Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Jjcmcom 0415

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 27

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication

Managing Impressions Online:


Self-Presentation Processes in the Online
Dating Environment
Nicole Ellison

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Department of Telecommunication, Information Studies, and Media
Michigan State University

Rebecca Heino
McDonough School of Business
Georgetown University

Jennifer Gibbs
Communication Department
Rutgers University

This study investigates self-presentation strategies among online dating participants,


exploring how participants manage their online presentation of self in order to accom-
plish the goal of finding a romantic partner. Thirty-four individuals active on a large
online dating site participated in telephone interviews about their online dating experi-
ences and perceptions. Qualitative data analysis suggests that participants attended to
small cues online, mediated the tension between impression management pressures and
the desire to present an authentic sense of self through tactics such as creating a profile
that reflected their ‘‘ideal self,’’ and attempted to establish the veracity of their identity
claims. This study provides empirical support for Social Information Processing theory
in a naturalistic context while offering insight into the complicated way in which ‘‘hon-
esty’’ is enacted online.

doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00020.x

Introduction
The online dating arena represents an opportunity to document changing cultural
norms surrounding technology-mediated relationship formation and to gain
insight into important aspects of online behavior, such as impression formation
and self-presentation strategies. Mixed-mode relationships, wherein people first
meet online and then move offline, challenge established theories that focus on

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 415
exclusively online relationships and provide opportunities for new theory develop-
ment (Walther & Parks, 2002). Although previous research has explored relation-
ship development and self-presentation online (Bargh, McKenna, & Fitzsimons,
2002; McLaughlin, Osbourne, & Ellison, 1997; Parks & Floyd, 1996; Roberts &
Parks, 1999; Utz, 2000), the online dating forum is qualitatively different from many
other online settings due to the anticipation of face-to-face interaction inherent in
this context (Gibbs, Ellison, & Heino, 2006) and the fact that social practices are still
nascent.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


In recent years, the use of online dating or online personals services has evolved
from a marginal to a mainstream social practice. In 2003, at least 29 million
Americans (two out of five singles) used an online dating service (Gershberg,
2004); in 2004, on average, there were 40 million unique visitors to online dating
sites each month in the U.S. (CBC News, 2004). In fact, the online personals category
is one of the most lucrative forms of paid content on the web in the United States
(Egan, 2003) and the online dating market is expected to reach $642 million in 2008
(Greenspan, 2003). Ubiquitous access to the Internet, the diminished social stigma
associated with online dating, and the affordable cost of Internet matchmaking
services contribute to the increasingly common perception that online dating is
a viable, efficient way to meet dating or long-term relationship partners (St. John,
2002). Mediated matchmaking is certainly not a new phenomenon: Newspaper
personal advertisements have existed since the mid-19th century (Schaefer, 2003)
and video dating was popular in the 1980s (Woll & Cosby, 1987; Woll & Young,
1989). Although scholars working in a variety of academic disciplines have studied
these earlier forms of mediated matchmaking (e.g., Ahuvia & Adelman, 1992; Lynn
& Bolig, 1985; Woll, 1986; Woll & Cosby, 1987), current Internet dating services are
substantively different from these incarnations due to their larger user base and more
sophisticated self-presentation options.
Contemporary theoretical perspectives allow us to advance our understanding of
how the age-old process of mate-finding is transformed through online strategies
and behaviors. For instance, Social Information Processing (SIP) theory and other
frameworks help illuminate computer-mediated communication (CMC), interper-
sonal communication, and impression management processes. This article focuses
on the ways in which CMC interactants manage their online self-presentation and
contributes to our knowledge of these processes by examining these issues in the
naturalistic context of online dating, using qualitative data gathered from in-depth
interviews with online dating participants.

Literature Review
In contrast to a technologically deterministic perspective that focuses on the char-
acteristics of the technologies themselves, or a socially deterministic approach
that privileges user behavior, this article reflects a social shaping perspective. Social
shaping of technology approaches (Dutton, 1996; MacKenzie & Wajcman, 1985;

416 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Woolgar, 1996) acknowledge the ways in which information and communication
technologies (ICTs) both shape and are shaped by social practices. As Dutton points
out, ‘‘technologies can open, close, and otherwise shape social choices, although not
always in the ways expected on the basis of rationally extrapolating from the per-
ceived properties of technology’’ (1996, p. 9). One specific framework that reflects
this approach is Howard’s (2004) embedded media perspective, which acknowledges
both the capacities and the constraints of ICTs. Capacities are those aspects of
technology that enhance our ability to connect with one another, enact change,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


and so forth; constraints are those aspects of technology that hinder our ability to
achieve these goals. An important aspect of technology use, which is mentioned but
not explicitly highlighted in Howard’s framework, is the notion of circumvention,
which describes the specific strategies employed by individuals to exploit the capaci-
ties and minimize the constraints associated with their use of ICTs. Although the
notion of circumvention is certainly not new to CMC researchers, this article seeks to
highlight the importance of circumvention practices when studying the social aspects
of technology use.1

Self-Presentation and Self-Disclosure in Online and Offline Contexts


Self-presentation and self-disclosure processes are important aspects of relational
development in offline settings (Taylor & Altman, 1987), especially in early stages.
Goffman’s work on self-presentation explicates the ways in which an individual may
engage in strategic activities ‘‘to convey an impression to others which it is in his
interests to convey’’ (1959, p. 4). These impression-management behaviors consist of
expressions given (communication in the traditional sense, e.g., spoken communi-
cation) and expressions given off (presumably unintentional communication, such as
nonverbal communication cues). Self-presentation strategies are especially impor-
tant during relationship initiation, as others will use this information to decide
whether to pursue a relationship (Derlega, Winstead, Wong, & Greenspan, 1987).
Research suggests that when individuals expect to meet a potential dating partner for
the first time, they will alter their self-presentational behavior in accordance with the
values desired by the prospective date (Rowatt, Cunningham, & Druen, 1998). Even
when interacting with strangers, individuals tend to engage in self-enhancement
(Schlenker & Pontari, 2000).
However, research suggests that pressures to highlight one’s positive attributes
are experienced in tandem with the need to present one’s true (or authentic) self to
others, especially in significant relationships. Intimacy in relationships is linked to
feeling understood by one’s partner (Reis & Shaver, 1988) and develops ‘‘through
a dynamic process whereby an individual discloses personal information, thoughts,
and feelings to a partner; receives a response from the partner; and interprets
that response as understanding, validating, and caring’’ (Laurenceau, Barrett, &
Pietromonaco, 1998, p. 1238). Therefore, if participants aspire to an intimate rela-
tionship, their desire to feel understood by their interaction partners will motivate
self-disclosures that are open and honest as opposed to deceptive. This tension

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 417
between authenticity and impression management is inherent in many aspects of
self-disclosure. In making decisions about what and when to self-disclose, individ-
uals often struggle to reconcile opposing needs such as openness and autonomy
(Greene, Derlega, & Mathews, 2006).
Interactants in online environments experience these same pressures and desires,
but the greater control over self-presentational behavior in CMC allows individuals
to manage their online interactions more strategically. Due to the asynchronous
nature of CMC, and the fact that CMC emphasizes verbal and linguistic cues over

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


less controllable nonverbal communication cues, online self-presentation is more
malleable and subject to self-censorship than face-to-face self-presentation (Walther,
1996). In Goffman’s (1959) terms, more expressions of self are ‘‘given’’ rather than
‘‘given off.’’ This greater control over self-presentation does not necessarily lead to
misrepresentation online. Due to the ‘‘passing stranger’’ effect (Rubin, 1975) and the
visual anonymity present in CMC (Joinson, 2001), under certain conditions the
online medium may enable participants to express themselves more openly and
honestly than in face-to-face contexts.
A commonly accepted understanding of identity presumes that there are multi-
ple aspects of the self which are expressed or made salient in different contexts.
Higgins (1987) argues there are three domains of the self: the actual self (attributes
an individual possesses), the ideal self (attributes an individual would ideally pos-
sess), and the ought self (attributes an individual ought to possess); discrepancies
between one’s actual and ideal self are linked to feelings of dejection. Klohnen and
Mendelsohn (1998) determined that individuals’ descriptions of their ‘‘ideal self’’
influenced perceptions of their romantic partners in the direction of their ideal self-
conceptions. Bargh et al. (2002) found that in comparison to face-to-face interac-
tions, Internet interactions allowed individuals to better express aspects of their true
selves—aspects of themselves that they wanted to express but felt unable to. The
relative anonymity of online interactions and the lack of a shared social network
online may allow individuals to reveal potentially negative aspects of the self online
(Bargh et al., 2002).
Although self-presentation in personal web sites has been examined (Dominick,
1999; Schau & Gilly, 2003), the realm of online dating has not been studied as exten-
sively (for exceptions, see Baker, 2002; Fiore & Donath, 2004), and this constitutes
a gap in the current research on online self-presentation and disclosure. The online
dating realm differs from other CMC environments in crucial ways that may affect self-
presentational strategies. For instance, the anticipated future face-to-face interaction
inherent in most online dating interactions may diminish participants’ sense of visual
anonymity, an important variable in many online self-disclosure studies. An empirical
study of online dating participants found that those who anticipated greater face-to-
face interaction did feel that they were more open in their disclosures, and did not
suppress negative aspects of the self (Gibbs et al., 2006). In addition, because the goal of
many online dating participants is an intimate relationship, these individuals may be
more motivated to engage in authentic self-disclosures.

418 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Credibility Assessment and Demonstration in Online Self-Presentation
Misrepresentation in Online Environments
As discussed, online environments offer individuals an increased ability to
control their self-presentation, and therefore greater opportunities to engage in
misrepresentation (Cornwell & Lundgren, 2001). Concerns about the prospect of
online deception are common (Bowker & Tuffin, 2003; Donath, 1999; Donn &
Sherman, 2002), and narratives about identity deception have been reproduced in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


both academic and popular outlets (Joinson & Dietz-Uhler, 2002; Stone, 1996; Van
Gelder, 1996). Some theorists argue that CMC gives participants more freedom to
explore playful, fantastical online personae that differ from their ‘‘real life’’ identities
(Stone, 1996; Turkle, 1995). In certain online settings, such as online role-playing
games, a schism between one’s online representation and one’s offline identity are
inconsequential, even expected. For instance, MacKinnon (1995) notes that among
Usenet participants it is common practice to ‘‘forget’’ about the relationship between
actual identities and online personae.
The online dating environment is different, however, because participants are
typically seeking an intimate relationship and therefore desire agreement between
others’ online identity claims and offline identities. Online dating participants
report that deception is the ‘‘main perceived disadvantage of online dating’’ (Brym
& Lenton, 2001, p. 3) and see it as commonplace: A survey of one online dating
site’s participants found that 86% felt others misrepresented their physical appear-
ance (Gibbs et al., 2006). A 2001 research study found that over a quarter of online
dating participants reported misrepresenting some aspect of their identity, most
commonly age (14%), marital status (10%), and appearance (10%) (Brym & Lenton,
2001). Perceptions that others are lying may encourage reciprocal deception,
because users will exaggerate to the extent that they feel others are exaggerating or
deceiving (Fiore & Donath, 2004). Concerns about deception in this setting
have spawned related services that help online daters uncover inaccuracies in
others’ representations and run background checks on would-be suitors (Baertlein,
2004; Fernandez, 2005). One site, True.com, conducts background checks on their
users and has worked to introduce legislation that would force other online dating
sites to either conduct background checks on their users or display a disclaimer
(Lee, 2004).
The majority of online dating participants claim they are truthful (Gibbs et al.,
2006; Brym & Lenton, 2001), and research suggests that some of the technical and
social aspects of online dating may discourage deceptive communication. For
instance, anticipation of face-to-face communication influences self-representation
choices (Walther, 1994) and self-disclosures because individuals will more closely
monitor their disclosures as the perceived probability of future face-to-face interac-
tion increases (Berger, 1979) and will engage in more intentional or deliberate self-
disclosure (Gibbs et al., 2006). Additionally, Hancock, Thom-Santelli, and Ritchie
(2004) note that the design features of a medium may affect lying behaviors, and that

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 419
the use of recorded media (in which messages are archived in some fashion, such as
an online dating profile) will discourage lying. Also, online dating participants are
typically seeking a romantic partner, which may lower their motivation for mis-
representation compared to other online relationships. Further, Cornwell and
Lundgren (2001) found that individuals involved in online romantic relationships
were more likely to engage in misrepresentation than those involved in face-to-face
romantic relationships, but that this was directly related to the level of involvement.
That is, respondents were less involved in their cyberspace relationships and there-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


fore more likely to engage in misrepresentation. This lack of involvement is less likely
in relationships started in an online dating forum, especially sites that promote
marriage as a goal.
Public perceptions about the higher incidence of deception online are also
contradicted by research that suggests that lying is a typical occurrence in everyday
offline life (DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996), including situations
in which people are trying to impress prospective dates (Rowatt et al., 1998).
Additionally, empirical data about the true extent of misrepresentation in this
context is lacking. The current literature relies on self-reported data, and therefore
offers only limited insight into the extent to which misrepresentation may be
occurring. Hitsch, Hortacsu, and Ariely (2004) use creative techniques to address
this issue, such as comparing participants’ self-reported characteristics to patterns
found in national survey data, but no research to date has attempted to validate
participants’ self-reported assessments of the honesty of their self-descriptions.

Assessing and Demonstrating Credibility in CMC


The potential for misrepresentation online, combined with the time and effort
invested in face-to-face dates, make assessment strategies critical for online daters.
These assessment strategies may then influence participants’ self-presentational
strategies as they seek to prove their trustworthiness while simultaneously assessing
the credibility of others.
Online dating participants operate in an environment in which assessing the
identity of others is a complex and evolving process of reading signals and decon-
structing cues, using both active and passive strategies (Berger, 1979; Ramirez,
Walther, Burgoon, & Sunnafrank, 2002; Tidwell & Walther, 2002). SIP considers
how Internet users develop impressions of others, even with the limited cues avail-
able online, and suggests that interactants will adapt to the remaining cues in order
to make decisions about others (Walther, 1992; Walther, Anderson, & Park, 1994).
Online users look to small cues in order to develop impressions of others, such as
a poster’s email address (Donath, 1999), the links on a person’s homepage (Kibby,
1997), even the timing of email messages (Walther & Tidwell, 1995). In expressing
affinity, CMC users are adept at using language (Walther, Loh, & Granka, 2005) and
CMC-specific conventions, especially as they become more experienced online (Utz,
2000). In short, online users become cognitive misers, forming impressions of others
while conserving mental energy (Wallace, 1999).

420 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Walther and Parks (2002) propose the concept of ‘‘warranting’’ as a useful con-
ceptual tool for understanding how users validate others’ online identity cues (see
also Stone, 1996). The connection, or warrant, between one’s self-reported online
persona and one’s offline aspects of self is less certain and more mutable than in face-
to-face settings (Walther & Parks, 2002). In online settings, users will look for signals
that are difficult to mimic or govern in order to assess others’ identity claims
(Donath, 1999). For instance, individuals might use search engines to locate news-
group postings by the person under scrutiny, knowing that this searching is covert

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


and that the newsgroup postings most likely were authored without the realization
that they would be archived (Ramirez et al., 2002). In the context of online dating,
because of the perceptions of deception that characterize this sphere and the self-
reported nature of individuals’ profiles, participants may adopt specific presentation
strategies geared towards providing warrants for their identity claims.
In light of the above, our research question is thus:
RQ: How do online dating participants manage their online presentation of self in order to
accomplish the goal of finding a romantic partner?

Method
In order to gain insight into this question, we interviewed online dating participants
about their experiences, thoughts, and behaviors. The qualitative data reported in
this article were collected as part of a larger research project which surveyed a national
random sample of users of a large online dating site (N = 349) about relational goals,
honesty and self-disclosure, and perceived success in online dating. The survey
findings are reported in Gibbs et al. (2006).

Research Site
Our study addresses contemporary CMC theory using naturalistic observations.
Participants were members of a large online dating service, ‘‘Connect.com’’ (a
pseudonym). Connect.com currently has 15 million active members in more than
200 countries around the world and shares structural characteristics with many
other online dating services, offering users the ability to create profiles, search
others’ profiles, and communicate via a manufactured email address. In their
profiles, participants may include one or more photographs and a written
(open-ended) description of themselves and their desired mate. They also answer
a battery of closed-ended questions, with preset category-based answers, about
descriptors such as income, body type, religion, marital status, and alcohol usage.
Users can conduct database searches that generate a list of profiles that match their
desired parameters (usually gender, sexual orientation, age, and location). Initial
communication occurs through a double-blind email system, in which both email
addresses are masked, and participants usually move from this medium to others as
the relationship progresses.

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 421
Data Collection
Given the relative lack of prior research on the phenomenon of online dating, we
used qualitative methods to explore the diverse ways in which participants under-
stood and made sense of their experience (Berger & Luckman, 1980) through their
own rich descriptions and explanations (Miles & Huberman, 1994). We took an
inductive approach based on general research questions informed by literature on
online self-presentation and relationship formation rather than preset hypotheses. In
addition to asking about participants’ backgrounds, the interview protocol included

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


open-ended questions about their online dating history and goals, profile construc-
tion, honesty and self-disclosure online, criteria used to assess others online, and
relationship development. Interviews were semistructured to ensure that all partic-
ipants were asked certain questions and to encourage participants to raise other
issues they felt were relevant to the research. The protocol included questions such
as: ‘‘How did you decide what to say about yourself in your profile? Are you trying to
convey a certain impression of yourself with your profile? If you showed your profile
to one of your close friends, what do you think their response would be? Are there
any personal characteristics that you avoided mentioning or tried to deemphasize?’’
(The full protocol is available from the authors.)
As recommended for qualitative research (Eisenhardt, 1989; Glaser & Strauss,
1967), we employed theoretical sampling rather than random sampling. In theoret-
ical sampling, cases are chosen based on theoretical (developed a priori) categories to
provide examples of polar types, rather than for statistical generalizability to a larger
population (Eisenhardt, 1989). The Director of Market Research at Connect.com
initially contacted a subsample of members in the Los Angeles and San Francisco Bay
areas, inviting them to participate in an interview and offering them a free one-
month subscription to Connect.com in return. Those members who did not respond
within a week received a reminder email. Of those contacted, 76 people volunteered
to participate in an interview. Out of these 76 volunteers, we selected and scheduled
interviews with 36 (although two were unable to participate due to scheduling
issues). We chose interview participants to ensure a good mix on each of our
theoretical categories: gender, age, urban/rural, income, and ethnicity. We focused
exclusively on those seeking relationships with the opposite sex, as this group con-
stitutes the majority of Connect.com users. We also confirmed that they were active
participants in the site by ensuring that their last login date was within the past week
and checking that each had a profile.
Fifty percent of our participants were female and 50% were male, with 76% from
an urban location in Los Angeles and 24% from a more rural area surrounding the
town of Modesto in the central valley of California. Participants’ ages ranged from 25
to 70, with most being in their 30s and 40s. Their online dating experience varied
from 1 month to 5 years. Although our goal was to sample a mix of participants who
varied on key demographic criteria rather than generalizing to a larger population,
our sample is in fact reflective of the demographic characteristics of the larger
population of Connect.com’s subscribers. Thirty-four interviews were conducted

422 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
in June and July 2003. Interviews were conducted by telephone, averaging 45 minutes
and ranging from 30 to 90 minutes in length. The interview database consisted of 551
pages, including 223,001 words, with an average of 6559 words per interview.

Data Analysis
All of the phone interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed, and checked for accu-
racy by the researcher who conducted the interview. Atlas.ti, a software program
used for qualitative content analysis, was used to analyze interview transcripts. Data

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


analysis was conducted in an iterative process, in which data from one informant
were confirmed or contradicted by data from others in order to refine theoretical
categories, propositions, and conclusions as they emerged from the data (Lincoln &
Guba, 1985). We used microanalysis of the text (Strauss & Corbin, 1998) to look for
common themes among participants. The data analysis process consisted of system-
atic line-by-line coding of each transcript by the first two authors. Following
grounded theory (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), we used an iterative process of coding.
Coding consisted of both factual codes (e.g., ‘‘age,’’ ‘‘female,’’ ‘‘Los Angeles’’) and
referential codes (e.g., ‘‘filter,’’ ‘‘rejection,’’ ‘‘honesty’’) and served both to simplify
and reduce data as well as to complicate data by expanding, transforming, and
reconceptualizing concepts (Coffey & Atkinson, 1996). New codes were added
throughout the process, and then earlier transcripts were recoded to include these
new conceptual categories. All of the data were coded twice to ensure thoroughness
and accuracy of codes. The researchers had frequent discussions in which they
compared and refined coding categories and schemes to ensure consistency. During
the coding process, some codes were collapsed or removed when they appeared to be
conceptually identical, while others were broken out into separate codes when fur-
ther nuances among them became apparent.
A total of 98 codes were generated by the first two authors as they coded the
interviews. Unitization was flexible in order to capture complete thought units.
Codes were allowed to overlap (Krippendorff, 1980); this method of assigning
multiple codes to the same thought unit facilitated the process of identifying
relationships between codes. See Appendixes A and B for more information on
codes.

Findings
These interview data offer insight into the self-presentation strategies utilized by
participants in order to maximize the benefits and minimize the risks of online
dating. Many of these strategies revolved around the profile, which is a crucial
self-presentation tool because it is the first and primary means of expressing one’s
self during the early stages of a correspondence and can therefore foreclose or create
relationship opportunities. These strategies are intimately connected to the specific
characteristics of the online dating context: fewer cues, an increased ability to man-
age self-presentation, and the need to establish credibility.

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 423
The Importance of Small Cues
When discussing their self-presentational strategies, many participants directly or
indirectly referred to the fact that they carefully attended to subtle, almost minute
cues in others’ presentational messages, and often seemed to take the same degree of
care when crafting their own messages. As suggested by SIP (Walther, 1992), subtle
cues such as misspellings in the online environment are important clues to identity
for CMC interactants. For instance, one participant said she looked for profiles that
were well-written, because ‘‘I just think if they can’t spell or . . . formulate sentences,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


I would imagine that they’re not that educated.’’ Because writing ability was
perceived to be a cue that was ‘‘given off’’ or not as controllable, participants noticed
misspelled words in profiles, interpreting them as evidence of lack of interest or
education. As one female participant put it, ‘‘If I am getting email from someone
that obviously can’t spell or put a full sentence together, I’m thinking what other
parts of his life suffer from the same lack of attentiveness?’’ These individuals often
created their own profiles with these concerns in mind. For instance, one participant
who found spelling errors ‘‘unattractive’’ composed his emails in a word processing
program to check spelling and grammar.
Many of the individuals we interviewed explicitly considered how others might
interpret their profiles and carefully assessed the signals each small action or com-
ment might send:
I really analyzed the way I was going to present myself. I’m not one of these
[people who write] all cutesy type things, but I wanted to be cute enough, smart
enough, funny enough, and not sexual at all, because I didn’t want to invite
someone who thought I was going to go to bed with them [as soon as] I shook
their hand. (PaliToWW, Los Angeles Female)2
In this case, the participant ‘‘really analyzed’’ her self-presentation cues and
avoided any mention of sexuality, which she felt might indicate promiscuity in the
exaggerated context of the profile. This same understanding of the signals ‘‘sexual’’
references would send was reflected in the profile of another participant, who pur-
posefully included sexually explicit terminology in his profile to ‘‘weed out’’ poor
matches based on his past experience:
The reason I put [the language] in there is because I had some experiences
where I got together [with someone], we both really liked each other, and
then it turned out that I was somebody who really liked sex and she was
somebody that could take it or leave it. So I put that in there to sort of weed
those people out. (imdannyboy, Los Angeles Male)
Participants spoke of the ways in which they incorporated feedback from others
in order to shape their self-presentational messages. In some cases, they seemed
genuinely surprised by the ways in which the digital medium allowed information
to leak out. For instance, one male participant who typically wrote emails late at
night discussed his reaction to a message that said, ‘‘Wow, it’s 1:18 in the morning,

424 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
what are you doing writing me?’’ This email helped him realize how much of a ‘‘night
owl’’ he was, and ‘‘how not attractive that may be for women I’m writing because it’s
very clear the time I send the email.’’ Over time, he also realized that the length of his
emails was shaping impressions of him, and he therefore regulated their length. He
said:
In the course of [corresponding with others on the site] I became aware of
how I had to present myself. Also, I became quite aware that I had to be very

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


brief. . . . More often than not when I would write a long response, I wouldn’t
get a response. . . . I think it implied. . . . that I was too desperate for
conversation, [that] I was a hermit. (joet8, Los Angeles Male)
The site displayed the last time a user was active on the site, and this small cue
was interpreted as a reliable indicator of availability. As one male participant said,
‘‘I’m not going to email somebody who hasn’t been on there for at least a week max.
If it’s been two weeks since she’s logged on, forget her, she’s either dating or there’s
a problem.’’
Overall, the mediated nature of these initial interactions meant that fewer cues
were available, therefore amplifying the importance of those that remained. Partic-
ipants carefully attended to small cues, such as spelling ability or last login date, in
others’ profiles in order to form impressions. In a self-reflexive fashion, they applied
these techniques to their own presentational messages, carefully scrutinizing both
cues given (such as photograph) and, when possible, those perceived to be given off
(such as grammar).

Balancing Accuracy and Desirability in Self-Presentation


Almost all of our participants reported that they attempted to represent themselves
accurately in their profiles and interactions. Many expressed incomprehension as to
why others with a shared goal of an offline romantic relationship would intentionally
misrepresent themselves. As one participant explained, ‘‘They polish it up some, like
we all probably do a little bit, but for the most part I would say people are fairly
straightforward.’’ However, as suggested by previous research on self-disclosure and
relationship development, participants reported competing desires. At times, their
need to portray a truthful, accurate self-representation was in tension with their
natural inclination to project a version of self that was attractive, successful, and
desirable. Speaking about this tendency towards impression management, one par-
ticipant noted that she could see why ‘‘people would be dishonest at some point
because they are still trying to be attractive . . . in the sense they would want this
other person to like them.’’

Ideal Self
One way in which participants reconciled their conflicting needs for positive self-
presentation and accuracy was to create profiles that described a potential, future

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 425
version of self. In some cases, participants described how they or others created
profiles that reflected an ideal as opposed to actual self: ‘‘Many people describe
themselves the way they want [to be] . . . their ideal themselves.’’ For example,
individuals might identify themselves as active in various activities (e.g., hiking,
surfing) in which they rarely participated, prompting one participant to proclaim
sarcastically, ‘‘I’ve never known so many incredibly athletic women in my life!’’ One
participant explained,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


For instance, I am also an avid hiker and [scuba diver] and sometimes I have
communicated with someone that has presented themselves the same way, but
then it turns out they like scuba diving but they haven’t done it for 10 years,
they like hiking but they do it once every second year . . . I think they may
not have tried to lie; they just have perceived themselves differently because
they write about the person they want to be . . . In their profile they write about
their dreams as if they are reality. (Christo1, Los Angeles Male)
In two cases, individuals admitted to representing themselves as less heavy than
they actually were. This thinner persona represented a (desired) future state for these
individuals: ‘‘The only thing I kind of feel bad about is that the picture I have of
myself is a very good picture from maybe five years ago. I’ve gained a little bit of
weight and I feel kind of bad about that. I’m going to, you know, lose it again.’’ In
another case, a woman who misrepresented her weight online used an upcoming
meeting as incentive to minimize the discrepancy between her actual self and the
ideal self articulated in her profile:
I’ve lost 44 pounds since I’ve started [online dating], and I mean, that’s one
of the reasons I lost the weight so I can thank online dating for that. [Because]
the first guy that hit on me, I checked my profile and I had lied a little bit
about the pounds, so I thought I had better start losing some weight so that it
would be more honest. That was in December, and I’ve lost every week since
then. (MaryMoon, Los Angeles Female)
In this case, a later physical change neutralized the initial discursive deception.
For another participant, the profile served as an opportunity to envision and ideate
a version of self that was future-focused and goal-oriented:
I sort of thought about what is my ideal self. Because when you date, you
present your best foot forward. I thought about all the qualities that I have, you
know, even if I sometimes make mistakes and stuff. . . . And also got together
the best picture I had, and kind of came up with what I thought my goals
were at the time, because I thought that was an important thing to stress.
(Marty7, Los Angeles Male)
Overall, participants did not see this as engaging in deceptive communication per
se, but rather as presenting an idealized self or portraying personal qualities they
intended to develop or enhance.

426 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Circumventing Constraints
In addition to impression management pressures, participants’ expressed desires for
accurate representation were stymied by various constraints, including the technical
interface of the website. In order to activate an online profile, participants had to
complete a questionnaire with many closed-ended responses for descriptors such as
age, body type, zip code, and income. These answers became very important because
they were the variables that others used to construct searches in order to narrow the
vast pool of profiles. In fact, the front page of Connect.com includes a ‘‘quick’’ search

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


on those descriptors believed to be most important: age, geographical location,
inclusion of photograph, and gender/sexual orientation.
The structure of the search parameters encouraged some to alter information to
fit into a wider range of search parameters, a circumvention behavior that guaran-
teed a wider audience for their profile. For example, participants tended to mis-
represent their age for fear of being ‘‘filtered out.’’ It was not unusual for users who
were one or two years older than a natural breakpoint (i.e., 35 or 50) to adjust their
age so they would still show up in search results. This behavior, especially if one’s
actual age was revealed during subsequent email or telephone exchanges, seemed to
be socially acceptable. Many of our participants recounted cases in which others
freely and without embarrassment admitted that they had slightly misrepresented
something in their profile, typically very early in the correspondence:
They don’t seem to be embarrassed about [misrepresenting their age] . . . in
their first reply they say, ‘‘oh by the way, I am not so many years, I am that
many years.’’ And then if I ask them, they say, well, they tend to be attracted to
a little bit younger crowd and they are afraid that guys may surf for a certain age
group of women, because you use those filters. I mean, I may choose to list
only those that are between X and Y years old and they don’t want to be filtered
away. . . . They are trying to be sort of clever so that people they tend to be
attracted to will actually find them. (Christo1, Los Angeles Male)
If lying about one’s age was perceived to be the norm, those who didn’t engage
in this practice felt themselves to be at a disadvantage (see Fiore & Donath, 2004).
For instance, one participant who misrepresented his age on his profile noted:
I’m such an honest guy, why should I have to lie about my age? On the other
hand, if I put X number of years, that is unattractive to certain people. They’re
never going to search that group and they’re never going to have an
opportunity to meet me, because they have a number in their mind just like
I do. . . . Everybody lies about their age or a lot of people do. . . . So I have to
cheat too in order to be on the same page as everybody else that cheats.
If I don’t cheat that makes me seem twice as old. So if I say I am 44,
people think that I am 48. It blows. (RealSweetheart, Bay Area Male)
In the above cases, users engaged in misrepresentation triggered by the social
norms of the environment and the structure of the search filters. The technical

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 427
constraints of the site may have initiated a more subtle form of misrepresentation
when participants were required to choose among a limited set of options, none of
which described them sufficiently. For instance, when creating their profiles, partic-
ipants had to designate their ‘‘perfect date’’ by selecting one from a dozen or so
generic descriptions, which was frustrating for those who did not see any that were
particularly appealing. In another case, one participant complained that there was
not an option to check ‘‘plastic surgery’’ as one of his ‘‘turn-offs’’ and thus he felt
forced to try to discern this from the photos; yet another participant expressed his

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


desire for a ‘‘shaved’’ option under the description of hair type (‘‘I resent having to
check ‘bald’’’).

Foggy Mirror
In addition to the cases in which misrepresentation was triggered by technical con-
straints or the tendency to present an idealized self, participants described a third
branch of unintentional misrepresentation triggered by the limits of self-knowledge.
We call this phenomenon ‘‘foggy mirror’’ based on this participant’s explanation:
People like to write about themselves. Sometimes it’s not truthful, but it’s how
they see themselves and that gives you a different slant on an individual. This is
how they really see themselves. Sometimes you will see a person who weighs 900
pounds and—this is just an exaggeration—and they will have on spandex,
you’ll think, ‘‘God, I wish I had their mirror, because obviously their mirror
tells them they look great.’’ It’s the same thing with online. (KarieK, Bay Area
Female)
This user acknowledges that sometimes others weren’t lying per se, but the fact
that their self-image differed from others’ perceptions meant that their textual self-
descriptions would diverge from a third party’s description. In explaining this phe-
nomenon, KarieK used the metaphor of a mirror to emphasize the self-reflexive
nature of the profile. She also refers to the importance of subtle cues when she notes
that a user’s self-presentation choices give one a ‘‘different slant on an individual.’’
The term ‘‘foggy mirror’’ thus describes the gap between self-perceptions and the
assessments made by others. The difference might be overly positive (which was
typically the case) or negative, as the below example illustrates. A male participant
explained:
There was one gal who said that she had an ‘‘average’’ body shape. . . . When I
met her she was thin, and she said she was ‘‘average,’’ but I think she has
a different concept of what ‘‘average’’ is. So I then widened my scope [in terms
of search parameters] and would go off the photographs. What a woman thinks
is an ‘‘average’’ body and what I think is an ‘‘average’’ body are two different
things. (joet8, Los Angeles Male)
In this case, the participant acknowledged the semantic problems that accom-
pany textual self-descriptions and adopted a strategy of relying on photographs

428 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
as visual, objective evidence, instead of subjective, ambiguous terms like ‘‘aver-
age.’’ To counter the ‘‘foggy mirror’’ syndrome in their own profiles, some indi-
viduals asked friends or family members to read their profiles in order to validate
them.
In regards to self-presentation, the most significant tension experienced by par-
ticipants was one not unique to the online medium: mediating between the pressures
to present an enhanced or desired self (Goffman, 1959) and the need to present one’s
true self to a partner in order to achieve intimacy (Reis & Shaver, 1988). In their

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


profiles and online interactions, they attempted to present a vision of self that was
attractive, engaging, and worthy of pursuit, but realistic and honest enough that
subsequent face-to-face meetings were not unpleasant or surprising. Constructing
a profile that reflected one’s ‘‘ideal self’’ (Higgins, 1987) was one tactic by which
participants reconciled these pressures. In general, although all of our participants
claimed they attempted to be honest in their self-presentation, misrepresentations
occurred when participants felt pressure to fudge in order to circumvent the search
filters, felt the closed-ended options provided by the site didn’t describe them accu-
rately, or were limited by their self-knowledge.

Establishing Credibility
The increased ability to engage in selective self-presentation, and the absence of
visual cues in the online environment, meant that accuracy of self-presentation
was a salient issue for our interviewees. The twin concerns that resulted from these
factors—the challenge of establishing the credibility of one’s own self-descriptions
while assessing the credibility of others’ identity claims—affected one another in
a recursive fashion. In an environment in which there were limited outside confir-
matory resources to draw upon, participants developed a set of rules for assessing
others while incorporating these codes into their own self-presentational messages.
For example, one participant made sure that her profile photograph showed her
standing up because she felt that sitting or leaning poses were a camouflage technique
used by heavier people. This illustrates the recursive way in which participants
developed rules for assessing others (e.g., avoid people in sitting poses) while also
applying these rubrics to their own self-presentational messages (e.g., don’t show self
in sitting pose).
Participants adopted specific tactics in order to compensate for the fact that
traditional methods of information seeking were limited and that self-reported
descriptions were subject to intentional or unintentional misrepresentation when
others took advantage of the ‘‘selective self-presentation’’ (Walther & Burgoon,
1992) available in CMC. As one participant noted, ‘‘You’re just kind of blind, you
don’t know if what they’re saying in their profile online is true.’’ Acknowledging the
potential for misrepresentation, participants also sought to ‘‘show’’ aspects of their
personality in their profiles versus just ‘‘telling’’ others about themselves. They cre-
ated their profiles with an eye towards stories or content that confirmed specific
personality traits rather than including a ‘laundry list’ of attributes. As one Los

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 429
Angeles male participant explained, ‘‘I attempted to have stories in my profile
somewhat to attempt to demonstrate my character, as opposed to, you know, [just
writing] ‘I’m trustworthy,’ and all that bit.’’ This emphasis on demonstration as
opposed to description was a tactic designed to circumvent the lack of a shared
social context that would have warranted identity claims and hedged against blatant
deception.
Another aspect of ‘‘showing’’ included the use of photographs, which served
to warrant or support claims made in textual descriptions. Profile photographs

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


communicated not only what people looked like (or claimed to look like), but
also indicated the qualities they felt were important. For instance, one man with
a doctorate included one photo of himself standing against a wall displaying his
diplomas and another of him shirtless. When asked about his choice of photos, he
explained that he selected the shirtless photo because he was proud of being in shape
and wanted to show it off. He picked the combination of the two photos because
‘‘one is sort of [my] intellectual side and one is sort of the athletic side.’’ In this case,
the photos functioned on multiple levels: To communicate physical characteristics,
but also self-concept (the aspects of self he was most proud of), and as an attempt to
provide evidence for his discursive claims (his profile listed an advanced degree and
an athletic physique).
To summarize, our data suggest that participants were cognizant of the online
setting and its association with deceptive communication practices, and therefore
worked to present themselves as credible. In doing so, they drew upon the rules they
had developed for assessing others and turned these practices into guidelines for their
own self-presentational messages.

Discussion
The primary goal of the online dating participants interviewed for this study was
to find someone with whom they could establish a dating relationship (although
desired commitment level and type of relationship varied across participants).
Given this, they attempted to achieve their goals while contending with the
unique characteristics of the online environment, engaging in strategies designed
to circumvent the constraints of the online dating environment while exploiting
its capacities. One constraint—the lack of nonverbal cues—meant that the task
of interpreting the remaining cues became paramount in regards to both assess-
ment of others and presentation of self. Since the goal of most online dating
participants was to identify and interact with potential romantic partners, indi-
viduals strove to highlight their positive attributes and capitalize on the greater per-
ceived control over self-presentation inherent in the medium. However, the future
face-to-face interaction they anticipated meant that individuals had to balance
their desire for self-promotion with their need for accurate self-presentation. In
response to the risk of misrepresentation online, made possible by the selective
self-presentation affordances of CMC, participants adopted various strategies to

430 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
demonstrate the credibility of their identity claims, recursively applying the same
techniques they employed to uncover representational ruses in others. Our find-
ings suggest that participants consistently engaged in creative workarounds (cir-
cumvention strategies) as they went through the process of posting a profile,
selecting individuals to contact, and communicating with potential romantic
partners. Our data also highlight the recursive process by which some partic-
ipants constructed rules of thumb for assessing others (e.g., an inactive account
indicates a lack of availability or interest) while simultaneously incorporating

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


these rules in their own messages (e.g., frequently making slight adjustments
to the profile).

Theoretical Implications
As individuals make initial decisions about potential partners, they form impressions
that help reduce uncertainty about the other (Berger & Calabrese, 1975). For this to
happen in the context of CMC, SIP argues, individuals will adapt their behaviors to
the cues that are available (Tidwell & Walther, 2002; Walther, 1992) to convey
information to one another. While empirical support for SIP has been demonstrated
(see Walther & Parks, 2002 for a review), this article is among the few to provide
evidence for SIP in a naturalistic setting. Our data show that in the initial interactions
of online dating participants, stylistic aspects of messages such as timing, length, and
grammar appear equally as important as the content of the message itself; this is
consistent with SIP’s formulation that when nonverbal cues are decreased, the
remaining cues become more salient to users. Previous laboratory studies of SIP
have tended to focus on the manipulation of a subset of cues. A unique contribution
of this study’s extension of SIP is its demonstration of the organic interplay of these
alternative sources of social information online.
Although much of the public debate about online dating has centered on the
medium’s inability to ensure participants’ truthful self-descriptions, our interview
data suggest that the notion that people frequently, explicitly, and intentionally ‘‘lie’’
online is simplistic and inaccurate. Exploring the question of whether participants
created a playful or fantastical identity online (Stone, 1996; Turkle, 1995) or were
more open and honest (Rubin, 1975), we found that the online dating participants
we spoke with claimed that they attempted to present an accurate self-representation
online, a finding echoed in our survey data (Gibbs et al., 2006). This study highlights
the fact that creating an accurate online representation of self in this context is
a complex and evolving process in which participants attempt to attract desirable
partners while contending with constraints such as those posed by technological
design and the limits of self-knowledge.
In some cases, the technical constraints of the site may have unintentionally
enabled acts of misrepresentation, for instance when participants slightly altered
information in situations in which they felt an arbitrary data point (in age, for
example) would significantly harm their chances of being discovered by a potential
mate. Additionally, self-reported descriptions that use subjective terms (e.g., ‘‘pretty’’

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 431
or ‘‘average’’) could also result in unintentional misrepresentation due to different
interpretations of these terms. Additionally, as Shah and Kesan point out, ‘‘Defaults
have a legitimating effect, because they carry information about what most people
are expected to do’’ (2003, p. 7). In the case of online dating, it may be that the
default settings in the search field (i.e., an age range, whether searches are limited to
profiles with photographs) influence user perceptions of the desirability or appro-
priateness of certain responses.
Additionally, our interview data suggest that online representations of one’s ideal

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


self—when combined with the increased accountability engendered by an antici-
pated face-to-face interaction—may serve as a tool to enable individuals to minimize
the discrepancy between their actual and their ideal selves. The ideal self refers to
qualities or achievements one strives to possess in the future (Bargh et al., 2002). In
the realm of online dating, it is interesting that participants reported using the profile
to ideate a version of self they desired to experience in the future. For some, the act of
constructing an online profile may begin a process of self-growth as they strive to
close the gap between actual and ideal self, such as the woman who misrepresented
her weight but then was able to achieve her goal of weight loss over time. Future
research is needed to assess the extent to which this phenomenon exists and its long-
term consequences for processes of self-growth.
More research is also needed to understand fully whether strategies designed to
circumvent constraints (technical or other) are perceived to be deceptive by users
and, if so, which norms govern their use. The literature on deception explores a wide
range of deceptive acts, ranging from the more mundane ‘‘diversionary responses’’ to
outright ‘‘lies’’ (Buller & Burgoon, 1994). Future research could work to develop
a taxonomy of online deception and acceptability, which takes into account the
nuances of social norms and the fact that some misrepresentation may be uninten-
tional or socially accepted. For instance, if a profile includes incorrect information
that is rectified immediately over email, is it a ‘‘lie?’’ More importantly, is it accept-
able? Also, more research is needed to understand more clearly the extent and
substance of participants’ actual concerns regarding online dating (i.e., misrepresen-
tation, effectiveness, safety) and how they overlap with the often sensationalized
discourse about online deception as represented in media accounts and social
narratives.

Practical Implications
Given that deceptive practices are a concern for online dating participants, future
research should explore the ways in which online dating sites could implement
design features aimed at addressing these issues. For instance, they could acknowl-
edge and incorporate aspects of a shared social context, similar to social networking
sites like Friendster (Donath & boyd, 2004), through the use of testimonials or social
network visualizations. Online dating sites could adopt some of the design features
used in e-commerce sites, such as testimonials, user rating systems, or social network
visualizations, where participants also must operate in an uncertain environment in

432 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
which warranting is difficult and deception can be costly (Resnick & Zeckhauser,
2002).
A second design consideration is the possibility that the technical characteristics
of some online dating sites may privilege objective characteristics (such as demo-
graphic features) and de-emphasize the process of seeing others as individuals rather
than as amalgams of various traits. The benefit, or capacity, of online dating is that
participants can use specific search parameters to cull a subset of profiles from
a larger database. Participants acknowledged that the online dating environment

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


placed more emphasis on certain kinds of information—information that might
not be very important in a face-to-face setting when chemistry was already estab-
lished. To compensate for or to circumvent these constraints, participants tried to
create profiles that stood out or evidenced aspects of self that they were particularly
proud of rather than a laundry list of features. They struggled to present themselves
as unique individuals within the constraints of a technical system that encouraged
homogeneity, negotiating a desire to stand out with the need to blend in. Future
research might examine the potential for developing self-presentation tools that
allow individuals more nuanced ways of expressing themselves in the online envi-
ronment, such as video presentations, more sophisticated communication tools, or
triangulated information from others on the site. Online dating sites may need to
reconsider the ways in which profiles are structured and the characteristics they
include; as Fiore and Donath argue, ‘‘the features of a person that Match.com
presents as salient to romance will begin to have some psychological and cultural
influences if 40 million Americans view them every month’’ (2004, p. 1395). If we
accept this claim, then it stands to reason that participants’ visions of self may
be impacted by their online self-presentations, especially if these presentations are
constrained.

Limitations
We chose to conduct interviews with online dating participants in order to gain
insight into how they perceived their experiences and the processes through which
they learned to avoid the pitfalls and exploit the possibilities of online dating.
However, there are several limitations that should be acknowledged in our method
and sample. Limitations of this study include the sampling of only participants
located on the West Coast. While Connect.com members are worldwide, we cannot
assess if regional or national differences affect the online dating experience. A major
limitation is the potential for self-selection bias, as participants volunteered for the
study. While demographically diverse, those that chose to volunteer might be biased
toward a more positive outlook on online dating or potentially more honest in their
online dating practices.
In addition, the self-reported nature of the data may have resulted in a social
desirability bias, making participants less likely to admit to intentional misrepresen-
tation. Finally, many of our findings may be specific to Connect.com’s model of
online dating, in which participants post profiles and select with whom they want to

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 433
communicate. Other online dating sites, such as eHarmony, utilize a very different
model, acting as online matchmakers where individuals who are found to be com-
patible are paired based on personality tests developed by ‘‘expert’’ psychologists.
Future research could assess whether variables like self-efficacy predict which model
users choose to utilize. Although our observations in this article were based on the
sample as a whole, we acknowledge that there may be differences (for instance, along
gender lines) which are beyond the scope of this article but which could be explored
in future research.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Although self-presentation and relationship formation have been studied in
other online contexts, tracing how these processes take place in the online dating
realm offers researchers unique insights into the crucial role of circumvention
techniques, the complicated nature of ‘‘honesty’’ in online environments, and the
social and psychological implications of the design and structure of these sites. From
a historical perspective, the goals of online dating participants are not that different
from those described by poets throughout the ages. What is different is the tools in
their repertoire and the constraints and opportunities they present. As O’Sullivan
writes, ‘‘From a functional perspective, it appears new technologies may be providing
nothing terribly new— just new ways of doing things that people have been doing
throughout the history of social interaction’’ (2000, p. 428). This study has attempted
to elucidate and explain some of these social practices as a window into the ways in
which new communication technologies are shaping us—and we are shaping
them—in the ongoing pursuit of romantic relationships.

Acknowledgments
The authors thank Karen Aroian, Ulla Bunz, Annika Hylmo, Edythe Hough, Patrick
O’Sullivan, Charles Steinfield, Joe Walther, and two anonymous reviewers for their
helpful comments and suggestions regarding this manuscript.

Notes
1 Similar to the concept of ‘‘workaround’’ employed by designers and software engineers,
users engage in circumvention strategies to neutralize constraints—or turn them into
capabilities. Prior CMC research has identified similar processes in interpersonal contexts.
For instance, O’Sullivan (2000) found that users chose mediated channels over face-
to-face communication in situations where a preferred impression was expected to be
violated in order to capitalize on the face-saving capabilities of mediated interaction.
Similarly, CMC researchers working in other contexts have noted the process by which
individuals adapt their behavior to compensate for the limitations imposed by the
medium in order to pursue their communication goals (Walther, Loh, & Granka, 2005).
2 All identifying information about our participants has been changed to protect their
confidentiality, although we have attempted to use pseudonyms that reflect the tone and
spirit of their chosen screen names. Additionally, at the request of our research site, we
have used a pseudonym in place of the site’s actual name.

434 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
References
Ahuvia, A. C., & Adelman, M. B. (1992). Formal intermediaries in the marriage market: A
typology and review. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 54(2), 452–463.
Baertlein, L. (2004). Demand for advice to online lovelorn is booming. BizReport. Retrieved
August 14, 2004, from http://www.bizreport.com/news/6516/
Baker, A. (2002). What makes an online relationship successful? Clues from couples who met
in cyberspace. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 5(4), 363–375.
Bargh, J. A., McKenna, K. Y., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2002). Can you see the real me?

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Activation and expression of the ‘‘true self’’ on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1),
33–48.
Berger, C. R. (1979). Beyond initial interaction: Uncertainty, understanding and development
of interpersonal relationships. In H. Giles & R. St. Clair (Eds.), Language and Social
Psychology (pp. 122–144). Baltimore: University Park Press.
Berger, C., & Calabrese, R. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond:
Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human
Communication Research, 1(2), 99–112.
Berger, P. L., & Luckman, T. (1980). The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the
Sociology of Knowledge. New York: Irvington Publishers.
Bowker, N., & Tuffin, K. (2003). Dicing with deception: People with disabilities’ strategies for
managing safety and identity online. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 8(2).
Retrieved January 25, 2006, from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol8/issue2/bowker.html
Brym, R. J., & Lenton, R. L. (2001). Love Online: A Report on Digital Dating in Canada.
Retrieved July 2, 2003, from http://www.nelson.com/nelson/harcourt/sociology/
newsociety3e/loveonline.pdf
Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Deception: Strategic and nonstrategic communication.
In J. A. Daly & J. M. Wiemann (Eds.), Strategic Interpersonal Communication
(pp. 191–223). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
CBC News. (2004). Online Dating Facts and Figures. Retrieved November 17, 2005, from
http://www.cbc.ca/consumers/market/files/services/onlinedating/facts.html
Coffey, A., & Atkinson, P. (1996). Making Sense of Qualitative Data: Complementary Research
Strategies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cornwell, B., & Lundgren, D. C. (2001). Love on the Internet: Involvement and
misrepresentation in romantic relationships in cyberspace vs. realspace. Computers in
Human Behavior, 17(2), 197–211.
DePaulo, B., Kashy, D., Kirkendol, S., Wyer, M., & Epstein, J. (1996). Lying in everyday life.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 979–995.
Derlega, V., Winstead, B., Wong, P., & Greenspan, M. (1987). Self-disclosure and relationship
development: An attributional analysis. In M. E. Roloff & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal
Processes: New Directions in Communication Research (pp. 172–187). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Dominick, J. R. (1999). Who do you think you are? Personal home pages and
self-presentation on the World Wide Web. Journalism and Mass Communication
Quarterly, 76(4), 646–658.
Donath, J. S. (1999). Identity and deception in the virtual community. In M. A. Smith &
P. Kollock (Eds.), Communities in Cyberspace (pp. 29–59). New York: Routledge.

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 435
Donath, J., & Boyd, d. (2004). Public displays of connection. BT Technology Journal, 22(4),
71–82.
Donn, J., & Sherman, R. (2002). Attitudes and practices regarding the formation of romantic
relationships on the Internet. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 5(2), 107–123.
Dutton, W. E. (1996). Information and Communication Technologies: Visions and Realities.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Egan, J. (2003, November 23). Love in the time of no time. The New York Times. Retrieved
November 25, 2003, from http://www.nytimes.com/2003/11/23/magazine/

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


23ONLINE.html?ex=1070719885&ei=1&en=fcd72235b67ffb79
Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). Building theories from case study research. Academy of
Management Review, 14(4), 532–550.
Fernandez, S. (2005, May 30). Getting to know you: Tell-all sites put online dater profiles to
truth test. The Washington Post. Retrieved June 5, 2005, from http://
www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2005/05/29/
AR2005052901071_pf.html
Fiore, A. T., & Donath, J. (2004). Online Personals: An Overview. Paper presented at the
meeting of ACM Computer-Human Interaction 2004, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved
October, 1, 2004, from http://smg.media.mit.edu/papers/atf/chi2004_personals_short.pdf
Gershberg, M. (2004, June 10). Funny odds of online dating. BizReport. Retrieved October 28,
2004, from http://www.bizreport.com/news/7383/
Gibbs, J. L., Ellison, N. B., & Heino, R. D. (2006). Self-presentation in online personals: The
role of anticipated future interaction, self-disclosure, and perceived success in Internet
dating. Communication Research, 33(2), 1–26.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for
Qualitative Research. Chicago: Aldine Publishing.
Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Anchor.
Greene, K., Derlega, V. L., & Mathews, A. (2006). Self-disclosure in personal relationships. In
A. Vangelisti & D. Perlman (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Greenspan, R. (2003). Socializing surfers shop for friends, dates. Clickz. Retrieved April 3,
2004, from http://www.clickz.com/news/article.php/3114251
Hancock, J., Thom-Santelli, J., & Ritchie, T. (2004). Deception and design: The impact of
communication technology on lying behavior. In E. Dykstra-Erickson & M. Tscheligi
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 2004 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems
(pp. 129–134). New York: ACM.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review,
94(3), 319–340.
Hitsch, G. J., Hortacsu, A., & Ariely, D. (2004). What makes you click: An empirical analysis
of online dating (Working Paper). Retrieved June 22, 2005, from http://
rover.cs.northwestern.edu/~surana/blog/extras/online_dating.pdf
Howard, P. N. (2004). Embedded media: Who we know, what we know and society online. In
P. N. Howard & S. Jones (Eds.), Society Online: The Internet in Context (pp. 1–28).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Joinson, A. N. (2001). Self-disclosure in computer-mediated communication: The role of
self-awareness and visual anonymity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(2),
177–192.

436 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Joinson, A. N., & Dietz-Uhler, B. (2002). Explanations for the perpetration of and
reactions to deception in a virtual community. Social Science Computer Review, 20(3),
275–289.
Kibby, M. (1997). Babes on the Web: Sex, identity and the home page. Media International
Australia, 84, 39–45.
Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content Analysis: An Introduction to its Methodology. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage.
Klohnen, E. C., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1998). Partner selection for personality characteristics:

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


A couple-centered approach. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(3), 268.
Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal
process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner
responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74(5), 1238–1251.
Lee, A. (2004, November 1). Online love may come with safety warning. Detroit News,
pp. 1A–2A.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic Inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lynn, M., & Bolig, R. (1985). Personal advertisements: Source of data for research on
interpersonal relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationship, 2, 337–383.
MacKenzie, D. A., & Wajcman, J. (1985). The Social Shaping of Technology. Maidenhead, UK:
Open University Press.
MacKinnon, R. C. (1995). Searching for the Leviathan in Usenet. In S. Jones (Ed.),
CyberSociety: Computer-Mediated Communication and Community. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
McLaughlin, M., Osbourne, K., & Ellison, N. (1997). Virtual community in a telepresence
environment. In S. Jones (Ed.), Virtual Culture: Identity and Communication in
Cybersociety (pp. 146–168). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook
(2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
O’Sullivan, P. B. (2000). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Impression management
functions of communication channels in relationships. Human Communication Research,
26(3), 403–431.
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication,
46(1), 80–97.
Ramirez, A., Walther, J. B., Burgoon, J. K., & Sunnafrank, M. (2002). Information-seeking
strategies, uncertainty, and computer-mediated communication: Toward a conceptual
model. Human Communication Research, 28(2), 213–228.
Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. W. Duck (Ed.),
Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research and Interventions (pp. 376–389).
Chichester, England: Wiley.
Resnick, P., & Zeckhauser, R. (2002). Trust among strangers in Internet transactions:
Empirical analysis of eBay’s reputation system. In M. R. Baye (Ed.), Advances in Applied
Microeconomics: The Economics of the Internet and E-commerce. (Vol. 11, pp. 127–157).
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Roberts, L. D., & Parks, M. R. (1999). The social geography of gender-switching in
virtual environments on the Internet. Information, Communication & Society, 2(4),
521–540.

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 437
Rowatt, W. C., Cunningham, M. R., & Druen, P. B. (1998). Deception to get a date.
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(11), 1228–1242.
Rubin, Z. (1975). Disclosing oneself to a stranger: Reciprocity and its limits. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 11(3), 233–260.
Schaefer, L. J. (2003, February 14). Looking for love, online or on paper. The New York Times,
p. A31.
Schau, H. J., & Gilly, M. C. (2003). We are what we post? Self-presentation in personal web
space. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(3), 385–404.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Schlenker, B. R., & Pontari, B. A. (2000). The strategic control of information: Impression
management and self-presentation in daily life. In A. Tesser, R. B. Felson, & J. M. Suls
(Eds.), Psychological Perspectives on Self and Identity (pp. 199–232). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Shah, R. C., & Kesan, J. P. (2003). Manipulating the governance characteristics of code. Info:
The Journal of Policy, Regulation and Strategy for Telecommunications, Information and
Media, 5(4), 3–9.
St. John, W. (2002, April 21). Young, single and dating at hyperspeed. The New York Times.
Retrieved September 2, 2004, from http://www.springstreetnetworks.com/press/pdf/
springst_youngandsingle.pdf
Stone, A. R. (1996). The War of Desire and Technology at the Close of the Mechanical Age.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for
Developing Grounded Theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Taylor, D., & Altman, I. (1987). Communication in interpersonal relationships: Social
penetration processes. In M. E. Roloff & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal Processes:
New Directions in Communication Research (pp. 257–277). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on
disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit
at a time. Human Communication Research, 28(3), 317–348.
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the Screen: Identity in the Age of the Internet. New York: Simon and
Schuster.
Utz, S. (2000). Social information processing in MUDs: The development of friendships in
virtual worlds. Journal of Online Behavior, 1 (1). Retrieved July 15, 2004, from http://
www.behavior.net/JOB/v1n1/utz.html
Van Gelder, L. (1996). The strange case of the electronic lover: A real-life story of
deception, seduction, and technology. In R. Kling (Ed.), Computerization and
Controversy: Value Conflicts and Social Choices (2nd ed., pp. 533–546). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Wallace, P. (1999). The Psychology of the Internet. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational
perspective. Communication Research, 19(1), 52–91.
Walther, J. B. (1994). Anticipated ongoing interaction versus channel effects on relational
communication in computer mediated interaction. Human Communication Research,
20(4), 473–501.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and
hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23(1), 3–44.

438 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Walther, J. B., Anderson, J. F., & Park, D. D. (1994). Interpersonal effects in
computer-mediated interaction: A meta-analysis of social and antisocial communication.
Communication Research, 21(4), 460–487.
Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). Relational communication in computer-mediated
interaction. Human Communication Research, 19(1), 50–88.
Walther, J. B., Loh, T., & Granka, L. (2005). Let me count the ways: The interchange of verbal
and nonverbal cues in computer-mediated and face-to-face affinity. Journal of Language
and Social Psychology, 24(1), 36–65.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-mediated
communication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of
Interpersonal Communication (3rd ed., pp. 529–563). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Walther, J. B., & Tidwell, L. C. (1995). Nonverbal cues in computer-mediated
communication, and the effect of chronemics on relational communication. Journal of
Organizational Computing and Electronic Commerce, 5(4), 355–378.
Woll, S. (1986). So many to choose from: Decision strategies in videodating. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 3(1), 43–52.
Woll, S. B., & Cosby, P. C. (1987). Videodating and other alternatives to traditional methods
of relationship initiation. In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in Personal
Relationships (Vol. 1, pp. 69–108). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Woll, S. B., & Young, P. (1989). Looking for Mr. or Ms. Right: Self-presentation in
videodating. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51(2), 483–488.
Woolgar, S. (1996). Technologies as cultural artifacts. In W. Dutton (Ed.), Information and
Communication Technologies: Visions and Realities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Appendix A: Sample Codes and Quotes


Code Sample Quotes
Honesty ‘‘So I think some people intentionally lie, and then I think
some people truly try to be honest, but they still don’t really
know what they want. And then I think there’s a third group
of people, that truly are honest and truly know what they
want and are just picky as hell, and they either meet someone
or they just give up.’’
‘‘I don’t know why anyone would want to do that [be dishon-
est]. I mean unless you just like have nothing else to do and
you just want to play with people I guess. I mean, sure I could
make myself a multimillionaire (laugh) and a Playboy model
I guess. If I was going to do it I might as well go all out.’’

(continued)

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 439
Appendix 1 Continued
Code Sample Quotes
Cues Masked/Amplified ‘‘Another guy was really, like, seemed so awesome on the
phone, had a really fun personality, worked in TV and we had
the greatest conversation. I met him in Santa Monica, where
he lived, and first of all he had no personality. And I was like,
‘oh my God, either it just radically changed overnight or he is

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


only good on the phone.’’’
‘‘I think if someone can write really well, that would come
out more easily than in person. What is good about the Inter-
net for a shy person is that you can show what you have to
offer more than you can in person. What is difficult for a shy
person is that it takes a little more time to open up and show
what you have to offer, and you can do that on the Internet
more, I think.’’

Appendix B: Most Important Codes with Frequencies*


Code Frequency
Honesty 266
Filter 222
Self-presentation 192
Photographs 180
Supply and demand 180
Desired qualities in partner 180
Cues/characteristics masked or amplified by medium 175
Strategies—online (things learned about online dating) 171
Self-knowledge/self-concept 163
Online vs. traditional dating 163
Physical attractiveness/appearance 150
Rejection 136
Effectiveness/efficiency of online dating 128
Context/weak, strong ties 119
* All codes with more than 100 occurrences.

About the Authors


Nicole Ellison is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Telecommunications,
Information Studies, and Media at Michigan State University. Her research explores
issues of self-presentation, relationship development, and identity in online environ-
ments such as weblogs, online dating sites, and social networking services.

440 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association
Address: Dept. of Telecommunications, Information Studies, and Media, Michigan
State University, 403 Communication Arts and Sciences, East Lansing, MI 48824
USA

Rebecca Heino is an Assistant Professor in the McDonough School of Business at


Georgetown University. She has centered her research on the use of communication
technologies—such as intranets and email—in organizations, specifically focusing
on organizational adoption, implementation, and privacy.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/11/2/415/4617726 by guest on 30 November 2022


Address: Georgetown University, McDonough School of Business, 37th & O St. NW,
Old North G-04, Washington, DC 20057 USA

Jennifer Gibbs is an Assistant Professor of Communication at Rutgers University.


Her research interests center around how individuals connect, collaborate, and
negotiate identity and differences in global, multicultural, and mediated contexts.
Address: Department of Communication, SCILS, 4 Huntington Street, Rutgers
University, New Brunswick, NJ 08901-1071 USA

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 11 (2006) 415–441 ª 2006 International Communication Association 441

You might also like