ED188074
ED188074
ED188074
ABSTRACT .
nV.
*****************************************'******************************
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best (that can be made
frcm the original document.
,************************************************4**********************
4
a
7
S.B. HOFFMAN
1.
Method of procedure
tested, a m;ore complete list of
Before these hypotheses were
original 16 (see Table 1 ) was
influence tactics adding to Falbo's
in each.cohort and 15 of each.gender,
r derived. Ninety persons, 30 their close
how they got their way in
.
In depth abo4t
..were interviewed
tiomes of,conflict over behavicrs, norms, and
relationships.during .Thitresponsea were content
analyzed and an
1000Xsonality di erences.
24actica were added, fOr a botal of 39 tactics (see;
additional
Mae .1) ,
-A,
.,,.;ir7.4r,14,1rmlr'-fovoN , 4E,, .
rated the
Similft to Falbo's procedure, a panel of expert judges
procedure was
pairwise similarity of these tactics ar4 a NW scaling
derived, however. Falbo's two
used. No simple dimensional scheme was then used
dimensions of direct/indirect and rational/nonrational were
If cons'ensus on ratings
by the judges to rate each of the 39 tactics.
into one of four quadrants
was achieved, a tactic was then placed
direct/umotional (hot),
kliown as strategies: direct/rational (dry),
(see Table 2).
indirect/rational (sl), and indirect/emotional (warm)
in subsequent data
Tactics were collapsed into strategies for use
analyses.
(Likelihood ot
The 39 tactics were incorporated into an LITU Scale
rate their
Influence Tactic Us *ale). Respondents were asked to of not at all
likelihood of using each tactic on a four-point scale
significant extant
likely to very likely, for each of four
relationships: intinte, parent, child, and best friend. As a
to designate
criterion validity index, respondents were also asked
whether they had or had not used a lactic on two self-rePorted
each of their designated
- occasions of influence in the past /ear in
relationships.
in this
Ten males and ten females in each birth cohort particpated
from university
study. The young adult sample was derived jobs, and exercise
undergraduates, middle-aged adults from classes,
retirement apartment house, and
salons, and older adults from jobs, a
Caucasian and
from a retired teachers' roster. All subjects were 18 21,
between the ages of
middle-class. Young adults were
middle-aged from 30 - 45, and old foam 60 80.
Data analysis
by biserial
COnvergent validity of the LITU Scale was deArmined
the LITU scale.
correlation at the actual tactic use scale with
have been due to social
Correlations were lag, but poor results may
behaviors, and limited use of
desirability biases in reporting actual
Content validity.was
behaviors in only two Occasions of influence.
stratetgies explained
high and reliability of subscales (the four
previously) was mcderately high.
measures analysis of
The LITU Scale was analyzed by a repeated
being the repeated measure,
variance SAS program (with relationships
independent variables). Dependent!
cOort and gender the two other each ct the .0our strategy
irariables V*re likelihood ct (Ise scorns for
noninfluence tactics.
subscales and an additiOnal sUbscale of
dividing through by total score
Subscale scores were standard
to cOntrol foc response set biza.
3
7717.?"e'ekg44440***414Ntri,+4,0*. , 1i. ry
1/41/1410tle-'701
than the middle-aged. The
friends, the young use more warm tactics
that the middle-aged use more warm
only anomalous finding here is other close relationships,
tactics in their marriages than in some with the other
although less than young adults do. In OoMbloation
inimicble-aged marriages, it
other finding of kigh use of dry tmctics
for use of many different tactics of
appears that marriage is an arena with the
influence for-the mdddle-aged. This must be tempered
adults arp most
reminder that for total influencing, middle-aged
else close bp them.
likely to influence their chil:jren than anyone
cohort x
The Direct tional Strategy. For the dry strategy, a
There is a very
relationihip interactiOn was significant (p 4 .04).
bir all cohorts:
similar pattern of use across relationships reported
likelihood of using dry tactics with
young adults report breatest
and least,with parents.
friends, less likelihood with intimates,
as do older adults.
Middle-aged adultssalso report the same pattern, a.relationship varies
HOgever, the level ofjuse of dry tactics within
relationship, ndddle-aged adults are
by cohort. In the intimate older or
significantly more likely bp use dry tactics than either adults do
cohorts. With-their parent's, young and middle-aged
younger
using dry tactics. With th6ir
not differ in their likelihoo0.of
significanqy more dry tactics than-
children, middle-aged adults use relationship;,as in the intimate
'older adults dd. And in the friend
using.significantly more dry
relotionship, mdddle-aged adults report
tactics than do young or old adults In analyzing this pattern of
cohorts dry tactics are more
findings, it appears that across all
equitable power
likely to be utilized in relationships with in some
structures, such as in best friend relationships and greater number of
because of their
marriages. The middle-aged,cohort, in more cf
re3ources4.is also able ba employ this less costly strategy to have
adults appear
their relationships. Interestly, mdddle-aged
than younger or older versOns, or
either more equitable marriages 1961) might have marriages more
because of "disenchantment" (Pineo, also use
more than older adults
like friendships. 14iddle-aged adults middle-age0 parentp possess more
piky tactics with their ,children, as friendshipS also appear to
resources elan older parrts,,\Maddle,aged
i/oung or older adults, if use of
more,equitable thantriendships'of
be a function of eqpal powet resources
dry tactics can be assumed bo
id a relationship.
. ,_. noT.
Discussion
influencing is a function of both culturally
In general, iL appears that
predispositions and the power differential of
socialized IxThavioral cohort is
Also, it appears that the youngest
individuals in a relationship. in all their
older cohorts to influence
more likely than middle aged or costly to them in
often they must use tactics that are (such as joking,
relationshi0s, although
Specifically, these are the warm tactics
order tO do this. relationships
guilt, or sarcasm). Middle aged adults in all their with
pretense, tactics, although they use rlost such tactics
are less likely to use warm parents are also most likely to
use these
their parents. Their eiderl that bOth adult children
their middle aged children. IL seems and so-
tactics with
have a difficult time directly influencinglioach other direct
and old pafents
these indirect means. The use of dry tactics, the most
must resort to and with
of all influence modes, is least likely with parents
and least costly
adult children.
likely than males to use warm
Female's, irue to stereotype, were more socializing 'experience
that the effect of early
.tactics. This would suggest resistent to current cultural trends to
teaching sex typed behavior is females, less
behaviors. Even the younge4k-cOhort of
instrumentalize female expected,
of indirect tactics. It mdght be
practiced in sex typing, made use begin to utilize these tactics
lesi
of women may
however, that future cohorts is a real
Whether the use of warm influence tactics
frequently. therefore resistant to cultural impact
genetic/biological sex difference and
cross-sequential research designs. '
1
b.
17.., '
REFERENCES
,4 e
44r,.11,c1r,
,r ,
TABLE I ,
INFLUENCE if/CMS
Name Example
16, although in
--*Asterisked tactics were inCluded in Falbd-'s original list ct
some cases in a slightly di,fferent form.
Some tactic
Note -- In the LITU scale,s,tactic names were not utilized.
reporting ct their use in
examples were phrased in such a way as to encourage
the_case ct socially undesirable tactics (e.g., nag, deceit).
9
,. , 7.. a. t0.4.,.111,..,;*.,*/!....rf' T:1
TAIW 2
QUADRANT STRNITGLES.
-0
00
se
"!
10