Effective Practices For Developing Reading Compreh
Effective Practices For Developing Reading Compreh
net/publication/254282574
CITATIONS READS
506 39,617
2 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by P. David Pearson on 20 September 2014.
Chapter 10
205
• Good readers try to determine the meaning of unfamiliar words and
concepts in the text, and they deal with inconsistencies or gaps as
needed.
• They draw from, compare, and integrate their prior knowledge with
material in the text.
• They think about the authors of the text, their style, beliefs, inten-
tions, historical milieu, and so on.
• They monitor their understanding of the text, making adjustments in
their reading as necessary.
• They evaluate the text’s quality and value, and react to the text in a
range of ways, both intellectually and emotionally.
• Good readers read different kinds of text differently.
• When reading narrative, good readers attend closely to the setting
and characters.
• When reading expository text, these readers frequently construct
and revise summaries of what they have read.
• For good readers, text processing occurs not only during “reading”
as we have traditionally defined it, but also during short breaks
taken during reading, even after the “reading” itself has com-
menced, even after the “reading” has ceased.
• Comprehension is a consuming, continuous, and complex activity,
but one that, for good readers, is both satisfying and productive.
(See Pressley and Afflerbach [1995] and Block and Pressley [2001] for re-
views of much of the research on good readers’ comprehension. The in-
tellectual ancestor to this chapter is “Developing Expertise in Reading
Comprehension” [Pearson, Roehler, Dole, & Duffy, 1992] in the second
edition of What Research Has to Say About Reading Instruction; this piece
also provides a good overview of the work upon which this characteri-
zation of good reading is based.)
Given knowledge about what good readers do when they read, re-
searchers and educators have addressed the following question: Can we
teach students to engage in these productive behaviors? The answer is a
resounding yes. A large volume of work indicates that we can help stu-
dents acquire the strategies and processes used by good readers—and
that this improves their overall comprehension of text, both the texts
used to teach the strategies and texts they read on their own in the future.
Modeling*
Guided Practice
Direct
Instruction*
Participating*
Scaffolding*
Facilitating*
Primarily
Student
0
0 Percent of Task Responsibility Assumed by the Student 100
As one moves down the diagonal from upper left to lower right, students assume more, and
teachers less, responsibility for task completion. There are three regions of responsibility:
primarily teacher in the upper left corner, primarily student in the lower right, and shared
responsibility in the center. (This figure is adapted with permission from Pearson and Gallagher
[1983]; the asterisked terms are borrowed from Au & Raphael [1998].)
That night Max wore his wolf suit and made mischief of one kind and
another.... Boy, I can really visualize Max. He’s in this monster suit and
he is chasing after his dog with a fork in his hand. I think he is really
starting to act crazy. I wonder what made Max act like that...Hm-m-
m...I bet he was getting a little bored and wanted to go on an adven-
ture. I think that is my prediction. (Pressley et al., 1992, p. 518)
Food Teeth
break Swallow and
enters
mouth
. food into . food travels
through tube
small bits
into stomach
Rodents g
ests
Food For
Garbage g
The Trickster
Habitats Deserts
Cities
Famous
Coyotes
lves
Wo
Coyotes Relatives Dogs
Hyenas
Humans Ways to
Harm Humans
Natural
Enemies
Ways to
Help Humans
Indeed, most people with relevant experience will agree that summariz-
ing is a difficult task for many children. Many children require instruc-
tion and practice in summarizing before they are able to produce good
oral and written summaries of text. Interestingly, research suggests that
instruction and practice in summarizing not only improves students’
ability to summarize text, but also their overall comprehension of text
content. Thus, instruction in summarization can be considered to meet
dual purposes: to improve students’ ability to summarize text and to im-
prove their ability to comprehend text and recall.
There are at least two major approaches to the teaching of summa-
rization. In rule-governed approaches, students are taught to follow a set
of step-by-step procedures to develop summaries. For example, McNeil
and Donant (1982) teach the following rules, which draw from the work
of Brown, Campione, and Day (1981) and Kintsch and Van Dijk (1978):
Rule 1: Delete unnecessary material.
Rule 2: Delete redundant material.
Rule 3: Compose a word to replace a list of items.
Rule 4: Compose a word to replace individual parts of an action.
Rule 5: Select a topic sentence.
Rule 6: Invent a topic sentence if one is not available.
T: What would be a good question about pit vipers that starts with the
word why?
S: (No response)
T: How about, “Why are the snakes called pit vipers?”
——
S: How do spinner’s mate is smaller than.... How am I going to say that?
T: Take your time with it. You want to ask a question about the spin-
ner’s mate and what he does, beginning with the word how.
S: How do they spend most of his time sitting?
T: You’re very close. The question would be “How does the spinner’s
mate spend most of his time?” Now you ask it.
——
T: That was a fine job, Ken, but I think there might be something to
add to our summary. There is more information that I think we
need to include. This paragraph is mostly about what?
S: The third method of artificial evaporation. (Palincsar & Brown,
1984, p. 138)
S1: My question is, what does the aquanaut need when he goes under
water?
S2: A watch.
S3: Flippers.
S4: A belt.
S1: Those are all good answers.
T:` Nice job! I have a question too. Why does the aquanaut wear a belt?
What is so special about it?
S3: It’s a heavy belt and keeps him from floating up to the top again.
T: Good for you.
S1: For my summary now: This paragraph was about what aquanauts
need to take when they go under the water.
S5: And also about why they need those things.
reciprocal teaching (on the cognitive side of the ledger). Second, the list
is long enough to guarantee selective application (based on the text and
the learning context) to any given text. There is no way that a teacher
could ensure that each strategy was applied to every text encountered
by a group of students.
Much of the research on SAIL and its intellectual cousin, transactional
strategies instruction, has been qualitative, looking in detail at the ways
that strategies are taught and learned. These studies suggest that SAIL and
similar programs offer a promising approach to comprehension instruc-
tion, with rich, motivating interactions around text and increasing so-
phistication of student strategy use over time. One quasi-experimental
study of SAIL has confirmed the effectiveness of the approach at im-
proving student comprehension (Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, & Schuder,
1996). In the study, second-grade students in SAIL classrooms outper-
formed students in comparable non-SAIL classrooms on standardized
measures of both reading comprehension and word attack. Students in
SAIL classrooms also remembered more content from their daily lessons
than students in non-SAIL classrooms. Additional evidence for the effi-
cacy of this “family” of transactional strategy instruction routines can be
found in Pressley’s (1998) recent review.
Summary
In this chapter, we have described effective individual and collective
strategies for teaching comprehension of text and discussed character-
istics of a balanced comprehension program into which such strategies
could be embedded. In Figure 10.6, we offer a tool for assessing the com-
prehension instruction environment in your own classroom. We hope
that this will aid readers in identifying both strengths and weaknesses
in comprehension instruction as well as serving as a summary of the ma-
terial presented in this chapter. We hope it will not prove overwhelm-
ing, even to those who are novices at comprehension instruction. Realize
that the use of even one of the techniques described in this chapter has
REFERENCES
Anderson, R.C., & Biddle, W.B. (1975). On asking people questions about what they
are reading. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 9,
pp. 9–129). New York: Academic Press.
Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in
reading comprehension. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal
(Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255–291). New York: Longman.
Anderson, R.C., Wilkinson, I.A.G., Mason, J.M., & Shirey, L. (1987, December).
Prediction versus word-level questions. In R.C. Anderson (Chair), Experimental in-
vestigations of prediction in small-group reading lessons. Symposium conducted at the
37th annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, St. Petersburg Beach, FL.
Armbruster, B.B., & Anderson, T.H. (1980). The effect of mapping on the free recall of
expository text (Tech. Rep. No. 160). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Center for
the Study of Reading.
Armbruster, B.B., Anderson, T.H., & Meyer, J.L. (1990). The framing project: A collabo-
ration to improve content area reading using instructional graphics (Tech. Rep.).
Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
OTHER RESOURCES
The reference section includes references to many books, chapters, and
articles that address specific comprehension strategies and approaches to