(Current Issues in Social Psychology) Jan-Willem Van Prooijen - The Psychology of Political Polarization-Routledge (2021)
(Current Issues in Social Psychology) Jan-Willem Van Prooijen - The Psychology of Political Polarization-Routledge (2021)
(Current Issues in Social Psychology) Jan-Willem Van Prooijen - The Psychology of Political Polarization-Routledge (2021)
Current Issues in Social Psychology is a series of edited books that reflect the state
of current and emerging topics of interest in social psychology.
Each volume is tightly focused on a particular topic and consists of seven
to ten chapters contributed by international experts. The editors of individual
volumes are leading figures in their areas and provide an introductory overview.
The series is useful reading for students, academics, and researchers of social
psychology and related disciplines. Example topics include: self-esteem, mind-
fulness, evolutionary social psychology, minority groups, social neuroscience,
cyberbullying, and social stigma.
Edited by
Jan-W illem van Prooijen
First published 2021
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
605 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10158
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2021 selection and editorial matter, Jan-Willem van Prooijen; individual chapters,
the contributors
The right of Jan-Willem van Prooijen to be identified as the author of the editorial material,
and of the authors for their individual chapters, has been asserted in accordance with
sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilized
in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or
hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information
storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks,
and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
A catalog record has been requested for this book
ISBN: 978-0-367-48717-1 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-0-367-48716-4 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-1-003-04243-3 (ebk)
Typeset in Bembo
by Newgen Publishing UK
Contents
SEC TI O N 1
Underlying processes of political polarization 15
2 When do psychological differences predict political
differences? Engagement and the psychological bases of
political polarization 17
CHRISTOPHER M. FEDERICO
SEC TI O N 2
The social context of political polarization 95
6 Support for populist parties: economic deprivation,
cultural backlash, or status anxiety? 97
JOLANDA JETTEN AND FRANK MOLS
newgenprepdf
vi Contents
Index 169
Contributors
Across the world, societies polarize politically. The tone of political debate has
hardened in recent years, and the political left and right increasingly seem to
perceive each other as enemies instead of as opponents. Extremist, nationalist,
and populist leaders who blame other groups for society’s problems have bene-
fited electorally from these developments.Various countries have elected popu-
list leaders in office in recent years (e.g., the US, Italy, Hungary, Brazil), and also
elsewhere nationalist movements have significantly shaped the political land-
scape (e.g., Brexit). Against the backdrop of these developments, the world faces
important challenges such as the COVID-19 pandemic, climate change, geo-
political conflict, terrorism, and immigration. Moreover, governmental officials
frequently report suspicions of foreign interference in national elections, and
the public is regularly exposed to fake news, alternative facts, and conspiracy
theories that fuel further polarization.
Political polarization is often visible through macro-level processes in society,
including protest movements, electoral support for relatively radical political
parties, and increasing levels of conflict between societal movements. Yet, at
the basis of political polarization are psychological, micro- and meso-level
processes that determine how individual citizens think and feel about people
with a different worldview, how they treat ethnic, religious, and sexual minority
groups, and how much they trust leaders and experts on issues such as climate
change, health, poverty, and international conflict. Any manifestation of polit-
ical polarization starts with the concerns of individual citizens, and therefore,
the social-psychological processes associated with polarized political views are
key to understanding the broader societal implications of this phenomenon. As
such, attempts to depolarize the political debate are more likely to be effective
if they take scientific knowledge of the psychology of political polarization into
account.
The present edited volume was inspired by these issues, and sought to pro-
vide a state-of-the-art overview of scientific knowledge on the psychology of
political polarization. To achieve this aim, leading experts on the psychology
of political polarization generously contributed a chapter in which they share
2 Jan-Willem van Prooijen
their views on this phenomenon. The current introductory chapter will briefly
illuminate what political polarization is exactly, and what the main theoretical
and practical reasons are to study this phenomenon. Moreover, it will provide a
short overview of the book.
Müller, 2016). Populism occurs at both the left and right, and indeed, polit-
ical parties that are considered left- or right-extreme often also are considered
populist (Akkermans, Mudde, & Zaslove, 2014; Schumacher & Rooduijn, 2013).
A notable difference with political extremism, however, is that populism can
occur throughout the political spectrum, including in the center. For instance,
Italy’s five-star movement is widely considered a populist party yet is not clearly
left-wing or right-wing (e.g., it has relatively left-wing positions on income
equality and sustainability, yet relatively right-wing positions on immigration).
Moreover, various well-known politicians have articulated rhetoric consistent
with populist leadership, yet are not politically extreme (e.g., Silvio Berlusconi
in Italy, or Boris Johnson in the UK, who both are center-right but not far-right;
Van Prooijen, 2018; see also Müller, 2016, for other examples).
Besides such support for broad political movements, political polarization
may be reflected in increased levels of conflict between citizens on more specific
policy issues. For instance, many citizens do not believe the scientific evidence
that climate change is real or that humans are causing it, yielding conflict with
citizens or political parties that support regulations to reduce CO2 emissions
(see Chapter 9). Such climate change skepticism is robustly associated with a
conservative ideology in the US, although the link with ideology is less clear
in various other countries (Hornsey, Harris, & Fielding, 2018). Likewise, the
COVID-19 pandemic has not only sparked agreement to global lockdown pol-
icies to contain the virus, but it has also inspired resistance and protest among
citizens who oppose the lockdown policies. Such anti-lockdown sentiments are
associated with conspiracy beliefs (Marinthe, Brown, Delouvée, & Jolley, 2020),
a common predictor of political polarization (Krouwel, Kutiyski,Van Prooijen,
Martinsson, & Markstedt, 2017). In sum, although political polarization can
take many forms, the common denominators are a strong conviction in one’s
own values and beliefs, and hostility towards those who are perceived to hold
different values and beliefs.
yet by and large, citizens’ life circumstances have improved substantially over
the past decades. Contrary to the bleak rhetoric of politically radical leaders,
objective metrics suggest that on average citizens of modern societies actu-
ally are safer, richer, happier, and healthier than ever before. Even over the last
50 years people have a longer life expectancy, are less likely to suffer from pov-
erty, unemployment, or sickness, are less likely to be sent to war, are less likely to
be victimized in a crime, are more likely to own a house and car, and so on (e.g.,
Pinker, 2018; Rosling, 2018). Why do these prosperous and successful societies
offer such fertile ground to political polarization?
One possible explanation is that the benefits of globalization are not equally
distributed among citizens, and that particularly those who are “left behind”
cause the political polarization in modern societies. Indeed, a 2020 United
Nations report shows that income inequality is on the rise throughout the
world.1 Moreover, psychological research reveals that higher levels of inequality
in societies predict a preference for radical leadership (Sprong et al., 2019).
A closer look suggests, however, that also this explanation needs more specifi-
city. Political polarization occurs not only among relatively poor but also among
relatively wealthy citizens (see Chapter 6; Mols & Jetten, 2017). Moreover,
during the past few decades life circumstances have improved not only for
wealthy people but also for the poorer segments of most societies (Rosling,
2018). All of this suggests that the psychology of political polarization is a com-
plex phenomenon that cannot be traced back to a single cause.
Overconfidence
As political polarization implies that people are ideologically entrenched in
their beliefs, it stands to reason that it is associated with high levels of belief
confidence. Consistent with this notion, in the US both the left and right
extremes consider their own beliefs as superior –that is, as more correct than
other viewpoints –on various specific policy issues (e.g., health care; illegal
immigration; taxes; Toner, Leary, Asher, & Jongman- Sereno, 2013). Also in
non-political estimation tasks political polarization is associated with increased
judgmental confidence (Brandt, Evans, & Crawford, 2015). Moreover, politic-
ally extreme beliefs are more stable over time than politically moderate beliefs.
Over the course of an election campaign, people at the edges of the political
spectrum displayed less variation in their self-reported political ideology than
moderates, suggesting that their convictions were less sensitive to social influ-
ence (Zwicker,Van Prooijen, & Krouwel, 2020).
High confidence in one’s own beliefs can be warranted, however, and
therefore does not necessarily imply overconfidence. If experts (e.g., psych-
ology professors; medical doctors) feel confident that they know more about
their specific domain of expertise than lay people, usually they are making a
warranted judgment. Likewise, political party elites with high levels of polit-
ical knowledge (“ideologues”) also have high confidence in their own beliefs
(Converse, 1964). One might question, however, how warranted the belief con-
fidence is of regular citizens who support relatively radical political movements.
Correctly understanding reality implies appreciating the many complexities
that are inherent to most societal problems, and to political decision making.
Political polarization is associated with a tendency to cognitively simplify such
complexities, however (Lammers, Koch, Conway, & Brandt, 2017). This makes
it likely that the high levels of confidence among political radicals frequently is
overconfidence.
An illustration of such overconfidence can be found in a study that took
place during the 2016 EU refugee crisis (Van Prooijen, Krouwel, & Emmer,
2018). Left-versus right- wing citizens endorsed diametrically different
solutions for this geopolitical problem, with the political left being more likely
to support inclusion of refugees in society, and the political right more likely to
support rejecting refugees at the border. The left and right extremes converged
in a belief that the solution to this problem was simple, however. Moreover, as
compared to moderates, extremists had higher confidence in their responses on
a test assessing their factual knowledge of the refugee crisis. This higher belief
confidence did not translate into more factually correct answers, suggesting
overconfidence. Moreover, their belief in simple solutions for the refugee
crisis mediated the link between political extremism and judgmental confi-
dence. Apparently, the political extremes were overconfident in their judgments
because this geopolitical problem looked simple to them.
Introduction 7
Findings in other settings also suggest that radical political beliefs are
associated with increased confidence yet not with increased factual know-
ledge. One study took place during a Dutch referendum with a clear pro-
establishment and a more radical anti- establishment (i.e., anti- EU) voting
option. Anti-establishment voting was associated with increased self-perceived
understanding of the referendum, but this self-perceived understanding did not
translate into more factual knowledge: in fact, anti-establishment voters scored
worse on a factual knowledge test of the referendum than pro-establishment
voters. In addition, anti-establishment voters were more likely to overclaim
non-political knowledge, as reflected in the extent to which they claimed to
recognize stimuli that they actually saw for the first time (Van Prooijen &
Krouwel, 2020). These insights point at a potential risk of political polariza-
tion: The excessive confidence that politically polarized citizens display often is
overconfidence, which may decrease the quality of decision making.
Intolerance
The link between political polarization and overconfidence emerges from the
conviction that one’s own beliefs are factually correct. An additional aspect of
political polarization, however, is that they are moralized judgments. The high
levels of belief superiority associated with political polarization therefore may
imply a perception of one’s own values as morally superior. Such moral abso-
lutism is a likely source of conflict in society, as strong moral convictions stimu-
late intolerance towards people who do not share these convictions (Skitka,
2010). Accordingly, dogmatic intolerance –defined as a tendency to reject,
and consider as inferior, any ideological belief that differs from one’s own –is
stronger at the political extremes than in the political center (Rollwage, Doling,
& Fleming, 2018; Van Prooijen & Kuijper, 2020). Such dogmatic intolerance
is associated with willingness to protest, denial of free speech to people with
different views, and support for antisocial behavior as a means to reach ideo-
logical goals (Van Prooijen & Krouwel, 2017).
While dogmatic intolerance pertains to people’s (lack of) acceptance of
incompatible beliefs, political polarization also may stimulate intolerance of
entire societal subgroups. According to the ideological conflict hypothesis,
people are prejudiced about groups that they assume to have different values
than themselves (Brandt, Reyna, Chambers, Crawford, & Wetherell, 2014).
Research designed to test this hypothesis has revealed that the high levels of
prejudice commonly observed at the political right towards a range of soci-
etal subgroups (e.g., Muslims; ethnic minorities; feminists) is associated with
the belief that these groups largely vote left-wing. This line of research also
has revealed high levels of prejudice at the political left, however, specifically
towards societal groups commonly assumed to vote right-wing (e.g., Christians;
business people; the military; for an overview, see Brandt et al., 2014). Consistent
8 Jan-Willem van Prooijen
with these insights, the political extremes derogate a larger number of soci-
etal subgroups than political moderates (Van Prooijen et al., 2015) and dis-
play relatively high levels of parochial altruism, that is, a willingness to sacrifice
their self-interest for the benefit of their group, if necessary by hurting people
from different groups (Van Prooijen & Kuijper, 2020). Taken together, these
findings are consistent with the assumption that political polarization is related
to intolerance, both towards people with different beliefs and to people who
belong to different groups.
Motivated reasoning
A basic human motivation is to have a coherently organized worldview where
people’s values, beliefs, and actions converge with their factual knowledge about
the world (Festinger, 1957). It is inevitable, however, that people regularly come
across scientific information that challenges this worldview. For instance, people
who believe that COVID-19 is “just a flu” are likely to encounter scientific evi-
dence indicating that the coronavirus is far more dangerous to humans than a
seasonal flu virus. People can resolve such discrepancies in various ways, which
may include updating their original beliefs (e.g., people accept that COVID-19
is different from, and more dangerous than, flu) or rejecting the scientific evi-
dence (e.g., people embrace beliefs such as that scientists are mistaken, that
science is “just an opinion,” or that scientists conspired with powerful interest
groups to rig the evidence).What determines whether people update their pre-
existing beliefs or reject scientific evidence when faced with such discrepant
information?
It is likely that the more strongly people are ideologically entrenched in their
beliefs, the less willing they are to change those beliefs when faced with new
information. Indeed, political polarization is empirically associated with cog-
nitive inflexibility (Zmigrod, Rentfrow, & Robbins, 2020). Such a decreased
willingness or ability to update beliefs when faced with incompatible infor-
mation increases the probability that people accept unscientific or otherwise
implausible statements as true. For instance, people may misperceive the sci-
entific consensus about a range of topics as consistent with their values (e.g.,
about the reality of anthropogenic climate change; Kahan, Jenkins-Smith, &
Braman, 2011), or deny ideologically inconvenient scientific findings altogether
(Washburn & Skitka, 2017). Additionally, people may embrace conspiracy
theories that support their values and further polarize society. The stronger
people identify as Republican, the more likely they are to believe theories
that Democrats are conspiring, and vice versa (Miller, Saunders, & Farhart,
2016; Uscinski, Klofstad, & Atkinson, 2016). Such intergroup conspiracy the-
ories can also polarize international relationships: during the 2019 trade war,
US and Chinese citizens both endorsed beliefs that the opposing country was
conspiring against them (although these intergroup conspiracy theories were
stronger in Chinese than US samples; see Van Prooijen & Song, 2020).
Introduction 9
Overview of the book
To examine the psychology of political polarization, what follows are eight
chapters that were contributed by political psychologists with substantial
expertise on these issues.The book is organized in two sections, each containing
four chapters. The first section (Chapters 2–5) focuses on underlying psycho-
logical processes: how do the emotional, cognitive, and motivational factors
that mostly take place within people’s minds contribute to political polariza-
tion? In Chapter 2, Christopher M. Federico focuses on how psychological
needs for security and certainty are related to political polarization. The author
discusses evidence for the idea that the extent to which these needs polarize
political beliefs depends on how engaged people are with the political system,
and how well they understand and care about the different options that are
provided by political elites. In Chapter 3, Alain Van Hiel, Jasper Van Assche, and
Tessa Haesevoets examine the political mindset of citizens who support radical
and populist movements. They argue that this mindset is characterized by pol-
itical cynicism, which may be a better framework than polarization to under-
stand these citizens. At the end of the chapter these authors provide evidence
that populism and cynicism indeed are closely related concepts, and offer prac-
tical suggestions for moderate parties to regain the trust of voters.
In Chapter 4, Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer provide a psycho-
logical profile of extreme Trump supporters.These authors review evidence for
three common explanations for Trump support, notably tribal loyalties (which
includes prejudice, ingroup favoritism, and bloodlust –that is, an aggressive
desire for attacks on perceived enemies), selective media exposure, and material
self-interest. While they find some evidence for each of these, the evidence
is relatively weaker for explanations associated with tribalism, and stronger
for selective media exposure and material self-interest. Finally, in Chapter 5,
10 Jan-Willem van Prooijen
results are mixed, and underscore the strength of people’s ideological resistance
to accept the reality of anthropogenic climate change.
Political polarization occurs in many societies around the world. Given
its impact on people’s lives, domestic policy, and international relations, it is
important to have an evidence-based understanding of this phenomenon.There
is not one single cause of political polarization, however, and it is unlikely that
there is one single solution for it. The present book aims to offer a piece to this
puzzle by examining the underlying psychological processes of polarization,
and the social context in which political polarization transpires. By establishing
these micro-and meso-level processes that shape political polarization among
regular citizens, psychology may contribute to meaningful interventions
designed to depolarize societies.
Note
1 See the UNDESA World Social Report 2020: www.un.org/development/desa/
dspd/world-social-report/2020-2.html
References
Akkermans, A., Mudde, C., & Zaslove, A. (2014). How populist are the people?
Measuring populist attitudes in voters. Comparative Political Studies, 47, 1324–1353.
Brandt, M. J., Evans, A. M., & Crawford, J. T. (2015). The unthinking or confident
extremist? Political extremists are more likely than moderates to reject experimenter-
generated anchors. Psychological Science, 26, 189–202.
Brandt, M. J., Reyna, C., Chambers, J. R., Crawford, J. T., & Wetherell, G. (2014). The
ideological-conflict hypothesis: Intolerance among both liberals and conservatives.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 27–34.
Burke, B. L., Kosloff, S., & Landau, M. J. (2013). Death goes to the polls: A meta-analysis
of mortality salience effects on political attitudes. Political Psychology, 34, 183–200.
Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.),
Ideology and its discontents, pp. 206–261. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe.
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2016). The mechanics of motivated reasoning. Journal of
Economic Perspectives, 30, 133–140.
Festinger, F. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Frimer, J. A., Brandt, M. J., Melton, Z., & Motyl, M. (2018). Extremists on the Left and
Right use angry, negative language. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(8),
1216–1223.
Hogg, M. A., Meehan, C., & Farqueharson, J. (2010). The solace of radicalism: Self-
uncertainty and group identification in the face of threat. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 46, 1061–1066.
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., & Fielding, K. S. (2018). Relationships among conspira-
torial beliefs, conservatism, and climate change skepticism across nations. Nature
Climate Change, 8, 614–620.
12 Jan-Willem van Prooijen
Howe, L. C., & Krosnick, J. A. (2017). Attitude strength. Annual Review of Psychology, 68,
327–351.
Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific
consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14, 147174.
Krouwel, A. (2012). Party transformations in European democracies. New York,
NY: SUNY Press.
Krouwel, A., Kutiyski, Y., Van Prooijen, J.-W., Martinsson, J., & Markstedt, E. (2017).
Does extreme political ideology predict conspiracy beliefs, economic evaluations
and political trust? Evidence from Sweden. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 5,
435–462.
Kruglanski, A. W., Gelfand, M. J., Bélanger, J. J., Sheveland, A., Hetiarachchi, M., &
Gunaratra, R. (2014). The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: How
significance quest impacts violent extremism. Advances in Political Psychology, 35(S1),
69–93.
Marinthe, G., Brown, G., Delouvée, S., & Jolley, D. (2020). Looking out for
myself: Exploring the relationship between conspiracy mentality, perceived personal
risk, and COVID-19 prevention measures. British Journal of Health Psychology, 25(4),
957–980.
McGregor, I., Prentice, M., & Nash, K. (2013). Anxious uncertainty and reactive
approach motivation (RAM) for religious, idealistic, and lifestyle extremes. Journal of
Social Issues, 69, 537–563.
Midlarsky, M. L. (2011). Origins of political extremism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L., & Farhart, C. E. (2016). Conspiracy endorsement as
motivated reasoning: The moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. American
Journal of Political Science, 60(4), 824–844.
Mols, F., & Jetten, J. (2017). The wealth paradox: Economic prosperity and the hardening of
attitudes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Mudde, C., & Kaltwasser, C. R. (2017). Populism: A very short introduction. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Müller, J.-W. (2016). What is populism? Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Pinker, S. (2018). Enlightenment now: The case for reason, humanism, science, and progress.
New York, NY: Viking Penguin.
Rollwage, M., Dolan, R. J., & Fleming, S. M. (2018). Metacognitive failure as a feature
of those holding radical beliefs. Current Biology, 28, 4014–4021.
Rosling, H. (2018). Factfulness: Ten reasons we’re wrong about the world—and why things are
better than you think. London, UK: Sceptre.
Schumacher, G., & Rooduijn, M. (2013). Sympathy for the ‘devil’? Voting for populists
in the 2006 and 2010 Dutch general elections. Electoral Studies, 32, 124–133.
Skitka, L. (2010). The psychology of moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology
Compass, 4(4), 267–281.
Sprong, S., Jetten, J.,Wang, Z., Peters, K., Mols, F.,Verkuyten, M. … Wohl, M. J. A. (2019).
“Our country needs a strong leader right now”: Economic inequality enhances the
wish for a strong leader. Psychological Science, 30, 1–13.
Toner, K., Leary, M., Asher, M.W., & Jongman-Sereno, K. P. (2013). Feeling superior is a
bipartisan issue: Extremity (not direction) of political views predicts perceived belief
superiority. Psychological Science, 24, 2454–2462.
Introduction 13
Uscinski, J. E., Klofstad, C., & Atkinson, M. D. (2016). What drives conspiratorial
beliefs? The role of informational cues and predispositions. Political Research Quarterly,
69, 57–71.
Van den Bos, K. (2018). why people radicalize: How unfairness judgments are used to fuel
radical beliefs, extremist behaviors, and terrorism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2018). Populism as political mentality underlying conspiracy the-
ories. In B. Rutjens and M. Brandt (Eds.), Belief systems and the perception of reality, pp.
79–96. Oxon, UK: Routledge.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Krouwel, A. P. M. (2017). Extreme political beliefs predict dog-
matic intolerance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 292–300.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Krouwel, A. P. M. (2019). Psychological features of extreme pol-
itical ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 159–163.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Krouwel, A. P. M. (2020). Overclaiming knowledge predicts anti-
establishment voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11, 356–363.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., Krouwel, A. P. M., Boiten, M., & Eendebak, L. (2015). Fear among
the extremes: How political ideology predicts negative emotions and outgroup dero-
gation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 485–497.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., Krouwel, A. P. M., & Emmer, J. (2018). Ideological responses to
the EU refugee crisis: The left, the right, and the extremes. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 9, 143–150.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Kuijper, S. M. H. C. (2020). A comparison of extreme religious
and political ideologies: Similar worldviews but different grievances. Personality and
Individual Differences, 159, 109888.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Song, M. (2020). The cultural dimension of intergroup con-
spiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology.
Washburn, A. N., & Skitka, L. J. (2017). Science denial across the political divide: Liberals
and conservatives are similarly motivated to deny attitude-inconsistent science. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 972–980.
Webber, D., Babush, M., Schori-Eyal, N., Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, A., Hettiarachchi, M.,
Bélanger, J. J., Moyano, M., Trujillo, H., Gunaratra, R., Kruglanski, A. W., & Gelfand,
M. J. (2018). The road to extremism: Field and experimental evidence that signifi-
cance loss-induced need for closure fosters radicalization. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 114, 270–285.
Zmigrod, L., Rentfrow, P. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2020). The partisan mind: Is extreme
political partisanship related to cognitive inflexibility? Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 149, 407–418.
Zwicker, M.,Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Krouwel, A. P. M. (2020). Persistent beliefs: Political
extremism predicts ideological stability over time. Group Processes & Intergroup
Relations, 23, 1137–1149.
Section 1
Underlying processes of
political polarization
Chapter 2
With the rise of sharp partisan and ideological divides in both the United States
and other nations, social scientists have labored to make sense of why we seem
further apart than before. In this vein, a sizable body of research has focused on
the role of political parties and the “elites” who lead them. According to these
accounts, citizens have diverged from one another in their political attitudes
and in their feelings toward their opponents as leaders of different political
parties have drifted apart from one another in their ideological viewpoints
(Abramowitz, 2010; Levendusky, 2009) and as parties have diverged in terms
of the groups they attract (Iyengar, Sood, & Lelkes, 2012; Mason, 2018). These
perspectives suggest a top-down process of polarization: citizens begin to drift
apart from one another in their attitudes and in their feelings toward political
opponents when political “elites” model more extreme ideological positions
and appear to represent more ideologically opposed social groups (Johnston,
Lavine, & Federico, 2017; see also Zaller, 1992).
However, research also suggests that polarized political opinions may have
deep psychological roots. As an influential line of research in personality and
social psychology indicates, political differences can reliably be predicted from
psychological differences in a bottom-up fashion. Individuals with varying per-
sonality traits, needs, and motives tend to adopt different political preferences
(Federico & Malka, 2018; Gerber, Huber, Doherty, & Dowling, 2011; Hibbing,
Smith, & Alford, 2014; Jost, 2017; Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2009; Jost, Sulloway,
Glaser, & Kruglanski, 2003; Mondak, 2010). In particular, those who are
dispositionally inclined to seek certainty and security are more likely to lean
to the right, whereas those who are more comfortable with uncertainty, ambi-
guity, and risk lean to the left (Jost et al., 2003, 2009; Jost, Federico, & Napier,
2013). These findings imply that polarization at the mass level is not merely
a byproduct of elite divisions; rather, it reflects fundamental variation among
human beings in basic psychological motivations (Jost et al., 2003; see also
Hibbing et al., 2014).
These two perspectives on political polarization have unfolded on largely
parallel tracks, without speaking too extensively to one another. In part, this
18 Christopher M. Federico
2008, 2009; Kemmelmeier, 1997; Van Hiel, Pandelaere, & Duriez, 2004; Van
Hiel et al., 2010); high conscientiousness and low openness to experience in the con-
text of the Big Five model of personality (e.g., Carney, Jost, Gosling, & Potter,
2008; Gerber et al., 2010, 2011; McCrae, 1996; Mondak, 2010); a preference
for conservation values (i.e., tradition, conformity, and security) over a function-
ally opposed set of openness values (i.e., stimulation and self-direction; Caprara,
Schwartz, Capanna,Vechionne, & Barbaranelli, 2006; Goren, 2012; Malka, Soto,
Inzlicht, & Lelkes, 2014; Schwartz, 1992, 2007; Thorisdottir et al., 2007); and a
tendency to prioritize binding moral concerns linked to ingroup loyalty, respect
for authority, and the maintenance of moral purity, all of which reflect a desire
for the security and certainty provided by strong social ties (Graham, Haidt, &
Nosek, 2009; Haidt, 2012; Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto, & Haidt, 2012).
Thus, across a variety of variables, those with differing existential and epi-
stemic orientations tend to polarize in their political preferences: stronger needs
for security and certainty are reliably associated with right-leaning orientations,
whereas weaker needs for security and certainty are associated with left-leaning
ones. This point is reinforced by recent meta-analyses of this literature (Jost,
2017). With respect to existential concerns, Jost, Stern, Rule, and Sterling
(2017b) examined data from 134 studies including 369,525 participants and
found “small-to-moderate” relationships between variables linked to the sali-
ence of fear and threat and support for right-wing policies, parties, and leaders.
With respect to epistemic concerns, Jost, Sterling, and Stern (2017a) aggregated
results from 181 samples including 133,796 participants and observed signifi-
cant relationships between needs for structure and order, need for closure,
intolerance of ambiguity, rigidity, and dogmatic and right-wing preferences
and between integrative complexity, analytic thinking, need for cognition, and
uncertainty tolerance and liberal preferences.
Though the tendency for those with varying needs for security and cer-
tainty to adopt politically polarized preferences is robust, it is also subject to
important boundary conditions. One of these pertains to whether needs for
security and certainty are conceptualized in subjective or objective terms. Though
the findings reviewed above suggest a general tendency for needs for security
and certainty—which reflect a kind of psychological “rigidity”—to go with
the politics of the right, Van Hiel, Onraet, and DePauw (2010) and Van Hiel,
Onraet, Crowson, and Roets (2016) find that “objective” behavioral measures
of intolerance of ambiguity and rigidity that assess participants’ ability to
perform cognitive operations requiring flexibility and attention to multiple,
inter-related aspects of a problem are more weakly related to conservatism
than “subjective” self-report measures that explicitly ask participants to indi-
cate how well various statements describe them (e.g., “I enjoy having a clear
and structured mode of life”; Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). Reinforcing this,
recent studies have failed to replicate earlier results (e.g., Oxley et al., 2008)
indicating a correlation between political preferences and strong physiological
responses to objective tasks in which participants are exposed to threatening
Psychological and political differences 21
and practices or how much diversity in culture and belief is desirable are more
likely to divide those with different existential and epistemic needs (Feldman
& Johnston, 2014; Johnston et al., 2017). In general, partisan and ideological
groupings in the United States and Western Europe are clearly differentiated
along this social axis; the right and parties of the right favor tradition and tighter
cultural norms, whereas the left and parties of the left are more open to change
and normative diversity (Ellis & Stimson, 2012; Hetherington & Weiler, 2009;
Kitschelt et al., 2010). To the extent that the engaged are more aware of these
distinctions between political-menu options, then we should find that engaged
citizens are more polarized in their partisan and ideological identifications as a
function of differences in needs for security and certainty.
With respect to partisanship, work by my colleagues and I provides ample
evidence for this prediction. For example, Federico and Reifen-Tagar (2014)
examined the relationship between authoritarianism and party identification
in the 2004 and 2008 American National Election Studies. We found that
respondents low and high in authoritarianism were more likely to diverge in
their partisan identifications when they were higher in educational attainment
(a variable linked to political awareness; Sniderman et al., 1991). Among
those with greater education, low authoritarians were more Democratic and
high authoritarians were more Republican. Similarly, Johnston, Lavine, and
Federico (2017) found that the tendency for those low in authoritarianism
to identify with the Democratic Party and those high in authoritarianism
to identify with the Republican Party was stronger among individuals who
scored higher on a composite measure of political engagement (reflecting both
knowledge and interest) in the 2000, 2004, 2008, and 2012 American National
Election Studies.
Parallel results emerge for other psychological variables. Johnston, Lavine, and
Federico (2017) also examined the moderating effects of engagement on the
relationship between other existential and epistemic variables and partisanship
in multiple representative samples of American adults. In their data, individ-
uals differing in need for closure, endorsement of conservation values (versus
openness values) and binding moral concerns, and the openness to experience
and conscientious dimensions of the Big Five were more likely to polarize in
their partisan preferences when they were high in engagement. Among the
more engaged, those high in need for closure, conservation values, binding
morality, and conscientiousness were more likely to gravitate toward the GOP,
whereas those low on these dimensions were more likely to identify with the
Democratic Party. In contrast, those low in openness were more Republican
and those high in openness were more Democratic at the upper end of the
engagement spectrum.
With respect to ideological self- placement, engagement has analogous
polarizing effects. For instance, Federico, Fisher, and Deason (2011) and
Johnston, Lavine, and Federico (2017) found that the relationship between
Psychological and political differences 25
high in needs for security and certainty are more left-wing in their economic
preferences.
Consistent with this “reversal” hypothesis, Johnston, Lavine, and Federico
(2017) found that individuals low and high in needs for security and certainty
polarize in opposite ideological directions depending on their level of political
engagement. They examined this prediction in ten nationally representative
datasets of American adults, using a range of existential and epistemic variables
(including authoritarianism, need for closure, dispositional risk aversion, con-
servation versus openness values, and the openness and conscientiousness
dimensions of the Big Five). Among engaged individuals, 21 out of 21 tests
indicated that those differing in needs for security and certainty polarized in
the “traditional” ideological direction: those low in needs for security and cer-
tainty preferred a more interventionist government, whereas those high in
needs for security and certainty preferred a more market-oriented approach.
In contrast, among those low in engagement, individuals with different exist-
ential and epistemic dispositions polarized in the opposite ideological direc-
tion: those low in needs for security and certainty preferred free markets and
those high in needs for security and certainty preferred that the government
step in to provide economic security.
Again, this result has been replicated internationally by other researchers.
Specifically, using the same 51-nation World Values Survey dataset referred to
earlier,Ariel Malka and his colleagues (2014) found that a preference for conser-
vation (over openness) values predicted right-wing, market-oriented economic
attitudes among those high in political interest but left-wing, interventionist
economic attitudes among those low in interest. Thus, both our research and
that of others broadly suggests that engagement may polarize issue attitudes as a
function of psychological needs, but that this effect may be somewhat complex
in the realm of economics.
Conclusion
In this chapter, I have reviewed work suggesting that psychological perspectives
on polarization need to account for the roles of both “bottom-up” and “top-
down” processes in the emergence of political differences. Like a growing
body of work in personality and social psychology (Jost et al., 2009, 2013), the
approach I outline here argues that a variety of individual differences in needs
for security and certainty intuitively prepare people to be more attracted to
some political identities and preferences than others. At the same time, drawing
on a venerable line of research in political science on elite opinion leader-
ship, it also suggests that differences in existential and epistemic needs will not
express themselves in terms of polarized political positions unless individ-
uals are politically engaged to learn what positions “match” their underlying
needs, traits, and motives. In support of this hybrid model, I have reviewed
28 Christopher M. Federico
research suggesting that individual variation in needs for security and certainty
is more likely to be associated with polarized partisan and ideological affinities
among the politically engaged. Moreover, my colleagues and I have also found
that the moderating role of engagement extends to the relationship between
existential and epistemic needs and issue attitudes, with the nature of pattern
being especially complex in the “hard” domain of economics. In the latter
case, engagement does not merely amplify the relationship between psycho-
logical dispositions and issue preferences. Rather, it actually reverses them, such
that needs for security and certainty are associated with right-wing economic
preferences among the engaged and left-wing economic preferences among
the less engaged.
Together, the findings reviewed here suggest a number of broader
implications. First, though they reinforce the general argument that psycho-
logical differences matter for the formation of political preferences, they also
suggest that these differences are translated into political differences via mul-
tiple mechanisms (Federico & Malka, 2018). Sometimes the connection is
direct, such that individuals with strong needs for security and certainty seek
the symbolic “safety” of the status quo by gravitating to the right and those
who are less sensitive to threat and uncertainty are more open to the change
implied by positions on the left. This process appears to be at work in rela-
tionship between existential and epistemic needs and social-issue preferences,
as well as the relationship between these needs and political identities (such as
partisanship and ideology) that have become discursively linked to social-issue
disagreements. The research reviewed here also suggests that this process is at
work in the relationship between strong needs for security and certainty and
left-wing economic preferences found among those low in political engagement.
In other cases, the connection is indirect or “menu-dependent” (Malka & Soto,
2015; see also Johnston et al., 2017), such that individuals with different needs
for security and certainty adopt different political positions mainly because they
have sorted into different political identities and take cues about issue positions
from elites who share those identities. This process is the one at work in the
relationship between strong needs for security and certainty and right-wing
economic preferences among the engaged.
Of course, the model I develop here (like others) is not without its limitations.
In particular, it speaks primarily to the political implications of “subjective,” self-
report measures of psychological dispositions; none of the studies in question
examined whether engagement also moderates the relationship between
“objective” behavioral measures of rigidity and threat sensitivity and political
preferences. As noted previously, subjective measures are more strongly related
than objective measures to political preferences (Van Hiel et al., 2016). Thus,
the patterns predicted by the approach outlined in this chapter may or may not
generalize to objective psychological measures. On one hand, it may be the
case that the relatively implicit cognitive and motivational differences tapped
by objective measures are simply more difficult for individuals to connect with
Psychological and political differences 29
politics than the differences measured by self-reports. In this case, the cognitive
“boost” provided by political engagement may be especially impactful with
respect to objective measures, leading to even stronger moderating effects of
engagement.
On the other hand, the asymmetry between subjective and objective psy-
chological dispositions may be more fundamental, leaving objective indices
unrelated to political preferences even among the engaged. One crucial diffe-
rence between the two types of measures potentially points in this direction.
Self-reports of needs for security and certainty—like survey items inquiring
about partisan and ideological identifications or issue attitudes—ask individuals
to consciously agree or disagree with symbol-laden content that has evalu-
ative implications. In contrast, behavioral measures usually involve tasks that
do not ask participants to make subjective semantic judgments (Van Hiel et al.,
2010). This suggests that psychological dispositions measured by self-reports
may be more matched with political preferences in terms of compatibility or
similarity (in that both ask for evaluative responses; Kraus, 1995). As such, the
self-perceptions and worldviews tapped by self-report psychological measures
may be more easily connected with (and integrated into) different symbolically
laden political belief systems. Indeed, for the politically engaged, the perceptions
and self-characterizations that comprise responses to self-report measures of
existential and epistemic needs may be thought of as extended, distal elements
of political belief systems. Conversely, the explicitly political aspects of these
belief systems may become more important aspects of the self for those who
are more politically aware (Federico & Ekstrom, 2018).
Another potential boundary condition on the model developed here is that
it might not apply equally to all variables indicative of “rigidity” or “sophistica-
tion.” As I note above, the present model is intended specifically as an approach
to understanding the interplay between “rigidity” in the form of subjective
individual-difference measures of needs for security and certainty and “sophis-
tication” in the form of domain-specific political knowledge and interest (e.g.,
Federico & Malka, 2018). But there are other forms of rigidity and sophisti-
cation, and they might exhibit dynamics different from those in my model. In
particular, rigidity can also be conceptualized as an outcome of investing the self
in specific political commitments, as opposed to a general trait prior to politics.
In this case, individuals with views that are polarized to the right and the left
may display forms of motivated rigidity reflecting a defensive commitment to
their political preferences and a tendency to parse the political world in black-
and-white terms—outcomes that are often regarded as signs of low sophistication
(see Zmigrod, Rentfrow, & Robbins, 2020)
Along these lines, when the scope of inquiry is extended beyond self-
reported traits, individuals at both extremes may show signs of rigidity, such
as higher levels of defensive bias (Ditto et al., 2019; Washburn & Skitka, 2017),
greater avoidance of opposing opinions (Frimer, Skitka, & Motyl, 2017), greater
intolerance of those in opposing political coalitions (van Prooijen & Krouwel,
30 Christopher M. Federico
2017; van Prooijen, Krouwel, Boiten, & Eedebak, 2015; see also Brandt &
Crawford, 2020), a stronger tendency to overclaim knowledge that one does
not objectively possess (van Prooijen & Krouwel, 2019), a greater prefer-
ence for simple solutions to problems (accompanied by greater judgmental
certainty; van Prooijen, Krouwel, & Emmer, 2018), and a more-pronounced
tendency to rigidly categorize political stimuli (Lammers, Koch, Conway, &
Brandt, 2017). Moreover, even generalized trait-like measures of rigidity are
associated with extremity to both the left and right (rather a right-wing orien-
tation) when extremity is operationalized as strength of partisan social identi-
fication as opposed to simple left–r ight self-placement (Luttig, 2018; Zmigrod
et al., 2020).
These findings—taken together with the theory and research that are the
primary focus of this chapter—suggest the need for several crucial concep-
tual distinctions. First, it is important for researchers to distinguish between
rigidity in the sense of subjective needs for security and certainty and rigidity
in the sense of motivated defensiveness or simplicity of perception. The former
may elicit sorting into conservative identities, especially among the politically
engaged, whereas the latter may result from committing the self to identities
and preferences on either the right or left. Second, it is necessary to distin-
guish between political engagement as a domain-specific form of sophistica-
tion and generalized indicators of cognitive sophistication. The former may act
primarily as a moderator, amplifying the sorting of individuals with different
existential and epistemic needs into polarized political camps. In contrast, the
latter may reflect the aforementioned rigidity that comes from having polit-
ical commitments at all, whether they be on the right or the left. With these
points in mind, researchers may find it easier to integrate the various strands
of an increasingly complex body of inquiry in the link between psychological
variables and political preferences.
Acknowledgment
The author would like to thank Jan-Willem van Prooijen for his comments,
suggestions, and editorial assistance.
Note
1 There are of course exceptions to this pattern, with some studies showing robust
relationships between variables related to needs for security and certainty and
attitudes in both the social and economic domains (see Jost et al., 2017b; see also
Azevedo et al., 2019; Gerber et al., 2010; Hennes, Nam, Stern, & Jost, 2012). However,
as Federico and Malka (2018, p. 14) note in their review of this literature, many of
these analyses (1) still show a stronger relationship between needs for security and cer-
tainty and social (versus economic) attitudes and (2) use measures of threat or inse-
curity that include explicit political content, thereby introducing potential artifacts.
Psychological and political differences 31
References
Abramowitz, A. I. (2010). The disappearing center: Engaged citizens, polarization, and
American democracy. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The
authoritarian personality. New York, NY: Harper.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality”. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 30. 47–92.
Azevedo, F., Jost, J. T., Rothmund, T., & Sterling, J. (2019). Neoliberal ideology and the
justification of inequality in capitalist societies: Why social and economic dimensions
of ideology are intertwined. Journal of Social Issues, 75, 49–88.
Bakker, B. N., Schumacher, G., Gothreau, C., & Arceneaux, K. (2020). Conservatives
and liberals have similar physiological responses to threat. Nature Human Behavior,
4(6), 613–621. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z
Baldassari, D., & Gelman, A. (2008). Partisans without constraint: Political polarization
and trends in American public opinion. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 408–446.
Braithwaite,V. (1997). Harmony and security value orientations in political evaluation.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 401–414.
Brandt, M. J., & Crawford, J. T. (2020). Worldview conflict and prejudice. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 61, 1–66.
Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller,W., & Stokes,W. (1960). The American voter. New York,
NY: Wiley.
Caprara, G. V., Schwartz, S., Capanna, C., Vecchione, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2006).
Personality and politics: Values, traits, and political choice. Political Psychology, 27, 1–28.
Carmines, E. G., Ensley, M. J., & Wagner, M. W. (2012). Political ideology in American
politics: One, two, or none? The Forum, 10, 1–18.
Carmines, E. G., & Stimson, J. A. (1980). The two faces of issue voting. The American
Political Science Review, 74(1), 78–91.
Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and
conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind.
Political Psychology, 29, 807–840.
Chirumbolo, A., Areni, A., & Sensales, G. (2004). Need for cognitive closure and pol-
itics: Voting, political attitudes and attributional style. International Journal of Psychology,
39, 245–253.
Cizmar, A. M., Layman, G. C., McTague, J., Pearson-Merkowitz, S., & Spivey, M. (2014).
Authoritarianism and American political behavior from 1952 to 2008. Political
Research Quarterly, 67(1), 71–83.
Converse, P. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. Apter (Ed.),
Ideology and discontent. New York, NY: Free Press.
Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it
matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Ditto, P. H., Liu, B. S., Clark, C. J., Wojcik, S. P., Chen, E. E., Grady, R.H., … Zinger,
J. F. (2019). At least bias is bipartisan: A meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias
in liberals and conservatives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 273–291. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691617746796
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and preju-
dice. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 41–113.
32 Christopher M. Federico
Fleishman, J. A. (1988). Attitude organization in the general public: Evidence for a
bidimensional structure. Social Forces, 67, 159–184.
Frimer, J. A., Skitka, L. J., & Motyl, M. (2017). Liberals and conservatives are similarly
motivated to avoid exposure to one another’s opinions. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 72, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.003
Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Personality
and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts.
American Political Science Review, 104(01), 111–133.
Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2011).The Big Five person-
ality traits in the political arena. Annual Review of Political Science, 14, 265–287.
Gerth, H., & Mills, C. W. (1953). Character and social structure. New York, NY: Harcourt,
Brace, & World.
Goren, P. (2012). On voter competence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. (2009). Liberals and conservatives use different sets of
moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029–1046.
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion.
New York, NY: Random House.
Hennes, E. P., Nam, H. H., Stern, C., & Jost, J. T. (2012). Not all ideologies are created
equal: Epistemic, existential, and relational needs predict system-justifying attitudes.
Social Cognition, 30(6), 669–688.
Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American
politics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie
variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 297–307.
Iyengar, S., Sood, G., and Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity per-
spective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76, 405–431.
Johnston, C. D., Lavine, H. G., & Federico, C. M. (2017). Open versus closed: Personality,
identity, and the politics of redistribution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61, 651–670.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651
Jost, J. T. (2017). Ideological asymmetries and the essence of political psychology.
Political Psychology, 38, 167–208. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12407
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political Ideology: Its structure,
functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–333.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2013). Political ideologies and their social
psychological functions. In M. Freeden, L. T. Sargent, & M. Stears (Eds.), Handbook of
political ideologies (pp. 232–250). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as
motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.
Jost, J. T., Sterling, J., & Stern, C. (2017). Getting closure on conservatism, or the pol-
itics of epistemic and existential motivation. In C. Kopetz & A. Fishbach (Eds.), The
motivation–cognition interface; From the lab to the real world: A Festschrift in honor of Arie
W. Kruglanski. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 56–87.
Jost, J. T., Stern, C., Rule, N. O., & Sterling, J. (2017). The politics of fear: Is there an
ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Social Cognition, 35, 324–353.
Kahan, D. M. (2015). What is the ‘science of science communication’? Journal of Science
Communication, 14(3), 1–12.
34 Christopher M. Federico
Luttig, M. D. (2018). The ‘prejudiced personality’ and the origins of partisan strength,
affective polarization, and partisan sorting. Advances in Political Psychology, 39(S1),
239–256.
Malka, A., & Lelkes, Y. (2010). More than ideology: Conservative-liberal identity and
receptivity to political cues. Social Justice Research, 23, 156–188.
Malka, A., Lelkes,Y., & Soto, C.J. (2019). Are cultural and economic conservatism posi-
tively correlated? A large-scale cross-national test. British Journal of Political Science, 49,
1045–1069. doi:10.1017/S0007123417000072, 1–25.
Malka, A., & Soto, C. J. (2015). Rigidity of the economic right? Menu-independent
and menu-dependent influences of psychological dispositions on political attitudes.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 137–142. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0963721414556340
Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for security and cer-
tainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 1031–1051.
Mason, L. (2018). Uncivil agreement: How politics became our identity. Chicago, IL: University
of Chicago Press.
McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
McClosky, H. (1958). Conservatism and personality. American Political Science Review,
52, 27–45.
McClosky, H., & Zaller, J. (1984). The American ethos. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological
Bulletin, 120, 323–337.
Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Morgan, G. S., & Wisneski, D. C. (2017). The structure of political ideology varies
within and between people: Implications for theories about ideology’s causes. Social
Cognition, 35, 395–414.
Noel, H. (2013). Political ideologies and political parties in America. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Osborne, D., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). Does personality matter? Openness correlates with
vote choice, but particularly for politically sophisticated voters. Journal of Research in
Personality, 46(6), 743–751.
Osborne, D., & Sibley, C. G. (2015). Within the limits of civic training: Education
moderates the relationship between openness and political attitudes. Political
Psychology, 36(3), 295–313.
Osmundsen, M., Hendry, D. J., Laustsen, S., Smith, K. N., & Petersen, M. B. (2020). The
psychophysiology of political ideology: Replications, reanalysis, and recommenda
tions. Journal of Politics.
Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M.V., Miller, M. S., Scalora, M., et al.
(2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667–1670.
Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
25, 1–65.
36 Christopher M. Federico
Van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A. P. M., Boiten, M., & Eendebak, L. (2015). Fear among
the extremes: How political ideology predicts negative emotions and outgroup
derogation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 485–497. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0146167215569706
Van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A. P. M., & Emmer, J. (2018). Ideological responses to
the EU refugee crisis: The left, the right, and the extremes. Social Psychological &
Personality Science, 9, 143–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550617731501
Washburn, A. N., & Skitka, L. J. (2017). Science denial across the political divide: Liberals
and conservatives are similarly motivated to deny attitude-inconsistent science. Social
Psychological & Personality Science, 9, 927–980. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506
17731500
Weber, M. (1948). From Max Weber: Essays in sociology. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive
closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062.
Wilson, G. D. (1973). The psychology of conservatism. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2016). An attempt to clarify the link between cognitive
style and political ideology: A non-Western replication and extension. Judgment and
Decision Making, 11, 287–300.
Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Zmigrod, L., Rentfrow, P. J., & Robbins,T.W. (2020).The partisan mind: Is extreme pol-
itical partisanship related to cognitive inflexibility? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 149, 407–418.
Chapter 3
Polarization and populism have become key words to explain recent political
developments (e.g., Abramowitz & Saunders, 2008; Inglehart & Norris, 2016;
Mudde, 2004; Simas, Clifford, & Kirkland, 2020). At first sight, both phenomena
seem to imply each other. Indeed, on the one hand, polarization seems to be
an important precursor of populism, and it is almost self-evident that, in times
of polarization, populism is on the rise. On the other hand, the mere presence
of populism seems to imply that it has been based on ever-increasing polariza-
tion. However, such connections are not obvious at all. Specifically, polarization
means that the pivotal political dimensions are stretched in such a way that
the contradictions they entail become ever greater. This is something gradual,
which evolves over time. Conversely, populism implies the separation of two
homogeneous and antagonistic groups, ‘the pure people’ versus the ‘corrupt
elite’ (Mudde, 2007). This means disruption, a break between the radical voter
and the existing political establishment.We argue that the idea of a rupture best
captures the distinction between moderates and radicals.
In fact, we argue that polarization is a process that only has relevance for
explaining differences among moderate citizens, not for differences between
moderates and radicals. We thus disclaim that polarization is at the basis of the
increasing support for populist and radical parties. We, however, further argue
that the term ‘populism’ as a psychological and individual-level variable does
not add much to our understanding of such ideology and the radical-populist
Weltanschauung. Instead, we propose that political cynicism offers a better
explanation of the radical mindset. Note that we use the terms radicalism and
populism as synonyms, as both terms usually refer to the same political parties,
which are invariably extreme on either the left-wing or the right-wing side of
the political spectrum.1
In the remainder of this chapter, we will first explain how psychological
polarization operates, and how it has been used in political psychology to
explain political polarization and extremism. Next, we present evidence that
radicalism is not so much about being extreme right-wing or left-wing, but
instead is characterized on both sides of the political spectrum by political
Radical and populist supporter mindset 39
cynicism. Radical parties thus primarily attract politically cynical citizens, and
we will show that the ‘old concept’ of political cynicism and the ‘new’ individual
differences variable of populism coincide and are indistinguishable.We will fur-
ther argue that political cynicism offers a better explanation of the mental state
of adherents of so-called populist parties than the concept of populism itself.
respect to the personal values they cherish. Specifically,Van Hiel (2012) found
that, especially on the extreme sides of the political spectrum, there is more
variability in personal values such as openness, conformity and safety than on
the moderate positions. Exactly these values have been hypothesized to be at
the basis of left–r ight ideology.
Second, also incompatible with the polarization hypothesis, is the finding
that it is not their sheer position on the left–r ight dimension that distinguishes
moderates and radical right-wing adherents the most, but other factors come
into play as well. Political party preferences often coincide with each other and
form clusters. For instance, in the typical Western European situation, those
who like the Social Democrats also often are positively inclined toward green
parties as well, but they tend to dislike right-wing parties (e.g., Van Hiel &
Mervielde, 2002). Analysis of party preferences in Belgian (Flemish) and Dutch
samples reveals four distinct party types: libertarian, traditional left, traditional
right and radical right.2 Well, what makes adherents of the radical right different
from the other voters? Van Assche,Van Hiel, Dhont, and Roets (2019) reported
that the radical right-wing parties mainly attract voters who are highly polit-
ically cynical and who have negative attitudes towards minorities and immi-
gration. The left–right dimension –operationalized in terms of right-wing
authoritarianism (Altemeyer, 1981) and social dominance orientation (Pratto,
Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle, 1994) –was less important to characterize the
radical-right position, but was particularly relevant to distinguish among the
adherents of the various moderate parties. Should polarization in terms of an
expanding left–r ight position be the real cause of radicalism and populism, then
supporters of a right-radical party would have to distinguish themselves from
the advocates of the other parties, especially on this dimension, that is, in terms
of left–r ight attitudes. But, such a supreme effect of left–r ight attitudes is not
what the data showed.
The data we just discussed pertained to the ideological attitudes of supporters
of radical and populist parties. Also note that we can make a similar case against
polarization solely based on the political level, that is, on the level of ideology
itself. Indeed, Mudde (2004) already stated that populist parties are ‘ideologic-
ally poor’. For example, they select left and right recipes that they embed in
their own story as they see fit. The Italian Five-Star movement, for example,
combines both typical left and right recipes, and thus cannot be called left or
right, which the party officials consider to be an outdated dichotomy (Mosca
& Tronconi, 2019). During the last elections of the Belgian parliament, the
Flemish radical right-wing party Vlaams Belang positioned itself on the left for
socio-economic themes. The radical right-wing PVV in the Netherlands had
also been in favor of policies that can be called progressive, especially in the
domain of welfare.
For all these reasons, both located at the individual-attitudinal level as well
as on the level of populist-radical ideology itself, polarization in terms of ever-
expanding differences between the left and the right does not offer the best
42 Alain Van Hiel et al.
seems as if this dimension took some time to crystallize into a topic in its own
right. Indeed, right-wing ideological attitudes were invariably seen as the basis
of prejudice. A lot of evidence was gathered for this over the years, in many
political contexts (Sibley & Duckitt, 2008). The idea that prejudice itself can
form a basis –alongside right-wing ideological beliefs –was not seen as an
interesting research question, as it seemed too self-evident. However, although
diversity has been a lingering issue for quite some time, it is becoming increas-
ingly important in political debate, and it has become a topic in its own right.
Attitudes towards diversity may show an increasingly looser connection with
left–r ight attitudes than in the past, and it may become an important source of
radicalization (in contrast to left–r ight attitudes, which do not have this radic-
alizing potential any more).
A second substantive alignment runs along the socio-economic axis. The
radical left draws on a reservoir of people who are anti-capitalist and perceive
globalization, international banking and unbridled capitalism as major threats
to society (see Van Hauwaert & Van Kessel, 2018). Traditional parties, and even
social democratic parties, have been unable to provide sufficient counterweight
to socio-economic globalization and deregulation. They have even actively
participated in this, and therefore they no longer offer an ideological home to
people who are against this evolution.
The research by Van Assche et al. (2019) shed some light on these issues,
by showing exactly how these alignments rearrange the political field. In a
regression analysis in which left–right ideology, political cynicism and ethnic
prejudice are included, the preference for radical-r ight parties was significantly
determined by increased political cynicism and ethnic prejudice, whereas the
relationship with left–r ight attitudes was less important. Specifically, the radical
right parties are particularly charming to voters who are highly politically
cynical and who are biased against minorities and negatively inclined towards
immigration. Hence, political cynicism and immigration attitudes on the right-
wing side, and political cynicism and undoubtedly anti-capitalism attitudes on
the left-wing side (this should still be demonstrated in a suitable political con-
text, like for instance in Southern Europe), are particularly useful to distinguish
the electorate of radical parties from supporters of moderate parties.
increased polarization over the last few years, then this would be of the greatest
relevance for our understanding of the electorate of traditional left-wing and
right-wing parties. Thus, even though adherents of moderate parties are ‘in the
system’, and even though they order their opinions on the shared, left–right
dimension, polarization can create serious disparities, which makes it difficult
to find common ground.
We guess that moderate political parties, however, do not have any other
option left other than to bridge their ideological differences, in order to be able to
reach compromises that are acceptable to all. Traditional parties urgently need to
depolarize, leave their typical left-versus-r ight discussions, and instead try to solve
together the most important problems, and as such act as role models for their
followers. There is no other option left than to become trust agents, to become
bridge builders at the service of society. Indeed, a mutual ‘combat strategy’ among
traditional parties would probably be best attuned with the attitudes of their
followers, but it underlines their powerlessness to arrive at solutions together. One
could say that the lack of unity and decisiveness comes across as a never-ending
struggle for power, at the cost of searching for substantial solutions and showing
genuine concern for citizens.These so-called ‘political games’ –real or portrayed
as such in the media –further increase political cynicism (e.g., Cappella &
Jamieson, 1997; Erber & Lau, 1990), and thus pave the way for the further success
of radical parties. In this view, not ‘psychological polarization’ (at the attitudinal
level), but ‘political polarization’ at the level of political parties, may fuel political
cynicism, and therefore, the electoral success of populist and radical parties.
The political counter-reaction, of course, can also come from non-traditional
parties. Western Europe is witnessing anti-radical initiatives that try to unite
citizens.The best-known example of such an attempt is the political movement
of Emmanuel Macron, La République en Marche (or, abbreviated, en Marche).
But, here again, the key to success resided not in firm left-wing or right-wing
recipes, but instead in the attempt to find a consensus that transcends left–r ight
contradictions.This new movement united the political middle and at the same
time wiped out the important political parties that had dominated France for
decades (especially the Social Democrats and Republicans). En Marche became
the challenger of the radical right (i.e., Front National). The positive alternative
presented by the Macron movement was striking, and paid off electorally. This
seems to be a better strategy than either to emphasize left–r ight contradictions,
or to portray radical parties as populist, demagogic and irresponsible (Stavrakakis
& Katsambekis, 2019). It is indeed doubtful that the latter strategy will bring
the radical voter back to the traditional herd, as has been shown by Stavrakakis
and Katsambekis, who analyzed the Greek political situation.
Conclusion
Do polarization and populism coincide? We have argued that there is
no direct link between polarization and populism, and that adherents of
Radical and populist supporter mindset 49
Notes
1 Although relatively rare, various authors (e.g., Muller, 2016) have noted that centric
populism is a possibility, and a common example in this respect is Silvio Berlusconi,
a populist who was center-r ight but not far-r ight.
2 The party landscape in these contexts lacks a successful radical left-wing party.
3 These measurement models show modest values on a number of fit indices. Both
the populism and political cynicism measures should thus be further refined in
future studies.
References
Abramowitz, A., & Saunders, K. L. (2008). Is polarization a myth? Journal of Politics, 7,
542–555.
Abrams, D. A.,Wheterell, M., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing
what to think by knowing who you are: Self-categorization and the nature of norm
formation, conformity, and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29,
97–119.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The
authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
Agger, R. E., Goldstein, M. N., & Pearl, S. A. (1961). Political cynicism: Measurement
and meaning. The Journal of Politics, 23, 477–506.
Akkerman,A., Mudde, C., & Zaslove,A. (2014). How populist are the people? Measuring
populist attitudes in voters. Comparative Political Studies, 47, 1324–1353.
Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarism. Winnipeg, Canada: Univ. Of Manitoba
Press.
Ansolabehere, S., & Iyengar, S. (1995). Going negative. New York: Free Press.
Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press.
Cappella, J. N., & Jamieson, K. H. (1997). Spiral of cynicism: The press and the public good.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R, Professional manual: Revised NEO
Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa,
Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Da Silva, D. (2009). Towards a pedagogy of trust. Departmental bulletin paper. Keisen
University Bulletin, 21, 85–101.
50 Alain Van Hiel et al.
Van Assche, J., Dhont, K.,Van Hiel, A., & Roets, A. (2018). Ethnic diversity and support
for populist parties: The “right” road through political cynicism and lack of trust.
Social Psychology, 49(3), 182–189.
Van Assche, J., Van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., & Roets, A. (2019). Broadening the individual
differences lens on party support and voting behavior: Cynicism and prejudice as
relevant attitudes referring to modern-day political alignments. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 49, 190–199.
Van Hauwaert, S. M., Schimpf, C. H., & Azevedo, F. (2020).The measurement of populist
attitudes: Testing cross-national scales using item response theory. Politics, 40, 3–21.
Van Hauwaert, S. M., & Van Kessel, S. (2018). Beyond protest and discontent: A cross-
national analysis of the effect of populist attitudes and issue positions on populist
party support. European Journal of Political Research, 57, 68–92.
Van Hiel, A. (2012). A psycho-political profile of moderates and left-wing and right-
wing extremists. European Journal of Political Research, 51, 166–203.
Van Hiel, A., & Mervielde, I. (2002). Explaining conservative beliefs and political
preferences: A comparison of social dominance orientation and authoritarianism.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 32, 965–976.
Vinokur, A., & Burnstein, E. (1974). Effects of partially shared persuasive arguments on
group-induced shifts: A group-problem solving approach. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 29, 305–315.
Chapter 4
A psychological profile of
extreme Trump supporters
Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
President Trump’s extraordinary tenure in office will end. Whether that end
comes in 2021 or in 2025 (or beyond), it will come. This is probably good
news to most people in the US and around the world, where Trump is almost
universally disliked (Wike, Poushter, Fetterolf & Schumacher, 2020). However,
Trump’s exit will not be the end of Trumpism. Approximately 140 million
Americans still approve of President Trump. When he leaves, they will remain.
To properly understand the Trump era, one must also understand his followers.
Our objective is to psychologically profile supporters of US President Donald
Trump, whom we characterize as extremists. Characterizing such a large sector
of society and supporters of a major political figure in a major democracy as
extremists might raise eyebrows. We suggest that a broad geopolitical and his-
torical perspective will support the view that President Trump’s supporters, as
numerous as they are, are indeed extreme.
Perhaps the least controversial thing about the Trump presidency is that it has
been unusual. Some political and economic regularities (Drezner, 2018) not-
withstanding, Trump had the least prior political experience of any US presi-
dent, and his policies (e.g., on NATO, Russia, free trade, executive authority)
and his personal behavior (e.g., braggadocio, insults, lack of financial transpar-
ency) make him a highly unusual US president or democratically elected head
of state. People will likely disagree about whether such an unusual presidency
has moved the country in the right or the wrong direction. But we suggest
that people of varying political stripes probably agree that an unusual presi-
dency it indeed has been.The Trump presidency itself being far from the main-
stream of presidencies renders Trump support extreme in the sense that it is
non-normative.
Trump’s support has been steady and around 42% of Americans.The “Trump
supporter” demographic is not homogeneous, however. Some of his supporters
are more tentative, conditional, and ambivalent whereas others are more fer-
vent, unconditional, and full- throated in their approval of their president.
Self-identified political conservatism has been strongly associated with levels
of Trump support (Frimer & Skitka, 2018). This is noteworthy in that many
54 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Tribalism
The first common media explanation for Trump support that we con-
sider is tribalism, which comes in three forms: prejudice against outsiders,
unwavering favoritism toward the ingroup, and bloodlust (Pew Research
Center, 2019b).
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 55
Prejudice
Trump supporters’ animosity toward “the other” can often surface in racialized
and gendered forms in that Trump supporters and opponents tend to look
(demographically) different from one another. According to Thought Co the
typical Trump supporter is white and male (Cole, 2019; Pew Research Center,
2019a) whereas Trump’s opponents tend to be more demographically diverse
(The Washington Post; Blake, 2015), with greater representation of black people,
Hispanics, and Asians (The L.A Times; Pearce, 2019). Women and the LGBTQ
community tend to be better represented among Trump’s opponents (Pew
Research Center, 2019a).
President Trump’s promises to “Make America Great Again” might signal
to white Americans that Trump aims to revert to laws that favor the white
majority as they formerly did during the nation’s less diverse past (The Atlantic;
Green, 2017; Khazan, 2018). Non-white immigrants have also been a major
focus of Trump’s attacks. According to The Chicago Counsel, immigration has
become a deeply polarizing issue (Kafura, 2019), with Republicans gener-
ally feeling threatened by immigrants (The Washington Post; Clement & Balz,
2019), and seeing it as a more important issue than do Democrats (MSNBC;
Benen, 2018). To Republicans, immigrants pose a threat to the country’s “vital
interests” (The Washington Post; Clement & Balz, 2019), the traditional American
way of life (USA Today; Collins, 2016), and to their job security (The Washington
Post; Bump, 2019).
President Trump’s signature promise to “build a wall” was central to both
his campaign and to his presidency (The New York Times; Hirschfield-David
& Baker, 2019). His anti-immigrant rhetoric included attacking the morality
of Mexican immigrants, labeling them as “rapists” and “drug smugglers,” and
framing them as an economic threat in the way they were framed as com-
peting for jobs and depleting the social welfare system (TIME; Arce, 2019). At
times, President Trump even advocated for violence against those attempting
to immigrate to the US (The Washington Post; Sonmez, 2018; The Los Angeles
Times;Vives & Castillo, 2019). The Washington Post contends that Trump’s tough
stance on immigration was particularly alluring to conservatives who were
unsatisfied with existing, more subtle approaches to immigration (Scott, 2019a),
and may have been a major factor in his ascension to the White House (Politico;
Kumar, 2019). Proposition 1a is that Trump support is rooted in a particularly nefarious
form of tribal prejudice based in racism, xenophobia, and other forms of antipathy toward
minorities.
Scientific evidence
Conservatives in general appear to be particularly racist against black people
and illegal immigrants.When asked to report their general feelings toward black
people, illegal aliens, and white people on 100-point feeling thermometers,
56 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
conservatives showed a ~12-point gap favoring whites over blacks, and a ~45-
point gap favoring whites over illegal immigrants (Schlenker, Chambers, &
Le, 2012). The analogous gaps for liberals were approximately half the size.
This apparent racism and prejudice against minorities has been found in sev-
eral studies (Meertens & Pettigrew, 1997; Sears & Henry, 2003; Terrizzi, Shook,
& Ventis, 2010), and appears to confirm Proposition 1a, that Trump support
may be rooted in racialized tribalism. However, this conclusion hinges upon
a correlational relationship between ideology and attitudes toward various
demographics, leaving open the possibility of a third variable explaining the
association.
Worldview conflict is an alternative explanation for conservatives’ prejudi-
cial gap: people tend to feel and express prejudice against anyone whose beliefs
conflict with their own (Brandt & Crawford, 2019; Chambers & Melnyk,
2006). For instance, gun rights advocates dislike gun restriction advocates just
as gun restriction advocates dislike gun rights advocates. And liberals tend to
dislike conservatives just as conservatives tend to dislike liberals. Black people
tend to hold liberal worldviews and favor liberal politicians. This political
leaning is evident in exit polls in which black voters overwhelmingly favored
Democrats over Republicans in recent US elections. Black voters favored
Democratic candidates over Republican candidates by 81%, 87%, 91%, and
77% in the 2016–2004 elections respectively (e.g., CNN, 2016). It is possible
that conservatives dislike black people because of black people’s race or because
of black people’s political beliefs, or both.
Attitudes toward conservatively minded black people are revealing of
the source of conservatives’ prejudice toward black people. Experimentally
crossing the race and the political beliefs of the target, Chambers, Schlenker,
and Collisson (2012) found that (white) conservatives expressed similarly posi-
tive attitudes toward conservatives, regardless of whether the conservative was
white or black. And they expressed similarly color-blind negative attitudes
toward liberals. Thus, conservatives’ prejudice against black people appears to
be explained by worldview conflict and may have little inherent basis in skin
color. (Note that we are not claiming that racism does not exist, nor that when
it exists it is always rooted in ideological conflict. We are merely citing data
that suggest that conservatives’ apparent racism may be rooted in ideological
conflict.)
Ingroup favoritism
Conservatives’ disdain for liberals (of all colors) could cause conservatives to
blindly rally behind their culture war leaders, resulting in extreme ingroup
favoritism and deference to authority. While running for president in January
2016, Donald Trump gained the impression that his supporters were unusually
dedicated and deferential to him. Expressing astonishment, he famously
stated: “They say I have the most loyal people, did you ever see that? I could
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 57
stand in the middle of Fifth Avenue and shoot somebody and I wouldn’t lose
voters. It’s like incredible!” (CNN; Diamond, 2016). According to Axios, 62%
of those who approve of his job performance claim they cannot think of any-
thing Trump could do to lose their support (Allasan, 2019). Scandals, such as
the Hollywood access tape, impeachment for abuse of power and obstruc-
tion of Congress, and mismanagement of the COVID-19 pandemic seemed to
have done little to weaken support among his followers (The Washington Post;
Farenthold, 2016), with his approval ratings remaining unusually stable and
between 39% and 45% (Jones, 2018). These anecdotes and statistics suggest
an unusually robust form of deference to and support for authority among
Trump’s supporters.
According to Vox, the Trump orbit is a hotbed for authoritarian submis-
sion: people who are especially likely to defer to authoritarian leaders hold
conservative values such as a defense of hierarchy and social order as a means
to an end of maintaining order and control in an uncontrollable world (Taub,
2016). Psychology Today suggests that President Trump displays qualities con-
sistent with authoritarian leadership, subsequently attracting support from like-
minded individuals looking to thwart perceived troublemakers (Azarian, 2017).
Proposition 1b is that Trump support is rooted in their extreme tendency to defer to their
ingroup leaders.
Scientific evidence
Conservatives have been found to have a pessimistic view of human nature,
seeing people as inherently selfish (Lakoff , 2002). This perspective then justi-
fies strong external forces, such as group loyalty and strong leaders, to maintain
social order. Surveys of liberals and conservatives repeatedly confirmed that
conservatives express more positive attitudes toward the idea of social con-
formity (Schwartz, Caprara, & Vecchione, 2010), a hierarchical social structure
(Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle, 1994), and submitting to author-
ities (Altemeyer, 2004; Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009). Moreover, Trump
supporters per se score especially high on standard measures of authoritar-
ianism obedience (Dunwoody & Plane, 2019; Ludeke, Klitgaard, & Vitriol,
2018; Womick, Rothmund, Azevedo, King, & Jost, 2018). These results seem
to support the proposition that Trump supporters are particularly deferential
to authorities.
Presumably, the same conclusion that Trump’s base is especially loyal toward
and supportive of him should be evident in public opinion data.To find out, we
conducted a novel analysis of all 1046 presidential approval polls (each sample
was ~1000 Americans) that Gallup (2020), conducted over the past 27 years,
beginning in 1993 (President Clinton) and ending in 2020 (President Trump),
with the objective of testing whether leader-directed ingroup favoritism, in
the form of approval of the ingroup president and disapproval of the outgroup
president, was stronger among Republicans than Democrats.
58 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
Ingroup Favoritism
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015 2020
Year
Figure 4.1 shows that ingroup favoritism has been generally strong, with
70+% of Americans displaying ingroup favoritism most of the time. Ingroup
favoritism appears to have strengthened over the past quarter-century, as the
lines generally slope upwards, indicative of the growing culture war. But did
Republicans display more ingroup favoritism than Democrats? The Democrats’
and the Republicans’ lines appear to cross over one another repeatedly in
Figure 4.1, with neither being consistently higher than the other. This appears
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 59
(controlling for the effects of the September 11th 2001 terrorist attacks yielded
a similar conclusion; see Model 3).
The disconnect between (previously reviewed) psychological theory and
research that suggests that Trump supporters are particularly authoritarian on
the one hand and publicly available data (that points to similarities between the
two sides) on the other hand begs for an explanation. One possibility is that the
scales used to support the conclusion that Republicans are more biased than
Democrats may themselves have been biased by conflating the act of obedi-
ence to authority with the act of obeying conservative authorities. For example,
an item on the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale asks participants to
agree or disagree with “It is always better to trust the judgment of the proper
authorities in government and religion than to listen to the noisy rabble-rousers
in our society” (Altemeyer, 2004, p. 86). This wording is confounding because
it leaves open the possibility that conservatives endorse this item more than
liberals because conservatives trust the judgment of any or even all authorities
(an authoritarian tendency) on the one hand, or because of a more generic
form of ingroup favoritism manifest as conservatives trusting the judgment
of their ideologically congenial authorities, like religious leaders. Some scales
appear to avoid this conservative authority confound by not specifying the
authority object, and instead asking general questions about behaviors such as
obedience in general (Schwartz, 1992). However, an object (e.g., an authority
figure) is logically necessary for obedience to happen. Not stating what that
object is might leave the subject to infer or imagine one; when people do infer
an obedience object, they tend to conjure the image of a conservative authority
figure (Frimer, Gaucher, & Schaefer, 2014).
To validly test the idea that conservatives are more obedient than liberals, the
object of obedience needs to be specified and the perceived ideology of the
authority figure needs to be taken into account. When asked about obeying an
ideologically diverse set of authority figures, from civil rights and environmental
leaders on the left to religious and military leaders on the right, conservatives
displayed more obedience only when the authorities were perceived to hold
politically right-wing views (Frimer et al., 2014). When the authority fig-
ures were from the political left, people on the political left displayed elevated
authoritarian tendencies. And when the authority figures were perceived to
be politically neutral (e.g., an office manager), liberals and conservatives were
similarly obedient. And when the authority figures in the RWA scale were
revised to be left-wing authorities, political liberals endorsed authoritarianism,
prejudice, and dogmatism (Conway, Houck, Gornick, & Repke, 2017). These
results are consistent with Realistic Group Conflict Theory, which posits that
people in general (liberals included) fall in line with the crowd and the leader
when groups compete for limited resources like political power (Sherif, Harvey,
White, Hood, & Sherif, 1961). Obedience to authority appears to not be
limited to, or even particularly elevated on, the political right vis-à-vis the left.
Rather, it appears to be a quite pronounced motive on both extremes (Frimer
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 61
et al., 2014), meaning that obedience to Trump might be a product of the larger
culture war.
Bloodlust
Another form of tribalism that might be at play in extreme Trump support is
bloodlust in the form of aggressive desires for attacks on perceived enemies.
President Trump has frequently insulted and attacked Democrats, referring to
them as “sick people” (ABC News; Scott, 2019b) and “Crooked Hillary,” having
issued more than an insult each day on Twitter alone (Frimer & Skitka, 2018).
The Nation characterized his verbal attacks on his political adversaries as tan-
tamount to President Trump “throwing red meat to his base,” which conjures
the image of Trump satiating the ravenous hunger of a vicious pack of wolves
(Abramsky, 2019). Proposition 1c is that Trump’s supporters’ tribal instincts cause them
to approve of Trump causing harm to their shared (perceived) enemies.
Scientific evidence
Conservatives score higher than liberals on standard measures of sadism, psych-
opathy, and general meanness (Lilienfeld, Latzman, Watts, Smith, & Dutton,
2014; Preston & Anestis, 2018) and extreme conservatives score especially high
on these traits (Duspara & Greitmeyer, 2017).These trends might appear to align
the media’s portrayal of Trump supporters as a pack of ravenous wolves that
enjoy a rhetorical bloodbath. But closer inspection points to a different con-
clusion, that Trump’s supporters do not approve of his attacks in the first place.
Elevated scores on a subclinical measure of psychopathy do not necessarily
qualify Trump supporters as psychopaths in the same way that being 1 kg over-
weight does not qualify a person as obese.To put conservatives’ higher desire for
interpersonal conflict in context, Frimer and Skitka (2020; Study 5) asked 1593
Americans from across the political spectrum to indicate whether they prefer
their political leaders to be culture warriors (e.g., “fight with opponents”)
or be governors for all (e.g., be a “country unifier”). Partisanship correlated
with preference in a manner consistent with prior psychopathy and ideology
findings, such that Republicans expressed a stronger preference for culture
warriors than Democrats did. However, people across the political spectrum,
strong Republicans included, preferred governors over warriors (Figure 4.2).
Inferences about Trump supporters’ self-reported preference for national
unity over culture war might be prone to the limits and biases of self-awareness
and qualified by the possibility of socially desirable responding, to which
conservatives tend to be particularly prone (Wojcik et al., 2015). Needed are
experimental designs that assess the reactions of Trump supporters to Trump’s
attacks on his adversaries wherein respondents are not aware of what a scripted
response might entail.To that end, Frimer and Skitka (2018) asked self-identified
“diehard Trump supporters” to react to a tweet in which President Trump
62 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Strong Republican
Republican
Neither
Democrat
Strong Democrat
1 2 3 4 5
Culture Warrior Governor for All
Figure 4.2 Self-
reported political leadership preference of American Democrats and
Republicans in 2019.
Note: All items were bipolar, with culture warrior items being low (1) and governor- for- all
items being high (5) on the 1– 5 scale. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
attacked a political rival (e.g., Hillary Clinton, Barack Obama) or a media per-
sonality (e.g., Mika Brzezinski, Joe Scarborough) or a less uncivil version of the
same. If Trump’s diehard base has bloodlust, we should expect them to express
greater approval of his uncivil attack than for the more civil rapprochement.
But this is not what was found. In several studies, diehard Trump supporters
evaluated Trump similarly after reading a civil or uncivil message from him;
when they did show a reaction, it was disapproving of Trump’s uncivil attack.
The rest of the political spectrum univocally disapproved of Trump’s attacks, and
this included (non-diehard) Trump supporters.These results were corroborated
in longitudinal analyses of public polling data, meaning that they were not an
artifact of the artificial lab setting. They also accord with decades of research
showing that attack ads backfire upon the attacker—they harm the reputation
of the attacker more than that of the attacked (e.g., Carraro & Castelli, 2010; see
Lau, Sigelman, & Rovner, 2007 for a review). In sum, research to date seems to
disconfirm the notion that Trump’s supporters have bloodlust for the perceived
political adversaries.
The information bubble
Along with tribalism, a second common explanation that the media some-
times offers for extreme Trump support implicates the regular consumption of
pro-Trump media and opinions. While the number of media sources and the
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 63
diversity of ideas available to the public have increased over time, people have
increasingly consumed content that confirms their attitudes while remaining
unexposed to information that might alter or challenge those beliefs. The
“Trump bubble” refers to a set of media outlets, citizens, and social media circles
that share content that exclusively promotes and validates President Trump’s
agenda, policies, and behavior. According to The Financial Times, the center of
the Trump bubble might be Fox News (Bond, 2017), whose pundits tend to
support President Trump on most matters. Social media also contributes to the
formation and maintenance of information bubbles. Sites like Facebook and
Twitter use algorithms to learn from a user’s previous behaviors to selectively
present more of the same. Business Insider proposes that these sorting algorithms
help to maintain a relatively narrow ideological cocoon that reinforces asym-
metrical, biased information bubbles and echo chambers (Bremmer, 2019).
Even when belief-challenging information enters the conversation, it rarely
affects attitudes. This is because people tend to interpret information in a way
that confirms their pre-existing opinions.According to The New Yorker, this con-
firmation bias might explain why extreme Trump supporters were undeterred
by evidence that challenges their allegiance (e.g., the Access Hollywood scandal;
Konnikova, 2016).
The information circulating within the Trump bubble may not be fully
accurate, in part because some of it is false news propagated by Russian bots
(Goldman, Barnes, Haberman & Fandos, 2020). However, it succeeds in
reinforcing the beliefs of those who strongly approve of the current presi-
dent (The Washington Post; Emba, 2016), and may even deepen their extremism
(The Guardian; Grimes, 2017). Information bubbles on the political right may
be particularly impervious to outside information. However, Huffpost suggests
that both Republicans and Democrats consume media content that supports
and confirms their existing beliefs (Grenoble, 2018). Proposition 2 is that Trump
supporters are in an information bubble filled with pro-Trump opinions, Russian bots,
and fake news.
Scientific evidence
Repeated exposure to statements supporting a singular point of view is suffi-
cient to increase attitude polarization (e.g., Hinsz & David, 1984). The more
statements supporting the viewpoint, the more extreme the observer’s attitudes
are likely to become. Simply put, repetition works. Information bubbles,
sustained by social media algorithms that selectively present information that
the user previously responded positively to, have made information consump-
tion increasingly asymmetric and unbalanced in recent years.
However, artificial intelligence sorting algorithms are not the only causes
of information bubbles. Among the most robust findings in social psych-
ology is confirmation bias, the tendency for people to seek out and inter-
pret information in a way that supports their existing opinions (e.g., Kunda,
64 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Scientific evidence
The literature on “pocketbook voting,” that is, the tendency to vote for and
support candidates and policies that maximize one’s own material self-interests,
is mixed and contentious, with some scholars suggesting that pocketbook
voting occurs and others suggesting it might not. Our review of the literature
finds support for the occurrence of a particular kind of pocketbook voting,
namely that people tend to favor candidates and policies that they believe will
advance their individual self-interests.
Early analyses seemed to support the notion that apparent pocketbook
voting is an epiphenomenon, propped up by unrelated psychological processes.
For example, analyses of survey data and experimental studies found that
correlations between personal finances and political attitudes were found only
when items measuring finances and attitudes were located in close proximity
to one another within a survey (Sears & Lau, 1983). People might have voiced
opposition toward a candidate as a form of scapegoating their personal financial
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 67
distress when their reasons for opposition were actually based in other factors.
However, Lewis-Beck (1985) evaluated Michigan Center for Political Studies-
Survey Research Center (CPS-SRC) election surveys and found no support
for the item-proximity moderator effect. Lau, Sears, and Jessor (1990) then
refined the conditions under which item proximity is likely to produce an
apparent pocketbook voting effect. Together, these studies seem to support the
idea that it is possible to socially prime a relationship between pocketbook
concerns and political attitudes.
The primed connection between personal finances and political decision
making may not be limited to surveys and questionnaires but might also extend
to the real political world. Politicians sometimes raise the psychological sali-
ence of personal finances in political decision making, which could induce
pocketbook voting. For example, Ronald Reagan successfully induced more
externalized attribution for voters’ financial improvement in his “Good
Morning America” campaign (Beschloss, 2016) and subsequently produced
a strong economic self-interest vote for himself in 1984 (Lau et al., 1990).
President Trump has drawn from the same playbook by explicitly linking his
presidency to the value of Americans’ pensions:
I won the election, the markets went up thousands of points, things started
happening. If, for some reason, I were not to have won the election, these
markets would have crashed. That will happen even more so in 2020. You
have no choice but to vote for me, because your 401(k), everything is going
to be down the tubes.
(Murray, 2019)
posed by then-candidate Ronald Reagan, “Are you better off today than you
were four years ago?” Some studies found support for retrospective pocketbook
voting (Tilley, Neundorf, & Hobolt, 2018) whereas others did not (Elinder
et al., 2015; Mutz, 2018).
The existing body of pocketbook voting literature is solely based on correl-
ational and observational studies, leaving open the possibility that unidentified
third variables confound or suppress observed effects. To our knowledge, no
studies have experimentally manipulated the perception that a particular can-
didate will benefit the respondent financially and then observed whether such
a manipulation altered level of support. In sum, it remains unclear whether
and to what degree extreme Trump support is rooted in perceived material
self-interest.
Conclusion
Our review of Trump support, in both its more tentative and extreme forms,
finds that all three major psychological claims advanced in the media were
consistent with the available psychological evidence. Extreme Trump support
appears to be rooted in tribalism, information bubbles, and perceived material
self-
interest. In general, the more nefarious theories (e.g., racism, Russian
bots, bloodlust) were less supported and the more banal ones (e.g., informa-
tion bubbles, self-interest) were more supported. Moreover, several of the effi-
cacious explanations generalized to extreme Trump opponents, meaning that
they might be a byproduct of a decades-long and escalating culture war. In this
way, Donald Trump’s presidency is not only stoking the culture war, but also is
a product of it. Focusing attention on ways to reduce intergroup conflict might
be a fruitful way of preventing the emergence of future would-be autocrats and
thereby preserving democracy.
References
Abramsky, S. (2019, November 19). Trump wants to treat undocumented migrants
like enemy combatants. The Nation. Retrieved from www.thenation.com/article/
archive/trump-immigration-military-terrorism/
Allasan, F. (2019, November 5). Poll: 62% of Trump supporters say nothing he could
do would change opinion. Axios. Retrieved from www.axios.com/monmouth-poll-
trump-approval-a05b8144-1d1b-4296-a0d4-6ca0390b05ee.html
Altemeyer, B. (2004).The other "authoritarian personality." In J. T. Jost & J. Sidanius (Eds.),
Key readings in social psychology. Political psychology: Key readings. New York: Routledge.
Arce, J. (2019, August 6). Trump’s anti-immigrant rhetoric was never about legality
—it was about our brown skin. TIME. Retrieved from https://time.com/5645501/
trump-anti-immigration-rhetoric-racism/
Azarian, B. (2017, December 31). An analysis of Trump supporters has identified 5 key
traits. Psychology Today. Retrieved from www.psychologytoday.com/ca/blog/mind-
in-the-machine/201712/analysis-trump-supporters-has-identified-5-key-traits
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 69
Bail, C. A., Guay, B., Maloney, E., Combs, A., Hillygus, D. S., Merhout, F., Freelon, D.,
& Volfovsky, A. (2020). Assessing the Russian Internet Research Agency’s impact on
the political attitudes and behaviors of American Twitter users in late 2017. PNAS
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(1),
243–250. doi:10.1073/pnas.1906420116
Bakshy, E., Messing, S., & Adamic, L. A. (2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news
and opinion on Facebook. Science, 348(6239), 1130– 1132. doi:10.1126/
science.
aaa1160
Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from left
to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber?. Psychological
Science, 26(10), 1531–1542
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interper-
sonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
497–529.
Bencherki, N., & Basque, J. (2018, August 6). Why so many Americans continue to
believe in Donald Trump. The Conversation. Retrieved from https://theconversation.
com/why-so-many-americans-continue-to-believe-in-donald-trump-100498
Benen, S. (2018, October 18). Republican voters see illegal immigration as the nation’s
#1 issue. MSNBC. Retrieved from www.msnbc.com/rachel-maddow-show/
republican-voters-see-illegal-immigration-the-nations-1-issue
Beschloss, M. (2016, May 7). The ad that helped Reagan sell good times to an uncer-
tain nation. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2016/05/08/
business/the-ad-that-helped-reagan-sell-good-times-to-an-uncertain-nation.html
Blake, A. (2015, April 10). The 10 most loyal demographic groups for Republicans
and Democrats. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/
news/ t he- f ix/ w p/ 2 015/ 0 4/ 0 8/ t he- 1 0- m ost- l oyal- d emographic-groups- f or-
republicans-and-democrats/
Bond, S. (2017, January 18). Trump supporters turn to Fox News first, says survey.
The Financial Times. Retrieved from www.ft.com/content/01bb80b8-dd9e-11e6-
9d7c-be108f1c1dce
Brandt, M. J., & Crawford, J. (2013, December 6). Replication-extension of “not for all
the tea in China!” Political ideology and the avoidance of dissonance-arousing situ-
ations (Nam, Jost, & Van Bavel, 2013, PLoS One). Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved
from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2365281
Brandt, M., & Crawford, J. (2019). Worldview conflict and prejudice. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology. 0065-2601. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/
bs.aesp.2019.09.002
Bremmer, I. (2019, January 11). America is more politically divided than ever, and the
government shutdown only makes that more apparent. Business Insider. Retrieved
from www.businessinsider.com/america-divided-politics-than-ever-democrats-
republicans-government-2019-1
Bump, P. (2019, August 13). It’s the mainstream anti-immigration rhetoric, not the
extreme, that’s shaping American politics. The Washington Post. Retrieved from
www.washingtonpost.com/politics/2019/08/13/its-mainstream-anti-immigration-
rhetoric-not-extreme-thats-shaping-american-politics/
Carr, J. (2019, July 11). Trump’s farm bailout program continues USDA’s racist
legacy. AG Mag. Retrieved from www.ewg.org/agmag/2019/07/trump-s-farm-
bailout-program-continues-usda-s-racist-legacy
70 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Carraro, L., & Castelli, L. (2010), The implicit and explicit effects of negative political
campaigns: Is the source really blamed? Political Psychology, 31: 617–645. doi:10.1111/
j.1467-9221.2010.00771.x
Cary, P. & Holmes, A. (2019, April 30).Workers barely benefited from Trump’s sweeping
tax cut, investigation shows. The Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.com/
us-news/2019/apr/30/trump-tax-cut-law-investigation-worker-benefits
Chambers, J. R., & Melnyk, D. (2006). Why do I hate thee? Conflict misperceptions
and intergroup mistrust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 1295–1311.
https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1177/0146167206289979
Clement, S., & Balz, D. (2019, September 9). Partisan divisions over immigration
widen after a year of turmoil at border. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.
washingtonpost.com/politics/partisan-divisions-over-immigration-widen-after-a-
year-of-turmoil-at-border/2019/09/09/2b4e6482-cfdf-11e9-8c1c-7c8ee785b855_
story.html
CNN. (2016, November 23). Exit Polls. CNN. Retrieved from www.cnn.com/
election/2016/results/exit-polls
Cole, L. N. (2019, June 29). Meet the people behind Donald Trump’s popularity.
ThoughtCo. Retrieved from www.thoughtco.com/meet-the-people-behind-
donald-trumps-popularity-4068073
Collins, E. (2016, August 10). Poll: Americans feel their way of life is under threat. USA
Today. Retrieved from usatoday.com/story/news/politics/onpolitics/2016/08/10/
american-values-congress-obama-approval/88518182/
Conway, I. L. G., Houck, S. C., Gornick, L. J., & Repke, M. A. (2017). Finding
the Loch Ness monster: Left-wing authoritarianism in the United States. Political
Psychology, 39(5), 1049–1067. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1111/
pops.12470
Diamond, J. (2016, January 24). Trump: I could ‘shoot somebody and I wouldn’t lose
voters’. CNN. Retrieved from www.cnn.com/2016/01/23/politics/donald-trump-
shoot-somebody-support/index.html
Ditto, P. H., Liu, B. S., Clark, C. J., Wojcik, S. P., Chen, E. E., Grady, R.H., … Zinger,
J. F. (2019). At least bias is bipartisan: A meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias
in liberals and conservatives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 273–291. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691617746796
Drezner, D. W. (2018, August 6). How Donald Trump is normal and abnormal. The
Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/news/posteverything/
wp/2018/08/06/how-donald-trump-is-normal-and-abnormal/
Dunwoody, P. T., & Plane, D. L. (2019). The influence of authoritarianism and out-
group threat on political affiliations and support for antidemocratic policies. Peace and
Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 25(3), 198–210. doi:10.1037/pac0000397
Duspara, B., & Greitemeyer,T. (2017).The impact of dark tetrad traits on political orien-
tation and extremism: An analysis in the course of a presidential election. Heliyon,
3(10), e00425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2017.e00425
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T., & Levine, J. M. (2009). Shared reality: Experiencing com-
monality with others’ inner states about the world. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
4(5), 496–521. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01161.x
Elinder, M., Jordahl, H., & Poutvaara, P. (2015). Promises, policies and pocketbook
voting. European Economic Review, 75, 177–194.
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 71
Emba, C. (2016, July 14). Confirmed: Echo chambers exist on social media. So what
do we do about them? The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.
com/news/in-theory/wp/2016/07/14/confirmed-echo-chambers-exist-on-social-
media-but-what-can-we-do-about-them/
Farenthold, D. (2016, October 8). Trump recorded having extremely lewd conversation
about women in 2005. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.
com/politics/trump-recorded-having-extremely-lewd-conversation-about-women-
in-2005/2016/10/07/3b9ce776-8cb4-11e6-bf8a-3d26847eeed4_story.html
Farenthold, D. A., Costa, R., & Wagner, J. (2017, January 20). Donald Trump is sworn
in as president, vows to end ‘American carnage’. The Washington Post. Retrieved
from www.washingtonpost.com/politics/trump-to-be-sworn-in-marking-a-
transformative-shift-in-the-countrys-leadership/2017/01/20/954b9cac-de7d-11e6-
ad42-f3375f271c9c_story.html
Festinger, F. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Frimer, J. A., Gaucher, D., & Schaefer, N. K. (2014). Political conservatives’ affinity for
obedience to authority is loyal, not blind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40,
1205–1214. doi:10.1177/0146167214538672.
Frimer, J. A., & Skitka, L. J. (2018). The Montagu principle: Incivility decreases
politicians’ public approval, even with their political base. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 115(5), 845–866. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/
pspi0000140.supp (Supplemental),
Frimer, J. A., & Skitka, L. J. (2020). Americans hold their political leaders to a higher
discursive standard than rank- and- partisans. Journal of Experimental Social
file co-
Psychology, 86, N.PAG. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.
103907
Frimer, J. A., Skitka, L. J., & Motyl, M. (2017). Liberals and conservatives are similarly
motivated to avoid exposure to one another’s opinions. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 72, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.003
Gallup. (2020). Presidential approval ratings –Donald Trump. Gallup. Retrieved from
https://news.gallup.com/poll/203198/presidential-approval-ratings-donald-trump.aspx
Goldman, A., Barnes, J. E., Haberman, M., & Fandos, N. (2020, February 25).
Lawmakers are warned that Russia is meddling to re-elect Trump. The New York Times.
Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2020/02/20/us/politics/russian-interference-
trump-democrats.html
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different
sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141
Green, E (2017, September 10). It was cultural anxiety that drove white, working-
class voters to Trump. The Atlantic. Retrieved from www.theatlantic.com/politics/
archive/2017/05/white-working-class-trump-cultural-anxiety/525771/
Greenberg, J., & Jonas, E. (2003). Psychological motives and political orientation: The
left, the right, and the rigid: Comment on Jost et al. (2003). Psychological Bulletin,
129(3), 376–382. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.376
Grenoble, R. (2018, February 6). Trump supporters consume and share the most
fake news, Oxford study finds. Huffpost. Retrieved from www.huffingtonpost.ca/
entry/trump-supporters-read-share-f ake-news_n_5a7a25f9e4b06505b4e8f07d?ri
18n=true
72 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Grimes, D. (2017, December 4). Echo chambers are dangerous –we must try to
break free of our online bubbles. The Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.
com/ s cience/ b log/ 2 017/ d ec/ 0 4/ e cho- c hambers- a re- d angerous- we-must-
try-to-break-free-of-our-online-bubbles
Guilford, G. (2018, May 3). The epic mistake about manufacturing that cost
Americans millions of jobs. Quartz. Retrieved from https://qz.com/1269172/
the-epic-mistake-about-manufacturing-thats-cost-americans-millions-of-jobs/
Hinsz,V. B., & Davis, J. H. (1984). Persuasive arguments theory, group polarization, and
choice shifts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10(2), 260–268. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0146167284102012
Hirschfield-David, J., & Baker, P. (2019, January 5). How the border wall is boxing
Trump in. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2019/01/05/us/
politics/donald-trump-border-wall.html
Hoffower, H. (2019, June 26). College is more expensive than it’s ever been, and the
5 reasons why suggest it’s only going to get worse. Business Insider. Retrieved from
www.businessinsider.com/why-is-college-so-expensive-2018-4
Hogg, M. A. (2007). Uncertainty- identity theory. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 39, 69–126. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(06)39002-8
Howard, P. (2019, October 23). Waiting for ratification, UAW strikers reflect America’s
anxiety about overall economy. Detroit Free Press. Retrieved from www.freep.com/
story/money/cars/general-motors/2019/10/23/gm-strike-uaw-us-economy/
4056332002/
Iyengar, S., & Hahn, K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological
selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59(1), 19–39. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.01402.x
Jones, J. M. (2018, December 21). Trump approval more stable than approval for prior
presidents. Gallup. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/opinion/polling-
matters/245567/trump-approval-stable-approval-prior-presidents.aspx
Jones, J. M. (2019, December 21). Trump approval more stable than approval for prior
presidents. Gallup. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/opinion/polling-
matters/245567/trump-approval-stable-approval-prior-presidents.aspx
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as
motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375
Kabaservice, J. (2018, November 9). Moderate Republicans aren’t dead. They’re hiding
in plain sight. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/out-
look/moderate-republicans-arent-dead-theyre-hiding-in-plain-sight/2018/11/09/
268c7b1c-e365-11e8-ab2c-b31dcd53ca6b_story.html
Kafura, C. (2019, October 8). Republicans and Democrats in different worlds on immi-
gration. The Chicago Counsel. Retrieved from www.thechicagocouncil.org/publica-
tion/lcc/republicans-and-democrats-different-worlds-immigration
Khazan, O. (2018, April 23). People voted for Trump because they were anxious, not
poor. The Atlantic. Retrieved from www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2018/04/
existential-anxiety-not-poverty-motivates-trump-support/558674/
Konnikova, M. (2016, December 25). The psychological research that explains helps
the election. The New Yorker. Retrieved from www.newyorker.com/science/maria-
konnikova/the-psychological-research-that-helps-explain-the-election
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 73
Kumar, A. (2019, November 8). Immigration helped Trump win in 2016. It could help
spell his demise in 2020. Politico. Retrieved from www.politico.com/news/2019/11/
08/trump-immigration-disarray-067042
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–
498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think (2nd ed.). Chicago,
IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Lau, R., Sears, D., & Jessor,T. (1990). Fact or artifact revisited: Survey instrument effects
and pocketbook politics. Political Behavior, 12(3), 217–242.
Lau, R. R., Sigelman, L., & Rovner, I. B. (2007), The effects of negative political
campaigns: A meta-analytic reassessment. Journal of Politics, 69: 1176–1209. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00618.x
Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, H. (1944).The people's choice. New York: Duell,
Sloan & Pearce.
Leonhardt, M. (2019, October 9). Americans spend twice as much on healthcare as they
did in the 1980s. CNBC. Retrieved from www.cnbc.com/2019/10/09/americans-
spend-twice-as-much-on-health-care-today-as-in-the-1980s.html
Lewis-Beck, M. (1985). Pocketbook voting in U.S. national election studies: Fact or
artifact? American Journal of Political Science, 29(2), 348–356. doi:10.2307/2111171
Lilienfeld, S. O., Latzman, R. D.,Watts, A. L., Smith, S. F., & Dutton, K. (2014). Correlates
of psychopathic personality traits in everyday life: Results from a large community
survey. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.
Long, H. (2016, March 29). U.S. has lost 5 million manufacturing jobs since 2000. CNN
Business. Retrieved from https://money.cnn.com/2016/03/29/news/economy/us-
manufacturing-jobs/index.html
Long, H. (2017, December 14). Trump chose five families to show how the tax bill
benefits them. Their stories show the reality is complicated. The Washington Post.
Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2017/12/14/trump-
chose-five-f amilies-to-show-how-the-tax-bill-benefits-them-their-stories-show-
the-reality-is-complicated/
Long, H. (2018, February 10). In U.S., wage growth is being wiped out entirely by infla-
tion. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/business/2018/
08/10/america-wage-growth-is-getting-wiped-out-entirely-by-inflation/
Ludeke, S. G., Klitgaard, C. N., & Vitriol, J. (2018). Comprehensively-measured authori-
tarianism does predict vote choice: The importance of authoritarianism’s facets,
ideological sorting, and the particular candidate. Personality and Individual Differences,
123, 209–216.
McEnery, T. (2011, February 9). 10 iconic American factories that have closed in
the past two years. Business Insider. Retrieved from www.businessinsider.com/
major-iconic-factory-closings-2008-2011-2
Meertens, R. W., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Is subtle prejudice really prejudice. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 61(1), 54–71. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1086/
297786
Murray, S. (2019, August 15). ‘You have no choice but to vote for me,’ Trump tells
N.H. rally. Politico. Retrieved from www.politico.com/story/2019/08/15/
trump-new-hampshire-rally-manchester-1466285
74 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Mutz, D. C. (2018). Status threat, not economic hardship, explains the 2016 presiden-
tial vote. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
115(19), E4330–E4339. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718155115
Nam, H. H., Jost, J. T., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2013). “Not for all the tea in China!” Political
ideology and the avoidance of dissonance-arousing situations. PLoS ONE, 8(4), 1–8.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059837
Olorunnipa, T., & Parker, A. (2019, July 27). Trump campaign sees political advantage in
a divisive appeal to working-class white voters. The Washington Post. Retrieved from
www.washingtonpost.com/politics/trump-campaign-sees-political-advantage-in-a-
divisive-appeal-to-working-class-white-voters/2019/07/26/39234f00-aef1-11e9-
8e77-03b30bc29f64_story.html
Overack, L. (1941). Campaign finance in the presidential election of 1940. The American
Political Science Review, 35(4), 701–727. doi.org/10.2307/1948076.
Pearce, M. (2019, October 23). As Trump seeks reelection, immigrant voters stand in his
path. The L.A Times. Retrieved from www.latimes.com/politics/story/2019-10-23/
trump-attacks-immigrants-new-naturalized-citizen-voter-registration
Pew Research Center (2019a, August 9). An examination of the 2016 electorate, based
on validated voters. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from www.people-press.org/
2018/08/09/an-examination-of-the-2016-electorate-based-on-validated-voters/
Pew Research Center (2019b, October 10). Partisan antipathy: More intense, more
personal. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from www.people-press.org/2019/10/10/
partisan-antipathy-more-intense-more-personal
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orien-
tation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763.
Preston, O. C., & Anestis, J. C. (2018). Psychopathic traits and politics: Examining affili-
ation, support of political issues, and the role of empathy. Personality and Individual
Differences, 131, 142–148. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.034
Rapier, G. (2018, May 18). Housing prices are soaring in America –here’s where they’ve
climbed the most since the housing crisis. Markets, Business Insider. Retrieved from
https://markets.businessinsider.com/news/stocks/how-americas-home-prices-
have-fared-since-the-housing-crisis-2018-5-1024994866
Reich, R. (2019, May 19). The Trump economy is hurting most Americans.
Statistics won’t fool voters. The Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.com/
commentisfree/2019/may/19/trump-economy-americans-voters-unemployment
Schlenker, B. R., Chambers, J. R., & Le, B. M. (2012). Conservatives are happier than
liberals, but why? Political ideology, personality, and life satisfaction. Journal of Research
in Personality, 46(2), 127–146. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/
j.jrp.2011.12.009
Schwartz, S, H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
25, 1–65.
Schwartz, S. H., Caprara, G. V., & Vecchione, M. (2010). Basic personal values, core pol-
itical values, and voting: A longitudinal analysis. Political Psychology, 31(3), 421–452.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00764.x
Scott, E. (2019a, May 10). For Trump and some of his supporters, violence against
immigrants appears totally acceptable. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.
washingtonpost.com/politics/2019/05/10/trump-some-his-supporters-violence-
against-immigrants-appears-totally-acceptable/
Psychology of extreme Trump supporters 75
Scott, R. (2019b, March 28). President Donald Trump calls Democrats ‘sick people,’ takes
a victory lap on Mueller report during rally in Michigan. ABC News. Retrieved from
https://abcnews.go.com/Politics/president-donald-trump-hold-campaign-rally-
michigan-1st/story?id=61987526
Sears, D. O., & Freedman, J. L. (1967). Selective exposure to information: A critical
review. Public Opinion Quarterly, 31(2), 194–213. https://doi.org/10.1086/267513
Sears, D. O., & Henry, P. J. (2003).The origins of symbolic racism. Journal of Personality &
Social Psychology, 85(2), 259–275. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.85.2.259
Sears, D. O., & Lau, R. R. (1983). Inducing apparently self- interested political
preferences. American Political Science Review, 27, 223–253.
Sonmez, V. (2018, November 26). Trump says U.S. agents ‘had to use’ tear gas against
migrants at the border. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.
com/ p olitics/ t rump- s ays- u s- a gents- h ad- t o- u se- t ear- g as- a gainst- m igrants- a t-
border/2018/11/26/a2b66a00-f1ba-11e8-80d0-f7e1948d55f4_story.html
Steinour, H. (2019, February 22). The soaring cost of US child care, in 5 charts. Quad-
City Times. Retrieved from https://qctimes.com/lifestyles/parenting/the-soaring-
cost- o f- u s- c hild- c are- i n- c harts/ a rticle_ 8 8234ec1- 7 c23- 5 5c8- b 86b- 7 ce3e42f
34c7.html
Stroud, N. J. (2008). Media use and political predispositions: Revisiting the con-
cept of selective exposure. Political Behavior, 30(3), 341–366. doi.org/10.1007/
s11109-007-9050-9.
Tankersley, J. (2018. October 11).White Americans gain the most from Trump’s tax cuts,
a report finds. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2018/10/11/
business/trump-tax-cuts-white-americans.html
Taub, A. (2016, March 1). The rise of American authoritarianism. Vox. Retrieved from
www.vox.com/2016/3/1/11127424/trump-authoritarianism
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social con-
servatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality & Individual
Differences, 49(6), 587–592. https://doi-org.uwinnipeg.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.paid.
2010.05.024
Tilley, J., Neundorf, A., & Hobolt, S. B. (2018). When the pound in people’s pocket
matters: How changes to personal financial circumstances affect party choice. Journal
of Politics, 80(2), 555–569. doi.org/10.1086/694549
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Krouwel, A. P. M. (2019). Psychological features of extreme pol-
itical ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 159–163.
Vives, R., & Castillo, A. (2019, October 9). Trump’s policies, as well as anti-
immigrant violence, disturbed these Latinos. Now they’re taking action. The Los
Angeles Times. Retrieved from www.latimes.com/california/story/2019-10-09/
trump-immigration-violence-latinos
Vosoughi, S., Roy, D., & Aral, S. (2018).The spread of true and false news online. Science,
359(6380), 1146–1151. doi.org/10.1126/science.aap9559
Webb, T. L., Chang, B. P. I., & Benn, Y. (2013). ‘The ostrich problem’: Motivated
avoidance or rejection of information about goal progress. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 7, 794–807. doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12071
Wike, R., Poushter, J., Fetterolf, J., & Schumacher, S. (2020, January 8). Trump
ratings remain low around globe, while views of U.S. stay mostly favorable. Pew
Research Center. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.org/global/2020/01/08/trump-
ratings-remain-low-around-globe-while-views-of-u-s-stay-mostly-favorable/
76 Laura Kinsman and Jeremy A. Frimer
Wojcik, S. P., Hovasapian, A., Graham, J., Motyl, M., & Ditto, P. H. (2015). Conservatives
report, but liberals display, greater happiness. Science, 347, 1243–1246. doi:10.1126/
science.1260817
Womick, J., Rothmund, T., Azevedo, F., King, L. A., & Jost, J. T. (2019). Group-based
dominance and authoritarian aggression predict support for Donald Trump in the
2016 U.S. presidential election. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(5), 643–
652. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550618778290
Zitner, A. (2016, May 26). Donald Trump viewed as change agent, economic steward.
The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved from https://blogs.wsj.com/washwire/2016/05/
26/donald-trump-viewed-as-change-agent-economic-steward/
Chapter 5
Philosophers and social theorists have long argued that a plurality of opinions
and constructive debate are fundamental components of a healthy society
(Arendt, 1968; Mill, 1859; Mutz, 2006). It is commonly assumed that disagree-
ment about political topics promotes societal progress. For example, without
continual debate about voting and immigration policies, a system that treats
people in the fairest way possible would be difficult to establish. Additionally,
disagreement can foster greater political knowledge, ultimately promoting a
more informed and engaged electorate (Putnam, 2000). This perspective is
commonly echoed when scholars advocate for greater attitude diversity (e.g.,
liberal and conservative perspectives) in various spaces, such as congressional
chambers, university campuses, and social media (An et al., 2011; Duarte et al.,
2015; Lukianoff & Haidt, 2019).
At the same time, researchers have also highlighted a growing trend of atti-
tude polarization (Abramowitz, 2010). The attitudes that political elites (e.g.,
party leaders) and everyday citizens hold toward various issues (e.g., abortion)
have become increasingly divergent and extreme over time (Abramowitz &
Saunders, 2008). From this perspective, attitude disagreement is generally
described as a social problem (Bishop, 2009). Political polarization can lead to
a variety of insidious consequences, ranging from a lack of contact between
people who hold different political viewpoints (Pew, 2014) to partisan gridlock
(Jacobson, 2016), and even long-term conflict and violence (Bar-Tal, 2013).
Thus, understanding the roots of political polarization is a pivotal question
in improving relations among people and forging more constructive political
discourse.
In this chapter, I review how relational motivations to connect and affiliate
with other people contribute to political polarization. I also highlight ways in
which these goals diverge across the political spectrum. Specifically, I outline
how political conservatives (those on the right) and liberals (those on the left)
differ in the relational goals that they prioritize. I discuss how these motivations
contribute to beliefs about the degree of attitude similarity within political
groups, as well as the accuracy of those beliefs among both everyday citizens
78 Chadly Stern
and political elites. I also discuss how these motivations impact the actual degree
of attitude agreement that develops within political groups. Throughout this
chapter, I outline how these processes lead both liberals and conservatives to
contribute to political polarization. Lastly, I propose psychologically informed
approaches that could be used to address the social problems that polariza-
tion creates and to foster more positive relationships between liberals and
conservatives.
Once people adopt shared ideas about basic concepts in the world (e.g., the
meowing animal is a cat), they form more elaborate beliefs and attitudes about
those concepts. People not only know what a “cat” is or what it means to iden-
tify as “Black” in their culture, but also have preferences about which animals
they think would be good pets and attitudes toward different racial groups.
How do people come to feel that these preferences, attitudes, and beliefs are
valid? Festinger (1950) famously made the distinction between “physical” and
“social” aspects of the world. He argued that a person can test and validate their
perceptions of physical forms of reality, such as walking through what they
perceive to be an open door to determine whether the door is actually open.
In contrast, social forms of reality cannot be subjected to physical tests and
empirical inquiry. A person who believes that cats are better pets than dogs or
that Black people are inferior to White people cannot directly test the validity
of their perspectives. Instead, they reference other people, and similar others in
particular, to validate their attitudes and beliefs.To the degree that similar others
agree with one’s own attitudes and beliefs, those perspectives are infused with
a sense of legitimacy.
more supportive of Hillary Clinton and Joe Biden (moderate liberals) over the
more ideologically extreme Bernie Sanders in the 2016 and 2020 presidential
elections, respectively (Siders, 2020). However, local party leaders often display
the exact opposite behavior. Local party leaders are more likely to prefer an
ideologically extreme (versus moderate) candidate, and the tendency to do so is
especially pronounced among Republicans (Broockman, Carnes, Crowder-
Meyer, & Skovron, in press).
What drives these counterintuitive decisions? The preferences and motiv-
ations of party leaders contribute to candidate support to a greater degree
that does strategic selection of candidates (Hassell, 2018). It is possible that
local party leaders’ relational goals contribute to their preferences for extreme
candidates. Conservative party leaders perceive their electorate as being more
conservative than it actually is (i.e., as being strongly conservative). It is feasible
that they prefer an extreme candidate whose views are consistent with those
perceptions as a means of “sharing reality” with the electorate. In contrast, lib-
eral party leaders perceive their electorate as being less liberal than it actually
is (i.e., as being moderately liberal). In turn, they might prefer a more extreme
candidate as a means of expressing uniqueness from the electorate.
It is important to note that the candidate preferences of party leaders might
vary based on the scope of the electorate. Local party leaders appear inclined
to prefer more extreme candidates. However, party leaders who are focused on
national elections might prefer moderate candidates due to the more expansive
scope of the electorate. Nevertheless, to the degree that party leaders do some-
times choose more ideologically extreme (versus moderate) candidates, this
process has the potential to further divide everyday people in their political
stances. Politicians can act as a knowledge base for citizens grappling with how
to feel about an issue (Leeper & Slothuus, 2014), and observing more ideo-
logically extreme candidates would produce more politically extreme attitudes
among the populace.
are more likely to construct homophilous networks (Boutyline & Willer, 2017).
Additionally, when conservative (versus liberal) elites used moral-emotional
language in their Twitter messages, these messages more readily spread (Brady,
Wills, Burkart, Jost, & Van Bavel, 2019). Ideological asymmetries in message
diffusion were most pronounced when the messages invoked religion and pat-
riotism, both of which revolve around social connection and group loyalty.
These findings are consistent with the perspective that conservatives’ stronger
goals to connect and affiliate with like-minded others lead them to construct
more attitudinally similar networks than do liberals’.
Conservatives’ greater desire for social cohesion is oriented toward ingroup
members and like- minded others (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009; Stern,
West, Jost, & Rule, 2014). In turn, conservatives’ greater attitude similarity is
constrained to self-selected networks that reinforce their views. For example,
conservatives are more likely than liberals to report that most of their close
friends share their political views (Pew, 2014). Greater similarity on a local level
tends to correspond to greater distinction from others an a more expansive
level (Henrich & Boyd, 1998), such that conservatives should have less homo-
geneous attitudes than do liberals when gauged on a broader level. Indeed,
Colleoni, Rozza, and Arvidsson (2014) found that Republicans on Twitter
were more homogeneous when they were following the accounts of fellow
Republicans, but Democrats overall were more homogeneous. This pattern is
also reflected in nationally representative survey responses. When assessed on
the level of the country, liberals in the United States display greater homogen-
eity in their attitudes than do conservatives, and this difference consistently
occurs over a period of 40 years (Ondish & Stern, 2018). Overall, conservatives
possess greater attitude homogeneity on a local and self-selected level, whereas
liberals possess greater attitude homogeneity on a national level.
These patterns of attitude homogeneity among liberals and conservatives
further contribute to polarization. Forming a small social network that is highly
homogeneous distances members of that network from broader discussions and
viewpoints within a society. In other words, a person in a highly homogeneous
group will be less likely to have their attitudes and beliefs challenged. Without
exposure to diverse viewpoints, extreme perspectives on topics are more
likely to be adopted over time (Abelson, 1995; Binder, Dalrymple, Brossard,
& Scheufele, 2009). The exact topics on which people become more extreme
vary across locations and are determined, at least to some degree, through the
importance of the topic in that area (Liu & Latane, 1998). Thus, conservatives’
greater attitude homogeneity on a local and self-selected level can contribute
to greater extremity on certain issues. In contrast, liberals’ greater attitude
similarity on a national level could lead them to adopt more extreme positions
as a means of removing themselves from a homogeneous network. Ironically,
if liberals consistently become more extreme in their positions across various
issues to individuate themselves, they reinforce the very homogeneity that
they wish to escape. Liberals and conservatives both moving to more extreme
Relational goals & political polarization 85
Any social problem will be derived from a variety of factors, and effective
interventions target multiple sources of an issue (Lewin, 1947). The suggestions
outlined above give a sample of strategies that could be integrated into a larger
multi-step approach that creates more effective discourse between liberals and
conservatives. Actual similarity on non-political topics (e.g., favorite ice-cream
flavor) could be initially highlighted to increase interest in an interaction and
provide topics of conversation for people who hold opposing political views.
Perceptions of similarity on various non-political topics (even in the absence
of actual similarity) could then be experimentally induced prior to an inter-
action to create a general feeling of similarity that sustains an interaction over
time. Additionally, topics for both actual and perceived similarity could be
tailored to meet liberals’ and conservatives’ relational goals. Altogether, this type
of approach could begin the process of building a fruitful bond between indi-
viduals with opposing political views. Relationships also have the potential to
cascade, in the sense that a new social connection can spread through a person’s
network (Gottman, 1983; Rubin, 1985). In this way, the relational goals that
currently lead to homogeneous political networks could lead to more hetero-
geneous networks.
Connecting people on the left and right serves many positive goals, including
a general improvement in relationships among people. However, potentially
one of the most important goals is to engage people with diverse viewpoints
in sustained and civil discourse about pressing political issues. Discussions in
which people with varying perspectives respectfully engage (and disagree) with
one another create more connected communities and instill support for the
rules and laws of a society (Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950; Mutz, 2006). As
some scholars have noted, exposure to dissimilar political views can reinforce
the negative consequences of polarization (Bail et al., 2018). However, negative
reactions based on individual pieces of information tend to occur in contexts
where people possess little or no prior information about the other person
(Fiske & Taylor, 2013). If common bonds are first forged through the intro-
duction of non-political similarity, exposure to political attitudes that challenge
one’s own would be less threatening, in turn creating an environment for more
constructive discussion.
Concluding remarks
The existence of diverse and oppositional attitudes about political topics is
an inevitable consequence of human psychology. However, the adoption of
more extreme and polarized attitudes has increased in recent years and has
contributed to adverse social relations. In this chapter, I have described how
relational motivations to connect and affiliate with other people contribute
to political polarization. I further outlined how ideological differences in
relational motivation can lead both liberals and conservatives to contribute
to polarization, but for different underlying reasons. As a point of optimism,
88 Chadly Stern
References
Abelson, R. P. (1995). Attitude extremity. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude
strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 25–41). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Abramowitz, A. (2010). The disappearing center: Engaged citizens, polarization, and American
democracy. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Abramowitz, A. I., & Saunders, K. L. (2008). Is polarization a myth? The Journal of
Politics, 70(2), 542–555.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The
authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4),
709–716.
An, J., Cha, M., Gummadi, K., & Crowcroft, J. (2011). Media landscape in Twitter: A
world of new conventions and political diversity. In Fifth International AAAI Conference
on Weblogs and Social Media.
Arendt, H. (1968). Between past and future. New York: Viking Press.
Bail, C. A., Argyle, L. P., Brown, T. W., Bumpus, J. P., Chen, H., Hunzaker, M. F., … &
Volfovsky, A. (2018). Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase pol-
itical polarization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(37), 9216–9221.
Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from left
to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber?. Psychological
Science, 26(10), 1531–1542.
Baron, J., & Jost, J. T. (2019). False equivalence: Are liberals and conservatives in the
United States equally biased? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(2), 292–303.
Bar-Tal, D. (2013). Intractable conflicts: Socio- psychological foundations and dynamics.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interper-
sonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
497–529.
Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York:
Doubleday.
Binder, A. R., Dalrymple, K. E., Brossard, D., & Scheufele, D. A. (2009). The soul of a
polarized democracy: Testing theoretical linkages between talk and attitude extremity
during the 2004 presidential election. Communication Research, 36(3), 315–340.
Bishop, B. (2009). The big sort: Why the clustering of like-minded America is tearing us apart.
Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Bougher, L. D. (2017). The correlates of discord: identity, issue alignment, and political
hostility in polarized America. Political Behavior, 39(3), 731–762.
Relational goals & political polarization 89
Boutyline, A., & Willer, R. (2017). The social structure of political echo chambers:
Variation in ideological homophily in online networks. Political Psychology, 38(3),
551–569.
Bradner, E. B. (2016). The GOP resistance to Donald Trump. Retrieved from www.
cnn.com/2016/05/05/politics/paul-r yan-donald-trump-republican-resistance/
index.html
Brady, W. J., Wills, J. A., Burkart, D., Jost, J. T., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2019). An ideological
asymmetry in the diffusion of moralized content on social media among political
leaders. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(10), 1802–1813.
Broockman, D. E., Carnes, N., Crowder-Meyer, M., & Skovron, C. (in press). Why local
party leaders don’t support nominating centrists. British Journal of Political Science.
Broockman, D. E., & Skovron, C. (2018). Bias in perceptions of public opinion among
political elites. American Political Science Review, 112(3), 542–563.
Cacciatore, M. A., Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2016).The end of framing as we know
it … and the future of media effects. Mass Communication and Society, 19(1), 7–23.
Cavazza, N., & Mucchi-Faina, A. (2008). Me, us, or them: Who is more conformist?
Perception of conformity and political orientation. The Journal of Social Psychology,
148(3), 335-346.
Colleoni, E., Rozza, A., & Arvidsson, A. (2014). Echo chamber or public sphere?
Predicting political orientation and measuring political homophily in Twitter using
big data. Journal of Communication, 64(2), 317–332.
Ditto, P. H., Liu, B. S., Clark, C. J., Wojcik, S. P., Chen, E. E., Grady, R. H., … & Zinger,
J. F. (2019). At least bias is bipartisan: A meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias in
liberals and conservatives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(2), 273–291.
Duarte, J. L., Crawford, J. T., Stern, C., Haidt, J., Jussim, L., & Tetlock, P. E. (2015).
Political diversity will improve social psychological science. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 38, 1–13.
Duncan, F. (2014). Normcore: Fashion for those who realize they’re one in 7 billion. Retrieved
from www.thecut.com/2014/02/normcore-fashion-trend.html.
Dvir-Gvirsman, S. (2015a). Size matters: The effects of political orientation, majority
status, and majority size on misperceptions of public opinion. Public Opinion Quarterly,
79(1), 1–27.
Dvir-Gvirsman, D. (2015b).Testing our quasi-statistical sense: News use, political know-
ledge, and false projection. Political Psychology, 36(6), 729–747.
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2015). From gulf to bridge: When do moral arguments
facilitate political influence?. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(12),
1665–1681.
Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political
Psychology, 24(1), 41–74.
Festinger, L. (1950). Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57(5), 271–282.
Festinger, L., Cartwright, D., Barber, K., Fleischl, J., Gottsdanker, J., Keysen, A., &
Leavitt, G. (1948). A study of a rumor: Its origin and spread. Human Relations, 1(4),
464–486.
Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. (1950). Social pressures in informal groups: A study
of a housing community. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: From brains to culture. Atlanta, GA:
Sage.
Goel, S., Mason, W., & Watts, D. J. (2010). Real and perceived attitude agreement in
social networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(4), 611–621.
90 Chadly Stern
Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children become friends. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 48(3, Serial No. 201).
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different
sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2009). Planet of the Durkheimians, where community, authority,
and sacredness are foundations of morality. In J. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir
(Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification (pp. 371–401).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Hall, A. B. (2015). What happens when extremists win primaries? American Political
Science Review, 109(1), 18–42.
Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes
the subjective objective. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of
motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context (Vol. 3, pp. 28–84). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Hassell, H. J. (2018). Principled moderation: Understanding parties’ support of mod-
erate candidates. Legislative Studies Quarterly, 43(2), 343–369.
Hennes, E. P., Nam, H. H., Stern, C., & Jost, J. T. (2012). Not all ideologies are created
equal: Epistemic, existential, and relational needs predict system-justifying attitudes.
Social Cognition, 30(6), 669–688.
Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998).The evolution of conformist transmission and the emer-
gence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(4), 215–241.
Hess,Y. D., & Ledgerwood, A. (2014). Bolstering system-justifying beliefs in response to
social exclusion. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 17(4), 494–508.
Higgins, E. T. (2019). Shared reality: What makes us strong and tears us apart. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Hoch, S. J. (1987). Perceived consensus and predictive accuracy: The pros and cons of
projection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(2), 221–234.
Holtz, R. (2003). Intragroup or intergroup attitude projection can increase opinion cer-
tainty: Is there classism at college? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(9), 1922–1944.
Hoyle, R. H. (1993). Interpersonal attraction in the absence of explicit attitudinal infor-
mation. Social Cognition, 11(3), 309–320.
Huber, G.A., & Malhotra, N. (2017). Political homophily in social relationships: Evidence
from online dating behavior. The Journal of Politics, 79(1), 269–283.
Imhoff, R., & Erb, H. P. (2009). What motivates nonconformity? Uniqueness seeking
blocks majority influence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(3), 309–320.
Iyengar, S. (1990). Framing responsibility for political issues: The case of poverty. Political
Behavior, 12(1), 19–40.
Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N., & Westwood, S. J. (2019). The
origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annual
Review of Political Science, 22, 129–146.
Jacobson, G. C. (2016). Polarization, gridlock, and presidential campaign politics in
2016. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 667(1), 226–246.
Johnston, R., Manley, D., & Jones, K. (2016). Spatial polarization of presidential voting
in the United States, 1992–2012: The “big sort” revisited. Annals of the American
Association of Geographers, 106(5), 1047–1062.
Jost, J. T. (2017). Ideological asymmetries and the essence of political psychology. Political
Psychology, 38(2), 167–208.
Relational goals & political polarization 91
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as
motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.
Jost, J. T., Ledgerwood, A., & Hardin, C. D. (2008). Shared reality, system justification,
and the relational basis of ideological beliefs. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
2(1), 171–186.
Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political
orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating, and
personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 633–645.
Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L., & Schaller, M. (2010). Renovating
the pyramid of needs: Contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 292–314.
Kim, K. R., Kang, J. S., & Yun, S. (2012). Moral intuitions and political orientation:
Similarities and differences between South Korea and the United States. Psychological
Reports, 111(1), 173–185.
Koch, A., Imhoff, R., Dotsch, R., Unkelbach, C., & Alves, H. (2016). The ABC
of stereotypes about groups: Agency/ socioeconomic success, conservative–
progressive beliefs, and communion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
110(5), 675–709.
Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J.Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W.Y., & Sleeth-Keppler,
D. (2002). A theory of goal-systems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34,
311–378.
Lau, I.Y., Chiu, C.Y., & Lee, S. L. (2001). Communication and shared reality: Implications
for the psychological foundations of culture. Social Cognition, 19, 350–371.
Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2014). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public
opinion formation. Political Psychology, 35, 129–156.
Leonardelli, G. J., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2010). Optimal distinctiveness
theory: A framework for social identity, social cognition, and intergroup relations. In
M. P. Zanna (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 43, pp. 65–115). San
Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics. Human Relations, 1, 5–41.
Liu, J. H., & Latané, B. (1998). The catastrophic link between the importance and
extremity of political attitudes. Political Behavior, 20(2), 105–126.
Lukianoff, G., & Haidt, J. (2019). The coddling of the American mind: How good intentions
and bad ideas are setting up a generation for failure. New York: Penguin Press.
Mallett, R. K., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Expect the unexpected: Failure to
anticipate similarities leads to an intergroup forecasting error. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 94(2), 265–277.
Markus, H. R. (2008). Pride, prejudice, and ambivalence: Toward a unified theory of
race and ethnicity. American Psychologist, 63(8), 651–670.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4),
370–396.
Mill, J. S. (1979). On liberty. New York: Penguin. (Original work published 1859.)
Miller, P. R., & Conover, P. J. (2015). Red and blue states of mind: Partisan hostility and
voting in the United States. Political Research Quarterly, 68(2), 225–239.
Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary for
attraction? A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 25(6), 889–922.
92 Chadly Stern
Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Oishi, S., Trawalter, S., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). How ideological
migration geographically segregates groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
51, 1–14.
Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect political
attitudes. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Nilsson, A., & Erlandsson, A. (2015). The Moral Foundations taxonomy: Structural val-
idity and relation to political ideology in Sweden. Personality and Individual Differences,
76, 28–32.
Ondish, P., & Stern, C. (2018). Liberals possess more national consensus on political
attitudes in the United States: An examination across 40 years. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 9(8), 935–943.
Pew (2014). Political polarization and the American public. Retrieved from www.people-
press.org/2014/06/12/section-3-political-polarization-and-personal-life/
Pinel, E. C., & Long, A. E. (2012). When I’s meet: Sharing subjective experience with
someone from the outgroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3), 296–307.
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community.
New York: Simon and Schuster.
Rabinowitz, M., Latella, L., Stern, C., & Jost, J. T. (2016). Beliefs about childhood vac-
cination in the United States: Political ideology, false consensus, and the illusion of
uniqueness. PloS One, 11(7).
Reis, H. T., Collins, W. A., & Berscheid, E. (2000). The relationship context of human
behavior and development. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 844–872.
Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind: Investigations into the nature of belief systems
and personality systems. New York: Basic Books.
Ross, L. D., Lelkes,Y., & Russell, A. G. (2012). How Christians reconcile their personal
political views and the teachings of their faith: Projection as a means of dissonance
reduction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(10), 3616–3622.
Rubin, L. B. (1985). Just friends: The role of friendship in our lives (pp. 213–225). New York:
Harper & Row.
Rusbult, C. E., Olsen, N., Davis, J. L., & Hannon, P. A. (2001). Commitment and rela-
tionship maintenance mechanisms. In J. Harvey & A. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic
relationships: Maintenance and enhancement (pp. 87–113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Siders, D. (2020). ‘It sounds pretty insane, actually’: Democrats relive 2016 primary all over
again. Retrieved from www.politico.com/news/2020/03/07/democrats-2016-
primary-122951
Sinclair, S., Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Colangelo, A. (2005). Social tuning of auto-
matic racial attitudes: the role of affiliative motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89(4), 583–592.
Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1977). Abnormality as a positive characteristic: The
development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 86(5), 518–527.
Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The human pursuit of difference.
New York: Plenum.
Stern, C., & Crawford, J. (2021). Ideological conflict and prejudice: An adversarial col-
laboration examining correlates and ideological (a)symmetries. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 12, 42–53.
Relational goals & political polarization 93
Stern, C., & Ondish, P. (2018). Political aspects of shared reality. Current Opinion in
Psychology, 23, 11–14.
Stern, C., & West, T. V. (2016). Ideological differences in anchoring and adjustment
during social inferences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(11), 1466–1479.
Stern, C., West, T. V., Jost, J. T., & Rule, N. O. (2014). “Ditto heads”: Do conservatives
perceive greater consensus within their ranks than liberals? Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 40(9), 1162–1177.
Stern, C., West, T. V., & Schmitt, P. G. (2014). The liberal illusion of uniqueness.
Psychological Science, 25(1), 137–144.
Testa, C. (2012). Is polarization bad? European Economic Review, 56(6), 1104–1118.
Tidwell, N. D., Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Perceived, not actual, similarity
predicts initial attraction in a live romantic context: Evidence from the speed-dating
paradigm. Personal Relationships, 20(2), 199–215.
van Prooijen, J. W., & Krouwel, A. P. (2019). Psychological features of extreme political
ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(2), 159–163.
Waytz, A., Iyer, R., Young, L., Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2019). Ideological differences in
the expanse of the moral circle. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–12.
West, T. V., Magee, J. C., Gordon, S. H., & Gullett, L. (2014). A little similarity goes a
long way: The effects of peripheral but self-revealing similarities on improving and
sustaining interracial relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1),
81–100.
Yilmaz, O., Harma, M., Bahçekapili, H. G., & Cesur, S. (2016). Validation of the moral
foundations questionnaire in Turkey and its relation to cultural schemas of individu-
alism and collectivism. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 149–154.
Section 2
Blacks, women, immigrants, refugees, … all have cut ahead of you in line.
But it’s people like you who have made this country great.You feel uneasy.
It has to be said: the line cutters irritate you. They are violating rules of
fairness.You resent them, and it is right that you do.
(Hochschild, 2016, p. 139)
[t]he discontented middle class, whose envy of those more prosperous than
themselves was considerably less than their fear of sinking from bourgeois
status into the proletariat. This was exactly the same kind of social group
living in a state of anxiety that Adolf Hitler later gathered around him to
provide his first large body of followers.
(Zweig, 1942/2009, p. 83)
Economic deprivation
When, where, and why would people turn to populist parties with an anti-
immigrant agenda? These are some of the questions social scientists have been
asking for many decades. From the outset, there was considerable interest in
Support for populist parties 99
the idea that economic deprivation, which causes increased conflict over scarce
resources, might provide answers to these questions (Sherif, Harvey, White,
Hood, & Sherif, 1961; for a meta-analysis see Riek, Mania, & Gaertner, 2006).
Researchers subsequently claimed to have unearthed empirical evidence for
a link between economic downturn and harsh attitudes towards minorities
(Assumptions 1 and 2). For example, Hovland and Sears (1940) studied the link
between cotton prices (which they used as a proxy for economic conditions)
and black lynching in the Southern part of the US, and found that lynching
increased when cotton prices fell and the economy slowed down. Although
there are now serious concerns about the statistical validity of these findings
(Green, Glaser, & Rich, 1998), this paper has become a classic in social psych-
ology and contributed to the above assumptions becoming accepted in many
social science circles as established knowledge.
Hovland and Sears’s proposition seemed to make perfect sense in the late
1940s, when social scientists turned their attention to the question of how the
Holocaust in Nazi Germany could have happened. This outlook also fit well
with Dollard and colleagues’ frustration–aggression thesis, according to which
failure to attain goals creates frustration and outgroup aggression (Dollard,
Miller, Doob, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939), and with later work arguing frustra-
tion can lead to scapegoating of minorities (Allport, 1954). Although different
strands of social science research focused on different aspects of this puzzle (e.g.,
stereotyping, prejudice, discrimination, scapegoating), Hovland and Sears’s idea
that “harsh times produce harsh attitudes” seemed well suited to make sense of
this dark episode in human history. After all, the Holocaust happened in the
wake of the 1930s’ Great Depression, when ordinary Germans faced economic
hardship (e.g., hyperinflation, mass unemployment, increased competition for
jobs and affordable housing), and Hitler’s regime was said to have harnessed
these grievances, thereby channeling frustration with economic conditions into
aggression towards Jews and other minorities.
Social psychologists have played a prominent role in disseminating the “harsh
times producing harsh attitudes” paradigm in research into intergroup conflict
and polarization. However, the idea may have gained wider traction because it
aligned well with at least two other strands of thought. First, the idea of a causal
link between frustration, aggression, and lashing out seemed to fit well with
Marxian understandings of society, and the idea that uprisings will take place
once working-class deprivation and frustration reach a tipping point. Second,
from the 1960s onwards, rational choice theorizing became popular (Harsanyi,
1969), and although these theorists were not seeking to explain or predict the
rise and fall of populist movements, from their theoretical standpoint (in which
they conceive of humans as selfish rational utility maximizers) it also made sense
to conceive of intergroup tensions as tensions arising from competition over
scarce resources.
When considering the above, it is not surprising to see such widespread
faith in the notion that “harsh times produce harsh attitudes,” and so much
100 Jolanda Jetten and Frank Mols
follow-up research to tease out the finer details. For example, researchers would
later distinguish between egoistic (individual) and “socio-tropic” (collective)
concerns, and between “realistic conflict threat” (perceived conflict over scarce
resources) and “symbolic threat” (perceived threat to the group’s values) (e.g.,
Esses, Dovidio, Jackson, & Armstrong, 2001; Stephan & Stephan, 1996).
When evaluating the evidence for economic deprivation thinking however,
evidence is rather mixed for the assumption that support for populist parties
with a strong anti-immigrant agenda is higher (a) in times of economic down-
turn (Assumption 1), and/or (b) in areas hardest hit by economic downturn
(Assumption 2). Of direct relevance here are findings of more contemporary
analyses of voter attitudes. For example, Mols and Jetten (2016, 2017; see also
Jetten, 2019) analyzed election results in different countries, and found no evi-
dence for the widely accepted view that economic crises or peaks in immi-
gration provide fertile soil for populist parties. If anything, this research casts
doubt on the above-mentioned assumptions, showing that populist parties are
often remarkably popular in times of economic prosperity (see also Mudde,
2007; Mudde & Rovira Kaltwasser, 2018). Consistent with this reasoning too,
Mutz (2018) observed in her research that the US economy was recovering
well during the 2016 election that saw Donald Trump come to power with
a strong anti-immigrant agenda. Put differently, these findings suggest that
there has been a tendency to rely too heavily on economic relative deprivation
explanations, thereby neglecting counterfactuals.
When it comes to the third assumption on who is most likely to be attracted
to populist parties, in line with relative deprivation theory (Walker & Smith,
2001), the reasoning typically goes that those who are most deprived are most
vulnerable economically and that might explain why they feel most threatened
by immigrants with whom they fear they might have to compete for jobs and
housing. This widespread belief that (realistic and symbolic) threat perceptions
will be more pronounced among those at the poorer end of the wealth spec-
trum who are hit hardest by economic downturn (Assumption 3) is remarkably
pervasive, and it is hence not surprising that so many commentators view “the
poor” as most prone to polarize and vote for a populist party.
While we are not questioning that such a relationship may exist at times
(manual workers have been found to be drawn to populist parties: Lubbers &
Scheepers, 2000), it is also clear that there are some important exceptions to
that widespread assumption. For example, when Donald Trump won the 2016
US presidential election, securing 46.1% of votes, it gradually became clear
that his support base was not limited to poor working-class voters in so-called
rustbelt states (states that had been negatively affected by economic down-
turn, Assumption 2), but included voters in more affluent parts of the country,
like Florida. Furthermore, the first exit poll results immediately challenged
Assumption 1, showing that Trump attracted voters from all income categories,
and that 45% of those voting for Trump had a college degree. Similar findings
were encountered in Gallup pre-election survey data research, which found
Support for populist parties 101
that Trump voters earned more than average, and that they were less likely than
non-voters to have been affected by globalization and immigration (Rothwell
& Diego-Rosell, 2016).
This is also evident from Mutz’s (2018) analysis of a representative panel
data set from 2012 to 2016. She found little support for the hypothesis that
an individual’s personal wealth and changes to their wealth position were
associated with a vote for Donald Trump. Mutz (2018) concludes instead that
“The 2016 election […] was an effort by members of already dominant groups
to assure their continued dominance and by those in an already powerful and
wealthy country to assure its continued dominance” (p. 9). This conclusion fits
well with the cultural backlash and status anxiety hypothesis, which we will
explain further below.
Other evidence that populist parties can be remarkably popular among
voters who are least affected by economic downturn and relatively well
off can be obtained from other opinion poll research. For example, a study
conducted in 2010 in the Netherlands revealed that voters of the right-wing
populist party, the Party for Freedom (PVV), are 40% more likely to be less
well educated than the average voter, but 21% more likely to earn an income
higher than the median (see Mols & Jetten, 2017). Likewise, exit poll research
revealed that two-thirds of those turning out to vote in the Brexit referendum
were middle class. Of all those who voted “Leave,” 59% were middle class (A,
B, or C1), as opposed to 24% of voters in the lowest two social classes (D, E)
(Dorling, 2016).
It thus appears that, even though the idea that populist parties appeal to the
unemployed, the uneducated, and blue-collar workers seems to make perfect
sense, as does the idea of this being explained by deprivation triggering frus-
tration, aggression, and scapegoating (Dollard et al., 1939), the research evi-
dence tells a more complex story. For example, although some individual-level
analyses seemed to confirm the suspected link between unemployment and
populist voting (e.g., Betz, 1994; Jackman & Volpert, 1996; Lubbers, Gijsberts,
& Scheepers, 2002), there are many researchers who fail to find support for this
link (e.g., Jetten, 2019; Mols & Jetten, 2017; Norris, 2005). As several authors
have pointed out, on the whole the evidence for the economic argument is
rather mixed and inconclusive (for overviews, see Inglehart & Norris, 2016;
Mudde, 2007). It has also been noted that this relationship does not automatic-
ally manifest in macro-level national trends (Inglehart & Norris, 2016; Lubbers
et al., 2002). If anything, it is not uncommon to see populist parties thrive in
times of economic growth, low unemployment, low immigration and asylum
seeking (e.g., Gibson, 2002; Jetten, 2019; Mols & Jetten, 2017).
But why is it often middle-class voters on above-average incomes who
are drawn to populist parties and movements? One possible answer would
be cultural backlash and resistance against the rise of progressive values,
disseminated through education. After all, voters may earn an above-average
personal or household income, but still feel alienated culturally from those
102 Jolanda Jetten and Frank Mols
Cultural backlash
A promising alternative explanation to the economic deprivation account is
the cultural backlash thesis, according to which the current surge in votes for
populist parties (and associated polarization) can be explained not as a purely
economic phenomenon, but in large part as a reaction against rapid progres-
sive cultural change (Inglehart & Norris, 2016; Norris & Inglehart, 2019).
Traditional left–r ight politics, so Inglehart and Norris argue, has become over-
laid with a new cleavage dividing populism from cosmopolitan liberalism, with
progressive causes (e.g., gender equality, same-sex marriage, refugee rights, pol-
itical correctness), rather than socio-economic status, becoming the new polit-
ical and ideological battleground. In this reading, which builds on earlier work
interpreting the rise of new social movements in Europe as a silent revolu-
tion (Inglehart, 1977) and subsequent research interpreting the rise of populist
radical-r ight parties as a “silent counter-revolution” (Ignazi, 1992), it is not so
much macro-economic conditions or voters’ personal income that determines
their voting behavior, but opposition to multiculturalism, gender equality, and
other progressive values. Based on this line of research, it has been predicted
that populist parties will thrive when the educated ruling elite are perceived as
zealously promoting progressive norms/causes, and with insufficient regard for
the interests and preferences of society’s less-well-educated voters.
Inglehart and Norris (2016) put their thesis to the test when analyzing data
from different national surveys (e.g., European Social Survey), expecting to
Support for populist parties 103
encounter strongest support for populist parties among the older generation,
men, those lacking college education, and people with traditional values. The
results of this analysis confirmed not only (a) that the cultural and the eco-
nomic cleavage form distinct dimensions of party competition, but also (b) that
the rise of populist parties reflects, above all, a reaction against a wide range of
rapid cultural changes that seem to be eroding the basic values and customs of
Western society (Inglehart & Norris, 2016). This sentiment is well articulated
by Mike, a Tea Party voter, who was interviewed by Hochschild (2016). Mike’s
views are summarized as follows:
the Federal government wasn’t on the side of men being manly. Liberals
were certainly on the wrong side of that one. It wasn’t easy being a man. It
was an area of numerous subtle challenges to masculinity, it seemed. These
days, a woman didn’t need a man for financial support, for procreation,
even for the status of being married. And now with talk of transgender
people, what, really, was a man? It was unsettling, wrong. At the core, to be
a man you had to be willing to lose your life in battle, willing to use your
strength to protect the weak. Who today was remembering all that?
(Hochschild, 2016, p. 202)
We know from earlier studies that populist support tends to be stronger among
the older generation, men, and the less well educated (Lubbers et al., 2002;
Norris, 2005), and these findings align well with Norris and Inglehart’s (2019)
cultural backlash thesis. However, because more educated voters tend to also
earn higher incomes, it remains unclear whether declining support for populist
parties among the well educated reflects reduced economic anxiety, socializa-
tion into progressive values, or both. What is more, researchers examining the
relative importance of “education” have revealed puzzling effects that defy such
explanations. For example, researchers have uncovered a curvilinear pattern,
suggesting populist radical-right parties receive their strongest support from
the mid-school stratum (Arzheimer & Carter, 2006; Evans, 2005). As we will
see in the next section, this is not the only area in which we can witness non-
linear patterns, and in our view it is worth zeroing in on such patterns because
this will enable us to do more justice to the complex processes underpinning
support for populist parties, and to recognize what existing explanations (eco-
nomic deprivation and cultural backlash) fail to capture.
to, when they fear a decline in status or when they feel that their wealth is not
growing fast enough, their support for populism becomes easier to understand.
The main lesson to emerge from our work is that populist voting may well
be a phenomenon associated with status protection and aspiration to secure
upward social mobility. It is this phenomenon that we call the wealth paradox:
even though relatively speaking, a person can be relatively gratified, they might
still feel relatively deprived when they feel entitled to future gains or when they
fear previous gains might be lost in the future (Jetten, 2019 Jetten et al., 2015;
Mols & Jetten, 2017).
Social identity theorizing (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) helps to understand when
status anxiety is most likely to occur. Building on work by Scheepers, Ellemers,
and Sintemaartensdijk (2009; see also Ellemers & Bos, 1998; Scheepers &
Ellemers, 2005), we have argued that perceptions of the permeability of bound-
aries might enhance the fear of status loss among the more affluent and this
makes those who are objectively relatively gratified fear (future) relative depriv-
ation. In contrast, high-status group members have been found to be relatively
magnanimous towards lower-status groups, including minorities, when they
do not feel that the minority group is challenging their higher status, or when
their high status is secure and not under threat (Bettencourt, Dorr, Charlton,
& Hume, 2001; Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992; Sachdev & Bourhis, 1985;
Turner & Brown, 1978). For example, Harvey and Bourhis (2011) found in a
context where wealth positions of poor and wealthier participants were fixed
for the duration of the study that those in a wealthy group were most likely to
share their resources equally with subordinate poor group members, presum-
ably because their high wealth position was secure and this allowed them to be
tolerant and generous.
What is more, another key principle in social identity theorizing relating to
the legitimacy of wealth relations might also play a role in the extent to which
status anxiety is triggered. The more that economic prosperity is associated
with a sense of entitlement, the more there will be a fear that “our” resources,
and resources that “we are entitled to” might be lost to immigrants, justifying
anti-immigrant sentiments (Jetten, Ryan, & Mols, 2017; LeBlanc, Beaton, &
Walker, 2015).
In sum, in line with social identity theorizing (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), when
wealth positions are locked in because group boundaries are impermeable and
when a position of affluence is stable and legitimate, we might see that a higher
wealth position may lead to noblesse oblige, whereby the wealthy feel they have
to look after those who are less well off, and this should be associated with
tolerance and support for minority groups such as immigrants. However, at
other times, and in particular when those lower in the hierarchy challenge the
legitimacy of the status quo, wealthy groups may experience status anxiety and
may strike hard to protect their privileged position. The negative treatment of
those who are less well off may be seen as justified in such cases to protect the
106 Jolanda Jetten and Frank Mols
status quo and the superiority of the high-wealth group (see Jetten, 2019; Mols
& Jetten, 2017).
There is some empirical support for the prediction that fearing the loss of
one’s advantaged status is associated with populist support. For example, Major,
Blodorn, and Major-Blascovich (2016) asked what accounts for the widespread
support for Donald Trump in the 2016 US elections.They found that reminding
white Americans high in ethnic identification that non-white racial groups will
outnumber whites in the United States by 2042 led to increased status threat
for the group.That is, they were more concerned about the declining status and
influence of white Americans as a group and they were more likely to report
increased support for Trump and anti-immigrant policies.
In a similar vein, we recently obtained empirical support for the notion that
the fear that one will lose social standing (in our studies this was operationalized
as the fear of losing wealth) is associated with greater opposition to immigrants
(Jetten, Mols, & Steffens, 2020). In a first study, we explored support for our
predictions in a community sample in Australia (N = 498). We found here,
correlationally, that fear of falling (operationalized as concerns about personal
future income as well as Australia’s future income) was associated with greater
opposition to immigration to Australia. We then experimentally studied the
effects of potential (Study 2, N = 294) and actual (Study 3, N = 166) down-
ward mobility among participants who were randomly allocated to a rela-
tively wealthy group in a hypothetical society. In Study 2, we found that those
allocated to a wealthier group and who learned that their position might
change in the second part of the study (i.e., a manipulation of the perme-
ability of group boundaries) experienced elevated collective angst compared to
a condition where participants had been told their position would remain the
same over the course of the study (i.e., the impermeability of group boundaries
condition). In turn, such heightened collective angst predicted higher levels of
opposition to immigration, providing evidence of mediation.
Our third study showed that collective angst was higher when wealthy
participants were confronted with actual downward mobility (varying from
stability of their status, to slight wealth loss, to more extreme wealth loss).
Furthermore, mediational analysis showed that, the greater the wealth loss, the
more collective angst participants reported and this was predictive of greater
opposition to immigration.
In a final study we aimed to model a context where a relatively wealthy
group feels that their wealth is stagnating over time while an initially poorer
group is quickly gaining wealth over time (Study 4, N = 151). Here too, we
find that fear of falling among the wealthy is associated with more opposition
to immigration, with the effect being mediated by collective angst.
We conclude from these studies that the anticipation that the economic
future looks less rosy than the present evokes collective angst amongst more
wealthy segments of society, which, in turn, fuels prejudice towards immigrants
and opposition to immigration.
Support for populist parties 107
In conclusion
Populism researchers have long been aware that income is a poor predictor of
populist voting (e.g., Mudde, 2007; Norris, 2005), and, more importantly here,
that populist parties have a remarkable capacity to unite strange bedfellows
(Arzheimer & Carter, 2006; Evans, 2005; Ford & Goodwin, 2015, Ivarsflaten,
2005; Rooduiyn, 2018), in this case the poor and the wealthy. However, as
Mudde (2007, p. 226) pointed out more than a decade ago, what remains poorly
understood is why groups with diametrically opposed socio-economic interests
would end up supporting the same party and/or leader, and it would seem fair
to argue that this has not changed signficantly. Such strange patterns are par-
ticularly difficult to explain if we rely exclusively on conventional economic
deprivation thinking. These patterns become easier to understand, though,
once we take account of the different motivations that may drive those at the
poorer end of the wealth spectrum to vote for a populist leader than those at
the wealthier end.
We have argued here and elsewhere that economic deprivation accounts may
provide a solid explanation for populist support among the “have-nots,” but not
so much for the “haves.” For the latter, it is concerns about losing their status
(i.e., status anxiety) and frustration with a ruling class that is perceived to be out
of touch with the values of ordinary working families (cultural backlash) that
might better account for their attraction to populism. It would be of interest
to explore the interplay between status anxiety and cultural backlash in greater
detail in the future to better understand why it is that populists can count on
support from the poor as well as the more affluent.
Acknowledgment
This research was supported by the Australian Research Council’s Discovery
Project funding scheme (DP170101008) and an Australian Research
Council Laureate Fellowship awarded to the first author (FL180100094).
Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Jolanda Jetten,
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072,
Australia.
Note
1 In this chapter we made the conscious choice to use the term “populist” in an
inclusive way, to describe parties and leaders deploying powerful anti-establish-
ment rhetoric, and proposing nativist welfare policies and radical measures to curb
immigration and multiculturalism. We feel this is justified because our aim is to
analyze the psychological processes that underlie the appeal of such parties and
proposals, rather than to contribute to debate about further refinements in classifi-
cation of different kinds of populist parties. Moreover, even though populist parties
can include both left-wing as well as right-wing parties, because of our interest in
108 Jolanda Jetten and Frank Mols
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Arzheimer, K., & Carter, E. (2006). Political opportunity structures and right-wing
extremist party success. European Journal of Political Research, 45(3), 419–443. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6765.2006.00304.x
Bettencourt, B. A., Dorr, N., Charlton, K., & Hume, D. L. (2001). Status differences and
in-group bias: A meta-analytic examination of the effects of status stability, status
legitimacy, and group permeability. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 520–542.
Betz, H. G. (1994). Radical right-wing populism in Western Europe. Basingstoke, UK:
Macmillan.
Dambrun, M., Taylor, D. M., McDonald, D. A., Crush, J., & Meot, A. (2006). The rela-
tive deprivation–gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans toward
immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
91(6), 1032–1044. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1032
de Botton, A. (2004). Status anxiety. Melbourne, Australia: Hamish Hamilton.
de Tocqueville, A. (1835/2000). Democracy in America. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago.
Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration
and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. https://doi.org/10.1037/
10022-000
Dorling, D. (2016). Brexit: The decision of a divided country. British Medical Journal,
354, i3697. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.i3697
Ehrenreich, B. (1990). Fear of falling: The inner life of the middle class. New York: Harper
Perennial.
Ellemers, N., & Bos,A. E. R. (1998). Social identity, relative deprivation, and coping with
the threat of position loss: A field study among native shopkeepers in Amsterdam.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(21), 1987–2006. doi:DOI 10.1111/j.1559–
1816.1998.tb01357.x
Esses, V. M., Dovidio, J. F., Jackson, L. M., & Armstrong, T. L. (2001). The immigra-
tion dilemma: The role of perceived group competition, ethnic prejudice, and
national identity. Journal of Social Issues, 57(3), 389–412. https://doi.org/10.1111/
0022-4537.00220
Evans, J. A. (2005). The dynamics of social change in radical right-wing populist party
support. Comparative European Politics, 3, 76–101. https://doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.
cep.6110050
Ford, R., & Goodwin, M. J. (2015). Different class? UKIP’s social base and political
impact: A reply to Evans and Mellon. Parliamentary Affairs, 69(2), 480–491. https://
doi.org/10.1093/pa/gsv012
Gibson, R. K. (2002). The growth of anti-immigrant parties in Western Europe. Lewiston,
NY: Edwin Mellen Press.
Green, D. P., Glaser, J., & Rich, A. (1998). From lynching to gay bashing: The elusive
connection between economic conditions and hate crime. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 75(1), 82–92. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.82
Support for populist parties 109
Grofman, B. N., & Muller, E. N. (1973). The strange case of relative gratification and
potential for political violence: The V-curve hypothesis. American Political Science
Review, 67(2), 514–539. https://doi.org/10.2307/1958781
Guimond, S., & Dambrun, M. (2002). When prosperity breeds intergroup hos-
tility: The effects of relative deprivation and relative gratification on prejudice.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(7), 900–912. https://doi.org/10.1177/
014616720202800704
Harsanyi, J. C. (1969). Rational-choice models of political behavior vs. functionalist
and conformist theories. World Politics, 21(4), 513–538. https://doi.org/10.2307/
2009665
Harvey, S. P., & Bourhis, R. Y. (2011). Discrimination in wealth and power structures.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 15, 21–38.
Hochschild, A. R. (2016). Strangers in their own land: Anger and mourning on the American
right. New York: The New Press.
Hovland, C. I., & Sears, R. R. (1940). Minor studies of aggression: VI. Correlation of
lynchings with economic indices. The Journal of Psychology, 9(2), 301–310. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1940.9917696
Ignazi, P. (1992). The silent counter-revolution: Hypotheses on the emergence of
extreme right-wing parties in Europe. European Journal of Political Research, 22(1),
3–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475–6765.1992.tb00303.x
Inglehart, R. (1977). The silent revolution: Changing values and political styles among Western
publics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Inglehart, R., & Norris, P. (2016). Trump, Brexit, and the rise of populism: Economic
have nots and cultural backlash. HKS working paper no. RWP16-026. Available at
SSRN: http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2818659
Ivarsflaten, E. (2005). The vulnerable populist right parties: No economic realignment
fuelling their electoral success. European Journal of Political Research, 44(3), 465–492.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6765.2005.00235.x
Jackman, R. W., & Volpert, K. (1996). Conditions favouring parties of the extreme right
in Western Europe. British Journal of Political Science, 26(4), 501–521. https://doi.org/
10.1017/S0007123400007584
Jetten, J. (2019). The wealth paradox: Prosperity and opposition to immigration.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(6), 1097–1113. https://doi.org/10.1002/
ejsp.2552
Jetten, J., Mols, F., & Postmes,T. (2015). Relative deprivation and relative wealth enhances
anti-immigrant sentiments: The v-curve re-examined. PloS One, 10(10), e0139156.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139156
Jetten, J., Mols, F., & Steffens, N. K. (2020). Prosperous but fearful of falling: The
wealth paradox, collective angst, and opposition to immigration. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220944112
Jetten, J., Ryan, R., & Mols, F. (2017). Stepping in the shoes of leaders of populist right-
wing parties: Promoting anti-immigrant views in times of economic prosperity.
Social Psychology, 48, 40–46.
LeBlanc, J., Beaton, A. M., & Walker, I. (2015). The down-side of being up: A new
look at group relative gratification and traditional prejudice. Social Justice Research, 28,
143–167.
Lipset, S. M. (1955). The radical right: A problem for American democracy. British
Journal of Sociology, 6(2), 176–209. doi:10.2307/587483
110 Jolanda Jetten and Frank Mols
Lubbers, M., Gijsberts, M., & Scheepers, P. (2002). Extreme right-wing voting in
Western Europe. European Journal of Political Research, 41(3), 345–378. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1475–6765.00015
Lubbers, M., & Scheepers, P. (2000). Individual and contextual characteristics of the
German extreme right- wing vote in the 1990s: A test of complementary the-
ories. European Journal of Political Research, 38(1), 63–94. https://doi.org/10.1111/
1475–6765.00528
Major, B., Blodorn, A., & Major-Blascovich, G. (2016). The threat of increasing diver-
sity: Why many White Americans support Trump in the 2016 presidential election.
Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 21(6), 931–940. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1368430216677304
Mols, F., & Jetten, J. (2016). Explaining the appeal of populist right-wing parties in times
of economic prosperity. Political Psychology, 37(2), 275–292. https://doi.org/10.1111/
pops.12258
Mols, F., & Jetten, J. (2017). The wealth paradox: Economic prosperity and the hardening of
attitudes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Moscatelli, S., Albarello, F., Prati, F., & Rubini, M. (2014). Badly off or better off than
them? The impact of relative deprivation and relative gratification on intergroup dis-
crimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 248–262. doi: 10/1037/
a0036704
Mudde, C. (2007). Populist radical right parties in Europe. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Mudde, C., & Rovira Kaltwasser, C. (2018). Studying populism in comparative per-
spective: Reflections on the contemporary and future research agenda. Comparative
Political Studies, 51(13), 1667–1693. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414018789490
Mullen, B., Brown, R., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of salience,
relevance, and status: An integration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22,
103–122.
Mutz, D. C. (2018). Status threat, not economic hardship, explains the 2016 presidential
vote. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(19), E4330–E4339. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1718155115
Norris, P. (2005). Radical right: Voters and parties in the electoral market. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2019). Cultural backlash and the rise of populism: Trump, Brexit,
and authoritarian populism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Osman, M., Lv, J. Y., & Proulx, M. J. (2018). Can empathy promote cooperation when
satus and money matter? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 40(4), 201–218. https://
doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2018.1463225
Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving
more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 99(5), 771–784. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020092
Piff, P. K., Stancato, D. M., Côté, S., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Keltner, D. (2012). Higher
social class predicts increased unethical behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 109(11), 4086–4091. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1118373109
Postmes, T., & Smith, L. G. E. (2009). Why do the privileged resort to oppression: A
look at some intragroup factors. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 769–790. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01624.x
Support for populist parties 111
Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., & Gaertner, S. L. (2006). Intergroup threat and outgroup
attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 336–
353. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_4
Rooduijn, M. (2018). What unites the voter bases of populist parties? Comparing the
electorates of 15 populist parties. European Political Science Review, 10(3), 351–368.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755773917000145
Rothwell, J. T., & Diego-Rosell, P. (2016). Explaining nationalist political views: The
case of Donald Trump. Available at SSRN: http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2822059
Sachdev, I., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1985). Social categorization and power differentials in
group relations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 415–434.
Scheepers, D. T., & Ellemers, N. (2005). When the pressure is up: The assessment of
social identity threat in low and high status groups. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 41(2), 192–200.
Scheepers, D. T., Ellemers, N., & Sintemaartensdijk, N. (2009). Suffering from the pos-
sibility of status loss: Physiological responses to social identity threat in high status
groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(6), 1075–1092. doi:10.1002/ejsp.609
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). Intergroup
cooperation and competition: The Robbers’ Cave experiment. Norman, OK: University
Book Exchange.
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1996). Predicting prejudice. International Journal
of Intercultural Relations, 20(3–4), 409–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767
(96)00026-0
Tajfel, H., & Turner J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W.
G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48).
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Turner. J. C., & Brown, R. (1978). Social status, cognitive alternatives, and intergroup
relations. In H.Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psych-
ology of intergroup relations (pp. 202–234). New York: Academic Press.
Walker, I., & Smith, H. J. (Eds.). (2001). Relative deprivation: Specification, development, and
integration. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/
CBO9780511527753
Wang, Z., Jetten, J., & Steffens, N. K. (2019). The more you have, the more you want?
Higher social class predicts a greater desire for wealth and status. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 50(2), 360–375. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2620
Zweig, S. (1942/2009). The world of yesterday: Memoirs of a European. London, UK:
Pushkin Press.
Chapter 7
Now even as we speak, there are those who are preparing to divide us, the
spin masters and negative ad peddlers who embrace the politics of any-
thing goes. Well, I say to them tonight, there’s not a liberal America and a
conservative America; there’s the United States of America. There’s not a
black America and white America and Latino America and Asian America;
there’s the United States of America.
President Barack Obama
Our nation has become too divided. Too many Americans feel like they’ve
lost hope. Crime is harming too many citizens. Racial tensions have gotten
worse, not better. This isn’t the American Dream we all want for our
children.
President Donald Trump
Societies often experience division. From the American political divide over
COVID-19 (Brownstein, 2020; Conway, 2020a; Malloy & Schwartz, 2020), to
Syria’s ongoing civil war (e.g., BBC News, 2020), to the European Union’s dis-
unity on Brexit (e.g., Hunt & Wheeler, 2017), finding common ground can
often feel like an insurmountable task. This sentiment is particularly evident
in modern-day America, where people find themselves choosing between one
of two seemingly mutually exclusive sides: Republican or Democrat, climate
change proponent or denier, refugee immigration or travel ban, COVID-19-
is-dangerous or COVID-19-is-benign. These “us versus them” divisions per-
meate politics, religion, race, and socio-economic class relations, interpersonal
relationships, and even (alternative) facts. Ironically, it seems that one of the
very few points of agreement among opposing ideologues –as exemplified
by Presidents Barack Obama and Donald Trump –is that America is increas-
ingly divided. And indeed, an overwhelming amount of evidence suggests
that America is a very politically divided nation, perhaps more divided than
it has been in a hundred years or more (McConnell, Margalit, Malhotra, &
Levendusky, 2018; Schaeffer, 2020).
The agreement paradox 113
Reactance
Perceived pressures for agreement threaten to take away people’s freedom to
choose; and a lot of research and theory shows that people resent having their
freedom taken away. This phenomenon has been explained under the rubric of
psychological reactance theory (Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Dillard & Shen, 2005),
which states that when people’s freedom is taken away (e.g., if they are forced
to agree), they feel a desire to resist the pressure or undergo attitude change to
reestablish their freedom (e.g., Silvia, 2006).
Often, these effects produce immediate push-back. For example, controlling
parenting, which involves coercion and punishment, can backfire as it often
causes children to feel reactance (van Petegem, Soenens,Vansteenkiste, & Beyers,
116 Lucian Gideon Conway et al.
2015). Similarly, movie ratings that limit adolescents from viewing adult movies
sometimes backfire, as those movie ratings attract adolescents to these movies
because of reactance (Varava & Quick, 2015). Pop-up ads on websites that force
web surfers to watch the ad once again take away the viewer’s freedom on the
webpage. As a result, these ads are perceived as intrusive and irritating (Edwards,
Li, & Lee, 2002).
As the reactance literature demonstrates, however, coercive pressure does
not always produce immediate non-compliant behavior. While many different
moderators have been established for the relationship between pressure and
deviant behavior (see, e.g., Grandpre, Alvaro, Burgoon, Miller, & Hall, 2003;
Laurin, Kay, & Fitzsimons, 2012; Quick & Stephenson, 2008), the important
moderators for our purposes revolve around the difference between feeling
reactance and acting upon it. Often, people have emotional reactance to pressure,
but they do not act on that emotion. For example, persons may feel particu-
larly annoyed at their boss for imposing a communication ban –they may feel
a strong desire to reestablish their freedom –but they nonetheless will comply
with the ban because they believe that it would be professionally imprudent to
be seen as engaging in deviant behavior (see Booth & Hern, 2017; Conway &
Schaller, 2005). Similarly, persons may feel reactance against political correctness
(PC) norms that dictate what they can and cannot say, but may nonetheless
comply with those norms for self-presentational reasons (see Conway et al.,
2009); or athletes may feel reactance against public norms about standing for
the national anthem, but may comply with those norms for fear of losing their
place on the team (see Korman, 2017).
This suggests the following vital point: even when reactance is not acted on
in the short term, it psychologically exists behind the scenes. It is a crack in the
foundation of the artificially manufactured agreement. Importantly, the things
that propped up the artificial agreement may change; and the reactance will
remain. The boss who enforced the policy may move on –yet the employees
will still feel very frustrated at having their freedom taken away and will still
want to reassert that freedom when they believe they can. PC norms may cause
people to comply with those norms –yet they may feel privately frustrated,
looking for the first chance to lash out and reestablish their freedom.Thus, even
when the public pressures appear to be working, reactance may nonetheless
grow like foundational cracks, and eventually undermine the very agreement
they were intended to create.
Informational contamination
Feeling pressure to agree with a particular position not only has emotional
consequences, but research grounded in attribution theory (Kelley, 1973)
suggests it also impacts cognitive judgments (e.g., for discussions, see Conway
& Repke, 2019; Conway & Schaller, 2005; Conway et al., 2009, 2017). How
people evaluate the veracity of others’ opinions depends not only on what
The agreement paradox 117
they say their opinions are, but also the context in which they express them
(Newtson & Czerlinsky’s, 1974).
This idea importantly interfaces with a second consequence of agreement
pressure: informational contamination. Informational contamination occurs when
the value of expressed agreement is informationally discounted because it is
viewed as the result of public pressure to agree and thus does not represent a
“real” consensus. Consider a simple example. All else being equal, if you heard a
group of people all expressing a positive opinion about Louisianans, you would
be likely to think of Louisianans more positively than you did before. However,
if you found out that, right before you entered the room, an influential professor
(who was still present) had said he was from Louisiana and expected people to
talk positively, you would discount the positive “Louisiana agreement” you heard.
You would in that case likely attribute the positive agreement to the pressure
for agreement and not to the potential reality of Louisianan goodness (see
Conway et al., 2009). The information contained in the agreement would be
contaminated by the perceived pressure for agreement that seemed to produce it.
Consistent with the informational contamination hypothesis, findings
from Eagly, Wood, and Chaiken (1978) demonstrate that perceptions of a
communicator’s true opinions are highly susceptible to perceived external
pressures, such that a communicator is viewed as more manipulative and less
sincere when they express opinions consistent with perceived external pressure.
As a result, people do not trust the subsequently expressed opinion as much in
cases where external pressures appear salient. Similarly, research suggests that
perceiving behavior as a function of overt pressure from an authority figure
leads to attributions about the constraints of obedience as opposed to a reflec-
tion of the person’s actual beliefs (Conway & Schaller, 2005; Fein, 1996; Pryor,
Rholes, Ruble, & Kriss, 1984).
As with reactance, informational contamination often occurs simultan-
eously with the emergence of apparent agreement. There is an irony here. The
very thing that produces the short-term artificial consensus –pressures for
agreement –is also the thing that makes it unsustainable in the long term. It is
like constructing a building with cracks in the foundation –sooner or later it
is destined to collapse.
Business decisions
Often businesses feel pressure to come to a consensus (see Dong & Xu, 2016).
Very few businesses want to feel a deep sense of division within their own ranks.
One response to this desire for business unity –in the face of potential disagree-
ment –is to try and manufacture unity by pressuring people into agreement
from the top down. What is the consequence of this approach? In a set of five
studies using business scenarios, Conway and colleagues (2005) demonstrated
how a business approach that manufactures short-term agreement through
pressure can backfire.
In all the scenarios, participants imagined that they were a member of a
committee that was going to make a vital business decision for the company,
and they then witnessed agreement in favor of one of the options under con-
sideration. Researchers varied whether or not the agreement appeared to
have been manufactured by pressure from the company president: in some
conditions, the company president put pressure on the committee to choose
one option (the one ultimately agreed upon), while in another condition the
president did not do so.
Did this pressure produce backfiring? In line with an agreement paradox
approach, it depended on the likelihood that participants themselves would
feel directly influenced by the president in the scenario. No backfiring of the
pressure manipulation occurred when participants believed that they them-
selves might still be under the direct influence of the president –his prior
command was comparatively more likely in that instance to produce compli-
ance. However, just as in real-world situations where parameters often change,
in conditions where the context changed and the president who gave the ori-
ginal order was no longer in charge when the key decision was made, the
pressure from the president backfired and produced more deviance (Conway
et al., 2005).
Consistent with our discussion of how pressure backfires, mediational ana-
lyses revealed that when backfiring of pressure occurred, it occurred largely
because the pressure produced both informational contamination and react-
ance (both independent mediators; Conway et al., 2005). These were cracks in
the foundation of the artificial agreement produced by the command.
In summary, this work reveals some of the components of the agreement
paradox. All else being equal, pressure from authorities can manufacture an
artificial consensus –but the agreement is fragile. The things that prop up
artificial agreement –in this case, the continued presence of the authority
figure who gave the command –are often temporary. But the reactance and
informational contamination they create last beyond that moment, and when
the prop is removed, the agreement collapses. Thus, pressure can produce
short-term agreement, but the pressure ultimately backfires and produces
division instead.
The agreement paradox 119
These studies show some of the basic features of the agreement paradox: a
norm designed to manufacture positive communication about groups may
succeed in the short term. But those norms cannot possibly be ever-present in
all situations –people may move to a new context where the norm is not in
operation. And when they do, both informational contamination and reactance
are still present, which in turn produces a backfiring for the norm. This is how
a counterintuitive finding can consistently emerge –how a norm specific-
ally designed to produce positive communication can instead produce negative
communication.
One study suggests it does. Conway and colleagues (2017) measured a sample
of moderate (slightly left-leaning) American participants’ voting preferences for
Trump and Clinton during the 2016 general election season. At a general level,
they found that the more people felt a sense of general reactance to and con-
tamination by existing communication norms, the more likely they were to
support Trump over Clinton. The key effects generally held even when con-
trolling for participants’ own ideology, demonstrating that the effect of commu-
nication norm concerns went beyond liberal or conservative boundaries, but
independently predicted voting preferences for both groups.
More importantly, Conway et al. (2017) also manipulated the salience of PC
norms. In one condition, participants were reminded of the existing PC norms
and given a justification for their continued existence. In other conditions,
participants were not reminded. In line with the agreement paradox, results
clearly showed that priming PC norms made people more likely to support
the non-normative candidate: when PC norms were made salient, participants
were more likely to express a preference for Trump (but not Clinton).
Why would primng PC norms make people more likely to support the
non- normative candidate? This seems counterintuitive on the surface but
makes sense if one considers it through the lens of the agreement paradox.
Agreement pressures cause people to feel reactance and informational contam-
ination regardless of their ideology. When a non-normative candidate appears
(even one who, like Trump, is largely disliked), people feel at the very least that
this candidate is working against the pressure (and accompanying undesired
consequences). Thus, whereas they may not otherwise like Trump, priming the
restrictive PC norms reminds people of their dislike of restricted freedom, and
as a result increases their support for him. Indeed, in Conway et al. (2017),
Clinton had a sizeable lead on Trump in conditions where PC norms were
not made salient, but this lead was essentially eradicated when PC norms were
primed.
political bias (see e.g., Guber, 2012; McCright & Dunlap, 2011) and selective
exposure (see e.g., Stroud, 2011). However, a recent series of studies (Conway
& Repke, 2019) suggests part of the explanation involves perceived pressure
for agreement producing reactance and informational contamination. In their
first study, Conway and Repke (2019) measured the degree of informational
contamination in participants’ assessment of the scientific consensus of climate
change. Specifically, they gave participants a brief summary of a published
study showing 97 percent scientific consensus on climate change and then
asked several questions about participants’ attributions of the consensus (e.g.,
“I believe that the 97 percent agreement exists because there is a larger polit-
ical agenda that puts pressure on scientists to conform to the views endorsed
by the agenda”). Conway and Repke (2019) then used this informational
contamination measurement to predict participants’ support for both govern-
mental and civic action regarding climate change. They found that perceived
informational contamination decreased the likelihood of supporting govern-
mental and civic action in response to climate change –and, importantly,
this effect remained even when controlling for participants’ ideology. In other
words, informational contamination about scientific consensus eroded support
for climate change-based action, even when ideologically Americans would be prone
to support such action.
Two additional studies used experimental scenarios to test both informa-
tional contamination’s and reactance’s role in the political pressure → rejection
of governmental action path. In both studies, an agreement in favor of a pro-
sustainability law emerged in a political context. The researchers manipulated
whether or not a strong political leader (Study 2) or a highly visible law (Study
3) put pressure for public agreement. In both studies, they also measured infor-
mational contamination by asking questions about the degree that an apparent
consensus for a pro-sustainability law was artificial versus genuine (e.g., “At
the assembly where everyone spoke in favor of the law, to what degree do you
think the unanimous discussion in favor of the law occurred because of the
influence of the President?”). They measured reactance by asking questions
about the degree that pressure to conform with the law made participants want
to reestablish their freedom to choose (e.g., “In the scenario, to what degree did
the President’s discussion at the meeting make you want to do the opposite of
what he said, just to show him that you could not be told what to do?”). In both
studies, political pressure to engage in specific environmental policies operated
through informational contamination and reactance to decrease support for
governmental action –an indirect path that was overwhelmingly significant
in both studies. Taken together, this set of studies suggests that part of the div-
ision in environmental sustainability attitudes in modern America isn’t just
about pre-existing ideological differences; rather it is that many Americans per-
ceive that there has been public pressure for agreement which has influenced
science –and thus they both react against that emotionally and informationally
discount that information.
The agreement paradox 123
Studies 1a–d
Participants
One thousand and ninety- eight Mechanical Turk (MTurk) participants
completed questionnaires for two larger parent projects. The first project
(Studies 1a and 1b) pertained to authoritarianism, threat, and voting.The second
project (Studies 1c and 1d) pertained to authoritarianism and perceptions of
modern American groups. Across all studies, participants completed either a
right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) or left- wing authoritarianism (LWA)
scale, questionnaires related to their voting intent in the 2020 election, and
questionnaires related to their support of potentially divisive political behaviors.
All studies showed roughly similar patterns, and thus here for brevity we present
pooled analyses of all studies combined.
124 Lucian Gideon Conway et al.
Measures
RWA/LWA
Participants in all studies were randomly assigned to complete either a 20-item
version of Altemeyer’s RWA scale (Altemeyer, 1996) or a parallel 20-item LWA
scale (Conway et al., 2018).
Control measures
In order to separate the effects of authoritarianism from those of political
ideology (Conway, 2020b), we controlled directly for self-reported political
ideology (via a standard two-item self-identification ideology scale anchored
by liberal/Democrat and conservative/Republican). We further controlled for
biological sex assigned at birth and age.
I often feel like Donald Trump wants people to fit a particular narrow tem-
plate, and that makes me want to be exactly the opposite of what he wants
in order to show that he can’t tell us what to do.
126 Lucian Gideon Conway et al.
These items formed a coherent scale and were averaged into a single reactance
score (alpha = 0.88).
Participants additionally completed a three- item scale of informational
contamination about Trump that was adapted from prior research (Conway
et al,. 2017). For example: “I distrust a lot of what Donald Trump says because
I assume it is reflective of some political agenda.” These items formed a
coherent scale and were averaged into a single informational contamination score
(alpha = 0.91).
As expected, when controlling for ideology, age, and sex, LWA was posi-
tively related to both informational contamination (partial r = 0.25, p = 0.001)
and reactance (partial r = 0.43, p < 0.001). Parallel analyses revealed that RWA
was negatively related to informational contamination as expected (partial
r = –.024, p = 0.002), but was not significantly related to reactance (partial
r = –0.07, p = 0.373). To analyze the degree that reactance and informa-
tional contamination accounted for the LWA/ RWA → divisive outcomes
relationships, we performed a series of mediational analyses according to
recommended current practices (Hayes, 2018). Specifically, we used the
PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2018; model 4) to compute normal inferential tests
of indirect effects, bootstrapped indirect effect sizes (using 5000 samples), and
bootstrapped indirect effect confidence intervals (using 5000 samples). Each
analysis tested the degree that there was an indirect effect in an LWA/RWA →
potential mediator → divisive outcome model, such that LWA/RWA affects
divisiveness through the mediator. In each case, every test also controlled for age,
biological sex assigned at birth, and self-reported political ideology.
Results are presented in Tables 7.2 (LWA) and 7.3 (RWA). As seen there,
not only do both key agreement paradox variables (informational contamin-
ation and reactance) consistently mediate the LWA → voting intent relation-
ship, they also significantly mediate the effect of LWA on the politically divisive
attempt to remove Trump from office prior to the 2020 election. For RWA, the
expected pattern emerged for informational contamination, such that high-
RWA persons showed lower informational contamination, which accounted
for their voting intent and opposition to Trump’s removal. However, no effect
of note emerged for reactance.2
These results suggest that, ironically, the kinds of people most likely to
care about consensual agreement –authoritarians –are also especially likely
to diverge on divisive behaviors. Although counterintuitive on the surface,
this makes sense from an agreement paradox lens. Persons who care about
agreement are also more sensitive to challenges to that agreement –and thus
are more prone to reactance and informational contamination. Authoritarians
are especially likely to batten down the psychological hatches when it seems
that consensus might be imposed from the outside. As a result, authoritarians’
desire for consensus might produce local, short-term consensus –but it might
lead them to actually divide society in the long term.
The agreement paradox 127
Concluding thoughts
Many modern societies are divided. In the present chapter, we have reviewed
evidence that part of this divide might be explained by the double-edged sword
of agreement pressures –pressures that build a superficial consensus in the short
term, but that often backfire in the long term.
We recognize that many of the connections in this chapter are loosely drawn.
Indeed, we do not claim to have demonstrated the agreement paradox fully in
a societal context –such a demonstration would require longitudinal studies
carried out over multiple years. Instead, we have outlined a perspective, drawn
predictions based on that perspective, and provided evidence for some of those
predictions.This evidence largely concerns the mechanisms that would produce
backfiring of pressures for agreement –informational contamination and react-
ance. We have shown that, when people feel agreement pressure, they often
respond in ways consistent with potential backfiring. We have also shown that
the kind of people most likely to be sensitive to such pressures, authoritarians,
are most likely to diverge on ultimately divisive behaviors.Thus, we believe that
this work, while still comparatively new, provides a path forward for researchers
to explore new ways to understand political divisions.
Notes
1 We focus primarily on the United States to illustrate divided societies because the
context of the data we discuss largely occurs in the United States, and we do not
wish to imply more cross-cultural relevance than the data suggest. While we would
guess the agreement paradox framework presented here would have relevance for
most societies, that inference goes far beyond the known data. As such, the work
presented here should be construed as a useful case study in one cultural context –
to test the usefulness of the agreement paradox lens. This context is, of course, no
more or less important than any other context.
2 We also asked participants one parallel reactance question for Democratic leader-
ship and one parallel informational contamination question for Democratic leader-
ship. Consistent with Conway and McFarland’s (2019) argument that elections are
more influenced by perceptions of the incumbent, these perceptions of Democratic
leadership did not significantly mediate any of the RWA → outcome relationships
reported in Table 7.3 (all indirect effect ps > 0.10).
References
Ahmadian, S., Azarshahi, S., & Paulhus, D. L. (2017). Explaining Donald Trump via com-
munication style: Grandiosity, informality, and dynamism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 107, 49–53.
Allen, J. T. (2010). How a different America responded to the Great Depression. Pew
Research Center.Retrieved from www.pewresearch.org/2010/12/14/how-a-different-
america-responded-to-the-great-depression/
The agreement paradox 131
Conway, L. G., III, & Repke, M. A. (2019). The psychological contamination of pro-
environmental consensus: Political pressure for environmental belief agreement
undermines its long-term power. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62, 12–21.
Conway, L. G., Repke, M. A., & Houck, S. C. (2017). Donald Trump as a cultural revolt
against perceived communication restriction: Priming political correctness norms
causes more Trump support. Journal of Social and Political Psychology,5(1), 244–259.
Conway, L. G., III, Salcido, A., Gornick, L. J., Bongard, K. A., Moran, M., & Burfiend,
C. (2009). When self-censorship norms backfire: The manufacturing of positive
communication and its ironic consequences for the perceptions of groups. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 31, 335–347.
Conway, L. G., III, & Schaller, M. (2005). When authority’s commands backfire:
Attributions about consensus and effects on deviant decision making. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 311–326.
Conway, L. G., III, & Schaller, M. (2007). How communication shapes culture. In
K. Fiedler (Ed.), Frontiers of social psychology: Social communication (pp. 107– 127).
New York: Psychology Press.
Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P.T., Anderegg,W. R. L.,Verheggen, B., Maibach, E.W., …
Green, S. A. (2016). Consensus on consensus: A synthesis of consensus estimates on
human-caused global warming. Environmental Research Letters, 11(4). doi: 10.1088/
1748–9326/11/4/048002
Dillard, J. P., & Shen, L. (2005). On the nature of reactance and its role in persuasive
health communication. Communication Monographs,72(2), 144–168.
Dong,Y., & Xu, J. (2016). Consensus building in group decision making. Singapore: Springer.
Duckitt, J., Bizumic, B., Krauss, S.W., & Heled, E. (2010). A tripartite approach to right-
wing authoritarianism: The authoritarianism– conservatism–traditionalism model.
Political Psychology, 31, 685–715.
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Chaiken, S. (1978). Causal inferences about communicators
and their effect on opinion change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(4),
424–435.
Edwards, S. M., Li, H., & Lee, J. H. (2002). Forced exposure and psychological reactance:
Antecedents and consequences of the perceived intrusiveness of pop-up ads. Journal of
Advertising, 31(3), 83–95. americans-fight-election_us_57cb226ae4b0e60d31df66a2
Fein, S. (1996). Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence
bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1164–1184.
Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political
Psychology, 24, 41–74.
Fitz, N. (2015). Economic inequality: It’s far worse than you think. Scientific American.
Retrieved from www.scientificamerican.com/article/economic-inequality-it-s-far-
worse-than-you-think/
Grandpre, J., Alvaro, E. M., Burgoon, M., Miller, C. H., & Hall, J. R. (2003). Adolescent
reactance and anti-smoking campaigns: A theoretical approach. Health Communication,
15(3), 349–366.
Guber, D. (2012). A cooling climate for change? Party polarization and the politics of
global warming. American Behavioral Scientist, 57(1), 93–115.
Guo, J. (2015). The real reasons Donald Trump’s so popular —for people totally
confused by it. The Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/
news/wonk/wp/2015/12/12/the-four-basic-reasons-that-explain-why-donald-
trump-actually-is-so-popular/
The agreement paradox 133
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hunt, A., & Wheeler, B. (2017). Brexit: All you need to know about the UK leaving the
EU. BBC News. Retrieved from www.bbc.com/news/uk-politics-32810887
Hunter, B. (2017). Peterson grant bid torpedoed. Retrieved from www.torontosun.
com/2017/04/03/peterson-grant-bid-torpedoed
Jackson, J. W. (1993). Realistic group conflict theory: A review and evaluation of the
theoretical and empirical literature. Psychological Record, 43(3), 395–415.
Johnson, C. D., Gormly, J., & Gormly, A. (1973). Disagreements and self-esteem: Support
for the competence- reinforcement model of attraction. Journal of Research in
Personality, 7(2), 165–172.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28,
107–128.
Korman, C. (2017). Why one Division III football player gave up the game to take a
knee. USA Today. Retrieved from http://ftw.usatoday.com/2017/10/gyree-durante-
albright-college-division-iii-player-kneel-kicked-off-team-national-anthem-protest
Laurin, K., Kay, A. C., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2012). Reactance versus rationalization:
Divergent responses to policies that constrain freedom. Psychological Science, 23(2),
205–209.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an
interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 68, 518–530.
Levine, J. P. (1994). The role of jury nullification instructions in the quest for justice.
Legal Studies Forum, 18, 473.
Ludeke, S. G., Klitgaard, C. N., & Vitriol, J. (2018). Comprehensively-measured authori-
tarianism does predict vote choice: The importance of authoritarianism’s facets,
ideological sorting, and the particular candidate. Personality and Individual Differences,
123, 209–216.
Malloy, T., & Schwartz, D. (2020, March 9). Biden crushes Sanders in democratic race,
Quinnipiac University national poll finds; More disapprove of Trump’s response to
coronavirus. Quinnipiac University Poll. https://poll.qu.edu/images/polling/us/
us03092020_untz23.pdf
Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An
empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 72–90.
McConnell, C., Margalit, Y., Malhotra, N., & Levendusky, M. (2018). The economic
consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. American Journal of Political Science,
62(1), 5–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12330
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). The politicization of climate change and
polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociological
Quarterly, 52(2), 155–194.
Meadors, M. (2016). Is political correctness really “killing America”? Huffington Post.
Retrieved from www.huffingtonpost.com/marvin-meadors/is-political-correctness-
_b_8026152.html
Newtson, D., & Czerlinsky, T. (1974). Adjustment of attitude communications for
contrasts by extreme audiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 829–837.
Noble, J. (2015). Trump comments on McCain war record spark outrage. USA Today.
Retrieved from www.usatoday.com/story/news/politics/elections/2016/2015/07/
18/trump-mccain-war-hero-captured/30347573/
134 Lucian Gideon Conway et al.
Paikin, S. Genders, rights and freedom of speech. TVO. The Agenda, 26 Oct. 2016.
Web. 28 Nov. 2016. “http://tvo.org/video/programs/the-agenda-with-steve-paikin/
genders-r ights-and-freedom-of-speech”
Passini, S. (2017). Different ways of being authoritarian: The distinct effects of authori-
tarian dimensions on values and prejudice. Political Psychology, 38, 73–86.
Pelham, B. W., & Blanton, H. (2002). Conducting research in psychology: Measuring the
weight of smoke. Pacific Grove, CA: International Thompson Publishing.
Plous, S. (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision- making. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Pryor, J. B., Rholes, W. S., Ruble, D. N., & Kriss, M. (1984). A developmental ana-
lysis of salience and discounting in social attribution. Representative Research in Social
Psychology, 14, 30–40.
Quick, B. L., & Stephenson, M.T. (2008). Examining the role of trait reactance and sen-
sation seeking on perceived threat, state reactance, and reactance restoration. Human
Communication Research, 34(3), 448–476.
Rodriguez, C. G., Moskowitz, J. P., Salem, R. M., & Ditto, P. H. (2017). Partisan selective
exposure: The role of party, ideology and ideological extremity over time. Translational
Issues in Psychological Science, 3(3), 254
Saad, L., & Jones, J. M. (2016). U.S. concern about global warming at eight-year high.
Gallup. Retrieved from www.gallup.com/poll/190010/concern-global-warming-
eight-year-high.aspx (accessed 2/2/2017).
Schaeffer, K. (2020, March 4). Far more Americans see 'very strong' partisan conflicts now
than in the last two presidential election years. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.
org/fact-tank/2020/03/04/far-more-americans-see-very-strong-partisan-conflicts-
now-than-in-the-last-two-presidential-election-years/
Silvia, P. J. (2006). Reactance and the dynamics of disagreement: Multiple paths from
threatened freedom to resistance to persuasion. European Journal of Social Psychology,
36(5), 673–685.
Stroud, N. J. (2011). Niche news: The politics of news choice. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Suedfeld, P., & Rank, A. D. (1976). Revolutionary leaders: Long- term success as
a function of changes in conceptual complexity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 34(2), 169.
Tetlock, P. E. (1994). Political psychology or politicized psychology: Is the road to scien-
tific hell paved with good moral intentions? Political Psychology, 15, 509–529.
Tidwell, N. D., Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Perceived, not actual, similarity
predicts initial attraction in a live romantic context: Evidence from the speed-dating
paradigm. Personal Relationships, 20(2), 199–215.
van Petegem, S., Soenens, B.,Vansteenkiste, M., & Beyers,W. (2015). Rebels with a cause?
Adolescent defiance from the perspective of reactance theory and self-determination
theory. Child Development, 86(3), 903–918.
van Prooijen, J. W., & Krouwel, A. P. (2019). Psychological features of extreme political
ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(2), 159–163.
Varava, K. A., & Quick, B. L. (2015). Adolescents and movie ratings: Is psychological
reactance a theoretical explanation for the forbidden fruit effect? Journal of Broadcasting
& Electronic Media, 59(1), 149–168.
Verbeek, A. G. (2011). The League and the law: Arthur L. Gilliom and the problem of
due process in Prohibition-era Indiana. Indiana Magazine of History, 107(4), 289–326.
Chapter 8
generating shared norms of outrage (Thomas, Smith, & McGarty, 2018; see also
Klandermans, 1984) and the unique role of moralization in predicting protest
and violence (Skitka et al., 2014;Van Zaal et al., 2011), the effect of the conver-
gence between an individual’s own moral views and the moral views of others
has received scant research attention, even though its effects on protest violence
may be pronounced.
Understanding the impact of moral convergence on protest violence is
important given the proliferation of online social networks in the 21st cen-
tury. Online social networks such as Twitter and Facebook have allowed people
to be exposed to the opinions of millions of others and violent protests are
often preceded by back-and-forth discussions on social media platforms about
moralized societal topics, such as unfairness, injustice, and racism (Manjoo,
2017). Social media platforms, in other words, are increasingly being used by
people to express their moral disapproval with social and political developments,
such as government corruption, the killing of unarmed citizens by police, or
the removal of culturally meaningful symbols and statues (Steinert-Trelkeld,
2017). Due to the scale of social networks, moralized messages that refer to
injustice and unfairness can spread to millions of others and reflect the moral
views of a given population. Social media discussions thus encode signals of
moralization such that people expose others to, and are exposed to others’,
moral views on a daily basis.
These social media dynamics shape moral convergence. Due to the pro-
liferation of social media platforms and the increasing diversity in Western
nations (Tavernise, 2018), people in the West are increasingly confronted with
the diverse moral views of others (Keating & Karklis, 2016). In response, some
people are trying to cluster themselves in digital “echo chambers” where they
are confronted primarily with those who agree with their moral views (e.g.,
ideological clustering on social media platforms such as Twitter, Facebook, or
Gab; Barberá, Jost, Nagler, Tucker, & Bonneau, 2015; Dehghani et al., 2016).
Some people are clustering themselves in geographical areas where they are
primarily confronted with those who agree with them (Motyl, 2016; Motyl,
Iyer, Oishi, Trawalter, & Nosek, 2014). This tendency to create social networks
with high levels of viewpoint homogeneity, especially in response to being
exposed to viewpoint diversity, suggests that (a) people at times are exposed
to viewpoint diversity in their social networks and (b) people try to create
conditions under which moral convergence tends to be relatively high. Thus,
understanding how the convergence and nonconvergence of moral views
shape protest violence has the potential to illuminate how social networks may
impact protest violence.
Below, I first discuss the role of individual moralization on violence; then
how individual moralization interacts with others’ moralization to create moral
(non)convergence and how this impacts protest violence. I, lastly, discuss future
research directions and practical implications.
138 Marlon Mooijman
In other words, we found evidence for the idea that expressions of moralization
on social media can predict when future protests will take on the dynamics that
lead to higher rates of arrest and violence. These findings are consistent with,
and go beyond, previous work showing how people’s self-reported moral con-
viction regarding a political issue (e.g., that abortion is immoral) correlates posi-
tively with people’s self-reported endorsement of violent tactics (e.g., bombing
an abortion clinic; Skitka & Morgan, 2014; Zaal et al., 2011). These findings
thus strongly suggest that rates of moralization on online social networks are
associated with street-level violence.
Please note that an increase in moral language on a social network could
simply reflect an increase in moralization; yet, given that people use social
media to view the opinions of peers, it may also be the case that an increase in
moral language reflects moral convergence. Accordingly, most of the time we
are unable to tease individual moralization apart from moral convergence and
thus draw conclusions about the effects of moralization that are in fact effects
of moral convergence. Below I will review an emerging body of work that
suggests that conditions of moral convergence rather than individual rates of
moralization drive support for protest violence.
in the protests, that police and government buildings were attacked in mul-
tiple cities, and that dozens of protesters died in the violence (Eltagouri, 2018;
Iranian Students’ News Agency, 2018; Rahimpour, 2018). These nationwide
protests provided a unique opportunity to go beyond reliance on convenient
Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) samples
(Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010) and collect data from people engaged
in an ongoing protest. Protests initially erupted on December 28th, 2017 when
relatively small groups of people started protesting across Iran, particularly in
Mashhad—the second most populous city in Iran. The last large-scale protest
was reported on January 14th, 2018 in Tehran—Iran’s capital and most popu-
lous city—with several thousand protesters participating. We started translating
the measures into Farsi on December 30th, 2017 and started collecting data
from participants on January 6th, 2018.
We found that, the more Iranian participants (who participated in the
protests) moralized the protest (e.g., indicated that the protests were a reflec-
tion of their moral conviction) and perceived that other Iranians also moralized
the protest, the more they endorsed using violence during these protests. In
fact, individual rates of moralization positively predicted the endorsement of
violence only when participants perceived that others also moralized the pro-
test. Individual rates of moralization negatively predicted the endorsement of
violence when participants perceived that others did not moralize the pro-
test. Put differently, perceiving that others moralized the protest only positively
predicted the endorsement of violence when participants themselves moralized
the protest; perceiving that others did not moralize the protest did not predict
the endorsement of violence when participants themselves moralized the pro-
test. This suggests that neither individual rates of moralization nor social norms
solely explained the endorsement of violence. Instead, the interaction between
personal moralization and the perceived moralization that others engaged in
predicted violence: moral convergence in particular predicted the endorsement
of violence.
These findings also suggest that individual rates of moralization can nega-
tively predict violence endorsement when participants perceive that others
do not moralize the protest.These findings are consistent with the notion that
people endorse violence less when moral convergence is low because they
have become less certain about the moral status of the protest goal, unable
to go along with what others also believe (because there is a misfit between
personal moral attitudes and others’ moral attitudes), and lack the feeling
that there is the power of a shared vision to push for meaningful change.
Endorsing violence under conditions of moral nonconvergence may seem
like a fruitless or even risky strategy. Advocating violence when others do not
may lead people to be punished or socially excluded, as happened with the
US far-r ight group “Proud Boys.”This group’s endorsement of using violence
against ideological opponents such as Antifa put them on the radar of the
Southern Poverty Law Center and Justice Department, which led its leader
Converging moral views in social networks 141
The Unite the Right rally (also known as the Charlottesville rally) was a
far-r ight rally in Charlottesville, Virginia, USA, from 11–12 August 2017.
The rally occurred amidst the backdrop of controversy generated by the
removal of several Confederate monuments.
Participants were informed that we were interested in their opinion about the
counter-protesters who protested against the far-right protesters. Participants
in the moral (non)convergence condition indicated to what extent they
considered it a reflection of their morals to protest these far-right protesters
and were then informed that “the majority of (versus few) people in the United
142 Marlon Mooijman
States share your particular moral values. Other people in the United States
think about this protest in a similar (versus different) manner compared to you.”
Participants then indicated to what degree they endorsed using violence against
others during these protests.
We found that people endorsed violence against far-r ight counter-protesters
in particular when they moralized gun control and perceived that their ingroup
also moralized this cause. This could be explained by their increased moral
attitude certainty, sense of identity fusion with other ingroup members, and
perceived power of a shared moral vision to push for political change on gun
control. These effects were stronger the more participants identified with the
relevant reference group (e.g., Americans), suggesting that moral convergence is
in part an interpersonal phenomenon. Although the abovementioned work on
moral convergence is relatively new, the emerging body of work thus suggests
that the convergence of one’s own moral views with the moral views of ingroup
members can increase violence—these findings suggest that when we measure
moral attitudes, language, and rhetoric in online social networks, we are often
capturing both the individual rates of moralization as well as the convergence
between individual moral views and the moral views of others.
Of course, people are not only confronted with the views of ingroup
members and at times find themselves exposed to the moral views of outgroup
members (on TV, face to face, in posts on social media). Although little research
to date has examined this question, it seems plausible that converging on moral
views with an outgroup member (e.g., a Democrat finding themselves agreeing
with a Republican) is less likely to foster violence and moral nonconvergence
is likely to foster violence because it emphasizes similarities versus differences.
In other words, perceived group membership is likely to be a boundary condi-
tion to the effect of moral convergence on political tolerance. Given the tenets
of social identity theory, moral convergence could also impact perceived group
membership categories (e.g., Democrats and Republicans could categorize
themselves in the superordinate category “Americans” as a consequence of
sharing moral values; Ellemers, 2012) but the stability of some group member-
ship categories (e.g., gender, race, ethnicity, nationality) suggests that ingroup
moral convergence increases whereas outgroup moral convergence decreases
the deterioration of nonviolent protest norms.
This line of reasoning might seem to present a paradox. Aren’t the most
radical people most likely to use violence to achieve their political goals? And
don’t they, by definition, share the fewest of their moral values with the rest of
society (Webber, Kruglanski, Molinario, & Jasko, 2020)? The key here, in my
view, is that radical fringe groups have high levels of viewpoint homogeneity
within their subgroup and actively police and censor viewpoints that deviate
from the ingroup consensus (e.g., religious cults, extreme political movements).
Without high levels of viewpoint homogeneity, small fringe groups would
quickly be subsumed in larger groups and cease to exist. Some fringe groups
Converging moral views in social networks 143
Implications
These findings provide important implications for the impact of social networks
on protest norms. As morally relevant messages multiply in social networks,
perceived convergence may increase, as suggested by the findings reviewed
in this chapter. This may increase the degree to which people overcome
their objections to using violence aimed at perceived ideological opponents.
Importantly, impressions of moral convergence on online social networks can
be biased given the tendency for online social networks to function as digital
echo chambers (Barberá et al., 2015). Some estimates suggest that seven out
of ten Americans are connected to an online social network and that polit-
ical polarization has been increasing for decades (Doherty, 2014). In addition,
a host of research has demonstrated that people start to understand others’
views by anchoring on their own views and projecting these on to others
such that people tend to assume that others’ view are similar to theirs unless
they know otherwise (Camerer, Loewenstein, & Weber, 1989; Keysar, Lin, &
Barr, 2003; Nickerson, 1999;Van Boven, Dunning, & Loewenstein, 2000;Van
Veelen et al., 2016; see Mitchell, Banaji, & Macrae, 2005; Saxe, 2005). This
suggests that online social networks such as Twitter and Facebook have the
potential to be catalysts for violence and the undermining of nonviolent
protest norms.
These findings also come at a time when some polls suggest that a minority
of US college students consider it acceptable to use violence against ideo-
logically dissimilar others (Rampell, 2017), the deplatforming of controversial
speakers on college campuses seems have increased in the last years (Fire, 2020),
and about two out of three US Democrats indicate not wanting to date a
Trump supporter (Brown, 2020). Although these societal developments allude
to a more general political intolerance rather than violence, they fit with this
chapter in so far as groups that increasingly converge on a specific set of moral
views become less and less tolerant of ideologically dissimilar others.
Indeed, in the past few years, protesters in the United States have clashed vio-
lently with police and counter-protesters on numerous occasions, whether this
was due to protests by the far-r ight or the invitation of controversial speakers
144 Marlon Mooijman
on college campuses. The findings reported in this chapter shed some light on
these social developments while also providing suggestions for counteracting
the increasing acceptability of violence. Increased support for using violence at
protests occurred primarily when people perceived that they shared moralized
views with others. This implies that decreasing the moralization of attitudes
and diluting the perception that relevant others from one’s group agree with
one’s moral position may attenuate the rise of the acceptability of violence.
Although people may try to “sort” themselves into networks with high levels
of moral convergence, the findings discussed in the current chapter imply that
combating moral convergence may be effective at decreasing the acceptability
of using violence at protests. In particular, convergence is most relevant in rela-
tion to our direct social circle, as this is where we derive most of our sense of
identity and meaning, and attempts to increase moral diversity should thus be
targeted towards people’s ingroups.
However, although moral convergence is an important determinant of
violent protest, it is by no means a sufficient condition for violent protest.
Whether violence emerges at a protest might also depend on additional
factors, such as the base rates of violent inclinations amongst the protesting
population, the likelihood of escalatory tactics deployed by the police, and the
degree to which the protest is aimed at being peaceful or not. Nonetheless,
a key decision-making problem for government officials is to predict which
protests will turn violent and how many resources should be allocated to
prevent protests from dissolving into chaos and violence. Although more
research should be done to replicate and extend the findings reviewed in this
chapter, the reviewed findings suggest that the construct of moral (non)con-
vergence can be of help.
These findings also offer a warning about the potential effects of perceived
versus actual moral homogeneity. Perceived moral homogeneity is likely to be
higher than actual homogeneity in moral views, as social media tends to act as
an echo chamber: people often use their social networks to be connected to
similarly minded others and, as such, assume that others share their viewpoints.
Future studies could, then, measure people’s actual moral views, the perceived
moral views of others, and confront people with others’ actual views. Future
research could also manipulate whether these views are coming from the
ingroup or outgroup. Based on the evidence reviewed in this chapter, it may be
that exposing people to moral diversity within their own social groups makes
them more politically tolerant and less likely to endorse violence as a means
of furthering moralized goals. Even when protests are justified and their goals
are widely shared by members of society (e.g., the killing of George Floyd in
Minneapolis in 2020), the destruction, chaos, and violence that can come in
their wake often end up undermining public support for the protest’s goals
(Feinberg, Willer, & Kovacheff, 2020; Wasow, 2020). Given the crucial role that
protests play in a democratic society, understanding and preventing the emer-
gence of violence at protests seem useful for us all.
Converging moral views in social networks 145
References
Atari, M., Mostafazadeh Davani, A., Kogon, D., Kennedy, B., Saxena, N., Anderson,
I., & Dehghani, M. (2020) (under review). Extremists of a feather, hate together:
Morally homogeneous networks and use of hateful rhetoric.
Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from left
to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological
Science, 26, 1531–1542. doi: 10.1177/0956797615594620
Beale, A., & Kehrt, S. (2017). Behind Berkeley’s semester of hate. The New York Times.
Retrieved 2019-05-01, from www.nytimes.com/2017/08/04/education/edlife/
antifa-collective-university-california-berkeley.html
Becker, J. C. & Tausch, N. (2015) A dynamic model of engagement in normative and
non- normative collective action: Psychological antecedents, consequences, and
barriers. European Review of Social Psychology, 26, 43–92.
Brown, A. (2020). Most Democrats who are looking for a relationship would not con-
sider dating a Trump voter. PEW Research Center. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.
org/f act-tank/2020/04/24/most-democrats-who-are-looking-for-a-relationship-
would-not-consider-dating-a-trump-voter/
Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Weber, M. (1989). The curse of knowledge in eco-
nomic settings: An experimental analysis. Journal of Political Economy, 97, 1232–1254.
Casey, N. (2018). Deadly siege of Venezuela rebels led by former action hero transfixes
country. The New York Times. Retrieved 2019- 05-
01, from www.nytimes.com/
2018/01/15/world/americas/venezuela-rebels-maduro.html
Chotiner, I. (2020). How violent protests change politics. The New Yorker. Retrieved 2020-
06-01, from www.newyorker.com/news/q-and-a/how-violent-protests-change-
politics
Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative
conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human
behavior. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp.
201–234). New York: Academic Press.
Dehghani, M., Johnson, K., Hoover, J., Sagi, E., Garten, J., Parmar, N. J., … Graham,
J. (2016). Purity homophily in social networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 145, 366.
Doherty, C. (2014). Seven things to know about polarization in America. Pew Research
Center. www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/06/12/7-things-to-knowabout-
polarization-in-america
Ellemers, N. (2012). The group self. Science, 336, 848–852. doi: 10.1126/science.
1220987
Eltagouri, M. (2018). Tens of thousands of people have protested in Iran. The
Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-05-01, from www.washingtonpost.com/news/
worldviews/w p/2 018/0 1/0 3/t ens-o f-t housands-o f-p eople-p rotested-i n-i ran-
this-week-heres-why/?noredirect=on&utm_term=.7b3ecabe6f89
Erdbrink, T. (2018a). Deadly Iran protests prompt warning of harsher response. The
New York Times. Retrieved 2019- 05- 01, from www.nytimes.com/2018/01/01/
world/middleeast/iran-protests.html
Erdbrink,T. (2018b). Iran can’t keep dictating lifestyle, its president warns. The New York
Times. Retrieved 2019- 07-10, from www.nytimes.com/2018/01/08/world/
middleeast/iran-rouhani-protests.html
146 Marlon Mooijman
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Kovacheff, C. (2020). The activists’ dilemma: Extreme
protest actions reduce popular support for social movements. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 19, 1086–1111.
Foundation for Individual Rights in Education (FIRE, 2020). Retrieved from www.
thefire.org/research/disinvitation-database/
Hatewatch staff. (2019). Proud Boys threatens Portland mayor: “I’m coming for you”.
Southern Poverty Law Center. Retrieved 2019- 05-
01, from www.splcenter.org/
hatewatch/2019/01/25/proud-boy-threatens-portland-mayor-im-coming-you
Heim, J. (2017). Recounting a day of rage, hate, violence and death. The Washington
Post. Retrieved 2019-05-01, from www.washingtonpost.com/g raphics/2017/local/
charlottesville-timeline/?utm_term=.689a53d02f06
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x0999152x
Hofmann, W., Meindl, P., Mooijman, M., & Graham, J. (2018). Morality and self-
control: How they are intertwined and where they differ. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 27, 286–291.
Iranian Students’ News Agency. (2018). 20 killed in recent riots. Iranian Students’ News
Agency (ISNA). Retrieved 2018-02-17, from www.isna.ir/news/96101708562/
Keating, D., & Karklis, L. (2016).The increasingly diverse United States of America. The
Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-05-01, from www.washingtonpost.com/g raphics/
national/how-diverse-is-america/
Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition,
89, 25–41.
Klandermans, B. (1984). Mobilization and participation: Social- psychological
expansisons of resource mobilization theory. American Sociological Review, 2,
583–600.
Kunst, J. R., Boos, B., Kimel, S.Y., Obaidi, M., Shani, M., & Thomsen, L. (2018).
Engaging in extreme activism in support of others’ political struggles: The role of
politically motivated fusion with outgroups. PLoS ONE, 13, 1–30.
Manjoo, F. (2017). The alt-majority: How social networks empowered mass protests
against Trump. The New York Times (30 January 2017). Retrieved from www.
nytimes.com/2017/01/30/technology/donald-trump-socialnetworks-protests.
html?mcubz=0
Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & MacRae, C. N. (2005). The link between social cogni-
tion and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 17(8), 1306–1315
Mooijman, M., Hoover, J., Lin,Y., Ji, H., & Dehghani, M. (2018). Moralization in social
networks and the emergence of violence during protests. Nature Human Behaviour,
2, 389–396.
Mooijman, M., Meindl, P., Oyserman, D., Monterosso, J., Dehghani, M., Doris, J. M., &
Graham, J. (2018). Resisting temptation for the good of the group: Binding moral
values and the moralization of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
115, 585.
Mooijman, M., Meindl, P., & Graham, J., (2020). On self-control moralization. In A.
Mele (Ed.), Surrounding self-control (pp. 259–274).Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mooijman, M., Atari, M., & Dehghani, M. (under review). Converging moral views and
endorsing violence during the 2018 Iranian protests.
Converging moral views in social networks 147
Van Stekelenburg, J., & Klandermans, B. (2013).The social psychology of protest. Current
Sociology, 61, 886–905.
Van Veelen, R., Otten, S., Cadinu, M., & Hansen, N. (2016). An integrative model of
social identification: Self-stereotyping and self-anchoring as two cognitive pathways.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 3–26.
Van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Put your money
where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based
anger and group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 649–678.
Walker, I., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1984). Relative deprivation theory: An overview and con-
ceptual critique. British Journal of Social Psychology. 23, 301–310.
Wasow, O. (2020). How 1960s Black protests moved elites, public opinion, and voting.
American Political Science Review, 114, 638–659.
Webber, D., Kruglanski, A., Molinario, E., & Jasko, K. (2020). Ideologies that justify pol-
itical violence. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 107–111.
Zaal, M. P., Laar, C.V., Ståhl, T., Ellemers, N., & Derks, B. (2011). By any means neces-
sary: The effects of regulatory focus and moral conviction on hostile and benevolent
forms of collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 670–689.
Chapter 9
Alternative climate facts
Central to the ideological polarization of climate change is the divide in
factual beliefs—liberals and conservatives disagree about the scientific evi-
dence showing that greenhouse gas emissions from human activity are driving
global warming (Leiserowitz et al., 2019). This disagreement follows a recent
trend in the politicization of facts, leading to new terms like “fake news” and
“alternative facts.” Some have dubbed this the “post-truth” era (Lewandowsky
et al., 2017).
Stark divides in factual beliefs across ideological lines may be driven by
different sources of information (via selective exposure; e.g., Stroud, 2010) or by
150 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
motivated reasoning where information that does not align with prior beliefs
or tribal allegiances is likely to be rejected (e.g., Ditto et al., 2019). Whatever
their source, facts play a crucial role in the partisan debate over climate change.
For many politically contested issues, factual beliefs comprise only a part of
the discussion; for example, the central debate over abortion concerns the def-
inition and value of human life, largely philosophical questions that rely on
morals and values for an answer rather than facts alone.Yet for climate change
the debate is most centrally about facts; it is not the value of a clean and healthy
environment that is generally at issue, it is the science of what is necessary to
achieve that healthy environment that is contested. In such cases, polarization
seems odd and unnecessary: provide people with the scientific consensus and
beliefs will converge. But the era of alternative facts reigns, and partisans instead
engage in heated debates around scientific evidence bearing not just on the
causes of climate change but on its very existence.
Ideological polarization, however, does not stop at whether climate change
is occurring. Differences in attitudes towards climate change may result not
only from politicized facts about climate change, but also from different
values and reactions to its consequences or solutions. For example, given that
conservatism is related to endorsing the societal status quo, the system-and
status quo-threatening consequences of climate change may be perceived dif-
ferently across political groups (Feygina et al., 2010). Additionally, common
solutions for addressing climate change involve government intervention,
conflicting with a conservative small-government worldview (e.g., Campbell
& Kay, 2014).
Addressing, mitigating, and adapting to climate change will require col-
lective action from a majority of the public. Given strong ideological polar-
ization on climate change, research must focus on engendering bipartisan
recognition of climate change and support for climate policies. Although
not exclusively faulting conservatives—many liberals lack the motivation or
behavioral urgency needed to address climate change as well—intervention
research on this topic requires a careful consideration of the central role of
political ideology.
Accordingly, this chapter serves as a review of research on experimental
interventions designed to increase belief in climate change, support for cli-
mate change policy, or willingness to behave in ways to reduce climate change.
Each section details a type of intervention (grouped by their focus on a par-
ticular construct or utilization of a similar manipulation as a way of influen-
cing outcomes) and each discusses how well the various interventions succeed
in reducing political polarization or garnering increased climate support from
conservatives, Republicans, or climate skeptics. Although there are other types
of interventions than those currently discussed, this chapter focuses on the most
commonly researched ones. The chapter ends with a summary of the most
promising interventions and recommendations for future research.
Bridging the climate change divide 151
Negative emotions
Experimental interventions targeting negative emotions have examined both
overall negative affect and specific discrete emotions like fear or anger. Some
research shows that pessimistic, or negatively framed, messages can motivate
people to mitigate climate change (Hornsey & Fielding, 2016; Schwartz &
Loewenstein, 2017). Inducing fear specifically can garner higher perceptions
of climate change risk (Skurka et al., 2018) and increase concern about global
warming and willingness to reduce emissions (Nolan, 2010).
There is some evidence that focusing on negatively valanced emotions is
especially effective for conservatives. In an experiment focused on manipu-
lating perceptions of efficacy, Feldman and Hart (2016) found that an efficacy
message about reducing global warming increased fear for conservatives, and
fear in turn was positively related to climate change activism intentions (the
same mediation path, in contrast, was not significant for liberals or moderates).
In a similar study, fear was positively related to support for climate policies for
conservatives but not liberals (Feldman & Hart, 2018a). Hornsey, Fielding, et al.
(2016), however, did not find that a high-fear or guilt message was particularly
persuasive for climate skeptics.
152 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
Positive emotions
Several experimental manipulations have successfully increased support for cli-
mate policies via hope as a mediator (Feldman & Hart, 2016, 2018a; Nabi et al.,
2018). Additionally, providing potential solutions to global warming decreased
skepticism from before to after learning about the solutions (Feinberg &
Willer, 2011). In contrast to the majority of studies on hope, Hornsey, Fielding,
et al. (2016) found that, although an optimistic climate message increased
people’s hope, it did not lead to an increase in motivation to mitigate climate
change; in fact, the researchers observed that the optimistic message led to
lower perceptions of climate change risk. van Zomeren et al. (2019) reported
similar findings, where optimistic messages increased participants’ hope but not
intended collective action on climate change.
In contrast to the earlier findings—that fear may be particularly influential
for conservatives—Feinberg and Willer (2011) found that dire, fear-inducing
messages backfired among those high in system justification (and who tend
to be more conservative, e.g., Jost et al., 2017), and that optimistic messages
reduced global- warming skepticism for high system justifiers. In addition,
some research indicates that hope is related to both policy support and climate
activism for conservatives (Feldman & Hart, 2016, 2018a).
Psychological distance
Given the distant and abstract nature of climate change (e.g., Moser, 2010),
researchers have argued that effective interventions should make climate change
more concrete and local (Weber, 2016). Indeed, construal level theory (CLT)
suggests that distance can be perceived psychologically, rather than only phys-
ically; psychological distance “is a subjective experience that something is close
or far away from the self, here, and now” (Trope & Liberman, 2010, p. 440).
When things are perceived as closer, people tend to see them as more concrete
Bridging the climate change divide 153
rather than abstract (Trope & Liberman, 2010). In correlational studies, there is
a link between psychological distance of climate change and people’s attitudes
towards it, such that people have more concern about climate change the closer
they feel to its impacts (e.g., A. S. Singh et al., 2017). In other words, these
studies find that climate change concern is positively associated with believing
that the impacts of climate change are primarily felt now (as opposed to the
distant future), occurring nearby oneself, and experienced by people similar to
oneself. Conversely, other research suggests that people facing direct physical
risk (e.g., living by the coast) only perceive an increased risk of climate change
when they directly attribute nearby events (e.g., floods) to climate change
(Brügger et al., 2015; Whitmarsh, 2008). Nevertheless, numerous experimental
studies have been conducted to determine if and when psychological distance
can be utilized as an effective way to increase concern for and action towards
climate change.
Overall, psychological distance manipulations have not been effective in
improving attitudes towards climate change, including belief (Herring et al.,
2017), negative affect (Rickard et al., 2016), or risk perceptions (Altinay, 2017;
Chu & Yang, 2018; Rickard et al., 2016;Wiest et al., 2015). Furthermore, studies
have found a lack of evidence that proximal manipulations increase support for
climate mitigation policies or projects (e.g., Brügger et al., 2016; Schuldt et al.,
2018), behavioral intentions (Altinay, 2017; Chu & Yang, 2018), or perceived
harm of sea-level rise (Mildenberger et al., 2019). Interestingly, some studies
have actually shown that framing climate change as more distant or global
(relative to local) can increase concern about climate change and sea-level rise
(Joslyn & LeClerc, 2016), and increase people’s perceptions of the severity of
climate impacts (Spence & Pidgeon, 2010). Some studies manipulate both spa-
tial and temporal distance (i.e., emphasizing how the effects of climate change
are happening sooner rather than later) to investigate the interaction effects,
although there is no clear pattern of findings (Joslyn & LeClerc, 2016; Rickard
et al., 2016). The few studies that find distance to be an effective manipulation
highlight the nuance of the phenomenon—perhaps policy support is limited
to local policies (Wiest et al., 2015), manipulations only work by indirect (not
direct) effects (Jones et al., 2017), and interventions need to be more wide-
spread and tailored rather than short one- time messages (Romero- Canyas
et al., 2019).
For conservatives, there is some evidence that manipulating distance
influences attitudes. When climate change is framed as more distant (relative
to a control condition), conservatives have less support for climate policy (Chu
& Yang, 2018; Hart & Nisbet, 2012), lower risk perceptions (Chu & Yang,
2018), and lower negative affect (Chu & Yang, 2018). Similarly, conservatives
displayed more support for policy and higher negative affect when climate
change was framed as spatially close rather than far (with a complex relation
to the timing of the impacts; Rickard et al., 2016). Most importantly, local
or proximate frames of climate change led to less political polarization in
154 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
climate change attitudes (Chu & Yang, 2018; Rickard et al., 2016), and even
eliminated political differences in behavioral intentions (Wiest et al., 2015).
Illustrating the potential effectiveness of highlighting local climate impacts
for conservatives, Romero-Canyas et al. (2019) conducted an effective field
experiment that increased perceptions of and concerns for global warming—
in a conservative region. There is some evidence that decreasing psychological
distance could be an effective way to increase conservatives’ engagement with
climate change. However, more research is needed on the interactions between
emotion, ideology, and psychological distance (e.g., Chu & Yang, 2019; Lu &
Schuldt, 2015).
Scientific consensus
Scientists have come to a consensus about anthropogenic climate change, with
evidence that around 97% of published papers endorse its reality (Cook et al.,
2016). Initial correlational evidence demonstrated that understanding this
scientific agreement about climate change is related to both climate change
beliefs and support for climate policies (Ding et al., 2011; McCright et al.,
2013).This research is consistent with the Gateway Belief Model (GBM) which
posits that scientific consensus acts as a gateway belief for downstream attitudes
about climate change and ultimately influences policy support (van der Linden
et al., 2015).
Most studies find that a consensus message increases people’s beliefs about
the amount of scientific agreement, including both posttest-only studies (e.g.,
Bolsen & Druckman, 2018; Brewer & McKnight, 2017; Myers et al., 2015)
Bridging the climate change divide 155
and pre–post studies (e.g., Goldberg et al., 2019; van der Linden et al., 2019).
Additionally, there is some evidence that conservatives have larger increases in
consensus estimates in response to a consensus message than liberals do (e.g.,
Goldberg et al., 2019; van der Linden et al., 2019).
However, evidence for an effect of a consensus message on other climate
change attitudes—like belief or policy support—is mixed. Although the GBM
does not predict a main effect of consensus information on climate change
beliefs (as a two-stage model, it predicts an indirect effect; van der Linden et al.,
2015), many studies test this direct effect on a variety of climate change atti-
tude variables. Several studies show that a consensus message is effective for
increasing people’s belief in climate change, support for policy, and support for
action on climate change (Bolsen et al., 2014; Brewer & McKnight, 2017; Cook
& Lewandowsky, 2016; Deryugina & Shurchkov, 2016; van der Linden et al.,
2019; van der Linden, Leiserowitz, Rosenthal, et al., 2017). One study found
that a consensus manipulation reduced political polarization in belief in various
attitudes about global warming (Bolsen et al., 2014).
On the other hand, several studies provide evidence that receiving consensus
information does not directly impact climate beliefs (other than perceived sci-
entific agreement). Several posttest-only experiments do not find significant
effects of consensus information on belief in climate change, policy support, or
behavioral intentions (e.g., Deryugina & Shurchkov, 2016; Dixon et al., 2017).
A few studies even suggest a potential backfire effect of consensus information,
where conservatives report less belief in climate change, less trust in scientists,
or increased psychological reactance (Cook & Lewandowsky, 2016; Kahan
et al., 2011; Ma et al., 2019). Despite the handful of studies suggesting a poten-
tial backfire effect with conservatives, multiple studies do not provide evidence
of a backfire effect or interaction between the consensus manipulation and
political ideology (e.g., Brewer & McKnight, 2017; Dixon et al., 2017; Myers
et al., 2015).
Policy framing
In addition to measuring policy support as an outcome of interventions,
researchers have manipulated various aspects of climate policies and measured
responses. Generally, the interventions involve various emphasis frames—
messages that highlight different aspects of a policy or issue (Druckman, 2001).
Others, however, utilize source frames by attaching various political sources
to a policy or by telling participants that policies or messages are endorsed by
certain political groups (e.g., Cohen, 2003). Experimental research has applied
both emphasis and source frames to climate policies in an attempt to increase
support and reduce political polarization.
Emphasis frames
In a large survey experiment, Stokes and Warshaw (2017) revealed that people
were more supportive of a renewable energy policy when it was framed as redu-
cing air pollution, increasing jobs, and minimizing costs (compared to the same
policy with no additional information). Similarly, Aklin and Urpelainen (2013)
found that participants were more supportive of a clean energy policy when
learning about its benefits, but this increase was mitigated when participants
learned about the costs as well. Other studies find that the specific wording
of policies is important, particularly that people dislike taxes and are more
supportive of regulations framed as a “carbon offset” rather than a “carbon
tax” (e.g., Hardisty et al., 2010). Importantly, this “carbon offset” framing also
eliminated political polarization on support for regulation (Hardisty et al.,
2010), perhaps because Republicans are especially averse to policies framed
as taxes and directed towards consumers (Hardisty et al., 2019). Campbell and
Kay (2014) provide evidence that it is climate policy solutions, rather than the
problem of climate change itself, that may drive climate change attitudes. For
example, they found that Republicans were less skeptical about climate change
after learning about a free-market solution compared to a government regu-
lation solution. Given conservative aversion to large government and taxes,
conservatives may be especially influenced by the type of solution posed and
whether it involves a tax. Furthermore, disconnecting policy from climate
change—instead framing it in terms of air pollution or energy security—can
improve Republican support (Feldman & Hart, 2018b), which is in line with
polling data that finds evidence of conservative support for renewable energy
(Leiserowitz et al., 2017). Taken together, these findings show that emphasis
frames are potentially powerful for garnering conservative policy support and
point to the influence of specific wording when it comes to climate policy.
However, few of these studies measured actual behavior. Illustrating the diffi-
culty of changing actual policy support, Binder et al. (2015) provided evidence
of the effectiveness of shifting the wording of a carbon emissions policy in a
survey experiment, but failed to find the same effect in a field experiment
measuring actual voting behavior.
Bridging the climate change divide 157
Source frames
Several studies have manipulated the source of a policy, or the group endorsing
the policy, and measured support for it. Most studies demonstrate that, when
Democrats endorse a climate policy, Democrats are more likely to support it,
and when Republicans endorse a climate policy, Republicans are more likely
to endorse it (Fielding et al., 2020; Van Boven et al., 2018). Ehret et al. (2018)
found similar results, although the effects among Republicans were not as strong.
However, other studies provide contrasting results.Although they manipulate the
source of an environmental message (rather than a specific policy), Bolsen et al.
(2019) and Zhou (2016) failed to find consistent effects of source frames. Zhou
(2016) did not find any significant effects of Republican-endorsed messages,
and Bolsen et al. (2019) did not find consistent effects of source frames, with
little evidence that messages from Republicans were especially persuasive for
garnering Republican policy support. Moreover, these studies find that source
frames can backfire when attached to climate messages, at times leading to more
climate skepticism and less policy support for Republicans (Bolsen et al., 2019;
Zhou, 2016). Given that the studies with effective source frames all attached
them to a specific policy (e.g., cap-and-trade), perhaps source frames are only
successful when paired with specific climate policies.
Other interventions
Health
Correlational research suggests that people generally respond positively to
information about the health benefits of mitigating climate change, even
158 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
skeptical audiences (Maibach et al., 2010). However, experimental tests pro-
vide mixed evidence. Some studies show that people have more belief in cli-
mate change and support for climate policy after learning about health risks,
as well as endorse car pollution reduction policies when they are framed as
protecting public health rather than the environment (Levine & Kline, 2017;
Walker et al., 2018). More importantly, related research found that conservatives
in particular were more supportive of climate policy and reducing air pollution
when pollution was framed as affecting health rather than the climate (Petrovic
et al., 2014).
There are just as many studies, however, that find nearly the opposite effects
of framing climate change as a health issue. Framing climate change as a health
issue did not increase support for renewable or clean energy (Hanus et al.,
2018), belief in climate change (Hart & Feldman, 2018; McCright et al., 2016),
or support for reducing greenhouse gas emissions (McCright et al., 2016).
Furthermore, providing information about the health risks of climate change
compared to the environmental risks was unsuccessful at increasing support
for policy (Bernauer & McGrath, 2016; Hart & Feldman, 2018) or political
action on climate change (Bernauer & McGrath, 2016; Levine & Kline, 2017).
As opposed to the research discussed earlier, several studies do not find a mod-
erating role of ideology, suggesting that conservatives are similarly affected by
health information as liberals (e.g., Bernauer & McGrath, 2016; Levine & Kline,
2017). The intervention might be effective, but the current state of research is
too unclear to determine when and for whom it is effective.
Morality
Much of the literature on climate change and morality stems from Moral
Foundations Theory (MFT), which suggests there are five moral domains,
or foundations, in which people root their morality: harm, fairness, ingroup,
authority, and purity (Graham et al., 2011). MFT has implications for political
polarization in that conservatives generally weigh each of the five foundations
relatively equally in their morality, whereas liberals tend to emphasize harm and
fairness (Graham et al., 2009). Put another way, liberals emphasize the individu-
alizing foundations—morality relating to the welfare of the individual—and
conservatives value the binding foundations—morality relating to the welfare
of groups.
Perhaps because of this nuance in moral beliefs, general moral manipulations
have been ineffective for both liberals and conservatives (Albertson & Busby,
2015; Severson & Coleman, 2015). Targeted interventions, however, have more
potential. Perhaps the most promising research using morality manipulations
considers the effect of varying moral messages—focusing on either liberal moral
foundations (harm, care, fairness) or conservative ones (purity, sanctity, loyalty,
authority)—on liberals and conservatives separately. For example, Feinberg and
Willer (2013) provided participants with a neutral message, a message describing
Bridging the climate change divide 159
the harm of environmental degradation and need to care for the Earth (targeting
liberal morality), or a message emphasizing the need to keep the Earth pure and
sacred (targeting conservative morality). Liberals were generally unaffected by
moral messages, displaying high levels of belief in global warming across all
conditions. Conservatives, however, had more belief in global warming when
shown a pure and sacred message than the other two messages. Furthermore,
the pure and sacred message (framing the effects of climate change more in
terms of contamination than harm) reduced political polarization in global-
warming belief, and eliminated it for general environmental attitudes. Similar
manipulations—messages framed in terms of conservative morality—were also
effective in increasing conservatives’ belief in and concern for climate change
(Wolsko, 2017; Wolsko et al., 2016). Day et al. (2014) demonstrated a similar
effect—with liberals more supportive of issues framed in terms of liberal moral
foundations and vice versa for conservatives—for a variety of issues, including
concern for the environment. Although conservative moral arguments show
promise in increasing conservatives’ belief in climate change, more research is
needed on the topic due to the limited number of experimental studies.
climate change effects. These studies generally found that, while liberals often
demonstrated a ceiling effect and supported climate change policies no matter the
intervention, conservatives were particularly influenced by these interventions.
These studies were either targeted specifically towards conservatives (e.g.,
emphasizing the free market or employing a Republican source) or appealed to
personal outcomes (e.g., localizing climate change consequences).
Despite the promise of these interventions, however, it is important to note
that their effects were not consistent across studies. Even among studies that
were the most targeted towards conservatives—using conservative values and
conservative sources—interventions did not dependably reduce polarization.
For example, in a sample of Republicans, Zhou (2016) did not find any sig-
nificant effects in climate policy support of an economic or national security
message, even from a Republican source. For studies that attached Republican
sources to climate policies or messages, the results are similarly inconsistent
(e.g., Bolsen et al., 2019; Ehret et al., 2018). As such, the primary conclusion of
this review of interventions points to the stability of climate change attitudes in
the face of attempts to change them and the difficulty of garnering bipartisan
support for climate policies.
There is no one-size-fits-all way to increase engagement in climate change;
every type of intervention included mixed results, with some studies showing
the effectiveness of the intervention and others demonstrating its inability to
influence attitudes. Interventions that theoretically should work often don’t
(e.g., Zhou, 2016) and polarization that is always supposed to exist sometimes
doesn’t (e.g., Severson & Coleman, 2015). Insufficient power is a problem in
some studies, but not in others.The one constant seems to be the general resist-
ance of a substantial portion of people to believe in or act on the very real and
imminent threat of human-caused global climate change.
The hyper- polarized political environment in US politics creates many
problems at many levels, but none of these problems is more pressing than the
policy paralysis it has engendered regarding action to combat global climate
change. Future research must continue to examine interventions and investigate
the potential reasons for why they do not reduce polarization as consistently
as would be expected. Bipartisan support for climate policy is not impossible
(Leiserowitz et al., 2017), but the path to reduced polarization remains unclear
during an increasingly urgent moment for climate policy.
References
Aklin, M., & Urpelainen, J. (2013). Debating clean energy: Frames, counter frames, and
audiences. Global Environmental Change, 23(5), 1225–1232. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.gloenvcha.2013.03.007
Albertson, B., & Busby, J. W. (2015). Hearts or minds? Identifying persuasive messages
on climate change. Research & Politics, 2(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/
2053168015577712
162 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
Feldman, L., & Hart, P. S. (2018a). Is there any hope? How climate change news imagery
and text influence audience emotions and support for climate mitigation policies.
Risk Analysis, 38(3), 585–602. https://doi.org/10.1111/r isa.12868
Feldman, L., & Hart, P. S. (2018b). Climate change as a polarizing cue: Framing effects
on public support for low-carbon energy policies. Global Environmental Change, 51,
54–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.05.004
Feygina, I., Jost, J.T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global
warming, and the possibility of “system-sanctioned change.” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 326–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209351435
Fielding, K. S., Hornsey, M. J.,Thai, H. A., & Toh, L. L. (2020). Using ingroup messengers
and ingroup values to promote climate change policy. Climatic Change, 158, 181–199.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-019-02561-z
Goldberg, M. H., van der Linden, S., Ballew, M. T., Rosenthal, S. A., & Leiserowitz, A.
(2019).The role of anchoring in judgments about expert consensus. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 49(3), 192–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12576
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different
sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping
the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366–385. https://
doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199107000-00016
Gustafson, A., Rosenthal, S. A., Ballew, M. T., Goldberg, M. H., Bergquist, P., Kotcher,
J. E., Maibach, E. W., & Leiserowitz, A. (2019). The development of partisan polariza-
tion over the Green New Deal. Nature Climate Change, 9(12), 940–944. https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41558-019-0621-7
Hanus, N., Wong-Parodi, G., Hoyos, L., & Rauch, M. (2018). Framing clean energy
campaigns to promote civic engagement among parents. Environmental Research
Letters, 13, 034021. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748–9326/aaa557
Hardisty, D. J., Beall, A. T., Lubowski, R., Petsonk, A., & Romero-Canyas, R. (2019). A
carbon price by another name may seem sweeter: Consumers prefer upstream offsets
to downstream taxes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 66, 101342. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101342
Hardisty, D. J., Johnson, E. J., & Weber, E. U. (2010). A dirty word or a dirty world?
Attribute framing, political affiliation, and query theory. Psychological Science, 21(1),
86–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609355572
Hart, P. S., & Feldman, L. (2018). Would it be better to not talk about climate change?
The impact of climate change and air pollution frames on support for regulating
power plant emissions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 60, 1–8. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.08.013
Hart, P. S., & Nisbet, E. C. (2012). Boomerang effects in science communication.
Communication Research,39(6),701–723.https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650211416646
Herring, J., VanDyke, M. S., Cummins, R. G., & Melton, F. (2017). Communicating
local climate risks online through an interactive data visualization. Environmental
Communication, 11(1), 90–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2016.1176946
Hornsey, M. J., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). A cautionary note about messages of
hope: Focusing on progress in reducing carbon emissions weakens mitigation
motivation. Global Environmental Change, 39, 26–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
gloenvcha.2016.04.003
Bridging the climate change divide 165
Hornsey, M. J., Fielding, K. S., McStay, R., Reser, J. P., & Bradley, G. L. (2016). Are people
high in skepticism about anthropogenic climate change necessarily resistant to influ-
ence? Some cause for optimism. Environment and Behavior, 48(7), 905–928. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0013916515574085
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). Meta-analyses of the
determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change, 6,
622–626. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2943
Jones, C., Hine, D. W., & Marks, A. D. G. (2017). The future is now: Reducing psycho-
logical distance to increase public engagement with climate change. Risk Analysis,
37(2), 331–341. https://doi.org/10.1111/r isa.12601
Joslyn, S. L., & LeClerc, J. E. (2016). Climate projections and uncertainty com-
munication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 222–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/
tops.12177
Jost, J. T., Stern, C., Rule, N. O., & Sterling, J. (2017). The politics of fear: Is there an
ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Social Cognition, 35(4), 324–353.
https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.4.324
Kahan, D. (2017).The “Gateway Belief ” illusion: Reanalyzing the results of a scientific-
consensus messaging study. Journal of Science Communication, 16(05). https://doi.org/
10.22323/2.16050203
Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scien-
tific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/
13669877.2010.511246
Kennedy, B., & Hefferon, M. (2019, August 28). U.S. concern about climate
change is rising, but mainly among Democrats. Pew Research Center. www.
pewresearch.org/ f act- t ank/2 019/0 8/2 8/ u - s -c oncern- a bout- c limate- c hange-
is-r ising-but-mainly-among-democrats/
Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role
of affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change, 77, 45–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10584-006-9059-9
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Rosenthal, S., Kotcher, J., Bergquist, P., Gustafson, A.,
Ballew, M., & Goldberg, M. (2019). Politics and global warming, November 2019.
New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication. https://
climatecommunication.yale.edu/wp-content/uploads/2020/01/politics-global-
warming-november-2019b.pdf
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Cutler, M., & Rosenthal, S. (2017).
Trump voters and global warming. New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change
Communication. https://climatecommunication.yale.edu/publications/trump-
voters-global-warming/
Levine, A. S., & Kline, R. (2017). A new approach for evaluating climate change
communication. Climatic Change, 142(1–2), 301–309. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10584-017-1952-x
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K. H., & Cook, J. (2017). Beyond misinforma-
tion: Understanding and coping with the “post- truth” era. Journal of Applied
Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 353–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.jarmac.2017.07.008
Lu, H., & Schuldt, J. P. (2015). Exploring the role of incidental emotions in support for
climate change policy. Climatic Change, 131(4), 719–726. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10584-015-1443-x
166 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
Ma, Y., Dixon, G., & Hmielowski, J. D. (2019). Psychological reactance from reading
basic facts on climate change: The role of prior views and political identifi-
cation. Environmental Communication, 13(1), 71–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17524032.2018.1548369
Maibach, E. W., Nisbet, M., Baldwin, P., Akerlof, K., & Diao, G. (2010). Reframing cli-
mate change as a public health issue: an exploratory study of public reactions. BMC
Public Health, 10(1), 299. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-10-299
Markowitz, E. M., & Shariff, A. F. (2012). Climate change and moral judgement. Nature
Climate Change, 2, 243–247. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1378
McCright, A. M., Charters, M., Dentzman, K., & Dietz,T. (2016). Examining the effect-
iveness of climate change frames in the face of a climate change denial counter-frame.
Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 76–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12171
McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Xiao, C. (2013). Perceived scientific agreement and
support for government action on climate change in the USA. Climatic Change, 119,
511–518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-013-0704-9
Mildenberger, M., Lubell, M., & Hummel, M. (2019). Personalized risk messaging can
reduce climate concerns. Global Environmental Change, 55, 15–24. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.01.002
Moser, S. C. (2010). Communicating climate change: History, challenges, process and
future directions. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 1(1), 31–53. https://
doi.org/10.1002/wcc.11
Myers, T. A., Maibach, E., Peters, E., & Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Simple messages help set
the record straight about scientific agreement on human-caused climate change: The
results of two experiments. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0120985. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0120985
Myers, T. A., Nisbet, M. C., Maibach, E. W., & Leiserowitz, A. A. (2012). A public health
frame arouses hopeful emotions about climate change. Climatic Change, 113(3–4),
1105–1112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-012-0513-6
Nabi, R. L., Gustafson, A., & Jensen, R. (2018). Framing climate change: Exploring the
role of emotion in generating advocacy behavior. Science Communication, 40(4), 442–
468. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018776019
Nisbet, E. C., Cooper, K. E., & Garrett, R. K. (2015). The partisan brain: How dis-
sonant science messages lead conservatives and liberals to (dis)trust science. Annals
of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 658(1), 36–66. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0002716214555474
Nolan, J. M. (2010). “An Inconvenient Truth” increases knowledge, concern, and
willingness to reduce greenhouse gases. Environment and Behavior, 42(5), 643–658.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916509357696
Petrovic, N., Madrigano, J., & Zaval, L. (2014). Motivating mitigation: When health
matters more than climate change. Climatic Change, 126(1–2), 245–254. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10584-014-1192-2
Rickard, L. N.,Yang, Z. J., & Schuldt, J. P. (2016). Here and now, there and then: How
“departure dates” influence climate change engagement. Global Environmental Change,
38, 97–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.03.003
Romero-Canyas, R., Larson-Konar, D., Redlawsk, D. P., Borie-Holtz, D., Gaby, K.,
Langer, S., & Schneider, B. (2019). Bringing the heat home: Television spots about
local impacts reduce global warming denialism. Environmental Communication, 13(6),
740–760. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2018.1455725
Bridging the climate change divide 167
Schuldt, J. P., Rickard, L. N., & Yang, Z. J. (2018). Does reduced psychological distance
increase climate engagement? On the limits of localizing climate change. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 55, 147–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.001
Schwartz, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). The chill of the moment: Emotions and
proenvironmental behavior. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 36(2), 255–268.
https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.16.132
Severson, A. W., & Coleman, E. A. (2015). Moral frames and climate change policy
attitudes. Social Science Quarterly, 96(5), 1277–1290. https://doi.org/10.1111/
ssqu.12159
Singh, A. S., Zwickle, A., Bruskotter, J. T., & Wilson, R. (2017). The perceived psycho-
logical distance of climate change impacts and its influence on support for adap-
tation policy. Environmental Science & Policy, 73, 93–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.envsci.2017.04.011
Singh, S. P., & Swanson, M. (2017). How issue frames shape beliefs about the importance
of climate change policy across ideological and partisan groups. PLoS ONE, 12(7),
e0181401. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181401
Skurka, C., Niederdeppe, J., Romero-Canyas, R., & Acup, D. (2018). Pathways of influ-
ence in emotional appeals: Benefits and tradeoffs of using fear or humor to promote
climate change- related intentions and risk perceptions. Journal of Communication,
68(1), 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx008
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring
affective image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–
1032. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01801.x
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy
support and opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948. https://doi.org/10.1111/
risa.12140
Spence, A., & Pidgeon, N. (2010). Framing and communicating climate change: The
effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change,
20(4), 656–667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.07.002
Stokes, L. C., & Warshaw, C. (2017). Renewable energy policy design and framing influ-
ence public support in the United States. Nature Energy, 2(8), 17107. https://doi.org/
10.1038/nenergy.2017.107
Stroud, N. J. (2010). Polarization and partisan selective exposure. Journal of Communication,
60(3), 556–576. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010. 01497.x
Tannenbaum, M. B., Hepler, J., Zimmerman, R. S., Saul, L., Jacobs, S., Wilson, K., &
Albarracín, D. (2015). Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and
theories. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1178–1204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039729
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance.
Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963
Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Psychological barriers to bipartisan
public support for climate policy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(4), 492–507.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617748966
van der Linden, S. L., Leiserowitz, A. A., Feinberg, G. D., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). The
scientific consensus on climate change as a gateway belief: Experimental evidence.
PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0118489. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118489
van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2019). The gateway belief model: A
large-scale replication. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62, 49–58. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.01.009
168 Jacob B. Rode and Peter H. Ditto
van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., Rosenthal, S., & Maibach, E. (2017). Inoculating the
public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges, 1(2), 1600008.
https://doi.org/10.1002/gch2.201600008
van Zomeren, M., Pauls, I. L., & Cohen-Chen, S. (2019). Is hope good for motivating
collective action in the context of climate change? Differentiating hope’s emotion-
and problem-focused coping functions. Global Environmental Change, 58, 101915.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.04.003
Walker, B. J. A., Kurz, T., & Russel, D. (2018). Towards an understanding of when non-
climate frames can generate public support for climate change policy. Environment and
Behavior, 50(7), 781–806. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916517713299
Weber, E. U. (2016). What shapes perceptions of climate change? New research since
2010. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(1), 125–134. https://doi.org/
10.1002/wcc.377
Whitmarsh, L. (2008). Are flood victims more concerned about climate change
than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behav-
ioural response. Journal of Risk Research, 11(3), 351–374. https://doi.org/10.1080/
13669870701552235
Wiest, S. L., Raymond, L., & Clawson, R. A. (2015). Framing, partisan predispositions,
and public opinion on climate change. Global Environmental Change, 31, 187–198.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.12.006
Wolsko, C. (2017). Expanding the range of environmental values: Political orientation,
moral foundations, and the common ingroup. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 51,
284–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.04.005
Wolsko, C., Ariceaga, H., & Seiden, J. (2016). Red, white, and blue enough to be
green: Effects of moral framing on climate change attitudes and conservation
behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.jesp.2016.02.005
Zhou, J. (2016). Boomerangs versus javelins: How polarization constrains communi-
cation on climate change. Environmental Politics, 25(5), 788–811. https://doi.org/
10.1080/09644016.2016.1166602
Index
security 19 stress 113
selective exposure 64 sustainability 121–122
self-interest 65–68 symbolic threat 100
slavery 128
social capital 78 tax cuts 66
Social Comparison Theory 39 tribalism 54
social dominance orientation 41 Trump, Donald 98, 100, 112, 120–121;
social identity 105 support 53–68
social media 63, 83, 137 Twitter 84
social networks 83–85, 136–137
social relationships 78–85 violence 140
sophistication 28 Vlaams Belang 41
source frames 157
State of the Union 124 wealth paradox 103–106
status anxiety 103–106 WEIRD samples 140
status quo 150 White supremacists 141
stereotypes 119 World Values Survey 27
strategies 85 worldview conflict 56