Nihms 1678983
Nihms 1678983
Nihms 1678983
Author manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Author Manuscript
Abstract
Background: Agenesis of the corpus callosum (AgCC) involves congenital absence of all or part
of the corpus callosum. Because the disorder can only be firmly diagnosed via neuroradiology, it
has a short research history, and only recently has the cognitive syndrome become clear.
Purpose: Our purpose is to review the primary deficits in AgCC that constitute the core
syndrome.
Conclusions: The cores syndrome includes: (1) Reduced interhemispheric transfer of sensory-
motor information; (2) Reduced cognitive processing speed; (3) Deficits in complex reasoning and
novel problem-solving. These domains do not appear to reflect different neuroanatomical
Author Manuscript
Implications: These core deficits are expressed across various domains of cognitive, behavioral,
and social functioning. The impact of these deficits varies across development and may be
moderated by individual factors such as cooccurrence of other neurodevelopmental conditions,
general intellectual capacity, and environmental support.
Keywords
corpus callosum; agenesis of the corpus callosum
Introduction
Author Manuscript
This paper describes what we believe to be the core syndrome of agenesis of the corpus
callosum (AgCC) as displayed in adults who have few, if any, other neurological
abnormalities, and have grossly intact general intelligence. Although AgCC was previously
thought to be exceedingly rare, increased clinical use of neuroimaging has resulted in higher
detection rates in relatively normally functioning individuals. Studies conducted with this
larger pool of patients are providing greater understanding of the role of the corpus callosum
Corresponding Author: Warren S. Brown, Fuller Graduate School of Psychology, 180 N. Oakland Ave. Pasadena, CA 91101,
wsbrown@fuller.edu 626-584-5525—office.
Brown and Paul Page 2
in cognition and behavior. The accumulating data also provides families and clinicians with
Author Manuscript
more nuanced insights into the patterns of cognitive capacities that influence learning, daily
behaviors, and developmental progression of individuals with AgCC.
AgCC is often associated with a broader syndrome of brain malformation related to known
toxic-metabolic conditions or genetic causes, but in 55-70% of AgCC cases the cause is
Author Manuscript
unknown (Bedeschi et al., 2006; Schell-Apacik et al., 2008; Tang et al., 2009). When part of
a broader neurodevelopmental syndrome or accompanied by other congenital brain
malformations, the cognitive and behavioral impact of these other conditions would likely
obscure the moderate to mild deficiencies directly related to callosal absence. In order to
identify the specific contributions of the corpus callosum to higher cognitive abilities, this
review describes the AgCC syndrome as it appears in individuals without any (or only very
minor) other brain or body dysmorphology. These individuals typically have normal-range
IQs and appear neurologically “asymptomatic” (i.e., lack of symptoms apparent in
diagnostic procedures typically used in clinical neurology). In most of these individuals
AgCC is discovered through routine prenatal sonogram or neuroimaging motivated by
unrelated issues. In these cases, AgCC is likely to be the primary contributor to the cognitive
outcome, and thus we refer to these individuals as having “Primary AgCC”.
Author Manuscript
Although they have a common primary neurological finding, individuals with Primary
AgCC are somewhat heterogeneous with respect to other minor structural brain
abnormalities, some of which appear to be secondary to the callosal dysgenesis (e.g.
colpocephaly, Probst Bundles) and some of which may or may not be related to the callosum
(e.g. minor areas of heterotopia, interhemispheric cysts, interhemispheric lipoma). The
group also varies with regard to amount of residual callosal connection (complete vs.
partial), and most certainly varies at the molecular and synaptic level. To minimize the
negative influence of concomitant neurological abnormalities, this review focuses on
individuals with a full scale IQ above 80. Heterogeneity notwithstanding, we present what
we believe to be the core syndrome which results in the mild to moderate cognitive and
psychosocial deficits in Primary AgCC.
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 3
The aim of this paper is to briefly sketch the core syndrome of Primary AgCC (i.e., deficits
specifically related to callosal absence) which in turn contributes to more specific
neuropsychological and psychosocial deficits in AgCC. The following sections will offer
evidence from published literature (including much of our own research) demonstrating that
Primary AgCC is associated with a core syndrome involving:
interactions likely contribute to slower processing time and difficulty in complex problem-
solving, which are themselves inter-related. These core deficits contribute to many other
specific deficiencies we describe briefly (see figure 1).
and motor information in AgCC discovered that congenital absence of callosal connections
does not cause a full “disconnection syndrome” as is seen following commissurotomy
(Sperry, 1968; Bogen & Frederiks, 1985; Sperry, Gazzaniga, Bogen, Vinken, & Bruyn,
1969).
Bryden & Zurif, 1970; Buchanan, Waterhouse, & West Jr., 1980; Geffen, Nilsson, Quinn, &
Teng, 1985; Jeeves, 1979).
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 4
letters or dot-patterns above the level of chance. In contrast, participants with AgCC
Author Manuscript
The mediating effect of encoding complexity on interhemispheric transfer in AgCC has also
been demonstrated in studies of tactual-spatial information transfer using the Tactual
Performance Test (Dunn, Paul, Schieffer, & Brown, 2000; Sauerwein & Lassonde, 1994;
Sauerwein, Lassonde, Cardu, & Geoffroy, 1981), Finger Localization Test (Dunn et al.,
2000; Geffen et al., 1985; Sauerwein & Lassonde, 1994) and tactual recognition (Jeeves &
Silver, 1988). As with sensory transfer, individuals with AgCC exhibited intact performance
at the lowest levels of sensory-motor difficulty, with significant declines in performance
occurring as tasks requiring transfer of more complex (less easily encoded) information.
Author Manuscript
Slow processing speed was also implicated in a study of cognitive inhibition in adults with
AgCC. On the Color-Word subtest of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, we
found deficient cognitive inhibition and flexibility in adults with AgCC compared to age-
and IQ-matched controls, but regression analyses indicated that these differences in
cognitive control were primarily the consequence of slowed processing speed (Marco et al.,
2012). Thus, processing speed limitations may have wide-ranging implications for cognition
and behavior in AgCC. However, as found in studies of callosal damage in traumatic brain
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 5
injury, impairments in processing speed are exacerbated on tasks with greater information
Author Manuscript
Impairments in abstract reasoning (Brown & Paul, 2000; David, Wacharasindhu, &
Lishman, 1993; Gott & Saul, 1978), concept formation (Fischer, Ryan, & Dobyns, 1992;
Imamura, Yamadori, Shiga, Sahara, & Abiko, 1994), problem-solving (Brown, Anderson,
Symington, & Paul, 2012; Schieffer, Paul, & Brown, 2000), and generalization (Solursh,
Margulies, Ashem, & Stasiak, 1965) have all been observed in patients with AgCC. Such
deficiencies are particularly evident as task complexity increases (Schieffer, Paul, & Brown,
2000). For example, on the simplified (color) version of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices
Tests (Raven, 1960, 1965) performance of adults with AgCC was consistent with individual
Author Manuscript
Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) scores, but performance was impaired relative to FSIQ on the more
complex Standard Progressive Matrices (Schieffer, Paul, & Brown, 2000). The benefits of
practice in Primary AgCC are supported by patterns of academic achievement in a sample of
adults who performed within average range on basic mathematic calculations (a skill
practiced throughout school), but exhibited significant deficits on math reasoning (Wechsler
Individual Achievement Test-II; Hanna, 2018).
Paul, Erickson, Hartman, & Brown, 2016), adequately understanding non-literal and
complex language (Brown, Paul, Symington, & Dietrich, 2005; Brown, Symington, Van
Lancker-Sidtis, Dietrich, & Paul, 2005; Paul, Van Lancker-Sidtis, Schieffer, Dietrich, &
Brown, 2003; Rehmel, Brown, & Paul, 2016), exerting cognitive inhibition and flexibility
(Marco et al., 2012), formulating strategies (Brown et al., 2012) and effectively applying
imagination and creativity (Paul, Schieffer, & Brown, 2004; Young et al., under review). In
addition, these core cognitive deficits negatively impact social and emotional cognition,
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 6
resulting in difficulty reasoning abstractly about emotions in social context (Anderson, Paul,
Author Manuscript
& Brown, 2017; Paul et al., 2006), expressing emotions in words (Pazienza, Paul, & Brown,
2011), interpreting sarcasm and understanding subtle aspects of social interactions
(Symington, Paul, Symington, Ono, & Brown, 2010), recognizing emotion in faces
(Bridgman et al., 2014), imagining and inferring the mental, emotional, and social
functioning of others (Kang, Paul, Castelli, & Brown, 2009; Turk, Brown, Symington, &
Paul, 2010) and awareness of functional deficits (Kaplan, Brown, Adolphs, & Paul, 2012;
Mangum, 2018; Miller, Su, Paul, & Brown, 2018). Although they appear to be secondary
products of diminished interhemispheric interactions, slowed processing time, and deficient
complex problem solving, these associated cognitive and social deficits may result in
functionally significant impairments in adaptive skills needed in daily life (Miller, Su, Paul,
& Brown, 2018; Mangum, 2018) and reciprocal social communication (Paul, Corsello,
Kennedy, & Adolphs, 2014).
Author Manuscript
Moderating Factors
Expression of these core deficits will vary across the lifespan, as a consequence of
neuroanatomic variations and concomitant conditions, and in relation to individual traits and
context. We offer brief comments on each of these influences.
Schieffer, Paul, Schilmoeller, & Brown, 2000). Nonetheless, older children with AgCC may
fall behind their peers when tasks are sufficiently complex (Garrels et al., 2001) or novel
(Young et al., 2013) for their developmental level. Thus, tasks that can be mastered through
practice, such as reading and arithmetic, are more likely to be impaired in children with
AgCC than in adults. In contrast, tasks such as social interaction and complex problem
solving become increasingly complex in adolescence and remain complex and somewhat
novel throughout life, posing an ongoing challenge to individuals with AgCC (e.g., Kang et
al., 2009; Turk et al., 2010; Mangum, 2018; Miller, Su, Paul, & Brown, 2018).
It is reasonable to expect that the impact of AgCC would vary in relation to degree of
callosal absence, with partial AgCC resulting in less severe manifestations of these
deficiencies than complete AgCC. However, although most of the research cited in this
paper focused on complete AgCC, studies that included persons with partial AgCC found
Author Manuscript
their performances were distributed among the results of individuals with complete. While
the explanation for this is not yet clear, it is possible that outcomes in partial AgCC are
impacted by individual variations in how the remaining callosal interhemispheric
connections are organized (Wahl et al., 2009).
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 7
conditions (i.e., not Primary ACC). The nature and severity of these additional conditions
Author Manuscript
will influence expression of the core deficits from AgCC and at the extreme may render the
core deficits functionally irrelevant in daily life.
Conclusion
Research has accumulated over the last two decades allowing for the description of a pattern
of deficits characteristic of AgCC. We have argued for a core syndrome associated with
callosal absence in AgCC involving reduced interhemispheric transfer of sensory-motor
Author Manuscript
information, slowed cognitive processing speed, and deficits in complex reasoning and novel
problem-solving. However, because these cognitive deficiencies are typically mild to
moderate, they are often not easily recognized. It is our hope that better description of the
cognitive and psychosocial impact of AgCC will increase the likelihood of a diagnostic MRI
in these high-functioning cases, as well as provide more complete information and helpful
guidance to patients and their families regarding the likely consequences of this congenital
brain disorder.
Acknowledgements
The authors of this paper declare that they have no conflicts of interest with respect to the writing of this paper or
the information contained. The writing of this paper was also not supported by grant funding.
Author Manuscript
References
Anderson LB, Paul LK, & Brown WS (2017). Emotional intelligence in agenesis of the corpus
callosum. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(3), 267–279. doi:10.1093/arclin/acx001
[PubMed: 28431033]
Bedeschi MF, Bonaglia MC, Grasso R, Pellegri A, Garghentino RR, Battaglia MA, … Borgatti R
(2006). Agenesis of the corpus callosum: clinical and genetic study in 63 young patients. Pediatric
Neurology, 34(3), 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.08.008 [PubMed: 16504787]
Bogen J, & Frederiks J (1985). Split-brain syndromes. Handbook of Clinical Neurology (Vol. 45, pp.
99–106). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science Publishing Co.
Bridgman MW, Brown WS, Spezio ML, Leonard MK, Adolphs R, & Paul LK (2014). Facial emotion
recognition in agenesis of the corpus callosum. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6.
doi:10.1186/1866-1955-6-32
Brown WS (1991). The Bimanual Coordination Test: Version 1. The Travis Research Institute Papers.
Brown WS, Anderson LB, Symington MF, & Paul LK (2012). Decision-making in individuals with
Author Manuscript
agenesis of the corpus callosum: expectancy-valence in the Iowa Gambling Task. Archives of
Clinical Neuropsychology, 27(5), 532–544. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs052 [PubMed: 22721927]
Brown WS, Jeeves MA, Dietrich R, & Burnison DS (1999). Bilateral field advantage and evoked
potential interhemispheric transmission in commissurotomy and callosal agenesis.
Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 1165–1180. [PubMed: 10509838]
Brown WS, & Paul LK (2000). Cognitive and psychosocial deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum
with normal intelligence. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(2), 135–157.
Brown WS, Paul LK, Symington M, and Dietrich R (2005). Comprehension of humor in primary
agenesis of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia, 43, 906–916. [PubMed: 15716161]
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 8
Brown WS, Symington M, VanLancker D, Dietrich R and Paul LK (2005). Paralinguistic processing in
children with callosal agenesis: Emergence of neurolinguistic deficits. Brain and Language, 93,
Author Manuscript
Fischer M, Ryan SB, & Dobyns WB (1992). Mechanisms of interhemispheric transfer and patterns of
cognitive function in acallosal patients of normal intelligence. Archives of Neurology, 49(3), 271–
277. [PubMed: 1536630]
Garrels SR, Paul LK, Schieffer BM, Florendo EV, Fox MM, Turk AA, & Brown WS (2001). Abstract
problem solving in children with callosal agenesis [Abstract]. Presented at the Twenty-Ninth
Annual International Neuropsychological Society Conference, February 14–17, 2001 Chicago,
Illinois. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(2), 258.
Geffen G, Nilsson J, Quinn K, & Teng EL (1985). The effects of lesions of the corpus callosum on
finger localization. Neuropsychologia, 23, 497–514. [PubMed: 4033904]
Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Casey BJ, Kozuch P, King AC, Hamburger SD, & Rapoport JL (1994).
Quantitative morphology of the corpus callosum in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(5), 665–669.
Glass H, Shaw G, Ma C, & Sherr EH (2008). Agenesis of the corpus callosum in California
1983-2003: a population-based study. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 146A(19, 2495–
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 9
Guillem P, Fabre B, Cans C, Robert-Gnansia E, & Jouk PS (2003). Trends in elective terminations of
pregnancy between 1989 and 2000 in a French county (the Isère). Prenatal Diagnosis, 23(11), 877–
Author Manuscript
Jeeves MA, & Silver PH (1988). Interhemispheric transfer of spatial tactile information in callosal
agenesis and partial commissurotomy. Cortex, 24, 601–604. [PubMed: 3219875]
Jeeves MA, Silver PH, & Jacobson I (1988). Bimanual co-ordination in callosal agenesis and partial
commissurotomy. Neuropsychologia, 26, 833–850. [PubMed: 3194049]
Jeeves MA, Silver PH, & Milner AD (1988). Role of the corpus callosum in the development of a
bimanual skills. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 305–323.
Kang KH, Paul LK, Castelli F, & Brown WS (2009). Theory of mind in agenesis of the corpus
callosum compared to high functioning autism [Abstract]. Poster presented at the Thirty-Seventh
Annual Meeting International Neuropsychological Society: February 11–14, 2009 Atlanta,
Georgia, USA. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(S1), 168. doi:10.1017/
S1355617709090420
Kaplan JM, Brown WS, Adolphs R, & Paul LK (2012). Psychological profile in agenesis of the corpus
callosum [Abstract]. Poster presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting: October
13- 17, 2012 New Orleans, LA, USA. 198.19.
Author Manuscript
Kourtidou P, McCauley SR, Bigler ED, Traipe E, Wu TC, Chu ZD, Hunter JV, Li X, Levin HS, Wilde
EA (2013). Centrum Semiovale and Corpus Callosum Integrity in Relation to Information
Processing Speed in Patients with Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. Journal of Head Trauma
Rehabilitation, 28(6):433–41. DOI:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3182585d06
Koivisto M (2000). Interhemispheric interaction in semantic categorization of pictures. Cognitive
Brain Research, 9(1), 45–51. PII:S0926-6410(99)00042-7. [PubMed: 10666556]
Lassonde M, Sauerwein H, Chicoine AJ, & Geoffroy G (1991). Absence of disconnexion syndrome in
callosal agenesis and early callosotomy: brain reorganization or lack of structural specificity
during ontogeny? Neuropsychologia, 29(6), 481–495. [PubMed: 1944857]
Maertens M, & Pollmann S (2005). Interhemispheric resource sharing: Decreasing benefit with
increasing processing efficiency. Brain and Cognition, 58(2), 183–192. doi:10.1016/
j.bandc.2004.11.002. [PubMed: 15919550]
Mangum R (2018). Self-understanding of executive function in individuals with agenesis of the corpus
callosum. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Fuller Graduate School of Psychology, Pasadena,
California.
Author Manuscript
Marco EJ, Harrell KM, Brown WS, Hill SS, Jeremy RJ, Kramer JH, … Paul LK (2012). Processing
speed delays contribute to executive function deficits in individuals with agenesis of the corpus
callosum. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(3), 521–529. doi:10.1017/
s1355617712000045. [PubMed: 22390821]
Marion SD, Kilian SC, Naramor TL, & Brown WS (2003). Normal development of bimanual
coordination: Visuomotor and interhemispheric contributions. Developmental Neuropsychology,
23(3), 399–421. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2303_6 [PubMed: 12740193]
Mathias JL, Bigler ED, Jones NR, Bowden SC, Barrett-Woodbridge M, Brown GC & Taylor DJ
(2004) Neuropsychological and Information Processing Performance and Its Relationship to White
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 10
Matter Changes Following Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: A Preliminary Study,
Applied Neuropsychology, 11:3, 134–152. DOI:10.1207/s15324826an1103_2 [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
15590348]
Miller JS, Su JJ, Paul LK, & Brown WS (2018). Adaptive behavior in agenesis of the corpus callosum:
Self and informant reports. [Abstract]. Presented at the Forty First Annual Meeting International
Neuropsychological Society February 14–17, 2018 Washington, DC, USA. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 24(S1), 195.
Mueller KLO, Marion SD, Paul LK, & Brown WS (2009). Bimanual Motor Coordination in Agenesis
of the Corpus Callosum. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123(5), 1000–1011. doi:10.1037/a0016868
[PubMed: 19824766]
Njiokiktjien C, Ramaekers G, & Njiokiktjien C (1991). Absence of the corpus callosum:
Clinicopathological correlations. Pediatric behavioural neurology: The child’s corpus callosum
(pp. 235 – 250).
Njiokiktjien C, Valk J, & Ramaekers G (1988). Malformation or damage of the corpus callosum? A
clinical and MRI study. Brain Development, 10(2), 92–99. [PubMed: 3389478]
Norman W, Jeeves MA, Milne A, Ludwig T (1992) Hemispheric interactions: the bilateral advantage
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 11
Saul RE, & Sperry RW (1968). Absence of commissurotomy symptoms with agenesis of the corpus
callosum. Neurology, 18, 307.
Author Manuscript
callosum: linguistic analysis of the Thematic Apperception Test. Neuropsychologia, 48, 43–50.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.009 [PubMed: 19686767]
Ubukata S, Ueda K, Sugihara G, Yassin W, Aso T, Fukuyama H, & Murai T (2016). Corpus callosum
pathology as a potential surrogate marker of cognitive impairment in diffuse axonal injury. Journal
of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 28(2), 97–103. doi:10.1176/
appi.neuropsych.15070159
Wahl M, Strominger Z, Jeremy RJ, Barkovich AJ, Wakahiro I, Sherr EH, & Mukherjee P (2009).
Variability of homotopic and heterotopic callosal connectivity in partial agenesis of the corpus
callosum: a 3T diffusion tensor imaging and q-ball tractography study. American Journal of
Neuroradiology, 30(2), 282–289. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A1361 [PubMed: 19001538]
Wang LW, Huang CC, & Yeh TF (2004). Major brain lesions detected on sonographic screening of
apparently normal term neonates. Neuroradiology, 46(5), 368–373. doi: 10.1007/
s00234-003-1160-4 [PubMed: 15103432]
Weissman DH, & Banich MT (2000). The cerebral hemispherescooperate to perform complex but not
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 12
International Neuropsychological Society February 6–9, 2013 Waikoloa, Hawaii, USA. Journal of
the International Neuropsychological Society, 7P(S1), 44. doi:10.1017/S1355617713000362
Author Manuscript
Young CM, Folsom RC, Paul LK, Su J, Mangum R, & Brown WS (under review). Awareness of
consequences in agenesis of the corpus callosum: semantic analysis of responses.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.
Brown and Paul Page 13
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Agenesis of the corpus callosum, core syndrome, and specific deficits.
Author Manuscript
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 March 24.