Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

10 1016@j Jmathb 2020 100783

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Mathematical Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jmathb

A case for combinatorics: A research commentary


T
Elise Lockwooda,d,*, Nicholas H. Wassermanb, Erik S. Tillemac
a
Oregon State University, United States
b
Teachers College, United States
c
Indiana University, United States
d
University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: In this commentary, we make a case for the explicit inclusion of combinatorial topics in
Research commentary mathematics curricula, where it is currently essentially absent. We suggest ways in which re-
Combinatorics searchers might inform the field’s understanding of combinatorics and its potential role in cur-
Discrete mathematics ricula. We reflect on five decades of research that has been conducted since a call by Kapur
Curricula
(1970) for a greater focus on combinatorics in mathematics education. Specifically, we discuss
the following five assertions: 1) Combinatorics is accessible, 2) Combinatorics problems provide
opportunities for rich mathematical thinking, 3) Combinatorics fosters desirable mathematical
practices, 4) Combinatorics can contribute positively to issues of equity in mathematics educa-
tion, and 5) Combinatorics is a natural domain in which to examine and develop computational
thinking and activity. Ultimately, we make a case for the valuable and unique ways in which
combinatorics might effectively be leveraged within K-16 curricula.

1. Introduction and motivation

Although there have been recent exceptions, the K-16 mathematics curriculum has remained largely unchanged for decades.
Calculus, an important area of mathematics developed in the 17th century, remains the focal point of much of student’s requisite
mathematical learning, driving the algebra-heavy curriculum of middle and secondary schools. A common course sequence for high
school students to take is a year each of Algebra, Geometry, Algebra II, and Trigonometry/Pre-Calculus. Given this course sequence, it
would not be uncommon, or untrue, for such students to conclude that algebra is mathematics. Yet while the world has changed,
perhaps most drastically in the realm of computing and technology, course sequences in mathematics have remained the same. We
see this as problematic and detrimental for several reasons that we elaborate in this paper.
The goal of this commentary is to make a case for combinatorics. That is, we aim to make a case for the explicit inclusion of
combinatorial topics in the curriculum – both in K-12 classrooms and in introductory postsecondary mathematics courses – where it is
currently essentially absent. We regard such inclusion as beneficial both to students and to society. For students, this inclusion
provides accessible opportunities for inclusion around mathematical learning, and it strengthens their mathematical development by
deepening their mathematical thinking and expanding their notions of mathematics. For society, this inclusion offers opportunities to
improve civic engagement in a technological world.
As a research commentary, we specifically look at how existing research has, and future research could, draw the field’s attention
to the importance of combinatorics and its potential as a key element of K-16 curricula. The commentary focuses on five assertions
that emphasize the importance of combinatorics in mathematics education. Specifically, these assertions are: 1) Combinatorics is


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: Elise.Lockwood@oregonstate.edu (E. Lockwood).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2020.100783
Received 2 January 2020; Received in revised form 18 March 2020; Accepted 27 March 2020
0732-3123/ © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

accessible, 2) Combinatorics problems provide opportunities for rich mathematical thinking, 3) Combinatorics fosters desirable
mathematical practices, 4) Combinatorics can contribute positively to issues of equity in mathematics education, and 5)
Combinatorics is a natural domain in which to examine and develop computational thinking and activity. The goal of the paper is to
summarize existing research in combinatorics education related to each claim, and to highlight areas in which additional research is
needed to provide further evidence to substantiate these claims. In this way, we both articulate the current state of research in
combinatorics education and provide an agenda for future research. The first three of these assertions are rationales based in prior
calls related to research in combinatorics education (in particular Kapur’s (1970) appeal for more research into combinatorics
education nearly fifty years ago), and these rationales are still relevant today. We discuss the ways in which researchers have
attempted to answer these calls in the intervening fifty years, and we highlight potential places where researchers could continue to
make inroads in substantiating these particular claims. Then, we highlight two new arguments for the importance of combinatorics in
mathematics education that previously have not been emphasized explicitly, but that align with two trends in mathematics education
research today. By highlighting these new rationales, the paper makes a timely case for combinatorics education and research, both
offering a retrospective view of what has been done in the fifty years since Kapur’s paper, and proposing a forward-looking agenda for
what research might continue to support the case that combinatorics deserves a substantive place in K-16 mathematics education.
Before articulating the problem at hand, we first briefly specify a definition for combinatorics, as the topic sometimes faces
definitional confusion (Berge, 1971).1 In his Introductory Combinatorics textbook, Brualdi defines combinatorics in the following way:
“Combinatorics is concerned with arrangements of the objects of a set into patterns satisfying specified rules…Thus, a general
description of combinatorics might be that combinatorics is concerned with the existence, enumeration, analysis, and optimization of
discrete structures” (Brualdi, p. 1–2, emphasis in original; note here discrete means non-continuous). This is the definition that we
adopt for this paper. Others offer similar definitions (see, e.g., Cameron, 1994; English, 2005; Mazur, 2010; Tucker, 2002), all of
which capture the idea that combinatorics involves discrete, finite sets or structures and answers questions about the existence,
enumeration, and properties of such structures.

2. Context and motivation – a brief history of combinatorics in the curriculum in the U. S

First, we note our perspective as researchers in the United States, where the problem we are trying to articulate is especially acute.
However, we acknowledge that in some countries, including Germany (Höveler, 2017), Israel (Eizenberg & Zaslavsky, 2003, 2004),
Brazil (Borba, Pessoa, Barreto, & Lima, 2011), Spain (Batanero, Navarro-Pelayo, & Godino, 1997; Batanero, Godino, & Navarro-
Pelayo, 2005; Godino, Batanero, & Roa, 2005), and Hungary (Vansco et al., 2017), to name a few, combinatorics already has a strong
presence in school curricula. We do not have space to address how combinatorics is treated in each of these other countries, but the
fact that many other countries do include combinatorial topics serves as a motivation to enact change in the U.S. We now briefly
elaborate on the status and history of combinatorial topics in mathematics education in the U.S., which was part of the impetus for
writing this commentary.
Combinatorics is a subset of discrete mathematics, and so some discussion of combinatorics necessarily involves addressing
treatment of discrete mathematics more broadly. In early standards documents, NCTM (1989) included discrete mathematics (in
addition to algebra, functions, geometry, trigonometry, probability, statistics, pre-calculus) as an explicit content area to be in-
corporated across the high school curriculum, and NCTM offered resources to support this area of focus (e.g., NCTM, 1991). This was
relatively forward-thinking; it was based on NCTM's (1980) call for action that explicitly made a goal of having a “flexible curriculum,
with a greater range of options…to accommodate the diverse needs of the student population” (p. 17). Indeed, the specific inclusion
of discrete mathematics (including combinatorics) in the 1989 standards might have even been in response to Kapur’s calls in the
1970s, or to other societal developments such as handheld computing devices like calculators and personal computers. Some states,
such as New Jersey, made significant headway into having discrete mathematics standards in schools (Rosenstein et al., 1997).
Yet in the decades that have followed, discrete mathematics has slowly been removed from the K-12 curriculum, and it continues
to be on the periphery in undergraduate mathematics. Only a decade after the 1989 standards, NCTM (2000) removed discrete
mathematics as an explicit content area of focus, and there was only one mention of a combinatorial topic in the Grade 9–12
standards for Number and Operation: “develop an understanding of permutations and combinations as counting techniques.” There
were some combinatorial topics that were obliquely included with the data and probability strand (e.g., computing probabilities,
sample space, etc.), but incorporating combinatorial topics solely within probability is not clearly representative of including a
combinatorial topic within the curricula. The more recent Common Core State Standards for Mathematics (National Governors
Association Center for Best Practices, 2010), implemented in a majority of states in the U.S., further separate discrete mathematics
from schools. There are two standards that make explicit mention of combinatorial ideas – one in relation to algebra and the binomial
theorem, the other in relation to probability; however, both of these are (+) standards, which means they are optional – i.e., only for
those intending further mathematical study. In a concrete sense, combinatorics has been relegated out of the K-12 curriculum in the
U.S., and it is not framed as something that students should study as part of their mathematics education. We interpret this devel-
opment to be a move backward, particularly given how society is changing technologically. It represents a narrowing and stan-
dardization of mathematics, rather than an injection of diversity and flexibility into the curriculum. Furthermore, at the college level,

1
Many of the arguments we are making could also be applied to discrete mathematics more broadly; however, we focus on combinatorics both
because, as combinatorics education researchers, we are most qualified to speak specifically about combinatorics, and because we hope a narrower
focus will help to sharpen our arguments.

2
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Fig. 1. Kapur listed reasons for why combinatorial analysis are important for school mathematics (Kapur, 1970, p. 114).

although students have more opportunities to engage with combinatorial topics, typically these arise either as a small unit in a
transition-to-proof discrete mathematics class, or, perhaps, as an elective course for mathematics majors. That is, again, combina-
torics generally appears to be viewed as a relatively unimportant aspect of mathematical study.
Now, one might argue these developments happened for good reason, and society learned a lesson and changed course after
having attempted to incorporate discrete mathematics into schools in the 1990s. We, however, believe these developments have been
a misstep. Somehow over the years, the case for combinatorics became lost, and educators and policymakers forgot (or never in-
ternalized) the promise for how beneficial combinatorics could be for students. With this commentary, we aim to formulate and
elaborate this case for combinatorics.

3. Renewing assertions about the nature of combinatorics in mathematics education

We acknowledge that there have been individuals and groups who have made a general case for discrete mathematics (and, to
some extent, combinatorics) in the past. We seek to build on such prior arguments. In particular, we take time here to elaborate and
explore some of these previous rationales, attempting to defend why we have something novel to contribute to the conversation. We
first note that in 1970, Kapur published an article in Educational Studies in Mathematics, entitled “Combinatorial analysis and school
mathematics,” which argued for further inclusion of combinatorics into the curriculum. His list of 11 reasons is provided in Fig. 1.
Kapur did not offer empirical evidence for these claims, and this was not his goal. Rather, he sought to argue his case and to
suggest that the field take up the mantle of exploring and substantiating his claims via research. He presented a number of com-
binatorial problems that illustrated his points, and he said the following in his conclusion:
It will require a great deal of investigation, research, and experimentation to find to which problems of combinatorial mathe-
matics can go to school, but it is obvious that along with algebraic structures and geometric transformations, combinatorial
problems provide a rich storehouse for modernizing and revitalizing our school curriculum. It is the author’s hope that this source
will be fully exploited in the future (Kapur, 1970, p. 127).
Now, nearly fifty years later, we can examine how combinatorics education has advanced and how Kapur’s calls have been
addressed.

3
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Notably, in the intervening decades there were efforts to address the need for more discrete mathematics in the curriculum, most
of which involved broad discussions of discrete mathematics (rather than combinatorics specifically). These included, for example,
the work of Rosenstein et al. and the DIMACS group about discrete mathematics in schools (e.g., DeBellis & Rosenstein, 2004;
Rosenstein et al., 1997) and the Topic Study Group (TSG) 17 from ICME 2016, which focused on the teaching and learning of discrete
mathematics and yielded a book that argued for the value of discrete mathematics (e.g., Hart & Sandefur, 2017). Others have been
more specific to combinatorics. English (2005), for example, highlighted a 1986 Working Group (K-4) of the Commission on Standards
for School Mathematics that recommended combinatorics, in elementary school, as “an area of exploration within two of its themes for
curriculum development” (p. 121); she also argued that much of the book Discrete Mathematics across the Curriculum, K-12 (Kenney &
Hirsch, 1991) focused on “the teaching of combinatorics especially in the middle and secondary school years” (2005, p. 122). While
such publications did not typically entail research studies, these chapters offered instructional ideas and motivations for in-
corporating combinatorics practically into the classroom (e.g., Althoen, Brown, & Bumcrot, 1991; DeGuire, 1991; Dossey, 1991;
Evered & Schroeder, 1991; Miller, 1991; Schielack, 1991; Spangler, 1991). In addition, Maher and colleagues incorporated combi-
natorics as a key content area in their longitudinal study out of Rutgers University, and their work culminated in a book entitled
Combinatorics and reasoning: representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (Maher, Powell, & Uptegrove, 2011) that highlights
particularly important aspects of combinatorial thinking and activity among school-age children. Additional teachers and researchers
have made arguments for the importance of combinatorics over the years (e.g., Batanero et al., 2005; Hurdle, Warshauer, & White,
2016; Sriraman & English, 2004).

4. Assertions about combinatorics: findings from research and opportunities for additional investigation

In this section, we highlight five assertions about why, and in what ways, combinatorics might have value in school mathematics.
For each assertion, we summarize and exemplify existing literature related to that assertion, and, in this way, this section also offers
an extensive literature review of research on combinatorics education. Then, we also suggest ideas for what additional research might
be needed to better understand and substantiate each assertion systematically. Our aim is not merely to state the problem, but to
present some practical ideas for the kinds of research studies that might help make a case for (and ultimately enact change toward)
incorporating combinatorics into the K-16 curriculum.

4.1. Combinatorial tasks are accessible and require little mathematical background knowledge

One assertion is that combinatorics, as a mathematical domain, is accessible. By accessible, we mean that combinatorics problems
are characterized by: i) asking concrete questions with little technical terminology (i.e., what the question is asking is easy to
understand, with solutions often being concrete counting numbers that answer the question “how many”); and ii) requiring few
mathematical prerequisites for a student to have a chance to explore solutions to those problems. For example, consider the following
problem: You have twenty different books, and you want to take three of them with you on vacation. How many different possibilities are
there for which three books you could bring? Note that the vocabulary is easily understood – there are no technical mathematical terms,
which would require previous definition (e.g., “codomain”);2 and the question is concrete – the answer is a numerical value and does
not necessitate mathematical prerequisites to find it. This is not, however, to say the problem is easy. Such accessibility is an
important consideration for thinking about education and school mathematics, and the more accessible the material, the fewer
barriers there are to having students conceptually understand mathematical problems. An implication of combinatorics being ac-
cessible is that even very young children, as well as students who have not had much success with algebraic (or other mathematical)
topics, have a chance to engage meaningfully with interesting problems. We discuss how combinatorics education researchers have
implicitly supported this assertion, and we then suggest some ideas for research studies that could address this claim more explicitly.
Researchers have implicitly highlighted the accessibility of combinatorics by demonstrating student reasoning about counting
problems in their studies. In studies about combinatorial learning (e.g., Batanero, et al., 1997; English, 1991, 1993; Eizenberg &
Zaslavsky, 2004; Fischbein & Gazit, 1988; Fischbein & Grossman, 1997; Fischbein et al., 1970; Lockwood & Gibson, 2015; Lockwood
& Purdy, 2019a, 2019b; Lockwood et al., 2018; Maher & Martino, 1996a, 1996b; Maher et al., 2011; Tillema, 2013, 2014, 2018, in
press; Tillema & Gatza, 2016), counting problems often arise from everyday contexts. This tends to be true of the field of combi-
natorics in general, but it is certainly true of the set of problems explored in such research. For instance, consider a problem like the
Four-Topping Pizza Problem discussed by Maher, Sran, & Yankelewitz (2011), and at length throughout Maher et al. (2011):
Kenilworth Pizza has asked us to help design a form to keep track of certain pizza choices. They offer a cheese pizza with tomato sauce. A
customer can then select from the following toppings: peppers, sausage, mushrooms, and pepperoni. How many choices for pizza does a
customer have? List all the possible choices. Find a way to convince each other that you have accounted for all possibilities (Maher, Sran,
et al., 2011, p. 69).
Per Maher, Sran, et al., (2011), the students were in fifth grade when they solved this problem. In reading this problem, we

2
The one term that might need defining in this problem is “different.” But different, in general, does not have a precise mathematical definition
(i.e., it depends on the context and problem at hand). Indeed, we regard this as part of the fun with combinatorics problems, because students might
define “different” differently in this problem, and arrive at different answers. Precisely because of this phenomenon, combinatorics problems often
have the added benefit of engaging students in the mathematical activity of defining.

4
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

suggest two things. First, it is not difficult to understand what the question is asking. Ordering pizzas is a real world, concrete idea, a
context with which children may be familiar. Further, there is no technical terminology that needs to be unpacked in the question.
Second, even if students do not know how to solve the problem (or could not articulate a formula or general approach), they often can
begin to symbolize the set of outcomes (e.g., a sequence Pe, M, S would indicate a pizza that had peppers, mushrooms, and sausage).
This is in fact what Maher et al. document, that students listed outcomes. We contend that their engagement in meaningful math-
ematical justification to explain their work highlights the accessibility of such problems.
At the elementary level, researchers have shown that even very young children are able successfully to symbolize sets of outcomes
with the aid of concrete materials (English, 1991; English, 1993; Maher, Sran, et al., 2011; Nunes & Bryant, 1996; Piaget & Inhelder,
1975). At the middle grades or high school levels, researchers have demonstrated that students can symbolize the set of outcomes
using written lists (Speiser, 2011; Tillema, 2013), tree diagrams (e.g., Fischbein & Gazit, 1988), and arrays (Tillema, 2018). Re-
searchers have also demonstrated how symbolizing the set of outcomes can lead to productive and important conversations about
fundamental aspects of problems like whether to include ordered outcomes (Tillema, 2020) or how to organize a list to determine
whether it contains all possible outcomes (Muter & Uptegrove, 2011). Even with partial listing, students with no prior combinatorial
experience can identify patterns and structure within partial lists of outcomes (Lockwood & Gibson, 2016). This is what we mean
when we say that a counting problem is accessible – students are able to understand what a question is asking, they can begin a
solution path by symbolizing outcomes, and, in the process of symbolizing these outcomes, they can engage with and discuss key
combinatorial ideas.
This kind of accessibility stands in contrast with many other problems in school mathematics. For example, problems like the
following may not be inherently tricky or difficult, but they require certain background knowledge and expertise: “Prove the identity
sin2(x)+cos2(x) = 1,” “Determine the roots of the following polynomial,” “Find a function that is differentiable on R except at
x = 0,” and “Is the following topological subspace homeomorphic?” For these problems, it would be necessary to unpack certain
terms and concepts (e.g., what is a “root”) before one could even parse the problem statements. That is, common questions in many
other mathematical areas often have specific, technical language that needs to be explicitly defined in order for one to understand the
problem prior to attempting to solve it.
Researchers have implicitly shown that combinatorial problems are accessible. However, as part of an argument for incorporating
combinatorial problems (as opposed to other kinds of mathematical problems) into K-16 mathematics, there is still a need for clearer
empirical demonstrations about the accessibility of combinatorial problems. We offer a few possible ideas for studies that could not
only demonstrate the accessibility of combinatorics problems, but that could further show that this accessibility is actually beneficial
for students.
First, as a field we might simply explore the use of technical language in combinatorial problems versus algebra or geometry
problems. For instance, we might analyze prerequisite mathematical knowledge required to solve problems: either exploring how
many mathematical prerequisites there are for a typical problem, or how elementary those prerequisites are (e.g., a prerequisite of
solving equations would be more advanced, or less elementary, than one of addition). Either way, by analyzing problems in text-
books, the field might be able to interrogate the claim that combinatorial problems are “more accessible” than other content areas, in
terms of less technical language, or in terms of fewer, more elementary, mathematical prerequisites. We do not currently actually
have measures of accessibility, but such information could support (or refute) the claim that combinatorial problems are “more”
accessible than other domains. Second, we might explore this claim of accessibility empirically with students. Through interviews, we
could probe students’ understanding of different problems in terms of what problems are asking, to see whether combinatorics, as a
field, engages students in solving problems in a way that is “more accessible” than in other areas. That is, accessibility is funda-
mentally mediated by the interpreter, and exploring students’ interpretations (including different ages, groups, levels of mathematical
experience, etc.) would further provide a sense of whether “accessibility” is a characteristic relatively unique to combinatorics as
compared to other mathematical areas. If such evidence was found, it could bolster the rationale for incorporating these particular
kinds of problems into school mathematics. Third, introductory content and ideas, as opposed to those that are building on those
introductory foundations, tend to require less technical vocabulary. It may be that the “accessibility” argument about combinatorics
is really an observation related to the level of the content being studied. Because we study algebra for years, for instance, algebra
problems naturally use more technical vocabulary, as such problems build on introductory ideas studied in years past. Combinatorial
problems, on the other hand, may tend to be introductory simply because they are not built upon to the same extent that algebra
problems are in school, and so they may have less technical language. Interrogating this question might lead to interesting insights.
However, regardless of whether this is the case or not, from our perspective there seems to be more one can do with introductory
ideas in combinatorics than in other fields; that is, other fields necessitate building on concepts more quickly than one needs to in
combinatorics. Parsing the introductory nature of problems versus the mathematical domain would be a challenging but interesting
research endeavor and could provide further empirical evidence that could be used to inform the accessibility of combinatorial
problems.
The discussion around accessibility raises two issues, which we address briefly. The first is that accessibility does not mean
problems are trivial, a notion we elaborate further in the next assertion (Section 4.2) about combinatorial problems providing
opportunities for sufficiently challenging, rich mathematical thinking. The second is related to issues around equity. The fact that
combinatorics lacks prerequisites provides students opportunities to understand problems, and it also opens access for broader
populations of people to engage meaningfully with mathematical ideas and topics. In this way, we view combinatorics as a topic that
can be used to contribute positively to issues of equity and access that the field of mathematics education is currently considering.
This is a topic we discuss further in Section 4.4.

5
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

4.2. Combinatorics problems provide opportunities for challenging, rich mathematical thinking for all students

In noting that combinatorial problems are accessible, we do not want to imply that they are easy or trivial for students to solve.
Indeed, a number of researchers have documented the difficulties that students of all ages face in solving such problems (e.g., Annin &
Lai, 2010; Batanero et al., 1997; Eizenberg & Zaslavsky, 2004; Hadar & Hadass, 1981; Lockwood & Gibson, 2016).3 Yet, these
challenges do not mean that combinatorial problems are inaccessible or too difficult for students. Rather, we regard these difficulties
as suggesting that although combinatorial problems can be easy to engage with (because their statements are easy to understand),
they also require careful thought and certain practices to master or understand completely. This implies that such problems can
appeal to a wide swath of students because they are generally comprehensible while also providing engaging challenges. Kapur
(1970) captures this particular feature of combinatorial problems nicely: “Some of these problems look trivial in their statements and
they present great difficulties in their complete solutions, but most of these can be discussed at the school level” (p. 126). This sums
up our characterization of combinatorial problems as being accessible but still able to pose “great difficulties” (or we might say,
engaging challenges) in their complete solutions. We discuss three ways in which combinatorics educators and combinatorics edu-
cation researchers have supported this assertion. We then suggest some ideas for research studies that could address this claim more
explicitly
First, part of what makes combinatorial problems unique (with the potential to be so rich) is that there is not always a clear path
or procedure to solve a given counting problem. This can be seen, first, from mathematicians (textbook authors) providing com-
mentary on their field. Tucker (2002) says of his introductory chapter on counting “In this chapter we discuss counting problems for
which no specific theory exists” (p. 169). Brualdi (2004) says, “The solutions of combinatorial problems can often be obtained using
ad hoc arguments, possibly coupled with the use of general theory. One cannot always fall back on applications of formulas or known
results” (p. 2–3). Martin (2001) notes that “One of the things that makes elementary counting difficult is that we will encounter very
few algorithms. You will have to think. There are few formulas and each problem seems to be different” (p. 1). That is, unlike
problems in calculus, say, where there might be a clear procedure for solving a problem, there is not always a clear path or process to
a solution. This can make counting problems frustrating, but it also sets them apart as commonly requiring ingenuity, cleverness, and
novel problem-solving approaches.
Second, we look to mathematics educators. Annin and Lai (2010) discussed difficulties in teaching students to count, and they
note that, “What we often find challenging about teaching students to count, however, is that most problems do not cleanly fall into
one and only one standard category of counting problems. Rather, each problem typically involves different aspects that rely on
different techniques” (p. 404). That is, one pedagogical implication is that combinatorial methods cannot be reduced to a procedural
application, but they require thinking about each problem. Indeed, Lockwood, Wasserman, and McGuffey (2019) found that, even in
the simplest cases where problems fit clearly into the category of being “combination” problems, undergraduate students faced
difficulties in determining when to apply a formula for combinations. They also found that when given other problems that could be
solved using a combination formula, students did not recognize that they could apply a formula in those cases. The solution path for
students, even between relatively simple equivalent problem types, was unclear; they could solve one type of problem but not the
other. These comments, and supporting research studies, highlight perhaps some of what makes combinatorics difficult, yes, but they
also emphasize what makes combinatorics so unique and engaging – by their very nature, combinatorial problems offer great op-
portunities for critical thinking and reasoning, often introducing students to “big” mathematical ideas, such as equivalence (Lock-
wood & Reed, in press).
Third, in response to some of these claims about (and calls for) ways in which combinatorics can foster rich mathematical
thinking, the combinatorics education community has demonstrated very thoroughly that students can reason richly and deeply
within combinatorics. One way in which they have done this is to demonstrate sophisticated student understanding of particular
combinatorial topics, such as the multiplication principle (e.g., Lockwood & Caughman, 2016; Lockwood, Reed, & Caughman, 2017;
Lockwood & Purdy, 2019b, 2019b), bijections and isomorphism (e.g., Mamona-Downs & Downs, 2004; Muter & Maher, 1998; Powell
& Maher, 2003; Tarlow, 2011), combinations and the binomial theorem (Lockwood et al., 2018; Maher & Speiser, 1997; Speiser,
2011; Tillema & Burch, 2020; Wasserman & Galarza, 2019; Tillema (2011)), combinatorial proof (e.g., Engelke & CadwalladerOlsker,
2010; Erickson & Lockwood, in press; Lockwood, Caughman, & Weber, 2020; Lockwood, Caughman et al., 2020; Maher & Martino,
1996a, 1996b; Tarlow & Uptegrove, 2011), and equivalence (Lockwood & Reed, in press). Take, for instance, the multiplication
principle (accessible even to elementary students); it is perhaps the most basic counting principle, and yet research studies have
revealed nuances about how the principle is presented in textbooks (Lockwood et al., 2017) and how students reason about mul-
tiplication in counting (Lockwood & Purdy, 2019a, 2019b). In one study, Lockwood and Purdy (2019b) had undergraduate students
reinvent a statement of the multiplication principle, and they identified ways in which students dealt with the idea of independence
and the need for distinct composite outcomes in their statement of the multiplication principle. In addition, researchers (Lockwood &
Caughman, 2015; Lockwood & Purdy, 2019a) highlight subtle issues related to order that emerged for students when they used
multiplication – namely that students do not always recognize that the multiplication principle can introduce order into a counting
process. Across these studies, researchers have provided compelling evidence that students can think deeply about a variety of
combinatorial topics, and that, even though content and ideas are accessible (e.g., multiplication), combinatorics provides a domain
in which they are also sufficiently rich, and challenging, to promote deep mathematical thinking and reasoning. Much of this

3
We do not outline causes for such difficulty in this paper, as that is not our primary goal. Some inherent difficulties are discussed in Tucker
(2002) and Martin (2001), and reasons for difficulties have been summarized elsewhere; see, for example, Lockwood and Purdy (2019b).

6
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

evidence is qualitative in nature, with researchers demonstrating these findings through task-based interviews (e.g., English, 1991,
1993; Lockwood & Erickson, 2017; Tillema, 2018, in press) or teaching experiments (e.g., Lockwood, Swinyard, & Caughman, 2015;
Lockwood & Purdy, 2019a, 2019b; Tillema, 2013, 2014), and some interview-based studies that span long periods of time (Maher
et al., 2011).
Combinatorics education researchers have shown ways in which students reason richly within the domain of combinatorics, but
there is more work to be done to support the integration of such topics into the curriculum. There is a need to study what kinds of
classroom and teacher supports are necessary to ensure that such reasoning and rich thinking surface for students. We hypothesize
that there could be a two-pronged approach to such work. On the one hand, teachers themselves need sufficient professional de-
velopment in the area of combinatorics for them to be able to know how even very simple problems might be used to engender rich
and deep thinking. We could investigate teachers’ explorations of questions such as, What exactly are the nuances of the multi-
plication principle? or How might a combinatorial problem, or a set of combinatorial problems, be used to engage students richly in
thinking about multiplication? On the other hand, implementing classroom tasks is a challenge.
We have essentially argued that combinatorics, as a field, provides many problems that have great potential. There are, however,
two important pedagogical questions to consider. First, we must ask what precisely are the supports necessary to ensure im-
plementation matches potential in combinatorics. Research should help us understand whether there are domain-specific ways, in the
area of combinatorics, for helping to ensure implementation that supports rich thinking.
Second, we must ask what are typical instructional approaches used in combinatorics versus in other domains. Much instruction
in combinatorics is problem-based, and particular counting problems are leveraged to develop and apply important combinatorial
methods and practices. Algebra teaching, in contrast, can often follow a much stricter (less problem-based) procedural approach to
learning algebraic methods.4 However, this need not be the case, and we see opportunities to investigate the relationship between the
content that is being taught and the pedagogical approaches to teaching that content. We think it would be valuable to study whether,
for example, combinatorics itself as a domain inherently provides better opportunities for rich and challenging thinking than a
domain like algebra, or whether any perceived differences about combinatorics are actually a result of an implicit pedagogical
approach that provides such mathematical opportunities. One might accomplish this by exploring characteristics of teachers’ lesson
plans across these different domains, or by attempting to apply common pedagogical approaches from combinatorics to domains such
as algebra to examine potential domain differences and their pedagogical implications. Further research that attempts to differ-
entiate, isolate, and understand this interaction would be helpful to investigate whether combinatorics, a particular content area, has
some inherent benefits for rich and challenging thinking as opposed to other domains.

4.3. Combinatorics fosters desirable mathematical practices

There have also been claims that combinatorics can help to foster desirable practices (e.g., Lockwood & Reed, 2018; Maher et al.,
2011). In their presentation of Standards for Mathematical Practice, the CCSSM says, “The Standards for Mathematical Practice
describe varieties of expertise that mathematics educators at all levels should seek to develop in their students” (p. 5).” We follow the
CCSSM in characterizing mathematical practices as “rest[ing] on important “processes and proficiencies” with longstanding im-
portance in mathematics education” (p. 5). Essentially, these standards focus on broad practices, rather than content, that mathe-
matics students should develop over the course of their education. There has been research within the combinatorics education
community that both implicitly and explicitly connect students’ work on combinatorial problems to their engagement in these
practices.
For example, one such practice in the CCSSM is, Construct viable arguments and critique the reasoning of others, and there are
examples of students engaging in this practice in the context of combinatorial problems within the combinatorics education litera-
ture. For instance, Lockwood (2014b) showed instances of students trying to make sense of two potential answers, where they needed
to think about a hypothetical (incorrect) solution and make sense of it. In particular, students were considering potential solutions to
the Passwords problem: A password consists of eight upper-case letters. How many such 8-letter passwords contain at least three E’s?
(Lockwood, 2014b, p. 32). After attempting to solve the problem on their own, the students evaluated a correct answer that leveraged
8 8
a case breakdown by counting passwords with exactly three through eight E’s – this yielded a solution of k = 3 258 k . Alter-
k
natively, students also interpreted an answer of
8
3 ()
265 as a seemingly reasonable but common incorrect answer.5 As the students in
Lockwood’s study explored these answers, they certainly had to engage in the practice of constructing their own viable arguments,
first understanding and then critiquing the incorrect answer. There have been a number of studies that involve multiple students
(such as paired or small-group teaching experiments), where we see evidence of productive discussions between students as they
present, consider, and defend their own and others’ arguments (Lockwood & Reed, 2018; Eizenberg & Zaslavsky, 2004; Kavousian,
2008; Lockwood et al., 2015; Maher et al., 2011; Lockwood (2014a)). Often the social dynamic of having students working together

4
This is not arguing a relative comparison of pedagogical approaches in combinatorics as universally better than algebra. Indeed, Wasserman
(2019) points out a few important pedagogical challenges and dualities that exist with respect to the teaching of combinatorial ideas that need
further resolution.
5
According to Lockwood (2014b), the idea behind the incorrect answer is that one can first choose three spots in the password to be E’s. Then,
since the three E’s constraint is met, the remaining five positions can be any letters, including E. This process counts some outcomes more than once,
such as the all Es password, so this answer is too large. The case breakdown is correct.

7
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Fig. 2. A structured list of outcomes.

in these qualitative studies affords students opportunities to think and reason about their own arguments and the arguments of other
students. Because combinatorial problems cannot often be solved by applying a specific procedure (see previous assertion, Section
4.2), students may approach and solve problems very differently (i.e., not using the same prescribed procedure). This is an important
quality of combinatorial problems; it suggests that, perhaps more naturally than in other mathematical domains, students will have to
defend their own solutions and critique, or come to understand, someone else’s reasoning. Indeed, part of the fun in combinatorics is
realizing that very different-appearing solutions can, in fact, both be correct.
As another example, the CCSSM includes the practice to Look for and make use of structure. There are a number of studies that have
highlighted ways in which combinatorics is particularly suited for this kind of work. In introductory enumerative combinatorics,
there are natural structural distinctions among types of combinatorial objects that are being counted, and this can be a way to
distinguish between fundamental types of counting problems. For example, a difference between permutations and combinations can
be understood as two different types of counting problems that count two different types of objects (sequences and sets, respectively).
Many researchers (e.g., Lockwood & De Chenne, 2019; Lockwood et al., 2015, 2018; Maher et al., 2011; Powell, 2011; Tarlow, 2011)
have highlighted structural commonalities and differences among counting problems, and they have documented students’ reasoning
about such relationships. In addition, some researchers (most notably Batanero et al., 1997) discuss the effects of implicit combi-
natorial models of combinatorial problems, in particular highlighting differences between selection, distribution, and partition
problems (Dubois, 1984). The existence of such models demonstrates important structural aspects of counting problems and highlight
ways in which students may meaningfully distinguish between structures within combinatorics.
Another way that researchers have demonstrated that students look for and make use of structure within the domain of com-
binatorics is through numerical expressions that reflect a particular counting process (Lockwood & Reed, 2018; Lockwood, 2013).
Burch, Ataide-Pinheiro, and Tillema (2019) have noted this phenomenon with pre-service secondary teachers as the PSTs determined
binomial coefficients. In determining the coefficient of x 2 ·y 3 , one PST focused on the position of the two xs (e.g., xxyyy, xyxyy, etc.),
writing the total number of outcomes as a sum (4 + 3 + 2 + 1) while the other focused on the position of the three ys (e.g., yyyxx,
yyxyx, etc.), writing the total number of outcomes as a sum of sums ((3 + 2 + 1) + (2 + 1) + (1)). These different counting
processes were reflected in the numerical expressions they produced, and we would argue that thinking about these different
structures can be useful for students in developing justifications and understanding expressions and counting processes. Some re-
searchers (e.g., English, 1991; Halani, 2012; Lockwood, 2013; Lockwood & De Chenne, 2019; Lockwood & Gibson, 2016) have also
demonstrated how students might leverage structure of combinatorial objects in developing, refining, and implementing listing
strategies. For example, Lockwood (2013) discussed listing outcomes for the problem, “How many 3-letter ‘words’ are there using the
letters A, B, and C (repetition of letters allowed)?” (p. 255). Lockwood described a three-stage process to solve the problem, “to apply
the multiplication principle to consider the number of choices for the first letter, second letter, and third letter of the word” (p. 256).
This yields an answer of 3*3*3, and the resulting list of outcomes that is created using that listing process generates a list with a
particular structure (Fig. 2 below). Specifically, it generates an alphabetical list, and the way the outcomes are organized show
structural regularity in the set of outcomes that reflect the three-stage counting process.
6! 6!
As a final example related to structure, consider the following expression: 3 ! 2 ! + 2 ! 2 ! . We follow Lockwood (2013) in asserting
that a mathematical expression can reflect some combinatorial process, or have some combinatorial meaning. In this case, the
structure of the expression reflects having broken the problem into two (disjoint) cases, each of which was solved by considering
permutations of six elements and dividing by differing numbers of repeated elements. Now, consider the expression: 4· 3 ! 2 ! . Here the
6!

structure reflects a different process, one where there are 4 initial choices, each of which has the same number of options that is a
permutation of six elements with some repeated elements. That is, these numerical expressions reflect structure – in particular one’s
approach to counting – somewhat naturally. Perhaps of even more interest in this particular example is that these are both correct
and equivalent solutions to the same problem (the reader might confirm their numerical equivalence), despite them being very
different structurally (in particular, they both answer the question “How many numbers greater than 3,000,000 can be formed by
arrangements of 1, 2, 2, 4, 6, 6, 6?” (Tucker, 2012, p. 212)6).
We now consider a final way in which researchers have examined combinatorics as a domain in which to develop mathematical
practices, some researchers have explored justification and generalization within the context of combinatorics (e.g., Lockwood, 2011;
Lockwood & Reed, 2018; Reed & Lockwood, 2018; Maher et al., 2011). One of the central components of justification in combi-
natorics is providing an argument that demonstrates that one has counted all of the desired outcomes exactly once. For example,

6
The first expression breaks the solution into cases based on whether the first digit is a 4 or a 6, and the second expression treats the 6’s as distinct,
considers four options for first digits (the 4 or the distinct 6’s), then uses division to account for equivalence.

8
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Maher and colleagues (Maher & Yankelewitz, 2011; Maher, Sran, et al., 2011) have illustrated that as early as the 2nd and 3rd grade
children can begin to wrestle with how to justify that they have produced all possible outcomes (see also, English, 1991, 1993, 1996,
who built on Piaget & Inhelder, 1975), and that by the 5th grade some students in their study were able to use case-based arguments
to aid in their justifications. At the high school level, Tillema and Gatza (2017) have demonstrated the role that justification plays for
students prior to them being able to conclude that two different ways of counting the same set of outcomes are equivalent; namely to
conclude equivalence the students needed to first justify that each way of counting counted the desired set of outcomes exactly once.
In work focused on generalization, researchers have demonstrated that within combinatorics there are many opportunities for stu-
dents to generalize beyond just finding numerical or algebraic patterns. Indeed, students have been shown to focus on generalizing
structural patterns and relationships like those related to the binomial coefficients (Muter & Uptegrove, 2011) and the multiplication
of binomials (Tillema & Gatza, 2017). Moreover, Burch et al. (2019) showed that pre-service secondary teachers saw different
structures in their lists for the same problem, and that these different structures allowed the pre-service teachers to discuss which of
the structures were generalizable. Ellis, Tillema, Lockwood, and Moore (2017) have also illustrated that students can establish
“relations of similarity across problems or contexts” (p. 680) in the domain of combinatorics, a powerful basis for beginning to make
abstractions about when situations might require a particular combinatorial operation.
There is relatively strong qualitative evidence that students in individual interviews, pairs, or small groups can engage in the
mathematical practices outlined above. We see several potential areas for further research related to mathematical practices. First,
the studies cited above show that students can and do engage with particular mathematical practices in powerful ways. However, the
extent to which their engagement in these mathematical practices in a combinatorial setting might support them to engage in similar
practices in other mathematical domains has not been a focal point of study. In addition, the way that these mathematical practices
entail domain specific ways of thinking has also not been extensively studied. Such investigations could bolster an understanding
about mathematical practices, and they could speak to the assertion that combinatorics can be leveraged to help develop practices in
productive ways. Second, there have been relatively few studies that investigate how teachers might learn to support students in these
mathematical practices in their classroom instruction. Given that K-12 teachers may have limited experiences with combinatorics,
this research needs to include studies that provide information on teacher learning in this domain, studies on how teachers come to
see this domain as connected to the curricula that they teach, and studies that investigate how teachers implement and support
students to engage with mathematical practices in this domain. Notably, this might reveal several different approaches for in-
corporation at the K-12 level, such as integrating combinatorial topics within the development of existing courses and ideas, or as a
course on its own. At the post-secondary level, classroom studies on how instructors learn to support students to engage in these
mathematical practices may be slightly easier given that collegiate level combinatorics courses already exist.

4.4. Combinatorics can contribute positively to issues of equity in mathematics education

In this section, in light of the recent long overdue import given to issues around equity in mathematics education, we frame a new
assertion about the importance of combinatorics, namely that combinatorics can contribute positively to issues of equity in
mathematics education. We acknowledge that this is related to issues of accessibility, but we argue that this is a distinct rationale, as
it involves not just the nature of combinatorial content but the impact of that content on who has access to mathematics within
society. In particular, we focus on issues of equity and access for students who may not feel welcome as members of the mathematical
community, which overwhelmingly runs along gender and racial lines (Gutiérrez, 2008; Martin, 2015). The premise is for such
students, it is possible that some of their mathematical (non)identification may stem from their not being afforded access to different
mathematical domains, ones which might align more closely to their own mathematical ways of thinking and doing. By broadening
students’ ideas about what mathematics is, we can expand access to who believes they can do, and who sees value in, mathematics.
This is in contrast to current school emphases, which, we believe, mistakenly send the message to many students that they are not
“mathematics people.” Indeed, because combinatorics problems do not require substantial prerequisites or technical terminology, it is
particularly well-suited for addressing this issue. Even if students are “behind” in other areas of mathematics (i.e., they have not done
well in algebra), they can get a fresh start in a different area, one that is rooted in the familiar mathematical notion of counting but
that also involves sufficiently challenging and rigorous mathematics. Anecdotally, we have seen evidence of this in our own sec-
ondary teaching: high school students that struggled for years with algebra excelled in solving combinatorial problems. We now
outline three more specific ways in which combinatorial topics could be particularly related to current issues of equity in mathe-
matics education, and we suggest that additional research might be carried out in each of these areas.
First, counting problems can be naturally applied to issues related to social justice. There are many combinatorics problems that
can be used to explore relevant social issues. Gatza (2018) has used combinatorics problems to explore how middle grades students
understand racial bias in jury selection using problems like the following (see also Gatza, Tillema, & Burch, 2020).
Jury Selection Problem. Suppose we have a county with an eligible juror population that is 50% Black and 50% White. While there are
most commonly 12 people on juries, let’s assume we are only selecting the first TWO jurors. The first person selected will be the foreperson
(person who speaks on behalf of the jury) and the second person selected will be a regular juror. To simplify the situation, let’s start with the
assumption that there are 5 Black individuals and 5 White individuals in the county. Draw a 2D array for the total possible two-person
juries.
Gatza (2018) has used problems like this one to examine the mutually supportive nature of critical race consciousness devel-
opment and mathematical learning. He has supported students to: examine recent supreme court cases related to racial bias in jury
selection; promote conversations about the history of racial categories (e.g., what does it mean to have discrete racial categories?;

9
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

how has that played out historically?); and help differentiate between individual and structural components of racial bias and racism.
He has embedded this work in the context of thinking about mathematical issues that are important; for example, it is common for
students to consider the relationship between the number of racial categories and the number of possible racial compositions of juries
to be linear. Doing so is incorrect, as the relationship is non-linear. In this case, incorrectly applying a linear relationship also has a
socially significant consequence—it leads a person to dramatically underestimate the likelihood that racial bias is occurring in jury
selection. Using mathematics to explore relevant social issues is not unique to combinatorics, but because many counting problems
arise from readily accessible contexts there is great promise in using combinatorics for such purposes.
Second, combinatorics could expand students’ views of what mathematics is. Students may not regard discrete mathematics as
"math," perhaps because of how different it seems from algebra and calculus, to which they are accustomed. Presenting mathematics
as a singularity (algebra) potentially limits students’ conception of what mathematics is; pointing out discrete mathematics as “math”
can expose students to mathematics as a multiplicity. Even historically, discrete mathematics was not necessarily considered
mathematics by mathematicians. Euler, who first posed and solved the now-famous Königsburg bridge problem (paraphrased as, "Is
there a way to take a walk around the city, starting and ending at your house, going across all bridges exactly once?") in the 1700s,
did not even consider the problem to be “mathematics” because it was utterly different from other areas of the discipline. It is now a
quintessential problem in graph theory. Most students today would also be surprised to hear that the Königsburg bridge problem is a
mathematical question; it seems qualitatively different than a question such as, “Solve for x in the following equation: 5x – 7 = 13.”
We see this difference as an inroad to conversations about the nature and applications of mathematics. For example, in contemporary
times, combinatorics is at the root of how computing systems work, a discussion that we think has the potential to capture students’
imaginations (e.g., When I press an “A” on my keyboard why does the letter appear on my screen? How is a binary number system
involved in satellite communication?). These kinds of opportunities allow students to consider (like Euler did) that something they
may not recognize as mathematical is in fact deeply mathematical.
Combinatorics provides opportunities for students to see many additional mathematical topics and ideas that offer different
perspectives on the nature of mathematics. We see this both in the actual content, and in typical approaches to problem solving
within various domains. That is, combinatorics offers new, interesting topics that they may not see in algebra or calculus, such as
counting techniques and recurrence relations. But, combinatorics also offers a different way of approaching and solving problems. As
we have noted, counting requires ingenuity, and solutions do not follow predictable procedures or algorithms. Even seeing that
combinatorics requires a different way of approaching problems could expand students’ view of the mathematical process, and
students who are not satisfied with applying procedures and calculations may be compelled by what combinatorics as a mathematical
domain has to offer.
Consider, for instance, combinatorial proofs. These are not exercises in symbolic manipulation, but rather they entail articulating
enumerative arguments about certain sets of objects (see Erickson & Lockwood, in press; Lockwood, Caughman et al., 2020;
Lockwood et al., in press). They even look different than proofs in other domains, as they consist simply of sentences of text and may
not contain any symbols at all. This stands in contrast to the kinds of proof and argumentation to which many students at an early
mathematical level are accustomed, which primarily involve algebraic manipulation. As another example, consider the relationship
between discrete mathematics and computers, a topic we discuss further in Section 4.5. Mathematics related to computers involves
not continuous structures, but logic, recurrences, combinatorial arguments, algorithms, and more. These are topics that are funda-
mentally mathematical and are essential to computers, but, in the focus on the continuous mathematics of algebra and calculus, many
students never learn that mathematics entails such topics.
Third, if students can gain broader perspectives on the nature of mathematics, then combinatorics can broaden opportunities for
students to feel like they are capable at mathematics. Simply because of curricular emphases on algebra and calculus that we have
mentioned, students may conclude that if they do not excel at algebra, they are not mathematics people. We thus may lose people to
studying mathematics simply because they do not associate their interests, or talents, with mathematics. Indeed, ask many people if
they use mathematics in their jobs, and they will say no–that is, rarely do they do some activity that they associate with mathematics,
like “solving for x” or “applying the quadratic formula.” But if people regarded networks, or logistics operations, as graphs from
discrete mathematics, or recognized sets and enumeration as informing their daily technological interactions, they might have a
different view about mathematics. In other words, some who identify as non-math people may, in fact, be more likely to identify as
math people after realizing that mathematics is not only algebra (and by being given opportunities to study a fundamentally different
domain of mathematics). To be clear, these questions of what mathematics is and who can do mathematics are fundamentally issues
of access and equity. Currently, people who are considered (by themselves or others) as non-math people often base these evaluations
on their own perception of mathematics, one that is often skewed by their exposure to some domains of mathematics and not others.
Given that many students take sides, preferring algebra problems to geometry problems (or vice versa), it would be unsurprising to
find that some (other) students are more compelled by combinatorial problems; an equitable mathematical education would provide
students with the opportunity to recognize their mathematical interest(s) and potential.
In sum, by studying a different area of mathematics, students who previously did not identify with mathematics might now view
themselves positively with regard to mathematics. By providing students with an area where problems are easily accessible despite
prior study, students who struggle in some areas might find they excel in a different area. Finally, by incorporating combinatorics
problems, we can give opportunities for students to develop computational and other mathematical ways of thinking that give access
to technology-based (or other STEM) careers.
We thus see a great need for researchers to study the potential for discrete mathematics, and combinatorics in particular, to
capture the interest of and engage students who might otherwise dismiss mathematics. This simply has not been systematically
investigated. The increase in researchers pursuing equity-related educational issues has grown substantially in the last decade, and

10
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

here we call for more intersectional work applying equity-oriented research approaches to be done in this area. Results from such
studies could provide insight on the extent to which combinatorics can and should be used to broaden participation in mathematics.
For example, aside from Gatza’s work, we are not aware of studies that have leveraged combinatorial problems to explore issues of
social justice. Thus, we see potential for research studies that might actually examine the effectiveness of combinatorics in this
context. We also feel strongly that combinatorics could give students opportunities to broaden their perspective on the nature of
mathematics. Yet, there is a need to examine this phenomenon empirically. As an example, the field might examine students’ current
conceptions of mathematics (as related to algebra and calculus versus discrete mathematics and combinatorics), and investigate ways
to expand students’ views of mathematics through combinatorics. One might ask students, through surveys and interviews, about
their conceptions of mathematics, or about which kinds of problems they associate most with mathematics, before and after taking a
discrete mathematics course. By doing this for different levels and populations of students, we could see what kinds of perspectives
exist for various populations. In addition, we could give students problems from different domains and query the extent to which
those various problems reflect what the students believe about the nature of mathematics. Then, with such information, we could
investigate ways in which to help students actually broaden and expand their views of mathematics, and we could attempt to use
combinatorics to do so. This might entail giving students combinatorial tasks and also asking reflective questions that draw their
attention to the nature of mathematics. Lastly, explicit investigation about access and equity could be valuable. As an example, one
might study those who identify as “non-math” people in order to disentangle whether their identification is based on a singular
domain-specific view of mathematics. Or, one might explore the effect that taking a discrete mathematics has on mathematical self-
efficacy for such students. That is, does studying combinatorics actually shift people’s mathematical identities, or their sense of self-
efficacy in mathematics? In such cases, it might be important to attempt to explore whether shifts in identity are associated with the
content in particular, or with an implicit pedagogical approach to teaching that content. Similarly, one might study whether exposing
students to different areas of mathematics, including combinatorics, perhaps alters their future mathematical course-taking. Such
studies might pave the way for understanding the way that combinatorics might be used to expand access and equity to mathematics.
We find each of these to be potentially compelling, and the mathematics education research community needs to take up this issue to
explore whether these ideas in fact have merit.

4.5. Combinatorics is a natural domain in which to examine (and develop) computational thinking and activity

Our world is increasingly computer-oriented, and our professional and social interactions are regularly mediated by technology.
Combinatorics is an essential mathematical topic for computer scientists to know and to understand, but it is also increasingly
essential to everyday citizens being able to engage critically with our technological world. Even more, increasing numbers of fields
are emphasizing computing, and more and more jobs involve computational literacy and fluency. Along with others (e.g.,
Abramovich & Pieper, 1996; Buteau & Muller, 2017; Cetin & Dubinsky, 2017; diSessa, 2018; Hickmott, Prieto-Rodriguez, & Holmes,
2018; Lockwood, De Chenne, & Valdes-Fernandez, 2019; Papert, 1990; Pei, Weintrop, & Wilensky, 2018; Sinclair & Patterson, 2018;
Lockwood, DeJarnette, & Thomas (2019)), we are becoming convinced that it is our responsibility as mathematics educators to help
our students develop an understanding of computing and to be able to reason about and use computing as a part of their scientific and
mathematical work. There have been calls for more integration of computing into curricula (see, for example, an increasing number
of calls by federal and private funding agencies to focus on CS education (e.g., Blikstein, 2018)). With this increased attention on
seeing computing integrated more wholly into the curriculum, we contend that the field would do well to look to combinatorics as the
natural place to introduce fundamental computing structures and concepts into the mathematics curriculum.
There are inherent connections between discrete mathematics and computers and computer science, as data and information must
be discretized for computers to be able to process and work with such data. Kapur (1970) raised some of these connections, noting
that in combinatorics “enough motivation for working with computers can be provided” (p. 114) and “Students can appreciate the
powers and limitations of mathematics as well as the power and limitations of computers through combinatorial mathematics” (p.
114). These comments were from fifty years ago, and we now have much clearer examples of such connections to computers. For
example, one point of connection is that combinatorics and computing share some common processes. It is very common in com-
binatorics to break a problem (or proof) up into several cases (often disjoint) and solve each individually; by doing so in a systematic
way, we end up with a solution based on cases. This case-based, systematic, way of reasoning is also useful in computing, as many
computational approaches consider the space of possible cases, and design solutions to these different cases as a means of solving the
larger problem. Because computing, and discrete mathematics, are fundamentally about finite (and not infinite) processes, cases exist
and are a useful and productive approach in both domains. This is but one example of ways in which combinatorial topics and
approaches can complement ideas in computer science. While Kapur did not elaborate these ideas or show evidence with data, we
now have a chance to identify ways in which research has supported these claims over the last several decades, and to make new
arguments for additional research that could support these claims.
As an example of work that is exploring relationships between combinatorics and computational thinking and activity in parti-
cular, some researchers have investigated ways in which combinatorial thinking may be well-suited for fostering computational
thinking and activity, as well as how computational settings might reinforce and enrich students’ reasoning about combinatorics
problems (e.g., Fenton & Dubinsky, 1996; Lockwood & De Chenne, 2019; De Chenne & Lockwood, in press; Lockwood, De Chenne
et al., 2019). For instance, in their book Introduction to Discrete Mathematics with ISETL, Fenton and Dubinsky (1996) proposed a new
programming language (ISETL) that would explicitly help to reinforce topics of discrete mathematics in a computational setting. As
another example, Lockwood and De Chenne (2019) report on undergraduate students’ exploration of tasks in which they solved
counting problems in a computational setting of programming involving coding in Python. Lockwood and De Chenne provided

11
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

evidence “that students were able to reason about, understand, and use nested for loops and particular conditional statements within
those for loops to computationally generate lists of outcomes for three main types of problems” (p. 47), and they suggested that “the
symbols in the Python code seemed to enrich the students’ combinatorial reasoning by affording opportunities for the students to
make a connection to the kinds of outcomes they were counting” (p. 47). The students in their study, some of whom were novice
counters, expressed that they found the context to be an effective introduction to principles of computing.
As a field, then, we are gaining empirical evidence for ways in which combinatorics can help to enrich students’ computational
thinking and activity, but we see a great need for continued work in this area. Specifically, the field could conduct studies that
investigate what relationships students see between the structure of counting problems and computing languages, which could help
to identify what kinds of counting problems and structures students might use to support the development of computing languages
and vice versa. Researchers could also analyze extant curricular materials that identify combinatorics problems that afford oppor-
tunities for supporting the development of computational thinking. We also consider design experiment studies that investigate novel
curricular interventions aimed at supporting the mutual development of combinatorial reasoning and computational thinking to be
an important area for further research. That is, prior to implementation in classrooms, we think it is important to have studies that
test the affordances and limitations of particular curricular interventions in experimental settings in order to inform subsequent
classroom implementation. These studies could answer questions such as: What are the necessary ingredients for a curricular in-
tervention to mutually support combinatorial reasoning and computational thinking? In what ways is each mutually supported? What
particular combinatorial or computational concepts are particularly effective in facilitating connections between these two dis-
ciplines?

5. Discussion and conclusion

In this commentary, we have made a case that more attention should be paid to combinatorics within the K-16 mathematics
curriculum. We have argued that combinatorics as a discipline has potentially unique affordances within the realm of mathematics
education. In particular, two current trends in mathematics education suggest that this case for combinatorics is especially timely,
namely the increased attention to issues of access and equity, as well as the current national push toward computational thinking
initiatives in mathematics education. Through this commentary, we have synthesized existing literature from the past five decades in
combinatorics education research, while also identifying an agenda for the kinds of work and research that still needs to be done. To
conclude, we offer a vision of what a multi-faceted effort in this area might look like, which involves researchers, practitioners, and
policy-makers.
First, for researchers, we regard the exploration and substantiation of claims to be of utmost importance. That is, as a field, to
accomplish the incorporation of discrete mathematics and combinatorics into the curriculum, we need strong foundations on which
to make such arguments. While existing research provides some of these foundations, there is more to be done. In addition to in-depth
qualitative work, we need larger-scale quantitative studies that help us understand generalizations that can be made. We need not
just to rely on compelling theoretical arguments about the utility of discrete mathematics (e.g., that it might expand students’ sense of
what mathematics is), but we must continue to push for increasing the number and amount of empirical studies and evidence. In
addition, we need to draw on the expertise of equity-oriented researchers in exploring questions about mathematical identify, self-
efficacy, opportunity, etc., in relation to various mathematical domains. Such research and evidence will form the solid backbone of
arguments for broader incorporation.
Second, for practitioners, we need people willing to engage in the practical work of developing curricular ideas and resources in
K-16 mathematics. First, for K-16 mathematics educators, people must take up the work of generating curricular resources that are
age-appropriate for specific groups of students. This might involve multiple different approaches to thinking about the incorporation
and inclusion of discrete mathematics and combinatorics into the curriculum. We name two, as examples. First, we might consider
entire units, or courses, devoted to topics in combinatorics. These might be multi-week units, or even semester long courses in high
school, and these approaches would represent a valuing of combinatorics as mathematics in its own right – similar to units and
courses on topics in probability or statistics. As an alternative, we might consider ways that discrete and combinatorial topics might
be incorporated for the purpose of deepening students understanding of current curricular goals, for example, in algebra. One could
imagine that exploring counting problems, because they lend themselves to different equivalent expressions, might help students
attend to (and interpret) the structure of algebraic expressions (e.g., CCSSM (2010) standards HSA.SSEA.1 and A.2). That is, com-
binatorial problems might be used to help students learn algebraic (or other) topics. In either case, we need practitioners working on
practical ways to incorporate discrete mathematics and combinatorics into K-16 curricula. Second, for K-16 mathematics teacher
educators, we need to consider the practical necessity of teacher preparation. Existing teachers might not have the mathematical
expertise to feel comfortable teaching students combinatorial topics; teacher educators are the ones that must be tasked with the
challenge of how to prepare a teacher workforce for such incorporation. This includes activities like designing professional devel-
opment resources and rethinking teacher preparation program requirements and courses. Practitioners of mathematics teacher
education are a necessary component of realizing such goals.
Third, for policy-makers, we need public voices and larger policy mechanisms in place to help support and bolster such efforts.
Here, we make an analogy to statistics education. Over the past 20 years, statistics educators have been extremely successful at
shaping the mathematics curricula to include more statistics. Not only have they worked to build a research based in statistics
education, but they have done an excellent job in helping the public understand the importance and necessity of statistics in today’s
world; the public now generally believes statistics to be very important. Indeed, many in the public argue now that data and statistics
should be at least a, if not the, primary part of student’s mathematics education. The statistics education community has also

12
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

benefitted from other driving forces; the existence of an AP Statistics course as an alternative to AP Calculus in high school, for
example, has been used as a mechanism to increasingly incorporate statistics throughout the curriculum and to provide alternative
mathematical options for study. Hence, from this analogy, we see the need for voices to help make the argument to the public for the
importance of discrete mathematics and combinatorics to students’ mathematical education. Notably, we believe arguments that
leverage the current emphases on computational thinking, and on how combinatorial reasoning can help everyday citizens under-
stand the role and nature of computers and algorithms in their everyday lives (which are increasingly mediated by technological
interactions), could be especially compelling. Additionally, promoting the diversity of mathematics, and asking the public to demand
an education that sheds light on this mathematical diversity, because such diversity is important to conversations about access and
equity, is similarly important. Having voices to help shape public perception is an important feature in such efforts. Similarly, efforts
to entrench combinatorics as important aims in public policy and the public infrastructure is important. For example, having people
fight for the inclusion of standards that emphasize discrete mathematics topics; having public organizations such as the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), or the Mathematical Association of America (MAA), etc., vie for education to portray
the diversity of mathematics, including discrete; and perhaps even having organizations like the College Board consider the creation
of an AP Discrete Mathematics exam. All of these efforts would be useful toward accomplishing the broader aim for which we have
advocated in this commentary.
In sum, we believe the case for combinatorics has been lost (at least in the U.S.) in the shifting landscape of mathematics
education over the past 30 years. In this paper, we revive and renew these arguments, and implore the mathematics and education
communities (including researchers, practitioners, and policy-makers) to answer the calls made, in order to diversify and strengthen
the educational opportunities students are presented in mathematics. We see this renewal as vital to the ongoing conversations about
how all students can participate in rich mathematical thinking and activity.

Author statement

Descriptions in this manuscript are accurate and agreed upon by all authors.

References

Abramovich, S., & Pieper, A. (1996). Fostering recursive thinking in combinatorics through the use of manipulatives and computer technology. The Mathematics
Educator, 7(1), 4–12.
Althoen, S. C., Brown, J. L., & Bumcrot, R. J. (1991). Graph chasing across the curriculum: Paths, circuits, and applications. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.).
Discrete mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991 yearbook (pp. 30–43). Reston, VA: National Council of the Teachers of Mathematics.
Annin, S. A., & Lai, K. S. (2010). Common errors in counting problems. Mathematics Teacher, 103(6), 402–409.
Batanero, C., Navarro-Pelayo, V., & Godino, J. (1997). Effect of the implicit combinatorial model on combinatorial reasoning in secondary school pupils. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 32, 181–199.
Batanero, C., Godino, J., & Navarro-Pelayo, V. (2005). Combinatorial reasoning and its assessment. In I. Gal, & J. B. Garfield (Eds.). The assessment challenge in statistics
education (pp. 239–252). IOS Press.
Berge, C. (1971). Principles of combinatorics. New York: Academic Press.
Blikstein, P. (2018). Pre-college computer science education: A survey of the field. Retrieved fromMountain View, CA: Google LLC. https://goo.gl/gmS1Vm.
Borba, R., Pessoa, C., Barreto, F., & Lima, R. C. G. (2011). Children’s, young people’s and adults’ combinatorial reasoning. In B. Ubuz (Vol. Ed.), Proceedings of the 35th
Conference of the International group for the psychology of mathematics education: Vol. 2, (pp. 169–176). Ankara, Turkey: PME.
Brualdi, R. A. (2004). Introductory combinatorics (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Burch, L. J., Ataide-Pinheiro, W., & Tillema, E. S. (2019). Opportunities for Generalizing within Pre-Service Secondary Teachers’ Symbolization of Combinatorial Tasks. Brief
Research Report at the 41st Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics EducationSt. Louis, MO:
University of Missouri.
Buteau, C., & Muller, E. (2017). Assessment in undergraduate programming-based mathematics courses. Digital Experiences in Mathematics Education, 3, 97–114.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40751-016-0026-4.
Cameron, P. J. (1994). Combinatorics: Topics, techniques, algorithms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cetin, I., & Dubinsky, E. (2017). Reflective abstraction in computational thinking. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 47, 70–80.
De Chenne, A., & Lockwood, E. (2020). Student verification practices for combinatorics problems in a computational environment (In press) To Appear in the
Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Conference on Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education.
DeBellis, V., & Rosenstein, J. G. (2004). Discrete mathematics in primary and secondary schools in the United States. ZDM, 36, 77–81.
DeGuire, L. J. (1991). Permutations and combinations: A problem-solving approach for middle school students. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete
mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991 yearbook (pp. 59–66). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
diSessa, A. A. (2018). Computational literacy and “The big picture” concerning computers in mathematics education. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 20(1), 3–31.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2018.1403544.
Dossey, J. A. (1991). Discrete mathematics: The math for our time. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991
yearbook (pp. 1–9). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Dubois, J. G. (1984). Une systematique des configurations combinatoires simples. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15(1), 37–57.
Eizenberg, M. M., & Zaslavsky, O. (2003). Cooperative problem solving in combinatorics: The inter-relations between control processes and successful solutions. The
Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22, 389–403.
Eizenberg, M. M., & Zaslavsky, O. (2004). Students’ verification strategies for combinatorial problems. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(1), 15–36.
Ellis, A. B., Tillema, E., Lockwood, E., & Moore, K. C. (2017). Generalization across domains: The relating-forming-extending generalization framework. In E. Galindo,
& J. Newton (Eds.). Proceedings of the 39th annual meeting of the North American chapter of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 677–
684). Indianapolis, IN: Indiana University.
Engelke, N., & CadwalladerOlsker, T. (2010). Counting Two ways: The art of combinatorial proof. Proceedings of the 13th Annual Conference on Research in
Undergraduate Mathematics Education. Presented at the Special Interest Group of the Mathematical Association of America (SIGMAA) for Research in Undergraduate
Mathematics Education. Retrieved from http://sigmaa.maa.org/rume/crume2010/Abstracts2010.htm.
English, L. D. (1991). Young children’s combinatorics strategies. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, 451–547.
English, L. D. (1993). Children’s strategies for solving two- and three-dimensional combinatorial problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24(3),
255–273.
English, L. D. (1996). Children’s construction of mathematical knowledge in solving novel isomorphic problems in concrete and written form. The Journal of

13
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

Mathematical Behavior, 15(1), 81–112.


English, L. D. (2005). Combinatorics and the development of children’s combinatorial reasoning. In G. A. Jones (Vol. Ed.), Exploring probability in school: Challenges for
teaching and learning: Vol. 40, (pp. 121–141). Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Erickson, S. & Lockwood, E. (in press). Investigating combinatorial provers’ models of multiplication. To appear in the Electronic Proceedings of the 23rd Annual
Conference on Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education. Boston University, Boston, MA.
Evered, L. J., & Schroeder, B. (1991). Counting with generating functions. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991
yearbook (pp. 143–149). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Fenton, W., & Dubinsky, E. (1996). Introduction to discrete mathematics with ISETL. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Fischbein, E., & Gazit, A. (1988). The combinatorial solving capacity in children and adolescents. ZDM, 5, 193–198.
Fischbein, E., & Grossman, A. (1997). Schemata and intuitions in combinatorial reasoning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 34, 27–47.
Fischbein, E., Pampu, I., & Manzat, I. (1970). Effects of age and instruction on combinatory ability in children. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 40,
261–270.
Gatza, A. M. (2018). Investigating racism with mathematics: Racial bias, quadratics, and combinatorics. Paper Presentation at Annual Meeting of the National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Gatza, A. M., Tillema, E. S., & Burch, L. J. (2020). Achieving “strategic outcomes” for effective teaching: Using discrete mathematics to develop content knowledge,
design curriculum, and address issues of equity. Symposium at the American Mathematics Teacher Educators Annual Conference, Phoenix, AZ.
Godino, J., Batanero, C., & Roa, R. (2005). An onto-semiotic analysis of combinatorial problems and the solving processes by university students. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 60, 3–36.
Gutiérrez, R. (2008). A “gap-gazing” fetish in mathematics education? Problematizing research on the achievement gap. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
39(4), 357–364.
Hadar, N., & Hadass, R. (1981). The road to solving a combinatorial problem is strewn with pitfalls. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12(4), 435–443.
Halani, A. (2012). Students’ ways of thinking about enumerative combinatorics solution sets: The odometer category. Electronic Proceedings for the Fifteenth Special
Interest Group of the MAA on Research on Undergraduate Mathematics Education (pp. 59–68).
Hart, E. W., & Sandefur, J. (Eds.). (2017). Teaching and learning discrete mathematics worldwide: Curriculum and research. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International
Publishing.
Hickmott, D., Prieto-Rodriguez, E., & Holmes, K. (2018). A scoping review of studies on computational thinking in K-12 mathematics classrooms. Digital Experiences in
Mathematics Education, 4, 48–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s4075-017-0038-8.
Höveler, K. (2017). Children’s combinatorial counting strategies and their relationship to conventional mathematical counting principles. In E. W. Hart, & J. Sandefur
(Eds.). Teaching and learning discrete mathematics worldwide: Curriculum and research (pp. 81–92). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Hurdle, Z., Warshauer, M., & White, A. (2016). The place and purpose of combinatorics. Mathematics Teacher, 110(3), 216–221.
Kapur, J. N. (1970). Combinatorial analysis and school mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 3(1), 111–127.
Kavousian, S. (2008). Enquiries into undergraduate students’ understanding of combinatorial structures. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Vancouver, BC: Simon Fraser
University.
Kenney, M. J., & Hirsch, C. R. (Eds.). (1991). Discrete mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991 yearbook. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Lockwood, E. (2011). Student connections among counting problems: An exploration using actor-oriented transfer. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 78(3), 307–322.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-011-9320-7.
Lockwood, E. (2013). A model of students’ combinatorial thinking. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32, 251–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2013.02.008.
Lockwood, E. (2019). Using a computational context to investigate student reasoning about whether “order matters” in counting problems. In A. Weinberg, D. Moore-
Russo, H. Soto, & M. Wawro (Eds.). Proceedings of the 22nd annual Conference on research in undergraduate mathematics education (pp. 385–392). Oklahoma City,
Oklahoma.
Lockwood, E. (2014a). Both answers make sense! Using the set of outcomes to reconcile differing answers in counting problems. Mathematics Teacher, 108(4), 296–301.
Lockwood, E. (2014b). A set-oriented perspective on solving counting problems. For the Learning of Mathematics, 34(2), 31–37.
Lockwood, E., & Caughman, J. S. (2016). Set partitions and the multiplication principle. Problems, Resources, and Issues in Mathematics Undergraduate Studies, 26(2),
143–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/10511970.2015.1072118.
Lockwood, E., & De Chenne, A. (2019). Using conditional statements in Python to reason about sets of outcomes in combinatorial problems. International Journal of
Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-019-00108-2.
Lockwood, E., & Erickson, S. (2017). Undergraduate students’ initial conceptions of factorials. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology,
48(4), 499–519. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020739X.2016.1259517.
Lockwood, E., & Gibson, B. (2016). Combinatorial tasks and outcome listing: Examining productive listing among undergraduate students. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 91(2), 247–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-015-9664-5.
Lockwood, E., & Purdy, B. (2019a). An unexpected outcome: Students’ focus on order in the multiplication principle. International Journal of Research in Undergraduate
Mathematics Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-019-00107-3.
Lockwood, E., & Purdy, B. (2019b). Two undergraduate students’ reinvention of the multiplication principle. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 50(3),
225–267.
Lockwood, E., & Reed, Z. (2018). Reinforcing mathematical concepts and developing mathematical practices through combinatorial activity. In E. W. Hart, & J.
Sandefur (Eds.). Teaching and learning of discrete mathematics worldwide: Curriculum and research (pp. 93–110). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Lockwood, E., & Reed, Z. (2020). Using an actor-oriented perspective to explore one undergraduate student’s repeated reference to a particular problem in a com-
binatorial context (In press) In C. Hohensee, & J. Lobato (Eds.). Transfer of learning: Progressive perspectives for mathematics education and related fields.
Lockwood, E., Reed, Z., & Caughman, J. S. (2017). An analysis of statements of the multiplication principle in combinatorics, discrete, and finite mathematics
textbooks. International Journal of Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 3(3), 381–416. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-016-0045-y.
Lockwood, E., Swinyard, C. A., & Caughman, J. S. (2015). Patterns, sets of outcomes, and combinatorial justification: Two students’ reinvention of counting formulas.
International Journal of Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 1(1), 27–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-015-0001-2.
Lockwood, E., Wasserman, N. H., & McGuffey, W. (2018). Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different categories of combination problems?
International Journal of Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 4(2), 305–322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-018-0073-x.
Lockwood, E., Reed, Z., & Erickson, S. (2020). Undergraduate students’ combinatorial proof of binomial identities (In press) Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education.
Lockwood, E., Caughman, J. S., & Weber, K. (2020). An essay on proof, conviction, and explanation: Multiple representation systems in combinatorics. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 103, 173–189.
Lockwood, E., De Chenne, A., & Valdes-Fernandez, S. (2019). Affordances of solving counting problems in a computational environment. In M. Graven, H. Venkat, A.
Essien, & P. Vale (Vol. Eds.), Proceedings of the 43rd Conference of the International group for the psychology of mathematics education: Vol 3, (pp. 41–48). Pretoria,
South Africa: PME.
Lockwood, E., DeJarnette, A. F., & Thomas, M. (2019). Computing as a mathematical disciplinary practice. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 54. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jmathb.2019.01.004.
Maher, C. A., & Martino, A. M. (1996a). The development of the idea of mathematical proof: A 5-year case study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(2),
194–214.
Maher, C. A., & Martino, A. M. (1996b). Young children inventing methods of proof: The gang of four. In L. Steffe, P. Nesher, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.).
Theories of mathematical learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Maher, C. A., & Speiser, R. (1997). How far can you go with block towers? The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 16(2), 125–132.
Maher, C. A., & Yankelewitz, D. (2011). Representations as tools for building arguments. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and

14
E. Lockwood, et al. Journal of Mathematical Behavior 59 (2020) 100783

reasoning: Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (pp. 17–26). New York: Springer.
Maher, C. A., Powell, A. B., & Uptegrove, E. B. (Eds.). (2011). Combinatorics and reasoning: Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms. New York: Springer.
Maher, C. A., Sran, M. K., & Yankelewitz, D. (2011). Making pizzas: Reasoning by cases and by recursion. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.).
Combinatorics and reasoning: Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (pp. 59–72). New York: Springer.
Mamona-Downs, J., & Downs, M. (2004). Realization of techniques in problem solving: The construction of bijections for enumeration tasks. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 56, 235–253.
Martin, D. B. (2015). Comments prepared for turning the Common core into reality in every classroom plenary session. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics 2015
Research Conference: Boston, MA.
Martin, G. E. (2001). The art of enumerative combinatorics. New York: Springer.
Mazur, D. R. (2010). Combinatorics: A guided tour. Washington, DC: MAA.
Miller, D. W. (1991). Discovering and applying Euler’s formula. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete mathematics across the curriculum, K-12: 1991 yearbook
(pp. 96–103). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Muter, E. M., & Maher, C. A. (1998). Recognizing isomorphism and building proof: Revisiting earlier ideas. Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the North
American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 461–467).
Muter, E. M., & Uptegrove, E. B. (2011). Representations and connections. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and reasoning:
Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (pp. 105–120). New York: Springer.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1980). An agenda for action: Recommendations for school mathematics of the 1980s. Reston, VA: Author.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989). Curriculum and evaluation standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1991). Professional standards for teaching mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, & Council of Chief State School Officers (2010). Common core standards for mathematics.
Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (1996). Children doing mathematics. Oxford, United Kingdom: Blackwell Publishers.
Papert, S. (1990). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York, NY: Perseus.
Pei, C., Weintrop, D., & Wilensky, U. (2018). Cultivating computational thinking practices and mathematical habits of mind in lattice land. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 20(1), 75–89. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2018.1403543.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1975). The Origin of the Idea of Chance in Children. New York: W. W. Norton & Company Inc.
Powell, A. B. (2011). Block towers: Co-construction of proof. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and reasoning: Representing, justifying
and building isomorphisms (pp. 97–104). New York: Springer.
Powell, A. B., & Maher, C. A. (2003). Heuristics of twelfth graders building isomorphisms. In N. A. Pateman, B. J. Dougherty, & J. T. Zilliox (Vol. Eds.), Proceedings of
the 2003 joint meeting of PME and PMENA: Vol. 4, (pp. 23–30). Honolulu: CRDG, College of Education, University of Hawai’i.
Reed, Z., & Lockwood, E. (2018). Generalizing in combinatorics through categorization. In A. Weinberg, C. Rasmussen, J. Rabin, M. Wawro, & S. Brown (Eds.).
Proceedings of the 21st annual Conference on research in undergraduate mathematics education (pp. 311–325). San Diego, CA: San Diego State University.
Rosenstein, J. G., Franzblau, D. S., & Roberts, F. S. (Vol. Eds.), (1997). DIMACS series in discrete mathematics and theoretical computer science: Discrete mathematics in the
schools: Vol. 36American Mathematical Society and National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Schielack, J. F. (1991). Primary experiences in learning what (as well as how) to count. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete mathematics across the curriculum,
K-12: 1991 yearbook (pp. 44–50). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Sinclair, N., & Patterson, M. (2018). The dynamic geometrisation of computer programming. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 20(1), 54–74. https://doi.org/10.
1080/10986065.2018.1403541.
Spangler, D. A. (1991). The pigeonhole principle: A counting technique for the middle grades. In M. J. Kenney, & C. R. Hirsch (Eds.). Discrete mathematics across the
curriculum, K-12: 1991 yearbook (pp. 55–58). Reston, VA: National Council of the Teachers of Mathematics.
Speiser, R. (2011). Block towers: From concrete objects to conceptual imagination. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and reasoning:
Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (pp. 73–88). New York: Springer.
Sriraman, B., & English, L. D. (2004). Combinatorial mathematics: Research into practice. Mathematics Teacher, 98(3), 182–191.
Tarlow, L. D. (2011). Pizzas, towers, and binomials. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and reasoning: Representing, justifying and
building isomorphisms (pp. 121–132). New York: Springer.
Tarlow, L. D., & Uptegrove, E. B. (2011). Block towers: Co-construction of proof. In C. A. Maher, A. B. Powell, & E. B. Uptegrove (Eds.). Combinatorics and reasoning:
Representing, justifying and building isomorphisms (pp. 97–104). New York: Springer.
Tillema, E. S. (2011). Students’ combinatorial reasoning: The multiplication of binomials. Paper presented for the 33rd annual meeting of the North American chapter of the
international group for the psychology of mathematics education in North America. Reno, Nevada: University of Nevada.
Tillema, E. S. (2013). A power meaning of multiplication: Three eighth graders’ solutions of Cartesian product problems. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32,
331–352.
Tillema, E. S. (2014). Students’ coordination of lower and higher dimensional units in the context of evaluating sums of consecutive whole numbers. The Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 36, 51–72.
Tillema, E. S. (2018). An investigation of 6th graders’ solutions of combinatorics problems and representation of these problems using arrays. The Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 52, 1–20.
Tillema, E. S. (2020). Students’ solution of arrangement problems and their connection to Cartesian product problems (in press) Mathematical Thinking and Learning.
Tillema, E. S., & Burch, L. J. (2020). Leveraging combinatorial and quantitative reasoning to support the generalization of advanced algebraic identities. Invited paper
presentation at the international congress on mathematical education to the topic study group in the teaching and learning of discrete mathematics in Shanghia, China.
Tillema, E. S., Gatza, A., Tillema, E. S., & Gatza, A. M. (2016). A quantitative and combinatorial approach to non-linear meanings of multiplication. For the Learning of
Mathematics, 36(2), 26–33.
Tillema, E. S., Gatza, A. M., Tillema, E. S., & Gatza, A. M. (2017). The processes and products of students’ generalizing activity. Research report at the thirty ninth annual
meeting of the international group for psychology of mathematics education in North America. Indianapolis, IN: HAMTE.
Tucker, A. (2002). Applied combinatorics (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Vansco, Ö, Beregszaszi, E., Burian, H., Emese, G., Stettner, E., & Szitanyi, J. (2017). Complex mathematics education in the 21st century: Improving combinatorial
thinking based on Tamás Varga’s heritage and recent research results. In E. W. Hart, & J. Sandefur (Eds.). Teaching and learning discrete mathematics worldwide:
Curriculum and research (pp. 111–136). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Wasserman, N. H. (2019). Duality in combinatorial notation. For the Learning of Mathematics, 39(3), 16–21.
Wasserman, N. H., & Galarza, P. (2019). Conceptualizing and justifying sets of outcomes with combination problems. Investigations in Mathematical Learning, 11(2),
83–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/19477503.2017.1392208.

15

You might also like