Emotional Communication and Therapeutic Change - Wilma Bucci, William F. Cornell (Editor) - The New Middle Ages, 1, 2020 - Routledge - 9780367645618 - Anna's Archive
Emotional Communication and Therapeutic Change - Wilma Bucci, William F. Cornell (Editor) - The New Middle Ages, 1, 2020 - Routledge - 9780367645618 - Anna's Archive
Emotional Communication and Therapeutic Change - Wilma Bucci, William F. Cornell (Editor) - The New Middle Ages, 1, 2020 - Routledge - 9780367645618 - Anna's Archive
“A lot has been said and written on how the two contexts of our field—clinical
and experimental—can come together, but this book marvelously stands
out among the many attempts at exploring the interface between these two
contexts. Wilma Bucci goes directly into the heart of psychotherapy process,
and she does so in a truly interdisciplinary way: she looks simultaneously
from different perspectives such as psychoanalysis, cognitive psychology
and affective and social neuroscience. This is just what is needed, and the
theoretical parts come alive through many clinical vignettes. We also receive
a clear picture of the new developments of Wilma Bucci’s line of research
following her 1997 book, Psychoanalysis and Cognitive Science: A Multiple
Code Theory. This new book should be read by all those who are really
interested in the revision of psychoanalytic metapsychology and in the
scientific standing of psychoanalysis today.”
Paolo Migone, M.D.
Editor, Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane
(“Psychotherapy and the Human Sciences”)
www.psicoterapiaescienzeumane.it
“I have always had the utmost respect for Wilma Bucci’s thinking. I believe
it is important—even classic. So, despite the fact that she and I don’t always
agree, I am delighted to see this body of work brought together in a single
source. The field of psychoanalysis and, more broadly, cognitive and affective
neuroscience, need this collection. Here you will find statements of dual/
multiple code theory, for which Bucci is justly famous, as well as elaborations
and clinical applications of those views, including vivid case material. Bucci’s
highly significant work on dissociation—classic in its own right—is here too.
Psychoanalysts and their sympathizers should count their blessings that Bucci
has been there to represent them in the wider world of cognitive psychology
and neuroscience. This is a book with which every student of psychoanalysis
and neuroscience should be familiar.”
Donnel B. Stern, William Alanson White Institute, New York
Emotional Communication
and Therapeutic Change
In this book, Wilma Bucci applies her skills as a cognitive psychologist and
researcher to the fields of psychoanalysis and psychotherapy, opening up new
avenues for understanding the underlying processes that facilitate therapeutic
communication and change. Grounded in research geared to understanding
and demonstrating the clinical process (rather than the “outcome”) of ana-
lytic inquiry and therapeutic dialogue, Bucci’s multiple code theory offers
clinicians, researchers, trainers, and students new perspectives on the essen-
tial, often unlanguaged, foundations of the psychotherapeutic endeavor.
Understanding Psychotherapy
through Multiple Code Theory
Wilma Bucci
Edited by William F. Cornell
First published 2021
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2021 Wilma Bucci
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised
in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or
hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information
storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks,
and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Names: Bucci, Wilma, author. | Cornell, William F., editor.
Title: Emotional communication and therapeutic change: understanding
psychotherapy through multiple code theory / authored by Wilma Bucci;
edited by William F. Cornell.
Description: Abingdon, Oxon; New York, NY: Routeldge, 2021. |
Series: Relational perspectives |
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2020035687 (print) | LCCN 2020035688 (ebook) |
ISBN 9780367645601 (hbk) | ISBN 9780367645618 (pbk) |
ISBN 9781003125143 (ebk)
Subjects: LCSH: Psychotherapy. | Psychoanalysis. |
Psychotherapist and patient.
Classification: LCC RC480.B747 2021 (print) |
LCC RC480 (ebook) | DDC 616.89/14–dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2020035687
LC ebook record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2020035688
ISBN: 978-0-367-64560-1 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-0-367-64561-8 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-1-003-12514-3 (ebk)
Typeset in Times New Roman
by Newgen Publishing UK
Contents
List of figures ix
Editor’s preface: A cognitive scientist meets the couch xi
Acknowledgments: Building an interactive field xx
A personal note on theory and practice xxiv
Prologue: The need for evolution of the psychoanalytic
model xxvi
PART I
Evolution of the basic theory: Concepts and
contexts of multiple code theory 1
1 Symptoms and symbols: A multiple code theory of
somatization 3
2 The need for a “psychoanalytic psychology” in the
cognitive science field 21
3 The referential process, consciousness, and the sense of self 41
4 Symptoms and symbols revisited: Twenty years later 61
5 The power of language in emotional life 75
PART II
Clinical perspectives on emotional communication 99
6 Converging perspectives on emotional change in the
interpersonal field 101
7 The primary process as a transitional concept: New
perspectives from cognitive psychology and affective
neuroscience 126
8 The interplay of subsymbolic and symbolic processes in
psychoanalytic treatment: It takes two to tango, but who
knows the steps and who is the leader? 144
viii Contents
Index 223
Figures
This epigraph is taken from the chapter that begins this collection of Wilma
Bucci’s writing, which I read when first published in 1997 in an issue of
Psychoanalytic Inquiry devoted to exploring “Somatization: Bodily Experience
and Mental States”. I found this paper riveting and I noticed in the reference
list that Wilma had a book in press. I bought Psychoanalysis and Cognitive
Science: A Multiple Code Theory as soon as it came out, then contacted
Wilma, beginning what has proven to be a decades-long collaboration. It has
now been more than two decades since that first book, and we decided the time
was right to assemble a new book that gathered together many of her papers
written since then. The result, Emotional Communication and Therapeutic
Change: New visions of the “Talking Cure” Through the Lens of Multiple Code
Theory, is a collection of papers, revised lectures, and case discussions that
show a relentless, incisive, perpetually questioning mind at work.
Bucci’s multiple code theory has been a very timely arrival as efforts to
comprehend the presence and meanings of bodily experience have been emer-
ging in contemporary philosophy, psychotherapy, and psychoanalysis. Since
Freud, the verbal and symbolic order has been the primary means and vocabu-
lary of psychoanalytic treatment, but the reach and means of analytic inquiry
are now increasingly exploring visual, sensate, motoric, and visceral modes
of experience and expression within the bodies of patient and analyst alike.
Bucci titles her acknowledgments section “Building an Interactive Field,”
and she demonstrates throughout the chapters of this book her capacity to
build, question, and rebuild her models through her ongoing engagements in
a profoundly enriching interactive field of fellow cognitive and neuroscience
xii Editor’s preface
Bucci’s research steps out of the outcome-focused research models that have
come to pervade and pervert the functions of scientific inquiry into the psy-
chotherapeutic project and produce results that are eagerly promoted by
insurance companies, arguing that, “Comparing the outcomes of competing
theories is not useful if we do not identify the psychological mechanisms that
bring about the observed results” (Bucci 2013, p. 16). Bucci has stepped out
of the silos of preferred theories and efforts to prove that one is superior to
another. She asks a fundamental question: How can we understand and dem-
onstrate the means through which therapeutic change comes to be? Through
the evolution of her multiple code theory and the elucidation of the referential
process, Bucci has devoted herself to the study of therapeutic processes and
the identification of factors in psychodynamic therapies that foster change.
Consistent with a fundamental attitude in psychodynamic approaches to
therapy, the therapeutic work studied by Bucci is not focused primarily on the
alleviation or elimination of symptoms, but rather on grasping their meaning.
As Bucci stresses in her closing comments in Chapter 4:
Editor’s preface xiii
A major distinction that I hope I have made clear through this chapter
and that I want to emphasize particularly here is that symptoms may
operate as symbols—have symbolic functions—in the sense that their
expression may enable entry into a symbolic mode. In therapy, somatic
symptoms may provide a pathway to symbolizing emotional experience
that has been dissociated, particularly where other modes of expression,
such as memories, fantasies, and dreams, may not be accessible.
Virtually every aspect of the multiple code theory calls the adequacy of
manualized, cognitive-behavioral treatment into serious question for any
therapeutic goal beyond symptom relief and insurance reimbursement. Bucci
critiques the underlying assumptions of the theories underlying cognitive-
behavioral models of treatment and issues a challenge to clinicians of both
psychoanalytic and cognitive behavioral models to carry out research to iden-
tify and demonstrate potentially common factors that contribute to the effi-
cacy of varying methodologies:
the divergent models that had informed (and sometimes frustrated) my work.
Here was the demonstration of bodily experience—sensate and motoric—as
a form of psychic organization, as a means of coming to know and be known
by another. The subsymbolic domains are seen through the multiple code
theory as essential forms of psychic organization, as means of knowing and
learning, informing us about ourselves and others, consciously and uncon-
sciously. There is vast potential for understanding and emotional contact
when we open ourselves to how something is said to us, as well as how we
respond in pace, tone, postural shifts, facial expression, and so on. The mul-
tiple code theory provides a structure within which language and cognition,
so valued by Freud, and affect and the body, so valued by Reich, each have a
place, a value, and necessary functions through the interrelationship of three
fundamental forms of psychic organization: verbal symbolic, nonverbal sym-
bolic, and subsymbolic. Bucci began to recognize that the key to therapeutic
change was the gradual evocation of all three modes of experience within the
therapeutic process and their gradual linkage (the referential process) within
a psychodynamic relationship that is sufficiently emotional and personally
engaged.
In his classic book, Character Analysis, Reich insists that “the beginnings of
living functioning lie much deeper than and beyond language. Over and above
this, the living organism has its own modes of expressing movement which simply
cannot be comprehended with words (Reich, 1980, p. 359, italics in original).
Reich, in many ways foreshadowing contemporary neuroscience and parent–
infant research, grounded his therapeutic approach within the foundations of
the emotional and physical qualities of the mother–infant relationship and
the autonomic nervous system as they were known at that time.
Winnicott, in his emphasis on the developmental indwelling of the psyche
in the soma through the mother–infant relationship, also saw somatic experi-
ence as being at the heart of health and vitality:
Here is a body, and the psyche and the soma are not distinguished except
according to the direction from which one is looking. One can look at
the developing body or at the developing psyche. I suppose the word
psyche here means the imaginative elaboration of somatic parts, feelings,
and functions, that is, of physical aliveness … Gradually the psyche and
the soma aspects of the growing person become involved in a process of
mutual interrelation … At a later stage the live body, with its limits, and
with an inside and an outside, is felt by the individual to form the core for
the imaginative self.
(Winnicott, 1958, p. 244, italics in original)
For Winnicott, the infant discovers and elaborates the self through movement
(for which he created the notions of muscle pleasure and motility) through
their immersion in the subsymbolic realm:
Editor’s preface xv
So in every infant there is this tendency to move and to get some kind of
muscle pleasure in movement, and to gain from the experience of moving
and meeting something …What will quite soon become aggressive
behavior is therefore at the start a simple impulse that leads to a movement
and to the beginnings of exploration.
(Winnicott, 1984, pp. 93–94)
If the developing child feels increasingly free to release the body to its
being, to embody their subjectivity, they will develop a very peculiar
expression which we know as “sensuality.” This capacity to use the senses
is an acknowledgment of the body’s freedom of movement and the sen-
sual self has matriculated desire into gestural being. But sensuality is not
achieved by the self alone.
(Bollas, 1999, pp. 152–153)
Bollas infuses Winnicott’s properly British “good enough” with a vivid sense
of the eroticism and vitality of the forces of our early development.
Winnicott famously framed psychotherapy as a form of “play”:
Psychotherapy takes place in the overlap of two areas of playing, that of the
patient and that of the therapist. Psychotherapy has to do with two people
playing together. The corollary of this is that where playing is not possible
then the work done by the therapist is directed towards bringing the patient
from a state of not being able to play into a state of being able to play.
(Winnicott, 1971, p. 38, italics in original)
Here the linking and integrative functions of language within the referen-
tial process become crucial. The therapist’s words can become the means
by which the therapist and patient focus and deepen somatic, subsymbolic
experience. Quinidodoz (2003, p. 35), for example, describes her use of “incar-
nate language,” which she defines as a “language that touches as one that does
not confine itself to imparting thoughts verbally, but also conveys feelings
and the sensations that accompany those feelings” (emphasis in original).
Descriptive language on the part of the analyst—words often informed and
inflected by the therapist’s own somatic experience—is crucial here; such lan-
guage is experience-near, conveying a felt sense of one’s interior and somatic
states. Incarnate language is a way to speak to the patient’s body rather than
speaking about it, an avenue for the “sensualisation” of the emergent process.
Contemporary psychoanalysis has come to see and to articulate the cen-
trality of the therapeutic relationship in the vitality and efficacy of the
xviii Editor’s preface
As I read the posts on this forum, talk and write with colleagues
around the world, work with my consultation groups, and meet with my
clients—all now through virtual means—the questions and realities of
embodiment do indeed come to the forefront. I think the term “working
remotely” is far more accurate in capturing experiential reality than
“working virtually”.
We are, in fact, working remotely. We are not in the same room; we
are not in one another’s physical presence; and we are deprived of the
wealth of sensate, emotional, and nonverbal communications that
silently inform, enrich, and enliven our sessions (with a huge nod to
Wilma Bucci’s accounting of the place of subsymbolic experience in
the psychotherapeutic process). I hear (and myself experience) over and
over again the fatigue, exhaustion people experience working the “vir-
tual” realms all day long. It has given me new insights into the anxie-
ties and disconnections my younger clients experience when they spend
so much time with the misnamed “social” media. The screens create an
illusion of contact. The screens dominate our immediate experience with
Editor’s preface xix
two-dimensional, visual and vocal data. Our receptive tools and capaci-
ties are seriously diminished, and I think we are constantly consciously,
and unconsciously, trying to fill in the experiential gaps in our contact.
I often hear weary versions of, “It’s better than nothing.” But from
a somatic perspective, it is the areas of “nothing” that need to be
acknowledged. I have found it essential as these days of remote sessions
go on and on to not pretend that this is good enough, better than nothing.
I am finding it essential to acknowledge and inquire about the experiences
of absence, what is missing. This is an acknowledgement of elements of
our lived realities as we cannot be in close or physical contact with those
with whom we are working, with those we love who are now held at a dis-
tance. The experience of loss, anxiety, and grief in our sessions is a core
aspect of working somatically.
(April 8, 2020)
References
Bollas, C. (1999). The mystery of things. London: Routledge.
Bucci, W. (1997). Symptoms and symbols: A multiple code theory of somatization.
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 17, 2, 126–150.
Bucci, W. (2008). The role of bodily experience in emotional organization: New
perspectives on the multiple code theory. In F. S. Anderson (Ed.), Bodies in
Treatment: The unspoken dimension (pp. 51–76). New York: The Analytic Press.
Bucci, W. (2013) The referential process as a common factor across treatment modal-
ities. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 16, 16–23.
Cornell, W. F. (2001). There ain’t no cure without sex: The provision of a vital base.
Transactional Analysis Journal, 31, 233–239.
Cornell, W. F. (2015). Somatic experience in psychoanalysis and psychotherapy: In the
expressive language of the living. London: Routledge.
Quinidodoz, D. (2003). Words that touch: A psychoanalyst learns to speak.
London: Karnac.
Reich, W. (1980). Character analysis, 3rd edn. V. R. Carfagno (Trans.) M. Higgins and
C. M. Raphael (Eds.). New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Winnicott, D. W. (1958). Through paediatrics to psychoanalysis: Collected papers.
London: Karnac.
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing & reality: London: Tavistock.
Winnicott, D. W. (1984). Deprivation and delinquency. London: Tavistock/Routledge.
Acknowledgments
Building an interactive field
I have been doing this work for a long time and have had the great good
fortune of building close relationships with many people—clinicians and
researchers—whose collaboration and inspiration have been indispensable in
this work. This volume happened because my friend and colleague William
Cornell suggested that it was time for me to do a book that brought together
several papers, and because he offered to be the editor. I am extremely grateful
that he was willing to do this, not only because it is important to me to have
produced this book, but also because he is such an original thinker and elo-
quent writer that it seemed above and beyond expectations for him to be
willing to take on the role of editor of this volume. Bill’s work intersects
closely with mine in recognizing that subsymbolic bodily and sensory experi-
ence needs to be alive and present in the session and in the therapeutic rela-
tionship in order to bring about change; he is a master at knowing how to
bring about such activation in the consulting room. His characterization of
the therapeutic process from the perspective of somatic psychotherapy, and
his vivid clinical descriptions have provided validation for the ideas of mul-
tiple code theory and also opened new questions and ideas.
The evolution of multiple code theory has been interactive with the devel-
opment of methods of measurement from its beginning, but the project of
measurement took a giant leap when Bernard Maskit entered this work about
fifteen years ago. Bernie is a research mathematician who by now probably
knows more about psychoanalysis and psychotherapy than any mathemat-
ician ever has. Using some mathematical ideas, and also acquiring an enor-
mous amount of mysterious computer skills, he has developed the innovative
program that is the basis for our explorations into the psychotherapy process.
Bernie is the partner of my life as well as my work; following the theme of this
volume, his contributions transcend boundaries. He has brought his awesome
ability to think outside the box to our conversations about work and every-
thing else for the past 40 years and more.
There are many colleagues with whom I have collaborated and discussed
ideas over the years. Here I will just mention three people who were instru-
mental in developing the theory and research approach. Norbert Freedman
Acknowledgments xxi
was a mentor who became a colleague; his work in developing the interface of
theory and research in the clinical context was highly innovative and continues
to be influential. Erhard Mergenthaler introduced innovations in the com-
puter assessment of language style that are central to our research. Richard
C. Friedman’s broad and courageous vision of psychoanalysis as a living
theory that requires reexamination and revision provided inspiration, and his
personal encouragement provided support. I have also had the privilege of
supervising the research of many students who did their dissertations with
me at the Derner Institute of Adelphi University and elsewhere; I remember
each of them as a special collaborative relationship. Some of their work is
represented in this book; some is represented in other publications that are
referred to here and elsewhere. I wish I could thank each of my colleagues and
students, past and present, individually here.
I also want to mention several people with whom I actively collaborate
today. We work closely with Leon Hoffman, of the New York Psychoanalytic
Society and Institute (NYPSI), whose insights and creative energy are indis-
pensable to our thinking, our projects and our program of research, on many
levels. Also, through Leon’s efforts, NYPSI provides us with a base for our
work. With the resources of the Pacella Research Center, we have been able
to support several graduate students and postdoctoral fellows to participate
in the projects and we are beginning to establish a research presence in the
education of candidates. Here we are realizing at least a start at repairing the
split between the institutes and the universities that has hampered the devel-
opment of the psychoanalytic field from its early days.
Sean Murphy has been a central, indispensable member of our group; he did
his dissertation with me at Adelphi, graduating in 2012, and has been involved
in our research program ever since, continuously generating new ideas at the
interface of theory and practice, developing new projects, and spreading the
word. His grasp of technology is awesome and constantly expanding; he and
Bernie bring their different and complementary talents and skills to our col-
laborative research. Sean is also strongly, viscerally aware of the need for revi-
sion of theory and practice to meet current critical mental health needs. In
addition to working directly with us at NYPSI, and teaching research design
at the university level, he now has the position of data scientist at a nonprofit
service and advocacy organization. In this role, he is exploring the application
of features of the referential process to develop a protocol for responding
to calls on suicide hotlines. Sean’s work gives me the opportunity to add a
note of looking forward to the future of our theory and measures, opening a
broader direction of their application to urgent mental health needs.
The Clinician- Researcher seminar that we have established at NYPSI
has provided a new approach to repairing the split in the field. I thank Drs.
Wendy Olesker, Charles Jaffe and Christopher Christian for their invaluable
contributions in sharing their own treatments and their own perspectives.
Our Italian colleagues are adding a cross-cultural perspective and their own
xxii Acknowledgments
imaginative and creative ideas to this work; here I’ll mention particularly
Rachele Mariani, Attá Negri, Luigi Solano, and Marina Amore.
We are fortunate to include graduate and post-doctoral students as members
of our current research group at the NYPSI, including Karen Tocatly, who
has been central in our current project of developing a measure of the arousal
function of the referential process, and You Zhou and Xinyao Zhang, who
have worked closely with us on several theoretical and technical projects.
Mentioning You and Xin allows me to close these acknowledgments with
what feels like a necessary reference to the existential crisis in the world, the
widening plague that provides the context in which we now live, love, and
work. A colleague on a listserv noted that there was one word in Chinese for
both crisis and opportunity; I asked You and Xin whether they could fill in
the current usage of this word. Xin says: “the word that pops up in my mind is
”危机” (wei ji, pronounced “wei gee”), which usually means crisis in everyday
usage. But … if you split the word apart, 危 means danger/crisis and 机 means
opportunity … In this sense, perhaps a better matched English word is “crit-
ical time,” which equally implies urgency (i.e. a tiny difference might end up
with a big butterfly effect that is qualitatively different) with a more neutral
connotation. A similar word is “Kairos moment,” with a connotation of
criticality but more positive.” You Zhou says that the most frequently used
meaning of this word in China is
For me, this concept of wei ji provides a way to end these acknowledgments
with love and gratitude for my family, friends, colleagues and students, who
continue in these critical times to provide new turning points, new opportun-
ities, new tests, and new meanings to expand our explorations.
Earlier versions of the following chapters appeared elsewhere and are
adapted here by permission:
Chapter 3: The referential process, consciousness, and the sense of self (2002)
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22, 766–793.
Acknowledgments xxiii
I started thinking intensively about the need for a scientific study of psycho-
analytic ideas at a time when I had recently finished my graduate work in cogni-
tive psychology and psycholinguistics and had my first position at the Clinical
Behavior Research Unit at Downstate Medical Center. I was considering a
mixed career of clinical work, including psychoanalytic training, along with
research; I had been in the clinical program at the University of Michigan
a few years earlier. At Downstate, I had a few therapy patients, supervised
in a thoughtful and supportive way by the psychoanalytically oriented fac-
ulty there. They were the kind of very complicated, very difficult patients—
presumably not seen as suitable for analytic treatment—who were assigned to
inexperienced therapists in those years.
At the same time, I was a patient in psychoanalysis—lying on the couch,
in a fairly classical analysis, trying to following the basic rule: to say what-
ever comes to mind no matter how trivial or irrelevant it seemed, violating
normal conversational constraints, possibly insulting the person I was talking
to, causing pain and even shame for myself. I was asking myself how this pro-
cess was going to help me resolve the emotional and somatic difficulties that
had brought me to treatment. I assumed at the time that the practitioners of
the analytic treatment I was receiving had a clear scientific understanding of
the mechanisms underlying this process. But when I looked into the psycho-
analytic theory, there was nothing that answered my questions in the terms
I was looking for. The concepts were grounded in a theory of a century or so
earlier, the psychoanalytic metapsychology, sometimes as psychological and
sometimes as neurological concepts; and the process was far from being well
defined or amenable to systematic research.
Yet the process worked for me. I could actually feel the moments in the
treatment when something was changing; I gradually felt different within
myself, others saw me as different, I acted differently to some extent, people
responded differently to me. The modes of experiencing, thinking and relating
that opened up for me in psychoanalytic treatment have continued throughout
my life since then, as both life-saving on the one hand and endlessly fascin-
ating on the other. As a researcher, I saw the development and continuing
A personal note on theory and practice xxv
I began writing the introduction to this book in the summer of 2019, in the
context of almost three years of assaults on decency and humanity and on
our system of government, distinct from anything I had known before. Like
many others, I woke up each morning wondering what new horror would be
unveiled today. There were so many unthinkable events and they happened
so fast that whatever I read about one day was likely to be outdone by the
next day’s reports. And then came the late winter and spring of 2020, with its
plagues, floods, fires, protests, and continually deepening fears for our way of
life. This book is about a theory of emotional organization that addresses the
questions of how interacting and talking with another person can help to heal
the wide variety of emotional disorders that emerge in contexts such as these,
as in the other challenges of life, and what kind of interacting works best for
whom, when, and how. Related and new questions concerning the processes
of psychotherapy also need to be addressed when the threats to humanity
come ex machina, as has happened in the pandemic of the late winter and
spring of 2020.
In August, 2019, I read how the Trump administration was moving to block
immigrants who may need government aid:
Cuccinelli actually said, when asked about the poem by Emma Lazarus
inscribed on the Statue of Liberty, that a version of the poem can still
apply: ‘give me your tired, your poor—who can stand on their own two feet
and who will not become a public charge.’
Also in August 2019, a few days after two horrendous massacres in El
Paso and Dayton, the FBI along with local police departments arrested three
young men in their early twenties for allegedly making mass shooting threats.
Referring to one of these men, the Florida sheriff who made the arrest said,
“When you look at this kid’s background, he is the profile of a shooter. He lost
his job, he lost his girlfriend, he’s depressed, he’s got the ammunition and he
wants to become known for being the most prolific killer in American history.”
This was also the summer in which Jeffrey Epstein, the multi-millionaire
who was charged with operating a sex trafficking ring with teenagers, who had
socialized with Donald Trump, Bill Clinton, and Prince Andrew, and who
owned mansions and islands around the world, hung himself in a vermin-
infested cell in a Manhattan prison.
During this same summer, a new and brilliantly designed city playground
opened on our block. The area is designed with flexible equipment and zones
that allow people to create their own games. The playground teems with chil-
dren with their assorted caretakers—nannies, mommies, daddies and others.
We pass there on most days; in the several minutes we stand there, we observe
uncountable dramas that refute Tolstoy’s assertion that all happy families are
alike—or Aristotle’s earlier version that men are good in one way but bad in
many. One day, a little child—barely walking—noticed a still smaller barefooted
baby in a stroller, became fascinated with the baby’s feet and began to play
with them. The baby was at first a bit taken aback, then began to participate
actively in the play with these interesting objects. The caretakers exchanged
smiles, one checking that this was alright with the other responsible for the
little toes. Around the same time, another child started crying and three speed
racers of about two to three years old on little vehicles and wearing helmets
stopped briefly to look on with expressions of concern. These and many other
different dramas occur so constantly that it is difficult to stop watching. The
playground closed for several months in the spring of 2020 as the city worked
to contain the virus, but it is open again now, with the masks and hand sani-
tizer that characterizes New York City’s social experience today.
It seems to me that the various fields of psychology and psychotherapy
have important roles to play in addressing the patterns of shame and grandi-
osity and hatred and fear of the other that underly the horrors we are seeing
and experiencing, while also recognizing the varied possibilities of human
behavior. How could a little child who was delighted with a baby’s toes or
a two-year-old who was concerned about another child’s crying become
someone who needs to rape and kill?
In The Fire Next Time, James Baldwin (1962, pp. 81–82) refers to the role
of psychic change in the context of social conditions in this country:
xxviii Prologue
But in order to change a situation one has first to see it for what it is … To
accept one’s past—one’s history—is not the same thing as drowning in it;
it is learning how to use it. An invented past can never be used; it cracks
and crumbles under the pressures of life like clay in a season of drought.
How can the American Negro’s past be used? The unprecedented price
demanded—and at this embattled hour of the world’s history—is the
transcendence of the realities of color, of nations, and of altars.
Clearly it is not only the American Negro (using Baldwin’s term) for whom
such change is needed, but also the many people in the towns and countrysides
of America who are controlled by their fears of strange people from other
lands, or the young men who are driven to kill by their lack of power and
hope and love, and the many others—perpetrators and victims—about whom
we see and hear and read every day. Baldwin focuses on the need to transcend
the accepted categories of color, nations, and altars. He would probably have
referred as well to changes in the categories of gender and sexuality, but that
broadened perspective was not yet sufficiently accessible in 1962.
We can see a related process of transcending accepted categories in scien-
tific thought about the physical world. In Einstein’s general theory of rela-
tivity, the categories of space and time were redefined in terms of one another
as part of a single continuum, known as space-time. Einstein’s discoveries
concerning the interdependence of these dimensions opened a revolution in
the characterization of the physical world that continues today.
The chapters of this book focus on the need to revise or transcend accepted
concepts and categories in the fields of psychoanalysis and psychotherapy,
and on the need for a systematic theory of psychic organization and thera-
peutic change, rooted in current scientific work and able to be examined in
a research context. In the years since the publication of my 1997 book, the
development of multiple code theory has been informed by new directions of
investigation and exponentially growing advances in knowledge concerning
emotion, cognition and somatic functions, as well as by new recognition
of the inherent interconnections of these functions—within oneself and
with others. These advances have come from research in fields of cognitive
psychology and affective and social neuroscience. Advances in theory have
also come from the writings of clinicians based on their observations in the
therapy context.
We are quite used to seeing the two words mental and physical opposed
and would not quarrel with their being opposed in daily conversation. It
is quite another matter, however, if the concepts are opposed in scientific
discussion.
(Winnicott, 1954, p. 201)
xxx Prologue
Your body is part of your mind, not in some gauzy mystical way, but in a
very real biological way. This means there is a piece of your body in every
concept that you make, even in states that we think of as cold cognition.
(cited in Armstrong, 2019)
As Barrett (cited in Armstrong, 2019) also says, the brain must continu-
ally construct concepts that guide the body by integrating scraps of sensory
input with memories of similar experiences from the past; this can be seen
as a version of the multiple code concept of emotion schemas in the cogni-
tive science field. According to Barrett, creating this internal model of your
body in the world provides a basis for inferring the causes of the sensory
data that comes in through the sense organs, guides actions, and constructs
experiences.
Here it appears that current research, along with clinical observation, is
leading us to question accepted boundaries, not only between emotional and
mental functions, but also in relation to somatic ones. Based on this work, it
feels misleading to use the terms “emotion,” “mind,” and “body” separately.
The conceptual boundaries, like those of color, nation, and altar to which
Baldwin referred, need to be transcended and changed. Einstein referred to
a concept of space-time; Winnicott’s term “psyche-soma” comes close to
this—interpreting “psych” as incorporating what have been termed emotion
and mind.1 The multiple code concept of emotion schemas, Damasio’s (1994)
concept of dispositional representations, and the psychoanalytic concept of
drive all build on this network of functions.
In his terms, the infant–mother unit includes the “psyche-soma” of both. The
insights of Winnicott and many other clinicians have now been supported
Prologue xxxi
and extended by several decades of work on the mirror neuron system and
related processes.
The discovery of mirror neurons was important in providing a physical,
neurological basis for direct access to the experience of another person.
The discoveries began with the finding in 1992 that specific types of visuo-
motor neurons discharge both when a monkey executes a motor act and
when it observes a similar motor act performed by another individual, the
experimenter (Di Pellegrino et al., (1992). Since that time, there have been
many studies providing evidence—not surprisingly—for the operation of
mirror neurons in humans as in other primates, and many studies examining
their anatomy and their impact on social interaction. Research in the area
of social neuroscience with human subjects has provided evidence for the
role of the mirror mechanism in the integration and control of emotion at
all levels of experience and thought. As the neuroscientist Keysers (2011,
p. 104) writes:
without the physical feeling of thrill we sense when our thinking leads to
success, I doubt we would care to think at all. Our mind is grounded in
our bodies. Through the discovery of shared circuits, the body becomes
central not only to our own emotional lives but also to the exchanges
between our minds. To understand the actions of other individuals, we
need to map them onto our own body’s motor programs. To understand
their emotions, we need to map them onto our own visceral feelings.
The new findings on the integration of emotion and cognition, and on the
development of the “psyche-soma” can be applied as well to the processes of
communication between self and other. The implications are potentially great
for understanding how humans function in an interpersonal world, and the
struggles they confront, and for an understanding of how psychotherapy can
intervene to bring about change.
at any given moment, that can be worked on directly and intentionally, and
that we call “conscious.” The nature of the factors that determine the entry of
particular information into working memory at any given time has been, and
continues to be, studied intensively by cognitive psychologists.
It is very interesting and somewhat remarkable that Freud (1915, p. 167)
made an essentially similar point:
The “small content” to which Freud refers can be seen as conceptually equiva-
lent to the current concept of working memory or attention.
Freud then went on to struggle with the distinction between being con-
scious and unconscious in a number of—manifestly contradictory—ways. On
the one hand he says (1915, p. 177):
The core processes of the system Ucs. (the processes that Freud later
called ‘id’) are not unconscious. The id is the fount of consciousness, and
consciousness is primarily affective.
As I point out throughout this volume, we can account for the psycho-
therapy process more directly by focusing on the forms of experience,
including subsymbolic bodily and sensory forms, rather than on level of con-
sciousness. This approach then helps to address the question of when and
how different aspects of experience come into and out of focus in the thera-
peutic interaction for both participants and the implications of these shifts in
focus for therapeutic change.
Prologue xxxiii
Behind all these uncertainties, however, there lies one new fact, whose dis-
covery we owe to psychoanalytic research. We have found that processes
in the unconscious or in the id obey different laws from those in the pre-
conscious ego … In the end, therefore, the study of psychical qualities has
after all proved not unfruitful.
chapters in both sections of this volume address the nature of the therapeutic
communication from different perspectives.
The multiple code theory adds to this formulation the perspective on the
systematic nature and organization of experience that “has not yet been
actualized in symbolic form,” existing alongside of symbolic nonverbal and
verbal forms, in consciousness as well as out of awareness.
Emotion schemas
All our knowledge of ourselves and our worlds is based on the formation
of memory schemas; organized representations of knowledge of all types
that are activated and altered by new experience, and that determine how
new experience is perceived. The concept of memory schemas, introduced by
Bartlett (1932), has been a central concept in memory research since that time.
Emotion schemas, as defined in multiple code theory, are types of memory
schemas formed through the repeated occurrence of a set of subsymbolic
sensory, somatic, and motoric processes in relation to certain events of life.
I have termed these clusters of subsymbolic experiences the “affective core”
of the schema. Emotion schemas differ from other memory schemas in two
respects: the dominance of the subsymbolic processes of the affective core;
and the central role of interpersonal interactions in the formation of the
schemas.
The child feels a conglomerate of bodily experiences when her mother
abuses her verbally or physically; she probably does not call her feelings shame
or anger or terror, but she registers the painful experiences in relation to her
mother in her memory schemas. In her mind, and in her memory, she may then
also turn away from the image of her mother as the source of the painful acti-
vation, to avoid acknowledgment that her caretaker upon whom she depends
for life is also the source of danger to her life. She may also seek another
source, in other people or in herself, to account for the painful feelings. The
xxxvi Prologue
I will use the term “reverie” … to refer to the analyst’s (or the analysand’s)
day-dreams, fantasies, ruminations, bodily sensations and so on, which
I view as representing derivatives of unconscious, intersubjective
constructions that are jointly, but asymmetrically, generated by the ana-
lytic pair. These intersubjective constructions, which I have termed “the
analytic third” … are a principal medium through which the unconscious
of the analysand is brought to life in the analytic relationship.
Prologue xxxvii
Patient Therapist
But the shocking concept that Bion reveals to us, as I have said time and
again, is that there is a process that continuously transforms the data that
we receive from reality, so that reality is continuously being transformed
Prologue xxxix
into a movie sequence within our mind. This process comes to life in a
still largely unknown way … we continuously transform the sensory flow,
the flow of stimuli into a sequence of pictograms, into a dream sequence
unknown to us.
For many reasons, we are not yet in a place where the two contexts—clinical
and experimental—can come together readily, but we can work toward such
connections.
These conditions and views are well documented, and impose a responsibility
and a challenge for research. With a systematic theory, and the measures
xlii Prologue
Notes
1 The term “psychic” that Freud used throughout his writings—as in “psychic
apparatus,” “psychic determinism”—which might be interpreted as referring to
this general structure—has changed its meaning since his time, now referring to
supernatural functions or powers, which is the opposite of what I mean.
2 Here I note that both “attention” and “working memory” are psychological
constructs, whose definitions, like those of most psychological constructs, are in a
state of development and flux.
3 Earlier versions of the diagram were presented in the paper “Pathways of Emotional
Communication,” based on the work of Ogden, Bollas, Arlow and others, and with
somewhat different features in Bucci (2009); neither of these papers is included
in this volume. The diagram has subsequently been revised through presentations
and discussions with colleagues; the version presented here has not previously been
published.
4 This utterance is paraphrased from a treatment contained in the referential process
data base.
Prologue xliii
References
Armstrong, K. (2019). Interoception: How we understand our body’s inner sensations.
APS Observer, 32(8). Retrieved from www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/
interoception-how-we-understand-our-bodys-inner-sensations
Baddeley, A. D. (1994). Working memory: The interface between memory and cogni-
tion. In D. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 351–367). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Baldwin, J. (1962). The fire next time. New York: Vintage Books.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Bateman, A. & Fonagy, P. (2004) Mentalization-based treatment for borderline person-
ality disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bollas, C. (1995). Cracking up: The work of unconscious experience. London: Routledge.
Bower, G. H. (1970). Analysis of a mnemonic device. American Scientist, 58, 496–510.
Bucci, W. (1985). Dual coding: A cognitive model for psychoanalytic research. Journal
of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 33(3), 571–607.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (2009). The sleeping analyst, the waking dreams: Commentary on papers by
Richard Chefetz and David Mark. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 19, 415–425.
Damasio, D. C. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain.
New York: Avon Books.
Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Gallese V., & Rizzolatti G. (1992).
Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain
Research, 91, 176–180.
Ferro, A., & Nicoli, L. (2017). The new analyst’s guide to the galaxy: Questions about
contemporary psychoanalysis. A. Bompani (Trans.). London: Karnac.
Freud, S. (1915). The unconscious. SE, 14, 166–204.
Freud, S. (1938). An outline of psycho-analysis. SE, 23, 139–208.
Jones, E. (1946). A valedictory address. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
27, 7–12.
Keysers, C. (2011). The empathic brain. Cambridge, MA: Social Brain Press.
Ogden, T. H. (1999). The music of what happens in poetry and psychoanalysis.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 80, 979–994.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston.
Paivio, A. (1978). A dual coding approach to perception and cognition. In H. L. Pick
& E. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 39–52).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 9, 148–158.
Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human
amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53.
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, E., Schlicht, T., &
Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 36, 393–462
Solms, M. (2013). The conscious id. Neuropsychoanalysis, 15(1), 5–19.
newgenprepdf
xliv Prologue
Stern, D. B. (2013). Field theory in psychoanalysis, Part 2: Bionian field theory and
contemporary interpersonal/relational psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Dialogues,
23, 630–645.
Winnicott, D. W. (1954). Mind and its relation to the psyche-soma. British Journal of
Medical Psychology, 27(4), 201–209.
Winnicott, D. W. (1960). The theory of the parent–infant relationship. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 41, 585–595.
Trotta, D., & Rosenberg, M. (2019). New Trump rule targets poor and could cut legal
immigration in half, advocates say. Reuters. Retrieved from www.reuters.com/art-
icle/us-usa-immigration-benefits/new-trump-rule-targets-poor-and-could-cut-legal-
immigration-in-half-advocates-say-idUSKCN1V219N?il=0
Part I
The interaction of psychic and somatic processes has been a central concern
of psychoanalysis from its initial formulations (Freud, 1895, 1900) to the pre-
sent day. In contrast to Freud’s time, the interaction of emotion and somatic
illness is now also recognized in the medical field. It is not only the special
disorders identified as hysterias, nor even the medical entities traditionally
classified as psychosomatic, that are affected by such interaction; the field of
psychoneuroimmunology supports the view that
But behind all of these uncertainties there lies one new fact, the dis-
covery of which we owe to psychoanalytic research. We have learned that
processes in the unconscious or in the id obey different laws from those
in the preconscious ego. We name these laws in their totality the primary
process, in contrast to the secondary process which regulates events in
the preconscious or ego. Thus the study of mental qualities has after all
proved not unfruitful in the end.
(Freud, 1940, pp. 44–45)
The discovery that he saw as his first and major finding remains the fact
that he holds to most firmly at the end: the discovery of a mode of thought,
characterizing the unconscious or the id, which differs from the processes of
normal, rational, waking life.
The “dual/ multiple code” theory of emotional information processing
builds on this fundamental psychoanalytic solid ground. What we need to
understand—and what is really not so difficult to recognize —is that Freud’s
fundamental observations of two distinct modes of thought, their dynamic
interaction, and their interaction with somatic events do not entail the
assumptions of the energy model or the special assumptions of either the
first or second topographies and can be disembedded from these. The model
and the evidence supporting it have been discussed in detail elsewhere (Bucci,
1985, 1989, 1993, 1997) and will be outlined briefly here, focusing on issues
that are relevant to a new theory of somatization.
out, in a general and conceptual way, that two distinct formats of informa-
tion processing—both within the nonverbal system—are now being identi-
fied by cognitive scientists at a far more sophisticated model-building level
than ever before, and that in the subsymbolic formats, complex constructs
are being developed— really for the first time— which account systemat-
ically for the types of intuitive and implicit processing, involving visceral,
somatic, and motoric, as well as sensory, modalities, which are central to a
psychoanalytic model.
These subsymbolic processes also have their limitations. While such pro-
cessing is systematic, it is also highly specialized for specific functions. The
PDP models do not account for integration of subsystems in relation to
the overall goals or values of the organism in which they are implemented.
The symbolic processes of the nonverbal system fill this integrative and organ-
izing function (Norman, 1986).
The new multiple code theory thus expands Freud’s fundamental solid
ground to incorporate three—at least—rather than two basic systems of
thought: verbal versus nonverbal, and within nonverbal, symbolic versus
subsymbolic. By implication, the new model also emphasizes the crucial role
of connections among all these disparate systems and the corresponding
implications of failure of such connections.
basis of emotion (LeDoux, 1989). The multiple code formulation is also com-
patible, in part, with the definition of affects by Kernberg (1990) as incorpor-
ating symbolic representational, motoric, and visceral components; however,
it diverges from Kernberg’s inclusion of discharge phenomena within the
definition of affects and his corollary conception of affects as the “building
blocks” of drives (1990, p. 117). According to the multiple code theory, motiv-
ation is conceptualized in terms of the representational and directive proper-
ties of the emotion structures, independent of the particular source—internal
or external—of this activation and independent of energic notions based on
physiological need states.
Emotion structures may be activated by memory images or evoked by
language. Such activation—states of terror, loss or helplessness, pleasure
or desire—may then have physiological effects similar to the experiences
themselves. Any component of an emotion schema may be activated by any
other; images of persons, places, or objects may evoke somatic, as well as
behavioral, components of the schema or, conversely, be evoked by them.
In some cases, external stimulation may occur without consequent activation
of emotion schemas; in some cases, the emotion schema may occur in the
absence of apparent external cause or internal need. Any component of an
emotion schema, like any mental representation or process, may occur within
or outside of the focus of awareness. The dynamic unconscious, incorpor-
ating representations that are “warded off,” involves additional explanatory
factors, as will be discussed below.
In contrast, the referential connections for abstract and general terms such
as “truth,” “beauty,” “justice,” “postmodernism,” “epistemology,” derive
their meaning largely from connections to other words within the logical hier-
archies of language, and may be connected to nonverbal representations only
indirectly—if at all—through connections within the verbal hierarchies to
concrete and specific words. That is why it is useful to give examples when
presenting abstract material; it is also why intellectualization by patients—or
analysts—leaves the nonverbal, emotional representations untouched.
Cognitive models have generally failed to consider the complexity and dif-
ficulty of the referential process. Standard views of cognitive development
(Piaget, 1950; Bruner, 1966) have also failed to recognize the continuing
role of nonverbal processing, including emotional information processing,
throughout life. In both of these developmental theories, it is assumed that
earlier stages of concrete sensory and motoric processing drop out when
levels of formal, logical processing are attained. These standard approaches
to cognition must fail as the basis for a psychoanalytic theory, as Noy (1979,
p. 170) points out:
Evidence for dual or multiple coding and the referential process has been
developed in experimental cognitive psychology, in neuropsychology, and in
our own experimental, clinical, and psychotherapy research, as summarized
elsewhere (Bucci, 1984, 1985, 1988, 1989, 1993; Bucci & Miller 1993; Paivio,
1986). Recent research on cerebral lateralization and modularity of function
by Gazzaniga (1985), Kosslyn (1987), and Farah (1984) supports the new
multicomponent formulation; the new work takes us well beyond a simple
bicameral left brain/right brain dichotomy. Thus the underlying neuro-
physiological substrate for emotional information processing and the ref-
erential process would include activation of analogic and global nonverbal
representations, which are dominant in the right hemisphere; connections
across the corpus collosum to the more discrete, “nameable” images that we
now find to be associated with the left hemisphere, the primary site of sym-
bolic processing; mediating processes carried out by the image generating
10 Evolution of the basic theory
component within the left hemisphere; and connections within the left hemi-
sphere between discrete images and words.
rather than a means of warding off memory (Dodd & Bucci, 1987). The
basis for the fundamental rule of free association—that the apparently
irrelevant or trivial notions that may come into focus are actually outliers
of the warded-off schema that have escaped repression—may apply to such
specific external details, as to verbalization of viscerosensory experience.
The therapist may then make use of these small opportunities to open the
symbolic and interpersonal domains.
References
Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. (1988). The contribution of mother–infant mutual influ-
ence to the origins of self and object representations. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 5,
305–337.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. I. New York: Basic Books.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). On cognitive growth. In J. S. Bruner (Ed.), Studies in cognitive
growth (pp. 1–67). New York: Wiley.
Bucci, W. (1984). Linking words and things: Basic processes and individual variation.
Cognition, 17, 137–153.
Bucci, W. (1985), Dual coding: A cognitive model for psychoanalytic research. Journal
of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 33, 571–607.
Bucci, W. (1988). Converging evidence for emotion structures: Theory and method. In
H. Dahl, H. Kaechele, & H. Thomae (Eds.), Psychoanalytic process research strat-
egies (pp. 29–50). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bucci, W. (1989). A reconstruction of Freud’s tally argument: A program for psycho-
analytic research. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 9, 249–281.
Bucci, W. (1993). The development of emotional meaning in free association. In
J. Gedo & A. Wilson (Eds.), Hierarchical conceptions in psychoanalysis (pp. 3–47).
New York: The Guilford Press.
Symptoms and symbols 19
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science. New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W., & Miller, N. (1993). Primary process: A new formulation and an analogue
measure. In N. Miller, L. Luborsky, J. Barber, & J. Docherty (Eds.), Handbook of
dynamic psychotherapy research and practice (pp. 387–406). New York: Basic Books.
Dodd, M., & Bucci, W. (1987). The relation of cognition and affect in the orientation
process. Cognition, 27, 53–71.
Eagle, M. N. (1984). Recent developments in psychoanalysis: A critical evaluation.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Farah, M. J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential ana-
lysis. Cognition, 18, 245–272.
Fodor, J. A. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architec-
ture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.
Freud, S. (1895). Project for a scientific psychology. Standard Edition, 1, 295–391.
London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Standard Edition, 4 & 5. London:
Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. Standard Edition, 18, 12–66. London: Hogarth
Press.
Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psycho-analysis. New York: W. W. Norton.
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books.
Holt, R. R. (1985). The current status of psychoanalytic theory. Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 2, 289–315.
Hull, J., Ellenhorn, T., & Bucci, W. (1990). Attunement and the rhythm of dialogue in
psychotherapy: I Empirical findings. Paper presented to annual conference, Society
for Psychotherapy Research, Wintergreen, WV.
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Kernberg, O. F. (1990). New perspectives in psychoanalytic affect theory. New York:
Academic Press.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computa-
tional approach. Psychological Review, 94, 148–175.
Krystal, H. (1988). Integration and self-healing: Affect—trauma—alexithymia.
Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition &
Emotion, 3, 267–289.
Leventhal, H. (1984). A perceptual-motor theory of emotion. In K. R. Scherer,
& P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum,
pp. 271–291.
McDougall, J. (1989). Theaters of the body. New York: W. W. Norton.
Nemiah, J. C., & Sifneos, P. E. (1970). Affect and fantasy in patients with psycho-
somatic disorders. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in psychosomatic medicine, Vol.
2 (pp. 430–439). London: Butterworths.
Norman, D. A. (1986). Reflections on cognition and parallel distributed processing.
In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel
distributed processing (pp. 531–546). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Noy, P. (1979). The psychoanalytic theory of cognitive development. The Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child, 34, 169–215.
20 Evolution of the basic theory
I shall entirely disregard the fact that the mental apparatus with which
we are here concerned is also known to us in the form of an anatom-
ical preparation, and I shall carefully avoid the temptation to determine
psychical locality in any anatomical fashion. I shall remain on psycho-
logical ground.
(Freud, 1953a, p. 536)
Like the models in use in cognitive psychology today, Freud’s model of the
mind, the metapsychology, was constructed as an analogue to a physical
domain. The metapsychology was an attempt to account for psychological
concepts on the basis of the distribution of mental energy in the psychical
apparatus, using principles of Newtonian mechanics. The energic model was
retained in the structural as in the topographic theory. Although there are
important differences between these two theories, both assume that mental
energies derive from somatic sources, from the instincts or drives; that the
psychical apparatus is inactive until stimulated; that the building up of
instinctual energy produces unpleasure; and that mental activity is motivated
toward reducing this instinctual energy by discharging or binding it. Both
assume that language is associated with binding of energy and that nonverbal
functions are associated with the more primitive component of the appar-
atus: in the topographic model with the unconscious; in the structural model
with the id; and in both cases with the primary process of thought.
The failure of the energy model as a theory of biological systems has
been discussed in detail elsewhere (Bucci, 1997a; Eagle, 1984; Holt, 1985).
In general, the usefulness of theoretical models of mind depends on their fit
to the mental operations being modeled. As Holt and others have pointed
out, the human organism cannot usefully be construed as the kind of closed
system in which the principles of energy distribution, as postulated in the
metapsychology, might apply (Holt, 1989; von Bertalanffy, 1950). For this
and other reasons, many analytic theorists have advocated rejection of the
energy theory (Gill, 1976; Holt, 1976, 1989; Klein, 1976; Rubinstein, 1965;
Schafer, 1976). Unfortunately, in the process they have also rejected the
24 Evolution of the basic theory
It is obvious that its [the brain’s] behavior, and hence the behavior of an
organism, is determined not just by the logical machine that the mind
instantiates, but also by the protoplasmic machine in which the logic is
realized.
“conscious access to specific past events” (Squire, 1992, p. 210). Whereas con-
scious processing has previously been associated with intentional operations,
and unconscious processing with automatic functions (Posner & Snyder,
1975), processing outside of awareness has been shown to include intentional
and voluntary functions as well (Zbrodoff & Logan, 1986).
The pervasiveness and diversity of unconscious processes, as understood
today, require that the implications of the unconscious as a psychoanalytic
construct be reconsidered. The factors determining what is understood psy-
choanalytically as the systemic or dynamic unconscious, and the features
of such processing, need to be distinguished from the general modality of
processing outside of awareness. Beyond this, we may also find that it is
not the dimension of awareness or lack thereof that is most significant in
understanding psychic functioning, but the form and organization of thought.
This change in emphasis may be seen as a revisiting of the structural model in
a new light (Bucci, 2001).
From the perspective of cognitive science, we should also note an epistemo-
logical problem that was overlooked in Freud’s formulation of inference from
conscious to unconscious events. Analysts are directly aware only of their
own conscious experiences, the observations made through the medium of
their own perceptual systems. The patients’ conscious experiences, the sub-
jective representations and processes that occupy their awareness, are as
“unknowable” to the analyst directly as the contents of the patients’ uncon-
scious minds, and must themselves be inferred from their utterances and
behaviors. Here cognitive psychology has taken a more generalized and sys-
tematic step in the direction indicated by Freud, accounting for conscious as
well as unconscious mentation as occupying the same epistemological level
and as requiring similar inferential strategies.
I have examined your experimental studies for the verification of the psy-
choanalytic assertions with interest. I cannot put much value on these
confirmations because the wealth of reliable observations on which these
assertions rest make them independent of experimental verification. Still,
it can do no harm.
(cited in Grunbaum, 1984, p. 1)
Although Freud’s claims may appear somewhat cavalier, core aspects of his
methodological position remain sound. The need for naturalistic designs is
now increasingly recognized within the cognitive science field, again without
acknowledging the significance of psychoanalytic contributions in this
regard. Yuille (1986), Neisser (1976), and others have pointed to the inability
of experimental paradigms to study events as they naturally occur and the
distorted views of psychological processes that result. The need for natural-
istic designs is particularly evident where interpersonal issues and emotional
factors are involved.
The current emphasis on naturalistic designs may be seen, for example, in the
method of protocol analysis, an important tool in cognitive science research.
In this method, subjects are asked to give continuous verbal commentaries—
in effect, to think aloud—while solving problems or performing a variety of
tasks. In gathering the protocol, the exact wording of the instructions given
to the subjects may vary with the particular task, “but the simple instruction
to talk aloud while performing the task captures the essence”:
Several types of verbal reports may be generated using this procedure. The
simple instruction to talk aloud naturally while performing the task is most
30 Evolution of the basic theory
and emotional events, as they figure in a scientific theory, have the same status
as particles, the “big bang,” black holes, or life in the Bronze Age: all are
theoretical entities that cannot be observed directly and that have their exist-
ence defined in relation to other concepts and to observable events. From
its beginnings, psychoanalysis has been built on the interaction of sensory,
somatic, and emotional experience with cognitive and linguistic functions,
and psychoanalysis has gone beyond cognitive science in its recognition of
the multiple channels of experience and expression and the structure and
function of unattended thought. The psychoanalytic situation, with its fun-
damental rule and its controlled interpersonal setting, constitutes a unique
naturalistic research milieu for study of these questions.
On the other hand, the promise of psychoanalysis as a theory of mind and
a research milieu has not been fulfilled. While Freud’s goal was the devel-
opment of a theoretical model as a basis for the inference that is central to
psychoanalytic work, the necessary scientific procedures of theory develop-
ment and revision have not taken place. To demonstrate the contribution of
psychoanalytic concepts to the field of information processing, we need a the-
oretical framework that makes these concepts coherent and consistent, and
amenable to empirical investigation.
The multiple code model has been constructed as such a theoretical frame-
work, a general theory of emotional information processing that accounts
for adaptive as well as maladaptive functions and that may be applied to an
understanding of pathology and its repair in treatment. In the next section,
I briefly outline the application of multiple code concepts to some central
psychoanalytic ideas, and show how these applications may help to build a
bridge between psychoanalysis and cognitive science.
of oneself,” outside of the domain of the self over which one has intentional
control. Subsymbolic formats are dominant in emotional information pro-
cessing, as we shall see, and provide a systematic way to account for what we
know as empathy, intuition, and unconscious communication (Bucci, 2001).
In contrast to subsymbolic processing, symbols are discrete entities with
properties of reference and generativity. This means that they refer to entities
outside of themselves and may be combined to generate infinite varieties of
new forms. Symbols may be images or words.3 Language has been assumed to
be the primary medium of psychoanalysis (the “talking cure”), although it is
not the primary medium of thought and certainly not of emotion.
The three systems, with different contents and different organizing
principles, are connected by the referential links, which enable us to symbolize
and verbalize our emotional experience and to understand and resonate to
the words of others. Building on the work of (Paivio 1971, 1986), Kosslyn
(1987), and others, I have introduced the concept of the referential process as
the mechanism by which the multiple components of the human information-
processing system are connected (Bucci, 1984, 1997a). The basic mechanism
of the referential process, the mechanism of transformation from subsymbolic
information to nonverbal and then to verbal symbols, may be seen in parallel
form in the child’s development of the symbolizing function and the analytic
patient’s connecting of emotional experience to words. The infant forms an
image of mother on the basis of multiple ever-changing appearances, pro-
ducing an enduring prototypic image—we may say a memory schema—that
enables recognition of mother in the many varied contexts and forms in which
she appears; this enduring discrete entity can then be named. Similarly, the
analytic patient begins with arousal of subsymbolic emotional experience,
which is gradually connected to imagery and language. Prototypic images
and episodes constitute the lingua franca of the nonverbal representational
system, enabling the connection of multiple subsymbolic representations to
one another and to words.
Within the multiple code theory, emotions are defined as memory schemas
built up through repetitions of interactions with significant other people from
the beginning of life. The emotion schemas are represented as prototypic
events that share a common subsymbolic core of sensory, visceral, somatic,
and motoric experience. They incorporate our expectations of others and of
ourselves: how others will act towards us in particular circumstances, how
we are likely to act and react, and how we are likely to feel. One cannot dir-
ectly report the finely varying states of the subsymbolic components of the
schema, but one can describe instances of the prototypic events in which these
processes figure. In the narratives of such instantiations, the emotion schemas
can be told.
Within the emotion schema, any component that is activated has the poten-
tial to activate other elements, so that language or imagery may activate traces
of sensory or visceral experience or action, or the converse may occur. Like
34 Evolution of the basic theory
all memory schemas, the emotion schemas determine how one perceives
the world and are themselves changed by new perceptions. Like all memory
schemas, they may operate within or outside of awareness.
The formulation of the emotion schemas as memory schemas is built
on Bartlett’s (1932) early notion of memory schemas and is compatible
with current information-processing approaches to emotion theory (Lang,
1994; Scherer, 1984) as well as current research on the neurophysiology of
the emotions (Damasio, 1994; LeDoux, 1989). Stern’s (1985) concept of
representations of interactions that have been generalized refers essentially
to prototypic episodes as described here. The concept of emotion schemas is
also compatible with Kernberg’s (1990) definition of affects as incorporating
symbolic representational, motoric, and visceral components. Freud’s con-
cept of transference may itself be seen as a precursor of the concept of the
emotion schema:
however, turn attention away from the triggering imagery by distracting one-
self or redirecting attention in some way.
While avoidance may appear to control the emotional arousal, the individual
pays a high price. The painful subsymbolic sensory and visceral components
and tendencies toward action continue to operate, at least in trace form, but
now without emotional meaning and without capacity for symbolic regula-
tion. The individual may seek to find meaning—conscious or unconscious—
for the bodily activation: in some cases as having an independent somatic
source, as in somatization; in other cases as displaced to related but different
objects where the perceived connection does not threaten the self. When this
happens repeatedly, the emotion schema may then be reconstructed in this
dissociated or distorted form.
The occurrence of symptomatology and the imperviousness of patho-
logical schema to new experience may be accounted for on the basis of
the fundamental dissociation within the emotion schema and the distorted
attempts at repair. The response of avoidance is self-reinforcing: each time
the schema is evoked, the painful somatic and sensory experience is evoked
as well. In avoiding the people, events, or places associated with a painful
schema, in reality and in imagination, individuals then cannot take in poten-
tial new information about themselves and others; they cannot learn that the
dreaded expectations will not materialize in reality. The “vicious circle” of
pathology (Strachey, 1963) can be understood in these terms (Bucci, 1997a,
1997b, 2001).
verbalizing and symbolizing whatever they can: bodily feelings, vague images,
whatever comes to mind.
The conversion of the subsymbolic to the symbolic format operates first in
the nonverbal system. The patient thinks of an event, an image, a memory, a
dream, which may seem irrelevant but which is associated with the emotion
schema. The discrete images and episodes, including memories of the past and
events of the here and now, can then be translated into words and described
in narrative form.
The power of free association can be seen most clearly here. The apparently
trivial or irrelevant images and episodes that come to mind are likely to be
peripheral symbolic components of the emotion schema. These are permitted
into awareness even when the initial objects of the dissociated schema are
avoided—precisely because they are avoided—so that the patient does not
recognize the emotional meaning of what they say. The subsymbolic elem-
ents of the dissociated schema may be connected to words by this means. The
narrative of the connecting phase reveals the patient’s emotion schema as it
currently exists—as it has been retrieved from memory or played out in the
here and now. The power of the relationship may be seen here—in providing
both objects that enable the schema to be symbolized and an environment in
which the potentially unbearable feelings can be safely touched.
In the third phase, the patient, with the analyst, reflects on the images
and stories that have been told. The analyst may take the lead at this stage.
Optimally, new connections are made—within the patient’s emotion schemas
and between patient and analyst—which permit the cycle to begin anew at a
deeper level. Now the patient can begin to understand the emotional meaning
of her or his narrative in new terms.
Here is where the possibility of breaking the “vicious circle” is found. The
old story in a new interpersonal context is potentially a new story, not just a
retelling. The somatic elements of the activated schema occur in the session
in modulated form. The event is represented in a code that is shared; the tools
of logical differentiation and generalization can be intentionally invoked.
The connection of the displaced object to the activated memory schema can
be recognized; the differences in one’s own capacities and in the situation in
which the activation occurs can be recognized as well. The person of the ana-
lyst and the therapeutic context constitute prototypic imagery in the here and
now that may be newly entered into the schemas. The analytic relationship
potentially plays the same role in the reconstitution of the schema that the
caretaker and the earlier context played in its initial development.
Notes
1 This contrasts with the approach of the introspectionists (Titchener, 1915),
in which subjects’ verbalizations were taken at face value as constituting valid
representations of their own thought processes rather than as data from which
inferences may be made.
2 The type of processing that I term “subsymbolic” has features of “connectionist”
or parallel distributed processing systems based on properties of neural nets and
modeled by the mathematics of dynamical systems (Rumelhart, McClelland, &
PDP Research Group, 1986 ).
38 Evolution of the basic theory
References
Arlow, J. A. (1969). Unconscious fantasy and disturbances of conscious experience.
Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 38, 1–27.
Baars, B. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Bucci, W. (1984). Linking words and things: Basic processes and individual variation.
Cognition, 17, 137–153.
Bucci, W. (1985). Dual coding: A cognitive model for psychoanalytic research. Journal
of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 33, 571–607.
Bucci, W. (1989). A reconstruction of Freud’s tally argument: A program for psycho-
analytic research. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 9, 249–281.
Bucci, W. (1993). The development of emotional meaning in free association. In
J. Gedo & A. Wilson (Eds.), Hierarchical conceptions in psychoanalysis (pp. 3–47).
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (1995). The power of the narrative: A multiple code account. In J. Pennebaker
(Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 93–122). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Bucci, W. (1997a). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (1997b). Symptoms and symbols: A multiple code theory of somatization.
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 17, 151–172.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
21(1), 40–70.
Bucci, W., & Miller, N. (1993). Primary process analogue: The referential activity (RA)
measure. In N. Miller, L. Luborsky, J. Barber, & J. Docherty (Eds.), Psychodynamic
treatment research (pp. 387–406). New York: Basic Books.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain.
New York: Avon Books.
Eagle, M. N. (1984). Recent developments in psychoanalysis: A critical evaluation.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A
critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.
Freud, S. (1932). Third (revised) English edition of the interpretation of dreams.
London: Allen.
The need for a “psychoanalytic psychology” 39
Freud, S. (1953a [1900]). The interpretation of dreams. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 4
(pp. 1–627). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1953b [1895]). Project for a scientific psychology. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud
Vol. 1 (pp. 295–301). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1955 [1895]). Studies on hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The
standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 2 (pp.
3–305). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1958 [1912]). The dynamics of transference. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 12
(pp. 97–108). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1964a [1937]). Analysis terminable and interminable. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud,
Vol. 23 (pp. 216–253). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1964b [1940]). An outline of psycho-analysis. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud,
Vol. 23 (pp. 144–207). London: Hogarth Press.
Gill, M. M. (1976). Metapsychology is not psychology. Psychological Issues, 9,
71–105.
Grunbaum, A. (1984). The foundations of psychoanalysis. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Holt, R. R. (1976). Drive or wish? A reconsideration of the psychoanalytic theory of
motivation. Psychological Issues, 9, 158–197.
Holt, R. R. (1985). The current status of psychoanalytic theory. Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 2, 289–315.
Holt, R. R. (1989). Freud reappraised: A fresh look at psychoanalytic theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Jones, E. (1953). The life and works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 1. New York: Basic Books.
Kernberg, O. (1990). New perspectives in psychoanalytic affect theory. In Emotion:
Theory, research and experience (pp. 115–131). New York: Academic Press.
Klein, G. S. (1976). Psychoanalytic theory: An exploration of essentials. New York:
International Universities Press.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computa-
tional approach. Psychological Review, 94, 148–175.
Lang, P. J. (1994). The varieties of emotional experience: A meditation on James-
Lange theory. Psychological Review, 101, 211–221.
LaPlanche, J., & Pontalis, J.-B. (1973). The language of psychoanalysis. New York:
W. W. Norton.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and
Emotion, 3, 267–289.
McLaughlin, J. (1978). Primary and secondary process in the context of cerebral
hemispheric specialization. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 47, 237–266.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.
Norman, D. A. (1981). Perspectives on cognitive science. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Noy, P. (1979). The psychoanalytic theory of cognitive development. Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child, 34, 169–215.
40 Evolution of the basic theory
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. Solso
(Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–85).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rubinstein, B. B. (1965). Psychoanalytic theory and the mind-body problem. In
N. S. Greenfield, & W. C. Lewis (Eds.), Psychoanalysis and current biological thought
(pp. 35–56). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel
distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.
Schafer, R. (1976). A new language for psychoanalysis. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process
approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–
317). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Simon, H. A., & Kaplan, C. A. (1989). Foundations of cognitive science. In M. I. Posner
(Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 1–47). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with
rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.
Strachey, J. (1963 [1934]). The nature of the therapeutic action of psychoanalysis. In
L. Paul (Ed.), Psychoanalytic clinical interpretation (pp. 362–378). New York: The
Free Press.
Titchener, E. B. (1915). A beginner’s psychology. New York: Macmillan.
von Bertalanffy, L. (1950). The theory of open systems in physics and psychology.
Science, 3, 23–29.
Yuille, J. C. (1986). On the futility of a purely experimental psychology of cognition.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 467–471.
Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (1986). On the autonomy of mental processes: A case
study of arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 118–130.
Chapter 3
The premise of dual (or multiple) systems of thought has remained central to
psychoanalytic theory through its several transformations, while the nature
of the contrasting systems has repeatedly been redefined. In developing his
successive models of the psychical apparatus, Freud shifted from qualities
(conscious, preconscious, unconscious) to structures (id, ego, superego);
he then reunited these two forms of organization uneasily in his final sum-
mary formulation (Freud, 1940). The polarities of the primary vs. secondary
processes of thought, and verbal vs. nonverbal processes, have generally been
seen as related to both the qualities and agencies of mind. Correspondence
among these dimensions has typically been assumed as a “default” theoret-
ical position, thus the contents of the id are seen as unconscious, nonverbal,
and characterized by primary process thought, and the features of the ego as
the converse of these. Analysts also recognize that there may be unconscious
functions in the ego, that there are organized unconscious fantasies and sys-
tematic communication outside of the verbal mode, and that language may
appear in dreams and images in waking life, but they cannot account for these
observations using the metapsychology in any of its forms. Such inconsist-
encies constitute a dilemma for psychoanalytic theory and “embarrass” the
psychoanalytic methodology, as Arlow (1969) points out.
Elsewhere, I have introduced the systems of multiple code theory as
accounting for the same types of clinical observations that are covered in
psychoanalytic theory, but as explaining these within the more consistent
framework provided by cognitive science (Bucci, 1997). The multiple code
theory is based on processes and forms of thought rather than dimensions of
consciousness or agencies of mind. Using current work in neuropsychology,
this chapter extends the concepts of multiple code theory as these relate to
the qualities of consciousness as well as to issues of self-representation that
are central to the structural model. This permits elaboration of the clinical
implications of multiple code theory, particularly in relation to the develop-
ment of pathology and the process of repair in treatment and speaks, as well,
to some of the inconsistencies in the psychoanalytic concepts.
42 Evolution of the basic theory
on this theory, we can extend the concepts of the multiple code theory, par-
ticularly the notions of the referential process and the emotion schemas,
in relation to levels of consciousness and forms of self-representation. As
outlined in Table 3.1, Damasio defines three major categories of awareness
and three corresponding types of self-representation.
future, and the changing landscapes of one’s life. This is a complex biological
phenomenon with several levels of organization that evolve across the lifetime
of the organism. The self that is formed through extended consciousness and
that views the panorama of autobiographical memory is the autobiograph-
ical self. This is the traditional notion of self, linked to the ideas of iden-
tity and personhood, built out of the unique experiences that characterize an
individual’s life.
In neurological terms, the autobiographical self is a process of coordinated
activation and display of personal memories, based on a continuously
reactivated multisite network of dispositional representations. The images
that represent those memories explicitly are exhibited in multiple sensory cor-
tices, and they are held over time by working memory. The memories and
anticipations are treated as any other objects and become known to the simple
core self by generating their own pulses of core consciousness (Damasio,
1999, p. 221). Thus, to summarize, “Extended consciousness occurs when
working memory holds in place, simultaneously, both a particular object and
the autobiographical self, in other words when both a particular object and
the objects in one’s autobiography simultaneously generate core conscious-
ness” (Damasio, 1999, p. 222).
Having an emotion
In Damasio’s system, emotions4 are defined as essentially regulatory
mechanisms that operate typically in a nonconscious mode. They are specific,
well-orchestrated sets of biological responses that involve bodily and cogni-
tive functions, which are triggered by an initiating stimulus, and begin to solve
the problem triggered by the stimulus to maintain the organism within the
narrow homeostatic range capable of supporting survival. Functions related
to body state include autonomic activation, endocrine and other chemical
responses, immune system effects, and activation of muscles throughout the
body, including changes in body posture and movements of face and limbs.
Functions related to mental state include wired-in behaviors such as bonding,
nurturing, playing, and exploring; inhibition or enhancement of body signals
and alteration of their pleasant or unpleasant qualities; and changes in
mode of processing, such as speeding up or slowing down, or sharpening
or blurring of focus. These functions are based largely on the dispositional
representations that make up the proto-self.
Referential process and the sense of self 47
Many of the bodily functions are observable to others, regardless of the level
of awareness of the individual experiencing the emotional state: skin flushing
or becoming pale, muscles tensing in fear, or slumping in dejection. The cogni-
tive changes may also be observable, as in the actions associated with the wired-
in behaviors, or the racing of thought and speech in manic phases, and the
converse in depressive states. These responses may be recognized by an observer
even when the person in whom the state is activated is not aware of these effects.
Knowing a feeling
The public and physical indicators of the emotional state have an inwardly
directed and private face, which Damasio terms “feelings,” and which have
the potential to become conscious, to become known to the individual. The
knowing of feelings is the property of core consciousness, the agency of the
core self. The knowing may occur on several levels: as awareness of a bodily
change—heart pounding, mouth becoming dry, stomach tightening; as an
immediate eruption of anger, or terror, or desire, experienced as such, leading
often to immediate action, without reflection; or with broader meanings,
leading to more complex responses.
Knowing a feeling is the gateway to the emotion schemas, or dispositional
representations, of extended consciousness, the agency of the autobiograph-
ical self, built out of sensory and somatic components as these occur in the
multiple complex events of life. While the innate physiological components of
an emotion schema are shared across humans and other species as well, the
contents and contexts of the schema are unique, determined by the events of
each individual’s life.
Development of pathology
The dissociations within the emotion schemas may occur through a range of
developmental means. The somatic and sensory components of some events—
a rage directed at the caretaker, a reaction to a caretaker’s rage, a fear of aban-
donment and annihilation or, on other levels, the terrors of war or the pain
of a physical illness—may be experienced by a young child as potentially
overwhelming, even threatening their survival. One usually cannot control the
orchestrated playing out of the subsymbolic components of the schema. The
biological changes associated with the affective core—changes in heart rate or
blood pressure or respiration or skin response, or the racing or slowing down
of the mind—play out directly in response to a triggering event. Humans (and
presumably other organisms) may then resort to a variety of alternative means
to handle the arousal of intolerable affect—to prevent or reduce it in some way.
Attempts at avoidance
One possibility is avoidance. One can run away or turn away from the triggering
stimulus—as we move away when a snake crosses the path, or stay away from
other threatening situations when we can. One avoids the trigger, to avoid the
activation of the affective core. When the trigger is internal—when a thought
or memory emerges that begins to arouse the painful affective core—one can
attempt to avoid the pain by avoiding the thought. One turns attention away
from the activating image or memory, tries to stop thinking about it. In effect,
on some implicit mental level, one says to oneself, “Don’t go there,” and uses a
variety of means to try to stay away. Such avoidance may reduce the extended
consciousness of the painful event, and may reduce the core consciousness as
well. This may also in some cases actually reduce the activation of the physio-
logical and behavioral components of the schema—although they are likely
to continue to play out to some degree. The operation of avoidance in life and
in memory will interact:
broaden widely; one avoids not only the photographs, but also the room they
are in, and the apartment, and eventually the town.
In a couples therapy session, the wife moves to the far side of her chair, and
tightens and tenses her body when her husband complains about her lack of
sexual interest.
symbols in that they are discrete entities that categorize or chunk the analogic
representational field into finite units. They may also be “things in themselves,”
motoric or sensory components of the presently activated schema, rather than
representations of it, thus perhaps standing for the schema itself in a meto-
nymic rather than metaphoric way. The analyst’s presence, the experiences and
responses activated in the transference, enactments of all kinds, and certain
types of bodily experience may be early symbols or protosymbols of this sort.
In the couples therapy, the therapist notices the wife’s posture and movements
and says to her: “When he spoke just now, you moved away to the corner
of the chair, as far as you could, and clenched up as if you were protecting
yourself.” The movement as marked by the therapist became an early form
of symbol for the wife, activating core consciousness; she becomes aware
of her bodily position, where she is sitting and her inner state of tension.
This would mark the beginning of a perturbation in the flow of the habitual
emotion schema of abuse and fear.
In the couples therapy, the wife becomes aware that she is feeling afraid now.
She connects the way she is feeling in the session to the way she feels at home
Referential process and the sense of self 53
The wife, now in individual therapy, remembers related events in her early
life with her first family as well as in her marriage, and retrieves further
aspects of her schema of fear—that she feels powerless and alone, and that
being persecuted and abused is a way she has accepted to feel less alone.
As she recognizes her new interpersonal context and her own adult powers,
she gradually comes to respond differently—bodily and emotionally—to the
analyst and to her husband.
this activation, so as to repair the dissociation rather than to reinforce it, must
be addressed specifically for each patient.
The transference plays a unique role in this respect. The power of the trans-
ference is in the evocation of the patient’s emotion schema in relation to the
analyst in the here and now. The analyst may be recognized as “standing for”
other objects, but the persona of the analyst is a present factor that changes
the balance and context of the interaction. (To the extent that the change in
the emotion schema incorporates internalization of the new, real persona, the
question arises of whether transference can ever—or should ever—be fully
resolved.)
Notes
1 The term “subsymbolic” has been used widely in cognitive science to designate the
form of processing described here. This processing is subsymbolic in the sense of
underlying symbolic representations, in an information processing sense—that is,
subsymbolic formats may be “chunked” into units that are represented symbolic-
ally. The prefix “sub” may be misleading, however, as implying a lower or less com-
plex level of organization. The term nonsymbolic is more neutral and might have
Referential process and the sense of self 59
been a better choice to avoid any such implication. As is clear from my presentation
of multiple coding, I see subsymbolic information as more extensive and at least as
complex as symbolic forms.
2 The possibility of a fourth system that would be characterized as “verbal
subsymbolic” remains open to question. Paralinguistic aspects of language,
including pausing rhythms and intonation patterns, and aspects of the sound
of speech, as in onomatopoeia or more generally in poetry, may operate on a
subsymbolic level, as may emotional vocalizations (e.g. sighing, giggling), but the
words of language themselves appear to be intrinsically digital and discrete elem-
ents. The operation of a verbal nonsymbolic code needs to be explored further.
3 The dispositional representations as defined by Damasio operate on a different
explanatory level from the connectionist notions of neural networks. Dispositional
representations are defined as neurophysiological structures, whereas the
connectionist neural networks have the status of psychological constructs, as
discussed above. Subsymbolic processing, as outlined in multiple coding, is modeled
specifically by connectionist systems, whereas dispositional representations may be
understood as incorporating information in symbolic as well as subsymbolic forms.
4 The terms “emotion,” “feeling,” and “affect” have been given a wide range of
different, even conflicting, definitions throughout the emotion literature. The terms
“emotion” and “feeling” are used here specifically as defined by Damasio within
his theoretical framework; the terms “affective core” and “emotion schema” are
used as defined within the theoretical network of multiple coding; the connections
between these two sets of concepts are explicated throughout this chapter.
5 The existence and nature of subsymbolic structures underlying comprehension
of logical, scientific, and mathematical relationships are vividly illustrated in
Hadamard’s interviews with Poincaré, Einstein, and others, discussed in Bucci
(1997). Einstein specifically included bodily experience along with other types of
experience as underlying his creative work: “The psychical entities which seem to
serve as elements in thought are certain signs and more or less clear images which
can be ‘voluntarily’ reproduced and combined … The abovementioned elements
are, in my case, of visual and some of muscular type” (quoted in Hadamard, 1949,
pp. 142–143).
References
Arlow, J. A. (1969). Unconscious fantasy and disturbances of conscious experience.
Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 38, 1–27.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science. New York: The Guilford
Press.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
20, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2002). From subsymbolic to symbolic—and back: Therapeutic impact of
the referential process. In R. Lasky (Ed.), Symbolization and desymbolization: Essays
in honor of Norbert Freedman (pp. 50–74). New York: Other Press.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Avon Books.
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.
60 Evolution of the basic theory
(Bartlett, 1932). They differ from other memory schemas in their focus on the
interpersonal world, and in the dominance of sensory, motoric, and somatic
processes that make up their affective core; most of these processes operate in
subsymbolic form.
From a developmental perspective, Bowlby’s (1969) concept of internal
working models and Stern’s (1985) representations of interactions that have
been generalized (RIGs) are related concepts that emphasize the interper-
sonal nature of emotion schemas and the role of the caretaker. As defined
by Stern, RIGs are based on episodes that include “sensations, perceptions,
actions, thoughts, affects and goals,” and that occur repeatedly in a particular
temporal relationship. (Stern, 1985, p. 95). As specific episodes repeat, the
infant begins to form the prototypic memory structure, the RIG, which Stern
(1985, p. 95) characterizes as “an individualized, personal expectation of how
things are likely to proceed on a moment-to-moment basis.” These structures
develop from the beginning of life, in somatic, sensory and motoric forms,
well before language is acquired.
Comparison of concepts
The multiple code concept of emotion schemas and Damasio’s concept
of emotion based on dispositional representations share the following
components with the psychoanalytic notion of drive:
64 Evolution of the basic theory
In all these systems, what we call an emotion occurs when an initiating stimulus
activates a particular emotion schema with its affective core. In all systems,
there is an underlying organization developed through life that involves bio-
logical and cognitive functions; in all these systems, some manifestation of
this organization may be activated by particular stimuli whose significance
may not be recognized. These networks and response patterns, which may
begin as adaptations to particular challenges, may become maladaptive in
different situations. The goal of psychotherapy is to modify the underlying
organization so as to redirect the response to the activating stimuli.
The multiple code theory can also account for the observation that not all
states of stress or threatened homeostasis are maladaptive. In contrast to
severe, protracted, uncontrollable states of “distress,” mild, brief and con-
trollable states of stress can be experienced as pleasant, or exciting; these are
sought after, and can also stimulate emotional and intellectual growth and
development. People seek the excitement of competitive sports, to partici-
pate or to watch. They read novels and watch shows and movies that engage
and arouse them. Scientists and mathematicians seek challenging problems
to solve.
All these activities where excitement is intentionally sought and desired,
which raised questions for a drive theory based on the principle of inertia,
can be accounted for in terms of development of emotion schemas associated
with pleasure as well as with dread. The multiple code theory is neutral as
to the valence (positive or negative) of an emotion schema, incorporating
schemas of joy as well as fear and pain. As we know, Freud also recognized
problems with his energy model—for example, that sexual activity includes
pleasurable accumulation of energy, but did not resolve these problems.
66 Evolution of the basic theory
the relationship. The sensory, somatic and motoric contents of the affective
core are largely in subsymbolic form; connection of these contents to lan-
guage is needed for treatment to proceed. The communication of emotional
experience is a complex, multi-level process, which I have termed the referen-
tial process, and which includes three basic phases: arousal, symbolizing and
reflection/reorganization. The multiple levels of thought must occur in ther-
apist as well as patient in order that effective communication can take place.
I have discussed these functions elsewhere, and will review them briefly here.
Arousal
The patient knows something is bothering them, something has brought
them to treatment. Traces of a problematic dissociated emotion schema are
activated within the relationship, in the interaction of the two participants
and in different ways in the subjective experience of each. The experience of
each participant is largely in subsymbolic form in this phase, involving acti-
vation of the affective core of a schema, and their communication occurs
primarily on sensory, bodily, and motoric levels. The patient, like any person
trying to communicate emotional experience, has difficulty in connecting
their experience to language. The problem is particularly acute for the patient,
who is struggling with schemas that are dissociated, and who is likely to try
to avoid connection to painful experience rather than seek to formulate and
communicate it.
Symbolizing/n arrative
Images or sequences of images associated with the affective core come to
mind, perhaps in fleeting or disconnected form, perhaps in waking fanta-
sies or in events of the treatment relationship, perhaps as they appeared in
a dream. The images constitute instantiations of an emotion schema that
has been activated—one of the cluster of events that activate a similar set of
feelings and involve similar responses. In the optimal operation of the refer-
ential process, the patient can then go on to describe the image or event in the
kind of vivid and detailed language that indicates connection to emotional
experience in the speaker and is capable of evoking corresponding experience
in a listener.
Reflection/r eorganization
Once the material is shared, and the affect is present but sufficiently contained,
there is opportunity for a reorganizing phase in which the meaning of the
events that make up the schema may be further explored, new connections
may be discovered, and new schemas constructed.
68 Evolution of the basic theory
Many a time the “good hour” does not start propitiously. It may come
gradually into its own, say after the first ten or fifteen minutes. Then, a
dream may come, and associations, and all begins to make sense. In par-
ticularly fortunate instances a memory from the near or distant past, or,
suddenly, one from the dark days may present itself with varying degrees
of affective charge. And when the analyst interprets, sometimes all he
needs to say can be put into a question. The patient may well do the
summing up by himself, and himself arrive at conclusions.
(Kris, 1956, p. 446)
The problem, as clinicians know all too well, is that the first ten or 15 minutes
to which Kris refers may become 10 or 15 hours or weeks or even more.
The concepts of the emotion schemas and the referential process, in the
framework of the therapeutic relationship, provide a new perspective on
this transition phase. This is a phase of subsymbolic activation; much is
happening inside the patient, but not much that can be shared in symbolic
Symptoms and symbols revisited 69
We can see this as a prolonged arousal phase for both patient and analyst.
After just over a year of the analysis, Stefano came back from their second
summer break with a dark, worried look that Dr. Solano had never seen him
exhibit before. For once, Stefano seemed genuinely emotionally engaged;
when they shook hands at greeting, Dr. Solano had the feeling that he was res-
cuing someone who had been shipwrecked. The evening before, Stefano had
noticed a swelling on his neck; he went to the hospital where a thyroid nodule
with lymph node involvement was found. Stefano’s wife reacted by saying to
70 Evolution of the basic theory
him that now he would have to care about himself. Stefano’s brother, who
suffered from psychiatric problems, said, “At last something has happened to
you as well” (Solano, 2020, p. 1458).
With the help of the analysis, which provided a secure base, Stefano was
able to control his attempts at denial in this situation and to take appropriate
medical action to treat the nodule, which was ultimately found to be benign.
Dr. Solano says:
The focus on the somatic event played a transitional role enabling entry into
a symbolic mode
In time, they connected the emergence of the nodule on his neck with
the summer break in the analysis, presumably as an effect of the threat of
loss of the relationship, whose value Stefano may have felt but not expli-
citly recognized. His noticing the nodule just the evening before his return to
treatment, although it was presumably apparent before that, suggested that
he was able to bear his fear in the context of anticipation of Dr. Solano’s
support. In the process of working through this experience, Stefano also
became aware that avoidance of negative feelings entailed loss of contact
with positive ones.
Ogden reports that Mrs. B seemed to look at him intently at the beginning
and end of each hour; when he asked her about that, she said she was not
aware of doing it. Shortly after an intervention concerning the patient’s self-
doubts about her value as a mother and as an analysand, an incident occurred
in which she became terrified at the sound of a movement of his. She abruptly
turned around on the couch, for the first time in the analysis. She had a look
of panic on her face and said, “I’m sorry, I didn’t know what was happening
to you.” Ogden writes:
It was only in the intensity of this moment, in which there was a feeling
of terror that something catastrophic was happening to me, that I was
able to name for myself the terror that I had been carrying for some time.
I became aware that the anxiety I had been feeling and the (predomin-
antly unconscious and primitively symbolized) dread of the meetings
with Mrs. B (as reflected in my procrastinating behavior) had been dir-
ectly connected with an unconscious sensation/fantasy that my somatic
symptoms of malaise, nausea and vertigo were caused by Mrs. B, and
that she was killing me. I now understood that for several weeks I had
been emotionally consumed by the unconscious conviction (a “fantasy
in the body”: Gaddini, 1982, p. 143) that I had a serious illness, perhaps
72 Evolution of the basic theory
a brain tumor, and that during that period I had been frightened that
I was dying.
(Ogden, 1994, pp. 14, 15)
affective core. What we call a feeling of anger, or dread, or joy, or more fre-
quently a feeling we cannot name is such an instance of an emotion schema
that has been activated. The concepts of the emotion schema, and an emotion
as an activation of the schema, are based on current neurological findings
and related to many concepts in developmental and emotion theory, as well
in psychoanalysis.
Emotional and bodily health depends on connections within the emotion
schema enabling people to use the bodily information provided by the
affective core to determine what is good or bad for them. Emotional disorders
arise when schemas are dissociated; the strategies people use to maintain
the dissociation and to regulate the painful dissociated experiences of the
affective core, as well as the bodily effects of failures of regulation, become
the problems that bring people to treatment.
The physiological functions that make up the affective core of the emotion
involve adaptive processes that operate constantly to maintain physical
health. There is considerable evidence that prolonged and intense activation
of these processes in response to stressor events will lead to physical illness,
depending on each individual’s areas of vulnerability. There is no evidence
that specific events of life will lead to particular illnesses, or that recovery of
a specific memory will have a related curative power.
A major distinction that I hope I have made clear throughout this chapter,
and that I want to emphasize particularly here, is that symptoms may operate
as symbols—have symbolic functions—in the sense that their expression may
enable entry into a symbolic mode. In therapy, somatic symptoms may pro-
vide a pathway to symbolizing emotional experience that has been dissociated,
particularly where other modes of expression, such as memories, fantasies,
and dreams, may not be accessible. The pathway operates in the referential
process, playing out in both participants in the treatment process and in the
communication between them. Such functions need to be distinguished clearly
from Freud’s formulation of hysterical symptoms as symbolic expressions of
repressed unconscious wishes. This distinction has ramifications throughout
psychoanalytic theory that need to be addressed fully.
Acknowledgment
This article is a revised version of a paper presented at a conference of
the Italian Psychoanalytic Society and the International Psychoanalytical
Association, Rome, 2007.
References
Auchincloss, E. L., & Samberg, E. (Eds.) (2012). Psychoanalytic terms and concepts.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Alexander, F. (1950). Psychosomatic medicine. New York: W.W. Norton.
74 Evolution of the basic theory
Only the sounds of speech pass back and forth between analyst and
patient; ultimately the treatment seeks to reach beyond words, to the
diverse elements of experience—imagery, feelings, desires—which have
never been verbalized or have been wrongly named. The experiences
represented in the patient’s memory must be represented again in his
spoken language, and then re-represented in the analyst’s mind. A basic
question that psychoanalysis shares with other disciplines, including psy-
cholinguistics and cognitive psychology, as well as philosophy, concerns
the correspondence between these representational domains, and the val-
idity of the inference to experience that may be drawn from words.
(Bucci, 1985, p. 571)
The problem of connecting emotion and words is, ironically, well articulated
in literature. In the moments after Vronsky and Anna Karenina’s desires for
one another had been fulfilled for the first time, Anna was distraught. Count
Vronsky “stood before her, pale, his lower jaw quivering, and besought her
to be calm, not knowing how or why.” He speaks of his happiness, but she
experiences the word as a violation:
“Happiness!” she said with horror and loathing and her horror uncon-
sciously infected him. “For pity’s sake, not a word, not a word more.” … She
felt at that moment that she could not put into words the sense of shame, of
rapture and of horror at this stepping into a new life, and she did not want
to speak of it, to vulgarize this feeling by inappropriate words. But later too,
and the next day and the third day, she still found no words in which she
could express the complexity of her feelings; indeed she could not even find
thoughts in which she could clearly think out all that was in her soul.
(Tolstoy, 2000, p. 150)
There are many such examples in literature. It seems that it is possible to write
or talk in an emotionally evocative way about why and how it is not possible
to write or talk—or even think—about emotion. Yet the great literature of the
world is built on the power of language to express all manner of emotional
experience in words, as Tolstoy shows. T. S. Eliot has formulated this process
in his concept of the objective correlative:
Maskit, & Murphy, 2016) focused on the symbolizing component of the ref-
erential process; this chapter expands that discussion to include new work on
the reflecting/reorganizing function. Empirical support from linguistic, experi-
mental and clinical perspectives concerning the concepts of emotion schemas
and the referential process is reviewed, and linguistic measures for the functions
of the referential process that are in current use in our research are presented.
the new and powerful representational system of language has been over-
laid on a set of other representational systems that were previously avail-
able, but without the mechanisms for adequate integration of systems
being fully in place.
Modes of processing
Symbolic processing
We are most familiar with symbol systems; they are the systems that we asso-
ciate with thought, and that we can most readily manipulate and control.
78 Evolution of the basic theory
Subsymbolic processing
People are less likely to acknowledge the subsymbolic modes as processes
of systematic thought, yet they are central in mental life, within as well as
outside of awareness. Subsymbolic2 processes operate in continuous formats
based largely on analogic relationships rather than on the combination and
manipulation of discrete elements or features. From the beginning of life,
people experience gradations in sensations and feelings to which they are able
to attend, generally without attempting to label them. This applies for all sen-
sory modalities and for bodily and motoric experience. Subsymbolic functions
are involved in many everyday activities. The task of changing lanes on a
highway requires judging the speeds and distances of vehicles approaching
and passing, in relation to one’s own speed, in the real time of driving, and
then directing one’s steering, accelerating, and braking motions using those
judgments. The dimensions of speed and distance are inherently continuous;
the driver directs attention to the process, often pauses in an ongoing conver-
sation to focus on it, but does not explicitly compute any of the variables that
are involved. While skills are needed that must be acquired, and that improve
with practice, each situation that is encountered is unique. An expert mathem-
atician, given simulations and metrics, would be challenged to carry out the
multiple interacting computations even without time constraints, and could
not consider such computations in the real time of driving.
These types of processes occur in many situations: in hitting a moving
target from a moving base as in naval warfare; for the tennis player, who
visualizes where the ball is going to land, and starts running to that place even
before the ball leaves the opponent’s racket; in juggling and walking a tight-
rope; in creative cooking and tasting wine; in all forms of art; and in creative
The power of language in emotional life 79
focused on the events of the interpersonal world; and (2) they are organized
on the basis of the subsymbolic processes of the affective core, particularly
including processes associated with the maintenance of well-being, such as
changes in the functioning of circulatory, respiratory, visceral, endocrine,
and other physiological systems, as well as motoric processes associated with
action tendencies. Thus, episodes with similar affective core components will
cluster to form an emotion schema.
For example, an emotion schema may include related experiences of
arousal, heart beating faster and blood pressure rising in response to different
life events such as hearing footsteps late at night in a dark street, visiting a
doctor’s office, giving a public performance, or beginning a yearly holiday
visit to one’s family. The bodily experiences may occur in particular combin-
ations and may be joined with tendencies to actions such as running away,
hiding, attacking, which may be managed in a range of ways. The schemas
that are constructed are specific to each individual’s life, and also have elem-
ents that are culturally and socially shared.
The concept of the schema may be characterized as a combined exemplar-
prototype model, following the distinctions made by Ross and Makin (1999).
On the one hand, it may be characterized by an exemplar, a detailed descrip-
tion of a specific episode in which the constellation of bodily experiences
are activated with particular people in a particular time and place context
or alternatively, in more general terms, at varying degrees of generality and
abstractness. These contrasting modes of characterizing the schema will lead
to different modes of verbal expression and different bodily effects.
Some clusters of exemplars are given labels (such as anger, fear or shame
in English), or combinations of labels— as Tolstoy combined the terms
shame, rapture and horror to try to describe Anna’s state. In many or most
cases, people cannot find words to adequately label the schema that has been
activated.
Like all memory schemas, the emotion schemas involve active and con-
structive processes, constantly changing with new input and determining how
one sees the world (Bartlett, 1932). New episodes are continually incorporated
into the schemas, and the components and structure of the schemas change
throughout life. In retrieving an emotional memory, one does not retrieve a
fixed scenario, but activates a network of potential connections.
In neurological terms, memory schemas—including emotion schemas—
may be understood as dispositional representations (Damasio, 1994), sets of
dormant firing potentialities in ensembles of neurons distributed all over
the brain. The networks that make up the emotion schemas include sensory,
visual, and motoric features, and conceptual interpretation and evaluation.
The schemas may be activated by any of their components—the places,
people, sensations, and concepts that figure in them—and may in turn activate
any component. They can generate imagery by firing back to sensory cortices;
they can generate movements, and they can direct the internal biochemical
The power of language in emotional life 81
operations of the endocrine system, immune system, and viscera. The for-
mulation of emotion schemas as types of memory schemas based on varied
and distributed components is compatible with current evidence concerning
interaction among brain networks (Pessoa 2008; Phelps 2006), contrasting
with views of affect and cognition as separate systems.
Emotion schemas as defined here provide a link between the two approaches;
they are enduring networks of connections that may be activated in particular
contexts, and they are built on clusters of specific episodes represented in par-
ticular places and times. Each of these two faces of emotion, the enduring
and the momentary, has a particular relation to the expression of emotion in
language, as will be discussed.
The distinction between emotion states and episodes noted by Mulligan
and Scherer (2012) may be seen in relation to the distinction between semantic
and episodic memory as formulated by Tulving (2002) and others. As forms
of semantic memory, the emotion schemas are part of the individual’s general
body of knowledge, in subsymbolic as well as symbolic forms, concerning the
interpersonal world as this impinges on them. In contrast, the specific mem-
ories from which the schemas are constructed, like many episodic memories,
carry with them actual arousal of the components of the schema, including
the bodily components—usually in trace form. Suddendorf and Corballis
(2007), Tulving (2002) and others refer to episodic memory as mental time
travel; here the concept is extended to emotional time travel, involving acti-
vation of the affective-bodily-cognitive assembly associated with exemplars
of emotion schemas, playing out in specific memories or fantasies of oneself
with particular people, in a particular time and place.
are not necessarily) felt subjectively, and may exert control over attentional
pathways.
The subsystems combine to form emotional episodes. The profiles of emo-
tional episodes may correspond to emotions that are given category labels
in ordinary language, but are not restricted to limited sets of prototypical
patterns:
Core affect, the object and the emotion episode: Russell’s conceptual framework
From a contrasting perspective, Russell (2003) proposes a conceptual frame-
work whose two primitives are core affect and perception of affective quality;
his other concepts are defined in terms of those. For Russell, like Scherer, the
component processes are seen as cohering in emotion episodes. In Russell’s
formulation, the episodes begin with an antecedent event that is perceived in
terms of its affective quality, and that dramatically alters core affect. Other
components of an emotional episode identified by Russell may include
appraisal, based on perceptual-cognitive processing of the Object; instrumental
action directed at the Object; physiological and expressive changes, including
facial, vocal, and autonomic changes; and subjective experiences, including
metacognitive judgments.
As Russell states, some emotional episodes may sufficiently fit a prototype
of a specific or basic emotion to count as an instance of that emotion. As he
also emphasizes, however, the components of emotion concepts are not as
closely associated as has generally been assumed, even for what appear to be
prototypic instances of supposedly unified emotion categories such as sur-
prise (Reisenzein, 2000) and fear (LeDoux, 1996). Thus, Russell (2003, p. 166)
says:
abstract symbols of the verbal code. The limitations of the referential process
become apparent when one attempts to verbalize an experience that one has
never verbalized before, to describe a taste or smell, or to teach an athletic or
motoric skill: Anna could not find the words to express her feelings; she could
not even find her thoughts.
Three major functions of the referential process have been identified: an
arousal function that involves activation of experience with its subsymbolic
core; a symbolizing function that includes imagery and narrative; and a
reflecting/reorganizing (R/R) function. The functions may often proceed
sequentially, producing the process in its full form, but may also be interrupted,
or operate recursively.
The process operates in any conversational situation and in thought for
oneself, and may also be observed in literature. The bidirectional and inter-
personal nature of the process, at both subsymbolic and symbolic levels, and
the functions of reorganization of the emotion schemas, are seen most clearly
in psychotherapy. Each of the functions of the process is characterized by
recognizable and measurable features of language style. We will first describe
these functions and review empirical evidence for them, then describe sev-
eral computerized language measures that have been developed to trace this
process.
Arousal of a schema
The process begins with arousal of an emotion schema with its bodily
components. The schema may be activated by a smell, a song, a sight, or
another event, sometimes without the person being directly aware of the source
of the feeling, and often without being able to name an emotion. Emotion may
be communicated on the subsymbolic level in this phase, through modalities
such as body movement, gesture and facial expression, without the experi-
ence being formulated in verbal form. In response to Anna’s distress, Vronsky
becomes pale, his lower jaw trembles; Anna bows her head, becomes limp,
and falls to the floor at his feet. Anna certainly has thoughts at this time, but
she does not recognize them as such because they are subsymbolic in form.
Reflecting/r eorganizing
Following the reliving of an event, and its verbal description, the speaker (or
writer) may enter a more reflective mode, perhaps recognizing relationships
among events or distinctions between them that had not previously been iden-
tified, and modulating reactions in some way. Thus new emotional meanings
may be developed and emotion schemas may be modified. In some instances,
the reflection may also include generalized descriptions or emotion category
labels. After reliving the painful event, the person may see the situation dif-
ferently: “At first I just felt terribly humiliated, but now I understand that she
was very upset about something else.” In the process of emotional experi-
ence and communication, this phase is likely to play a modulating rather than
activating role.
cannot be expressed in words. The speaker is likely to talk about the struggle
itself, or may attempt to label a feeling that has been activated, but with little
or no connection to what the feeling is about, or to the objects or events that
have evoked it. The following is an example from a therapy session of the lan-
guage of this phase:
There are really two things on my mind right now. One is that yesterday
sınavda after leaving, no, before I came too, I felt sort of upset and probably ner-
cıkabilir vous and then after I left, I felt, I thought I’d be alright but I felt even
worse. And, again it was almost as if I were fighting even just letting
things come into my mind that were beginning to come into my mind.
I don’t know …
In terms of Eliot’s concept of the objective correlative, the words with high
WRAD weights are those that must be used in describing a “set of objects,
a situation, a chain of events which shall be the formula of that particular
emotion.” While the objects, situations and events may vary widely, a limited
set of function words is needed to point to objects and join events, and place
them in situational contexts, “such that when the external facts, which must
terminate in sensory experience, are given, the emotion is immediately evoked”
(Eliot, 1950, p. 100). The WRAD is dominated by the deictic and other
function words that point to, locate, and join together words representing
people, places, and events that make up episodes, applying across contents.
From an evolutionary perspective, we suggest that a deictic term such
as the serves the purpose of the gesture of pointing, while the conjunction
and functions as if physically placing things together; spatial prepositions
such as in and on, and tense markers position entities in specific contexts
of place and time. In this sense, function words, like gestures, and like the
rhythms and intonation patterns of speech, are themselves transitional
94 Evolution of the basic theory
between subsymbolic and symbolic forms. The WRAD identifies these points
of connection in discourse, although the speaker or writer does use these
function words intentionally, and the listener or reader does not attend dir-
ectly to them. Similar categories, modified by the different grammatical forms
of the language, have also been found in the Italian versions of this dictionary
(Mariani et al., 2013).
The new WRRL dictionary follows the same procedure of identifying
aspects of language style associated with different mental and emotional
functions. An initial analysis of how these stylistic features operate to
reorganize and modulate emotion is presented by Zhou and colleagues (2021,
in press). Emotion category terms, such as anger, fear, or shame, can also
enable people to learn from previous instances, distinguish among different
experiences or find relationships among them that they have not seen before.
The power of language to activate and express emotion, and to change emo-
tional meanings, has been recognized, explicitly and implicitly, by scientists
and psychotherapy researchers, and by poets and writers. The processes of
retrieval and the neural pathways involved in these functions have been traced
by some researchers mentioned here and many others. As our research has
begun to show, variations in emotion circuitry find their way to functional
terms of language that are not intentionally chosen by speakers; these elem-
ents of language style also have the power to feed back to enable rewiring
of the emotion schemas themselves. The theory of multiple coding and the
referential process offers a theoretical context and a framework for empirical
research into these effects.
Acknowledgment
This chapter was initially published in Italian translation as “Il ruolo del
linguaggio nella vita emotiva”, Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 53 (2019), 379–
404. It is an updated and expanded version of earlier work (Bucci, Maskit &
Murphy (2016).
Notes
1 Lyrics of popular song by M. Gordon, 1945; information downloaded from
wikipedia.org, September 5, 2013.
2 The term “subsymbolic” was taken from connectionist and parallel distributed
processing (PDP) approaches; it is used here to mean nonsymbolic, not to charac-
terize this form as less systematic or complex than symbolic forms.
3 Geneva Appraisal Questionnaire (GAQ): Format, development, and utilization
www.affective-sciences.org/researchmaterial. Also see Scherer (2001).
4 The full list of WRAD words in alphabetical order with their dictionary weights
can be found in Bucci and Maskit (2014).
5 The DAAP software transforms most filled pauses, such as uhm or hm, to the
word mm.
The power of language in emotional life 95
6 This variant of the WRAD measures, also known as the Intensity Index, is
defined as the mean amount in a segment by which the WRAD curve is above its
neutral value.
References
Baldwin, M.W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information.
Psychological Bulletin, 112(3), 461–484.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Brand, B. L., Lanius, R., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R. J., & Spiegel, D. (2012).
Where are we going? An update on assessment, treatment and neurobiological
research in dissociative disorders as we move toward the DSM-5. Journal of Trauma
and Dissociation, 13(1), 9–31.
Bretherton, I. (1994). Infants’ subjective world of relatedness: Moments, feeling
shapes, protonarrative envelopes, and internal working models. Infant Mental
Health Journal, 15(1), 36–42.
Bucci, W. (1985). Dual coding: A cognitive model for psychoanalytic research. Journal
of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 33(3), 571–607.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (2011). The role of subjectivity and intersubjectivity in the reconstruc-
tion of dissociated schemas: Converging perspectives from psychoanalysis,
cognitive science and affective neuroscience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28,
247–266.
Bucci, W., & Crisafulli, G. (2021 in press). Linguistic measures of the therapeutic
process in Carl Rogers’ case of Miss Vib. In special issue: “Empirical and Clinical
Studies of the Referential Process”, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
Bucci, W., & Kabasakalian-McKay, R. (1992). Instructions for scoring referential
activity (RA) in transcripts of spoken narrative texts. Ulm: Ulmer Textbank.
Bucci, W., & Kabasakalian-McKay, R. K. (2014). Manual for scoring RA scales.
Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.962956
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2006). A weighted dictionary for referential activity. In
J. G. Shanahan, Y. Qu, & J. Wiebe (Eds.), Computing attitude and affect in text
(pp. 49–60). Dordrecht: Springer.
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2007). Beneath the surface of the therapeutic interaction; The
psychoanalytic method in modern dress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 55, 1355–1397.
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2014). Weighted Referential Activity Dictionary (WRAD).
Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.962957
Bucci, W., Maskit, M., & Hoffman, L. (2012). Objective measures of subjective
experience: The use of therapist notes in process-outcome research. Psychodynamic
Psychiatry, 40(2), 303–340.
Bucci, W., Maskit, M., & Murphy, S. (2016). Connecting emotions and words: The
referential process. Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, 15(3), 359–383.
Corballis, M. S. (2009). Mental time travel and the shaping of language. Experimental
Brain Research, 192, 553–560.
96 Evolution of the basic theory
Clinical perspectives
on emotional communication
Chapter 6
Converging perspectives
on emotional change in the
interpersonal field
Let us bear clearly in mind that every human being has acquired, by the
combined operation of inherent disposition and the external influences in
childhood, a special individuality in the exercise of his capacity to love—
that is, in the conditions which he sets up for loving, in the impulses he
gratifies by it, and in the aims he sets out to achieve in it. This forms a
cliché or stereotype in him, so to speak (or even several), which perpetu-
ally repeats and reproduces itself as life goes on, in so far as external
circumstances and the nature of the accessible love-objects permit, and is
indeed itself to some extent modifiable by later impressions.
(Freud, 1912, pp. 105–106)
Adaptive dissociation
Dissociation may occur within and between emotion schemas. In adaptive
dissociation within schemas, the subsymbolic contents of the affective core
of the schema may play out to a large extent without direction by language,
and even without attentional focus. We see such instances of adaptive and
complex subsymbolic flow in many actions of everyday life, in sports, in the
arts, as well as in emotional interchange. Natural and adaptive forms of dis-
sociation occur between as well as within emotion schemas; different contexts
activate different schemas, leading to different states of being and feeling
and different modes of response. We have different personas as professional
people, mothers (or fathers), wives (or husbands), and in the many activities of
our lives. When I am with my students, I have or try to have a role of authority
or expert; when I am taking my tango lesson, I am a novice; in either of these
situations I am very absorbed and during that period of absorption I can put
the various anxieties and concerns of my personal life out of my mind.
Many of the young Black and Latino people who move from the ethnic
and social contexts in which they have their roots to enter elite institutions
have acute experiences of complex identity shifts. The experience was particu-
larly intense for those young students who were recruited for the Ivy Leagues
in the first waves of affirmative action in the late 1960s and early 1970s.
106 Clinical perspectives
Primary dissociation
Primary dissociation occurs when the regulation of behavioral response to
threat is disrupted, as in extreme trauma, so that the system remains in emer-
gency mode with potentially damaging effects on bodily systems. Cortisol
dysregulation and related processes weaken the capacity of the hippocampus
to regulate the stress response, and also affect its convergence function, inter-
fering with the formation of memories. Thus the ability of the hippocampus
to regulate stress is impeded, while the operation of the amygdala to stimu-
late stress is enhanced. Images of events may be encoded in the amygdalar
memory system, but these are likely to be dissociated and fragmented to some
Converging perspectives on emotional change 107
The air was instantly gray and full of objects moving at different speeds,
some rising and others falling. Pieces of things. My arm suddenly hurt.
Time slowed. I looked at the men in the truck bed with me and saw
mostly blank expressions. Disbelief. It wasn’t until they saw their own
blood everywhere that they responded.
(Busch, 2012, p. 273)
Later he says:
I thought I should jump another convoy out and go ice my arm, but
a Marine looked through the holes in my sleeve and saw blood. I had
to look to be sure it was my own … I remember a low audible static in
my head. It didn’t seem to be in my ears but rather somewhere inside
my mind.
(Busch, 2012, p. 276)
This gives some idea of the form of registration of experiences where the
level of physiological stress is such as to directly affect the encoding of events.
The information is not fully oriented to time and place, it is not connected
to the self; it remains in isolated and fragmentary form, registered mostly
through amygdalar memory and within sensory association areas, not
incorporated into coherent organized episodes. The narrative is difficult for
the reader to follow, difficult to bear.
LeDoux (2002, p. 225) says about amygdalar memories:
The good news is that even when the ability to form explicit memory is
impaired, we can store useful information about harmful situations. The
bad news is that if we don’t know what it is we are learning about, those
stimuli might on later occasions trigger fear responses that will be diffi-
cult to understand and control.
108 Clinical perspectives
There is evidence that memories laid down early in life, before the hippocampal
memory system is fully developed, are also of this fragmentary nature.
Secondary dissociation
In secondary dissociation, the direct physiological effects are less acute, but
the encoding of events is impaired because of their specific meanings. The
memory may initially have been formed to some degree in integrated form,
so may be potentially available for retrieval; then aspects of the event are
avoided, rather than the integrated memory never having been encoded. The
child avoids recognition or acknowledgment of the caretaker on whom they
are dependent for love and life as also the source of their terror and the object
of their rage. There are psychic as well as physical mechanisms that enable this
avoidance. The amygdala has output connections to areas involved in motor
control that underly physical actions of flight. The amygdala also has output
connections to the prefrontal cortex that motivate and direct the organism
to turn attention away from the source of the threat; these are central to the
dissociation process. Later, when events associated with the threat occur, the
avoidant pattern will spread to these. The dilemma is that while knowledge
of the threat may be avoided, the painful or conflictual affective responses to
the source will nevertheless be activated to some degree. While the immediate
physiological effects of the threatening experience may be reduced to some
extent by this turning away, these effects are likely to occur in chronic, perva-
sive forms that are at least as damaging.
The point I want to emphasize here is that whether the dissociation is primary,
involving fragmentary memories generated by the amygdala, or secondary, the
product of avoidance, turning away from knowledge of the threat, the person is
left with unexplained bodily or motoric or affective responses that are likely to
be activated in many contexts, by stimuli that are unrecognized, whose source
cannot be known—overwhelming arousal of feelings and response tenden-
cies, without any reason or any meaning, and without mechanisms of regula-
tion being available. The person is, in a sense, psychically lost—having lost the
connection to what they desire or fear, and the knowledge of how to respond.
involves taking in new knowledge about events in the world that have been
perceived as threatening in relation to one’s current situation and current
powers, but the particular nature of dissociated schemas is that they are set
up precisely to avoid such knowledge. The person will first use all means at
hand to avoid situations associated with the dreaded schema, in reality or
in imagination; then to avoid knowledge of emotional meanings should the
affective core of the schema be activated in trace form. This is a version of the
vicious circle of treatment about which Strachey wrote in 1934.
What we are now beginning to understand is that the vicious circle is the
impasse that is the opportunity—not an obstacle to treatment, but a pathway
into the rigid and dissociated schema, although one that is tangled and full of
briars and thorns (like the Prince’s journey to awaken the Sleeping Beauty).
The trace of the dreaded schema must be activated in the session and in the
relationship in order for change to come about, but activated in such a way
that the tangle of avoidance and protection can be penetrated to some extent,
and the schema can potentially be reconstructed rather than the dissociation
being reinforced.
source and meaning of the events that make up the schema may be fur-
ther explored, new connections may be discovered, and new schemas
constructed.
Kurt would insist that his experience, his pain, was beyond human com-
prehension. It didn’t help that his words, he felt, could not begin to convey
the dimensions of his experience; they inevitably trivialized it.
(Mark, 2009, p. 410)
Sometimes, after such an event, Kurt would tell Mark that it felt “simple and
pure” to hate him, or to feel so unjustly injured by him. Mark says that he
would want defensively to appeal to Kurt’s overall sense and history of him,
but as he knew, there was no such “overall sense”; Kurt was entirely “in the
moment.”
Here, in focusing on a specific emotion (“simple and pure” hatred), Kurt
is providing a source for his painful activation and using the therapist in this
112 Clinical perspectives
role. The theory would define this as a move toward the symbolizing phase;
Mark does not at this point describe the process in this way.
About three months into their work, Mark received a phone call from
Kurt; as they both knew, Kurt said, he had to quit therapy because Mark was
making things worse for him. They both knew this wasn’t going to help him;
in fact it was making him crazier. Here Kurt’s avoidance is being breached; the
connection to awareness of specific threatening events is experienced as going
too far, as potentially unbearable. This is a classic danger point for dropout
from treatment, in psychodynamic and other treatment forms.
Mark asks him to consider coming back for one more session. His con-
scious thought was that he was doing the professional thing by urging Kurt to
return; he says that he was less aware at the time that he needed him to do so:
He did return and told me that in the previous session, the one that
convinced him that therapy was making him crazier, he’d had the repeated
image of a small, though unidentifiable and indeterminate, animal who
would disappear through a “funnel, a black hole.” Then, at some point
in that session, in a way that felt terrifyingly numb, he was disappearing
through that black hole. Kurt was so disoriented after that session that
even in our subsequent one, he could not recall, nor even imagine, how he
was able to return home.
(Mark, 2009, p. 411)
Over the next several years of the treatment, Kurt experienced many similar
images, always within the session; he did not experience images outside of
the session and generally felt he was not able to visualize intentionally. He
always conveyed the images angrily; they horrified and humiliated him. In his
dissociated state, Kurt was not able to think of these images as the products
of his mind or as mental products at all; he experienced them as coming from
outside himself “as meaningless, sadistic intrusions into his mind.”
The images are nonverbal manifestations of the symbolizing phase. They
are carrying out the function of this phase—connecting to components of
the dissociated schema and arousing feelings of threat and humiliation,
almost unbearable but contained in the context of the relationship, reson-
ating in complex ways for the therapist. The images are not accepted by Kurt
as connected to the timeline of his autobiographical memory. Partly because
of this disconnection, Mark does not at first see the images as productive.
Mark’s goal for some period was to cure the patient of his images, to eradi-
cate them by explaining them away, but after a period of complex struggle
within himself he shifts to a process of using the images, helping Kurt feel
his way into them. Kurt proves to be willing and able to do this; the images
become more organized by the experience of their interaction. A pattern
occasionally emerged in their sessions in which Kurt would become silent,
and begin to look off in a way that signaled he was experiencing such an
Converging perspectives on emotional change 113
image, having what Mark called a “waking dream.” Typically, during this
period, Mark would feel what he described as “something generic, something
between eerie amazement, interest, and mild anxiety.” At times, however, he
would find himself feeling a particular affect, which seemed unrelated to what
was going on but would “fit” with the image that Kurt would subsequently tell
him. Mark says, “I would experience an affect that felt devoid of context, of
symbolization, while he would have a visual image that, for him, was equally
devoid of feeling or cognitive meaning.” In one such instance, Mark’s feeling
was sadness, a deep sense of loss. Kurt then describes an image of Popeye
jumping on a miniature platform, flexing his muscles and sticking his tongue
out, maybe in Mark’s direction, then lying down with an implement through
his heart.
Here the symbolizing phase incorporates a simultaneously intermodal
(subsymbolic/symbolic) and intersubjective connection. This is not mystical
but can be seen as a specimen case of the anticipatory power of embodied
communication: Mark’s perceptions incorporate feelings of sadness and loss
that are responsive to Kurt’s experience, which he has not yet told.
Kurt then has the association to a neighbor’s child who “proudly paraded
around like some action figure.” He initially experienced the association as
pleasurable, then his experience turned to sadness. He said that he felt that the
child’s parents were overly concerned with suppressing the behavior that they
saw as unruly, “maybe like his own parents had been with him” (Mark, 2009,
p. 412). From this they go on to a series of associations around Kurt having
been able to enjoy a recent social occasion, and feeling proud about this but
not having been able to express the pride explicitly in the session because
Mark saw him as more ill than he was.
Here they are expanding their associations, exploring Kurt’s fears about
what will happen to him if he feels proud—an implement through his heart—
and relating this to events of Kurt’s present and past life, and to aspects of
Kurt’s feelings about Mark and their relationship that had not previously
emerged. This represents extension of the narrative symbolizing into the
reorganizing phase. Through these connections, Kurt becomes able to own
his images, to see them as related to his history and himself.
in time; Suddendorf and Corballis (1997, 2007) and Tulving (2002) refer to
this as “time travel.” Fantasies of the future, like memories of the past, can
be episodic—specific situations occurring in a particular time and place in
relation to the self—or semantic—previously formulated, emerging as more
general and abstract without emotional connections. The difference in level
of awareness applies to the present—the “here and now”—as well (Tulving,
2002). Particular types of interactions in the session would be experienced
as autonoetic and characterized as episodic in this sense. On the neuro-
logical level, we know that episodic narratives, told in the state of autonoetic
awareness, involve activation of the prefrontal cortex with widely distributed
cortical and subcortical networks including connections to hippocampus and
amygdala. This is compatible with Damasio’s notion of extended conscious-
ness, associated with complex processing of emotional information:
We can see this as the basic process underlying what Bromberg (1998) writes
about in “Shadow and Substance” as allowing motoric, affective, imagistic,
and verbal elements to coalesce with narrative memory in the context of a
perceptual experience of the patient–analyst relationship. In contrast, the
network of connections underlying semantic memory largely involves cor-
tical zones. Information stored in semantic memory might be retrieved with
lower levels of hippocampal activation and without activation of emotional
circuitry.
The difference between episodic and semantic or prototypic memory is
apparent to clinicians. There are instances in which a patient tells a story—
which may be about a manifestly trivial event—in vivid language as if reliving
it, sometimes feeling intense affect, more than they anticipated. The telling
is likely to open some new connections for the speaker and to evoke intense
complex feelings in the listener. There are also contrasting instances in which
patients describe experiences of abuse or trauma that are in themselves hor-
rific and shocking, and that may be consciously accessible to them, but that
are told in a way that seems strangely flat. In such instances, therapists listen
with complex emotion of a different nature—often with horror at the situ-
ation that is depicted, but also often with discomfort, sometimes not experi-
encing the emotion that they would expect to feel in response to such terrible
experiences; sometimes feeling lethargic, even sleepy; sometimes feeling other
bodily responses that they do not understand.
In an APA demonstration tape by Jeremy Safran (2008), developed as
a demonstration of relational psychotherapy, a woman is seeking help
118 Clinical perspectives
In our view, in fact, there is a good case for using the term “isolation”
solely to denote a specific defensive process which ranges from compul-
sion to a systematic and concerted attitude and which consists in the
severing of the associative connections of a thought or act—especially its
connections with what precedes and succeeds it in time.
There are many possibilities for what a therapist might do on hearing a story
of this nature from a patient, depending on the therapist’s sense of the patient’s
capacity for affect regulation, the therapist’s own feelings and the nature of
the relationship that has been built. Even after thirteen years, a question such
as, “Where were you, where were your children, when you first heard about
this?”, which places the event in the context of time and place, might initiate
retrieval of episodic memories with the potential for opening connections to
dissociated components of the memory. The therapist will need to rely on
experience, knowledge, and subsymbolic intuitive processing to decide how to
proceed—to encourage the retrieval of the memory, with its powers and risks
of emotional arousal—or to maintain the dissociation.
Converging perspectives on emotional change 119
Even where the therapist does not seek to activate the connection to epi-
sodic memory, they need to activate and reinforce the connection for their
own self. At the very least, the therapist needs to acknowledge, nonverbally or
verbally, the effect of the story on him. Not to do so would leave the patient
unacknowledged, in the same isolated interpersonal situation that contributed
to their present state. Safran appropriately does not try to open emotional
exploration in the context of the demonstration tape, but does validate his
own emotional response by reflecting on the matter-of-fact way in which they
are talking about this catastrophic event.
In the treatment process, moments of interaction, including moments of
disconnection or impasse that are acknowledged and worked through like
narratives, provide opportunities for the connecting processes of the symbol-
izing phase to occur—connecting one’s own experience to the experience of
the other in the moment. Such events occurred frequently, in different forms,
in Mark’s treatment of Kurt; they may be seen as episodic events in the pre-
sent, in Tulving’s (2002) terms. The movement to episodic events incorporating
memories of the recent or distant past, with ownership by Kurt as related to
himself, on the timeline of his life, took a long time to achieve in that treatment.
When one thinks today about what one did yesterday, time’s arrow is bent
into a loop. The rememberer has mentally traveled back into her past,
120 Clinical perspectives
and thus violated the law of the irreversibility of the flow of time. She has
not accomplished the feat in physical reality, of course, but rather in the
reality of the mind, which, as everyone knows, is at least as important for
human beings as is the physical reality.
(Tulving, 2002, pp. 1, 2)
I suggest that the power of episodic memory is even more marvelous than
is represented in Tulving’s account. First, what Tulving calls the “reality of
the mind” needs to be viewed as the reality of the affective-bodily-cognitive
assembly that underlies the sense of self; the mind does not enter on its travel
in an unembodied state. The travel to the past or to the future involves the
whole system of the self, including activation in the present of sensory and
bodily experience associated with the past memory or fantasized future event.
Second, and equally crucial—although not emphasized in Tulving’s paper—
it may be true that remembering the past requires some form of time travel,
but it is travel to a place that has not existed as such before. The playing out
of past memory in the present is a new event in a new emotional context—a
living, not a reliving. A major aspect of episodic memory, emphasized by
Corballis (2009), Neisser (2008) and others, is its generativity. Episodic mem-
ories are not stored or retrieved in previously constructed units; according to
Corballis (2009, p. 555), “Our memories for episodes are made up of combin-
ations of people, actions, objects, places—along with qualities such as time of
day, weather, season, mood, emotional states and the like.”
From the perspective of the referential process, however, we can see that
the construction of episodic memory requires more than combinatorial
operations connecting elements of a memory. It is a more complex process
in which something that has been in analogic subsymbolic format emerges
as a new shape; this may be in nonverbal imagery or verbal form. The role
of such a reconstructive process is clear for schemas where primary dis-
sociation has occurred—that is, where amygdalar memories were registered
without connection to their source—but it also applies to all memories that
have been dissociated. This formulation, based on current research, is similar
in many ways to Freud’s early (1893) description of recollection as discussed
by Loewald (1980, p. 41) as originating in something old: “inscribed into the
organism as an unconscious memory trace (body memory),” leading to the
“creation of something new” which “had not existed in this form before.”
What is left out of Freud’s early formulation (and also of Tulving’s account)
is that the activation of an emotion schema, and the telling of a narrative, are
inherently relational acts: the speaker relates to the people who figure in the
memory or fantasy or dream, and also relates to the person who is listening
in the present. In the context of the psychoanalytic process, the patient does
not travel to the past (or future) alone. There has to be a reason and a value,
beyond self-exploration, to telling the story in the present context. The pur-
pose and effects of telling depend on the state of the listener; the referential
Converging perspectives on emotional change 121
Acknowledgments
Some of the material presented in this paper was previously presented in a
keynote address at Div. 39 of the American Psychological Association in San
Antonio, TX, in April 2009, and at the Conference within a Conference of
Division 39 of the American Psychological Association in Toronto, Canada,
in August 2009.
Notes
1 It should be emphasized that multiple code theory is a psychological model; the
concepts of the theory are psychological constructs, not neurological ones. To date,
however, the major advances in understanding emotional processes have come
from the field of affective neuroscience rather than from psychology. The theories
and observations of neuroscience are presented here as providing a source of evi-
dence for multiple code theory, along with evidence from clinical work and experi-
mental and developmental research. As in all fields, the implications of these data
are open to revision and discussion based on further evidence.
2 The multiple code concept of primary dissociation is comparable to the descrip-
tion of primary dissociation as given by van der Kolk, van der Hart and Marmar
(1996): “Memories of the trauma are initially experienced as fragments of the sen-
sory components of the event—as visual images; olfactory, auditory, or kinesthetic
Converging perspectives on emotional change 123
sensations; or intense waves of feelings.” (cited by Frewen & Lanius, 2006, p. 113).
These responses, cued by reminders of past traumatic events, are seen by van der
Kolk and colleagues as representing a defining diagnostic feature of PTSD and
are often associated with psychophysiological arousal, as indexed by increased
heart rate and electrical skin conductance. The multiple code concept of secondary
dissociation involves a more integrated level of initial encoding and subsequent
potential for retrieval of a memory that has particular relevance for psychosocial
treatment and is not addressed by van der Kolk and colleagues.
References
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1994). Working memory: The interface between memory and cog-
nition. In D. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 351–367).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The concept of episodic memory. In A. Baddeley, J. P. Aggleton,
& M. A. Conway (Eds.), Episodic memory: New directions in research (pp. 1–10).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Beebe, B., Knoblauch, S., Rustin, J., & Sorter, D. (2005). Forms of intersubjectivity in
infant research and adult treatment. New York: Other Press.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bromberg, P. M. (1998). Shadow and substance: A relational perspective on clinical
process. In Standing in the spaces: Essays on clinical process, trauma, and dissociation
(pp. 165–187). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
20, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2002). The referential process, consciousness, and the sense of self.
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22, 766–793.
Bucci, W. (2007a). New perspectives on the multiple code theory: The role of
bodily experience in emotional organization. In F. S. Anderson (Ed.), Bodies in
treatment: The unspoken dimension (pp. 51–77). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Bucci, W. (2007b). Dissociation from the perspective of multiple code theory: Part
I. Psychological roots and implications for psychoanalytic treatment. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 43, 165–184.
Bucci, W. (2009). The sleeping analyst, the waking dreams: Commentary on papers by
Richard Chefetz and David Mark. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 19, 415–425.
Bucci, W. (2011a) The role of subjectivity and intersubjectivity in the reconstruction
of dissociated schemas; converging perspectives from psychoanalysis, cognitive
science and affective neuroscience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28, 247–266.
Bucci, W. (2011b) The role of embodied communication in therapeutic change. In
W. Tschacher, & C. Bergomi (Eds.), The implications of embodiment: Cognition and
communication. Exeter: Imprint Academic.
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2007). Beneath the surface of the therapeutic interaction: The
psychoanalytic method in modern dress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 55, 1355–1397.
124 Clinical perspectives
Behind all these uncertainties, however, there lies one new fact, whose
discovery we owe to psychoanalytic research. We have found that processes
in the unconscious or in the id obey different laws from those in the
preconscious ego. We name these laws in their totality the primary process,
in contrast to the secondary process which governs the course of events in
the preconscious, in the ego. In the end, therefore, the study of psychical
qualities has after all proved not unfruitful.
(Freud, 1940, p. 164)
In the case of the primary process, psychical energy flows freely, passing
unhindered, by means of the mechanisms of condensation and displace-
ment, from one idea to another …; in the case of the secondary process,
the energy is bound at first and then it flows in a controlled manner.
(Laplanche & Pontalis, 1973, p. 339)
As they also note: “The opposition between the primary process and the
secondary process corresponds to that between the pleasure principle and the
reality principle” (Laplanche & Pontalis, 1973, p. 339). Whereas the aim of
Primary process as a transitional concept 127
the unconscious process was to establish a perceptual identity with the original
experience of satisfaction “by the shortest available route,” through “wishful
cathexis to the point of hallucination,” the secondary process seeks thought iden-
tity “with the connecting paths between ideas, without being led astray by the
intensities of those ideas” (Freud, cited by Laplanche and Pontalis, 1973, p. 340).
From a contemporary and more eclectic perspective, Auchincloss and
Samberg (2012) define the primary and secondary processes as “two funda-
mentally different modes of representation and/or organization of psycho-
logical life, which, at the descriptive level account for two types of ‘thought’,
different in both form and content.” As they outline these modes of psychic
life, the primary process is associated with dreams, fantasies, infantile levels
of thought, neurotic symptoms, psychotic states, and processes of free associ-
ation in treatment, as well as with creative forms of mentation in the arts and
religious rituals. The secondary process is associated with adaptive, active,
rational, mature, waking life.
Freud’s view of psychic structure and function may also be seen in rela-
tion to the ideas of philosophers, at least from Plato onward, as well as the
German philosophers of the late eighteenth and nineteenth centuries. In
The Republic, Plato describes a complex soul made of several parts—logical,
spirited, and appetitive—operating in more or less integrated ways. Kant’s
(2007) views concerning a tripartite model of mind, with elements of reason,
understanding, and sensibility, were prominent in German intellectual life in
the 1890s, when Freud was developing his theory in the Project for a Scientific
Psychology (1895), and in The Interpretation of Dreams (1900). Each of these
formulations struggled with related distinctions in different ways.
In Freud’s view, the different components of mind were distinguished
within a conceptual framework based on flow of psychic energy. From his
earliest formulations, and throughout the development of the metapsych-
ology, Freud continued to view the overall function of the mental apparatus
as the regulation and discharge of mental energy, and to account for the
distinction between the primary and secondary processes within this frame-
work. This is where we confront one of the central questions discussed in
this chapter. Many—perhaps most—contemporary analysts have rejected
the notion of psychic energy, “criticizing it as: based on multiple tautolo-
gies; misusing metaphor as fact; pervaded by contradiction, confusion and
imprecision; lacking explanatory value; reinforcing mind- body dualism;
and presenting a false link between psychoanalysis and neurophysiology”
(Auchincloss & Samberg, 2012, p. 78). The rejection of the energy theory
leaves the concept of the primary process without a systematic foundation. As
Holt (2002, p. 462) states, “There is no intrinsic reason why the various prop-
erties of disordered thought described by Freud constitute a theoretical unity,
once we abandon the notions of free and unneutralized cathexis.”
The association of the primary process with unconscious thought has also
been widely questioned. According to Laplanche and Pontalis (1973, p. 339),
128 Clinical perspectives
Sam: A herring.
Joe: A herring isn’t green.
Sam: So you paint it green.
Joe: But it doesn’t hang on a wall.
Sam: So you hang it on the wall.
Joe: But it doesn’t whistle.
Sam: So who cares if it whistles?
(Luborsky, 1988, p. 136)1
Primary process as a transitional concept 129
and Samberg (2012, p. 201) refer to the multiple code theory (Bucci, 1997a,
1997b):
In previous work (Bucci, 1997a, 1997b, 2002), I have defined the organization
of emotional experience in terms of the concept of emotion schemas, types
of memory schemas built as “clusters of memories of events of one’s life
in which subsymbolic sensory, visceral, and motoric processes are activated
in relation to different people in a variety of contexts” (Bucci, Maskit, &
Murphy, 2015, p. 365). My formulation also included the role of language in
the representation of the emotion schemas. I will return to this point in the
discussion of psychoanalytic concepts of primary process thought.
132 Clinical perspectives
of thought arises. Here I’ll look at this issue in relation to the several modes of
thought as specified in multiple code theory (Bucci, 1997a, 1997b).
Symbolic codes
Symbols are defined in multiple code theory from a general semiotic perspec-
tive as discrete representations with properties of reference and generativity—
that is, they are representations that refer to other entities and that may be
combined to generate an infinite array of new forms.3 Symbolic forms may be
words or images in all sensory modalities. People are familiar with the con-
cept of symbolic processing; there is an implicit assumption that thought is
generally verbal, and some recognition that we may think in pictures as well.
relief, maybe it was like, ‘I’m not going to worry about anyone or anything,
I’m just going to worry about tennis.’ ” I suggest that whatever happened
internally enabled Serena to put aside the “many things” that were “on the
line,” to which Mouratoglou referred, and to “just worry about tennis”; this
allowed the organization of her subsymbolic processing system to operate in
an optimal manner in her crucial game at the Wimbledon final.
We can contrast Serena’s experience with the experience of the Scottish
tennis player Andy Murray. He is a hero to the British people (at least those
who care about tennis); in 2013, he was the first British man to win Wimbledon
in 77 years. According to writer Louisa Thomas, he is also “a walking existen-
tial crisis” (Thomas, 2016a).
In the second round of the 2016 Wimbledon, he played Matthias Bourgue,
an unseeded player ranked 164 in the world. As Thomas described it:
Murray eventually struggled through to win this match and went on to win
the tournament. After his victory in the final, he broke down in tears, and for
several minutes the sobbing wouldn’t stop; he was barely under control when
he stepped up to receive his trophy. It was, he later said, a happier moment
than his original Wimbledon win in 2013, when he first felt the relief of so
much pressure. Presumably, he was sufficiently able to put aside the pressure
in 2016 and, as Serena described it, just worry about his tennis. But as Thomas
said, Andy knows that he will never be able to escape the position in which the
public in his country has put him, the stage on which he must live.
Like Serena Williams and all athletes, Andy operates best at a level of acti-
vation integrated with, but not overwhelming, his great skill and technique—
the complex, highly developed processes existing in his subsymbolic bodily
and sensory systems. The tennis announcers are constantly commenting on
the need for intensity of activation integrated with such complex mental
processes. In describing a great match, they make comments such as, “The
Primary process as a transitional concept 137
adrenalin is pumping for both guys; such a mental battle between these
two.” On the other hand, when a player makes many errors, announcers say
something like, “The adrenalin is flowing, he needs a little more control.” As
one of the announcers at the 2016 Olympics said about Madison Keys, the
powerful young American tennis player who is beginning to come to terms
with the demands of international tennis on center court, “She is showing
more maturity; rather than pulling the trigger with her nerves, she is being
more patient with her power.” It is not that she needs to bind or discharge her
power (in the old energic terms); she needs to be more patient with it, to know
how to use her power, to let it work.
Scientific work is infused with desire. Science has to be driven; you have to
want it; it’s like being in love. The solution to the problem is the object of
desire; you have to be hungry for it. In the broadest sense, I know that a
and b are connected, I have to know how. That is all of science—knowing
there is a connection, not knowing what the connection is, wanting to
know, having to know.
You are writing, say, about a grizzly bear. No words are forthcoming. For
six, seven, ten hours no words have been forthcoming. You are blocked,
frustrated, in despair. You are nowhere, and that’s where you’ve been
getting. What do you do?
There are psychological differences from phase to phase, and the first is
the phase of the pit and the pendulum. After that, it seems as if a different
person is taking over. Dread largely disappears. Problems become less
threatening, more interesting. Experience is more helpful, as if an ama-
teur is being replaced by a professional. Days go by quickly, and not a few
could be called pleasant, I’ll admit.
(McPhee, 2013)
Just as Serena was able to stop worrying about records and slams and
New York crowds, and focus on her tennis, the creative writer moves away
from his state of dread to a state in which he can use his knowledge and skills.
On the other hand, if the work does not go well, the writer, like the creative
scientist, will turn away to regulate himself and to allow his thoughts to open
up. In a letter to his daughter, also a writer, McPhee wrote:
Dear Jenny: What am I working on? How is it going? Since you asked, at
this point I have no confidence in this piece of writing. It tries a number
of things I probably shouldn’t be trying … After four months and nine
days of staring into this monitor for what has probably amounted in
aggregate to something closely approaching a thousand hours, that’s
enough. I’m going fishing.
(McPhee, 2013)
Notes
1 As his friends and colleagues knew (and miss very much now), Lester Luborsky
was a master at illustrating complex conceptual issues with very old jokes.
2 In this discussion, I use the words affect and emotion interchangeably, without
distinguishing between them. There is considerable controversy concerning the
definitions of these terms (and others such as feelings) that will not be discussed here.
3 This is a general definition of symbolic processing to be distinguished from Freud’s
concept of symbolism, as denoting a “mode of indirect and figurative represen-
tation of an unconscious idea, conflict or wish” (Laplanche and Pontalis, 1973,
p. 442).
4 Bernard Maskit (personal communication), whose areas are geometry and top-
ology (and who also, in the interests of full disclosure, is my husband).
5 This could be, in some sense, a difficulty for the scientist as well—the connection,
when discovered, may contradict previous networks of knowledge and be
experienced as disruptive to intellectual organization. In many cases, for the scien-
tist, such discovery is eventually greeted with even greater excitement and desire.
In rare instances, it is possible that the scientist might choose not to recognize this
discovery if it contradicts their life’s work.
References
Arlow, J. (1958). Panel: The psychoanalytic theory of thinking. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 6, 145–153.
Auchincloss, E. L., & Samberg, E. (Eds.). (2012). Psychoanalytic Terms and Concepts.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59,
617–645.
Bucci, W. (1997a). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (1997b). Symptoms and symbols: A multiple code theory of somatization.
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 17, 151–172.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
20, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2002). The referential process, consciousness, and the sense of self.
Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22, 766–793.
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2007). Beneath the surface of the therapeutic interaction: The
psychoanalytic method in modern dress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 55, 1355–1397.
Bucci, W., Maskit, M., & Hoffman, L. (2012). Objective measures of subjective
experience: The use of therapist notes in process-outcome research. Psychodynamic
Psychology, 40(2), 303–340.
Bucci, W., Maskit, M., & Murphy, S. (2015). Connecting emotions and words: The
referential process. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 359–383.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain. New York:
Harcourt.
142 Clinical perspectives
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Kalin, N. H. (2003). Parsing the
subcomponents of emotion and disorders of emotion: Perspectives from affective
neuroscience. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook
of affective sciences (pp. 8–24). New York: Oxford University Press.
Dorpat, T. (2001). Primary process communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 21,
448–463.
Freud, S. (1895). Project for a scientific psychology. Standard Edition, 1, 295–39.
London: Hogarth Pres.
Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Standard Edition, 4 & 5. London:
Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psycho- analysis. Standard Edition, 23, 144–207.
London: Hogarth Press.
Hadamard, J. (1945). The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Holt, R. R. (2002). Quantitative research on the primary process: Method and findings.
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 50, 457–482.
Kant, I. (2007 [1781]). Critique of pure reason, trans. F. M. Muller. Harmondsworth:
Penguin.
Kogan, A. et al. (2014). Vagal activity is quadratically related to prosocial traits, pro-
social emotions, and observer perceptions of prosociality. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 107(6), 1051–1063.
Laplanche, J., & Pontalis, J.- B. (1973). The language of psychoanalysis, trans.
D. Nicholson-Smith. New York: W. W. Norton.
Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic
systems modeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 169–194.
Luborsky, L. (1988). A comparison of three transference related measures applied to
the specimen hour. In H. Dahl, H. Kaechele, & H. Thomae (Eds.), Psychoanalytic
process research strategies (pp. 109–116). New York: Springer-Verlag.
McLaughlin, J. (1978). Primary and secondary processes in the context of cerebral
hemispheric specialization. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 47, 237–266.
McPhee, J. (2013). Draft No. 4. The New Yorker, April 29.
Nauta, W. J. H. (1971). The problem of the frontal lobe: A reinterpretation. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 8, 167–187.
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F.
(2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.
Noy, P. (1969). A revision of the psychoanalytic theory of the primary process.
International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 50, 55–170.
Perrotta, T. (2016). Serena Williams makes history with Wimbledon win, her 22nd
Grand Slam title. Wall Street Journal, July 9.
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 9, 148–158.
Pessoa, L. (2010). Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: From “what is it?” to
“what’s to be done?” Neuropsychologia, 48, 3416–3429.
Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human
amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53.
Primary process as a transitional concept 143
The concept of ineffable was the central theme of the panel where a previous
version of this chapter initially appeared (and of the 2008 Division 39 con-
ference as a whole). There was a related conference in Rome in July 2007 on
Psychoanalytic Theories of Unconscious Mental Functioning and Multiple
Code Theory. Two of the speakers, Giuseppe Moccia and Giuseppe Martini,
both members of the Italian Psychoanalytic Society, took us on a scholarly
guided tour of psychoanalytic and philosophical thought concerning the
domain of implicit or unconscious processes, starting with Freud’s (1915,
p. 166) original insight concerning the nonrepressible part of the uncon-
scious: “Everything that is repressed must remain unconscious; but let us state
at the very outset that the repressed does not cover everything that is uncon-
scious. The unconscious has the wider compass: the repressed is a part of the
unconscious.” Since Freud’s time, the fields of phenomenology and hermen-
eutics have more deeply studied and valorized that wider compass, as Martini
(2007) and Moccia (2007) pointed out, giving it many labels and emphasizing
many different aspects:
• the unrepresentable; the perturbing and ineffable sphere that escapes the
clarifying ambition of interpretation (Martini, 2007)
• the reality that escapes the word (Heidegger, 1982)
• the enigmatic question (Gadamer, 1989)
• the untranslatable (Ricoeur, 1970)
• the incomprehensible (both on a psychopathological level as referring to
delirium, but also in more general philosophical terms, as referring to
bodily experience) (Jaspers, 1963)
• the unthinkable, the unknown, unknowable, infinite without form
(Bion, 1962)
• the unthought known (Bollas, 1987).
There are also related concepts in the writings of Ferenczi, Winnicott, Piera
Aulagnier, Loch, Matte Blanco, Ferrari, and many others.
All these writers, philosophers, and psychoanalysts are attempting to char-
acterize the same epistemological domain, but their characterizations are
divergent and to some extent contradictory. The known that is unthought of
Bollas is different from the unknown, the unknowable of Bion. And both are
different from the incomprehensible of Jaspers and the unrepresentable of
Interplay of processes in treatment 147
Emotion schemas
Emotion schemas are types of memory structures that constitute the organ-
ization of the self in the interpersonal world. They are formed on the basis of
repeated interactions with caretakers and others from the beginning of life.
The subsymbolic sensory, somatic, and motoric representations and
processes constitute the affective core of the emotion schema—the source
of the varieties of arousal and pleasure and pain that constitute emotional
Interplay of processes in treatment 149
experience. In each event of life, the processes of the affective core will be
activated in relation to the people, places, and activities that figure in that
event; thus we build memories of people and events that give us pleasure or
pain, that activate happiness, or dread, or a wish to attack. Autobiographical
memory is built out of such events; this is the basis for the organization of the
self in the interpersonal world.
The emotion schemas develop in an interpersonal context; the baby who
laughs and smiles and has feelings of joy can see and hear the other person also
smiling and laughing and making the corresponding sounds; the expressions
of the other become incorporated in the schema of joy. If the child who cries
hears sympathetic sounds and sees a particular facial expression, along with
feeling a soothing touch, the child’s schemas of pain or fear will develop to
incorporate responses of turning to others and expectations that others can
help. If the caretaker typically responds to the child’s cries with annoyance or
withdrawal, schemas of negative expectations and associated responses will
develop.
some points interfere with the flow of the experience and at other times will
facilitate it.
this to a relatively high degree and may have somewhat more of a sense of
safety in negotiating the troubled waters.
What does the analyst bring, what does the analyst need, to support work
in this mode? Here are a few possibilities:
References
Bion, W. R. (1962). A theory of thinking. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis,
43, 306–310.
Bollas, C. (1987). The shadow of the object: Psychoanalysis of the unthought known.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Bromberg, P. M. (2006). The analyst’s “self-revelation”: Not just permissible, but
necessary. In Awakening the dreamer: Clinical journeys (pp. 128–150). Mahwah,
NJ: The Analytic Press.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
21, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2007a). Dissociation from the perspective of multiple code theory: Part
I. Psychological roots and implications for psychoanalytic treatment. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 43, 165–184.
Bucci, W. (2007b). Dissociation from the perspective of multiple code theory: Part
II. The spectrum of dissociative processes in the psychoanalytic relationship.
Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 43, 305–326.
Bucci, W. (2010) The uncertainty principle in the psychoanalytic process. In J. Petrucelli
(Ed.), Knowing, not-knowing and sort-of-knowing: Psychoanalysis and the experience
of uncertainty (pp. 203–214). London: Karnac Books.
Freud, S. (1915). The unconscious. Standard Edition, 14, 166. London: Hogarth Press.
Gadamer, H. G. (1989). Hermeneutics and psychiatry. In H. G. Gadamer (Ed.), The
enigma of health: The art of healing in a scientific age. J. Gaiger & N. Walker, Trans.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Hadamard, J. (1949). An essay on the psychology of invention in the mathematical field.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Heidegger, M. (1982 [1959]). On the way to language (P. D. Hertz, Trans.). San
Francisco: Harper & Row.
Jaspers, K. (1963). General psychopathology. J. Hoenig & M. W. Hamilton Trans.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Martini, G. (2007). New prospects on unconscious mental functioning and their
reflections on the clinical practice. Paper presented at Conference of the Italian
Psychoanalytic Society and the International Psychoanalytical Association, Rome.
Interplay of processes in treatment 155
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & Hinton, G. E. (1989). The appeal of parallel
distributed processing. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & PDP Research
Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of
cognition (Vol. 1: Foundations, pp. 3–44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Moccia, G. (2007). Psychoanalytic theories of unconscious mental functioning and
multiple code theory. Paper presented at Conference of the Italian Psychoanalytic
Society and the International Psychoanalytical Association, Rome.
Reik, T. (1964 [1948]). Listening with the third ear: The inner experience of a psychoana-
lyst. New York: Pyramid.
Ricoeur, P. (1970). Freud and philosophy: An essay on interpretation. New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Chapter 9
without an adequate capacity for multiple states and functions will lead a
limited life. People call on a pool of dissociative and integrative processes to
manage the wide range of challenges and problems of life; these may involve
positive explorations or retreat from experience. Sometimes the solutions
that are adaptive in one context will turn out to be maladaptive in others;
treatment may also involve further dissociation as well as new integration.
My second major point concerns our understanding of the nature of trau-
matic experience, its challenge to personality organization, and the various
ways in which people use the tools of adaptation that they possess to respond
to this challenge. Just as we see a theoretical tension in the definition of dis-
sociative processes, we must also recognize a similar kind of tension in the
definition of trauma and traumatic events.
The specific nature of trauma is a psychic injury that remains unhealed.
The process begins as an adaptive response to danger; the human organism,
like all organisms, mobilizes its defenses against a threat, with immediate
responses of fight or flight, in their many variations. In adaptive functioning,
the emergency response is regulated when the external danger is past. In some
cases, however, the regulation, resetting the response system to nonemergency
mode, does not occur or occurs only partially. The person appears unable to
register changes in their situation and continues to respond as if danger were
present or imminent; thus the initial response patterns of avoidance or attack
are replayed in a broadening range of situations, rather than modulated in
the context of the person’s current circumstances and current powers. The
expectations of danger and the protective responses may become dangers in
themselves, preventing the healing of the psychic wound that might occur nat-
urally over time. Treatment may activate the threat of danger and elicit further
defense; this is the “vicious circle” of the treatment of traumatic disorders.
Before I address the mechanisms underlying this vicious circle of treatment,
however, I wish to make a general point about the circularity of diagnosis that
complicates the problem further, but also provides a potential escape.
The girl opens up her face (her mouth opens, her eyes widen, her eyebrows
rise) and then closes it back, in a series of changes whose contour can be
represented by a smooth arch. Mother responds by intoning “Yeah” with
a pitch line that rises and falls as the volume crescendos and decrescendos
… The mother’s prosodic contour has matched the child’s facial-kinetic
contour.
(Stern, 1985, p. 140)
playing out of the schema in the present, the here and now. In either case,
whether through retrieval from memory or as enactment, the activation of an
emotion schema involves not only words and images, but also some degree
of arousal of the sensory and bodily experiences of the affective core. Just as
visual images are now known to activate the same neural pathways involved
in visual perception, the activation of the affective core of a schema involves
actual physical pathways of pleasure and pain happening in the body in the
present, to varying degrees.
The activation of the affective core in connection to the people and events
of life is crucial to the emotional information-processing system, to enable
emotional evaluation of events as they occur in terms of their impact on the
person’s well-being. The person perceives an element of the event—an object
in a particular place and at a specific time—or retrieves it from memory; the
emotional information about this event comes from the activation of the
subsymbolic sensory and somatic functions of the affective core. In adaptive
functioning, that is how we use feelings to evaluate events, to know whether
something is good or bad for us.
The child then attempts to deal with the threat in some way. They cannot
realistically attack or escape physically; they are small and weak and fear
being abandoned. What the child can do is turn attention away from the
threat and from the perception of the caretaker as the source of terror; dis-
sociation of the emotion schemas occurs through such a process. Bromberg
(2001, pp. 904–905) writes about a patient who says:
When I was little and I got scared—scared because Mommy was going to
beat me up—I’d stare at a crack in the ceiling or a spider web on a pane
of glass, and pretty soon I’d go into this place where everything was kind
of foggy and far away, and I was far away too, and safe. At first, I had to
stare real hard to get to this safe place. But then one day Mommy was
really beating on me and without even trying I was there, and I wasn’t
afraid of her. I knew she was punching me, and I could hear her calling
me names, but it didn’t hurt and I didn’t care. After that, anytime I was
scared, I’d suddenly find myself there, out of danger and peaceful. I’ve
never told anybody about it, not even Daddy. I was afraid to because
I was afraid that if other people knew about it, the place might go away,
and I wouldn’t be able to get there when I really needed to.
was waiting for death, and therapy was simply one more ritual among many”
(1998, p. 323).
The second major strategy of treatment, working toward the goals of reinte-
gration, requires the patient to break through the rituals and confront the
demons, to allow the activation of the dreaded schema in the present to some
degree, with its potential risks and rewards. Elsewhere (Bucci, 2002, 2003),
I have discussed in detail how the referential process works in the context of
the treatment relationship to bring about changes in the emotion schema—
changes in what we perceive and feel, and what we expect from others, not
only changes in what we do. This basic process applies in any uncovering
treatment, with variations depending on the nature of the emotional disorder.
The referential process involves three major phases: (1) arousal of the
affective core of the emotion schema; (2) experiencing imagery of a spe-
cific episode and telling it in concrete detail or reenacting aspects of it; and
(3) some reflection and examination of the episode. Reintegration of the
dissociated schemas requires repeated playing out of these phases in the inter-
personal context of the relationship, so that the affective core itself gradually
undergoes change in relation to perception of the present, memories of the
past, and expectations of the future. The change in the subsymbolic processes
of the affective core in relation to imagery and perception of objects and
events is what we mean by working through.
The referential process applies in treatment of all disorders, whether or not
specifically trauma related; the following specific issues need to be confronted
when one is working with patients with severe disorders reflecting massive
dissociation within and between the emotion schemas:
She was walking along the top of a seawall that began to get narrower
and narrower until she was at a place she couldn’t go forward without
falling into an abyss. But she couldn’t go back because she couldn’t turn
around. The scene then shifted to her looking at herself in a mirror and
suddenly noticing a second head growing out of the side of her own head.
The face wasn’t there yet, and she was terrified of it appearing. She didn’t
want to see it.
(Bromberg, 1998, p. 325)
In allowing herself to dream the dream, she was conveying that although
she felt her analysis might be leading her toward ‘the black hole’ of
madness she was no longer accepting the existential deadness of dissoci-
ation as the price for escaping potential retraumatization.
In time, over the course of the analytic work, Christina was able to experi-
ence anxiety for the first time and distinguish it from the traumatic dread that
Dissociation Part I: Psychological roots 171
had been her constant companion, telling her she was always on the edge of
the “black hole.” She could now recognize anxiety as something unpleasant
but bearable—as something she felt rather than a way of addressing the
world. She recognized that she was now taking the risk of pursuing a life that
included self-interest, and that in choosing to live life rather than wait for it,
she had accepted the inevitability of loss, hurt, and ultimately death as part
of the deal (Bromberg (1998, p. 328).
Strachey (1934) discusses the “neurotic vicious circle.” Issues similar to
those noted by Strachey apply in different ways to the broad range of patients
whom analysts see today. We need to recognize the risks and the rewards
of this uncovering process. The tradeoff of psychic numbness coupled with
chaotic intrusion on one side, against vulnerability to pain that is viewed
as unbearable on the other, exists to varying degrees and in different ways
for patients with all emotional disorders, not only for victims of abuse. The
challenge of the treatment is determined by the intensity of the threat and its
meaning for the individual. The challenge also depends on the mechanisms
of repair that were overlaid on the initial dissociation to enable the person to
go on. The circle will be broken as both the estimate of the magnitude of the
danger and the estimate of one’s own strength are revised through explor-
ation in the new context of the treatment relationship. The reward includes
vulnerability to pain and fear, but also feelings like bravery, love and joy—a
sense of self, a connection to others, and a sense of life.
Acknowledgment
Earlier versions of this chapter and Chapter 10 were presented in Palermo,
Italy in November 2005, at an international congress on the assessment
and treatment of traumatic experiences. Portions of these chapters were
also presented in April 2006 at an all-day seminar on “The Dissociative
Mind: Psychological Roots and Psychoanalytic Processes in Action”, at
Adelphi University, Garden City, Long Island. The collaborative research
and clinical perspective presented in this chapter has roots in a panel on
“Trauma, Dissociation and Conflict: The Space Where Neuroscience,
Cognitive Science, and Psychoanalysis Overlap,” held at the April
2002 meeting of the Division of Psychoanalysis (39) of the American
Psychological Association
References
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders IV (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Bowlby, J. (1969), Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bromberg, P. M. (1998). Standing in the spaces: Essays on clinical process, trauma, and
dissociation. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
172 Clinical perspectives
Timberlake, W., & Lucas, G. A. (1989). Behavior systems and learning: From misbe-
havior to general principles. In S. B. Klein, & R. R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary
Learning Theories (pp. 237–275). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
van der Kolk, B.A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psy-
chobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1, 253–265
van der Kolk, B. A., & Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of
traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
8, 505–525.
Chapter 10
Dissociation—Part II
The spectrum of dissociative processes in
the psychoanalytic relationship
The inside of the ego, which comprises above all the intellective processes,
has the quality of being preconscious. This is characteristic of the ego
and belongs to it alone … The sole quality that rules in the id is that of
being unconscious. Id and unconscious are as intimately united as ego
and preconscious; indeed, the former connection is even more exclusive.
In that passage, the insights that motivated Freud to revise his theory are
somehow lost. Freud’s unresolved struggle in characterizing the domain of
thought outside of standard linguistic and logical forms remains with us today.
We have difficulty emerging from the shadow of the energy model, playing
out in the concepts of unconscious, id, and primary process forms associated
with forbidden wishes and drives. Psychoanalysis retains a deep-rooted, but
somewhat unacknowledged, view of the nonverbal as the “other,” in the post-
modern sense of the other—the alien, the outsider, the not fully known, with
a corollary assumption that the full sense of knowing, in consciousness, must
involve standard logical principles and verbal thought. Within this frame-
work, the goal of treatment is to occupy that alien domain—to make the
unconscious conscious, to place ego where id has been.
Outside psychoanalysis, in cognitive science, neuroscience, and related
fields, there is now widespread recognition of multiple modalities of thought,
and more complex views of the features of the different systems. The char-
acterization of the systems and the basis for their differentiation has been
a matter of intensive empirical investigation and revision during the past
several decades. As I discussed in Chapter 9 (Bucci, 2007), the new findings
concerning the organization of thought that have emerged in empirical
research need to be considered in developing the psychoanalytic theory and
as a guide for treatment.
The multiple code theory retains the core psychoanalytic premise of
diverse modes of mentation, without basing the systemic distinctions on par-
ticular contents associated with fantasies, wishes, or desires on one hand, or
principles of reality on the other. The multiple code theory also retains the
fundamental psychoanalytic insight concerning interaction among cognition,
emotion, and bodily experience without calling on drives or related energic
concepts.
In contrast to the assumptions of the metapsychology, the modalities
characterized as subsymbolic and symbolic nonverbal and verbal forms of
thought are not distinguished as more or less dominant, higher order and
lower order ways of knowing, or as more or less alien, but as operating dif-
ferently from one another, following different principles. Status of conscious-
ness is not a determining factor in this differentiation; the relationship of
the different modes of thought to conscious or unconscious states and to
wishes and bodily needs is complex. Different states, different ways of being
and knowing may be activated and may be within awareness at certain times;
176 Clinical perspectives
social supports. The person (or other organism) then gradually returns to the
pre-threat state, the normal way of being.
Elizabeth Howell (2005) describes her experience on the morning of
September 11, 2001: exiting the subway at the station before the World Trade
Center, seeing the “twin towers three short blocks away, burning rapidly, like
matchsticks”; rushing across the Brooklyn Bridge to her home in Brooklyn;
closing her windows “against the now arrived black cloud of soot and burned
remains.” She says, “I remained calm. It was a heartrendingly emotional
time, but I thought that psychologically, I was fine.” We can see this as the
analgesia of the freezing state, the built-in physiological protection against
overwhelming arousal. Howell knew it was a “heartrendingly emotional time,”
but she was somehow insulated from it.
The freezing response gradually lifted; the impact of the experience then hit
her a few days later:
The numbness persisted throughout the war and into the liberation
(Kraft, 2004):
as more painful than the original events; the responses of numbing and anal-
gesia that accompanied the actual onslaughts of terror were not in place.
For many of the survivors, the strategies of distraction became less effective
as the demands of life eased:
Alex H. says that for many years after the war, he was so involved in the
fight for a new existence that he did not think about the past. Beginning
with no family, no schooling, and the wrong language, Alex says the daily
fight to establish himself used all his energy. In fact, he suppressed his
time in the concentration camp until 3 years before he came in to give
testimony. He describes the result of having accomplished his goals. “My
past is starting to haunt me … and I feel so depressed, very often. That
I actually feel that I—very often feel that I lived long enough.” [Testimony
of Alex H., 1985, p. 376]
As Kraft notes, Arnold C. did not talk about his emotional response or show
the disgust or anger that might have been activated in him by the narrative
(and that is generated in the listener or reader). “His motivation is to tell the
events clearly and directly” (Kraft, 2004, p. 364).
For many of the survivors, however, the retelling of specific episodes in the
oral testimony had the effect of activating emotional experience that had been
dissociated and the reliving was experienced as intolerable. There are many
examples of survivors who cry during testimony while describing a specific
event, express surprise at their emotions and then often apologize, indicating
182 Clinical perspectives
that they cannot control the pain of the memory and attributing it to the recall
of the specific event. There were some survivors who gave testimony more
than once. At the second interview, as Kraft (Kraft, 2004, p. 364) describes,
they all reported that giving testimony was deeply distressing, and they were
all surprised at the intensity of their distress. One said, “I didn’t realize that
it’s going to take me to the depths of depression for months. I didn’t realize
it.” Another reported that she needed to be tranquilized afterward because of
the powerful emotion that was released.
When they could tell their stories in general terms, they experienced the
value of educating new generations and commemorating the lives of those
who were lost. They may have gained a sense of purpose, hope, and sharing:
Alan Z. said he does not cry when he talks to individuals and to larger
groups about the loss of his family during the Holocaust: “Only when
I go back that far is there a lot of detail. You see, when you go to speak
somewhere to a school or to the synagogue, I don’t go into these details
where it makes me emotional.” [p. 364].
They learned to avoid the triggers, internal or external, in memory or
in life, that had the potential to activate the schemas.
I think the problem is … I’m afraid if I open it up, I’m going to have
nightmares that I had for years and years, and I will not allow this … I’m
afraid it might destroy me [Testimony of Martin S., 1988, p. 351].
We see the avoidance of detail throughout the testimonies, in many
cases stated quite explicitly:
The only question was, “Where were you during the war?” “I was in a
concentration camp.” That’s it. “I was in the partisans.” That’s it. “I was
hiding in the—some place.” That’s it. Nobody spoke any details. It seems
that the people wanted to block it out from their mind [Testimony of
Ruth A, 1994, p. 365].
Certain people, they stay with you and they can’t get away, they can’t,
they just can’t get away. Anyone, if he thinks, he sees the hole in his heart,
Dissociation Part II: Dissociative processes 183
She reported by telephone the next day that she’d had all of one hour
of really feeling good after that session. She said that she could feel her
body, her mind was clear and crisp, and she had a lot of energy. But she
was reporting this in the context of “Is that all that I get, one hour?!”
What she went on to say was that as soon as the hour had passed her
mind was flooded with new thoughts, images, sensations, and other pieces
of memory from an abortion, as a teenager. She was terribly distraught,
miserable, and the feeling of her suffering was again the most salient
experience in talking with J.
What this means is that analysts are necessarily genuine in their emotional
communication. The analyst is communicating what they feel, independent
of what they might say, even when they are not explicitly aware of what they
feel; and the actual emotional meaning of their expression is received by
the patient even when the patient may not be explicitly aware of what that
meaning is.
We are now beginning to know more about the wiring that connects
internal experience with perception of the expressions of others. Neurons,
termed mirror neurons, have been found in the frontal cortex of monkeys
and humans. These mirror neurons represent, in an individual’s brain, the
movements (or expressions) that the brain sees in another individual, and
produce signals to sensory and motoric structures so that the corresponding
movements or expressions are either “previewed” in simulation mode or actu-
ally executed in trace form by the viewer (Rizzolati et al., 1996; Rizzolati,
Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001). The implications of these new findings are poten-
tially enormous for understanding emotional communication in development
and throughout life.
Change in the emotion schemas depends on the connection between what
the patient knows emotionally about their own self, about the analyst, and
about their relationship, and what the analyst is expressing. What the patient
knows emotionally that is invalidated by the analyst raises the risk of reinfor-
cing the dissociated schema rather than enabling new connections. Bromberg
(1994, p. 356) expresses this precisely:
Analysts can decide how to work in treatment, while recognizing that what
they feel will be communicated on some level. This communication will occur
in a range of channels in face-to-face treatment and will occur in auditory
channels, paralinguistic as well as linguistic, when the patient is on the couch.
188 Clinical perspectives
I gather … that neither of you [Jung and Pfister] has yet acquired the
necessary coolness in practice, that you still engage yourselves, give away
a good deal of yourselves in order to demand a similar response. Permit
me, the venerable old master, to warn that one is invariably mistaken
in applying this technique, that one should rather remain unapproach-
able, and insist upon receiving. Never let us be driven crazy by our poor
neurotics.
(From Falzeder, 1994, p. 314; discussed by Friedman, 1997, p. 27)
The patient must be emotionally engaged, under the influence of the ana-
lytic situation, yet Freud (in his somewhat burnt state following the treatment
of Frau Hirschfeld) was saying that the analyst must remain unengaged.
Friedman (1997, 2005) has addressed these issues from a somewhat clas-
sical perspective in two searching (and engaged) papers. In the more recent
paper, he examines many aspects of the analyst’s involvement and response
or nonresponse to the patient’s appeal. He addresses the question of whether
“there might be a universal and peculiarly psychoanalytic something in the
analyst’s feelings that somehow deserves the name of love, since analysts
through the generations have seemed to think so”:
The patient may look for ordinary (forthright) love … but the classical
analyst hopes to avoid it, because, as Nussbaum points out, the pressure
Dissociation Part II: Dissociative processes 189
Martha Nussbaum (2005, p. 379; italics added), referring to the Stoic view
of the emotions, has argued that all major emotions have one central feature
in common: “the thought that the emotion’s object matters greatly for the
life of the person experiencing the emotion.” It is this element that Freud in
his writings on technique, like the Stoics, felt it important to avoid, as setting
oneself up for damages and reversals; putting oneself at the mercy of fortune,
as Nussbaum describes. It is this element that patients truly seek, and the
absence of this element, the absence of longing and suffering, of true human
vulnerability, is experienced by the patient, correctly in Nussbaum’s terms,
as the absence of actual “real” love. Friedman (2005, p. 386) concludes on a
note of failure concerning his attempt to identify a particular psychoanalytic
“something” that may be characterized as love: were the analyst to settle into
a love relationship in the ordinary sense, Friedman says, the patient’s freedom
would be at risk, since love is, to varying degrees, necessarily “confining and
demanding of the beloved,” and would in any case not be the idealized love
for which the patient yearns (p. 386).
The new work in affective neuroscience brings the question of the authen-
ticity of the analyst’s response front and center in a new way. The stoic
solution of avoidance is not sufficient. The analyst needs to experience
real activation, longing, suffering, vulnerability; to really care; to really feel
attacked; not in an “as if ” sense. The analyst’s experience must be real in
the moment.
Here is where our new understanding of dissociative processes as normal,
adaptive, and indeed necessary in emotional functioning provides a possible
resolution of the dilemma posed by Freud, and by Friedman, Nussbaum, and
many others. The analyst’s emotional experience, their schema of interaction
with the patient, could be fully genuine in the moment, but in a local and
dissociated form.
The fundamental analytic attitude that is needed here is to recognize that
there could be other emotional states and will be others while subjective con-
sciousness, working memory, is engaged with any given state. The particular
nature of this dissociation that makes it both tolerable and effective is that,
while one emotion schema is aroused and dominant in working memory, the
analyst knows that there are others in the wings. The schema that is activated
is genuine in the moment, but in the context of a different emotional frame-
work, including background knowledge that it is only one state, and that there
are others that will be activated in different contexts, and that they are all held
within one overall autobiographical frame.
190 Clinical perspectives
All of our self-states are self in relation to others. We each have a pool
of affective components and response patterns that emerge in different situ-
ations, just as we can know we are different with different people and hold
this knowledge within a more or less unitary sense of an autobiographical
self. Sometimes, of course, for everyone, the internal worlds collide; nothing
is simple.
I think we can talk about effective analytic work in this way—the power to
maintain diversity in one’s persona, expressed in particular forms in particular
contexts, connected sufficiently to the spine of autobiographical memory. It is
not only emotional authenticity in interaction with the patient, but also emo-
tional insight that is facilitated for the analyst by the capacity to enter flexibly
into different states that are activated by the actual interpersonal context. The
analyst can know emotionally only what they can feel, and they can process
and work only with what they can know.
itself. As for the patient who is able to open new connections, the reward of
our psychoanalytic self-examination will be new discoveries and a more vital
and expanding field.
Note
1 Drawn from a collection of more than 4000 testimonies held at the Fortunoff
Video Archive for Holocaust testimonies at Yale University.
References
Arlow, J. A., & Brenner, C. (1964). Psychoanalytic concepts and the structural theory.
New York: International Universities Press.
Bromberg, P. M. (1994). “Speak! That I may see you”: Some reflections on dissoci-
ation, reality, and psychoanalytic listening. The Journal of Analytical Psychology,
4, 517–547.
Bromberg, P. M. (1998). Standing in the spaces: Essays on clinical process, trauma, and
dissociation. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Bromberg, P. M. (2001). Treating patients with symptoms— and symptoms with
patience: Reflections on shame, dissociation and eating disorders. The Journal of
Analytical Psychology, 11, 891–912.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
21, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2007). Dissociation from the perspective of multiple code theory—Part
I: Psychological roots and implications for psychoanalytic treatment. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 41, 132–184.
Chefetz, R. (2006). Suffering as relatedness and affect regulations. Presented at
Seminar of International Society for the Study of Dissociation, Garden City, NY.
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.
Falzeder, E. (1994). My grand-patient, my chief tormenter: A hitherto unnoticed case
of Freud’s and the consequences. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 63, 297–331.
Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Standard Edition, 4 & 5. London:
Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psycho- analysis. Standard Edition, 23, 139–207.
London: Hogarth Press.
Friedman, L. (1997). Ferrum, ignis and medicina: Return to the crucible. Journal of
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 45, 20–36.
Friedman, L. (2005). Is there a special psychoanalytic love? Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 53, 349–375.
Gee, J. P. (1986). Units in the production of narrative discourse. Discourse Processes,
9, 391–422.
Howell, E. F. (2005). The dissociative mind. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Kraft, R. N. (2002). Memory perceived: Recalling the Holocaust. Westport, CT: Praeger.
192 Clinical perspectives
Embodied communication
and therapeutic practice
In the consulting room with Clara,
Antonio, and Ann
addressed the question in terms of the unconscious mind and was not par-
ticularly troubled by the problem. He saw this communication as immediate
and direct, similar to the mechanism of the telephone:
Just as the receiver converts back into sound waves the electric oscillations
in the telephone lines which were set up by sound waves, so the doctor’s
unconscious is able, from the derivatives of the unconscious which
are communicated to him, to reconstruct the unconscious, which has
determined the patient’s free associations.
(Freud, 1912, p. 115)
The new findings in the fields of affective and social neuroscience offer
a more systematic understanding of such perception of the experience of
another person, including an emphasis on the therapist’s recognition of
factors within their own self, as well as in the patient, that may contribute
to such perception. These findings have been influential in developing the
multiple code theory, including the concept of the referential process with its
three functions of arousal, symbolizing/narrative, and reflecting/reorganizing.
So as to illustrate in more detail the operation of this process, I offer
here three case examples drawn from recent papers. In these examples, the
therapists are working from quite different theoretical frames of reference,
and yet each is demonstrating a keen sense of the centrality of somatic and
subsymbolic experience—within themselves as well as their patients.1
In presenting these cases, I want to focus on the interactions that are
associated with embodied communication; these are dominant in the arousal
phase of the referential process and also remain active throughout the
treatment. This bodily communication provides the emotional context for
associated exploration in narrative and also in the enactments of the schema
in the therapeutic relationship. The patient “knows” the other and their own
self in relation to the other in their own subsymbolic system; the therapist
also “knows” the patient through the complementary entrainment. The
therapist’s response, in their own emotion circuitry, to the playing out of the
Embodied communication in consulting room 195
patient’s expression, is the best available source of knowledge and entry into
further shared knowledge of the patient’s dissociated and distorted schema.
This “knowing” of oneself is unlikely to be explicit for the therapist, at least
at first; they will need to go through a version of the referential process to
recognize the nature of their own experience. In this process, the therapist
will have associations and memories and will also reflect on these—including
examination of the way in which the experiences of their own life contributes
to their perception of the patient’s experience.
The converse of this, which needs to be recognized in all treatment
approaches, including the various forms of cognitive and behavioral and
exposure treatments, as well as experiential and client-centered approaches,
is that the therapist’s real emotional experience of the patient, including
experience of which they may not be aware, will necessarily be received by
the patient and responded to within their embodied emotional system. All
therapists need to recognize that this is occurring when making their decisions
regarding how to work.
I will first discuss the complex story of the treatment of Clara, which was
presented by her therapist, Dr. Marina Amore, in Milan in November 20172
and has been developed further in Amore (2012). In my comments at that
conference, in response to Dr. Amore’s presentation, I discussed Clara’s
emotional issues from the theoretical perspective of multiple code theory.
I also raised several questions concerning the treatment process, including
Dr. Amore’s experience of herself as well as her experience of Clara and their
relationship. My discussion here will include changes in the therapist as well
as the patient in the interactive field of the therapy, and in the drama of the
termination phase. These comments refer both to the initial presentation and
to Amore (2012).
describes it, “the flow of words is continuous” between them, but “words are
never enough to grasp and describe the ‘dark’ experiences that pervade her
inner world.” Clara talks about herself, but is frustrated by a sense of not
being able to understand what she really feels; she has the impression that her
speech is always incomplete, that she is incomplete. She is not able to describe
the horror she feels that leads to the panic attacks; not able to connect to the
specific experiences that distress her.
Marina suggests that they increase the frequency of the sessions from two
to three times a week, and includes the possibility of calling on the weekend.
The new setting changes the patterns of communication in the session and
provides a stronger foundation for their relationship. Here we can begin to see
the playing out of the phases of the referential process in the session and in
the treatment. This period of the treatment is dominated by increased arousal
of experience in the context of the new relationship. Clara is constantly testing
her experience of Marina in small incremental ways, seeing that Marina does
not react in ways that she learned in her childhood to expect. Yet Marina
senses that Clara continues to keep her at a distance from important aspects
of her experience.
During this period, Clara reports a dream in which they are climbing a
high mountain together, “as in a pilgrimage, to reach a sacred place where
an uncovered sarcophagus rests.” As they near the edge of the sarcophagus,
Clara puts her hand on Marina’s eyes. As Marina reports:
For both of us, the exploration of the dream makes explicit the thought
that the vision of what is her most intimate feeling, experienced fear-
fully as shapeless and unrepresentable, can somehow be intolerable and
harmful to me. If this happened as it happened when the mother saw her
blood, she would find herself alone again.
she knows that Clara is easily influenced by her every word. She knows also
that Clara is very sensitive to all of Marina’s nonverbal expressions, often
interrupting her talk to stare at Marina as if confused. In time, they under-
stand how Clara is responding to Marina’s frown, or a moving of her gaze, or
her smile, or her body movements from one position to another. Any of these
and other of Marina’s nonverbal expressions became unknown factors that
needed to be interpreted in order for Clara to proceed with her self-disclosure
in safety. (See Amore 2012 for more detail on these interactions.)
Clara’s comments also lead Marina to focus on her own bodily experience.
In some situations, Marina feels that her leg muscles are contracted “as if
prepared for a sudden jump forward”; she associates that with her fear that
because of the intensity and tension of their interaction, Clara may experi-
ence a sudden psychotic break. In other instances, when they focus on aspects
of their shared womanhood, Marina is able to experience her own body in a
stronger way and to convey this strength to Clara.
Three years into the work, Clara is no longer suffering from symptoms
of neuralgia or the panic attacks. She has become increasingly able to talk
about life experiences that had previously been felt as unbearable, as these
now emerge in memories and dreams. She connects the occurrence of the
attacks of neuralgia to a painful and annihilating sense of loneliness in her
childhood. She reports re-experiencing these feelings as an adult while lying
on the sofa in her living room, waiting in vain for someone to come and
reassure and comfort her.
Here we see evidence that movement into a symbolic narrative phase is
occurring more consistently in the sessions during this period of treatment,
and that she is more able to report fantasies, dreams and narratives of past and
future events. As Clara is building new connections between her somatic and
sensory experiences and her representations of others in the treatment, the
therapist is becoming a new “other” connected to Clara’s self-representation.
During this process, Clara is also building new connections in her memory of
early life events, and in her current relationships to others; she is able to form
a relationship of love and become a mother.
The development of the functions of the referential process in the treatment
was associated with Clara’s movement from the couch to a face-to-face pos-
ition with Marina. A reduction in the frequency of sessions also occurred
at this time. In the initial phases of the treatment, the use of the couch had
proven effective in helping Clara to maintain a closer contact with her own
volatile and intense inner experience. As the treatment progressed, the couch
and its implications themselves became objects of their exploration, serving
as indicators of changes within each of them and in their relationship. As they
discovered together, the use of the couch initially functioned for Clara to pre-
serve Marina as a supportive presence. As Marina observes (Amore, 2012),
“In this way, not even I could see, reflected by the frightened-frightening
Embodied communication in consulting room 199
expression on her face, her frailties. Clara needed to preserve me, as we had
also seen in the sarcophagus’s dream in which she covered my eyes from
something that could not be looked at.” The shift from the couch to face-
to-face contact reflected their shared observation that Clara had over time
come to feel more confident about Marina’s emotional solidity in the face
of her frailties, and more safe and confident in their relationship. As Marina
described in her conference paper:
This awareness now made her eager to look at me; defying the risk of
seeing the negative responses that she had fantasized for so long a time.
Now she could meet my gaze where finally she could see herself being
seen in her frailties. This shift in setting made possible a long phase of
comparison and mirroring that followed.
This can be seen as a time in which Clara was more able to talk about her
experience and reflect on it, and in which more lasting changes in her emotion
schemas may be seen.
Now Clara moves among images. The white box becomes a white room,
empty and isolated from the rest of the world. She sees herself as a little
girl, with her back turned, standing in front of an old radio that used to
belong to her grandparents. She is surprised by the memory. She wonders
why her parents gave it away; she is sorry they did. She watches herself
turn the knob trying to tune into a station. Then her attention is caught
by something little Clara is holding in her hand. “Oh my God … I forgot
all about it … It’s a dog … my stuffed animal! I never parted with it …
How could I have forgotten about it all these years?”
This is movement into the symbolizing phase, from proto symbol to symbol.
She moves on from talking about the pain to visual imagery and then to
images retrieved from the past. She smiles as she tells Marina how much com-
fort this stuffed animal had given her when she felt frightened and in danger.
She remembers that, “like the radio, one day it was simply gone: her parents
had given it to a younger cousin because, as they later explained, she had
Embodied communication in consulting room 201
outgrown it” (Amore 2012, p. 250). She has no recollection of how she felt at
that moment, or whether she cried, but now she is able to mourn the loss of
this object that was so important to her. Marina writes (Amore 2012, p. 250):
At the end of the session Clara’s neuralgia is gone and she feels she has
recovered an important part of her experience. I feel the same and am
deeply moved by the process I have witnessed and shared. I’m touched by
this unexpected finding, Clara can see it by my wet eyes.
I suggest that what was occurring here in Clara’s experience was activation
of the affective core of her early dissociated emotion schema of pain and
dread. It seems likely that the visual images she experienced in the moment in
Marina’s office, along with the attack of neuralgia, were components of the
painful experiences of her childhood, part of the affective core of an emotion
schema that were activated in the session at the point of termination. Visual
events, including the episodes of perceptual dysmorphism, were aspects of her
initial presenting complaints at the onset of her treatment, and visual events
such as flashes, and momentary brightening, which are associated with anx-
iety and prolonged stress (Sabel et al. 2018), may have been associated with
her attacks of neuralgia. Now she experiences these painful and frightening
feelings in the context of the new relationship with Marina, who cares for her
and is able to mourn with her. This type of experience may have happened
repeatedly in the treatment, perhaps in less dramatic and clear ways, and has
presumably also happened in her relationships outside the treatment, so that
new schemas involving new representations of her own self in relation to
others were gradually being built.
associated with that as well. Her parents took it away: she could do without
it; she did not need it. As the end of the treatment approaches, she is able to
let the memory and the sense of need return. She now has an “object”—the
new relationship with Marina has been internalized—that she can keep with
her, while still being a competent, brave self.
An explicit reference to shame emerges in Marina’s conference paper
with respect to the sexual advances from the professor who apparently
raped Clara; she is ashamed to have been a passive object of a man’s sexual
pleasure, ashamed to have been sexually disappointing to him. Her strategy
of distancing and normalizing the event doesn’t work; the teacher becomes
cold and detached. The first episode of dysmorphism happens in this
period; she also begins to engage in promiscuous sexual encounters without
desire or pleasure. Her attempt to write a thesis is compromised and she
withdraws from the work, although still maintaining some connection to
her university.
A recurring dream that Clara reports of a child pretending to be asleep
while being abused by an unknown adult couple who claimed to be her parents
seems relevant here; this is associated with the total numbness of her body
during her sexual encounters. I would be interested in the relationship of this
and other dreams to the avoidance of shame in her early self-representation.
In further examination of this complex case, I would also be interested in an
association between her relationship with her father, whom she might have
hoped to please by being strong, brave and intellectual, and her later self-
abnegating submission to her professor, who uses and then abandons her.
Presumably the working through of the bodily numbness and associated fan-
tasies, perhaps involving her father, could be a topic for another paper.
that activate it. For Clara, a dreaded schema involved her parents’ coldness
and rejection, her experience of this, and her expectation of abandonment
or abuse. Throughout Clara’s life, situations occurred, in many contexts with
many people, that activated this schema, and her painful and frequently
unsuccessful responses, without her recognition of the emotional meaning
of these events.
As Lewis also notes, Barthes also characterizes a photograph “as having the
capacity to wound him but only when ‘the wound is already in me, some-
where in my history’.” Barthes (1981) refers to this particular experience of
shared wounding as punctum and describes it as “irreducibly subjective, unin-
tentional and unpredictable,” related to experience of traumatic loss.
Here I take up the processes of “unworded, embodied” dialogue to which
Lewis refers in the context of the theory of multiple coding and the referen-
tial process (Bucci, 2008, 2011a, 2011b). From this perspective, I frame the
therapeutic interactions in terms of emotional communication rather than
unconscious communication; such interaction is characterized by forms of
nonverbal processing, which may occur at varying levels of awareness. Based
on this approach, I will provides an account of how Antonio and Lewis moved
back and forth between silence and words, and the nature of the connections
between and within them. I will also discuss both the metaphor of the umbil-
ical cord and the experience of shared wounding (or “punctum”) in relation
to the therapeutic process as described here.
Embodied communication in consulting room 205
His response to her earlier question of how she might be of help, saying
that her question sounded like a sexual one, at first seems to be a mani-
festly inappropriate, somewhat provocative (perhaps even passive-aggressive)
response to what seemed to be a standard question; however, this response
also provides information about his mode of interaction that needs to be
understood. In this context, I was struck by Jane’s description of Antonio as
“tall, stunningly handsome, with thick, curly dark hair and a dashing smile.”
He wasn’t just attractive; he had the wow factor—presumably he knew that.
I wondered how this affected their interaction, and how her reaction might
have been communicated to him. Jane acknowledges to herself that she finds
him very attractive, but she feels most concerned about how to work with
him in his silent, frozen, closed position; how to help him and whether he will
return. She acknowledges to him that issues of sexuality are likely to arise and
that her job will be to maintain a safe therapeutic space for him.
In general, Jane stays with his experience, does not question or direct. As
she continues to watch Antonio, in many sessions in the early months, she
feels herself freezing and disappearing; she tries to slow down her breathing
and heart rate, as one might do to stabilize oneself under conditions of threat;
she questions what is wrong with her. She associates her feelings with the
astronauts described by Kohut (1982), “who found unbearable the thought of
circling alone forever in space, ‘deprived of human meaning, human warmth,
human contact, human experience’ ” (cited by Lewis, 2018, p. 496).
In the context of current research on emotional communication, we can
suggest that some of what Antonio is feeling is being directly activated in her,
as she watches and listens and perhaps responds in sensory and bodily modes.
These feelings include physiological functions that she does not yet recognize,
which provide a potential wealth of information concerning Antonio’s experi-
ence, and which are also associated with emotion schemas developed in the
context of her own life. It is not that she is making inferences to Antonio’a
experiences; she is feeling them directly, in herself, in the context of her own
network of emotion schemas.
directly between them through the neural circuitry that allows individuals to
experience the actions and emotions of others. Keysers (2011) uses the term
“shared circuits” to describe the family of neural processes underlying such
communication. Barthes’ (1981) notion of the umbilical cord can be under-
stood as a metaphor for this process.
The communication that involves seeing (or hearing) others perform cer-
tain actions or showing certain bodily or facial expressions (as in a photo-
graph) involves subsymbolic experience, to a large extent within awareness.
What may not be accessible to awareness as connected to those feelings
are the representations of the people and events of life that evoked them.
In the referential process, the activation of the affective core connects to the
instantiations of an emotion schema in each person’s life—the events in which
these sensory and visceral experiences and actions were activated in relation
to other people in other places. Antonio and Jane had related experiences in
their lives—the contexts and specific instantiations were different but similar
components of the affective core had been activated. In Barthes’ terms,
similar wounds are already in each of them, somewhere in their history; still
mainly in subsymbolic form but connected to a schema of being wounded in
a particular way. This is the quality that he calls “punctum.”
For Antonio, the emotion schemas were severely dissociated, or perhaps
the traumas of his life were so intense that organized schemas had never been
formed, so that, in the early phase of treatment, he was apparently unable
to trace the connection of his feelings of pain and dread to the people and
events associated with them; he could not find the emotional meaning for
the feelings that had been aroused. Jane was able to trace connections within
her emotion schemas, to imagery and then in words. This allowed her to find
emotional meaning for her own experience, and presumably, as the treatment
continued, to enable Antonio to find emotional meaning for his.
She was also sweetly naive and maintained a subtly ironic sense of humor
about the struggles in her lonely life. I knew that she was profoundly
lonely, but I never quite understood how she kept herself so socially
isolated.
One evening, he happens to see Ann as she enters a movie theatre where he
is seated:
In the theater, I barely recognized this woman hunched down into her
overcoat, arms held tightly at her sides, unkempt hair over her face,
moving like a street person with the thorazine shuffle. She walked up
and down the aisle several times before choosing a seat far from others.
I could not tell if she had seen me.
(Cornell, 2008, p. 41)
As he watched Ann, Bill saw someone very different from the woman he usu-
ally saw in his office, and began to have a sense of the mechanisms that kept
her so alone.
In the next session, Ann indirectly acknowledged having seen him in the
theatre—asking what he thought of the movie. After responding to that,
he told her that he had seen her in the theater but couldn’t tell whether she
had seen him. She said that it looked like he was with a friend so she didn’t
want to intrude. “I was alone, as usual,” she said. With considerable care, Bill
then tells her that if he hadn’t known her, he would have found her way of
coming into the theater rather frightening, that her whole demeanor seemed
to emanate “Leave me the fuck alone.” Even knowing her, he said, he didn’t
feel he could approach her to say hello; all he could feel was the signal to stay
away. He asked her if that was what she was feeling and if that was what she
wanted to communicate. Ann was startled:
NO! Is that really what I look like? What I’m feeling is that everybody else
is at the movies with a friend, a partner, a boyfriend, a family, and I’m
alone, always alone, and people are staring at me. I hate it. I try to find
212 Clinical perspectives
a seat where I won’t bother anybody, and where I don’t have to see the
couples. I hate it so much that most of the time I can’t even get out the
door to go to the movie. But I didn’t know I looked so weird.
(Cornell, 2008, pp. 41–42)
He could see her anxiety and shame overwhelm her. He tells Ann that it felt
important to describe to her what he experienced, that he was worried that
it might shame her, but thought there was a lot they can learn from this. He
then suggests that “they bring the body that was in the theater” into his office:
I suggested that she put her coat back on, hunch into it, and shuffle into the
office. I felt sick to my stomach as I watched. I wanted to move to her, to tell
her to pull the hair out of her face, to look at me, or to do or say something
kind to her. I asked her to notice any feelings that came up in her and to
allow her body to move in any way it needed. Gradually, she became still and
then slumped to her knees, curling over, pulling her coat over her head. She
looked to me now like she was awaiting a beating. I thought of her stories
of beatings by her father, the teasing and taunting by her brothers, the delu-
sional ravings of her mother. But I did not feel compassionate. I felt irritated.
(Cornell, 2008, p. 42)
Bill was moving from his complex, subsymbolic bodily responses of revul-
sion and caring to begin to process the enactment in his symbolic system, by
connecting to stories of her childhood and her family. His perception of Ann
may also be arousing in him the feelings and associated responses she elicited
in her family at that time:
She just knelt there, curled over and inert. I wanted to kick her. I got
bored. I started thinking ahead to my evening after work. My bladder
began to ache. I wanted the session over. I felt I’d made a mistake in
talking to her about the theater, in intervening this way.
(Cornell, 2008, p. 42)
He does not act on the response plan that is aroused; he recognizes it expli-
citly and manages it through various processes of avoidance, defense and
reflection:
Still, she did not move. I forced myself to look at her inert form. She
looked like a supplicant. I began thinking of my Catholic upbringing
(Ann was also raised Catholic)—forced to genuflect, to kneel, to pray
for forgiveness, awaiting the sound of the nuns’ clickers informing us we
could stand up and move on. Submission . Defeat. Hatred. An object of
derision and disgust.
(Cornell, 2008, p. 42)
Embodied communication in consulting room 213
The interaction of Ann and Bill illustrates the recursive and interactive
sequence of perception/action mirroring, emotional experiencing, and antici-
pation that is associated with embodied communication, and how this may
play out and be used in the therapeutic context. Ann’s perception of the inter-
personal world has grown to incorporate the experience of people as staring
at her, shaming her, ridiculing her, abusing her. She has learned to respond
to that before it happens. Her anticipatory response is self-fulfilling; Bill feels
sick to his stomach, he is bored, he wants to kick her, to get away from her,
he suffers physically. He has learned through his body how Ann feels and the
feelings that Ann’s demeanor excites in the people around her. These lead him
as well to explicit imagery and associations to her life, and to his own life,
including the Catholic upbringing that he and Ann share, and the associated
feelings.
The occurrence of his negative reactions in a sense seems to raise questions
concerning the claim I made earlier that the therapist’s feelings would neces-
sarily be communicated to the patient. Bill does not kick her as he has the
urge to do, but he does feel boredom, anger, and the wish to escape. There are
a number of additional distinctions that need to be made, which he does not
explicate. This interaction occurred in the context of several years of work
together, several years of countless interactions in which he has seen and
reacted to multiple aspects of her—her sweet naiveté, her sense of humor;
countless times in which he has perceived her differently, reacted differently
from the reactions of her family in her early years. He is distressed in many
ways, some of which he explicitly recognizes, some of which he may not. He
wants to get away but does not, he endures his distress, manages it somehow,
explores it, stays with her.
She recognizes that she has evoked distress in him, that she has “scared
him” again, as she did in the theatre; perhaps she intended to do so, per-
haps it is one of her strategies of attack or warding off attack. She also sees
that he has allowed her to show this depth of feeling that she has seen as
unspeakable and has stayed with her. The treatment doesn’t end with the
214 Clinical perspectives
Concluding reflections
It has become increasingly clear that the way of knowing the experience
of others as usually understood in traditional theories of mind, by making
inference from observation of others to their inner experience, needs to
be revised. The multiple code theory, with its component of subsymbolic
processing, was developed in large part in recognition that human know-
ledge and information processing are not adequately modeled as symbol-
manipulating systems. This is a broader formulation that goes beyond the
emphasis on action seen in theories of embodied communication but is com-
patible with those theories, and may provide some elaboration of the under-
lying psychological model.
It is also important to recognize that while the therapist “knows” the other
immediately through their own bodily experience, this knowledge may be
valid or invalid. Therapists need constantly to examine the contribution of
their own life and bodily experiences to their knowledge of the other. The new
findings should not lead psychodynamic therapists to say they have found a
scientific basis for the elusive—and seductive—concept of projective identifi-
cation, but rather a more nuanced understanding of factors in themselves as
well as the patient that contribute to what they perceive.
Notes
1 In all these reports, since the patients are referred to by their first names, I use
the therapists’ first names as well when they are talking about themselves as
participants in the context of shared experience; the therapists’ surnames or both
names are used in their discussions of the case, or in reference to their publications.
This is current procedure for reports of interpersonal and relational treatments.
2 Paper presented at conference “New Perspectives on Symptoms and
Symbols: Neuroscience and Applications in Clinical Dissociation, Milan, Italy,
2017.” An earlier version of Clara’s case appeared in Amore (2012).
3 Cited from Abstracts of the Standard Edition, International Universities
Press, p. 89.
4 An earlier version of the case of Antonio appeared in Bucci (2018).
5 An earlier version of the case of Ann appeared in Bucci (2011b).
Embodied communication in consulting room 215
References
Amore, M. (2012). Clinical scenarios of “remembering”: Somatic states as a pro-
cess of emerging memory. Psychoanalytic Dialogues: The International Journal of
Relational Perspectives, 22(2), 238–252.
Barthes, R. (1981). Camera lucida: Reflections on photography. NewYork: Farrar,
Strauss and Giroux.
Bucci, W. (2001). Pathways of emotional communication. Psychoanalytic Inquiry,
20, 40–70.
Bucci, W. (2008). New perspectives on the multiple code theory: The role of
bodily experience in emotional organization. In F. S. Anderson (Ed.), Bodies in
treatment: The unspoken dimension (pp. 51–77). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Bucci, W. (2011a). The interplay of subsymbolic and symbolic processes in psychoana-
lytic treatment: It takes two to tango—but who knows the steps, who’s the leader?
The choreography of the psychoanalytic interchange. The Journal of Analytical
Psychology, 21, 45–54.
Bucci, W. (2011b). The role of embodied communication in therapeutic change: A
multiple code perspective. In W. Tschacher & C. Bergomi (Eds.), The implications
of embodiment: Cognition and communication (pp. 209– 228). Exeter: Imprint
Academic.
Bucci, W. (2011c). The role of subjectivity and intersubjectivity in the reconstruction
of dissociated schemas: Converging perspectives from psychoanalysis, cognitive
science and affective neuroscience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28, 247–266.
Bucci, W. (2018). Emotional communication in the case of Antonio. Psychoanalytic
Inquiry, 38, 518–529.
Cornell W. F. (2008). Self in action: The bodily basis of self- organization In
F. S. Anderson (ed.), Bodies in treatment: The unspoken dimension (pp. 29–49).
Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Crits-Cristoph, P., Gibbons, M. C., & Mukherjee, D. (2013). Psychotherapy process-
outcome research. In M. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (6th ed., pp. 298–340). New York: Wiley.
Freud, S. (1899). Screen memories. Standard Edition, 3, 301–22. London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1912). The dynamics of transference. Standard Edition, 12, 97–108.
London: Hogarth Press.
Keysers, C. (2011). The empathic brain. Cambridge, MA: Social Brain Press
Kohut, H. (1982). Introspection, empathy, and the semi-circle of mental health.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis and Self-psychology, 63: 395–407.
Leahy R. L. (2009). Resistance: An emotional schema therapy (EST) approach. In
S. E. Gregoris (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy: A guide for the practising clinician,
Vol. 2. (pp. 187–204). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Lewis, J. (2018). Bodies in dialogue: Empathic connectedness in the realm of the
unspeakable. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 38(7), 493–501.
McEwen, B. S., & Seeman, T. (2003). Stress and affect: Applicability of the concepts of
allostasis and allostatic load. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
(Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 1117–1137). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 9, 148–158.
216 Clinical perspectives
Reik, T. (1948), Listening with the third ear: The inner experience of a psychoanalyst.
New York: Pyramid Books.
Sael, B. A., Wang, J., Cárdenas-Morales, L., Faiq, M., & Heim, C. (2018). Mental
stress as consequence and cause of vision loss: The dawn of psychosomatic oph-
thalmology for preventive and personalized medicine. EPMA Journal, 9, 133–160.
Selye, H. (1950). Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. British Medical Journal,
1(4667), 1383–1392.
Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chapter 12
The tango can be debated, and we have debates over it, but it still encloses,
as does all that which is truthful, a secret.
The tango is a direct expression of something that poets have often tried to
state in words: the belief that a fight may be a celebration.1
How do two become one while remaining two? How does each person
experience the other within oneself while finding new parts of oneself
as well? The knowing—and not knowing—these multiple selves as they
impact one another is often center stage in our analytic work. Tango, like
the psychoanalytic relationship, requires trust, attunement, presence and
in-the-moment awareness, as both partners are constantly counterbalan-
cing each other and sharing the other’s weight. We plan to discuss the
many parallels of analytic work and the dance of Tango, and along the
way invite you to experience a feeling that is danced.
of Darwin, scientists have argued that different emotions give rise to specific
body movements, and are expressed through such movements in humans as
in all species. Feelings of joy and sadness, and all sorts of multiple complex
feelings that cannot be easily or directly named, are expressed in a person’s
face and body, and then communicated to others through activation of their
own bodily responses—perhaps in trace form. This kind of embodied emo-
tional communication is central to the psychoanalytic interaction, as I have
discussed in detail.
Here I am talking about a different idea—part of the same interactive cycle,
but starting in a different place. Particular body movements can give rise to
different emotional states—not just that we express how we feel through facial
expression and body movement, but that by moving our bodies in a certain
way we can make ourselves feel a certain way.
The recognition and use of the effects of certain types of movement on
emotion is directly related to body therapy, and also related to all forms of
psychotherapy. In psychology and neuropsychology, we are beginning to
learn more about these bidirectional connections. There is now evidence from
neural imaging studies that feedback from muscle movement is associated
with activation in brain regions such as the amygdala and underlies produc-
tion of neurotransmitters that elicit particular feeling states (Hettenlotter
et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2014). In very general terms, we can say that by moving
muscles in certain ways, a dancer might be doing the equivalent of giving their
body a shot of some neural transmitter, perhaps in some cases endorphins,
the body’s own self-produced opiates; in some cases oxytocin, the chemical
associated with yearning and love; in some cases transmitters associated with
anger, aggression, or passion.
I suggest that in tango we have a set of movements that activate a range of
different emotional experiences, including sadness, yearning, and anger, and
others that defy one’s attempts to name them, and that also activate memories
and fantasies—all communicated between two people. There are moments of
risk and uncertainty, not knowing what is coming, being on the edge. All of
this is happening in a limited specified time and place, the time of a song or a
few songs, and all of this may happen with a person who plays no other role
in one’s life, perhaps whom one knows only in the dance world.
There is some research concerning how particular body movements are
related to particular feeling states, including how specific movements commu-
nicate emotion to others (Melzer et al., 2019), and how particular movements
contribute to recognition of certain feelings within oneself (Maxwell &
Davidson, 2007). Here I want to suggest some hypotheses about these effects
specifically from the perspective of tango, as developed by Dardo, and also
experientially, as I have learned from him. In his own dancing and in his
teaching, Dardo is studying the effects of movement from the inside, teaching
what he finds out in himself. He gives instructions such as, “Be quiet and
experience your Achilles; you can feel where you come from, where you are
Nobody dances tango alone 219
going.” As he explains, from his own exploration inside his body, the Achilles
tendon connects to the inside; the toe-ball of the foot connects to the out-
side; one side moves the other. This is part of how the spiral of the dance
happens, the interplay of horizontal and vertical. The vertical direction—
ankle, knee, hip, and center—within our bodies is what makes the connection
of the ground to the self; the horizontal goes across to the other and involves
responding to the partner and moving together around the room. The center
is the point where the horizontal and vertical intersect, and also where self
and other connect.
This is not mystical. As a potential scientific direction, I could see this kind
of articulation of the experience of movements as a basis for hypotheses
concerning the relationship between certain combinations of body movement
and complex emotions, and also for hypotheses concerning communication
of emotion to others. At some point, I think it could happen that researchers
studying the relation of body movement and emotional communication could
use experts like Dardo in designing psychophysiological studies. For example,
the researcher might ask participants to focus on stretching the Achilles
tendon or pressing the toe-ball of the foot into the ground, then report how
they feel now, or perhaps report events of the past or fantasies that come to
mind. I could also foresee a study examining which parts of the brain light up
when participants view movements with characteristics such as those defined
by Dardo.
I think the relation of different kinds of movements to different emotional
states within oneself and to communication of emotion states to others is
part of the reason why tango becomes so addictive. Dardo’s instructions are,
“Follow yourself, feel your body, follow your body, learn from your body.”
Then he also says, “Take care of yourself in order to take care of others.”
This leads to the next unique aspect of tango, the nature of the partnering.
In tango, there is a basic set of movements and steps that can be learned, but
the sequence is not fixed. Tango is improvisational, there are no fixed combin-
ations. In finding a joint direction together, the leader has to know where the
follower is, and the follower has to know what the leader will do; the leader
signals intent through their body movement before they know; the follower
feels the direction in their body. They are not connecting experience to words,
but to movements that they share. They may follow particular steps, but they
220 Clinical perspectives
still have to create together, explore together. The art of the tango lies not so
much in what steps are taken as in how they are taken.
Here is where a new question emerges: one person (the designated leader)
must generate a sequence of steps so the two participants in the couple move
together as a kind of unit. The partner—the designated follower—has to feel
within their body what the other is generating. This is a particular kind of
talent—the talent of being a sensitive receptor tuned into another person,
feeling the other in one’s own body. But as Dardo is also teaching us now,
the moment of waiting, opening oneself to the other, is not enough. It is
also necessary, at the same time, for the partner to open to their own self, to
bring not only their energy but also their own creative sense of the movement,
to integrate their sense of the experience with the sense that the partner is
sending. Together they need to make a space into which each participant can
bring their creative energy, while the structure and flow of the dance remain.
This paragraph now seems depressing and dampening to me. The new
perspective on psychological organization that constitutes the framework
of this volume goes beyond the concept of autonomy. Just as there is no
infant without the one who cares for them, as Winnicott says, there is no
person without connections to others throughout life. We are all built on the
representations of the connections held within us; these connections continue
to be built continuously throughout life, and need to be examined where they
are interfering with life. It is not “autonomy,” but this new and different and
perplexing world of connections that needs to be built, in the organization of
memory and in current life. To some extent, at some points as in my lessons
with Dardo, the therapist’s “brain” leads; later, when new connections are
made, the patient can build the structures, in their relationships with others as
well as within the therapy.
In this small chapter to end this book, I have taken another look at the
tango floor to view the development and playing out of these connections in
microcosm. Nobody dances tango alone; no one knows quite what step will
happen until it does; each dance comes to life in the interaction between the
participants. As I am writing this closing chapter, in the terrible spring of
2020, it is also clear that no one knows quite what challenge will happen until
it does; the different meanings of anxiety as of other feelings emerge in the
interactions between us, in life situations as in treatment. Creative exploration
into the unknown is the essence of psychoanalytic treatment and perhaps a
source of hope. Such exploration gives psychoanalysis its unique potential—a
healing profession that is also a healing art.
Notes
1 Two quotes from Jorge Luis Borges, taken from www.azquotes.com
2 The colloquium was organized by Cleonie White and moderated by Anita
Lanzi; both are members of the White Institute. The presenters included
Velleda Ceccoli, a psychoanalyst and Clinical Associate Professor at the NYU
Postdoctoral Program in Psychotherapy and Psychoanalysis; Dardo Galletto,
master tango dancer, choreographer and teacher; and myself. Following the
formal presentations, the audience was invited to experience the dance with
members of Dardo’s company.
3 Argentine Tango—Igor Polk Dancing Site. Retrieved from www.virtuar.com/tango
References
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Hettenlotter, A., Dresel, C., Castrop, F., Cebellos-Baumann, A.O., Wohlschläger,
A.M., & Haslinger, B. (2008). The link between facial feedback and neural activity
within central circuitries of emotion: New insights from botulinum toxin-induced
denervation of frown muscles. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 537–542.
222 Clinical perspectives
Kim, M. J., Neta, M., Davis, F. C., Ruberry, E. J., Dinescu, D., Heatherton, T.
F., Stotland, M. A., & Whalen, P. J. (2014). Botulinum toxin- induced facial
muscle paralysis affects amygdala responses to the perception of emotional
expressions: Preliminary findings from an A-B-A design. Biology of Mood & Anxiety
Disorders, 4(11). Retrieved from https://biolmoodanxietydisord.biomedcentral.
com/articles/10.1186/2045-5380-4-11#citeas
Maxwell, J. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Emotion as motion: Asymmetries in approach
and avoidant actions. Psychological Science, 18, 1113–1119.
Melzer, A., Shafir, T., & Tsachor, R. P. (2019), How do we recognize emotion from
movement? Specific motor components contribute to the recognition of each
emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 July. Retrieved from www.frontiersin.org/art-
icles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01389/full
Index
(ECS) 177; levels of consciousness all treatment approaches 195 (see also
43–6 (see also consciousness; case studies; mirror neurons)
dispositional representations; emotion emotions 72; category terms 94; and
schemas) emotional intelligence 62; as episodes
defense, defenses: as forms of or states 81–3; labels of 80, 83–4,
dissociation 12–13, 53, 157; in 88; language of 190; as regulatory
isolation of affect 118, 165; as mechanisms 46; and somatization
undoing 165 (see also dissociation; 64; verbalization of 84–6 (see also
unconscious processes) Damasio, A. R.; Russell, J. A.;
dissociation: adaptive 105–6, 156–7; Scherer, K. R.)
avoidant, primary and secondary emotion schemas xxxiv–v, 10–11, 33, 42,
106–8, 149; biological substrate of 62–3, 72–3, 79–81, 102, 131, 148–9,
165–6; double consciousness in 106; 161–3; activation in therapy process
in emotion schemas 48, 149, 165; in 120, 138–40, 188; affective core of
emotional disorders 48–9, 108–9, 167; 63, 79–80, 102, 161, 166; change in
freezing response in 179–80; levels 69, 169, 187; as clusters of episodes
of 47–8; in memories of Holocaust 80, 162; in countertransference and
survivors 179–83; multiple code transference 34, 162; as dispositional
theory of 159–61, 175–6; in pathology representations 42, 63, 80, 162;
37, 42, 47–8, 163–7; reconstruction in dissociation within 49, 149; 165,
referential process 50–3, 161–3, 202–3, 210; effects of stress on 178–9;
169–70; relation to repression and information processing in 104–5,
defenses 12, 54; relation to trauma 122; integration of 185; and internal
156–9, 164, 183–5; self-repair of 50, working models 63; interpersonal
166–7; in somatic disorders 12–13; context of xxxvii–viii, 81, 103; as
treatment of 58, 168, 186–8 (see also memory schemas 33–4, 62–3, 102–3,
Bromberg, P.; Chefetz, R.; Mark, D.) 138; as mind-body constellations 202;
dispositional representations 43, 63 in multiple code theory 7, 10, 40, 76;
dreams: connection to symbolic mode and narratives 103; neural circuitry
69, 73, 85; dream work xxxii, 28, 140; of 43, 104, 176–8; object relations as
referential process in 10, 203 (see also 162; and referential process 161–3;
case studies; primary and secondary relation to consciousness 44; relation
processes) to drive theory 62–5; reorganization
dual code theory see Paivio, A. of 85; representations of interactions
drives 8; in Freud’s energy theory 23, that have been generalized (RIGs)
61–2, 174–5 63; role of caretaker in 14, 63;
dynamical systems xxxiii, 160; see also symbolic connections in 17; and
subsymbolic processes transference 103
emotional information processing 9–10,
Eagle, M. 3, 23 30, 32–4, 147, 160, 165; and emotional
Einstein, A. xxvii–ix, 59n5, 92–3, 147 intelligence 25
Eliot, T. S.: objective correlative 76, 93 episodic memory 82, 85, 91, 93; and
embodied communication 194–5; autonetic and noetic awareness
anticipatory power of 113; in case 116–7; hippocampal function in 165;
of Ann 213; in tango 144 (see also in therapeutic change 118–21 (see also
emotional communication) emotion schemas, narrative)
emotional communication 186–7; in exposure treatment xxxix
arousal phase 51, 67, 194; as empathy
32; in episodic memory 121; in Ferro, A.: Bionian perspective xxxvii;
formation of emotion schemas 103; symbolizing process xxxviii
gestures in 69, 85, 93, 139; projective flow experience 79; in subsymbolic
identification as 214; in tango 144; in processing 102
Index 225
free association xxxix, 10, 16, 18, Keysers, C. shared circuits xxx, 210
35–6, 52, 127, 140, 194; protocol Kris, E. good hour 68
analysis as 30 Krystal, H. 12, 15
Friedman, L. 189, 197
Freud, S. 3, 4, 21, 29, 41; on language: in alexithymia 15;
consciousness xxxi; drive theory of disconnection from bodily functions
61; energy model 23; on hysteria 56; emotional expression in 82–4, 86,
101; primary process 5, 174; screen 92–4; in energy model 23; measures
memories 203; stereotype plate 34; of 85, 88–92; in multiple code theory
structural model xxxii, 4, 23, 174; 77–8; as objective correlative 76;
topographic models 4–5, 174; on paralinguistic aspects of 59n2; in
transference 34; on the unconscious referential process 37, 86, 88, 110–11;
xxxi, 174–5 (see also defenses; in therapeutic process 33, 54, 56, 67,
unconscious processes) 75, 190; in working memory 56 (see
also episodic memory; referential
Galletto, D. 144, 150, 217, 221n2 activity; referential process)
Gates, H. L.; cross-over generation 106 LeDoux, J. E. 8, 34, 83, 104, 109;
amygdalar memories 107–8; fear
hippocampus, hippocampal functions: response 177; working memory and
and explicit memory system 105; and consciousness 114
stress response 106–7; in working Leventhal, H. 16
memory 114–15, 117–18, 165, Lewis, J. case of Antonio 204–10
177–8, 184 Loewald, H. W. 68, 101, 109, 120
Heidegger, M. 146–7 Luborsky, L. 128–9
Howell, E. F. 156, 179
Holt, R. 3, 28, 128–30 Mark, D. case of Kurt 111–13
Mariani, R. 92, 94
instincts 61–2; see also drives Martini, G. 146
internal working models 63; see also McDougall, J. 12, 14, 66
emotion schemas memory: autobiographical 112, 149;
interpersonal processes 81–2, 85, 193–5; declarative 26–7; episodic and
in autobiographical memory 149; semantic 116–20; implicit 26; and
in development 11, 34; in emotion narrative 117; short-term or working
schemas 15, 63, 79–80, 148–9, 179, 114–6; long-term 115–16; procedural
184; intrapsychic vs. interpsychic or non-declarative 26 (see also
communication 176; mirror neurons working memory; episodic memory;
in 103; in subsymbolic system 135; memory schemas)
in treatment context xxxvi–viii, memory schemas see Bartlett, F. C.;
16–17, 32, 66–7, 85, 116, 119, 152, emotion schemas
169, 185–7, 190 (see also Cornell, mental models: role in cognitive
W.F. case of Ann; Lewis, J. case of science and psychoanalysis 22–4;
Antonio; Schilbach, L.; Stern, Daniel; developmental 11; dual process theory
Stern, Donnell) 27; metapsychology as 23 (see also
intersubjectivity see interpersonal Freud, S.; multiple code theory)
processes; mirror neurons mentalization based treatment xxxix
mirror neurons xxx, 144, 187; see also
Jaspers, K. 146 interpersonal processes
Jones, E. xxviii Moccia, G. 146
multiple code theory 42, 77, 102–3,
Kaechele, H. 3 159–61; clinical implications of
Keller, H. 78 14–16; emotion schemas in 7–8;
Kernberg, A. 14, 34, 66 major concepts of xxxiii–vi, 5, 32–4;
226 Index
model of pathology in 22, 32–5, 41, proto-self 44–6; see also consciousness;
102, 176; nonverbal system in 6–7; Damasio, A. R.
origins of xxxii; processing modes in psychoanalytic psychology: as
77–9, 134; in psychoanalytic process integrative field 37
10–12, 35; referential process in 8–10; psychoanalytic situation: as research
somatization in 3, 12–14, 16–18; milieu 32, 37; therapeutic process
structural change in 42; subsymbolic in 35–6
thought in xxxiii; verbal system in 5–6 Psychotherapy Action Network xli
(see also emotion schemas; referential
process) referential activity: measures of 16–17,
88–9; see also narrative; referential
narrative: computerized measures of 91; process
in dreams 10; episodic memory and referential process xxxv–vi, 10, 33,
116, 120–1; in exposure therapy 87; in 66–9, 76–7, 84–6, 109–10, 139, 161,
free association 52; as metaphor 103; 169–70; activation of affective core
in referential process xxxv, 36, 51, 87, in 67; arousal of emotion schema
139, 194, 198; as symbolizing 33, 67, 85; in dreams 203; evidence for xli,
85, 113 86–8; in interpersonal context xxxvi;
Negri, A. 92 measures of 38n4; 88–92; Italian
Neisser, U. 21, 29, 120 measures of 68, 92, 94; narratives as
noetic awareness 116; see also episodic metaphors in 52–3; phases of 50–3;
memory, consciousness 67–8, 85–6, 110–11; protosymbols in
51–2, 197, 200; reorganizing in 86,
object relations see emotion schemas 88; as therapeutic process 35–6, 66–8;
Ogden, T. H.: example of emotion subjective experience in 113–7; in
schema xxxv–vi, xli; case of Ms. B termination phase 199–201, 204; and
70–2; reverie xxxv working through 102 (see also Amore,
M.; Cornell, W.F.; Lewis, J.; narrative)
Paivio A. xxxii, 9, 33, 77 Reik, T. 147–8, 194
parallel distributed processing see regression 54–5, 190
subsymbolic processing repression 12, 35, 101; compared to
pathology 42, 163–4; biological evidence dissociation 54; see also defense,
165; forms of 166; development of defenses
49–50; and emotion schemas 104, resistance 8, 15, 54–6, 62, 170, 190, 206
165; multiple code formulation of Ricoeur, P. 146–8
47–8, 167, 176–8; repair of 54; roots Rogers, C. 92
of 163; vicious circle of 34 (see also Russell, J. A. 83–4
dissociation)
Pessoa, L. xxviii, 81, 132, 202 Safran, J. 117–8
Phelps, E. A. xxviii, 81, 104, 132 Schacter, D. 26
primary process thought 55, 126; affect Scherer, K. R. 82–3
activation in 131–2; in awareness 128; Schilbach, L. xl
condensation and displacement in secondary process: revision of 130;
55; contradictions in 128–30, 174; in Freud on 5, 28, 126–8
creative writing 137–8; dream work in Selye, H. 65
28, 147; emotion schemas in 131–2; in semantic memory 82
energy model 126–7, 130, 133; Holt’s short-term memory 30, 114; see also
scoring system of 130; in scientific working memory
work 137; in sports 135–7; Freud on Solano, L. case of Stefano 117–18
126–7; multiple code reformulation of Solms, M. xxxi
53–5; 130–1, 133–5, 140 somatization 35; in case of Stefano
projective identification 153, 194, 214 69–71; in case of Mrs. B 70–2, 76;
Index 227