Units 1 and 2
Units 1 and 2
Units 1 and 2
The consonant-vowel structure has been suggested as a basic phonological unit based
on the following elements:
- Almost all languages have CCV or CV words. This is the most common structure
among languages. Consonant-vowel is known as a canonical (basic) structure,
because it is universally the most prevalent structure and the first to be acquired
by children. Although syllabic structure is language specific in terms of form and
the types of their means that are permitted in any of the three positions of the
syllable, this canonical structure exists in all languages and it is refer in most.
- If a language has CCV words, it also has CV words. For example, in English, “tree”
(CCV), but also “bee” (CV).
- In addition, hardly any language has V or VC words without CV ones. There are
some exceptions like the Arandic group of Australian aboriginal languages. Even
in a relatively small number of languages, this CV is the only type of syllable
permitted.
- The first systematic utterances of children are usually of this from regardless of
language type. Hence, children barely utterance at the age of 6 to 9 months
combines this basic structure by babbling consonants and vowels and not string
of just vowels or just consonants. Secondly, children distort adult speech saying
[bæ] for “bird” or [kA] for “cup”; a fact that is said to be common and seems to
obey to unconsciousness. Therefore, the syllable is considered as a unit of neural
programming, rather than primarily muscular or acoustic events.
Vowels and consonants are not an ordered in meaningless succession. They are rooted
in syllables, and every English word consists of at least one syllable. The study of syllable
structure can help us understand that words are nor a random selection of phonemes.
And, that there are some underlying principles that govern the combination of sounds.
The knowledge of this syllable structure constrains is relevant for a very understanding
of how sounds interact with each other, how words can be divided, and how this affects
pronunciation.
Syllables are hard to define. Although, there is no definition of the syllable that
phoneticians or phonologists currently agree on, here are some definitions of the
syllable:
- A syllable consists of a centre which has little or no obstruction to the airflow and
which sounds comparatively loud: before and after this centre there will be
greater obstruction to airflow and/ or less loud sound (vowel sound).
Articulatory and auditory terms.
- A fundamental but elusive phonological unit typically consisting of a short
sequence of segments, most typically a single vowel or diphthong possibly
preceded and/ or followed by one or more consonants. Phonological terms.
- In phonetics, a syllable is a group of sounds that are pronounced together.
4. THEORIES
Trask (1996) claims that the term syllable offers some problems of definition to the point
that there is no complete comprehension of what a syllable is. He insists that, although
native speakers usually find it easy to decide how many syllables are present in a word
or utterance, there are problems still in syllable division even by native speakers.
For example, although most native speakers would surely distinguish three syllables in a
word such as “punishment”, and just one syllable in a word such as “book”, it is difficult
to stablish the reasons and pattern which can work out with other words and which can
help to delimit the frontiers of each syllable. Thus, every English word consists of at least
one syllable. But there are words with two or more syllables that may have a different
number of syllables depending on the pronunciation. We can find that the same word
can be pronounced in different ways, such as “every” or “necessary”. This is because rage
of speech alters the number of syllables.
Indeed, syllables are hard to define. Scientifically, no one knows what physical action of
the speaker creates a syllable. That is, there is not a good knowledge of what physical
action produces a syllable. However, there have been several attends to define the
syllable. The syllable has been defined in different ways according to different criteria.
- The prominence theory→ in any utterance some sounds are set to be more
prominent or sonorous than others. That is, they are felt by listeners to stand out
from their neighbours. The number of syllables will be determined by the
number of peaks of prominence. Phonetically it is claimed that when identifying
the syllables listeners are responding to sonority. According to Carr (1999),
sonority is an acoustic effect. The most sonorous a sound, the most it resonates.
Vowels are characterized by an open articulation, so they resonate the most.
Since sonority has to do with open articulation, the most open sound is the more
sonorous and the loudest it resonates. Thus, sonority is the relative loudness of
a segment compare with others and each syllable will have a single sonority peak;
syllable boundaries or troughs occur at the point of relatively weak prominence
(valleys). So, the prominence theory argues that in a string of sound, some are
intrinsically more sonorous than others and that each peak of sonority
corresponds to the centre of a syllable. These peaks are best illustrated by vowels
which have greater carrying-power. The less sonorous sounds provide valleys of
prominence and are best illustrated by the closures and narrowing which
produce consonants. There is one of the weaknesses of this approach, is that this
approach gives a general guideline but it does not always indicate clearly where
the boundaries between an adjacent syllable’s fall.
- The pulse theory→ It is one of the approaches or main theories about the
syllable that pretends to define the syllable in articulatory terms. It is concerned
with the muscular activity that takes place during speech. According to this
theory, in any utterance there are a number of chest pulses, accompanied by
increases in air pressure, which determine the number of syllables uttered. This
can often be felt particularly in emphatic speech, but it is often difficult to detect
such a pulse in adjacent syllables; for example, when to vowels can co-occur. For
example, the word going has two syllables but is usually said in a single muscular
effort. Hence, this theory claims that syllables correlate with bursts of muscular
activity at the intercostal muscles or chest pulses. But experimental work has
shown that there is no such a simple correlation. Whatever syllables are they are
not simplemotor units. This theory suggests that the syllable rather than the
sound is the basic unit of speech. Furthermore, vowels sounds are nuclear to the
syllable and the render the chest pulse audible, while consonant sounds typically
occur at the onset and closure of the syllable. Some scholars state that there is
no empirical evidence in relation to the pulse theory due to the lack of tools that
could provide quantifiable parameters and data for the syllable.
- The phonological theory or linguistic approach→ according to this theory, a unit
of greater magnitude than the phoneme or sound segment is more usefully
defined in linguistic terms, that is, with reference to the structure of one
particular language rather than in general phonetic terms with universal
application. The reason is that it may be found appropriate to divide a similar
sound sequence differently in different languages. For example, if we have this
sequence of phonemes /discuss/, in Spanish we cannot break the syllable like
/di’scuss/ because there is no syllable that starts with sk- (that would be the case
of English).
Every language has phonemes and every language has its own common patterns in
which phonemes are arranged to form syllables and the syllables are arranged to form
langer units.
1. SYLLABLE STRUCTURE→Every syllable has structure: onset and rhyme, and the
latter include peak or nucleus and coda. We also want to describe what general
structures are possible for English syllables and what structures are impossible
(/ae/, /t/, /k/). The study of sequences of phonemes is called phonotactics, and
it seems that the phonotactic possibilities of a language are determined by the
syllabic structure. One of the reasons for studying phonotactics is the evidence it
may bring on syllable structure. According to Goldsmith (1990), most approaches
to the syllable have describe the syllable as a highly hierarchical structure, that
would be superimposed on a sequence of segments. This hierarchy would
contain various levels of segmental organization. The syllables’ major role would
be to govern the sequential distribution of segments. In order to account for the
sound structure of languages, it has been proposed that syllables are three-like
structures corresponding to various branching entities. That is, syllable structure
may be represented graphically by means of a tree diagram. Linguists have
explored every possible configuration for the internal structure of syllables. For
CVC syllables, the main concern is whether the vowel is grout with the prior
consonant known as the onset with the posterior consonant or coda, or with
neither.
- The first tree illustrates the theory of the flat syllable, where the vowel roots with
neither the onset nor the coda.
- The second tree illustrates the onset-rhyme theory where the vowel roots with
the coda to form a constituent called the rhyme (Goldsmith, 1990).
- The last tree illustrates the theory of body-coda organization where the vowel is
rooted with the onset to form a constituent called the body (McCarthy, 1979).
We will adopt the theory of the second tree.
The onset includes any consonants that precede the nuclear element and the rhyme
includes the nuclear element or peak as well as any marginal elements (usually
consonants) that may follow it. The rhyme in term further branches into peak or nucleus
and coda. The peak represents the nuclear element in a syllable while the coda includes
all the consonants that follow the peak in a syllable. Therefore, the rhyme associates the
nucleus with the coda, and the syllable head associates all these parts together.
The most cited piece of evidence for the special role of the onset rhyme distinction has
come from rhyming. In rhyming, it is the vowel in the syllable and any sounds that follow,
that determine if two words rhyme independently of the sounds in the onset. However,
not all these elements are required in every syllable. All languages require syllable nuclei.
In fact, the nucleus is the only universally obligatory component of a syllable.
The sound occupying the nucleus is normally a vowel. Occasionally syllables have a
syllabic consonant rather than a vowel. In English this is true for the lateral sound, nasals
and R in rhotic accents. And no syllable has more than one vowel. Depending upon the
language specific rules, syllables have certain numbers of consonants before and after
the vowel. In most languages, onsets are preferred yet optional, though they are
required in some languages in which every single syllable will have a consonant. No
language requires codas. In most languages codas are optional and they are restrictive
or even prohibited in others. Syllables that have no coda are open syllables. The most
common opened syllable is the consonant CV syllable (bee/boy). Syllables that have a
coda, are closed syllables. The most common closed syllable is CVC syllable. Thus, the
onset and coda are optional elements and each if felt will contain one or more
consonants.
The syllable is the unit in which phonotactic rules operate. Phonotactics may be defined
as the study of common syllabic patterns, that is, the part of phonology which studies
and describes the constrains on co-occurrence, the constrains on the combination of
sounds. Thus, phonotactics is a branch of phonology that studies the permissible strains
of phonemes in a particular language. These constrains are indeed very language
specific.
Examples of pseudowords: sad ped ren fop wub det tog lat zum vit bot bom cam gip mib
nim yob bup kag hig.
Syllable structure and relative prominence are interrelated. The structures of stress and
unstress syllables differ in certain small ways:
- Some elements can occur only as the peaks of unstressed syllables (shwa).
- Some phonemes have different pronunciations in stressed and unstressed
syllables.
The syllable is more usefully defined in linguistic terms, i.e. with reference to the
structure of a particular language rather than in general phonetic terms with universal
application.
Every English syllable has a center or a peak, an element which is syllabic. All vowels are
syllabic by definition. In addition, liquids and nasals become syllabic in certain positions.
Every word, phrase or sentence has as many syllables as it has syllabic elements.
The peak may be preceded by one or more non-syllabic elements, which constitute the
onset of the syllable, and it may be followed by one or more non-syllabic elements which
constitute the coda.
The English template→ Template is like a basic pattern. English had utmost three
consonants for the beginning of the syllable or onset, and in that case necessarily begins
with S-. They have utmost three consonants at the end for coda of the syllable in
uninflected forms. These constrains define what is called a well-formed template. But
this only establishes the limits for each constituent of the syllable and says nothing about
non-permissible sequences in English and yet follow this pattern. There are permissible
and non-permissible combinations of sounds in English. Hence, there are phonotactic
constrains in English.
The similarities between English and Spanish focus on the role of the nucleus, and the
typology of the syllables. In both languages, the nucleus is essential, and the coda and
onset are optional. Despite these similarities, there are also differences.
1. The nucleus of the English syllable may fall on a consonant (syllabic consonants).
Whereas in Spanish this is not possible. The Spanish language only accepts
vowels as nucleus of syllables.
2. Final consonant clusters in Spanish are not so common as in English. Rafael
Guerra (1983) carried out a research study on the frequency of syllabic structures
in spoken Spanish. Results showed that the basic syllable structure CV scores the
highest with 55.81%.
Phonotactics.
All languages built their words from a finite set of phoneme units. And it is also true that
in all languages there are constrains on the way in which these phonemes can be
arranged to form syllables. These phonotactic constrains severally limit the number of
syllables that would be theoretically possible if phonemes could be combined in an
unconstrained way.
So phonotactics may be define as the study of common syllabic patterns that is the part
of phonology which studies and describes the constraints on co-occurrence. Languages
have phonotactic constraints due to:
PHONOLOGICAL THEORY
1. Long vowels and diphthongs are never followed by /ŋ/. Examples: “long”, “king”,
“sung”, “bank”.
2. In onsets with three consonants, the first element is always /s/; the second
element must be a plosive, and the third element an approximant. Examples:
“scream”, “straight” or “splash”.
3. Short vowels are never the nucleus of an open syllable; a word like /næ/ is
impossible in English. Short vowels have to be followed by a coda (that is, in a
closed syllable), and for this reason they are called checked vowels. Vowels that
can occur both in open and closed syllables are free vowels.
4. The consonant /Ʒ/ occurs as an onset in words of foreign origin like “genre”, or
“gigolo”. In coda position, this sound occurs only in words of recent French origin:
“beige”, “rouge”, “camouflage”.
5. /ð/ only occurs word-initially in function words (example: this, thus, though).
Every language has phonemes and every language has its own common patterns in
which these phonemes are arranged to form syllables and the syllables are arranged to
form larger units. We want to describe what general structures or patterns are possible
for English syllables and what structures or combinations are impossible. We can define
two types of non-word monosyllables. On one hand, there are accidental gaps. These
are phonotactically legal word-like sequences, but happen not to occur in the language
(English). On the other hand, there are illegal syllables. These violate a phonotactic
constraint in English. These kinds are also known as pseudowords.
English has a large number of monosyllabic words. By examining the legal or permissible
sequences of phonemes in English monosyllabic words, we can get a good idea of what
types of syllable structures are legal or permissible in English. It is clear that English has
a very flexible syllable structure (I, me, spray, ant, bond, etc.)
There are languages at the opposite extreme that only have CV syllables. However, there
are nevertheless, considerable constraints on which phoneme sequences are
permissible in English syllables.
1. If two vowels occur in sequence, the syllable break is between the vowels (V.V)
→ chaos, neon. /keɪ.ɒs/
2. If one consonant occurs between two vowels and the second is strong (long
vowels, diphthongs, short vowels followed by 2 or more consonants) whether
stressed or not, the consonant is part of the second syllable→ arrive, repent.
/ə́.rɑɪv/ /rɪ́.pent/
3. If one consonant occurs between a strong vowel and a weak one, the consonant
is ambisyllabic (^) (in different pronunciations by the same person, it may end
one syllable or begin the next one)→ leaving /liːvɪ̂ŋ/
4. When a single consonant occurs between two weak vowels, the consonant may
be ambisyllabic (cinema) or it may go with the second vowel (preferiblemente
ambisyllabic)→ cinema, positive /sɪn̂əm̂ ə/ /pɒẑətɪ̂ v/ *^ se pone entre letras
5. If two vowels are separated by a consonant cluster, syllable division depends on
what consonants are in the cluster
5.1. If the consonant cluster is one which cannot occur in initial position, the
consonants are divided in such a way that the second syllable begins with a
single consonant or a cluster which can occur initially→ candy, atlas /kæn.di/
/æt.ləs/
5.2.a. If the cluster is of the type which can occur word-initially, and the following
vowel is strong, the whole cluster is part of the syllable with the strong vowel →
decline /dɪ.klaɪn/
5.2.b. If the second vowel is weak; the first consonant of the consonants is
ambisyllabic→ sacred /seɪk ̂rɪd/
Exercise
napkin /næp.kɪn/
striking /strɑɪk^ɪŋ/
hellinistic /hel^ɪ.nɪs^tɪk/
twilight /twaɪ.laɪt/
magnet /mæg.nɪt/
ambition /æm.bɪʃ^ən/
striated /straɪ.eɪt^ɪd/
actor /æk.tə/
phonetics /fən^e.tɪks/
liturgic /lɪ.tɜːdʒ^ɪk/
incredible /ɪn.kre.d^ɪb^əl/
nakedness /neɪ.kɪd.n^əs/
harmony /hɑːm^ən^ɪ/
celebrity /sɪ.leb^rət^ɪ/
alveolar /æl.vɪ.əʊl^ə/
cathedral /kə.θiːd^rəl/
exposure /ɪk.spəʊʒ^ə/
amalgamation /ə.mæl.gə.meɪʃ^ən/
synthesis /sɪn.θə^sɪs/
alphabet /æl.fəb^ɪt/
pronunciation /prə.nʌn.sɪ.eɪʃ^ən/
nostalgic /nə.stæl.dʒɪk/
merchandizeɪk /mɜː.tʃən.daɪz/
tutelage /tjuːt^ɪl^ɪdʒ/
oxygen /ɒk.sɪdʒ^ən/
accident /æk.sɪ.dənt/
*Ejercicio lamp y about en agenda (pasar foto). /S/ en inicio es como un appendix (se
ve más tarde) no es parte del onset (violacion del sonority principle).
STRUCTURE OF MONOSYLLABIC WORDS.
The onset.
According to the prominence theory, the maximum consonants in the onset is two, and
a three consonants cluster is considered here as a violation of the Sonority Sequencing
Principle and /s/ will be treated as an appendix. And we also have this Obligatory Contour
Principle, which does not accept two consonants with similar places of articulation to be
placed together (siempre se prefiere que las consonants vayan con el onset).
The Peak.
The peak of the syllable contains the “syllabic element”, the segment that is more
sonorous than both its neighbours. The rule that specifies the associations of vowels to
x-positions would be:
1. Associate a [- tense] vowel with 1 x-position
2. Associate each element of a diphthong with 1 x-position
3. Associate a [ + tense] vowel with 2 x-position
Giegerich is one of the main supporters of this theory and uses x-positions in syllable
tree-diagrams as a way to show the number of timing units in the syllable. That way,
differences in length between short and long vowels can be expressed in terms of x-
positions. In English, long vowels and diphthongs are tensed (tensas) while short vowels
are lax (laxas). The later are produced with less muscular effort.
The coda.
- There should be no violation of the rule (sonority sequencing principle).
- No 2 obstruents (oral stops, affricates and fricatives) in the coda.
- No 2 coronals (dentals, alveolar, palatals and palatoalveolars) in the coda.
Places of articulation:
1. Alveolar /t, d, n, l, s, z/
2. Dental /ɵ,ð/ Del 1 al 4 son coronals.
3. Palatal /j/
4. Palatoalveolar /t∫, dƷ, ∫, Ʒ/
5. Glottal /h/
6. Bilabial /p, b, m/
7. Labiovelar /w/
8. Postalveolar /r/
9. Labiodental /f, v/
10. Velar /k, g, ŋ/
The Rhyme.
The rhyme has to be analysed and recognized as a single phonological unit, since both
the coda and the peak function together and not in separate ways.
- A well-formed syllable contains no more then 3 x-position in the rhyme.
- A well-formed syllable contains minimally 2 x-positions in the rhyme, since lax
monophthongs (short vowels) occur only in closed syllables.
- Three cannot be two obstruents in the coda.
Appendices.
The elements which violate the sonority degree proposed by the sonority scale or occupy
a fourth position in the rhyme will be treated as appendices. They will depart from the
syllables.
SYLLABIFICATION OF POLYSYLLABIC WORDS
1. Nucleus formation: scan the word for the most sonorous segment in the word
and identify it as the nucleus/peak.
2. Onset maximization principle: assign consonants to the left of the peak to the
onset. Any consonants placed between two vowels tend to be placed in the onset
of the following syllable, instead of the coda of the previous syllable.
3. Coda formation: assign consonants to the right of the peak to the coda.
Appendices will always depart from the syllable.
4. Phonotactic constraints and rules for the analysis:
4.1. A maximum of 2 consonant positions are permitted in the coda (following
and not violating the sonority degradation).
4.2. Any number of coronals appearing after another consonant in a word-
final coda are treated as appendices. (we cannot have 2 coronals in the coda).
4.3. The coda cannot contain 2 obstruents.
4.4. We consider the distinction between tense (long)/ lax (short) vowels:
stressed lax vowel can only occur in closed syllables.
Speech is produced through the coordinated actions of the respiratory, resonatory, and
articulatory subsystems. In fact, impairments in one or more of these subsystems can
compromise speech intelligibility.
No sound can be produced without a supply of force or energy. For speech it is the
breathing mechanism that constitute the energy supply with the expiratory phase. In
order that sound will be generated the steady flow of energy in one direction has to be
converted into oscillations.
For speech, the larynx performs this function with the vocal cords/folds. The alternate
opening and closing of the vocal folds results in the ignition of successive puffs of air into
the space above the larynx. This stream of pulses constitutes the bases of the sound
generated by the larynx.
According to the Source-filter Theory of speech production there is a sound source, the
larynx, and a filtering of the sound source in the airway above the glottis by the actions
of resonators (oral cavity, nasal cavity and pharynx).
The source of speech is the glottis. The air passing through the vibrating vocal folds is
stimulated by the opening and closing movements. Before air can exit the mouth, it must
pass through the vocal tract, which acts as a filter by the action of resonators. This filter
damps (atenuar, sin suprimir del todo) some frequencies while intensifying others.
Which frequencies get down and which intensify depends on the shape of the vocal tract
at a given point in tongue. The frequencies that are intensified are the formants
(formantes) that we see on a spectrogram, and represent the sounds that resonate the
loudest in the particular filter formed by the vocal tract.
A wave is an energy transport. They transport energy, not matter. They can be described
as disturbances that travel through a medium, transporting energy from one location to
another location. Sound waves can be:
5. WHAT IS SOUND
6. PROPERTIES OF WAVES
Phonation
The glottal wave is periodic, that is it consists of repeated cycles of motion, and therefore
it must be made up of a fundamental frequency plus a range of harmonic. The
fundamental frequency has the greatest amplitude, and the amplitude of each
succeeding harmonic would be falling off progressively as the frequency increases.
Periodic vibration of the vocal folds is known as phonation.
When an event occurs repeatedly, then we say that the event is periodic, and we refer
to the time for the event to repeat itself as the period. So, the period of a wave is the
time for a particle on a medium to make one complete vibrational cycle. Mathematically,
the period is the reciprocal of the frequency and vice versa.
Frequency and period are different yet delated quantities. Frequency is the cycles per
second, and period is the seconds per cycle. The larynx is the sound source for all the
periodic sounds that occur in the course of speech.
Most of the filtering of sound spectrum is carried out by that part of the vocal tract
anterior to the sound source. In the case of a glottal source, the filter is the entire
supraglottal vocal tract.
The most important thing to know is that the source and filter are independent. The
larynx which is the glottal source gives raise to the fundamental frequency; while
resonators give raise to formant structure, which is the result of differences in
articulation which affects the shape and dimensions of the vocal tract.
9. VOWEL FORMANTS
In speech the fundamental frequency is changing all the time, but the spectrum of the
resulting sound always has the same general outline or envelope. This means a certain
sameness of quality is heard in a range of sound with different fundamentals. If this
were not the case, speech sound could not fulfil the linguistic function, they in fact have
or form differences in vowel quality, result from different vocal tract shapes.
The term used for a resonance of the system in this context is a formant or peak of
resonance. The perceived quality of a vowel can be described by the relative location of
the peaks in the lower part of the spectrum termed (called) formants.
Differences in formant structure are the results of differences in articulation which affect
the shape and dimensions of the vocal tract. The most important modifications are due
to alterations in the configuration of the tongue. A significant part of the information
listeners uses in distinguishing the sounds is carried by the disposition of the first
formant (F1) and the second formant (F2).
Formants
There is a relationship between F1 and F2 to either part of the tongue raised and degree
of elevation of the tongue.
F1 is related with the degree of elevation of the tongue. It is negatively correlated with
vowel height (high vowels will show a low F1, while lower ones will show a higher F1).
F2 is related to part of the tongue raised. It is positively correlated with vowel frontness.
One of the challenges of speech research is to determine what aspects of the acoustic
signal are relevant to the listener for perceiving the sounds of speech. It seems that not
all items of acoustic information are equally important from the point of view of speech
perception. However, there is no doubt that the perception of speech is necessarily
structured by the inherent constraints of the auditory system.
Formants Heed Hid Head Had Hard Hod Hawed Hood Who’d Heard Hud
F1 277 304
F2 2478 2511