The Impact of Perinatal HIV Infection On Older School-Aged Children's and Adolescents' Receptive Language and Word Recognition Skills
The Impact of Perinatal HIV Infection On Older School-Aged Children's and Adolescents' Receptive Language and Word Recognition Skills
The Impact of Perinatal HIV Infection On Older School-Aged Children's and Adolescents' Receptive Language and Word Recognition Skills
0197
The Impact of Perinatal HIV Infection on Older School-Aged Childrens and Adolescents Receptive Language and Word Recognition Skills
Elizabeth Brackis-Cott, Ph.D.,1 Ezer Kang, Ph.D.,2,3 Curtis Dolezal, Ph.D.,1 Elaine J. Abrams, M.D.,2,3 and Claude Ann Mellins, Ph.D.1
Abstract
Perinatally HIV-infected youths are reaching adolescence in large numbers. Little is known about their cognitive functioning. This study aims to describe and compare the receptive language ability, word recognition skills, and school functioning of older school-aged children and adolescents perinatally HIV infected (HIV-positive) and perinatally HIV-exposed but uninfected (seroreverters; HIV-negative). Participants included 340 youths (206 HIVpositive, 134 HIV-negative), 916 years old, and their caregivers. Youths completed the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition (PPVT-III) and the Reading Subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test, Third Edition (WRAT-3). Caregivers were interviewed regarding demographic characteristics and school placement of youths. Medical information was abstracted from medical charts. Both groups of youths scored poorly on the PPVT-III and WRAT-3 with about one third of youths scoring in less than the 10th percentile. The HIV-positive youths scored lower than the seroreverters (M 83.8 versus 87.6, t 2.21, p 0.028) on the PPVT-III and on the WRAT-3 (M 88.2 versus 93.8, t 2.69, p 0.008). Among the HIV-positive youths, neither CD4 cell count, HIV RNA viral load or Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) classication were signicantly associated with either PPVT-III or WRAT-3 scores. However, youths who were taking antiretroviral medication had lower WRAT-3 scores than youths not taking medication (M 95.03 versus 86.89, t 2.38, p 0.018). HIV status remained signicantly associated with PPVT-III and WRAT-3 standard scores after adjusting for demographic variables. Many youths had been retained in school and attended special education classes. Findings highlight poor language ability among youths infected with and affected by HIV, and the importance of educational interventions that address this emerging need.
Introduction ith the advent of antiretroviral treatment (ART), children living with perinatal HIV infection are reaching adolescence in large numbers. For example, in New York City, more than 67% of living persons diagnosed with HIV infection before 13 years of age are currently 13 years of age or older.1 These youths are primarily ethnic minorities living in impoverished, urban communities who contend with multiple stressors that place them at risk for poor cognitive functioning (e.g., exposure to substances in utero, attendance at schools in poor performance districts, comorbid behavioral problems, inconsistent school attendance because of poor health or health care appointments). From the be-
ginning of the epidemic, there have been consistent ndings of signicant neurologic, developmental, cognitive, and language decits in HIV-infected (HIV-positive) infants and younger children.26 Clinical reports indicate that older school age perinatally HIV-infected children present with signicant learning problems affecting their academic performance, developmental milestones, and ability to function independently.7 However, little is known about the cognitive functioning, reading ability, or school performance of perinatally HIV-infected adolescents. One study by Kullgren and colleagues8 that included adolescents found cognitive, adaptive, as well as behavioral delays in HIV-positive youths compared to normative samples. Unfortunately, their sample of 67 youths ranged in age from 316 years with a mean of 6.7
1 2 3
HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies at New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, New York. Department of Pediatrics, Harlem Hospital Center, New York, New York. College of Physicians & Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York.
415
416 years. Similarly, Jeremy and colleagues8a found poor neuropsychological functioning in HIV-positive children and adolescents (4 months to 17 years) compared to established norms and that neuropsychological functioning was worse for children with higher viral loads. However, a limitation of the study was the authors inability to look at specic age groups and the lack of an appropriate control group. Bisiacchi and colleagues8b compared a sample of 29 perinatally HIVinfected children and adolescents and 13 seroverters (615 years), and found signicant differences in executive functions in HIV-positive children compared to seroreverters. Also despite the documentation of longer lifespan among perinatally HIV-infected youth, little has been published on the school experiences of older HIV-positive children and adolescents. Mialky and colleagues8c found that among 85 school-age children (518 years, mean 9.9 years), 85% were attending public schools, 3% were in private schools, and 2% were home schooled. Five children were attending special education classes at the time of the study, 23.5% had repeated at least one grade, and 52% were receiving some special services in their school such as resource room (n 10), physical therapy (n 5), tutoring (n 4), speech therapy (n 3), and accelerated courses (n 2). As children transition to early and middle adolescence, it becomes increasingly clear that language and reading skills are the critical building blocks for literacy and future academic success912 with an important transition from learning to read and reading to learn.13 Poor educational achievement, including delayed reading and language skills in youth perinatally infected with HIV may have implications for their ability to understand their illness and its treatment, to adhere to complex medication regimens, and to make decisions about sexual and=or drug risk behaviors.1416 For example, studies of HIV-positive adults have found that those who were cognitively compromised or had low levels of health literacy had greater difculty with adherence, particularly when the regimen was complex.1516a As perintally HIV-infected youths age and assume more responsibility for their own health care, they too will require a degree of health literacy that, if not present, may seriously impede their capacity to effectively manage their disease. In summary, although there are a number of studies demonstrating neurocognitive and language decits in HIVpositive infants and young children, particularly in the domain of expressive language.1719 The data on preadolescents and older adolescents are limited. The few studies that exist cover too large an age range, do not focus on adolescents, or do not have appropriate control groups. Moreover, the language and reading skills of HIV-positive adolescents have not specically been described. It is unclear whether early discrepancies between HIV-positive and HIV-negative children persists over time or whether they might be diminished with age and improved health. Such information will be helpful to plan psychoeducational interventions for the growing population of older, perinatally HIV-infected youths. Using baseline data from Project CASAH (Child and Adolescent Self-Awareness and Health Study), one of the largest U.S.based studies of psychosocial determinants of behavior in a sample composed of both perinatally HIV- infected youths and perinatally HIV-exposed, but uninfected youths (seroreverters; HIV-negative) with similar age and demographic backgrounds, the goal of this study is to conduct secondary
BRACKIS-COTT ET AL. data analysis using two brief screening tools to describe and compare the receptive vocabulary skills, word decoding ability, and school achievement in both groups. Methods Participants and procedures Participants include 206 perintally HIV-infected older school-aged children and adolescents and 134 seroreverters, and their 340 primary caregivers. Inclusion criteria are (1) ages 9 to 16 years perinatally exposed to HIV, (2) English-speaking, (3) caregivers speak English or Spanish; and (4) caregiver with legal capacity to consent for the childs participation. Both adolescents who had and had not been formally told of their HIV status were included. During the study recruitment period (20032007), of 443 eligible participants, 11% refused contact with the research team and 6% could not be contacted by the study coordinators. Of 367 (83% of 443) approached to participate in the study 340 caregiver=youth dyads (93% of 367) enrolled. New York State condentiality laws preclude data collection on families who refused participation. Participants were recruited from four urban medical centers in New York City. Two sources of data were collected: (1) caregiver and youth interviews and (2) medical chart abstractions for HIV-positive youth. Caregivers and children were interviewed simultaneously but separately during two 60- to 90-minute sessions (approximately 1 month apart). Among the 340 caregiver=youth dyads enrolled, 96% completed both interview sessions. Since the data for this analysis were derived from the second interview session the study cohort included 325 caregiver=youth dyads. This study received local Institutional Review Board approval and was compliant with the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA) of 1996. All caregivers provided written informed consent for themselves and youths and youths provided written assent. Monetary reimbursement for time and transportation was provided. Assessments Child receptive language ability and word recognition skills. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), is a widely used, well-validated test of receptive language abilities.20 The examiner reads each word aloud; the child chooses which of four pictures best illustrates the word. Standard scores range from 40160. Research shows a strong correlation between the total score of the PPVT-III and the Full Scale IQ (r 0.90) and Verbal IQ scores (r 0.91) from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children.20 The PPVT-III was also validated using the Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test.20 The Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-3)21 measures the development of reading, spelling, and arithmetic skills for individuals ages 575 years. For this study, we used the reading subtest of the WRAT-3, which focuses on specic coding skills: Readingrecognizing and naming letters and pronouncing printed words. The child is asked to read up to 42 words aloud. Standard scores range from 45155. The information presented in the technical manual provides adequate psychometric properties of the WRAT-3.21 Demographics. Caregivers were asked about child and caregiver age, gender, ethnic identity, education, work status of caregiver, as well as family income.
LANGUAGE ABILITY OF HIV-POSITIVE YOUTH AND SEROREVERTERS Table 1. Characteristics of Youths and Their Primary Caregivers by Child HIV Status
Total Sample HIV-positive Seroreverters n (% of 325) n (% of 196) n (% of 129) Child Variables Race=Ethnicity African American Latino Age 910 1112 1314 1516 Female Current grade in school 2nd4th 5th6th 7th8th 9th12th Caregiver Variables Relationship to childa Biologic parent Relative caregiver Nonrelative caregiver HIV infecteda Race=Ethnicity African American Latino Ageb 1929 3039 4049 5059 6082 Female Education 09th grade 10th12th grade >High school
417
Data analyses HIV-positive youths and seroreverters were compared on demographic variables, PPVT-III, WRAT-3, and school-related characteristics. w2 tests were used for dichotomous and categorical variables, and t tests were used for continuous variables. Variables such as age and education have been categorized for presentation in the tables, but were retained in their original continuous form for t test analyses. In addition, multivariate regression analyses were conducted to test the association between HIV status and PPVT-III=WRAT-3 standard scores while adjusting for demographic variables. Results The vast majority of demographic variables were similar between HIV-positive youths and seroreverters including child race=ethnicity, age, gender, and grade in school (Table 1). Signicantly fewer HIV-positive youths were living with a birth parent (36% versus 70%; w2 36.9, p < 0.001), and thus, by denition, an HIV-positive caregiver (31% versus 69%; w2 42.9, p < 0.001). Also, HIV-positive youths had older caregivers than seroreverters (t 2.54, p 0.01). Among the HIV-positive youths, the mean CD4 cell count was 602 (median 572, standard deviation [SD] 317) and only 10% had CD4 less than 200 cells=mm3. The median HIV RNA viral load was 3150 copies per milliliter (mean 14,722; SD 25,516); 34% had undetectable viral loads ( 400 copies per milliliter) and only 5% had viral load values 100,000 or more copies per milliliter. The majority of HIV-positive youths were taking antiretroviral medications (84%). Receptive language ability Among the total sample, the mean PPVT-III standard score was 85.3 (Table 2). PPVT-III percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to 96th percentile; 60% of all youths scored below average (<25th percentile) and 37% scored less than 10th percentile. Among the HIV-positive youths, the mean standard score was 83.9 and PPVT-III percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to 96th percentile. Sixty-two percent scored below average (<25th percentile) and 39% scored less than 10th percentile. Among the seroreverters, the mean PPVT-III standard score was 87.5 and PPVT-III percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to 86th percentile. Fifty-six percent of the seroreverters scored below average (<25th percentile) and a third (34%) scored less than 10th percentile. The HIV-positive youths scored signicantly lower (M 83.8) than the seroreverters (M 87.6) on the PPVT-III (t 2.21, p 0.028). Word recognition
180 (55) 96 (30) 100 77 90 58 164 75 86 90 73 (31) (24) (28) (18) (51) (23) (27) (28) (23)
114 (58) 58 (30) 51 50 58 37 99 38 55 58 44 (26) (26) (30) (19) (51) (19) (28) (30) (22)
66 (51) 38 (30) 49 27 32 21 65 37 31 32 29 (38) (21) (25) (16) (50) (29) (24) (25) (23)
160 (49) 73 (23) 92 (28) 149 (46) 167 (51) 116 (36) 13 68 114 61 69 284 (4) (21) (35) (19) (21) (87)
70 (36) 53 (27) 73 (37) 61 (31) 101 (52) 72 (37) 7 30 67 47 45 170 (4) (15) (34) (24) (23) (87)
90 (70) 20 (16) 19 (15) 88 (68) 66 (51) 44 (34) 6 38 47 14 24 114 (5) (30) (36) (11) (19) (88)
HIV-positive vs. HIV-negative ap < 0.001; bp < 0.05. Due to missing data, some values do not sum to the total Ns listed. Due to rounding, percentages may not sum to 100.
School-related characteristics. Caregivers were asked about school placement of youth (regular education class versus special education class), and whether the youth had ever skipped a grade, been held back, suspended or expelled. Child health status. CD4 lymphocyte count (cells=mm ), HIV RNA viral load (copies per milliliter), and clinical disease category according to CDC classication system for the closest date to the research interview were obtained through medical record abstraction for the HIV-positive youths. Based on variation in assay methodology all viral load 100,000 or more copies per milliliter were coded as 100,000 copies per milliliter and all values 400 or less copies per milliliter were coded as undetectable. In addition, caregivers of HIV-positive youths were asked whether their child was taking antiretroviral therapy.
3
Among the full sample, the mean WRAT-3 score was 90.5 and WRAT-3 percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to100th percentile. Half of youths (49%) scored below average (<25th percentile) and 28% scored less than 10th percentile. Among HIV-positive youths, the mean WRAT-3 score was 88.3 and WRAT-3 percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to 97th percentile. Half of the HIV-positive youths (54%) scored below average (<25th percentile) and a third (33%) scored less than 10th percentile. Among the seroreverters, the mean WRAT-3 score was 93.8 and WRAT-3 percentile scores ranged from less than 1st to 100th percentile. Forty percent
418 Table 2. PPVT and WRAT Scores by Child HIV Status Child variables PPVT Mean standard score (SD)b Range Mean percentile score (SD) Range WRAT-3 Mean standard score (SD)c Range Mean percentile score (SD)c Range Total sample n 322a 85.30 (14.39) 22127 24.08 (22.44) 096 90.44 (18.02) 44139 34.30 (30.08) 0100 HIV-positive n 196 83.82 (14.81) 22127 22.09 (21.16) 096 88.23 (17.92) 44128 30.54 (29.66) 097
BRACKIS-COTT ET AL.
Seroreverters n 129 87.55 (13.47) 59116 27.10 (24.03) 086 93.77 (17.72) 44139 39.97 (29.93) 0100
a Note. 2 youths refused to complete the WRAT-3. HIV-positive vs. Seroreverters; bp < 0.05; cp < 0.01. PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; WRAT-3, Wide Range Achievement Test, Third Edition; SD, standard deviation.
scored below average (<25th percentile) and 21% scored less than 10th percentile. The HIV-positive youths scored signicantly lower (M 88.2) than the seroreverters (M 93.8) on the WRAT-3 (t 2.69, p 0.008). For both the PPVT-III and the WRAT-3, the full range of scores was represented and normally distributed with no extreme outliers. Associations between demographics, health status, and ability Correlations, t tests, and analyses of variance (ANOVAs) assessed the associations between PPVT-III and WRAT-3 standard scores and all demographic variables in Table 1. Males and youths in higher grades had higher PPVT-III scores; younger youths had higher scores on the WRAT-3. All other analyses were not statistically signicant. Among HIVpositive youths, none of the health status variables obtained from medical charts (CD4 count, viral load, CDC classication) were signicantly associated with either PPVT-III or WRAT-3 scores; all correlation coefcients were close to zero. However, youths who were taking antiretroviral medication had lower WRAT-3 scores than youths not taking medication (M 95.03 versus 86.89, t 2.38, p 0.018). In regression analyses, HIV status remained signicantly associated with PPVT-III and WRAT-3 standard scores after adjusting for
childs gender, household income, caregiver education, child race=ethnicity, and whether the caregiver was the childs biologic parent (Table 3). School-related characteristics Over a third of the youths (35%) in both groups (37% of HIV-positive, 32% of HIV-negative) had been retained in school, 33% were currently attending special education classes (37% of HIV-positive versus 28% of HIV-negative) and 47% had a history of special education placement (52% HIVpositive, 39% HIV-negative, w2 4.77, p < 0.05; Table 4). Across groups, special education placement was most frequently attributed to problems with reading or math. Discussion Our sample of perinatally HIV-infected and exposed youths scored poorly on measures of language functioning: 37% scored less 10th percentile on the PPVT-III and 28% scored less than 10th percentile on the WRAT-3. Poor scores are suggestive of poor verbal ability including limited vocabulary and lack of basic skills needed for reading. In addition, large numbers of youths were academically retained (35%) and placed in special education classes (47%). Overall,
Table 3. Multivariate Regression Models Predicting Cognitive Functioning among HIV-Positive and HIV-Negative Adolescents PPVT - III b (SE) Gender (1 male, 2 female) Income (categories) Caregiver years of education Race=ethnicity: Latinoa Race=ethnicity: Othera Caregiver is biologic parent (0 no, 1 yes) Child HIV status (0 HIV-negative, 1 HIV-positive) 4.31 0.70 0.14 1.98 2.32 1.18 4.18 (1.65) (0.33) (0.28) (1.95) (2.44) (1.89) (1.79) p 0.009 0.034 0.622 0.312 0.342 0.533 0.020 WRAT - 3 b (SE) 2.01 0.95 0.02 1.47 5.40 4.57 5.04 (2.06) (0.41) (0.35) (2.44) (3.07) (2.36) (2.23) p 0.331 0.021 0.950 0.548 0.079 0.054 0.024
a Reference category African American. PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; WRAT-3, Wide Range Achievement Test, Third Edition; SE, standard error.
LANGUAGE ABILITY OF HIV-POSITIVE YOUTH AND SEROREVERTERS Table 4. School-Related Characteristics for HIV Infected and Uninfected Youth School-related characteristics Currently in school (yes) Ever held back in school (yes) Currently in special education class Ever attended special education class (yes)a Reason for special education placement ever Reading problems Math problems Attention problems Discipline problems
HIV-positive vs. HIV-negative; ap < 0.05.
419
Total sample n (% of 325) 324 (99) 112 (35) 109 (34) 154 (47) n (% of 153) 95 (62) 84 (55) 58 (40) 35 (23)
HIV-positive n (% of 196) 195 (99) 72 (37) 73 (37) 102 (52) n (% of 101) 64 (63) 56 (55) 38 (38) 25 (25)
Seroreverters n (% of 129) 129 (100) 40 (31) 36 (28) 52 (40) n (% of 52) 31 (60) 28 (54) 20 (39) 10 (19)
the performance of this cohort of perinatally HIV-exposed youths was well below age expectations. HIV-positive youths performance was statistically worse as compared to the seroreverters on both measures of reading and language ability and the HIV-positive youths were more likely to have attended special education classes. Our data suggest that infected youths continue to have learning problems at higher than expected rates as they age and enter adolescence. Because HIV remains a stigmatized disease and teachers are rarely disclosed to about a childs status, it will be important for medical doctors working with this population to be aware of how HIV inuences youths ability to function in school. The stress of living with a chronic stigmatized illness coupled with a sense of educational failure places youths with HIV at further risk for behavioral and health problems. For example, poor language skills may limit youths ability to understand their illness and compromise their ability to adhere to challenging medication regimens.15,16 Conversely, better cognitive functioning, particularly in the verbal domain, may operate as a protective factor against early sexual activity during adolescence, whereas lower intelligence may be a risk factor.14 Furthermore, our school-based ndings that large number of these youths are being retained in school and attending special education classes further demonstrates how these perinatally HIV infected and affected youths are struggling. It is noteworthy to report that, among the HIV-positive youths, delays were not associated with most recent CD4 count or viral load. Previous studies that found viral load to be unrelated to cognitive test scores, speculated that the lack of association with viral load might be due to the fact that viral load values can uctuate with changes in adherence.2224 These biomedical markers only reect current health status and do not necessarily represent past medical history which may have included long periods of illness, immune suppression and active viremia. Furthermore, cognitive functioning may be indirectly related to disease severity, for example with increased number of absences from school among sicker children. Our nding that youths who were currently taking antiretroviral therapy had lower scores on the WRAT-3 could be explained by sicker children being more likely to be prescribed medication or conversely could be related to side effects of the medications.25 This nding needs more thorough analysis and assessment to fully understand the association, however, such analysis is beyond the scope of this investigation.
Although HIV-positive youths had signicantly lower scores on both measures of language and reading ability, it is important to note that 34% of seroreverters scored less than 10th percentile on the PPVT-III and 21% scored less than 10th percentile on the WRAT-3. By denition, these youths are living with an HIV-positive parent or have already lost a parent to death. In addition to dealing with the stressors of inner city living, these youths are often caretakers for their parents and isolated due to their family secret, often leading to behavior problems or symptoms of anxiety or depression.2630 Similar to the HIV-positive youths, seroreverters are living in impoverished, urban communities in poor performance school districts. Unfortunately, HIV-exposed but uninfected youths are often difcult to identify, as they are typically not followed in specialized HIV care clinics. HIVpositive youths are more likely to have regular contact with medical, mental health, and social service providers to help families navigate systems to get the services they need, but HIV-negative youths are less likely to benet from such services. There is no sign that the numbers of uninfected, but HIV-affected children are diminishing, as HIV disease continues to spread in women of child-bearing age. Our data suggest that seroreverters may be at risk for poor outcomes given the association between cognitive functioning and risk behaviors31,32 that can in turn lead to sexually transmitted diseases (STDs), pregnancy, and behaviorally acquired HIV disease. Medical providers treating seroreverters may be in a unique position to identify youths who are struggling and make the appropriate mental health and social service referrals. There are a number of limitations to this study that should be considered when interpreting our results. Participants were recruited from HIV primary care clinics in New York City and ndings may not generalize to perinatally HIVexposed youths in other settings. Also, although we attempted to recruit both groups from similar communities based on the demographics of pediatric HIV disease, other factors (e.g., differential rates of study refusal or access to services) may have altered the group effects. Due to issues of condentiality and HIPAA, no data were collected on participants who refused to participate or who were not approached. Nor were data collected to assess whether children were bilingual or their level of acculturation in general, all of which could account for some of the ndings. In addition, Project CASAH is study of psychosocial determinants of youth risk and resilience, with a
420 particular interest in mental health and thus included limited measurement of cognitive or academic functioning that could be administered in a short time. The WRAT-3 has limited validity when it comes to predicting actual academic achievement as there is a difference between ones ability to successfully decode words and comprehend text. It also remains to be seen how well suited the PPVT-III, while highly correlated with overall IQ in typically developing children, is as a proxy for overall IQ or general verbal ability in those with learning difculties or general developmental delays. More thorough evaluation of cognitive functioning would be helpful in strengthening our ndings. Notwithstanding these limitations, this study demonstrates poor verbal and reading ability among youths infected with and affected by HIV, and highlights the importance of educational interventions that address this emerging need. As this population ages, further research should continue to examine the relationship between markers of disease severity, related psychosocial factors, and cognitive functioning and attempt to clarify specic cognitive domains that may be more sensitive to effects of HIV among adolescents including motor functioning, executive skills, and information processing speed,33 so that appropriate educational or remedial programs can be developed and incorporated into ongoing care for this population. Acknowledgment This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH63636; Principal Investigator: Claude Ann Mellins, Ph.D.), and by a center grant from the National Institute of Mental Health to the HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies at NY State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University (P30-MH43520; Principal Investigator: Anke A. Ehrhardt, Ph.D.) Author Disclosure Statement No competing nancial interests exist. References
1. NYC DOHMH Pediatric & Adolescent HIV AIDS Surveillance Update New York City: Semiannual Report. December 2007, p. 5. 2. Belman AL, Muenz LR, Marcus JC, et al. Neurologic status of human immunodeciency virus 1-infected infants and their controls: A prospective study from birth to 2 years. Pediatrics 1996;98(6 Pt 1):11091118. 3. Coplan J, Contello KA, Cunningham CK, et al. Early language development in children exposed to or infected with human immunodeciency virus. Pediatrics 1998;102:8. 4. Epstein LG, Sharer LR, Goudsmit J. Neurological and neuropathological features of human immunodeciency virus infection in children. Ann Neurol 1988;23:S19S23. 5. Mellins CA, Levenson RL, Zawadzki R, Kairam R, Weston M. Effects of pediatric HIV-infection and pre-natal drug exposure on mental and psychomotor development. J Pediatr Psychol 1994;19:617628. 6. Willen EJ. Neurocognitive outcomes in pediatric HIV. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2006;12:223228. 7. Wolters PL, Brouwers P, Moss HA. Pediatric HIV disease: Effect on cognition, learning, and behavior. Sch Psychol Q 1995;10:305328.
BRACKIS-COTT ET AL.
8. Kullgren KA, Morris MK, Bachanas PJ, Jones JS. Prediction of cognitive, adaptive, and behavioral functioning in preschool and school-age children with HIV. Child Health Care 2004;33:241256. 8a. Jeremy RJ, Kim S, Nozyce M, et al. Neuropsycholgical functioning and viral load in stable antiretroviral therapyexperienced HIV-infected children. Pediatrics 2005;115: 380387. 8b. Bisiacchi PS, Suppiej A, Laverda A. Neuropsychological evaluation of neurologically asymptomatic HIV-infected children. Brain Cogn 2000;43:4952. 8c. Mialky E, Vagnoni J, Rutstein R. School-age children wtih perinatally acquired HIV infection: Medical and psychosocial issues in a Philadelphia cohort. AIDS Patient Care STDs 2001;15:575579. 9. Denti L, Guerin G. Confronting the problem of poor literacy: Recognition and action. Reading Writing Q 2004;20: 113122. 10. Durham RE, Farkas G. The importance of early language skills: An explanation for social class. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, San Francisco, CA. www.allacademic.com=meta=p109174_ index.html (Last accessed March 26, 2009). 11. Ippolito J, Steele JL, Samson JF. Introduction: Why Adolescent literacy matters now. Harvard Educ Rev 2008;78:16. 12. Shapiro ES. From research to practice: Promoting academic competence for underserved students. School Psych Rev 2008;37:4651. 13. Chall JS. The Academic Achievement Challenge: What Really Works in the Classroom? New York: Guilford Press, 2000. 14. Halpern CT, Joyner K, Udry R, Suchindran C. Smart teens dont have sex (or kiss much either). J Adolesc Health 2000;26:213225. 15. Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, et al. Medication adherence among HIV adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology 2002;59:19441950. 16. Wagner GJ. Predictors of antiretroviral adherence as measured by self-report, electronic monitoring, and medication diaries. AIDS Patient Care STDs 2002;16:599608. 16a. Kalichman SA, Rompa D. Functional health literacy is associated with health status and health-related knowledge in people living with HIV-AIDS. J Acquir Immune Dec Syndr 2000;25:337344. 17. Coppan J, Contello K, Cunningham CK, et al. Early language development in children exposed to or infected with human immunodeciency virus. Pediatrics 1998;102:14. 18. Epstein LG, Sharer LR, Loeske JM, et al. Neurologic manifestations of human immunodeciency virus in children. Pediatrics 1986;78:678687. 19. Wolters PL, Brouwers P, Civitello L, Moss HA. Receptive and expressive language function of children with symptomatic HIV infection and relationship with disease parameters: A longitudinal 24-month follow-up study. AIDS 1997;11:11351144. 20. Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Examiners Manual, 3rd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1997. 21. Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd ed. Lutz, FL: PAR=Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 1993. 22. Martin SC, Wolters P, Toledo-Tamula MA, et al. Cognitive functioning in school-aged children with vertically acquired HIV infection being treated with highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART). Dev Neuropsychol 2006;30:633657.
421
30. Stein JA, Riedel M, Rotherum-Borus MJ. Parentication and its impact on adolescent children of parents with AIDS. Fam Proc 1999;38:193208. 31. Bogart LM, Collins RL, Ellickson PL, Klein DJ. Adolescent predictors of generalized health risk in young adulthood: A 10-year longitudinal assessment. J Drug Issues 2006;36:571 596. 32. Luster T, Small SA. Factors associated with sexual risk-taking behaviors among adolescents. J Marriage Fam 1994;56: 622632. 33. Askinazi LA, Grant ML, Legido A, Bagarazzi ML The relationship between performance on neuropsychological test measures and immunologic and virologic markers of disease progression in HIV infected children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:653654.
Address reprint requests to: Elizabeth Brackis-Cott, Ph.D. HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies 1051 Riverside Drive New York, NY 10032 E-mail: eb372@columbia.edu