SLUSE
SLUSE
SLUSE
THEORIES OF EMOTION
WHAT IS EMOTION……………………………………………………………………………..2
THE PHSIOLOGY OF EMOTION………………………………………………………………3
1
[Type here]
2
[Type here]
To emote is to
the experience and expression of emotions
It's important to note that the physiology of emotion is highly interconnected with
cognitive processes, personal experiences, and social factors. Emotions are not
solely determined by biological mechanisms but are also influenced by individual
differences, cultural norms, and learned behaviors. As research continues to
advance, our understanding of the physiology of emotion will likely become more
nuanced, providing new insights into the complex nature of human emotions.
3
[Type here]
While plentiful research suggests that many facial expressions are universal, such
as a frown to indicate sadness, sociocultural norms and individual upbringings play
a role in our behavioral responses. For example, how love is expressed is different
both from person to person and across cultures.
Behavioral responses are important to signal to others how we’re feeling, but
research shows that they’re also vital to individuals’ well-being. A study in
the Journal of Abnormal Psychology found that while watching negative and positive
emotional films, suppression of behavioral responses to emotion had physical
effects on the participants. The effects included elevated heart rates. This suggests
that expressing behavioral responses to stimuli, both positive and negative, is
better for your overall health than holding those responses inside. Thus, there are
benefits of smiling, laughing and expressing negative emotions in a healthy way.
The physiological and behavioral responses associated with emotions illustrate that
emotion is much more than a mental state. Emotion affects our whole demeanor
and our health. Furthermore, our ability to understand others’ behavioral
responses plays a huge role in our emotional intelligence, which will be discussed in
more detail later.
4
[Type here]
Emotional psychologist Paul Ekman identified six basic emotions that could be
interpreted through facial expressions. They included happiness, sadness, fear,
anger, surprise and disgust. He expanded the list in 1999 to also include
embarrassment, excitement, contempt, shame, pride, satisfaction and amusement,
5
[Type here]
though those additions have not been widely adapted. Similarly, in the 1980s,
psychologist Robert Plutchik identified eight basic emotions which he grouped into
pairs of opposites, including joy and sadness, anger and fear, trust and disgust, and
surprise and anticipation. This classification is known as a wheel of emotions and
can be compared to a color wheel in that certain emotions mixed together can
create new complex emotions.
More recently, a new study from the Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology at
the University of Glasgow in 2014 found that instead of six, there may only be
four easily recognizable basic emotions. The study discovered that anger and
disgust shared similar facial expressions, as did surprise and fear. This suggests
that the differences between those emotions are sociologically-based and not
biologically-based. Despite all the conflicting research and adaptations, most
research acknowledge that there are a set of universal basic emotions with
recognizable facial features.
sees a bear while walking along in the woods, James and Lange would suggest that
the person would tremble and then realise that, because they are trembling, they are
afraid. The James-Lange theory of emotion, proposed by psychologist William
James and physiologist Carl Lange in the late 19th century, suggests that emotions
are the result of physiological responses to external stimuli. According to this
theory, when an individual encounters a stimulus, the body first experiences a
physiological response, such as increased heart rate, sweating, or trembling. It is
only after these bodily changes occur that the individual becomes aware of the
corresponding emotion, such as fear, anger, or happiness.
Complex emotions vary greatly in how they appear on a person’s face and don’t
have easily recognizable expressions. Grief looks quite different between cultures
and individuals. Some complex emotions, such as jealousy, may have no
accompanying facial expression at all.
Critics of the James-Lange theory have argued that physiological responses are
not specific enough to account for the wide range of emotional experiences and
that emotional experiences can occur without noticeable physiological changes.
Additionally, research in the field of emotion has revealed that the relationship
between physiological responses and emotional experiences is more complex
than proposed by the James-Lange theory. According to James, his theory is that
the bodily changes follow directly the perception of the exciting fact. Our feelings
of the same changes as they occur and this is called the emotion Essentially,
emotions stem from your interpretation of your physical sensations. For
example, your heart beating wildly would lead you to realize that you are
7
[Type here]
afraid. In this theory, a stimulus of some sort (for example, the large snarling dog)
produces a physiological reaction. This reaction, which is the arousal of the “fight-
or-flight” sympathetic nervous system (wanting to run), produces bodily
sensations such as increased heart rate, dry mouth, and rapid breathing. James
and Lang believed that physical arousal led to the labeling of the emotion (fear).
Simply put, “I am afraid because I am aroused,” “I am embarrassed because my
face is red, “I am nervous because my stomach is fluttering,” and “I am in love
because of my heart rate increases when I look at her or him.”
2. Simultaneous response: In response to the stimulus, the individual's body undergoes specific
physiological changes, such as increased heart rate, changes in breathing pattern, or release of
stress hormones. At the same time, the individual experiences the corresponding emotion.
The key distinction between the James-Lange theory and the Cannon-Bard theory is that in the
latter, the physiological response and emotional experience are seen as independent and
occurring simultaneously. In other words, according to the Cannon-Bard theory, encountering a
8
[Type here]
stimulus triggers both the physiological response and the emotional experience at the same
time.
One of the central tenets of the Cannon-Bard theory is that emotions are generated in the
brain's subcortical structures, particularly the thalamus, which relays sensory information to
both the cerebral cortex (responsible for conscious thought and decision-making) and the
autonomic nervous system (responsible for regulating involuntary bodily functions). This
suggests that emotional experiences are not solely dependent on bodily changes but are also
influenced by cognitive and perceptual processes.
Critics of the Cannon-Bard theory have argued that it oversimplifies the complex interplay
between physiological responses and emotional experiences. Additionally, subsequent research
has shown that there is a bidirectional relationship between physiological responses and
emotional experiences, with each influencing the other in a dynamic manner.
In summary, while the James-Lange theory emphasizes the role of physiological responses as
the primary cause of emotional experiences, the Cannon-Bard theory proposes that emotions
and physiological responses occur simultaneously and independently in response to external
stimuli. Both theories have contributed to our understanding of emotions, and contemporary
research continues to explore the intricate connections between physiological responses and
emotional experiences. Physiologists Walter Cannon and (1927) and Philip Bard (1934)
theorized that the emotion and the physiological arousal occur more or less at the same time.
Cannon, an expert in sympathetic arousal mechanisms, did not feel that the physical changes
aroused by different emotions were distinct enough to allow them to be perceived as different
emotions. Bard expanded on this idea by stating that the sensory information that comes into
the brain is sent simultaneously (by the thalamus) to both the cortex and the organs of the
sympathetic nervous system. The fear and the bodily reactions are, therefore, experienced at
the same time-not one after the other. “I am afraid and running and aroused!” The Cannon-
Bard theory of emotion, proposed by Walter Cannon and Philip Bard in the 1920s and 1930s,
suggests that emotional and physiological responses occur simultaneously in response to a
stimulus. According to this theory, when an individual encounters a stimulus, the brain
processes the information and triggers both a physiological response (such as increased heart
rate or sweating) and an emotional experience (such as fear or happiness) at the same time.
This theory suggests that the physiological and emotional responses are independent of each
other, but they occur simultaneously due to the brain's processing of the stimulus. This is in
contrast to the James-Lange theory, which proposes that physiological responses precede the
emotional experience.
9
[Type here]
Schachter-Singer Theory
This theory, developed by Stanley Schachter and Jerome E. Singer, introduces the
element of reasoning into the process of emotion. The theory hypothesizes that
when we experience an event that causes physiological arousal, we try to find a
reason for the arousal. Then, we experience the emotion. The Schachter-Singer two-
factor theory of emotion is a theory outlining two components of emotion, an initial
10
[Type here]
physiological arousal and a subsequent cognitive process. The cognitive identification or label
of the emotion is assigned based on the person's immediate environment. This theory was
introduced by Stanley Schachter and Jerome Singer in the 1960s.
One principal component of this theory is the interplay between physical arousal and the
cognitive identification of that arousal. According to the Schachter-Singer theory of emotion, in
order to feel an emotion, a person must both feel a physiological arousal and label the arousal.
In this context, physiological arousal describes physical processes of the body like increased
heart rate, perspiration, trembling, blushing, or appearing pale.
This theory also explains that a person's emotional response is formed from an
interpretation of their immediate environment. When a person has a physiological
response, they can use their proximate environment to interpret the emotion. The
theory explains that if a person has an apparent emotional explanation of their bodily
response, the person is unlikely to look for another justification from the environment.
These are far from the only theories of emotion that exist, but they provide great
examples of how the ideas about how emotion is generated differ from each other.
What all theories of emotion have in common is the idea that an emotion is based
off some sort of personally significant stimulus or experience, prompting a
biological and psychological reaction.
Benefits Of Understanding
Emotions
As discussed, emotions have helped humans evolve and survive. According to
Ekman, who developed the wheel of emotion, “It would be very dangerous if we
11
[Type here]
didn’t have emotions. It would also be a very dull life. Because, basically, our
emotions drive us — excitement, pleasure, even anger.” That is why it’s important
that we’re able to understand emotions as they play such an important role in how
we behave.
Ekman argues that emotions are fundamentally constructive. They are influenced
by what is good for our species overall and what we learned during our upbringing.
They guide our behavior in a way that should lead us to a positive outcome.
However, emotions can become destructive if the emotions we’ve learned are the
correct response no longer fit our situation, or if subconscious emotions cause
reactions that we are unable to understand. Being in touch with your emotions and
turning your understanding into action is called emotional awareness. Being able to
do this with others as well is referred to as emotional intelligence.
Emotional intelligence is the ability to perceive, control and evaluate emotions. The
term was first coined by researchers Peter Salovey and John D. Mayer and found
popularity through Dan Goleman’s 1996 book. They define it as the ability to
recognize, understand and manage our own emotions as well as recognize,
understand and influence those of others. The study of emotional intelligence has
gained much popularity since the mid-1990s, with business professionals,
relationship coaches and more using the term to encourage others to improve their
lives. Many researchers believe that emotional intelligence can be improved over
time, while some argue that it’s a trait we’re born with or without.
Regulating emotion in the self and others: Managing emotions so that all parties
are motivated towards a positive outcome.
Those who have emotional intelligence open themselves to positive and negative
emotional experiences, identify the emotions and communicate those emotions
appropriately. Emotionally intelligent people can use their understanding of their
emotions and the emotions of others to move toward personal and social growth.
Those with low emotional intelligence may unable to understand and control their
12
[Type here]
emotions or those of others. This could leave others feeling badly when they don’t
understand their emotions, feelings, or expressions.
Clearly, there are personal and professional benefits to improving your emotional
intelligence. In Forbes, Co-Chair of Nobel Peace prize-nominated campaign and
New York Times best-selling author Chade-Meng Tan addressed the benefits of
improving emotional intelligence. He pointed out that high emotional intelligence
correlates with better work performance, makes people better leaders and creates
the conditions for personal happiness. He stated, “There are also compelling
personal benefits, and the most basic of those occur in three categories: calmness
and clarity of mind, resilience and more satisfying relationships.”
Whether or not that is true is certainly debatable, but emotional intelligence has
served humans well throughout our evolution and history. It played a role long
before it was officially defined, and likely will for years to come.
13
[Type here]
14