Pudmed 1
Pudmed 1
Pudmed 1
DOI: 10.1111/mcn.12584
bs_bs_banner
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Estomih Mduma12 |
Beena Mark R. Koshy13 | Conaway14 | James A. Platts‐Mills2 |
2 |
Richard L. Guerrant Mark D. DeBoer1
1
Department of Pediatrics, University of
Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, USA Abstract
2
Center for Global Health, Department of Although many studies around the world hope to measure or improve developmental progress in
Medicine, University of Virginia, children to promote community flourishing and productivity, growth is sometimes used as a
Charlottesville, Virginia, USA surrogate because cognitive skills are more difficult to measure. Our objective was to assess how
3
Department of Nutrition Sciences, Penn childhood measures of anthropometry correlate with measures of child development in low‐
State University, University Park,
income settings with high prevalence of poor nutrition and enteric disease, to inform studies
Pennsylvania, USA
4 considering growth outcomes in the absence of direct child developmental skill assessment.
Department of Psychology, University of
Venda, Thohoyandou, South Africa Children from the MAL‐ED study were followed from birth to 24 months of age in field sites in 8
5
Department of Global Health and Primary low‐ and middle‐income countries across 3 continents. Monthly weight, length, and head
Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway circumference measurements were performed. At 24 months, the Bayley Scales of Infant and
6
Center for Nutrition and Food Security, Toddler Development was administered. We correlated cognitive measures at 24 months with
icddr‐b, Dhaka, Bangladesh
anthropometric measurements from birth to 2 years comparing 3 constructs: absolute attained
7
Department of Paediatrics and Child Health,
monthly measures, summative difference in measures from the mean growth curve, and rate
Aga Khan University, Karachi, Pakistan
8 of change in measures. Growth faltering at multiple time periods is related to Bayley cognitive
Department of Microbiology, Federal
University of Ceará, Fortaleza, Brazil outcomes at 24 months. Birthweight, overall growth by 18–24 months, and rate of growth in the
9
Department of Psychology, PRISMA, Iquitos, 6‐ to 18‐month period were most associated with 24‐month developmental scores. In this study,
Peru head circumference measurements, compared with length, was more closely linked to cognitive
10
Department of Psychology, Siddhi Memorial scores at 24 months. Notably, all studies between growth and cognitive outcomes exhibited low
Hospital, Bhaktapur, Nepal
r2 values (0.001–0.049). Anthropometric measures, particularly head circumference, were related
11
Department of Psychology, Temple
to cognitive development, although explaining a low percent of variance. When feasible, direct
University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
12
measures of child development may be more useful.
Haydom Global Health Research Centre,
Haydom, Tanzania
13 KEY W ORDS
Department of Developmental Pediatrics,
Christian Medical College, Vellore, India early childhood development, stunting, malnutrition, growth, global health, cognition
14
Department of Public Health Sciences,
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia,
USA
Correspondence
Rebecca J. Scharf, Division of Developmental
and Behavioral Pediatrics, University of
*
see MAL‐ED‐Network‐Investigators (2014), complete list of MAL‐ED investigators are listed in the Appendix section.
The Etiology, Risk Factors and Interactions of Enteric Infections and Malnutrition and the Consequences for Child Health and Development Project (MAL‐ED) is carried
out as a collaborative project supported by the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, the Foundation for the NIH, and the National Institutes of Health, Fogarty Interna-
tional Center.
Matern Child Nutr. 2018;14:e12584. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mcn © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1 of 11
https://doi.org/10.1111/mcn.12584
2 of 11 bs_bs_banner
SCHARF ET AL.
1 | I N T RO D U CT I O N
Key messages
Growth faltering in young children is a challenge worldwide in settings
of undernutrition and enteric disease. Children not meeting their • Stunting has been associated with cognitive delay in
growth potential may not be meeting their developmental potential smaller studies around the world.
either, leading to diminished flourishing of societies. Poor linear • Stunting has sometimes been used as a proxy for
growth, or stunting (height‐for‐age z‐score < −2), may follow child- estimating global rates of cognitive development in
hood enteric disease or undernutrition (R. E. Black, Victora, et al., children.
2013; Checkley et al., 2008; Guerrant, Oria, Moore, Oria, & Lima, • This analysis of three growth parameters (length, weight,
2008; Lu, Black, & Richter, 2016; Prendergast & Humphrey, 2014), and head circumference) in early life in a large, recent
and some studies around the world have estimated rates of cognitive international birth cohort found that physical growth in
development and productivity based upon levels of stunting (R. E. Black, early life was related to cognitive development at age 2.
Alderman, et al., 2013; Bornstein et al., 2012; Grantham‐McGregor
• In particular, head circumference was most correlated
et al., 2007). Many studies in global health settings are interested
with 2‐year‐old Bayley cognitive scores. However,
in child development as an outcome but use height attainment as a
variance explained was low, and direct measures of
proxy outcome (Bhutta et al., 2013; Dewey & Adu‐Afarwuah,
child development add important information.
2008; Humphrey et al., 2015; Prendergast & Humphrey, 2014).
Anthropometry has been used as a surrogate for cognitive develop-
ment, as cognitive development is harder to measure and is better
measured at older ages. For example, worldwide estimates of chil-
dren not meeting their developmental potential have been based be examined at certain time points (e.g., monthly measurements) to
on stunting rates along with poverty indicators (Bornstein et al., determine if measurements from specific time points are most related
2012; Grantham‐McGregor et al., 2007; Subramanian, Ozaltin, & to developmental progress (Borghi et al., 2006). Third, it may be useful
Finlay, 2011). Although growth has previously been related to devel- to examine, taking into account the starting point and rate at which a
opmental skills in smaller studies around the world (Chang, Walker, child grows, the summative growth achieved; overall size may take
Grantham‐McGregor, & Powell, 2002; Gandhi et al., 2011; Guerrant, into account growth patterns along the way and present a summary
Deboer, Moore, Scharf, & Lima, 2013; Tarleton et al., 2006), more of the time period (de Onis et al., 2004). Finally, studying rate of
information is needed about relationships between growth and growth may be useful (Sansavini et al., 2014; Zhang, McArdle, &
development. Nesselroade, 2012). Children may have an acceleration or decelera-
Measuring early growth monitors health and development (de Onis tion in growth rate, or may follow along a growth curve (z‐score) con-
et al., 2012), and several early anthropometry measurements—weight sistently, which may have particular relationships to their brain
(de Onis, Blossner, Borghi, Morris, & Frongillo, 2004), length (Berkman, development.
Lescano, Gilman, Lopez, & Black, 2002; Prendergast & Humphrey, Because post‐natal growth has been routinely and is frequently
2014), and head circumference (Ivanovic et al., 2004; Miller et al., collected around the world as an outcome in studies of enteric dis-
2016)—carry particular implications for maturation (American Academy ease, we used data from a large, global cohort of children from
of Pediatrics, 2015). Children in settings of undernutrition and enteric research study sites to examine how growth faltering from birth to
disease are at risk for insufficient brain growth, as well as linear growth. 24 months is related to cognitive outcomes measured by the Bayley
One commonly used marker of brain growth is head circumference Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley, 2006). Our goal
(Eichorn & Bayley, 1962; Wright & Emond, 2015), particularly in the was to evaluate measures of growth as predictors of cognitive devel-
first year of life when growth is most rapid before the fontanelles are opment at 24 months using three growth parameters (length, weight,
closed and skull sutures fuse (Alamo‐Junquera et al., 2014; Scharf, and head circumference) and four constructs for evaluating growth
Stroustrup, Conaway, & DeBoer, 2015). Early growth has implications (birthweight, individual measures at specific time points, summative
for adult health (Barker, 2006; DeBoer et al., 2012). Birthweight is growth, and rate of growth). We sought not to examine all proximal
one measure of a child's endowment from the prenatal period; this determinants of development, but instead to determine which compo-
may be reflective of maternal nutrition, illness, or stress and genetic nents of growth best correlate, to inform future studies considering
factors influencing fetal development (Binkin, Yip, Fleshood, & anthropometric outcomes when direct cognitive assessment is not
Trowbridge, 1988; Patrick et al., 2005). Next, growth estimates can possible.
SCHARF ET AL. bs_bs_banner
3 of 11
and weighed to the nearest 10 g (0.01 kg). Scales (Seca 354 or Detecto birth (proxy for birthweight).
8440) were calibrated weekly using standard weights. Length was 2. Attained size: WAZ, LAZ, and HCZ at monthly cross sections from
measured using a measuring board. Length was measured using a 0 to 2 years.
measuring board (Seca 417, ShorrBoard, UNICEF 0114500, or Seca 3. Summative growth: Area between child's growth curve and the
217). Hair clips, socks, and shoes were removed, and two field workers WHO growth curve; areas calculated from birth to 24 months
recorded the lengths to the nearest 0.1 cm. Head circumference was and in 6‐month intervals as
measured using nonstretch Teflon measuring tape (Seca 212) by
positioning the tape just above the eyebrows, above the ears, and 24
∑ ðWAZ i −WAZi−1 Þ*ðti −ti−1 Þ;
around the biggest part of the back of the head to the nearest 0.1 cm. i¼0
not consider where the child falls in relation to the average growth anthropometry and Bayley scores measured at 24 months. Participants
curve. A high growth rate may indicate strong growth in well‐ without 24‐month Bayley scores (compared to those with 24‐month
nourished children as well as catch‐up growth among malnourished Bayley measures) had similar gender proportions (51% male vs. 48%
children. Cumulative growth combines both the magnitude and rate male, χ2 p value = .11) but were more likely to be from rural settings
components of growth. Because each construct is calculated with (52% vs. 46%, χ2 p value = .02) and had higher enrolment weight
different units, we standardized the growth constructs into sample‐ (3.19 kg vs. 3.07 kg, t test p value < .0001). Enrolment weights (taken
Xi −μ in the first 2 weeks of life) of the study sample had a mean of
based z‐scores ( zi ¼ , where Xi is the child's growth construct
σ 3.07 kg and ranged by research site, from a mean of 2.77 kg in the
value, μ is the total sample mean of the growth construct, and σ is
Bangladesh research site to 3.48 kg in the Brazil site. Ninety‐seven
the standard deviation of the growth construct) to compare effect
percent of children in the analysis had birthweights in the WHO
sizes across constructs.
normal range (2,000–5,000 g) at study start (Figure S2). Over the 2‐
We used multivariable linear regression to estimate the associa-
year study period, there was variation among the research sites with
tion between each construct of WAZ, LAZ, and HCZ with separate
regards to height, weight, and head circumference achieved; for
regression models at different ages with cognitive outcome at
example, head circumference z‐scores ranged from −2.01 in the India
24 months, adjusting for research site and enrolment weight.
site to 0.20 in the South Africa site.
These analyses were completed to specifically examine the non-
causal association between anthropometry and early childhood devel-
opment to assess the ability of anthropometry to serve as convenient 3.2 | Enrolment weight and child development
and field‐ready proxy for child development by 24 months where col- We first evaluated enrolment weight as a predictor of the cognitive
lection is limited to anthropometric observations, for example, cross‐ subscale at 24 months. Figure 1 shows the adjusted mean Bayley
sectional survey data. Therefore, other potential contributors to cogni- cognitive score for each enrolment WAZ category, adjusted for study
tive outcomes in children (socio‐economic status, early language expo- site. Children born with a birthweight z‐score more than two standard
sure, developmental stimulation, parental education or income, play deviations below the mean had the lowest Bayley scores at
opportunities, poor diet, poverty, infection, etc.) were not included as 24 months.
covariates in the present analysis, but analyses were examined and
commented upon in Section 4.
3.3 | Monthly anthropometry measures and
cognitive development
3 | RESULTS
When assessing the associations between anthropometry measure-
ments taken at each month separately with Bayley scores, adjusting
3.1 | Cohort characteristics for birthweight and research site, we found that the score on the
We examined data from 1,210 children across eight research sites in cognitive subtest was significantly related to weight, length, and head
the MAL‐ED study. There was wide variation in achieved growth circumference measurements. The regression analysis estimates for
between the sites (Table 1). Out of 1,887 newborn infants who had each measurement are plotted by month as related to cognition in
enrolment weights to be included in the study and were eligible for Figure 2. Overall, the growth constructs in the second year of life were
analysis, we examined data from 1,210 children who had both more positively correlated with cognitive scores than constructs in the
TABLE 1 Sample population, enrolment weight, and 24‐month anthropometry z‐scores for each research site
Enrolment Enrolment Enrolment 24‐month 24‐month 24‐month
Research sitea N % female weight (kg) length (cm) HC (cm) LAZ WAZ HCZ
All 1210c 48.7 3.07 ± 0.51 49.2 + 2.2 34.2 + 1.6 −1.56 ± 1.14 −1.05 ± 1.17 −1.04 ± 1.23
Bangladesh (Dhaka) 188 50.5 2.77 ± 0.40 48.3 + 2.0 33.6 + 1.4 −2.04 ± 0.95 −1.63 ± 0.97 −1.87 ± 0.96
Brazil (Fortaleza) 140 42.1 3.48 ± 0.53 49.8 + 2.1 35.2 + 1.4 −0.02 ± 1.10 0.34 ± 1.22 NAb
India (Vellore) 227 53.7 2.93 ± 0.46 49.3 + 2.1 33.4 + 1.3 −1.92 ± 0.97 −1.65 ± 0.94 −2.01 ± 0.78
Nepal (Bhaktapur)d 224 46.4 3.15 ± 0.46 50.1 + 2.1 34.2 + 1.2 −1.34 ± 0.92 −0.93 ± 0.90 −0.95 ± 0.88
Peru (Loreto) 197 45.7 3.08 ± 0.44 48.6 + 1.9 33.7 + 1.4 −1.89 ± 0.87 −0.79 ± 0.91 −0.58 ± 0.96
Pakistan (Naushahro Feroze) 245 50.6 2.90 ± 0.49 48.7 + 2.4 33.9 + 1.5 NA −1.65 ± 0.99 NA
South Africa (Venda) 213 48.8 3.31 ± 0.47 49.6 ± 1.9 35.5 + 1.3 −1.71 ± 1.04 −0.51 ± 0.98 0.20 ± 1.02
Tanzania (Haydom)d 254 51.3 3.38 ± 0.47 48.9 + 2.4 35.1 + 1.4 −2.67 ± 1.02 −1.33 ± 1.01 −0.77 ± 0.98
Note. HC = head circumference; LAZ = length‐for‐age z‐score; WAZ = weight‐for‐age z‐score; HCZ = head‐circumference‐for‐age z‐score.
a
Data represented as mean ± SD.
b
NA: Data not of sufficient quality to include in the study.
c
Total N used in the analyses (children with anthropometry and Bayley scores to 24 months) excludes participants from the Tanzania and Nepal sites as the
Bayley data were not collected with sufficient reliability to be included in the analyses.
d
Bayley cognitive data not included in analyses.
SCHARF ET AL. bs_bs_banner
5 of 11
(a) WAZ
0.75
0.25
0.00
-0.25
-0.50 6 12 18 24
Age (months)
(b)
0.75 LAZ
0.25
FIGURE 1 Mean 24‐month Bayley cognitive score by category of
enrolment weight z‐score (adjusted by site). Overall model linear 0.00
trend test p = 0.02
-0.25
-0.50 6 12 18 24
first year of life. Of note, head circumference was more strongly
Age (months)
associated with cognitive skills than LAZ or WAZ; for an average
increase of 1 HCZ, Bayley cognitive score (range of 0 to 15) increased (c) HCZ
0.75
1 z-score increase in HCZ
0.37 points.
cognitive score per
Change in 24-mo
0.50
3.4 Summative anthropometry measures and child
|
0.25
development
0.00
We next assessed how summative growth area from the growth curve
was related to Bayley cognitive scores (Figure 3). Summative linear -0.25
growth was not linked to 24‐month Bayley scores at any of the time
intervals, whereas summative weight area was only significantly -0.50 6 12 18 24
associated with 24‐month Bayley scores from 18 to 24 months. Age (months)
Summative head circumference from 12 to 18 and 18 to 24 months FIGURE 2 Associations between attained growth z‐scores from 1 to
was significantly related to cognitive skills. Therefore, taking into 24 months of age with Bayley Cognitive Score at 24 months of age.
account summative growth over time, head circumference was again Mean difference (95% confidence interval) in cognitive score at
a stronger predictor of cognitive scores than weight and length and 24 months associated with an increase in attained growth z‐score by 1
at each month (analysed in separate regressions) from 1 to 24. (a)
later total growth (the 18‐ to 24‐month period in particular) was most
Weight‐for‐age z‐score (WAZ). (b) Length‐for‐age z‐score (LAZ). (c)
closely linked (Figure 3). Head‐circumference‐for‐age z‐score (HCZ). Regressions are adjusted
for research site and enrolment weight
3.5 | Rate of growth and child development weight, length, or head circumference [assessed separately] vs.
Finally, we examined slope, or rate of growth, over time. Growth others). Children born in low anthropometry categories did not
rates over 6‐month periods were most related to Bayley cognitive exhibit significantly different relationship between rate of growth
score between 6 and 18 months (Figure 4). Growth rate in length and 24‐month Bayley scores.
6 to 12 months, and head circumference 6 to 12 and 12 to Our analyses focused on whether measures of childhood anthro-
18 months were positively related to cognitive scores. Rapid rate pometry were associated with later measures of Bayley scores of
of change for weight, length, and especially head circumference at cognition. However, in a separate sensitivity analysis, we assessed
18 to 24 months was related to lower scores. As a sensitivity anal- whether these measures remained associated after adjusting for
ysis, we assessed whether the relationship between growth rate the presence of confounding factors, including maternal reasoning
(slope) and Bayley score differed when participants were stratified abilities (score on the Raven Combined Progressive Matrices),
by enrolment anthropometry measure category (i.e., children who maternal education (years of schooling), and child stimulation (score
were smaller than two standard deviations below the mean for on Home Observation of the Measured Environment). Inclusion of
6 of 11 bs_bs_banner
SCHARF ET AL.
0.5
0.5
0.0
0.0
-0.5
-0.5 -1.0
6
12
18
24
12
4
6
0-
0-
-1
-2
6-
-
12
18
6-
12
18
Time window Time window
(months) (months)
(b)
(b) LAZ Area on Cognitive LAZ Slope
1.0
score per 1 z-score
0.5
0.5
0.0
0.0 -0.5
-1.0
-0.5
12
4
6
0-
-1
-2
24
12
6
18
6-
0-
12
18
-
-
6-
18
12
Time window
Time window (months)
(months) (c)
HCZ Slope
score per 1 z-score increase in
1.0
Change in 24-mo cognitive
increase in summative HCZ
Change in 24-mo cognitive
1.0
0.5
score per 1 z-score
0.5 0.0
-0.5
0.0
-1.0
12
4
6
-0.5
0-
-1
-2
6-
12
18
6
12
4
-1
-2
0-
6-
Time window
12
18
gestational age. However, children who required prolonged hospitali- the cognitive assessments for the Naushahro Feroze, Pakistan, research
zation after birth or were born <1,500 g were excluded from the study. site. BK oversaw the cognitive assessments for the Vellore, India,
A total of >97% of participants were in the normal range for research site. RS oversaw the cognitive assessments for the Bhaktapur,
birthweight by WHO; thus, it does not appear that a significant Nepal, research site. AOV oversaw the cognitive assessments for the
number of these children were born prematurely. Iquitos, Peru, research site. MRC gave input to the analyses, helped to
Following developmental skills longer than 24 months will be run the analyses, and gave critical feedback to the manuscript. LEMK
informative as later assessments have better established correlations and LP give leadership to the cognitive committee for the MAL‐ED
with adult metrics; growth and developmental assessment data until study, conceptualized the analyses, and gave critical input. RLG
age 5 are currently being collected and will be the subject of future contributed to the conceptual framework, critically read, and revised
analyses. Our study team is gathering data on cognitive, language, the manuscript. All authors critically read and approved the manuscript.
and executive function skills in these children at age 5 and examining
the many predictors of school readiness skills, including growth. ORCID
Children grow most quickly in the first 3 years of life and thus may Rebecca J. Scharf http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3381-5594
be the most vulnerable to influences, both positive and negative, on
growth as well as brain development and cognition. These data may RE FE RE NC ES
have relevance for the planning and interpretation of studies with a Alamo‐Junquera, D., Sunyer, J., Iniguez, C., Ballester, F., Garcia‐Esteban, R.,
goal of assessing or improving childhood potential. This highlights the Forns, J., … Julvez, J. (2014). Prenatal head growth and child neuropsy-
chological development at age 14 months. American Journal of
critical nature of early childhood and the importance of promoting
Obstetrics and Gynecology, 212(5), 661. e1‐11.
health in order to foster growth and development (Chan, Lake, &
American Academy of Pediatrics (2015) Bright futures: Prevention and
Hansen, 2016; Lo, Das, & Horton, 2016). health promotion for infants, children, adolescents, and their families.
The first 2 years of life represent tremendous growth, both In: Bright futures. (ed American Academy of Pediatrics). American
physically and cognitively. We found that measurements of head Academy of Pediatrics, Elk Grove, IL.
circumference were most predictive of cognitive scores at age 2. Barker, D. J. P. (2006). Adult consequences of fetal growth restriction.
Clinical Obstetrics and Gynecology, 49, 270–283.
Several steps may be helpful in monitoring childhood development:
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. San
(a) direct measures of developmental progress because measures of Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
growth or attained size are not strong proxy measures for early child
Belfort, M. B., Rifas‐Shiman, S. L., Sullivan, T., Collins, C. T., McPhee, A. J., Ryan,
development, (b) active growth surveillance in childhood, and (c) P., … Makrides, M. (2011). Infant growth before and after term: Effects
including head circumference in surveillance measurements in settings on neurodevelopment in preterm infants. Pediatrics, 128, e899–e906.
of malnutrition and enteric disease. Interdisciplinary studies of nutri- Berkman, D. S., Lescano, A. G., Gilman, R. H., Lopez, S., & Black, M. M.
(2002). Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection
tion, morbidity, and child development are best served by direct
during infancy on cognition in late childhood: A follow‐up study. Lancet,
assessment of development using accepted tools when possible. 359, 564–571.
Following anthropometry provides another opportunity to monitor Bhutta, Z. A., Das, J. K., Rizvi, A., Gaffey, M. F., Walker, N., Horton, S., …
child health and development and identify those children who may the Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Evidence‐based
be at risk for delays. This may allow for health workers or those in interventions for improvement of maternal and child nutrition: What
can be done and at what cost? Lancet, 382, 452–477.
education to monitor children with delayed growth more closely.
Binkin, N. J., Yip, R., Fleshood, L., & Trowbridge, F. L. (1988). Birth weight
and childhood growth. Pediatrics, 82, 828–834.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Black, M. M., Walker, S. P., Fernald, L. C., Andersen, C. T., DiGirolamo, A. M.,
The authors thank the families and children, as well as the staff, of the Lu, C., … Lancet Early Childhood Development Series Steering
Committee. (2016). Early childhood development coming of age:
MAL‐ED Network for their important contributions and tireless work Science through the life course. Lancet, 389, 77–90.
and Drs. Crystal Patil, Ben McCormick, and Laura Caulfield for critical Black, R. E., Alderman, H., Bhutta, Z. A., Gillespie, S., Haddad, L., Horton, S.,
review of the manuscript. … Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Maternal and child
nutrition: Building momentum for impact. Lancet, 382, 372–375.
CONF LICT S OF INT ER E ST Black, R. E., Victora, C. G., Walker, S. P., Bhutta, Z. A., Christian, P., de Onis,
M., … Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Maternal and
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. child undernutrition and overweight in low‐income and middle‐income
countries. Lancet, 382, 427–451.
CONT R IBUT IONS Borghi, E., de Onis, M., Garza, C., Van den Broeek, J., Frongillo, E. A.,
Grummer‐Strawn, L., … Martines, J. C. (2006). Construction of the
RJS, ETR, JAPM, and MDD conceptualized and ran the analyses, and World Health Organization child growth standards: Selection of
drafted the manuscript. ES and EM oversaw the data collection, growth methods for attained growth curves. Statistics in Medicine, 25, 247–265.
measurements, and cognitive assessments for the Haydom, Tanzania, Bornstein, M. H., Britto, P. R., Nonoyama‐Tarumi, Y., Ota, Y., Petrovic, O., &
Putnick, D. L. (2012). Child development in developing countries:
research site. AM oversaw the data collection and cognitive assessments
Introduction and methods. Child Development, 83, 16–31.
for the Venda, South Africa, research site. CA oversaw the data collec-
Chan, M., Lake, A., & Hansen, K. (2016). The early years: Silent emergency
tion, growth measurements, and cognitive assessments for the Fortaleza, or unique opportunity? Lancet, 389, 11–13.
Brazil, research site. FT oversaw the data collection and cognitive Chang, S. M., Walker, S. P., Grantham‐McGregor, S., & Powell, C. A. (2002).
assessments for the Dhaka, Bangladesh, research site. MR oversaw Early childhood stunting and later behaviour and school achievement.
SCHARF ET AL. bs_bs_banner
9 of 11
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 43, between enteric pathogens, malnutrition, gut physiology, physical
775–783. growth, cognitive development, and immune responses in infants and
Checkley, W., Buckley, G., Gilman, R. H., Assis, A. M. O., Guerrant, R. L., children up to 2 years of age in resource‐poor environments. Clinical
Morris, S. S., … Childhood Malnutrition and Infection Network. (2008). Infectious Diseases, 59, S193–S206.
Multi‐country analysis of the effects of diarrhoea on childhood Miller, L. C., Joshi, N., Lohani, M., Singh, R., Bhatta, N., Rogers, B., … Patrick,
stunting. International Journal of Epidemiology, 37, 816–830. W. (2016). Head growth of undernourished children in rural Nepal:
Chin‐Lun Hung, G., Hahn, J., Alamiri, B., Buka, S. L., Goldstein, J. M., Laird, Association with demographics, health and diet. Paediatrics and Interna-
N., … Gilman, S. E. (2015). Socioeconomic disadvantage and neural tional Child Health, 36, 91–101.
development from infancy through early childhood. International Jour-
Murray‐Kolb, L. E., Rasmussen, Z. A., Scharf, R. J., Rasheed, M. A., Svensen,
nal of Epidemiology, 44, 1889–1899.
E., Seidman, J. C., … MAL‐ED Network Investigators. (2014). The MAL‐
Cornelio‐Nieto, J. O. (2007). The effects of protein‐energy malnutrition ED cohort study: Methods and lessons learned when assessing early
on the central nervous system in children. Revista de Neurologia, child development and caregiving mediators in infants and young
44(Suppl 2), S71–S74. children in 8 low‐ and middle‐income countries. Clinical Infectious
Daelmans, B., Darmstadt, G. L., Lombardi, J., Black, M. M., Britto, P. R., Lye, Diseases, 59, S261–S272.
S., … Lancet Early Childhood Development Series Steering Committee. de Onis, M., Blossner, M., Borghi, E., Morris, R., & Frongillo, E. A. (2004).
(2016). Early childhood development: The foundation of sustainable Methodology for estimating regional and global trends of child
development. Lancet, 389, 9–11. malnutrition. International Journal of Epidemiology, 33, 1260–1270.
DeBoer, M. D., Lima, A. A. M., Oria, R. B., Scharf, R. J., Moore, S. R., Luna,
de Onis, M., Onyango, A., Borghi, E., Siyam, A., Blössner, M., Lutter, C., …
M. A., … Guerrant, R. L. (2012). Early childhood growth failure and the
WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. (2012). Worldwide
developmental origins of adult disease: Do enteric infections and mal-
implementation of the WHO child growth standards. Public Health
nutrition increase risk for the metabolic syndrome? Nutrition Reviews,
Nutrition, 15, 1603–1610.
70, 642–653.
Dewey, K. G., & Adu‐Afarwuah, S. (2008). Systematic review of the efficacy Patrick, P. D., Oria, R. B., Madhavan, V., Pinkerton, R. C., Lorntz, B., Lima, A.
and effectiveness of complementary feeding interventions in develop- A. M., … Guerrant, R. L. (2005). Limitations in verbal fluency following
ing countries. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 4, 24–85. heavy burdens of early childhood diarrhea in Brazilian shantytown chil-
dren. Child Neuropsychology, 11, 233–244.
Eichorn, D. H., & Bayley, N. (1962). Growth in head circumference from
birth through young adulthood. Child Development, 33, 257–271. Platts‐Mills, J. A., Babji, S., Bodhidatta, L., Gratz, J., Haque, R., Havt, A., …
MAL‐ED Network Investigators. (2015). Pathogen‐specific burdens of
Fagan, J. F., & McGrath, S. K. (1981). Infant recognition memory and later
community diarrhoea in developing countries: A multisite birth cohort
intelligence. Intelligence, 5, 121–130.
study (MAL‐ED). The Lancet Global Health, 3, E564–E575.
Gandhi, M., Ashorn, P., Maleta, K., Teivaanmaki, T., Duan, X. L., & Cheung,
Y. B. (2011). Height gain during early childhood is an important predic- Prendergast, A. J., & Humphrey, J. H. (2014). The stunting syndrome in devel-
tor of schooling and mathematics ability outcomes. Acta Paediatrica, oping countries. Paediatrics and International Child Health, 34, 250–265.
100, 1113–1118. Sansavini, A., Pentimonti, J., Justice, L., Guarini, A., Savini, S., Alessandroni,
Georgieff, M. K. (2007). Nutrition and the developing brain: nutrient prior- R., … Faldella, G. (2014). Language, motor and cognitive development of
ities and measurement. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 85, extremely preterm children: Modeling individual growth trajectories
614S–620S. over the first three years of life. Journal of Communication Disorders,
49, 55–68.
Grantham‐McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L.,
Strupp, B., … the International Child Development Steering Group. Scharf, R. J., Stroustrup, A., Conaway, M. R., & DeBoer, M. D. (2015). Growth
(2007). Child development in developing countries 1—Developmental and development in children born very low birthweight. Archives of
potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 101, F433–F438.
369, 60–70.
Shonkoff, J., & Phillips, D. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The
Guerrant, R. L., Deboer, M. D., Moore, S. R., Scharf, R. J., & Lima, A. A. M. science of early childhood development. Washington DC: The National
(2013). The impoverished gut—A triple burden of diarrhoea, stunting Academies Press.
and chronic disease. Nature reviews Gastroenterology & Hepatology, 10,
220–229. Shonkoff, J. P. (2011). Protecting brains, not simply stimulating minds.
Science, 333, 982–983.
Guerrant, R. L., Oria, R. B., Moore, S. R., Oria, M. O. B., & Lima, A. A. M.
(2008). Malnutrition as an enteric infectious disease with long‐term Streimish, I. G., Ehrenkranz, R. A., Allred, E. N., O'Shea, T. M., Kuban, K. C. K.,
effects on child development. Nutrition Reviews, 66, 487–505. Paneth, N., … ELGAN Study Investigators. (2012). Birth weight‐ and
Humphrey, J. H., Jones, A. D., Manges, A., Mangwadu, G., Maluccio, J. A., fetal weight‐growth restriction: Impact on neurodevelopment. Early
Mbuya, M. N. N., … Sanitation Hygiene Infant Nutrition Efficacy Human Development, 88, 765–771.
(SHINE) Trial Team. (2015). The Sanitation Hygiene Infant Nutrition Subramanian, S. V., Ozaltin, E., & Finlay, J. E. (2011). Height of nations: A
Efficacy (SHINE) trial: Rationale, design, and methods. Clinical Infectious socioeconomic analysis of cohort differences and patterns among
Diseases, 61, S685–S702. women in 54 low‐ to middle‐income countries. PLoS One, 6, e18962.
Ivanovic, D. M., Leiva, B. P., Perez, H. T., Olivares, M. G., Diaz, N. S., Urrutia,
Tarleton, J. L., Haque, R., Mondal, D., Shu, J. F., Farr, B. M., & Petri, W. A.
M. S. C., … Larraín, C. G. (2004). Head size and intelligence, learning,
(2006). Cognitive effects of diarrhea, malnutrition, and Entamoeba
nutritional status and brain development: Head, IQ, learning, nutrition
histolytica infection on school age children in Dhaka, Bangladesh. Amer-
and brain. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1118–1131.
ican Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 74, 475–481.
Lo, S., Das, P., & Horton, R. (2016). A good start in life will ensure a sustain-
Thompson, R. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Developmental science and the
able future for all. Lancet, 389, 8–9.
media—Early brain development. American Psychologist, 56, 5–15.
Lu, C., Black, M. M., & Richter, L. M. (2016). Risk of poor development in
young children in low‐income and middle‐income countries: An estima- Wright, C. M., & Emond, A. (2015). Head growth and neurocognitive
tion and analysis at the global, regional, and country level. The Lancet outcomes. Pediatrics, 135, E1393–E1398.
Global Health, 4, e916–e922. Zhang, Z. Y., McArdle, J. J., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2012). Growth rate models:
MAL‐ED Network Investigators. (2014). The MAL‐ED study: A multina- Emphasizing growth rate analysis through growth curve modeling.
tional and multidisciplinary approach to understand the relationship Journal of Applied Statistics, 39, 1241–1262.
10 of 11 bs_bs_banner
SCHARF ET AL.
SUPPORTING I NFORMATION
Carreon, Danny: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Additional Supporting Information may be found online in the
Charu, Vivek: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
supporting information tab for this article.
Dabo, Leyfou: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Doan, Viyada: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Graham, Jhanelle: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
How to cite this article: Scharf RJ, Rogawski ET, Murray‐Kolb Hoest, Christel: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
LE, et al. Early childhood growth and cognitive outcomes: Find- Knobler, Stacey: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
ings from the MAL‐ED study. Matern Child Nutr. 2018;14: Lang, Dennis: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
e12584. https://doi.org/10.1111/mcn.12584 McCormick, Benjamin: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
McGrath, Monica: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Miller, Mark: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Mohale, Archana: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
APPENDIX A
Nayyar, Gaurvika: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
M A L ‐ E D I N V E S TI G A T O R S Psaki, Stephanie: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Acosta, Angel Mendez: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Rasmussen, Zeba: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Chavez, Cesar Banda: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Richard, Stephanie: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Flores, Julian Torres: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Seidman, Jessica: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Olotegui, Maribel Paredes: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Wang, Vivian: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Pinedo, Silvia Rengifo: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Blank, Rebecca: FNIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Trigoso, Dixner Rengifo: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Gottlieb, Michael: FNIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Vasquez, Angel Orbe: A.B. PRISMA, Iquitos, Peru; Tountas, Karen: FNIH, Bethesda, MD, USA;
Ahmed, Imran: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Amour, Caroline: Haydom Lutheran Hospital, Haydom,
Pakistan; Tanzania;
Alam, Didar: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Mduma, Estomih: Haydom Lutheran Hospital, Haydom,
Pakistan; Tanzania;
Ali, Asad: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Ahmed, Tahmeed: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Pakistan; Ahmed, A. M. Shamsir: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Bhutta, Zulfiqar A.: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Dinesh, Mondol: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Pakistan; Tofail, Fahmida: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Qureshi, Shahida: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Haque, Rashidul: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Pakistan; Hossain, Iqbal: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Shakoor', Sadia: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Islam, Munirul: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Pakistan; Mahfuz, Mustafa: ICDDR‐B, Dhaka, Bangladesh;
Soofi, Sajid: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Chandyo, Ram Krishna: IOM, Tribuhvan University, Kathmandu,
Pakistan; Nepal;
Turab, Ali: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Shrestha, Prakash Sunder: IOM, Tribuhvan University, Kathmandu,
Pakistan; Nepal;
Yousafzai, Aisha K.: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Shrestha, Rita: IOM, Tribuhvan University, Kathmandu,
Pakistan; Nepal;
Zaidi, Anita K. M.: Aga Khan University, Naushahro Feroze, Ulak, Manjeswori: IOM, Tribuhvan University, Kathmandu,
Pakistan; Nepal;
Bodhidatta, Ladaporn: AFRIMS, Bangkok, Thailand; Black, Robert: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Mason, Carl J.: AFRIMS, Bangkok, Thailand; Caulfield, Laura: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Babji, Sudhir: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Checkley, William: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Bose, Anuradha: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Chen, Ping: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
John, Sushil: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Kosek, Margaret: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Kang, Gagandeep: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Lee, Gwenyth: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Kurien, Beena: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Yori, Pablo Peñataro: JHU, Baltimore, MD, USA;
Muliyil, Jayaprakash: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Murray‐Kolb, Laura: Pennsylvania State University, Univer-
Raghava, Mohan Venkata: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; sity Park, PA, USA;
Ramachandran, Anup: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; Schaefer, Barbara: Pennsylvania State University, Univer-
Rose, Anuradha: Christian Medical College, Vellore, India; sity Park, PA, USA;
Pan, William: Duke University, Durham, NC, USA; Pendergast, Laura: Temple University, Philadelphia, PA,
Ambikapathi, Ramya: FIC, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA; USA;
SCHARF ET AL. bs_bs_banner
11 of 11
Abreu, Claudia: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Svensen, Erling: University of Bergen, Norway; Haydom
Brazil; Lutheran Hospital, Haydom, Tanzania;
Bindá, Alexandre: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Tor, Strand: University of Bergen, Norway;
Brazil; Patil, Crystal: University of Illinois, Urbana‐Cham-
Costa, Hilda: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, paign, IL, USA;
Brazil; Bessong, Pascal: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Di Moura, Alessandra: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Mahopo, Cloupas: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Filho, Jose Quirino: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Mapula, Angelina: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Leite, Álvaro: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Nesamvuni, Cebisa: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Lima, Aldo: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Nyathi, Emanuel: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Lima, Noelia: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Samie, Amidou: University of Venda, Thohoyandou,
Lima, Ila: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, South Africa;
Brazil; Barrett, Leah: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Maciel, Bruna: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Gratz, Jean: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Brazil; Guerrant, Richard: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Moraes, Milena: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Houpt, Eric: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Brazil; Olmsted, Liz: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Mota, Francisco: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Petri, William: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Brazil; Platts‐Mills, James: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Oria, Reinaldo: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Scharf, Rebecca: UVA, Charlottesville, VA, USA;
Brazil; Shrestha, Binob: Walter Reed/AFRIMS Research Unit,
Quetz, Josiane: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Kathmandu, Nepal;
Brazil; Shrestha, Sanjaya Kumar: Walter Reed/AFRIMS Research Unit,
Soares, Alberto: Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Kathmandu, Nepal.
Brazil;