Tymoczko Tonality - An Owner's Manual 2023
Tymoczko Tonality - An Owner's Manual 2023
Tymoczko Tonality - An Owner's Manual 2023
Metric Manipulations in Haydn and Mozart: Chamber Music for Strings, 1787-1791, Danuta Mirka
Songs in Motion: Rhythm and Meter in the German Lied, Yonatan Malin
A Geometry of Music: Harmony and Counterpoint in the Extended Common Practice, Dmitri
Tymoczko
In the Process of Becoming: Analytic and Philosophical Perspectives on Form in Early Nineteenth-
Century Music, Janet Schmalfeldt
Audacious Euphony: Chromatic Harmony and the Triad's Second Nature, Richard Cohn
Beating Time and Measuring Music in the Early Modern Era, Roger Mathew Grant
Enacting Musical Time: The Bodily Experience of New Music, Mariusz Kozak
Hearing Homophony: Tonal Expectation at the Turn of the Seventeenth Century, Megan Kaes Long
A Blaze of Light in Every Word: Analyzing the Popular Singing Voice, Victoria Malewy
Sweet Thing: The History and Musical Structure of a Shared American Vernacular Form, Nicholas
Stoia
Hypermetric Manipulations in Haydn and Mozart: Chamber Music for Strings, 1787-1791, Danuta
Mirka
DMITRI TYMOCZKO
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s
objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a
registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and certain other countries.
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, United States of America.
© Oxford University Press 2023
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or
transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford
University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, by license, or under terms agreed with the
appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of
the above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above.
You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any
acquirer.
Library of Congress Control Number: 2022042508
ISBN 978–0–19–757710–3
eISBN 978–0–19–757712–7
DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780197577103.001.0001
to anyone who cares enough
to make it all the way through
Contents
2. Rock Logic
1. A melodic principle
2. A harmonic principle
3. A first loop family
4. Two more families
5. Shepard-tone passacaglias
6. Minor triads and other trichords
7. A fourth family
8. Other modalities
9. Function and retrofunction
10. Continuity or reinvention?
4. Repetition
1. Repetition reimagined
2. Repeating contrapuntal patterns
3. The geometry of two-voice sequences
4. Three voices and the circle of triads
5. Three voices arranged 2 + 1
6. Four voices
7. Contrary-motion sequences
8. Melodic sequences and near sequences
9. Near sequences
10. Sequences as reductional targets
5. Nonharmonic Tones
1. The first practice and the SNAP system
2. Schoenberg’s critique
3. Monteverdi’s “Ohimè”
4. The standardized second practice
5. A loophole
6. After nonharmonicity
7. Functional Progressions
1. A theory of harmonic cycles
2. A more principled view
3. Rameau and Bach
4. Functional melody, functional harmony
5. Fauxbourdon and linear idioms
6. Sequences
7. Bach the dualist
8. Modulation
1. Two models of key distance
2. Enharmonicism and loops in scale space
3. Minor keys
4. Modulatory schemas
5. Up and down the ladder
6. Modal homogenization and scalar voice leading
7. Generalized set theory
9. Heterogeneous Hierarchy
1. Strategy and reduction
2. Two models of the phrase
3. Chopin and the Prime Directive
4. An expanded vocabulary of reductional targets
5. Simple harmonic hierarchy
6. The four-part phrase
7. Grouping, melody, harmony
8. Beyond the phrase: hierarchy at the level of the piece
Conclusion
Appendix 1: Fundamentals
Appendix 2: Deriving the Spiral Diagrams
Appendix 3: Sequence and Transformation
Appendix 4: Corpus Analysis, Statistics, and Grammar
Terms, Symbols, and Abbreviations
Bibliography
Index
Preface and Acknowledgments
This book grew like a city, each gleaming draft built over the ruins of a
previous version. The earliest stratum is pedagogical. After many years of
teaching tonal harmony, I had accumulated an unstructured miscellany of
ideas, techniques, and analyses. Some were the product of computational
corpus study—a practice I adopted around 2000, first using MIDI files and
Max/MSP, and then using Python, music21, and actual scores. My plan was
to use this material to put pressure on what I took to be theoretical
oversimplification. I wanted to demonstrate, against Schenkerian
orthodoxy, that harmonies in baroque and classical music followed a largely
nonhierarchical chord-to-chord grammar. I wanted to show that upper-voice
configurations in four-voice counterpoint obeyed a simple grammar of hand
positions. I wanted to argue that nonharmonic tones were not “merely
decorative,” and to trace the “irreducible seventh” back to the Renaissance.
I wanted to identify protofunctional tendencies in sixteenth-century music.
The goal was to provide more detailed information than could be found in
standard textbooks, augmented by conceptual heuristics I used in the
classroom.
This led to the aesthetic or critical layer. For the more I dove into the
grammatical minutiae, the more I began to question the value of
disinterested description: by avoiding issues of musical relevance, I was
tacitly accepting assumptions that should instead be put into question. This
felt increasingly problematic, pedagogically, aesthetically, and even
morally. Why should twenty-first-century musicians care about the detailed
grammar of sixteenth- or eighteenth-century practice? Who needs a truly
accurate account of largely extinct functional-harmonic conventions? What
is the aesthetic value of recapitulation? Though I love music, and consider it
intrinsically worthy of study, I felt increasingly strongly that aesthetic
significance should be a component of grammatical inquiry. Music theory
without philosophy is empty.
The final, theoretical layer introduced the spiral diagrams that were to
play a central role in the completed book. Originally designed as
pedagogical simplifications, I gradually came to realize that they were
something more. For by freeing the mind from the burden of imagining
higher-dimensional voice-leading geometry, they left more energy for
understanding genuinely musical relationships, often in a way that
connected past and present. A major development here was my discovery
that Princeton graduate composers were interested in ideas I had taken to be
purely theoretical and historical. Their influence pushed me to connect
sequences to repetition and transformation more generally, to think about
how one might use complex motivic networks in contemporary contexts,
and to consider how pieces like the Waldstein might serve as models for
contemporary composition. More significantly, these connections led to the
idea of the quadruple hierarchy, one of the central themes in the manuscript
as it now stands. The result is that the oldest and newest sections of the
book are found side-by-side: for while I had long used the heuristics of
OUCH theory in the classroom (§3.6), I happened upon the general picture
of chords-as-scales only after I thought the book was essentially finished
(§3.7).
This last revision, so drastic and so late, raised the disturbing possibility
that I had ceased writing a book in any conventional sense: what had begun
as a journey had turned into a way of life, my manuscript evolving into a
repository for my latest thinking on all things music. Every time I learned
something new, or changed my mind, I updated the relevant file on my
computer. Having faced the prospect of spending the rest of my life
endlessly writing and rewriting the same words—along with the attendant
familial disapproval—I resolved to stop. The result is this snapshot of my
thinking as of the time of publication. Already I worry that readers will be
reluctant to climb so far out on my own little conceptual limb. And though I
have tried to be clear, I am aware that the material is extremely challenging.
Many people helped me along the way. Thirty-five years ago, John
Stewart sat next to me at the piano and showed me his version of OUCH
theory, planting seeds that were to bear fruit decades later. In a graduate
seminar at UC Berkeley, David Huron taught me about corpus study and
computational musical analysis. Richard Cohn introduced me to musical
geometry and provided valuable support when my thinking was in its early
stages. Clifton Callender, Noam Elkies, Rachel Hall, and Ian Quinn joined
me in working out the geometrical approach to voice leading.
More recently, Nick DiBerardino, Robert Andrew Scott, Jacob Shulman,
and Steve Taylor provided invaluable comments on drafts of the
manuscript. Robert’s incredibly detailed copyediting, of both music and
examples, went well beyond the call of duty. Jason Yust not only read the
manuscript, and shared portions of it with his class, but also served as a
useful sounding board for many half-baked thoughts. Mark Liberman was a
gracious consultant about linguistic and statistical matters. Michael Bruschi
and Christopher Peacocke read early drafts through chapter 5, the former
leading me to add preludes to each chapter. I have already mentioned the
graduate students in Music 527: Jenny Beck, Yihan Chen, Wei Dai, Nick
DiBerardino, Molly Herron, Natalie Miller, James Moore, Mauro Windholz,
and Zhoushu Ziporyn. The next semester, undergraduates in Music 306
user-tested portions of the book—leading to yet more rewriting. Others who
have helped me, by some combination of reading excerpts, answering
emails, and providing materials, include Giovanni Albini, Matthew Arndt,
Fernando Benadon, Ed Berlin, Eli Berman, William Caplin, David Cohen,
Michael Cuthbert, Harrison Davis, Donnacha Dennehy, Chris Douthitt,
Noam Elkies, David Feurzeig, Kenneth Forkert-Smith, Andrew Gelman,
Robert Gjerdingen, Aaron Grant, Dan Harrison, James Hepokoski, Julian
Hook, Nori Jacoby, Corey Kendrick, Megan Long, Rudresh Mahanthappa,
Nathan John Martin, Nathaniel Mitchell, Marco Buongiorno Nardelli, Drew
Nobile, Ali Rahmjoo, Peter Schubert, Ian Sewell, David Temperley, Yo
Tomita, and Dan Trueman. I am sure that this list is incomplete, and I
apologize to anyone I have missed.
My children, Lukas and Katya, allowed me to share their journey into
music, gently teaching me that I wasn’t as smart as I thought. Several of
their pieces turned into musical examples.
Last but not least comes my partner, emotional lodestone, and intellectual
hero Elisabeth Camp, who makes a cameo in a few footnotes. I am
embarrassed that neither of my books is good enough to be dedicated to her.
1
Implicit Musical Knowledge
1. Gesualdo’s trick
When confronted with a passage like this, I try to follow what I call the
Prime Directive:
whenever you find an interesting musical technique, try to generalize it to every possible
chordal and scalar environment.
In this case we have two voices moving in parallel with the third alternating
between prime and inverted forms of a sonority, arrayed in close position
and without voice crossings. Thus generalized, Gesualdo’s trick can be
found in many other places: Figure 1.1.3 shows Mozart using it in the first-
movement development of his C major piano sonata K.309, now with
descending minor rather than major thirds; Figure 1.1.4 shows one of its
several appearances in The Rite of Spring, with the parallel voices forming
major sevenths; Figure 1.1.5 shows Rudresh Mahanthappa using it in the
twenty-first century. Figure 1.1.6, meanwhile, applies the technique in
diatonic space whose unit of distance is the scale step. Here two voices are
separated by diatonic third, moving downward in parallel, with the last voice
alternately a fifth and sixth above the bass. What results is the descending-
third “Pachelbel” sequence (rechristened the “Romanesca” by Robert
Gjerdingen), a central idiom of both modal and functionally tonal
composition.6 From the standpoint of the Prime Directive, Gesualdo,
Stravinsky, Mahanthappa, and Pachelbel are all doing the same thing.
Figure 1.1.3. In mm. 73–76 of K.309, I, Mozart uses Gesualdo’s trick but with parallel minor thirds
rather than major.
Figure 1.1.4. The second measure of R114 in The Rite of Spring. Here the parallel voices form major
sevenths and articulate a melody; the third voice is alternately a tritone above the bottom voice and a
tritone below the top.
Figure 1.1.5. Rudresh Mahanthappa’s 2006 composition “The Decider” linearizes Gesualdo’s trick,
moving the perfect fourth in parallel, the middle voice alternately a major second below the top voice
and a major second above the bottom.
Figure 1.1.6. The Pachelbel sequence moves two voices in parallel diatonically, alternately adding the
third above and below.
The Prime Directive leads to my guiding idea, that Western music displays a
hierarchical structure in which the same basic procedures occur on multiple
levels simultaneously. This is the quadruple hierarchy of surface voices
moving inside chords that move inside more familiar scales that are
themselves moving through chromatic space—or as I will sometimes call it,
the collectional hierarchy, as the number of levels can vary. Figure 1.2.1
presents a clear example, the pianist’s right hand moving sequentially along
three voices that articulate the familiar “omnibus” schema.9 It is not
unreasonable to think that these triadic voices form a three-note scale, a
series of melodic positions available throughout the piano’s range, with
right-hand melody moving systematically along this scale very much like
“doe a deer” moves along the diatonic collection. Yet these three voices form
triads that would normally be considered chordal.
Figure 1.2.1. In mm. 38–41 of Beethoven’s Op. 54, II, voices move systematically along the top three
notes of the omnibus pattern, as if they formed a three-note scale.
Figure 1.2.3. Transposing the motive C–D–E along the C major triad. The top line interprets the
second note as being a scale step above the first; the bottom line interprets it as a scale step below the
second. These lead to different forms on G.
Figure 1.2.5. Here the voice moves according to a regular pattern (“up by third, down by step”) along
a series of melodic slots that are themselves moving. The example is drawn from the first movement
of Mozart’s G major piano sonata, K.283.
3. Philosophy
Another motivation for this book is my sense that music theory presents
philosophical challenges whose complexity is not always appreciated. As a
discipline that crosses the boundary between science and art, it requires both
musical intuition and methodological sophistication, yet it is extremely
difficult for a single human being to acquire expertise in both domains. The
result is a tendency to neglect some of the most perplexing issues in what is
an inherently challenging intellectual enterprise.
The most obvious issue is the divergence between eye and ear, between
structure apparent in musical scores and what can be heard by even well-
trained listeners. This is a familiar problem in twentieth-century contexts,
exemplified by worries about the audibility of twelve-tone and other
modernist modes of musical organization.16 But analogous problems
surround earlier music as well: for instance, both introspection and
experimental research suggest that long-term tonal closure (the return to the
tonic at the end of a long piece) is not particularly salient for most listeners,
yet the architecture of sonata form seems to presume its importance—the
purported function of the recapitulation being to resolve the “large-scale
dissonance” created by the appearance of the second theme in a “foreign”
key.17 Were earlier composers fixated on structures that even well-trained
listeners cannot hear? Did they wrongly expect their audience to experience
recapitulation as resolving a dissonance? Was classical music written for a
small elite with absolute pitch? These are uncomfortable questions, and
contemporary theorists are understandably reluctant to confront them head-
on, yet they are essential for understanding the aesthetic significance of
classical music.
The divergence between eye and ear undermines the common music-
theoretical inference from patterns-in-scores to listeners’ awareness of those
patterns. This what-you-see-is-what-you-hear principle is central to projects
as different as Fred Lerdahl’s a prioristic psychology, which assumes that
“experienced listeners” recreate in their minds a completely accurate copy of
the musical score, and Gjerdingen’s historicist “schema theory,” which
proposes score analysis as a tool for reconstructing the “situated
psychology” of earlier listeners.18 Thus Leonard Meyer writes, “the structure
of the affective response to a piece of music can be studied by examining the
music itself,” a statement echoed by Gjerdingen and Byros.19 We know this
statement is false in the case of long-term tonal closure, and we have reason
to worry that it fails in other cases as well: for instance, earlier composers
avoided parallel perfect intervals even in cases where they are very difficult
to hear (e.g., when tucked away in inner voices). Consequently, any
identification of what is seen with what is heard will underestimate the
complex mixture of aural and conceptual, of heard and imagined, at play in
the Western notated tradition.20
To be clear, I accept that musical styles are characterized, in part, by
recurring regularities and patterns, and that our ability to perceive such
patterns underwrites our ability to appreciate and understand music.
Furthermore, I agree that theory can enrich our perceptual experience by
sensitizing our ears to structure: having written this book, I regularly find
myself hearing the idioms and patterns I discuss, even when listening to
unfamiliar pieces without a score. But I reject the claim that structure-in-
scores is ipso facto available to listeners’ conscious reflection, or even that
an ideal listener should consciously try to track compositional technique.
Gjerdingen compares the listener to a figure-skating judge observing a series
of standard moves like the Lutz or triple axel. I will argue that music
perception is more complex than that: composers can navigate spaces of
musical possibility without being able to say exactly what they are doing just
as listeners can understand what they are hearing without being able to
describe their understanding in words. Music is written for audiences, not
judges.
One way of putting this point is that there is no fixed relationship between
compositional technique and communicative meaning, or how something is
made and what it says. In some circumstances, artists efface their methods,
deliberately aiming to create illusions. (Outside of music we have magic
tricks and special effects; inside it, we have Chopin’s arpeggiation patterns
or Ligeti’s “touches bloquées,” simple physical gestures creating outsized
impressions of complexity.) In other cases, technique is meant to be
apprehended only holistically and tacitly: for example, sound engineers and
record producers make countless decisions about reverb, panning, filtering,
microphone placement, and amplification, all in service of creating a
coherent and aesthetically pleasing sonic field, but with no expectation that
audiences will consciously track these decisions (“wow, nice use of a high-
pass filter”). In still other circumstances there is a closer connection between
technique and content, as when composers intend listeners to recognize the
return of a theme. But many practices are difficult to categorize: when it
comes to sonata form there is a genuine question about whether we should
expect an idealized listener to consciously track specific technical events
such as the “medial caesura” or whether we should think of those events as
tools for creating compelling energetic trajectories. Much the same might be
said for galant schemata, which some theorists might compare to words in a
language and others might consider technical tools for generating a certain
sort of musical flow.
If theory’s fundamental challenge is the gap between eye and ear, then the
runner-up is inferring aesthetic norms from musical evidence. This is an
issue that can arise even with perfect perception of every musical detail. For
suppose we notice that rock and classical music use different harmonic
progressions: should we conclude that rock musicians are deliberately
rejecting classical norms, or simply speaking a novel musical language?21
Or we observe Beethoven effacing standard sonata-form signposts such as
the medial caesura: should we read this as a deliberate rejection of the past
or as a new set of formal conventions that can be understood on its own?22
Do popular music’s pervasive accents on beats 2 and 4 represent a deviation
from a genuine psychological expectation that beat 1 should carry the
strongest accent, or an alternate metrical norm? Here it is all too easy to
project inappropriate expectations onto earlier styles, powering up the
hermeneutic engines when our oversimplifications fail.
Consider the nineteenth-century theorist Ebeneezer Prout’s approach to
one of the more vexed problems in historical theory, the prohibition on
parallel perfect intervals in the presence of nonharmonic tones. Prout
endorses the common view that suspensions represent (or “substitute” for)
their tones of resolution, and thus cannot be used to mask parallels. He then
immediately concedes that this norm was not obeyed by the “Old Masters”
in the case of perfect fifths, proposing a sonic explanation—that this
violation is “less unpleasing” and “quite unobjectionable” (Figure 1.3.1).
This notion of a musical rule not followed in practice should make us
suspicious, for how are we to know that a norm exists, if not through
practice? Textbooks are unreliable, as authors might just be wrong about the
music they intend to describe. (Consider the prohibition of split infinitives, a
more-or-less illusory rule with no grounding in actual linguistic behavior.23)
Perhaps instead of describing composers as habitually violating their own
rules we should look for rules that accurately describe compositional
behavior. Or perhaps our so-called rules are generalizations theorists have
made about complex musical traditions—with apparent departures better
described as limitations of the generalizations themselves. Here it is
important that earlier composers were trained from an early age and in a
hands-on manner, a pedagogical approach that can preserve idioms resisting
systematic generalization.24 In replacing this hands-on tradition with
textbooks and college classes, we tend to reify the regularities, endowing
them with a metaphysical weight they do not deserve.
Figure 1.3.1. Prout’s 1889 discussion of suspension-masked parallel perfect intervals.
Figure 1.4.1. The tendency for leaps (solid lines) and steps (dotted lines) to reverse direction in
Palestrina’s masses, the fugues from The Well-Tempered Clavier, and artificial music based on these.
In the real music, there is a substantial tendency for leaps to reverse direction, even in the middle of
the tessitura; this tendency is much smaller in the artificial music.
Figure 1.4.2. The tendency of melodic leaps to reverse direction in a variety of repertoires. Due to the
frequent use of the pentatonic scale, a three-semitone interval has been classified as a step in Sacred
Harp and rock.
Such evidence needs to be viewed with caution, not just out of general
principle but also because musical concepts, including “voice” and “step,”
can be difficult to pin down. In sophisticated tonal music we find melodies
alternating between two logical voices, arpeggiating chords (i.e. moving by
step along a nonscalar collection), and inserting embellishing tones between
leap and reversal (Figure 1.4.3). These examples remind us that the principle
of post-skip reversal is native to the context of counterpoint instruction, a
simplified environment in which many musical techniques are off the
table.44 Simple counting will not necessarily answer the musical question.
Figure 1.4.3. Skips that do not immediately reverse direction due to multiple abstract voices, stepwise
motion along a triadic alphabet, and melodic embellishment.
What we can say is that computational analysis often provides limited and
defeasible evidence in favor of music-theoretical common sense. Leaps lead
disproportionately to changes of melodic direction in many repertoires. Vast
tracts of Renaissance polyphony decorate triads with the nonharmonic tones
of traditional contrapuntal theory. And, of course, countless functionally
tonal passages exhibit the tonic-subdominant-dominant-tonic organization
described in harmony texts. Here and elsewhere empirical investigation
undercuts the more extreme forms of music-theoretical skepticism—a
complete rejection of “post-skip reversal,” or of Roman-numeral analysis, or
of the very notion of nonharmonicity.45 In the past, such skepticism has
often served theoretical ambition, the rejection of received wisdom paving
the road for more radical alternatives. It is difficult to see how this approach
can survive the era of abundant musical data.
At the same time, however, the following chapters will uncover numerous
challenges to textbook verities: nonharmonic tones that cannot be “reduced”
to consonances, or classical idioms that do not conform to any simple model
of harmonic progression. Conversely, we will find untheorized regularities in
a variety of genres. Perhaps the most consequential are the continuities
between sixteenth-century modality and eighteenth-century functionality—a
very gradual harmonic simplification beginning in the Renaissance and
continuing through the classical era, often invisible to the analytic eye. As a
result, there is hardly any music that is purely “modal” in the sense of being
untainted by functional principles, nor any that is purely “functional” by
virtue of having shorn all connections to earlier practice. Consistent with this
picture are the differences between dialects of functional tonality, with
baroque music retaining more vestiges of Renaissance practice. Corpus
analysis thus suggests we replace the binary opposition of modality and
functional tonality (and perforce the assumption that these represent separate
and coherent “systems” of musical thinking) with a more continuously
changing set of contrapuntal and harmonic conventions.46
We are, in other words, led in equal and opposite directions: on the one
hand to the conclusion that traditional theory is generally correct, and on the
other to the realization that its basic assumptions sometimes fail. One might
draw an analogy to Newtonian physics, which provides accurate descriptions
of the macroscopic world despite being fundamentally wrong about its
microscopic structure. To the engineer, these failures are minor and can often
be ignored. To the philosopher, they are cataclysmic, for the failure of
Newtonian physics points to a fundamentally counterintuitive picture of the
universe. For all the success of classical mechanics, the world simply is not
made of tiny billiard balls proceeding along deterministic paths. Something
altogether different is going on.
5. Schema
Figure 1.5.3. Sequential and nonsequential variants of Robert Gjerdingen’s “Romanesca” schema.
Figure 1.5.4. The percentage of triads above 3̂ that are in first inversion.
Figure 1.5.5. Three ways to harmonize stepwise descending thirds with alternating 53 and 63
sonorities. The first is common in virtually all triadic music; the second can be found in the baroque
but is rare thereafter; the third is common only in modal music. Here “D4A2” means “descending
fourth, ascending second.”
Figure 1.5.6. Two contrary-motion sequences featuring diatonic consonances. They can be reversed to
create registral expansion.
Figure 1.5.7. Two examples of a contrary-motion schema alternating thirds and sixths: Fanny
Mendelssohn Hensel’s “Januar,” mm. 15–18 (from Das Jahr, H. 385); and a passage from Klaus
Badelt’s film score Pirates of the Caribbean: Curse of the Black Pearl (2002, heard at 2:07:32). The
first forms a brief fifth sequence, while the second combines an ascending and descending fifth to
return to its starting point.
Figure 1.5.8. A motive from Meredith Monk’s Anthem (2020), analyzed in relation to the traditional
contrary-motion schema. Both feature “antiparallel” motion where the upper voice ascends by fifth
(T4 or four diatonic steps), while the bottom voice descends by fourth (T–3).
6. Outline
This book is framed by two largely analytical chapters. Chapter 2 argues that
a variety of rock progressions, all unusual from the standpoint of classical
theory, reflect straightforward features of musical geometry—and hence that
the intuitive competence of the rock musician is in part a matter of knowing
one’s way around the space of chromatic triads. This chapter is meant to
provide an accessible introduction to my general approach. Chapter 10
makes a broadly similar point about Beethoven, focusing on what I call the
“Ludwig” schema, and considering some of the philosophical challenges his
music poses. This is more of a culmination, linking technical issues in voice-
leading geometry with philosophical questions about analysis. Together, the
two chapters suggest that a range of different musical styles can be linked by
a subterranean geometrical logic.
Sandwiched in between are seven more theoretical chapters examining
various features of modal and functionally tonal syntax. Chapter 3 considers
the voice-leading system, arguing that counterpoint is not fundamentally a
matter of avoiding parallels, or even balancing of independent melodic lines,
but of understanding the interdependence between harmonic and melodic
forces. As we will see, general linear impulses such as the preference for
descending stepwise melodies will tend to generate different kinds of
harmonic progressions in different contexts—so that one and the same
melodic tendency produces modal results in one situation and functional
results in another. My approach here recalls the Schenkerian idea that
melodic phenomena generate harmonic patterns “at the musical surface.”
But unlike Schenker, I argue that the voice-leading system often generates a
distinctively modal logic which reverses classical harmonic norms.
Chapter 4 then zeroes in on the particular subject of repetition, proposing
a new theoretical device, the repeating contrapuntal pattern, that can be
found from the Renaissance to the present day. This is perhaps the newest
chapter of the book, using abstract notions of symmetry to identify an aspect
of musical competence crossing stylistic boundaries; its argument continues
in appendix 3, which connects classical sequences to twentieth-century
transformations. Chapter 5 considers the nonharmonic system coordinating
the behavior of dissonant and nonharmonic tones. I argue that nonharmonic
tones are not simply “decorative,” particularly in the case of suspensions.
This leads to a more general consideration of the ways in which
nonharmonic tones can serve harmonic ends—and hence the impossibility of
drawing a clean distinction between harmonic and nonharmonic realms. The
discussion again serves as a case study for my broader approach: traditional
theories of nonharmonicity work well in general, while also breaking down
in alarming ways.
The latter part of the book turns to the harmonic system, a set of initially
implicit norms that were eventually codified by figures such as Rameau and
Riemann. Chapter 6 interprets the rise of harmonic functionality as a gradual
process spanning more than two centuries, using analysis and corpus study
to tease out a simpler form of protofunctionality appearing early in the
sixteenth century. Chapter 7 analyzes the local procedures of mature
functionality as the product of three independent subsystems—harmonic
cycles, fauxbourdon, and sequences. Here the main innovations are (1)
tracing functional practice to basic affordances of geometrical space; (2)
using the circle of diatonic thirds to highlight the quasi-sequential nature of
functional harmony; (3) including fauxbourdon as a core functional
subsystem; and (4) expanding our conception of sequential practice. Chapter
8 discusses scales and keys, which I present as chordlike objects operating at
a higher hierarchical level; I also use voice leading to propose a new theory
of enharmonicism. Chapter 9 turns to higher-level organization, considering
melodic strategies, harmonic recursion, and eight-bar phrase structure.
Each chapter is prefaced by a short prelude setting the stage for the more
detailed investigation to follow; for some readers, these may provide the
most concentrated source of interesting material. Where appropriate, I end
each chapter by considering twentieth- and twenty-first-century applications
of its techniques. Some readers will want to chart a path reflecting their
particular interests: the examination of earlier music is mostly confined to
chapters 2–6, while functional tonality is the focus of chapters 7–10. Those
who are interested in analysis can find detailed discussions of Monteverdi’s
“Ohimè, se tanto amate” in §5.3, the opening of the Rondo-Burlesque from
Mahler’s Ninth Symphony in §5.5, the Kyrie of Palestrina’s Pope Marcellus
Mass in §6.5, Marenzio’s “Ahi Dispetata Morte” in §6.7, four “dualist”
pieces by J. S. Bach (invention, prelude, chorale, and fugue) in §7.7, several
Chopin pieces in §9.3, and, in chapter 10, five nineteenth-century
movements by Beethoven, Schubert, and Wagner. Once the basic framework
of chapters 2–4 is taken on board, chapters can be read out of order, as
loosely connected essays on different aspects of musical structure.
Any theory that is concerned with voice leading needs to grapple with
Heinrich Schenker, whose musical ideas are in equal parts insightful and
infuriating—and whose moral and aesthetic views are often downright
repulsive. One of the major surprises for me, over the course of my writing,
is how often I have found myself close to Schenkerian insights.56 Like
Schenker, I believe music is often governed by a deep contrapuntal logic that
is obscured when we focus too closely on roots and Roman numerals. I
endorse the Schenkerian idea that chords can exist “below the musical
surface,” though for me this means treating them as small scales along which
voices move. I likewise share Schenker’s perception that sequences are
fundamentally connected to certain sorts of linear intervallic structures, or
“repeating contrapuntal patterns” as I call them. I frequently adopt a broadly
Schenkerian strategy of analyzing music by “reducing” it to familiar
templates or schemas, eliminating surface detail to reveal a simplified
backbone. On a more detailed level, I often follow Schenker by assigning
voice exchanges to the musical surface.57 Finally, I use voice leading as a
lens for understanding higher-level phenomena such as modulation. In all
these ways, I will try to infuse the relatively flat models of voice-leading
geometry with a richer hierarchical structure.
But beyond these broad similarities are a host of important differences.
The biggest is that I am fundamentally skeptical about the long-range
connections that characterize so much Schenkerian practice—the idea that
there are aesthetically and perceptually meaningful connections between
notes separated by dozens or hundreds of measures. In my view this
represents a fantastical disregard for human perceptual limitations.58 Almost
as important are two different conceptions of musical reduction: for me it is
a process of modeling, speculative hypotheses about the cognitive structures
that might produce a musical passage, whereas for Schenker it reveals
something like a musical essence.59 Furthermore, I view scales (both
familiar and generalized) as important bearers of harmonic structure, while
Schenkerian theory affords them no privileged status. I also think sequences
can provide alternatives to the chorale-like backgrounds of Schenkerian
analysis, allowing us to understand an irregular musical surface as distorting
a more-regular background. Finally, I am largely unconcerned with unity as
an aesthetic value and with reductional stages beyond the first. All of which
is to say that I try to incorporate Schenkerian insights in ways that are both
empirically informed and epistemologically modest, tempered by geometry,
corpus study, and schema theory.
The notated tradition has often been a source of undue anxiety. We are bad
listeners if we do not have a sense of resolution when a sonata recapitulates
its second theme in the tonic key. We are bad historians if we hear tonic-
dominant resolution in sixteenth-century music. We are bad composers if we
need the piano, or fail to imagine an entire piece in a single blazing instant.
We are aesthetically unsophisticated if we happen to prefer rock to Wagner.
Over and over again, we are told that we are Ordinary while the dead
composers are Geniuses—and that we should subordinate our preferences to
those of our aesthetic betters. One possible response is to reject the learned
tradition in favor of simpler and more accessible styles. My hope is that this
book could point toward another: that by generalizing and demystifying
basic compositional techniques, it will allow us to appreciate the notated
tradition’s virtues while also defusing some of its elitism—giving us a
middle path between uncritical acceptance and outright rejection. This is the
final and most unlikely meaning of my title.
1
Babbitt was a partisan of logical positivism, whose narrowminded and aggressive skepticism
drove many theorists away from analytic philosophy as such; however, I believe that music theory has
much to learn from its emphasis on clear reasoning.
2
This is not the only project that music theorists can undertake: we can study perception, either
using experiments or introspection; we can study the historical concepts that have actually guided
music creation; we can engage in various forms of criticism and evaluation. I will be concerned with
these issues only secondarily, focusing instead on the intuitive knowledge of basic pitch structure.
3
McCreless 2011 considers the metaphor of musical ownership from many different points of
view.
4
Note that Gesualdo substitutes G major for the G minor implied by the sequence (§4.9).
5
Cohn 1996 and 2012, pp. 106–9.
6
Hook 2002 reports that John Clough (2000) offered a similar analysis.
7
Stravinsky’s four-voice passage involves the same abstract registral configurations as Gesualdo’s,
with alto doubling bass in one chord form and tenor doubling soprano in the other. In both passages,
the four-voice sonorities are related by pitch-space inversion.
8
Later in the same solo, Tyner uses the technique with four-note pentatonic subsets (Figure A3.11).
9
Yellin 1998, Telesco 1998, and §4.6.
10
Throughout this book I notate scale degrees so that middle C is as close to 60 as possible.
11
Lewin (1986, p. 355, note 19) makes a similar point about Schubert’s “Morgengruss.” Whether
the Beatles were explicitly thinking in these terms is a complex and likely unanswerable question.
12
If we interpret D as a passing tone connected by step to both C and E, then it is unclear how to
start the figure on G.
13
Shepard tones can be used to produce a sequence that appears both to fall continuously and also
to remain fixed in register; the illusion is accomplished by gradually fading in higher-register
harmonics while fading out the lower-register harmonics (Shepard 1964).
14
Peter Westergaard (1975) asserted that leaps of a third or more inherently marked a change of
voice below the surface, a claim that initially sounds counterintuitive. But when background voices
are triadic and connected by stepwise voice leading, the statement is almost tautologous: since the
background of Heu me Domine contains almost no leaps, leaps on the surface are necessarily due to
motion from one background strand to another, precisely as Westergaard suggests.
15
The investigation of hierarchically embedded musical “alphabets” begins with Simon and
Kotovsky 1963, continuing with Simon and Sumner 1968, Deutsch and Feroe 1981, and Lerdahl 2001.
The idea of extending set-theoretical techniques to arbitrary collections is implicit in Clough 1979a
and 1979b. This book fuses these ideas with the theory of voice leading, considering arbitrary
hierarchies of arbitrarily moving collections, with analogous transformations occurring at each
hierarchical level, and motion at one level often counteracting motion at another.
16
Lerdahl 1988.
17
Charles Rosen writes, “material presented outside the tonic must have created, in the eighteenth
century, a feeling of instability which demanded to be resolved” (1971, p. 74, my italics). The
experiments of Cook 1987 and Marvin and Brinkman 1999 suggest that this may not be true.
18
The diagram in Lerdahl 2020, p. 23, formalizes the inference from the musical surface to the
“heard structure.” See Tymoczko 2020a for more.
19
See Meyer (1956, p. 32), Gjerdingen (1996, pp. 380–81, and 2007, pp. 16–19), and Byros (2012,
pp. 282–84, which is admirably clear about these issues, and from which several of the preceding
references are drawn).
20
Aural comprehension is sometimes used as a criterion of musical content—which we might call
the what’s-there-is-what-you-hear principle. For example, Hadjeres, Pachet, and Nielsen 2017 use
listener data to judge whether a computer can be said to “compose like Bach.” I doubt those listeners
were sensitive to the structural details that interest theorists, composers, and analysts.
21
Stephenson 2002, chapter 5. The issue here is comparable to questions about whether a speaker
uses “bad grammar” or speaks a slightly different dialect; chapter 2 interprets rock music with
reference to specifically modal norms.
22
Hepokoski’s notion of “dialogic form” (2010) places a very heavy weight on inferred cultural
norms; chapter 10 interprets Beethoven as less beholden to existing convention.
23
Pullum and Huddleston 2002. Weber (1817–1821) 1846 is impressively nondogmatic about
musical practice.
24
Gjerdingen 2020.
25
This point is further obscured by the music-theoretical conflation of the grammatical and the
pedagogical: introductory textbooks simultaneously function as our most complete descriptions of
musical syntax, leaving us with an extensive literature articulating true but simplistic generalizations,
and comparatively little scholarship which tries to describe musical norms at a more granular level.
26
See the prelude to chapter 6 for specific references.
27
Margolis and Laurence 2003. Hilary Putnam notes that “momentum equals mass times velocity”
was once true by definition; yet in special relativity this statement is false, true only in the limit of
very small velocity (Putnam 1988, p. 8).
28
Wittgenstein 1953.
29
See Rosch 1978, which develops Wittgenstein’s observations in a scientific context. Caplin
(2013, p. 127) associates this attitude with “postmodernism,” but I think it is close to the consensus
among analytic philosophers and cognitive scientists, two groups not known for their postmodern
attitude. Lawrence 2020 observes that Caplin operates with a “classical” theory of concepts.
30
For instance, Caplin 2004, example 6, Mozart K.310, III, mm. 173–174, and the opening themes
of Beethoven’s Op. 127, I and IV. Richards’s term “closural function” (2010) is virtually synonymous
with the pre-Caplin “cadence,” underscoring the potentially terminological nature of the debate.
Burstein 2015 also critiques Caplin’s approach.
31
Effacing the medial caesura is, as Mark Richards (2013a, 2013b) has noted, a common tactic in
Beethoven’s music (Figure P10.4, §10.3).
32
I think Caplin is right to point out that 5̂–1̂ bass motion typically plays an important role in
signaling strong cadences in the classical style, just as I think Hepokoski and Darcy are right to note
that second themes are very often preceded by pauses. But in making these true observations into
definitions, they reject the very possibility of counterexamples. I see Brown 2005 as declaring that he
will only use the word “tonal” to describe pieces that are susceptible to Schenkerian analysis.
33
The fallacy begins with a claim that “no Scotsman would do x” (Flew 1975). When presented
with a counterexample, a Scotsman who does in fact do x, the claimant responds by arguing that the
doer is “no true Scotsman.” In this way it becomes definitionally true that no Scotsman would do x,
since anyone who does x is not a (true) Scotsman. This renders a potentially true generalization into a
trivial fact about linguistic usage.
34
For sandwiches see Lund 2014 and Scherer 2015; Burstein (2015, p. 105) notes that traditional
theorists often emphasized the importance of judgment in concept application.
35
Powers 1992b, p. 212: “Nowadays professional folksingers and composer of folksongs talk
glibly about their Dorian and Mixolydian tunes, and so too sophisticated jazz practitioners and
textbooks alike, just as composers and theorists of art music used to compose using ‘dominants’ with
‘flatted leading-tones’ and call it ‘modal harmony.’ ”
36
Both Schoenberg and Schenker, for example, placed a heavy emphasis on musical relationships
of dubious perceptibility, justifying their methods by appeals to a nebulous and problematic notion of
“genius” (Arndt 2013).
37
There are interesting analogies between the history of science and the history of music theory,
which both struggle to chart a path between the twin dangers of triumphalism (or “whiggishness”) and
relativism. I suspect that the history of music theory, having survived whiggishness in the form of a
Hegelian-Schenkerian progressivism, is currently in the late stages of its relativist or historicist phase,
with a more realist, computational era on the horizon. See Wootton 2015.
38
Labov 1975 describes a case in which a speaker used constructions he declared to be
ungrammatical.
39
Compare Weber (1817–1821) 1846, pp. 453–54: “Theorists really possess incomparably less of
theory than practitioners themselves. For the former teach false rules while the latter act according to
those which are true. The rules which the former lay down, infinitely more often prove erroneous than
correct; while the latter produce elevated works of art, from which we might and should long since
have deduced better rules.”
40
McEnery and Wilson 1996.
41
Mistakes no doubt remain, however, and readers are invited to send corrections by email.
42
Von Hippel and Huron 2000.
43
The artificial datasets are amnesiac or “Markov twin” versions of the musical data that have the
same (first-order) melodic statistics: note x in the Markov twin uses the same intervals, with the same
probabilities, as note x in the original music, but has no memory of the previous interval. For more,
see von Hippel and Huron 2000. Note that the artificial datasets do exhibit a tendency toward “post-
skip reversal” at their registral extremes; Huron and von Hippel are not wrong about the existence of
this effect, just about its magnitude.
44
Von Hippel and Huron were concerned both with the universal claims of Leonard Meyer and
Eugene Narmour, and also with the situated pronouncements of Western pedagogues. Their data more
directly bears on the former.
45
Schenker rejected (local) Roman-numeral analysis while Schoenberg rejected the very notion of
nonharmonicity. These views are echoed by contemporary figures such as Robert Gjerdingen (1996)
and Ian Quinn (2018).
46
For the “two systems” see Dahlhaus 1990, pp. 59–65.
47
Gjerdingen and Bourne 2015. Gjerdingen’s project is influenced by Meyer 1996.
48
The connection is explicit in Holtmeier 2007.
49
If we assert that the bass F (3̂) is a chord tone, then the upper-voice E♭4 on the second quarter is
the seventh of iii7 (F–A♭–C–E♭), which moves up by step.
50
The “cadenza doppia” is the formula in the top voice of Figure 1.5.1, where the leading tone
moves up to the tonic, often forming a suspension, before ultimately moving back to the leading tone
and resolving once again to the tonic.
51
In discussion, theorists have sometimes suggested that Bach’s cadential idiom “works” because
the bass and upper voices become temporarily out of sync, with the seventh resolving to 3̂
prematurely, before the upper voices reach the tonic. But consider this explanation from the point of
view of a student attempting to imitate Bach: here it is crucial that the “falling out of sync” (if that is
what it is) almost always occurs in a particular context and on a particular set of scale degrees. This
limitation is apparent only when we switch perspectives from that of the passive listener, confronting a
fixed body of earlier music, to that of the composer, who needs to know when the idiom is
permissible.
52
Temperley 2006 also argues for abstract schemas, while Byros 2017 distinguishes “microtheory”
from “macrotheory.” Rabinovitch (2018, 2019, and 2020) also tries to systematize schematic thinking,
though from a different point of view. Holtmeier 2011 notes that eighteenth-century musicians had
easy access to an elaborate theoretical discourse containing generalizations about topics such as
sequences, the rule of the octave, and the fundamental bass.
53
Gjerdingen 2007, p. 34.
54
Holtmeier 2011 makes a similar point.
55
These two intervals, the fourth and fifth, are the diatonic scale’s closest approximation to a half-
octave.
56
Burstein (forthcoming) describes the difficulties of assigning a univocal view to Schenker
himself or Schenkerians more generally. In this book, I will typically be concerned with views shared
by many different scholars—what Burstein describes as a “Schenkerian practice” originating with an
“idealized” Schenker.
57
Cf. Brown 2005, p. 78.
58
Forte 1959 aptly compares Schenker to Freud, whose work was considered much more scientific
in 1959 than it is now.
59
Cf. Schenker (1935) 1979, p. xxiii: “The musical examples which accompany this volume are
not merely practical aids; they have the same power and conviction as the visual aspect of the printed
composition itself (the foreground). That is, the graphic representation is part of the actual
composition, not merely an educational means.”
Prelude
Transposition Along a Collection
measured in triadic steps, or steps along the three note “scale” E ♭ –G–B ♭
(Figure P2.2). Actually, that oversimplifies: it is only the last two notes of
the motive that move along the triad, with the first a neighbor note one scale
step above the second note; to analyze the passage we therefore need to
juggle scalar and chordal distances simultaneously.
Figure P2.1. Moving a three-note motive up by step along the diatonic scale (T1).
Figure P2.2. In m. 7 of “West End Blues,” Louis Armstrong transposes the second two notes of his
motive downward by chordal step along the E♭ major triad (t–1), with the first note a neighbor one
scale step above the second.
In Figure P2.3, Domenico Scarlatti transposes along both chord and scale,
the right hand moving up by one triadic step while the left hand ascends by
two scalar steps. (For the sake of illustration, I am ignoring the slight
difference between the hands’ patterns and considering some notes to be
nonharmonic.) We see that for A–C–E, transposition by triadic step (C–E–A)
is almost equivalent to transposition by two diatonic steps (C–E–G). Readers
can explore this idea by shifting arbitrary motives along the intrinsic scale
defined by their own notes; this produces a range of interesting results,
particularly when combined with transposition along the scale, or when one
declares some notes to be nonharmonic. The passages in Figure P2.4 lie
somewhere between traditional motivic development and modern set theory;
as a group, they suggest that musical identity is up to us, determined by an
object’s transformational properties rather than fixed for all time by its
intrinsic constitution.
Figure P2.3. Domenico Scarlatti’s Sonata in A minor, K.3, mm. 3–6. The right-hand motive moves up
by chordal step (t1), while the left-hand variant moves up by two scale steps (T2), with the two
transpositions producing very similar results. (Here and elsewhere, I use p and n for passing and
neighboring notes.) Since t1 and T2 are almost the same, t–1 almost cancels T2 so that the combination
t–1T2 does very little. The bottom system analyzes the efficient voice leading between the top system’s
(A, C, E) and the bottom system’s (G, C, E) as a combination of transpositions along both chord (t–1)
and scale (T2).
Figure P2.4. Transposing two motives along the intrinsic scale defined by their pitches. Two different
stepwise transpositions are shown, one treating all notes as harmonic, the other treating some as
nonharmonic. Boldface n labels potential chromatic neighbors, regular n labels potential diatonic
neighbors. The last measure of the third line transposes a triad along its fifth, interpreting the second
note as a third above the first.
An alternate strategy moves each voice upward by step along the chord
itself. First we compress the chord’s notes into a single octave, forming a
scale from its notes; then we move each voice by the same number of steps
along that scale. This yields a series of voicings sharing the same abstract
registral arrangement and returning to their initial configuration in a higher
octave (Figure P2.6).1 Once again, transposition along a collection preserves
intervals as measured by that collection: here, the middle note is always one
chordal step above the lowest, while the top is four chordal steps above the
middle. This approach is familiar in guitar pedagogy, where it is used to
construct inversions that keep the left hand in roughly the same position; in
chapter 3 we will find that it plays a role in traditional figured-bass theory as
well.
Figure P2.6. Forming registral inversions by moving each voice along the intrinsic scale formed by
the chord’s notes.
Transposition along the chord is significant here because it preserves a
tangible aspect of musical structure: voicing, or the pattern of registral
spacing measured in steps along the chord itself. It is also important because
the most efficient voice leading between two chords—the mapping that
collectively moves the voices by the smallest overall amount—will always
connect chords that are voiced in the same way. Thus if we want to find
efficient voice leading between two chords, we should arrange them using
the same spacing-in-chordal steps. Figure P2.7 maps the preceding
example’s C3–D3–A4 to a minor triad spaced in exactly the same way.
There are three fundamental possibilities depending on whether the C in the
initial chord is mapped to the root, third, or fifth of the minor triad.2 We
conclude that chordal-step distance will be relevant whenever composers are
concerned with efficient voice leading, which is much of the time. Indeed,
composers might find themselves using transposition-along-the-chord
unknowingly, as a byproduct of trying to minimize contrapuntal motion. In
motivic contexts, composers intentionally move musical objects along the
chord; in contrapuntal contexts, these same transformations can arise
implicitly and without conscious knowledge. The phenomenon of
transposition-along-a-collection spans the border between implicit and
explicit.
Figure P2.7. Three spacing-preserving voice leadings from C3–D3–A4 to a minor triad; the first maps
the C to the triad’s root, the second to the triad’s third, and the last to the triad’s fifth. All chords are
voiced with the middle note one chordal step above the bass and four chordal steps below the soprano.
The deep point is that any collection of notes, from chord to motive to set,
can be associated with two different scales: an external or enclosing scale—
chromatic, diatonic, or some other contextually relevant collection—and the
intrinsic scale formed from its own notes. To transpose along the first scale
is to transpose in the ordinary sense, preserving intervals as measured along
the scale; to transpose along the second is to generalize the notion of chordal
inversion as just described, preserving intervals as measured along the chord.
Recent music theory has largely concerned itself with the first of these
transformations, whereas traditional theory often concerned itself with the
second, either implicitly (e.g., as a means of obtaining efficient voice
leading) or explicitly (e.g., close and open position). The intrinsic scale is a
missing link between traditional tonal theory and modern set theory,
obscured by the habit of conceiving sets in exclusively chromatic terms.
Once we train ourselves to think within a variety of different scales, starting
with “diatonic set theory” and generalizing from there, we will eventually
find our way to the intrinsic scale and the concepts implicit in traditional
theory.
There are many other uses for the concept of the intrinsic scale. One, to be
discussed in the next two chapters, harmonizes melodies using transposition
along both chord and scale. Another connects chords related by pitch-class
inversion and voiced in the same way; such spacing-preserving progressions
will invariably preserve the distance between at least one pair of voices.3
What results are exactly the passages we explored in §1.1, where two voices
move in parallel while the remaining notes alternate to create similarly
spaced, inversionally related chords. In other words, the seeming
contrapuntal wizardry of Gesualdo et al. is a straightforward consequence of
a concern for efficient voice leading, or attention to chord voicing as
measured in chordal steps (Figure P2.8). The recent discipline of “neo-
Riemannian theory” is largely concerned with these progressions, and
particularly those where the parallel voices are stationary; the concept of the
intrinsic scale gives us an intuitive handle on this complex literature, and on
the musical practices motivating it.4
Figure P2.8. Spacing-preserving progressions from a chord to its pitch-class inversion. Intrinsic
spacing is listed to the right of each staff. Open noteheads show the parallel voices. The progressions
on the left move to different registral inversions of the same target chord in a voicing-preserving way.
The progressions on the right also transpose the destination chord chromatically.
Transposition along the chord and transposition along the scale combine
to form a kind of doubly parallel motion along a pair of hierarchically nested
collections. I will notate x-step transposition along the chord as tx, with a
lowercase “t,” and y-step transposition along the scale as Ty, using a
boldface T for chromatic transposition and regular T for transposition along
scales such as the diatonic.5 Together, the two operations generate a wide
range of harmonies from a single starting point, all broadly similar in
arranging the same intervals in the same abstract registral configuration;
indeed these two transpositions are the only operations that preserve both
chord type and distance in chordal steps.6 For an n-note chord in an o-note
scale, transposition by n chordal steps is equivalent to transposition by o
scale steps, so we can always replace tn with To and vice versa. The two
operations commute, so txTy (tx and then Ty) is the same as Tytx (Ty and then
tx). Subscripts add as we would expect, with txTy and taTb combining to form
tx+aTy+b. We will see that there are even circumstances in which we might
want to consider inversion along both chord and scale, notated ixIy.
Remarkably, there is a simple and intuitive geometry of hierarchically
nested transposition, equally applicable to voice in chord, chord in scale, and
scale in chromatic aggregate. We can use the following recipe to represent
transposition along both an n-note collection (“chord”) and an o-note
collection (“scale”) containing it.
A. Draw a spiral with n loops, attaching its end to its beginning. In this
book I will always a draw clockwise spiral beginning at 12 o’clock and
moving inward until it reaches 9 o’clock for the nth time, then
connecting the end of the spiral clockwise to its starting point (Figure
P2.9).
B. Mark off o equally spaced points along this spiral, labeling them with
consecutive scale tones. To do this, divide the circle into o equal pie
slices; move along the spiral placing a point at the border of every nth
slice, with n the size of the chord: every border for one-note chords,
every other border for two-note chords, every third for three-note
chords, and so on. In this book I will usually place the C chord at the
innermost point at 12 o’clock and move clockwise along the spiral in a
descending fashion, labeling B, B♭, A . . . for the chromatic scale, B, A,
G . . . for the diatonic, and so on.
Figure P2.10 shows the resulting diagrams for major chords and diatonic
scales in chromatic space. Each point represents an entire collection of notes,
a complete major chord or complete diatonic scale. (In my experience,
readers can be confused by this point, so I will repeat it: in this book, points
on the spiral diagrams will always represent entire chords rather than single
notes.) The major-chord spiral winds around the circle three times, with
chords positioned at 12, 3, 6, and 9 o’clock (every third clock position); the
scalar spiral winds around the circle seven times, with chords placed at every
seventh slice. Readers can verify that a chord’s angular position corresponds
to the sum of its pitch classes, with each one-slice clockwise motion
decreasing the chord’s sum by one.7 Given a diagram, we can determine the
size of the chord from the number of loops and the size of the scale from the
number of points. Do not be concerned by the line’s self-intersection, as this
is an artifact of depicting an intrinsically higher-dimensional geometry on a
flat piece of paper; as appendix 2 explains, the actual line does not intersect
itself.
Figure P2.9. Spirals representing chords with one, two, three, and seven notes.
Figure P2.10. Spiral diagrams for major triads in the chromatic scale and seven-note diatonic
collections in the chromatic scale. Each point represents a complete chord: “Cmaj” in the first example
represents the C major triad, while “Cdia” in the second represents the C diatonic scale.
Figure P2.11 illustrates. The third rule is in many ways the most important,
as it allows us to understand every path as a combination of loops and slides,
or chordal and scalar transpositions. Before using a particular model, we will
typically calculate the voice leading linking nearby chords; this is the
combination of loops and slides that counteract each other as much as
possible, and it changes from diagram to diagram. Chapter 2 will explain
how to do this.
Figure P2.11. In the spiral diagrams, clockwise motion descends and counterclockwise motion
ascends (– and + respectively). Path a slides along the spiral and transposes along the scale. Path b
loops around the space and transposes along the chord; this requires leaving the spiral at some point.
Paths c and d represent the same voice leading as they start and end at the same point and involve the
same total quantity of angular motion (90° clockwise). Path c shows that this voice leading is T–5t1,
combining a clockwise loop (t1) with a five-step counterclockwise slide T–5.
The rest of this book will use the spiral diagrams to model intuitive
musical knowledge—and particularly the efficient voice leadings that result
when an operation-along-a-chord nearly counteracts the corresponding
operation-along-a-scale. We will focus on a small collection of spaces,
analyzing rock music with the 3-in-12 graph of chromatic triads, early music
with the 2-in-7 and 3-in-7 graphs of diatonic dyads and triads, and
functionally tonal modulation using the graph of 7-in-12 diatonic scales. In
modeling melody, we will sometimes consider very small scales such as the
2-in-3 graph of two-note triadic subsets. We will also use multiple spiral
diagrams to describe nested musical motion, the same basic structure
representing the motion of voice in chord, chord in scale, and scale in
aggregate. With a little practice, the reader can learn to translate geometrical
path to musical notation and vice versa; with a little more practice, the reader
can construct and manipulate the spiral diagrams for any chord in any scale.
The spiral diagrams can also be extended to include multiple chord types and
a wider range of voice leadings, including those with voice exchanges, and
those in which chord tones are doubled.9 No mathematics is required, and no
particular music-theoretical skill beyond an ability to manipulate
hierarchically nested transposition. My hope is that the spiral diagrams can
function as the music-theoretical equivalent of a consumer product, allowing
readers to enjoy the benefits of sophisticated musical geometry without
mastering its details—much as we can use GPS without fully understanding
the general-relativistic calculations it performs.
Readers who want to dive straight into the technical details can turn to
appendices 1 and 2; those who prefer a more analytical introduction should
instead proceed to chapter 2.
1
If we start with a chord whose notes are compressed within a single octave, the new algorithm
returns the same results as the traditional process of moving bottom note to top.
2
The next chapter’s prelude will explain how to derive the remaining spacing-preserving voice
leadings from these three options.
3
NB: “inversion” here means “pitch-class inversion,” the turning-upside-down of a chord’s
intervals, rather than “registral inversion,” the rearranging of its notes in register. In this case, the
identity of the parallel voices is determined by the registral inversions of the chords in the progression.
4
Spacing-preserving voice leadings between inversionally related chords can be obtained by
inverting a chord twice, along both the intrinsic and extrinsic scale; see appendix 1, appendix 3, and
Tymoczko 2020b for more.
5
This notation was first used by Julian Hook (2003, 2008).
6
Here I am using “chord type” to mean “transpositional set class.” If we consider pitch-class
inversions (like the major and minor triad) to be the same, then we need to include the “generalized
neo-Riemannian voice leadings” that send a chord to its similarly spaced inversional partner as shown
in Figure P2.8 (e.g., the “parallel” voice leading that lowers a major triad’s third by semitone or raises
the minor triad’s third by semitone). These act as half a transposition-along-the-chord (Tymoczko
2020b).
7
Here we need to use “clock arithmetic,” where we take the remainder when divided by the size of
the scale. So, for instance, the C major triad contains C, E, and G or 0, 4, 7 in pitch class notation,
which together sum to 11 (0 + 4 + 7). A♭ major contains A♭, C, and E♭, or 8, 0, 3, which also sum to
11. E major is E, G♯, and B, or 4, 8, and 11, which sum to 23, which is 11 when we divide by 12 and
take the remainder. B major, G major, and E♭ major are three pie-slices clockwise, since their notes
sum to 8 modulo 12.
8
Rule 3 is equivalent to the statement that two paths correspond to the same voice leading if one
can be smoothly transformed into the other without breaking the path or moving its endpoints; such
deformations are studied by topologists (Tymoczko 2020b). In summing the intervals in each voice,
we do not use clock arithmetic but rather real numbers representing paths in pitch-class space
(appendix 1, Tymoczko 2011a).
9
Readers may find it useful to visit the website https://www.madmusicalscience.com, where they
can find movies and software.
2
Rock Logic
In many musical styles, melodic steps tend to descend while leaps tend to
ascend.1 In Figure 2.0.1, for example, the singer steps downward from 3̂ to
1̂, descending by step within the chord before leaping back to 3̂ to start the
next verse. It is reasonable to think that the melody motivates the song’s
inversion of traditional harmonic norms: if we decide to associate each
melodic note with a major triad in the D major scale, the I–V–IV–I
progression is one of a small number of possibilities. The subversion of the
tonic-subdominant-dominant paradigm, in other words, may not be a
rebellion against classical harmonic practice, but rather a straightforward
solution to a basic musical problem.
This chapter will use this idea to motivate a model of rock harmony, one
that emphasizes the melodic origin of its harmonic idioms. Though the
underlying techniques apply equally well to ascending or leaping melodies,
my main focus will be on stepwise descent. The goal is to understand the
implicit knowledge of talented but possibly untrained musicians—principles
that they might plausibly absorb from thousands of hours of listening and
playing. This project is broadly comparable to that of constructing a
grammar for natural language, another formal system that humans learn
without explicit instruction. This embodied and intuitive grammar contrasts
with the explicit doctrines of the notated tradition, from species counterpoint
to twelve-tone composition.
1. A melodic principle
Figure 2.1.2. “Weak” root progressions (ascending fifths and thirds, descending steps) generally
permit more opportunities for stepwise descent, with the major third being the only interval equally
balanced between strong and weak.
The model has Schenkerian overtones in suggesting that harmonic
patterns can arise out of fundamentally melodic aims—in this case, phrase-
level descending motion from one tonic-triad note to another. But rather than
trying to show that stepwise descending melody necessarily reproduces the
conventions of functional harmony, I will suggest that it leads to different
kinds of progressions in different harmonic contexts. In A Geometry of
Music, I used complicated geometrical models to try to capture this
interdependence of harmony and melody, describing twisted, singular, and
higher-dimensional spaces in which each musical voice is represented by its
own dimension. Here I will use the simpler spiral diagrams for the same
purpose. Figure 2.1.3 reproduces the voice-leading space for major triads in
the chromatic scale. For reasons to be explained in §2.6, I label points using
the triad’s root; readers should keep in mind, however, that each point
represents an entire chord.
Figure 2.1.3. The spiral diagram for chromatic triads. Each point represents a complete major triad.
We know that slides along the spiral correspond to transposition along the
scale while loops represent transposition along the chord. Unfortunately, it is
not obvious from these principles how to understand radial motion between
nearby chords, say from C major vertically outward to E major. This is a
general problem: for each new spiral diagram, we will need to calculate the
voice leadings between nearby points. Here, transposition-along-the-chord
largely counteracts transposition-along-the-scale, leaving efficient voice
leading as the result.
There is a general recipe that works for any spiral diagram: to find a direct
path between two chords, slide your finger in the desired direction
(clockwise or counterclockwise) from one to the other along the spiral; if
you are looking for a purely radial path, then both directions are equivalent.3
The number of chords you touch, not counting the first, is the transposition
along the scale (Tx), with positive and negative values of x corresponding to
counterclockwise and clockwise motion respectively. Meanwhile, the
number of times you revisit your initial angular position, y, becomes the
transposition along the chord t±y, with its sign opposite that of the scalar
transposition. The two combine to form Txt–y or T–xty, a composite
representing the most direct voice leading between the two chords.
Example 1. Radial motion from C to E. Imagine sliding counterclockwise
along the spiral from C major to E major. Not counting our starting point, we
touch four chords for T4. (Remember that I use boldface T for chromatic
transposition, and regular T for transposition along a scale such as the
diatonic.) We return once to our initial angular location of 12 o’clock, for t–1,
with a negative sign because our scalar transposition is positive. The
combination of these two motions, shown in Figure 2.1.4, moves C down to
B, keeps E fixed, and moves G up to G♯; this is the LP voice leading of neo-
Riemannian theory.4 The same reasoning shows that radial motion from E
major out to A♭ major, and from A♭ major inward to C major, also produces
T4t–1 or LP.
Figure 2.1.4. Calculating the radial path from C to E. We move counterclockwise along the spiral by
four steps to reach E, for T4; we pass our original angular position once, for t–1.
to A♭, also produces T–4t1, or the PL voice leading. This allows us to express
all motions on the diagram in musical notation (Figure 2.1.5).
Figure 2.1.5. The voice leadings on circular chord space represented in notation: vertical motion
corresponds to t–1T4 or t–1T–4 (LP or PL) while horizontal motion corresponds to chromatic
transposition (Tx).
Figure 2.1.7. (top) A descending sequence of augmented triads that can be labeled in a variety of
ways. (bottom line) By lowering a note of each augmented triad, we can obtain a variety of root
progressions exhibiting descending stepwise voice leadings. These progressions all move one step
clockwise on the spiral diagram.
Figure 2.1.8. In this chapter, we will generally imagine inner voices moving in doubly parallel motion
with the melody while the bass moves independently.
2. A harmonic principle
two units apart, since it takes two semitonal shifts (e.g., C→C♯ and F→F♯)
to transform one into the other. This conception of “diatonic distance” is a
voice-leading distance that reflects how far the scale’s notes have to move to
get to their destination; this is similar to, but not quite the same as, a model
based on common tones. Scalar voice-leading distance can be represented by
the circular graph of diatonic scales in chromatic space, where motion
between adjacent points represents the single-semitone voice leading
between fifth-related diatonic collections, and angular distance is equivalent
to scalar distance (Figure P2.10). We will return to this idea in chapter 8.
I will define the harmonic distance between two major triads as the
smallest distance from a scale containing one chord to a scale containing the
other (Figure 2.2.2). Thus the C major triad is distance 0 not just from F
major and G major (since the C, F, and G triads all belong to the C diatonic
scale), but also from D major (since the G diatonic scale contains both C
major and D major triads) and B♭ major (since F diatonic contains B♭ major
and C major). Meanwhile, the C major triad is one unit away from the E ♭
diatonic, and the B ♭ diatonic scale is one step away from F diatonic). By
similar reasoning the C and A major triads are one unit away from each
other. Major-third-related triads, like C and E, or C and A ♭ , are two units
away from each other, whereas semitonally related triads are three steps
away. Maximally separated are tritone-related triads, which are four steps
apart. This notion of triadic distance is very closely related to the circle-of-
fifths distance between triads, here justified by the fact that fifth-related
triads create coherent diatonic backgrounds or macroharmonies.8 It is, in
other words, a voice-leading distance at another hierarchical level.
Figure 2.2.2. Diatonic distance between major triads, defined as the minimum distance between a
diatonic scale containing the first chord and a diatonic scale containing the second.
The distance traversed by a repeating chord progression can then be
defined as the total number of modulatory steps involved in the entire
progression, with the proviso that we return to the initial chord and scale
(Figure 2.2.3). My proposal is that rock musicians favor chord progressions
that are diatonically close—that is, lying in a single diatonic scale or a small
collection of closely related scales. If this is so, then we should expect more
fifth-related triads like C and F, and major-second related triads, like C and
B♭, than semitonally or tritone-related triads (like C and B or C and F♯).9
The suggestion is that rock musicians have implicitly absorbed a weak
diatonic norm through extensive exposure to Western music.10
Figure 2.2.3. Measuring the total diatonic distance of a progression; the progression D–A–E–G has
diatonic distance 2, since it requires modulating from D diatonic to A diatonic and back.
Figure 2.2.5 rewrites the spiral diagram using Roman numerals, with
darkness representing harmonic proximity to the tonic chord. Once again,
the figure leads us to expect an abundance of progressions by ascending
fifths, ascending and descending major seconds, and, to a lesser extent,
ascending minor thirds. It does not give us reason to expect many
descending fifths or descending thirds, progressions traditionally associated
with functional tonality. For this reason, I will say that the gameboard
embodies a retrofunctional norm.13 Starting with two very basic musical
preferences—descending melodic steps and a preference for diatonically
close progressions—we have derived substantive expectations about rock
harmony, expectations that are both different from those appropriate to
classical music, and (as we will shortly see) consistent with what we find in
the repertoire.
Figure 2.2.5. The spiral diagram for major triads, rewritten with Roman numerals and with dark
chords being harmonically closer to I.
Conversely, the rules of functional harmony are precisely not the rules you
would come up with if you were to construct a musical syntax on your own
(if, say, you were a talented but not formally trained musician sitting in your
bedroom with a guitar). Instead, you would be more likely to come up with
the inverse—a retrofunctional norm, favoring ascending rather than
descending fifths and ascending rather than descending minor thirds. This
retrofunctional norm, I will argue, is an intuitive counterknowledge arising
from the deep structure of musical space, a subterranean musical practice
distinct from, and to some extent opposed to, the culturally sanctioned
routines of the notated tradition.
Figure 2.4.3. Progressions that can be generated by eliminating a nontonic chord from the “Air Near
My Fingers” progression.
At the level of the phrase, rock songs tend to adopt one of two strategies.
“Helpless” and “Back in Black” return to the tonic at the start of each two-
bar unit, overlapping their phrases to generate a feeling of constant forward
progress. Here, harmonic arrival coincides with melodic departure, and the
music is largely devoid of cadences in the sense of phrase-terminating
pauses. “Eight Days a Week,” “Sympathy for the Devil,” and “I Can’t
Explain” instead return to the tonic at the end of each two-bar unit; this
creates a much stronger sense of arrival, allowing for a feeling of rest
between each repeating unit. Here it is reasonable to speak of cadences in a
relatively traditional sense. 19
The third loop family is built on the I–V–♭VII–IV progression featured in
songs like “Natural Woman” (written for Aretha Franklin by Goffin and
King) and Duran Duran’s “Rio.” The former is shown in Figure 2.4.4, the
descending voice leading clear on the musical surface. This 6–5 progression
is the retrograde of the classical tradition’s ascending 5–6 sequence (Figure
2.4.5), giving rise to a descending chromatic line. Unlike the other
progressions we have considered, it plays a significant role in the classical
tradition—appearing in the music of Beethoven (Op. 31, no. 1, the Waldstein
sonata), Schubert (the late G major quartet, etc.), and many other composers.
In classical contexts it usually articulates a descent from tonic down to
dominant, whereas in popular music it often returns plagally to the tonic
after just four chords.20 Thus classical composers embed the progression
within a broadly functional movement from tonic to dominant whereas
popular music is more likely to use it as an alternative to tonic-dominant
functionality. The four-chord pattern generates two three-chord progressions
we have already encountered, I– ♭ VII–IV–I and I–V–IV–I, and one new
progression, I–V– ♭ VII–I, used in the Beatles’ “I’m a Loser” and Guns-N-
Roses’ “Sweet Child O’ Mine.”
Figure 2.4.4. “Natural Woman.”
Figure 2.4.5. Ascending and descending 6–5 sequences.
5. Shepard-tone passacaglias
Figure 2.5.2 traces this endless Urlinie throughout the progression: the
note D4, which ends the vocal line, continues downward by step in the guitar
to C♯ (over V) and then B (over IV) before arriving at A3 at the next repeat
—whereupon it continues downward to G3 before shifting upward by octave
to the guitar’s high G4. The music thus embeds a hidden canon or round
endlessly descending by step (Figure 2.5.3). Unlike a traditional round,
however, where the melodic lines are articulated by actual instruments, this
canon is constructed by listeners—supported by rich, strummed guitar
chords that suggest but do not completely determine specific voices. It is we
who connect the D4 in the vocal melody to the subsequent D4 in the guitar,
we who hear the common tones and stepwise motions linking each chord to
the next. Nevertheless these are reasonable connections to make: the
stepwise motions in Figure 2.5.2 are really there in the music, and can be
identified even by the casual listener. (Sing the canonic line along with the
recording and you will realize how obvious it is.) To me this is one of the
most fascinating differences between voice leading in classical music and
rock—in the former, it is embodied in notation while in the latter it is
partially constructed by the audience.21
Figure 2.5.2. Each voice in “Helpless” connects to the next-lowest voice across the repeat.
Figure 2.5.3. “Helpless” embeds a three-voice continuously descending canon. Voices enter every two
measures.
Figure 2.5.4. The canon in “Eight Days a Week.” Voices enter every two measures.
Figure 2.5.5. “You Won’t See Me” explicitly articulates a continuous stepwise descent.
Figure 2.5.7. Three songs in three keys played at the same time without dissonance.
Figure 2.5.8. The fundamental canon of rock. Voices enter every two measures.
One moral is that rock centricity is often a matter of phrasing and musical
emphasis rather than abstract pitch relationships. O-Zone’s “Dragostea Din
Tei” provides a marvelous example, modulating from C major in the chorus
to A minor in the verse without ever departing from its repeating C–G–a–F
loop (to be discussed shortly). This is initially accomplished hypermetrically,
prolonging the G chord for two measures so that the verse begins on A minor
rather than F major (16 bars into the music, at about 0'30"). The remaining
modulations operate entirely by orchestration, the bass synthesizers dropping
out a bar before each section change; their return creates a musical accent
which, along with the melody, helps shift the sense of tonal center back and
forth between A minor and C major, even while preserving the four-bar
harmonic rhythm. These modulations suggest that centricity can be a matter
of delicate hints rather than unmistakable signposts.26
Our progression C–G–D–A–(C), with its three ascending fifths and one
ascending minor third, is cousin to the most famous minor third in all of
music theory, Rameau’s double emploi. Rameau interpreted functional
harmony using a sequence of four descending diatonic fifths d–G–C–F
whose phrase structure was offset relative to its harmonic structure: C–F–d–
G–(C) rather than d–G–C–F–(d). His “double emploi” is the minor-third
juncture between the ends of the fifth sequence, sometimes elided into a ii65
composite. These two chord loops are retrogrades: three fifths and a minor
third, ascending in rock and descending in functional harmony. This
resemblance underscores the sense in which rock music is genuinely
retrofunctional, its chords inverting classical expectations—not out of
cussedness but for reasons of contrapuntal logic.
By now, readers will be wondering how to fit minor triads into the spiral
model. A first answer is that Figure 2.1.3 can be used to represent any type
of three-note chord in chromatic space: major chords, minor chords,
diminished triads, 026 trichords like C–D–F♯, three-note chromatic clusters,
and so on. (This is why I labeled the figure by root, without specifying the
complete chord type.) That is, its basic principles are completely
independent of a chord’s specific intervallic content, depending only on the
size of the chord and the size of the scale: sliding along the spiral transposes
along the scale, while looping around the space transposes along the chord,
so that moving radially outward, in any twelve-tone scale, combines
transposition down one chordal step with transposition up four chromatic
semitones (t–1T4). For the C minor triad, radial motion corresponds to the
voice leading (C, E♭, G) → (B, E, G); for an 026 trichord it is (C, D, F♯)→
(A♯, E, F♯); and for a chromatic cluster it is (C4, D♭4, D4) → (F♯3, E4,
F4).27 (Inward radial motion is the inverse, transposing one step upward
along the chord and four semitones chromatically downward.) In each case,
radial motion involves the same total amount of upward and downward
semitonal motion, so that the sum of the paths in all voices is zero semitones.
This universality is one of the most remarkable features of the geometry, a
single n-in-o graph describing any n-note chord in any o-note scale.
There is, however, one important difference between these various spiral
diagrams: the closer the chord is to the augmented triad, the tighter the
connection between clockwise motion and descending stepwise voice
leading. Figure 2.6.1 distorts augmented triads to obtain the 025 trichords on
the spiral diagram. With a nearly even chord, like the major or minor triad,
we do not need to distort the augmented triad very much, and the result is
smooth descending voice leading (e.g., Figure 2.1.7). The 025 triad requires
a larger amount of distortion, and this makes for a larger amount of contrary
motion as we move clockwise along the spiral diagram. In the aggregate,
adding the motion in all voices, descent still dominates: each clockwise turn
produces a voice leading whose melodic intervals sum to –3. But as chords
become more and more uneven, the connection to stepwise melodic motion
is less and less clear (appendix 1).
Figure 2.6.1. The position of 025 chords on the spiral diagram.
Of course, we would really like to represent both major and minor chords
at the same time. One way to do this is to superimpose two versions of our
graph on top of each other, as in Figure 2.6.2.28 To use this new figure we
need to know that the direct clockwise path between parallel major and
minor triads is the “parallel” voice leading that alters the chord’s third by
semitone: thus moving clockwise from C major to C minor lowers E to E♭,
Figure 2.6.3. The passacaglia from Shostakovich’s E minor piano trio. The numbers above the figure
show how to interpret these motions on Figure 2.1.3, using a single point to represent major, minor, or
diminished triads with that root. For augmented triads, the three radial points collapse into a single
chord.
In the rest of the chapter, I will mostly follow the strategy of grouping
major and minor together, sacrificing detail in the name of graphical
simplicity; readers who prefer more precision can use Figure 2.6.2 instead.
Chapter 3 will introduce the diatonic analogues of these structures, in which
major and minor triads appear as transpositionally related species of the
same genus, the diatonic triad. This provides yet a third strategy for
simultaneously modeling major and minor triads.
7. A fourth family
Figure 2.7.1. “Hey Joe.” Despite the ascending semitone B–C, we can hear a canonic descent E–D–
C♯–B–(C), with the melodic E descending to the inner-voice C across the repeat.
Figure 2.7.2 shows some of the many songs that can be derived from this
pattern by eliminating chords and replacing major with minor. These are
some of the most important progressions in all of rock, including i–VI–VII–i
and a–F–C–G, which can appear either as i–VI–III–VII in A minor or I–V–
vi–IV in C major.32 Since the outer chords in the five-chord sequence
occupy the same angular position, they are linked by symmetrical voice
leading in which one note ascends and one note descends; from the
standpoint of the spiral diagram, neither belongs “before” or “after” the
other. We can therefore exchange them to obtain E–G–D–A–C. Neil Young’s
“Keep on Rockin’ in the Free World” uses the progression’s three-chord
subset e–D–C in its verse and the four-chord e–G–D–C in the chorus, the
minor tonic creating a completely diatonic progression while allowing for
stepwise descent from VI to i (Figure 2.7.3). Another four-chord subset, E–
G–A–C, appears in Dave van Ronk’s version of “House of the Rising Sun,”
to be discussed in §2.10. Boyce and Hart’s “Steppin’ Stone,” recorded by
Paul Revere and the Raiders, uses van Ronk’s progression in the context of
an explicit and continuous melodic descent: the backing melody 8̂– ♭ 7̂–6̂–
♭6̂ connects smoothly to the main vocal 5̂–4̂–♭3̂–1̂, forming a descending
octave (Figure 2.7.4).33 Interestingly, the melodic composite articulates the
canon implicit in the version of the progression that has a minor tonic; it is
not inconceivable that the writers heard this line in the progression, writing
the melody in the way a listener might track the Shepard-tone passacaglia in
the final product. From this perspective, the melody’s G–e descent, rather
than a departure from a stepwise norm, is faithful to the progression’s inner
logic.
Figure 2.7.2. Progressions in the “Hey Joe” family. “Heart-Shaped Box” is starred because its
progression usually features ascending voice leading.
Figure 2.7.3. Neil Young’s “Keep on Rockin’ in the Free World” uses subsequences of the progression
e–G–D–A–C.
Figure 2.7.4. The vocal parts in “Steppin’ Stone” articulate a continuous stepwise descent, while the
harmonies present a four-chord subsequence of e–G–D–A–C. The vocal pitches are implied by the
triads’ descending voice leading.
8. Other modalities
Figure 2.8.1 shows the spooky prayer near the end of Heinrich Schütz’s
setting of Psalm 84 (“Wie lieblich sind deine Wohnungen”), the eighth of his
Psalmen Davids (1619, SWV 29). It is an intensely powerful rendering of
the Psalm’s most direct address to God, eleven consecutive root-position
triads of uncertain tonal provenance—the unmetered homophonic chant a
sharp contrast with the preceding polyphony. When I first encountered this
music, I heard resonances with rock that led me to gently question my sanity.
Why should Schütz’s early seventeenth-century composition remind me of
Jimi Hendrix or Aretha Franklin? It took some time before I was ready to
accept these resonances as something more than accidental; and it took
longer still before I was confident enough to imagine that they might reflect
deep musical values—a shared interest in closely related harmonies and
stepwise descending melody. I now think it is not so surprising that we
should find resemblances here: rock musicians and early seventeenth-
century composers both operated in a context where functional-harmonic
constraints were comparatively weak, available options rather than
inviolable laws. In such environments, musicians are free to explore the
fundamental geometry of triadic voice leading, and in particular the
connection between descending voice leading and retrofunctional harmony.
Figure 2.8.1. “Wie lieblich sind deine Wohnungen,” mm. 136–146.
Figure 2.8.4. A retrofunctional sequence of ascending fifths at the end of Bach’s “Es woll uns Gott
genädig sein” (BWV 311, Riemenschneider 16).
9. Function and retrofunction
One of the most interesting features of rock is the coexistence of modal and
functional harmony. Songs like “Sympathy for the Devil” and “I Can’t
Explain” juxtapose mixolydian I– ♭ VII–IV–I verses with functional
dominants in their choruses. The Beatles’ “With a Little Help from My
Friends” does something similar, beginning with ascending fifths supporting
a two-voice compound melody (Figure 2.9.1).38 The retrofunctional fifths
then reverse direction to produce a functional ii–V7–I progression, now
supporting 2̂–3̂ in the melody. In the figure, I interpret this 2̂–3̂ as moving
between voices, with the arrival on 1̂ implied but not explicitly stated. (The
tonic scale degree arrives in measure 3 but without harmonic support, while
the accompaniment clearly states the 4̂–3̂ descent.) The chorus harmonizes a
strong 2̂–1̂ with an archetypal rock-mixolydian progression, ♭ VII–IV–I.
This ascending-fifth pattern then shifts up by fifth, from ♭VII–IV–I to IV–I–
V, arriving on a functional half-cadence with the guitar part recapitulating
the phrase’s 5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂ descent. The verse’s three-chord units thus alternate
ABAB between function and retrofunction. In the second verse, this ABAB
alternation is highlighted by the vocals, with Ringo Starr taking the modal
unit (I–V–ii) and the background vocals singing the functional unit (ii–V7–
I). This association continues through the bridge, where the background
singers ask a functional question (vi–V/V) which the lead vocal answers
retrofunctionally (I–♭VII–IV). The contrast between harmonic styles is thus
reinforced by the orchestration.
Figure 2.9.1. The Beatles’ “With a Little Help from My Friends” alternates functional and
retrofunctional motion.
“In rock music the harmonies tend to go backwards, except when they
don’t.” This sounds like a parody of untestable theorizing . . . yet it is
entirely possible that rock music does in fact juxtapose distinct harmonic
subsystems, one functional and one modal, each with its own characteristic
repertoire of harmonic moves.39 Insofar as the systems are kept separate,
occurring for instance in different sections of a song, then it is not
unreasonable to declare the music to be harmonically polyvalent. Such
juxtapositions have been dubbed “code switching” by Richard Cohn,
borrowing a term from linguistics to describe nineteenth-century
chromaticism.40 Just as bilingual speakers can move smoothly between two
different languages, so too do popular musicians shift between the default
voice leading of the modal system and more conventionalized functional
routines. To be sure, the line between languages can vary from individual to
individual: to my ears, tunes like “With a Little Help from My Friends”
really do move back and forth between distinct harmonic styles, but for
others the song may be unified creole, a coherent fusion of elements that are
no longer distinct.
I hear a similar sort of code switching in a piece such as Thomas Morley’s
“April Is in My Mistress’ Face” (1596), whose 38 measures are divided into
four short sections, each setting a single line of text. The opening is strongly
functional, with paired voices articulating T–(S)–D–T progressions that
would be unobjectionable in the high baroque (Figure 2.9.2).41 The second
phrase shifts to B ♭ , its ascending-fourth progressions including a series of
ascending leading tones. The third phrase is homophonic and declamatory,
starting with primary triads in B ♭ major and shifting to G minor as the
thought of fall is introduced. Here it is possible to perceive the major/minor
opposition as carrying some of the “happy/sad” valence it acquires later.42
Figure 2.9.2. Thomas Morley’s “April Is in My Mistress’ Face” begins functionally, but ends
retrofunctionally.
Figure 2.9.3. The end of Morley’s piece is based on a diatonic version of the descending 6–5
decoration, embellished with a voice exchange. The voice exchange highlights the canon in the outer
voices.
The connection between early music and rock is in one sense obvious: bands
had names like “Renaissance,” wrote lyrics about knights and royalty, and
played folksongs like “Scarborough Fair.” Amidst political turmoil, an
idealized past provided an alternative to an unsatisfactory present, with
modal harmony its sonic emblem. This was clear to conservative musicians,
who largely rejected modality in favor of functional harmony—to the point
where one can have a pretty good sense, even before listening, of the
harmonic vocabulary of a song like “The Ballad of the Green Berets” or
“Stand by Your Man.” What results is a strange historical inversion whereby
an older modal practice was associated with progressive politics while newer
functionality symbolized traditional values.
When I was young, I assumed the triadic modality of rock was linked by a
continuous tradition to the modality of the Renaissance. Surely, I imagined,
people never stopped strumming dorian and mixolydian tunes in informal
venues such as pubs and homes, uncorrupted by the learned tradition. What
happened in the 1960s, I imagined, was simply that this marginalized and
ubiquitous musical tradition was raised to public consciousness. But having
looked for evidence of this continuity, I find myself much less convinced
than I used to be. American and English folk recordings made before 1950
tend to be harmonically spare, featuring either unaccompanied melodies,
drones, or perhaps oscillating between two chords. It is surprisingly difficult
to find recordings that are (a) clearly modal, (b) made before 1950, and (c)
feature three or more distinct triads.47 Insofar as these recordings are
representative, folk modality tends toward monody.
Furthermore, the picture of folksongs as rigid vessels for modal practice is
at odds with the extraordinary flexibility of the vernacular tradition:
surveying early twentieth-century recordings, one finds the same tunes
appearing in different forms—changing modes and time signatures, and
acquiring a variety of harmonizations. Consider the earliest recording of
“The House of the Rising Sun,” made for Vocalion in 1933 by the
Appalachian musicians Clarence (“Tom”) Ashley and Gwen Foster, under
the name “Rising Sun Blues.” The piece, transcribed in Figure 2.10.1, is a
sixteen-bar major-mode blues with an ABBC structure, a standard twelve-
bar blues that repeats its middle four bars. As such it is largely functional,
oscillating between I and IV before reaching the V–I cadence. Its most
unusual chord—a iv2 that occurs under a melodic ♭ 7̂ not present in the
harmony—can perhaps be seen as a minor subdominant (an instance of
“modal mixture”) though it also has an affinity with the VI that appears in
later versions of the tune. (In some verses of the Ashley/Foster recording,
this iv2 moves to V43 on beat three.) A 1935 variant by Homer Callahan
(under the name “Rounder’s Luck”) has simple i–V–i harmony, while other
versions are monophonic (Georgia Turner, 1937), or blues-based (Roy Acuff
and His Smoky Mountain Boys, 1938). These performances are considerably
more functional than most of the songs in this chapter, and indeed a later
recording by Ashley and Doc Watson sands down the rough edges of the first
version, softening the iv2 and hewing more closely to blues convention.
Figure 2.10.1. Ashley and Foster’s 1933 recording of “The House of the Rising Sun.”
Figure 2.10.2. Bob Dylan’s 1962 harmonization of “The House of the Rising Sun.” The melody is an
idealization that does not try to capture the nuances of Dylan’s performance.
Figure 2.10.3. Gordon Heath and Lee Payant’s 1955 version of “Scarborough Fair.”
Such examples suggest that, in the absence of recordings, folk songs are
an unlikely vehicle for the faithful transmission of modal harmony. Instead
of fixed and unchanging songs we find endless interpretations, united chiefly
by lyrics and secondarily by melody. As far as I can tell, early twentieth-
century American and English folk music is if anything more functional than
its descendants: triadic modality, with its blatantly nonfunctional
progressions, is largely a phenomenon of the late 1950s and 1960s, not
obviously continuing any widespread folk practice.53 In this sense, the
modality of the 1960s is best understood as the delayed echo of modernism,
popular musicians rejecting functionality a half-century after classical
composers did.
Thus if there are resonances between Renaissance and rock, this is likely
due to intrinsic triadic logic rather than actual historical connection. What
sustains the modal “state of nature,” what leads to its reappearance in
disconnected eras, is that it offers straightforward solutions to basic musical
problems—stepwise descending melodies and closely related triads. In other
words, it is a “syntax” supported not by convention but by constraints
inherent in our musical materials. Musicians need not be aware of these
constraints to be bound by them; they simply need to be interested in writing
music that feels right (with their intuitions prioritizing stepwise melody,
diatonically close harmonies, and so forth). By analyzing this feeling of
rightness we can transform the implicit into the explicit, revealing hidden
roads connecting different musical languages. If we are lucky, this new
understanding will suggest new compositional possibilities as well.
1
This asymmetry can also be found in birdsong and human speech, suggesting a potential origin in
respiration, the sharp intake and gradual release of breath required for both speaking and singing. See
Tierney, Russo, and Patel 2008 (music and speech), Tierney, Russo, and Patel 2011 (the “motor
constraint hypothesis”), and Savage, Tierney, and Patel 2017 (birdsong and music).
2
For the terms “strong” and “weak,” see Meeùs 2000. Stephenson 2002 makes a similar
observation about the prevalence of weak progressions in rock, though he explains the phenomenon
differently.
3
In general, the initial direction is important. If you want a short clockwise path from C to F, you
need to start by sliding clockwise; the algorithm in this paragraph will produce the path T5t–1. If you
were to instead slide counterclockwise, the algorithm will calculate the shortest counterclockwise path
from C to F, T–7t1; this is a different but equally important voice leading. For a purely radial path the
two directions produce the same result.
4
The “LP” voice leading moves a major triad’s root down by semitone and fifth up by semitone;
“PL” moves a major triad’s third down by semitone and fifth up by semitone. I call this “the major-
third system” (Tymoczko 2011a). For a comparison between voice-leading geometry and neo-
Riemannian theory see appendix C of Tymoczko 2011a and Tymoczko 2020b.
5
As appendix 1 explains, this notation means “C moves to B,” “E moves to E,” and “G moves to
G♯.”
6
Camp 2018.
7
Yust 2015a explores distorted and quantized sequences.
8
See Tymoczko 2011a.
9
To be sure, there are some styles, like heavy metal, that emphasize these diatonically distant
progressions precisely for their ominous and unusual quality; we will ignore this “contratonal” music
for the time being. For “contratonality” see Huron 2007a, p. 339.
10
Of course, many rock musicians have absorbed this norm through musical training as well.
11
De Clerq et al. 2011. See also Figure 3.4 in Temperley 2018. Pinter 2019 surveys the early
history of ♭VII in popular music.
12
Note that there is not a single tritone progression on the list—and that there is a huge gap
between the diatonic intervals of a fifth and major second, and the rest. The relatively greater
popularity of fifths over major seconds, which both have diatonic distance 0, is likely explained by the
longstanding norm of fifth motion. A secondary factor may be the fact that fifth-related triads are
diatonically “superclose” since each pair belongs to two different diatonic collections, whereas major-
second-related pairs belong to just a single collection.
13
Stephenson 2002 notes that the chord progressions in rock music are the opposite of those in
classical music, and suggests that this is the explanation for their popularity: rock harmony, in his
view, is fundamentally oppositional, a matter of doing what was not done in earlier styles. My
explanation instead proposes that rock musicians are making affirmative use of the possibilities
afforded by deep musical relationships; if anything, rock harmony is more intuitive than classical
harmony. Their oppositional quality is more byproduct than motivation—though unfamiliarity is likely
part of their attraction.
14
See §3.9 for more on the origins of this progression. It represents a popular-music instance of
what Samarotto 2004 describes as the “sublimating” of ♯ 4̂.
15
“When they clean the street, I’ll be the only shit that’s left behind.”
16
Rowe 1983.
17
My argument is not that ♭III, V, and VII are harmonically similar, the way dominant chords like
V and vii° are; instead, they are contrapuntally similar in the sense that any of them can be inserted
between I and ♭ VII without disrupting the stepwise descending voice leading. For the claim that
major-third relations can play similar harmonic functions, see Cohn 1999; this view, as Cohn notes, is
cousin to Lendvai’s minor-third-based “axis system” (§3.8).
18
Temperley (2018, p. 35) surveys the many writers who have commented on the dual meanings of
this progression.
19
See Moore 2012, p. 85, on “open” and “closed” phrases.
20
In the Eagles’ “Hotel California,” the descending 6–5 moves from tonic to dominant in tonally
functional fashion. Temperley 2018, p. 57, notes the descending chromatic line. One can interpret this
as a variant of Gesualdo’s trick where a single voice moves chromatically while the other two alternate
between root position and first inversion.
21
The composer Paul Lansky has spoken about composing so as to allow the listener to chart their
own way through a piece (Perry and Lansky 1996).
22
Feurzeig 2010 cites examples from Miles Davis (“Blue in Green”) to Philip Glass (the
“Spaceship” harmonies from Einstein on the Beach) and Ligeti (the fourth movements of the Horn
Trio and Violin Concerto), including the Shostakovich passage analyzed in §2.6.
23
There are various other ways to characterize this class of progressions, for instance as those
involving a diatonic distance less than 6, or those whose roots lie in a pentatonic scale (Biamonte
2010).
24
See Temperley 2011b. In rock, IV chords are almost as likely to go to I as V chords are. The Alt-
J song “(Interlude 3)” from An Awesome Wave presents a four-chord ascending-fifth sequence F–C–
G–D, but I hear G and not F as the tonic despite the hypermetric accent on F.
25
This is a picture that can be perceived either as a duck or a rabbit, but not both at the same time.
26
One can make a similar point about Boston’s “Peace of Mind,” which may seem to shift into C♯
minor at the start of the chorus despite its generally E major tonality. See Harrison 2016, pp. 70–72
(on “Cherry Cherry”), Doll 2017, and Nobile 2020.
27
In this last example, I use pitches to show how voices move, as discussed in appendix 1.
28
This graph is similar to familiar music-theoretical constructions such as the Tonnetz and
Douthett and Steinbach’s “Cube Dance” (1998). Here we reconstruct these graphs by superimposing
spiral diagrams (appendix 1).
29
To determine this, we can ask what spacing-preserving voice leading from C major to C minor
moves its voices by intervals collectively summing to –1; see Tymoczko 2020b.
30
As before, different paths between the same points represent different voice leadings: the
counterclockwise path from C major to E minor, for example, corresponds to the voice leading (C, E,
G) → (E, G, B).
31
The song is credited to Billy Roberts but likely derives from Niela Miller’s 1955 “Baby, Please
Don’t Go to Town.”
32
These and many other progressions are discussed in Doll 2017, an extensive survey of rock
harmony. Richards 2017 focuses on the “axis progression” a–F–C–G.
33
Minor Threat’s version expresses the continuity between these two vocal lines, the single
vocalist alternating between them.
34
See Cohn 1996, 1999, 2012, Tymoczko 2011a. Agmon’s “within-key chromaticism” (2020) is
very close to my own.
35
For an insightful treatment of Grieg’s late style, see Taylor 2019.
36
The chord sequence C–G–F–C, with descending voice leading, evokes the “fauxbourdon rule-of-
the-octave” idiom, I–V6–IV6–I64, though here it occurs over a different bass and in the wrong key
(§7.5).
37
Bach frequently modulates from major to relative minor by a sequence of ascending fifths
(§7.7). The bass here instantiates something very close to Byros’s “le–sol–fi–sol” schema (2012) but
on the wrong scale degrees.
38
Steve Forbert’s “Goin’ Down to Laurel,” from Alive on Arrival, uses the same progression.
39
Many other theorists have commented on rock harmony’s polystylistic qualities, including
Everett 2004, Osborn 2017, and Temperley 2018 (pp. 54ff).
40
Cohn 2012.
41
The F♮ in m. 9 provides a hint of modality.
42
See Meier 1988, Part II, chapter 7.
43
The voice leading recalls that of mm. 10–11 in Wagner’s Tristan, which is (C, F, A♭, D) → (B,
F♯, A, D♯) when we remove crossings. Here, the A♭→A voice becomes G♯→B.
44
The parallel thirds appear in the first sequence’s alto and tenor (disrupted by the substitution of
6
I for the iii) and as sixths in the second sequence’s soprano and tenor.
45
Note the ubiquitous parallel tenths between outer voices.
46
Ewell (2020) has emphasized that this focus on functional harmony is also a reflection of elitist,
ethnocentric, and racist forces.
47
There are a number of continuous modal traditions that are unlikely to have served as a historical
bridge between Renaissance and rock. Gregorian chant is one. Irish folk music is another, with modal
elements present from the transcriptions of Edward Bunting (1773–1843) to the recordings of the early
twentieth century.
48
Rings (2013) notes a similar progression in the work of Len Chandler, an oboist in the Akron
Orchestra, who would likely have been familiar with the learned modality of the early twentieth
century. See Grieg’s Elegie (§8.7) for the sort of descending bass line that might be the ancestor of
Dylan’s lament.
49
Wagner 2003.
50
Later uses of the progression, such as “Stairway to Heaven,” further detach it from its origins in
the lament: here, the i–III64–IV6–VImaj7 returns to i, decorating the idiomatic modal-rock VI–VII–i
rather than the functional-harmonic i–V–i.
51
Compare measure 3 of Figure 7.2.6.
52
Kloss 2012 presents a thorough archaeology of the song, showing that both major and minor
variants have an extensive history. The earliest published transcriptions resembling the standard
modern version appear in the very late 1870s, though in aeolian rather than dorian.
53
We see a similar progression when we consider other folk traditions, such as shape-note
polyphony: the clear modality of its eighteenth-century tunes is gradually regularized and made more
functional in the nineteenth century, returning only as twentieth-century practitioners become more
interested in historical authenticity. Thanks here to Rachel Hall.
Prelude
The Tinctoris Transform
Figure P3.3. Efficient voice leading minimizes the change in a chord’s center of gravity. The example
presents dyadic, triadic, and seventh-chord progressions in chromatic space; above each I identify the
change in center of gravity, equivalent to the average of the melodic motions in all voices.
Figure P3.4. The diatonic consonances spanning an octave or less, arranged from small to large.
Composers often move along the arrows.
They are also manifest in the structure of our geometrical models, where
the amount of angular motion corresponds to the change in center of gravity.
For example, on the 3-in-12 diagram (Figure 2.1.3), radial motion from C
major to E major lowers the root by semitone while raising the fifth by
semitone; since the ascending and descending semitones cancel, center of
gravity is preserved. In general, two chords will have the same angular
position when they can be linked by voice leading in which the change in
center of gravity due to looping (transposition along the chord) exactly
counteracts the change in center of gravity due to sliding (transposition
along the scale): in this case T4 raises the center of gravity by one third of an
octave, while t–1 lowers it by that same amount. Readers who look ahead to
Figures 4.2.7 and 3.8.4 will see that tritone-related perfect fifths occupy the
same radial position in 2-in-12 space, while minor-third-related seventh
chords occupy the same radial position in 4-in-12 space; this is the
geometrical origin of the idioms in the previous paragraph.
Efficient voice leading also requires that vertical configurations
themselves change by only a small amount. Figure P3.4 arranges the
diatonic consonances by size. Two-voice consonant counterpoint tends to
move by short distances on this graph: in the two-voice passages of
Palestrina’s masses, for example, about 90% of the fifths move to sixths or
thirds, while about 80% of the unisons move to thirds.6 (Note that this claim
implicitly uses the new coordinate system: to say “fifths often move to
sixths” is to say that voices configured as a fifth often move so that they end
up configured as a sixth.) By contrast, imperfect consonances often move to
other imperfect consonances by parallel or contrary motion. Putting these
points together, we can formulate a useful rule of thumb: if you are on a
perfect consonance, move along Figure P3.4 to a nearby imperfect
consonance (or, less likely, jump over an imperfect consonance to reach the
nearest perfect consonance); if you are on an imperfect consonance, move
about half the time along the dotted lines to another imperfect consonance,
and otherwise to a nearby perfect consonance. This positive counterpoint
gives musicians an affirmative goal, a set of plausible options going beyond
a negative approach built on prohibitions. It aims to capture the intuitive
knowledge of a composer who knows what to do next: “if I am located at
this point in musical space, then my most obvious destinations are . . .” Such
knowledge becomes increasingly important as additional voices bring more
contrapuntal options, or when composers are constrained by harmonic rules.
A second advantage of the configurational perspective is that it helps us
focus on structural relationships that are independent of transposition; in this
respect it is the earliest forerunner of what we have come to call musical set
theory.7 Two progressions can be said to belong to the same category if they
exemplify the same series of vertical configurations, differing only in
transpositional level: “an octave followed by a major sixth,” “a major triad
in 53 position followed by a minor triad in 64 position,” “a root-position
half-diminished seventh in close position followed by a root-position
dominant seventh in close position,” and so on. In 1614, Thomas Campion
used this idea to categorize four-voice voice leadings between root-position
triads, extending the Tinctorian perspective from two voices to four (Figure
P3.5).8 In A Geometry of Music I said that such progressions are individually
transpositionally related; here I will say they are related by the Tinctoris
Transform.
Figure P3.5. Two of Thomas Campion’s contrapuntal categories, along with his notation of the shared
vertical configurations. The arrows and T labels are my own, showing that progressions within each
category are related by the independent transposition of each chord.
Paths on our spiral diagrams are related by the Tinctoris Transform if they
differ only in the amount of sliding involved; this is true whether we are
considering a simple diagram representing only the transpositions of a single
chord type or a more complex diagram superimposing multiple chord types.
By ignoring slides, we can group together a large number of superficially
different possibilities, revealing similarities that may not be intuitively
obvious. The 3-in-12 diagram of chromatic major triads, for example,
contains only three fundamental kinds of motion, the clockwise loops of 0,
1, or 2 turns (t0, t–1, t–2); all other motions can be obtained by adding slides
to these (Figure P3.6). This sort of simplification can be analytically useful:
for example, the three canonic sequences in Figure P3.7 repeatedly apply a
transformation of the form Txt1, differing only in the amount of sliding
involved. While they might initially seem unrelated, they are similar from
the configurational point of view.
Figure P3.6. Voice leadings from the central C major triad to triads on each line differ only in their
transpositional component.
Figure P3.7. Three canonic sequences that repeatedly apply the same voice-leading pattern, differing
only in their diatonic transposition.
Figure P3.8. Approaching perfect and imperfect consonances in Ockeghem, Josquin, and Palestrina.
Values are percentages, with each column adding up to 100 (within rounding).
Figure P3.9. (left) An abstract diagram relating a dyad’s change in center of gravity to its overall size.
When the change is zero, the voice leading involves perfectly contrary motion and the overall voice-
leading size is minimized; voices move in a balanced way by approximately the same distance. As we
increase the change, the melodic intervals become more unbalanced, until we reach oblique motion,
where all the movement is in one voice; beyond this, we have similar motion where the overall size of
the voice leading increases. (The line of obliqueness is half the change in interval size.) (right) A
concrete example using imperfect consonances. Underneath each progression, I count the total number
of melodic steps in all voices.
1
Since the reference voice has position 0, the two systems both use n changing numbers to label
the notes of an n-note chord. We could also measure the configuration’s location using the sum of the
chord’s pitches instead of their average.
2
Transposition is a structure-preserving perceptual symmetry, much as “translation” (physical
movement) is a symmetry of 3D space. By factoring out transposition and translation, we can simplify
musical or physical problems.
3
Tinctoris measures an interval’s location relative to the tenor (his “reference” voice), rather than
the interval’s center of gravity; this difference is not fundamental, as the location of the tenor can be
computed from the center of gravity and vice versa.
4
Lewis 1964, p. 10.
5
These root motions correspond to 12/n, where 12 is the size of the octave and n is the size of the
chord. As we move away from these optimal intervals, we increase the size of the resulting voice
leading.
6
Motion from unison to unison, fifth to fifth, or octave to octave will produce parallel perfect
intervals so long as the center of gravity changes.
7
See Nolan 2003 and Schuijer 2008 for histories of set theory that begin more recently.
8
This example comes from Schubert 2018, who hypothesizes that Thomas Tallis may have used it
in composing his 1570 Spem in Alium. I think we can find evidence for the strategy throughout the
sixteenth century, most obviously in those repertoires that make extensive use of close-position root-
position triads (§6.1).
9
Here the reasoning is more complicated. The prohibition on voice overlap dictates that large
melodic intervals be harmonized not with near-parallel motion but significant changes of spacing.
These changes of spacing increase voice independence and are often, but not always, more efficient
than the near-parallel alternatives.
10
For example, Laukens 2019 or Levine 1989.
11
This amounts to discarding some of the information contained in the relative configuration, for
instance by treating its numbers as unordered and conceiving them modulo the octave. Morris 1995
incorporates both pitch and pitch-class information.
3
Line and Configuration
Figure 3.1.2. Moving one clockwise step from C to F combines T–4 and t1. The third moves up by
step and the root moves down by third.
Figure 3.1.3. A series of clockwise moves in the space produces a descending-fifth progression in
which thirds and sixths alternate.
There are two nonobvious ways to analyze motion along the diagram. The
first is to assign every path a number corresponding to the angular distance
traveled, or, equivalently, the number of basic voice leadings required to
generate that path. Since there is only one chord at every angular coordinate,
radial motion is completely determined by angular motion; as we will see,
this allows us to collapse the spiral into a circle. Alternatively, we can adopt
the Tinctorian strategy of decomposing voice leadings into combinations of
transposition along both chord and scale, or loops and slides. From this
perspective, there are just two basic possibilities, corresponding to those that
transpose along the chord and those that do not; these use an odd or even
number of basic voice leadings, respectively. Within each category, voice
leadings differ only by a change in center of gravity, controlled by the scalar
transposition Tx (Figure 3.1.4).
Figure 3.1.4. The voice leadings along the dyadic circle expressed in musical notation; the top staff
applies an odd number of basic voice leadings, combining transposition along the scale with one-step
transposition along the dyad (t1); the bottom staff applies an even number of basic voice leadings and
involves no transposition along the dyad.
Figure 3.1.6. The start of the last movement of Beethoven’s E♭ major piano sonata, Op. 27, no. 1.
The dyadic logic of the 2-in-7 spiral interacts with the triadic system in
complex ways. In the descending voice leading the initial dyad is almost
always the root and third of a triad, while the second dyad can either be
root/third or third/fifth, producing authentic and deceptive resolutions
respectively (Figure 3.1.7). A passing tone is often inserted into the melodic
third to create a resolving tritone. The ascending-fifth voice leading embeds
into a wider range of progressions; here again a passing tone yields 6̂–7̂–1̂
against 4̂–3̂, combining plagal root motion with an authentic resolution of
the tritone (7̂–1̂/4̂–3̂). In baroque music this figure is often harmonized as
shown in the middle column of Figure 3.1.7, producing a pair of eighth-note
harmonies over a stable bass (ii–vii°6 or IV–vii°64).4 The rightmost column
instead supports the voice leading with a changing bass, an option that is
increasingly preferred as functional harmony develops. The contrast between
these two approaches reflects a shift from a dyadic-contrapuntal conception
of functionality, where linear patterns are predominant, to a later and more
triadic orientation.
Figure 3.1.7. Triadic harmonizations of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading. The descending form
typically appears as root and third of the initial triad (top line, “1–3”), moving either to root-third or
third-fifth of a second triad. The ascending form can also appear as third-fifth of the initial triad
(bottom line, “3–5”). In both cases, a passing tone can create a resolving tritone. The rightmost
column harmonizes the dyadic progression with a change of bass.
Figure 3.1.8. The second theme of the first movement of Beethoven’s Op. 23 violin sonata. While the
root progressions are mysterious, the sequence of basic voice leadings is systematic, ascending by
fifth, descending, and then ascending again.
Figure 3.1.9. The opening of the middle section of the second mazurka in Clara Wieck Schumann’s
Soirées Musicales, Op. 6. The top staff shows various appearances of the ascending and descending
basic voice leadings, designated by open and closed noteheads respectively. The annotations indicate
whether the basic voice leading appears as the bottom (b) or top (t) third of the triad.
Figure 3.1.10. Common schemas and patterns analyzed as paths on the imperfect consonance’s spiral
diagram.
2. Voice exchanges
Any voice leading can be decomposed into two parts: a crossing-free
component that preserves spacing as measured along the intrinsic scale and a
voice exchange rearranging the notes of a single chord, with paths
collectively summing to 0.8 (As we will see, this factoring of voice leadings
into voice exchanges and a crossing-free residue is a point of contact
between Schenkerian theory and voice-leading geometry.) The diatonic third
has two elementary voice exchanges, a small one moving each voice by two
steps (third down to root and root up to third), and a large one moving each
voice by five steps (third up to root and root down to third). More
complicated voice leadings—for instance, keeping the third fixed while
moving the root up by octave—can be generated from these two, perhaps in
combination with one or more transpositions along the chord.9 Figure 3.2.1
uses curved arrows to represent the elementary voice exchanges. This new
geometry can represent every two-voice voice leading connecting any
imperfect consonances in the same diatonic collection.
Figure 3.2.1. Curved arrows represent voice exchanges: dotted and solid lines are the two- and five-
step exchanges, c0 and c1.
We can also apply the two-step voice exchange separately from the basic
voice leading to produce the sequence in Figure 3.2.3. Here the voices move
apart in near contrary motion, the bass descending by thirds while the top
voice alternates steps and thirds. Figure 3.2.4 shows the opening of the last
movement of Beethoven’s A♭ major piano sonata, Op. 26. The music begins
with the basic voice leading in a descending-fifth sequence that continues
across the textural change. (Here the schema appears in the context of
shifting scales tonicizing ii and vi, adding chromatic variety without
disrupting the contrapuntal logic.) Beethoven follows this with the two-step
voice exchange, moving the hands apart so that the tenor plays A♭ while the
top voice plays C; the basic voice leading then produces F–D, or vii°6/V, at
which point the pattern restarts between alto and bass under an upper-voice
pedal. The opening music now returns but with the two hands in contrary
motion—vertical dyads cycling through the two-step voice exchange, the
“antiparallel” voice leading, the five-step voice exchange, and a second
antiparallel voice leading. Here we have all the imperfect system’s atomic
moves: the basic voice leading, the two voice exchanges, and their
combination. This fusion of contrapuntal and harmonic is typical of
Beethoven, its joyous simplicity laying bare the fundamental mechanisms of
functional coherence.
Figure 3.2.3. Alternating the basic voice leading and the two-step voice exchange leads to a wedge
sequence in which harmonies move by fifth.
Figure 3.2.4. The start of the last movement of Beethoven’s A♭ major piano sonata, Op. 26.
3. Other intervals
Chapter 2 showed that a single graph can be used to represent voice-leading
relationships among the chromatic transpositions of any three-note chord.
The same is true of the other spiral diagrams: to generate Figure 3.1.1 we
need only the size of the chord, which tells us how many times our “line of
transposition” wraps around the circle, and the size of the scale, which tells
us how many equally spaced points to place on the line. This means that the
graph represents any two-note interval in any seven-note scale: no matter
what scale we choose, we can label its notes with the letters A–G, redefining
the familiar names to refer to the notes of our new scale, and no matter what
interval we choose, we can form a descending “basic voice leading”
combining four-step descending transposition along the scale with one-step
ascending transposition along the chord, or T–4t1 (Figure 3.3.1). Though
superficially different, these “basic voice leadings” move their voices in
structurally similar ways.
Figure 3.3.1. The basic voice leadings for diatonic fifths, seconds, and unisons. The figure shows how
they all combine four-step transposition downward along the scale with one-step transposition upward
along the chord, or T–4t1.
Figure 3.3.4. A phrase from “Heavenly Spark” represented on the circular space for diatonic fourths.
Figure 3.3.5. The opening of Thelonious Monk’s “Pannonica” decorates a pair of upper voices
forming fourths and fifths (bottom staff).
Beethoven superimposed two basic voice leadings in a musical context,
and we can do the same in the theoretical domain. Figure 3.3.6 combines the
circular spaces for all three diatonic consonances, with unisons and octaves
on the outside, fourths and fifths in the center, and thirds and sixths between
those two. Angular position on this graph represents the sum of the chords’
pitch classes, while radial position mirrors the prelude’s abstract graph of
diatonic consonances; small radial motions thus change the vertical
configurations by small amounts (Figure 3.3.7). This is essentially the
Möbius strip representing all two-note diatonic consonances, reconstructed
as the sum of three separate spiral diagrams.12 Expressed in musical
notation, its basic motions appear as shown in Figure 3.3.8, horizontal lines
embedding the contrary-motion pattern of Figure 1.5.6 (a version of the
“Ludwig” schema [§10.1]) and the vertical direction representing parallel
motion. This is a Tinctorian approach to two-voice counterpoint,
emphasizing parallel and contrary motion rather than the locations of the
individual voices.
Figure 3.3.6. Superimposed spiral diagrams for the three diatonic consonances. Changes in interval
size correspond to radial motion in this space; changes in center of gravity correspond to angular
motion.
Figure 3.3.7. The cross section of dyadic chord space embeds the graph of dyadic consonances from
Figure P3.4.
Figure 3.3.8. Consonant, two-voice diatonic counterpoint decomposed into parallel and contrary
motion. Open noteheads show imperfect consonances.
Figure 3.3.9 presents an even more abstract version of the space, taking
advantage of the fact that diatonic spirals contain exactly one chord at every
angular position: the new space compresses the spiral’s two strands into a
single circle, each angular segment representing a basic voice leading and
switching between the interval’s two inversions (fourth and fifth, third and
sixth, unison and octave). Despite its abstraction, it represents the very same
voice leadings as the earlier spiral diagrams, providing a simple model of
many different intervals (Figure 3.3.10).
Figure 3.3.9. An abstract graph of two-note diatonic consonances. Set class corresponds to radial
position while center of gravity corresponds to angular position.
Figure 3.3.10. Three sequences with descending-fifth root progressions. In the first, the upper voices
move clockwise along the middle ring from FD to GB to EC; in the second they move along the outer
ring from FC to FB to EB. The third mixes these two as shown by the dotted lines.
Figure 3.4.1. The circle of diatonic triads, along with its basic voice leading. Repeated applications of
the basic voice leading pass through each inversion of each triad before returning to the initial
configuration transposed by octave.
Figure 3.4.5. Ockeghem, Gloria from the mass De plus en plus, mm. 127ff. Numbers represent steps
on the triadic spiral diagram (positive counterclockwise, negative clockwise).
Figure 3.4.6. Palestrina, Kyrie from the mass Descendit angelus Domini, mm. 39–40.
Figure 3.4.7. Israel Kamakawiwoʻole’s “Over the Rainbow.”
Two further points about functional harmony: first, the primary triads I,
IV, and V divide the triadic circle nearly evenly, into arcs of length 2, 3, and
2; this means that as a group they collectively minimize common tones,
articulating three maximally distinct harmonic poles. There are two other
nearly even divisions of the circle that contain the tonic triad, I–ii–V–I and
I–IV–vii°–I, both paradigmatic T–S–D–T progressions; this suggests a
geometrical interpretation of harmonic functions as maximally disjoint
regions of triadic space, involving triads with the fewest possible common
tones.19 Second, there is an interesting symmetry between the two fifth-
progressions IV–I and V–I: each has a direct voice leading in which two
voices move stepwise, ascending for the dominant and descending for the
subdominant, and an indirect voice leading in which two voices move by
third and one moves by step (Figure 3.4.10). Both of these indirect voice
leadings can be embellished with passing tones to create a diminished triad,
producing a standard descending-fifth progression and an “ascending
subdominant” respectively. The dualistic symmetry between these two kinds
of fifth motion, and the tension between functional tonality and descending
stepwise voice leading, will be topics for chapter 7.
Figure 3.4.10. Direct and indirect voice leadings for V–I and IV–I progressions.
Figure 3.4.11. An outline of Clara Wieck Schumann’s “Liebst du um Schönheit,” Op. 12.
The answer, I think, is that its basic conceptual structures are neither the
homogenized harmonic patterns of Roman-numeral analysis, nor the equally
homogenized linear patterns of Schenkerian analysis, nor the highly
particular idioms of existing schema theory. Instead, they are somewhere in
between, linear/harmonic hybrids arising from the fundamental geometry of
diatonic objects: a small set of moves on various spiral diagrams. These
hybrid structures are more general than schemas and more specific than
“dominant-tonic progressions” or “parallel motion in tenths”; instead, they
are that subset of contrapuntal possibilities that is compatible with Roman-
numeral harmonic grammar, pruned from the larger vocabulary of earlier
music. A major lesson of voice-leading geometry is that this subset is both
small and surveyable: that the linear drive to write parallel tenths, if it is to
be compatible with functionally harmonic grammar, will manifest itself in a
limited number of ways. These are not idioms so much as possibilities. It is
not that harmony or counterpoint is primary; instead, only a few moves have
both harmonic and contrapuntal virtues.
Figure 3.5.1. Five upper-voice configurations and a diagram of their most common connections.
Figure 3.6.4. A grammar of triadic upper-voice configurations, extended to allow for C↔O
transitions. DI and OO have been relabeled as Unusual (U) to reflect their rarity in Bach.
These rules account for about 85% of the four-voice triadic voice leadings
in the Bach chorales and about 75% of those in the four-voice passages of
Palestrina’s masses. The approach simplifies the long lists of voice leadings
sometimes found in textbooks, allowing students to produce idiomatic part-
writing with a minimum of pain: the focus on upper-voice configurations
helps them avoid parallels, encourages them to think about spacing, and
gives them a positive set of options to pursue.36 They know, for example,
that if they are in H position they will be moving to either C or O on the next
chord; furthermore, they know that it is impossible for upper voices to form
parallel octaves when following these rules, and unlikely for them to form
parallel fifths.37 The rules generalize traditional figured-bass pedagogy,
extending its upper-voice configurations to all possible arrangements
(consistent with the principles of good spacing), and not just those that lie
comfortably under the hand. More importantly, they clarify that changes in
one line have implications for the others, arising from norms of vertical
spacing.
It can be instructive to label vertical configurations in preexisting pieces.
Let us imagine that Bach began composing Figure 3.6.5 with the bass and
harmony—perhaps improvising at the keyboard while paying particular
attention to the outer voices. Now observe that the upper voices use a
default, close-position harmonization right up to the point at which this
would produce parallels (C♯–D in the tenor and bass). Here he shifts to the
half-open position by moving the tenor down one triadic step, to A rather
than D. At this point one might expect him to return to close position, but
given his harmonic choices, this would double the leading tone and produce
an augmented fourth in the tenor. A second half-open voicing would create
parallel octaves while O and UDI are equally problematic; Bach’s best option
is to move the tenor down another triadic step to F ♯ and an open-octave
voicing. At this point, he is free to return to close position, which he does for
the rest of the phrase. So far there is nothing particularly marvelous about
the individual melodies; it is only when Bach seizes the opportunity to add
tenor-voice passing tones that the counterpoint comes alive, transforming an
awkward harmonic skeleton into something attractive. “OUCH theory”
provides a plausible if speculative genealogy of the passage—one that
suggests how an attention to vertical configurations can help create a
genuine counterpoint of melodic lines. My suspicion is that composers rarely
if ever conceive of counterpoint in purely linear terms, always keeping one
eye on the subtle play of vertical configurations.
Figure 3.6.5. A hypothetical derivation of the opening of J. S. Bach’s “Ich dank’ dich, lieber Herr”
(BWV 347, Riemenschneider 2).
OUCH theory works because upper voices often form complete triads
while the bass moves independently. This naturally raises the vexed question
of doublings. Bret Aarden and Paul T. von Hippel have produced an
astonishing survey of pedagogical advice on the subject, considering forty
different texts, all of which seem to disagree.38 A particular focus of
controversy is the permissibility of doubling the bass in first-inversion major
triads such as I6; the question is whether these chords can support upper
voices in C or O position, or whether they should instead use one of the
other configurations. For all the theoretical disagreement, compositional
practice is surprisingly clear: Figure 3.6.7 shows that in Bach’s chorales, the
bass is the most frequently doubled note except when it is a leading tone.39
And in Palestrina’s masses we find something even more remarkable. If we
conceive vertical configurations as combining bass, middle element (a third
or fourth above the bass), and top (fifth or sixth), then Palestrina’s practice is
to double the bass most often, the middle element next, and the top last.40
The major exception is V6, where the leading tone is less likely to be
doubled. Palestrina’s reluctance to double the top voice may reflect its status
as an active or variable note, free to move between fifth and sixth while still
forming a consonance; doubling this note would eliminate that freedom by
turning one of these options into a dissonance.
Figure 3.6.7. Doublings in four-voice passages in J. S. Bach’s chorales and selected Palestrina masses.
Each number is a percentage representing the proportion of chords of that type with the given
doubling.
Figure 3.6.8. Using half-open chords to avoid parallels. In the first measure, upper-voice close-
position triads create parallels between bass and alto; replacing the second close-position triad with a
half-open voicing removes the problem.
Figure 3.7.1. Three progressions grouping voices in different ways. In each case, the top group moves
by T–1 and T1t–1 while the bottom group moves by T–1t1 and T1. Underneath, I analyze these motions
as combining motion of chordal background inside scale, and motion of voices inside chordal
background.
Figure 3.7.5. The opening of Tristan. The lower staff shows a background voice leading with voices
or scale degrees numbered from bottom to top. The melodic “Desire” motif ascends along these scale
degrees, sounding voice 1, voice 2, voice 3, voice 4, and then voice 1 in a higher octave.
Figure 3.7.6. The climax of the Tristan prelude, mm. 77–83. The lowest staff shows the background
against which the outer voices move.
OUCH theory thus treats as universal what is really just one particular
way of organizing musical lines, setting bass against upper voices; this is
why it struggles when voices are divided into pairs or when the soprano is
opposed to the lower voices. With more parts come even more possibilities:
the first forty-voice voice leading in Thomas Tallis’s Spem in alium traces a
very large number of paths from G major to C major (Figure 3.7.7). It would
boggle the mind to try to analyze this progression in forty-note chord space
where every triad appears more than fifty thousand times, each
corresponding to a different doubling. Much more intuitive is the idea that
the forty voices move within a shifting three-note scalar field, pursuing their
own independent combinations of scalar and chordal transposition. As in
Figure 3.7.1, we can imagine harmonies moving in parallel along the scale
(Ty) while voices move inside them (tx).47
Figure 3.7.7. The first forty-voice voice leading in Spem in alium. Boldface numbers show how many
voices sound each interval. The voices together articulate four voice leadings on the triadic circle, two
of which are shown: the clockwise five-step path and the counterclockwise two-step path (labeled –5
and 2 on the figure). The remaining paths, labeled –12 and 9, can be obtained by sliding
counterclockwise and clockwise along the spiral respectively. Together, the four voice leadings
contain all the intervals in which voices move by fifth or less.
There are three basic points here. The first is that as a very general matter,
triadic counterpoint in four or more voices often sets groups of voices
against one another: bass in contrary motion to the upper voices, soprano
and alto against tenor and bass, and so forth. The second point is that over
the course of Western music history composers seem to have become
increasingly aware of the hierarchical possibilities inherent in this
arrangement, writing music in which surface voices move along abstract
backgrounds whose scale degrees themselves articulate nontrivial voice
leadings, a significant musical development occurring within the functional
era. The third point, which will be the focus of chapter 8, is that functional
tonality adds yet another hierarchical level, with voices moving inside scale-
like chords that move within seven-note scales that are themselves
modulating through chromatic space: wheels within wheels, spiral diagrams
nested like the epicycles of Ptolemaic astronomy.
8. Seventh chords
A diatonic seventh chord contains a fifth between root and fifth, and another
fifth between third and seventh. Figure 3.8.1 shows that as we move
clockwise along the spiral, we apply a basic voice leading to each of the
fifth-pairs in turn. It is as if the seventh-chord diagram divides the dyadic
fifth-diagram in half: two steps on the seventh-chord diagram takes one step
on each fifth-pair’s dyadic diagram; where the fifth’s basic voice leading
links fifth-related harmonies, the seventh’s basic voice leading links third-
related harmonies, with the diatonic third being half the diatonic fifth.
Figure 3.8.1. Clockwise one-step motion on the circle of diatonic sevenths produces a sequence of
descending-third progressions in which the seventh descends (T–2t1). Two-step motion produces
descending-fifth progressions (T–4t2, e.g., from Cmaj7 to Fmaj43), while three-step motion produces
ascending steps (T–6t3, Cmaj7 to Dmaj2). The stems show that the four-voice voice leadings can be
factored into two pairs of voices each articulating the diatonic fifth’s basic voice leading.
Figure 3.8.2. “All the Things You Are” outlines the descending-fifth sequence of seventh chords.
Open noteheads show melodic tones.
Figure 3.8.3. “I Will Survive,” written by Freddie Perren and Dino Fekaris and sung by Gloria
Gaynor, also follows the descending-fifth logic. During the C major chord, the melody descends to a
lower voice.
Figure 3.8.5. Motion from diminished to dominant seventh: one counterclockwise step corresponds to
the “common-tone” resolution (+3); in-box clockwise motion preserves three common tones and can
function as vii°7–V7 (–1); one out-of-box clockwise step corresponds to an authentic resolution (–5).
Figure 3.8.6. One-step clockwise motion from one dominant seventh to another. This produces either
an authentic resolution, its tritone substitution, a deceptive motion from V7 to V2/ii, or a descending
major-third progression.
Figure 3.8.10. V/ii acting as a neighboring chord to V7 in Mozart’s C major piano sonata K.309, I,
mm. 3–4.
Figure 3.8.11. Supertonic dominant sevenths moving directly to the tonic in Grieg (Op. 68, no. 4, mm.
52–53) and John Williams (“Hedwig’s Theme” from the Harry Potter movies). In the last example,
the common-tone dominant seventh replaces the cadential dominant.
Figure 3.8.12. Common-tone voice leading over an authentic V–I bass: Beethoven uses this pattern in
Op. 59, no. 1, I, mm. 361–362; in Chopin’s Op. 50, no. 2, mm. 77–79, the common-tone diminished
seventh arrives while the bass is still on 5̂, and is sustained through the tonic arrival.
Figure 3.8.13. Three passages that link common-tone dominant and diminished sevenths, in contexts
that evoke the blues IV7: Scott Joplin’s Pine Apple Rag (1908, mm. 2–4 of the trio); Charles
Trevathan’s racist “Bully Song” (1896, mm. 1–10, harmonizing ♭3 with VI7 rather than IV7); and
Antonio Maggio’s “I Got the Blues” (1908, mm. 8–11).
Figure 3.8.14. A semitonally ascending diminished seventh in Beethoven’s Op. 2, no. 1, IV, mm. 28–
29.
Figure 3.9.2. A descending-fifth seventh-chord progression embedding the nonfunctional triads from
“Moro Lasso.” Randy Newman uses the descending-fifth progression in “When She Loved Me” from
Toy Story 2 (at “still I waited for the day”).
Figure 3.9.3. The Beatles’ I–II–IV–I derived from a functional seventh-chord idiom.
in the bass and F ♯ –C–F at the top: a dominant-seventh sharp nine over a
tonic pedal and with an additional major ninth. The chorale takes a series of
clockwise steps that pass through seven inversions, repeatedly applying the
basic voice leading before returning to the one-semitone transposition of the
opening chord. This is abstractly parallel motion, with each chord belonging
to the same set class and spaced in the same pattern of chordal steps—and
though the progression is complex and clangorous, I hear its chords as
audibly similar, reflecting their similar spacing and intervallic constitution.
One could use this chorale to generate even-more-complex passages in
which different groups of voices combine different transpositions along the
chord tx with the same transposition along the scale Ty, as in §3.7. Here we
start to glimpse how we might generalize traditional compositional
procedures to arbitrary chord-and-scale environments, precisely as suggested
by the Prime Directive.
Figure 3.9.4. A chorale built from the basic voice leading for a seven-note chord in twelve-note space;
each chord is T7t–4 of its predecessor and is voiced 3, 5, 3, 2, 3, 3 in intrinsic steps.
1
Visit https://www.madmusicalscience.com/cs.html for interactive versions of this space, and
https://www.madmusicalscience.com/graphs/2in7.pdf for a printable version of the graph. Other
graphs can be found by replacing “2” and “7” with the appropriate numbers.
2
For more on this perspective, see chapter 4 of Tymoczko 2011a.
3
This voice leading reduces the dyad’s center of gravity by one: if we number notes so that C = 0,
D = 1, E = 2, and so on, then the chord (E, G) is (2, 4), with a sum of 6 and center of gravity (or
average) of 3; the basic voice leading sends (E, G) to (C, A), or (0, 5), with a sum of 5 and an average
of 2.5.
4
As Nicolas 2019 points out, Étienne-Denis Delair’s 1690 Traité d’accompagnement pour le
théorbe, et le clavessin recommends the ascending basic voice leading for ascending-fifth bass lines,
often producing a quasi-functional result.
5
The end of the phrase ends with an e-b ascent that continues the G–d–a pattern of the opening
phrase, though I would not put much emphasis on this connection.
6
See m. 182; this is one of countless instances of Beethoven transforming a theme so as to reveal a
disguised sequence.
7
Rabinovitch (2018, 2019, 2020) likewise grounds schemata in deeper contrapuntal principles;
though he starts from very different theoretical principles, I believe his approach is largely consistent
with mine. The link between galant schemas and dyadic geometry is also supported by Holtmeier’s
observation that galant composers favored outer-voice imperfect consonances (2011).
8
The voice leading (E4, G4, C5) → (D4, A4, E5) can be decomposed into the voice exchange (E4,
G4, C5) → (C4, G4, E5) and the spacing-preserving (C4, G4, E5) → (D4, A4, E5); the latter has both
chords in “open position,” with each voice two steps above its predecessor. When chords are related
by transposition, the spacing-preserving component is a transposition along the chord.
9
To move the root up by octave while keeping the third fixed, we can transpose both notes up
along the chord, sending C up to E and E up to C. We then apply the five-step voice exchange to move
E up to C and C down to E. The result leaves E fixed and in the same voice while raising C by octave
in the other. In other words, we have (C4, E4) → (E4, C5) → (C5, E4), for a composite of (C4, E4) →
(C5, E4).
10
The fifth is the closest diatonic approximation to the half-octave. In a scale with an even number
of notes, such as the chromatic or octatonic, then tritone transposition allows for perfectly balanced
contrapuntal motion, connecting maximally close thirds.
11
Thanks here to Gunnlaugur Björnsson.
12
In this space, the line of unisons acts like a mirror, “reflecting” paths backward into the interior
of the graph. See Tymoczko 2011a, chapter 3.
13
T–5t2 is equivalent to T2t–1 since T7t–3 does nothing and T–5t2 + T7t–3 = T2t–1. Three applications
of T–5t2 produces T–15t6 or T–1 since T–14t6 also does nothing. The descending basic voice leading
turns (E, G, B), or (2, 4, 6) in diatonic note-numbers, into (D, G, B), or (1, 4, 6), decreasing the sum
from 12 to 11 and center of gravity from 4 to 3.66.
14
These voice-leading patterns were the subject of explicit discussion in early figured-bass
treatises; see, for example, the examples on p. 77 of Buelow 1992 or the discussion on pp. 202–3 of
Bach (1762) 1949.
15
Chapter 6 will discuss the application of Roman numerals in an early-music context. These near-
palindromic phrases are fairly common in the Renaissance (e.g., Figure 8.6.11 or the start of
Palestrina’s “Stabat Mater”).
16
Note that the top line of the ukulele jumps above the vocal at the dominant, producing the
familiar effect of overlap.
17
The Pachelbel/Prinner combination is an exact A2D4 sequence that has been truncated and
repeated: b°–C–G–a–e–F–C–[b°]. Caplin (2015, p. 51) mentions this progression.
18
Free use of small clockwise steps can sometimes be found in functional music, for example the
opening of Mozart’s G minor quintet K.516 (g–f♯°6–f6–C64–E♭ 6–D6), the opening of the Waldstein
(§3.7), or the Chopin excerpts in §9.3.
19
Agmon 1995 and Quinn 2005.
20
Compare Figure 3.4.8 and Figure 3.1.7; see also the first measure of J. S. Bach’s chorale “O
Haupt voll Blut und Wunden” (BWV 244.54, Riemenschneider 74), which uses the same
Pachelbel/Prinner hybrid, starting on 4̂ rather than 6̂.
21
See Buelow 1992 and Bach (1762) 1949. Robert Gauldin (1997) calls my “half open”
configuration the “open/octave” position.
22
For example, imagine we form a scale containing two notes in all possible octaves (i.e., . . . C3,
G3, C4, G4 . . . ). Measured in steps along that scale, DI is 001 or 011 and corresponds to collections
like C3, C3, G3 or G3, G3, C4. In this same scale H is 012 (i.e., C3, G3, C4 or G3, C4, G4) while OO
is 013 (C3, G3, G4) and 023 (C3, C4 G4). For C and O, we form a scale containing three distinct notes
in every octave (i.e., . . . C3, E3, G3, C4, E4, G4 . . . ). Three consecutive notes in that scale form a
close-position triad (C, or 012 in steps along that scale), while three notes such as C3, G3, E4 form an
open-position triad (O or 024).
23
The chord at the end of m. 4 is either half-open or open, depending on how we read the melodic
A4–G4–A4 figure (§5.1).
24
For instance, the descending basic voice leading moves (D, D, A) or (1, 1, 5) to (C, F, F) or (0, 3,
3), changing the chord’s sum from 7 to 6 and the center of gravity from 2.33 to 2.
25
Again, efficient voice leading requires small changes in configuration: when every voice moves
efficiently, the total configuration moves by short distances on the graph.
26
For example, (D4, D4, F4)→ (C4, E4, E4).
27
Readers might notice that this figure is remarkably similar to the graph of two-note consonances
(Figure P3.4), with close position playing the role of thirds and open position playing the role of sixths
—a curious and largely coincidental resonance between two rather different music-theoretical
constructions.
28
Zarlino (1558) 1968, Schubert 2008.
29
A three-voice diatonic consonance contains either zero or two imperfect consonances. If both
chords in the voice leading contain two consonances, then at most one of the three pairs of voices in
each chord contains an interval that is not an imperfect consonance. So the third pair must contain an
imperfect consonance in both chords.
30
As discussed in §5.2, I consider the 64 chord to be harmonic. The phrase also features a
conspicuous point of imitation, with the middle voice picking up the top voice’s “Kyrie” a bar later.
31
A simple computer survey suggests they occur in less than 3% of Renaissance voice leadings.
See also Huron 1991.
32
Huron 2016.
33
When a chord divides the scalar octave exactly evenly, “spacing in chordal steps” is equivalent
to “spacing in scale steps.” As the chord divides the octave less and less evenly, the two notions
diverge.
34
It is interesting to ask whether (C, G) and (C, C, E) should be considered sonorities in their own
right or subsets of a triad; Renaissance and functional composers would likely give different answers.
35
This is comparable to Figure 2.1.8, only now with a wider range of inversions in the bass.
36
For examples of long lists of options, see Kostka and Payne 2003, who borrow from McHose
1947.
37
For example a DI↔C transition can produce parallel fifths only if moving between a close, root
position triad and a fifth with one note doubled; H↔O transitions can produce parallel fifths only
between adjacent voices, as in (D4, A4, D5) → (C4, G4, E5).
38
Aarden and von Hippel 2004.
39
Note that the bass is doubled if and only if the upper voices are in C or O position.
40
Huron 2016 tries to derive doubling practices from principles of upper-voice spacing, but he
exaggerates the rarity of the DI position; in Palestrina, DI is very common, and in Bach, it is
reasonably common. I describe it as “unusual” only in a pedagogical context.
41
Aarden and von Hippel 2004 contains a good summary of the justifications given for various
doubling rules. Schoenberg ([1911] 1983, p. 59) argues that the doubled third is acoustically defective.
42
Weber (1817–1821) 1846, pp. 190–92.
43
A fact that first came to my attention a few years back, when I awoke from a deep sleep, sat bolt
upright in the middle of the night, and said to myself, “Holy cow, how did I never notice the ascending
thirds in the Waldstein?” Whereupon my waking self had to reconstruct this analysis to figure out what
I meant.
44
Autoharps play triads, which are the collections relevant to this discussion, but provide no voice
leading from one chord to the next; harps supply voice leadings via their pedals, but sound seven-note
collections.
45
Previous theorists have tried to translate Schenker’s metaphors of “surface” and “depth” into
perceptual terms, postulating that humans naturally experience harmony in hierarchical fashion
(Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983, Lerdahl 2001, and Rohrmeier 2020).
46
Milton Babbitt (1987, p. 148) emphasized that the opening of Tristan spells out a “Tristan” chord
in its upper voice (G ♯ –B–D–F ♯ on Figure 3.7.5). I see this as the byproduct of the same general
relationships that produce the ascending thirds in the Waldstein.
47
Schubert 2018 proposes a similar analysis.
48
Erno Lendvai (1971) argues that major or minor chords have the same function when their roots
belong to the same diminished seventh, at least in Bartók’s music. I find this somewhat plausible in
the case of dominant-functioning chords, less so in the case of tonic and subdominant.
49
See, for example, m. 12 of the second E major fugue from The Well-Tempered Clavier or mm.
19, 31, and 71 of the second F major fugue.
50
The name is meant to emphasize their relation to the common-tone diminished seventh, not the
presence of common tones in the actual progression. Clendinning and Marvin 2016 use the term
“common-tone German sixth” to describe one of these progressions.
51
Other examples can be found in Haydn’s Symphony no. 100 (“Military”), I, mm. 195–197 (VI65
→V ), Beethoven’s Symphony no. 2, I, mm. 40–43 (V65/V→vii°7/V→V43/IV), and Kabalevsky’s
7
Since the diatonic scale has an odd number of notes, the voices exchange
intervals when they return to the tonic: where the right hand initially
ascended from C by fourth, it now descends from C by fifth, and where the
left descended from A♭ by fifth, it now ascends from A♭ by fourth (Figure
P4.2). The humor lies in the impression of invertible counterpoint: in the
consequent, each hand plays the intervals of the antecedent’s other hand, but
on the same notes as before. To my ear, contour and rhythm trump pitch-
class content, and the theme seems to switch hands. (The quarter-note delay
contributes to this impression by directing our ears to the left hand.) The
passage manages to sound trivial and brilliant at once, a paradoxical effect of
“counterpoint” that is hardly worth the name.
Figure P4.2. The opening phrase as a single sequence.
On the brilliant side of the ledger, the final left-hand fourth is decorated
by stepwise contrary motion, producing Gjerdingen’s “converging cadence.”
In the opening, this forms a half cadence, tonicizing E♭ while reinforcing its
status as dominant; it also serves the rhythmic function of filling four
measures with seven chords. In the consequent the voices start converging
one chord earlier, on F rather than B ♭ —a result of the hands having
exchanged melodic intervals. (Rhythmic displacement conceals this, the
consequent’s left hand only one quarter-note ahead of the antecedent.) This
creates a stronger sense of B♭-as-dominant, and hence a stronger tonicization
of the final E♭, reinforced by the melodic E♭. This is a nice example of the
power of schematic vocabulary: the two phrases are nearly identical,
descending by fifth from A♭ to E♭, yet because of the different positions of
the converging cadence they manage to form a period, moving from a half
cadence to a full cadence on the dominant. At which point the sequence has
returned to its original state so that the repeat follows organically—meaning
that the restatement is also a continuation of the underlying process. The
result is almost deconstructive in its simplicity, the nearly pure unfolding of
an algorithm that generates invertible counterpoint, a canon, a tonally
functional period, a half cadence, a modulation to the dominant, and the
conventionally mandated repeat. It is simultaneously funny and profound.
The four-bar middle section, meanwhile, presents a near sequence that
states a brief melody on E♭, D♭, and A♭.2 Figure P4.3 shows that the motive
outlines an octave-displaced version of the diatonic third’s ascending basic
voice leading. The circular pattern of roots E ♭ –B ♭ –D ♭ –A ♭ –E ♭ outlines a
minor third (§2.5). Here the thirds on B♭ and D♭ are superimposed to form a
1
See Caplin 1998, chapter 16, for more on these rounded-binary interior episodes. Steve Taylor
observed that this passage echoes the consequent of the slow-movement opening theme; it is also very
similar to the third variation of Op. 1, no. 3, III (discussed in §4.10), and the second theme of Op. 18,
no. 5, IV (mm. 36ff).
2
It is also a microscopic version of what we will call, in chapter 8, the up-and-down-the-ladder
schema.
3
Rosen 1998, p. 545.
4
C. P. E. Bach advertised his father’s “original thought” in the contrapuntal domain (Wolff 2001, p.
8). One component of this originality is Bach’s distinctive approach to the diatonic third’s ascending
basic voice leading (§3.1, 4.6, 7.7).
4
Repetition
Voice leadings are atomic musical moves that can be combined arbitrarily.
One of the central discoveries of the Renaissance was that they could be
repeated, chained together algorithmically to convert an isolated event into
an ongoing process. Repeating contrapuntal patterns are an important feature
of sixteenth-century music, where they are often subtle and disguised (e.g.,
the ascending fifths in Figure 3.6.6, whose sequential structure is not
immediately obvious).1 This disguised repetition evolves into the overtly
repetitive sequences of the baroque, which embed repeating contrapuntal
patterns into larger musical blocks. Nineteenth-century composers make
increasing use of contrary-motion “wedge” sequences, sometimes nontonal,
paving the way for the very flexible transformations of modern music—a
style that permits almost any conceivable transformation at any level of the
quadruple hierarchy. Appendix 3 considers this practice as an extension and
generalization of the traditional sequences that are the focus of this chapter.
1. Repetition reimagined
Figure 4.1.2. An abstract model of repetition in which a cell’s melodic lines are transposed and
permuted as they repeat.
Figure 4.1.3. Three examples of the schema. (top) An ascending-step sequence from Marianna
Martines’s “Vo solcando un mar crudele” (from Scelta d’Arie Composte per suo diletto, 1767), mm.
122ff. (middle) “Three Blinde Mice” (1609) uses permutation but not transposition. (bottom) A
sequence with both transposition and permutation from the first G♯ minor fugue in The Well-Tempered
Clavier, mm. 86ff.
This model resolves some longstanding difficulties surrounding the
identification of repeating sequential units. We can distinguish the simple
period, which is the length of the minimal block of music sufficient to
generate the sequence, or the cells as I have defined them, from the grand
period, which is the time it takes for all voices to cycle back to their initial
melodic positions, all transposed by the same amount (not counting octave
displacements that may differ from voice to voice). Previous theorists have
often focused on the grand period to the exclusion of the simple period; in
large part this is because they have neglected permutation as a sequence-
generating operation.4 In the presence of permutation, however, the grand
period will contain multiple simple periods: the sequences in Figure 4.1.3 all
have a simple period of one bar, but it takes three bars for “Three Blinde
Mice” to cycle back to its original melodic configuration and two bars for
the G♯ minor fugue to do so. In some cases, different groups of voices will
have different simple periods (e.g., Figure 4.5.7).
Sequences can be stable or unstable in two different ways. A collection of
transposing arrows is harmonically stable when its transpositions differ only
by octave. These sequences are structure-preserving in a very general way,
producing transpositionally related blocks regardless of their cells’ content.
Harmonically unstable sequences create contrary-motion “wedge” patterns
that move through unrelated vertical states (Figure 4.1.4). Here again the
grand period will typically contain multiple simple periods, the voices
cycling through different transpositions until they return to their initial
alignment. In earlier music, unstable sequences often exploit unique features
of the sequential unit to create a consonant result. This typically involves the
sort of invertible counterpoint found in Figure 4.1.5.5
Figure 4.1.4. Schoenberg’s “Angst und Hoffen” (The Book of the Hanging Gardens, Op. 15, no. 7)
contains a wedge sequence that transposes the voices by different amounts.
Figure 4.1.5. An invertible-counterpoint sequence in mm. 57–60 of the second E minor prelude in The
Well-Tempered Clavier. The sequence uses invertible counterpoint at the twelfth (mm. 1–2).
Look at the left side of Figure 4.2.1 while covering the right side with your
hand. If you wanted to repeat this voice leading, how would you do it?
Figure 4.2.1. Two ways to repeat the voice leading on the left.
Rather than a single correct answer there are two possibilities: continuing
the melodic motions within each voice to form a contrary-motion wedge, or
permuting the melodic intervals so that they move from voice to voice.
Either yields a sequence with a repeating cell that is just one note long.7 I
will call these one-note sequences repeating contrapuntal patterns.
Repeating contrapuntal patterns can be associated with phrases like “voice X
moves up by 3 semitones and then this melodic interval moves to voice Y;
voice Y moves down by one semitone and then this motion shifts to voice
X.” We can think of them, loosely, as combining two separate voice
leadings, one that determines the melodic intervals (“move voice X up by
three semitones”) and another that permutes these melodic intervals among
the voices (“its motion moves to voice Y”).8 Figure 4.2.2 contains repeating
contrapuntal patterns from Josquin, Beethoven, and Stravinsky.
Figure 4.2.2. Three sequences whose units have just a single chord. (left) Josquin, from the Sanctus of
the mass L’Ami Baudichon, mm. 14ff; (middle) the Beethoven passage in Figure P4.1; (right) one of
the main motives of Stravinsky’s Firebird.
The upshot is that there are deep connections between the seemingly
distinct domains of repetition, canon, and voice leading. These are reflected
by two different ways we can conceive of repeated moves on a spiral
diagram: as harmonic sequences and as generators of canons. This
connection is in the first instance theoretically surprising, as one might not
expect that repetition had anything to do with canon. It is also interesting
historically, as it is likely that the canonic aspect was initially paramount: in
a passage like Figure 4.2.5, Palestrina was presumably more concerned with
imitation than the ascending-fifth pattern of harmonies.9 As time passed,
however, these moments of close imitation perhaps came to be valued for
their harmonic patterning as well; thus when J. S. Bach or Mozart make
sequential use of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading, its harmonic aspect
is ascendant (Figure 4.3.2). In this way, a concern with counterpoint and
imitation could gradually yield to a focus on harmonic repetition without any
clear line dividing the two. Chapter 6 will argue that repeating contrapuntal
patterns could also spark awareness of abstract structural concepts such as
“root” and “fifth,” thus setting the stage for inversional equivalence.
Figure 4.2.5. The ascending basic voice leading in the top two voices of the Sanctus of Palestrina’s
mass Ave regina coelorum, mm. 18–20.
Figure 4.2.6. The exposition of the first E minor fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier (mm. 11–12) is
a sequence with two-measure units, whose initial notes articulate the basic voice leading from one unit
to the next.
Figure 4.2.7. The round in Ligeti’s Passacaglia ungherese makes a complete circle in the space of
chromatic major thirds.
Figure 4.2.8. In a large-unit sequence, not all chords need be related by the same voice leading. Here,
the first chords in each unit relate by T–4t2, while the second add the complicated voice exchange
crtcrfcrtctf, with crt the pairwise voice exchange of root and third, crf root and fifth, and ctf third and
fifth.
The isomorphism between sequence and voice leading allows us to use the
spiral diagrams to explore the geometry of sequential possibility. For
suppose we have a two-voice, harmonically consistent repeating
contrapuntal pattern: if it has no voice exchanges then it is either canonic or
transpositional, depending on whether it takes an odd or even number of
steps on the dyadic spiral; if it has voice exchanges, then it is either canonic
or contrary-motion, again depending on how many steps it takes (Figure
4.3.1).11 Readers will recall that these sequence-types—transpositional,
contrary-motion, and canonic—were all discussed in this chapter’s prelude.
The spiral diagrams show that they are the only harmonically consistent two-
voice possibilities. Geometry also justifies our decision to include
permutation alongside transposition as a sequence-generating operation:
after all, any harmonically stable voice leading generates a sequence, and
some of these permute their voices.
Figure 4.3.1. Sequences generated by repeated clockwise motion on the dyadic circle. The top line
does not involve any voice exchanges while the bottom line incorporates the two-step voice exchange
(symbolized by the asterisk; see the dotted curves on Figure 3.2.1). This turns parallel sequences into
canonic sequences and canonic sequences into contrary sequences.
For sequences with an odd number of voice exchanges, canons result from
an even number of steps on the dyadic circle, while antiparallel motion
results from an odd number of steps (bottom staff of Figure 4.3.1). The most
common sequences use the two-step voice exchange: on its own it produces
a simple round; combined with one-step clockwise motion it produces
antiparallel motion in which one voice moves by fourth and the other moves
by fifth in the opposite direction. The combination of stepwise motion with
the two-step voice exchange is also reasonably common, particularly in its
ascending form (e.g., Figure 7.6.10). For a sequence with an even number of
voice exchanges, the situation is reversed: an odd number of steps produces
a canonic sequence, and an even number produces antiparallel motion.
Sequences with larger motions on the dyadic circle tend to be rare as they
involve more unbalanced motion between the two voices.14
We now turn to harmonically unstable sequences. Any two-voice canonic
repeating contrapuntal pattern, whether harmonically stable or not, will
preserve its canonic structure when either of its dyads is moved by exact
contrary motion.15 This means that any such pattern can be generated by
applying contrary-motion distortion to either parallel motion or Pathétique-
style alternating antiparallel intervals, depending on the interval of
transposition as measured from one grand period to the next (Figure 4.3.4).16
For a particular interval, the various possibilities will be represented by a
matrix such as that in Figure 4.3.5. The most common harmonically unstable
canon is the dyadic 5–6; it can be embedded into a triadic sequence as root-
fifth, fifth-third, or both simultaneously—in which case we have three
canonic voices, one sounding third and fifth, another fifth and root, and
another root and third (Figures 4.3.6–7).17 This structure can be transformed
via dyadic expansion into a 5–10 sequence (e.g., the outer voices on the right
side of Figure 2.9.3). The music in Figure 4.3.8 makes sophisticated use of
these possibilities: my reconstruction begins in the top line with an
antiparallel ascending-fifth sequence; every other chord is then subjected to
contrary-motion contraction, the top three voices lowered by step while the
bass rises by the same amount; finally we apply “third substitution” to the
last three chords, lowering the bass by third and raising the fifth of the
upper-voice triad. This produces a familiar harmonic loop, four descending
fifths and one third, b–e–A–D (§2.5).
Figure 4.3.4. Two-voice canonic sequences derived from either parallel or antiparallel motion by
expanding or contracting every other chord. This produces sequences with even or odd intervals of
transposition respectively. On the bottom left, a pattern from m. 56 of the second B♭ minor fugue in
The Well-Tempered Clavier.
Figure 4.3.5. The matrix of two-voice, two-chord canonic patterns repeating down by step. The
examples can be read right-to-left to produce ascending-step patterns.
Figure 4.3.6. The 5–6 voice-leading pattern is an example of invertible counterpoint at the tenth, with
the G–A voice moving down by fifth to C–D while the C–C voice moves up by sixth to A–A. This is
equivalent to transposing G–A down by tenth and transposing the whole pattern up by sixth. This
figure can be embedded in a triadic 5–6 sequence in two separate ways.
Figure 4.3.7. Two ascending-step sequences from The Well-Tempered Clavier, each involving a
canonic relation among two voices. The top system is a reduction of mm. 13–19 of the second C major
fugue, and has the canon between the third-fifth and fifth-root voices; the bottom is from the second
F♯ minor fugue, mm. 47–48, and has its canon between the root-third and fifth-root voices.
Figure 4.3.8. Barbara Strozzi’s “Il Contrasto dei Cinque Sensi,” mm. 3–4. The top staff presents an
antiparallel fifth sequence; on the second staff, the voices of every other chord are moved together by
step; the third staff applies third substitution to the final three chords, lowering the bass by third and
raising the alto by step.
The interesting conceptual point is that canon and contrary motion arise as
byproducts of the basic mechanics of sequential voice leading, rather than
requiring deliberate compositional artifice: they are outcroppings that the
rock-climber may choose to grasp or not. The presence or absence of
canonic patterning is thus a matter of whether composers decide to draw out
the canonic potential latent in their material, with noncanonic sequences
often obscuring preexisting canonic relationships (Figure 4.3.9).18 In the
case of contrary motion, the handhold is less commonly grasped, though
there are circumstances where it is analytically useful to be aware of it: a
nice example comes from the opening of Beethoven’s Tempest sonata, a
sequence that could form a contrary-motion wedge, though Beethoven
chooses not to (Figure 10.3.2).19 These facts allow us to unpack metaphors
like “the will of the tones,” connecting low-level musical affordances to
high-level compositional devices like canon and contrary-motion.20 An
intuitive sense for this connection lies behind a good deal of compositional
expertise.
Figure 4.3.9. Measures 9–12 of the trio of Maria Szymanowska’s Polonaise no. 1, from 18 Dances of
Different Genres. The figuration disguises rather than highlighting the canon inherent in the voice
leading.
With larger chords, the subject of voice exchanges becomes much more
complex. Setting them aside for the moment, we can repeat the preceding
analysis: for an n-note chord, an n-step motion on the spiral diagram, or
motion by any integer multiple of n, will produce a transpositional sequence
in which all voices move in parallel; any other motion will produce canonic
sequences. Figure 4.4.1 generates three-voice sequences by taking one, two,
four, five, and seven clockwise steps on the triadic circle. The resulting
schemas are all very common. Because the triad divides the octave very
evenly, short motions on the triadic circle produce little melodic activity and
the canonic imitation is very subtle.
Figure 4.4.1. Repeating contrapuntal patterns generated by short clockwise motion along the triadic
circle. Each column is related by the Tinctoris Transform. The –2 and –4 patterns contain descending
three-note scale fragments, while the –5 and –7 patterns contain descending triadic arpeggios. The
examples can be read backwards to produce ascending sequences (+1, +2, etc.).
Figure 4.4.2. An ascending-fifth canonic sequence from Barbara Strozzi’s “Gli amanti falliti,” mm.
32–33. This uses the +5 sequence, the retrograde of the –5 pattern in the previous example.
Figure 4.4.3. A sequence from the second C♯ minor fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier, mm. 10ff.
The sequence uses the descending-fifth canonic voice leading labeled –5 on Figure 4.4.1.
Figure 4.4.4. The opening of Barbara Strozzi’s “L’amante modesto” uses the +7 sequence, the
retrograde of –7 on Figure 4.4.1.
Figure 4.4.5. (left) Each new entry can appear either a fifth above the previous entry, or at the same
pitch level. (right) Using repeated entries to avoid second-inversion triads.
Figure 4.4.6. The Chase schema in Orlando Gibbons’s In Nomine a 4, mm. 17–19.
Figure 4.4.7. In mm. 190ff of Josquin’s “Benedicite, omnia opera,” we have a descending-fifth
sequence featuring two superimposed instances of the diatonic third’s descending basic voice leading,
with all four voices in canon. After a brief ascending-step sequence, where the upper voices continue
to inhabit the circle of imperfect consonances, we have seven entries of a canonic sequence based on
the Chase schema.
Figure 4.4.8. A Chase-schema variant in mm. 96–111 of the scherzo of Beethoven’s Third Symphony
Op. 55. The upper staff is a reduction of Beethoven’s actual music, while the lower shows the three-
voice Chase schema.
Figure 4.4.9. Measures 158–161 of the six-part Ricercare from The Musical Offering, BWV 1079.
The sequence superimposes a triadic canon in the right hand with a dyadic canon in the left.
Figure 4.5.1. Two passages that take advantage of the tritone’s symmetry, moving the bass down by
fifth and the upper-voice tritone down by semitone.
Figure 4.5.3. The same pattern using diatonic thirds. The upper voices can either be root-third or
third-fifth pairs.
The preceding examples have the bass leaping and upper voices moving
stepwise. A package of related progressions shifts the melodic motion
toward the upper voices: Figure 4.5.4 appears in The Well-Tempered Clavier,
the bass descending by third while upper voices take turns ascending by
fourth. In Beethoven’s first D major piano sonata, the asymmetry is even
more extreme, with the bass descending by step while the upper voices
ascend by fourth and fifth (Figure 4.5.5).24 These passages are quite different
from one another, and one might not think to associate them; yet they exhibit
a fundamentally similar organization. The differences are (a) the interval in
the upper voices (fifth, third, etc.); (b) how the bass is transposed relative to
the upper voices; and (c) the degree of transpositional motion applied to the
underlying pattern. Together, these parameters generate a range of options
whose sonic diversity disguises their shared structure.
Figure 4.5.4. (top) We can transpose the chords of the sequence to shift the motion to the upper
voices, turning a descending-fifth sequence into a more balanced descending-third sequence. (bottom)
The second F major fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier, mm. 57–60.
Figure 4.5.5. Beethoven’s Op. 10, no. 3, IV, begins with contrary-motion thirds, leading to contrary-
motion fauxbourdon in the second full measure. That passage relates via the Tinctoris Transform to the
sequences we have been exploring.
Triadic composers also make extensive use of rounds dividing the voices
2 + 1, typically as a device to enliven static harmony. The most common
pattern dates back to the early sixteenth century, adding passing tones to the
two-voice round marked *0 in Figure 4.3.1 (Figure 4.5.6; see Figure 6.1.7
and §6.7 for Renaissance examples). It can appear either as IV–I or as V–I to
form “Prinner,” “Fenaroli,” and “standing on the dominant” schemas.25
Figure 4.5.7 contains one of my favorite classical rounds, the nonschematic
build to the final cadence of the first movement of Beethoven’s Op. 10, no.
3. One would hardly think to look for contrapuntal trickery here, yet the
upper voices embed a two-voice canon in contrary motion with the bass.26
Such rounds differ from standard sequences only in that their harmonies do
not transpose; by grouping them with familiar sequences we can deepen our
appreciation for the many functions of transformed repetition.
6. Four voices
Four voices can be divided as two pairs, a triad plus an independent voice, or
a single four-voice unit. The simplest four-voice sequences superimpose two
basic voice leadings moving along their own spiral diagrams, as in Figures
3.3.2 and 4.4.7. Figure 4.6.1 instead moves its voices in contrary motion:
here each pair forms a two-step interval containing every other note of a
four-note scale, with consistent contrary motion reproducing the complete
collection at every step. (This “thinking within the chord” is analogous to a
familiar whole-tone technique discussed in appendix 3.) Figure 4.6.2 shows
a related passage from the beginning of Louis Andriessen’s De Staat, with
fifths moving by contrary stepwise motion along the scale B–C–E–F. In such
cases, the evenness of the four-note scale will determine the regularity of
melodic intervals: for the diminished seventh, voices always move by three
semitones; for other chords they move by approximately three semitones,
with the value of “approximate” depending on the chord’s unevenness
(distance from the completely even diminished seventh). This contrary-
motion pattern can be extended to triadic contexts by doubling a note (Figure
4.6.3).
Figure 4.6.1. Measures 43–44 of Wagner’s Tristan prelude, with pairs of voices moving in contrary
motion within a four-note chord, initially A–C–D♯–F♯, then G♯–B♯–D♯–F♯; this produces the
complete four-note collection at every step. Geometrically, the voices move in opposite directions
along the 2-in-4 spiral diagram, which has only one loop since the two-step interval is
transpositionally symmetrical.
Figure 4.6.2. The opening of Louis Andriessen’s De Staat, with pairs of voices moving in contrary
motion along the scale B–C–E–F. Each hand alternates between the fifths F–C and E–B, always
forming the full tetrachord.
Figure 4.6.3. A contrary-motion sequence that moves along a four-note collection with two copies of
the note G; each verticality states the complete four-note collection (C major with doubled G). The
technique appears in mm. 19–24 of Burgmüller’s “Ballade” (Op. 100, no. 15); it can be generalized to
any trichord.
Figure 4.6.4. The “omnibus” sequence features three voices sounding the third, fifth, and seventh of
the dominant seventh chord (a diminished triad), with the fourth voice sounding the root.
When all four voices form a single cycle, the analytical situation can
become bewilderingly complex. The repeating contrapuntal pattern of Figure
4.6.7 is a four-voice Chase schema taking a two-step counterclockwise path
on the space of triads with doubled third, equivalent to Figure 3.8.1. This
schema was a favorite of J. S. Bach’s, permeating his four-voice music and
likely internalized as part of his improvisational practice.27 Figure 4.6.8
shows a marvelously disguised appearance at the end of the chorale “Was
mein Gott will, das g’scheh allzeit” (BWV 244.25, Riemenschneider 115).
We hear two literal instantiations of the schema before the harmonic rhythm
halved. This is accomplished in two different ways: first by staggering the
ascending-third melodies so that they occur separately, and second by
expanding the harmonies so that they last for two beats instead of one (in the
second line of the example). Despite its complexity, the ascending-fifth
canon remains audible through the penultimate chord.
Figure 4.6.7. A repeating voice-leading pattern that generates a four-voice ascending-fifth canon. The
pattern moves the root and one third down by step, and the fifth and a second third up by third (by way
of passing tones). J. S. Bach often uses this pattern with scale degrees 4̂–3̂–2̂ in the top voice against
6̂–7̂–1̂ in the bass.
Figure 4.6.8. The hidden canon at the end of “Was mein Gott will, das g’scheh allzeit.”
Together these passages exemplify what might be called the subterranean
mathematics of Western music, a mathematics that becomes increasingly
complex as we add voices. I imagine that earlier composers were proud of
their sequential discoveries, rightly considering them novel solutions to
ubiquitous compositional challenges—and a way to stamp their individuality
on the shared language of functional harmony. In earlier times, such
solutions would need to be learned or discovered piecemeal, without the
benefit of an overarching theoretical framework. For a composer working in
a consensus style, this schematic approach might be entirely sufficient. In
our own more fragmented time, the more abstract approach can be useful,
allowing us to apply sequential techniques to any chord in any scale.
7. Contrary-motion sequences
Figure 4.7.4. The “Morte” schema uses the ascending-fifth wedge, often with an augmented sixth
tonicizing the dominant.
The –1/+6 and –6/+1 patterns require special comment, for their outer
voices form genuine step-sequences that, despite the bass, do not factor into
a pair of fifths. It is possible to create a fifth-sequence by adding an
additional voice: in Figure 4.7.5 the upper voices use the diatonic fifth’s
basic voice leading, forming a half-note sequential unit that exploits the
bass’s preexisting periodicity. Many contrary motion step-sequences factor
into a pair of fifths in exactly this way: the climactic sequence in The Well-
Tempered Clavier’s first F major fugue is a good example, featuring
descending steps in the canonic upper voices against ascending sevenths in
the bass (Figure 4.7.6).28 Figure 4.7.7 shows the Hammerklavier’s opening
theme, where ascending steps B♭–C–D appear against descending sevenths,
again factored into a pair of descending fourths. Once again the middle voice
completes the diatonic third’s ascending basic voice leading, though you
would never notice unless you were looking for it.
Figure 4.7.5. With an added voice, the four-beat step sequence becomes a two-beat fifth sequence.
Figure 4.7.6. Contrary motion in the first F major fugue from The Well-Tempered Clavier, mm. 56ff.
The first sequence has a two-measure melodic period and moves by step/seventh; the second has a
one-measure period and moves by fourth/fifth. (In the first, the upper voices articulate two instances
of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading, as shown above the music.) Across the juncture between
them, the sixteenth-note figure moves down by third and the ascending eighth notes are doubled a
third below. We can imagine B♭–C–D in the left hand of the fourth measure continuing smoothly with
E–F–G in the next.
Figure 4.7.7. Beethoven’s Hammerklavier, Op. 106, mm. 5–6, is a contrary-motion sequence. The
outer voices, on their own, are a step-sequence with a one-measure period; the middle voice creates an
ascending-fifth sequence with a half-measure period.
The interesting point is that all these contrary-motion sequences are
similar from the Tinctorian perspective, sharing the same pattern of relative
motion between the hands; we can derive all of them by transposing the list
of consonances and (perhaps) eliminating some dyads. All these patterns
exemplify the same basic constraint on antiparallel motion: when the
harmonic interval is the fifth, the motion is balanced, with the voices moving
by almost the same interval; as we move away from this interval, to the
third, second, and finally unison, the voices become less and less balanced
until the melodic motion is confined to one voice and we recover our
original list of consonances. Conversely, since there are only a few ways to
harmonize contrary-motion patterns with traditional contrapuntal and
harmonic principles, contrary motion becomes an important tool for
nineteenth- and twentieth-century harmonic expansion—as in passages like
Figure 4.1.4, where voice leading is the engine that generates the atonal
harmonies.
Transpositional sequences are public knowledge, found in virtually every
composer’s work and discussed in virtually every harmony textbook.
Contrary-motion sequences are more esoteric, a secret compositional art not
often subjected to theoretical or pedagogical scrutiny. One gets the sense of
magic tricks jealously guarded from the public—and perhaps not equally
fascinating to every composer. (These sequences are ubiquitous in
Beethoven, common in J. S. Bach and Domenico Scarlatti, and rare in
Mozart.) Insofar as theorists have managed to penetrate this veil of secrecy,
they have typically glimpsed only a fraction of the whole, identifying
isolated sequences without realizing they are species of a single larger
genus.29 In part this reflects a gap in our theoretical knowledge: without the
Tinctoris Transform, it is difficult to see the unity among these patterns, or
why there would be anything interesting to say about contrary motion in
general. Readers are hereby enjoined to keep faith with this esoteric
tradition, revealing only what is absolutely necessary to those who have not
yet purchased a copy of this book.30
Figure 4.8.1. Structured arpeggiation in the opening of J. S. Bach’s first cello suite, BWV 1007.
More closely analogous are melodic patterns that transpose as they repeat.
These are theoretically interesting because they often imply hierarchical
networks in which the distinction between harmonic and nonharmonic plays
a structural role. In the prelude to chapter 2, we saw Louis Armstrong
moving a motive downward along the E♭ major chord, with the first note a
diatonic upper neighbor to the second (Figure P2.2). We can model this with
two different kinds of transformational arrow, little-t arrows representing
intervals within the triad and big-T arrows representing intervals within the
scale (Figure 4.8.2). Here the distinction between harmonic and
nonharmonic has a transformational significance, with different kinds of
tones moving differently from one motive-form to another.
Figure 4.8.2. The sequence in Louis Armstrong’s “West End Blues” (Figure P2.2) modeled with both
horizontal and vertical arrows. The horizontal or between-motive arrows transpose the harmonic tones
along the E♭ major triad; the vertical or within-motive arrows generate nonharmonic tones by
Things get more complicated when composers transpose along both chord
and scale. Amy Beach’s “Fire-Flies” (Op. 15, no. 4, Figure 4.8.3) begins by
moving a quarter-note motive upward along both the D and A melodic-minor
scales. Beach then moves a variant motive upward by one step along the A
minor triad (acting as the top of F♯ half-diminished in the last measure). The
two motives interpret similar pitch structures with different transformational
arrows: in the first, the vertical dyad is two scale steps, while in the second it
is a one-step triadic interval and hence either a third or a fourth.
Distinguishing these two kinds of transposition can be difficult: a good rule
of thumb is that, over a fixed harmony, transposition along the chord
preserves the distinction between harmonic and nonharmonic, while
transposition along the scale does not. This is shown by the open noteheads,
which change positions in the first motive but stay fixed in the second.
Figure 4.8.3. Transposition along both chord and scale in mm. 9–11 of Amy Beach’s “Fire-Flies,” Op.
15, no. 4.
Once again, these examples show that intervallic content is not something
fixed, but rather a variable whose value is revealed by musical development:
one and the same motive can be subjected to multiple interpretations, treated
first as a collection of scalar intervals and then as a collection of triadic
intervals. This is particularly important in the analysis of fugue subjects,
whose notes can be associated either with the tonic or dominant scale
degree; the former are answered down by fourth while the latter are
answered down by fifth (Figure 4.8.4). Thus, rather than thinking of tonal
answers as distorting or altering the original subject, we can think of them as
exact transpositions of complex hierarchical structures.31 In other words, a
fugue’s alternating tonic-dominant entries are an exact melodic sequence
that moves along the tonic-dominant perfect fifth. Creating a fugue subject
involves not just writing a melody but also deciding on its pattern of
hierarchical dependencies.32
Figure 4.8.4. The subject and answer to the first B♭ minor fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier; here,
“t” refers to transposition along the root-fifth dyad. The subject’s G♭5 sounds like a displaced neighbor
to the preceding F5; however, it transforms as a sixth above the initial B♭.
Though I have studied classical music for more than three and a half
decades, I have only recently come to appreciate the complexity of its
melodic procedures—its motives moving in regular and quasi-regular ways
along chords, scales, chordal subsets, and a variety of bespoke collections
(Figure 4.8.6).33 Sometimes it can be difficult to unpack the relative
contributions of transposition along chord, transposition along the scale, and
structured arpeggiation. And sometimes we find operations not easily
captured by familiar music-theoretical terminology: in Figure 4.8.7, Mozart
moves an intervallic pattern from a three-note triad to a four-note seventh
chord, collections that are not themselves related by transposition. Debussy
used this sort of “interscalar transposition” to shift melodies from the seven-
note diatonic collection to the six-note whole-tone scale and back. Mozart’s
chordal use of this technique is more likely to pass unnoticed, in large part
because we think we understand his music better than we do.
Figure 4.8.6. Measures 136–138 of “October,” from Fanny Mendelssohn Hensel’s Das Jahr, H. 385.
The music presents two short sequences that move along the four-note scale A♭–B♭–C–E♭ (the “horn
fifth scale”). The passage uses two-step intervals within the four-note scale, always forming either A♭–
C or B♭–E♭.
Figure 4.8.7. The second movement of Mozart’s K.533, mm. 96–98, presents the same melodic
intervals in four-note and three-note collections.
9. Near sequences
Figure 4.9.3. This passage alters the standard ascending-step sequence in two ways—first repeating
the C major chord that functions as a quasi-tonic, and second repeating the A minor chord that
functions as a quasi-dominant; this leads to consecutive descending fifths (V–I–IV) or consecutive
descending thirds (I–vi–IV).
Figure 4.9.4. Duke Ellington’s “Satin Doll” starts as a standard ascending-step sequence, but with the
units expressing ii–V rather than V–I functionality. At the third iteration, the root stays fixed while the
melody ascends by three semitones, continuing the pattern in an unpredictable way.
Figure 4.9.5. The ascending 5–6 in the Waldstein sonata’s first-movement coda, mm. 251ff, with first-
inversion chords functioning as both tonics and dominants. The fundamental bass contains the roots of
the triads that would normally have those harmonic functions.
Stepping back a little further, we can locate sequences within the broader
class of transformationally restricted passages. Where a standard sequence
repeatedly applies a single transformation, these latter passages draw from a
small collection of transformations, often closely related. The five excerpts
in Figure 4.9.6 exhibit broadly similar structure, lines divided into groups
that move in doubly parallel motion along both chord and scale. The first is
exactly sequential; the second is almost sequential, alternating between
three- and four-step descending diatonic transposition, while the last three
combine the two transpositions more freely; taken together they suggest a
continuum from sequence to free composition, determined by which features
of contrapuntal structure are held fixed. Figure 4.9.7 models one region of
that continuum, exemplifying the various combinations of repeating
contrapuntal pattern (R), harmonic consistency (H, or the use of a single
chord-type), and preservation of spacing-in-chordal-steps (S). The passages
featuring R are all paradigmatically sequential; the combination SH is not, as
it permits arbitrary combinations of transposition along both chord and scale.
Yet the result is regular enough so as to bear comparison with sequences in
the strict sense.
Figure 4.9.6. Palestrina’s passage is an exact sequence whose upper voices have a one-chord unit and
whose bass has a two-chord unit; Beethoven’s is a near sequence in which the scalar transposition
varies irregularly between descending fourth and fifth. The last three passages freely combine
transposition along chord and scale.
It is not until the third variation that Beethoven becomes fully explicit
about the harmonic logic, lifting the curtain on his compositional process to
reveal a completely sequential structure (Figure 4.10.4).37 Or perhaps what
is revealed is the nature of functional music more generally, since
Beethoven’s theme is about as typical as one could imagine. In either case,
the result is a profound reimagining of the variation form, proceeding not by
progressive embellishment (embodied in ever-faster note values decorating a
thematic original), but by progressive analysis, stripping away decoration to
reveal a latent musical logic (Figures 4.10.5–6). The entire third variation is
a series of perturbed fifths, reminiscent of the Pathétique’s A♭ major episode.
One can hardly appreciate the profundity of this moment unless one is
sensitive to the near sequences buried within the ostensibly freely composed
theme.
Figure 4.10.4. Overtly sequential structure at the end of the third variation of Beethoven’s Op. 1, no.
3, II.
Figure 4.10.5. Transformations of the consequent in Op. 1, no. 3, II.
Figure 4.10.6. A reduction of the third variation in Beethoven’s Op. 1, no. 3, II. Aside from the two
descending thirds E♭–c and F–d, the harmony is entirely composed of descending-fifth progressions.
Chapters 7 and 9 will explore these issues further. Readers who are
particularly interested in modern music—or who would like to see
“Gesualdo’s trick” analyzed as a hierarchical sequence-by-inversion—might
want to read appendix 3 while these topics are still fresh.
1
See Schubert 2008, chapter 8, on the Renaissance aesthetic of disguised repetition.
2
The octave, permutation, and transposition symmetries (OPT) are discussed in Callender, Quinn,
and Tymoczko (2008) and Tymoczko 2011a; appendix 3 will incorporate inversion (I) as well.
3
Taneyev [1909] 1962, Segall 2014, and Collins 2015 and 2018; the notation is also similar to
Henry Klumpenhouwer’s model of voice permutation (Klumpenhouwer 1991, see also Harrison
1988). Thanks here to Julian Hook.
4
See Bass 1996, Moreno 1996, Harrison 2003, Ricci 2004, Sprick 2018, and Waltham-Smith 2018.
Both Bass and Schoenberg ([1911] 1983, p. 283) require that sequential periods contain multiple
chords, which is false of many descending-fifth sequences (e.g., Figure 4.3.2). It was the work of
David Feurzeig, many years ago, that provoked me to think about the sequential role of permutation.
5
Taneyev (1909) 1962. Any sequence with differing transpositional subscripts technically involves
some sort of invertible counterpoint.
6
There is a repetition calculator at https://www.madmusicalscience.com/ allowing users to explore
repeating contrapuntal patterns in a hands-on way.
7
Readers may notice that the two solutions recall the first two phrases of the Waldstein, which
opens with a contrary-motion wedge and continues with the diatonic third’s ascending basic voice
leading (§3.7).
8
Geometrically, this resembles the operation known as “parallel transport,” the moving of a vector
along another vector. Here we move a path (the voice leading) along another path that determines the
permutation; in wedge sequences the second path is the same as the first, while in harmonically stable
sequences it is the line of transposition.
9
Repeating contrapuntal patterns are an important part of the Renaissance practice of stretto fuga
(Milsom 2005).
10
Each notation has distinct advantages: crossed T arrows apply to larger sequential units, but are
sensitive to chords’ position in register; dual transpositions Txty are not sensitive to register and
generalize more readily, but may apply only to some chords in the sequence.
11
Thus contrary-motion sequences require voice exchanges. This follows from the fact that
crossing-free voice leadings preserve spacing in chordal steps; contrary-motion sequences change
spacing and thus require voice exchanges.
12
For example, Figures 4.2.2, 4.2.5, or 5.3.4.
13
For other examples see Mozart’s F major piano sonata K.533, I, mm. 24–25, or Beethoven’s C
minor piano sonata, Op. 10, no. 1, mm. 151ff.
14
One sometimes finds patterns like (E5, G5) → (D4, F5) → (C3, E5), as at the end of the
Pathétique episode discussed in the prelude.
15
Suppose we have a two-voice canonic repeating contrapuntal pattern with the top voice moving
by interval x from first chord to second, and bottom voice moving by y. Since the sequence is canonic,
the top voice moves by y from second chord to third while the bottom voice moves by x; both voices
move by x + y from first chord to third. If we keep the first and third chords the same, while expanding
the second so that the top voice moves by x + 1 and bottom voice by y – 1, we preserve the canonic
relationship. If we are willing to use fractional scale-steps (e.g., quarter-tones), then any sequence can
be turned into a purely parallel sequence in this way.
16
Voice exchanges are a particular kind of contrary-motion expansion and contraction; the
“threads” in Gosman 2012 unwind voice exchanges to reveal the parallel motion, much as we are
discussing here.
17
Harmonically unstable sequences involve invertible counterpoint: in the case of the 5–6, the
ascending step is initially a fifth above the constant lower voice, and then a sixth below the constant
upper voice; this is invertible counterpoint at the tenth.
18
In 1719, Moritz Johann Vogt explicitly noted the imitative qualities of the diatonic third’s basic
voice leading (Morgan 1978, p. 84).
19
In other places, such as the development of Op. 59, no. 1, IV, mm. 123ff, Beethoven realizes the
same motive’s contrary-motion potential.
20
Arndt 2011.
21
Slottow 2018.
22
For more, see the end of the Agnus of Palestrina’s eight-voice mass Fratres ego enim accepi, the
final phrase of Bach’s “Meine Seele erhebet den Herren” (BWV 10.7, Riemenschneider 358), mm.
17–21, the second F major fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier, mm. 29–33, and the first movement of
Beethoven’s Second Symphony, mm. 88–95. Traces can also be found in mm. 11–12 of Morley’s
“April Is in My Mistress’ Face” (Figure 2.9.2). The parallel thirds in Gosman 2009 are closely related.
23
Examples include the second D minor fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier (mm. 11–12), the
second theme of Mozart’s Symphony no. 40, I (mm. 56–58), and Brahms’s Op. 116, no. 2 (m. 63).
24
Note that the (G, B) → (F♯, D) progression in the second measure belongs both to the opening
contrary-motion fifth pattern and the subsequent fauxbourdon pattern. That fauxbourdon pattern links
the top voice to the middle voice, suggesting a series of voice overlaps; this could be modeled using
voice crossings in the two-note scale containing the chord’s root and fifth.
25
Examples include Mozart’s C minor piano fantasy, K.475, mm. 102–105 (standing on the
dominant), Haydn’s C major quartet Op. 50, no. 2, IV, mm. 211ff (ambiguous between dominant and
predominant), and Mozart’s C major piano concerto K.467, II, mm. 45–50. Bass 1996, note 3,
describes the last example as a “purely melodic” sequence.
26
The right-hand dyads ascend systematically through 2-in-3 space (Figure 3.7.3), moving
clockwise and skipping the D–A perfect consonance: (A, F ♯ )→ (D, F ♯ )→ (F ♯ , A) → and so on,
alternating τ –1t2 and t1, with τ being transposition along the dyad and t transposition along the triad.
The example also shows that the left hand hints at its own canonic structure.
27
Other examples include the end of “Es ist das Heil uns kommen her” (BWV 155.5,
Riemenschneider 335), and Figure 7.7.9.
28
Thanks to Daniel Harrison for bringing this example to my attention.
29
Yellin 1998, Rice 2015.
30
Just kidding.
31
The tonic note moves down by fourth, the dominant down by fifth; consequently, composers can
choose between a real answer in the dominant, a nearly subdominant answer, or some mixture of the
two. In many of The Well-Tempered Clavier’s fugue subjects, only the first dominant note is echoed at
the fifth below; but some have more thoroughly subdominant answers (e.g., the first E♭ major and G♯
minor fugues).
32
Similarly, Shostakovich’s more chromatic fugue subjects combine diatonic and chromatic
intervals.
33
This “horn fifths” pattern exploits the same structure that gives rise to Figures 4.6.1–3,
alternating between the vertical dyads A♭–C and E♭–B♭.
34
Russell 2018.
35
See also Figure 4.3.8, Monteverdi’s “Cosi sol d’un chiara fonte” (Madrigals, Book 8, no. 4, part
2 of “Hor ch’el Ciel e la Terra,” mm. 14ff), and Beethoven’s First Symphony, Op. 21, I, mm. 81–86.
When triads are in close position, this can produce free alternations of descending thirds and fifths,
with either the fifth or the third and fifth moving up by step.
36
See chapter 8 of Tymoczko 2011a.
37
In the last variation the falling fifths of the third variation are factored into falling thirds (§7.2).
38
Meyer 1982, Gosman 2009, and Yust 2015a all explore ideas related to sequential reduction.
Christensen 1993a refers to Rameau’s “mechanization” of harmonic motion.
39
This analysis is my own simplification of one by David Damschroder (2018, p. 30), who should
not be held responsible for it. The melodic F is viewed as a decoration of the A ♭ , while the other
melodic thirds are treated differently.
Prelude
Three Varieties of Analytical Reduction
Figure P5.1. (left) The opening of the first Minuet from Bach’s Partita in B♭, BWV 825. (right)
Measures 3–4 of Bach’s D minor two-part invention BWV 775.
Figure P5.2. Beethoven’s Op. 54, II, mm. 14–15. De-arpeggiation turns the D♯ into a chromatic
neighbor.
Figure P5.3. Three analyses of the right hand at the start of the first G major prelude in The Well-
Tempered Clavier. The first postulates a three-voice chorale, while the others postulate a melodic
sequence moving along a small scale. The choice of the pattern’s final melodic interval determines the
scalar voice leading.
Schema theorists would argue that this gap between theory and practice
can be explained by the way earlier composers learned their trade—as a
practice, a set of musical routines, a spoken language whose grammar was
not explicitly theorized. (And of course at an early age, rather than as young
adults in college classrooms.) English speakers naturally and unconsciously
accept the dummy subject in a sentence like “it’s raining”: by the time we
are old enough to wonder what “it” is, we are so accustomed to the phrase
that we do not feel its strangeness. In much the same way, a young musician
might play hundreds of passages such as those in Figures P5.4–6 before
noticing that they could be said to contain forbidden parallels. Such passages
would simply be part of the language, the way “it’s raining” is part of ours.
As a result, we should not be surprised to find arpeggiated accompaniments
suspended somewhere between genuine polyphony and undifferentiated
harmony.
This response is fine as far as it goes, but I suspect that there is also a
deeper issue here: eighteenth-century musicians recognized a class of
musical objects that were qualitatively different from, and analytically
irreducible to, chorale-like arrangements of pitches—nonspecific
“accompanimental stuff” that augments the concrete soprano and bass voices
of figured-bass notation. The picture of chords-as-scales allows us to model
this richer ontology without abandoning analytical rigor: in Figure P5.7, for
example, we can postulate a concrete bass and soprano, the latter articulated
by the top of an arpeggio that moves along a shifting triadic alphabet.5 This
arpeggio activates a background not localized to any specific register and
substantially immune to standard contrapuntal strictures. (There is no
possibility of forbidden parallels within the abstract background, nor
between background and surface; parallels occur between concrete voices
only.6) From a hierarchical perspective there is nothing suspect about this
passage; but if we try to reduce it to specific pitches, then we can avoid
parallels only at the cost of radically inconsistent analysis.7
Figure P5.7. The start of the trio of Beethoven’s Op. 2, no. 3. Carl Schachter’s reduction postulates a
doubled A3 that is not motivated by the music; my alternative features a regular melodic sequence in a
shifting scalar background.
Western music has traditionally been a music of rules: rules about how
chords are constituted, rules constraining the relation between harmonic and
nonharmonic, rules governing the avoidance of parallels or antiparallels,
rules about which notes can be doubled, rules about how chords progress,
and countless others besides. The rules regulating nonharmonic tones are
perhaps the most important, in large part because they ensure a certain sort
of teachability—allowing us to mark our students right and wrong without
consulting our aesthetic preferences. They are also intimately connected to
the practice of reductive analysis, the replacing of nonharmonic tones with
their harmonic partners. In many styles this produces a harmonic skeleton
that can be said to carry the weight of the musical argument. The
nonharmonic system thus allows theorists to play the role of structural
engineers, distinguishing the load-bearing and ornamental walls of the
musical house.
All of which works much better in theory than in practice. For as we will
see, received views of nonharmonicity are empirically inadequate,
assimilating suspensions to decorations even though they resist our best
attempts at reduction. They are plagued by inconsistency, as when they treat
the permissibility of parallel fifths on different musical levels. And they are
aesthetically problematic, encouraging us to ignore the myriad ways in
which nonharmonic tones can contribute to fundamentally harmonic
narratives. We do not have to stray far from the Austro-Germanic canon to
find composers like Domenico Scarlatti blatantly disregarding traditional
rules—or styles in which the distinction between “chordal” and
“nonchordal” is inherently blurry.1 These problems have provoked some
theorists to protest against the very notion of nonharmonicity, searching for
alternatives in which harmonic and nonharmonic are on a more equal
footing.2 Indeed, this is one motivation for the recent turn to figured-bass
nomenclature, where there is a less-sharp distinction between the harmonic
and linear realms.3
One issue here is the theoretical tendency to overvalue rules at the
expense of concrete idioms, a Platonic streak running throughout the history
of Western musical thought. Beyond that, however, is a deeper and more
uncomfortable fact about the language itself, namely that composers
sometimes viewed the nonharmonic system with a degree of irony, writing
music in which purportedly nonharmonic notes had important harmonic
effects. The nonharmonic system was less like a system of moral
commandments than it was like the speed limit, often seen as a nuisance and
sometimes obeyed only grudgingly.4 (This is obviously true of Monteverdi,
Domenico Scarlatti, and Mahler, but I believe it holds for Palestrina and J. S.
Bach as well.) Like the speed limit, the rules were real enough, explaining
behavior that would otherwise remain inexplicable. But a deep
understanding of earlier music requires acknowledging the gap between the
theoretical precepts of the nonharmonic system and the meaning of the
music produced under its aegis. This leads to an awkward terminological
problem, for we should neither claim that “nonharmonic tones are harmonic”
nor assert its negation. Nonharmonicity, or perhaps nonharmonicity, is an
inherently unstable notion, both useful and misleading, explanatory and yet
false to the music it purports to describe.5
Misunderstanding this point sometimes leads pedagogues to discourage
the very techniques that enabled composers to express themselves freely.
Thus we forbid practices that were merely somewhat uncommon:
consecutive strong-beat parallels, parallels masked by suspension, and
antiparallel fifths in four or fewer voices.6 Haydn’s reported comments about
Kirnberger aptly express this point: “too cautious, too confining, too
everlastingly many infinitely tiny restrictions for a free spirit.”7 This
persnickitude is often associated with the abdication of aesthetic
responsibility, for by forbidding antiparallels we allow ourselves to avoid
questions about whether a particular passage is pleasing or stylistic: the
whole subject is simply off the table. An alternative would be to
deemphasize rules in favor of more directly musical questions, emphasizing
fluid creativity over legalistic niceties. Sometimes what looks like rigor is
really just a desire to make it through the day with our red pencils intact.
1. The first practice and the SNAP system
The first great era of nonharmonic control was based on what I call “the
SNAP system”: suspensions, neighbors, anticipations, and passing tones.
Though often said to originate with Josquin, this prima prattica stabilized
somewhat later, in the music of Palestrina and contemporaries such as
Lassus and Byrd.8 Earlier polyphony features an array of incomplete
neighbors, upward-resolving suspensions, and the occasional inexplicable
moment (Figure 5.1.1). The Palestrina style largely eliminates these,
retaining the cambiata as the sole surviving incomplete neighbor. The
resulting music is astonishing in its systematicity, almost completely free of
the occasional glitches that mark most human productions. Among his other
achievements, Palestrina was one of the great proofreaders in human history.
Figure 5.1.1. Unusual nonharmonic tones. In mm. 106–107 of Josquin’s motet “Liber generationis,”
we have both an incomplete neighbor and an upward-resolving suspension (A–B on beat 4). In mm.
18–19 of Clemens’s “Concussum est Mare,” we have a nonharmonic tone that is both leapt-to and
leapt-away-from.
C–E ♭ –G–B ♭ ). The problem is the starred G, a step away from the
suspension’s resolution; it is approached and left by leap and hence cannot
be assimilated to any known species of Renaissance dissonance. We are
forced either to invent an entirely new nonharmonic tone solely to account
for this figure, or else admit that there are suspensions that cannot be
reduced to a consonant background.
Figure 5.1.6. In mm. 9–11 of the Benedictus of Palestrina’s mass Io mi son giovinetta (1570), the
fourth above the bass resolves a suspension. In mm. 5–6 of the Kyrie of the mass Ave regina
coelarum, the leapt-to A is harmonic, which suggests that the alto F is similarly harmonic; otherwise,
it has to stand for the following E.
Figure 5.1.7. “121” neighbor and passing tones that cut across the binary division of the beat,
sometimes called “fake suspensions,” from Palestrina’s Pope Marcellus Mass (Gloria, m. 9) and
Adrian Willaert’s motet O salutaris hostia (m. 36). This idiom often gives rise to a 64 chord,
occupying the first half of the nonharmonic tone’s duration.
Figure 5.1.8. Three irreducible seventh chords in mm. 72–75 of the Gloria of Palestrina’s mass Veni
Sancte Spiritus. The starred notes cannot be dissonances according to standard contrapuntal theory.
Figure 5.1.9. The Renaissance seventh’s standard resolutions. From a modern perspective, all involve
root progressions of a descending third or descending fifth.
Suspensions are also unique in being the only nonharmonic tones that
regularly license parallels at the level of the harmonic skeleton—both in the
Renaissance and later (Figure 5.1.10). This poses another obstacle to the
view that suspensions are merely decorative, as it seems incoherent to say
that the first passage in Figure 5.1.11 is grammatical, the second is
nongrammatical, and the first stands for, embellishes, or represents the
second. The challenge is exacerbated by the cadential idiom in Figure
5.1.12; here parallels are often said to be permissible because they occur
“only on the surface,” disappearing when we consider the harmonic
skeleton. But how can it be that some parallels are allowed because they
occur only on the surface while others are allowed because they occur only
in the harmonic skeleton?
Figure 5.1.10. Suspensions masking parallel fifths in the harmonic skeleton, in the Kyrie of
Palestrina’s mass Assumpta est Maria (m. 54), and Bach’s chorale Ich hab mein Sach Gott
heimgestellt (BWV 351, Riemenschneider 19, m. 7).
Figure 5.1.11. The progression on the left is common, while the progression on the right almost never
appears. Should we say that the former stands for the latter?
Figure 5.1.12. An idiom in which parallel fifths appear “on the surface.” Here the anticipated tonic
appears alongside 5̂–4̂–3̂ to create fifths. For an example, see Bach’s Chorale “Freuet euch, ihr
Christen” (BWV 40.8, Riemenschneider 8, m. 4).
Figure 5.1.16. Sounding dominant seventh chords in mm. 11–12 of the Sanctus of Palestrina’s mass
Aeterna Christi munera and m. 38 of the Agnus of the mass In semiduplicibus majoribus (II).
2. Schoenberg’s critique
Figure 5.2.1. Two interpretations of the 64 chord in the Kyrie from Palestrina’s Descendit angelus
Domini, mm. 39–40.
Figure 5.2.2. The most common suspension figures in Palestrina’s masses, transposed so that the bass
of the first sonority is C.
Figure 5.2.5. Many different nonharmonic tones conspiring to produce the same “dominant
thirteenth” sonority in Chopin’s mazurkas: Op. 6, no. 3, mm. 89–90; Op. 41, no. 1, mm. 41–44; Op.
41, no. 2, mm. 1–2; and Op. 41, no. 2, mm. 18–20.
Figure 5.2.6. Three appearances of the “Tristan chord” that could be analyzed as the byproduct of
nonharmonic tones.
3. Monteverdi’s “Ohimè”
Figure 5.3.1. Some contrapuntal oddities in Monteverdi’s madrigals. In A the upper voices are easiest
to understand as rhythmically displaced passing tones rather than suspensions: the F5 in m. 1 is
dissonant and hence not a preparation. In B the lowest voice arrives at F♯ a quarter note before the rest
of the voices; this could be either a leapt-to anticipation or a weak-beat suspension of the E. In C the
top voice resolves the suspension D upward to E. Example D is a standard cadential figure occurring
under an upper-voice pedal D5. In E the C5 seventh is unprepared. In F the top voice leaps away from
the seventh (G5 down to C).
Figure 5.3.2. The nonharmonic tones in A conspire to produce fourth chords. In B the harmonies are
difficult to identify.
expect E♭.) One interpretive option is to take them at face value, as leaping-
third anticipations not entirely foreign to the tradition (e.g., the first passage
in Figure 5.4.6). Another is to imagine a hocket with rhythmic and registral
displacement, analyzing the high-register A and C in m. 2 as incomplete
neighbors attached to the preceding low-register G and B ♭ (Figure 5.3.3).
This is how the figure later appears (e.g., top two voices of mm. 6–8, and
alto and tenor in mm. 9–11). This reading also reflects the text, sundering the
sighs into male and female (low and high), rather than allowing them to
sound together as a unified syntactical structure.34 In either case, the crucial
point is that nonharmonic reduction has become an interpretative rather than
mechanical process: as in late nineteenth-century music, it is genuinely
unclear how to reduce the surface to a well-behaved harmonic skeleton—
with none of the answers being completely convincing.
Figure 5.3.3. The beginning of “Ohimè,” along with a reading in which the incomplete neighbors
embellish their predecessors.
The next six measures (“se tanto amate di sentir dir “Ohimè” / “if you so
love to hear me say alas”) present a familiar ascending-fifth sequence that
develops the preceding bars both melodically and harmonically (Figure
5.3.4).35 Cantus, tenor, and alto sing a six-beat melodic canon that
incorporates the “alas” motif, with Quinto and alto doubling at the third
below; this canon is essentially the ascending version of the diatonic third’s
basic voice leading, split into different octaves and with the bass sounding
chord roots. The sequential unit’s third half-note is harmonically variable,
supporting a first-inversion triad with a bass that either sustains the
preceding bass note or descends by third.36 As in rock modality, these
ascending-fifth root progressions produce descending-step voice leading.37
One can admire the compositional technique in designing the incipit of the
canon to fit with the “Ohimè” continuation, so that what initially sounds like
development transmutes into something more like resolution or explanation.
Figure 5.3.4. “Ohimè,” mm. 5–19, with brackets marking the six-beat sequence. Underneath, an
analysis of the opening canon.
The next text fragment (“deh perché fate” / “then why do you”) features a
new motive that overlaps with the end of the canon; here the suspensions
resolve by leap.38 Figure 5.3.5 analyzes the passage as a distorted ascending-
third sequence. The outer-voice counterpoint here is essentially parallel, with
the top voice initially singing chordal fifths and then roots (“thinking within
the chord,” §3.7). Monteverdi obscures this structure in two ways: first by
reversing the order of the phrases in the sequential repeat, and second by
reaching a cadence at the end of the first unit. This makes the second unit
feel like both a continuation and a phrase beginning; our ears hear some of
the organization in Figure 5.3.5 but only obscurely. (The A cadence also ends
the ascending-fifth harmonic motion that began in m. 3, B ♭ –F–c–g–d–A
crossing motivic and phrasal boundaries.) The reordering transforms the
relatively standard deceptive progression A–B♭ into the more dramatic A–F
(“the major-third system”). The passage continues with “chi dice ‘Ohimè’
morire?” (“kill the one who says ‘Alas?’ ”), returning to the A major triad via
another major-third progression, with the cadential dominant extensively
decorated by incomplete neighbors, an unprepared seventh, and octave-
displaced passing motions—a free counterpoint of lines decorating a stable
but hazy harmony.
Figure 5.3.5. A hypothetical derivation of mm. 8–15. Question marks are suspensions resolved by
leap. The result is an ABBA palindrome.
The piece’s second large section starts with the upper two voices moving
in parallel thirds (Figure 5.3.6), freely harmonized by descending steps and
descending fifths, and shifting position from root-third to third-fifth. (The
underlying logic here is shared with the Pachelbel progression, which
features descending melodic voices and is codified as a specific piece of
functional vocabulary; in the Renaissance, the same basic relationships are
exploited more freely.) This quasi-sequence dissolves in a wonderful
reduction of the harmonic rhythm, the voices singing a slow, sad “doloroso”
on ascending scale fragments. The voices take turn ascending by fourth and
fifth, harmony secondary to linear motion until the final cadence on D. The
phrase repeats up a fourth and with a fuller texture; the last melodic ascent is
displaced upward by third, producing a ♭ 9 incomplete neighbor and an
unapologetic seventh chord in m. 37. It is interesting that the two phrases
both start on a root-position D triad: the first begins with the descending
second (d–d–C–F– . . .), the parallel thirds sounding root-third, while the
second begins with the descending fifth (D–g–F–B♭– . . .), the parallel voices
sounding third-fifth.39
Figure 5.3.6. “Ohimè,” mm. 20–38, along with a reduction of each phrase’s opening.
Figure 5.3.7 presents the poem’s “turn” (“but if, my love, you wish to let
me live and live for me . . .”), set in a lydian-inflected B♭ and one of just two
phrases in the piece that are not immediately repeated.40 The shift to a faster
tempo and brighter tonality effectively conveys the poem’s move toward
amatory hope. Harmonically it is the piece’s most straightforward music,
replete with functional progressions and organized around an ascending bass
line from B♭2 to B♭3, with the upper voice outlining a stepwise descent from
F5 to B♭4.41 Contrapuntally, it is noteworthy for the dissonances in mm. 41–
The final section delivers the punchline, the promise of endless satisfying
sighs shared between lovers (Figure 5.3.8). We begin with emphatic V–I–V
progressions in G and C minor, the second containing undisguised parallel
fifths. The music then returns to the opening “Ohimè,” now harmonized with
parallel first-inversion triads; these create deceptive-resolution pairs (e.g.,
D–E♭, C–d, B♭–c), leading to a b6–g cadence closer to Poulenc than standard
Renaissance harmony. (Here the opening bars’ octave-displaced hocketing
has been regularized to a single line.) The lower voices then echo this music,
alternating tonic and dominant dyads in G dorian and recalling some of the
analytical challenges of the opening (Figure 5.3.9).
Figure 5.3.8. “Ohimè,” final section.
Figure 5.3.9. In mm. 53–55, we can read harmonies changing in quarter notes, as in the surrounding
phrases, or syncopated half-note motion.
This is the sort of passage that provoked the most famous police action in
Western music history, Giovanni Artusi’s attempt to enforce the authority of
the learned tradition. Much of the commentary around this incident centers
on Giulio Cesare Monteverdi’s suggestion that text-setting justifies a
seconda prattica that departs from strict counterpoint. To my mind, however,
that is something of a red herring. More fundamental is the attempt to shift
the debate from inviolable musical rules toward acts of aesthetic judgment.
The crucial point, in other words, is the suggestion that we need to consider
whether a given passage is aesthetically fitting, rather than whether it obeys
the time-independent rules of the prima prattica.43
To be sure, the particular judgment Monteverdi asks us to make is one that
compares musical effects to poetic texts, but put that aside for the moment;
focus instead on the general form of the question. “Do I find this particular
musical passage to be compelling?” That question can be asked without
reference to a text, precisely as we do when modern music takes a more
flexible attitude toward the nonharmonic realm. Here the Monteverdis can
start to seem uncannily contemporary, as if they had anticipated modernity
three centuries too early: for the fundamental freedom they desired—to have
counterpoint judged by its aesthetic effects rather than by an unquestionable
set of musical laws—was one that composers did not obtain until the early
twentieth century. By contrast, Artusi seems to think that aesthetic judgment
should come into play only after we have verified that the music conforms to
the timeless norms of the strict style. What makes Monteverdi radical is the
subjecting of the rules themselves to aesthetic evaluation: for each of his
unusual contrapuntal effects, we are supposed to ask, “do I appreciate this
passage?” regardless of whether it violates traditional norms.44
Construed like this, the debate starts to look like a draw at best.45
Historians have sometimes dismissed Artusi as an ineffectual conservative,
pointlessly resisting the inexorable force of musical progress. This may be
justified so long as we focus narrowly on the question of prima prattica
rules. But if we turn to the more general question “should nonharmonic
tones conform to some rigorous contrapuntal grammar, whatever it may be?”
then we have to admit that history gave an answer more Artusian than
Monteverdian. Yes, the classical tradition incorporated some of Monteverdi’s
specific innovations, but it subjected them to rules as inviolable as those of
the prima prattica: in this sense, Monteverdi was an evolutionary dead end,
an unheard avatar of compositional freedom. While it may be true that Artusi
had a fundamentally scholastic mind, it is also true that the notated tradition
retained a scholastic streak right up to the twentieth century. That
scholasticism still survives, for though we live in a Monteverdian world, we
tend to become Artusi the moment we step into our classrooms.
4. The standardized second practice
Figure 5.4.3. An incomplete neighbor in m. 5 of “Wie schön leuchtet der Morgenstern” (BWV 172.6,
Riemenschneider 323).
Figure 5.4.4. An incomplete neighbor to an incomplete neighbor in m. 6 of the second B♭ major fugue
in The Well-Tempered Clavier.
Pedal tones are more complicated, in part because they are both a
rhetorical effect and species of nonharmonic tone: one can create the sense
of a pedal by changing chords over a static bass note, even if that bass is
always part of the prevailing harmony. Formally, a pedal tone is any note
that is initially harmonic and sustained through one or more chord
changes.49 In practice, however, this definition is overbroad, encompassing
such phenomena as Monteverdi’s “suspensions that resolve by leap,” which
may not feel much like pedals. Actual pedal tones are more schematic,
almost always occurring on the tonic or dominant scale degree, and typically
returning to harmonic status before they move: to my knowledge, Mozart’s
piano sonatas contain no exceptions to these principles among their hundred
or so pedals, though J. S. Bach’s chorales do contain a few (Figure 5.4.5).50
Pedals are unique in the nonharmonic bestiary insofar as they are
irreducible: where the other nonharmonic tones can be said to embellish
some specific note, pedals do not; removing them requires recomposition
rather than reduction. Many textbooks dodge this issue by stipulating that the
rest of the music embellishes the pedal tone itself, but I think this conflates
the syntactic process of nonharmonic reduction with the very different
enterprise of summarizing reduction.
Figure 5.4.5. Three unusual pedal tones in the Bach chorales: “Jesu Leiden, Pein und Tod” (BWV
245.14 Riemenschneider 83, mm. 11–12), “Nun danket alle Gott” (BWV 252, Riemenschneider 330,
mm. 3–4), and “O Gott, du frommer Gott” (BWV 399, Riemenschneider 315, mm. 1–2).
By the late baroque, many composers had once again become formulaic in
their treatment of nonharmonic tones. J. S. Bach in particular can be
astoundingly systematic: of more than ten thousand nonharmonic tones in
his chorales, all but about ten are well behaved. Most of the anomalies,
illustrated in Figure 5.4.6, involve what might be called “arpeggiated
anticipations,” where an arpeggiating voice (always moving down by third,
usually from tonic to subdominant) reaches the next harmony an eighth note
early; this is reminiscent of the start of “Ohimè.” There are a few other
unusual moments, including a trio of weak-beat suspensions, the
nonstandard pedals in Figure 5.4.5, a couple dozen “121” passing and
neighboring tones, a few upward-resolving suspensions, and so forth. Here
Monteverdian anarchy has been domesticated, standardized into a “second
practice” whose rules are almost as inviolable as Palestrina’s. This system
remains in force throughout much of the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries,
with few composers coming anywhere close to Monteverdi’s radical
freedom.
Figure 5.4.6. Anomalous nonharmonic tones in the chorales: “Herzlich lieb hab ich dich, o Herr”
(BWV 245.40, Riemenschneider 107, mm. 14–15), “Meinen Jesum laß ich nicht, weil . . .” (BWV
154.8, Riemenschneider 152, mm. 10–11), “Christus, der uns selig macht” (BWV 245.15,
Riemenschneider 81, mm. 11–12).
5. A loophole
The nonharmonic system does not directly deal with sonic resultants, but
only with the linear behavior of dissonant voices—a technical loophole
licensing almost any imaginable dissonance.56 Beethoven’s Ninth Symphony
uses incomplete neighbors to sound a complete harmonic minor scale, the
same scandalous set that stomps away at the opening of Stravinsky’s “Dance
of the Adolescents” (Figure 5.5.1).57 This Schreckensfanfare is one of
several famous moments where Beethoven uses nonharmonic tones to create
unusual harmonies, joined by the Eroica’s “false horn entry,” and the end of
the first movement of the Lebewohl sonata. If such anomalies are rare in
earlier music, it is largely for reasons of taste: as a general rule, earlier
composers intended to create consonant surfaces in which nonharmonic
tones avoided extreme dissonances.58 These intentions start to change over
the course of the nineteenth century even while the musical syntax remains
the same. The risk is that we focus too much on the stable syntax at the
expense of the evolving aesthetics.59
Figure 5.5.1. The same seven-note sonority appears in the last movement of Beethoven’s Ninth
Symphony (m. 208, just before the first vocal entrance) and Stravinsky’s Rite of Spring.
A–G–F ♯ over E minor, the melody ascending by fifth while the harmony
moves by step. The impression is less of harmony as container for the
melody than of melody as an independent actor that creates tension by
shifting its relation to the harmonic structure.
The music now turns to the relative major, oscillating between I, V, and
various decorations of V (mm. 24ff). The general technique—extensive
chromatic activity above a dominant pedal—is familiar, here taken to
Mahlerian extremes. Measure 26 gives us a flurry of eighth notes, A7 leading
to G7, the most conventional of the common-tone dominant sevenths (§3.8).
The chromaticism increases as the melody rises: in m. 28 we have a second
flurry on another common-tone dominant, B7 over G.60 This leads to the
remarkable explosion of mm. 29–30, with the upper voices descending at
different speeds along the minor seventh G♯–B–D♯–F♯, each note acquiring
a semitonal upper neighbor.61 The result is gleaming and uncategorizable, as
deliciously painful as nitrous oxide in a dentist’s chair.
The second phrase echoes its predecessor, now with a chipper violin
melody and still circling around the “B7 over G” dominant (mm. 32ff). This
leads to a long sequence of descending triads that I hear pointing to E7, or
V/V in D minor. The expected cadence is elided with the start of the next
phrase, the only interruption of the four-bar phrasing since m. 7. This is a
fine example of Mahler’s ability to use purely instrumental forces to convey
complex emotional states like intrusion, digression, and hesitation. The
subsequent music once again invokes the ascending voice leading of the
common-tone diminished seventh: here we have a surprising E–G–B–D
moving to the expected E–G♯–B–D, with the G ascending to G♯ over the
course of two measures, the E minor seventh the distant cousin of the
standard common-tone diminished seventh E–G–A♯–C♯.62 This ascending
semitonal voice leading continues through the phrase, moving through F–A–
B( ♭ )–D, to F ♯ –A–B–D ♯ , seventh chords ratcheting upward underneath a
truncated opening motif.63 I found this passage completely unintelligible
until I became aware of the retrofunctional idioms in §3.8.64
So goes my account of the opening section’s harmony. On the page it may
seem straightforward, but it was formulated over countless hours spread over
many years, with many wrong turns and rethinkings along the way. I have no
expectation that readers will agree with it, and indeed Mahler’s surface
presents numerous details that argue against my reading.
1. The V/V in m. 8 occurs over an E–A fifth that does not belong to the
underlying harmony, creating a D–D♯–E cluster on the downbeat; I treat
the E as a reattacked pedal tone surviving from the preceding harmony,
though it appears in a new octave as part of an active bass line (Figure
5.5.3). As in an improvisation, the bass player simply arrives at the
harmony a bit late.
2. My analysis treats the wind parts in mm. 15–18 as largely independent
of the underlying harmony, the neighbor notes moving from weak to
strong eighth notes with every repetition of the four-note motive (Figure
5.5.4, top staff). Here they act as a kind of orchestrational thickening
hinting at pandiatonicism.
3. The suggestion of a stable V in mm. 23–26 is undercut by bass motion
that could be taken to represent chord changes (Figure 5.5.5).
4. The “e minor seventh” in m. 38 is actually a minor ninth, occurring over
a towering clash: C–C♯–D–E–F–A on beat 4 of m. 37 and D–E–F–G on
beat 1 of m. 38 (Figure 5.5.6). The D–E–F bass could suggest that E is
passing.
These are genuine issues, but I know of no better analysis. The problem is
that Mahler’s language is so contrapuntally and harmonically inventive as to
make objective analysis almost impossible; there are simply too many ways
to parse the extended chords, pervasive linear dissonances, and unusual
progressions.65 (As we will see in the prelude to chapter 7, nonharmonic
reduction typically requires some prior understanding of the music’s
harmonic vocabulary.) While I strongly suspect that Mahler imagined some
reductive template along the lines of Figure 5.5.2, that template is all but
unrecoverable. The upshot is that nonharmonic reduction is no longer
something broadly intersubjective or syntactical, but rather autobiographical
and interpretive, a record of one’s personal coming-to-terms with a
refractory and ambiguous musical surface. Here we have the familiar
modernist slippage between compositional process and musical result, albeit
in the context of functional tonality.
Once again, I find myself pulled in two directions. In some sense, the
basic theoretical picture of “skeleton with decoration” seems reasonably apt:
if one function of analysis is to tell us how we might make a piece, then I
find my reduction to be successful, for I can just about imagine turning
Figure 5.5.2 into Mahler’s actual score. (And when I listen to the piece, I
have a sense of tonal coherence that seems consistent with the reduction.) At
the same time, the very act of reduction slights Mahler’s frenzied
counterpoint, its lines careening as close to chaos as those of Nancarrow or
Schoenberg. If Western norms of dissonance treatment were produced by a
conversation between composer and theorist, one gets the sense that the
dialogue has broken down. This is virtually explicit in the movement’s
sardonic dedication “to my brothers in Apollo”: superficially, the music
proves that Mahler could write counterpoint; but the pervasive delirium is
the sign of something deeper—an ironic attitude toward syntax itself. If the
meaning of this music lies in its clashes, extended sonorities and vectors of
dissonance, then it represents a deliberate challenge to the very idea of
nonharmonic reduction.
6. After nonharmonicity
intersecting to saturate chromatic space from B♭3 to E♭4. The beauty of this
music, to my mind, lies in the skillful blending of multiple logics—one
nontonal and set-theoretical, another tonal and harmonic, a third
emphasizing a descending fifth from B♭4 to E♭4. The same combination can
be found in jazz, originating with the blues and theorized under the rubric
“playing outside” (Figure 5.6.2). Unlike their weightless atonal cousins,
these sets acquire differential significance as they move in and out of the
key, flashes of color like fish in a coral reef.
Figure 5.6.1. The opening of Shostakovich’s Ninth String Quartet.
Figure 5.6.2. The fourth chorus of McCoy Tyner’s solo on “Pursuance,” from A Love Supreme.
Brackets show moments where the melodic 035 sets align with the harmony.
Figure 5.6.3. Mozart’s Symphony no. 36, K.425, mm. 158ff, with Schenker’s hypothesized perception
of the chromatic ascent.
Figure 5.6.4. Chopin’s Nocturne, Op. 27, no. 2, mm. 32–33, in which complete triads slide
chromatically from dominant to tonic.
Figure 5.6.5. The end of Kabalevsky’s “Ditty” (Op. 30, no. 2), where parallel chromatic major thirds
create the harmonies.
In effect, Schenker took passing tones as the model for all dissonances,
and nonharmonic reduction as the model for reduction more generally.72
Here I have reversed his argument, suggesting that suspensions are
fundamentally less reducible than passing tones, and using that irreducibility
to propose that reduction is generally problematic. My claim is that the
nonharmonic system, rather than a means of prolongation, is a mechanism
for generating dissonances whose meaning and metaphysics is up for grabs
—with musicians and theorists sometimes disagreeing about their status. In
the bottom passage of Figure 5.2.4, it is vital to hear the “composite sound”
formed by the passing D♯ and the harmonic A; just as in Figure 5.2.1 it is
vital to hear the composite sound of the 64 formed by the suspension and the
consonant sixth. If this is right, then there is an inconsistency at the heart of
traditional musical syntax, a mismatch between the grammatical rules
composers followed and the harmonic meaning of the music they made.
One underappreciated feature of that grammar is its combining of
intervals from distinct alphabets (§4.8). In earlier music, nontriadic alphabets
usually produce decorative notes subordinate to those of the prevailing
harmony.73 Twentieth-century musicians sometimes combined alphabets
without this sort of subordination: Figure 5.6.6, from the end of Stravinsky’s
Rite of Spring, is similar to Louis Armstrong’s line in “West End Blues”
(Figure P2.2, §4.8), lower notes moving along the B♭ minor triad, with the
top voice moving along the C major triad, two or three semitones above its
lower neighbor; here, a complex transformational network creates a variety
of set classes sharing the same abstract structure. While the network is
reminiscent of the one we used to describe Armstrong’s phrase, it has a very
different meaning: the first note of Armstrong’s motive is an out-of-harmony
neighbor that resolves to a chord tone, whereas the top note of Stravinsky’s
chord is part of the harmony itself.
Figure 5.6.6. The Rite of Spring, upper voices at R196.
1
Sutcliffe 2003, chapter 5.
2
See, for example, Schoenberg (1911) 1983, Narmour 1988, or Quinn 2018.
3
See Gjerdingen 2007 and Holtmeier 2007, who essentially invert Riemann’s argument that
figured-bass notation makes too little distinction between harmonic and nonharmonic (Riemann 1896,
p. 107).
4
In the course of a sensitive discussion, Weber (1817–1821) 1846, p. 830, protests against “the
furious persecution against every thing that has the name of fifths.”
5
For clarity, I will continue to use the standard term “nonharmonic” even though it occasionally
leads to paradoxical language.
6
Sometimes pedagogues forbid practices that are actually common, such as cadences in which
inner-voice leading tones do not resolve, or doubling the third of a first-inversion tonic. Some of these
superstrict prohibitions can be traced back to dogmas contemporaneous with the music in question,
suggesting a longstanding conflict between theoretical regularization and compositional heterogeneity.
See, for example, Vicentino’s prohibitions on simultaneous leaps in ensembles of various sizes
(Vicentino [1555] 1996, book 2, chapter 30, Arthur 2021).
7
Quoted in Mirka 2015, p. 165.
8
See, for example, Taruskin 2005, volume 2, chapters 14–16, which identifies Josquin as the
source of the “ars perfecta.” Monteverdi’s brother Giulio Cesare instead identifies Ockeghem (Strunk
1950, p. 408). Counterpoint textbooks tend to take Palestrina’s practice as representative of this
broader and more varied tradition.
9
See the prelude to chapter 7. John Miller performed the extraordinary service of digitizing all of
Palestrina’s well-attested masses. See Sailor and Sigler 2017 for a quantitative study focusing on
dyadic pairs.
10
Unfortunately, further improvements take much more work; as with most programming projects,
a small amount of effort produces the lion’s share of the results.
11
This is one way of understanding Schoenberg’s statement that dissonances are “comprehended”
with reference to consonances (Schoenberg 1984, p. 259, and Tymoczko 2011a, p. 185): that the
nontriadic surface is understood in terms of a triadic object that never appears.
12
By my count, freestanding anticipations, not decorating suspensions and not themselves
decorated by other notes, comprise less than 1% of Palestrina’s dissonances—making them less
frequent than ascending neighbors. Freestanding anticipations are much more common in Josquin.
13
This figure sometimes appears in eighth notes (sixteenths in our notation), as in m. 10 of the
Benedictus of the mass Ave Regina coelorum.
14
Knud Jeppesen ([1946] 1970, p. 124) notes that accented third-species dissonances are
acceptable for Palestrina only when they form this slippery figure, and not in the context of
unidirectional melodic lines.
15
I call these “121 passing and neighboring tones”: three melodic notes with durations 1, 2, and 1,
with the nonharmonic tone having duration 2 and the harmonies progressing in a 2, 2 rhythm. Bach
uses the 121 neighboring and passing tones in BWV 393 (Riemenschneider 275) m. 3 and BWV 103.6
(Riemenschneider 120) m. 4.
16
See m. 47 of the Gloria of Dufay’s mass Ave Regina coelorum (three voices) and m. 25 of the
Credo of the mass Se la face ay pale (four voices). Dufay’s music contains many other moments that
resist nonharmonic reduction—for example, m. 7 of the Kyrie of the mass Ave Regina coelorum,
where an irreducible seventh is formed by passing motion. In Palestrina’s music, the idiom occurs at
least once per movement on average. Gauldin 1995a discusses this figure.
17
Guillotel-Nothmann 2018 connects suspension resolutions to the origins of root functionality.
18
Fétis (1840) 1994, pp. 32–33, and Christensen 2019. Christensen emphasizes the unpreparedness
of some of Monteverdi’s sevenths (p. 33 of Fétis [1840] 1994), whereas I am more focused on Fétis’s
apparent misunderstanding of the suspension idiom (p. 32 of same).
19
Compare Lerdahl (2020, p. 18): “once passing and neighboring tones and suspensions are
stripped away in Classical music, every chord is a triad.”
20
Numerous theorists have considered suspensions “essential” or “primary” dissonances, as
opposed to the less-essential, more decorative nonharmonic tones: Vincenzo Galilei (Palisca 1956, p.
88), Christoph Bernhard ([~1640] 1973, p. 79), Fux ([1725] 1971, p. 55), Fenaroli and other
partimento theorists (Sanguinetti 2012, pp. 103 and 125ff), and Jeppesen (1946) 1970. Even Rameau
often assigns a different fundamental bass to a suspension and its resolution (Christensen 1993a, pp.
123ff; this view is echoed in Schoenberg [1911] 1983, pp. 316–17). Others instead treat suspensions as
purely decorative, including Riemann (1896, p. 107), Prout (Figure 1.3.1), and Schenker ([1910] 1987,
I, pp. 261 and 266). Beach 1974 identifies Kirnberger as the progenitor of this second perspective.
Thanks here to David Cohen.
21
Schoenberg (1911) 1983, p. 309. The claim about medicine is outdated: health-care providers
have learned that they often need to consider non-medical matters such as economics, environment,
and sociology.
22
Jeppesen (1931) 1939, p. xi.
23
For an interpretation of functional tonality and twelve-tone music as formal systems, see Boretz
1970 and 1972, Westergaard 1975, and Dahlhaus 1990, pp. 61–62 (particularly the thought that there
may not exist additional “systems”).
24
Note that in making this claim, I am using the term “harmonic” in a minimalist sense, essentially
as a label for residual preferences that cannot easily be explained by linear or melodic factors; I do not
presuppose inversional equivalence, root functionality, or any other sophisticated theoretical ideas.
Quinn (2018) offers an alternative definition of “harmonic” in terms of tonic-independent principles;
on this definition, the tendency for 64 to resolve to 53 would be a purely contrapuntal law—as would
a preference for descending-fifth progressions, if I read Quinn correctly.
25
Harrison 1994 considers harmonic function as arising from particular scale-degree motions;
while this is not how I always think of function (§7.1), I find it analytically useful here.
26
The ascending 7̂ passing tone is virtually mandatory when the 6̂–1̂ is in the bass, the IV6 chord
leaping to I only in the context of IV–IV6–I arpeggiations.
27
Narmour 1988 makes a similar argument.
28
Schoenberg (1911) 1983, p. 322.
29
Compare the Sanctus of Palestrina’s mass Assumpta est Maria, m. 31, or the Sanctus of the Pope
Marcellus Mass, m. 53.
30
Recall also that the harmonic/nonharmonic distinction has a transformational significance (§4.8).
31
Christoph Bernhard ([~1640] 1973, p. 90) associates many of these effects with improvisation,
an association echoed by Palisca (1994, pp. 54ff). The tradition of using nonharmonic tones to produce
quartal harmonies, often evoking folk music, continues in works such as Schubert’s C major string
quintet, D. 956, III, and Chopin’s Mazurka Op. 6, no. 2.
32
See, for example, Fétis (1840) 1994, p. 31, or the heightened language in McClary 2004, pp.
183ff (“shriek,” “grinding dissonances,” “shouting,” “BITCH!,” “transgression,” “bitter,”
“unpleasant,” “brutal assault”).
33
McClary’s extreme language is no doubt intended to get us to hear Monteverdi’s dissonances as
striking and outrageous, as his contemporaries might have. However, it is not obvious that ordinary
Renaissance listeners (as opposed to theorists) would have had such extreme reactions—perhaps the
fleeting dissonances would have eluded earlier ears precisely as they elude so many contemporary
listeners. Or perhaps Renaissance audiences were accustomed to more extreme levels of dissonance in
improvisational contexts, as suggested in Palisca 1994, chapter 3.
34
Octave-displaced melodies also appear in the bass voice at mm. 16, 25, and 35.
35
That is, the ascending B♭–F fifth of the previous phrase continues to the C–G–D–A of the next;
this is noted in Chafe 1992 (p. 92). Salzer 1983 reads the opening chords as a prolongation of G minor.
36
Chafe 1992 proposes that Monteverdi’s language is hexachordal and based on major and minor
triads (pp. 24–31). The sequence in mm. 6–12, however, suggests that it is fruitful to include the
diminished triad alongside major and minor: the b°6 in m. 6 plays a similar role to the g6 in m. 10;
these are both instances of a ubiquitous Renaissance voice-leading pattern, which can appear either in
authentic (vii°6–I) or plagal ( ♭ vii6–I) forms. To capture this similarity, it is useful to treat the
diminished triad as a genuine sonority, and hence to postulate a diatonic rather than hexachordal
background.
37
Salzer 1983 interprets the chords in the fifth-sequence as dominants of their predecessors rather
than “ascending subdominants” of their successors. I think this is an anachronistic flattening-out of a
three-dimensional Renaissance harmonic landscape.
38
To my ear, these figures have less the character of a true seventh, or even a suspension, than of a
syncopation or perhaps a resonance that has been sustained just slightly longer than the harmony it
belongs to—like a fleeting pedal tone.
39
This is the “chord repeats, pattern continues” technique discussed in §4.9.
40
See Theune 2007 on poetic turns.
41
Salzer 1983 reads the phrase as an interruption form.
42
One might try a quarter-note reading where the first two beats are B♭maj7 followed by a°, but this
involves an unusual harmonic progression and a nonstandard resolution of the seventh.
43
On “fittingness” in aesthetics, see Danto 1981, p. 207, and Camp 2017. McClary is similarly
suspicious of what she calls the “ ‘the words made me do it’ defense” (2004, p. 182).
44
These two perspectives—one humanist and evolutionary, the other scholastic and timeless—map
onto broader worldviews whose conflict was playing out during these same decades. See Tomlinson
1990 and Wootton 2015.
45
Palisca 1994 makes a similar point, as does Chafe (1992, p. 5): “what distinguishes Monteverdi’s
music from both late Baroque rationality and the sixteenth-century style is the relative unpredictability
of his dissonances and rationalizations.”
46
Christoph Bernhard’s unpublished 1640 treatise contains one of the most interesting
seventeenth-century discussions of nonharmonicity; interestingly, it does not discuss pedal tones.
47
Christoph Bernhard describes this explicitly: “part of a note is cut off, so that this may be placed
in front of the following note in the degree immediately below” ([~1640] 1973, p. 108).
48
A modest degree of recursion is characteristic of the nonharmonic system: in Figure 5.1.4, for
example, we have an anticipation being decorated by a neighbor. One can also find neighbor tones
decorating passing tones and suspensions, and so on.
49
This minimal definition is found in Gauldin 1997. Other textbook authors such as Aldwell and
Schachter (2002) and Kostka and Payne (2003) place more restrictions on pedal tones. The bass G in
Figure 9.3.1 is unusual: if we want to consider the iiø7 to be harmonic then the chordal G is attacked a
measure too early; it is perhaps comparable to the bass A in m. 13 of Figure 5.5.2.
50
Pedal tones are the only species of nonharmonic tone largely limited to specific scale degrees.
Some genres, such as string quartets, are more lenient with pedal tones.
51
Bach’s keyboard music occasionally presents additional anomalies, including suspensions
resolving by leap.
52
Or, more neutrally, its significance; I use “semantics” in a general and metaphorical sense.
53
Or that they represent multiple voices as discussed in the prelude to this chapter.
54
That is, one might have a small penalty for the standard notes of the SNAP system (ensuring that
the computer prefers an analysis with no nonharmonic tones to one with some), a slightly higher
penalty for incomplete neighbors and pedal tones, and the highest penalty for “rogue notes” with no
standard explanation. The strategy was devised by my student Jeffrey Hodes in his undergraduate
thesis (Hodes 2012).
55
Assuming the key is correct.
56
This is something like the converse of the twelve-tone technique of creating consonant music by
hiding unwanted notes in an unobtrusive instrument.
57
Steve Taylor notes that the same chord appears at m. 327 of the first movement of Mahler’s
Second Symphony.
58
One sign of this intention is that nonharmonic tones generally form consonances with each other.
59
Meyer 1996 also emphasizes Romantic composers’ repurposing of earlier techniques.
60
Note that the “A7 over G” common-tone dominant resolves to G7 and is associated with the
diminished seventh C♯–E–G–B♭; the “B7 over G” resolves to C and is associated with C–D♯–F♯–A.
61
See Lewis 1983, pp. 148–50, for a polytonal interpretation involving the dominant of E minor.
My analysis evokes what jazz musicians call “upper structures,” chromatic, stacked-third extensions
of what is fundamentally V7 in C (cf. Levine 1989).
62
It is also possible to identify the chord on m. 38 as a G7, with bass E an accented neighbor,
though that makes the underlying voice leading less clear.
63
The B/B♭ oscillation once again evokes the common-tone diminished seventh.
64
Note the three-note trumpet figure landing successively on G, D, and A.
65
Lewis 1983 contains a careful reading of the opening measures that disagrees with mine in
almost every detail.
66
Temperley 2007 and Nobile 2015. Christopher Doll modifies this to “melodic/harmonic
stratification” (2017).
67
Whether Schoenberg himself distinguished these two senses of “emancipation” is an interesting
question. As Hinton 2010 and Arndt 2011 point out, his description of emancipation changed over
time, from something anticipated to something that had occurred.
68
Exactly what determines this sense of “belonging” is a very complex matter; in popular music,
instrumentation plays a vital role, with guitars often playing pure triads while the vocalist provides
additional notes.
69
See Figure 1.1.4, which suggests a distorted B♭ minor. When the embellishing tones acquire their
own centricity, we have “polytonality.” Thus there is a hidden connection between the emancipation of
dissonance and polytonality.
70
This comes out most clearly in his various complaints over modernism, most notably “Further
consideration of the Urlinie: II.” The precise role of traditional contrapuntal principles, and their
specific applicability or inapplicability to various levels of reductive analysis, is a major point of
disagreement among Schenkerians.
71
Schenker (1926) 1996, p. 10. This point is echoed by Rothgeb (“the passing tone is . . . totally
without harmonic significance,” 1975, p. 268) and Lerdahl (2020, p. 18).
72
Schenker (1910) 1987, I, pp. 261ff, and Rothgeb 1975, pp. 273–74.
73
Fugue subjects are an exception, using different interval types to relate harmonic notes (§4.8).
Prelude
Functional and Scale-Degree Analysis
Figure P6.2. (top) The second-inversion triad in m. 19 of the Agnus from Palestrina’s mass De Beata
Marie Virginis (II) (starred) is plausibly harmonic, occurring in a sequence of four ascending fifths (or
five, if we ignore the a6). (bottom) The second-inversion triad in m. 21 of the Agnus of the mass In
minoribus duplicibus (starred) has a much weaker claim to harmonicity, being produced by a passing
tone.
In this book, I generally default to a fairly strict form of scale-degree
analysis. First, because it gives us a vocabulary for exploring how
collections like iii6 can gradually change their function (e.g., moving to IV in
the Renaissance, largely disappearing in the classical period, and acquiring
dominant function in the nineteenth century). Second, because it gives us a
common language in which to express disagreement or uncertainty about a
chord’s perceived function: for example, one listener may hear the cadential
64 as a dominant, while another may be more sensitive to its tonic qualities,
with neither being right or wrong.5 Scale-degree analysis provides a shared
vocabulary for discussing these disagreements, rather than encoding the
purportedly “correct” interpretation directly in its notation.6 And third,
because the scale-degree approach leads to greater methodological clarity.
For the scale-degree theorist, a term like “dominant” is roughly synonymous
with “V or vii°” and “dominants proceed to tonics” is a testable claim: we
simply check whether scale degrees 5, 7, and 2 typically proceed to scale
degrees 1, 3, and 5. This is something even a computer can do.
By contrast, functional analysis can sometimes come dangerously close to
circularity: at the extreme, labels like “dominant” are utterly divorced from
their scale-degree content, applied to any chord that is perceived (by the
analyst) as engendering a certain kind of expectation.7 Here the claim
“dominants progress to tonics” becomes definitional, free of any empirical
content whatsoever—an introspective report that some sonority arouses an
expectation of a tonic arrival.8 Here, harmonic analysis becomes a language
for talking about one’s experience—sometimes described as a matter of
“chord meaning.”
None of which is to imply that it is misguided: describing subjective
experience is a reasonable pursuit, and there is certainly something to be
gained from the thought that tonic function can be expressed by scale
degrees 1, 3, and either 5 or 6. Problems arise when one particular approach
is tacitly assumed to be the only option. Eminent scholars have claimed that
Roman numerals necessarily presuppose inversional equivalence, or that
terms like “dominant” assume the primacy of the bass voice.9 Similarly,
theorists sometimes complain about the “trivial transliteration of note-
content”—as if it were a mistake to aspire to somewhat objective
descriptions against which we can test our assertions.10 (Very often these
same theorists underestimate the degree of regularity to be found in local
harmonic progressions, regularity that becomes unmistakable when we stoop
to the level of “trivial transliteration.”) In rejecting scale-degree analysis,
theorists deprive themselves of a useful tool for charting the development of
harmonic functionality—indeed, virtually the only tool that can trace the
subtle ways in which configurations of scale degrees change their behavior
over the course of the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries.
My argument can be reformulated as a question. Suppose a musician is
interested in scale-degree content, desiring a relatively theory-neutral
account of the tertian harmonies in a piece. How should they go about this?
The two options are to create an entirely new language that is virtually
equivalent to, and intertranslatable with, familiar Roman numerals (for
instance, using “scale degrees 7 and 3 over 5̂” in place of “iii6”), or,
alternatively, to use familiar Roman numerals with a slightly different set of
implications. The first has massive costs in time and effort: when confronted
with these new terms, readers need to stop and think about what is meant,
whereas traditional Roman numerals can be immediately associated with
collections of scale degrees. The second runs the risk of unwanted
implications, but this can be forestalled by being clear about how one is
using words. One approach requires that readers internalize an entirely new
nomenclature, all in the fruitless attempt to stave off misunderstanding; the
other requires only that we trust our readers to be attentive, flexible, and
charitable enough to take us at our word.
1
See Rothgeb 1996.
2
Rameau (1722) 1971, pp. 71–73 (book 2, chapter 6).
3
This debate can be traced back at least to Weber (1817–1821) 1846, pp. 463–64; for more recent
discussions see Beach 1967 and Ninov 2016.
4
As Carl Schachter puts it, “there is no such ‘thing’ as a I chord in C major, but only an idea that
can find expression” in manifold ways; that single “idea” is presumably what is labeled by the Roman
numeral “I” (Schachter 1990, p. 166, quoted in Rothgeb 1996; my italics). Damschroder 2016, p. 5,
expresses a similar sentiment.
5
Rothgeb 1996 describes functional analysis as recording what a “perceptive listener” hears; such
perceptions are much more variable than scale-degree content.
6
In this respect my proposed reconceptualization of Roman numerals is consonant with the trend
toward the more theory-neutral labels of figured bass (Gjerdingen 2007 and Holtmeier 2007).
7
Some theorists divorce terms like “dominant” from scale-degree content altogether (Lerdahl
2001, Nobile 2016).
8
Some theorists use “subdominant” to refer to IV chords that progress plagally to the tonic rather
than authentically to V, so that “subdominant chords progress to tonics” becomes a matter of
definition; for the scale-degree theorist these same words constitute a falsifiable claim (Swinden 2005,
p. 253).
9
Dahlhaus 1990, p. 73. Speaking about the Renaissance cadence, Taruskin writes: “the progression
in the bass, from the fifth scale degree [ . . . ] to the final, is congruent with what we are accustomed to
calling a V–I or dominant–tonic progression. To call it that is to think of the motion of the lowest part
as the essential cadential approach, and to associate the gesture toward closure with the ‘dominant’
harmony” (Taruskin 2005, vol. 2, p. 496, my italics).
10
Rothgeb 1996, paragraph 11.
6
The Origins of Functional Tonality
Scholars have situated the origin of functional tonality in the early 1400s, the
popular music of the early 1500s, Monteverdi’s works of the early 1600s,
Corelli and the 1670s, and even after J. S. Bach.1 To my mind this is like
trying to identify the precise moment in which dinosaurs evolved into birds:
functional harmony developed gradually over centuries, and there is no
single point at which the process began or ended.2 This is because the
transition was largely implicit, a matter of composers learning patterns and
idioms without necessarily recognizing them explicitly—embodied routines
that only later became the subject of explicit theoretical attention. To trace its
development, we need to look toward the musical knowledge implied by
music itself. Statistics are essential here, if only to point us in the right
direction.
Once we understand the development of tonal functionality as gradual and
implicit, we can incorporate a number of earlier insights. In what follows, I
will agree with Lowinsky that a simple form of protofunctionality appeared
in the early 1500s as an optional set of ionian-mode idioms emphasizing
root-position I, IV, and V.3 Over the next two centuries, these routines
colonized an increasing swath of music, ultimately coming to resemble
universal laws rather than genre-specific tendencies. During this time, they
also increased in complexity, moving beyond simple oscillations to longer
progressions featuring a wider variety of sonorities. Thus I also agree with
Fétis and Dahlhaus, who have drawn attention to the early 1600s as a time
when additional functional vocabulary was being standardized. The mid-
seventeenth-century enthusiasm for sequences represents another inflection
point, and a reason for highlighting Corelli.
Where I depart from earlier theorists is in rejecting a unified or essentialist
conception of functionality. The evolution of functional harmony, like the
evolution of birds, involved numerous separate processes occurring at
different times. One is the restriction of harmonic vocabulary to a small
number of chords and progressions. Another is modal homogenization, the
reduction of the available modes to just major and minor. The development
of inversional equivalence is a third. Broad acceptance of seventh chords and
second-inversion triads is a fourth. A fifth is the standardization of
modulatory pathways. Interest in sequential patterning is another. Yet others
concern melodic routines, from the leading tone’s increasing tendency to
resolve, to the development of the 3̂–2̂–1̂ and 5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂–1̂ templates that
underlie so many baroque and classical phrases. My guiding principle is that
these processes are largely independent, separable, and linked by no single
logic.4 As a result, we find some varieties of functional tonality that are
focused on primary triads, and others that make heavy use of seventh chords,
secondary diatonic triads, or sequences. No one key unlocks all the doors.
Again, many of these changes occurred in musical practice long before
they were noticed by theorists. To say this is not to imply that history is
guided by a hidden hand, or that it evolves toward greater artistic perfection,
but merely that it sometimes develops in a way that is akin to natural
language: from 800 to 1400, for example, English gradually lost its case
markers in a bottom-up process driven by the changing behavior of ordinary
speakers. To study this process by looking at medieval grammars, rather than
the records of linguistic behavior, is to get things backward—for the
grammarians neither noticed nor cared about these changes as they were
occurring.5 The development of harmonic functionality is analogous, a very
gradual regimentation of musical behavior that focuses musical attention on
a core set of harmonic and melodic routines. To explore it, we need to
consider the utterances of musical speakers, rather than the theories
surrounding those utterances. Paradoxical as it may sound, it is possible that
contemporary theorists, using contemporary tools, may end up
understanding some aspects of this grammar better than its native speakers.6
27, and the occasional use of F♮ over the C major chord.9 One gets the sense
that each period embellishes a single harmonic template, rather like a
variations form—for instance, antecedent phrases always start with four bars
of I and end with two bars of V, but mm. 5–6 can contain either IV or I;
similarly, consequents always end with a bar of V and two bars of I, but the
preceding five measures are more flexible. There is also an eight-bar
“cadential interlude” after the third period, presenting two iterations of a IV–
V–I cadence, as if reinforcing the previous consequent’s close.10
Figure 6.1.2. The opening of Dalza’s first Pavana alla Venetiana.
Figure 6.1.7. The end of “Se ben hor non scopro el foco,” omitting the repeat of mm. 4–6 on Figure
6.1.6.
Figure 6.1.8. Deriving the four-voice canon: starting with an ascending-fifth sequence, every other
chord is contracted by contrary motion. Rhythmic displacement allows one voice to be duplicated at
the unison.
These pieces are so bewilderingly functional that they seem to challenge the
thesis of gradual evolution—suggesting perhaps that functional tonality did
spring forth fully formed in this early vernacular tradition. But on closer
inspection, it is clear that we are dealing with a distinctive protofunctional
dialect that both resembles and differs from later styles.
We can start with the resemblances.
First, these pieces seem to be harmonic in conception: the heavy use of
root-position triads, often in close position and related by “default” voice
leading, coupled with the deemphasizing of contrapuntal devices such as
imitation, all suggest that chords are genuine objects of compositional
focus.16 Unapologetic parallelism reinforces the centrality of harmony
(Figure 6.2.1).
Figure 6.2.1. Parallelism in Dalza’s second Pavana alla Venetiana (mm. 53–55).
Figure 6.2.2. Phrase structure in which a stronger V–I follows a weaker V–I (from the opening of
Dalza’s Pavana alla Ferrarese).
Figure 6.2.3. The “tendency” of IV–V and IV–I progressions. The number +19.9 indicates that the
likelihood of a V chord increases by +19.9 percentage points after a IV, relative to its overall
likelihood—or in other words, that IV chords “tend” to go to V chords. Negative values indicate
progressions that are suppressed.
Figure 6.2.4. The likelihood of root-position triads on the various ionian-mode scale degrees. The
mediant is, after the leading tone, the least likely to support a root-position triad, indicating a
preference for I6 over iii, even in genres that typically favor root-position triads.
Figure 6.2.5. The Prinner combines two standard ionian features, I6 as a default harmonization of 3̂
and the vii°6–I cadence.
Related to this is the importance of IV: the two harmonic cycles I–IV–I
and I–V–I are, if not equal, then at least comparable in significance.
Furthermore, IV–I progressions sometimes have a subtle “dominant” quality
by virtue of an ascending 6̂–7̂–8̂ line. In other cases, I–IV progressions have
a tonicizing character, with tonic moving to subdominant so as to evoke
cadential formulae (e.g., the end of Figure 6.1.7). Figure 6.2.7 compares the
relative proportion of IV and V chords in a range of ionian and major-mode
music from the sixteenth century to the present. In some genres, IV is almost
as common as V, falling from about 45% in my sample of Lassus to
something more like 15% in Beethoven—a change due to both the declining
use of IV and the increasing preference for V.
Figure 6.2.7. The relative proportion of IV and V: 100% means only IV is used, 0% means only V is
used, and 50% represents an even balance between the two.
Fourth, though it is not apparent from the preceding musical examples, the
different modes feature different harmonic patterns. In dorian, for example,
the subdominant is often deemphasized relative to the subtonic, and pieces
often revolve around i, ♭ VII, III, and V.22 Here again we might think of
contemporary popular music, where major, minor, and mixolydian all
emphasize different chord progressions. Early protofunctionality is a mode-
bound option rather than an inviolable law governing all music—analogous
perhaps to the blues in 1960s rock, a package of musical moves that can be
adopted or avoided according to taste.
Fifth, inversional equivalence does not play a robust compositional role,
for the simple reason that the harmonic vocabulary is so restricted: there can
be no question about the equivalence of vi and vi6 in genres where vi6 hardly
appears. Instead we have a proliferation of root-position primary triads that
deemphasize the melodic function of the bass in favor of its harmonic role in
articulating roots. It follows that simple harmonic functionality does not
require inversional equivalence; the basic prerequisite for harmonic function
is harmonic limitation.
Sixth, the melodic tendency of noncadential leading tones is weak. In
functional music, outer-voice leading tones almost always move to the tonic.
In proto-functional music, noncadential leading tones are less constrained,
often progressing downward (Figure 6.2.8). This suggests that there are
important differences between cadential and ordinary dominants: leading-
tone resolution is a function of the cadential suspension rather than the
dominant as a harmonic entity or the leading tone as a melodic scale degree.
Figure 6.2.8. A descending leading tone in Dalza’s Tastar de Corde 1, mm. 85ff.
Carl Dahlhaus has cautioned that we should not take these resemblances at
face value, since some of these pieces originated with a cantus-tenor duet to
which additional voices were later added.
Provided that the significance of a composition depends on the tradition out of which it arose
—provided therefore that a procedure’s origin determines its meaning—then a frottola like
Oimè et cor must be characterized as a two-voice composition with a supplemental bass.23
Figure 6.3.2. The cantus/tenor duet, and close-position triads, in the first phrase of Tromboncino’s
“Ah partiale e cruda morte!”
A little thought reveals that this is not so mysterious, for as long as upper
voices form complete close-position major and minor triads, then cantus and
tenor will necessarily form an acceptable dyadic sequence of fifths and
sixths; conversely, as long as the tenor forms a third, fifth, or sixth with the
cantus, the alto can add the note that completes the close-position triad. (In
the opening of “Ah partiale e cruda morte!” the tenor is usually a third below
the cantus, falling to the fifth/sixth before the cadences.) Looking at
Tromboncino’s music, it is very difficult to know whether he began with a
two-voice framework or some analogue to OUCH theory that produced a
two-voice framework as byproduct. The deceptive cadence in the second-to-
last line provides some evidence in favor of the second option, for in the
cantus/tenor framework there is no deception and the passage repeats to
puzzling effect (Figure 6.3.3). This suggests that even if Tromboncino
started with a two-voice framework, he was already imagining the four-
voice structure. We should not put too much interpretive weight on these
questions of compositional method, as their importance tends to recede once
we recognize that different paths can lead to similar destination.
Figure 6.3.3. Close position upper voices in mm. 23–27 of Tromboncino’s “Ah partiale e cruda
morte!” The repeated cadences are attractive in the full arrangement, but oddly pedestrian in the
cantus/tenor duet.
Figure 6.3.4. A cadence in which voices converge on E, but which modern listeners will likely hear as
emphasizing A.
Figure 6.3.5. (top) The end of Josquin’s mass De beata virgine. The final V–I and IV–I progressions
lack the suspension formula and the final IV–I has no converging pair. (bottom) The end of the mass
Sub tuum presidium, attributed to Josquin. This is an E phrygian piece that ends with a cadence in
which upper voices converge on B, not used as a tonal center in the Renaissance.
Figure 6.3.6. Possible bass notes under a converged-upon C, along with the percentage of cadences
with that bass. The cadences on the sixth above the bass are very rare.
This divergence between theory and practice reminds us that earlier music
theorists are not “native informants” who provide authoritative information
about their musical culture, but rather theory builders who try to describe,
systematize, and explain that culture.26 Rather than taking their assertions at
face value, we should check their theories against the data provided by
music itself. Doing the history of theory without evaluating theoretical
claims is like doing the history of science without considering how
experiments turned out. It is also vital to bear in mind that earlier theorists
operated in an intellectual environment substantially different from our own:
in the early 1500s it was still possible to uphold Aristotle’s view that the
equatorial regions were uninhabitable, even though European sailors had
actually been there, or that heavy objects fall faster than lighter objects, even
though anyone could demonstrate the contrary.27 If Renaissance thinkers
upheld tradition over evidence in these matters, how sensitive should we
expect them to be to subtle changes in musical practice? The question is not
meant to be contemptuous but rather to acknowledge that early sixteenth-
century minds had their own ideas about the relative weight of evidence and
authority, that the habit of empirical thinking was still in its infancy, that
they did not have our tradition of accumulating intellectual progress, that
they lacked common terms for “discovery” and “innovation,” and that they
were largely unacquainted with probabilistic inference.28 Aron’s remarks
about the cadence were thus constrained by tradition in ways we may not
fully appreciate.29 All of which presents a fascinating topic for historians of
science, and a dangerous pitfall for unwary historicists. Music historians
have chided their “presentist” colleagues for unthinkingly projecting
contemporary concepts onto the music of the past, but they have been much
less sensitive to the equally erroneous assumption that earlier theorists
operated with contemporary standards of descriptive accuracy.30
One final example of the complex interplay between origin and meaning.
We have reason to believe that Renaissance composers had at least some
notion of inversional equivalence, for the repeating contrapuntal patterns in
chapter 4 are defined precisely by the fact that structurally analogous notes
move in similar ways: in the Josquin passage in Figure 4.2.2, root moves
down by step while third moves up by third, while in Figure 3.4.6 and mm.
20–24 of Figure 3.6.6, root and third move down by step while fifth stays
fixed. To construct these sequences a composer has to understand which
motions go in which voices, and this is tantamount to an implicit notion of
inversional equivalence. (It is difficult to repeatedly lower the root if you
cannot identify it!) Imitation thus provides one potential route toward
inversional equivalence, for in asking “how can I create a consonant three-
voice transposing round?” composers could easily be led to contrapuntal
patterns in which roots, thirds, and fifths moved in characteristic ways.
Does it follow that composers thought of inversionally related chords as
having the same musical function? Not at all, for it is entirely possible to
have an abstract understanding of concepts like “root,” “third,” and “fifth,”
while still believing that register is fundamental to a chord’s contextual
function and meaning. But at the same time, it seems likely that this weaker
version of inversional equivalence formed the foundation for the later and
more robust notion. (Furthermore, it is plausible that the early preference for
I6 over iii results from a perceived resemblance between E–G–C and C–E–
G.) The interesting question is how the version of inversional equivalence
implicit in sixteenth-century practice developed into the stronger form
explicit in the eighteenth century. This is a story of gradually shifting
musical meaning, written jointly by composers and theorists, and it begins in
weak and untheorized tendencies such as the favoring of I6.
Against Dahlhaus’s carefully hedged historicism I would therefore
propose an alternative in which “origin” and “meaning” can come apart—
one in which composers follow their ears as they grope toward the new,
sometimes using earlier techniques to create music that exemplifies
innovative and untheorized structure. If we believe music can develop in the
way that natural language often does, if we believe that composers
sometimes develop novel techniques—sometimes without an abstract
understanding of their own musical practice, and with written theory trailing
behind—then we need something more sophisticated than Dahlhaus’s
identification of origin and meaning. And if Renaissance music exhibits
weaker versions of tendencies that strengthen throughout the sixteenth and
seventeenth centuries, then it is futile to make invidious contrasts between
approaches that “rely on common-practice tonality as a prism through which
to view early music” and “methods that respect the integrity and self-
sufficiency of the languages of early music.”31 Used cautiously,
contemporary theory and data-driven analysis can help us toward a
genuinely historical perspective, revealing as-yet-undiscovered regularities
in early music, and leading to a more nuanced view of functional harmony
itself. To label this project “anti-historicist” or “presentist” is implicitly to
reserve the term “history” for the study of verbal discourse. The history of
practice is part of the history of theory.
Figure 6.4.2. The progressions whose frequency changes most from Ockeghem to Palestrina. The bass
interval is given in parentheses with D5 = descending fifth or ascending fourth, 0 = no motion, and A5
= ascending fifth or descending fourth. Numbers are percentages of all two-chord complete-triad
progressions.
Jeppesen writes that in Palestrina “the vertical, harmonic requirements
assume merely the exclusively consonant, full harmony of the chords, in
which modulatory relations [i.e., progressions] play only a small part.”34
This description of purely contrapuntal harmony-free music seems better
suited to Ockeghem, where the bass typically moves by unison or step—
exactly as we would expect if it were just another voice, not charged with
the dual responsibility of supporting meaningful vertical harmonies (Figure
6.4.3).35 By contrast Palestrina has a notable preference for major triads over
minor, for root-position over first-inversion chords, and for bass leaps by
fourth or fifth—leaps that almost invariably support root-position triads.36
To my mind, this suggests that harmonies are becoming objects of
compositional concern, and that the lowest voice is starting to embrace a
second role in supporting the overall harmony. This change from pure
counterpoint toward counterpoint-with-harmonic-patterning represents a
genuine difference within the prima prattica, to the point where one might
say that Ockeghem, Josquin, and Palestrina speak different musical dialects.
And this remains true even if these changes are largely implicit and
unselfconscious—a matter of internalized routines rather than explicit
thinking.
Figure 6.4.3. The most common progressions in Ockeghem are those in which the bass moves by
unison or step. Numbers are percentages of all two-chord complete-triad progressions. A2, D2, and 0
identify bass motion.
Figure 6.4.7. The five most common voice leadings for vii°6–I progressions in Bach’s chorales. Of
these, the majority involve 4̂ rising to 5̂, rather than resolving downward.
Figure 6.4.8. The frequency of the two most common dominant chords over the supertonic, vii°6 and
V43.
Figure 6.4.9. In the nineteenth century, V43 gradually comes to replace vii°6 as the primary dominant
over 2̂. However, it inherits its voice leading from the earlier chord.
Figure 6.5.5 annotates the final Kyrie. The section opens with paired
voices in close canon, each articulating the descending form of the diatonic
third’s basic voice leading; because that voice leading produces a canon at
the fifth, the motive’s two transpositions sound almost the same notes
(Figure 6.5.6). The bass supports this fifth-based structure to create iconic
harmonic cycles, rather like Figure 4.10.2, with the canonic lines either the
upper or lower third of the governing triad. This is a wonderful example of
the inseparability of sixteenth-century harmony and counterpoint, the music
being at once harmonically significant and also generated by melodic
imitation. The next motive picks up the end of this figure, repeating entries
on C and G to create an accelerating (G, B) → (E, C) → (F, A) loop that lasts
until the end of the movement (Figure 6.5.7, cf. the lower-right pattern in
Figure 3.1.10). Measure 75 is of particular interest, with a IV64 chord
decorating the piece’s final V–I progression; that chord is itself embellished
with a irreducible seventh I have labeled vi65. Here the F, while dissonant
with the bass, serves both as resolution of the suspension G and as
preparation of a suspension that resolves to E—a second-inversion triad that
cannot be dismissed as merely nonharmonic.
Figure 6.5.5. The end of the Kyrie in the Pope Marcellus Mass. Each cadence is marked by an
asterisk.
Figure 6.5.6. The basic voice leading in the final Kyrie. The fifth-related entry pairs sound almost the
same notes.
Figure 6.5.7. Motivic entries at the end of the Kyrie of Palestrina’s Pope Marcellus Mass.
6. A broader perspective
Figure 6.6.4 shows the proportion of diatonic triads found in a broad range
of music. Here I have anachronistically categorized chords by root,
considering I, I6, and I64 as forms of the same chord.54 What emerges is a
fairly dramatic increase in the number of V chords across the entire era.
There is also a clear emergence of IV as a privileged third sonority: up
through about Gombert, the subdominant chord is just another chord, no
different from ii, vi, or iii. After this point it starts to emerge as special, only
to be supplanted by the supertonic in the seventeenth century. Again we see
the prehistory of functionality moving through distinct phases, with the late
sixteenth century being relatively subdominant-focused, and the seventeenth
century being comparatively more oriented toward the supertonic. If this is
so, then we need to be careful not to generalize about functional harmony as
such. Instead, we will want to recognize the existence of multiple
functionalities, some featuring IV and others ii.55
Figure 6.6.5. The “tendency” of V→I and ii→V, defined for V→I as the likelihood of I following a V
minus the simple likelihood of V. This is the increase in the probability of one chord given that you
have just heard the other.
Figure 6.6.6. The relative popularity of the ii–V, IV–V, and vi–V progressions.
Figure 6.6.7. Percentage of all root-position vi (or vi7) chords that move directly to root-position V (or
V7).
Figure 6.6.8 shows the proportion of progressions that conform to the full-
fledged grammar of functional harmony described in §7.1. The picture here
is of a very gradual transition from modality to functional harmony: a gentle
and consistent rise from the early polyphony of Ockeghem and Josquin to
the music of Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven, punctuated by outliers such as
Dalza, the frottolists, and Juan del Encina—and, in the other direction,
figures such as Castello. This data suggests it would be foolish to try to draw
a sharp line between modality and functionality; instead there is a very slow
evolution that takes place over several centuries, and which is particularly
dramatic in the century from 1550 to 1650. (As Jeppesen put it: “the entire
course of the development of music in the sixteenth and seventeenth
centuries was toward harmonic stability and tonal centralization.”57) We find
independent evidence for this process in individual pieces, crude
computational surveys, and large collections of human analyses. It is hard to
understand how scholars could once have favored a sudden or saltationist
perspective, for the data is as close to evolutionary as one could possibly
imagine.58
Marenzio’s “Ahi dispietata morte” (1585) sets a text written more than two
centuries earlier, one of a large group of sonnets lamenting the death of
Laura de Noves, Petrarch’s muse and unrequited love.59 In many madrigals
the specter of death is a metaphor for consummated desire or a gambit in the
game of sexual persuasion; here, Laura is really dead, and the poet is left to
reflect on grief, loneliness, and the consolations of memory. Besides being
intrinsically worthy of contemplation, Marenzio’s setting provides an
opportunity to review many of the topics we have been exploring.
Like many pieces of its time, it is composed of fragments that individually
conform to functional conventions, but are juxtaposed in surprising ways.
The piece begins and ends with E triads, and was likely conceived as E
centered. However, many of the E chords are major triads proceeding
directly to A: in this sense, they are “constrained toward the future” (§6.4,
§8.6), acting like dominants even though they may have been intended as
tonics; for this reason, I have analyzed the opening in an anachronistic A
minor. The piece opens with the pair of phrases in Figure 6.7.1, one
outlining E–a–E, the second answering with a–E–a. The soprano soars far
above the lower voices (an effect Monteverdi used in “Cruda Amarilli”), and
each voice sings a scale fragment, two ascending and two descending. The
cadence uses an irreducible seventh, introducing the suspension E with the
metrically weak 64 chord and blurring harmony and counterpoint into an
idiomatic whole.60
Figure 6.7.1. The opening of Marenzio’s “Ahi dispietata morte.”
The second phrase again features A minor scale fragments, but more
straightforwardly; I have analyzed the opening C–E dyad as A minor even
though the A does not arrive for two-and-a-half quarter notes. At the end of
the phrase, the upper voices prolong the word “vita,” overlapping with the
next phrase (“L’una m’à posto in doglia” / “The one plunged me into grief”).
Here we leave the A minor/E major orbit, the tenor/bass third A–C slipping
down to inaugurate a sequence of basic voice leadings, (G, B) → (E, C) →
(F♯, A), forming a harmonic cycle in G. The upper voices then repeat this
music up by fourth, a simple sequence grounded in imitation. As Figure
6.7.2 shows, the diatonic third’s basic voice leading continues throughout the
passage, shifting from (F♯, A) to (F♯, D) and continuing with thirds on G
and C; this superimposing of dyads is reminiscent of Figure P4.3. The dyads
lead to a deceptive cadence decorating vi with IV6, initiating a series of
fauxbourdon sixths and ending with a firm cadence on A.
Figure 6.7.2. The diatonic third’s descending-fifth basic voice leading in mm. 8–10.
Figure 6.7.4. The ascending-third sequence in mm. 14–16, leading to a IV64–I alternation.
The piece’s next phrase sets the words “et lei che se n’è gita seguir non
posso” / “and she who has gone I cannot follow.” This virtuosic music is
based on the Chase schema, with the underlying melodic pattern appearing
canonically eleven times as the music moves from A minor to C major
(§4.4). Canon here is a metaphor for following, with the notes portraying
what the words claim is impossible—a moment of negative text-painting
embodying the fantasy of mortality overcome. Figure 6.7.5 unpacks the
mechanics, rebarring the music to group the entries into ascending-step
triples (C–C–G, D–D–A, E–E–B). The final entries extend up by seventh
rather than fifth, leading to a cadence on C. This piece was my first
encounter with the Chase schema, and I remember being dumbfounded by
Marenzio’s contrapuntal and expressive virtuosity—admiration that was
later tempered by the realization that he was reusing a well-known piece of
Renaissance vocabulary.
Figure 6.7.5. The Chase schema in the seventh phrase of the madrigal.
At this point, more than halfway into the piece, we have had almost no
signs of E centricity, save an opening phrase that is easily heard in A minor.
Instead, the music has offered a trio of strong cadences on A (mm. 7, 13, 20)
and two less-marked turns to C (mm. 16 and 24). This is not to suggest that
it is impossible to hear the phrygian signals; on the contrary, I imagine that
cognoscenti would interpret the score as E centered. My point is that the
music does very little to establish E as a genuine, psychologically real
center; a casual listener from any culture would be hard pressed to single out
the E as the primary tone.
What is fascinating is that the second half of the piece starts to insist on a
recognizably phrygian modality (Figure 6.7.6).62 The contrapuntal virtuosity
of “I cannot follow” gives way to a homophonic passage on “ch’ella nol
consente” (“she will not let me,” a slightly odd blaming of the deceased for a
problem not of her making). The details of this little passage are fascinating:
it begins with what seems like functional music in G, ii–IV–V–i supporting
2̂–1̂–7̂–1̂ in the melody, but the cadential tonic is a minor chord that
progresses to A major, evoking a phrygian close.63 (The continuation, which
stays on A rather than moving to D, solidifies this impression.) The effect is
of an overlapping cadence, but without the standard suspension—a
surprising harmonic turn where the phrase seems to acquire an extra chord.
The next bit of music is transitional and tonally unstable. The lower two
voices sing “Ma pur ogni or presente” (“But in every moment”) in canon at
the fifth, using the basic voice leading to move through thirds and sixths on
D, G, C, and F. This is yet another example of the connection between the
basic voice leading and canon. The top three voices then vary this material in
singing “nel mezzo del meo cor” (“in the middle of my heart”), leading to a
cadence on a phrygian E that cannot be interpreted as a dominant. We
immediately turn to homophonic declamation with the C major “madonna
siede” (“my lady sits”), setting the stage for the final section.
That music, based on the words “et qual è la mia vita, ella sel vede” (“and
what my life is now, she sees”), proposes that the dead live on in our
remembrance (Figure 6.7.7). The texture is wonderfully suspended, slowing
the harmonic rhythm to luxuriate in a series of prolonged third-related
harmonies, enlivened by stepwise oscillation (e.g., the four A–G motions in
mm. 33–34, the four F–E motions in mm. 34–35, etc.).64 Here there are
almost no hints of (proto-)functional harmony. The basic contrapuntal
technique is shown in Figure 6.7.8; it is based on a four-note descending
figure that was first presented in mm. 11–12. Harmonically, the figure
appears in two forms: in one, the sustained voice creates oscillations
between fifth-related triads (“option 1” on the figure, used in the middle of
the phrase). The second and more modal version adds a second sustained
voice a third below the first, leading to a series of third-related triads, usually
arranged to form VI6–i progressions in which 6̂ acts as a descending
tendency tone (mm. 33–35, 41–43). To my ear, these VI6–i progressions
recall the vocabulary of minor-mode rock (e.g., Patti Smith’s “Ask the
Angels”).
I have listed the appearances of the figure above the score, labeled by its
highest note, since the beginning is variable. The basic pattern can support a
unison canon at the distance of one half-note, but a third unison entry cannot
be introduced until three half-notes after the second—as at the end of the
piece, where we have four E entries in a row. However, it is possible to
accelerate the canon by bringing in an entry at the third (Figure 6.7.8). This
leads the music down by third from C to A for a phrygian cadence, the third
such cadence in less than ten measures; we then move downward to F and
back, but instead of continuing on from A to C, the soprano entry in m. 40 is
distorted to reach a high E. This opens onto a deeply spiritual ending and the
most extended phrygian passage in the piece—transforming the earlier E-as-
possible-dominant to an unmistakable E-as-phrygian-tonic. It is heartfelt and
expressive, music that is as alive today as it was more than four hundred
years ago: a poem of the mind in the act of making peace with death.
Figure 6.7.8. The contrapuntal logic behind the final passage of Marenzio’s “Ahi dispietata morte.”
But is it functionally tonal? I confess that the more I think about this
question, the more confused I become. It largely depends on whether we
think the glass is half empty or half full: clearly, Marenzio’s music is not
“functionally tonal” if we require that music treat the harmonic norms of the
eighteenth and nineteenth centuries as if they were inviolable musical laws.
But at the same time, it is clearly music where notions of tonic, dominant,
and subdominant, not to mention harmony, sequence, and deceptive cadence,
have some use. It is music that seems to move purposefully through key
areas, starting on an E-as-quasi-dominant, exploring the white-note A and C
areas, before introducing the phrygian flavors of its second half. If
“functionally tonal” means “music where the concepts of functional
harmony can be useful and enlightening,” then the music is functionally
tonal—despite the fact that it is in the phrygian mode.
Which should we emphasize: the differences from eighteenth-century
practice or the similarities? In some sense, the question is whether
functionality comes in degrees. To me, the affirmative answer is natural: this
music’s departures from later norms do not nullify the equally clear
continuities with that tradition. But in saying this I am conscious that I am
drawing on a twenty-first-century conception of functional harmony as
heterogeneous and largely conventional. (And perhaps more to the point: I
am drawing on my enculturation into popular styles that treat functionality
as an option rather than a requirement.) To a nineteenth-century musician
who had been taught to conceive of musical rules as natural laws,
comparable to the as-yet-unchallenged laws of Newtonian physics, things
might well seem different. Perhaps from that perspective, little departures
would have a much greater significance—as alienating and shocking as
minor departures from the laws of nature. Perhaps from that perspective,
genuine functionality would require that music rid itself of all errors and
fully embody the truth. That perspective may be gone at the level of our
conscious thinking, and yet still survive in our unconscious habits,
preventing us from acknowledging protofunctionality when we hear it.
1
Korte 1929, Georgiades 1937, and Besseler 1950 (early 1400s, all cited in Dahlhaus 1990, p. 83);
Lowinsky 1961 (early 1500s); Dahlhaus 1990 and Fétis (1840) 1994 (Monteverdi); Bukofzer 1947
(Corelli); Gauldin 1995b (after Bach).
2
This view is consonant with Christensen 1993b and Taruskin 2005, vol. 2, pp. 471–72. On avian
evolution, see Varricchio and Jackson 2016.
3
Lowinsky 1961.
4
Compare Taruskin’s suggestion that there is a unified logic based on fifths: “we [modern readers]
are therefore much more fully aware than anyone could have been at the time of the range of
implications the new cadential structure [i.e., the root-position V–I cadence] carried within it” (2005,
vol. 2, pp. 471–72, my italics).
5
In fact, there were relatively few such grammars; medieval grammarians were largely concerned
with Latin rather than “vulgar” language. In contrast to the standard practice of historical linguistics,
Bent explicitly recommends founding musical grammar on contemporaneous grammatical theorizing
(1998). This view is so foreign to historical linguistics that I am unable to find any linguists who have
considered it.
6
For a linguistic analogue see Labov 1975, p. 107: “this puts us in the somewhat embarrassing
position of knowing more about a speaker’s grammar than he does himself.”
7
The opposition between zeroth-order and first-order is broadly related to Dahlhaus’s opposition
between Riemannian and Rameauian conceptions: Riemann tends to emphasize zeroth-order chord
categories (“functions”); while Rameau tends to emphasize first-order progressions (Dahlhaus 1990,
Christensen 1993). Bobbitt (1955) and Meeùs (2000, 2018) emphasize first-order probabilities. There
are also second-order probabilities concerning the likelihood that a two-chord sequence such as ii6–V
will continue in a particular way, third-order probabilities governing the successors to three-chord
sequences, and so on.
8
This figure categorizes chords by root; one gets a very similar graph if one looks only at root-
position triads.
9
In general, the piece exhibits an intriguing and playful mix of scalar inflections. Since it was
published in tablature, it is unclouded by questions about musica ficta.
10
Dalza’s other Venetian Pavanes—which indeed feel more like variations on a single template
than distinct pieces—include similar cadential interludes.
11
For the sake of clarity, my realization includes parallel perfect intervals which would be avoided
in practice. The figure’s bottom level is very similar to a graph appearing in Rameau’s 1737
Génération harmonique (Christensen 1993a, p. 194). Unlike Rameau, I consider the sequence a
distorted quantization of an exact contrary-motion sequence (Yust 2015a). This recalls the strategy we
used to model rock harmony (Figure 2.1.7).
12
As the tradition develops, 4̂–3̂ is increasingly harmonized by V7–I. Other important possibilities,
not appearing on my simple model, include V harmonizing 5̂, IV harmonizing 1̂, and vii°6–I
harmonizing 4̂–3̂.
13
Including the octave repeat, the entire structure can be seen as a near sequence whose interval of
repetition changes from a fourth (B–E) to a fifth (E–B).
14
For a justification of my rebarring, see Boone 2000, pp. 36ff.
15
This same sequence appears in many other Renaissance pieces, including Monteverdi’s 1610
Vespro della Beata Vergine (Rotem and Schubert 2021).
16
Dahlhaus 1990, p. 288: “Many details [of the frottolas] cannot be explained without recourse to
the concept of a chord.”
17
“The I–IV, IV–I, and V–I fifth-relations in [Dalza’s] Pavanna alla ferrarese are so strikingly
marked that Lowinsky was unquestionably right to see in them something new” (Dahlhaus 1990, pp.
109–10).
18
This is a relatively small sample so things might change if we looked at more music.
19
Recall Gjerdingen’s remarks about I6, iii, and the rise of functional tonality (§1.5). It is also
possible that the preference for I6 reflects the influence of the mixolydian mode, where root-position
mediant triads are unavailable.
20
Compare McClary 2004, p. 15.
21
Compare Figure 6.1.5, m. 9; Figure 6.1.6, m. 4; Figure 2.4.1; Pinter 2019; and McClary 2004, p.
218.
22
For example, Dalza’s “Calata ala spagnole 1” and the “Caldibi Castigliano.”
23
Dahlhaus 1990, p. 281 (my italics). In a similar vein, Bent writes that “the failure to
acknowledge a discant-tenor duet . . . can invalidate an analysis” (1998).
24
Thanks here to David Cohen. Richard Taruskin (2005, vol. 2, pp. 465–72) makes a similar point.
25
Parncutt 1988.
26
See Powers 1992a, pp. 43–4 for a similar view.
27
See Wootton 2015, pp. 72–73.
28
Hacking 1975.
29
Even in 1597 Thomas Morley was describing the consonance of the perfect fourth with a long
list of tenor-based precepts like “the fourth above the tenor is consonant only if the bass is a third or
fifth below the tenor,” paying homage to the dyadic tradition despite his own music’s manifestly
triadic and functional qualities.
30
Compare Bent 1998 and Dahlhaus’s remarks on the relation between V7 and vii°, discussed at
the end of the next section. Bent requires that “analytical tools be harmonized with early techniques
and vocabulary, to the extent that these can be recovered and extended, just as what is incompatible
with how they parsed their music should be avoided” (1998, p. 24).
31
Jesse Anne Owens, in the preface to Wiering 2001, p. ix.
32
Interestingly enough, this seems to reflect both an increasing preference for “major” modes and
an increasing preference for major triads within those modes. Major-mode pieces—interpreted as
“pieces ending on a major triad that is in the key signature, and which is the most-frequent chord in
the piece”—rise from a total of about 14% in Ockeghem, to 18% in Josquin, to 30% in Palestrina.
Meanwhile, the proportion of major triads among the consonances within a broad selection of minor-
mode pieces (i.e., pieces whose last root supports a minor triad in the key signature) changes from
22% in Ockeghem to 19% in Josquin to 29% in Palestrina; the corresponding numbers for ionian and
mixolydian are 24% for Ockeghem, 34% for Josquin, and 51% for Palestrina.
33
Some but not all of this is due to Ockeghem and Josquin’s greater preference for progressions
that move from a complete triad to an open fifth. In Palestrina, 27% of root-position major triads move
down by fifth to a root-position triad vs. only about 9% for minor triads, a difference that is too large
to be due to cadences alone.
34
Jeppesen (1931) 1939, p. xi.
35
The relative balance between major and minor, root position and first inversion, likewise
suggests that consonance is the primary harmonic consideration, as if Ockeghem had only weak
preferences among the different vertical possibilities.
36
Carl Dahlhaus claimed that Renaissance harmony is marked by “bass formulas” in which “the
difference between 53 and 63 sonorities over the same bass has little or no importance” (1990, p. 145):
in Palestrina, however, bass motion by fourth or fifth overwhelmingly tends to support root-position
triads. Meier (1988, p. 51) argues that the bass voice, along with the concept of the chord, becomes
increasingly important during the sixteenth century.
37
I used my computer to select one-flat pieces ending on an F triad and with F major as the most
common chord, and zero-flat pieces ending on C and with C major as the most common triad; I then
averaged the behavior in these two groups. Roig-Francolí 2018 considers some of Victoria’s proto-
baroque tendencies.
38
The preference for 63 chords over 7̂ is unsurprising, since the root-position triad is diminished.
39
That is, pieces with no key signature and which end on a C triad.
40
For clarity, I eliminate a number of chords whose zeroth-order tendency is very similar to their
tendency before or after vii°6.
41
This is a good place to reiterate that all my code and data are available online.
42
Dahlhaus (1990, p. 63) and Bent (1998, p. 44). Dahlhaus views the Renaissance “parallel
cadence” as combining 3–5 and 6–8 interval progressions, endowing the third above the bass with an
ascending tendency. The classical seventh-above-the-root, by contrast, has a descending tendency and
is rarely doubled.
43
See Tymoczko 2011a, chapter 7.
44
V43 is awkward from an earlier perspective since the fourth above the bass is dissonant, yet its
tone of resolution, the third above the bass, is present in the chord; in Renaissance music it is very
rare, usually arising from a double suspension (i.e., D and F suspended against G, resolving downward
to C and E, with B forming the Renaissance seventh and moving up to C). I suspect composers like
Corelli and Bach still shared this point of view.
45
See Buelow 1992, p. 35, and Holtmeier 2007, p. 36. C. P. E. Bach writes: “to the chord of the
major sixth and the minor third, the perfect fourth is sometimes added without express indication”
([1762] 1949, p. 239). Thinking of V43 as an (optional) extension of vii°6 is very different from
thinking of vii°6 as a subset of V43.
46
Taruskin (2005, vol. 2, p. 655) articulates a common texture-based view: “the style of
Palestrina’s Kyrie does not differ especially from the ars perfecta idiom [i.e., the Renaissance
polyphonic style beginning with Josquin] with which we are familiar, because the Kyrie is a sparsely
texted, traditionally melismatic item where textual clarity was not of paramount concern. [ . . . ] It is in
the ‘talky’ movements of the Mass—the Gloria and the Credo—that the special post-Tridentine
qualities emerge.”
47
Jeppesen (1944–1945) 1975 agrees with this reading, arguing that the movement is in
mixolydian for the first eight measures and ionian thereafter.
48
This belongs to the same “tonal type” as the earlier Kyrie: it uses high clefs, has no signature,
and ends on a C triad (Powers 1981).
49
Lowinsky, by contrast, favors a double-mode theory where functional harmony arises
simultaneously in both ionian and aeolian (1961, pp. 8–10).
50
Stein 2002.
51
Cook 1987, Marvin and Brinkman 1999.
52
Long (2018, 2020) emphasizes the role of rhythm and phrasing in engendering harmonic
expectation. But it seems to me that the phenomena she discusses can appear in both modal and
functional contexts: regularly phrased eight-bar dance music, like irregularly phrased polyphony, can
be more or less functional. For this reason, I think we need an account of functionality that is
separable from issues of phrasing and rhythm, even if in actual historical practice these phenomena
were often intertwined.
53
For the Goudimel pieces, I have taken the preexisting melody as primary; it mostly appears in
the tenor but sometimes in the upper voice; for Monteverdi, I have used the upper voice, with no
implication that it is preexisting.
54
One gets a reasonably similar graph if one considers only root-position triads, or just first-
inversion chords.
55
Rock and jazz represent twentieth-century poles of this dichotomy: rock musicians favor IV and
jazz musicians ii.
56
Tendency here is the difference between the chord’s probability after a dominant and its overall
zeroth-order probability, as in Figure 6.4.6.
57
Jeppesen (1944–1945) 1975, p. 100.
58
Fétis (1840) 1994 (pp. 30ff) was an early advocate for the saltationist view. More recently,
Harold Powers (1981, p. 467) argued against the gradualist picture—in retrospect, placing too much
weight on theoretical statements, and not enough on musical practice. Long 2020 makes a similar
point.
59
Einstein 1948, p. 659, notes that this same text was set by Palestrina.
60
Note the similarity to Figure 5.1.5, which is in many ways its mixolydian counterpart.
61
The third quarter of m. 18 is harmonically obscure, with my preferred reading being a IV64
whose root F is decorated by a suspension G.
62
Coluzzi 2015 explores modal flexibility (commixtio tonorum) in another Marenzio madrigal.
63
See §7.4 on the ii–IV progression, which is largely eliminated in later music. Here again we see
functional practice coming gradually into focus: in this music, the poles of tonic, subdominant, and
dominant are fairly clear, but many peripheral conventions are absent.
64
Newcomb 2007 describes this (perhaps uncharitably) as “chordal noodling.”
Prelude
Could the Martians Understand Our Music?
Figure P7.3. Bach’s D minor two-part invention, BWV 775, mm. 7–14. I analyze m. 11 as ii and IV
simultaneously.
Figure P7.4. Three modulatory strategies. In pivot-chord modulation, there is an overlap between
syntactical progressions in two different keys. In direct modulation, keys do not overlap, but the new
key begins with an unambiguous dominant. Garden-path modulations do not strongly signal the start
of the second key, creating a nonsyntactical progression across the key boundary.
Figure P7.5. Seemingly modal progressions occurring across key changes: Bach’s “In allen meinen
Taten” (BWV 13.6, Riemenschneider 103, mm. 3–4) and Grieg’s “Gade” (Op. 57, no. 2, mm. 99–
103).
Figure P7.7. Two approaches to testing harmonic theories. On the left, different theories are evaluated
by a single objective analysis; on the right, different theories determine different optimal analyses,
with theory and analysis evaluated as a package.
book 2 of The Well-Tempered Clavier, we hear a long series of descending fifths, E–a♯–D♯–g♯–c♯–
Figure 7.1.1. The frequency of diatonic chords in Bach and Mozart, combining triads and sevenths.
Figure 7.1.2. In functional tonality, bass degrees 1̂, 3̂, 5̂, and 7̂ typically support tonic and dominant,
while the remaining degrees support the other harmonies.
We have seen that the simplest forms of functionality are built around
elementary subdominant and dominant cycles: I–IV–I, I–V–I, and I–IV–V–I,
all articulated by root-position chords. As functional harmony develops, the
dominant cycle increases in complexity, coming to play a greater and greater
role, while the subdominant cycle remains static or even decreases in
prominence. Figure 7.1.3 provides a theory of harmonic cycles in baroque
and classical music, adapted from A Geometry of Music.2 Here I arrange the
diatonic triads in a chain of thirds descending from tonic to dominant: chords
are completely free to move rightward and vertically, but leftward motion
must proceed along one of the arrows. The size of each symbol corresponds
to its frequency. In a typical functional piece, more than 95% of the
progressions will conform to this model, particularly if we exclude sequence
and fauxbourdon; conversely, all the root progressions allowed by the model
are common.3 This two-way fit poses a challenge for those who would like
to replace Roman numerals with particular schemas, as one would need a
fine-tuned collection of idioms—no doubt varying with genre—to reproduce
the progressions on the map.
Figure 7.1.3. A model of functional harmony where rightward and vertical motion is unrestricted but
leftward motion follows the arrows.
This model incorporates a variety of earlier approaches to functional
harmony. Inversionally equivalent chords are placed above one another,
meaning that they act similarly. Functional categories are encoded by
horizontal position, with neighboring sonorities (e.g., IV and ii, or vii° and
V) participating in similar progressions. The model also privileges certain
types of root progression: since the “strong” progressions (descending third,
descending fifth, and ascending step) move rightward by one, two, or three
steps, the model will permit more of these motions than their “weak”
counterparts (ascending third, ascending fifth, and descending step). Where
the simple protofunctionality of §6.1 was decidedly Riemannian, privileging
primary triads as locations in harmonic space, our more sophisticated model
is more Rameauian, highlighting a specific way of moving from tonic to
dominant.4
With just eleven chords, the map can be learned by example and without
explicit instruction. It is also consistent with figured-bass theory, as its
guiding principle is that bass lines move along an abstract chain of
descending thirds from 3̂ to 5̂, with 53 preceding 63 over the same bass, and
ascending thirds permissible only when they support 53→63 progressions
(e.g., from root position to first inversion, Figure 7.1.4). Interested readers
might try constructing alternative models that produce an approximately
root-functional output without making any overtly root-functional
assumptions: they will find, I suspect, that a thirds-based arrangement is
virtually unavoidable. And while the model includes a few progressions that
occur only in specific inversions, it can be approximately reformulated using
roots (Figure 7.1.5).
Figure 7.1.4. The descending-thirds arrangement as a figured-bass composer might conceive it, along
with a translation into contemporary chord symbols. The main numbers represent bass degrees; the
superscripts represent vertical configurations: 63 chords on 1̂ and 53 position chords on 7̂ are rare.
Figure 7.1.5. A root-functional version of the model, with some arrows requiring specific inversions.
Figure 7.1.6. The functional flexibility of the mediant, which can act as a pre-predominant, a
dominant, and a tonic substitute.
Figure 7.1.7. Some salient major-mode differences between earlier and later functional dialects.
Figure 7.2.2. Nested interval cycles in The Well-Tempered Clavier’s first A major prelude.
Two aspects of this structure are particularly relevant. First, its layers can
be rearranged to produce the basic voice leadings for the primary sonorities
of functional harmony (Figure 7.2.3). Indeed, each layer of the ouroboros
can be extended to form a “maximally even” diatonic chord, with
overlapping collections linked by their basic voice leading.9 Second, the
tertian layer can be used to reformulate the third-based harmonic grammar:
rather than taking triads to be indivisible musical atoms, we can describe
music as moving along the descending-third chain of pitch classes as shown
in Figure 7.2.4. In other words, harmonic cycles often generate complete
chains of diatonic pitch classes, descending by third through two abstract
octaves.10 This descending chain of thirds will often continue across
harmonic cycles to produce an ongoing pitch-class sequence.
Figure 7.2.3. Basic voice leadings for the fifth, third, triad, and seventh chord, derived by rearranging
contiguous segments of the interval cycles on Figure 7.2.1.
Figure 7.2.4. The harmonic grammar expressed with pitch classes. Again, rightward moves are
unrestricted while leftward motion follows the arrows.
Figure 7.2.5. Upper-voice regularity harmonized to form slightly irregular triads. The two staves
present the same sequence of descending thirds in slightly different ways.
These ideas can be used to interpret “Oh, Pretty Woman,” Roy Orbison’s
1964 ode to sexual objectification (Figure 7.2.6). The melody descends
through a single octave, moving downward by three thirds and a step;
underneath it, the harmony moves by third through two abstract octaves,
initially in parallel with the melody, but then inserting an E7 between D
major and A major. These descending thirds extend from the end of the first
statement through the beginning of the second, continuing the sequence
across the verse’s repetition. The bridge’s “down by third, up by step”
melody can be derived by omitting notes from the descending-third sequence
(e.g., F D b c E C a f, etc.); here it is supported by a pair of nonsequential
harmonic cycles—articulated in the electric guitar by a series of ascending
triadic voice leadings.11 Once again the pattern continues through the repeat,
the melody skipping only one note on the circle of thirds (or, if we view it as
an elided two-voice pattern, switching voices from bottom to top). Orbison’s
short song thus presents all three levels of the ouroboros: descending steps
on the melodic surface, embellishing descending thirds in the abstract
progression of pitch classes, mostly grouped into descending-fifth triadic
progressions, and supporting a descending-step melodic sequence in the
bridge.12 The beauty of this music is its multivalence: it is not based on fifths
or thirds or steps but rather on a delicate balance of all three, requiring only
the slightest compositional nudge to bring out one or another of these
aspects.
Such music is repetitive in two different ways. First, and most obviously,
it passes repeatedly through the same abstract set of harmonic possibilities.
Second, and more subtly, its phrases can be more or less sequential
depending on how a composer chooses to parse the abstract chain of
descending thirds. This sequential structure is latent in the grammar,
available for composers to exploit if they choose—rather than something put
into a particular piece, it can be drawn out of its generic fabric (cf. §4.3). At
some basic level, mature functionality simply is the descending sequence of
thirds—sometimes manifest on the musical surface and sometimes locked
away in the musical subconscious (Figure 7.2.7).13 The magic of this style
lies in the interaction between overt and covert, symmetry and asymmetry,
sequential melodic descent filtered through a nonsequential grammar.
Figure 7.2.7. One of the second themes of the first movement of Beethoven’s Op. 22, twice
descending by thirds from F to F.
Figure 7.3.2. A model of functional progressions inspired by Rameau. Chords can use third
substitution to move forward along the arrows to either bracketed chord, inserting either a descending
third or ascending step into the sequence of descending fifths (e.g., C–a–d–G or C–d–G). This graph
allows for seven progressions, all but one of which is common in functional music; all but one make a
single rightward pass through Figure 7.1.3.
Figure 7.3.3. Each of Rameau’s three basic progressions articulates a segment of the descending-third
chain of pitch classes.
Figure 7.3.4. Derivation of the I–vi–IV–ii–vii°–V–I progression from Rameau’s descending-fifth
paradigm. The boxed progressions can be interpreted as elided versions of the seventh-chord
progression on the bottom staff. Black noteheads on the bottom staff are missing from the associated
triads.
Figure 7.3.6. The fundamental harmonic progression of the prelude, with the descending scalar bass.
The bass line here derives from the A major statement.
Figure 7.3.7. The D major prelude makes four passes through this flowchart.
Figure 7.3.8. The prelude’s constantly descending bass line, with each beat representing a measure.
Though Bach was hardly a loyal Rameauian, the choices available in his
open-form prelude closely track the basic concepts of Rameau’s theory.
There are no ascending thirds or fifths in Figure 7.3.7, but rather a
preponderance of descending fifths (65%) and descending thirds (19%), with
a smaller number of ascending and descending steps (10% and 5%
respectively, the latter mostly moving from tonic to leading-tone seventh).20
For me, the root motions are salient: it feels as if the dancer is moving in a
particular way rather than visiting a collection of familiar places. In other
words, it exhibits a Rameauian functionality based on types of root
progression rather than a Riemannian functionality based on locations such
as “subdominant.” Yet the churning motion-oriented music also conforms to
the precepts of the thirds-based grammar, presenting no anomalous moves
and touching on a large fraction of the permitted progressions.
The near-equivalence between Rameau’s theory and my own is an
example of theoretical underdetermination, our ability to construct different
theories to account for one and the same set of observable facts. In physics
we have the Lagrangian, Hamiltonian, and vector formulations of Newtonian
mechanics. In music we have Rameau’s descending-fifth theory, my own
descending-thirds theory, and many others besides: theories based on idioms,
functional categories, linear relationships, and so on. The interesting thought
is that none of these theories is right or wrong; instead, they offer different
perspectives on a musical practice that was largely understood tacitly. In
choosing among them we must ask which gives the simplest and most
compelling explanation of the broadest range of musical behavior. Different
theorists will make different choices, reflecting preferences that are as much
aesthetic as scientific.
4. Functional melody, functional harmony
Seventh chords are intrinsic to Rameau’s theory, not just in providing a tool
for analyzing descending-third progressions, but also in coordinating
descending-step melody with functionally tonal harmony. In reality,
however, seventh chords are not nearly so prevalent as Rameau’s theory
suggests. How then do real-life functional composers manage the tension
between triadic functionality and descending melody, given the tendency of
“strong” progressions to generate ascending melodic steps? That is, how do
functionally triadic composers negotiate the fact that efficient triadic voice
leading often leads to ascending melodies?
Here I think we can give five separate answers, each of which helps us
understand something different about the style. The first is that functional
composers use a wide range of root progressions, including “weak”
progressions that give rise to descending triadic voice leading. While it is
true that ascending thirds are generally rare, descending steps are reasonably
common, and ascending fifths ubiquitous: progressions like IV–I and I–V
are important in virtually all functional genres.21 Functional tonality does not
so much avoid ascending-fifth progressions per se as ascending fifths not
involving the tonic triad.
The second answer is that functional harmony involves a norm of larger
voice leading, expressed by larger motions on the triadic circle; in particular,
simple harmonic cycles like I–V–I and I–IV–I often make a complete circle
through the space (Figure 7.4.1). These larger voice leadings permit the
addition of passing tones that create brief seventh chords, turning
contrapuntal lemons into harmonic lemonade. Thus, insofar as functional
composers have to negotiate a conflict between harmonic and melodic
forces, this is largely a matter of avoiding obvious contrapuntal possibilities
—ascending-third progressions and ascending fifths not involving the tonic.
Practically speaking, the remaining options are more than sufficient to
produce effective music.
Figure 7.4.1. Functional tonality often travels the long way around the triadic circle.
Figure 7.4.3. Measure 5 of the first A♭ major fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier.
Figure 7.4.4. Modal possibilities for melodic 5̂–4̂–3̂ over harmonic V–?–I.
Figure 7.4.5. Modal and functionally tonal approaches to similar musical problems.
Figure 7.4.6. Harmonizing descending melodic steps in the Bach chorales and Mozart piano sonatas.
The notation I(64) indicates that the tonic chord could be in any inversion including second, whereas
I64 requires second inversion.
This suggests that functional-harmonic restrictions likely drove the
expansion from triads to seventh chords, partly as a way of recovering lost
melodic possibilities: scale-degree progressions such as 2̂–1̂–7̂, which had
indigenous modal harmonizations, gradually acquired functional alternatives
involving sevenths and I64, alternatives that allowed descending-fifth
progressions to harmonize a greater range of melodic possibilities.24 If this is
right, then it seems unlikely that we can derive functional rules from voice-
leading considerations, as some theorists have wanted.25 Instead something
like the opposite is true: at the most general level, melodic goals generate
nonfunctional triadic progressions; to counteract this tendency, composers
need to replace those triadic progressions with more complex alternatives
involving seventh chords and the cadential 64.
A final answer is that surface voices sometimes descend relative to an
ascending background. Leonard Cohen’s “Hallelujah” uses this strategy
alongside the others discussed in this section. The reduction in Figure 7.4.7
is largely determined by the melody, whose primary notes are shown with
open noteheads; these ascend by sixth from G4 before falling back to C5.26 I
analyze the final phrase as descending through the triadic background,
moving soprano–alto–tenor before cadencing on a C4 that is the same
abstract voice as the C5 that ends the second phrase, only now in a lower
octave.27 The strategy here is essentially the inverse of Neil Young’s
“Helpless”: there we found omnipresent descending voice leading giving
rise to modal harmony, with the vocal melody ascending by triadic step
against a descending triadic background; here we have omnipresent melodic
ascent, with the vocal melody descending through the voices from one verse
to the next. The two songs, with their two different harmonic languages, are
in effect mirror images, with “Helpless” descending while “Hallelujah” rises
toward the heavens.
Figure 7.4.7. An outline of Leonard Cohen’s “Hallelujah.” The numbers above the example show the
steps each progression takes along the circle of diatonic triads (Figure 7.4.1).
Figure 7.5.3. Two approaches to harmonizing a descending scale: the traditional rule of the octave
(top) creates harmonic accents on metrically weak beats, while the fauxbourdon-based alternative
(bottom) does not.
Figure 7.5.5. In the second theme of the first movement of Brahms’s G major violin sonata, Brahms
uses the fauxbourdon ROTO to create a strong sense of retrofunctionality.
Figure 7.5.6. Standing on the predominant in the Symphony Op. 11, no. 2, I, mm. 108–115, by Joseph
Bologne, Chevalier de Saint-Georges.
Figure 7.5.7. “Passing 64” chords in Bach, Mozart, and Haydn. More than 90% involve tonic or
dominant.
Figure 7.5.9. The 75–63 fauxbourdon sequence in mm. 34–37 of Monteverdi’s madrigal “Longe da te,
cor mio” (Book IV).
Figure 7.5.10. The “pseudochord” idiom in its tonic and dominant forms.
6. Sequences
Figure 7.6.1. Sequences by interval of transposition in Corelli, Bach, and Mozart. Numbers are
percentages in the work of each composer.
Figure 7.6.8. The use of seventh chords allows for a greater variety of stepwise descending thirds.
Figure 7.6.9. Ascending-third sequences in theory and practice.
Figure 7.6.10. A disguised ascending-third sequence at the opening of Pergolesi’s Stabat Mater, P. 77.
The stepwise upper voices are decorated by voice exchanges and octave displacement (E6→E5). The
sequence calls for ii° instead of iv on the second beat of m. 3.
Figure 7.7.1. The diatonic third’s descending and ascending basic voice leadings and their functional
uses.
These patterns resonate with the music-theoretical tradition known as
“dualism,” which conceives of ascending fifths as inversions of descending
fifths, their “upside-down” counterparts. Dualist music theorists typically
operate in chromatic space, interpreting the major key’s dominant as the
chromatic inversion of the minor key’s subdominant; however, the two
forms of the basic voice leading can also be related by diatonic inversion
around the supertonic. From this point of view, the “ascending subdominant”
is the inversional counterpart of the V–I progression (Figure 7.7.2). But this
oversimplifies things, since both dominant and subdominant permit a
passing note that forms a resolving tritone: paradoxically IV–I is both a
musical antipode to V–I, and also an alternative expression of the same
functional logic. Could there be functional dialects where “dominant” is a
superordinate genus containing both descending- and ascending-fifth
species?53
Figure 7.7.2. The descending G–C voice leading is the diatonic inversion of the ascending F–C voice
leading; both permit a passing tone that creates a resolving tritone.
Figure 7.7.3. Descending and ascending step- and third-sequences using the two kinds of voice
leading. The first two letters indicate the type of sequence, while the last indicates whether the
descending or ascending basic voice leading is used. Thus A2a indicates an ascending-step sequence
using the ascending form of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading.
Figure 7.7.4. Measures 3–4 of the first two-part invention (BWV 772), along with its inversion in
measures 19–20.
Figure 7.7.5. Two episodes from the first invention, the first using the inverted theme to ascend by
fifth; the second using theme and inversion to descend by fifths.
Figure 7.7.6. The opening sequence of the first C♯ major prelude from The Well-Tempered Clavier.
Open noteheads show the diatonic third’s ascending basic voice leading.
(c) “Ach, lieben Christen, seid getrost” (BWV 256, Riemenschneider 31).
The previous pieces contrast the two forms of the diatonic third’s basic voice
leading, but in different ways: the two-part invention presents the ascending
fifth as both the descending fifth’s inversional dual and functional
equivalent, while the C♯ major prelude uses ascending and descending fifths
as contrasting directions in an orderly modulatory path. “Ach, lieben
Christen, seid getrost,” a freestanding chorale based on an anonymous
sixteenth-century melody, gives us a third approach, moving quickly from
ascending-fifth progressions to descending-fifth cadences, as if compressing
the prelude’s harmonic narrative down to the phrasal level. Its mercurial
contrasts feel spontaneous and embodied, the product of internalized
keyboard routines rather than a series of chord changes we are intended to
follow.
The music begins on A minor but immediately switches to C major for the
rest of the phrase (Figure 7.7.9); this is a standard chorale gambit, inserting
the global tonic at the start of an opening phrase in a subsidiary key, as if
paying lip service to monotonality.56 The IV–I ascending subdominant
features the diatonic third’s basic voice leading, supporting a “merely
passing” note that “just happens” to create a resolving tritone. The phrase
ends with a cadential schema reused by four of the chorale’s five phrases,
with the inner-voice leading tone falling by third.57 The second phrase
returns from the relative major to the relative minor by a sequence of
ascending fifths, C–G–d–a. This is a favorite Bach schema, the diatonic
third’s basic voice leading allowing for the tonicization of each successive
fifth; it is closely related to the four-voice Chase-schema variant in §4.6.
Relative to that schema, the seemingly superfluous vii°/V is both standard
and expected—sandwiched inside the ascending basic voice leading (C5,
E4) → (B4, G4).
Figure 7.7.9. “Ach lieben Christen, seid getrost,” highlighting the diatonic third’s basic voice leading.
The next phrase cancels the Picardy third with Renaissance decisiveness,
shifting directly from the cadence on A major to the dominant of C. Once
again, we return to the relative minor by the ascending-fifth schema, C–G–
d–a, now extended to a half cadence in A minor. The next phrase modulates
to G major by descending fifths, arriving at a dominant that moves to G by
way of a IV6–viiø7–I progression. Many analysts would consider the IV6
“merely passing,” but the chorale contains three C6–f ♯ ø7–G progressions,
only one of which is preceded by D; it is a particular schema that uses the
ascending basic voice leading, not a generic process of prolongation.58 This
last phrase loosely echoes the second, beginning with two more repetitions
of the four-voice Chase schema. In the D–a progression, the thirds switch
roles so that each staff twice articulates the diatonic third’s basic voice
leading: (G, B) → (F♯, D) → (A, C) and (D, B) → (F♯, A) → (E, C).59 At
this point the fifths reverse direction, descending to the final cadence as in
m. 4.
Here, much more than in the previous pieces, one can start to feel the
inadequacy of triadic Roman-numeral theory. For while virtually everything
in the chorale can be made consistent with that theory, this requires a series
of analytical tricks—the delicate placement of key boundaries, the use of
secondary dominants to license unusual triadic progressions, and so on. Such
tricks will not completely erase the sense of mismatch between triadic
grammar and Bach’s practice: theory tells us to value descending-fifth
progressions and smooth progressions between keys, whereas Bach seems to
delight in ascending-fifth progressions that meander from one key to
another. There is no path to the music that starts from the precepts in a
standard harmony text. But once we focus on the basic voice leading, it
starts to clarify: for here we find a systematic contrast between the familiar
ascending-fifth schema, appearing in both explicit and disguised form, and
the descending-fifth logic of the cadence.
(d) The second B minor fugue from The Well-Tempered Clavier. The last
piece of The Well-Tempered Clavier paints a similar picture on a bigger
canvas, using the two forms of the basic voice leading to articulate large
formal regions. Its episodes all use the diatonic third’s basic voice leading,
with episodes 1, 3, and 7 ascending and the rest descending. The subject has
two parts, the first an inverted arch combining basic voice leadings in near-
contrary motion, the second a 6̂–5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂ descent. The piece presents two
harmonizations of this descent, each exploiting a different dyadic pattern
(Figure 7.7.10).60 The first uses sequence D3a on Figure 7.7.3 and leads to a
series of energetically charged ascending-fifth episodes while the second
uses seventh chords and the diatonic fifth’s basic voice leading; this
produces a variety of harmonizations spanning the gamut from fauxbourdon
to descending fifths.61 The music thus charts a path from unusual to
conventional, gradually relaxing in intensity over the course of its ninety-
nine measures—much as our earlier examples balanced ascending-fifth
tension with descending-fifth release.
Figure 7.7.10. The subject of the second B minor fugue from The Well-Tempered Clavier, along with
two harmonizations of the linear descent.
Formally, the piece is in three large sections: the first twenty-six measures
contain a single episode and state the subject on B, F♯, and B; this music is
dominated by an energetic, trilling countersubject and the diatonic third’s
ascending basic voice leading. The central second section lasts from m. 27 to
m. 80 and introduces the oscillatory, descending-fifth countersubject. Here
we have very little B minor, with two entries on F♯ and one each in D major,
A major, and E minor; this music is dominated by descending-fifth
progressions. The third section is a transposed and condensed repeat of the
second, supplemented by a cursory final cadence.
Figure 7.7.11 uses open noteheads to highlight two fundamental voice-
leading patterns: the contrary-motion near sequence harmonizing the
subject’s opening and the countersubject’s basic voice leading. The
abundance of white shows how completely the two schemas saturate the
music. The first episode warps the subject’s final measure into an episode
that exploits pattern A2a on Figure 7.7.3, moving along the chain of
ascending fifths from A major to its relative minor. The m. 26 arrival on C♯7
signals the end of the first section and the disappearance of the trilling
countersubject. Bach’s willingness to drop this motive contradicts
compositional nostrums about unity and motivic consistency: instead, the
piece simply moves on to something new.
The first half of the second section is outlined in Figure 7.7.12. In some
ways this can be considered a second beginning, the counterpoint finally
having achieved its final form. It is interesting that this new countersubject
appears only after the piece has left the tonic, as if reserving its home-key
debut for the recapitulatory third section. Bach’s countersubject has a
characteristic baroque flexibility, appearing once as fauxbourdon, three times
as descending fifths, and twice as the intermediate 75–63 sequence (Figure
7.7.13).62 One weakness of triadic theory is its inability to capture this
flexibility: fauxbourdon is purely linear, the descending-fifth sequence
paradigmatically harmonic, and the 75–63 D3A2 sequence is an
uncomfortable hybrid that cannot easily be assimilated to either category.
Like the Renaissance seventh, this eighteenth-century idiom challenges the
distinction between harmonic and nonharmonic, opening a gap between
Bach’s thinking and our own.
Figure 7.7.12. An outline of the first half of the fugue’s middle section.
Figure 7.7.13. The six appearances of the 7–6 pattern, transposed to B.
The fourth entry gives way to a descending-step episode whose bass has
the same oscillating character as the second countersubject, as if echoing the
new musical mood (see Figure 7.7.12, and pattern D2d on Figure 7.7.3). It is
a canonic sequence whose upper voices outline the descending form of the
diatonic third’s basic voice leading, highlighted here for the first time. The
following A major entry gives us a descending-fifth harmonization of the
second countersubject, a diatonic 65–42 sequence in sevenths. This is
followed by a return of the diatonic third’s ascending basic voice leading,
embedded in a highly chromatic sequence, with the voices outlining exactly
the same notes that featured in the earlier ascending-fifth episode. (Compare
the reduction of episode 3 in Figure 7.7.12 to that of episode 1 in Figure
7.7.11.) Like that earlier passage, the music ascends by fifths from A to f♯,
once again moving from major to relative minor. Discounting the
recapitulatory third section, this is the last ascending-fifth sequence in the
piece: having dominated the first half of the fugue, its nonstandard logic
gives way to descending-fifth convention.
The rest of the piece has some wonderful moments: a joyous cascade with
outer voices in a lightly disguised canon (episode 4 on Figure 7.7.14); a brief
ascending-fourth sequence based on the first two measures of the theme,
creating the false impression of stretto (entry 8 on the same figure); and a
final descending-fifth episode in which the bass loops the end of the subject
under a florid upper-voice countermelody, with the outer voices decorating
the diatonic third’s descending basic voice leading (episode 5). With the
return to the tonic, the fugue settles into a recapitulation. The expressive
effect is somewhat unclear, the recapitulation functioning neither as a
triumphant return nor a counterweight to any lengthy tonal excursion;
instead, it is a slight tweaking of the degree-of-sameness in a relatively
homogeneous piece. The final close is perfunctory, the subject shrinking to
its first four notes and appearing in the lower voices before the entire Well-
Tempered Clavier careens to a halt on an unexpected Picardy third.
Figure 7.7.14. An outline of the second half of the middle section of the fugue.
***
If it is true that J. S. Bach’s music is often governed by a dualistic contrast
between ascending and descending fifths, then what consequences follow? A
blunt answer is that for all its accuracy, Roman-numeral thinking is often
boring, insensitive to the contrapuntal structures that give direction to its
atomic and independent harmonic cycles. Rather than mere conventions,
these contrapuntal structures reflect a preexisting musical geometry; thus,
though they may sometimes appear in historical sources or partimento
manuscripts, they can also be rediscovered independently by musicians as
they learn their way around the keyboard. This independent rediscovery is
part of the process of developing a distinctive compositional voice.
A sense for this contrapuntal logic is a prerequisite for enlightening
analytical reduction. Consider the opening of Petzold’s Minuet in G, not
composed by Bach but evidently valued by him. Every Schenkerian analysis
I know treats the opening melody as embellishing a neighboring 5̂–6̂–5̂
progression, the E–F♯–G ascent a superficial prolongation of the primary E
(Figure 7.7.15).63 The assumption seems to be that the subdominant’s third
has a tendency to descend, acting as a neighbor to 5̂. The preceding analyses
suggest that the progression is instead an expressively marked illustration of
C. P. E. Bach’s ascending tendency. On this hearing, the tonic and stable G–
A–B–C–D is answered by a more active ascending-subdominant C–D–E–
F♯–G, the C–B bass and E–F♯–G melody creating a subdominant-dominant
hybrid. Habits formed by Haydn and Mozart will not help us hear this music,
nor adjudicate among our interpretive options. Nor will it do to retreat to
subjectivity, declaring analysis the record of our own responses to
decontextualized notes: for interpretation should be cognizant of a
composer’s vocabulary—or at a minimum, not conducted in ignorance
thereof.
A similar point could be made about Figure 7.7.16, which at first glance
seems to juxtapose two unrelated syntactical units, IVmaj2–ii7–V7–I in the
dominant and a descending-step sequence that returns to the tonic. To a
traditional theorist, the progression from V to vii°43/ii is an inessential
juncture, a pair of words belonging to different sentences. To a Schenkerian,
the local progressions and modulations are superficial, generated by a deeper
linear process in which the tenth (C4, E5) descends by step through the
octave to (C3, E4). Those who have been sensitized to the diatonic third’s
basic voice leading, however, may notice the descending-step dyadic
sequence (G, B) → (F, D) and (F, A) → (E, C): this gives higher-order
direction to the harmonic cycles while continuing the modulatory journey up
the ladder of fifths (C major→G major→D minor→ . . .). Bach embeds the
dyadic sequence in a series of nonsequential triads, superimposing the first-
unit (F, D) with the second-unit (F, A), so that (G, B) is root-third of a G
major triad while (F, A) is third-fifth of D minor (c.f. Figure P4.3). This
dyadic logic joins two Roman-numeral cycles and cuts across the parallel
tenths of Schenker’s analysis. The compositional thinking here is
recognizably similar to that which produced the four-voice Chase schema in
“Ach, lieben Christen, seid getrost,” or the ascending-fifth sequences of the
B minor fugue; indeed, virtually the same progression occurs in the G major
cello suite, extending the ascending fifths past the supertonic to the relative
minor (Figure 7.1.17).64
Figure 7.7.16. A sequence of ascending basic voice leadings in mm. 8–15 of The Well-Tempered
Clavier’s first prelude, along with Schenker’s reading.
Figure 7.7.17. Measures 9–14 of the prelude to the G major cello suite, BWV 1007.
1
This preference for tonic and dominant is embedded in the traditional “rule of the octave,” to be
discussed shortly; the connection supports Holtmeier’s 2007 contention that the rule of the octave is
not simply a schema but a theory of functional tonality.
2
My theory is indebted to a long tradition of “harmonic maps” (Riemann 1896, Piston 1941,
McHose 1947, Kostka and Payne 2003).
3
Some bass lines are rare for linear reasons, such as ii6–V6 with its leaping tritone or V6–I6 with its
unresolved leading tone.
4
See Christensen 1993b, p. 97.
5
It is possible that the mediant’s harmonic flexibility helps explain its infrequency: by blurring
tonic and dominant, it undercuts the dichotomy at the heart of functional syntax. Nonsequential
mediants are most common at the beginning and end of the functional tradition, virtually disappearing
during the classical style.
6
See mm. 32–33 of Mozart’s Piano Sonata in C major, K.279, II, or m. 13 of his C minor sonata
K.457, I (with the leading tone in an inner voice); the tenths often evoke the Prinner schema.
7
Horn and Huron (2015) make a related point about modal homogenization, which occurs on an
even larger timescale.
8
John Clough (1979b and 1994) and Jason Yust (2009) have explored this structure as it relates to
prolongation (§9.1–§9.3). I will be more concerned with its role in generating harmonic and melodic
patterns, including sequences and near sequences. An unpublished paper by Yust, written when he was
a graduate student, anticipates several features of my approach.
9
Because the size of the diatonic scale is a prime number, every “maximally even” chord is a near
interval cycle and every interval cycle can be extended to form a maximally even chord. Since the
ouroboros contains fifths, thirds, and steps, it contains basic voice leadings for nearly even chords of
every size.
10
For example, I–IV–V7 forms the cycle (G–E–C)–(C–A–F)–(F–D–B–G), returning to its starting
note two octaves below. Very simple cycles such as I–V–I and I–IV–I skip a few notes on this chain of
thirds.
11
The A major triad at the end of the bridge sounds like C: V/ii, suggesting another repeat; instead,
it becomes the global tonic by compositional fiat.
12
Temperley 2018 notes the bridge’s sequential structure.
13
There are many well-known examples of descending-third melodies, including the opening of
the Hammerklavier’s first-movement development (Rosen 1971, pp. 407–13), the opening of
Brahms’s Fourth Symphony, and Brahms’s Op. 121, no. 3.
14
One could of course make an analogous point about many non-Western styles.
15
This blurring of third-related harmonies is already present in the 1722 Treatise on Harmony,
which derives the deceptive cadence by noting that C–E–A can replace C–E–G (Rameau [1722]
1971). In that book, C–E–A can have a fundamental bass of either A or C.
16
This is particularly clear if one examines the musical examples Rameau offers, which sometimes
add content to his explicit theorizing. These examples, like my own reconstruction, highlight the
centricity-defining role of I and V7.
17
Nathan Martin has referred to Rameau’s “astonishing ingenuity” in deriving common
progressions from his descending-fifth paradigm (Martin 2018). But if functional music tends to move
along the descending circle of thirds, then this ingenuity is easily explained; what makes it seem
astonishing is the nonobvious relation between Rameau’s descending-fifth paradigm and (what I take
to be) the more basic circle of descending thirds.
18
Since the second distortion undoes the first, quarter notes 8–10 are shifted up by third relative to
the underlying pattern. The second distortion avoids the tonic chord’s seventh, abandoning the leading
tone to create a brief moment of triadic arrival; my analysis includes this missing seventh in
parentheses.
19
Readers can visit my https://www.madmusicalscience.com to hear artificially generated music
using Bach’s template. Those who find Bach’s figuration challenging can use another pattern, relaxing
whatever stylistic rules they want—the important thing is guiding the perpetually descending sevenths
through a range of keys.
20
Rameau himself might not have considered them true descending-step progressions, as he
considered vii° an incomplete V7.
21
The most common ascending-third progression is vi–I6, and the most common descending-step
progressions are vi–V and V–IV6. The functionally anomalous cadential six-four allows for vi–I64 and
ii–I64 (§9.5), while vii° (sometimes thought to be a metonym for V7) allows for I–vii°.
22
Compare the second B major fugue, which exploits the double counterpoint latent in the two
parallel options.
23
For modal ii(6)–IV–V progressions see Figures 6.5.5 (m. 56) and 6.7.6 (m. 25).
24
The ascending use of 4̂ in I–V43–I6 is also relevant here, as it allows I–V–I to harmonize 3̂–4̂–5̂.
25
See Headlam 2012 and Yust 2018, chapter 10, for expressions of this Schenkerian hope.
26
The only descents come from “weak” progressions in both more and less conventional forms (a
IV–I–V found in virtually all functional music, and I–vi–I and IV–vi–IV progressions more idiomatic
to rock) and slightly longer voice-leading paths (IV–V and V/vi–vi, the latter using a seventh). I hear
I–vi–I and IV–vi–IV as weaker versions of subdominant-tonic oscillation, with vi substituting for IV
in the first case and I in the second. These variants start to become popular in the middle of the
nineteenth century (§10.7).
27
This ascending voice leading continues through the repeat even while the vocal melody
disguises it, with the middle voice now sounding the melodic G4. This is illustrated, on my example,
by the two open noteheads in the final chord, one representing the last note of the first verse, the other
the first note of the second verse.
28
Traditional theorists rarely distinguished fauxbourdon from harmonic cycles: Rameau ([1722]
1971, p. 126), for example, adds a fundamental bass to fauxbourdon, while Weber ([1817–1821] 1846,
p. 457) includes fauxbourdon passages in his discussion of chord grammar. Later theorists such as
Schenker and Lerdahl also conceive of functional harmony as unified.
29
The contrary-motion outer voices are a common idiom; see Figures 3.2.5 (“fauxbourdon”) and
10.6.9, or Beethoven’s Op. 18, no. 5, IV, m. 59ff.
30
It is sobering to contemplate the divergence between this musical “rule of the octave” and the
alternative found in historical treatises—a theoretical construct that is perhaps more accurate as a
description of general functional principles than of musical practice (Christensen 1992, Holtmeier
2007; see Sanguinetti 2012, p. 20, who cites a rare discussion of the fauxbourdon ROTO in Valente).
Once again we find an embodied and untheorized musical knowledge, idioms learned by ear or by
score study, passed on from composer to composer without leaving clear theoretical traces.
31
Both Temperley (2011) and Rothgeb (1975) describe the fauxbourdon ROTO progression as
unusual.
32
Brahms frequently uses retrofunctional V–IV progressions in his second themes; often these
involve some version of the “fauxbourdon ROTO,” but sometimes they do not. For other examples,
see the second themes of the first movements of the G major string sextet, Op. 36, and the C minor
piano quartet, Op. 60.
33
Compare the fauxbourdon ROTO harmonization of 8̂–7̂–6̂–5̂ to the template in Figure 6.1.3.
34
My label is a play on the term “standing on the dominant,” coined by Erwin Ratz and
popularized by Caplin 1998; Gjerdingen’s “Indugio” schema (2007) is closely related. For similar
examples from Mozart, see K.279, I, mm. 25ff; K.283, III, mm. 33ff; and K.309, I, mm. 47ff.
35
Even in the sixteenth century, Zarlino encouraged composers to be freer in their use of second-
inversion harmonies ([1558] 1968, p. 193).
36
Harrison 2003 explores this connection in detail; Allsop 1992 explores the prehistory of the
Corelli style.
37
William Caplin has noted that the “7” sonorities in these passages have a quasi-harmonic
character (1998, p. 31, footnote 29), and I have argued for the importance of the underlying down-a-
third-up-by-step root-pattern (2011a, chapter 7).
38
The “Crucifixus” from Bach’s B minor mass uses the 7–6 and 7–43 options almost
interchangeably (Biamonte 2012).
39
Examples can also be found in Brahms: Op. 76, no. 4, mm. 28–30 (discussed in Tymoczko
2011a, chapter 8), and Op. 119, no. 2, mm. 15–16.
40
One might think that this idiom counts as evidence against that prohibition, except that the vast
majority of apparent I–ii–I6 progressions occur in precisely this context, with the predominant
invariably lacking its fifth.
41
The second G major prelude in The Well-Tempered Clavier contains an extended example of the
idiom in which the parallel thirds exceed their usual bounds.
42
To generate this list, I programmed my computer to identify passages of melodic repetition; I
then analyzed each sequence by hand, eliminating the relatively small number that I considered not to
be true sequences. I also compared the results to my own analyses of individual pieces to check for
completeness (appendix 4).
43
Hook 2020 uses a similar system of categorization. Longer sequences can usually (but not
always) be reduced to just the first and last chord in the repeating unit.
44
This sequence appears commonly in Beatles songs, for instance “You Never Give Me Your
Money.”
45
Interestingly, Bach uses the A5D3 sequence, the inversion of the common D5A3, at least twice
in The Well-Tempered Clavier: in the first F major fugue (m. 13) and in the second C♯ minor fugue
(m. 23).
46
To these we can add fauxbourdon and the “down a third up a step” sequence, though the latter is
rare after the baroque.
47
See Riemann 1896, pp. 120ff, and Caplin 1998, p. 29, for the view that sequences are almost
entirely linear. Schenker went further, denying that sequences existed and speaking of linear
progressions instead; see Schenker (1935) 1979, Slottow 2018, and Sprick 2018. Weber (1817–1821)
1846 argues that sequential progressions are similar to those used by harmonic cycles (p. 430), though
somewhat looser (p. 432).
48
Note that here it is important to consider the melodic E5 as at least quasi-harmonic, as it
continues the sequential pattern of mediants-acting-like-dominants, even though it quickly resolves to
an orthodox V7.
49
This pattern uses both the ascending-third (I–V7/vi) and ascending-step patterns (V7/vi–IV–V) in
Figure 4.4.1.
50
Margulis 2013.
51
Bach (1762) 1949, p. 205.
52
Alternately, Nathan Martin has suggested to me that it may be an echo of the pre-Renaissance
idea that imperfect consonances resolve to perfect consonances.
53
Delair’s 1690 figured-bass treatise recommended the diatonic third’s ascending basic voice
leading for ascending-fifth bass lines (Nicolas 2019).
54
This analysis echoes some of Schoenberg’s observations about the piece (Neff 2012).
55
The F major two-part invention has a very similar harmonic plan, modulating upward by fifths
I–V–ii–vi, descending by fifth-related chords vi–ii–V–V/IV–IV to reach the subdominant, and ending
by modulating back up by fifth.
56
Typically these “orphan tonics” also participate in the harmonic narrative of the opening phrase,
though sometimes in an unorthodox way (e.g., a vi–I6 progression).
57
To call this “non-normative” is to engage in speculative fiction; in reality, this is a standard
cadence that should be included in any realistic description of leading-tone behavior.
58
Burstein (2018, p. 6) writes that the IV6–I progression is “atypical,” but by my count I is the
second most common successor to IV6 in Bach’s chorales.
59
This could be described as a variable sequence with changing permutation.
60
The Well-Tempered Clavier’s second F♯ minor fugue also juxtaposes these harmonic strategies,
initially harmonizing 6̂–5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂ with the descending-third sequence before shifting to descending
fifths for the rest of the piece. In that fugue, however, the descending-third harmonization is fleeting
and momentary, with the bulk of the piece tracing out long sequences of descending fifths.
61
Compare the two harmonizations of the descending tenths in Figure 3.4.11.
62
These intermediate forms typically thwart the turn to the relative major that otherwise
characterizes the minor-mode entries, the III+maj7 a second-level Baroque dominant (e.g., A+maj7–f♯ in
m. 29 and G+maj7–e in m. 73). Compare Rings (2011, p. 152) on the evanescent nature of fugal key
areas.
63
See for example Proctor 2004 or Schachter 2016.
64
Note that Figure 7.7.11 contains the same pattern we have been considering, (E, G♯)→ (D, B)
and (D, F♯)→ (C♯, A), with (D, B) and (D, F♯) superimposed.
Prelude
Chromatic or Diatonic?
combination T–7t4 shifts B to B♭ while keeping all other melodic slots fixed; this is the basic voice
leading.
Figure P8.3. Beethoven’s passage takes eighteen counterclockwise steps along the triadic spiral and
eight clockwise steps along the spiral of diatonic scales.
1
This is exactly the same reasoning that allows us to analyze “doe, a deer” as a single diatonic
pattern moving up by scale step (Figure P2.1).
2
These nested diagrams recall Figure 3.7.4 but represent different musical levels: where the earlier
figure depicted dyads moving inside scale-like triads, our new diagram represents chord-like triads
inside seven-note scales.
3
See Cohn 1997 for the analysis and appendix 3 for the neo-Riemannian transformations.
8
Modulation
This chapter uses the spiral diagrams to model scales and modulation. We
begin with some conceptual issues surrounding key changes, including the
notion of “modulatory distance” and the meaning of enharmonic spelling in
a fixed chromatic universe. We then construct a circular model of key
relations, using it to describe several common modulatory schemas. This
leads to a reconsideration of the seventeenth-century process of modal
homogenization, by which other modes take on the characteristics of ionian.
Finally, we survey some features of scalar thinking in the twentieth and
twenty-first centuries. In many ways, the conceptual work has already been
done; the task is simply to apply familiar techniques to new musical
problems.
What does it mean for two modes to be “close”? One answer focuses on
modulations that minimize the motion of scale degrees, reducing the overall
sum of distances as measured from tonic to tonic, supertonic to supertonic,
and so on. From this point of view C major and G major are not close, since
their scale degrees are almost half an octave apart; instead, the most
proximate keys are the parallel major and minor—as well as semitonally
related keys such as C and D ♭ major (Figure 8.1.1). This notion of key
distance is most relevant when instruments do the same thing before and
after the modulation: one singer, for example, singing the same melody in
both old key and new. (If you can sing a tune in C major, you can probably
sing it in D♭ major whereas G major may be out of your range.) It underlies
modulatory practices such as “sidestepping” or “pumping-up,” familiar in
jazz and popular music but sometimes found in classical music as well.1
Figure 8.1.1. Scale-degree preserving mappings from C major to C minor and D♭ major. The open
notehead is the tonic.
To get a feel for this second approach, it is useful to play the scales at the
piano with a fixed fingering; different scalar voice leadings will correspond
to different ways of shifting your fingers as you modulate from one key to
another, with each finger tracking an abstract voice.3 Suppose for example
you play a C major scale with the fingering shown at the top of Figure 8.1.7.
The simplest flatward modulation lowers the leading tone by semitone,
lowering the note played by the fourth finger of the right hand, and moving
the tonic from third left-hand finger to the right-hand thumb. A series of five
such modulations sends the tonic of the first key to the leading tone of the
last, but a series of seven sharpward modulations maps the tonic of C major
to the tonic of C♯ major. In both cases, the fingers embody the melodic slots,
taking on new scale-degree roles as you modulate. The size of the
modulation corresponds to the total distance moved by all fingers.
Figure 8.1.7. Two paths between the same points on the scale circle. (a) The 150° (five pie-slice)
clockwise path from C major to D♭ major. (b) The 210° (seven pie-slice) counterclockwise path from
C major to C♯ major. The first maps the tonic to the leading tone; the second tonic to tonic.
notes (D ♭ ♭ ). You will find that your left-hand third finger, which formerly
played the tonic, is now playing the leading tone (Figure 8.2.1). Contrast that
modulation with the series of key changes that ascends by fifth from C major
to E major and then returns to the white notes as above, by lowering the
second, third, sixth, and seventh scale degrees. Here each finger ends up
back on its initial note. We can say that the first sequence of modulations
forms a nontrivial voice leading from the C diatonic scale to itself, moving
each voice downward along the scale; the second produces a trivial voice
leading that leaves each voice exactly where it began.
Figure 8.2.1. Two modulatory sequences; solid arrows are descending major-third modulations that
lower the second, third, sixth, and seventh scale degrees; dashed arrows raise the fourth scale degree.
All of which is very clear on the spiral diagram in Figure 8.2.2. The first
sequence moves in a complete clockwise circle, taking one third of a turn
with each modulation, while the second takes four counterclockwise steps
before moving four steps clockwise; the different outcomes thus reflect the
geometrical fact that loops represent transpositions along the chord. Here,
however, the “chord” is what we would usually call a “scale,” a seven-note
diatonic collection embedded within the twelve-note chromatic aggregate.
Other than that, there is nothing unusual going on: if a series of modulations
makes a complete loop surrounding the center of scale space, then it will
change the scale-degree function of the abstract voices (here represented by
our seven fingers); if the modulations do not make such a circle, then the
abstract voices will return to their original scale-degree functions. The spiral
diagram allows us to see this as a specific manifestation of a more general
phenomenon that applies to both chords and scales. Enharmonic equivalence
is produced by loops in scale space, just as chordal inversion is produced by
loops in chord space. Indeed, enharmonicism just is chordal inversion at the
scalar level.4
Figure 8.2.2. The two modulatory sequences from Figure 8.2.1 graphed in 7-in-12 space.
What is confusing is that notation normally but not always tracks the
underlying scalar voice leading, with the seven letter names A–G
corresponding to the seven abstract voices or melodic slots. Thus when we
lower the white-note scale’s leading tone we add a flat to B, indicating that
melodic slot B has moved down by semitone. Each one-step clockwise
motion on the circle adds a flat or subtracts a sharp; counterclockwise moves
add a sharp or subtract a flat. If we rigorously follow this rule, then
nontrivial modulatory sequences will connect different spellings of the same
scale: the modulations on the left of Figure 8.2.1 produce C major, A♭ major,
F♭ major, and D♭♭ major. The spelling does not indicate the presence of two
distinct harmonic objects, for in a fixed chromatic universe, C major and D♭♭
major are one and the same—acoustically, conceptually, and in every other
sense. Rather, the spelling indicates the presence of a nontrivial path, a loop
in voice-leading space that changes the voices’ scale-degree roles: in C
major, the C slot (or “voice” or finger) has tonic function, while in D ♭ ♭
major, the D slot is tonic.5 The modulatory sequence on the right of Figure
8.2.1 adds four sharps and then subtracts them, sending C major to C major,
with the identity of the spelling reflecting the triviality of the associated
voice leading. This is conceptually the same phenomenon we encountered in
Figure 1.2.2, where a series of background voice leadings made a complete
loop in chord space, so that a single pitch acquired a new scale-degree label;
in both cases, surface voices need to move in order to remain in the same
place.
However, there are also merely notational enharmonic changes that do not
correspond to any scalar voice leading. In Gottschalk’s Le Mancenillier (Op.
11, Figure 8.2.3), the music shifts from G ♯ minor to the parallel major
(spelled as A♭) and then down by fifth to C♯ minor; this entire passage then
and D♯ major, both requiring double sharps. His flat-side respelling of these
keys is merely notational in the sense that it does not record any genuine
pitch-class voice leading: if we were listening to the piece, or watching a
pianist depress keys in slow motion, we would have no way of knowing that
the respelling had occurred. Similarly, if we were to model its scalar voice
leadings with our fingers there would be no difference between playing G♯
major and A ♭ major; the switch from sharps to flats has no observable
consequence. By contrast, genuine enharmonicism involves an observable
change in the melodic slots’ scale-degree roles.
Figure 8.2.3. Scalar voice leadings in Gottschalk’s Le Mancenillier, Op. 11. The flat-side respellings
are merely notational and do not represent scalar voice leading.
Figure 8.2.5. (left) A progression dividing the octave equally, which is awkward to spell; (right) a
two-leveled repeating contrapuntal pattern involving a three-note chord (stemmed) inside a seven-note
scale.
3. Minor keys
So far we have constructed two models of key distance, one emphasizing the
proximity of parallel major and minor, the other emphasizing the proximity
of relative major and minor. Since both relationships are important, it would
be nice to be able to model them simultaneously. This led the eighteenth-
century theorist F. G. Vial to propose the structure in Figure 8.3.1; one of its
diagonal axes corresponds to motion along the circle of fifths while the other
alternates between parallel and relative modulations. Vial’s graph is an
accurate model of functional practice—which is to say, it represents the most
common modulations as short-distance geometrical paths. But it obscures
the circular geometry of enharmonic equivalence: after all, the two paths in
Figure 8.3.2 look similar, but only one returns its scale degrees to their initial
position. The model gives us no insight into the unified logic governing both
chord and scale.
Figure 8.3.1. F. G. Vial’s model of major and minor keys. The NE/SW diagonal represents motion
between fifth-related keys; the NW/SE diagonal alternates between relative and parallel modulations.
Figure 8.3.2. The two progressions in Figure 8.2.1 look abstractly similar in Vial’s space but produce
very different effects: the solid path moves every abstract voice down one scale degree while the
dashed path returns each voice to its starting position. This difference is not apparent in Vial’s
geometry.
For this reason, I favor an alternative that more closely resembles the
spiral diagrams. Figure 8.3.3 derives this model in three stages. The first
superimposes the circular spaces for the diatonic, acoustic (i.e., melodic-
minor ascending), and harmonic minor scales, using dotted lines to connect
the collections belonging to G and D minor; as in Figure 3.3.9, I represent
each spiral with a single circle whose arcs apply the basic voice leading. The
top-right circle improves on the model by duplicating the diatonic collection
and rearranging the scales’ radial positions: now the circular arcs represent
shifts between a single key’s scale-forms rather than applications of a basic
voice leading. The lowest graph reflects this same information more
compactly, no longer distinguishing the minor scale-forms. In all three
models, spatial extension allows minor keys to provide shortcuts between
otherwise-distant keys. This nonlocality arises not from deep features of
musical geometry but rather the convention of treating distinct scales as
syntactically equivalent, with changes between minor scale-forms not
counting as modulations even though they move through scale space.
Figure 8.3.3. Three models of key space. Top left: the superimposed diagrams for the diatonic,
harmonic minor, and melodic minor collections, moving outward. Top right: a version that rearranges
each scale’s radial positions while separating major and natural minor. In both of these graphs,
boldface type indicates natural minor, regular type is harmonic minor, and italic is melodic minor.
Bottom: a more compact diagram that does not distinguish the three specific minor-scale forms.
These models reconcile circular voice-leading logic with the insight that
parallel and relative keys are both closely related. Because A minor is
spatially extended, it can be close to C major, G major, D major, and A
major. The first and last are the relative and parallel relationships. The
proximity to G major reflects the single-semitone voice leading that turns a
G major tonic into an A melodic-minor leading tone.8 And though
modulations from D major to A minor are uncommon in D major pieces,
they are quite common in G major: indeed, the modulatory pathway I–V–ii
(G–D–a) is one of the most important in functional tonality. Thus the model
is reasonably accurate analytically, even though it is a two-dimensional
simplification of a vastly more complicated seven-dimensional space.9
It is useful to augment this model with information about the global tonic.
Figure 8.3.4 borrows a strategy from chapter 2, using darker colors to
represent keys whose tonic triads are contained in the C major scale; these
form a stack of fifths ascending from subdominant to mediant, F–C–G–d–a–
e, all clustered in a single region of circular space.10 This in turn shows that
enharmonicism requires chromaticism: because modulation typically uses
efficient scalar voice leadings, there is no obvious sharpward path from iii
(the sharpest of the diatonic keys) to IV (the flattest). Hence a sequence of
diatonic modulations will tend to cancel out, adding and subtracting the
same small collection of sharps and flats, and not requiring enharmonic
respelling. This means that composers who understand key areas diatonically
—limiting themselves to the triads contained in a single diatonic scale—will
not travel in complete circles through key space. It is only once key areas are
conceived chromatically, allowing for sharpward motion from mediant to
subdominant, that enharmonicism becomes commonplace. This is what I
take to be the truth behind Lewin and Cohn’s claim that such modulatory
sequences force us to abandon “diatonically based models”: not that we need
to abandon the basic picture of chords inside scales, but rather that we need
to consider modulatory destinations in chromatic space, rather than limiting
ourselves to keys whose tonics inhabit a single diatonic collection.
Figure 8.3.4. Circular key space, with key areas darkened to reflect proximity to C major.
4. Modulatory schemas
Figure 8.4.1. A schematic outline of standard modulations in baroque and classical music.
In their details, the grammars of chord and key are mirror images: chords
tend to progress from tonic to secondary destinations to dominant, while
keys move from tonic to dominant to secondary destinations. Chordal norms
are more stable than the modulatory norms: from 1600 to the present we can
find pieces deploying TSDT harmony in recognizably similar ways, even
while patterns of modulation change: for example, classical phrases often
end by tonicizing the dominant, but this modulation is rare in jazz or rock-
and-roll. Modulatory norms are also form-dependent in a way that chord
grammar is not: sonatas are structured as journeys from tonic to foreign keys
and back, while fugues and rondos permit more frequent returns to the tonic.
Beyond these very general conventions are some more specific
modulatory schemas. These typically occur in sonata-form developments,
where the majority of classical-style modulations are found. One of the
simplest is what I call the helicopter drop, a sudden and dramatic turn to a
distant key, followed by a gradual return to the tonic. The first-movement
development of Beethoven’s Pathétique starts by jolting the listener from G
minor to E minor, whereupon it descends by fifths back to the tonic (Figure
8.4.2).12 The first-movement development of the second Razumovsky
quartet is similar, moving through a circle of major-third related minor triads
(g–e♭–b) to arrive at a B minor that feels more distant than it actually is. The
remaining seventy measures pursue a single ascending-step near sequence
(Figure 8.4.3). Particularly interesting are the reinterpretations of the A and
A ♭ chords, marked by asterisks on the example: in both cases, sequential
logic dictates that the chords should be dominants moving up by fourth;
instead, they act as tonics moving down by third (§4.9). These
transformations extend the long sequential climb so that it twice ascends
from A to C before finally breaking free to rise to the tonic. (Note also the
two enharmonic respellings, signifying two loops in scale space and a
recapitulation in G♭♭♭ minor.) The embedding of diverse thematic material
within a single modulatory sequence is characteristic of Beethoven’s
practice.
Figure 8.4.2. The development of Beethoven’s Pathétique sonata, Op. 13, I. Enharmonic
reinterpretation of the diminished seventh takes us to a distant E minor, from which the music
descends by fifths back to a converging cadence and the tonic.
Figure 8.4.4. A paired-sequence design in the development of Haydn’s Symphony no. 62, I.
The ascending fifths ratchet up the tension, their sonic signature a sequence
of Sturm-und-Drang i–V7/V progressions; this leads to an energetic peak
that is discharged by more conventional harmonic movement. In larger
pieces the schema sometimes occupies only one part of the development.
The appearance of V/vi as a point of farthest remove has sometimes been
identified as a characteristic of the early eighteenth century; however, the
up-and-down-the-ladder schema appears throughout classical music,
particularly in the relatively simple developments of Mozart’s piano
sonatas.15 Figures 8.5.1–3 provide some shorter examples from the sonatas
of Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven. The development of the first movement
of K.332 is notable in that Mozart alters an expositional sequence to
conform to the ascending-fifth schema—an unusual rewriting that testifies to
the idiom’s gravitational pull (Figure 8.5.4). Figure 8.5.5 lists a few other
notable examples; we will encounter several more in chapter 10.
Figure 8.5.1. The development of the first movement of Haydn’s Divertimento in C major,
Hob.XVI:1.
Figure 8.5.2. The development of the first movement of Mozart’s K.332, I.
Figure 8.5.3. The development of the first movement of Beethoven’s Op. 49, no. 2.
Figure 8.5.4. In the development of K.332, I, Mozart changes a descending-fifth sequence into an
ascending-fifth sequence.
Figure 8.5.5. Some other developments that use the up-and-down-the-ladder schema.
This theory of modulation can also help elucidate the phenomenon of modal
homogenization, by which the various Renaissance modes came to take on
the characteristics of ionian. This process maps ionian-mode procedures into
a new domain, implying something like a voice leading between scalar
collections—a “modulation,” as it were, taking place over the span of
decades rather than seconds. Our two notions of key distance correspond to
the two ways in which this can happen: in a scale-degree-preserving
transformation, the modal tonic stays fixed while other degrees change
position—as when the seventh degree is raised from a modal subtonic to a
functional leading tone (Figure 8.6.1). In a scale-degree-changing
transformation, we have a fixed vocabulary of pitches that change meanings,
centricity shifting from one note to another. This necessarily involves
ambiguity during the transition, the multiplicity of interpretive possibilities
reflecting a gradual change of scale-degree roles.16
Figure 8.6.2. Satie’s third Gnossienne can be heard in either A minor or E minor.
Figure 8.6.5. The last phrases of the first two verses of Scheidt’s 1624 “Gelobet siest du, Jesu Christ.”
The first-verse phrase is easily heard in C, the second is more strongly in G.
As a general rule, mixolydian tends to move toward ionian by way of
scale-degree-preserving mappings. Marenzio’s 1586 three-voice villanelle
“Vorria parlare e dire” is representative: F ♯ outnumbers F ♮ almost 5-to-1,
with the mixolydian seventh appearing only in mm. 4–5 (Figure 8.6.6). The
harmonic progressions can mostly be parsed into functionally plausible
cycles, though there are a few nonfunctional moments (e.g., IV–vi–V in mm.
2–3 and I–ii–I6 in mm. 13–14, the shift from D to A in m. 11). The most
striking is the V– ♭ VII–V in mm. 4–6, with its staggered parallelism
(reminiscent, perhaps, of pop-music power chords).25 Here we have a
mixolydian that could be described as substantially ionian, but with a strong
subtonic emphasis.
At the same time, there are some remarkably early examples of minor-
mode functionality. Morley’s “Leave, alas, this tormenting” is composed out
of short harmonic cycles which are unusual mainly in that they modulate
somewhat faster than we might expect (Figure 8.6.14). We begin with very
clear G minor, but immediately move to a stereotypical cadence in C minor,
only to be wrenched immediately into D minor before settling back into G
minor for eighteen measures. The III chord on the second system is a slight
but noteworthy inclination toward major. There are a few other moments
where Morley uses major triads in not-completely-functional ways (VI6–III6
on the third system, perhaps VII instead of v6 shortly thereafter, VII6 on the
bottom system), but these are exceptional; overall the music is strongly
functional, particularly when compared to pieces like Schütz’s St. Matthew
Passion.
Figure 8.6.14. The harmonies in Morley’s 1600 “Leave, Alas, This Tormenting.”
Figure 8.7.4. This table can be read in two ways. First, each column shows the notes of the column-
label scale as they appear on the circle of fifths: the diatonic scale is a compact stack of seven fifths,
the acoustic scale has a compact core of five fifths (a pentatonic scale) with two outliers, and so on.
Second, the table records the “brightness” of those scales’ modes, expressed as the sum of its intervals
above the tonic pitch; here each cell labels the tonic of a mode of the column-label scale, whose
“height” is given by the row label. See Figure 8.7.5.
Figure 8.7.5. Mode height as the sum of the intervals above the tonic.
Figure 8.7.6. The principles of chord-scale compatibility associate chords with scales containing
them; here, four common scales that accompany G–B–F, functioning as G7.
Figure 8.7.7. A triply nested sequence from Wheels within Wheels in which a seven-note scale
(bottom) iterates its ascending basic voice leading inside the eleven-note scale lacking F♯ (T8t–5).
Inside this scale, a six-note scale (middle) moves according to its ascending basic voice leading (T6t–
5); inside this, a four-note scale (top) moves according to the voice leading T3t–2. Chords with open
noteheads are generalizations of the melodic minor scale as discussed in the text.
Beyond that, the spiral diagrams reveal analogies between different chord-
and-scale domains. While the full picture is too complex to explain here, it is
worth surveying briefly. Suppose we have a spiral diagram with just one
chord at each angular position—that is to say, an n-note chord in an o-note
scale, with n not dividing o and the two numbers not sharing any common
factor: 3 in 7, 7 in 10, 5 in 12, and so on. In that case, the most-even n-note
chord will be a near interval-cycle, a circular ordering of notes all but one of
which are the same size, with the unusual interval one scale step larger or
smaller than the others. For example, the diatonic seventh chord E–G–B–D
is a near interval cycle, a stack of three two-step intervals (E–G, G–B, B–D)
with the fourth interval being just one step smaller (D–E, a step rather than a
third). Similarly, the diatonic scale is a stack of seven-semitone perfect fifths
(F–C, C–G, G–D, etc.) with the final interval (B–F) again just a chromatic
step too small. In this sense the seventh chord E–G–B–D is the structural
analogue of the C diatonic collection. Because both are near interval cycles,
we can form a generalized “circle of fifths” by shifting the position of the
unusual interval: with the diatonic seventh chord, we move D down by step
to C, connecting E–G–B–D to E–G–B–C, with the C major collection we
move B down by semitone to B♭, connecting C diatonic and F diatonic; this
is the descending basic voice leading for each collection. Thus we have both
harmonic and contrapuntal analogies between E–G–B–D and C diatonic:
harmonically, both are near interval cycles, contrapuntally both are linked by
a generalized circle of fifths.
To extend this analogy to other collections, we exploit the fact that we can
generate nearly even chords by scrambling the voice leadings on the
generalized circle of fifths.30 For example, the melodic minor scale is the
second-most even seven-note chord in twelve-tone equal temperament; it
can be generated by applying the voice leading E→E ♭ , which links the F
major scale to the B♭ major scale, one step too early on the circle of fifths:
that is, to C major rather than F major (Figure 8.7.8). The stack of fourths
D–E–G–A is the second-most even diatonic tetrachord; it can be generated
by applying the voice leading B→A one step too early on the circle of
diatonic thirds, to the E diatonic seventh chord rather than to the C major
seventh chord. In exactly the same way, we can obtain the third-most-even
seven-note scale, the harmonic major scale, by applying the voice leading
A→A ♭ two steps too early (i.e., once again to C major, rather than to B ♭
major).31 This procedure gives us a generalized recipe for producing chordal
analogues in different chord-and-scale domains. Once again, these
“analogues” will be similar both in their internal structure and their
contrapuntal relationships. The second-most even chord, for example, will
be “gapped” when expressed in terms of the interval that characterizes the
maximally even chord (Figure 8.7.9). The third-most even sonority will be
an inversionally related pair forming a doubly gapped stack of the
characteristic interval. And all of these objects will be linked by an
analogous system of single-step voice leadings. Thus one and the same
description can be applied to familiar seven-note scales, three- and four-note
diatonic collections, and countless other collections as well—a remarkable
system of analogies linking different chord-in-scale domains (Figures
8.7.10–12).
Figure 8.7.8. Scrambling the generalized circle of fifths to obtain analogous chords in different
musical domains.
Figure 8.7.9. When a spiral diagram has just one chord at every angular position, the four most-even
chords will be linked by the same single-step voice-leading relationships and will have the same
structure. Chord 1 is the most-even chord with a, b, c, connected by the basic voice leading (“the
generalized circle of fifths”). Chord 2 is the second most even chord, with its basic voice leading
linking 2a to 2b, and so on. Chord 3 comes in two inversionally related variants, 3a and 3A. This is the
abstract pattern shared by Figures 8.7.3–4 and 8.7.10–12.
Figure 8.7.10. The most-even triads in the diatonic scale.
Figure 8.7.11. The most-even four-note chords in the diatonic scale.
Figure 8.7.12. The most-even six-note chords in the eleven-note scale containing all the standard
chromatic pitch classes except B. For legibility, the circles have been straightened into lines.
1
Lewin 1984 and McCreless 1996; two familiar examples are the end of the scherzo of
Beethoven’s Hammerklavier Op. 106 and Miles Davis’s “So What.”
2
These pairings are the primary theoretical objects studied in Brinkman 1986, Agmon 1989, and
Rings 2011. These earlier theorists do not associate them with abstract musical voices as I do here; for
Agmon they are formal mathematical objects, and for Rings they are subjective perceptions.
3
Each finger plays a note that has a scale-degree role, corresponding to the three basic elements of
the model.
4
Yust 2018 pursues similar considerations using different mathematics; Tymoczko and Yust 2019
detail the close connections between voice-leading spaces and Yust’s Fourier models.
5
In isolation we cannot distinguish between C major and D ♭ ♭ major. But we can distinguish a
modulation taking C major to C major from one taking C major to D ♭ ♭ major. We could of course
rewrite these as modulating from D ♭ ♭ major to D ♭ ♭ major and from D ♭ ♭ major to E ♭ ♭ ♭ ♭ major,
respectively.
6
The words here come from Cohn 1996 and echo the sentiment in Lewin 1984, a very challenging
and obscure paper; Cohn 2012, pp. 2–3, reiterates the view. Waltham-Smith 2018 also seems to
endorse a similar perspective. Klumpenhouwer 2011 argues that we can avoid any sense of paradox if
we simply label keys chromatically.
7
As a keyboardist, J. S. Bach daily confronted an instrument in which the diatonic scale is a subset
of the twelve chromatic notes, and indeed celebrated this embedding in his most famous composition.
Were E ♭ minor and D ♯ minor different places then there would be little sense in the E ♭ minor/D ♯
minor prelude-and-fugue pair in Book I of The Well-Tempered Clavier.
8
Nowadays we tend to think that Kellner’s model puts the minor keys in the correct position, but
Heinichen’s model, which highlights the proximity of G major and A minor, has some benefits.
9
These models are consistent with, but more intuitive than, the three-dimensional lattice used in
§7.4 of Tymoczko 2011a. Some readers may prefer even simpler concentric models like Figure 8.1.5,
which is fine so long as one remembers that minor keys are “smeared.”
10
Compare Agmon 2013, building on observations made by the eighteenth-century musician
Joseph Riepel.
11
Note the IV–V7–I harmonization of the ascending basic voice leading for diatonic thirds (§3.1).
12
Other examples include the first-movement developments of Beethoven’s Op. 7, no. 1 (deceptive
cadence to ♭VI followed by a descending-third sequence to ♭II, skipping ahead one sequential step to
V), Op. 31, no. 2 (§10.3), and Op. 31, no. 3 (shift to VI followed by a descending fifth movement to
IV, which immediately ascends to the ii6 that starts the recapitulation.
13
Examples include The Well-Tempered Clavier’s first C ♯ and D major preludes, its second C
major fugue, Bach’s F major two-part invention, and the opening of the G major cello suite. Romantic
examples include Chopin’s D major prelude, discussed earlier, and the second song of Schumann’s
Dichterliebe.
14
The nineteenth-century theorist Ernst Friedrich Richter cites Mozart’s K.533, I, an “up-and-
down-the-ladder,” as a paradigmatic development; his analysis closely tracks my description of the
schema (Richter 1852, pp. 32–34). Richter’s discussion is cited in Beach 1983, which provides a
Schenkerian discussion of developments that emphasize or close on V/vi. This option creates
something like the opposite of the helicopter drop: a gradual journey away from the tonic followed by
a quick and surprising return.
15
See Rosen 1988. In the online supplement to this book, I summarize the developments of all of
Mozart’s piano sonatas.
16
This ambiguity is facilitated by the fact that centricity in Renaissance polyphony is often fairly
subtle (Powers 1958).
17
On Shady Grove, Jerry Garcia and David Grisman perform the song “Wind and Rain” in a way
that I hear as strongly mixolydian; Crooked Still, on Crooked Still Live, plays a very similar version—
almost a transposition of Garcia and Grisman’s—that I nevertheless hear in ionian. Brittany Haas, who
played on Crooked Still’s recording, told me she initially heard the piece in mixolydian even while the
other musicians heard it in ionian.
18
These affiliations may reflect the presence of structurally similar pentachords, with the C
diatonic scale containing an 02457 pentachord on C and G, and an 02357 pentachord on D and A
(Judd 1992). These pairs are broadly consistent with those noted by Meier 1988 and Ceulemans 2017.
19
In determining the mode of a mass movement, I looked at key signature, final chord, and most
frequent chord. Thus my “mixolydian” pieces had no key signature with G major as the final and most
frequent chord. The frequency constraint was meant to eliminate ionian pieces that happened to end on
G. I also checked mode assignments by hand for plausibility.
20
The subdominant emphasis in mixolydian is noted by Kerman (1981, pp. 69–70) and Jeppesen
([1931] 1939, pp. 81–82); the subdominant emphasis in phrygian is noted by Zarlino ([1558] 1983,
chapters 20 and 30). Jeppesen (1931) 1939, pp. 59–82, is largely consistent with my own account.
Meier (1988, p. 135) notes Palestrina’s tendency to cadence on C even in mode 7 (mixolydian rather
than hypomixolydian).
21
See Christensen 1993b, who also emphasizes Zarlino’s classification in terms of the quality of
the third above the root.
22
Zarlino (1558) 1983, chapters 20 and 30.
23
Dorian is two accidentals away (F→F ♯ , C→C ♯ ), while aeolian is three. Phrygian is four
semitones from ionian if we fix the tonic (i.e., raise 2̂, 3̂, 6̂, and 7̂), but only three if we allow the tonic
to move (i.e., lower 1̂, 4̂, and 5̂).
24
Bach’s settings of the melody are similarly ambivalent: in BWV 91.6 (Riemenschneider 51) the
opening melodic note is a tonic; whereas in BWV248.28, it is a dominant (though I hear it gradually
acquiring tonicity as the chorale progresses). See Burns 1993 and 1995 for more.
25
Einstein 1948, chapter 8, contains numerous examples of early canzone villanesca with
extensive passages in parallel root-position triads; Einstein notes that these fifths “almost completely”
disappear in Marenzio (p. 586), though one remains in Figure 8.6.6.
26
The need for a term (like “phrygian mode”) to describe the similarities between Hildegard and
Stravinsky stands as a point against Powers’s suggestion that we abandon the term “mode” as a
musical descriptor, replacing it instead with “tonal type.” Hildegard and Stravinsky do not use the
same “tonal type,” but both their pieces can reasonably be said to be in phrygian.
27
Schein’s setting is likely the model for Bach’s.
28
The bass/alto canon begins in mm. 11–12 and continues across the sequential boundary.
Overlapping ascending fifths and the “descending 6–5” also occur in the Kyrie of Byrd’s Mass in
Three Voices (§3.5).
29
In other words, the piece exploits the fact that the harmonic minor scale contains collections
related by chromatic but not scalar transposition, a property often referred to as “ambiguity”
(Rothenberg 1978 and Balzano 1980).
30
Tymoczko 2004 and 2011a. Junod et al. (2009) note some of these relationships in the 7-in-12
context.
31
To obtain the inversion of the harmonic minor scale, we do the same in the sharpward direction,
applying the displacement G→G♯ not to D major but to C major.
Prelude
Hearing and Hearing-As
Figure P9.2. The music as the product of a 3 + 2 echo. Accented notes are played on the synthesizer;
the rest are produced by the echo.
I cannot offer any general theory of musical perception, nor any reliable
way to separate “hearing as” from “hearing plus thinking.” But a lifetime of
experience has convinced me of two facts. First, that it can be genuinely
difficult to know whether we are thinking something or experiencing it
directly—and hence that we can be drastically mistaken about what we hear.
(Indeed, I suspect that this confusion explains why so many ambitious
composers wasted so much energy manipulating inaudible pitch-
relationships.) And second, that theory can sometimes enrich our experience,
leading us to understand music in a way that genuinely increases our
enjoyment. The challenge for theory is to stretch our ears while remaining
realistic about human limitation. Done badly, it is like playing tennis without
a net, elevating blatantly implausible claims to the status of institutional
common sense. Done well, it is a delicate and intimate form of science that
increases the world’s supply of aesthetic value. No hard-and-fast rules
determine the boundary between these two activities, only taste and good
judgment.
1
Compare Brewer 2015 (“N-rays”), National Research Council 2014 (eyewitnesses), Thomas 1971
(the supernatural), Jost et al. 2009 (implicit bias), Goldstein et al. 2008 (wine).
2
Of course, most absolute-pitch listeners are directly sensitive to interval quality. I have, however,
met some who reported the sort of insensitivity discussed here.
3
The distinction originates with Wittgenstein’s (1953) distinction between “seeing as” and “seeing
plus thinking” which in turn was inspired by the earlier movement of Gestalt psychology. See
DeBellis 1999 for related discussion.
4
Rosen 1988, p. 197.
5
Repeated listening is not the answer, as it gives the listener cognitive access to information they
may not be able to perceive directly. Suppose a non-absolute-pitch listener learns a piece well enough
that they know what key they are in at any one time. This is compatible with their not having an
immediate and palpable sense of long-term tonal closure: they may know that a piece is returning to
the tonic, while having absolutely no sense of “dissonance being resolved.” In the same way, they
could memorize a twelve-tone piece accurately enough to identify, by ear, any divergences from the
score. But this tells us nothing about whether listeners are sensitive to twelve-tone structure in the
aesthetically relevant sense. Repeated experience allows us to think the thought “now I am back in the
home key” (or “now I am hearing the retrograde inversion of the row, starting on B ♭ ”), but this is
different from being sensitive to the relevant musical properties.
6
Cook 1987, Marvin and Brinkman 1999. It is entirely possible to notice that the music has
returned to the tonic key without any associated sense of release.
7
Davies 1983. Hansberry 2017 argues, correctly in my view, that scale-degree identity is to some
extent under our conscious control; indeed, I recall hearing about a new-music soprano who assigned
scale degrees to twelve-tone pieces, hearing them in relation to imaginary tonics.
8
Crane and Piantanida 1983.
9
Heterogeneous Hierarchy
Rameau once wrote that it is almost impossible to give definite rules for
melody, as good taste plays a central role in its development.2 Schenker took
perhaps the single largest step toward unraveling this mystery when he
proposed that music typically involves a slower melody taking place beneath
the musical surface, with phrases often embellishing stepwise descents from
one tonic-triad note to another, and with bass lines moving from tonic to
dominant before leaping back to the tonic. This template is so ubiquitous
that examples are almost superfluous, but Figure 9.1.1 provides one anyway
—the opening phrase of Mozart’s G minor symphony K.550, embedding a
linear descent from D down to F♯, while reserving the strong root-position
dominant for the end of the phrase.
Figure 9.1.1. The opening of Mozart’s Symphony no. 40, K.550 features a linear descent from 5̂ to 7̂.
I am firmly convinced that this idea can help us understand a wide range
of music, and I regularly encourage young composers to listen for this
“slower melody” in styles both old and new. What is more challenging is
understanding its significance. Attention to melodic structure has sometimes
been set in opposition to the recognition of local harmonic patterns, as if the
two were somehow incompatible. To my mind, however, there is no conflict
at all: the art of functional composition consists in writing satisfying phrases
while also satisfying the constraints of the local harmonic grammar. This
sort of double organization is familiar from the world of sports and games.
In basketball, players execute higher-order aims (driving to the basket,
advancing the ball to the other end of the court, blocking an opponent’s
lanes) within the constraints given by the rules (no more than two steps per
dribble, no contact with opposing players, etc.). Similarly, chess players
pursue strategic goals (developing pieces, controlling the center) within local
constraints governing pieces’ motions. If the harmonic grammar provides the
rules of the game, then phrase-level melodic templates are the higher-level
goals.
What makes a melody directed? Schenker’s answer, which I endorse, is
that directed melodies often articulate coherent voice-leading structures.
Simple melodies, such as “Helpless,” have just one structural voice,
descending stepwise from one tonic-triad note to another. More complex
melodies, such as “Eight Days a Week,” dance between multiple voices of
an abstract background that itself moves in a coherent way—somewhat like
an Alberti bass, but less predictably. Still more complex is the technique of
moving contrary to a background voice leading, as when Beethoven ascends
along the predominantly descending Waldstein progression (§3.7). This
produces a dual melodic logic superimposing multiple directed narratives: in
the Waldstein, the two hands take separate paths from tonic to dominant,
bass descending by step while the melody ascends by thirds.
These facts imply that directed melodies will often articulate the doubly
parallel motion represented by our spiral diagrams—that is, the combination
of big-T transposition along the scale, and little-t transposition along the
chord. The nonharmonic system ensures that out-of-chord notes resolve by
step to in-chord notes; and registral limitations ensure that these within-
chord notes tend to outline close-position triads and seventh chords. The
resulting “background” provides melodic destinations, points of rest between
which the surface moves. (Again, I think of this as a kind of dancing, surface
voices leaping among background voices in an aesthetically pleasing way.)
Figure 9.1.2 analyzes the opening of Mozart’s Fortieth Symphony, where
hierarchical transposition is intrinsic to melodic directedness, the glue
binding harmony and melody together.
Figure 9.1.2. Dual transpositions in the opening of Mozart’s Symphony no. 40.
One way of putting this is that individual harmonic cycles have different
degrees of cadential weight, with phrases moving from less-conclusive to
more-conclusive over the course of a phrase. Figure 9.2.2 quantifies this
notion in a rough-and-ready way, considering whether a harmonic cycle
contains a predominant, ends with a root-position tonic-dominant
progression, and so on.7 Cadential weight, thus defined, generally increases
throughout a phrase, with the final cycle longer and stronger than its
predecessors. We can use this approach to construct a simple-but-effective
algorithm for harmonizing chorale-style melodies. Suppose we have a series
of melody notes n1, n2, and so on, to be harmonized by a series of chords C1,
C2, . . . Given a chord Ci harmonizing melodic note ni, we can choose a new
chord Ci + 1 by multiplying two probabilities, the (first order, phrase-
independent) probability that a particular chord follows chord Ci according
to a harmonic grammar and the (zeroth order) likelihood that that chord is
found at that point in the phrase. Figure 9.2.3 shows that this fairly simple
algorithm can produce a reasonably idiomatic output, comparable to the
efforts of a good second-semester undergraduate.8
Figure 9.2.2. (top) A simple model of cycle strength, assigning “strength points” to various features of
the cycle. (bottom) Modeled in this way, cycles increase in strength throughout the phrase in Bach's
chorales.
Figure 9.2.3. A computer-generated harmonization of the opening two phrases of the melody of
“Christus, der ist mein Leben” (BWV 281, Riemenschneider 6).
All of which starts to narrow the gap between traditional harmonic theory
and the Schenkerian alternative, at least as concerns the structure of
individual phrases. For both parties can agree on the following:
P1. Phrases tend to begin and end with root-position tonic chords;
P2. Melodies tend to decorate stepwise descents from one note of the tonic
chord to another, perhaps after an initial ascent;
P3. Melodies often jump between the degrees of a series of abstract
scalelike chords that articulate close-position harmonies connected by
familiar voice leadings (§9.1);
P4. Bass lines tend to reserve the root-position dominant for the final
cadence.
These principles imply that traditional harmonic theorists will often find the
phrase-level structures emphasized by Schenkerian analysis. Similarly, they
will often be able to reinterpret orthodox Schenkerian graphs as highlighting
these features of functional phrases. From a practical point of view, the two
approaches are speaking a very similar language.10
However, they differ over subtle questions about the meaning and scope
of these features. Where traditional theory asserts that harmonic cycles are
concatenated sequentially, placed one after another like beads on a string,
Schenkerians argue that early-phrase progressions are nested inside a single
“structural” progression that lasts for the duration of the phrase (Figure
9.2.5). This view is described with admirable clarity in a pair of recent
textbooks, Steven Laitz’s The Complete Musician and Jane Clendinning and
Elizabeth West Marvin’s The Musician’s Guide to Theory and Analysis.11
Both use the term “the phrase model” to describe the recursive conception:
what makes the final dominant cadential, on this view, is not its rhythmic
role within the conventions of the eight-bar phrase, or its use of root-position
dominant chords and 2̂–1̂ melody, but rather the fact that it “connects”
syntactically to the initial tonic, with the earlier T–S–D–T cycles
hierarchically embedded within a higher-level “structural” progression.12
Harmonic analysis thus requires us to determine which chords belong to the
structural progression and which do not.
Figure 9.2.5. Steven Laitz’s interpretation of the opening of Haydn’s G minor sonata, Hob. XVI:44, II
(from The Complete Musician). Where a traditional theorist would see four successive harmonic
cycles, Laitz postulates that three of the cycles are embedded within the fourth. Above the staff, I note
the use of a schema from Figure 9.1.3 to relate tonic and dominant.
Figure 9.2.6. Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s five-stage reductional analysis of the opening of Mozart’s
K.331, I. Above the staves I add my own description of the reductional level. The first three are
compatible with traditional theory.
Figure 9.3.2. Chopin’s structured arpeggiation embellishes three underlying voices, each of which
appears in two octaves. This is represented by the networks, which start in the center, move left, and
then move right.
Figure 9.3.3. Three increasingly detailed outlines of Chopin’s A minor prelude.
Figure 9.3.4. The A minor prelude takes short clockwise moves in two different geometrical spaces.
The first A minor mazurka (Op. 7, no. 2) is similar, interpolating VI6 into
iv64–i to create a strikingly modal opening, but proceeding onward to a
functional phrase with a clear 5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂–1̂ linear descent.20 Once again these
descending diatonic triads lead to nonfunctional sevenths, descending voice
leading now chromatic and futuristic rather than modal and nostalgic (Figure
9.3.5). The second A minor mazurka is similar but with the diatonic triads
giving way to chromatic trichords; the tetrachords are reserved until the
piece’s coda (Figures 9.3.6–7). An interesting wrinkle here is the scrambling
of the stepwise descents: rather than repeating the diatonic triad’s basic voice
leading, (A, C, F) → (A, C, E) → (G, C, E) → (G, B, E), Chopin gives us
(A, C, F) → (G, C, F) → (G, B, F) → (G, B, E), moving along the dotted
lines in Figure 8.7.10. This is the technique of Chopin’s E minor prelude,
now expressed with diatonic triads rather than chromatic sevenths.21 Here
we see something like an explicit awareness of the Prime Directive.
Figure 9.3.5. The A minor Mazurka, Op. 7, no. 2, opens with a series of short descending moves on
the triadic spiral, creating a iv–VI–i progression; the eight bars feature two loosely parallel iv–i–V–i
progressions in A and E minor. The contrasting middle section uses descending semitonal voice
leading between seventh chords.
Figure 9.3.6. The opening of the A minor Mazurka, Op. 17, no. 4. The move from A–C–F to G–B–E,
instead of taking place in the order Alto–Bass–Tenor, occurs in the order Bass–Tenor–Alto, producing
a series of nontriadic harmonies.
Figure 9.3.7. Chromatic seventh-chord descent in the coda of Op. 17, no. 4.
One final example allows for a more direct comparison with Schenker.
The first contrasting phrase of the Revolutionary Etude again features
descending stepwise voice leading embodied by triads and sevenths.
Schenker and I both identify the same initial voice leading, a chromatic 6–5
sequence using minor triads (Figure 9.3.8). But my analysis continues the
stepwise descent through the following dominant sevenths, while Schenker
instead follows the surface, inventing a 5–6 sequence to rationalize its voice
leading. His graph includes a provocative footnote:
all conventional concepts, such as functional harmony, modulation, etc., fail in regard to
measures 21–41. It would be wrong, for instance, to speak of a modulation to B-flat major, a-
flat minor, etc. The voice leading of the middleground alone offers the correct solution to the
problem.22
Figure 9.4.1. The theme of the variations movement of Beethoven’s Op. 109.
Figure 9.4.2. (left) My analysis of the opening of Beethoven’s theme. (right) A graphical
representation, with the vertical axis in scale steps and the horizontal axis in quarter notes.
Figure 9.4.3. The theme as a superimposition of two thirds cycles. Vertical dyads are connected by
either a basic voice leading (bvl+ or bvl–) or the two-step voice exchange (curved arrow).
Three pieces of evidence support this analysis. First, Beethoven makes the
contrary-motion design explicit in the piece’s third variation, where the
music articulates exactly the reduction I have proposed—the composer
helpfully analyzing his own music (Figure 9.4.4). This unveiling of covert
sequential structure is reminiscent of the Op. 1, no. 3 variations, which also
use the diatonic third’s basic voice leading (§4.10). Second, this kind of
thematic design, based on contrary motion and diatonic thirds, is
characteristic of Beethoven: we have already encountered examples in §3.2,
and will encounter many more in the next chapter. And third, the exploration
of dyadic logic continues throughout the theme: in mm. 9–10, we have a
curious descending-step sequence combining the two forms of the diatonic
third’s basic voice leading (Figure 9.4.5, compare pattern D2a on Figure
7.7.3). So the basic voice leading, which initially alternates with voice
exchanges, now appears more explicitly and without the voice exchanges.
Figure 9.4.4. The third variation of Op. 109 (bottom staves of each line) exposes the contrary-motion
structure of the theme (top two staves).
Figure 9.4.5. My analysis of the second half of Beethoven’s theme. Inward-pointing stems show the
descending form of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading; outward-pointing stems represent the
ascending form.
The minuet from the Op. 22 piano sonata is broadly analogous to the Op.
109 theme, again emphasizing contrary motion and the diatonic third’s basic
voice leading (Figure 9.4.7). Here, however, it opens with an ascending-fifth
contrary motion pattern E♭–B♭–F, again decorated with voice exchanges but
now with each dyad preceded by its diatonic lower neighbor. Note that on
my analysis the initial melodic D belongs to the lower voice and continues in
the left hand. The underlying schema appears in many of Beethoven’s
compositions, including the preceding opus number (Figure 9.4.8); it is
easily missed if one is not expecting it.28
Figure 9.4.7. The Op. 22 minuet opens by decorating the ascending form of the diatonic third’s basic
voice leading.
Figure 9.4.8. The opening of the minuet of Beethoven’s First Symphony, Op. 21.
Figure 9.5.1. While the standard cadential 64 chord decorates the dominant, an alternative decorates
the tonic: m. 30 of the first Agnus of Palestrina’s mass Quando lieta sperai; Chopin’s Mazurka, Op.
17, no. 3, mm. 39–40; the opening of the slow movement of Mozart’s A minor piano sonata, K.310,
where the G–B♭ suspensions embellish a deceptive resolution.
Secondary dominants often require a similar approach. Consider the two
analyses in Figure 9.5.2. The first is not so much wrong as incomplete, for
while it correctly notes the presence of harmonic cycles in G major, D major,
and E minor, it implausibly postulates four separate key changes, as if the
mere presence of a dominant were sufficient to immediately cancel the
prevailing key. The better analysis depicts a multileveled structure—
identifying E minor both as the tonic of its own IV65–V65–i cycle and also
as vi in a higher-level G major cycle. This description recognizes the
psychological reality that very brief excursions to foreign keys do not fully
dislodge our memory of the home tonality: when we return to the tonic after
a short digression to E minor, we are capable of experiencing the entire
progression in a persisting G major. (It is not known whether listeners
experience the E minor as having any tonic quality, or whether we simply
accept V7/vi–vi as an idiom, perhaps a substitute for iii–vi.) We might say
that centricity is “sticky,” changing more slowly than harmonies themselves.
Musicians exploit this stickiness by briefly digressing from the home key, a
technique that is as old as secondary dominants and as recent as “playing
outside” (§5.6).37
Figure 9.5.2. Two analyses of the opening of Bach’s “Herr Jesu Christ, dich zu uns wend” (BWV 332,
Riemenschneider 136).
Figure 9.5.3. The opening of the last movement of Haydn’s final piano sonata, Hob.XVI:52.
Such examples clearly show that Western music can exhibit a limited
degree of hierarchical embedding, with subsidiary progressions decorating
larger patterns that can be felt to follow familiar harmonic trajectories. This
sort of recursive organization has generated significant interest among both
musicians and cognitive scientists, as it evokes the structure of natural
language: the hierarchy in Figure 9.5.2 seems analogous to that in a phrase
like “the kitten we got at the shelter has grown into an affectionate cat,”
where the sentence “we got the kitten at the shelter” is embedded inside “the
kitten has grown into an affectionate cat.” This suggests that music may
exploit the same cognitive capacities as natural language, with musical
“sentences” like I–V–I capable of being embedded inside one another to
form progressions like (I–V–I)–(vi–V/vi–vi)– . . . 40
This analogy has led some theorists to propose that secondary dominants
generally decorate the chords to which they apply.41 If so, it is wrong to hear
a direct relationship between a secondary dominant and the preceding chord:
instead, we should hear X–V7/Y–Y as a version of X–Y, with the second
harmony embellished by a V7/Y chord at a lower level, symbolically X–
(V7/Y⇒Y). (This is analogous to the way we connect “the kitten” but not
“the shelter” to the verb phrase “has grown”; it is the kitten rather than the
shelter that has grown.) Thus in the penultimate measure of Figure 9.5.2, the
meaningful progression is from G major to E minor, and not from G major to
A65. The G–A65 is like the misleading adjacency “the shelter has grown” in
the English sentence.
Once again, however, the complexity of musical practice thwarts simple
generalizations. For if secondary dominants invariably worked as just
described, then we would not find progressions of the form X–(V7/Y⇒Y)
unless we also found X–Y. Yet virtually any nonsyntactical progression X–Y
can be normalized by the insertion of the appropriate secondary dominant,
including V–IV, V–ii, I64–vi, and so on. Figure 9.5.4 provides a pair of
examples from the Bach chorales in which secondary dominants cancel the
implications of the preceding chord, allowing the music to rescind its
outstanding harmonic obligations.42 The paradox is that secondary
dominants are simultaneously a clear example of harmonic recursion and
also evidence for its limited scope: it is not much of an exaggeration to say
that any secondary dominant could signal either a momentary digression to a
foreign key, hierarchically embedded within a persisting tonic, or a shift to a
new key that simply abandons any existing implications. This suggests
harmonies have only weak implications for chords beyond their immediate
successors.
Figure 9.5.4. Secondary dominants licensing unusual harmonic progressions: “Wie schön leuchtet der
Morgenstern” (BWV 36.4, Riemenschneider 86) has a V–ii if the applied dominant is removed;
“Fröhlich soll mein Herze springen” (BWV 248.33, Riemenschneider 139) has a I64–vi without the
applied dominant.
Figure 9.6.3. Grace VanderWaal’s 2017 “So Much More Than This” arranges the three units A, B, and
C to form an ABBC phrase, while Marshmello’s 2018 “Happier” arranges similar units to form an
ABAC period.
Figure 9.6.4. Sam Cooke’s 1961 “Cupid” nests two eight-bar sentences inside a sixteen-bar period.
The initial motive is transposed relative to the prevailing chord, changing harmonic notes to
nonharmonic notes and vice versa.
Figure 9.6.5. Triply nested groupings in Mozart’s K.311, III.
More complex construction can arise from elision, with one or more bars
shared by overlapping formal units. I hear the opening of the
Hammerklavier’s scherzo as a four-bar AA'A'A" that ends with a weak
cadence: heard on its own, these measures outline a weakly closed I–vi–IV–
V–I harmonic cycle, with the first three chords hierarchically decorated by
their own dominants (Figure 9.6.6). The final bar starts a second schema, a
four-bar “Ludwig” featuring outer-voice contrary motion and organized as
four-bar sentence; heard on its own, it would make a reasonably convincing
opening.47 To understand this music, we therefore need to recognize the
weak close in m. 4: whether we refer to it as a “cadence” or a form of
“noncadential closure” is immaterial; the important point is its role in
dividing the phrase into two overlapping halves, 4 + 4 somehow adding up
to 7.
Figure 9.6.6. The opening of the scherzo of Beethoven’s Hammerklavier, Op. 106. The first four
measures and the last four measures each sound like coherent four-bar phrases.
Here I have been using the terms “sentence” and “period” to refer to very
general grouping strategies that can exist on a variety of temporal levels.
William Caplin, our most influential theorist of phrase structure, instead
defines “sentence” and “period” as thematic patterns of what he calls
“formal functions,” largely determined by bass motion and always having a
length of eight “real” bars.48 This has, in my opinion, three problematic
consequences. First, it deemphasizes one of the most interesting features of
the classical style, the reuse of similar grouping strategies on different
temporal levels.49 Second, it deprives us of vocabulary for expressing
similarities between classical-style phrases and those in nonclassical styles
such as rock. Third, it directs our attention away from relatively
intersubjective features of musical structure, such as the contrast between
ABÁC and AÁBC grouping, and toward much more subjective judgments
about rhetorical role.
The second theme of the first movement of Beethoven’s Op. 31, no. 3,
illustrates the difficulties that can arise. From the perspective of melody,
motive, and grouping, Figure 9.6.7 is a period, an (AB)(ÁC) phrase with an
unmistakable pause on an unstable ii6. Caplin, however, does not recognize
it as such, taking its lightly embellished 3̂–4̂–5̂–1̂ bass to indicate that the
entire structure is “cadential,” lacking “initiating function” altogether.50 But
his writing also justifies another interpretation that highlights the left hand’s
ascending-step sequence. This would make the phrase functionally medial, a
continuation without proper beginning.51 Thus we could interpret one and
the same phrase as a standard period, a cadential structure lacking initiating
function, or a sequence expressing medial function.
Figure 9.6.7. Three features of the second theme of Beethoven’s Op. 31, no. 3, I, suggesting three
different formal interpretations. First, its rhythmic and motivic antecedent/consequent design; second,
its cadential bass line; third, its disguised sequential structure. This last feature may explain the
decision to begin with a first-inversion triad.
Here, finally, we start to glimpse the full complexity of the classical style,
which obeys a local harmonic grammar while articulating longer-range
melodic connections and also grouping music into conventional four-unit
patterns, sometimes instantiated on multiple temporal levels, all occurring
inside scales that follow higher-level modulatory pathways and alongside
complex networks of motivic and thematic relationships which may
themselves make topical and generic references, while invoking the large-
scale conventions of forms such as the sonata. And that is completely
ignoring rhythm, orchestration, timbre, and any number of vitally important
phenomena. Music is difficult not so much because of the complexity of its
syntactical systems, but because it juggles so many simultaneously.
And though we can consider these systems separately, they sometimes
interact in complex and interesting ways. One example is when the local
harmonic grammar is overridden across phrase boundaries. In the first eight
bars of Figure 8.5.2, a V–ii progression occurs across mm. 4–5 of a sentence
form. Here the rhetorical structure ameliorates the harmonic hiccup: rather
than a baldly retrofunctional V–ii, I hear a first musical thought that ends on
the dominant followed by a new thought beginning on the supertonic. Such
examples show what is at stake in questions about the nature and location of
cadences; for whatever we call these I64–V progressions, we want to
recognize them as punctuations, important junctures that weaken the grip of
the local harmonic constraints.52
More common are those cases in which harmony supports grouping
structure. Chapter 6 noted that dominants and tonics are oriented oppositely
in time: dominants are approached freely, but constrained in how they move;
tonics move freely, but are approached in only a small number of ways
(§6.4). This differential orientation is fundamental to the logic of the period,
which begins with maximum harmonic stability, and hence maximal
uncertainty about the future, progresses to a high degree of harmonic
instability (and hence predictability), and then retraces its steps to achieve
repose.53 From this point of view, one might be tempted to consider Figure
9.7.1 an eight-measure period in which IV plays the role of the dominant,
appearing as the final chord of the first four-bar unit and leading to I at the
end of the second. This intuition motivates Drew Nobile to define
“dominant” and “tonic” in terms of the phrasal and entropic tendencies in
Figure 9.7.2. In effect, Nobile uses “dominant” to refer to a particular
position within the phrase, rather than within the more abstract harmonic
cycles that make up the phrase. I prefer the more traditional terminology, in
part because I prefer its linking of functional names to particular scale-
degree collections. Nevertheless, I feel the force of Nobile’s insight: there
are genres in which phrasal convention can lead us to hear a wider range of
harmonies as functioning like “dominants” in the broadest sense of the term
—unstable sonorities creating strong expectations of a coming tonic.54 Both
phrase structure and harmonic cycles can create tension and release.
Figure 9.7.1. Drew Nobile analyzes this passage from the Eagles’ “Take It Easy” as a period structure
with the subdominant chord playing the role of the dominant.
Figure 9.7.2. The energetic trajectory of an abstract period; it begins with low tension, moves to high
tension at the half cadence; and retraces its steps to end with low tension.
1
For similar views see Meyer 1996, p. 259, and Webster 2010 (“multivalence”). The approach also
resembles the “post-human organicism” of Watkins (2017) and Arndt (2019).
2
Rameau (1722) 1971, p. 155.
3
The technique seems to be genuinely schematic insofar as it is usually applied to phrase-initial
motives using tonic and dominant harmonies rather than motives more generally.
4
The upper-voice dyad moves by a loop (t1) and a slide (T1) on the 2-in-3 spiral diagram; the
three-note scale itself oscillates between D–F♯–A and C♯–E–G.
5
My analysis highlights the ascending basic voice leading (B, G) → (D, F ♯ ) to emphasize the
continuity with the following gesture.
6
Of course, no sensible person would imagine this to be so: traditional harmonic theory was never
meant as a recipe for producing beautiful music, any more than English grammar provides a paint-by-
numbers kit for producing beautiful poetry; the grammar plays a necessary but not sufficient role in
the production of compelling functional music. Critics of traditional harmonic theory sometimes treat
its limitations as fundamental flaws (Schenker [1930] 1997, pp. 1–9, Gjerdingen 1996 and 2015).
7
To be clear, this is only a very rough approximation of a complex issue; cadential weight is
affected by such factors as rhythm and temporal duration.
8
Here the algorithm chooses a voice leading Ci→Ci + 1 from the actual voice leadings in Bach’s
chorales.
9
Caplin 2004 rejects this term, but I think it captures the point that functional tonality extends the
harmonic logic of the cadence to the rest of the phrase. Caplin also tends to draw a sharp distinction
between “cadence” and “noncadence,” whereas I think the distinction is inherently fuzzy.
10
Sewell 2021 notes that Schoenberg provided simple melodic reductions along the lines I have
been describing.
11
Laitz 2008, Clendinning and Marvin 2016. Caplin 1998 has a similar view.
12
Though it derives from Schenker, the Laitz-Clendenning-Marvin phrase model is often presented
in starkly harmonic terms, shorn of the linear features fundamental to traditional Schenkerianism.
13
Tymoczko 2011a, chapter 5.
14
These later reductional stages tend to eliminate all the nontonic chords between the initial tonic
and the final cadence, regardless of the musical surface.
15
As recently as 2011, it was possible to publish an article in a prominent music-theory journal
broaching the question of how Schenkerian theory might, in principle, be tested (Temperley 2011a).
Morgan 1978 considers antecedents to Schenkerian analysis, all of which are compatible with the
version of traditional theory I articulate here; the distinctively Schenkerian focus on very long-range
pitch connections has little or no theoretical precedent.
16
The fact that Schenkerian theory is associated with racist aesthetic opinions adds moral urgency
to these already pressing problems (Ewell 2020).
17
Note that the bass reaches 5̂ for the cadence while the melody hints at a 3̂–2̂(–1̂) descent,
returning to the third scale degree for the repeat. Carl Czerny ([1830] 1848, I, p. 92) has an alternative
voice-leading reduction that departs more radically from the musical surface.
18
These chords, A–d♯ ø43–d♯°43–d♯°65–d♯°65[ ♭ 3], transform A-as-dominant into A-as-tonic by
way of a French sixth that continues the ascending-third triadic sequence. Rings 2011 considers these
sorts of changes of scale-degree function.
19
For more on the “second practice,” see Kinderman and Krebs 1996 and Cohn 2012. The
particular idiom used here is the descending major-third version of the game described in §8.5 of
Tymoczko 2011a. My analysis has many points of overlap with that in Meyer 1996 (pp. 93–97); it
contrasts with those of Kramer (1990) and Subotnik (1991), who both read this piece as challenging
the very idea of analytical reconstruction. Hatten 2014 offers a sensitive discussion of the work’s
poetics.
20
The Op. 24, no. 4 mazurka similarly decomposes its final V–I cadence into a pair of descending
thirds in accordance with the logic of the 3-in-7 spiral diagram. Like the opening of Op. 7, no. 2, the
V–iii–I can likewise be understood either as a nod toward modality or as a drawn-out suspension.
21
See §4.10, Tymoczko 2011a.
22
Schenker (1933) 1969, p. 58.
23
See Schmalfeldt 1991 and Cohn 1992.
24
See Neumeyer 1987, Temperley 2006 and 2011, and Froebe 2015 for related views. It may be
that Schenkerian schemas have their origins in something more than arbitrary convention—for
example, the preference for descending stepwise melodies may be rooted in the physiology of
respiration (chapter 2).
25
Beethoven’s theme also recalls his earlier Op. 18, no. 5 minuet. Its I–V–I–IV progression would
be a falling fifth sequence were the initial chord F♯ instead of E. This same near sequence appears as a
chord progression in Figure 6.1.3 and as a succession of keys in Bach’s D major prelude (§7.3).
26
The location of the final cadence is interesting, and one could make the case that it belongs on
the downbeat, with both melody and bass delaying the arrival until beat 3.
27
As Robert Morgan put it, in Schenker’s theory, “stepwise motion forms the basis for all melodic
content” (Morgan 2014, p. 19).
28
It is also the diatonic inversion of the two-voice fauxbourdon framework at the start of the fifth
Brandenburg concerto (Figure 7.5.2). Yust 2015b refines Schenker’s reading of the piece.
29
The direct shift from B ♭ 7 to G7 is an example of what I have called “the minor-third system”
(Tymoczko 2011a). The switch from descending thirds to descending steps occurs in many Beethoven
pieces, including Op. 31, no. 3, I, mm. 68–70.
30
The middle section answers 5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂–1̂ in G minor with 6̂–5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂ in C minor, shifting the
rhythmic emphasis from tonic to dominant in preparation for the I64–V arrival.
31
Schenker’s reading ignores the B♭65 in the middle section, reducing it to a G minor triad.
32
Schenker’s invention of C7 in the opening is reminiscent of Dahlhaus’s denial of the autonomy
of vii°6 (§6.4).
33
“It should be evident now that the analytic procedure is one of reduction; details which are
subordinate with respect to larger patterns are gradually eliminated” (Forte 1959). Arndt (2018, p. 39)
argues that the focus on surface-to-depth reduction is a later accretion; Schenker proposed an idealist
theory of the Genius’s creative process, which proceeds from the background to foreground. In this
book, I am concerned with Schenker as he has been received rather than the historical figure.
34
Gjerdingen and Bourne 2015.
35
“Always the same but not in the same way,” Schenker’s motto, appearing at the front of Der
Tonwille and later works.
36
This is precisely why some theorists like the notation V64--5 3; my own preference is to notate
the scale degrees and trust that readers understand the idiom.
37
It is also clear that musical experience is often multileveled and hierarchical: we can hear a tonic
scale degree as decorating the third of a V chord that is itself unstable relative to the tonic. My
explanation of this phenomenon is that the tonic scale degree is unstable relative to the local harmony
while the local harmony is itself unstable relative to our functionally tonal harmonic expectations
(§7.1).
38
This example comes from Muns 2008.
39
Other examples include the opening of the B ♭ major prelude from the first book of The Well-
Tempered Clavier (featuring diatonic chords with a very weak dominant function), Chopin’s Mazurka
Op. 50, no. 3, mm. 53–61 (featuring secondary subdominants), and Figure 9.6.6 (secondary dominants
embellishing descending thirds). Salzer (1952) 1962 uses these brief moments of harmonic recursion
to argue against traditional harmonic theory.
40
Among those who have made this connection are Bernstein 1976, Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983,
Katz and Pesetsky 2011, and Rohrmeier 2011 and 2020.
41
Brown 2005 (pp. 76ff) and Rohrmeier 2011 both make this argument, though they stop short of
explicitly demonstrating that their recursive principles actually account for the regularities in
functional pieces—or, what is more difficult, demonstrating that their recursive grammars rule out the
patterns we do not find in functional tonality. Rohrmeier 2020 seems to acknowledge that the
hierarchical approach does not expand our ability to describe what happens in actual pieces, merely
functioning as a theory of analytical intuitions about music.
42
For more examples, see Figures 10.4.7 (I–ii–IV–V upon removing the applied dominant) and
10.6.5 (V–ii6) and the end of the trio of Beethoven’s Op. 28, III, where D64, functioning locally as a
cadential 64, moves via secondary dominant to B minor and the end of the movement.
43
Rothstein 1989 emphasizes the classical style’s eight-bar norm.
44
Richards 2011 calls these “trifold sentences.”
45
Rothstein 1989, pp. 125–30, considers the origin of these phrase templates.
46
Lerdahl 2020, p. 89, makes the same point.
47
Note that this is the same ascending-fifth schema I identified in the Op. 22 minuet, on the same
pitches: IV–I–V in B♭ major (§10.1).
48
Caplin 2013 observes that we can analyze phrase structure in terms of motivic material, melodic
strategy, or harmony and bass. Temperley 2018, p. 90, favors a grouping-based strategy for reasons
similar to mine. Brody 2016 pursues the issue in a more general way, proposing “thematic design” and
“tonal structure,” as axes along which different theoretical approaches might disagree. In this section I
focus mostly on thematic design and motive; in the next I consider tonal structure.
49
Caplin 2009b allows for nested formal functions, but conceives them as generalized
“beginning/middle/end” patterns rather than the specific grouping strategies characteristic of sentence
and period.
50
See Caplin 1998, p. 111. Although Caplin in principle allows that cadential function is
determined by myriad factors, in practice he is almost exclusively focused on the bass: this leads him
to describe Beethoven’s entire theme as having cadential function although it is the start of the second
group. In this context, Richards argues that tonic chords are only “weak indicators of cadential
function” (2010, p. 26).
51
Caplin analyzes the sequential second theme of Beethoven’s Op. 2, no. 2, I as a “continuation”
(1998, pp. 112–13).
52
This provides another reason to reject Caplin’s strict division between “cadence” and
“noncadence.”
53
The association of tension with certainty about the future, and repose with uncertainty, may seem
counterintuitive; but recall that the tension is associated with the sense that the chord has to move in a
particular way. The uncertainty of repose is associated with the fact that the tonic has no analogous
obligations.
54
Nobile’s perspective derives from Lerdahl 2001, and recalls Long’s (2018, 2020) proposal that
we consider rhythm and phrasing as essential to functional tonality. These perspectives are closely
related to the ideas discussed in §9.2; indeed, one way of interpreting the Schenkerian notion of a
“structural dominant” is as “the dominant that occurs when we are expecting the end of the phrase.”
Here, phrasal role supersedes note-content as a determiner of function. This approach works best with
very regularly phrased music.
55
Caplin and Schoenberg present the opening of Beethoven’s Op. 2, no. 1, as a model sentence; to
my mind, however, it is unusual insofar as its basic idea has just one chord, giving it a more urgent
and developmental character than the typical Mozartian sentence (I–V, V–I). See BaileyShea 2004.
56
This is the origin of the “changing-note archetype” discussed in Gjerdingen 1988, later
rechristened the “Meyer” schema.
57
See Levinson 1997. Explaining what makes a piece “coherent and pleasingly dramatic” is no
easier than explaining what makes a good movie.
58
Reti 1951.
59
Marvin 2012–2013 points out that this challenge is particularly acute in the case of sonata-form
recapitulations. There is also the additional challenge of devising a Schenkerian analysis that is
compatible with traditional formal divisions (P. Smith 1994, C. Smith 1996, and Brody 2021).
60
This thought experiment is inspired by the experiment of Cook 1987, later repeated by Marvin
and Brinkman 1999. Gjerdingen 1999 expresses skepticism about some details of Cook’s experiment
even while endorsing his general conclusions.
61
Yust 2018 emphasizes keys within a largely Schenkerian framework, though this does not lead to
major changes in analytical practice.
62
Kirlin 2014.
63
Hepokoski and Darcy 2006, p. 232. On the same page they write: “The attaining of the ESC is
the most significant event within the sonata . . . It is here that the presence of the tonic becomes finally
secured as real rather than provisional.” In private conversation, Hepokoski stressed that he is
speaking of a compositional norm and making no claims about perception; nevertheless, he continues
to use the term “resolution” in this context (Hepokoski 2021, p. 12).
64
The first quote is from Caplin 1998, p. 161, continuing “and to provide symmetry and balance to
the overall form by restating the melodic-motivic material of the exposition.” The second is from
Caplin 1998, p. 163.
65
Scott Burnham speaks of “the embarrassment of trying to fit a large-scale repetition into a
dramatic narrative” (1995, p. 18). This embarrassing combination of drama and formulaic repetition is
echoed by Debussy’s alleged wisecrack about smoking a cigarette during classical-form developments
(cf. Rosen 2001, p. 117, suggesting the piece was by Beethoven, or Taruskin 2005, vol. 3, p. 22,
suggesting it was by Brahms). William Marvin (2012–2013) notes that an analogous problem arises
within the confines of Schenkerian theory.
66
Margulis 2013.
67
Rothstein observes that the classical style’s origins in dance music and opera were emphasized
by Leonard Ratner and Charles Rosen respectively (1989, p. 130); to these, he adds popular music as a
third source.
68
The quotation comes from a 1918 interview with the Boston Post, quoted in Warfield 2011, p.
299.
69
In a fascinating 2017 article, Yoel Greenberg suggests that recapitulations arose from a
concatenation of two different practices: “the double return of the opening theme in the tonic in the
middle of the second half of a two-part form, and the thematic matching between the ends of the two
halves of two-part form.”
70
Byros 2015 also makes the connection between formal conventions and scale-degree schemas.
71
Hepokoski writes: “To feel this music musically—to identify with this music as music—is to
monitor the moment-to-moment tensions of the drives toward the succession of boundary cadences”
(2021, p. 8). I find it difficult to believe that typical eighteenth-century classical-music audiences
engaged in this sort of “monitoring.”
Prelude
Why Beethoven?
Much the same point could be made about the use of register in the first
movement of the Tempest (Figure P10.2). Once again we see Beethoven’s
music oscillating dramatically, where a typical classical piece would stay in
a narrower range. And once again we see Beethoven using a basic musical
parameter in a form-defining way, most notably by constructing a long
registral arch linking transition and second theme—eliding the medial
caesura that typically separates those two formal regions. Registral
separation also seems to be a significant parameter, with the hands
expanding to the edges of Beethoven’s piano in the middle of the second
theme; this then initiates a grand contrary-motion sequence that returns to a
more normal configuration.2
Figure P10.2. Registral position of the two hands, averaged over a four-beat window, in the first
movement of the Tempest sonata.
Figure P10.4. Measures 44–66 of Beethoven’s Op. 10, no. 1, I, which move from transition to second
theme with no medial caesura. Hepokoski and Darcy describe this moment as a “caesura fill” even
though it could also be a “standing on the dominant” (albeit at a quiet dynamic); had the music
proceeded to a pause before its second theme, this passage almost certainly would have been identified
as such.
Figure P10.5. The Waldstein’s “filled medial caesura” features rhythmic deceleration.
1
Bernstein 1956.
2
We would find a similar contrast were we to consider dynamics, with Beethoven’s ranging from
pianissimo to fortissimo rather than the forte-and-piano opposition of many classical sonatas.
3
Edmund Burke (1757) 1999, Part 3, chapter 25: “the beautiful in music will not bear that loudness
and strength of sounds, which may be used to raise other passions; nor notes which are shrill, or harsh,
or deep; it agrees best with such as are clear, even, smooth, and weak. [In addition,] great variety, and
quick transitions from one measure or tone to another, are contrary to the genius of the beautiful in
music. Such transitions often excite mirth, or other sudden and tumultuous passions; but not that
sinking, that melting, that languor, which is the characteristical effect of the beautiful as it regards
every sense.”
4
Hoffmann 1989.
5
Dahlhaus makes a similar point (1991, p. 83).
6
Lalite et al. 2009.
7
Dahlhaus 1991, Schmalfeldt 1995 and 2011.
8
Similar points can be made about the second theme of the first movements of the Eroica (where
there is no pause at all between the beat 1 dominant arrival and the beat 2 start of the main theme) and
the Tempest, to be considered subsequently. See Richards 2013a.
9
Compare the medial caesura in the first movement of the Fifth Symphony, which expands and
transforms the “knock of fate” motive into the start of the second theme (§10.4): here the caesura is
again doing formal work over and above its conventional function.
10
In fact, we have some evidence that this is not the case, as contemporaneous “programmatic”
readings do not emphasize the medial caesura (e.g., the “Eroica” program in Marx [1859] 1997; see
Burnham 1995).
11
The subtitle of Caplin 1998 is “A Theory of Formal Functions for the Instrumental Music of
Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven,” while Hepokoski and Darcy introduce their book by writing “from
one perspective the Elements is a research report, the product of our analyses of hundreds of
individual movements by Haydn, Mozart, Beethoven, and many surrounding composers of the time”
(2006, p. v).
10
Beethoven Theorist
If a chord divides the octave very evenly, then it can be linked by efficient
voice leading to its transpositions. If its notes are clustered close together,
then its voice exchanges will be small. The minor third is interesting insofar
as it is balanced between these two extremes, being halfway between the
unison and the tritone—or in other words exactly one fourth of an octave.
This means that the most efficient voice leading connecting it to its tritone
transposition is exactly the same size as its smaller voice exchange. Two
contrary-motion voices, when restricted to this interval class, will alternate
between these two possibilities: (E ♭ 4, C5) → (C4, E ♭ 5) → (A3, F ♯ 5) →
Figure 10.1.10. The ascending-fifth Ludwig near the end of the exposition of the first movement of
Beethoven’s Op. 10, no. 3, mm. 93–96.
Figure 10.1.11. The ascending-fifth Ludwig in the opening of Op. 31, no. 1, III. Here the voice
leading ascends while the chords descend.
Figure 10.1.12. Another contrary-motion pattern in Op. 31, no. 1, IV, mm. 28–31. The D–F♯ dyad
appears one step higher than in the standard schema.
Figure 10.2.3. Measures 25–31 of Op. 2, no. 2, I, present five successive Ludwig units, with the left
hand octave-displaced.
Figure 10.2.4. The Op. 109 variations theme contains eight consecutive units (§9.4).
Figure 10.2.5. Measures 11–18 of the Courante from Élisabeth Jacquet de La Guerre’s Suite in D
minor (Pièces de clavecin, book 1, 1687) present five Ludwig units that alternate directions.
Figure 10.2.8. A passage from Op. 2, no 3, IV, mm. 127–133, that begins with contrary motion along
the F major triad and progresses to an ascending-fifth Ludwig, itself moving by near-contrary motion
along the diatonic scale.
Figure 10.2.9. (top) The contrary-motion pattern from Figure 1.5.6 can be doubled at the third and
extended through a complete octave to produce a series of mostly functional harmonies. (bottom)
Beethoven uses this strategy in the opening of Op. 130, I, mm. 18–19.
Figure 10.2.10. The second theme of Op. 2, no. 1, IV, mm. 34–42, combines free 5–6 ascent in the left
hand with a right-hand contrary-motion melody that descends through thirds and sixths.
Slightly more interesting is the idea that the Ludwig functions as a tool for
producing the dramatic gestural waves characteristic of Beethoven’s style.
The repeating passage from mm. 57–60 in the first movement of the Eroica
is exemplary, part of a massive sonic accretion using register (expanding by
two octaves), dynamics (full-orchestra crescendo), rhythm (quarter notes to
repeating eighth notes and ultimately sixteenths), and orchestration
(trumpets and timpani, Figure 10.2.11).16 From this perspective the Ludwig
is a pitch-domain analogue of the crescendo.
Figure 10.2.11. Measures 57–60 of the Eroica first movement, containing three consecutive Ludwig
units with the last two linked across voices.
Figure 10.2.13. One of the second themes in the Hammerklavier’s first movement (mm. 66–68) is a
four-unit Ludwig that connects smoothly back to itself.
3. The Tempest
Figure 10.3.2. The opening of the Tempest uses a Ludwig to articulate three basic motives.
Figure 10.3.3. The arpeggio, cascade, and turn motives as they appear in the exposition. Each tick
represents one measure.
Figure 10.3.5. The transition (mm. 21–40) as a disguised ascending-fifth sequence using fauxbourdon
triads.
Figure 10.3.7. Since registral inversion produces parallel fifths (middle), Beethoven alters the motive
(bottom).
The repeat expands the register by transposing along the dyad, first by t1
then by t2, the right hand constantly ascending but always in a slightly
different way (Figure 10.3.8). The continuation reinterprets vii°7 of A minor
as vii°7 in the relative major, contracting the register with a descending-third
contrary-motion sequence that restarts the left hand’s eighth-note motion;
Beethoven liked this effect so much he reused it in the first-movement
development of his Second Symphony.37 We arrive, finally, on a two-
measure Ludwig fragment repeated no fewer than five times. (Here I take
the outer voices to be primary, A–B–C–E against C–B–A–G♯, with the latter
voice doubled at the third.) Its fragmentary nature gives this music the
character of a closing theme; however, both the quiet dynamic and the
dominant-pedal bass undercut the cadential arrival at m. 74.38
Figure 10.3.8. (top) Registral expansion by transposition along the dyad (mm. 63–69); (bottom) the
contrary-motion descending-third sequence returning the hands to their original alignment (mm. 69–
73).
could connect smoothly with the E♭ 6–c♯°43–g♯°7 of the exposition but this
would upset the two-chord grouping; Beethoven instead omits the E ♭ and
proceeds directly to c♯° (Figure 10.3.13). Thus we can trace the ascending-
step sequence throughout the movement: E–a, F♯–b, G–C, A–d, [B–e], C–f,
C♯–f♯, D–g, E–A, lacking only the tonicization of E.44 We could say that the
recapitulatory repeat simultaneously continues the ongoing (if difficult-to-
perceive) sequential process.
Figure 10.3.12. The recapitulation’s two arioso laments, with the second transposed to D minor for
comparison.
From there the recapitulation largely repeats the exposition, with the
exception of the upper-voice pedal discussed earlier. The parallelism lasts
until the retransition, where repeated octaves lead to a spooky coda,
compressing the closing theme to a single measure, a fitting depiction of the
storm’s aftermath.45 For all its originality, the movement is almost entirely
schematic, composed of a handful of sequences stitched together with a few
moments of i–V–i functionality. Beethoven arrays these materials in the
service of a compelling formal trajectory, the large-scale registral, motivic,
and harmonic shapes portraying a musical storm that is sublime in every
sense of the term—overwhelming and rigorous music that escapes the
gravitational pull of the ordinary to achieve something unprecedented.
The first movement of the Fifth Symphony is in many ways cousin to the
Tempest: largely sequential, with a middle-register minor tonic, interrupting
its recapitulation for a doloroso aria, and opening with the juxtaposition of
heterogeneous material.46 The Tempest’s arpeggio is replaced by a fortissimo
“Knock of Fate,” appearing at the start of the form’s major sections:
exposition, transition, second theme, development, recapitulation, second
theme recapitulation, and, in the coda, the final thematic statement. (It is
missing from the recapitulation’s transition, replaced by the oboe lament.)
Both pieces are unusually taut in their overall form, suffused with motivic
relationships and exemplifying the analyst’s dream of unity. But where the
Tempest’s sequences are obvious, the Fifth’s are covert and disguised.
(a) Exposition. Schenker and Tovey both warn against taking the main
motive to be the first measure’s G–G–G–E ♭ ; for them the central theme is
this figure combined with its repetition down by one diatonic step, the
“down by third, up by step” melody so central to functional logic (§7.2).47 I
am not so certain. The opening measure articulates a systematic pattern of
chordal intervals that can be traced throughout the first phrase—a disguised
patterning, no doubt produced by the compositional subconscious, that lends
the music an audible if hard-to-describe consistency (Figure 10.4.1). This
pattern of chordal intervals becomes increasingly explicit over the course of
the movement, as if Beethoven were gradually revealing the underlying
structure (Figure 10.4.2).48
Figure 10.4.1. Measures 7–18 as a distorted arpeggio, cycling through three scale degrees that are
themselves moving.
Figure 10.4.2. Several appearances of the (0, 0, –1, –1) interval sequence.
Figure 10.4.3. The symphony’s four-bar phrases distribute two harmonies in many different ways.
Figure 10.4.4. The second large phrase (mm. 26–44) as an accelerating double sentence.
role as 3̂–1̂ in E♭ major. The exposition’s entire second half is thus a single
musical gesture, a steadily rising scalar bass beginning in stillness and
moving toward climactic affirmation (Figure 10.4.9). As Bernstein said,
there is something inevitable here, a palpably new focus on long spans of
musical time. This is not so much a matter of divine inspiration or
compositional genius, but rather musical thought: unusually long paragraphs
formed from entirely schematic material.
Figure 10.4.7. The second theme (mm. 71–94) as an ascending-step near sequence.
Figure 10.4.8. The Ludwig at the end of the second theme (mm. 96–102). The repeat creates four
units of continuous Ludwiging.
gesture: what initially sounds like 5̂–3̂ in E♭ is undercut by a low D♭–C that
turns it into 4̂–2̂ in F minor. This begins a sixty-five-measure “up-and-down-
the-ladder,” ascending by fifth from F to C to G, standing on the dominant,
and descending by fifth to C and F. This leads to a mysterious fauxbourdon
passage, a transfigured second theme ascending from F minor to B♭ minor
The ascent up the ladder of the fifths takes place by way of a grand
contrary-motion gesture. Figure 10.4.11 provides a reduction, the top staff
cascading downward by six octaves from C7 to C1 while the lower ascends
from A ♭ 2 to E ♭ 4; at each stage the top staff descends by an octave and a
fourth, or ten diatonic steps, while the lower voice ascends by fifth. The
voice leading uses a descending-fifth Ludwig that moves from (A♭, C) to (D,
F) to (G, B), but the initial dyad is the upper third of F minor rather than the
lower third of A♭ major; this converts the A♭–D progression of descending-
fifth dyads into an f–d° progression of descending-third triads.56 Once again
dyadic logic augments the triadic logic of Roman numerals.
Figure 10.4.11. The two contrary-motion sequences in the first half of the development.
Figure 10.4.13. Three of Schenker’s development sketches: (top) his long-range sketch; (middle) a
more detailed sketch; (bottom) his interpretation of the contrary-motion Ludwig.
(c) Recapitulation and coda. Oboe solo aside, the first half of the
recapitulation is fairly literal. In part this is because the transition requires
only a single change of harmony, vii°7/V moving to V rather than V/III. The
major difference is timbral: pizzicato lower strings and an increased role for
the winds, leading to the lament that replaces Fate’s second knock.58 The
second theme is transformed by the “pattern continues, chord repeats”
technique of §4.9; this takes I to IV and an expanded “standing on the
dominant” using ♭ 6̂ and ♮ 6̂ (Figure 10.4.14). Amusingly, the second theme’s
Figure 10.4.14. The recapitulation’s version of the second theme (mm. 319ff).
The coda is most notable for what it does not do: in place of schematic
cleverness and voice-leading trickery, we have raw compositional force,
often featuring a stripped-down texture of octaves or dyads that evoke the
Knock of Fate. The overall impression is of accumulating energy: six
overlapping phrases grouped into three paragraphs, outlining large scalar
trajectories and building to a climax surpassing anything heard so far. Like
the Waldstein, it presents a long process of rhythmic change, beginning with
roughly fifty measures of mostly eighth-note motion, slowing to quarter
notes for the next fifty measures, arriving on a singular half-note at the start
of the fifth phrase, and reinstating eighth notes for the final thirty bars.
Motivically, it separates the opening motive’s pitch and rhythm, highlighting
its pitch content in the second phrase and focusing on rhythm in the
remainder.59
The coda begins with pulsing eighth-note harmony, strings and winds
alternately intoning the weak-weak-weak-strong rhythmic motto; the two
Ludwig units loosely recall the transition (Figure 10.4.15, compare mm. 34ff
of Figure 10.4.4).60 These arrive on a second-inversion triad that overlaps
with the start of the second phrase, simultaneously a cadential 64 and a
tonic-functioning beginning. The lower voice combines the Knock of Fate
pitches with the rhythm of the second-theme horn call, abandoning its
cadence in favor of a natural-minor descent in tenths, the counterpoint
contracting to bare dyads. Once again, we bypass the cadence, scale-
fragments instead changing direction and taking up the WWWS rhythmic
motto; offbeat eighths add a third voice that converts the tenths into tonic-
dominant harmony, as if the language were gradually rebuilding itself. One
has the feeling of massive forces reversing direction, an ocean liner slowly
coming about, with phrase 2 descending from A5 to D4 and phrases 3 and 4
ascending back to C5.
Figure 10.4.15. The coda’s first three phrases, mm. 374–422.
All in all, the coda feels somewhat beyond analysis, devoid of the non-
obvious relationships that make verbal commentary worth reading. Here
Beethoven seems to be well on his way to a music of gesture, made from
contrasts of direction, grouping, and density rather than complexities of
harmony and melody. Though it uses familiar scales and harmonies, its
aesthetics feel genuinely novel, somewhere between the worlds of Haydn
and Penderecki. It is interesting that this music completes such a taut and
intricate movement, as if Beethoven’s expressive energy could no longer be
contained within the idioms of the classical style. The coda breaks its
generic bonds, bursting forth in a frenzy of unstructured energy.
5. The “Pastorale” sonata, Op. 28
Figure 10.5.2. The descending 7–6 compared to Beethoven’s descending-third fauxbourdon sequence.
Figure 10.5.3. Thinking within the chord in the repeat of the Op. 28 theme.
Figure 10.5.5. The second phrase loosely echoes the first-phrase horn call.
Figure 10.5.6. The fauxbourdon-based transition, mm. 40–62.
Figure 10.5.7. The transition as a continuous process, the parallel first-inversion chords continuing
through the repeat.
Unlike the previous pieces, the recapitulation involves only minor changes
to the exposition: inserting eighth notes into the first statement of the main
theme (mm. 279–280), adding an extra flourish in mm. 304–307, and
transposing mm. 316–319 so as to remain in the tonic. The coda returns to
the main theme and adds a brief cadence. It is difficult for me to perceive
this as anything other than perfunctory, for it neither contributes to what I
consider a genuine musical narrative, nor resolves anything I hear as long-
term tonal dissonance. In this respect it contrasts with the more varied
recapitulations of the Tempest and the Fifth Symphony. How is it that
Beethoven could be simultaneously so radical along some dimensions and so
conventional along others? What marked the difference, for him, between
unassailable features of sonata form, and those that were available for
experimentation? How did he understand the relation between large-scale
repetition and musical storytelling?
6. Schubert’s Quartettsatz
These three analyses should convey a sense of the intelligent play I hear in
Beethoven’s music. While his pieces are not always so pattern-based, nor so
heavily Ludwig-focused, these movements reflect an important strand of his
thinking—one that I think has been obscured by a fixation on genius,
heroism, divine inspiration, the Idee, musical unity, and the (Burkean)
sublime. I now want to consider how Beethoven’s techniques are
transformed in two pieces composed in his shadow, Schubert’s Quartettsatz
and the prelude to Wagner’s Lohengrin. I hear both composers as attempting
to match Beethoven’s expressive clarity while expanding his musical
vocabulary—Schubert in the direction of greater lyricism and formal
flexibility, and Wagner through a more thoroughgoing rejection of schematic
composition. If Beethoven’s language is one of limitation, a redeploying of
musical resources toward broader stretches of musical time, then both
Schubert and Wagner can be understood as trying to enrich their local
vocabulary while preserving Beethoven’s command of large-scale
architecture.
The Quartettsatz’s exposition is divided into three parts: we begin with
three phrases in C minor, the last of which moves to A ♭ ; we then hear a
lyrical second theme that leads to a two-part transition occurring after the
second theme. The transition begins with an ascending-step sequence whose
dominant sevenths never resolve, arriving at a G dominant that is V7 of the
global tonic—as if we were treading water tonally. We then hear a short,
repeated phrase that transforms this dominant into a tonic, leading to an
authentic cadence and a trio of themes in G major. This three-part structure
is in uneasy dialogue with the gestural targets of the classical-era sonata,
blurring, rearranging, and otherwise problematizing events such as
transition, medial caesura, and the “essential expository cadence.”68
Schubert’s combination of schematic detail and large-scale freedom has
prompted a great deal of criticism, but to a twenty-first-century musician it
suggests a genuine theoretical insight, that formal conventions and large-
scale key-relations are less significant than one might think.69 In this respect,
Schubert continues a process that begins with Beethoven’s more limited
problematizing of sonata-form conventions.
(a) Exposition. The opening measures present three basic elements: the
tremolo texture, the eighth-note neighboring figure, and the four-bar lament
bass (Figure 10.6.1). Both the motive and its development have a static
character, intrinsically accompanimental and always appearing on the same
pitch classes. We open with a four-voice canonic round expanding from
middle C to the quartet’s full four-and-a-half-octave range, low C2 to high
G6. Harmonically it articulates a third-focused tonic-subdominant Ludwig,
with the substitution of i64 for V43/iv evoking the fauxbourdon ROTO
(Figure 10.6.2). To the attentive analyst, the weak-beat i64 signals an
interruption, the unaccented second-inversion triad syntactically passing, yet
appearing at the end of each two-bar unit. Thus we could in principle infer
that the canon, when it breaks, must proceed to something other than V.
Eventually, the music satisfies this expectation with a dramatic Neapolitan,
the Ludwig continuing to the dominant through a voice exchange. In the
next six-bar phrase, the motive, such as it is, appears for the first time in an
upper voice, repeating just once before reaching a cadence with an unaltered
predominant. This is a softer and less angry sound, a notable contrast with
the previous Neapolitan.
Figure 10.6.1. The opening of Schubert’s Quartettsatz.
Figure 10.6.2. The opening fuses Ludwig and fauxbourdon ROTO.
The third phrase is shown in Figure 10.6.3. The ♭2̂ appears as part of the
Figure 10.6.4 details the resemblances among the opening theme, the A♭
major theme, and the fauxbourdon ROTO schema: the opening substitutes
V2/IV for V6, bypassing the schema’s distinctive V6–IV6 progression, while
the second theme subposes new bass notes to avoid the equally characteristic
IV6–I64–ii6. The second-theme melody ascends upward against this
descending background, an arpeggiation that continues when it repeats at the
octave. This pattern of octave restatement will continue throughout the
exposition, which repeats almost as much as the first movement of
Beethoven’s Op. 28. The cadential phrase-extension provides a classical-
music take on the Renaissance ascending-fifth sequence, secondary
dominant concealing the V–ii retrofunctionality and minor tonic reinforcing
the descending voice leading (Figure 10.6.5).71 Its repetition is thus a
continuation, a single sequence of ascending fifths perturbed by third
substitutions.72
Figure 10.6.4. (top) Comparison of the C minor and A♭ major themes, and the fauxbourdon ROTO.
The long-delayed cadence arrives as the tremolos return and the mode
changes to minor, a shocking reversion to the bravura opening—with the
neighbor-note motive again in the foreground (Figure 10.6.6). This music
has the character of a sonata-form transition, and indeed it is possible to
imagine moving directly from the E♭7 in m. 26 to the A♭ minor in m. 61—an
alternative that would be rhetorically if not tonally more straightforward than
Schubert’s actual composition. As in Beethoven, texture, mode, and gesture
serve a thematic function, recalling the opening even in the context of
considerable variation.73 Figure 10.6.7 proposes a stepwise descending
background voice leading against which the surface moves; this effect of
continuous descent disguised by registral displacement is similar to the
Shepard-tone passacaglias considered earlier.74 We arrive on the dominant in
C minor, as if all the preceding music had gotten us exactly nowhere; I hear
this as Schubert’s take on the medial caesura, a reference that is clearer in
the recapitulation, where the texture contracts to a single voice (Figure
10.6.16).75
Figure 10.6.6. The beginning of the transition, mm. 61–80.
Figure 10.6.7. Descending chromatic voice leading in the transition.
The form then becomes a bit mysterious. To my ear, the preceding phrase
is transitional rather than thematic, motivically linked to the (faux) medial
caesura by the melodic F♯–G–A♭–G, and beginning on III of C minor. But
the phrase ends with a perfect authentic cadence and the next phrase, as
Aaron Grant notes, could be heard as closing: a six-measure sentence
containing a full statement of the fauxbourdon ROTO, bringing to
completion a motif present throughout the exposition (Figure 10.6.9).76 The
reference here is not simply one of motivic content, but of schematic
identity: both the C minor opening and the A ♭ major melody hint at the
fauxbourdon ROTO without stating it clearly, the former descending 8̂–7̂–6̂–
5̂–4̂, and latter 8̂–7̂–6̂, and continuing the schema’s harmony even further.77
In the G major theme, the schema emerges complete and undisguised,
Schubert finally coming clean about his material.78 The melody, meanwhile,
ascends in contrary-motion tenths and sixths against the bass, as in the
opening of the Fifth Brandenburg Concerto (c.f. Figure 7.5.2 and the
fauxbourdon passage in Figure 3.2.5).
Figure 10.6.9. The first G major theme, mm. 99–104. The melody alternates sixths and tenths above
the descending bass.
The first G major theme starts on G4 and repeats up an octave; the next
theme begins on the higher-octave G5, repeating at a stratospheric G6 (with
the cello instead switching to pizzicato). The descending octave moves from
bass to melody, sliding chromatically down from tonic to dominant, and then
diatonically down to tonic (Figure 10.6.10). Figure 10.6.11 shows that this
melody transports the preceding bass line’s intervals into the chromatic
scale, a subtle continuity that might initially escape the ear. The eight-
measure sentence is based on the Waldstein/Natural Woman progression,
descending (I–V)–(♭VII–IV)–(♭VI–♭III). Normally, this ♭III would proceed to
V, as in Morley’s “April Is in My Mistress’ Face”; here, Schubert drops
down to a striking root-position Neapolitan, as if extending the sequence
further—a dramatic continuation that transforms the Waldstein schema into
something novel.79 There follows a straightforward closing melody, a six-
measure sentence ascending by octave and repeating in the higher register,
the cello’s double pedal both instantiating the omnipresent three-note motive
and introducing an idea that will become important in the development
(Figure 10.6.12). This is the exposition’s third six-measure sentence.80
Figure 10.6.10. The second G major theme, mm. 113–124, along with its underlying voice leading.
Numbers show 90° arcs on the circle of chromatic triads (Figure 2.1.3).
Figure 10.6.11. The two G major themes present the same intervals in different scales.
Figure 10.6.12. The closing theme (mm. 125–130) as a six-measure sentence.
The following melody picks up the cello pedal, reversing the process at
the beginning of the piece: where the transition from C minor to A♭ major
steered the neighbor figure from foreground to the background, the cello’s
accompanimental pattern here moves from background to foreground. The
octave displacement also continues the exposition’s registral play: where the
exposition repeated themes in a higher octave, this theme contains its own
higher-octave repeat. The harmony exploits the minor-third system to arrive
at B♭ minor, linking A♭7 to F65 by semitonal voice leading across the repeat;
its repeated iiø43–V7 progressions evoke the transition’s iv6–V65 units
(Figure 10.6.7).
The third section contains what would be a textbook “up-and-down-the-
ladder” were the piece in B ♭ major: melodies descending chromatically as
the harmony rises i–v–ii–vi to a V/vi that is approached by augmented sixth.
Like the opening of the exposition, the development goes nowhere tonally,
starting with the closing theme’s G major and ending on the dominant of G
minor. This V/vi slides deceptively to the false tonic B ♭ major and the
recapitulation of the lyrical theme.
Of course, the piece is not in B♭ major and the recapitulation begins in the
wrong key. One could take this as a sign of Schubert’s flexible attitude
toward modulation, or perhaps even compositional frivolity: perhaps, having
found himself in the midst of a B♭ major up-and-down-the-ladder, he simply
followed its local promptings, trusting that he could make his way back to
the tonic.82 A more colorful possibility is that the key represents a confession
of musical theft, for the lyrical melody may derive from one of Mozart’s
more languid and proto-Schubertian concoctions, the adagio of the piano
sonata K.332.83 Figure 10.6.15 shows that the two phrases outline almost the
same contrapuntal substrate, both subposing a novel bass under the
fauxbourdon ROTO’s I64. Mozart then returns to the schema, giving us ii6
and I6 right where we expect. These last two chords are preceded with
pseudo-dominant root-position mediants, which I have labeled III+/ii and iii;
Schubert’s theme replaces these with standard-issue dominants. Perhaps,
having borrowed Mozart’s B♭ opening for an A♭ second theme, Schubert felt
In any case, the theme immediately shifts to E♭ major for its repeat. While
close to C minor, this E♭ major will not take us smoothly back to the tonic:
Looking back at this analysis, I am struck by three things. The first is its
schematic familiarity. The main harmonic and contrapuntal devices all
appear in earlier chapters: the Ludwig, the fauxbourdon ROTO, descending
chromatic sequences, the ascending-step near sequence that skips the
mediant, and the “up and down the ladder.” If I were ever to teach a course
on “composing like Beethoven,” or to advise a student struggling with that
task, I could imagine using this piece as a model; for like much of
Beethoven’s music, it illustrates the power of avoiding certain kinds of
invention. It is interesting not because of its material, but because of the
expressive use to which it puts that material. Schubert’s comfort with
Beethoven’s vocabulary is all the more impressive for the fact that it was
largely untheorized: he did not learn the Ludwig or the fauxbourdon ROTO
from a textbook; instead, he likely discovered them directly in music.
The second feature is the omnipresence of stepwise descending voice
leading. Figure 10.6.17 graphs several of the piece’s schemas in the relevant
spiral diagrams.85 The similarity of procedure is clear, with all these patterns
moving by small clockwise steps. Particularly interesting is the relation
between the diatonic fauxbourdon ROTO and the chromatic Waldstein
sequence, a geometrical analogy that Schubert highlights with a clear
thematic parallelism (Figure 10.6.11). Here we see something like the Prime
Directive, an awareness that musical patterns can be transported from one
chord-and-scale environment to another. We are not yet in the world of
Chopin or Wagner: rather than setting sail onto on the open seas of
chromatic counterpoint, Schubert prefers to stick closer to the shores. But we
can start to see the glimmerings of a new and more general understanding of
musical possibility.
Figure 10.6.17. Four passages from the Quartettsatz modeled on spiral diagrams. On the top left, the
basic “fauxbourdon ROTO” pattern C–e°–F6–C64, used at multiple points in the movement; top right,
the two chromatic triadic sequences, the Waldstein C–G6–B♭–F6 (second theme) and the ascending-
fifth C–G–D–A (development, dotted line). On the bottom, the seventh chord sequence C7–c7–D2,
used in the transition.
Finally, there is the piece’s deconstruction of classical form, with its three
separate tonal areas, its two secondary themes, its oddly placed medial
caesura, and its rearranged recapitulation. In this respect it continues
Beethoven’s efforts to loosen the formal conventions of earlier classical
music. Schubert’s music conveys the sense of sonata form as abstracted and
transformed: tonally stable thematic zones interspersed with transitions, any
of which might end with a medial caesura. Directed modulatory architecture
is giving way to contrasts of character and mood, the piece presenting not an
orderly march from tonic to dominant and back, but a meandering tour
through tonal and expressive space. For me this is liberating, suggesting that
Schubert was a kindred spirit: someone who did not feel that large-scale key
relations were central to musical coherence, someone who understood sonata
form as optional and conventional, rather than a musical law of nature. The
piece, in other words, makes implicit but genuinely theoretical claims about
the nature of musical form.
Figure 10.7.1. The opening of the “Dawn” section of Félicien David’s The Desert.
Figure 10.7.2. Measures 1–12 of the Lohengrin prelude. The first three stanzas all begin with this
material.
After this shared introduction, the stanzas proceed freely, in something
like the way the successive voices in Renaissance polyphony diverge after
stating a motive. The first is in some ways the most hermetic: what sounds
like a repeat of the I–vi–I is interrupted by a descending chromatic 6–5
sequence, f ♯ –C ♯ –E–B (Figure 10.7.3). The A major context makes this
sound more like free-floating chromaticism than the typical descent from
tonic to dominant—a perception reinforced by the tonicization of E major.
Having arrived on B, we descend by fifth to A major, leading to an
unexpected phrygian cadence on G♯. Outer voices then descend in staggered
tenths, moving unsystematically through third-related harmonies to arrive at
E major halfway through the bar.
Figure 10.7.3. Measures 13–20, the end of the prelude’s first stanza.
It is easy to overlook how novel this harmony is, particularly when we are
thinking about Wagner’s later chromaticism. For though functional and
diatonic, the prelude is worlds apart from the schematic language of the
classical style: the proliferation of secondary triads decenters tonic and
dominant, and the music “wanders” even when it remains diatonic. Wagner
tends to disguise his schemas, as when he occludes the converging cadence
with semitonal motion or presents the Waldstein progression on unusual
scale degrees. One can draw a contrast with those Romantic composers who
present their wandering moments as departures from a tonic-dominant
background. Already in Lohengrin, wandering is becoming the norm.
The second stanza begins as the first, with the winds sounding the theme
in the lower register while the strings add a countermelody that, for me, is
largely textural (Figure 10.7.4). The second half of the phrase, shown in
Figure 10.7.5, uses generalized fauxbourdon to reach a cadence on F. The
countermelodies ascend by third as the harmony descends, as in the
Waldstein (Figure 10.7.6). The next four measures juxtapose phrygian and
authentic cadences on E, modality and functionality alternating as if they
were equally plausible options.89 The last dominant returns to E by way of
vi, hinting at the I–vi–I motto before descending by thirds B–G♯–[E]–c♯–A–
f ♯ . The relation between this music and the end of the first stanza
exemplifies Wagner’s innovative conception of musical identity: the two
passages seem to traverse similar terrain without quite being variations,
passing through the same stations in their own individual way. This formal
flexibility again recalls the Renaissance.
Figure 10.7.4. Measures 21–27, the start of the second stanza.
Figure 10.7.5. Measures 28–36, the end of the second stanza.
The third stanza largely repeats the second, though in a new register and
with more powerful orchestration: the A major theme enters in the horns,
bassoons, and lower strings, centered around middle C and supported by the
full force of the winds and strings (Figure 10.7.7). An extra ♭ II in m. 49
resolves as an ascending subdominant to an F that moves via the major-third
system to A7, the brass entering fortissimo for the arrival on D (Figure
10.7.8). The following music can be heard as a decorated I–vi–I, D–
(b⇐F ♯ ⇒b)–(A⇒D), leading to two iterations of the converging cadence
suggesting different keys—the first beginning in D and the second ending in
A, with no definitive point of modulation in between. (As in the Quartettsatz
or Beethoven’s Op. 28, a repeated modulation can be heard in two different
ways.) This takes us to a strong dominant whose deceptive resolution
overlaps with the start of the final large phrase.90
Figure 10.7.7. Measures 37–50, the third stanza. This music largely repeats the second stanza.
Figure 10.7.8. The fourth stanza, mm. 51–57.
Figure 10.7.9. The contrary-motion wedge of mm. 58–72, along with an analysis.
Figure 10.7.10. The death of Kastchei from Stravinsky’s Firebird (R195). The lower voices feature
the repeating contrapuntal pattern shown in Figure 4.2.2, while the upper voices descend
chromatically and unsystematically, producing a wide range of sonorities.
1
Eventually, Lewin gave up and I spent the rest of the semester happily writing fugues.
2
For example, Caplin 2004, p. 69.
3
McClary 1991 and Fink 2004.
4
Burnham 1995.
5
One can add transpositions to this template but that does not change the basic Tinctorian situation.
6
Small intervals like the minor second have a small voice exchange but a large voice leading to
their tritone transposition; large intervals have large voice exchanges, but a small voice leading to their
tritone transposition. The minor third is perfectly balanced between these two poles.
7
Examples include the first-movement developments of the Kreutzer (or perhaps “Bridgetower”)
sonata, the Second Symphony, the Waldstein, the Eroica, and the Fifth Symphony (discussed later).
8
Thanks here to Nathan Mitchell, who also pointed out the relevance of Rink’s “Morte” schema
(Figure 4.7.4).
9
Steve Taylor points out that the second half of the opening adagio melody is similar to the third-
movement antiparallel episode discussed in the prelude to chapter 4—thus linking all three
movements thematically. The first-movement introduction can also be interpreted as a disguised
Ludwig variant over a perpetually descending bass; mm. 7–8 are a potential source for Figure 4.6.1
and Tristan more generally.
10
Beethoven exploits the schema’s contrary-motion potential by continuing it through multiple
octaves: in this case, the descending line appears on A4 to A3 and A2, while the ascending line moves
from A1 to A3 and A4.
11
Schindler 1840, p. 197; 1841, II, pp. 83–84. For a generally Beethovenian piece, see Clementi’s
minuet from Op. 10, no. 1; for some Clementi Ludwigs, see Op. 7, no. 2, I, mm. 4–5, Op. 10, no. 1,
III, mm. 1–2, Op. 13, no. 4, mm. 45–49 (initially published as Op. 14, no. 1). For a Beethovenian use
of the diatonic third’s basic voice leading, see Op. 7, no. 2, I, mm. 78–79 (compare Figure 3.2.4).
12
That is, the one consonance that is not invertible at the octave, the fifth, is also the only
consonance that cannot be expanded by a third: C–G is consonant, but C–B is dissonant.
13
Due to registral limitations, continuous contrary-motion Ludwiging is often disguised by octave
displacements.
14
Beethoven sometimes uses this change-of-direction strategy in developmental passages,
including the first-movement developments of his second (mm. 146ff) and third (mm. 186ff)
symphonies.
15
Note that the two kinds of motion exploit two different geometries: the logic of contrary motion
is essentially a dyadic note-against-note logic, while the logic of parallel motion is more robustly
triadic.
16
This is the second theme in Hepokoski and Darcy’s reading.
17
This is broadly speaking a devotion to musical poverty, a kind of asceticism or humility; some
theorists have connected this to Beethoven’s interest in popular or revolutionary music (Biamonte
2006, Ferraguto 2019).
18
This theme resembles one of the second themes of Op. 7, I, mm. 60–68.
19
Kinderman 2009a, p. 357.
20
Cavett-Dunsby 1988 considers register in the transposed second-theme recapitulations of
Mozart’s “Haydn” quartets. Bergé and D’hoe 2009 note the registral issue here.
21
Beethoven’s description of how a pianist should play the piece (Burnham 2009).
22
Kant (1788) 1997, p. 133.
23
On Beethoven and monumentality, see Rehding 2009 and Dahlhaus 1991.
24
An 1820 conversation book contains the phrase “the starry skies above and the moral law within
—KANT!” (Thayer 1967, p. 747). For the inscription, which Kant considered among the most
sublime ever uttered, see Schindler (1860) 1996, p. 365, and Kant (1790) 1970, p. 160.
25
Bergé and D’hoe 2009 survey approaches to the motivic content.
26
Dahlhaus 1991 suggests that the piece represents a decisive break with earlier conceptions of
form. See Schmalfeldt 1995 and Horton 2014 for discussion.
27
This is the parsing given in Tovey 1931. Caplin 2009a also stresses the continuity between the
Tempest and Beethoven’s style. Vande Moortele 2009 surveys readings of the piece’s form.
28
See Dahlhaus 1991 and especially Schmalfeldt 1995 and 2011. Hamilton 2009 connects the
opening to the practice of improvised preluding.
29
Webster 2010 surveys the equally unusual form of Op. 10, no. 3, I.
30
Schmalfeldt (1995, p. 62) questions the analysis as a period because the consequent is
sequential; however, I believe sequential themes are common in Beethoven. Rothstein 2009, following
Riemann and Uhde, notes that the entire period forms a 2 + 4 structure if one takes the Largo half as
fast as the Allegro. Both the phrase and its constituent halves are examples of Goldenberg’s
“question/answer pairs” (2020).
31
James Hepokoski (2009), John Nathan Martin (2010), and David Damschroder (2016) all read
C♯–E–G–B♭ in m. 11 as a common-tone diminished seventh of G minor. This reading not only breaks
the harmonic sequence but postulates a very rare chord, the minor-key ct°7. Hatten 2009 agrees with
my reading: a dominant C♯–E–G–B♭ moving retrofunctionally to a dominant-of-the-dominant. This is
a common Beethoven progression (§3.8).
32
Op. 18, no. 6, II, m. 18, contains the same ascending-fifth pattern. Damschroder’s Schenkerian
analysis (2016) turns the sequence’s parallel motion into a non-parallel chorale. Burnham 2009 notes
the near omnipresence of first-inversion triads.
33
Hepokoski (2009) suggests that the movement has no second theme since it lacks a medial
caesura; instead, he hears this music as a “dominant lock” (or “standing on the dominant”). For me it
has too much individuality for that, its simple tonic-dominant harmony characteristically
Beethovenian.
34
My reading of the second-theme phrasing is broadly compatible with that of Schmalfeldt and
Caplin. Both of them, however, read the m. 63 cadence as the conclusion of a large thematic group
stretching back to m. 42, asserting a strong formal juncture between the nearly parallel presentations
of the second theme: thus they parse the music as (AB)(B´), with the B´ representing, in Caplin’s
words, “an entirely different thematic region” (Caplin 2009a, p. 111). I find it more natural to imagine
something more like (A)(BB´) with A and B briefly overlapping, the A material forgotten as the music
takes up a new subject. Thus I would consider m. 55 simultaneously weakly cadential with respect to
the preceding music and initiating with respect to what follows.
35
Caplin’s invocation of “invertible counterpoint” is really a metonym for a more general issue,
namely the way registral shifts can extinguish (what he considers) cadences by removing the required
root-position dominant (as in Example 4.7 of Caplin 2009a).
36
I hear the bass articulating Byros’s le–sol–fi–sol schema, but on the wrong scale degrees,
somewhat like the transition in the first movement of Beethoven’s Second Symphony (Byros 2012,
2015). This raises the delicate question, endemic to schematic analysis, of whether to read the passage
as a deliberate deforming of a preexisting norm, or as a new Beethovenian norm of its own.
Schmalfeldt 1995 hears an A major tonic but notes its potential for dominant function.
37
The diminished seventh is in m. 165 and the third-sequence starts in m. 166.
38
Schmalfeldt (1995), Hepokoski (2009), and Rothstein (2009) do not hear closure arriving until
m. 87; Caplin (2009a) instead hears those measures as a “post-cadential codetta.” Both hearings strike
me as reasonable.
39
Tovey 1931.
40
Cohn 2005 reads many of my “stock patterns” as “introverted motives” rather than schemas.
41
The ♮ 6̂–♭6̂–5̂ approach is a common developmental arrival.
42
Kinderman 2009b observes that the D minor lament anticipates the opening of the baritone
recitative in Beethoven’s Ninth Symphony.
43
Since each ascending-step modulation adds five flats, reinterpreting VI of the preceding minor
key as V of the next, the three sequential steps add fifteen total flats and complete a full circle in scale
space.
44
Skipping this tonicization is fairly standard, since the supertonic is diminished in minor.
45
Jander 1996 contextualizes the piece’s storm tropes.
46
Hepokoski 2009 notes this connection.
47
Schenker (1921–1923) 2004. Tovey (1935) 1981 snarkily observes that no piece was ever
derived from just four notes.
48
This same sort of gradual clarification characterizes the second theme of Op. 23, I (Figure 3.1.8),
the variations movement of Op. 1, no. 3 (§4.10), the variations movement of Op. 109 (§9.4), and the
idea of the “disguised model.”
49
If we disregard rotations, then there are four ways of arranging two harmonies in four bars:
AAAA, AAAB, AABB, and ABAB. All four appear on Figure 10.4.3. While I doubt that Beethoven
was consciously working through these possibilities, I do believe he was deliberately searching for
variety in the context of a limited harmonic vocabulary.
50
Lerdahl (2020, p. 89) makes a similar observation about the less-symmetrical opening music.
William Caplin would likely call this a compound sentence with two stages of fragmentation.
51
Schenker (1923–1924) 2005 notes that the dominant form of this arpeggiation gave Beethoven
particular difficulties.
52
Lewin 1986 colorfully describes this moment’s enharmonic play.
53
Riemann 1903, p. 38; Imbrie 1973. My own preference is for the Riemann/Imbrie reading,
though I think both are reasonable. Justin London (2004, p. 92) proposes yet another analysis in which
the second theme begins on a hypermetrical strong beat (m. 63). This disagreement suggests that
hypermeter can be fairly subjective. Temperley 2008 considers hypermetrical transitions in general.
54
In the transformation from horn call to violin melody, some harmonic notes become
nonharmonic; this leads Auerbach to dismiss the motivic connection as “invalid” (2021, p. 91). Like
Reti, I consider it to be significant (Reti 1951).
55
This near sequence often seems to appear in E♭, as in the second paragraph of the Eroica, the
second theme of the first movement of the C minor violin sonata, and the end of Op. 31, no. 3—
though it is also found in the first movement of the D major violin concerto. The shift from the
German augmented sixth to the fully diminished seventh chord, with the slightly odd ♭ 6̂–♮ 6̂–5̂ bass,
appears in a number of other pieces as well (e.g., Op. 10, no. 3, I, m. 199). Here ♭ 6̂ is “sublimated”
(Samarotto 2004).
56
Beethoven uses almost the same voice leading in the Eroica scherzo’s ascending-step sequence
(m. 143). Schenker (1923–1924) 2005 recognizes a connection between this moment and the closing
theme, but not its schematic identity.
57
B♭ minor continues the “down the ladder” portion of the schema one step too far.
58
The introduction to the second theme, which shifts from horns to bassoons, confronts conductors
with a delicate decision, as it is distinctly less powerful than the exposition; I might consider adding a
single horn to the bassoons, strengthening the texture while still preserving some timbral difference.
59
The last movement of Brahms’s C minor piano quartet Op. 60 also seems to separate the pitch
and rhythm of the Fifth Symphony’s opening notes.
60
Schenker reduces the coda’s upper voice to C–D–E♭, rather than the E♭–F–G of the schema; the
issues here are largely analogous to those surrounding the Op. 22 minuet (§9.4).
61
Sommer 1986 reads the form against the rhythm of repetition. Tovey 1931 is much closer to my
reading.
62
There is a cadence on the dominant of A in m. 59, an F♯ minor theme in m. 63, a weak A major
cadence in m. 70, a return to F ♯ minor in m. 71, and a half cadence on its dominant in m. 77.
Longyear and Covington (1988) compare this to “backing slowly into a parking space.” See also
Hepokoski and Darcy (2006, p. 120) on “tonally migratory” phrases.
63
The upper three voices move in doubly parallel motion, perhaps developing the fauxbourdon
opening; the chordal sevenths resolve up by step in the first and third measures.
64
Indeed, it was this passage that inspired Figure 6.1.3, which is as much Beethoven’s discovery as
it is mine. An analogous passage can be found in the closing theme of the first movement of the A
major sonata Op. 2, no. 2, there with V7 supporting 4̂.
65
It is also possible to hear a non-octave-repeating Ludwig taking D to D, though that reading cuts
against the rhythmic organization.
66
Thanks here to Yuqi Liang.
67
The outer voice round at m. 230 is reminiscent of m. 170 of the first movement of the Second
Symphony, which extends the pattern from triad to seventh chord.
68
See Grant 2018 for a helpful survey of Schubert’s three-part expositions, which generally
articulate similar trajectories. Barry 2014 considers whether the piece may be “Schubert’s most
extreme treatment of sonata form” (32); Webster 1978 suggests it is not in sonata form. Longyear and
Covington (1988) suggest that Beethoven’s Op. 28 is a precursor. Other insightful discussions include
Bruce 1969, Fieldman 2002, Smith 2006, Mak 2008, and Hunt 2014.
69
See Fieldman 2002 for a survey of these criticisms.
70
Webster 1978 notes that these quick transitions are characteristic of Schubert; Bruce 1969, Navia
2016, and Duane 2017 hear an earlier transition starting in m. 13. Smith 2006 considers the various
roles of D♭ major and the Neapolitan in the movement.
71
Here is a place where it is helpful to view a I64 chord as tonic, a functionally tonal analogue to
the harmonic 64 in Figure 5.2.1.
72
In Roman numerals, IV–I–V–ii–[I replacing vi]–V–ii–[I replacing vi]–V–I. The I–ii–V–I and I–
V–ii–I are four-chord double emploi progressions: in the more familiar five-chord double emploi (e.g.,
I–IV–ii–V–I or the pop-music I–V–ii–IV–I), we hear third-related chords in succession; in the four-
chord version, one such chord replaces another.
73
Duane 2017 uses computational analysis to argue that texture can signify thematicity in
Schubert’s expositions.
74
For example, §2.5 or §7.3. The last movement of Mozart’s E♭ major piano sonata, K.282, mm.
48–56, has a similar sequence, lowering the notes of a dominant seventh so that V7 becomes iv of the
key a step higher.
75
Richard Cohn (1999) has proposed that major-third-related key areas can serve similar
functional roles, implying that the second theme’s A♭ major could be a tonic substitute. I personally
hear A♭ major as a distinct key rather than a flavor of tonic, a hearing that comports with the broader
three-key practice surveyed in Grant 2018.
76
Grant 2018 describes a number of Schubertian three-key movements in which the thematic
material initially has a closing character that is retrospectively reinterpreted as thematic.
77
Many commentators have noted the motivic connection, including Beach 1994 and Barry 2014;
the schematic relationship is less frequently discussed.
78
Again, this bears comparison to the gradual revelation of structure we found in Beethoven.
79
The Neapolitan appears in the next sequential unit, which would continue ♭ V– ♭ II. For a rock
analogue see Led Zeppelin’s “Kashmir,” which similarly extends the 6–5 pattern beyond its standard
length. Peter Smith 2006 associates this moment with the prominent Neapolitan of the opening.
80
The others are the second phrase of the opening section and the G major theme.
81
Compare Figure 10.4.7.
82
Beach 1994 suggests that the B♭ key area is part of a movement-long C–B♭–A♭–G progression. I
do not place much aesthetic weight on these sorts of long-term pitch relations.
83
There is an extensive literature on Schubert’s borrowings, including Cone 1970, Edwards 1997,
and Humphreys 1997. Thanks here to René Rusch.
84
This kind of repositioning of the opening music appears famously appears in the first movement
of Mozart’s D major piano sonata K.311, I. Schubert makes a few other very small changes to the
recapitulation, including using minor chords in the second-theme Waldstein sequence.
85
Yust 2015a also notes the omnipresence of stepwise descending voice leading in Schubert,
though he analyzes it differently.
86
See Liszt 2016, pp. 117–18 (also found in Trippet 2010, p. 19), and Wagner 1907, pp. 232–33.
Kramer 2002 discusses the antisemitism of Wagner’s program and various efforts to resist this reading.
87
Deathridge 2008. The connection to the desert suggests that the antisemitism may have been
intrinsic to the music’s conception, rather than a late-breaking addition: for Wagner, the contemporary
world is an antispiritual Jewish-dominated desert.
88
The anticipations in m. 8 recall the third-based anticipations in the opening of Monteverdi’s
“Ohimé,” while softening the parallel fifths across the phrase break.
89
One could potentially hear this music as juxtaposing full cadences in E with half-cadences in A;
I, however, hear an E center throughout.
90
Kramer 2002 reads this final wedge as the second half of the fourth stanza; to me it sounds new.
91
Wagner’s symbolic use of register bears comparison with the end of Monteverdi’s “Cosi sol d’un
chiara fonte” (Madrigals, Book 8), where a Ludwig schema generates wide registral separation that
mirrors the words “I am so far from my salvation.”
92
Reynolds 2015; Gauldin 2004 explores chromatic wedges in Wagner’s later music.
93
In comparison to other Beethoven pieces, the Ninth Symphony does not strike me as being
heavily saturated with Ludwigs or other classical schemas; in fact, I suspect that its nonschematic
character represents its greatest influence on Wagner.
94
See Figure 4.1.4. Segall 2010 contains an interesting example from Schoenberg’s Verklärte
Nacht.
Conclusion
Past and Future
For centuries theorists have dreamed of a Platonic and extra-human music,
with listening the unwitting contemplation of ratio and number.1 There is a
lot wrong with this picture: to take it literally is to imagine that music could
be independent of physics, biology, culture, and history; yet the association
of ratio and consonance depends on the physics of vibrating objects, and our
pleasure in these relationships depends on our bodies and upbringing.
Platonism feeds the temptation to ignore complexity and detail, encouraging
us to think that nonharmonic tones must “stand for” their harmonic partners,
or that the return to the tonic key must create a sense of resolution, or that
beloved pieces must have an Urlinie. The very framework of rigid norms not
actually embodied in practice, of “geniuses” taking liberties with a priori
musical laws, reflects an urge to deny the human elements of music-making.
It is understandable if twenty-first-century musicians want to put Platonism
behind us.
Yet for all that it is not completely misguided. Around the world we can
hear musicians deploying an organizational strategy whose origins date to
the early Renaissance: surface voices moving inside chords that move inside
scales that are themselves moving inside the aggregate. The spiral diagrams
provide an elegant tool for visualizing this structure, a unified set of
geometrical models revealing the interdependence of harmony and
counterpoint: wheels within wheels, each turning independently of the
others. Styles can be characterized, in part, by the settings of this complex
machine: in a consonant, diatonic, and imitative environment, with little
constraint on harmonic progression, it can produce the transcendent stasis of
high-Renaissance polyphony; in a more homophonic and slightly less
diatonic context, the hedonic yawp of AC/DC. Augmented by a complex
harmonic grammar, we can approach the Newtonian clockwork of a Bach
prelude, or the Romantic subjectivity of a Beethoven sonata. With extended-
tertian sonorities and an expanded modal vocabulary, we move toward
impressionism, jazz, and minimalism. No doubt new settings remain to be
discovered, new ways of recombining the same basic ingredients.
The collectional hierarchy can be the explicit object of attention, as when
musicians move motives along both chord and scale, or when surface voices
ascend against a descending background—as in the Waldstein or the
Revolutionary Etude. More subtle are pieces like “Eight Days a Week,” Heu
me Domine, or “Helpless,” where voices might ascend along a continually
descending scale in order to remain stationary—or those moments where
composers exploit dyadic rather than triadic logic. For here the quadruple
hierarchy can arise not because composers are paying attention to it, but
because musical culture has settled upon idioms that exploit its unique
characteristics. This is most obviously true of those countless embodied
routines that produce efficient voice leading by combining similar operations
at multiple hierarchical levels: big-T little-t transposition, or in the case of
Gesualdo’s trick, big-I little-i inversion (appendix 3). Here the quadruple
hierarchy is like a collective unconscious, supplying hidden and non-obvious
structure to a vast range of practices and techniques. Some parts of this
structure are easily visible, like the tip of an iceberg; others lurk far below
the musical surface.
In its full generality, the collectional hierarchy lies beyond the horizon of
our intuition: there are few if any musicians today who can sit down at their
instrument, choose some arbitrary three-note subset of some arbitrary seven-
note scale, and fluently manipulate the resulting possibilities. Instead, we
tend to internalize the more limited set of musical routines relevant to a
particular style. Over the course of this book, we have identified theoretical
origins for nine of the eleven schemas listed in the appendix of Gjerdingen’s
Music of the Galant Style: dyadic space for the Quiescenza, Fonte, and
Monte (§3.1), triadic space for the Prinner and Romanesca (§3.4), a canonic
fourth-progression for the Fenaroli (Figure 4.5.6), and basic sentential logic
for the Meyer, Do-Re-Mi, and Sol-Fa-Me (§9.1).2 Such patterns are not
isolated idioms specific to the galant period but reflections of very general
voice-leading possibilities known to composers in many genres. This tracing
of schemas to abstract geometrical structure underwrites my claim that
musical knowledge is often implicit, known in ways that may not be directly
accessible to conscious self-reflection. For it is often the case that there are
only a small number of ways to achieve basic musical goals, such as
harmonizing stepwise descending melodies using closely related triads.
Given enough time, musicians will eventually discover these possibilities,
thereby internalizing a kind of geometrical knowledge without realizing it.
Culture will preserve these discoveries, transmitting them as important
idioms or musical figures of speech. Perhaps a particular style will favor
some possibilities to the point where they become markers of its sound-
world. Listeners, having internalized these routines, may develop a sense for
that core vocabulary even without being able to express their knowledge in
words.
Many chapters of this story remain untold. In Bebop and later jazz styles,
the descending voice leading of the diatonic fifth-sequence is chromaticized,
allowing roots to descend by semitone, tritone, or fifth. Here the 2-in-12
diagram of chromatic dyads can model both root progressions and upper-
voice pairs (Figure C1). Charlie Parker’s “Blues for Alice” uses this
chromaticized space to reimagine the blues as a sleek machine, the chord-
roots making three complete turns through the 2-in-12 diagram, moving
radially or by clockwise step, with upper-voice fifths always sliding
downward (Figure C2).3 This is an ancient musical logic, part of a dialogue
beginning before Ockeghem’s “generalized fauxbourdon” and still audible in
Neil Young’s “Helpless” (§2.3, §3.4, §7.3). From this point of view, Bebop is
more Romantic than modernist; for rather than rejecting functional
convention, Bebop musicians complexified it, adding new stories to the
building rather than razing it to the ground.
Figure C1. Bebop harmony extends the circle of dyadic fifths by allowing tritone substitution. This
replaces the 2-in-7 diatonic spiral with the 2-in-12 chromatic spiral. The lower diagram can be read
either as referring to root progressions or to upper-voice pairs connected by efficient voice leading.
Figure C2. The harmonic structure of Charlie Parker’s “Blues for Alice.” The numbers above the staff
refer to motion on the previous example’s 2-in-12 circle. Upper voices include the important tones in
Parker’s melody.
1
Both Leibniz and Thelonious Monk described music as a kind of unconscious mathematics
(Rohrmeier 2020).
2
See appendix 3 for another perspective on both the Fenaroli and the “horn fifths” schemas.
3
My diagram factors the upper voices into fifth-related pairs such as third-seventh, fifth-ninth, and
so on. When these pairs are related by chromatic transposition (e.g., two tritones or two fifths) they
can be modeled by the diagram in Figure C1. In other cases, we have to postulate that upper voices
move within a diatonic field whose modulations are directed by the roots.
4
See §1.1, appendix 3, and Tymoczko 2023.
5
Particularly important here is the development of musicxml, a cross-platform format for
representing musical scores, and music21, a software package for analyzing and creating scores
(Cuthbert and Ariza 2010).
APPENDIX 1
Fundamentals
Figure A1.1. Inversion occurring at both the triadic and chromatic levels. The two inversions preserve
the same pair of notes and combine to produce an efficient voice leading.
Formally, a voice leading can be defined as a collection of paths in pitch-
class space, measured along some scale. Voice leadings are an important
component of implicit musical knowledge, representing possible routes from
one chord to another. We can write (C, E, G) (C, F, A), meaning “C,
whatever octave it is in, stays fixed, while E, whatever octave it is in, moves
up by semitone, and G, whatever octave it is in, moves up by two
semitones.” This voice leading is measured in chromatic space; measured
Figure A1.7. The cross section of three-note chord space. Each chord appears in all three of its modes;
“mode 1” is an ordering with the smallest interval between the first two notes. Each boundary acts
geometrically like a mirror, exchanging voices’ pitches and labels: if voice 1 has C and voice 2 has E,
then exchanging them sends voice 1 to E and relabels it voice 2, while sending voice 2 to C and
relabeling it voice 1.
Figure A1.8. A simplified version of Beethoven’s Op. 109 variations theme, with its voice leadings
analyzed into three components: voice crossings (cxy, with x and y identifying the crossed voices),
transposition along the diatonic scale (Tx), and transposition along the triad (tx). Underneath the
example, I give a (diatonic) set-theoretical analysis, consisting solely in the observation that every
chord is a diatonic triad (024). Beneath that, I identify the passage’s set-class voice leadings, obtained
by omitting the T component. The second staff of music shows an alternate realization of these same
set-class voice leadings, keeping the E major harmony fixed. This music is related by the Tinctoris
Transform to Beethoven’s original. For clarity, I separate out the two components of the “c23t1” voice
leading. When voices cross, they exchange numbers, so that c21 exchanges the notes in the first and
second voices while also renumbering them accordingly. Below, I graph the music in set class space.
1
These objects differ from the pitch-class intervals of earlier music theory in distinguishing
multiple routes between the same points: thus “start at G and move down by four semitones” is
different from “start at G and move up by eight semitones,” even though both connect G to E♭. See
Tymoczko 2011a and 2023.
2
The opening of Schoenberg’s Op. 36 violin concerto is a nice example, moving from F♯2, B2,
D♯3, E3, A3, B♭3 to the inversionally related A♭2, D3, F3, G3, C4, D♭4, with both hexachords spaced
(3, 1, 1, 2, 1) in chordal steps. The registral distance between the three-note chromatic cluster is
preserved.
3
See http://www.madmusicalscience.com/quadruple.html.
4
A subtlety: if we think of chords as multisets with specific doublings, then (C, C, E, G) → (A, C,
F, A) could be understood as a bijective voice leading from C-with-doubled-root to F-with-doubled-
third (Callender, Quinn, and Tymoczko 2008, Tymoczko 2011a).
5
This conception of “voice exchange” is minimal and technical, referring to a specific type of
voice leading. Schenkerian theory uses a more robust notion (Cutler 2009).
6
See Tymoczko 2008 and 2011a. In my earlier work I used scalar and interscalar interval matrices
to represent these voice leadings; the spiral diagrams provide a geometrical representation of these
algebraic objects.
7
If we take 7-in-12 space to represent diatonic modes, then angular neighbors are modally similar:
for instance, a one-step clockwise move changes C ionian to C mixolydian.
8
I sometimes treat doubling as part of chordal identity (so that, for example, CGG and CCG lie on
different rings of Figure 3.5.3), representing changes of spacing by curved arrows.
9
Headlam 2012 criticized voice-leading geometry on the grounds that it could not accommodate
cardinality equivalence or incomplete chords. I think that the hierarchical picture allows us to model
many of these phenomena.
10
For example, Figure P5.7 when the bass sounds D. The upper voices continue to move along the
three-note subset.
11
Callender 1998.
12
Tymoczko 2020b.
APPENDIX 2
Figure A2.1. (a) Three-dimensional helices each containing the transpositions of an individual mode.
(b) When viewed from above these produce the spiral diagrams.
Figure A2.2. The modes of (C, G). Perfect fifths sum to 7 + 24i where i is an integer, while perfect
fourths sum to 19 + 24i. As the sum increases by 12, we move from one mode to the other, hopping
between the helices in the previous example.
Both recipes exploit the fact that musical space is structured like
wallpaper, composed of many equivalent regions containing different
versions of the same basic objects (e.g., chords or set classes, illustrated
metaphorically by Figure A2.5). We can represent music using any one of
these regions, each of which contains just one point for each object (except
at their boundaries, which contain multiple points representing the same
object). As we move off a region’s boundary, we either “bounce off” a
mirrorlike point, or “teleport” between boundary points representing the
same musical object. In either case, we have to apply some musical
transformation to all the coordinates in our region. This is because we have
entered a new region of pitch-space that is equivalent to the first but contains
different pitches (e.g., reordered and transposed by octave, relative to the
previous region). It follows that geometrical boundaries can be identified
with specific musical transformations, and hence that geometrical modeling
can illuminate musically significant operations such as transposition along
the chord.
Figure A2.5. Musical space is like a piece of wallpaper, constructed out of many similar regions. Any
path in this larger space can be represented in a single region, though paths may appear to “bounce
off” certain boundary points (β), or “teleport” between others (α). These boundaries can be associated
with specific musical transformations.
Figure A2.6. Representing music using a one-octave region of chromatic pitch space. Within the
region, paths can be determined by subtracting start-coordinate from end-coordinate; when moving off
the boundary, one first has to apply a transformation to all coordinates.
For two-note chords, we begin with the Cartesian plane representing all
ordered pairs of chromatic pitches. The square at the center of Figure A2.7 is
defined by the equations 12 ≤ x + y ≤ 24, and 0 ≤ y – x ≤ 12. Every ordered
pair of pitches is equivalent to either a single point in the square’s interior or
a pair of points on its boundary.4 Figure A2.8 rotates the square and labels
the points representing unisons, major thirds, and their inversions. Points on
the left edge (summing to 12) are related by t1 to points on the right
(summing to 24): for example, the left-edge sixth (2, 10) is duplicated as the
right-edge third (10, 14), and the left-edge third (4, 8) is duplicated as the
right-edge sixth (8, 16).5 According to Recipe 2, moving off the right
boundary requires us to apply t1 to every point in the space, while moving
off the left boundary requires us to apply t–1. (The top and bottom
boundaries meanwhile reorder each dyad while perhaps transposing notes by
octave.6) Figure A2.8 uses curved lines to represent the process of
“teleportation,” but this is merely a graphical contrivance that does not
substantively alter the figure. The result is recognizable, modulo a little
stretching and twisting, as the spiral diagram for chromatic major thirds and
minor sixths.
Figure A2.7. A derivation of two-dimensional chord space. The square is bounded by the lines y = x
(unisons), y = x + 12 (octaves), x + y = 12, and x + y = 24. The two lines y = x + 4 and y = x + 8
contain major thirds and minor sixths respectively; these are the two strands of the spiral. The dashed
arrows represent transposition up by one step along the chord, sending root up to third and third up to
root; these link chords on the x + y = 12 face with their equivalents on the x + y = 24 face.
Figure A2.8. The region shown in the previous figure, rotated by 45 degrees. For ease of reading, I
have shown only the unisons, octaves, major thirds, and minor sixths. Transposition moves
horizontally, while chords lying on a vertical line sum to the same value. Chords on the left boundary
relate by t1 to chords on the right boundary; when we move off the right to reappear on the left, we
need to apply t1 to all the points in the region.
Figure A2.9. Two equations that determine the region representing chord space for chords of any size:
the first determines a “generalized triangle” containing all set classes whose notes are in order while
spanning no more than an octave; the second determines the circular dimension whose coordinate is
given by the sum of chords’ pitch classes. Here c is an arbitrary constant. In three voices with an
octave of size 12, and choosing c as 0, the equations are x1 ≤ x2 ≤ x3 ≤ x1 + 12 and 0 ≤ x1 + x2 + x3 ≤
12.
Figure A2.10. Chromatic major triads lie on three line segments in the region determined by the
equations in Figure A2.9. This region is three-dimensional, though here I draw it in two dimensions.
Chords on the same vertical line sum to the same value. Chords on the left boundary are related by t1
to chords on the right. Recipe 2 tells us that as we move off the right boundary we need to apply t1 to
every chord in the region.
Figure A2.11. Dominant seventh chords lie on four line segments in the region determined by the
equations in Figure A2.9. This region is intrinsically four-dimensional. Each line contains a different
mode of the dominant seventh. Chords on the same vertical line sum to the same value. Chords on the
left boundary are related by t1 to chords on the right. Recipe 2 tells us that as we move off the right
boundary, reappearing on the left, we need to apply t1 to every chord in the region.
The spiral diagrams come in three flavors (Figure A2.12). When the size
of the chord n divides the size of the scale o, then there are n distinct chord-
forms sharing the same angular position, linked by the transposition o/n. In
this case there is a diagonal voice leading (To/nt–1) that moves chords
radially.8 By repeating it, we can generate all the spacing-preserving
leadings between chords at the same angular position, passing through each
of the chord’s inversions or modes and eventually returning each note to its
starting pitch. When the size of the chord is instead relatively prime to the
size of the scale, there is exactly one chord at each and every radial position;
here a basic voice leading moves chords by one angular position, generating
by repeated application all the voice leadings on the spiral.9 The third type of
graph occurs when the size of the chord shares a common factor with the
size of the scale; in this case, there are between 1 and n chords sharing the
same angular position. We can think of these spaces as having both a
diagonal voice leading (which moves radially, passing through some subset
of the modes before returning each voice to its starting position) and a basic
voice leading (which connects angular neighbors).
Figure A2.12. The three kinds of spiral diagram. (left) When the size of the chord (n) evenly divides
the size of the scale (o) we have n copies of each chord sharing the same angular position; the
diagonal action D moves inward or outward by one radial unit and generates all the voice leadings
connecting chords at the same angular position; these combine with chromatic transpositions (T) to
generate the remaining voice leadings. (middle) When the size of the chord is relatively prime to the
size of the scale, no two transpositions share the same angular position; here the basic voice leading B
moves chords by one angular unit; repeated applications of this basic voice leading generate all the
voice leadings on the figure. (right) When the size of the chord shares a common factor with the size
of the scale, there is both a diagonal action and a basic voice leading.
1
Thanks to Nori Jacoby, who helped me work out the multiple-helix derivation.
2
If we want distance in the xy plane to represent voice-leading distance, we need the n-fold spiral
arrangement used in the book. This is because To/nt–1 is small for nearly even n-note chords.
3
This double helix is the two-note analogue to Roger Shepard’s helical representation of pitches
(Shepard 1964).
4
Imagine a pair of pitch classes x and y: you can always arrange them in pitch so that one is less
than an octave above the other, satisfying 0 ≤ y – x ≤ 12. Since transposition along the chord increases
or decreases their sum by 12, repeated transpositions will eventually satisfy 12 ≤ x + y ≤ 24, producing
either a unique point in the interior or a pair of points on the boundaries. The two quantities x + y and y
– x are the two Tinctorian coordinates from the prelude to chapter 3.
5
In each case, the right-edge duplicate has the left edge’s second pitch as its first pitch, and the
octave-transposition of the left edge’s first pitch as its second pitch.
6
The bottom boundary swaps a dyad’s notes; the top boundary swaps them but also transposes the
first note up by octave and the second note down by octave. These octave shifts change as we move
from region to region.
7
For details see Tymoczko 2020b.
8
The adjective “diagonal” comes from mathematics, reflecting the fact that a single group acts
simultaneously as a transposition along both chord and scale.
9
Hook 2008 and 2011 was the first to explore the basic voice leading, beginning with the case of
the diatonic scale and eventually extending this idea to other seven-note collections. He also
introduced the big-T, little-t notation used in this book. Tymoczko 2008 and 2011a explored the
general combination of transposition along both chord and scale, but had no way to represent these
relationships geometrically.
APPENDIX 3
Figure A3.8. Contrary motion in the string parts at R70 of The Rite of Spring. (a) Descending seconds
against ascending major thirds, forming a series of diminished seventh chords. (b) The same technique
applied to nearly symmetrical dominant seventh chords.
Figure A3.9. Jazz sequences in which the upper voices descend semitonally while the bass descends
by fifth. As long as the initial scale contains the tritone transposition of the initial bass note, then the
resulting harmonies will always belong to one of its transpositions.
Figure A3.10. Here the left-hand voicings ascend by two semitones while the right-hand voicings
descend by one semitone; because the voicing forms an octatonic subset, the resultant harmonies are
octatonic as well. The entire passage expresses a sequence of descending-fifth harmonies.
Figure A3.11. At 2’57” in “Passion Dance,” McCoy Tyner plays four-note pentatonic subsets in his
right hand, moving in doubly parallel motion along both chromatic and pentatonic scales; the left hand
plays chromatic fourth chords. Though his two hands move independently and in contrary motion, the
music articulates diatonic subsets. This need not require deliberate planning as it tends to happen
naturally. Compare Figure 1.1.8.
Figure A3.13. We can obtain greater harmonic variety by transposing each successive unit.
Figure A3.14. Measures 15–18 of Bach’s first two-part invention present a near sequence combining
transposition and inversion (the music is in the lower system of Figure 7.7.5). The transposition moves
the upper voice downward by 10 and 11 diatonic steps in alternation; the inversion moves the top note
of the original sixteenth-note figure up by octave to become the bottom note of the inverted figure.
Figure A3.18. Two analyses of the sequence in Figure 1.1.7, the first using transposition and the
second using inversion.
Figure A3.21. Using unstated chord tones to transpose an arbitrary collection along the C major triad.
Here B and D are interpreted relative to tacit chordal degrees.
1
Taneyev (1909) 1962 discusses displacements in time as well as pitch.
2
This canon repeats after nine measures, but we can choose not to observe the repeat. The example
comes from Noam Elkies, who notes that the use of multiple temporal delays is unusual; most canons
involve just a single temporal delay. See also Gauldin 1996.
3
The figure’s Petrushka analysis originates in a collaboration with Rachel Hall and Jason Yust,
never published because I felt dissatisfied with its formalism; in retrospect, I was looking for
something like the generalized transformational theory described here (Hall, Tymoczko, and Yust
2010).
4
Horizontal arrows are generally more flexible than vertical arrows. In Figure A3.4, we cannot
analyze the Stravinsky passage using arrows as the upper intervals are three times as large as the lower
intervals; in the Bartók passage, we can use a vertical inversional arrow, as the two parts move in
exact contrary motion. Vertical inversion arrows are characteristic of “K-nets” (Klumpenhouwer 1991
and Lewin 1990).
5
This “quantization” operation is essentially the mathematical floor function: it leaves notes in the
B minor triad untouched, sending any other note downward until it reaches a note in the B minor triad.
Avoiding octaves thus requires diatonic transposition by eight rather than seven steps: F♯5 quantizes
to F♯5, but Pärt harmonizes melodic F♯6 with D5 rather than F♯5.
6
Ligeti borrowed this technique fifty years later in “Monument.”
7
Russell 2018. Early Stravinsky often moves instrumental lines relative to surface voice leading,
as in the opening bassoon parts of The Firebird or R43 of The Rite of Spring. This orchestrational
habit may have led him to consider how the technique could be used in more obviously contrapuntal
ways. In Figure A3.7, Stravinsky moves the string parts within the augmented triads, forming diatonic
melodies. This is not how the passage appears in the original ballet or 1911 suite; it may have been
recomposed after The Rite of Spring.
8
Alternatively one could apply this sort of thinking within the chord, for instance by playing a two-
step interval as measured within a four-note chord (i.e. notes 1–3 or 2–4). The two voices can then be
moved by either an even or odd number of tetrachordal steps to produce subsets of just two harmonies
(notes 1–3 or 2–4). Versions of this technique are used in the Fenaroli and “horn fifths” schemas, as
well as a Prinner variant (cf. Figures 4.5.6, 4.6.1–2, and 4.8.6).
9
The Rite of Spring is full of interesting contrary-motion patterns, for instance at R44 + 4 (seventh
chords in contrary motion forming large octatonic subsets), R60 (triads moving in contrary motion in
winds and trumpets), throughout the “Sacrificial Dance” (e.g., R192–R194, upper voices against
bassoons, or R196, trombones moving down octatonically while winds ascend along the B♭ minor and
C major triads forming a nearly octatonic composite). See Russell 2018.
10
Stuckenschmidt 1965. Here again contrary motion functions to produce harmonic expansion.
11
Nine of the twelve superimpositions of three-note fourth chords produce a diatonic subset.
Different patterns of left-and-right hand motion are more or less likely to preserve diatonicism.
Suppose, for example, left and right hand are each playing three-note fourth chords. If they are close
together on the circle of fifths, they will belong to the same diatonic or pentatonic collection. If one
hand moves up by three semitones while the other moves down by two, their relative position on the
circle of fifths will change by only one; by contrast, if one hand stays fixed and the other moves by
semitone, their configuration changes by 5. The latter change is more likely to turn a diatonic subset
nondiatonic.
12
See https://www.madmusicalscience.com/robots.html.
13
Since the index numbers are related by octave, this collection of arrows is harmonically stable,
preserving the pitch-class intervals between lines.
14
Compare m. 61 of the first A minor fugue in The Well-Tempered Clavier.
15
Crossed Tx and Ix arrows describe sequences using symmetry operations that can apply to
extended passages of music; dual transformations like Txty and Ixiy describe sequences using voice
leadings that apply to just a single chord. The latter description has the advantage of being insensitive
to a chord’s registral position and internal intervallic structure. Thus it is often useful to go back and
forth between the two. We could, for example, convert a neo-Riemannian voice leading into a large-
unit sequence by writing down an instance of that voice leading, describing it with crossed Ix arrows,
and using these arrows to form a larger sequence. See Tymoczko 2020b on the difference between the
symmetry group and the homotopy group.
16
These patterns are nondualist since both major and minor chords descend; a dualist sequence
would move them in opposite directions (Tymoczko 2020b).
17
This piece is the subject of two extended analyses by David Lewin (1981, 1987, pp. 128ff) and a
third by Jack Boss (2019). Lewin’s second analysis prefigures his later interest in K-nets by analyzing
contrary motion using vertical inversional arrows. I prefer horizontal transpositional arrows instead.
18
I do not provide any notation for the pattern’s absolute transpositional level. Voice leadings
conforming to the schema are related by the Tinctoris Transform.
19
Ethan Haimo suggests that the basic objects of this music are more elastic than set classes,
neighborhoods containing set classes related by small “perturbations” (i.e., efficient voice leadings);
see Haimo 1996 and Tymoczko 2020b.
20
In other words, the augmented triad (B, E♭, G) allows for an ascending-step melody with both
Viennese chords that follow it, the (A, D, A♭) in m. 7 and its near-transposition down by minor sixth,
the distorted (D♭, F = G♭, C) in m. 8. In both cases, the ascending registral semitone is audible in the
music (G5–A♭5 and B4–C5). The idea of elided inversion is useful at the end of the piece, connecting
the final chord of sequence δ to the return of α.
21
Traditional transformational theory often focuses on disembodied pitch classes, assigned to
octave and instrument according to unformalized compositional whim (Lewin 1987).
22
Tymoczko 2011a, chapter 4.
APPENDIX 4
Though it has one foot in the creative arts and the other in the sciences,
music theory has been somewhat slow to embrace empirical methods.
Figures such as David Huron and Fred Lerdahl have tried to push the field
in that direction, the former influenced by empirical psychology, the latter
by the introspective methods of Chomskian linguistics. But I think it is fair
to say that many theorists do not consider their profession to be even
broadly continuous with science. Those who do tend to follow Huron in
adopting the language and style of psychological discourse: formalized
theories, papers written as experiments, and null-hypothesis significance
testing, often in a context of skepticism toward traditional music-theoretical
verities. In this book I have instead chosen to adopt a more traditional style,
for three reasons.
The first, I admit, is constitutional: I believe I will have more fun and
reach more readers if I speak somewhat informally and leave out the
significance tests. After all, corpus analysis can be a powerful tool for
traditional scholarship—as it was for both Knud Jeppesen and Allen Irvine
McHose.1 Having programmed a computer to identify non-harmonic tones,
I discovered the Renaissance sevenths when they caused my algorithm to
fail, at which point I was able to theorize about them in a relatively
traditional way. Similarly, having programmed a computer to identify
cadences in Renaissance music (operationalized as two voices converging
stepwise on an octave or unison with one decorated by a suspension), I was
able to search hundreds of pieces for patterns like that at the bottom of
Figure 6.3.5. Here the computer pointed me toward examples whose
analytical significance was intuitively clear. In such cases, machines act as
virtual assistants, increasing our access to musical examples. Humans can
take advantage of these tools without adopting the language of statistical
science.
A second reason is that large effects can often be seen without
sophisticated statistics. My Bach-chorale analyses contain 45 instances of
I–X–I6, where X is a single chord over bass scale-degree 2̂; 34 of these are
I–vii°6–I6 progressions, 10 are I–V43–I6, and only 1 is I–ii–I6. Meanwhile
the Mozart piano sonatas contain about 115 progressions of this form
without a single indisputable I–ii–I6. Such regularities speak for themselves.
Statistics are necessary when we are dealing with subtle effects not obvious
to the naked eye—a new drug that slightly decreases the length of an
illness, an early-childhood intervention that slightly improves test scores a
decade later, and so on.2 The musical behaviors I care about tend to be
considerably more obvious than this. And even when we are dealing with
subtle topics, such as the gradual increase in root-position fifth-progressions
over the course of the sixteenth century, we can often observe the relevant
phenomena analytically, once we have been alerted to them by corpus study
(§6.5).
The final issue is that there are significant methodological questions that
need to be resolved before we can profit from rigorous statistical
methodology. Consider David Huron’s treatment of the ii–vii°6 idiom,
discussed in connection with Figure P7.1. Huron considers two possible
analyses of the passage in Figure A4.1, neither of which I endorse; the best
analysis, in my view, recognizes two eighth-note chords rather than one
quarter-note harmony.3 Huron’s analysis of this idiom underwrites his claim
that more than 20% of Bach’s supertonic chords progress to tonics—a
number that exceeds my own by a factor of at least 10.4 Or consider the
Tempest’s m. 11 G–B ♭ –C ♯ –E chord, which Hepokoski and Martin call a
“ct°7/iv” and which Hatten and I consider a vii°7/V (m. 11, shown in Figure
10.3.4). These cases show us that simple Roman-numeral labeling,
sometimes disparaged as “trivial transliteration of note content,” requires a
nontrivial sense of a composer’s vocabulary. The paradox of analysis is that
we have to know what we’re looking for in order to find it.
Figure A4.1. Three analyses of the sixth measure of Bach’s “Liebster Jesu, wir sind hier” (BWV 373,
Riemenschneider 328). The first two are considered by David Huron; the third is my preferred
analysis.
For these reasons, I have decided to follow the philosophy of open data—
writing in a relatively informal mode but releasing all my code and data so
that readers can reproduce and criticize my results.5 Those who worry about
my subjective choices can reanalyze my data, for instance automatically
labeling all the “nonharmonic consonances” in the Bach chorales, or
removing any modulations I have introduced into Renaissance pieces.
Readers will no doubt discover errors in both my analyses and my code.
But a policy of openness allows others not only to correct those mistakes
but also to use my data for work I have not anticipated. My hope is that this
combination of humanistic writing and data transparency will satisfy both
nonspecialists who are primarily interested in high-level conclusions and
quantitative researchers who are more interested in the fine details.
This data serves as the input to musical “grammars,” or theories of what
does or does not typically happen in a particular musical genre. In this
book, I have simultaneously tried to bolster traditional theories while
identifying their limits. The first effort involves, for example, the
suggestion that traditional theories of nonharmonicity work reasonably well
in Renaissance, baroque, and classical music, allowing us to identify
consonant harmonies decorated by familiar nonharmonic formulae (§5.1–
§5.4); the proposal that the descending-third arrangement of triads, from I
down to V, captures the most common progressions in functional music
(§7.1–§7.3); and the claim that sequential progressions are harmonically
significant (§7.6). Such proposals are meant to counter skeptics who want
to replace traditional theory with something radically different. The second
part of the project acknowledges the schema-theoretical point that music is
fundamentally idiomatic, reconceiving traditional precepts not as
exceptionless laws but as largely true generalizations. We cannot expect that
every passage in every piece will conform to those generalizations, nor
even that there will be any explanation for anomalous occurrences.
Sometimes exceptions happen.
It is a truism of linguistics that grammaticality is not the same as
frequency: that there can be perfectly grammatical sentences that have never
been uttered, and are unlikely ever to be: Noam Chomsky famously gave
the example of the rare-but-grammatical “colorless green ideas sleep
furiously.” Is it wrong to link “grammaticality” with frequency in the
musical case?
My first answer is that Chomsky’s example was meant to highlight the
rarity of particular sequences of words rather than sequences of abstract
grammatical categories. The extremely rare sentence “colorless green ideas
sleep furiously” corresponds to the very common sequence <ADJECTIVE>
<ADJECTIVE> <NOUN> <VERB> <ADVERB>. If we translate this
point to the musical domain, then the analogues to word-sequences are
something like actual segments of musical notation, with Roman numeral
sequences like V–IV being analogous to abstract sequences like <NOUN>
<ADJECTIVE>. And at the level of grammatical categories, the question
about frequency and grammaticality is by no means resolved. There are
some sequences, like <NOUN> <NOUN> <NOUN> <VERB> <VERB>
<VERB>, that are almost unknown in English (e.g., “people cats dogs like
hate sigh,” a description of the sadness expressed by people who are hated
by those cats that are liked by dogs). Some linguists do take the rarity of
this abstract sequence to indicate that it is indeed ungrammatical, while
others consider the form to be grammatical in an idealized sense.
Second, the linguistic divergence between frequency and grammaticality
is underwritten by semantics: though we might initially be baffled by
“people cats dogs like hate sigh,” we can upon reflection extract its
meaning. (Even with “colorless green ideas sleep furiously” we can answer
questions like: “were the ideas awake?” or “how did they sleep?”6) By
contrast, it is much harder to extract any meaning from “like hate sigh
people cats dogs.” Lacking semantics, it is unclear whether music supports
any analogous distinction. If asked “is root-position I–V–IV–I a
grammatical chord progression?” I can say only that it is common in rock
and rare in classical music; in that sense, it is “grammatical in,” or
“characteristic of,” the one style but not the other. At the same time, were I
to hear a I–V–IV–I progression in classical music, I would register it as
unusual without experiencing any further difficulties.7 In particular, I have
no experience of being able to “comprehend” I–V–IV–I in rock but not
classical music; instead, I have the expectation that the progression occurs
in some styles but not others. Violating these expectations leads to mild
surprise rather than genuine perplexity.
It might be possible to separate frequency and acceptability were
classical music a living tradition. For suppose two Viennese classical-era
composers disagreed about whether the I–ii–I6 progression was
grammatical in their shared language: if one were to compose successful
pieces making use of the progression, and if these pieces were accepted by
the community as exemplars of the classical style (rather than archaic
evocations of earlier music), then this would provide evidence that the
progressions were grammatical despite their infrequency. It is not clear that
composition can still function in this way, as contemporary ears are no
longer capable of delivering eighteenth-century judgments. For suppose that
a charismatic music theorist constructed a theory implying that I–ii–I6 was
acceptable (say, because the supertonic was “merely passing”); and suppose
that theorist’s followers began composing mock-classical music featuring
extensive I–ii–I6 progressions.8 It seems to me that this would tell us less
about the classical style than about the contemporary theoretical
community. The question is not what we think about classical harmony but
what its native speakers thought.9
For all these reasons, I think it is best to understand musical grammars as
interpretive summaries of statistical tendencies. That is, they aspire to
provide a useful description of musical behavior as being “standard” or
“unusual” to varying degrees. (While the simplest grammars might use just
two categories, we have seen that it is often useful to adopt a more fine-
grained picture in which standardness comes in degrees.10) This sorting
serves a variety of practical functions. For the analyst, it directs attention
toward those features of a piece that are genuinely exceptional, preventing
us from wasting time “explaining” common occurrences. For the student,
trying to compose in the style, it can provide suggestions about what might
come next. For the theorist, it can point toward hypotheses about why
musical practice might have developed as it did (e.g., that mature
functionality fleshes out the simpler I–IV–V template with a series of
descending-third progressions [§7.2]). For the cognitive scientist, it can hint
at the mental representations that constitute competence in a musical style.
My word “interpretive” is meant to serve a double function. First, it
registers the reality that we can construct multiple theories accounting for
the same set of observable facts. For instance, I have proposed that the
progression I–ii–I6 is ungrammatical or unusual in mature functional music;
I explain most of the exceptions as instances of the pseudochord idiom
(§7.5).11 Others may assert that I–ii–I6 is grammatical, proposing auxiliary
explanations that restrict its appearance to the “pseudochord” idiom. This
kind of pluralism, or “underdetermination,” is endemic to science, where
equivalent theories often account for the same observations.
The word “interpretive” also points to the holistic nature of the
grammatical enterprise. Consider once again the interdependence of
harmonic labeling, nonharmonic-tone identification, and harmonic
theorizing discussed in the prelude to chapter 7. This holism gives us yet
another reason to be cautious about the prospects for a simple approach to
corpus study—and in particular about the possibility of theory-neutral
datasets of analyzed music. For suppose it is true that the identification of
nonharmonic tones needs to be carried out on the basis of expectations
about what is common in a particular repertoire; if so, then one of the basic
canons of scientific method, the separation of data collection and hypothesis
formation, may be inappropriate. If analysis is theory-dependent, then it is
circular—though not necessarily viciously so.
Music theory is a difficult discipline, requiring equal comfort with the
two cultures of art and science, and on occasion the third culture of
philosophical reasoning as well. It is also an activity of diminishing cultural
prestige, once fit to inhabit the Quadrivium, now demoted to the study of a
relatively marginal aspect of human behavior—extremely marginal if we
concern ourselves with notated composition. If the new era of
computational corpus study exacerbates the first problem, requiring
theorists to master yet another complex skill, it ameliorates the second,
putting music theory in dialogue with computer science, information theory,
linguistics, 3D visualization, and cognitive science. To me this is yet
another reason to be encouraged about its future.
1
Working before computers, both scholars had to gather data separately for each music-
theoretical question—an earlier study of parallel fifths would not facilitate a later study of root
motions, for example. One of the main virtues of computers is that they allow us to ask multiple
questions of the same data.
2
There is a “replication crisis” currently unfolding across the social and biomedical sciences,
sometimes described as an epidemic of untrustworthy and implausible results. See Simmons, Nelson,
and Simonsohn 2011. These difficulties provide another reason to focus on larger effects.
3
Huron 2007b.
4
Huron 2007a, p. 266. The other issue is Huron’s decision to consider I64 a standard tonic rather
than an anomalous cadential sonority, hierarchically dependent on the following dominant (§9.5). He
and I once had a chance to examine his data, determining that a large proportion of his ii–I
progressions belonged to these two categories.
5
Readers can go to https://www.madmusicalscience.com/taom to download my code, data, and a
python program allowing them to reproduce my quantitative examples, as well as the other programs
described in the text.
6
See Camp 2004. The issue is complicated, as there are semantically defective sentences that are
judged grammatical (e.g., “more people have been to Russia than I have,” Wellwood et al. 2018).
7
For example, there are retrofunctional moments in Brahms (chapter 7, footnote 29) and Bach:
“Nun freut euch, lieben Christen g’mein” (BWV 388, Riemenschneider 183), mm. 3–4; “Komm,
Gott Schöpfer, Heiliger Geist” (BWV 370, Riemenschneider 187), m. 1; and “So gibst du nun, mein
Jesu, gute Nacht” (BWV 412, Riemenschneider 206), mm. 26–27.
8
Indeed, one periodically finds this progression in theory texts, despite its extreme rarity in actual
music.
9
My suggestion here is vaguely reminiscent of Wittgenstein’s “Private Language Argument”
(Wittgenstein 1953), which argues that linguistic meaning crucially requires the presence of a
community. My argument is that judgments of grammaticality may require the presence of an
appropriate community, at least insofar as they depart from judgments about frequency.
10
Thus the V2–I progression in Figure 1.5.1 can be simultaneously unusual relative to a coarse-
grained norm, while still occurring frequently enough to be characteristic of Bach’s style. For a very
different understanding of “musical grammar” see Zbikowski 2017.
11
For me, the almost-invariable absence of the ii chord’s fifth is an important datum, suggesting a
linear configuration rather than a true harmony.
Terms, Symbols, and Abbreviations
Bijective voice leading. A voice leading in which each note of one chord is
connected to exactly one note of the other (appendix 1).
Ix, Ix, ix, ιx. These symbols refer to x-step inversion (pitch-space reflection)
along various collections: the chromatic scale (Ix), a scale such as the
diatonic (Ix), a chord (ix), and a chordal subset (ι x) (appendix 1).
Pitch class. An octave-free note type such as C, C♯, or D. Spelling does not
matter and the note can occur in any octave.
Scale. An abstract musical object whose notes can appear in any octave; it
can be modeled as a circular ordering of pitch classes.
Spiral diagram. The main theoretical models used in this book, representing
the geometry of a pair of hierarchically nested transpositions (prelude to
chapter 2, prelude to chapter 3, §2.1, appendices 1–2).
Surface voice. A surface melodic stream containing notes that are felt to
connect.
Tx, Tx, tx, τ x. These symbols refer to x-step transposition along various
collections: the chromatic scale (Tx), another scale such as the diatonic (Tx),
a chord (tx), and a chordal subset (τ x) (prelude to chapter 2).
Tonal. An ambiguous term that can refer to music that is functionally tonal
(see above), or non-atonal. In addition the adjective sometimes refers to
keys and modes (“tonal center,” “tonal region,” “tonal plan”). I typically
use the term as a synonym for “non-atonal,” encompassing techniques
common to a range of modal and functionally tonal styles.
Voice exchange. A voice leading from a chord to itself in which the sum of
all the intervals moved by all voices equals zero.
Voice leading. A collection of paths in pitch class space that indicate how
the notes of one chord move to those of another; see (C, E, G) → (C, F, A)
(appendix 1).
Aarden, Bret, and Paul T. von Hippel. 2004. “Rules for Chord Doubling (and Spacing): Which Ones
Do We Need?” Music Theory Online 10(2).
Agmon, Eytan. 1989. “A Mathematical Model of the Diatonic System.” Journal of Music Theory
33(1): 1–25.
Agmon, Eytan. 1995. “Functional Harmony Revisited: A Prototype-Theoretic Approach.” Music
Theory Spectrum 17(2): 196–214.
Agmon, Eytan. 2013. The Languages of Western Tonality. New York: Springer.
Agmon, Eytan. 2020. “The Webern in Mozart: Systems of Chromatic Harmony and Their Twelve-
Tone Content.” Music Theory Spectrum 42(2): 173–92.
Aldwell, Edward, and Carl Schachter. (1969) 2002. Harmony and Voice Leading. 3rd ed. Belmont:
Wadsworth.
Allsop, Peter. 1992. The Italian “Trio” Sonata: From Its Origins until Corelli. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Arndt, Matthew. 2011. “Schenker and Schoenberg on the Will of the Tone.” Journal of Music Theory
55(1): 89–146.
Arndt, Matthew. 2013. “Schenker and Schoenberg on the Eye of the Genius.” Theoria 20: 39–120.
Arndt, Matthew. 2018. The Musical Thought and Spiritual Lives of Heinrich Schenker and Arnold
Schoenberg. New York: Routledge.
Arndt, Matthew. 2019. “Schoenberg—Schenker—Bach: A Harmonic, Contrapuntal, Formal Braid.”
Zeitschrift der Gesellschaft für Musiktheorie 16(1): 67–97.
Arthur, Claire. 2021. “Vicentino versus Palestrina: A Computational Investigation of Voice Leading
across Changing Vocal Densities.” Journal of New Music Research 50(1): 74–101.
Auerbach, Brent. 2021. Musical Motives: A Theory and Method for Analyzing Shape in Music. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Babbitt, Milton. 1987. Words about Music. Edited by Stephen Dembski and Joseph N. Straus.
Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Bach, Carl Philipp Emanuel. (1762) 1949. Essay on the True Art of Playing Keyboard Instruments.
Edited and translated by William Mitchell. New York: W. W. Norton.
BaileyShea, Matthew. 2004. “Beyond the Beethoven Model: Sentence Types and Limits.” Current
Musicology 77: 5–33.
Balzano, Gerald. 1980. “The Group-Theoretic Description of 12-Fold and Microtonal Systems.”
Computer Music Journal 4(4): 66–84.
Barry, Barbara. 2014. “Schubert’s ‘Quartettsatz’: A Case Study in Confrontation.” Musical Times
155(1928): 31–49.
Bass, Richard. 1996. “From Gretchen to Tristan: The Changing Role of Harmonic Sequences in the
Nineteenth Century.” 19th-Century Music 19: 263–68.
Beach, David. 1967. “The Functions of the Six-Four Chord in Tonal Music.” Journal of Music
Theory 11(1): 2–31.
Beach, David. 1974. “The Origins of Harmonic Analysis.” Journal of Music Theory 18(2): 274–306.
Beach, David. 1983. “A Recurring Pattern in Mozart’s Music.” Journal of Music Theory 27: 1–29.
Beach, David. 1994. “Harmony and Linear Progression in Schubert’s Music.” Journal of Music
Theory 38(1): 13–17.
Bent, Margaret. 1998. “The Grammar of Early Music: Preconditions for Analysis.” In Tonal
Structures in Early Music, edited by Cristle Collins Judd, 15–59. New York: Garland.
Bergé, Pieter, and Jeroen D’hoe. 2009. “To Play or Not to Play: Motivic Connections in the First
Movement of Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of
Analysis and Performance, edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 9–38.
Walpole: Peters.
Bernhard, Christopher. (~1640) 1973. “The Treatises of Christoph Bernhard.” Translated by Walter
Hilse. The Music Forum 3: 1–197.
Bernstein, Leonard. 1956. The Joy of Music. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Bernstein, Leonard. 1976. The Unanswered Question. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Besseler, Heinrich. 1950. Bourdon und Fauxbourdon. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.
Biamonte, Nicole. 2006. “Modality in Beethoven’s Folk-Song Settings.” Beethoven Forum 13(1):
28–63.
Biamonte, Nicole. 2010. “Triadic Modal and Pentatonic Patterns in Rock Music.” Music Theory
Spectrum 32(2): 95–110.
Biamonte, Nicole. 2012. “Variations on a Scheme: Bach’s ‘Crucifixus’ and Chopin’s and Scriabin’s
E-minor Preludes.” Intégral 26: 47–89.
Bobbitt, Richard. 1955. “Harmonic Tendencies in the Missa Papae Marcelli.” The Music Review 14:
273–88.
Boone, Graeme. 2000. “Marking Mensural Time.” Music Theory Spectrum 22(1): 1–43.
Boretz, Benjamin. 1970. “Sketch of a Musical System (Meta-Variations, Part II).” Perspectives of
New Music 8(2): 49–111.
Boretz, Benjamin. 1972. “Meta-Variations, Part IV: Analytic Fallout (1).” Perspectives of New Music
11(1): 146–223.
Boss, Jack. 2019. Schoenberg’s Atonal Music: Musical Idea, Basic Image, and Specters of Tonal
Function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Brewer, William F. 2015. “Perception Is Theory Laden: The Naturalized and Philosophical
Implications.” Journal for General Philosophy of Science 46: 121–38.
Brinkman, Alexander R. 1986. “A Binomial Representation of Pitch for Computer Processing of
Musical Data.” Music Theory Spectrum 8: 44–57.
Brody, Christopher. 2016. “Parametric Interaction in Tonal Repertoires.” Journal of Music Theory
60(2): 97–148.
Brody, Christopher. 2021. “Second-Reprise Opening Schemas in Bach’s Binary Movements.” Music
Theory Spectrum 43(2): 257–79.
Brown, Matthew. 2005. Explaining Tonality. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Bruce, Robert. 1969. “The Lyrical Element in Schubert’s Instrumental Forms.” Music Review 30(2):
131–37.
Buelow, George. 1992. Thorough-Bass Accompaniment According to Johann David Heinichen.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Bukofzer, Manfred. 1947. Music in the Baroque Era: From Monteverdi to Bach. New York: W. W.
Norton.
Burke, Edmund. (1757) 1999. A Philosophical Enquiry into the Origins of the Sublime and Beautiful
and Other Pre-Revolutionary Writings. London: Penguin.
Burnham, Scott. 1995. Beethoven Hero. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Burnham, Scott. 2009. “Singularities and Extremes: Dramatic Impulse in the First Movement of
Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and
Performance, edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 39–60. Walpole: Peters.
Burns, Lori. 1993. “J. S. Bach’s Mixolydian Chorale Harmonizations.” Music Theory Spectrum
15(2): 144–72.
Burns, Lori. 1995. Bach’s Modal Chorales. Hillsdale: Pendragon Press.
Burstein, L. Poundie. 2015. “The Half Cadence and Related Analytic Fictions.” In What Is a
Cadence? Theoretical and Analytical Perspectives on Cadences in the Classical Repertoire, edited
by Markus Neuwirth and Pieter Bergé, 85–116. Leuven: Leuven University Press.
Burstein, L. Poundie. 2018. “The Practice of Music Theory, and Music Theory versus Practice.” In
Teaching Music Theory, edited by Rachel Lumsden and Jeffrey Swinkin, 1–12. New York: W. W.
Norton.
Burstein, L. Poundie. 2020. “Those Boring, Arcane Part-Writing Exercises.” Gamut: Online Journal
of the Music Theory Society of the Mid-Atlantic 9(1).
Burstein, L. Poundie. forthcoming. “Analysis nach Schenker.” In Handbuch Musikanalyse. Methode
und Pluralität, edited by Ariane Jeßulat, Oliver Schwab-Felisch, Jan Philipp Sprick, and Christian
Thorau, translated by Oliver Schwab-Felisch. Kassel: Bärenreiter.
Byros, Vasili. 2012. “Meyer’s Anvil: Revisiting the Schema Concept.” Music Analysis 31(3): 273–
346.
Byros, Vasili. 2015. “Hauptruhepuncte des geistes: Punctuation Schemas and the Late-Eighteenth-
Century Sonata.” In What Is a Cadence? Theoretical and Analytical Perspectives on Cadences in
the Classical Repertoire, edited by Markus Neuwirth and Pieter Bergé, 215–51. Leuven: Leuven
University Press.
Byros, Vasili. 2017. “Mozart’s Vintage Corelli: The Microstory of a Fonte-Romanesca.” Intégral 31:
63–89.
Callender, Clifton. 1998. “Voice-Leading Parsimony in the Music of Alexander Scriabin.” Journal of
Music Theory 42(2): 219–33.
Callender, Clifton, Ian Quinn, and Dmitri Tymoczko. 2008. “Generalized Voice Leading Spaces.”
Science 320: 346–48.
Camp, Elisabeth. 2004. “The Generality Constraint, Nonsense, and Categorial Restrictions.”
Philosophical Quarterly 54(215): 209–31.
Camp, Elisabeth. 2017. “Perspectives in Imaginative Engagement with Fiction.” Philosophical
Perspectives 31: 74–102.
Camp, Elisabeth. 2018. “Why Maps Are Not Propositional.” In Non-Propositional Intentionality,
edited by A. Grzankowski and M. Montague, 19–45. New York: Oxford University Press.
Caplin, William E. 1998. Classical Form: A Theory of Formal Functions for the Instrumental Music
of Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven. New York: Oxford University Press.
Caplin, William E. 2004. “The Classical Cadence: Conceptions and Misconceptions.” Journal of the
American Musicological Society 57(1): 51–118.
Caplin, William E. 2009a. “Beethoven’s Tempest Exposition: A Springboard for Form-Functional
Considerations.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and Performance,
edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 87–125. Walpole: Peters.
Caplin, William E. 2009b. “What Are Formal Functions?” In Musical Form, Forms & Formenlehre:
Three Methodological Reflections, edited by Pieter Bergé, 71–89. Leuven: Leuven University
Press.
Caplin, William E. 2013. “Teaching Classical Form: Strict Categories versus Flexible Analyses.”
Tijdschrift voor Muziektheorie 18(3): 119–35.
Caplin, William E. 2015. “Harmony and Cadence in Gjerdingen’s ‘Prinner.’” In What Is a Cadence?
Theoretical and Analytical Perspectives on Cadences in the Classical Repertoire, edited by
Markus Neuwirth and Pieter Bergé, 17–58. Leuven: Leuven University Press.
Cavett-Dunsby, Esther. 1988. “Mozart’s ‘Haydn’ Quartets: Composing Up and Down without Rules.”
Journal of the Royal Music Association 113(1): 57–80.
Ceulemans, Anne-Emmanuelle. 2017. “Lasso, Meier, Powers. The Reality of the Modes under
Scrutiny.” Paper presented to the 9th European Music Analysis Conference, Strasbourg.
Chafe, Eric. 1992. Monteverdi’s Tonal Language. New York: Schirmer.
Christensen, Thomas. 1992. “The ‘Règle de l’Octave’ in Thorough-Bass Theory and Practice.” Acta
Musicologica 64(2): 91–117.
Christensen, Thomas. 1993a. Rameau and Musical Thought in the Enlightenment. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Christensen, Thomas. 1993b. Review of Carl Dahlhaus’s Studies on the Origins of Harmonic
Tonality and Joel Lester’s Between Mode and Keys. Music Theory Spectrum 15(1): 94–111.
Christensen, Thomas. 2019. Stories of Tonality in the Age of François-Joseph Fétis. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Chua, Daniel K. L., and Alexander Rehding. 2021. Alien Listening: Voyager's Golden Record and
Music from Earth. New York: Zone Books.
Clendinning, Jane Piper, and Elizabeth West Marvin. 2016. The Musician’s Guide to Theory and
Analysis. 3rd ed. New York: W. W. Norton.
Clough, John. 1979a. “Aspects of Diatonic Sets.” Journal of Music Theory 23: 45–61.
Clough, John. 1979b. “Diatonic Interval Sets and Transformational Structures.” Perspectives of New
Music 18(1–2): 461–82.
Clough, John. 1994. “Diatonic Interval Cycles and Hierarchical Structure.” Perspectives of New
Music 32(1): 228–53.
Clough, John. 2000. “Flip-Flop Circles and Their Groups.” Paper presented to the national meeting
of the Society for Music Theory, Toronto.
Cohn, Richard. 1992. “The Autonomy of Motives in Schenkerian Accounts of Tonal Music.” Music
Theory Spectrum 14(2): 150–70.
Cohn, Richard. 1996. “Maximally Smooth Cycles, Hexatonic Systems, and the Analysis of Late-
Romantic Triadic Progressions.” Music Analysis 15(1): 9–40.
Cohn, Richard. 1997. “Neo-Riemannian Operations, Parsimonious Trichords, and Their ‘Tonnetz’
Representations.” Journal of Music Theory 41(1): 1–66.
Cohn, Richard. 1999. “As Wonderful as Star Clusters: Instruments for Gazing at Tonality in
Schubert.” 19th-Century Music 22(3): 213–32.
Cohn, Richard. 2005. “‘This Music Crept by Me upon the Waters’: Introverted Motives in
Beethoven’s ‘Tempest’ Sonata.” In Engaging Music: Essays in Analysis, edited by Deborah Stein,
226–35. New York: Oxford University Press.
Cohn, Richard. 2012. Audacious Euphony. New York: Oxford University Press.
Collins, Denis. 2015. “Taneyev’s Theories of Moveable Counterpoint in the Music of J. S. Bach.”
Bach 46(2): 22–45.
Collins, Denis. 2018. “Approaching Renaissance Music Using Taneyev’s Theories of Movable
Counterpoint.” Acta Musicologica 90(2): 178–201.
Collins Judd, Cristle. 1992. “Modal Types and ‘Ut, Re, Mi’ Tonalities: Tonal Coherence in Sacred
Vocal Polyphony from about 1500.” Journal of the American Musicological Society 45(3): 428–
67.
Coluzzi, Seth J. 2015. “Speaking in (and out of) Mode: Structure and Rhetoric in Marenzio’s O
Mirtillo, Mirtillo anima mia (1595).” Music Theory Spectrum 37(2): 253–74.
Cone, Edward T. 1970. “Schubert’s Beethoven.” The Musical Quarterly 56(4): 779–93.
Cook, Nicholas. 1987. “The Perception of Large-Scale Tonal Closure.” Music Perception 5(2): 197–
205.
Cook, Nicholas. 1989. “Music Theory and ‘Good Comparison’: A Viennese Perspective.” Journal of
Music Theory 33(1): 117–41.
Crane, Hewitt D., and Thomas P. Piantanida. 1983. “On Seeing Reddish Green and Yellowish Blue.”
Science 221: 1078–80.
Cuthbert, Michael Scott, and Christopher Ariza. 2010. “Music21: A Toolkit for Computer-Aided
Musicology and Symbolic Music Data.” In Proceedings of the 11th International Society for Music
Information Retrieval (ISMIR 2010), August 9–13, 2010, Utrecht, Netherlands, edited by J.
Stephen Downie and Remco C. Veltkamp, 637–42.
Cutler, Timothy. 2009. “On Voice Exchanges.” Journal of Music Theory 53(2): 191–226.
Czerny, Carl. (1830) 1848. School of Practical Composition. Translated by John Bishop. London:
Robert Cocks & Co.
Dahlhaus, Carl. 1967. “Zur chromatischen Technik Carlo Gesualdos.” Analecta Musicologica 4: 77–
96.
Dahlhaus, Carl. 1990. Studies in the Origins of Harmonic Tonality. Translated by Robert Gjerdingen.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Dahlhaus, Carl. 1991. Ludwig van Beethoven: Approaches to His Music. Translated by Mary
Whittall. New York: Oxford University Press.
Damschroder, David. 2016. Harmony in Beethoven. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Damschroder, David. 2018. Tonal Analysis: A Schenkerian Perspective. New York: W. W. Norton.
Danto, Arthur. 1981. The Transfiguration of the Commonplace: A Philosophy of Art. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Davies, Stephen. 1983. “Attributing Significance to Unobvious Musical Relationships.” Journal of
Music Theory 27(2): 203–13.
de Clerq, Trevor, David Temperley, David Lustig, and Ethan Tan. 2011. “A Corpus Study of Rock
Music.” http://rockcorpus.midside.com. Accessed September 26, 2020.
Deathridge, John. 2008. Wagner Beyond Good and Evil. Berkeley: University of California Press.
DeBellis, Mark. 1999. “The Paradox of Musical Analysis.” Journal of Music Theory 43(1): 83–99.
Deutsch, Diana, and John Feroe. 1981. “The Internal Representation of Pitch Sequences in Tonal
Music.” Psychological Review 88(6): 503–22.
Doll, Christopher. 2017. Hearing Harmony: Toward a Tonal Theory for the Rock Era. Ann Arbor:
University of Michigan Press.
Douthett, Jack, and Peter Steinbach. 1998. “Parsimonious Graphs: A Study in Parsimony, Contextual
Transformations, and Modes of Limited Transposition.” Journal of Music Theory 42(2): 241–63.
Duane, Ben. 2017. “Thematic and Non-Thematic Textures in Schubert’s Three-Key Expositions.”
Music Theory Spectrum 39(1): 36–65.
Edwards, George. 1997. “A Palimpsest of Mozart in Schubert’s Symphony No. 5.” Current
Musicology 62: 18–39.
Einstein, Alfred. 1948. The Italian Madrigal. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Everett, Walter. 2004. “Making Sense of Rock’s Tonal Systems.” Music Theory Online 10(4).
Everett, Walter, and Tim Riley. 2019. What Goes On: The Beatles, Their Music, and Their Time. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Ewell, Philip A. 2020. “Music Theory and the White Racial Frame.” Music Theory Online 26(2).
Ferraguto, Mark. 2019. Beethoven 1806. New York: Oxford University Press.
Fétis, François-Joseph. (1840) 1994. Esquisse de l’histoire de l’harmony. Translated by Mary Arlin.
Hillsdale: Pendragon Press.
Feurzeig, David. 2010. “On Shifting Grounds: Meandering, Modulating, and Möbius Passacaglias.”
Paper presented to the national meeting of the Society for Music Theory, Indianapolis.
Fieldman, Hali. 2002. “Schubert’s Quartettsatz and Sonata Form’s New Way.” Journal of
Musicological Research 21: 99–146.
Fink, Robert. 2004. “Beethoven Antihero: Sex, Violence, and the Aesthetics of Failure, or Listening
to the Ninth Symphony as Postmodern Sublime.” In Postmodern Modes of Hearing, edited by
Andrew Dell’Antonio, 109–53. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Flew, Antony. 1975. Thinking about Thinking: Do I Sincerely Want to Be Right? London: Collins
Fontana.
Forte, Allen. 1959. “Schenker’s Conception of Musical Structure.” Journal of Music Theory 3(1): 1–
30.
Forte, Alan, and Stephen Gilbert. 1982. Introduction to Schenkerian Analysis. New York: W. W.
Norton.
Froebe, Folker. 2015. “On Synergies of Schema Theory and Theory of Levels: A Perspective from
Riepel’s Fonte and Monte.” Zeitschrift der Gesellschaft für Musiktheorie 12(1): 9–25.
Fux, Johann. (1725) 1971. The Study of Counterpoint. Edited and translated by Alfred Mann. New
York: W. W. Norton.
Gauldin, Robert. 1995a. A Practical Approach to Sixteenth-Century Counterpoint. Long Grove:
Waveland.
Gauldin, Robert. 1995b. A Practical Approach to Eighteenth-Century Counterpoint. Long Grove:
Waveland.
Gauldin, Robert. 1996. “The Composition of Late Renaissance Stretto Canons.” Theory and Practice
21: 29–54.
Gauldin, Robert. 1997. Harmonic Practice in Tonal Music. New York: W. W. Norton.
Gauldin, Robert. 2004. “The Theory and Practice of Chromatic Wedge Progressions in Romantic
Music.” Music Theory Spectrum 26(1): 1–22.
Gelman, Andrew, John Carlin, Hal Stern, David Dunson, Aki Vehtari, and Donald Rubin. 2013.
Bayesian Data Analysis. New York: Chapman and Hall.
Georgiades, Thrasybulos. 1937. Englische Diskantetheorie aus der ersten hälfte 15. Jahrhunderts.
Munich: Musikwissenschaftliches Seminar der Universität München.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 1988. A Classic Turn of Phrase: Music and the Psychology of Perception.
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 1996. “Courtly Behaviors.” Music Perception 13(3): 365–82.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 1999. “An Experimental Music Theory?” In Rethinking Music, edited by
Nicholas Cook and Mark Everist, 161–70. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 2007. Music in the Galant Style. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 2015. “Musical Grammar.” In The Oxford Handbook of Critical Concepts in
Music Theory, edited by Alexander Rehding and Steven Rings, 651–72. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Gjerdingen, Robert. 2020. Child Composers in the Old Conservatories: How Orphans Became Elite
Musicians. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gjerdingen, Robert, and Janet Bourne. 2015. “Schema Theory as a Construction Grammar.” Music
Theory Online 21(2).
Goldenberg, Yosef. 2020. “Continuous Question-Answer Pairs.” Music Theory Online 26(3).
Goldenberg, Yosef. 2021. “Intriguing Interpretation of Dyads in Common-Practice Tonal Music.”
Gamut: Online Journal of the Music Theory Society of the Mid-Atlantic 10(1).
Goldstein, Robin, Johan Almenberg, Anna Dreber, John Emerson, Alexis Herschkowitsch, and Jacob
Katz. 2008. “Do More Expensive Wines Taste Better? Evidence from a Large Sample of Blind
Tastings.” Journal of Wine Economics 3(1): 1–9.
Gosman, Alan. 2009. “Canonic Patterns: Reframing the Puzzle.” Theory and Practice 34: 1–29.
Gosman, Alan. 2012. “Canonic Threads and Large-Scale Structure in Canons.” Gamut: Online
Journal of the Music Theory Society of the Mid-Atlantic 5(1).
Grant, Aaron. 2018. “Schubert’s Three-Key Expositions.” PhD diss., Eastman School of Music.
Greenberg, Yoel. 2017. “Of Beginnings and Ends: A Corpus-Based Inquiry into the Rise of the
Recapitulation.” Journal of Music Theory 61(2): 171–200.
Guillotel-Nothmann, Christophe. 2018. “Conditional Asymmetry and Spontaneous Asymmetry of
Harmonic Progressions: The Changing Status of the Directional Tendency of Root Progressions in
Madrigal Cycles from Verdelot to Monteverdi (c. 1530–1638).” Music Theory Online 24(4).
Hacking, Ian. 1975. The Emergence of Probability: A Philosophical Study of Early Ideas about
Probability, Induction and Statistical Inference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hadjeres, Gaëtan, François Pachet, and Frank Nielsen. 2017. “DeepBach: A Steerable Model for
Bach Chorales Generation.” Proceedings of Machine Learning Research 70: 1362–71.
Haimo, Ethan. 1996. “Atonality, Analysis, and the Intentional Fallacy.” Music Theory Spectrum
18(2): 167–99.
Hall, Rachel, Dmitri Tymoczko, and Jason Yust. 2010. “Upright Petrouchka, Sideways Neapolitans,
and Proper Scales.” Paper delivered to the national meeting of the Society for Music Theory,
Indianapolis.
Hamilton, Kenneth. 2009. “Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata in Performance.” In Beethoven’s Tempest
Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and Performance, edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and
William Caplin, 127–62. Walpole: Peters.
Hansberry, Benjamin. 2017. “What Are Scale-Degree Qualia?” Music Theory Spectrum 39(2): 182–
99.
Harrison, Daniel. 1988. “Some Group Properties of Triple Counterpoint and Their Influence on
Compositions by J. S. Bach.” Journal of Music Theory 32(1): 23–49.
Harrison, Daniel. 1994. Harmonic Function in Tonal Music: A Renewed Dualist Theory and an
Account of Its Precedents. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Harrison, Daniel. 2003. “Rosalia, Aloysius, and Arcangelo: A Genealogy of the Sequence.” Journal
of Music Theory 47: 225–72.
Harrison, Daniel. 2016. Pieces of Tradition: An Analysis of Contemporary Tonality. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Hasty, Christopher. 1981. “Segmentation and Process in Post-Tonal Music.” Music Theory Spectrum
3: 54–73.
Hatten, Robert S. 2009. “Interpreting Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata through Topics, Gestures, and
Agency.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and Performance, edited by
Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 163–80. Walpole: Peters.
Hatten, Robert S. 2014. “Performing Expressive Closure in Structurally Open Contexts: Chopin’s
Prelude in A minor and the Last Two Dances of Schumann’s Davidsbündlertänze.” Music Theory
Online 20(4).
Headlam, David. 2012. Review of A Geometry of Music. Music Theory Spectrum 34(1): 123–43.
Hepokoski, James. 2009. “Approaching the First Movement of Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata through
Sonata Theory.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and Performance, edited
by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 181–212. Walpole: Peters.
Hepokoski, James. 2010. “Sonata Theory and Dialogic Form.” In Musical Form, Forms &
Formenlehre: Three Methodological Reflections, edited by Pieter Bergé, 71–89. Leuven: Leuven
University Press.
Hepokoski, James. 2021. A Sonata Theory Handbook. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hepokoski, James, and Warren Darcy. 2006. Elements of Sonata Theory: Norms, Types, and
Deformations in the Late-Eighteenth-Century Sonata. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hinton, Stephen. 2010. “The Emancipation of Dissonance: Schoenberg’s Two Practices of
Composition.” Music & Letters 91(4): 568–79.
Hodes, Jeffrey. 2012. “A New Roman-Numeral Analyzer for Classical Music.” Undergraduate thesis,
Princeton University.
Hoffmann, E. T. A. 1989. E. T. A. Hoffmann’s Musical Writings. Edited by David Charlton.
Translated by Martyn Clarke. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Holtmeier, Ludwig. 2007. “Heinichen, Rameau, and the Italian Thoroughbass Tradition: Concepts of
Tonality and Chord in the Rule of the Octave.” Journal of Music Theory 51(1): 5–49.
Holtmeier, Ludwig. 2011. Review of Robert Gjerdingen’s Music in the Galant Style. Eighteenth-
Century Music 8(2): 307–48.
Hook, Julian. 2002. “Uniform Triadic Transformations.” Journal of Music Theory 46(1/2): 57–126.
Hook, Julian. 2003. “Signature Transformations.” Paper presented to the national meeting of the
Society for Music Theory, Madison.
Hook, Julian. 2008. “Signature Transformations.” In Music Theory and Mathematics: Chords,
Collections, and Transformations, edited by Jack Douthett, Martha M. Hyde, and Charles J. Smith,
137–60. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Hook, Julian. 2011. “Spelled Heptachords.” In Mathematics and Computation in Music, Proceedings
of the Third International Conference of the Society for Mathematics and Computation in Music,
edited by Carlos Agon, Emmanuel Amiot, Moreno Andreatta, Gérard Assayag, Jean Bresson, and
John Mandereau, 84–97. New York: Springer.
Hook, Julian. 2020. “Generic Sequences and the Generic Tonnetz.” Journal of Music Theory 64(1):
63–103.
Horn, Katelyn, and David Huron. 2015. “On the Changing Use of the Major and Minor Modes 1750–
1900.” Music Theory Online 21(1).
Horton, Julian. 2014. “Dialectics and Musical Analysis.” In Aesthetics of Music: Musicological
Perspectives, edited by Stephen Downes. New York: Routledge.
Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge Grammar of the English
Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Humphreys, David. 1997. “Something Borrowed.” Musical Times 138(1853): 19–24.
Hunt, Graham G. 2014. “When Structure and Design Collide: The Three-Key Exposition Revisited.”
Music Theory Spectrum 36(2): 247–69.
Huovinen, Erkki. 2002. Pitch-Class Constellations: Studies in the Perception of Tonal Centricity.
Turku: Acta musicologica Fennica.
Huron, David. 1991. “The Avoidance of Part-Crossing in Polyphonic Music: Perceptual Evidence
and Musical Practice.” Music Perception 9(1): 93–104.
Huron, David. 2007a. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Huron, David. 2007b. “On the Role of Embellishment Tones in the Perceptual Segregation of
Concurrent Musical Parts.” Empirical Musicology Review 2(4): 123–39.
Huron, David. 2016. Voice Leading: The Science behind a Musical Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Imbrie, Andrew. 1973. “‘Extra’ Measures and Metrical Ambiguity in Beethoven.” In Beethoven
Studies, edited by Alan Tyson, 45–66. New York: W. W. Norton.
Jacoby, Nori, Eduardo A. Undurraga, Malinda J. McPherson, Joaquín Valdés, Tomás Ossandón, and
Josh H. McDermott. 2019. “Universal and Non-Universal Features of Musical Pitch Perception
Revealed by Singing.” Current Biology 29: 3229–43.
Jander, Owen. 1996. “Genius in the Arena of Charlatanry: The First Movement of Beethoven’s
Tempest Sonata in Cultural Context.” In Musica Franca: Essays in Honor of Frank A. D’Accone,
edited by Irene Aim, Alyson McLamore, and Colleen Reardon, 585–630. Hillsdale: Pendragon
Press.
Jeppesen, Knud. (1931) 1939. Counterpoint: The Polyphonic Vocal Style of the Sixteenth Century.
Translated by Glen Haydon. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Jeppesen, Knud. (1944–1945) 1975. “Problems of the Pope Marcellus Mass.” In Palestrina: Pope
Marcellus Mass, edited and translated by Lewis Lockwood, 99–130. New York: W. W. Norton.
Jeppesen, Knud. (1946) 1970. The Style of Palestrina and the Dissonance. New York: Dover.
Jost, John, Laurie A. Rudman, Irene V. Blair, Dana R. Carney, Nilanjana Dasgupta, Jack Glaser, and
Curtis D. Harding. 2009. “The Existence of Implicit Bias Is beyond Reasonable Doubt: A
Refutation of Ideological and Methodological Objections and Executive Summary of Ten Studies
That No Manager Should Ignore.” Research in Organizational Behavior 29: 39–69.
Junod, Julien, Pierre Audétat, Carlos Agon, and Moreno Andreatta. 2009. “A Generalization of
Diatonicism and the Discrete Fourier Transform as a Mean [sic.] for Classifying and
Characterising Musical Scales.” In Mathematics and Computation in Music, edited by Elaine
Chew, Adrian Childs, and Ching-Hua Chuan, 166–79. New York: Springer.
Kant, Immanuel. (1788) 1997. Critique of Practical Reason. Edited and translated by Mary Gregor.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kant, Immanuel. (1790) 1970. Critique of Judgment. Translated by John Henry Bernard. New York:
Free Press.
Katz, J., and David Pesetsky. 2011. “The Identity Thesis for Language and Music.” Unpublished
manuscript available at https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/000959.
Kerman, Joseph. 1981. The Masses and Motets of William Byrd. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Kinderman, William. 2009a. Beethoven. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford.
Kinderman, William. 2009b. “The First Movement of Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Genesis, Form,
and Dramatic Meaning.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and
Performance, edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 213–34. Walpole: Peters.
Kinderman, William, and Harold Krebs, eds. 1996. The Second Practice of Nineteenth-Century
Tonality. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Kirlin, Phillip B. 2014. “A Probabilistic Model of Hierarchical Music Analysis.” PhD diss.,
University of Massachusetts.
Kloss, Jürgen. 2012. “‘. . . Tell Her to Make Me a Cambric Shirt’: From the ‘Elfin Knight’ to
‘Scarborough Fair.’” https://www.justanothertune.com/html/cambricshirt.html.
Klumpenhouwer, Henry. 1991. “A Generalized Model of Voice-Leading for Atonal Music.” PhD
diss., Harvard University.
Klumpenhouwer, Henry. 2011. “History and Structure in Cohn’s Audacious Euphony.” Intégral 25:
159–75.
Korte, Werner Fritz. 1929. Die Harmonik des frühen XV. Jahrhunderts in ihrem Zusammenhang mit
der Formtechnik. Münster: Suhrbier & Bröcker.
Kostka, Stefan, and Dorothy Payne. 2003. Tonal Harmony. 4th ed. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Kramer, Lawrence. 1990. Music as Cultural Practice: 1800–1900. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Kramer, Lawrence. 2002. “Contesting Wagner: The Lohengrin Prelude and Anti-Anti-Semitism.”
19th-Century Music 25(2–3): 190–211.
Labov, William. 1975. “Empirical Foundations of Linguistic Theory.” In The Scope of American
Linguistics, edited by Robert Austerlitz, 77–134. Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press.
Laitz, Steven. 2008. The Complete Musician. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lalite, Philippe, Emmanuel Bigand, Joanna Kantor-Martynuska, and Charles Delbé. 2009. “On
Listening to Atonal Variants of Two Piano Sonatas by Beethoven.” Music Perception 26(3): 223–
34.
Laukens, Dirk. 2019. Jazz Guitar Chord Dictionary. Antwerp: Jazz Guitar Online. Free ebook
available from http://www.jazzguitar.be.
Lawrence, John Y. 2020. “Toward a Predictive Theory of Theme Types.” Journal of Music Theory
64(1): 1–36.
Lendvai, Erno. 1971. Bela Bartók: An Analysis of His Music. London: Kahn and Averill.
Lerdahl, Fred. 1988. “Cognitive Constraints on Compositional Systems.” In Generative Processes in
Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition, edited by John Sloboda,
231–59. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lerdahl, Fred. 2001. Tonal Pitch Space. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lerdahl, Fred. 2020. Composition and Cognition. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Lerdahl, Fred, and Ray Jackendoff. 1983. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Lester, Joel. 1998. “J. S. Bach Teaches Us How to Compose: Four Pattern Preludes of the Well-
Tempered Clavier.” College Music Symposium 38: 33–46.
Levine, Mark. 1989. The Jazz Piano Book. Petaluma: Sher Music.
Levinson, Jerrold. 1997. Music in the Moment. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Lewin, David. 1981. “A Way into Schoenberg’s Opus 15, Number 7.” In Theory Only 6(1): 3–24.
Lewin, David. 1984. “Amfortas’s Prayer to Titurel and the Role of D in ‘Parsifal’: The Tonal Spaces
of the Drama and the Enharmonic C♭/B.” 19th-Century Music 7(3): 336–49.
Lewin, David. 1986. “Music Theory, Phenomenology, and Modes of Perception.” Music Perception
3(4): 327–92.
Lewin, David. 1987. Generalized Musical Intervals and Transformations. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Lewin, David. 1990. “Klumpenhouwer Networks and Some Isographies That Involve Them.” Music
Theory Spectrum 12: 83–120.
Lewis, Christopher Orlo. 1983. “Tonality and Structure in the Ninth Symphony of Gustav Mahler.”
PhD diss., University of Rochester.
Lewis, Clive Staples. 1964. The Discarded Image. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Liszt, Franz. 2016. The Collected Writings of Franz Liszt, vol. 3, part 2. Edited and translated by
Janita R. Hall-Swadley. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
London, Justin. 2004. Hearing in Time. New York: Oxford University Press.
Long, Megan Kaes. 2018. “Cadential Syntax and Tonal Expectation in Late Sixteenth-Century
Homophony.” Music Theory Spectrum 40(1): 52–83.
Long, Megan Kaes. 2020. Hearing Homophony. New York: Oxford University Press.
Longyear, Rey M., and Kate R. Covington. 1988. “Sources of the Three-Key Exposition.” Journal of
Musicology 6(4): 448–70.
Lowinsky, Edward. 1961. Tonality and Atonality in Sixteenth-Century Music. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Lund, Jeb. 2014. “Is a Hot Dog a Sandwich? An Extended Meditation on the Nature of America.”
Guardian, July 3. Available at https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2014/jul/03/is-a-hot-
dog-a-sandwich-nature-america.
Mak, Su Yin. 2008. “Et in Arcadia Ego: The Elegiac Structure of Schubert’s Quartettsatz in C Minor
(D. 703).” In The Unknown Schubert, edited by Barbara M. Reul and Lorraine Byrne Bodley, 145–
53. New York: Routledge.
Margolis, Eric, and Stephen Laurence. 2003. “Concepts.” In The Blackwell Guide to the Philosophy
of Mind, edited by Stephen P. Stich and Ted A. Warfield, 190–213. Oxford: Blackwell.
Margulis, Elizabeth. 2013. On Repeat. New York: Oxford University Press.
Martin, Nathan John. 2010. Review of Beethoven’s ‘Tempest’ Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and
Performance. Theory and Practice 35: 169–89.
Martin, Nathan John. 2018. “Schenker and/or Rameau.” Paper presented to the national meeting of
the Society for Music Theory, San Antonio.
Marvin, Elizabeth West, and Alexander Brinkman. 1999. “The Effect of Modulation and Formal
Manipulation on Perception of Tonic Closure by Expert Listeners.” Music Perception 16(4): 389–
408.
Marvin, William. 2012–2013. “ ‘Und so weiter’: Schenker, Sonata Theory, and the Problem of the
Recapitulation.” Theory and Practice 37–38: 221–40.
Marx, Adolf Bernhard. (1859) 1997. Musical Form in the Age of Beethoven: Selected Writings on
Theory and Method. Translated by Scott Burnham. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McClary, Susan. 1991. Feminine Endings: Music, Gender, and Sexuality. Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press.
McClary, Susan. 2004. Modal Subjectivities. Berkeley: University of California Press.
McCreless, Patrick. 1996. “An Evolutionary Perspective on Nineteenth-Century Semitonal
Relations.” In The Second Practice of Nineteenth-Century Tonality, edited by William Kinderman
and Harald Krebs, 87–113. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
McCreless, Patrick. 2011. “Ownership, in Music and Music Theory.” Music Theory Online 17(1).
McDermott, Josh, and Marc Hauser. 2005. “The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and
Evolution.” Music Perception 23(1): 29–59.
McEnery, Tony, and Andrew Wilson. 1996. Corpus Linguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University
Press.
McHose, Allen. 1947. The Contrapuntal Harmonic Technique of the 18th Century. New York: Crofts.
Meeùs, Nicolas. 2000. “Toward a Post-Schoenbergian Grammar of Tonal and Pre-Tonal Harmonic
Progressions.” Music Theory Online 6(1).
Meeùs, Nicolas. 2018. “Harmonic Vectors and the Constraints of Tonality.” Music Theory Online
24(4).
Meier, Bernhard. 1988. The Modes of Classical Vocal Polyphony: Described According to the
Sources, with Revisions by the Author. Translated by Ellen S. Beebe. London: Broude Brothers.
Meyer, Leonard. 1956. Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Meyer, Leonard. 1982. “Process and Morphology in the Music of Mozart.” Journal of Musicology
1(1): 67–94.
Meyer, Leonard. 1996. Style and Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Milsom, John. 2005. “Imitatio, Intertextuality and Early Music.” In Citation and Authority in
Medieval and Renaissance Musical Culture: Learning from the Learned, edited by Suzannah Clark
and Elizabeth Eva Leach, 141–51. Woodbridge: Boydell.
Mirka, Danuta. 2015. “The Mystery of the Cadential Six-Four.” In What Is a Cadence? Theoretical
and Analytical Perspectives on Cadences in the Classical Repertoire, edited by Markus Neuwirth
and Pieter Bergé, 157–84. Leuven: Leuven University Press.
Moore, Alan. 2012. Song Means: Analyzing and Interpreting Popular Song. Burlington: Ashgate.
Moreno, Jairo. 1996. “Theoretical Reception of the Sequence and Its Conceptual Implications.” PhD
diss., Yale University.
Morgan, Robert. 1978. “Schenker and the Theoretical Tradition: The Concept of Musical Reduction.”
College Music Symposium 18(1): 72–96.
Morgan, Robert. 2014. Becoming Heinrich Schenker. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Morris, Robert D. 1995. “Equivalence and Similarity in Pitch and Their Interaction with PCSet.”
Journal of Music Theory 39(2): 207–43.
Muns, Lodewijk. 2008. “Why I Am Not a Schenkerian.” Unpublished manuscript available at
https://lodewijkmuns.nl/WhyIamNotaSchenkerian.pdf.
Nápoles López, Néstor, Claire Arthur, and Ichiro Fujinaga. 2019. “Key-Finding Based on a Hidden
Markov Model and Key Profiles.” In 6th International Conference on Digital Libraries for
Musicology, 33–37. New York: Association for Computing Machinery.
Narmour, Eugene. 1988. “Melodic Structuring of Harmonic Dissonance: A Method for Analysing
Chopin’s Contribution to the Development of Harmony.” In Chopin Studies, edited by Jim
Samson, 77–114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
National Research Council. 2014. Identifying the Culprit: Assessing Eyewitness Identification.
Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Navia, Gabriel Henrique Bianco. 2016. “The Medial Caesura in Schubert’s Sonata Forms: Formal
and Rhetorical Complications.” PhD diss., University of Arizona.
Neff, Severine. 2012. “Schoenberg as Theorist.” In Schoenberg and His World, edited by Walter
Frisch, 55–84. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Neumeyer, David. 1987. “The Ascending Urlinie.” Journal of Music Theory 31(2): 275–303.
Newcomb, Anthony. 2007. “Marenzio’s Riffs.” In Luca Marenzio e il madrigale romano, edited by
Franco Piperno, 115–43. Rome: Accademia Nazionale di Santa Cecilia.
Nicolas, Patrice. 2019. “Challenging Some Misconceptions about the Règle de l’octave.” Music
Theory Online 25(4).
Ninov, Dimitar. 2016. “Functional Nature of the Cadential Six-Four.” Musicological Annual 52(1):
73–96.
Nobile, Drew. 2015. “Counterpoint in Rock Music: Unpacking the ‘Melodic-Harmonic Divorce.’”
Music Theory Spectrum 37(2): 189–203.
Nobile, Drew. 2016. “Harmonic Function in Rock Music: A Syntactic Approach.” Journal of Music
Theory 60(2): 149–80.
Nobile, Drew. 2020. “Double-Tonic Complexes in Rock Music.” Music Theory Spectrum 42(2): 207–
26.
Nolan, Catherine. 2003. “Combinatorial Space in Nineteenth- and Early Twentieth-Century Music
Theory.” Music Theory Spectrum 25(2): 205–41.
Okazaki, Miles. 2015. Fundamentals of Guitar. Fenton, MO: Mel Bay.Osborn, Brad. 2017. “Rock
Harmony Reconsidered: Tonal, Modal and Contrapuntal Voice-Leading Systems in Radiohead.”
Music Analysis 36(1): 59–93.
Palisca, Claude. 1956. “Vincenzo Galilei’s Counterpoint Treatise: A Code for the Seconda Pratica.”
Journal of the American Musicological Society 9(2): 81–96.
Palisca, Claude. 1994. Studies in the History of Italian Music and Music Theory. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Parncutt, Richard. 1988. “Revision of Terhardt’s Psychoacoustical Model of the Root(s) of a Musical
Chord.” Music Perception 6: 65–94.
Perry, Jeffrey, and Paul Lansky. 1996. “The Inner Voices of Simple Things: A Conversation with Paul
Lansky.” Perspectives of New Music 34(2): 40–60.
Pinter, D. 2019. “The Magnificent Flat-Seventh.” Soundscapes 22. Available at
https://www.icce.rug.nl/~soundscapes/VOLUME22/Magnificent_flat-seventh.shtml.
Piston, Walter. 1941. Harmony. New York: W. W. Norton.
Powers, Harold. 1958. “Mode and Raga.” The Musical Quarterly 44(4): 448–60.
Powers, Harold. 1981. “Tonal Types and Modal Categories in Renaissance Polyphony.” Journal of
the American Musicological Society 34(3): 428–70.
Powers, Harold. 1992a. “Is Mode Real? Pietro Aron, the Octenary System, and Polyphony.” Basler
Jahrbuch für Historische Musikpraxis 16: 9–52.
Powers, Harold. 1992b. “Modality as a European Cultural Construct.” In Secondo convegno europeo
di analisi musicale: Atti, edited by Rosanna Dalmonte and Mario Baroni, 207–20. Trento:
Università degli studi di Trento, Dipartimento di storia della civiltà europea.
Proctor, Gregory. 2004. “A Schenkerian Look at Lully.” Journal of Seventeenth-Century Music 10(1).
Putnam, Hilary. 1988. Realism and Reason. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Quine, Willard van Orman. 1953. From a Logical Point of View. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Quinn, Ian. 2005. “Harmonic Function without Primary Triads.” Paper presented to the national
meeting of the Society for Music Theory, Boston.
Quinn, Ian. 2018. “Tonal Harmony.” In The Oxford Handbook of Critical Concepts in Music Theory,
edited by Alexander Rehding and Steven Rings, 467–97. New York: Oxford University Press.
Rabinovitch, Gilad. 2018. “Gjerdingen’s Schemata Reexamined.” Journal of Music Theory 62(1):
41–84.
Rabinovitch, Gilad. 2019. “Implicit Counterpoint in Gjerdingen’s Schemata.” Music Theory &
Analysis 6(1): 1–49.
Rabinovitch, Gilad. 2020. “Hidden Polyphony, Linear Hierarchy, and Scale-Degree Associations in
Galant Schemata.” Indiana Theory Review 36(1–2): 114–66.
Rameau, Jean Paul. (1722) 1971. Treatise on Harmony. Translated by Philip Gossett. New York:
Dover.
Reef, John. 2020. “A ‘Proto-Theme’ in Some of J. S. Bach’s Fugal Works.” Music Theory Online
26(4).
Rehding, Alexander. 2009. Music and Monumentality: Commemoration and Wonderment in
Nineteenth-Century Germany. New York: Oxford University Press.
Reti, Rudolf. 1951. The Thematic Process in Music. New York: Macmillan.
Reynolds, Christopher Alan. 2015. Wagner, Schumann, and the Lessons of Beethoven’s Ninth.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Ricci, Adam. 2004. “A Theory of the Harmonic Sequence.” PhD diss., University of Rochester.
Rice, John. 2015. “The Morte: A Galant Voice-Leading Schema as Emblem of Lament and
Compositional Building-Block.” Eighteenth-Century Music 12(2): 157–81.
Richards, Mark. 2010. “Closure in Classical Themes: The Role of Melody and Texture in Cadences,
Closural Function, and the Separated Cadence.” Intersections: Canadian Journal of Music 31(1):
25–45.
Richards, Mark. 2011. “Viennese Classicism and the Sentential Idea: Broadening the Sentence
Paradigm.” Theory and Practice 36: 179–224.
Richards, Mark. 2013a. “Beethoven and the Obscured Medial Caesura: A Study in the
Transformation of Style.” Music Theory Spectrum 35(2): 166–93.
Richards, Mark. 2013b. “Sonata Form and the Problem of Second-Theme Beginnings.” Music
Analysis 32(1): 3–45.
Richards, Mark. 2017. “Tonal Ambiguity in Popular Music’s Axis Progressions.” Music Theory
Online 23(3).
Richter, Ernst Friedrich. 1852. Die Grundzige der musikalischen Formen und ihre Analyse. Leipzig:
Georg Wigand.
Riemann, Hugo. 1896. Harmony Simplified. London: Augener.
Riemann, Hugo. 1903. System der musikalischen Rhythmik und Metrik. Leipzig: Breitkopf and
Härtel.
Rings, Steven. 2011. Tonality and Transformation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Rings, Steven. 2013. “A Foreign Sound to Your Ear: Bob Dylan Performs ‘It’s Alright, Ma (I’m Only
Bleeding),’ 1964–2009.” Music Theory Online 19(4).
Rohrmeier, Martin. 2011. “Towards a Generative Syntax of Tonal Harmony.” Journal of Mathematics
and Music 5(1): 35–53.
Rohrmeier, Martin. 2020. “The Syntax of Jazz Harmony: Diatonic Tonality, Phrase Structure, and
Form.” Music Theory & Analysis 7(1): 1–62.
Roig-Francolí, Miguel A. 2003. Harmony in Context. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Roig-Francolí, Miguel A. 2018. “From Renaissance to Baroque: Tonal Structures in Tomás Luis de
Victoria’s Masses.” Music Theory Spectrum 40(1): 27–51.
Rosch, Eleanor. 1978. “Principles of Categorization.” In Cognition and Categorization, edited by
Eleanor Rosch and Barbara Bloom Lloyd, 28–49. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rosen, Charles. 1971. The Classical Style. New York: W. W. Norton.
Rosen, Charles. 1988. Sonata Forms. New York: W. W. Norton.
Rosen, Charles. 1998. The Romantic Generation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rosen, Charles. 2001. Critical Entertainments. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rotem, Elam, and Peter Schubert. 2021. “Stretto fuga—How to Make a Masterpiece out of Two
Intervals.” Video, 17:50. https://youtu.be/zGZjzBvXCzg.
Rothenberg, David. 1978. “A Model for Pattern Perception with Musical Applications. Part I: Pitch
Structures as Order-Preserving Maps.” Mathematical Systems Theory 11: 199–234.
Rothgeb, John. 1975. “Strict Counterpoint and Tonal Theory.” Journal of Music Theory 19(2): 260–
84.
Rothgeb, John. 1996. “Re: Eytan Agmon on Functional Theory.” Music Theory Online 2(1).
Rothstein, William. 1989. Phrase Rhythm in Tonal Music. New York: Schirmer.
Rothstein, William. 2009. “Riding the Storm Clouds: Tempo, Rhythm, and Meter in Beethoven’s
Tempest Sonata.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of Analysis and Performance,
edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 235–72. Walpole: Peters.
Rowe, Dorothy. 1983. Depression: The Way Out of Your Prison. New York: Routledge and Kegan
Paul.
Russell, Jonathan. 2018. “Harmony and Voice-Leading in The Rite of Spring.” PhD diss., Princeton
University.
Sailor, Malcolm, and Andie Sigler. 2017. “The Insufficiently Stimulated Ear: A Corpus Study of
Dissonance Treatment from DuFay to Victoria.” Paper presented to the national meeting of the
Society for Music Theory, Arlington.
Salzer, Felix. (1952) 1962. Structural Hearing. New York: Dover.
Salzer, Felix. 1983. “Heinrich Schenker and Historical Research: Monteverdi’s Madrigal Oimè, se
tanto amate.” In Aspects of Schenkerian Theory, edited by David Beach, 135–52. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Samarotto, Frank. 2004. “Sublimating Sharp 4: An Exercise in Schenkerian Energetics.” Music
Theory Online 10(3).
Sanguinetti, Giorgio. 2012. The Art of Partimento. New York: Oxford University Press.
Saslaw, Janna. 1992. “Gottfried Weber and the Concept of Mehrduetigkeit.” PhD diss., Columbia
University.
Savage, Patrick E., Steven Brown, Emi Sakai, and Thomas E. Currie. 2015. “Statistical Universals
Reveal the Structures and Functions of Human Music.” Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 112(29): 8987–92.
Savage, Patrick, Adam Tierney, and Annirudh Patel. 2017. “Global Music Recordings Support the
Motor Constraint Hypothesis for Human and Avian Song Contour.” Music Perception 34(3): 327–
34.
Schachter, Carl. 1990. “Either/Or.” In Schenker Studies, edited by Heidi Siegel, 165–80. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Schachter, Carl. 2016. The Art of Tonal Analysis. Edited by Joseph Straus. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1910) 1987. Counterpoint, vol. 1. Edited by John Rothgeb. Translated by John
Rothgeb and Jürgen Thym. New York: Schirmer.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1921–1923) 2004. Der Tonwille: Pamphlets in Witness of the Immutable Laws
of Music, vol. 1. Edited by William Drabkin. Translated by Ian Bent, William Drabkin, Joseph
Dubiel, Timothy Jackson, Joseph Lubben, and Robert Snarrenberg. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1923–1924) 2005. Der Tonwille: Pamphlets in Witness of the Immutable Laws
of Music, vol. 2. Edited by William Drabkin. Translated by Ian Bent, William Drabkin, Joseph
Dubiel, Joseph Lubben, William Renwick, and Robert Snarrenberg. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1926) 1996. The Masterwork in Music, vol. 2. Edited by William Drabkin.
Translated by Ian Bent, William Drabkin, John Rothgeb, and Heidi Siegel. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1930) 1997. The Masterwork in Music, vol. 3. Edited by William Drabkin.
Translated by Alfred Clayton and Derrick Puffett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1933) 1969. Five Graphical Music Analyses. Translated by Felix Salzer. New
York: Dover.
Schenker, Heinrich. (1935) 1979. Free Composition. Edited and translated by Ernst Oster. New York:
Longman.
Scherer, Josh. 2015. “A Bro and a Philosopher Debate the True Meaning of a Sandwich.”
https://firstwefeast.com/eat/2015/04/philosophy-of-meat-bread.
Schindler, Anton. 1840. Biographie von Ludwig van Beethoven. Münster: Achendorff.
Schindler, Anton. 1841. The Life of Beethoven. Translated by Ignaz Moscheles. London: Henry
Colburn.
Schindler, Anton. (1860) 1996. Beethoven as I Knew Him. Edited by Donald W. MacArdle.
Translated by Constance S. Jolly. Mineola: Dover.
Schmalfeldt, Janet. 1991. “Towards a Reconciliation of Schenkerian Concepts with Traditional and
Recent Theories of Form.” Music Analysis 10(3): 233–87.
Schmalfeldt, Janet. 1995. “Form as the Process of Becoming: The Beethoven-Hegelian Tradition and
the Tempest Sonata.” Beethoven Forum 4: 37–71.
Schmalfeldt, Janet. 2011. In the Process of Becoming. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schoenberg, Arnold. (1911) 1983. Theory of Harmony. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Schoenberg, Arnold. 1984. Style and Idea. Edited by Leonard Stein. Translated by Leo Black.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Schubert, Peter. 2008. Modal Counterpoint, Renaissance Style. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schubert, Peter. 2018. “Thomas Campion’s ‘Chordal Counterpoint’ and Tallis’s Famous Forty-Part
Motet.” Music Theory Online 24(1).
Schuijer, Michiel. 2008. Analyzing Atonal Music: Pitch-Class Set Theory and Its Contents.
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Segall, Christopher. 2010. “K-Nets, Inversion, and Gravitational Balance.” Theory and Practice 35:
119–45.
Segall, Christopher. 2014. “Sergei Taneev’s Vertical-Shifting Counterpoint: An Introduction.” Music
Theory Online 20(3).
Sewell, Ian. 2021. “When All You Have Is a Hammer: Surface/Depth as Good Comparison.” Music
Theory Spectrum 43(2): 197–220.
Shepard, Roger. 1964. “Circularity in Judgements of Relative Pitch.” Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 36(12): 2346–53.
Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson, and Uri Simonsohn. 2011. “False-Positive Psychology:
Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as
Significant.” Psychological Science 22(11): 1359–66.
Simon, Herbert, and Kenneth Kotovsky. 1963. “Human Acquisition of Concepts for Sequential
Patterns.” Psychological Review 70(6): 534–46.
Simon, Herbert, and Richard Sumner. 1968. “Pattern in Music.” In Formal Representation of Human
Judgment, edited by Benjamin Kleinmuntz, 219–50. New York: Wiley.
Slottow, Steven. 2018. “To Be or Not to Be: Schenker’s Versus Schenkerian Attitudes towards
Sequences.” Gamut: Online Journal of the Music Theory Society of the Mid-Atlantic 8(1).
Smith, Charles. 1986. “The Functional Extravagance of Chromatic Chords.” Music Theory Spectrum
8: 94–139.
Smith, Charles. 1996. “Musical Form and Fundamental Structure: An Investigation of Schenker’s
Formenlehre.” Music Analysis 15: 191–92.
Smith, Peter. 1994. “Brahms and Schenker: A Mutual Response to Sonata Form.” Music Theory
Spectrum 16: 77–71.
Smith, Peter. 2006. “Harmonic Cross-Reference and the Dialectic of Articulation and Continuity in
Sonata Expositions of Schubert and Brahms.” Journal of Music Theory 50(2): 143–79.
Sommer, Heinz-Dieter. 1986. “Beethovens kleine Pastorale Zum 1. Satz der Klaviersonate op. 28.”
Archiv für Musikwissenschaft 43(2): 109–27.
Sprick, Jan Philipp. 2018. “Sequences: Between Affirmation and Destruction of Tonality.” Music
Theory Online 24(4).
Stein, Beverly. 2002. “Carissimi’s Tonal System and the Function of Transposition in the Expansion
of Tonality.” Journal of Musicology 19(2): 264–305.
Stephenson, Kenneth. 2002. What to Listen for in Rock: A Stylistic Analysis. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Strunk, Oliver. 1950. Source Readings in Music History. New York: W. W. Norton.
Stuckenschmidt, Hans Heinz. 1965. “Debussy or Berg? The Mystery of a Chord Progression.” The
Musical Quarterly 51(3): 453–59.
Subotnik, Rose Rosengard. 1991. Developing Variations: Style and Ideology in Western Music.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Sutcliffe, W. Dean. 2003. The Keyboard Sonatas of Domenico Scarlatti and Eighteenth-Century
Musical Style. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Swinden, Kevin. 2005. “When Functions Collide: Aspects of Plural Function in Chromatic Music.”
Music Theory Spectrum 27(2): 249–82.
Taneyev, Sergei. (1909) 1962. Convertible Counterpoint in the Strict Style. Translated by G. Ackley
Brower. Boston: Bruce Humphries.
Taruskin, Richard. 2005. The Oxford History of Western Music. New York: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, Benedict. 2019. Towards a Harmonic Grammar of Grieg’s Late Piano Music. New York:
Routledge.
Telesco, Paula J. 1998. “Enharmonicism and the Omnibus Progression in Classical-Era Music.”
Music Theory Spectrum 20(2): 242–79.
Temperley, David. 2006. Review of Music in the Galant Style. Journal of Music Theory 50(2): 277–
90.
Temperley, David. 2007. “The Melodic-Harmonic ‘Divorce’ in Rock.” Popular Music 26(2): 323–42.
Temperley, David. 2008. “Hypermetrical Transitions.” Music Theory Spectrum 30(2): 305–25.
Temperley, David. 2011a. “Composition, Perception, and Schenkerian Theory.” Music Theory
Spectrum 33(2):146–68.
Temperley, David. 2011b. “The Cadential IV in Rock.” Music Theory Online 17(1).
Temperley, David. 2018. The Musical Language of Rock. New York: Oxford University Press.
Thayer, Alexander Wheelock. 1967. Thayer’s Life of Beethoven. Edited by Elliot Forbes. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
Theune, Michael. 2007. Structure & Surprise: Engaging Poetic Turns. New York: Teachers &
Writers Collaborative.
Thomas, Keith. 1971. Religion and the Decline of Magic. New York: Scribner.
Tierney, Adam, Frank Russo, and Annirudh Patel. 2008. “Empirical Comparisons of Pitch Patterns in
Music, Speech, and Birdsong.” The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123: 3721.
Tierney, Adam, Frank Russo, and Annirudh Patel. 2011. “The Motor Origins of Human and Avian
Song Structure.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
108(37): 15510–15.
Tomlinson, Gary. 1990. Monteverdi and the End of the Renaissance. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Tovey, Donald. (1935) 1981. Symphonies and Other Orchestral Works: Essays in Musical Analysis.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Tovey, Donald. 1931. A Companion to Beethoven’s Pianoforte Sonatas. London: Associated Board of
the Royal Schools of Music.
Trippet, David. 2010. “Liszt on Lohengrin (or: Wagner in absentia).” The Wagner Journal 4(1): 4–21.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2003. “Progressions Fondamentales, Functions, Degrés, une Grammaire de
l’Harmonie Tonale Élémentaire.” Musurgia 10(3–4): 35–64.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2004. “Scale Networks in Debussy.” Journal of Music Theory 48(2): 215–92.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2008. “Scale Theory, Serial Theory, and Voice Leading.” Music Analysis 27(1):
1–49.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2011a. A Geometry of Music. New York: Oxford University Press.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2011b. “Dualism and the Beholder’s Eye.” In The Oxford Handbook of
Riemannian and Neo-Riemannian Music Theories, edited by Ed Gollin and Alex Rehding, 246–67.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2020a. “Review-Essay on Fred Lerdahl’s Composition and Cognition.” Music
Theory Online 26(1).
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2020b. “Why Topology?” Journal of Mathematics and Music 14(2): 1–56.
Tymoczko, Dmitri. 2023. “Hierarchical Set Theory.” Journal of Mathematics and Music 17(2): 282–
90.
Tymoczko, Dmitri, and Jason Yust. 2019. “Fourier Phase and Pitch-Class Sum.” In Proceedings of
the 7th International Conference of Mathematics and Computation in Music, edited by Mariana
Montiel, Francisco Gómez-Martín, and Octavio A. Agustín-Aquino, 46–58. New York: Springer.
Vande Moortele, Steven. 2009. “The First Movement of Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata and the
Tradition of Twentieth-Century Formenlehre.” In Beethoven’s Tempest Sonata: Perspectives of
Analysis and Performance, edited by Peter Berge, Jeroen D’hoe, and William Caplin, 293–314.
Walpole: Peters.
Varricchio, David J., and Frankie D. Jackson. 2016. “Reproduction in Mesozoic Birds and Evolution
of the Modern Avian Reproductive Mode.” The Auk 133(4): 654–84.
Vicentino, Nicola. (1555) 1996. Ancient Music Adapted to Modern Practice. Edited by Claude V.
Palisca. Translated by Maria Rika Maniates. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Von Hippel, P., and David Huron. 2000. “Why Do Skips Precede Reversals? The Effect of Tessitura
on Melodic Structure.” Music Perception 18: 59–85.
Wagner, Napthali. 2003. “ ‘Domestication’ of Blue Notes in the Beatles’ Songs.” Music Theory
Spectrum 25(2): 353–65.
Wagner, Richard. 1907. Richard Wagner’s Prose Works, vol. 3. Translated by William Ashton Ellis.
London: Kegan, Paul.
Waltham-Smith, Naomi. 2018. “Sequence.” In The Oxford Handbook of Critical Concepts in Music
Theory, edited by Alexander Rehding and Steven Rings, 577–601. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Warfield, Patrick. 2011. “The March as Musical Drama and the Spectacle of John Philip Sousa.”
Journal of the American Musicological Society 64(2): 289–318.
Watkins, Holly. 2017. “Toward a Post-Humanist Organicism.” Nineteenth-Century Music Review 14:
93–114.
Weber, Gottfried. (1817–1821) 1846. Theory of Musical Composition. Translated by James F.
Warner. Boston: Wilkins, Carter, and Company, and O. C. B. Carter.
Webster, James. 1978. “Schubert’s Sonata Form and Brahms’s First Maturity.” 19th-Century Music
2(1): 18–35.
Webster, James. 2010. “Formenlehre in Theory and Practice.” In Musical Form, Forms &
Formenlehre: Three Methodological Reflections, edited by Pieter Bergé, 123–39. Leuven: Leuven
University Press.
Wellwood, Alexis, Roumyana Pancheva, Valentine Hacquard, and Colin Phillips. 2018. “The
Anatomy of a Comparative Illusion.” Journal of Semantics 35(3): 543–83.
Westergaard, Peter. 1975. An Introduction to Tonal Theory. New York: W. W. Norton.
Wiering, Franz. 2001. The Language of the Modes: Studies in the History of Polyphonic Modality.
New York: Routledge.
Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1953. Philosophical Investigations. Edited by G. E. M. Anscombe and R.
Rhees. Translated by G. E. M. Anscombe. Oxford: Blackwell.
Wolff, Christoph. 2001. Johann Sebastian Bach: The Learned Musician. New York: W. W. Norton.
Wootton, David. 2015. The Invention of Science. New York: HarperCollins.
Yellin, Victor. 1998. The Omnibus Idea. Warren, MI: Harmonie Park Press.
Yust, Jason. 2009. “The Step-Class Automorphism Group in Tonal Analysis.” In Mathematics and
Computation in Music: MCM 2007, edited by T. Klouche and T. Noll, 512–520. New York:
Springer.
Yust, Jason. 2015a. “Distorted Continuity: Chromatic Harmony, Uniform Sequences, and Quantized
Voice Leadings.” Music Theory Spectrum 37(1): 120–43.
Yust, Jason. 2015b. “Voice-Leading Transformation and Generative Theories of Tonal Structure.”
Music Theory Online 21(4).
Yust, Jason. 2018. Organized Time: Rhythm, Tonality, and Form. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Zarlino, Gioseffo. (1558) 1968. The Art of Counterpoint. Translated by Guy A. Marco and Claude V.
Palisca. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zarlino, Gioseffo. (1558) 1983. On the Modes. Edited by Claude V. Palisca. Translated by Vered
Cohen. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Zbikowski, Lawrence M. 2017. Foundations of Musical Grammar. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Index
For the benefit of digital users, indexed terms that span two pages (e.g., 52–53) may, on occasion,
appear on only one of those pages.
Notes are indicated by “n” following the page number. Figures are indicated by f following the page
number.
cadence 19–21, 28, 30, 61–63, 99–101, 211n.6, 218, 219f, 233, 254f, 258n.4, 261–62, 265, 266, 267f,
270–74, 276n.33, 283f, 287f, 290, 296–97, 302, 348–49, 349n.57, 377–78, 380, 386f, 422n.20,
437–39, 438f, 442, 443, 449n.71, 455–56, 473–75, 476–77, 477n.35, 477f
as harmonic cycle 413–14
authentic 388–89, 521–22
converging 102–3, 151–52, 201–2, 377f, 519–20, 529–30
deceptive 253, 265, 270–74, 290, 321n.15
final 412, 414
half 78, 265, 290, 444f
mixolydian 306–8
perfect authentic 510–11
Phrygian 301–2, 385–86, 388–89, 520, 521–22
Renaissance 256n.9, 279n.42, 569
Schenkerian interpretation 416–18, 418f
caesura 377–78
filled 453–54, 454f, 455f
medial 17, 20, 380–82, 449, 451, 453–54, 454f, 455f, 455–56, 473–75, 476, 483–84, 496, 504–5,
508–9, 516f, 516–17
Callahan, Homer
“Rounder’s Luck” 83–84
Callender, Clifton 155n.2, 536n.4, 542n.11
Camp, Elizabeth 52n.6, 234n.43
Campion, Thomas 91, 91f
Caplin, William 20, 21–22, 116n.17, 151n.1, 333n.34, 334n.37, 340n.47, 414n.9, 415n.11, 439–42,
443n.52, 444n.55, 448, 455–56, 457n.2, 475n.27, 477–78, 477n.34, 478n.38, 483n.50
The Carter Family 269
Carthy, Martin 85
Cavett-Dunsby, Esther 472n.20
centricity 68, 148–49, 247–48, 275, 288–90, 299, 322n.16, 384, 390–91, 416–17, 434
Ceulmans, Anne-Emmanuelle 385n.18
Chafe, Eric 229–11nn.35–6, 235n.45
Chopin, Frederic 3, 9–10, 17, 34, 116n.18, 200–1, 224–26, 247–48, 249, 311, 334–35, 379n.13 §9.3,
516
Etude, Op. 10, no. 1 420–21, 420f
Etude, Op. 10, no. 12 (Revolutionary) 136 §9.3, 432, 528
Prelude, Op. 28, no. 2 (A minor) 188–89 §9.3
Prelude, Op. 28, no. 4 (E minor) 198, 198f, 201–2
Prelude, Op. 28, no. 5 (D major) 374–75, 375f
Mazurka, Op. 6, no. 2 227n.31
Mazurka, Op. 6, no. 3 213f, 225f
Mazurka, Op. 7, no. 2 422, 423f
Mazurka, Op. 17, no. 3 433f
Mazurka, Op. 17, no. 4 334–35 §9.3
Mazurka, Op. 24, no. 4 422n.20
Mazurka, Op. 30, no. 2 207f
Mazurka, Op. 33, no. 1 236f
Mazurka, Op. 41, no. 1 225f
Mazurka, Op. 41, no. 2 225f
Mazurka, Op. 50, no. 2 144f
Mazurka, Op. 50, no. 3 434–35n.39
Nocturne, Op. 27, no. 2 250f
chorale(s) 13, 35, 82, 96, 134–36, 149–50, 149f, 188–89, 203, 204–5, 205f, 206–7, 412–13, 424–25,
431, 432, 476n.32, 531f
chord
added-sixth 236f, 241–42
as objects of compositional focus 264–65
as scale §1.2, Ch. 2 (prelude), 53 §3.10, Ch. 5 (prelude), 540
augmented 52–53, 53f, 69, 70–71, 71f, 275, 559f, 565–66
augmented sixth 141, 186f, 377–78, 380, 480, 486n.55, 514
common-tone diminished seventh 96–97 §3.11, 375, 476n.31, 496–98
common-tone dominant seventh §3.11, 242–44
common-tone (other chords) 496–98
diatonic distance between§2.2
dyads, geometry of §§3.2–4
Farben 95f, 558f
fourth 9–10, 227n.31, 228f, 560, 561f
grammar 29–30, 119, 239 §6.6, Ch. 7, Appendix 4
pivot 305–6, 307f
power 387
seventh 12, 19–20, 27, 28 §3.11, 257–58 §7.3, 329–30
seventh chords, geometry of §3.11
seven-note 137, 149–50, 149f, 240, 241f, 366–67, 396–97, 534–35
six-four 214–16, 222, 226, 257–58, 286, 333–34, 508n.71
So What 95f
symmetrical 52–53, 394–96, 538f, 558–60
thinking within 31–33 §3.10, 179–81, 231, 495–96, 497f, 559n.8
triads, geometry of §§3.6–10
Tristan 224–26, 225f
unprepared seventh 173–75, 218n.18, 228–29, 228f
voicing Ch. 3 (prelude)
Christensen, Thomas 202n.38, 218n.18, 221n.20, 257n.2, 259n.7, 260n.11, 313n.4, 332n.30, 386n.21
chromatic 4–5, Ch. 2 (prelude), 59–60, 63–64n.20, 87, 89f §3.12, 190n.32, 198, 242–44, 246–48,
249, 250f, 251f, 254f, Ch. 8 (prelude), §§8.1–3, 384 §8.7, §9.3, 511–12, 512f, 514, 519–20,
524n.92, 525f, 565–66
cluster 68–69
figures, transposing diatonically 11–12, 566–67
neighbor 40f, 204–5, 204f
sequence §1.1, 53f, 63–64, 64f, 69–71, 147–48, 163–65, 164f, 260–61, 334–35, 353, 481, 488,
489–90, 510f, 515–16, 517f, 520, 528–29, 563–65
set theory 14–15, 94–95, 95f
sevenths 140, 140f, 420f
space 10, 68–69, 134, 137, 344
substitution 114, 114f, 118–19
transposition Ch. 2 (prelude), §2.1, 107, 135f
triad 33, 45–46, 49f §2.1, 69–70, 92 see also modulation
chromaticism 75, 78–79, 96, 132–34, 148–49, 227, 242–44, 358–60, 374, 419, 422, 520
heavily diatonic 75, 146
Chua, Daniel 310n.18
circle of fifth(s) 54–55, 99–100, 198, 199f, 321–22, 371–72, 380–82, 394–96, 396f, 398f, 399f,
560n.11
generalized 397–99
clausula vera 99–100, 271–72, 555
Clementi, Muzio 463–65
Clendinning, Jane
The Musician’s Guide to Theory and Analysis 142n.50, 414–16
Clough, John 6n.6, 15n.15, 316n.8
Cobain, Kurt 59
Cohen, Leonard
“Hallelujah” 330, 330f
Cohn, Richard 3, 4n.5, 21–22, 60n.17, 75n.34, 78–79, 147n.57, 148–49, 149n.63, 358n.3, 369–71,
374, 421n.19, 425n.23, 480n.40, 509n.75
Collins, Denis 156n.3
Collins, Shirley 85
Collins Judd, Cristle 385n.18
Coltrane, John
Giant Steps 290–91
Coluzzi, Seth 299n.62
computational analysis 212, 214, 238–39, 240f, 255 §6.4, 295, 308, 309–10, 336n.42, 385f, 386f,
447–48, 508n.73, 532, 534–35, 569, 573–74
computational harmonization 412–13, 414f
Cone, Edward 514n.83
contrary motion 3, 31, 31f, 32f, 53, 69, 87, 90–91, 93–94, 104, 105–6, 105f, 110–11, 125–26, 134,
137, 151–52, 153–54, 153f, 155, 158–60, 158f, 165, 167–68, 171, 175–76, 178–81, 178f, 180f,
181f, 182f §4.7, 260–62, 263, 264f, 331n.29, 341–42, 350–51, 360, 409–11, 427–28, 428f, 429,
430–31, 432, 438–39, 451, 457, §§10.1–3, 478–80, 489–91, 489f, 495–98, 501–3, 510–11, 518,
522–24, 525f, 526, 530–31, 536, Appendix 3
Cook, Nicholas 16n.17, 209n.8, 291n.51, 404n.6, 447n.60
Cooke, Sam
“Cupid” 440f
Corelli, Archangelo 75–76, 257, 280–81n.44, 311, 334–35 §7.6, 457
Op. 1 336
counterpoint 2–3, 33, 60, 74, 87, 89f, 90–91, 96–98, 104, 113, 119–20, 122f, 125–26, 128–29 §3.10,
§3.12, Ch. 4 (prelude), 161–62, 186, 203, 227, 231, 234–35, 238–39, 249, 275, 276–77, 286, 296–
97, 351–53, 420–21, 432, 459–60 §10.2, 492, 527, 530–31, 540
Baroque 11–12
chromatic 516
geometry of elementary functional 261–62
instruction 2, 23, 24, 205–6, 212n.8
invertible Ch. 4 (prelude), 158–59, 158f, 167n.17, 169f, 328n.22, 466f, 477–78, 477n.35
of J.S. Bach 3 §7.7
language of 8–9
of Mahler 246
modal 203–4, 222–23, 445–46
Morte schema 186, 186f
negative and positive 90–91, 93–94, 154
parallel 6–8, 265f, 387, 468–69, 495, 498–500
parallel and contrary 110–11, 126f, 467–68
Renaissance 222–23
species 47, 209, 214n.14, 218, 432
of Stravinsky 6–8 see also chord (thinking within), contrary motion, doubly parallel motion,
OUCH theory
Crane, Hewitt 404n.8
Cuthbert, Michael Scott 3, 532n.5
Cutler, Timothy 536n.5
Czerny, Carl 420n.17
Dahlhaus, Carl 26n.46, 149n.63, 223n.23, 256n.9, 257, 257n.1, 259n.7, 265n.16, 265n.17, 269,
269n.23, 270, 271–72, 274–75, 274n.30, 277n.36, 279–82, 431n.32, 453n.5, 453n.7, 473n.23,
475n.26, 475n.28
Dalza, Joan Ambrosio 259–60, 260f, 265–66, 265n.17, 265f, 266f, 267, 268n.22, 277–78, 295
Pavana alla Venetiana 259–60, 260f, 265f
Tastar de Corde 269f
Damschroder, David 202n.39, 254n.4, 476n.31, 476n.32
Danto, Arthur 234n.43
Darcy, Warren 20, 21–22, 380–82, 448, 453–54, 454f, 455–56, 455n.11, 469n.16
Davies, Stephen 404n.7
Davis, Miles 66n.22
“So What” 361, 361n.1
De Clerq, Trevor 56n.11
Deathridge, John 518n.87
DeBellis, Mark 403n.3
Debussy, Claude 4, 79–81, 84–85, 88–89, 192, 203, 226–27, 247–48, 448n.65
Prelude to the Afternoon of a Faun 5f
Deutsch, Diana 15n.15
disguised model 198, 476, 483n.48, 486–88
Doll, Christopher 68n.26, 72n.32, 247n.66
doubly parallel motion 41–42, 53, 54f, 60, 125–26, 132, 195–96, 496n.63, 555, 561f, 563–65
Douthett, Jack 70n.28
Duane, Ben 507n.70, 508n.73
Dufay, Guillaume 23, 214–16, 215–16n.16, 291
Duran Duran
“Rio” 63–64, 67f
Dylan, Bob
“The House of the Rising Sun” 84–85, 84f, 84n.48
“Knockin’ on Heaven’s Door” 59–60, 60f
“Tangled Up in Blue” 61
Faithfull, Marianne 85
Farrenc, Louise
Souvenir des Huguenots, Op. 19 193, 194f
fauxbourdon 34, 107–9, 108f, 165–66, 177n.24, 178f, 191–92, 233–34, 241–42, 297, 311, 312–13
§7.5, 336–39, 340n.46, 350–53, 429n.28, 433, 476, 476f, 488, 489–90, 495–500, 496f, 496n.63,
498f, 499f, 501
generalized 114–16, 521–22, 528–29
rule of the octave (ROTO) 76, 77n.36, 116, 148n.60, 331–32, 383f, 409–11, 498f, 501, 506f, 507f,
507–8, 510–11, 514–16, 517f, 519–20, 526
Fekaris, Dino
“I Will Survive” 138–40, 140f
Fenaroli, Fedele 221n.20
Feroe, John 15n.15
Ferraguto, Mark 471n.17
Fétis, François-Joseph 21–22, 218–20, 227n.32, 257, 295n.58
Feurzeig, David 65–66, 158n.4
Fieldman, Hail 504–460nn.68–9
Fink, Robert 457n.3
Flew, Antony 20n.33
flow 17, 407 §10.2
folk music 21n.35, 23, 82–84, 85–86, 227n.31
Formenlehre 21, 407, 449, 454–55
Forte, Allen 35n.58, 428–29, 429f, 431n.33
Foster, Gwen
“Rising Sun Blues” 83–84, 83f
Franklin, Aretha 63–64, 74–75
Froebe, Folker 426n.24
frottola 262–66, 265n.16, 269–71, 270f
Fux, Johann 221n.20
Kamakawiwoʻole, Israel
“Over the Rainbow” 111f, 114–16
Kant, Immanuel 473, 473n.24
Katz, Jacob 435n.40
Kellner 362–63, 364f, 373n.8
Kerman, Joseph 385n.20
Kern, Jerome
“All the Things You Are” 138–40, 139f
Kinderman, William 421n.19, 472n.19, 481n.42
King, Carole 4, 63–64
“Natural Woman” 63–64, 63f, 67–68
Kirlin, Phillip 209n.11, 448n.62
Kloss, Jürgen 85n.52
Klumpenhouwer, Henry 156n.3, 369n.6, 555n.4
knowledge 96, 237, 528
background 309–10
conscious 41
geometrical 528
implicit 47, 257, 471–72
intuitive 90–91
musical 1–4, 45–46, 332n.30, 528
public 188
self 402
stylistic 308
Korte, Werner Fritz 257n.1
Kostka, Stephen 128n.36, 237n.49, 312n.2
Kramer, Lawrence 421n.19, 518n.86, 522n.90
MacColl, Ewan 85
Maggio, Antonio
“I Got the Blues” 145f
Mahanthappa, Rudresh 4, 6
“The Decider” 7f
Mahler, Gustav 96, 210–11 §5.5, 247–48
Symphony no. 2 240n.57
Symphony no. 9 34, 142f §5.5
Marenzio, Luca 34, 227 §6.7
“Ahi dispietata morte” §6.7
“Vorria parlare e dire” 387, 388f
Margulis, Elizabeth 342n.50, 448n.66
Marshmello
“Happier” 440f
Martin, Nathan 323n.17, 344n.52, 476n.31, 570
Martines, Marianna
Sonata in A major 208f
“Vo solcando un mar crudele” 157f
Marvin, Elizabeth West 16n.17, 142n.50, 291n.51, 404n.6
The Musician’s Guide to Theory and Analysis 414–16
Marvin, William 447n.59, 448n.65
Marx, Adolf Bernhard 455n.10
mathematics 2, 45–46, 96, 320, 367n.4, 457–58, 540, 553n.8
musical 154, 183–84, 316, 471–72, 527n.1
McClary, Susan 227–10nn.32–3, 234n.43, 267n.20, 267n.21, 457n.3
McCreless, Patrick 4n.3, 361n.1
McDermott, Josh 310n.18
McEnery, Tony 23n.40
McHose, Allen 128n.36, 312n.2, 569
Meeùs, Nicolas 48n.2, 259n.7
Meier, Bernhard 79n.42, 277n.36, 385n.18
melodic slot 12, 13–14, 14f, 134–36, 357, 359f, 363–64, 367–68, 432, 535–36
Meyer, Leonard 3, 16, 24n.44, 26n.47, 74, 202n.38, 240n.59, 356, 407n.1, 421n.19
Michaelis
“Passando per una rezzola” 262–63, 263f
Milsom, John 162n.9
minimalism 201–2, 394–96, 527
Mirka, Danuta 211n.7
modernism 1–2, 85–86, 184, 249n.70
modes 2, 56f, 98, 257–58, 268, 275n.32, 290, 291f, 320, 361, §§8.6–7, 537–38
modulation 2–3, 15, 34–35, 45–46, 68, 151–52, 269, 320, 347–48, 348f, 355–56, Ch. 8 (prelude), Ch.
8, 403–4, 422–25, 447, 481n.43, 483–84, 522, 524–26, 529n.3, 570–71
choral and scalar 375
chord-first 375
direct 306, 307f
garden-path 306, 307f
pivot-chord 306, 307f
Monk, Meredith 4, 31–33, 104
Anthem 32f
Monk, Thelonious
“Pannonica” 109–10, 109f, 527n.1
Monteverdi, Claudio 19–20, 23–24, 104, 210–11, 218n.18 §5.3, 237–38, 248–49, 257, 291, 292,
292f, 468
controversy with Artusi 226–27, 234
“Cosi sol d’un chiara fonte” (Madrigal 8.4) 194–95n.35, 522n.91
“Cruda Amarilli” (Madrigal 5.1) 296–97
“Longe da te, cor mio” (Madrigal 4.19) 335f
“Ohimè, se tanto amate” (Madrigal 4.11) 34 §5.3, 241–42, 519n.88
“Tu se’ morta” (Orfeo) 224–26
Vespro della Beata Vergine 263n.15
Monteverdi, Giulio Cesare 212n.8, 234
Moore, Alan 63n.19
Morgan, Robert 169n.18, 418n.15, 428n.27
Morley, Thomas 79–81
“April Is in My Mistress’ Face” 79, 80f, 81f, 173n.22, 227, 273n.29, 511–12
“Leave, alas, this tormenting” 391–93
Morris, Robert 94n.11
Mozart, W.A. 6, 69–70, 76–77, 141–44, 161–62, 166n.13, 188, 191–92, 206n.5, 206f, 226–27, 237,
291, 295, 311–12, 312f, 314–15, 314n.6, 329f, 333n.34, 334f, 336, 337f, 338f, 339f, 354–55, 380,
380n.15, 409–11, 444n.55, 453, 455–56, 463–65, 468–69, 472n.20, 515n.84, 569–70
Clarinet Concerto, K.622 144n.53
Piano Fantasy, K.475 178n.25
Piano Concerto, K.488 207f
Piano Sonata, K.279 193, 195f, 239, 240f, 451, 452f
Piano Sonata, K. 282 445f, 445–46, 508n.74
Piano Sonata, K. 283 14f
Piano Sonata, K. 284 409–11, 411f
Piano Sonata, K. 309 6f, 143f
Piano Sonata, K. 310 20n.30, 191f, 433f
Piano Sonata, K. 311 438, 441f
Piano Sonata, K. 331 417f
Piano Sonata, K. 332 381f, 382f, 514–15, 515f
Piano Sonata, K. 333 77f, 208f
Piano Sonata, K. 533 193f, 380n.14
Piano Sonata, K. 545 166f
String Quintet, K. 516 116n.18
Symphony no. 36, K.425 250f
Symphony no. 40, K.550 176n.23, 408–09, 414
Violin Sonata, K. 304 439f
Muns, Lodewijk 434n.38
musica ficta 218, 260n.9, 290
Ockeghem, Johannes 93f, 114–16, 212n.8, 275–78, 276f, 277f, 295, 526, 528–29
Mass De plus en plus 116f
oenophile(s) 402
orbifold 547
ornament 303
Osborn, Brad 78n.39
OUCH theory 96–97 §3.6, 132, 136–37, 271
O-Zone
“Dragostea Din Tei” 68
Tallis, Thomas
Spem in alium 91n.8, 136–37, 138f
Taneyev, Sergei 155–57, 159n.5, 555n.1
Taruskin, Richard 212n.8, 256n.9, 257n.2, 258n.4, 272n.24, 282n.46, 448n.65
Taylor, Benedict 75n.35
Taylor, Steve 151n.1, 240n.57, 460n.9
Telesco, Paula 10n.9
Temperley, David 29n.52, 56n.11, 61n.18, 63–64n.20, 67n.24, 78n.39, 209n.8, 247n.66, 303n.3,
320n.12, 333n.31, 418n.15, 426n.24, 439n.48, 486n.53
Thayer, Alexander 473n.24
Theune, Michael 233n.40
thinking within the chord 31–33 §3.7, 179–81, 231, 495–96, 497f, 559n.8
Thomas, Keith 402n.1
“Three Blinde Mice” (traditional) 157–58, 157f
Tierney, Adam 47n.1
Tinctoris, Johannes Ch. 3 (prelude), 220–21
Liber de arte contrapuncti 87–88
Tinctoris Transform Ch. 3 (prelude), 110–11, 119–20, 141, 172f, 178f, 184–86, 187–88, 459n.5, 467,
534–35, 542–43, 543f, 549n.4, 564f, 565–66
Tomlinson, Gary 235n.44
tonicization 132–34, 151–53, 193, 336–37, 347–48, 349–50, 380, 477–78, 481, 500, 520
Tovey, Donald Francis 475n.27, 478n.39, 482–83, 485–86, 495n.61
transcription 83n.47, 85, 404
Trevathan, Charles
“Bully Song” 145f
triad see chord
Trippet, David 518n.86
Tromboncino, Bartolomeo 271
“Ah partiale e cruda morte!” 270, 270f, 271f
“Se ben hor non scopro el foco” 263, 263f
Turner, Georgia 83–84
Tymoczko, Dmitri 16n.18, 41n.4, 42n.6, 44n.8, 50n.4, 70n.29, 155n.2, 367n.4, 398n.30, 457–58,
530n.4, 533n.1, 536n.4, 537n.6, 543n.12, 552n.7, 553n.9, 555n.3, 563n.15, 563n.16, 566n.19
A Geometry of Music 13–14, 44n.8, 48–49, 50n.4, 55n.8, 75n.34, 91, 97n.2, 110n.12, 148n.59,
148n.61, 155n.2, 198, 213n.11, 280n.43, 312–13, 334n.37, 335n.39, 374n.9, 375, 398n.30,
416n.13, 421n.19, 422n.21, 430n.29, 533n.1, 536n.4, 537n.6, 553n.9, 566n.22
The Thousand Faces of Form 149–50
Wheels within Wheels 396–97, 397f
Tyner, McCoy 4, 9–10, 9f, 114–16, 560
“Pursuance” (A Love Supreme) 248f
“Passion Dance” (The Real McCoy) 9f, 561f