Dell Hymes 1967
Dell Hymes 1967
Dell Hymes 1967
times not; one’s speech network, based for example on the practice of
a common profession (say, sociology), may extend across communities,
the common profession providing sufficiently common rules of speech.
Obviously part of the work of definition is borne by the commu-
nity, and the difficulties of defining it are here by-passed. Criteria of
frequency of interaction, regularity of interaction, focus of interaction,
contiguity, degree of commonalty of pattern, etc., may be invoked,
each perhaps representing a continuum. The essential thing is that the
object of description is an integral social unit. Probably it will be use-
ful to reserve the notion of speech community for the social unit most
specifically characterized for a person by common locality and primary
interaction (Gumperz, 1962, esp. 30-32). I have essentially drawn dis-
tinctions of scale and kind of linkage within Gumperz’ wholly general
concept of linguistic community which amounts to any distinguishable
intercommunicating group.
Speech Situation. Within a community one readily detects many
situations associated with (or masked by the absence of) speech. Such
contexts of situation will often be naturally described as ceremonies,
fights, hunts, meals, love-making and the like. It would not be profitable
to convert such situations en masse into parts of a sociolinguistic de-
scription, by the simple expedient of relabelling them in terms of
speech. (Notice that the distinctions made with regard to speech com-
munity are not identical with the concepts of a general communicative
approach, which must note the differential range of communication by
speech, film, art object, music.) Such situations may enter as contexts
into the statement of rules of speaking as aspects of setting (or of
genre). In contrast to speech events, they are not in themselves gov-
erned by such rules, or one set of such rules throughout. A hunt, for
example, may comprise both verbal and nonverbal events, and the
verbal events may be of more than one type.
In a sociolinguistic description, then, it is necessary to deal with
activities which are in some recognizable way bounded or integral.
From the standpoint of general social description they may be regis-
tered as ceremonies, fishing trips, and the like; from particular stand-
points they may be regarded as political, aesthetic, etc., situations,
which serve as contexts for the manifestation of political, aesthetic,
etc., activity. From the sociolinguistic standpoint they may be regarded
as speech situations.
Speech Event. The term speech event will be restricted to activi-
ties, or aspects of activities, that are directly governed by rules for the
use of speech. An event may consist of a single speech act, but will
often comprise several. Just as an occurrence of a noun may at the
same time be the whole of a noun phrase and the whole of a sentence
(e.g., Fire!), so a speech act may be the whole of a speech event, and of
a speech situation (say, a rite consisting of a single prayer, itself a
20 DELL HYMES
That is, the type of speech event called hukum has as its focus a dis-
agreement arising from a charge that an offense has been committed, and
as its expected outcome, a legal ruling, based on precedent and carrying
special sanctions. The Yakan examples are from a paper by Frake, Struck
by Speech, to appear in a volume on the ethnography of communication
edited by John Gumperz and myself.
As the varied wording of the Yakan account has shown, where the
focus of a speech event requires special attention, it seems most natu-
rally to be an aspect of this portion of the heuristic scheme. Contrast
in focus is often important for comparative study. Thus litigation
INTFJUCI’ION OF LANGUAGE 23
has not proceeded very far. Work of Joel Sherzer and myself with some
American Indian data suggests the possibility of adapting a linguistic
mode of statement. In such a format, generalizations applying through-
out a speech event are stated at the outset in a sort of lexicon. The
sequential form of the act itself is stated in a sort of syntax by means
of context-sensitive rewriting rules (Chomsky, 1965). When prose de-
scriptions of events have been so restated, there has been a consider-
able gain in understanding of structure. The explicit form of statement
makes demands upon description that go beyond what is usually in
prose accounts. The form of the event is disengaged, as it were, from
the verbal foliage obligatory in prose sentences, and can be more
readily seen. Such formal restatement is essential, if comparative work
is to proceed. One must be able to compare events within a society,
and across societies, in concise and standard format. Such comparison
cannot depend upon memorizing or shuffling of prose paragraphs
vastly different in verbal style. And it is through some formal restate-
ment that one can commit oneself to a precise claim as to what it is a
member of a society knows in knowing how to participate in a speech
act.
A grave defect in many studies which examine the interaction of
language and social setting has been the failure to state precisely ( a )
the difference and ( b ) the interrelationship between values pertaining
to the sociolinguistic feature, on the one hand, and the values pertain-
ing to the social context in which it can occur.
A related defect has been failure to state precisely the differince,
and the interrelationship, between the normal, ordinary, or “un-
marked value of a sociolinguistic feature, on the one hand, and the
“marked,” or specially loaded values, on the other. Studies of the use
of a given code in multilingual situations, like studies of modes of
address, may state the range of contexts in which the code can occur,
appending information about its use in each, but without contrasting
the effects of varying code and context. However, as the Uruguayan
linguist, J. P. Rona, has insisted, sociolinguistics deals not only with
linguistic facts in contexts, but with linguistic facts having social value
in contexts.
Just as the linguistic sign is a relation between a linguistic form
and a linguistic value (e.g., the form “dog” and meaning “dog”), so a
sociolinguistic feature, such as a choice of code, is a sign, a relation
between a form (here, the linguistic fact, such as a code) and a socio-
linguistic value (say, respect, or formality). The set of code-varieties
within a community may thus be analyzed as a semantic set. One may
determine for normal contexts the dimensions of meaning along which
choice of code-variety implies contrast. To complete the analysis, one
must then state separately the domains-settings, role relationships or
whatever-across which the normal meanings of code choice are de-
INTERACTION OF LANGUAGE 27
fined. Having done so, one may now state the ways in which code
choice may be used to insult, to flatter, to boast-the marked, specially
loaded uses-by stating the corresponding, diflerent relations between
code values and domain values that govern these uses.
All these relationships can take the form of linguistic rules, such
as those developed by Chomsky (1965) for handling lexical elements.
In effect, one specifies form, content, and context-an overt element,
semantic values, and rules governing its selection in those values.
Such a form of statement is no more than an elaboration of the form of
statement familiar to us in dictionaries now, e.g., ?each (form), “a
tack sailed with the wind coming more or less from abeam” (semantic
value), with the specification (selection rule), “in nautical usage”. To
pursue such a form of statement in sociolinguistics,however, will have
as consequence the inseparability of sociolinguistic analysis from the
full-scale analysis of social life itself, for it is in the analysis of social
life that the requisite rules of selection for sociolinguistic features are
to be found and stated. (For a detailed example of this mode of
analysis, see my “Quasi-Korean Modes of Address”, submitted to
Anthropological Linguistics, with the Korean data on Richard Howell
( 1965), it can be shown that values of authority and intimacy assigned
to the modes of address (values often assigned to choice of code in some
societies) are in fact properties of the social relationships in terms of
which the use of the modes of address is to be defined. The modes of
address themselves form a set on a single dimension of social distance.
The formal separation of the set of linguistic choices and the set of
social relationships reveals the true nature of the relationship between
them. For major detailed work on Speech Communities from this
standpoint see Gumperz ( 1964) and Labov ( 1966).)
Such a mode of analysis permits formal treatment of many of the
functions served in acts of speech. The conventional means of many
such functions can indeed be analyzed as relations among components,
e.g., message-form, genre and key in the case of the -y form of the ac-
cusative plural of masculine nouns in Polish, which has the value
-solemn” in the genre of poetry, and the value “ironic, pejorative” in
the genres of non-poetic speech. Functions themselves may be statable
in terms of relations among components, such that poetic function, for
example, may require a certain relationship among choice of code,
choice of topic and message-form in a given period or society.
It would be misleading, however, to think that the definition of
functions can be reduced to or derived from other components. Such a
thought would be a disabling residue of behaviorist ideology. Ulti-
mately the functions served in speech must be derived directly from
the purposes and needs of human persons engaged in social action, and
are what they are: talking to seduce, to stay awake, to avoid a war.
The formal analysis of speaking is a means to the understanding of
28 DELL HYMES
KROBER,
l
by Stanley Diamond on anthropofogical approac es to education. )
A. L. Evolution, history, and culture. In Sol Tax (Ed.), Euolution after
Donuin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960, 1-16.
LABOV,WILLIAMA. The Social Stratification of English in New York City.
Washington, D.C.: Center for Applied Linguistics, 1966.
MALINOWSKI, BRONISLAW. Coral gardens and their magic, Vol. 11. London: Allen
and Unwin. 1935.
MEAD,MARGARET. Public opinion mechanisms among primitive peoples. Public
Ovinion Ouarterlv,“ - 1937,
. 1., 5 1 6 .
WEINRE& U&. Languages in contact. New York: Linguistic Circle of New
York, 1953.
WHITELY,W. H. Social anthropology, meaning and linguistics. Man, new series,
1966, 1, 139-157.