Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Sequeira_2009_IJP

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Article

Electrical autonomic correlates of emotion

SEQUEIRA, Henrique, et al.

Abstract

This paper focussed on how electrophysiological autonomic data may contribute to better
understand neural substrates of emotional processing. The utility of autonomic
electrophysiological markers for assessing emotional and cognitive processes is presented in
the context of an important bodily arousal interface. Components of general autonomic control
are reviewed and relevant neural modulations of specific autonomic variables were discussed.
The role of autonomic feedback on central processes is emphasized and neural influences on
autonomic activities as an index of arousal dimension, the electrodermal activity (EDA), are
outlined. An overview of brain mechanisms governing generation and control of EDA is
presented, and the contribution of electrodermal parameters as indices of emotional activation
illustrated by data related to diurnal emotional reactivity and to non consciously subjective
emotionality. Conclusions highlight the role of electrical autonomic expressions as tools to
explore emotional components of mind-body-mind relationships.

Reference
SEQUEIRA, Henrique, et al. Electrical autonomic correlates of emotion. International Journal
of Psychophysiology, 2009, vol. 71, no. 1, p. 50-56

DOI : 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.009
PMID : 18723054

Available at:
http://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:11827

Disclaimer: layout of this document may differ from the published version.
International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Psychophysiology


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / i j p s yc h o

Electrical autonomic correlates of emotion


Henrique Sequeira a,⁎, Pascal Hot b, Laetitia Silvert c, Sylvain Delplanque d
a
Université de Lille I & Laboratoire de Neurosciences Fonctionnelles et Pathologies, CNRS UMR 8160, CHRU, Hôpital Salengro, EFV, 59037 Lille cedex, France
b
LPNC-UMR CNRS 5105, Université de Savoie, UFR LLSH, Jacob-Bellecombette, BP1104, 73000 Chambéry, Cedex, France
c
LAPSCO, 34 av. Carnot, 63037 Clermont-Ferrand Cedex, France
d
Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, rue des Battoirs 7, 1205 Geneva, Switzerland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Available online 23 July 2008 This paper focussed on how electrophysiological autonomic data may contribute to better understand neural
substrates of emotional processing. The utility of autonomic electrophysiological markers for assessing
Keywords: emotional and cognitive processes is presented in the context of an important bodily arousal interface.
Autonomic nervous system Components of general autonomic control are reviewed and relevant neural modulations of specific autonomic
Cardiovascular
variables were discussed. The role of autonomic feedback on central processes is emphasized and neural
Consciousness
influences on autonomic activities as an index of arousal dimension, the electrodermal activity (EDA), are
Electrodermal activity
Electrophysiology outlined. An overview of brain mechanisms governing generation and control of EDA is presented, and the
Emotion contribution of electrodermal parameters as indices of emotional activation illustrated by data related to
Mind diurnal emotional reactivity and to non consciously subjective emotionality. Conclusions highlight the role of
electrical autonomic expressions as tools to explore emotional components of mind-body-mind relationships.
© 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction mechanisms and peripheral autonomic responses, assumed that any


organ influenced by the autonomic nervous system (ANS) could be a
The electrophysiological approach of the mind is normally carried potential index of mind activity. In line with these premises, the use of
out through central and peripheral electrical measures of the nervous the autonomic responses as markers of emotion, attention, decision-
system activity. In recent years central measures (electrical, magnetic, making, motor preparation, reward or punishment anticipation,
metabolic, hemodynamic) have acquired impressive temporal and unconscious detection, has been strongly developed since the 80s
spatial resolution and led to increasingly detailed functional maps of (e.g., Öhman et al., 2000). Current emotional research, particularly
brain activity. In spite of these technical improvements, somatic sustained by emergent disciplines as affective neuroscience (Pank-
(electromyography, eye-tracking) and autonomic (pupillometry, elec- sepp, 1998) and affective sciences (Davidson et al., 2003), seems to
trodermal activity, cardiovascular) peripheral electrical measures renew with the analysis of the ANS as a major vehicle of the activation
have been used successfully as indicators of mind expressions. Though dimension of emotion (e.g., Lang et al., 1993).
these measures are often contrasted along the dimensions of spatial The present paper focuses on how electrophysiological autonomic
and temporal resolution, early psychophysiological research proposed data may contribute to better understand the neural substrates of
that they can provide sensitive information on central activation and emotional processing. The autonomic activity was presented as a
thus can be useful as complementary probes of higher mental pro- relevant body–brain interface, and particularly how neuroelectrical
cesses (Duffy, 1951). and behaviourally expressions of arousal, indexed by electroder-
Autonomic electrical recordings, obtained for the first time at the mal activity (EDA), could be a pertinent body window on emotional
end of the nineteenth century (Féré, 1888), largely preceded the processes.
recording of cerebral signals in humans (Berger, 1929). The psy-
chometer, an instrument allowing to record autonomic measures, 2. Autonomic versus neurocognitive electrophysiology
became extremely popular as a way of revealing aspects of mental life
and constituted a surprising belief in machines for reading thoughts The autonomic activity corresponds to the physiological expres-
(Newman and Blanton, 1968). Fifty years later, the activation arousal sions, mostly electrical and hormonal, under the control of the ANS
theory (Lindsley, 1951), describing a continuity between central and represents the neural activity related to the brain and body
regulation. Historically, the use of the term autonomic implied that
such part of the nervous system was functionally independent of any
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +33 3 20 44 62 81; fax: +33 3 20 44 67 32. voluntary nervous or cognitive control. Phylogenetically, the ANS is a
E-mail address: henrique.sequeira@univ-lille1.fr (H. Sequeira). major and ancient component of the nervous system which controls

0167-8760/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.009
H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56 51

visceral targets like cardiovascular tissues (heart, blood vessels),


smooth muscles (most visceral organs), glands (endocrine and
exocrine) and sensory systems (eyes, skin), having specific roles in
physiological and behavioural adaptation.
Considering the role of the ANS in basic organic functions, at first
glance it seems quite surprising to consider it as a relevant way to
index brain functions. In addition, the capacity of central electro-
physiology to provide real-time indices of the flow of information
processing makes it difficult to admit the relevance of autonomic
electrophysiology as a window on brain activity. The main disadvan-
tage of the use of electrical autonomic measures is that they are
indirect indices of brain activity. These measures possibly reflect many
sources of variance and only a small minority will have a significant
psychological relevance. For example, heart rate is modulated by Fig. 1. Outline of the global organization of the autonomic control. Cortical, limbic and
mechanical (venous return of blood), hemodynamic (baroreceptor hypothalamic influences are directed to premotor (PMN) and preganglionic (PGN)
neurons mainly located at bulbar and spinal levels. Autonomic efferences are
feedback), humoral (circulating catecholamines) and neural (vagal
reintegrated at ganglionic level (G) before reaching autonomic targets (at). Broken
tone and sympathetic activation) factors. Then it is necessary to arrows indicate sensory autonomic information.
embed the psychological question in a complex physiological context,
mainly related to the maintenance of vital functions (Öhman et al.,
2000). In other words, any mind-autonomic related activity interacts ganglionic neurons that are directly connected with autonomic targets
with this vital activity. On the contrary, the main advantage is that are located. All motor and sensory autonomic information is organized
variations in any autonomic measure can be easily related with and transmitted by the two main autonomic nervous divisions — the
functional consequences. Indeed, in contrast to central measurements parasympathetic (PS) and sympathetic (S) components.
that are linked to the processing, the autonomic measures are closed The parasympathetic division has anatomical and neurochemical
to the effectors and reflect the outcomes of the processing and characteristics that allow different functional adaptations, often in
adjustments of the function. That is why measuring both central and opposition to those of the sympathetic system. The parasympathetic
peripheral activities can offer a better understanding of how infor- outflow is under the control of autonomic neurons located in several
mation is processed and what are the functional consequences. Also, nuclei of brain stem and sacral spinal cord whereas the sympathetic
autonomic recordings have the ability to provide quite easily tonic outflow is under the exclusive control of spinal thoracic neurons. Both
indices of subject's ongoing activity and phasic variations of his outflows are modulated by descending tonic and transient influences
response to a specific stimulus (Venables, 1991). As underlined by from the brainstem, the hypothalamus, the limbic system and even
Cacioppo and Tassinary (1990), nearly every bodily system, especially cortical areas (Cecheto and Saper, 1990; Sequeira et al., 1995). These
the autonomic one, has been monitored by psychophysiologists with influences act on different neuronal functional subsets that control
the aim of gaining some insight into the workings of human brain. specific peripheral targets like visceral motor, muscle vasomotor, skin
Before considering possibilities afforded by combining autonomic vasomotor, pilomotor and sudomotor units. Following the traditional
and neurocognitive recordings, it seems relevant to go further on the doctrine of the autonomic balance, most autonomic targets are under
analysis of autonomic control and its bodily expressions. a dual antagonist control exerted respectively by PS and S components
(e.g., cardiac vagal inhibitory control versus cardiac sympathetic ex-
3. The autonomic activity as a body–brain interface citatory control). However, although such reciprocal model of ANS
function is largely valid, it does not explain the entire range of
The autonomic control mainly regulates the internal environment autonomic control. As an alternative, Berntson et al. (1991) proposed a
in order to maintain the body homeostasis. It facilitates by this way multidimensional model to account for autonomic regulation, based
adaptive responses of the endocrine, immune, sensori-motor and on the notion of autonomic space. According to these authors, the
cognitive systems (Porges, 1995; Sequeira et al., 2000). In this context, multiple modes of autonomic control do not lie along a single con-
ironically, the brain is perhaps the less resilient to disruptions of tinuum extending from parasympathetic to sympathetic dominance
homeostasis. In fact, the brain is one of the first organs to be damaged but rather distribute within a two dimensional space. This model,
by lack of oxygen, glucose, pH imbalance and overheating (Clarke and verified empirically, incorporates the recognized complexities of auto-
Sokoloff, 1993). The brain is, in the truest sense, embodied and the nomic control and therefore allows us to better understand subtle
autonomic control appears as a crucial interface to maintain the autonomic variations and particularly those under the influence of
brain function: the mind (Niedenthal, 2007). However, the role of higher structures.
autonomic activity cannot be reduced to maintain brain or body In general, sympathetic outflow responds to the emergency
immediate homeostasis, it provides support to complex behaviours, as situations (fight and flight reactions) and facilitates the expenditure
emotional reaction. In that sense, autonomic activity is reactive to a of energy (emotion, cold, exercise, pain) whereas parasympathetic
stimulus but also contains an anticipatory dimension. division is more active during rest and sleep and promotes restoration
and conservation of energy. For example, life-threatening situations
3.1. Components of autonomic control modulate deeply the sympathetic outflow that includes vasoconstric-
tion in the skin and splanchnic vessels, vasodilatation in skeletal
Fig. 1 illustrates that the autonomic control is exerted by neuronal muscles, piloerection, sweating and release of adrenaline from adrenal
networks located in the central and peripheral nervous systems (e.g., medulla (Folkow, 2000). Apart from that, it is known that sympathetic
Sequeira et al., 2000). The central autonomic network contains two nerve activity in the skin is mainly regulated by environmental
kinds of autonomic neurons, premotor and preganglionic neurons. temperature and central activations related to affective and cognitive
These neurons are distributed at three levels: hypothalamus, brain states (Wallin and Fagius, 1986). Therefore, autonomic electrophysiol-
stem (nuclei of cranial nerves, rostro-ventrolatral medulla, nucleus of ogy has the ability to provide information on global orientation of
the tractus solitarius), and spinal cord (intermediolateral cell column) brain dynamics as revealed by arousal levels and brings enough phasic
(Jordan, 1997). Preganglionic neurons send fibers to autonomic information to help to “extract subprocesses” (Williams et al., 2004).
ganglia, small and complex peripheral neuronal centers, where post- In brief, neuronal descending subsets could integrate influences from
52 H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56

specific central processes and contribute to explain why it is possible receiving a growing attention (Barsalou, 1999; Prinz, 2002), neurobe-
to record distinct autonomic parameters associated with different havioural effects of ascending visceral afferent information have been
emotions (Collet et al., 1997; Kreibig et al., 2007; Rainville et al., 2006). mainly considered in the domain of emotion. Indeed, even if the
However, future assessment of autonomic patterns should include original hypothesis proposed by James (1894) that emotional
multidimensional peripheral physiological measurements in order to experience was mainly regulated by somatovisceral feedback is no
increase the reliability of autonomic signatures of emotional states. In longer viable, considerable evidence has accumulated showing
addition, new research directions concern the use of appraisal models specific effects of autonomic feedback on cortical or identified mental
of emotion to scheme detailed predictions of specific physiological activities (Berntson et al., 2002; Williams and Clayton, 2001).
changes expected to occur as a consequence of specific appraisal The current idea about the impact of autonomic afferences on
results (e.g., Delplanque et al., submitted for publication; Scherer and emotion and cognition is illustrated by bottom-up visceral regulation
Ellgring, 2007). (Berntson et al., 2003), somatic markers and decision-making
(Bechara et al., 1999; Damasio, 1994), and autonomic feedback on
3.2. Autonomic responses central regulation of arousal (Nagai et al., 2004). An important finding
of the last study was that a decrease in autonomic arousal measured
“Eyes wide, heart pounding, hands clammy”: In those terms, Kutas peripherally was accompanied by an increase in cortical excitation.
and Federmeier (1998) clearly express well known daily life sensa- Hence, the cortical activation was greatest after the subject had
tions, typically linked to autonomic variations triggered by affective performed the biofeedback relaxation exercise to reduce autonomic
events. They implicitly show the relevance of autonomic electro- arousal, indexed by electrodermal responses. These findings illustrate
physiology in recording variations of pupillary responses, cardiovas- the fact that biofeedback aimed at increasing autonomic arousal may
cular activity and electrical properties of the skin as potential markers have the potential to change cortical seizure threshold and therefore
of affective or cognitive expressions. For example, pupillary responses to reduce the occurrence of epileptic seizures.
have been correlated with emotional processing, cognitive load or In sum, models of emotional processing are highly diverging about
degrees of alertness, and task-evoked changes in pupil size can be the role of physiological responses to the emergence of an emotional
observed within the first several hundred milliseconds after the experience. In the hard version of ascending control, autonomic feed-
stimulus presentation (Beatty, 1986). Electrodermal and cardiovascu- back is required to experience emotion, whereas soft version con-
lar responses are acknowledged to index respectively the activation siders this useful but not necessary. Otherwise, autonomic responses
level and the valence of emotional stimuli (Bradley and Lang, 2000; are considered as a top-down intrinsic vehicle involved in emotional
Solbakk et al., 2005). Various aspects of cardiovascular functioning expression. Autonomic responses seem coupled with changes in
(heart rate variability, blood pressure, peripheral blood flow) can also regulation of affective states by means of descending and ascending
be correlated with emotions (Lang et al., 1993), passive and active circuits that support temporal delayed top-down and bottom-up
attention (Öhman et al., 2000), or motor processes (Sequeira and Ba- driven processing. An important direction for future certainly needs to
M'Hamed, 1999). For instance, heart rate acceleration often accom- increase coupling between neurocognitive measurements and auto-
panies perception of unpleasant compared with pleasant pictures nomic indices enable to express refined levels of bodily arousal, as
(Palomba et al., 1997; Solbakk et al., 2005) and heart rate deceleration indexed by electrodermal activity.
can be observed during detection tasks (Lacey and Lacey, 1978).
Furthermore, Jennings (1992) showed interesting cognitive–auto- 4. Electrodermal activity: a window on the arousal dimension
nomic interactions associated to a simple motor preparation task. In of emotion
particular, inhibition of the motor response during the warning period
was accompanied by heart rate decelerations. Thus, changes in EDA variations depend on the quantity of sweat secreted by eccrine
electrical properties of the skin have been used successfully to study sweat glands. These glands are mainly located in the hypodermis of
several dimensions of mind and from a long time ago (Boucsein, 1992). palmar and plantar regions and generate sweat excreted through
Current work carried on electrodermal properties related to im- sweating ducts (Groscurth, 2002). Such secretion is under the control
plicit or explicit arousal aspects of affective and cognitive processing of sympathetic innervation which transmits influences from the
(Critchley, 2002; Tranel and Damasio, 1985; Williams et al., 2004), central nervous system to the eccrine glands. Sweating variations are
largely explains why this autonomic marker has been used recently as sensitive markers of events having a particular signification for
a choice index of most emotional and cognitive activations in a large individuals, usually related to emotional, novelty or attentional fields.
panel of psychophysiological studies. In spite of interesting use of For this reason, the eccrine sweating activity is also known as palmar,
autonomic responses to index psychological processes, the multi- mental or emotional sweating (Ashina et al., 2003) in opposition to the
determination of physiological variables makes difficult to provide thermoregulatory sweating. Consequently, the EDA recording has
sufficient information for strong inference about mental activity been extensively used for more than a century in rats, cats, monkeys
(Cacioppo and Tassinary, 1990). Therefore, as discussed by Berntson and humans (see Sequeira and Roy, 1997 for a review). Following the
et al. (2007), multivariate and multilevel approaches, which include publication of guidelines on EDA research by the Society for
physiological and behavioural components, may facilitate the identi- Psychophysiological Research (Fowles et al., 1981), the skin conduc-
fication and development of autonomic markers of affective and tance method became the international standard technique to record
cognitive processes. and analyse the EDA.
The EDA is constituted of two main components: electrodermal
3.3. Autonomic responses feedback on central processes level and electrodermal responses. Electrodermal level corresponds to
slow spontaneous electrical fluctuations of sweat gland activity which
Spontaneous or induced activity of autonomic targets continuously result from the interaction between tonic discharges of sympathetic
provides sensory feedback to the brain and thus contributes to innervation and local factors (skin temperature and hydratation)
regulate lower nervous outflow, brainstem vital circuits and higher (Macefield and Wallin, 1996). Electrodermal responses are correlated
forebrain networks (see Berntson et al., 2003 for a review). Autonomic with phasic sympathetic nervous discharges (Lidberg and Wallin,
influences on higher neurobehavioural processes seem be transmitted 1981) and are characterized by their amplitude and three main tem-
by specific neural ascending pathways implicating brainstem circuits, poral measures: their latency, their duration (rise time and recovery
the amygdala and the basal forebrain areas. Although the idea of an time), and their surface. In addition to the important analysis of
autonomic feedback on human cognition as a whole has been recently amplitude, electrodermal temporal parameters had been interpreted
H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56 53

tract or reticulo-spinal pathways (Sequeira et al., 1995). Thus, neural


studies clearly support the implication of the EDA as a bodily win-
dow of reticular, limbic and cortical areas related to major mind
components such as emotion, preparation to action and vigilance
processes.

4.2. Electrodermal activity as an index of emotional activation

EDA is a good indicator of reticular activation and therefore seems


to reflect the energetic dimension of behaviour and particularly of
emotion. Indeed, the amplitude of electrodermal responses increased
linearly as ratings of arousal increased, regardless of emotional va-
lence (Bradley and Lang, 2000). This effect is observed when emo-
tional pictures (Winton et al., 1984) or emotional words (Manning and
Melchiori, 1974) are used. Lang et al. (1993) further showed a positive
correlation between arousal reports and conductance response ampli-
tudes in more than 80% of subjects. The EDA amplitude as a good
indicator of emotional arousal is illustrated below by some of our
Fig. 2. Global representation of the central control of electrodermal activity resulting experimental data obtained to different emotional demands.
from animal and human data. Excitatory (+) and inhibitory (−) descending influences
are integrated by preganglionic neurons (PGN) located at the spinal cord. These neurons
send final impulses to sympathetic ganglia (G) and to sweat glands (SG). The spinal cord
4.2.1. EDA as a marker of diurnal emotional reactivity
is under the strong control of reticular activating (RAS) and inhibiting (RIS) systems Classical data have shown that changes in autonomic activation
which are mainly influenced by frontal (Fro), parietal (Par), cingular (Cin) and preceding the onset of a stimulus, could modulate emotional
amygdaloid (Amy) brain regions. Note that these regions can activate or inhibit brain responses in normal (Lazarus, 1985) and in brain injured subjects
stem reticular structures whereas corticospinal fibers (CT) can activate both reticular
(Damasio, 1994). Furthermore, focusing on physiological components
and spinal levels. Other abbreviations: Hip, hippocampus; Occ, occipital; Tem, temporal.
of emotional reactivity, several authors reported spontaneous diurnal
fluctuations of autonomic variables like tonic and phasic EDA
(Venables and Christie, 1973; Hot et al., 1999). These data on diurnal
tentatively as indicators of particular emotional or cognitive function variations of EDA raise the question about temporal organization of
(e.g., see discussion, Bundy and Fitzgerald, 1975). Consequently, the psychophysiological states sustaining the emotional activation. There-
facility of the EDA measurement allows the use of electrodermal fore, the research of temporal patterning between components of
recordings as choice tools to explore a large panel of mind expressions: emotional reactivity is particularly relevant because affective dis-
emotions, vigilance states (Ba-M'Hamed et al., 1993), hemispheric orders, like mood ones, have been considered as the consequence of
specializations, decision-making (Amiez et al., 2003), reasoning bias temporal dysregulations of physiological systems allowing the tonic
(Carbonnell et al., 2006), and several behavioural adaptations such expression of emotions (Wirz-Justice and Van den Hoofdakker, 1999).
as grasping, ambulation and tactual exploration (Edelberg, 1973). Toward this end, Hot et al. (2005) investigated diurnal variations of
However, some clarification is needed concerning the contribution of autonomic activation and their relationships with emotional reactiv-
brain mechanisms implicated in emotion and cognition to peripheral ity components in healthy subjects. EDA, affective evaluation, and
electrodermal responses. emotional experience were recorded before or during the presenta-
tion of emotional pictures through several sessions in the daytime.
4.1. Central control of EDA Stimuli were selected from the International Affective Picture System
(Lang et al., 1988) and healthy undergraduate participants were
Fig. 2 illustrates central mechanisms of EDA, largely known from included in the study. Each participant performed seven experimental
data acquired in animal and in human experiments. Human data were sessions and sets of neutral and unpleasant pictures were presented in
obtained in association with the cognitive electrophysiology and counterbalanced orders between participants. Tonic (skin conduc-
neuroimaging techniques, in normal, psychopathological and brain- tance level, SCL) and phasic (skin conductance responses, SCR) EDA
damaged subjects. Skin regions allowing easy electrodermal record- was concomitantly recorded. After each picture presentation, par-
ings are directly innervated by postganglionic sudomotor neurones, ticipants indicated their affective evaluation by estimating the va-
located in ganglia of the lateral sympathetic chains (Jänig and Kümmel, lence and the arousal of pictures with the SAM rating system (Self-
1981). These neurons are under the direct influence of spinal Assessment Manikin; Bradley and Lang, 1994). At the end of the
sudomotor neurons, located in the intermediolateral nucleus of the session, participants reported the emotional experience induced by
spinal cord (Cabot, 1990) which are under permanent excitatory and the experimental session by means of a visual analogue scale.
inhibitory impulses from supraspinal centers, corresponding mainly to Fig. 3 partially illustrates the major results: (1) pre-stimulus SCL
the classical reticular formation of the brain stem (Roy et al., 1974). values increased during the daytime; (2) SCRs to unpleasant pictures
Reticular structures have excitatory (reticular activating system, RAS) but not to neutral ones presented a significant amplitude increase
or inhibitory (reticular inhibiting system, RIS) effects on EDA through during the daytime; (3) the self-reported emotional impact of sessions
descending neural circuits. also increased during the daytime; and (4) the analysis of temporal
Several studies based on neurophysiological brain imaging and relationships between variables revealed that the SCL and SCRs values
neuropsychological methods have demonstrated strong influences of to unpleasant pictures were positively correlated with the emo-
hypothalamus, limbic system (amygdala and hippocampus, cingulated tional experience. Taken together, this study showed a close temporal
gyrus) and cortical frontal areas on EDA elicitation (Lee et al., 1988; relation between EDA and emotional experience and thus contributes
Mangina and Beuzeron-Mangina, 1996). Zahn et al. (1999) suggested to emphasize the role of temporal autonomic patterns as a basic
that reduced EDA following cerebral damages and in particular in indicator of emotionality. These findings illustrate the relevance of
frontal regions mirrors impairment in mental processing associated to autonomic electrophysiology as a means to assess subjective emo-
EDA rather than a direct control of frontal regions on EDA. It also tional experience that can subserve emotional reactivity in healthy
was found that cortical influences could be mediated by the pyramidal participants and probably in psychiatric patients.
54 H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56

Fig. 3. Diurnal variations of mean amplitude of skin conductance responses (SCR) to unpleasant and neutral pictures (A) and mean values of emotional experience obtained by z-
transformation of individual data (B). Error bars represent the standard error to the mean. Adapted from Hot et al. (2005).

4.2.2. EDA, emotional words and consciousness place on two different days of the same week. The aim of the pre-
The contribution of the EDA recordings is not limited to the experimental phase was to determine, for each participant, the pre-
investigation of consciously reported subjective emotionality and can sentation duration of the masked words for the experimental phase
be interestingly extended to the investigation of non conscious
processing. In that sense, EDA constitutes a particularly pertinent
window on the mind when subjectively reported experience is not
possible. Indeed, a growing body of evidence suggests that judgment
of the affective relevance of a stimulus for the individual, especially
the fear relevance, can be made without the identification of the
precipitating stimulus. In this frame, a good deal of research has been
devoted to evaluate how individual reacts to aversive stimuli even if
these are not consciously identified. To this end, some studies have
measured electrodermal reactivity, as an index of emotional arousal,
to non-identified fearful pictures (Öhman and Soares, 1994). Results
have been interpreted in terms of biologically prepared fear reactions
and thus more likely to occur to pictorial stimuli. In these conditions,
one can wonder whether non-biologically prepared aversive visual
stimuli, like words, may elicit autonomic activation when they are not
consciously perceived. In spite of a study regarding the emotional
verbal material in phobic participants (Van den Hout et al., 2000), it
appeared important to demonstrate that any autonomic activation
related to affective value of words may be independent from any kind
of access of this value to consciousness. A study was conducted to
assess whether a non conscious physiological activity, like skin con-
ductance responses, could reflect the discrimination of the emotional
value (unpleasant versus neutral) of non consciously perceived words
(Silvert et al., 2004).
Fig. 4 illustrates the primary results. French emotional words with
a negative value (e.g., war, cancer, murder, etc.) and neutral words
(e.g., table, door, notebook, etc.) were presented on a computer
monitor to undergraduate female participants. The experiment was Fig. 4. Mean √SCR to neutral and emotional (unpleasant) words in the masked and
constituted of two phases, pre-experimental and experimental, taking unmasked modes of presentation. Adapted from Silvert et al. (2004).
H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56 55

in which EDA was recorded. The experimental phase included a Acknowledgements


masked presentation session followed by an unmasked presentation
session. During the masked presentation session, the words were This paper was essentially written during an invited stay of H.S.
projected for the duration determined for each participant in the pre- at the Ashikaga Sleep Research Center (ASRC, Ashikaga Institute of
experimental phase. At this duration, participants were unable to Technology, Japon). We are especially grateful to Prs T. Kobayashi,
identify the words and discriminate their emotional valence above Y. Saito and O. Mandai for all stay facilities.
chance level. During the unmasked presentation session, the words
lasted 150 ms and were easily readable, as reported by all participants. References
EDA was continuously recorded, except during the 5 min rest period
between the two sessions. SCRs elicited by unmasked words, and also Amiez, C., Procyk, E., Honore, J., Sequeira, H., Joseph, J.P., 2003. Decision-making and
by masked words, were of greater magnitude when the words were electrodermal skin conductance responses in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 267–275.
emotional than when they were neutral. Ashina, M., Suzuki, A., Mori, M., Kanesaka, T., Hattori, T., 2003. Emotional sweating response
in a patient with bilateral amygdala damage. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 87–93.
This study demonstrates that, in normal participants, masked
Ba-M'Hamed, S., Sequeira, H., Roy, J.C., 1993. Bilateral electrodermal activity during
verbal material is autonomically discriminated in the absence of sleep and waking in the cat. Sleep 16, 695–701.
stimulus identification and of conscious valence identification. The Barsalou, L.W., 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577–609.
Başar, E., 2005. A nebulous Cartesian system. In the brain-body-mind incorporation.
autonomic activation could be a marker of pre-attentive detection
Webbing principles of Descartes, Darwin, Einstein, Cajal., Bergson, Berger and
of emotional stimuli, especially arousal negative ones. Thus, among Hayek. Proceedings of Mind and Electrophysiology International Meeting,
the great number of autonomic activity indicators, EDA parameters Guadalajara, p. 15.
constitute a powerful tool to investigate both the subjectively enlight- Beatty, J., 1986. The pupillary system. In: Coles, M.G.H., Donchin, E., Porges, S.W. (Eds.),
Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications. Guilford Press, New York,
ened and hidden parts of emotional processing. pp. 43–50.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A.R., Lee, G.P.,1999. Different contributions of the human
5. Conclusions amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J. Neurosci. 19,
5473–5481.
Berger, H., 1929. Über das Elektroenkephalogramm des Menschen. Arch. Psychiatr.
This review attempts to outline how the functioning of visceral Nervenkr. 87, 527–570.
targets, regulated by phylogenetically old parts of the nervous Berntson, G.G., Cacioppo, J.T., Quigley, K.S., 1991. Autonomic determinism: the modes of
autonomic control, the doctrine of autonomic space, and the laws of autonomic
system and sustaining vital or crucial adaptive organic systems, can constraint. Psychol. Rev. 98, 459–487.
be related to more recent and complex forms of brain organization Berntson, G.G., Shafi, R., Sarter, M., 2002. Specific contributions of the basal forebrain
and emotional or cognitive expressions. First, electrical autonomic corticopetal cholinergic system to electroencephalographic activity and sleep/waking
behaviour. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 2453–2461.
indices were present at the very beginning of electrophysiological
Berntson, G.G., Sarter, M., Cacioppo, J.T., 2003. Ascending visceral regulation of cortical
methods applied to the exploration of mind processes and remain affective information processing. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 2103–3109.
relevant in most current experimental designs, in particular those Berntson, G.G., Cacioppo, J.T., Grossman, P., 2007. Whither vagal tone. Biol. Psychol. 74,
295–300.
including emotional processes. For example, even though recent
Boucsein, W., 1992. Electrodermal Activity. Plenum, New York. 456 pp.
studies focussed on imaging data from MRI and PET activation in Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., 1994. Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and
high cortical processing like decision-making or in exploration of the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59.
emotional learning (e.g., Cacioppo and Gardner, 1999; Phelps, 2006), Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., 2000. Measuring emotion: behavior, feeling and physiology. In:
Lane, R.D., Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion. Oxford University
physiological responses are necessary and crucial to establish if Press, Oxford, pp. 242–276.
conditioning or emotional processing have been performed (Bechara Bundy, R.S., Fitzgerald, H.E., 1975. Stimulus specificity of electrodermal recovery time: an
et al., 1999). Autonomic responses proved to be sophisticated enough examination and reinterpretation of the evidence. Psychophysiology 12, 406–411.
Cabot, J.B., 1990. Sympathetic preganglionic neurons: cytoarchitecture, ultrastructure,
to allow to disentangle mental processes like emotion, motor prepa- and biophysical properties. In: Loewy, A.D., Spyer, K.M. (Eds.), Central Regulation of
ration, attention or consciousness. In particular, EDA indexation Autonomic Functions. Oxford university Press, Oxford, pp. 44–67.
often confined to some recurrent questions like somato-autonomic Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., 1990. Inferring psychological significance from physiolog-
ical signals. Am. Psychol. 46, 16–28.
relationships, hemispheric lateralization and preparation to the Cacioppo, J.T., Gardner, W.L., 1999. Emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 191–214.
action, gained significant extension to understand emotional states Carbonnell, L., Vidal, F., Sequeira, H., Caverni, J.P., 2006. A reasoning bias revealed by
in neuropsychology, neurology and neuropathology fields (e.g., electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology 43, 387–393.
Cecheto, D.F., Saper, C.B., 1990. Role of the cortex in autonomic function. In: Loewy, A.D.,
Critchley, 2002). Second, autonomic electrical changes may consti-
Spyer, C.B. (Eds.), Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions. Oxford University
tute an important tool to understand complex bidirectional relation- Press, New York, pp. 208–223.
ships between the mind and the body. As previously recalled, studies Clarke, D.D., Sokoloff, L., 1993. Circulation and energy metabolism of the brain. In:
Siegel, G.J., Agranoff, B.W., Albers, R.W., Molinoff, P.B. (Eds.), Basic Neurochemistry.
showed the impact of subtle emotional or cognitive processes on
Raven Press, New York, pp. 645–680.
autonomic indices. The analysis of these top-down influences may Collet, C., Vernet-Maury, E., Delhomme, G., Dittmar, A., 1997. Autonomic nervous system
be enriched (1) by theoretical improvements as those proposed by response patterns specificity to basis emotions. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 62, 45–57.
the concept of somatic markers (Damasio, 1994), autonomic space Critchley, H.D., 2002. Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. Neuroscientist
8, 132–142.
(Berntson et al., 2003) and polyvagal theory (Porges, 2007), (2) by Critchley, H.D., Melmed, R.N., Featherstone, E., Mathias, C.J., Dolan, R.J., 2002. Volitional
new technical skills in the autonomic field, as the electrogastrogram control of autonomic arousal: a functional magnetic resonance study. NeuroImage
(Vianna and Tranel, 2006), and (3) by methods coupling electrical 16, 909–919.
Damasio, A.R., 1994. Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. Grosset/
autonomic indices (EDA, heart rate) with cognitive electrophysiolo- Putman, New York. 175 pp.
gical (EEG, event-related potentials, magnetoencephalography) or Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K.R., Goldsmith, H.H., 2003. Handbook of Affective Sciences.
brain imaging techniques (fMRI), as illustrated by several innovative Oxford University Press, New York. 1218 pp.
Delplanque, S., Grandjean, D., Chrea, C., Aymard, L., Cayeux, I., Margot, C., Velazco, M.I.,
works (e.g., Lim et al., 1999; Critchley et al., 2002; Williams et al., Sander, D., Scherer, K.R. Sequential unfolding of novelty and pleasantness appraisals of
2004; Solbakk et al., 2005; Takahashi et al., 2005; Gemignani et al., odors: evidence from facial electromyography and autonomic reactions, submitted
2006). Third, direct measures of brain electrical, magnetic, and meta- for publication.
Duffy, E., 1951. The concept of energy mobilization. Psychol. Rev. 58, 30–40.
bolic activities have the high potential to tells us when, what and
Edelberg, R., 1973. In: Stellar, E., Sprague, J.M. (Eds.), Mechanisms of electrodermal
where about mind components. The increasing knowledge about adaptations for locomotion, manipulation, or defense. . Progress in Physiological
electrical autonomic measures illuminates a conception of how emo- Psychology, vol. 5. Academic Press, New York, pp. 155–209.
Féré, C., 1888. Note sur les modifications de la résistance électrique sous l'influence des
tional and cognitive processes interact with the body and joins the
excitations sensorielles et des émotions. C.R. Soc. Biol. 5, 217–219.
global view of “brain-body-mind incorporation” proposed by Başar Folkow, B., 2000. Perspectives on the integrative functions of the “sympatho-
(2005). adrenomedullary system”. Auton. Neurosci. 83, 101–115.
56 H. Sequeira et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 71 (2009) 50–56

Fowles, D.C., Christie, M.J., Edelberg, R., Grings, D.T., Venables, P.H., 1981. Committee Panksepp, J., 1998. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal
report: publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psycho- Emotions. Oxford University Press, New York. 466 pp.
physiology 18, 232–239. Phelps, E.A., 2006. Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala.
Gemignani, A., Sebastiani, L., Simoni, A., Santarcangelo, E.L., Ghelarducci, B., 2006. Hypnotic Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 27–53.
trait and specific phobia: EEG and autonomic output during phobic stimulation. Porges, S.W., 1995. Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our
Brain Res. Bull. 69, 197–203. evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal Theory. Psychophysiology 32, 301–318.
Groscurth, P., 2002. Anatomy of sweat glands. In: Kreyden, O.P., Böni, R., Burg, G. (Eds.), Porges, S.W., 2007. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74, 116–143.
Hyperhidrosis and Botulinum Toxin in Dermatology. Karger, Basel, pp. 1–9. Prinz, J., 2002. Furnishing the Mind: Concepts and their Perceptual Basis. MIT Press,
Hot, P., Naveteur, J., Leconte, P., Sequeira, H., 1999. Diurnal variation of tonic electro- Cambridge, MA.
dermal activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 27, 223–230. Rainville, P., Bechara, A., Naqvi, N., Damasio, A.R., 2006. Basic emotions are associated
Hot, P., Leconte, P., Sequeira, H., 2005. Diurnal autonomic variations and emotional with distinct patterns of cardiorespiratory activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 61, 5–18.
reactivity. Biol. Psychol. 69, 261–270. Roy, J.C., Delerm, B., Granger, L., 1974. L'inhibiton bulbaire de l'activité électrodermale
James, W., 1894. Physical basis of emotion. Psychol. Rev 1, 516–529 Reprinted 1994, chez le chat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 37, 621–632.
Psychol. Rev. 101, 205–210. Scherer, K.R., Ellgring, H., 2007. Multimodal expression of emotion: affect programs or
Jänig, W., Kümmel, H., 1981. Organization of the sympathetic innervation supplying the componential appraisal patterns? Emotion 7, 158–171.
hairless skin of the cat's paw. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 3, 215–230. Sequeira, H., Roy, J.C., 1997. In: Jordan, D. (Ed.), Neural control of electrodermal activity.
Jennings, J.R., 1992. Is it important that the mind is in a body? Inhibition and the heart. Central Nervous Control of Autonomic Function, vol. 11. Harwood Academic Publish-
Psychophysiology 29, 369–383. ers, Amsterdam, pp. 259–293.
Jordan, D., 1997. Central Control of Autonomic Function. Harwood Academic Publishers, Sequeira, H., Ba-M'Hamed, S., 1999. Pyramidal control of heart rate and arterial pressure
Amsterdam. 304 pp. in cats. Arch. Ital. Biol. 137, 1–16.
Kreibig, S.D., Wilhelm, F.H., Roth, W.T., Gross, J.J., 2007. Cardiovascular, electrodermal, Sequeira, H., Ba-M'Hamed, S., Roy, J.C., 1995. Fronto-parietal control of electrodermal
and respiratory responses patterns to fear and sadness-inducing films. Psychophy- activity in the cat. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 53, 103–114.
siology 44, 787–806. Sequeira, H., Viltart, O., Ba-M'Hamed, S., Poulain, P., 2000. Cortical Control of somato-
Kutas, M., Federmeier, K.D., 1998. Minding the body. Psychophysiology 35, 135–150. cardiovascular integration: neuroanatomical studies. Brain Res. Bull. 53, 87–93.
Lacey, B.C., Lacey, J.I., 1978. Two way communication between the heart and the brain. Silvert, L., Delplanque, S., Verpoort, C., Sequeira, H., 2004. Autonomic responding to
Am. Psychol. 33, 99–113. aversive words without conscious valence discrimination. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 53,
Lang, P.J., Öhman, A., Vaitl, D., 1988. The International Affective Picture System 135–145.
(Photographic slides). Center for Research in Psychophysiology, Univ of Florida, Solbakk, A.K., Reinyang, I., Nielsen, C.S., 2005. ERP indices of resource allocation diffi-
Gainesville, FL. culties in mild head injury. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 743–760.
Lang, P.J., Greenwald, M.K., Hamm, A.O., 1993. Looking at pictures: affective, facial., Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M., Yoshida, H.,
visceral., and behavioural reactions. Psychophysiology 30, 261–273. Wada, Y., 2005. Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation
Lazarus, R.S., 1985. The psychology of stress and coping. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 7, and their association with personality traits. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 55, 199–207.
399–418. Tranel, D., Damasio, A.R., 1985. Knowledge without awareness: an automatic index of
Lee, G.P., Arena, J.G., Meador, K.J., Smith, J.R., Loring, D.W., Flanigin, H.F., 1988. Change facial recognition by prosopagnostics. Science 228, 1453–1454.
in autonomic responsiveness following bilateral amygdalotomy in humans. Van den Hout, M.A., De Jong, P., Kindt, M., 2000. Masked fear words produce increased
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 1, 119–129. SCRs: an anomaly for Ohman's theory of pre-attentive processing in anxiety.
Lidberg, L., Wallin, B.G., 1981. Sympathetic skin nerve in relation to amplitude of skin Psychophysiology 37, 283–288.
resistance responses. Psychophysiology 18, 268–270. Venables, P.H., 1991. Autonomic activity. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 620, 191–207.
Lim, C.L., Gordon, E., Rennie, C., Wright, J.J., Bahramali, H., Li, W.M., Clouston, P., Morris, J.G., Venables, P.H., Christie, M.J., 1973. Mechanisms, instrumentation, recording techniques,
1999. Dynamics of SCR, EEG, and ERP activity in an oddball paradigm with short and quantification of responses. In: Prokasy, W.F., Raskin, D.C. (Eds.), Electrodermal
interstimulus intervals. Psychophysiology 36, 543–551. Activity in Psychological Research. Academic Press, New York, pp. 23–109.
Lindsley, D.B., 1951. Emotion. In: Stevens, S.S. (Ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Vianna, E.P.M., Tranel, D., 2006. Gastric myoelectrical activity as an index of emotional
Wiley, New York, pp. 473–516. arousal. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 61, 70–76.
Macefield, V.G., Wallin, B.G., 1996. The discharge behaviour of single sympathetic Wallin, G., Fagius, J., 1986. The sympathetic nervous system in man — aspects derived
neurones supplying human sweat glands. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 61, 277–286. from microelectrode recordings. Trends Neurosci. 9, 63–67.
Mangina, C.A., Beuzeron-Mangina, J.H.,1996. Direct electrical stimulation of specific human Williams, C.L., Clayton, E.C., 2001. Contribution of brainstem structures in modulating
brain structures and bilateral electrodermal activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 22, 1–8. memory storage processes. In: Gold, P.E., Greenough, W.T. (Eds.), Memory
Manning, S.K., Melchiori, M.P., 1974. Words that upset urban college students: measured Consolidation: Essays in Honor of James L. McGaugh. American Psychological
with GSRs and rating scales. J. Soc. Psychol. 94, 305–306. Association, Washington, DC, pp. 141–163.
Nagai, Y., Goldstein, L.H., Critchley, H.D., Fenwick, P.B.C., 2004. Influence of sympathetic Williams, L.M., Brown, K.J., Das, P., Boucsein, W., Sokolov, E.N., Brammer, M.J., Olivieri, G.,
autonomic arousal on cortical arousal: implications for a therapeutic behavioural Peduto, A., Gordon, E., 2004. The dynamics of cortico-amygdala and autonomic
intervention in epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. 58, 185–193. activity over the experimental time course of fear perception. Cogn. Brain Res. 21,
Newman, E., Blanton, R., 1968. The early history of electrodermal research. Psychophy- 114–123.
siology 6, 453–475. Winton, W.M., Putnam, L.E., Krauss, R.M., 1984. Facial and autonomic manifestations of
Niedenthal, P., 2007. Embodying emotion. Science 18, 1002–1005. the dimensional structure of emotion. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 20, 195–216.
Öhman, A., Soares, J.J., 1994. “Unconscious anxiety”: phobic responses to masked stimuli. Wirz-Justice, A., Van den Hoofdakker, R.H., 1999. Sleep deprivation in depression: what
J. Abnorm. Psychology 103, 231–240. do we know, where do we go? Biol. Psychiatry 46, 445–453.
Öhman, A., Hamm, A., Hugdahl, K., 2000. Cognition and the autonomic nervous system. Zahn, T.P., Grafman, J., Tranel, D., 1999. Frontal lobe lesions and electrodermal activity:
In: Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., Berntson, G.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiol- effects of significance. Neuropsychologia 37, 1227–1241.
ogy. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 533–575.
Palomba, D., Angrilli, A., Mini, A., 1997. Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses
and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 27, 55–67.

You might also like