Hodson Thesis 2020
Hodson Thesis 2020
Hodson Thesis 2020
A THESIS
DEPARTMENT OF SOCIOLOGY
BY
DENTON, TX
AUGUST, 2020
First and foremost, this entire undertaking would not exist if it were not for Black
not being just another mediocre white kid. I can’t document every single person whose
tweets nudged me to stop eavesdropping and take charge of my social justice education, but
Kendall, Michelle Taylor, and Zahira Kelly, respectively); getting blocked by @thetrudz was
one of the best worst things that has ever happened to me. I humbly thank them, and I will
A college degree was rare where I came from, a master’s almost fictional. Thank you
to everyone who did not talk down to me or others, but instead cultivated our learning. As
for what I learned once I got there, my instincts told me that this multidisciplinary approach
would structure my interests best, and it has been more than I dreamed it could be. I had the
alongside the master’s process, and those were the theories that rang true for my
for guiding me toward key components that made this project viable: Profs. Sara Ishii,
Danielle Phillips-Cunningham, Agatha Beins, and Claire Sahlin from MWGS; Prof. Linda
Marshall from Social Work (ret.); and Prof. Sharla Snider in the Department of Professional
Education. Within Sociology, thank you to Prof. Cynthia Cready at University of North
Texas and Prof. Philip Q. Yang at TWU for getting me started in statistics. Special thank you
ii
to the Federation of North Texas Area Universities for facilitating richer and more diverse
learning spaces.
Jillian Morales (MWGS) and Ivy Naude (Sociology); I thank yall for saving me from
erroneous paperwork! Many thanks also to the staff at the Blagg-Huey Library, especially
Shelia Bickle in the archives and Jimmie Lyn Harris, who, sadly, passed away last month after
Earlier drafts of this thesis received vital theoretical feedback from Michelle
Vaughan, PhD, of Wright State University, important readability notes from Dana Sayre,
MA, and incalculable editing and formatting support from Criss Forshay.
from current and recent students, including Elizabeth Gabhart, PhD, of Tarrant County
College, Cassie Withey-Rila of the University of Otago, Natalie Frodin, MS, of University of
North Texas, Beth Fawcett, PhD, Avery Labelson, MA, Petina Powers, Danielle Barber, and
Kay Jae Johns of TWU, and Arielle Webb, MS, of Palo Alto University — I thank each of
you for sharing your journeys with me and letting me share in yours.
Thanks to the judge and to J. Selah Richards for helping me find myself again after
caregiving. Thanks to Julia Martin, for remaining a dear friend long after our commonalities
ended. Other folks cheering this work on include Kathleen Yacharn, Jelly Locklin Rains
(THE!), Heidi Hickman, Gabriel Uhuru, Este Bagato (and Pocket House!), Terri Lynne
Hudson, Abigail Grieg, Chelsea Elliot (and Baby Zab), Beverly Cruël-Garcia, and Jess
Wagstrom. You are each amazing and I am proud to know you. Grad school is, regrettably, a
iii
bit antisocial, so I must quietly recognize some folks whose paths diverged along the way:
KAK, KEK, NEK, SWS, CE, HD, TMJ, LW, RC, AYB, EM, MG, CM in Denton, and CM
in Lubbock. May your paths ahead be clear and calm. I’d also like to thank Melissa Smith-
Wilkerson and the staff of Alzheimer’s Association of North Texas for helping me stay
connected to caregivers. What is a calling? This is a calling, and yall help me feel seen.
Thank you to the committee who made this happen. To Profs. Jim Williams and
Jessica Gullion of TWU and to Prof. Dale Yeatts of UNT: none of you ever told me I
couldn’t do something, yet you each helped my expectations become more reasonable in
unreasonable times. I hope there are more moments of shared recognition in our futures.
To Joni and Ava (and Pixie and Snowy): thank you for being the family I come home
to. I knew there was no one else I’d want alongside me when I wrote a thesis and I knew
there was no one else I’d want to be alongside in an apocalypse, but I had no idea we’d all do
both at the same time! Yall have kept me fed when I should’ve starved, raised my spirits
when bleakness leered, and never let me lose my humanity to the process, no matter how
hard it got. Let us build and realize your dreams next, whatever shape the world takes on.
Caregiving can decimate your spirit, but afterward there is more room for
compassion and understanding to grow than you ever knew possible. This thesis and all
endeavors to follow are dedicated to the memory of my adopted grandparents, E.E. and
M.T. Hodson — Ed and E — who were my cheatcode into the middle class and later set the
examples by which I continue to live: unconditional love, chosen family, and mutual care at
every opportunity. I hope I can continue to honor and share your gifts for as long as it takes,
until every person can feel as loved and secure as I felt when you were both still here.
iv
ABSTRACT
G.J. HODSON
AUGUST 2020
In the coming decades, there may not be enough professional caregivers to meet the
needs of elderly and disabled persons; family and friends will take on more informal care
roles than ever before, with great social impact, yet the social literature lacks consistency and
breadth. This exploratory, quantitative study organizes the disparate social literature on
caregiving and dementia care and suggests a critical theoretical framework to interpret social
factors more fully. Utilizing the Caregiving in the U.S. dataset from 2015, three hypotheses test
this framework for a dementia care population to identify variable combinations that exert
outsized influence on care outcomes. Findings confirm two hypotheses: people of color tend
to develop dementia far earlier than whites and persons who are marginalized in multiple
ways tend to perform more activities of daily living; these findings demonstrate a role for
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ....................................................................................................... ii
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................... v
Chapter
I. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................... 1
vi
III. DATA AND METHODS .............................................................................................. 61
Data ....................................................................................................................... 61
Sample ..................................................................................................... 61
Variables ............................................................................................................... 62
Dependent Variables............................................................................. 62
Independent Variables .......................................................................... 63
Intervening Variables ............................................................................ 64
Data Analysis ....................................................................................................... 65
Bivariate Correlations ........................................................................... 65
Multivariate Linear Regressions .......................................................... 66
V. CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................ 80
Summary ............................................................................................................... 80
Discussion ............................................................................................................ 81
Limitations ........................................................................................................... 85
Recommendations .............................................................................................. 86
Conclusion ........................................................................................................... 88
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................. 90
vii
LIST OF TABLES
Tables Page
viii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figures Page
1. Age of Onset (Dementia/Memory Problems) ................................................................ 71
ix
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
Along with an increase in life expectancy, many nations have begun to see an
increase in conditions associated with old age, especially dementia. A person who develops
recognizing the change. As such, dementia has a deeply social impact, altering the way others
view and interact with the person and creating dependence upon others that only increases
over time. Those who serve persons with dementia — whether family members, friends, or
domestic workers hired for the purpose — are their caregivers. Caregivers may work to
maintain or improve the quality of life of those they serve (whether that service is paid or
unpaid), but when their support cannot stabilize or improve the condition (as is the case for
dementia), it can be hard to reconcile the care role with cultural, economic, and familial
notions of success and vocation. Research on dementia and on caregiving at large show that
outcomes for the caregiver and care recipient are deeply intertwined with social facets of
their lives: status, identity, and personal support, among others (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:15-
29, 70-118). Unfortunately, research into caregiving and dementia care has not necessarily
dementia, and dementia care dyads, then test a social inquiry into dementia care while
applying intersectional and queer theories by Crenshaw and Halberstam. This chapter
1
CAREGIVING AND DEMENTIA CARE
Grusky & Weisshaar 2014:1066-1077), there is usually an unspoken expectation that the
portion of years where a person can be vibrant and independent are also expanded (Lock
2002:33), however this is so far not the case. Publishing with the National Institute on Aging
and the National Institutes for Health, the World Health Organization (2011:6-8) reports
developed nations are but the first to face unprecedented proportion and growth for elderly
and disabled populations. This phenomenon has raised the visibility of persons whose roles
Elders in the U.S. can now live for decades after retirement age, but their health has
not kept pace (Eifert et al. 2016; cf. Halberstam 2005:153). However, caregiving is not only
about elders; over a quarter of people ages 18-65 in the U.S. have a disability or chronic
condition, and this figure is also growing (Scales et al. 2019:21). Patients with conditions that
were once a death sentence (such as cerebral palsy or AIDS) now have life expectancies that
approach those of the general population. Caregiving can also include recovery support for
injury, temporary illness, and surgery. The proverbial broken leg takes 6-8 weeks to recover,
during which time an otherwise healthy adult may rely on others for transportation,
groceries, bathing, and logistics; the recovery time for life-threatening forms of cancer or
from a traumatic brain injury, meanwhile, can last well over a year and forever change family
roles. Care outcomes tend to worsen for caregivers as frequency of tasks and duration
increase (Hunt et al. 2017:18-22) — these factors are also linked to a higher risk of elder
2
Giving care to the sick, the elderly, and the injured is a significant social behavior
among humans (George & Ferraro 2016; Montgomery & Kosloski 2013; Schulz & Eden
2016:86); this significance has not always been assumed, however (Seaman 2018). Since the
beginning of the Industrial Age, changed standards of health, hygiene, and even violence
have lowered death rates, while biomedical discoveries have enabled life to be prolonged,
particularly near its end (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:20). As (white, able-bodied) middle-class
women — traditionally caregivers of both children and the infirm (Schulz & Eden 2016:50,
53-55) — gradually joined the workforce (Suitor, Gilligan, & Pillemer 2016), so too did
families begin to spread out rather than living geographically close. Urban complexity and
dual-income priorities also led to lower birth rates, leaving aging parents with fewer children
to serve as caregiver; a growing portion of elders have no children at all (Schulz & Eden
2016:61-62).
Estimates range from 17-39 percent of U.S. adults giving some type of informal care
to another adult each year (Fox, Duggan, & Purcell 2013), of which dementia care accounts
for nearly a quarter (Hunt et al. 2017:6); other common ailments including recent surgery,
cancer, and loss of mobility (Hunt et al. 2015:29). The tasks involved in caregiving depend
upon the condition of the care recipient, but tend to be measured in two types: activities of
daily living (ADLs) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs; Hunt et al. 2015:8;
Roth, Fredman, & Haley 2015; Schulz & Eden 2016:45). ADLs are those activities that are
necessary to live: feeding oneself, minimal hygiene, or being able to move oneself short
distances. IADLs, on the other hand, are those tasks that are necessary in order to function
in society: obtaining groceries and preparing meals, managing finances and housework, and
3
arranging support. High-need caregivers number over 8.5 million, or 3.5 percent of all adults
over age 20 (Schulz & Eden 2016:50, 306; Vitaliano et al. 2011); care is considered "high-
need" in any case involving assistance with two or more ADLs, cancer, probable dementia,
or once daily care is required for longer than 2 years (Schulz & Eden 2016:74, 82-83). The
endurance of informal caregivers grants a silent economic and logistical reprieve for their
doctors and communities, but not without costs for the caregiver (Bruhn & Rebach
In the U.S., caregiving is predominated by care for elders, who receive most of their
care from spouses or their (middle-aged) children (Schulz & Eden 2016:50, 53-54). The rapid
growth of the U.S. elder population has led to more hiring of home healthcare workers
2018), and strains in the long-term care insurance industry (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:86;
Gleckman 2019; Kwak & Polivka 2014). Even if all families could afford professional
support or institutionalization (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:86), the industry is not large enough to
meet the care needs of elders (Gaugler et al. 2019:40-41; Redfoot, Feinberg, & Houser 2013)
in the present, let alone meet projections (West et al. 2014). As was done before the
separation of production spheres (Fraser 2016), families and communities are stepping up
informally (i.e., without pay or training — Hunt et al. 2015:42-45) to fill this gap.
The combination of informal and formal care may not be enough (Redfoot et al.
2013; Roth et al. 2015; Schulz & Eden 2016:51-53). Baby Boomers (born 1946-1964), whose
unusual size as a birth cohort is the reason for their name and official Census designation,
are expected to need more care in their later years than the younger population will be able
4
to provide at previously observed care participation rates (Feinberg & Spillman 2019;
Redfoot et al. 2013; Ryan, et al. 2012). This disparity may be a warning to future generations,
or it may be unique to Baby Boomers, about whom care gaps have been predicted since the
earliest Millennials were born (Kane & Kane 1980). Either way, the Boomer care gap is
already emergent and changing the relationship between age and care participation; as of
2014, Millennial adults (born 1980-1996) already represent one quarter of all caregivers and
16 percent of dementia caregivers (Hunt et al. 2017:8) — the highest of any birth cohort on
As with the concept of caregiving, our understanding and terminology for dementia
have evolved over the last half-century. Physicians no longer believe that moderate memory
loss is normal for a person at any age; the terms senility and demented have been deprecated in
favor of framing all cognitive decline as some form or other (however advanced) of
dementia. A syndrome rather than one specific disease (Haeusermann 2017), dementia
indicates a gradual degradation of the person’s synapses. Although some rare and externally
traced types of cognitive decline can be reversed (Dwolatzky & Clarfield 2003), the
overwhelming majority are associated with aging and general health, and these are
irreversible (Gaugler et al. 2019:8). Irreversible dementias are collectively called Alzheimer’s
Disease and Related Dementias (ADRD), although Alzheimer’s itself is now a catch-all for
cases of irreversible dementia that do not follow other, more specific pathologies (such as
indicates that most people who will eventually develop dementia begin synaptic decline as
much as twenty years before symptoms are noticeable (Gaugler et al. 2019:5); once
5
symptoms appear, cognitive decline can affect any or every aspect of a person’s ability to
Dementia is diagnosed in 10 percent of all U.S. elders over age 65, and early onset
dementia is estimated to affect another 200,000 persons under age 65 (Gaugler et al.
2019:17). The proportion of U.S. elders diagnosed with dementia appears to have plateaued,
yet the numbers continue to grow as Baby Boomers age (Gaugler et al. 2019:19-23).
Researchers note that the trajectory and care of early onset dementia can be dramatically
different from other forms, especially Alzheimer’s (Caceres et al. 2016). There is no cure or
treatment for ADRDs in sight, only medications that can ease the descent (Gaugler et al.
2019:10-11); from diagnosis forward (maybe even earlier), the person with dementia grows
Because physical symptoms of dementia tend to come in later stages, the impact of
about reality can clash due to memory loss, language difficulties, and erratic behavior on the
part of the person experiencing cognitive decline (Beard, Knauss, & Moyer 2009).
Supporting a person with dementia can entail anything (and, eventually, everything) from
transportation and decision-making to hygiene and feeding (Gaugler et al. 2019:31-33). Care
for persons with dementia is said to be among the most challenging of all caregiving
situations, and accounts for nearly half of all elder care (Gaugler et al. 2019:31-32).
Out of every five dementia caregivers, two report being solo caregivers (Hunt et al.
2017:23), but feelings of isolation are common among others as well (Schulz & Eden
2016:104-105). Studies associate social isolation with higher stress from caregiving (called
6
“caregiver burden”), especially when care lasts longer than 2 years (Schulz & Eden 2016:306;
Vitaliano et al. 2011). Even professional dementia caregivers report higher levels of isolation
than other caregivers (Morgan, Innes, & Kosteniuk 2011), exacerbating the infrastructural
Caregiving: the act of supporting another person through daily living activities. The
term caregiving became necessary in the latter half of the twentieth century to differentiate it
from caretaking (the care for a place, a thing, or some other object). Although the term is
common in U.S. research (U.K. research often uses carer and dementia advocates prefer care
partner), the parameters of its application are inconsistent and rarely explained, leading to
wide disparities and conflicts between findings; studies that sample only adults who have
provided some amount of care some time in the last year will demonstrate very different
findings from those that include care of children with developmental disabilities but sample
only caregivers who served for six months or more. (Child-rearing specialties sometimes
substitute caregiver for parent since the term is more inclusive, but the raising of children
without illness or disability has its own connotations and fields of research and is
consistently excluded from caregiving research.) For this study, the following terms are used
Caregiver: any person who performs caregiving; often linked to the person(s) for
whom they care through possessive pronouns (e.g., “Ed’s caregiver”). The term remains
controversial outside academia — Bruhn and Rebach report that “as many as 75 percent of
7
family caregivers who provide personal care do not self-identify themselves as caregivers
until personal care and homemaker assistance becomes an economic and psychological
burden” (2014:7) — yet there is evidence that intense caregiving experiences alter caregivers’
self-perception (158-160).
Care Dyad: the pairing of a care recipient and their caregiver (usually primary). As will
be discussed in the literature review, many studies discuss care dyads while focusing their
inquiry or analyses on either the caregiver or the care recipient, not both. The term care dyad
is here utilized in the interest of reifying the social nature of care work.
Formal and Informal (Care): a distinction made between care/givers as to whether care
work is paid (formal) or unpaid (informal), with implications that formal care also involves
training and supervision while informal care comes from family or friends. Efforts to
compensate informal caregivers (Eifert et al. 2016; Glenn 2010:174-175) and inconsistent
parameters for what constitutes “care” (Hunt et al. 2017:6n) are blurring this boundary.
Professional caregivers are outside the scope of this study, so all care that is not otherwise
stays in a medical or long-term care facility, family (or other support) legally appoint a
Secondary Caregiver: any informal caregiver who is not primary or solo. Although social
support and distribution of roles have received some attention, research has yet to develop
8
Care/Support System/Network: the care dyad and all who support its caregiving efforts.
As no standard yet exists, this study uses care as described above and support only to refer to
the direct social support or logistical assistance given to care dyads; similarly, I use network
for only family and social involvement, whereas system connotes active participation and may
age. In the U.S., 65 is a common cut-off, beyond which everyone is an elder, a senior, or an old
person. Elder is most common in social research, senior is more popular in marketing, and old
who are subjected to multiple social disparities. Rather than merely calculating material
marginalizations interact with one another (often, but not always, amplifying material
outcomes) within a specific context; recognizing the complex nature of discrimination and
priorities from every level of society (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:5; Schulz & Eden 2016:59-66);
as such, this and other studies utilizing a U.S. sample (even secondarily) must center studies
of the local population and not uncritically conflate it with others. Therefore, only studies on
U.S.-based samples are referenced. In the interest of person-led praxis, the following terms
will be favored over other conventions where possible (i.e., not quoting or citing specific
9
materials): Black or Black American over African American; LGBTQ over LGBT; men and
women over male and female; and people of color over “minorities.”
PROBLEM STATEMENT
The purpose of this study is to explore the complex social interactions that affect
approaches to the topics of dementia and dementia care. This will be accomplished through
a critical literature review, followed by testing hypotheses over some of the socially
determined niches of dementia care. These three hypotheses will be tested using the 2015
wave of National Alliance for Caregiving’s (NAC/AARP) Caregiving in the U.S. via
RATIONALE
This thesis draws from many disciplines to raise critical questions about how
caregiving has been researched and how future research may be organized, suggesting a
theoretical framework is necessary to connect and interpret dementia care across a fuller
range of social interactions, from the personal to the systemic. This study adds to
interpreting dementia care. It is the first study to apply intersectional quantitative analysis to
the topic of dementia caregiving, and its findings will inform the work of communities,
medical personnel, and service practitioners in the position of serving dementia caregivers.
Pointing a more social lens on dementia care will also advance our contemporary response
10
STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS
This study introduces and complicates literature and research practices around
inequalities in care dynamics and lack of disciplinary grounding have come together to
normalize research that does not adequately address the social factors in dementia care.
Theoretical approaches to filling this research gap are discussed, with two critical theories
(Crenshaw 1991, 1993; Halberstam 2005) selected to advance and interpret social meanings
from the data. In Chapter 3, the dataset and methods are outlined, as well as the data and
discussed and recommendations offered on future directions for dementia care research
11
CHAPTER II
LITERATURE REVIEW
This critical literature review curates findings from the periphery of several
sociological framework. Research into informal caregiving in the United States is broadly
followed by a summation of research specific to dementia care and how dementia care dyads
may constitute a unique social system. Multiple theoretical approaches are reviewed for
depth and relevance in developing this connection, with critical theories by Crenshaw and
Halberstam selected to advance the present analysis and three hypotheses introduced for
testing.
As will be elaborated in the Theoretical Framework, social literature has not been
standardized for the topic of caregiving; instead, caregiving is a diffuse topic, often
While the focus here is caregiving for one condition — dementia — there, too, literature
often lacks the theoretical groundwork to assert a focused body of work; rarely in the
literature (and never with coordinated standards) do studies elucidate the care dyad, its
members, and its support system outside of sweeping biomedical and public health
outcomes.
In this section, the history and literature of informal caregiving are broadly
12
introduced. Despite growing belief that caregiving is innate in humans, the term and its
research paradigm are less than a century old, only recently differentiated from other
responsibilities by improved medical care and shifting family and gender dynamics.
Demographics are presented, with health and income inequalities appearing to affect both
who needs care and who provides it. Research into family dynamics is discussed, although
there is limited qualitative research (and little research of any orientation for non-family
caregivers). Caregiving research trends are reviewed and found to demonstrate problems in
Research on caregiving emerged by the 1980s (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; Roth et al.
2015; van Durme et al. 2012), with early and ongoing attention to how informal caregiving
can negatively affect a caregiver's physical, emotional, and/or financial wellness (Beard &
Fox 2008; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:161; Seeher 2013). Caregivers may face greater risks than
non-caregivers for stress (Crimmins & Vasunilashorn 2016), isolation (Adler & Mehta 2014;
Musich et al. 2017), depression (Roth et al. 2015), relationship challenges (Schulz & Eden
2016:104), and early mortality (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:162-163; George & Ferraro 2016;
Roth et al. 2013; Schulz & Eden 2016:100), yet caregiving is not without its positive aspects
(Pinquart & Sörensen 2003; Roth et al. 2015). Negative consequences for caregivers appear
to be moderated by the caregiver's age, race, and gender (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:111; Schulz
& Eden 2016:63). Studies link social support for the caregiver to better outcomes for the
Most informal caregivers come from the care recipient’s immediate family according
13
to availability and expectations of the relationship (Cornwell & Schafer 2015); income may
be a factor as well (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:86). Available spouses are the default caregiver;
selection among adult children varies by the size of the family, genders of the adult children
and the parent, and individual availability or willingness (Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Suitor et
al. 2016). Siblings, grandchildren, and other family members care less frequently, with non-
relatives giving care up to 15 percent of the time (Hunt et al. 2015:20; Nichols et al. 2011).
Attempts to discern patterns in caregiver selection remain scattered, but family role and
having a close relationship with the care recipient seem to overshadow individual personality
traits (Cornwell & Schafer 2015; Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Pillemer & Suitor 2006; Suitor et
al. 2016). Most caregivers see their role as a duty, not an option (Hermanns & Mastel-Smith
2012), yet the 50-60 percent of caregivers who report not having a choice are at higher risk
for adverse outcomes (Schulz & Eden 2016:56, 109). The proportion of care provided by
families has slowly declined since the 1980s (Nichols et al. 2011), leading to explosive growth
in professional and institutional care (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:20; Eifert et al. 2016; Kane &
Kane 1980).
structured, public-interest surveys that report broad patterns from a nationwide view
(Feinberg & Spillman 2019; Kim et al. 2018; Raschick & Intergsoll-Dayton, 2004). These
large-scale studies set the foundation for predictions, trajectories, and policies toward
caregiving from a macrosociological level (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; George & Ferraro
2016), but overshadow microsociological studies into the nuances of care dynamics that may
challenge existing theories, methods, and assumptions (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; Seaman
14
2018). Condition-centered studies of caregivers, meanwhile, tend to assert the perspectives
of medical practitioners and intervention providers (recent examples include Bailes, Kelley,
& Parker 2016; Cloyes, Hull, & Davis 2018; Gitlin, et al. 2017; Gouin et al. 2016; Zhang &
Lee 2017) instead of care dyads or their families, leading to studies from the nursing field
that attempt to bridge the gap (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:22; Koehly 2017).
the visibility of caregiving often convey its scope in large dollar figures, usually either the
cost or savings to society (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:185-186; Schulz & Eden 2016:131;
a given year to become caregivers (Cook, Snellings, & Cohen 2018; Schulz & Eden
2016:127-131); “savings to society” estimates what cumulative healthcare costs are avoided
expensive interventions. Both models reduce the concepts of wellness and care to dollar
amounts and may fail to report in a way that is locally relatable by communities and families
The most common instruments in caregiving research are scales of caregiver burden,
which quantify the stress or intensity of caregiving experiences (Beard et al. 2009; Bruhn &
Rebach 2014:109; Hunt et al. 2015:7-8). Some of these instruments count ADLs/IADLs, the
number of hours spent providing care, or some combination of the two (Bruhn & Rebach
2014:131; Hunt et al. 2015:39-40; Pinquart & Sörensen 2004). Caregiver burden is also a
15
(2002); however, no measure of caregiver burden or experience has yet become an academic
affects caregivers’ physical and emotional health (Hunt et al. 2015:7-8) and can lead to
“caregiver burnout” (Hermanns & Mastel-Smith 2012; Nichols et al. 2011; Schulz & Eden
2016:34-35), but unexplored factors outside the care dynamic also play a role (Hunt et al.
Caregivers are notably more vulnerable to negative health outcomes if they lack
social support or coping skills, but both members of the care dyad can suffer if a caregiver’s
subjective assessment of the care situation is poor (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:154-157, 163).
Alternately, higher income, more education, and legal marital status seem to provide a
protective effect against worse health outcomes (George & Ferraro 2016); social and other
forms of support seem to help diminish some of the stressors of dementia care by
improving adaptability (Roth et al. 2015). Caregiver burden and other measures reinforce
that caregiving interacts not only with health status but with socioeconomic status,
perpetuating inequalities.
Care dyads are subject to the same social forces that create health disparities. Aging,
disabilities, and caregiving reflect economic and social stratifications that not only compound
but exacerbate one another (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:20, 84-93; George & Ferraro 2016;
Richardson & Brown 2016; Schulz & Eden 2016:61). Notably, not only the health of the
care recipient is at stake, as caregivers and care recipients both demonstrate health
vulnerabilities from care circumstances; these are also mediated by gender, race, and
16
socioeconomic factors (Apesoa-Varano et al 2015; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:87, 116; Cook et
al. 2018). While many of these disparities mirror the patterns of social status and health
furthermore, care dyads interface with race and LGBTQ status in unique, sometimes
counterintuitive ways.
et al. 2015; Friedemann & Buckwalter 2014; Suitor et al. 2016). Daughters and daughters-in-
law perform at least one-third of all caregiving (Hunt et al. 2015:6; Oliker 2011; Schulz &
Eden 2016:55), with wives representing another 8-10 percent (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:87;
Hunt et al. 2015:20). Adult sons who do participate in care are less likely to be married and
yet tend to contribute less intensively than daughters (Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Friedemann
& Buckwalter 2014); husbands who take on caregiving late in life (perhaps for the first time)
commonly report a loss of personal identity that may be linked with their perceived gender
roles (Hermanns & Mastel-Smith 2012). Even heterosexual dual-income families depend on
women to perform most care responsibilities (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:22-23; Suitor et al.
2016). The younger a woman is when she first becomes a caregiver, the greater her risk for
Historically, marriage, divorce, and complex blended families have affected caregiver
selection and involvement (Piercy 2007; Schulz & Eden 2016:62-63); in particular, care dyads
who are married to one another may gain positive effects (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:161).
LGBTQ adults seem to give care at a higher rate than non-LGBTQ adults — also
demonstrating equal engagement by men and women (Hunt et al 2015:81) — yet are erased
17
when care research is structured around the nuclear family and other cisheteronormative
assumptions (Halberstam 2005:10; Oliker 2011). This gap may exclude 1 in 11 caregivers
(Hunt et al. 2015:14) and disproportionately affect young adult caregivers (Flinn 2018;
Hodson forthcoming); Millennials — the adult cohort most likely to come from blended
early and often (Flinn 2018; Fry 2017). Research has found no significant difference in the
outcomes of care dyads based on the sex, marital status, or LGBTQ identification of
caregivers.
People in general are becoming caregivers earlier in life than in decades past (Bruhn
& Rebach 2014:9; Moon & Dilworth-Anderson 2015), reflecting perhaps the smaller sizes of
contemporary families, a growing sense of idealism and service among young people (Cook
et al. 2018; Pew 2015), and a relaxing of traditional gender norms for men. Younger
caregivers, though beleaguered and less prepared (Brodaty & Donkin 2009), show greater
physical resilience against care burden than older caregivers (Hunt et al. 2015:51, 53; Schulz
& Eden 2016:101), even in cases of dementia (Hunt et al. 2017:21); however, there is
concern that people born after 1980 (Millennials and Generation Z) will devote more years
to caregiving across their lifespans than any previous birth cohorts (Cook et al. 2018; Flinn
2018). Older caregivers, especially those over 65 (who are more expected and expecting to
give care; Scommegna 2016; Suitor et al. 2016), tend to face challenges that are more
financial (Eifert et al. 2016) and physical (Pinquart & Sörensen 2003). The risk of early death
is reportedly greater for older caregivers than comparable non-caregivers (Schulz & Eden
2016:100), although this finding has been recently disputed (Roth et al. 2015). Caregivers of
18
“middle” age (more or less Generation X) may have the best combination of resources, but
also carry the greatest number of responsibilities, not least the likelihood of working full
time, raising children, or even both (Berry, Apesoa-Varano, & Gomez 2015; Bruhn &
The relationship between health disparities and care disparities is most evident
among people of color, who (particularly Black men) tend to experience major illness earlier
and more severely than other races (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:99). More specifically, people of
color tend to demonstrate more comorbid conditions, greater ADL needs, and earlier onset
of dementia when compared to whites (Angel, Mudrazija, & Benson 2015; Schulz & Eden
2016:61, 110). Rodriguez (2018) suggests that premature death amongst marginalized
communities exacerbates disparities in later life. Several studies point to people of color
being more resilient against social isolation and caregiver burden than whites (Schulz &
Eden 2016:113), but critics note these studies may lack sufficient controls for other
intervening socioeconomic factors, which can have a multiplier effect on health and life
expectancy (George & Ferraro 2016). Caregivers of color, who are less likely to care alone
and more likely to involve non-relatives than whites (Hunt et al. 2015:20; Schulz & Eden
2016:21), generally reporting more positive and fewer negative consequences to care (Angel
et al. 2015; Schulz & Eden 2016:110); this is especially the case with Black caregivers
(Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015). White caregivers tend to be older, more likely to be supported
by family members, and are less likely to be employed (or more likely to be retired) than
19
Caregiving as a topic of research is neither broad nor deep, but demographic
projections for the U.S. are increasingly urgent. Family and friends make up most informal
caregivers, yet professional caregivers are doing more than ever before. Large-scale studies
have helped define and publicize the scope of caregiving, yet they omit broader support
systems. Inequalities of broader society are reflected and amplified by caregiving, but
complications also exist. Women give the most care and seem to be the most vulnerable to
its sacrifices, yet people of color seem uniquely protected against some of its worst
consequences. Younger caregivers (i.e., Millennials) are participating in care earlier and more
intensively than previous generations at their age, shifting norms and demonstrating diversity
academic fields and approaches. Care research has no home discipline or set of methods
around which to develop; often, it exists only as a niche within literature on the care
recipient’s condition (van Durme et al. 2012). Broad studies of caregivers, on the other hand,
frequently favor conventional or convenient designs that may not fit. Scholars have noted
such weaknesses in care research as narrow sampling (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; George &
Ferraro 2016; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003), recording and analysis that oversimplify U.S. racial
dynamics (Angel et al. 2015; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:110), and reporting statistics for non-
probabilistic samples (Pinquart & Sörensen 2004; Schulz & Eden 2016:60; Werner, Karnieli-
Miller, & Eidelman 2013). Methods get mimicked uncritically, allowing a single facet of a
care recipient (such as their age, healthcare, or disease) to overshadow potentially meaningful
20
dynamics from their entire support network (Davey & Szinovacz 2008). Advocates are
calling for more inclusion of the care recipient’s perspective in care research (Schulz & Eden
Although public and public interest datasets convey the scale of informal care by
counting and categorizing caregivers, these datasets only fulfill bureaucratic priorities if
interviews alike prefer to examine caregiving through a single unit of analysis — the primary
caregiver — despite evidence that larger care networks are present and effective (Nichols et
al. 2011; Schulz & Eden 2016:106). Critics note that surveying a single caregiver ignores
social networks beyond the care dyad (Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Koehly 2017); the level of
primary from several options have all been recommended for further inquiry. Examining the
entire phenomenon of caregiving can be challenging because of just how unseen caregiving
tends to be (Seaman 2018). Research designs that emphasize one caregiver and one care
recipient dealing with one condition may not adequately reflect facts on the ground and risk
furthering this invisibility. Isolation and time limitations also can make care dyads difficult to
recruit, especially for lengthy interviews or observation (Bédard et al. 2001; van Durme et al.
2012), and many families from low-income backgrounds — and families of color in general
— reject the construct of “caregiving” entirely (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; Bruhn & Rebach
2014:87, 100-105). Deeper still are the limitations of care research grounded in superficial
assumptions and care ontology; studies that focus on “family care” or “elder care” or
21
George and Ferraro (2015) invoke “the complexity of change” to endorse social
aging research with “considerable audacity and fortitude,” but care scholarship lacks diversity
in both designs and cases (Remler & van Ryzin 2011; Schulz & Eden 2016:56). While the
over-65 population of the U.S. remains overwhelmingly white, the proportion of whites in
this segment is decreasing (Angel et al. 2015; Eifert et al. 2016; combined with lower life
expectancies for people of color, the concentration of racial diversity in people born after
1960 (Schulz & Eden 2016:59-60) risks functional segregation of age itself. This racialization
of U.S. aging may already be underway in formal care, where 26 percent of professionals are
immigrants, who are vulnerable to misinterpretation, exploitation, and mutual distrust (Scales
et al. 2020). Reliance on professional caregivers nearly doubled from 2008 to 2018, and may
continue to do so (Scales et al. 2020); demographers warn that the U.S. population has not
grown fast enough from births alone to meet the anticipated care needs (formal or informal)
of aging Baby Boomers (Kane & Kane 1980; Schulz & Eden 2016:185; Scommegna 2016).
Both quantitative and qualitative approaches risk repeating societal erasures and
diminish academic rigor when they look at gender, race, and class as separate phenomena
from health status, quality of life, or caregiving outcomes (Kelley et al. 2017; Lovell &
Wetherell 2011; Oliker 2011). Increasingly, health in old age is being understood not as a
random occurrence but as the culmination of a person’s life experiences and their
socioeconomic advantages and disadvantages (Richardson & Brown 2016), but this approach
is not often employed in studies of elder care. Angel et al. caution that caregiving surveys do
not adequately address "the way in which social aspects of racial and ethnic diversity are
affected by the life course and social policy," connecting this oversight to poor clarity of
22
what is meant by "social support" (2015:8; cf. Cornwell & Schafer 2015). Without far more
rigorous analysis of many social factors and how they interact with caregiving, Roth et al.
warn that caregiving for a sick relative cannot be demonstrated to be any more stressful than
having a sick relative without providing them care (2015:310-11) — except, as is common
with such critiques, in cases of dementia care (cf. Pinquart & Sörensen 2003).
A growing number of health researchers are calling for multi-level analyses that
target health inequalities (Gkiouleka et al. 2018; Richardson & Brown 2016; Richman &
Zucker 2019). Studies commonly differentiate “high need” care situations — including
dementia (Schulz & Eden 2016:91) — for having stronger feelings of exhaustion,
overwhelm, and burnout (Gaugler et al. 2019:34-36; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003; Schulz &
Eden 2016:74), but the “high need” designation lacks consistent standards. The grounding
of caregiver studies with theory and/or a dedicated discipline may offer better frameworks
around care research, built on nuanced categories (Angel et al. 2015; George & Ferraro
2016) that will, in turn, better inform care dyads and those who support them.
Caregiving scholarship draws from a wide range of traditions, which can lead to
questionable methods and results that are difficult to generalize. Scholars of caregiving and
health have an opportunity to fill the gap of social meanings, particularly the interrelatedness
of social inequalities and clearer understandings of social support, but to do so will require
streamlining diffuse design approaches. At the same time, informal care research must
expand its scope to include not only caregivers but dyads and support networks, not only
health and burden measures but a layering of positive and negative socioeconomic factors
and their systemic roots over the life course. Care research has an urgent need to advance, as
23
generational changes are beginning to outpace foundational assumptions and nationwide
care gaps loom; the scale of care likely to be needed in the coming decades will have a
dramatic effect on the attitudes and opportunities of Millennials and Generation Z over their
lifespans.
In this section, dementia care dyads are explored as a unique and significant social
system. Dementia care and its scholarship are outlined relative to other care dyads, followed
by some exploration of research on how this system interacts with other social systems.
Outcomes from these interactions are summarized and trends are detailed.
Caregiving research is often framed by the care recipient's condition, and dementia
care is considered one of the most intensive and challenging forms of caregiving (Bruhn &
Rebach 2014:131). Early caregiving research framed family members as “the ‘real’ victims”
of dementia (Beard et al. 2009:227), and even today, “The most prevalent discourses in the
US depict family caregiving as a necessary, but onerous stopgap measure until a better,
biomedical solution is available” (Seaman 2018:64; cf. Cockerham 2013; Mol 2002:7-13n).
Dementia caregivers report high levels of burnout (Bookman & Kimbrel 2011), as well as
difficulties receiving adequate support — social or otherwise (Hunt et al. 2017:25) — yet no
current or near-future treatment for dementia can significantly lower the need for people
who monitor and protect people with dementia (Stites, Rubright, & Karlawish 2018). In
total, nearly 6 million people in the U.S. are living with Alzheimer’s or another dementia and
over three-quarters will need daily care by the late stages (Gaugler 2019:17; Scales et al.
24
2020). Although the proportion of elders experiencing dementia appears to have plateaued,
the population of people with dementia continues to grow (Gaugler et al. 2019:23;
Scommegna 2016).
Caregiving is generally considered "high-need" if daily care lasts for longer than two
years, but dementia care becomes difficult much sooner and lasts an average of 4-6 years
(Gaugler et al. 2019:34-36; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003; Vitaliano et al. 2011). Caregiving for a
person with dementia never stabilizes for long, but gradually expands in scope until the care
recipient loses mobility or dies. Both the intensity and the duration of care increase the
likelihood of negative outcomes for the 16 million dementia caregivers in the U.S. (Gaugler
et al 2019; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:131; Seltzer & Li 1996); further, 66 percent of caregivers
cohabitate with their care recipients, more than any other form of care (Gaugler et al.
2019:31; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003), leaving them with little reprieve. Dementia care
severity (Roth et al. 2013; Pinquart & Sörensen 2004; Seeher 2013), and the “functional
limitations and behavioral symptoms” associated with dementia (Schulz & Eden 2016:85) are
noted to affect subjective well-being more than high counts of care needs (Schulz & Eden
2016:103; Pinquart & Sörensen 2004). A sense of guilt over what more can or should be
done is common, if usually unwarranted (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:163; Seaman 2018).
The risks of harm from isolation, depression, burnout, sleep deprivation, and
worsened health are higher for dementia caregivers than for other caregivers (Bailes et al.
2016; Bookman & Kimbrel 2011; Greenwood, Mezey, & Smith 2018), particularly for adult
children and grandchildren (Nichols et al. 2011); even formal caregivers working with people
25
with dementia report higher levels of stress and depression than professionals caring for
other conditions (Morgan et al. 2011). Burden measures often come in higher for dementia
caregivers than for most other forms of care (Bailes et al. 2016; Hunt et al. 2017:18) and are
sometimes customized for dementia care (reviewed in Nichols et al. 2011), but it is
important to assess these figures relative to the progression of the disease, since the mid- to
late-stages are associated with worse behavioral problems and a higher intensity of care
A strong connection between the dyad often predates informal caregiving (Bruhn &
Rebach 2014:10; Suitor et al. 2016), yet caregiver assignment can be more accurately
predicted by the age or condition of the prospective care recipient than by the prospective
caregiver’s own circumstances (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:10). Dementia caregivers describe
their role as an act of love and familial commitment (Dunham & Cannon 2008), but this
attitude is more pronounced in women and families of color than with men and whites
(Bruhn & Rebach 2014:100-105; Shurgot & Knight 2005). Dementia caregivers appear more
likely to themselves have a chronic illness or disability than other caregivers (Bruhn &
Rebach 2014:153-154; Eifert et al. 2016; Wang, Robinson, & Carter-Harris 2014); however,
this connection has not been explored in the literature. Forty percent of dementia caregivers
report being sole caregivers (Hunt et al. 2017:23), but feelings of isolation are common
Dementia care tends to begin with immediate family, falling to spouses first and then
adult children (Gaugler et al. 2019:31; Nichols et al. 2011), although siblings and
grandchildren are more involved in dementia care than other informal caregiving (Nichols et
26
al. 2011). Some families must change caregivers at least once, but studies are rare (Davey &
Szinovacz 2008); wives, daughters, and siblings seem to be at greater risk of burnout than
husbands, sons, and distant relatives (Nichols et al. 2011:29), yet spouses of persons with
dementia may increase their own risk of developing dementia by giving care (Vitaliano et al.
2011). Most comparative studies have only compared dyads of one relationship type
(spouses, for example) against another (adult children, often daughters), with units selected
across — as opposed to within or beyond — families (Nichols et al. 2011; Schulz & Eden
The disparity between men and women’s prevalence as caregivers is stronger for
dementia care than for other forms of care; wives, daughters, and even daughters-in-law
together perform over two-thirds of all dementia care (Gaugler et al. 2019:31; Friedemann &
Buckwalter 2014; Nichols et al. 2011), though not without “frustrations” over family men
who expect to make decisions but not to participate in hands-on care (Dunham & Cannon
2008:48). Despite some qualitative variations in how care is performed, research into the
gender of dementia caregivers has reported no significant difference in health outcomes for
Negative caregiver outcomes are often moderated by race or ethnicity (Angel et al.
2015). As with caregiving broadly, families of color show better resilience against negative
care outcomes than white families, but also a stronger aversion to the core research concepts
of “caregiving”, “care burden”, and “dementia” itself (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; Bruhn &
Rebach 2014:102; Schulz & Eden 2016:22). Whites are the only racial group to care more
27
often for spouses than parents (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:105, 107), while people of color care
more often than whites for other relatives and non-relatives (Nichols et al. 2011).
Marital status has a strong effect on dyad makeup from both sides. Spouses make up
12 percent of dementia care dyads; outside of spousal dyads, dementia caregivers are more
likely to be married than other caregivers or the general populace (Hunt et al. 2017:35), and
care recipients who are unmarried rely more heavily on siblings, grandchildren, and in-laws
(Nichols et al. 2011). When caring for someone other than a spouse, however, dementia care
has been found to create a greater strain on marriages than other forms of care (Schulz &
Eden 2016:104). Millennial caregivers, who demonstrate the lowest marriage rates on record
and are more likely to identify as LGBTQ, represent a quarter of all dementia caregivers
Challenges can vary according to the age dynamic of any care dyad, and dementia
exhibits strong differences by age and age difference. Dementia care seems to face more
severe financial challenges when the caregiver is aged 18-35 (Flinn 2018); their careers,
educations, and resilience against economic oppression may be less developed compared to
both past cohorts of the same age range and older family members of the present, although
there is also evidence that Millennials face greater economic hardship than any other
generation since World War II (Fry 2017). Grandchild caregivers seem to be especially
vulnerable to social isolation, and commonly experience both role conflict and
unpreparedness (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:153; Nichols et al. 2011). Younger caregivers show a
greater physical resilience against care burden, even in cases of dementia (Hunt et al.
2017:21), but Millennials and Generation Z face a ballooning prevalence of care that could
28
overwhelm other expectations about family and work over the life course (Cook et al. 2018;
Eifert et al. 2016). Older caregivers also demonstrate financial strain but report the greatest
impact to their health (Lovell & Wetherell 2011). Just over a third of dementia caregivers are
themselves over 65 (Gaugler et al. 2019:31); some argue dementia care can increase the
mortality of older caregivers, especially spouses (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:162-163; George &
Ferraro 2016; Roth et al. 2013). Dementia caregivers of the Baby Boomer cohort report
higher levels of responsibility and greater disruptions to physical and emotional health
(Lovell & Wetherell 2011; Moon & Dilworth-Anderson 2015; Musich et al. 2017). Middle-
aged caregivers describe not only strong emotional challenges but also the greatest conflicts
of role and responsibility (Bookman & Kimbrel 2011). Caregivers supporting an elder while
also parenting children under 18 are known as the “sandwich generation” (Berry et al.
Lower income and less education attainment are each associated with better care
outcomes for adult children, though researchers also associate lower income with a stronger
sense of family duty (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:87); neither of these patterns has been explored
deeply in the context of dementia care. Willert and Minnotte note that while research into
previous section, such as interviewing the caregiver while treating the care recipient as a
medical case rather than examining the dyad holistically (Seaman 2018). Research into
29
dementia caregivers focuses on biomedical outcomes, utilization of social services, and intra-
family dynamics (Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Schulz & Eden 2016:106), centering not only the
pathological interpretation of dementia but also the so-called nuclear family over other care
dynamics (Apesoa-Varano et al. 2015; Nichols et al. 2011); this narrow view not only
excludes over a third of contemporary caregivers who are unmarried or not caring for
immediate family, it also centers middle class white families in a way that is outdated,
increasingly irrelevant, and methodologically specious (Eifert et al. 2016). The “cost/savings
to society” paradigm is less widely utilized because dementia is seen (somewhat inaccurately)
as an affliction of people who have aged out of the workforce; indeed a quarter of dementia
caregivers are over age 65, and yet 21 percent of these older caregivers still work (Hunt et al.
Dementia care research often happens at the local or state level, but such studies can
be difficult to place in conversation with one another, or with national studies, due to
methodological inconsistencies. Publicly available datasets such as the federal National Survey
of Caregivers and NAC/AARP’s Caregiving in the U.S. record and report which caregiving cases
center on dementia, but researchers must be careful working with the smaller samples, where
convention favors homogeneous samples that hinder reliability (George & Ferraro 2016).
Even studies that measure biomedical factors upon social phenomena stand accused of poor
sampling (Roth et al. 2015). Perhaps the greatest weakness in dementia care research is the
high number of caregiving studies that lack probability samples (Angel et al. 2015; Pinquart
& Sörensen 2003, 2004; Schulz & Eden 2016:60; Werner et al. 2013), and yet go on to report
statistics probabilistically (such as Wang et al. 2014). The abundance of unstandardized, by-
30
the-numbers statistical methods may hide the opportunity for more innovation and breadth
Assumptions that caregiving carries a cumulative harm for the caregiver also persists
in dementia care research, which then focuses on quantifying the harm rather than
complicating it (Dunham & Cannon 2008; Hermanns & Mastel-Smith 2012; Roth et al.
2015). Studies that highlight "positive aspects of caregiving" tend to make an explicit
exception for dementia care (such as Roth et al. 2015; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003), although
such studies exist for dementia care as well (Beard et al. 2009). Though, pathologically,
can shake up entire families and communities. Some of the social and demographic trends
for dementia caregiving follow those of caregiving in general, but others are amplified. In
particular, the role of spouses and daughters is more prominent, and age differences (both
between different caregivers and between a caregiver and their care recipient) have stronger
impact. Caregivers of color demonstrate resilience in dementia care, but their coverage in
literature is inconsistent and often lacks nuance. Specialized burden measures exist for
dementia caregivers, but quantitative studies do not tend to record stages of the disorder in
undervalue social aspects. Care dyads reflect the social status of the care recipient as well as
the caregiver, but studies have only interrogated the influence of their biomedical condition
upon the caregiving experience. Beard and Fox suggest that social research encourages
31
outlook wherein the dementia is only a disability of the person’s relationship to memory or
time (2008:1510); this approach has tremendous implications for dementia caregivers as well,
and turns questions of wellness away from biomedical fixes and toward the broader social
the care recipient’s decline” (2014:10), with stress and stress variance that are “interactional”
(156). Dementia care dyads exist in a mutually constrained tension where the actions and
impacts of one affect the other, perhaps more than any other form of caregiving. At the
heart of the care dyad is a constant question of prevention versus response, balancing
between the interests of a thinking, feeling adult — who may not realize or accept they are
sick and getting sicker — and the caregiver (and support system) that expects to outlive
them. Social shifts can occur to both the caregiver and the care recipient at the same time;
the less a person with dementia is capable of performing socially, the more responsibility
that falls upon the caregiver to either compensate or retreat. In a meta-ethnographic review
of international empirical studies and policy documents, U.K. researchers place a person
with dementia at the center of a large ecosystem where interactions with the caregiver, health
workers, and every level of society interact to affect their sense of personhood (Bosco et al.
2019). Similarly, Beard et al. endorse revisiting dementia as a "social process" to shift
dementia toward an "experience of memory loss rather than giving prominence to biological
an Irish study emphasizes, “Living with dementia cannot be isolated from interactions with
32
others,” (Greenwood et al. 2018:44, citing Daly et al. 2012). While the contexts of these
studies is beyond the scope of this thesis, their systematic viewpoints reiterate the social
significance not only of dementia, but of its care and of the greater support system of its
caregiver(s). Dementia care dyads already stand out in the literature; dementia care
demonstrates enough qualities of a distinct social system that many care studies separate
samples into two categories: dementia care and all others (see Pinquart & Sörensen 2003 for
Perhaps it is not always the case that the care receiver’s age, gender, etc., influence
caregiving, but it is certainly true in dementia care, yet these social statuses are only
superficial facets of a dementia care system. Without a systemic framework, we cannot, for
example, uncover how the layers of socioeconomics within a care network may interact with
the socioeconomic trajectory of the person with dementia; dementia care may even be
ideologically sensitive: some families see dementia as either a mental illness (Stites et al. 2018)
At the heart of this social system is not only the care recipient, but the care dyad, a
“socially constructed dialectic” (Seaman 2018:64), wherein the caregiver and care recipient
must navigate one another’s realities and together navigate the “real” of others (Beard et al.
2009; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:154; Hermanns & Mastel-Smith 2012). Between its fuzzy
beginning (Seltzer & Li 1996) and its unavoidable end (Beard & Fox 2008), a dementia care
dyad builds, sustains, and ultimately loses an inner world of their own (Hermanns & Mastel-
Smith 2012; Seaman 2018). Not merely an alliance or social network (Cornwell & Schafer
2015), the depth and duration of this social entanglement fosters social consequences for
33
both members of the dyad and their external relationships as well (Schulz & Eden 2016:104-
106). Although linear and cyclical models of dementia care exist (Bruhn & Rebach
2014:157), the trajectory of dementia symptoms is usually erratic. Caregivers may lack
certainty from week to week, hour to hour, about when and how their loved one's cognitive
losses will manifest (Dunham & Cannon 2008), so adaptability is critical (Bruhn & Rebach
2014:163; Roth et al. 2015). Many caregivers report worrying about letting their guard down
at the wrong time (Seaman 2018), which can lead to hypervigilance, adrenal fatigue, and
burnout.
The responsibilities of dementia caregivers extend far beyond mere ADLs and
IADLs (Pinquart & Sörensen 2004; Schulz & Eden 2016:76-77). Dementia caregivers
navigate ever-shifting dynamics (Berry et al. 2015; Bruhn & Rebach 2014:163) in a way that
tries to honor the present capabilities of the care recipient without exposing them to danger
or emotional harm (Beard et al. 2009; Berry et al. 2015). Serving as the care recipient's
primary (and sometimes only) buffer from the world, not only do caregivers decide whether
or how to intervene, they also determine when intervention must escalate and processes
must change (Dunham & Cannon 2008). A person with dementia may choose to pull back
from social engagement (Gauthier et al. 2013), but it is more common for their social role to
be withdrawn by others (Beard et al. 2009), leaving the care dyad to adapt roles and identities
on their own. Bosco et al. highlight social inclusion within their model of dignity, adding that
while dignity as a concept can be very individualized, “people know when they are not being
34
The caregiver is generally expected to intervene if the care recipient is
uncomfortable, unsafe, or unruly (Berry & Apesoa-Varano 2017), reflecting respectively the
subjective feelings of the care recipient, the security of the care recipient (and possibly
others), and the subjective social expectations of any other persons around. Under this
dynamic, it falls to informal caregivers to "discipline and control [...] the aging body" and to
researchers to reduce caregiver strain; family caregivers, particularly adult children, can see
this power dynamic as a "role reversal" (Dunham & Cannon 2008:46, 52), and often describe
feeling ambivalent about it (Seaman 2018). Notably, people with dementia are mistreated
more often than any other elders (Schulz & Eden 2016:107; van Durme et al. 2012); they
also frequently experience depression or anxiety, although studies have not yet discerned
The dementia care dyad is a power dynamic that can be uncomfortable for both
members, but the erratic nature of dementia means there is no “script” available to help
individual caregivers balance the particulars of safety, ability, and agency for themselves and
their care recipients (Beard et al. 2009; Berry & Apesoa-Varano 2017; Gauthier et al. 2013;
Seltzer & Li 1996). Berry et al. (2015) distinguish stages of caregiving by whether dyad
actions are collaborative or unilateral, which is itself distinct from a spectrum of cooperation
versus resistance.
The perspectives of people with dementia are often overlooked in research; over 80
percent of people with dementia live “in the community” — that is, not in any form of care
facility (Hunt et al 2017:32) — and 8 percent receive no assistance (Berry & Apesoa-Varano
35
2017; Gaugler et al. 2019:31), yet Dunham and Cannon warn that dementia care studies
“often focus on the powerlessness of the care recipient” (2008:46). Beard and Fox argue that
“loss of self” for persons with dementia is “a dominant trope in America, reflecting a
coherence and rationality of time, space, and selfhood” (2008:1510). The person
experiencing cognitive loss may themself resist the words “Alzheimer’s” or “dementia” and
complaints” on aging (Beard et al. 2009; Gauthier et al. 2013). While it may be easy to frame
the complicated dynamics of dementia care as the caregiver's problem, Beard and Fox
pendular process of maintaining past social roles and incorporating new ones" (2008:1511).
Persons with dementia often retain a sense of their own social lives (Beard & Fox
2008); impeded more by stigma than by ability (Greenwood et al. 2018), they remain
sensitive to rudeness, being ignored, and becoming isolated (Dunham & Cannon 2008).
Negative reactions — not only from strangers, but even from friends and loved ones — can
include talking down to the person, placing premature restrictions, or isolating them
(Gauthier et al. 2013; Johnson et al. 2015; Stites et al. 2018). Elder social ties are unique,
molded more by grief and health factors than employment or geography (Cornwell &
Schafer 2015), and especially vulnerable to race, gender, and socioeconomic status; persons
who face more and greater adversities have fewer and weaker connections than persons who
do not (Cornwell & Schafer 2015). Although persons with dementia who have strong social
support do not show slower cognitive decline than those with weaker support (Nichols et al.
36
2011), they may be less vulnerable to depression, anxiety, and other secondary consequences.
Feelings of guilt, denial, or loneliness can lead a person with dementia to overcompensate —
to “do what they can” (Berry et al. 2015) — however the person with dementia may have
For dyads who have active families or support systems, a caregiver must not only
make difficult decisions but justify them to others (Berry & Apesoa-Varano 2017; Seaman
2018); the primary caregiver is entrusted with learning the most about dementia and
disentangling the person from the disease for others (Beard & Fox 2008). Tensions arise not
only over what the caregiver can do but when, as many actions are warranted at one stage
but not others (Berry et al. 2015). Beard and Fox (2008:1510) invoke Kitwood and Sabat to
social psychology” (also cited in Dunham & Cannon 2008; Seaman 2018), ascribing moral
deficiency (cf. Mol 2002:7-13n) upon the person with dementia for having "excess
disability." Caregivers or others who attempt to bring the person with dementia to "reality"
are asserting a worldview that is confusing at best, placing their own needs above that of the
Dunham and Cannon emphasize the "bio-power" of this dynamic, noting, “the
family caregiver is controlled by the same process of power/knowledge that disciplines the
aging body with dementia” (2008:46), while Seaman (2018:68) describes the dynamic as a
relationship of "non-action" that leaves caregivers feeling "tired, but not powerless." Very
little scholarship examines the assumptions and power dynamics at play, although this trend
is improving (Berry et al. 2015; Unson et al. 2015). Beard and Fox recommend,
37
“Accentuating the socio-moral necessity to care for people with dementia in ways that
recognize and preserve their dignity as human beings” (2008:1511). A care recipient's
resistance may have nothing to do with a caregiver's actual malfeasance, nonetheless it may
be their best method for disrupting power (Dunham & Cannon 2008). A long-term, primary
caregiver could be rejected at some random point, but in most cases they take on a singular
prominence (Dunham & Cannon 2008); by the late stages of dementia, this bond culminates
into a connection that is more instinctual than cognitive: when all others are forgotten, even
the self, this (nameless, story-less, just familiar) entity makes things better, solves problems,
Also overlooked in research is the extent to which the best interest of the care
recipient may conflict with the best interest of the caregiver (Hermanns & Mastel-Smith
2012). Dementia caregivers may experience not only isolation from being “stuck at home” or
having obligations with their time (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:109; Schulz & Eden 2016:103-
106), but a functional withdrawal of family and friends (Adler & Mehta 2014; Beard et al.
2009; Greenwood et al. 2018) who are unsure how to connect with the caregiver and/or be
around the care recipient. As loved ones pull away from the person with dementia,
caregivers, too, are subject to severe social isolation (Dunham & Cannon 2008; Seaman
2018), which may worsen if caregivers do not find solidarity with other caregivers (Adler &
Mehta 2014); what does that say about the caregiver if the care recipient becomes isolated
under the unspoken assumption that the person is already “lost” — is the caregiver lost as
well, or their efforts “inconsequential” (Beard & Fox 2008; Seaman 2018)? Prioritizing the
comfort of others may endanger the comfort of the person with dementia; conversely,
38
caregivers may risk alienating limited social support if they ignore the discomforts of
outsiders. Choosing between the two may exacerbate decision fatigue. To keep the person
with dementia safe and comfortable, caregivers may employ misdirection or even outright
lying (Berry et al. 2015; Dunham & Cannon 2008). Whatever challenges of selfhood or life
course the caregiver perceives from taking time out to give care, their actual sense of a social
role or sense of self is subject to being altered in the experience and its fluctuations (Beard &
Dementia caregivers who report the greatest physical impact are not those whose
care recipients have the worst health, but those who began care with low social support
(Bruhn & Rebach 2014:162), yet dementia caregivers report difficulties receiving adequate
support (Hunt et al. 2017:3). Studies of elder social networks find that the greatest benefit
comes from those who can offer material support, though wellness monitoring through
regular interactions can also be beneficial (Cornwell & Schafer 2015). Families, employment,
and professional support (up to and including institutionalization) all involve systemic
interdependence that shifts along the care recipient's trajectory, but only professional
Though limited, findings suggest that informal caregiving can offer critical support,
foment family conflict, or both (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:155-160; Schulz & Eden 2016:106).
Dementia caregivers may relieve pressure upon a family by taking on care and
simultaneously failing to meet family expectations about their own employment, financial
success, or life goals (Eifert et al. 2016; Halberstam 2005:4-10). The paradigm of family care
is affected by shifts in the traditional family structure; few studies have explored the role of
39
divorce, step-children, and blended families in caregiving at all (Davey & Szinovacz 2008;
Schulz & Eden 2016:63), let alone in regards to memory-salient dementia care. Employment
has an effect on caregiving and caregiving has an effect on employment, though only
negative impacts seem to receive much attention (Schulz & Eden 2016:106). There may be
questions about the legitimacy of caregiving as a “career” for caregivers who leave other
jobs, or suspicion over shared finances (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:153-157; Hunt et al. 2015:62).
Social factors and social networks of the caregiver become intertwined, even
indistinguishable, from that of the care recipient, until only the caregiver’s merged support
network survives the death of the care recipient; however, not only their social support
systems interrelate.
The relevant practitioners who interact with the care dyad likely shift according to
the stages of the disease, although the literature does not address this. Over the course of
several years, the care recipient’s general practitioner may give over to a specialist, informal
support gives way to formal interventions from community or public programs, and paid in-
home support becomes institutionalization. Each of these transitions and forms of support
has been reviewed in literature by the relevant practitioner journals (Cornwell & Schafer
2015; Dam et al. 2016; Henning-Smith, Lahr, & Casey 2019; Parker et al. 2002), but they
have not yet been aggregated to center the dyad’s perspective. At heart of many of these
2017; Spector et al. 2012; cf. Cockerham 2013; Mol 2002:7-13n), “an ever-complicated
(Seaman 2018:64). Foucaultian analyses of caregiver studies (such as Dunham & Cannon
40
2008; Roth et al. 2015; Seaman 2018) caution that many caregiver studies are merely an
extension of clinical dementia research and may serve interests of the clinic (Foucault
[1963]1994:117-122) without informing the interests of the populations being studied. The
caregiver must serve as a conduit between the relevant system and the care recipient; this
sort of case management is believed to be getting more difficult for all caregivers (Eifert et
al. 2016), and for dementia caregivers may be difficult to prioritize. That said, "person-
centered care" is gaining traction among intervention and medical practitioners (Whitlatch &
Orsulic-Jeras 2018:S59), which may help them meet caregivers partway; Schulz and Eden
important facets.
isolated topic, but one belonging to a greater social context: its power dynamics, its history in
the nuclear family and biomedicine, and its epistemological quandaries do not belie but
exemplify the sort of interdisciplinary topics at which social research excels. To understand
informal caregivers in the U.S. is to come one step closer to understanding the people of the
U.S.
Conclusion
with the gender, race, marital status, age, and income of caregivers. Support that benefits the
caregiver will also benefit the care recipient; the inverse is true as well. Demographic and
socioeconomic factors for both members of the dyad are known to affect dyad health and
41
outcomes, but their cumulative interactions are rarely explored. Peer-reviewed articles on
caregiving and caregivers commonly emphasize interventions and practitioners, lacking not
only a longitudinal depth but also a sociological one and reinforcing simplistic views of
dementia care. Social and systemic perspectives are overlooked in care literature, particularly
for high-engagement families and atypical caregivers, and even analyses of interdependence
and power have yet to solidify the importance and relevance of caregiving to U.S. society.
The symptoms of dementia are experienced more socially than physically, yet
biomedicalization has sidelined social inquiry. Social factors of caregiving are likely
mechanisms of this system, but require deeper, more multidimensional research. The
opportunity exists to develop nuance in caregiving research by centering not the caregiver or
care recipient, but the mutual dyad, and to view that dyad as one system operating among
many toward greater Durkheimian solidarity (George & Ferraro 2016). With a systemic
outlook on dementia care dyads and their support systems, researchers can expand our
understanding of informal social systems, further distinguish dementia care from other
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
and Black feminists who wanted "concrete action," bell hooks (1991:5) makes a case for
theorization. She cautions that this work could be easily hidden or co-opted, but that the
work is necessary to break down the "false dichotomy between theory and practice" in
feminism and support a throughline from individuals' childhood traumas to their "healing"
42
group actions in the present and investing toward collective progress in the future (hooks
1991:5).
little theory being applied (Dunham & Cannon 2008). If the work of scholarship is to inform
pathological outcomes or financial impacts, there is no shortage of studies that will answer
these questions with statistical precision; however, if the work of scholarship is to recognize
something about the human experience in how and why caregivers exist — now, not one
hundred years ago nor a hundred years in the future — and if that care scholarship is at all to
be healing, it needs to reconcile theory with practice, and like hooks it necessitates
"engagement with collective sources" (1991:3) While a wide range of caregiver studies do
exist, their theoretical grounding is just as extreme as their sample sizes: so broad that no
depth can be gained or so narrow that no impact can be measured. For every study applying
broad social psychology to caregivers, there is a new and distinct theory targeting caregivers,
only to be forgotten after one or two studies. In between, there is nothing about how
individuals engage institutions or small groups in the caregiving context. It is feasible that
caregiving research could thrive if it had the structure and accountability of a theoretical
canon or a discipline overseeing its rigor; as it has neither, however, care scholarship
demonstrably suffers.
This review of theories makes the case that care research is a social phenomenon and
could be implanted in sociology without slowing from existing research trajectories. Multi-
layer frameworks require versatile theories, but with sociological designs scholars can expand
care research in every direction. As the power dynamics of dementia care specifically are
43
explored, the need for a critical perspective emerges, with the post-Foucault model of queer
Social inquiries must confront the question of positivism: whether truths uncovered
are fully knowable or probabilistic at best (Moradi & Grzanka 2017). Even so-called “hard”
sciences now make room for post-positivistic stances, but the divide itself is still quite binary;
one cannot study planets in the same way one studies quanta because the physics function
differently, so scholars must specify their scope. When social scientists design a study, that
study usually takes either a quantitative or a qualitative stance. These viewpoints are
specialized to extreme contexts of inquiry and only rarely and with great care can qualitative
and quantitative methods be applied to the same subject at the same time. As Adler and
Mehta assert, “in the real world things are ‘connected’” (2014:8). Outside of mixed methods,
which are rare and cumbersome, it is theory that helps connect the different stances and
show their relevance to one another. Sociology explicitly includes all levels of social
engagement and has set many precedents for linking different contexts to one another,
making it ideal for developing a systematic read of caregivers. We see that caregiving
situational stance (qualitative), but developing their relationship to one another (and to
greater society) requires theories and frameworks that can bridge the micro and the macro.
What is needed is a theoretical approach that can connect many layers at once from the
44
The relationship between caregiving and sociology need not be one-way. In making
their case for aging research, George and Ferraro summarize the social sciences as asking,
“what mechanisms convince societal members to create and sustain societies, even when
those mechanisms require members to sacrifice some of their own resources, gains, and
autonomy?” (2015:np). This framing perfectly describes the impact caregiving research can
offer sociology, and may even lead to new, non-bureaucratic answers to Durkheim's
matter how much structural arrangements appear to differ across societies, the functions
they serve are the same” (2015:np). This invites the sort of multi-modal frameworks where
The review above demonstrates that care research tends to polarize toward the
macro- and micro-levels, with little development in-between, nor momentum to fill the gaps
(George & Ferraro 2016). This pattern marginalizes caregiving as a phenomenon that is
& Eden 2016:163). Apesoa-Varano, Barker, and Hinton refer to this divide as “curing”
versus “caring” (2011), and Seaman indicates a further discernment is occurring between
“caring for” and “caring about” (2018:65). Here, “curing” is broad shorthand for the
categorizing, studying, and treating that which is errant. Theoretical reflections on how
biomedicine is enacted find that the emphasis remains binaristic — sick or not sick (Mol
45
this logistical (curing, or curing-adjacent, since no cure exists for dementia) outlook, it may
make sense to honor life by prolonging it, increasing a prominent quantitative measure, and
words, there is a lot of measuring and very little attention to care as a social or systemic
process. A logistical approach is direct and simple, finding facts on the ground as they are.
There is no need to challenge statistical conventions, nor look beyond the most
and delimit the "others." A logistical approach leaves no room, nor has any need, for
disciplinary tradition or theoretical framework, but neither does it offer any indication what
is to be measured when life cannot be prolonged, as is currently the case for persons with
dementia.
And yet, even a logistical viewpoint calls for better data than purely quantitative
studies have been able to provide. Caregiving scholarship often suffers from inconsistent
rigor and unresolved contradictions, leaving very few studies that can be compared to one
another directly (Davey & Szinovacz 2008; Pinquart & Sörensen 2003; Schulz & Eden
2016:45). Even Medical Sociology — rooted as the practitioner's ally (Cockerham & Ritchey
1997; Mol 2002:7-13n) — has advanced beyond mere logistics to recognize the need for
studies of patients' social factors, because disparities effect a measurable difference upon
46
caregiving as an isolated, biomedical, aging-related, and inherently negative phenomenon to
be understood and enhanced (George & Ferraro 2016; Roth et al. 2015). Studies often
measure outcomes for the care recipient or for the caregiver but not both (Schulz & Eden
something other than income or procreation are rarely explored. With each caregiving study
serving a different research tradition, gaps and inequalities can always be someone else's
problem. This trajectory leaves not one rich and intricate canon to be organized and
developed, but several pools of oppositional chaos; here, studies of high and low quality
alike receive very little attention or follow-up because they have no way to stand out.
Discomfort on the part of would-be research participants is seen as a failing of the recruit,
not of the research itself (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:110). Further, biomedical inquiries might
discern that social support helps improve outcomes for the care dyad, but examining the
tension between “medical” and “social” priorities is not even new in caregiving: a 1980
article from the Rand Corporation warns that discussions over long-term care carry the same
tension, and that favoring the biomedical perspective alone creates a setback for social policy
(Kane & Kane 1980). Invoking “heroic medical measures” and disability, the authors specify,
“The very permanence and intractability of these problems argue for societal provisions to
47
protect the elderly from a permanent patient role for decades before their death” (Kane &
Kane 1980:241), and yet this very phenomenon is a growing reality for U.S. elders (Eifert et
al. 2016). Forty years later and still without a firm methodology or design structure,
caregiving research remains disconnected from social and economic topics, even from its
own context — and it shows little sign of dramatically improving the lives of caregivers.
Many social inequalities continue or are even exacerbated in a caregiving context. Caregiving
Caregiving research has some footing in several disciplines — of which nursing, itself
sometimes seen as an elevated version of caregiving, may be the best fit (Bruhn & Rebach
2014:22) — and even within social theory, but efforts to unify all facets of caregiving are
rare. In 2014, Bruhn and Rebach published The Sociology of Caregiving, a textbook attempting
to pull together many of the disparate research threads. While it is a useful compilation of
research findings up to that point, it suffers many of the logistical limitations discussed
above: efforts to place caregiving in a global context rely on numbers and scale, with
layers. The authors rightly point out how social inequalities interact with care dynamics
(2014:79-119), but only in the process of attempting to organize the chaos — not to apply it
forward and further understanding. Continuing the pitfall of serving pathological interests
through simplistic designs, many facts and figures are presented without commentary. No
efforts are made to reconcile disparate methods or address academic distribution of care
topics (in fact, while there are chapters devoted to policy and social media, no chapter or
segment reviews caregiving research methodology). Perhaps the weakness of this text is a
48
continuation of the sidelining of caregiving as only a tangent to social problems, not a social
problem or social system in its own right, but more likely it is simply trying to do too much
with a subject that lacks cohesion. Caregivers are not a monolith (Cook et al. 2018;
Montgomery & Kosloski 2013; Morgan et al. 2011), and treating all care dynamics the same
is perhaps a better exercise for research on altruism or anthropology (Roth et al. 2015;
Seaman 2018) than for sociology. To see the systematic-social, it is necessary to recognize
that disease itself can be social (Beard et al. 2009), that dyads interact with and outside of
families and larger support networks (Bosco et al. 2019), and that caregiving looks
dramatically different for a broken leg than it does for dementia. Before a holistic viewpoint
can be developed, perhaps fitting care experiences into a spectrum or small matrix, some of
the most distinct care dynamics must be outlined more clearly (Brodaty & Donkin 2009).
This study takes the first step by separating dementia care and deepening its quantitative and
literary context.
inherent discord of priorities; the goal of medicine — to prolong life — is secondary at best
to the dementia caregiver, while quality of life is secondary at best to models of pathological
have come from the life course perspective, feminist ethics of care, and social role theory
(though not other approaches from Sociological Social Psychology), but none has succeeded
ongoing study. Life course perspectives tend to reiterate that, aside from parenting, some
level of caregiving is associated with old age (Bruhn & Rebach 2014:41-42), but this
49
overlooks demographic shifts and cultural contexts — again, caregiving as we know it exists
in this time and place for a reason. Scholars of ethics of care work to elevate the study of
compassion and qualitative judgment to an academic level, but struggle to disentangle the
construct from gender essentialist implications and sharpen the distinction between literal
and metaphorical “care” (Barnes & Brannelly 2008; Jordan 2020). Role theorists look to
discrepancies (which affect measures of caregiver burden) rather than consistencies (Bruhn
& Rebach 2014:8-9, 157; Savundranayagum & Montgomery 2010). Other theories that have
gained traction include predicting caregiver outcomes with the stress-process model (Moon
& Dilworth-Anderson 2015; Nichols et al. 2011) and development of a caregiver identity
theory (Friedemann & Buckwalter 2014; Montgomery & Kosloski 2013), but they are
designed once again to support interventions, to meet curing (or curing-adjacent) priorities.
These findings tend to speak to the role of the caregiver within a family or a biomedical
To break away from the curing vs. caring tension requires a theoretical framework
that can be applied across the macro/micro disconnect, that is just as concerned with large-
scale disparities as it is with individual outcomes, and ideally that also considers the agency
and quality of life of the person with dementia (Berry et al. 2015). Having rejected the
concept of victory over a disease that cannot yet be stopped, this study approaches
addressed without decontextualizing and recontextualizing the social powers that led to this
50
questions about why and how current intervention models even exist (i.e., in whose
service?). The only theories that meet all these criteria are critical theories.
By applying a critical approach, we allow for the possibility that caregivers’ social
experiences may run deeper than a mere lack of social support. It is not enough that
caregivers are doing too much work or that they feel isolated doing so; they are being
systematically excluded from societal relevance by a society that has no place for them and
caregivers may be regarded highly for their contributions, caregiving is not a common nor
comfortable topic in the U.S. because it is incongruous with expectations and values. Fraser
invokes capitalistic roots in a growing “crisis of care” and how the familial care gap is being
filled for the Global North with immigrants from the Global South (2016:112; see also
Glenn 2010:181; Halberstam 2005:19; Scales et al. 2020). Not only are existing inequalities
being exacerbated by the work of caregiving, but new ones are being created.
Seaman also points out that in the biomedicalization model, the future has become
"the site where solutions to our medical ills lie," and cautions that all dementia care work
could be invalidated if/when a cure is found (2018). The sense of time as experienced by
caregivers is a good example of just how far removed from “everyday” society many
caregivers are. In a series of 2011 blog posts for National Public Radio, astrophysicist Adam
Frank approaches social science in discussing how one’s relationship with time is “particular
to our culture and our history” (2011a). Frank blames contemporary “time-logic” on
51
over-consumption, and precarious economies (2011c). Frank further postulates that, “As
individuals, the desire to build a new [time-logic] springs from our deeply felt need to reclaim
value and balance in our lives” (2011a:np, emphasis in original). Essentially, what Frank asserts
is that time is social, and in doing so, he implies that anyone who does not participate in the
anti-social.
Queer theories also address anti-social relationships with time. Halberstam suggests
that alongside the centering of work-time, there is also a “reproductive temporality” and that
deviation — any deviation — constitutes “queer time” (2005:4-6; cf. George & Ferraro
2016). Because there is no room in the normative society for anyone who “leaves the
inheritance,” such a person is othered; temporally, they are “queer” irrespective of their
label of vague/unsortable and transgressive outsider-dom). Queer time might especially fit
someone who exits a traditional (employed, procreative) life course to take care of someone
with neither reproductive nor income potential; unlike marginalized persons or directly
experienced hardships, a caregiver has some level of choice in the matter (even if the choice
is between becoming a caregiver or letting a loved one decline alone and unsupervised).
specifically about dementia care, Beard et al. remind us that efforts to “fix” persons with
dementia are “predicated on the superiority of present-time orientation, where the goal is to
restore ‘reality’” (2009:228). The authors discourage “enforcing our cognitive reality by
52
letting go of normative expectations and ‘joining’ people with dementia in their worlds”
(Beard et al. 2009:228), and in doing so suggest there may be nothing wrong with not fitting
the common time-logic at all. To be clear, most caregivers struggle with their care recipients’
altered reality and probably would retain a role in the dominant time-logic, but it is not the
person with dementia who isolates them for the deviation; the assertion of a single and
universal “way” of being in the world means that those existing in queer time — whether or
not by choice — must be marginalized, lest the practice spread further (Halberstam 2005:10-
12, cf. Montgomery & Kosloski 2013; Schulz & Eden 2016:105). This is how you get queer
subjectivity.
describing queer space as “new understandings of space enabled by the production of queer
what is created there is not just for LGBTQ people; it certainly is not for everyone (there is
still a normative, cisheterocapitalist reproduction cycle out there, humming along, from
which queer subjects are discarded), but it might be for anyone. Queer subjectivity, then, is
Recall that dementia caregivers live as much or more in the world of their care
recipient as their own; their understandings of space and time become fluid, not entirely
their own, and that others’ positivistic constructs of reality are irrelevant at best (harmful at
their life, there is room for their counterpublic in queer space and time, themselves forged
and scarred by the AIDS crisis, its victims, and its caregivers (Halberstam 2005:2); witnesses
53
to dementia in particular are familiar with a “constantly diminishing future.” Queer
subjectivity may apply to any caregiving situation, but especially to those who delay or
sacrifice normative life courses that were otherwise available. As a research framework
(which Halberstam uses elsewhere in the same book to develop counternormative archives
for Brandon Teena and other queer narratives that had been altered and accepted in
normative media), it says first and foremost that there is valuable information in non-
normative experiences; these experiences are not always a matter of failings or of limited
research gap waiting to be filled!), but of choices that are not recognized. Queer subjectivity
is taking the opportunity to give illicit recognition, to see the unseen, and perhaps to allow
research orientation, a clear methodology is vital to its application. Scholars who call for
more “specialized categories of caregivers” (Brodaty & Donkin 2009:224) specifically link
this need to growing evidence of health inequality (Cook et al. 2018). Both quantitative and
qualitative approaches risk repeating societal erasure and diminish academic rigor when they
look at gender, race, and class as separate phenomena from health status, quality of life, or
caregiving outcomes. Developing such categories would require studies to both identify
significant patterns across large surveys of caregivers and offer context for the patterns as
measured. Public datasets allow and invite secondary inquiry to clarify these patterns, but
giving them context will require a theoretical foundation that is both sensitive to the variety
of care dyads and recognizes their origins in broader hegemony. Fortunately, studies are
54
calling for more intersectional perspectives in health and aging research (Evans 2019; Ferrer
et al. 2017; Gkiouleka et al. 2018; Hankivsky 2012; Richardson & Brown 2016).
who face multiple forms of systemic oppression at the same time (Crenshaw 1991; Hancock
2016:12). In the simplest terms, persons who face both racism and sexism in the workplace
demonstrate lower wages and fewer promotions than those who face racism or sexism alone
(Crenshaw 1991), but these forces also have implications upon activism, politics, and social
science (Crenshaw 1993; Hancock 2016:7-11; Moradi & Grzanka 2017). Intersectionality
asserts that not only is it significant to recognize when and how these disparities amplify one
another, but that systems of redress refusing to acknowledge more than one form of
theories collectively asserting that we can come to understand macro-social power dynamics,
if only through ever-more precise (and complex) matrixes of separation (Grusky &
Weisshaar 2014:4-15, 45, see also Tumin 37 in the same volume; Wright 2005:12-21, 26, see
also Breen 41 in the same volume); instead, intersectionality asserts that to comprehend the
magnitude of a person’s “total” marginalization, you do not divide the forces against them
and add them up, but combine them and pay attention to their cumulative, qualitative impact
seeded by the time Anna Julia Cooper wrote in 1892 of the “unique position” of Black
women as not merely Black and not merely women, but “confronted by both a woman
55
question and a race problem” (Guy-Sheftall 1995:45; cf. Crenshaw 1991). The Combahee
River Collective expanded this sentiment in 1977, stating, “We [...] find it difficult to separate
race from class from sex oppression because in our lives they are most often experienced
simultaneously” (Guy-Sheftall 1995:234; cf. Moradi & Grzanka 2017). Kimberlé Williams
Crenshaw named intersectionality and cemented its academic stature in two law articles in
the early 1990s, situating the need for multifaceted frameworks against oversimplified legal
battles over equal pay and censorship (1991, 1993). While its applications comprise any
situation where social marginalizations do not merely accumulate but create unique and
The face value for quantitative applications may seem low because intersectionality
discrimination, harmful binaries, and having to compromise to pursue recourse and identity.
together several disparities and calculating their cumulative harm, and theorists fearing
dilution and appropriation now push back against scholars who conflate intersectionality
with any study invoking multi-layered identities (Moradi & Grzanka 2017). What matters is
not whether there is a mathematical approach, but where the math is rooted. Material
differences do exist, they can be traced to social and systemic discrimination, and they can be
calculated for a specific population, but this process must originate from the population
itself (Moradi & Grzanka 2017). Intersectionality does not resist quantitative inquiry, only
the quantitative predilection for rigid standardization. Oppressive forces are in a constant
56
state of flux (indeed, perhaps time — or Queer Time — should be added to the
intersectionality toolbox, but this question is outside the scope of the present study), so no
scholar can ever say they have identified the textbook formula for intersectional enumeration
for all caregivers; we can only say that we have observed certain patterns within a particular
population at a particular time and, from there, calculate the (socio-)economic impact.
The call for intersectional analyses to hone and apply quantitative methods is
growing (Bauer & Scheim 2019), including in health contexts (Green, Evans, & Subramanian
2017; Hankivsky 2012; Richman & Zucker 2019; Warner & Brown 2011). Combining
intersectionality and statistical analysis will not only improve the quality of statistical
methods (by, for example, contextualizing systems of oppression in interaction effects), but
also continue the advance of feminist inquiry beyond theory and ontology (Scott & Siltanen
constituted predates and anticipates Michel Foucault’s well-known arguments about power”
(2016:164; cf. Foucault [1963]1994:99-100). Studies that look at, for example, race and
gender together can improve the present but also elevate the history (individual and
collective) of participants (Richardson & Brown 2016), which will also expand other critical
standpoints.
because “unique characteristics of various racial and ethnic minorities interact with the
challenges common to all [care situations] and result in substantially different outcomes”
(Angel et al. 2015). When scholars treat caregiving as a gendered phenomenon, they obscure
57
the ways multiple marginalizations place the disadvantaged caring for the advantaged; in the
middle class, it may be women caring for men, but in long-term care facilities it is the
working class caring for the middle class; in cities it is the less educated caring for the highly
educated; in many states, it is people of color caring for whites; and across the country it is
often the immigrant caring for the citizen (Glenn 2010:181; Scales et al. 2020). These
oppressive dynamics do not operate separately, but in constant interaction with one another,
rooted in many of the same assumptions about a positivistic, right or wrong world; this is
why intersectionality must remain interpretive and contextual, and why caregiving must be
Conclusion
This section has reviewed some of the theoretical frameworks that have previously
been applied to dementia care and caregiving research at large and detailed the challenges
that have emerged. In promoting a sociological research stance toward dementia caregiving,
this study moves away from interventionist perspectives that have little interest in the
support systems of caregivers and has faltered in the face of health inequalities. Critical
theories are suggested as an alternative that can meet the needs of dementia caregivers, their
care recipients, and practitioners of all levels. Halberstam’s queer subjectivity and
intersectionality are selected to advance the current analysis through inquiry of how othering
HYPOTHESES
This study examines the effect of intersecting social marginalizations upon dementia
58
existing quantitative dataset is utilized to demonstrate that critical theories can be applied to
data collected in a straightforward, positivistic manner. The following three hypotheses will
test whether social disparities demonstrate a significant impact upon dementia caregivers; to
a lesser extent, they will also explore the potential for sub-categorizing dementia caregivers
H1: The age of dementia onset among care recipients varies by race. This hypothesis places a
considered “early onset” if it is diagnosed before age 65, but a racial disparity that favors
whites could indicate that people of color are being ignored or systematically excluded in the
H2: Dementia caregivers with normative characteristics will demonstrate less physical strain than
non-normative caregivers. This hypothesis tests whether reports of low physical strain are
Physical strain tends to be one of the lower burden measures among dementia caregivers;
model, these social indicators may demonstrate that normative caregivers have an outsized
those othered for having few of the following normative traits: white, woman, over 50,
H3: Dementia caregivers with higher social marginalization will demonstrate a higher ADL index
than other caregivers. This hypothesis tests whether the ADL index measures significantly
59
higher among caregivers who face more intersections of social adversity — in other words, a
measure of care intensity will be assessed to determine whether caregivers who are othered
by society are othered amongst caregivers. ADLs are often counted as an “objective”
measure of care burden, but this hypothesis asserts that care dyads who face historical and
ongoing disadvantages will see health and care disparities reflected in their care challenges.
socially marginalized caregivers and compare their ADL indexes against caregivers with the
fewest marginalized traits: person of color, woman, lower income, less education, employed,
60
CHAPTER III
care variables utilized. Statistical methods (correlation matrixes and multivariate regression)
DATA
This study utilizes Caregiving in the U.S., a periodic, public interest survey of adult
caregivers that includes both objective and subjective items about the care dyad. Four waves
of this dataset exist (1997, 2003, 2009, and 2015, although 2015 employs a new
methodology) and are available to the public for download without registration. A new wave
stress, and an index of total ADLs were examined within the 2015 wave (conducted in late
Sample
From a random online sample of 7,660 nationwide adults, 1,248 online surveys were
completed by adults who had given care to some other adult in the prior 12 months
(NAC/AARP 2015). Oversamples are not included as they fall outside the scope of this
study. The sample is weighted according to the specifications recommended by the dataset’s
61
VARIABLES
Cases have been restricted to dementia caregivers who answered all necessary items
for the dependent variables. This section describes the variables as originally coded and
initial modifications made therefrom; invalid responses (Not Sure/Don’t Know or Refused) are
omitted.
Dependent Variables
For dependent variables, this study utilizes one indicator of dementia scope and
three indicators (one objective and two subjective) of care burden. For the scope of
dementia, age of onset estimates when the care recipient began to receive care for dementia;
this interval-ratio variable, measured in years, was calculated by subtracting the survey’s item
for duration of care from the age of the care recipient. This calculation does not recognize
potential changes in caregivers or situations where the care recipient previously received care
for something other than dementia, yet it is the closest we can get with the available data to
age of diagnosis or visible onset; a dichotomous version tracks those who are estimated to
For subjective caregiver burden, two survey questions record physical strain or
emotional stress from caregiving as reported by caregivers. For both 5-level scales, the
lowest item (recorded as 1) was Not a strain at all (Physical Strain) or Not at all stressful
(Emotional Stress) and the highest item (recorded as 5) was very much a strain or very stressful;
the available responses approximate respondent attitudes but cannot be quantified rationally,
62
The remaining dependent variable is an index of ADLs as counted by NAC/AARP,
based on cumulative (recorded as No = 1 and Yes = 1, but here modified so that No = 0, Yes
This dataset records a total of six ADLs, so the range for ADL count is 0-6; the higher the
count, the more types of daily support the caregiver delivers. Since the severity of ADLs are
Note here that other studies tend to combine the mobility measures — getting in and
out of beds and chairs and getting to and from the toilet — into one item, resulting in only five total
ADLs; NAC and AARP do not explain this methodological decision, but do report a 16-
point difference between the mobility measures as separated (Hunt et al. 2015:40).
Independent Variables
caregiver age, caregiver primary status, and caregiver employment status. Relationship is
recorded as a 36-item categorical variable referring to the relationship between the dyad —
63
whether they are partners (coded as 4: Companion/Partner, 20: Spouse, or 36: Same-Sex Partner),
other type of relative, or not related (coded as 24: Friend, 25: Guardianee, 26: Neighbor, or 27:
Other Non-relative) — based on responses to the survey question “Who are you caring/did
you care for?” Three dyad relationships (Family, Partnered, and Parent) are recoded
calculated ordinally (Sole Caregiver = 1, Primary but not sole = 2, Someone else is primary = 3, and
Shared equally = 4) that reports whether the respondent served as primary caregiver (No = 0,
Yes = 1); this study also created a dichotomous (No = 0, Yes = 1) variable for solo caregivers
(all solo caregivers are primaries, but not all primaries are solo), and inverted solo to report
whether a caregiver has Unpaid (informal) Support (No = 0, Yes = 1). Employment is a
= 1) reporting whether the caregiver says they were employed and caregiving simultaneously
Intervening Variables
education attainment, household income, and caregiver age. As women are central to two of
the hypotheses, gender is here discussed as a dichotomous variable for woman (initially
coded as Male = 1, Female = 2, here recoded so that Man = 0 and Woman = 1; see
Introduction for notes on terms of inclusion). Race was recorded across multiple survey
questions and merged in data collection as a 5-item categorical variable (White = 1, Black = 2,
Asian = 3, Other = 4, and Hispanic = 5); for this analysis, two dichotomous variables report
64
when a caregiver is white (Person of Color = 0, White = 1) or a person of color (White = 0,
Person of Color = 1), and a third dichotomous variable reports on Black and non-Black people
of color (Non-Black POC = 0, Black = 1). Education attainment depicts highest education
attainment (initially coded ordinally as Less than High School = 1, High School grad/GED = 2,
Some College = 3, Technical School = 4, College grad = 5, Graduate school/Grad work = 7) and is
here recoded dichotomously based on whether the caregiver has attained some certification
Certification = 1); as will be discussed in the next chapter, another dichotomous variable was
created for caregivers reporting the least education (High School Diploma or Beyond = 0, No
Diploma = 1). Household income (in dollars, initially merged from several questions into the
were, respectively, less than or more than 50000 but otherwise unspecified) is presented here
dichotomously (Less than 50000 = 0, 50000 or More = 1); note that the U.S. national average
household income for 2014 was $53,657 (DeNavas-Walt & Proctor 2015). Caregiver age was
recorded in years as an interval-ratio variable, but is here presented dichotomously (Ages 50+
= 0, 18-49 = 1); an inversion of this variable (Ages 18-49 = 0, 50+ = 1) is also utilized for
older caregiver.
DATA ANALYSIS
Bivariate Correlations
65
significance — but not the strength — of superficial relationships by measuring correlation
between all categories within the variables compared. Because this method involves a
correlation matrix that subdivides into smaller batches, some variable categories have been
controlled cross-tabulation.
variables. Statistically significant regression models report the strength and direction of a
predicted relationship, as well as its significance; errors between expected and actual
outcomes are minimized by squaring and adding them together and letting the lowest
possible total determine the effectiveness of the regression (this technique is called ordinary
least squares). These interactions are relevant to intersectional concepts because models of
co-occurring variables can demonstrate an impact upon a dependent variable that is more (or
more complicated) than the sum of their individual interactions (Evans 2019:95); such
models can be merged into a single variable (called an interaction effect) for further analysis.
By exploring data in this manner, complex patterns become predictions, explaining variance
(with each other or with interval-ratio variables) as dummy variables. A dummy variable
must be stated strictly in terms of a single category (i.e., “dummy” and “not-dummy”), with
66
the largest category conventionally serving as referent (“not-dummy”). Regressions can
theoretically consider many variables at once, but when significance is equal, a simple
The two remaining hypotheses, H2 and H3, are tested via multivariate linear
regression in Chapter 4. Under guidance from the theoretical framework, each hypothesis
will test three models to predict variance with the variables introduced here. Models will
include a set of variables as predictors, ranging between two and nine each, and the
significance of the optimal combination will test the hypothesis. These inquiries seek to
identify niches within dementia care dyads where social disparities are most evident.
67
CHAPTER IV
RESULTS
This chapter reviews the results of the data analysis. Descriptive analysis for primary
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS
The social aspects of dementia care are numerous and complex. Table 2 shows the
distribution of demographics and care traits among dementia caregivers and confirms much
Hunt et al. 2017) have disclosed about this sample. While the distribution of race closely
resembles the make-up of the United States, caregiver age skews in favor of older caregivers
and gender demonstrates a clear skew in favor of women (although men are more
represented in dementia care than in caregiving at large). Education is split nearly down the
middle; income skews in favor of households making more than $50,000 a year, though at
dementia care is between a parent (or parent-in-law) and adult child, with care of partners
representing another quarter. More caregivers support a person who is not related to them
than care for siblings, grandparents, and other relatives combined. Of dementia caregivers,
one in four has a child under 18 living with them (i.e., they constitute part of a sandwich
generation).
68
Table 2. Independent and Intervening Variables Used in Analysis of
Social Aspects of Dementia Care Dyads, 2015 (NAC/AARP)
Variable Category (n) Sub-Category % Std. Dev. Skew
(n or mean)
Gender Women (150) 57.8 .49480 -.319
Men (109) 42.2 .49480 .319
Race White (172) 66.4 .47325 -.698
Person of Color (87) 33.6 .47325 .698
Black (35) 13.4 .49239
a
.423
a
Employment is also a significant phenomenon for dementia care. While over a third
of dementia caregivers do not work, among those who do nearly two out of three are
working at least 35 hours per week. Among primary dementia caregivers, more are operating
without help from friends or family (37.7 percent) than are (20.5 percent); this indicates that
studies excluding secondary caregivers may leave out two out of every five informal roles in
dementia care. Dementia caregivers report that one in five of their loved ones requires
constant supervision.
69
Hypothesis 1
H1: The age of dementia onset among care recipients varies by race.
Recall that early onset dementia has a distinct pathological trajectory from those dementias
that are recognized mostly beyond age 65. Table 3 shows the challenges discussed above
with simplistic pathological determinants that ignore social phenomena, where age of onset
appears to vary across race. H1 can be explored (if not fully confirmed) using descriptive
statistics. By comparing means across three categories of race (White, Non-Black Person of
Color, and Black), age of care recipient and, by extrapolation, age of onset, reveal themselves
to be much lower for people of color than for whites. The average white dementia care
recipient is over seven years older (79.2) than the average person of color (about 71.5), and
this disparity grows by another year when comparing age of onset (means: 75.1 for White
and 67.0 for Persons of Color). Even though each of these race categories demonstrates
about the same proportion in the dementia subpopulation as they do in the U.S. population,
Table 3. Medians for Age of Onset, Controlled for Race, 2015 (NAC/AARP)
Variable White Non-Black POC Black
(N) (172) (52) (35)
mean Std. Dev. mean Std. Dev. mean Std. Dev.
Age of Care Recipient 79.2 13.43800 71.6 16.05000 71.5 14.55700
Age at Onset (est.) 75.1 14.95530 67.1 19.37383 67.0 16.96655
Early Onset 16.1% .36855 36.5% .48605 34.5% .47908
When these figures are graphed, both the starkness of the distribution and its
70
cases by calculated age of onset, controlling for race. The distribution curve for whites is the
most robust, as it has the most cases, but it also distinctly crests far to the right of (higher
than) Age 65, the benchmark for early onset dementia. The distribution curves for Black and
non-Black people of color, on the other hand, crest very close to Age 65. These curves are
rather flat due to their smaller number of cases. The significance of these findings must be
onset has too few cases spread across too many categories to compare as an interval-ratio
variable, but its reduction to the early onset dichotomous variable can be utilized instead.
Table 4 reports the results of this comparison, confirming a significant, weak relationship (χ²
71
= 12.302, Φ = -0.218 and Cramer’s V = 0.218). H1 is confirmed through controlled
descriptive statistics. The age of dementia onset among care recipients is higher among
people of color than among whites. Since nearly half of all cases of dementia for people of
color are diagnosed before age 65, the early onset benchmark seems to conflate experiences
study’s remaining hypotheses can be tested. Many have not demonstrated a significant
relationship with the outcome variables, but those that do are reported in Table 5. Some
moderate linear relationships are evident, notably the negative relationships employment
status demonstrates with outcomes when stratified by a demographic; that is, non-Black
persons of color who are employed tend to report significant lower levels of physical stress;
also, an observed reduction in self-reported emotional stress for caregivers who are
relationship between employed caregivers and physical strain, for example, appears to be
concentrated in caregivers who are neither white nor Black. As discussed in the literature
review, research on non-Black people of color (recorded as Black, Hispanic, Asian, or Other)
72
is limited but has repeatedly suggested that communities of color have a strong sense of
family duty that may mitigate stressors noted in general care populations. This assertion is
especially common for Black caregivers, and yet they lack a significant relationship (even a
negative one) here. Similarly, a negative relationship between employment and emotional
stress appears to be concentrated in women. In both cases, the correlation coefficient may
reflect social role fulfillment (communities of color fulfilling cultural expectations of family
care, women who are relieved to have responsibilities outside the home) or it may reflect a
low requirement of care: a caregiver who is able to work may not be overwhelmed by
responsibilities at home, or they may have plenty of unpaid support to share the burden.
Note that these findings do not rule out a relationship between variables, only
confirm the strength and direction of straightforward linear relationships. Table 5 shows
only significant relationships that are unstratified or controlled for a single intervening
73
variable. Exploring more than two variables at a time (or three variables, if one is a control)
ordinary least squares to approximate a normal distribution and predict variance for a
dependent variable. The remaining two hypotheses will be tested for significance through
three models each of regression; if multiple models are found to be significant, the most
elegant model (highest variance predicted with fewest variables) will be the most effective.
Hypothesis 2
H2: Dementia caregivers with normative characteristics will demonstrate less physical strain
than non-normative caregivers.
For dementia care dyads, the following traits are so common that their overlap may
constitute a normative niche within dementia care: white, woman, over 50, higher income,
not employed, caring for a parent. If the niche is significant, it could skew results for all non-
variables listed demonstrates a negative relationship with physical strain, but the higher the
Results from three models are reported in Table 6. Model 2a is the simplest: what is
the strength of the relationship between white woman and physical strain in dementia care?
Model 2b includes all six of the variables listed to determine whether the niche gains
significant accuracy with all factors included. Tweaking from among these six variables did
74
not improve significance much, so three further dichotomous variables are added for Model
2c: less education (high school diploma or less), normal onset (i.e., dementia care begins at or
after age 65), and unpaid support. These three variables showed some significance when
This demonstrates that complex social factors (including, intersectionality would note,
oppression) not only build upon one another in small, individual increments, they can also
compound for an even greater effect than the sum of their parts. By adding more normative
variables, we may get closer to demonstrating the hypothesis. Unfortunately, this sort of
75
the broad applicability of findings diminishes — and for this reason none of the models met
Having failed to reject the null hypothesis, no normative niche is confirmed for this
and physical strain was observed. It appears that an analysis centered on normative social
forces is not effective for demonstrating intersectional and othering dynamics among
dementia caregivers.
Hypothesis 3
H3: Dementia caregivers with higher social marginalization will demonstrate a higher ADL
index than other caregivers.
To demonstrate that social forces from outside caregiving can still affect caregivers, some of
the starkest social factors need to be discussed in their caregiving context. No studies have
explored racism, sexism, or income inequality in the context of care dyads (although some
reviews discuss homophobia in the context of LGBTQ caregivers, and as discussed above
health research in general is embracing complex social and life course considerations), but
these forces are inescapable because caregiving does not happen in a vacuum. Forms of
discrimination and systemic oppression interact with one another, and intersectional analysis
allows us to explore how. This test looks at ADL index, one of the most quantitative
measures available for caregiver burden. To test whether the measure of multiple
demographic categories are tested: person of color, woman, lower income, less education,
76
employed, and sandwich generation. This hypothesis must demonstrate statistical
Table 7 reports the results of the three models tested for H3.
Model 3a tests the significance of women of color’s relationship with ADL index. Model 3b
tests all seven variables listed at once. Neither demonstrates a significant relationship, yet
Model 3b is close (its significance measured just outside the standard p-value for regressions,
p ≤ 0.05). In this model, education was a surprisingly weak variable (far lower than in Model
2C); as stated above, the dichotomous split around high school diploma is very close to fifty-
fifty, potentially offering too little variance to be significant. A new variable was recoded
from the original dataset: a dichotomous measure for caregivers who had the least education
(no high school diploma). Model 3c replaced the lower education variable for least education
77
and identified a significant relationship. In Model 3c, education becomes the most impactful
predictor, alone accounting for variance of nearly one (0.874) ADL point — not a small
difference for a variable with only a range of 6 and a mean of 2.1. This indicates that, among
the most marginalized dementia caregivers (women of color with lower income who are
employed and part of a sandwich generation), not having a high school diploma raises by
It is important to clarify what the finding implies. It does not necessarily follow that
the same impact or even the same kind of impact — intersectional interactions are
contextual. A person without a high school diploma is more likely than a person with a
diploma to come from a family facing tremendous disadvantage, socially and health-wise, so
their loved ones tend to need much more from them on average. There is not likely to be
any direct or indirect relationship between a caregiver’s high school diploma and a present
care recipient’s care needs, but it might offer insight into how caregivers are selected.
It does not appear children from smaller families (therefore fewer available
caregivers) attain less education than those with many siblings (in fact, the opposite seems
more likely; Falbo & Polit 1986); more likely, a family who is choosing the caregiver for an
elder with dementia may favor, among those available, persons with if not the least
education, then the meagerest income or job prospects or the fewest meaningful
accomplishments in the eyes of their family. Whatever an individuals’ personal reason for
Halberstam), by not pursuing the national baseline for education, the person may signal that
78
making more money is not a priority; such a person may live “outside the logics of capital
accumulation” and thus be regarded by their family as one of Halberstam’s queer subjects
(2005:10). In terms of economic contribution, a person who has very little education may be
regarded non-normative, increasing the likelihood their family would thrust into another
Other disadvantages compound for even stronger effects, indicating that caregiving
consideration.
79
CHAPTER V
CONCLUSION
SUMMARY
This quantitative study presents a path forward for informal dementia care research
in the United States. As demonstrated in the literature review, caregiving and dementia care
have seen social inquiry languish while diffuse studies revisit pathology, demographics, and
micro-interactions without coordination or innovation; this study makes a case for stronger
three hypotheses, two were confirmed: that age of dementia onset among care recipients
varies by race (H1) and that dementia caregivers who are highly marginalized socially report
performing more ADLs for their care recipients (H3). The other hypothesis, that a normative
caregiver will report lower levels of physical strain, failed to attain significance. These
hypotheses were informed by Halberstam’s queer subjectivity, which asks if anyone who is
not actively procreating and building capital can be othered in society, even if they are for
reasons of family. These hypotheses were further developed through an intersectional lens to
show that dementia caregivers do not exist outside the world of the social (and particularly
of discrimination and systemic disparity), but instead have their care experiences affected by
social factors outside the care dyad. These findings should inform future research and policy
80
priorities as the U.S. and other post-industrial economies approach a surge in elder
DISCUSSION
Halberstam or Barret or Foucault to perceive that the caregiver is both everywhere and
nowhere in society. As thousands of generations have done before, families are stepping up
every day to tend to loved ones who can no longer care for themselves, and yet society does
not normalize caregiving or caregivers. Perhaps worse is dementia care; caring for someone
who cannot recover and may one day forget you does not build families or make the family
any money. Caregiving is an action outside of normative time and space — what is a bedtime
when your loved one is sundowning? where is home when someone who once comforted
you gets lost in their own body? are we allowed to care about things and people and
relationships that society does not make an explicit time and space for, and if so how? This
study does not ask those questions overtly, but they are present. If caregiving is going to
meet the needs of elders in the coming decades, some of these tensions between the seen
and the unseen, the valued and the unvalued, will require reconciliation.
racism, and sexism, there is no term for the exclusion of people who prioritize care over
81
capital. Like many systemic otherings, it does not depend on the violent acts of individuals
who wish harm, only broad inaction or negligence, facilitated by a general lack of inclusion,
no normative time and space where one may be a caregiver and still be regarded as
normative in society. This is especially true for dementia caregivers, whose support happens
on a scale of years, not weeks, and who spend at least some portion of their time
accommodating a fluid, subjective reality that may exist for no one else. Through the lens of
Halberstam, caregiving emerges as queer subject, a (paradoxically social, in that it is the act
Informal care exists outside of the economy because there is no place for it within
the economy, and yet that economy depends on free care labor. Its place is unwelcome,
othered, queer, deviant. Caregiving is not inherently anti-capitalist, and yet the evidence
presented here requires we ask whether capitalism might be anti-caregiving. If this is the
case, caregivers would find themselves in good company; at the heart of capitalism as we
know it is a hegemony that systemically (and sometimes overtly) disfavors women, people of
color, workers, LGBTQ folks, asexuals, non-monogamists, couples who are childless by
choice, and anyone who does not measure quality of life in dollars, years, or number of lives
given, taken, or saved. At the heart of these others is a sense of mutualism, either because
that is what capitalism rejects about them or in defense against the rejection. Is the caregiver
collaboration in care research and demonstrates the practice through a quantitative inquiry.
Scholarship up to this point has been so diffuse one could call it anti-disciplinary, but by a
82
lack of intention rather than a deliberate deconstruction. Because this study has also
ontological questions about creating new academic structures while accommodating post-
structuralist knowledge and even priorities. In approaching this study with critical theories,
my intention was to not overlook the process of othering in our social worlds; too many
fields of research were built first and made accessible much later, but caregiving could be
different. Caregiving, as much as sociology itself, is about people and for people, but can a
foundation be built that leaves room for future discoveries and corrections?
transgression, and intersectionality tells us that transgressions face social punishment with
material consequences. The theories employed in this study were selected because any
mistakes of their application will be more informative than the correct application of
uncritical social science. If it is found that the author has misappropriated queer subjectivity
or Black feminism, then let the corrections be fluent and accessible and waste no time in
creating space for caregivers, who may not even see themselves well enough to know they
The quantitative analysis presented here is only a step in the direction of uncovering
the full, non-normative mechanics of informal care. Many dementia caregivers face a
powerful separation between their inner and outer inner worlds, but understanding of their
lived experience can be improved if we expand the scope of inquiry to include the networks
of support, the selection of individual caregivers by families, and the dense interdependence
of the act of care itself. Demonstrating the commonalities within and between types of
83
caregivers (not only by condition, but also by social and cultural contexts) can reinvigorate
scholarship but also deepen the connectivity of caregivers between themselves, giving them
H1 confirms that caregiving, pathology, and social factors affect one another and
warrant consideration together. The mean disparity in ages of onset between whites and
people of color is severe enough, we are left to ask whether 65 is all that early for people of
color; since early onset dementia has its own pathological trajectory, how can we know
whether people of color are receiving the most appropriate medical response? The
affecting healthcare and caregiving and warrants further examination. Critical theories beg us
to go one step further and ask how the universal demarcation of age 65 contributes to the
othering of people of color with dementia and therefore how it serves white supremacy.
the most common caregiver demographics. This finding remains instructive about non-
The findings for H3 return this inquiry to its intersectional perspective and confirm
that, though small in this model, dementia caregivers can show marginalization in their work
and/or selection. H3 expands evidence from H1 that social disparities interact with care
circumstances. Unlike the pathological lens for H1 (i.e., the age that determines whether
dementia is early onset or not), Model 3c presents only social relationships as predictors:
84
how is the person racialized and genderized, what is their economic and educational access,
This study confirms two hypotheses and fails to confirm one. H1 confirms that the
determinants can directly correlate to care needs on the ground. The social lives of caregivers
The prominence of caregivers seems poised to rise further as elder and disabled
emersion challenges normative family and economic priorities. Scholars cannot deny that
care dynamics demonstrate social influence, nor that quantitative research benefits from
can dementia caregivers see their circumstances only through the lens of one sick relative:
the circumstances of dementia care are just as systemic as they are personal.
LIMITATIONS
By emphasizing that new truths available in a secondary source, this study forgoes
the opportunity to ask further questions from participants or researchers involved. Its
emphasis on dementia care will also limit the reliability of results for other types of
caregivers. Findings will only be directly applicable to experiences within the United States
The design of this study is nascent and requires further development. That said,
these choices have been made deliberately to specifically highlight gaps in existing literature,
85
outline opportunities for methodological innovation, and more effectively elucidate the
RECOMMENDATIONS
Developing a stronger sociological base for care research will require the
development of both knowledge and design; before caregiving can begin to consider
standardization under sociology or some other field, some cohort of investigators will have
to agree that this is worthwhile and commit to building bridges between the research niches
that currently exist. These bridges will require social scientists to become more familiar with
biomedical scholarship (and vice versa) and to improve social inquiry of the middle ground
between the four farthest corners of care scholarship: hyperlocal qualitative, logistical
biomedical practice, aloof quantitative demography, and abstract theory. Researchers may
also develop interdisciplinary or trans-disciplinary research that links caregiving trends with
interdependence (psychology), and mutual aid (political science). These approaches all have
something to show one another, but without networking across all levels care research will
While building a framework for care research that crosses levels and disciplines could
help to fill research gaps, some of the limitations could be improved by implementing more
theoretical approaches that contextualize individual care dynamics in greater economic and
power trends. By exploring the concept of normativity, I attempted to place the positivistic
in conversation with the post-positivistic, but such an undertaking will require a larger
86
such an undertaking to make it clear that defining normativities creates others out of the
non-normative; though the outcomes are vastly different, there is a fine line between
The health of greater families and support systems cannot be discerned from any
available caregiver dataset, but the present findings bolster the case for more comprehensive
inquiry of whole networks and systems around dementia care, not only the primary dyad.
Understanding the dyad requires a more systemic reflection on the dyad’s familial support
and circumstances. However, even in available datasets, these findings can be expanded by
than in quantitative analysis. This confluence of methods may be off-putting to readers who
mixed methodologies.
The techniques utilized here found weak significant results; future studies may
consider linking intersectional approaches with other multivariate techniques, such as factor
analysis, cluster analysis, and try to improve upon these findings. Although the NAC/AARP
dataset was selected for its breadth of both demographic and subjective variables, this study
warrants repeating with a larger (probably federal) dataset, such as the National Survey of
Caregivers. Politicization affects what research is performed and shared from the federal level;
non-federal secondary datasets can aid in filling potential gaps, but would be bolstered if
87
they were more normalized in academic literature and their range and complexity taught,
Research into how dementia is framed in popular culture may help to alleviate the
disconnect and elevate the agency of people in early stages of dementia and the humanity of
those in late stages. For all the reminders in dementia care scholarship that dementia is not a
mental illness, it is still socialized like one. Such research may want to invoke the literature of
disability studies, particularly calls to amplify the voices of disabled people in defining their
own experiences.
sense of stakeholder in studies of caregiving. The researcher and the medical practitioner
offer important insights but can still overlook elements of care that may be more apparent to
caregivers, their extended support system, and even persons with dementia themselves. Only
by including multiple (and, ideally all) of these perspectives will care research advance
CONCLUSION
As this study was being completed, the world took on great, collective, health-
anxious gasp as a new and highly contagious virus transversed every populated continent.
While epidemiologists go to work to inform the public and mitigate the impact, topics of
mutual care and social isolation are suddenly prominent and urgent. Where those
conversations lead remains to be seen, but somewhere between the panic and the boredom
there have also been indications that not everyone who beats COVID-19 is able to fully
recover. It is too early to tell what the impact will be upon the elder population — a high
88
enough death rate for people over 65 could completely upend predictions of a “care gap” —
but two other populations are changing in demographically significant ways. Firstly, in the
United States, elders of color (especially Black men) have been the most likely to die in the
first wave of COVID-19, as decades of continued health disparity and systemic disadvantage
take their toll. Secondly, there is a growing likelihood that some portion of COVID-19
survivors will become permanently disabled in ways that have not yet been identified,
Caregiving has always been relevant, but never more urgent than right now. How we
care for others and expect to be cared for may set the stage for the next century of social
habits, expectations, and connections. From dementia caregivers, we can learn to accept a
reality that is not entirely our own, to recognize and respect the agency of others, and to
develop and sustain strong support systems — informal and formal — that serve all
participants well.
89
REFERENCES
Adler, Richard, and Rajiv Mehta. 2014. Catalyzing Technology to Support Family Caregiving.
Washington, DC: National Alliance for Caregiving.
Angel, Jacqueline L., Stipica Mudrazija, and R. Benson. 2015. Racial and Ethnic Inequalities in
Health. Oxford: Elsevier Science & Technology.
Apesoa-Varano, Ester C., Judith C. Barker, and Ladson Hinton. 2011. "Curing and Caring:
The Work of Primary Care Physicians with Dementia Patients." Qualitative Health
Research 21(11):1469-1483.
Apesoa-Varano, Ester C., Yajarayma Tang-Feldman, Susan C. Reinhard, Rita Choula, and
Heather M. Young. 2015. "Multi-Cultural Caregiving and Caregiver Interventions: A
Look Back and a Call for Future Action." Generations: Journal of the American Society on
Aging 39(4):39-48.
Bailes, Christine O., Colleen M. Kelley, and Nadine M. Parker. 2016. "Caregiver Burden and
Perceived Health Competence when Caring for Family Members Diagnosed with
Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia." Journal of the American Association of Nurse
Practitioners 28(10):534-540.
Barnes, Marian, and Tula Brannelly. 2008. "Achieving Care and Social Justice for People
with Dementia." Nursing Ethics 15(3):384-395.
Bauer, Greta R., and Ayden I. Scheim. 2019. "Advancing Quantitative Intersectionality
Research Methods: Intracategorical and Intercategorical Approaches to Shared and
Differential Constructs." Social Science & Medicine 226:260-262.
Beard, Renée L., and Patrick J. Fox. 2008. "Resisting Social Disenfranchisement: Negotiating
Collective Identities and Everyday Life with Memory Loss." Social Science & Medicine
66:1509-1520.
Beard, Renée, Jenny Knauss, and Don Moyer. 2009. "Managing Disability and Enjoying
Life: How We Reframe Dementia through Personal Narratives." Journal of Aging Studies
23:227-235.
Bédard, Michel, D. W. Molloy, Larry Squire, Sacha Dubois, Judith A. Lever, and Martin
O'Donnell. 2001. "The Zarit Burden Interview: A New Short Version and Screening
Version." The Gerontologist 41(5):652-657.
Berry, Brandon, and Ester C. Apesoa-Varano. 2017 “Medication Takeovers: Covert
Druggings and Behavioral Control in Alzheimer’s.” Social Science & Medicine 188:51-59.
Berry, Brandon, Ester C. Apesoa-Varano, and Yarin Gomez. 2015. "How Family Members
Manage Risk Around Functional Decline: The Autonomy Management Process in
Households Facing Dementia." Social Science & Medicine 130:107-114.
90
Bookman, Ann, and Delia Kimbrel. 2011. "Families and Elder Care in the Twenty-First
Century." The Future of Children 21(2):117-140.
Bosco, Alessandro, Justine Schnieder, Donna Maria Coleston-Shields, and Martin Orrell.
2019. “Dementia Care Model: Promoting Personhood through Co-Production.”
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 81:59-73.
Brodaty, Henry, and Marika Donkin. 2009. "Family Caregivers of People with Dementia."
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 11(2):217-228.
Bruhn, John G., and Howard M. Rebach. 2014. The Sociology of Caregiving. New York:
Springer.
Caceres, Billy A., Mayu O. Frank, Jin Jun, Melissa T. Martelly, Tina Sadarangani, and Paloma
C. de Sales. 2016. "Family Caregivers of Patients with Frontotemporal Dementia: An
Integrative Review." International Journal of Nursing Studies 55:71-84.
Cloyes, Kristin G., William Hull, and Andra Davis. 2018. "Palliative and End-of-Life Care
for Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender (LGBT) Cancer Patients and their
Caregivers." Seminars in Oncology Nursing 34(1):60-71.
Cockerham, William C. 2013. "The Social Causation of Health and Disease." Pp. 1-26 in
Social Causes of Health and Disease. Oxford: Polity Press.
Cockerham, William C. and Ferris J. Richey. 1997. “Introduction: An Overview of Medical
Sociology.” Pp. xi-xxvi in Dictionary of Medical Sociology. Westport, Connecticut:
Greenwood Press.
Cook, Sarah K., Lauren Snellings, and Steven A. Cohen. 2018. "Socioeconomic and
Demographic Factors Modify Observed Relationship between Caregiving Intensity and
Three Dimensions of Quality of Life in Informal Adult Children Caregivers." Health and
Quality of Life Outcomes 16:169.
Cornwell, Benjamin, and Markus Schafer. 2015. "Social Networks in Later Life." Pp. 181-
201 in Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences. Oxford: Elsevier Science and Technology.
Crenshaw, Kimberlé. 1991. "Mapping the Margins: Intersectionality, Identity Politics, and
Violence Against Women of Color." Stanford Law Review 43(6):1241-1299.
------. 1993. "Demarginalizing the Intersection of Race and Sex: A Black Feminist Critique of
Antidiscrimination, Feminist Theory and Antiracist Politics." University of Chicago Legal
Forum:139-167.
Crimmins, Eileen M., and Sarinnapha M. Vasunilashorn. 2016. "Biodemography: Adding
Biological Insight into Social, Economic, and Psychological Models of Population and
Individual Health Change with Age." Pp. 55-75 in Handbook of Aging and the Social
Sciences., edited by L. George, and K. Ferraro. Oxford: Elsevier Science & Technology.
Dam, Alieske E. H., Marjolein E. de Vugt, Inge P. M. Klinkenberg, Frans R. J. Verhey, and
van Boxtel, Martin P. J. 2016. "A Systematic Review of Social Support Interventions for
91
Caregivers of People with Dementia: Are They Doing What They Promise?" Maturitas
85:117-130.
Davey, Adam, and Maximiliane E. Szinovacz. 2008. "Division of Care Among Adult
Children." Pp. 133-159 in Caregiving Contexts: Cultural, Familial, and Societal Implications.
New York: Springer.
DeNavas-Walt, Carmen, and Bernadette D. Proctor. 2015. Income and Poverty in the United
States: 2014. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau. Accessed May 11, 2020.
https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2015/demo/p60-
252.pdf
Dunham, Charlotte, and Julie Cannon. 2008. "‘They're Still in Control Enough to Be in
Control’: Paradox of Power in Dementia Caregiving." Journal of Aging Studies 22(1):45-53.
Dwolatzky, T., and A. M. Clarfield. 2003. "Correspondence: Reversible Dementias." Journal
of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 74:1008-1010.
Edelman, T. 2018. "Buying and Selling Nursing Homes: Who's Looking Out for the
Residents?" Accessed September 6, 2019. http://medicareadvocacy.org/buying-and-
selling-nursing-homes-whos-looking-out-for-the-residents
Eifert, Elise K., Rebecca Adams, Sharon Morrison, and Robert Strack. 2016. "Emerging
Trends in Family Caregiving using the Life Course Perspective: Preparing Health
Educators for an Aging Society." American Journal of Health Education 47(3):176-197.
Evans, Clare R. 2019. "Adding Interactions to Models of Intersectional Health Inequalities:
Comparing Multilevel and Conventional Methods." Social Science & Medicine 221:95-105.
Falbo, Toni, and Denise F. Polit. 1986. "Quantitative Review of the Only Child Literature:
Research Evidence and Theory Development." Psychological Bulletin 100(2):176.
Fazio, Sam, Douglas Pace, Janice Flinner, and Beth Kallmyer. 2018. "The Fundamentals of
Person-Centered Care for Individuals with Dementia." The Gerontologist 58(S1):S10-S19.
Feinberg, Lynn F., and Brenda C. Spillman. 2019. "Shifts in Family Caregiving — and a
Growing Care Gap." Generations: Journal of the American Society on Aging 43(1):73-77.
Ferrer, Ilyan, Amanda Grenier, Shari Brotman, and Sharon Koehn. 2017. "Understanding
the Experiences of Racialized Older People through an Intersectional Life Course
Perspective." Journal of Aging Studies 41:10-17.
Flinn, Brendan. 2018. Millennials: The Emerging Generation of Family Caregivers. Washington, DC:
AARP Public Policy Institute.
Foucault, Michel. 1994. The Birth of the Clinic: An Archaeology of Medical Perception. Translated by
A.M. Sheridan Smith. New York: Vintage.
Fox, Susannah, Maeve Duggan, and Kristen Purcell. 2013. Family Caregivers Are Wired for
Health. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. Accessed December 24 2018.
http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2013/Family-Caregivers.aspx
92
Frank, Adam. 2011a. "Beyond the Punch-Clock Life: The Tyranny of Modern Time II."
Accessed April 25, 2020. http://www.npr.org/blogs/13.7/2011/09/27/140818962/
beyond-the-punch-clock-life-the-tyranny-of-modern-time-ii.
------. 2011b. "The Future of Time." Accessed April 25, 2020.
http://www.npr.org/sections/13.7/2011/10/12/141217278/the-future-of-time.
------. 2011c. "The Tyranny of Modern Time." Accessed April 25, 2020.
http://www.npr.org/blogs/13.7/2011/09/23/140718434/time-crisis-why-you-don-t-
care-about-today-s-equinox.
Fraser, Nancy. 2016. "Contradictions of Capital and Care." New Left Review 100(Jul/Aug):99-
117.
Friedemann, Marie-Luise, and Kathleen C. Buckwalter. 2014. "Family Caregiver Role and
Burden Related to Gender and Family Relationships." Journal of Family Nursing
20(3):313-336.
Fry, Richard. 2017. "5 Facts about Millennial Households." Accessed August 8, 2019.
https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/09/06/5-facts-about-millennial-
households/
Gaugler, Joseph, Bryan James, Tricia Johnson, Allison Marin, and Jennifer Weuve. 2019.
2019 Alzheimer's Disease Facts and Figures. Chicago: Alzheimer’s Association.
Gauthier, S., A. Leuzy, E. Racine, and P. Rosa-Neto. 2013. "Diagnosis and Management of
Alzheimer's Disease: Past, Present and Future Ethical Issues." Progress in Neurobiology
110:102-113.
George, Linda K., and Kenneth F. Ferraro. 2016. "Aging and the Social Sciences: Process
and Prospects." in Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences, edited by L.K. George and
K.F. Ferraro. Oxford: Elsevier Science & Technology.
Gitlin, Laura N., Helen C. Kales, Katherine Marx, Barbara Stanislawski, and Constantine
Lyketsos. 2017. "A Randomized Trial of a Web-Based Platform to Help Families
Manage Dementia-Related Behavioral Symptoms: The WeCareAdvisor." Contemporary
Clinical Trials 62:27-36.
Gkiouleka, Anna, Timothy Huijts, Jason Beckfield, and Clare L. Bambra. 2018.
"Understanding the Micro and Macro Politics of Health: Inequalities, Intersectionality
& Institutions — A Research Agenda." Social Science & Medicine 200:92-98.
Gleckman, Howard. 2019. "Another Shock to the Long-Term Care Insurance Industry."
Accessed September 6, 2019. https://www.forbes.com/sites/howardgleckman/2019/
04/01/another-shock-to-the-long-term-care-insurance-industry/
Glenn, Evelyn N. 2010. Forced to Care. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Gouin, Jean‐Philippe, Chelsea Estrela, Kim Desmarais, and Erin T. Barker. 2016. "The
Impact of Formal and Informal Support on Health in the Context of Caregiving
Stress." Family Relations 65(1):191-206.
93
Green, Mark A., Clare R. Evans, and S. V. Subramanian. 2017. "Can Intersectionality Theory
Enrich Population Health Research?" Social Science & Medicine 178:214-216.
Greenwood, Nan, Gillian Mezey, and Raymond Smith. 2018. "Social Exclusion in Adult
Informal Carers: A Systematic Narrative Review of the Experiences of Informal Carers
of People with Dementia and Mental Illness." Maturitas 112:39-45.
Grusky, David B., and Katherine R. Weisshaar, eds. 2014. Social Stratification: Class, Race, and
Gender in Sociological Perspective. 4th ed. Boulder: Westview Press.
Gurrentz, Benjamin. 2018. Millennial Marriage: How Much Does Economic Security Matter to
Marriage Rates for Young Adults? Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau.
Guy-Sheftall, Beverly, ed. 1995. Words of Fire: An Anthology of African-American Feminist
Thought. New York: New Press.
Haeusermann, Tobias. 2017. "The Dementias — A Review and a Call for a Disaggregated
Approach." Journal of Aging Studies 42:22-31.
Halberstam, Judith. 2005. In a Queer Time and Place: Transgender Bodies, Subcultural Lives. New
York: New York University Press.
Hall, Donald E. 2003. "Introduction: What ‘Queer Theories’ Can Do for You." Pp. 9-18 in
Queer Theories. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Hancock, Ange-Marie. 2016. Intersectionality: An Intellectual History. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Hankivsky, Olena. 2012. "Women's Health, Men's Health, and Gender and Health:
Implications of Intersectionality." Social Science & Medicine 74(11):1712-1720.
Henning-Smith, Carrie, Megan Lahr, and Michelle Casey. 2019. "A National Examination of
Caregiver use of and Preferences for Support Services: Does Rurality Matter?" Journal of
Aging and Health 31(9):1652-1670.
Hermanns, Melinda, and Beth Mastel-Smith. 2012. "Caregiving: A Qualitative Concept
Analysis." The Qualitative Report 17(Art. 75):1-18. Accessed June 25, 2019.
http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR17/hermanns.pdf
Hirschman, Karen B., and Nancy A. Hodgson. 2018. "Evidence-Based Interventions for
Transitions in Care for Individuals Living with Dementia." The Gerontologist 58(S1):S129-
S140.
Hodson, G.J. forthcoming. "Modeling Types of Informal Caregivers: Gender, Condition,
and Social Age." Gender and Generations: Advances in Gender Research, Vol. 30, edited by V.
Demos and M.T. Segal. Somerville: Emerald.
hooks, bell. 1991. "Theory as Liberatory Practice." Yale Journal of Law and Feminism
4(1):Article 2.
94
Hunt, Gail Gibson, Rick Greene, C. Grace Whiting, Susan Reinhard, Lynn Friss Feinberg,
Rita Choula, Jordan Green, and Ari Houser. 2015. Caregiving in the U.S. 2015.
Washington, DC: National Alliance for Caregiving & AARP Public Policy Institute.
Hunt, Gail Gibson, C. Grace Whiting, Matthew Baumgart, Lisa Weber-Raley, and Michael
Panek. 2017. Dementia Caregiving in the U.S. Washington, DC: National Alliance for
Caregiving.
Johnson, Rebecca, Kristin Harkins, Mark Cary, Pamela Sankar, and Jason Karlawish. 2015.
"The Relative Contributions of Disease Label and Disease Prognosis to Alzheimer's
Stigma: A Vignette-Based Experiment." Social Science & Medicine 143:117-127.
Jordan, Ana. 2020. "Masculinizing Care? Gender, Ethics of Care, and Fathers’ Rights
Groups." Men and Masculinities 23(1):20-41.
Kane, Robert L., and Rosalie A. Kane. 1980. "Long-Term Care: Can our Society Meet the
Needs of its Elderly?" Annual Review of Public Health:227-53.
Keefe, Bronwyn. 2017. "Pride and Prejudice: Breaking Down Socially Constructed Attitudes
and Moving Towards a ‘Convergence’ in Aging and Disability Studies." The Gerontologist
57(6):1192-1193.
Kelley, Dannielle E., Megan A. Lewis, and Brian G. Southwell. 2017. "Perceived Support
from a Caregiver's Social Ties Predicts Subsequent Care-Recipient Health." Preventive
Medicine Reports (8):108-111.
Kim, Heejung, Sangeun Lee, Jooyoung Cheon, Soyun Hong, and Mido Chang. 2018. "A
Comparative Study to Identify Factors of Caregiver Burden between Baby Boomers
and Post Baby Boomers: A Secondary Analysis of a US Online Caregiver Survey." BMC
Public Health 18:579.
Koehly, Laura M. 2017. "It's Interpersonal: Family Relationships, Genetic Risk, and
Caregiving." The Gerontologist 57(1):32-39.
Kwak, Jung, and Larry J. Polivka. 2014. "The Future of Long-Term Care and the Aging
Network." Generations: Journal of the American Society on Aging 38(2):67-73.
Lock, Margaret. 2002. Twice Dead: Organ Transplants and the Reinvention of Death. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Lovell, Brian, and Mark A. Wetherell. 2011. "The Cost of Caregiving: Endocrine and
Immune Implications in Elderly and Non Elderly Caregivers." Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews 35(6):1342-1352.
Marwit, Samuel J., and Thomas M. Meuser. 2002. "Development and Initial Validation of an
Inventory to Assess Grief in Caregivers of Persons with Alzheimer's Disease." The
Gerontologist 42(6):751-765.
Mol, Annemarie. 2002. The Body Multiple: Ontology in Medical Practice. Durham: Duke
University Press.
95
Montgomery, Rhonda J. V., and Karl D. Kosloski. 2013. "Pathways to a Caregiver Identity
and Implications for Support Services." Pp. 131-156 in Caregiving across the Lifespan,
edited by R.C. Talley, and R.J.V. Montgomery. New York: Springer.
Moon, Heehyul, and Peggye Dilworth-Anderson. 2015. "Baby Boomer Caregiver and
Dementia Caregiving: Findings from the National Study of Caregiving." Age and Ageing
44:300-306.
Moradi, Bonnie, and Patrick R. Grzanka. 2017. "Using Intersectionality Responsibly:
Toward Critical Epistemology, Structural Analysis, and Social Justice Activism." Journal
of Counseling Psychology 64(5):500-513.
Morgan, Debra, Anthea Innes, and Julie Kosteniuk. 2011. "Dementia Care in Rural and
Remote Settings: A Systematic Review of Formal or Paid Care." Maturitas (68):17-33.
Musich, Shirley, Shaohung S. Wang, Sandra Kraemer, Kevin Hawkins, and Ellen Wicker.
2017. "Caregivers for Older Adults: Prevalence, Characteristics, and Health Care
Utilization and Expenditures." Geriatric Nursing 38:9-16.
NAC (National Alliance for Caregiving), and AARP Public Policy Institute. 2015.
"Caregiving in the U.S.", Accessed September 6, 2019.
https://www.caregiving.org/research/open-data/
National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, and World Health Organization. 2011. Global Health and Aging.
Washington, DC: National Institutes of Health.
Nichols, Linda O., Jennifer Martindale-Adams, Robert Burns, Marshall J. Graney, and
Jeffrey Zuber. 2011. "Typical and Atypical Dementia Family Caregivers: Systematic and
Objective Comparisons." The International Journal of Aging and Human Development
72(1):27-43.
Oliker, Stacey. 2011. "Sociology and Studies of Gender, Caregiving, and Inequality." Sociology
Compass 5(11):968-983.
Parker, Michael W., Jeffrey M. Bellis, Phillip Bishop, Mary Harper, Richard M. Allman,
Cynthia Moore, and Paula Thompson. 2002. "A Multidisciplinary Model of Health
Promotion Incorporating Spirituality into a Successful Aging Intervention with African
American and White Elderly Groups." The Gerontologist 42(3):406-415.
Pew Research Center. 2015. The Whys and Hows of Generations Research. Washington, DC: Pew
Research Center. Accessed 2019 August 8. http://www.people-
press.org/2015/09/03/the-whys-and-hows-of-generations-research/
Piercy, Kathleen W. 2007. "Characteristics of Strong Commitments to Intergenerational
Family Care of Older Adults." The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and
Social Sciences 62(6):S381-S387.
Pillemer, Karl, and J. J. Suitor. 2006. "Making Choices: A within-Family Study of Caregiver
Selection." The Gerontologist 46(4):439-448.
96
Pinquart, Martin, and S. Sörensen. 2003. "Differences between Caregivers and
Noncaregivers in Psychological Health and Physical Health." Psychology and Aging
18(2):250-267.
------. 2004. "Associations of Caregiver Stressors and Uplifts with Subjective Well-being and
Depressive Mood: A Meta-Analytic Comparison." Aging & Mental Health 8(5):438-449.
Raschick, Michael, and Berit Ingersoll-Dayton. 2004. "The Costs and Rewards of Caregiving
among Aging Spouses and Adult Children." Family Relations 53(3):317-325.
Redfoot, Donald, Lynn Feinberg, and Ari Houser. 2013. The Aging of the Baby Boom and the
Growing Care Gap: A Look at Future Declines in the Availability of Family Caregivers.
Washington, DC: AARP Public Policy Institute.
Remler, Dahlia K., and Gregg G. van Ryzin. 2011. Research Methods in Practice: Strategies for
Description and Causation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Richardson, Liana J., and Tyson H. Brown. 2016. "(En)Gendering Racial Disparities in
Health Trajectories: A Life Course and Intersectional Analysis." SSM - Population Health
2:425-435.
Richman, Laura S., and Alyssa N. Zucker. 2019. "Quantifying Intersectionality: An
Important Advancement for Health Inequality Research." Social Science & Medicine
226:246-248.
Rodriguez, Javier M. 2018. "Health Disparities, Politics, and the Maintenance of the Status
Quo: A New Theory of Inequality." Social Science & Medicine 200:36-43.
Roth, David L., William E. Haley, Martha Hovater, Martinique Perkins, Virginia G. Wadley,
and Suzanne Judd. 2013. "Family Caregiving and all-Cause Mortality: Findings from a
Population-Based Propensity-Matched Analysis." American Journal of Epidemiology
178(10):1571-1578.
Roth, David L., Lisa Fredman, and William E. Haley. 2015. "Informal Caregiving and its
Impact on Health: A Reappraisal from Population-Based Studies." The Gerontologist
55(2):309-319.
Ryan, Lindsay H., Jacqui Smith, Toni C. Antonucci, and James S. Jackson. 2012. "Cohort
Differences in the Availability of Informal Caregivers: Are the Boomers at Risk?" The
Gerontologist 52(2):177-188.
Savundranayagam, Marie Y., and Rhonda J. V. Montgomery. 2010. "Impact of Role
Discrepancies on Caregiver Burden among Spouses." Research on Aging 32(2):175-199.
Scales, Kezia, Arielle Altman, Stephen Campbell, Allison Cook, Angelina Del Rio Drake,
Robert Espinoza, and Jodi M. Sturgeon. 2020. It's Time to Care: A Detailed Profile of
America's Direct Care Workforce. Bronx: PHI.
Scales, Kezia, Jodi M. Sturgeon, Robert Espinoza, Stephen Campbell, and Allison Cook.
2019. Envisioning the Future of Home Care: Trends and Opportunities in Workforce Policy and
Practice. Bronx: PHI.
97
Schulz, Richard, and Jill Eden, a Committee on Family Caregiving for Older Adults, Board
on Health Care Services, Health and Medicine Division, and National Academies of
Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2016. Families Caring for an Aging America.
Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Scommegna, Paola. 2016. "Family Caregiving." Today's Research on Aging (33). Washington,
DC: Population Reference Bureau.
Scott, Nicholas A., and Janet Siltanen. 2017. "Intersectionality and Quantitative Methods:
Assessing Regression from a Feminist Perspective." International Journal of Social Research
Methodology 20(4):373-385.
Seaman, Aaron T. 2018. "The Consequence of "Doing Nothing": Family Caregiving for
Alzheimer's Disease as Non-Action in the U.S." Social Science & Medicine 197:63-70.
Seeher, Katrin, Lee-Fay Low, Simone Reppermund, and Henry Brodaty. 2013. "Predictors
and Outcomes for Caregivers of People with Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic
Literature Review." Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association 9:346-
355.
Seltzer, Marsha M., and Lydia W. Li. 1996. "The Transitions of Caregiving: Subjective and
Objective Definitions." The Gerontologist 36(5):614-626.
------. 2000. "The Dynamics of Caregiving: Transitions during a Three-Year Prospective
Study." The Gerontologist 40(2):165-178.
Shurgot, Gia R., and Bob G. Knight. 2005. "Influence of Neuroticism, Ethnicity, Familism,
and Social Support on Perceived Burden in Dementia Caregivers: Pilot Test of the
Transactional Stress and Social Support Model." The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological
Sciences 60(6):331.
Spector, Aimee, Martin Orrell, Astrid Schepers, and Niamh Shanahan. 2012. "A Systematic
Review of ‘Knowledge of Dementia’ Outcome Measures." Ageing Research Reviews
11(1):67-77.
Stites, Shana D., Jonathan D. Rubright, and Jason Karlawish. 2018. "What Features of
Stigma Do the Public Most Commonly Attribute to Alzheimer's Disease Dementia?
Results of a Survey of the U.S. General public." Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the
Alzheimer's Association 14(7):925-932.
Suitor, J. Jill, Megan Gilligan, and Karl Pillemer. 2016. "Stability, Change, and Complexity in
Later-Life Families." in Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences, edited by L. George and
K. Ferraro. Oxford: Elsevier Science & Technology.
Unson, Christine, Deborah Flynn, Mary A. Glendon, Elayne Haymes, and Diane Sancho.
2015. "Dementia and Caregiver Stress: An Application of the Reconceptualized
Uncertainty in Illness Theory." Issues in Mental Health Nursing 36(6):439-446.
98
Van Durme, Thérèse, Jean Macq, Caroline Jeanmart, and Micheline Gobert. 2012. "Tools
for Measuring the Impact of Informal Caregiving of the Elderly: A Literature Review."
International Journal of Nursing Studies 49(4):490-504.
Vitaliano, Peter P., Michael Murphy, Heather M. Young, Diana Echeverria, and Soo Borson.
2011. "Does Caring for a Spouse with Dementia Promote Cognitive Decline? A
Hypothesis and Proposed Mechanisms." Journal of the American Geriatrics Society
59(5):900-908.
Wang, Xiao R., Karen M. Robinson, and Lisa Carter-Harris. 2014. "Prevalence of Chronic
Illnesses and Characteristics of Chronically Ill Informal Caregivers of Persons with
Dementia." Age and Ageing 43:137-141.
Warner, David F., and Tyson H. Brown. 2011. "Understanding how Race/Ethnicity and
Gender Define Age-Trajectories of Disability: An Intersectionality Approach." Social
Science & Medicine 72(8):1236-1248.
Werner, Perla, Orit Karnieli-Miller, and Shmuel Eidelman. 2013. "Current Knowledge and
Future Directions about the Disclosure of Dementia: A Systematic Review of the First
Decade of the 21st Century." Alzheimer’s & Dementia 9:e74-e88.
West, Loraine A., Samantha Cole, Daniel Goodkind, and Wan He. 2014. 65+ in the United
States: 2010. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau.
Whitlatch, Carol J., and Silvia Orsulic-Jeras. 2018. "Meeting the Informational, Educational,
and Psychosocial Support Needs of Persons Living with Dementia and their Family
Caregivers." The Gerontologist 58:58.
Willert, Bria, and Krista L. Minnotte. 2019. "Informal Caregiving and Strains: Exploring the
Impacts of Gender, Race, and Income." Applied Research in Quality of Life (2019).
Accessed May 16, 2020. doi.org/10.1007/s11482-019-09786-1
Wright, Erik Olin 2005. Approaches to Class Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zhang, Jingjun, and Diana T. F. Lee. 2017. "Meaning in Stroke Family Caregiving: A
Literature Review." Geriatric Nursing 38(1):48-56.
99