The Routledge Handbook of Public Service Interpreting
The Routledge Handbook of Public Service Interpreting
The Routledge Handbook of Public Service Interpreting
Edited by
Laura Gavioli and Cecilia Wadensjö
Designed cover image: © Getty Images | CCeliaPhoto
First published 2023
by Routledge
4 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
605 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10158
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2023 selection and editorial matter, Laura Gavioli and Cecilia
Wadensjö; individual chapters, the contributors
The right of Laura Gavioli and Cecilia Wadensjö to be identified as the
authors of the editorial material, and of the authors for their individual
chapters, has been asserted in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the
Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced
or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other
means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and
recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without
permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks
or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and
explanation without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-P ublication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-P ublication Data
Names: Gavioli, Laura, editor. | Wadensjö, Cecilia, 1954– editor.
Title: The Routledge handbook of public service interpreting /
edited by Laura Gavioli, Cecilia Wadensjö.
Description: First edition. | Abingdon, Oxon;
New York, NY: Routledge, 2023. |
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2022029106 | ISBN 9780367278427 (hardback) |
ISBN 9781032391151 (paperback) | ISBN 9780429298202 (ebook)
Subjects: LCSH: Public service interpreting. | LCGFT: Essays.
Classification: LCC P306.947 .R68 2022 | DDC 418/.02—dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2022029106
ISBN: 978-0 -367-27842-7 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-1-032-39115-1 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-0 -429-29820-2 (ebk)
DOI: 10.4324/9780429298202
Typeset in Bembo
by codeMantra
CONTENTS
Introduction 1
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
PART 1
Theoretical and methodological approaches 15
v
Contents
PART 2
Exploring PSI settings 123
PART 3
Training and professionalization 259
vi
Contents
18 Role play as a means of training and testing public service interpreting 292
Magnus Dahnberg
Index 429
vii
CONTRIBUTORS
Sabine Braun is a Director of the Centre for Translation Studies at the University of Surrey,
and a Co-Director of Surrey’s Institute for People-Centred Artificial Intelligence. Her re-
search explores human-machine interaction and integration in translation and interpreting,
especially to improve access to critical information, media content and vital public services.
Jeremy L. Brunson holds a PhD in Sociology. He is the Executive Director of the Divi-
sion of Equity, Diversity and Inclusion at Gallaudet University. His published work includes
analyses of video relay service, the academic field of Interpreting Studies, the invisible labour
of deaf people and legal interpreting.
viii
Contributors
Magnus Dahnberg, PhD, is a Senior Lecturer and Director of the Institute for Interpret-
ing and Translation Studies (TÖI), Stockholm University. He defended his thesis in 2015
on interpreter-mediated conversations as role play. He is the former head of Swedish Armed
Forces Interpreter School, a Swedish-Russian interpreter and translator.
Emilie Jouin is a linguistics engineer at the CNRS and works at the ICAR laboratory. She
collaborates in studies on the analysis of natural interactions. She is specialized in legal and
ethical issues about the collection and processing of audiovisual data.
Mira Kadrić is a Professor of Interpreting Studies and Didactics of Translation at the Uni-
versity of Vienna. She obtained degrees in Translation and Interpreting, PhD in Interpreting
and Law and Habilitation in Interpreting Studies and Didactics of Translation. Her research
focuses especially on empirical work on legal, political and diplomatic interpreting.
Demi Krystallidou is a Senior Lecturer in Translation Studies at the Centre for Translation
Studies, University of Surrey, U K. Her research focuses on linguistically and culturally
ix
Contributors
mediated healthcare communication in primary, secondary care and mental healthcare, and
on interprofessional education in Health Sciences and Translation Studies.
Ian Mason is a Professor Emeritus at Heriot-Watt University. In his long career, he taught
translating, translation and interpreting studies. Recent work draws attention to a serious
mismatch between public expectations of interpreters’ performance and attested interpreter
behaviour. Current research focuses on matters of community, identity and communication
rights in interpreted encounters on n on-verbal communication and on reader response to
translations.
Raffaela Merlini is a Senior Lecturer in English language and Translation in the Depart-
ment of Law, Economics, Politics and Modern Languages at LUMSA University in Rome,
Italy. She has published in the field of Dialogue Interpreting, principally on the interactional
and socio-psychological dynamics of f ace-to-face interpreter-mediated talk in a variety of
institutional contexts.
Charlotta Plejert is an Associate Professor of Speech and Language Sciences. Her research
interests cover atypical interaction, often with a focus on multilingualism. She has co-edited
several volumes, for example Multilingualism and Ageing (De Bot, Plejert & Gram Simonsen,
2020) and Multilingual Interaction and Dementia (Plejert, Lindholm & Schrauf, 2017).
Sonja Pöllabauer holds a position as Professor for Interpreting Studies at the Centre
for Translation Studies, University of Vienna. She has been involved in projects on in-
terpreting in asylum procedures, healthcare interpreting and interpreter-mediated com-
munication in institutional settings, as well as the organization of training courses for lay
interpreters.
x
Contributors
Christopher A. Stone is a reader (a ssociate professor) in Interpreting and Translation at the
University of Wolverhampton, UK. His research interests include multimodal interpreter-
mediated interactions, in situ or via broadcast media. He maintains an active interpreting
practice and at the time of writing is the president of the World Association of Sign Language
Interpreters ( WASLI).
Anna Claudia Ticca is a Researcher in Linguistics. Her main interest is the study of v ideo-
recorded social interactions in multilingual contexts, in which interpreters may participate.
Her research is also dedicated to identifying the interactional skills of professionals and rein-
vesting the results into vocational training in education, health and interpreting.
xi
Contributors
work settings; more recent work focuses on the development of interpreting provisions in
m id-late 20th-century Britain, particularly in the voluntary sector.
xii
INTRODUCTION
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
Modern linguistics embraces the idea that human sense making of words and other commu-
nicative resources is a social, dynamic activity. Inspired by the literary scholar and philos-
opher Michail Bachtin (1986), among others, language and communication researchers are
increasingly applying a dialogical view of language and mind (see, e.g. Linell 2009). Dialogic
theory is also the basis for several studies of interpreter-mediated interaction. Applying a
dialogical view of language and mind means, among other things, that interpreters are per-
ceived of as active participants, with their own agency, rather than as passive instruments.
Also, the everyday perception of interpreting of spontaneous talk in interaction as a simple
transfer of clear-cut messages in one language to equally unambiguous messages in an-
other language must be dismissed. A significant number of studies show that spontaneous
interpreter-mediated interaction, spoken and/or signed, are complex events, both linguisti-
cally and socially. For instance, participants may produce talk not necessarily to add content,
but also to organize the very talking. Nevertheless, in a certain sense, interpreting can be
described as a ‘monologising practice in a dialogically organized world’ (Wadensjö 2004) in
that, when interpreters perform consecutively, they tend to treat participants’ talk chunk by
chunk, regardless of whether they have been allocated the turn or take it on their own initia-
tive. When an utterance is rendered in another language, if not checked for ambiguities, it is
thus treated as a more or less unambiguous chunk of talk. Doing so, interpreters are expected
to be observant of linguistic details and of how talk fits in with the on-going exchange, an
expectation which involves interpreters’ familiarity with the larger, institutional and cultural
contexts.
Within interpreting studies, indeed all authors in this volume, perceive of interpreters as
active participants with their own agency. Yet, it can be argued, in line with, for example,
Inghilleri (2012) and Määttä (2015), that also researchers who do not see interpreters as
translation machines, but assume that they are active, sense-making participants with their
own agency, paradoxically still tend to cement the simple conduit metaphor in the view of
both communication and interpreters’ work, by their exclusive focus on interpreters’ choice
of words and expressions in the other language, that is, looking at (a nd evaluating) interpret-
ing exclusively on the basis of the ‘monologizing’ component of their activity, as one would
look at source texts in relation to target texts.
DOI: 10.4324/9780429298202-1 1
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
2
Introduction
the court should consider the interpreter as a communication promoter and point to the need
to build mutual respect for how representatives of each profession exercise their professional
discretion, that is, how the actors take decisions founded in w ell-established professional
knowledge and professional ethics (Laster and Taylor 1994: 126). This argument, published
already in the 1990s, is no less valid today. In this volume, interpreters’ mandate and obliga-
tions in terms of professional discretion are discussed by Norwegian scholar Hanne Skaaden
(Chapter 16).
Definitions of ‘interpreting’
Research on interpreting can roughly be divided into studies focusing on the interpreting
individual and/or this individual’s cognitive processes on the one hand, and, on the other
hand, on interpreting as linguistic, communicative and social interaction. These two cat-
egories of studies are characterized by their different ways of delimiting interpreting as a
research object. In the former case, a definition of interpreting similar to that proposed by
Franz Pöchhacker is applied:
3
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
interpreting as interaction implies that all interpreter utterances are seen as having a poten-
tial impact on the content and the progression of an interpreter-mediated conversation. The
notion of a communicative pas de trois (Wadensjö 1998: 12), a communicative dance for three,
if you will, is to express that a triadic, bilingual, interpreter-mediated encounter has specific
communicative conditions that are worthwhile exploring systematically in their own right.
In spontaneous spoken interaction, the meaning that participants attribute to what is said
and done is instantly co-created as interaction unfolds. In interpreter-mediated conversations,
the interpreter is deeply involved in this sense making. The content and the progression of
talk will depend on how and when the interpreter renders in a new language what has been
said in the other. In a social encounter where the interpreter interprets consecutively, most
of the interpreter’s renditions will function as implicit coordinating moves (Wadensjö 1998: 109).
The interpreter’s talk is a prerequisite without which the monolingual participants will have
trouble going on with their exchange. The better they adapt to this specific communicative
situation, by waiting for their turns and making room for the interpreter’s renditions, the
easier the interpreter’s task will be. By speaking in parallel, by addressing the interpreter or
someone else present for side comments, by speaking very fast, indistinctly or for a very long
time, they may elicit explicit coordinating moves (Wadensjö 1998: 109) from the interpreter,
such as asking for repetition, for clarification, for time to interpret or time to s elf-correct, or
advising participants to respect each other’s right to be updated. Obviously, the more compe-
tent an interpreter is in terms of vocabulary and contextual knowledge, interpreting and co-
ordinating ability, and linguistic fluency in both languages, the better the conditions will be
for the monolingual parties to participate in the interpreted conversation, provided though,
they are focused on being each other’s interlocutors. Clearly, talking through an interpreter
is not an easy task and, for some, it may be unusual or even feel odd. So, explicit coordinat-
ing moves may sometimes be ‘the way’ in which the interlocutors learn how to participate
in interpreted interaction. Thus, learning how to use explicit coordination smoothly and
collaboratively may be an important issue in interpreters’ professionalization.
Defining interpreting as interaction opens up for studies not just of participants’ speech
production but also of their sense making based on other communicative resources (gestures,
body orientation, gaze direction, handling of artefacts and more). From this broader defini-
tion of interpreting also follows that interpreters’ p rofession-specific task in conversational
situations is perceived of as t wo-fold – to render others’ talk in a new language and, to facil-
itate the m
icro-organization of participants’ turns at talk, which, in turn, enables translation
to take place. In conclusion, while interpreting definitely includes translation activity, the
risk in defining interpreting as ‘a type of Translation’ is that of narrowing the researcher’s
view specifically to focus on words spoken as a relation between source texts and target
texts, while defining interpreting as interaction broadens the possible constituent parts of
‘interpreting’ and allows for looking at interpreters’ and primary parties’ agency in the light
of the institutional structures and frameworks within which interpreters operate, and at the
specific conditions for communication inherent in triadic or multiparty, bilingual and me-
diated encounters.
4
Introduction
situations. Collections of recorded encounters, then transcribed in detail for analysis, have
provided evidence of talk organization in interpreter-mediated settings and of interpreters’
practices in rendering and coordinating talk in interaction. Other studies like Tate and
Turner (1997/2002) or Leanza (2005; see Böser 2016 for a review) have instead collected data
through surveys or interviews with the participants in the interactions, both interpreters and
service providers, both individually and in focus groups. The latter data accounted for the
perceptions of the interlocutors about the interpreter-mediated encounters, based on their
expectations and experiences. The chapters in this volume refer, in various ways, to both
types of empirical research.
Empirical research, either showing interpreter-mediated interactional practices or high-
lighting the participants’ perceptions, has unveiled issues which make up for the debate in
public service interpreting nowadays. We deem four of them are particularly worth men-
tioning. The first is related to the features of talk in interaction, made evident in analyses
of authentic encounters. Studies on conversation, inspired by the work of sociologist Erv-
ing Goffman and then re-organized into an analytical methodology (Sacks, Schegloff and
Jefferson 1974), with a focus on institutional interaction (Drew and Heritage 1992), have
shown that no talk can be produced without the interlocutors reacting to each other’s con-
tributions and making sense of them. In conversation, even small interactional signals like
“m hm” or eye gaze, or even silence, contribute to the c o-construction of the interaction (see
Gardner 2001). These items may allow for the other interlocutor to go on or start speaking
or may indicate that they are expected to speak to someone else than the interlocutor who
is keeping silent. The fact that, in talk, even silence is a form of participation created more
than a challenge to a view of interpreting in which one of the interlocutors, the interpreter,
needs to interfere as little as possible in communication. In fact, interpreters do contribute
in talk sometimes with ‘co-constructive’ contributions like providing feedback to allow for
the interlocutors to actually produce talk. This is for instance the case in healthcare inter-
action where the interpreters help hesitating patients to go on talking (Leanza, Isabelle and
Rosenberg 2013; Theys et al. 2020) or in asylum seeking interactions where the interpreters
collaborate with the applicants’ narration development, while still allowing themselves space
to render (Pöchhacker 2012: 64–66; Pöllabauer, this volume and also Wadensjö, Rehnberg
and Nikolaidou 2022). How to precisely handle the features of talk, and participation in talk,
in the profession and in the training of professionals is still a matter of debate (see Ticca et al.
this volume, Niemants et al. this volume).
The second issue foregrounding empirical research concerns the subjects that are ob-
served, the interpreters in particular. The following are some questions pointing to the prob-
lems raised. If an interpreter, in the interaction, provides mhm and allows for continuation of
a participant’s turn, co-constructing an extended one, is it evidence of a feature of talk or of
the interpreter’s lack of professionalism? Is mhm the talk equivalent of note taking in speech
consecutive interpreting or a manifestation that the interpreter has lost talk coordination?
Focus groups and interviews show interpreters’ recurrent complaints that institutional pro-
viders and service seekers do not appropriately relate to the interpreters’ work, expecting
machine behaviour on the one hand or personal protection and even advocacy on the other.
Is this an expression of the interpreters’ n on-expertise? Of their inefficiency in coordinating
talk? Does it suggest the necessity of familiarizing interpreting service users with what it
means to talk through an interpreter? These questions have to do with the actual reliabil-
ity of the professionals observed in empirical studies. So, while on the one hand empirical
studies provide an amount of information about the interpreter-mediated encounter, infor-
mation may be skewed by the quality of informants. The quality of informants is, however,
5
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
‘ informative’ in sè, both in relation to the interpreting professionals available (a s well as the
professional service providers) and in the type of interaction they construe. The chapters in
this volume show the relevance of inquiries based on informants, but the problem of which
informants is likewise addressed (most explicitly in Chapter 16 by Hanne Skaaden).
The third issue raised by empirical research has to do with the specificities of the services
involved. While engagement in talk with one of the participants has been found relevant in
healthcare interpreting to optimise talk, putting the patients at ease (Penn and Watermeyer
2012) or encouraging them to talk about their problems (Gavioli 2012: 213–14; see also
Angelelli 2004), in other types of settings, such engagement may not work in the same way.
In immigration procedures, for instance, direct answers of interpreters to the service provid-
ers, though ‘quicker’ and possibly efficient in terms of talk exchange, reduce the opportunity
for the immigration applicants to show themselves as capable and competent, an opportunity
that is fundamental in this type of encounter (Mason 2009: 62). So, as in monolingual talk,
in interpreted talk different practices come at stake in different settings and may account for
the situated effectiveness of the interpreter’s work. The characteristics of the main settings
where PSI occurs, dealt with in section 2 of this volume, give a clear idea of the complexity
of public service interpreting work and account for the necessity that interpreters are able to
interpret the situation, together with (and ‘in’) the utterances that make it up, and are prepared
to exercise discretion in accommodating their interpreting practices to the situated activity.
The fourth issue highlighted by empirical data is the human intensity of the situations
involved in PSI. Illness, poverty, lack of freedom, rape, murder and other types of violence
are often the object of healthcare, legal and support services offered to foreign residents. Such
situations pose, more than others, the problem of empathic involvement of the interpreter
as a person as well as the problem of the treatment of empathy in talk (how to show its rele-
vance, how to render it). The problem is d ouble-sided, calling for the management of both
situations of potential sympathy for those who are perceived as the victims, and repulsion,
for those who are in horrible faults, like violent people and murderers (see Gustafsson this
volume). In these cases, it may not be easy for interpreters to discern what is the best possible
service that they can provide.
The chapters in the volume tackle the four issues summarized above by providing con-
crete examples as well as reflections helping interpreters and providers to grasp the nuances
and responsibilities concerning their participation in situations that may involve various
kinds of sensitivity and challenges. They provide more knowledge about the diverse situa-
tions and suggestions about how to provide effective service.
6
Introduction
these ad hoc solutions have been underlined in the literature (e.g. by Pöchhacker and Kadriç
1999), and possible ways of making them fruitful have been discussed (Bührig and Meyer
2004; Jansson, Wadensjö and Plejert 2017). What ad hoc interpreting has highlighted first
and foremost, however, is an unequivocal necessity for interpreting services’ availability in
public service encounters.
The second reason is related to the participation, in service encounters, of an interloc-
utor, the interpreter, specifically working on communication. The difference in language
is not the only difference highlighted in interpreter-mediated public service encounters, a
difference in the type of knowledge possessed by service providers and seekers is very much
at stake too. The work of interpreters (see, e.g. Raymond 2014) has highlighted the amount
of competence needed in building shared knowledge, by making clear or explicit those items
that may not mean much to one of the interlocutors (see also Mason 2006). While such a
difference in knowledge is well known from studies in monolingual interaction, interpreted
interaction as well as narratives of interpreters’ experiences give clear illustrations of (a) cases
in which the differences may be more relevant and crucial, and (b) ways in which they can be
treated. These cases show the functioning of asymmetric communication and may improve
communication in public services, not only when foreign speakers are involved, but for all
service seekers.
The third and possibly less obvious reason why interpreting is a powerful resource for
public services is that interpreters’ experience provides an incredibly rich amount of infor-
mation about what such services are in fact. A number of studies in this volume provide nar-
ratives showing that services may still be inadequate and unprepared to work in a changed
environment where service-seekers are no longer monolingual and autochthonous and sug-
gest possible improvements in the regulations or in the training of service providers. This
point too, which is well argued in Chapter 2 by Kristina Gustafsson, may provide benefits
not only for PSI, but for public services more in general.
Despite these strong points which make PSI an undoubtable resource, some studies, par-
ticularly in healthcare settings, have found that interpreters may negatively interfere in the
dynamics of public service encounters (Davidson 2000; Hsieh 2007; see Gavioli and Merlini
this volume). This poses again the problem of staff adequate preparation and the search for
effective ways of training both interpreting and service providers. In this volume, we have
dedicated an entire section, the third one to the problems associated with the training of the
personnel involved in PSI. These problems are related to poor knowledge of the PSI situa-
tions, including little or no knowledge of how interaction works, the languages involved and
the necessity to include also practical training, how to involve service providers, how to train
those who train interpreters and service providers.
7
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
Indeed, as will be shown in the chapters in this volume, in an asymmetric type of interac-
tion as we have in public service encounters, little or no familiarity with the services combined
with little or no knowledge of the language used make interpreted PS interactions even more
asymmetric, leading to argue that this, sometimes, huge imbalance needs to be addressed in
interpreting to allow for communication to occur (Mason and Ren 2012). As explained by
Claudio Baraldi in Chapter 3, the concept of mediation comes from studies in monolingual
conflict management, and, as such, conflict may easily be intended as an idea inherent to me-
diation, even in reference to interpreted mediation. Since huge asymmetries may provoke con-
flict, then mediation in PSI may involve dealing with potential or emerging ‘cultural’ conflicts.
While, as this volume shows, there is no doubt that PSI occurs in situations of strong
asymmetry and with vulnerable participants, attributing asymmetry and vulnerability to
cultural differences may have several drawbacks. First, using culture as an explanatory tool
for obstacles in communication may result in ‘othering’ minority patients, thus hiding rather
than highlighting communication problems (Felberg and Skaaden 2012). Second, interpret-
ers’ attempts to explain what may be perceived as unusual participants’ behaviour in terms
of different habits, traditions or values may in fact result in the production of stereotypes
(Barbieri 2009). Third, mediating ‘cultures’ by attributing individuals to cultural groups
deprives these individuals of the opportunity of participating in the interaction ‘as per-
sons’, with personal expression being interpreted (a nd possibly misinterpreted) in the light of
‘g roup features’ (Baraldi 2012: 323).
While mediation of ‘cultures’, whatever it means, may be one way to look at mediation
in PSI, restricting the concept to cultures has clear limitations. Studies observing interpreted
interactions (for instance Wadensjö 1998; Angelelli 2004, 2012; Penn and Watermeyer 2012)
have suggested that interpreting work can enhance both understanding and positive rela-
tionships through interactional practices. Possibly the strongest theoretical explanation of
mediation as occurring through interpreting work is provided by Wadensjö’s concept of co-
ordination (1998: 105), which we discussed above. In their coordinating activity, interpreters
are active agents who influence and regulate communication, generating a common focus
and sustaining the definition of encounters.
Coordination occurs through the selection of rendition forms as well as other interpret-
ers’ contributions, including forms of intercultural mediation: original utterances’ intended/
possible meaning is negotiated interactionally and the renditions to follow are designed as to
allow participants share understanding and rapport.
Shared understanding and rapport does clearly not necessarily mean that the interlocutors
are empathic with each other or that they can accommodate with each other perspectives.
Quite the contrary, as shown in the volume, problematic and even conflictual situations
occur in PSI and there may be cases where ambiguity and deception are involved too. Inter-
action, even conflictual interaction, however, occurs with at least two – in the case of in-
terpreted talk at least three – participants ‘participating’ – even to construct ambiguities,
deception and conflict. Even though the latter situations pose tough constraints on interpret-
ing and possibly the necessity of pointing to the existence of ambiguity (or deception) quite
explicitly, t alk-coordination, we believe, is the type of mediation activity mostly at stake in
interpreter-mediated work.
8
Introduction
interpreting in the settings which have been most in need of PSI services; the third provides
reflections and suggestions on interpreter as well as provider training, with an aim to im-
prove PSI services. Below, we provide a sketch of the main issues dealt with in each chapter,
in the order given in the table of contents. The threads connecting the chapters are however
many more than those that can be highlighted in this introduction: a section called ‘Related
topics’ at the end of each chapter guides the reader to explore links and connections among
the topics and problems dealt with in the contributions.
The opening chapter by Carmen Valero-Garcés offers an overview of the main challenges
characterising the field of Public Service Interpreting, most notably, the great variety of lan-
guages to cope with (together with a lack of interpreters for many of these languages), the
asymmetric relationships involved in situations where health, freedom and other fragilities
are at stake, and the effort of the public sector in providing adequate (or sometimes inade-
quate) services in these situations. The main controversies are discussed regarding interpret-
ers’ ethics and participation in the development of PSI as a profession.
The following three chapters deal with qualitative research perspectives and discuss, from
different angles, ways in which interpreters’ participation in public service encounters gives
evidence of and may contribute to social change. Kristina Gustafsson in C hapter 2 introduces
an ethnographic approach to the study of PSI. On the basis of interviews with interpreters
working in public settings, she shows unexpected features of building dialogic relationships
guaranteeing equal access and representation in the interaction. Besides illustrating an ap-
plication of the ethnographic approach to the study of PSI, Gustafsson suggests that the per-
spective of interpreters as well as the narration of their experiences may be a rich source of
information not only about the provision of interpreting service, but also, and most notably,
about the provision of public service to minorities, showing aspects that would otherwise get
overlooked. In Chapter 3, Claudio Baraldi discusses sociological approaches to the notion
of agency suggesting that agency does not cover any action by participants in the interaction,
but those which create visible social change. While this perspective on agency may be chal-
lenging for interpreters’ participation, in that interpreters have the task to guarantee that the
other participants participate first and foremost and in their own will, Baraldi shows examples
from authentic interpreter-mediated interaction where mediators are given the opportunity
and take the chance to make a difference, by promoting migrant women’s inclusion and
providers’ attention for their health and psychological conditions. In Chapter 4, Ian Mason’s
contribution discusses notions foregrounding a pragmatic view of PSI research. Starting
from the ideas of identity, position and power, Mason first highlights a distinction between
institutional and interactive power and then looks at how interactive power may affect in-
stitutional power through conversational uptake, reinforcing weak participants’ voices or,
alternatively, the power of institutional representatives. Mason’s reflection offers a compre-
hensive outlook on the complex relationship between language and context and shows that
notions like positioning, cultural assumptions and power are dynamic ones. He suggests that
the intersections among these notions account for the construction and rendition of meaning
in context, thus moving towards what may be called a social pragmatics of interpreting.
An increasing interest of PSI studies for data, like transcripts of interpreted interactions, has
brought to the creation of collections that can be stored and classified as to become shared re-
search materials. Thus corpus-based methods of archiving, categorizing and interrogating the
data are now finding their way into interpreting studies in general, and PSI studies in particu-
lar. This expanding field of research is presented and discussed by Bernd Meyer in Chapter 5,
together with an illustration of one of the few, possibly the only publicly available corpus of
PSI today, the Community Interpreting Database (A ngermeyer, Meyer and Schmidt 2012).
9
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
The two chapters concluding part 1 reflect on the integration of different media in public
service interpreting and translation. First, the use of technology to assist, complement and/or
replace human interpreters has started to expand in the PSI area, with an increased demand
for distant communication to both cope with the problem of finding suitable interpreting
services when needed and to deal with isolation requirements, which not least the Covid-19
pandemics brought to the fore. In C hapter 6, Sabine Braun, Khetam Al Sharou and Özlem
Temizöz discuss the main types of technologies used in interpreting interaction in healthcare
and focus on the ways in which technology re-shapes the interaction as well as the connected
challenges for interpreters and service providers. In the seventh and last chapter of part 1,
Mustapha Taibi discusses the issues involved in translating written documents for the public
service. While the medium, written rather than spoken language, allows for more consulta-
tion with the public service stakeholders, the issue of accessibility is one of fundamental im-
portance in public service translation, requiring a strong commitment of translators to orient
to the readers’ expectations and knowledge as well as the situation in which these documents
need to be read and understood. A possibly extreme example given by Taibi, still offering a
clear idea of what ‘situated’ public service translation may mean, is that of crisis scenarios, in
which full and clear information may help reduce the loss of lives.
Moving to part 2, the first three chapters deal with PSI in legal settings, face-to-face and
remote. Philipp Angermeyer in C hapter 8 highlights general as well as particular contextual
issues affecting interpreting in court, for instance distinguishing between interpreting in
cross-examination or inquisitorial proceedings, in which questions have different purposes
and targets. The chapter also discusses the contribution of studies from different disciplines,
like linguistics, anthropology, sociology and law and addresses the crucial issue of personal
deixis in situated court interpreting activities. In Chapter 9 Sonja Pöllabauer outlines the de-
velopment of the subfield ‘interpreter-mediated asylum interviews’, providing an overview
of recurrent and salient issues on research exploring authentic discourse data. One of these
issues is dealt with at length in Chapter 10 by Christian Licoppe. Drawing on video record-
ings of naturally occurring courtroom proceedings concerning asylum cases, this chapter
demonstrates how the introduction of video links in courtrooms can affect the conditions
for interpreters’ work, and also for the production of other participants’ questions and nar-
ratives. Using ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis (EM/CA), Licoppe generates
new knowledge about the impact of participants’ location in relation to each other, and about
the interdependence between actors’ communicative projects. At the same time, he demon-
strates the explanatory power of EM/CA as a theoretical and methodological approach to
studies of consecutively interpreted interaction. A very particular type of legal setting is that
of v ictim-survivors of domestic violence and abuse, dealt with in C hapter 11 by Rebecca
Tipton. Besides showing cases of testimony of women’s experiences of domestic violence, the
chapter offers a more general reflection on the idea of personal and contextual vulnerability.
The other major traditional area in which PSI services are most needed, besides court
and other legal settings, is health care. Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini in Chapter 12
discuss clinician-patient interpreting on the basis of two apparently divergent purposes, that
of providing appropriate medical therapy and that of giving patients care, attention and reas-
surance. Following studies on healthcare in monolingual contexts, eliciting patients’ stories
of experience, worries and fear is helpful for the clinicians to provide adequate cures and it
is thus part of the task of interpreting to consider this double goal in medical encounters.
Still in the medical area, C hapter 13 by Charlotta Plejert deals with the specific situation of
mental health. Here, patients’ tests include work based on repeating sounds, naming familiar
or unfamiliar objects, recognising situations. While such tests may not be easy for patients
10
Introduction
11
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
in adjusting their contributions (for instance in self-repair and in asking for clarification or
repeat). It is not likewise easy to establish the relationship between interpreters’ memory
load to their coordinating competence or to various types of contributions from the other
participants, to participants’ knowledge of institutional procedures, mutual expectations and
so forth. The chapter gives food for thought for reflections on such a link/connection and
suggests that exercises on monitoring cognitive load are needed in the training of interpret-
ers for PSI.
Covid-19 accelerated the development of online education worldwide, even if the phe-
nomenon is far from new. In C hapter 20, Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden provide an overview
of research on what has been called blended learning, that is, combination of online and
on-campus education, as this has developed in various disciplines and educational programs.
Subsequently, the authors account for the blended learning model that has been established
in the BA programme for public service interpreters at Oslo Metropolitan University. The
chapter shows what learning aims can be acquired online and for what aims on-campus ac-
tivities seem more appropriate. Also, the authors emphasize that didactics that stimulate stu-
dent interactivity is essential in creating opportunity for learning, whether on-site or online.
The two following chapters focus on the use of recordings and transcripts of authen-
tic mediated encounters in the training of interpreters. Natacha Niemants,
interpreter-
Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe in Chapter 21 explore the use of the so-called
Conversation Analytic Role-Play Method (CARM), originally developed to train for dispute
mediation, in the training of interpreters. While this method, as other types of role-plays,
involves simulation, it has the advantage of showing interpreting problems which really took
place, selecting them over a range of authentic materials and asking trainees to discuss how
the problem might be solved in the specific situation. Similar to what occurs in conversa-
tion analysis, in which recordings and transcripts allow researchers to repeat the event in a
sort of ‘slow-motion’ mode which makes the event analysable, in CARM training, trainees
can deal with the interpreting problem ‘in slow-motion’, discussing possible renditions and
their consequences. C hapter 22 by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véronique Traverso and Emilie
Jouin provides a recounting of the REMILAS (Refugees, Migrants and their Languages
in healthcare services) research project. The project examines communication and mutual
understanding in multilingual health, mental and social care consultations, thus linking up
to other contributions in the volume both dealing with asylum seeking and healthcare. The
focus of the chapter is on the development of a training program used to train both interpret-
ers and providers and based on natural interpreter-mediated talk. Besides providing more
suggestions about how to use authentic data in training, the chapter shows a completely new
program based on self-learning modality and accessible via digital instruments.
Chapter 23 aims at introducing a specific kind of interprofessional education (IPE) as a
teaching and learning model in the field of PSI. Demi Krystallidou takes education in the
healthcare s ector – where IPE was first d eveloped – as a case in point and shares her experi-
ences and critical reflections concerning the use of IPE in h ands-on training sessions, where
PSI students and medical students learn to collaborate in practice. In Chapter 24, Tatjana R.
Felberg and Gry Sagli share their experiences of training public service providers represent-
ing different institutions in how to communicate via interpreters. The authors argue for the
importance of such training and for making it easily available for various groups of public
service providers. The final chapter in the handbook is devoted to education and training of
public service interpreter teachers. In C hapter 25 Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer outline
research on the education of teachers for dialogue interpreting, with a specific focus on PSI,
without differentiating between signed and spoken language interpreting or any particular
12
Introduction
institutional setting. The chapter discusses methodological and didactic approaches to teach-
ing and learning which implies that the issues, knowledge and skills brought up can be ap-
plicable not just to teachers but in a wider field of interpreting.
References
Angelelli, Claudia (2004) Medical Interpreting and Cross-cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
——— (2012) “Challenges in interpreters’ coordination of the construction of pain”, in Coordinating
Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam, Benja-
mins: 251–68.
Angermeyer, Philipp S., Bernd Meyer, and Thomas Schmidt (2012) “Sharing community interpreting
corpora: A pilot study”, in Multilingual Corpora and Multilingual Corpus Analysis, Thomas Schmidt
and Kai Wörner (eds). Hamburg Studies in Multilingualism 14, Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 2 75–94.
Bachtin, Mikhail (1986) Speech Genres and Other Late Essays, Caryl Emerson and Michael Holquist
(eds), trans. Vern W. McGee. Austin, University of Texas Press.
Baraldi, Claudio (2012) “Interpreting as dialogic mediation: The relevance of expansions”, in Coor-
dinating Participation in Dialogue Iinterpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam,
Benjamins: 297–326.
Barbieri, Viola (2009) “La traduzione della diversità culturale nell’interazione mediata”, in La Media-
zione L inguistico-Culturale: una prospettiva interazionista, Laura Gavioli (ed). Perugia, Guerra: 203–30.
Böser, Ursula (2016) “Interviews and focus groups”, in Researching Translation and Interpreting, Claudia
Angelelli and Brian James Baer (eds). London/New York, Routledge: 236–46.
Bührig, Kristin and Bernd Meyer (2004) “Ad hoc interpreting and achievement of communicative
purposes in briefings for informed consent”, in Multilingual Communication, Juliane House and
Jochen Rehbein (eds). Amsterdam, Benjamins: 43–61.
Davidson, Brad (2000) “The interpreter as institutional gatekeeper: The social-linguistic role of inter-
preters in Spanish–English medical discourse”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (3): 3 79–405.
Drew, Paul, and John Heritage (1992) Talk at Work. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Felberg, Tatjana R., and Hanne Skaaden (2012) “The (de)construction of culture in interpreter-
mediated medical discourse”, Linguistica Antverpiensia, New Series. Themes in Translation Studies 11:
67–84.
Gardner, Rod (2001) When Listeners Talk: Response Tokens and Listener Stance. Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
Gavioli, Laura (2012) “M inimal responses in interpreter-mediated medical talk”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam, Benjamins:
201‒28.
Goffman, Erving (1981) Forms of Talk. Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania Press.
Hsieh, Elaine (2007) “Interpreters as co-diagnosticians: Overlapping roles and services between pro-
viders and interpreters”, Social Science & Medicine 64: 924–37.
Hsieh, Elaine, and Brenda Nicodemus (2015) “Conceptualizing emotion in healthcare interpreting: A
normative approach to interpreters’ emotion work”, Patient Education and Counseling 98: 1474–81.
Inghilleri, Moira (2012) Interpreting Justice: Ethics, Politics, and Language. New York/L ondon, Routledge.
Jansson, Gunilla, Cecilia Wadensjö, and Charlotta Plejert (2017). “Managing complaints in multi-
lingual care encounters”, Multilingua 36 (3): 3 13–46. (Published o n-l ine 28.03.17) Open Access.
https://doi.org/10.1515/multi-2016– 0 0043
Laster, Kathy, and Veronica L. Taylor (1994) Interpreters and the Legal System. Sydney, The Federation Press.
Leanza, Yvan (2005) “Roles of community interpreters in paediatrics as seen by interpreters, physi-
cians and researchers”, Interpreting 7 (2): 67–192.
Leanza, Yvan, Boivin Isabelle, and Ellen Rosenberg (2013) “The patient’s Lifeworld: Building mean-
ingful clinical encounters between patients, physicians and interpreters”, Communication & Medicine
10: 13–25.
Linell, Per (2009) Rethinking Language, Mind and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories
of Human Sense-Making. Charlotte, NC, Information Age Publishing.
Määttä, Simo (2015) “Interpreting the discourse of reporting: The case of screening interviews with
asylum seekers and police interviews in Finland”, Translation & Interpreting 7 (3): 21–35. http://d x.
doi.org/10.12807/t i.107203.2015.a02
13
Cecilia Wadensjö and Laura Gavioli
Mason, Ian (2006) “On mutual accessibility of contextual assumptions in dialogue interpreting”, Journal
of Pragmatics 38: 359–73.
——— (2009) “Role, positioning and discourse in f ace-to-f ace interpreting”, in Interpreting and Trans-
lating in Public Service Settings, Raquel De Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle Perez, and Christine Wilson (eds).
Manchester, St. Jerome: 5 2–73.
Mason, Ian and Wen Ren (2012) “Power in face-to-face interpreting events”, Translation and Interpret-
ing Studies 7 (2): 233‒52.
Merlini, Raffaela (2009a) “Seeking asylum and seeking identity in a mediated encounter: The projection
of selves through discursive practices”, Interpreting 11 (1): 57–92.
Penn, Claire, and Jennifer Watermeyer (2012) “W hen asides become central: Small talk and big talk
in interpreted health interactions”, Patient Education and Counseling 88: 391–98.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2012) “Interpreting participation: Conceptual analysis and illustration of the in-
terpreter’s role in interaction”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi
and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 45–70.
——— (2016) Introducing Interpreting Studies. 2nd ed. London/New York, Routledge.
Pöchhacker, Franz, and Mira Kadriç (1999) “The hospital cleaner as healthcare interpreter: A case
study”, The Translator 5 (2): 147–60. Special Issue on Dialogue Interpreting Ian Mason (ed).
Raymond, C.W. (2014) “Epistemic brokering in the interpreter-mediated medical visit: Negotiating
‘patient’s side’ and ‘doctor’s side’ knowledge”, Research on Language and Social Interaction 47 (4):
426–46.
Reddy, Michael J. (1979) “The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about lan-
guage”, in Metaphor and Thought, Andrew Ortony (ed). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press:
284–324.
Roy, Cynthia B. (2000) Interpreting as a Discourse Process. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Sacks, Harvey, Emmanuel Schegloff, and Gail Jefferson (1974) “A simplest systematics for the organi-
zation of t urn-taking for conversation”, Language 50: 696–736.
Tate, Granville and Graham Turner (1997/2002) “The code and the culture: S ign-language interpreting –
in search of the new breed’s ethics”, in The Interpreting Studies Reader, Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam
Shlesinger (eds). London, Routledge: 373–83.
Theys, Laura, Demi Krystallidou, Heidi Salaets, Cornelia Wermuth, and Peter Pype (2020) “Emotion
work in interpreter-mediated consultations: A systematic literature review”, Patient Education and
Counseling 103 (1): 33–43.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1992) Interpreting as Interaction: On Dialogue Interpreting in Immigration Hearings and
Medical Encounters (PhD diss.). Linköping Studies in Arts and Science No. 83, Linköping, Tema
Kommunikation.
——— (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (2004) “Dialogue interpreting: A monologising practice in a dialogically organised world”, Tar-
get. International Journal of Translation Studies 16 (1): 105–24. https://doi.org/10.1075/t arget.16.1.06wad
Wadensjö, Cecilia, Hanna Sofia Rehnberg, and Zoe Nikolaidou (2022) “Managing a discourse of
reporting: The complex composing of an asylum narrative”, Multilingua (published online May 24,
2022), https://doi.org/10.1515/multi-2022- 0 017.
14
PART 1
Introduction
Although public service interpreting (PSI) is one of the first forms of intercultural commu-
nication in history, it has only recently been defined as a professional and communicative
activity. It has also become the subject of academic research. According to Wadensjö (1998:
49), whose definition of PSI was one of the first to be recorded, PSI refers to interpreting
in public services to facilitate communication between staff and laypeople meeting for a
particular purpose. While Wadensjö refers to PSI as a form of social interaction, capturing
the perspectives of both parties involved in institutional encounters, Mikkelson (1996: 19)
offers a definition based on PSI as a concern at the macro level of society, considering it an
activity that facilitates equal access to legal services, healthcare, education and social services
to groups of people belonging to cultural or linguistic minorities who generally have lower
levels of education and income and are often unfamiliar with or unaccustomed to the new
social reality in the country in which they reside. Ozolins (2000: 32), among many others,
emphasises what he calls the ‘institution-driven’ characteristic of PSI, which highlights how
the institutional policies of each country affect its professionalisation.
The difficulties in defining this field of practice are also illustrated by the absence of a
common name. The variety of expressions used to address this activity illustrates the point:
community interpreting, liaison interpreting, interpreting in social services, dialogue inter-
preting, PSI and translation, and there are even specific names based on professionals and
their areas of expertise, such as healthcare interpreter, intercultural health mediator, cultural
interpreter, community interpreter, legal interpreter or public service interpreter to name
but a few. The two terms most used nowadays to refer to the activity are PSI and commu-
nity interpreting. The latter is the most employed expression in some countries, including
the United States, Canada, Australia, and New Zealand. The United Kingdom and some
European countries prefer PSI or public service interpreting and translation over community
interpreting to prevent confusion with translation/interpreting work performed by amateurs
on a voluntary basis (Corsellis 2002: 32).
Since the second half of the twentieth century, PSI has developed significantly as an aca-
demic discipline within Translation and Interpreting Studies (TIS). A closer look at research
in TIS shows abundant literature describing cases where family members, children, friends
DOI: 10.4324/9780429298202-3 17
Carmen Valero-Garcés
or anyone speaking or understanding two languages may help break language barriers in
hospitals, police stations, social work offices or immigration departments. The literature
also highlights training experiences or projects related to lesser-used languages (Bot 2003;
Burdeus-Domingo et al. 2020).
What emerges from this literature is PSI’s journey from an informal activity to an occu-
pation and eventually to a fully fledged profession. Tseng’s (1992) professionalisation model
of new occupations in four stages might serve as a framework of reference (for an analysis of a
different model of the professionalisation process, see Skaaden, 2018). Tseng’s model outlines
four stages: (1) market disorder and fierce competition among the practitioners of the pro-
fession in question, with a complete lack of social recognition; (2) development of consensus
about practitioners’ aspirations; (3) creation of professional associations and codes of conduct,
giving professionals higher social recognition and prestige; (4) adherence to the code of eth-
ics and control of entry to the profession, which consolidates the profession’s establishment.
Nevertheless, as Mikkelson (1996) points out, progressing from stages 1 to 4 does not happen
overnight, and boundaries between the different stages may be blurred and not identifiable.
This chapter explores some of the controversies and critical issues that have dominated
this process. It will include reflections on critical issues related to institutional relationships
and PSI, codes, norms and PSI’s professionalisation.
Types of interactions
Narrowing the gap between the administration and each resident in a specific area requires a
smooth operating system which Corsellis (2008: 71–89) visualises as a chain formed by three
fundamental components: public service providers; interpreters, mediators or intermediaries
that make communication possible; and users that are not proficient in the language in which
the services are provided.
Institutional encounters have many traits in common, but the interactions are diverse
(Agar 1985). There is an institution-individual relationship, and there is also an individual-
individual relationship. In the first type of interaction – institution-individual – the mi-
grant approaches the institutions or public service, seeking a service that existed before the
migrant/individual arrived at the host location. Consequently, the institutions have estab-
lished protocols, values, and operations specifically required to access their services. In the
second type of interaction – that is, individual-individual – providers do not systematically
apply the same criteria to every service-
seeker: instead, they act according to a personal
framework supported by the training they have received, their familiarity with potential
cultural differences, their linguistic knowledge, their own life experience within and outside
their country, or even their prejudices. Furthermore, the complexity of intercultural rela-
tionships that vary depending on the context of the interaction (legal, medical, educational,
social) must be considered; this leads to various relationships between actors from horizontal
to hierarchical ( Jiménez Salcedo 2010: 45). Thus, eliminating the language barrier is not the
18
General issues about public service interpreting
only issue in such encounters; other barriers must be overcome to facilitate understanding
and provide or receive the service in question.
The interaction’s success depends on the collaboration of all the actors involved in the
communicative chain described by Corsellis (2008). The mere linguistic involvement of the
interpreter is not sufficient; other professionals that act as providers (social workers, NGO
workers, public sector workers, and so on) must also collaborate. Such collaboration may,
however, be hindered for several reasons. Corsellis’s volume (2008, Chap. 7 and 8: 118–
7 4) describes the importance of training public sector workers to work with PSI and across
cultures. She underlines the need to promote interdisciplinarity between all the parties in-
volved and explores ideas about the policy and management skills needed to provide an
organisational framework. In her words, ‘Management of change requires a clear analysis of
an existing situation, identification of targets and the development of practical incremental
steps to cover the gap within agreed timescales’ (Corsellis 2008: 9). Accordingly, she claims
that the first step in achieving this objective is policy: a national commitment to providing
what is needed. In addition, she recommends a coordinated national approach ‘because a
piecemeal approach has associated challenges and risks’. Related topics are discussed in this
volume by Felberg and Skaaden, and by Krystallidou.
Challenges
One of the most significant challenges to providing accurate PSI is the vast number of lan-
guages that PSI services require and the difficulties of providing language-
specific training
and education for interpreters in many such languages, particularly those of limited diffusion.
This situation has given rise to some complaints, mainly in the legal sector (EULITA 2016),
concerning the qualifications held by the interpreters contracted and the actual quality of
their work. Moreover, low pay rates are causing many experienced and qualified interpreters
and translators to reject working for the public sector. Consequently, as Benhaddou (2012:
93–95) reports, Spanish institutions have lowered the required minimum qualifications,
which stems from the reality that otherwise no qualified interpreters would be available.
Low payment and lowered qualifications have affected PSI also in the United Kingdom, as
Staton (2019) shows. Related issues and other problems affecting PSI will be discussed below.
The power relations between institutions and service users present another challenge.
These relations precede any difficulties immigrants who do not speak the language have
when they establish contact and eventually apply for service. The privileges that institutions
have over citizens are legitimised by the executive power from which they arise and, there-
fore, allow institutions to impose their rules. Institutions’ power also implies responsibili-
ties, for example, communicating with their clients. Both Prunč (2012) and Skaaden (2018)
have considered this duality. Researchers like Lippi-Green (1994), Skutnabb-K angas (1999),
Blanchet (2016) and Wallace and Monzó-Nebot (2019) have highlighted the limits of the
tacit policies of institutional and individual monolingual practices. These researchers call
for measures to alleviate the substantial economic, administrative, and ideological obstacles
encountered when individuals and institutions attempt to support and maintain linguistic
and cultural diversity.
This complex situation requires institutional adaptation. It is the task of institutional
powers to take the first step to adapting services in cases of significant multiculturalism.
The act of calling on an interpreter or mediator represents an effort by providers to adapt
an existing protocol of action to a new need. This adaptation can hardly be a smooth one
for providers in that it introduces a change in the service culture of institutions that have
19
Carmen Valero-Garcés
(…) outsourced management by a private company has not proved ideal, but policymak-
ers have shown little sensitivity. Despite criticism of the lack of guarantees for quality in
the original contract (which focused on language ability and devoted little attention to
interpreting), the requirement for competence in interpreting completely disappeared
from the new request for tenders in 2012.
20
General issues about public service interpreting
those services (Wallace and Monzó-Nebot 2019). As the Covid-19 pandemic has evidenced,
safe communication between society and its minorities is in the interest of society at large.
Different countries have adopted similar solutions to cope with PSI’s challenges: special-
ised qualifications exist, although not always in languages of lesser diffusion, yet govern-
mental authorities still fail to make them a requirement. As a result, many language service
providers appoint non-professionals (often at a lower rate of pay) who cannot undertake
highly specialised interpreting, thus putting the rights of migrants and the integrity of insti-
tutional representatives at risk.
The aim to guarantee the right to communicate and the need to improve the quality of
translation and interpreting services has led several organisations at national and interna-
tional levels to call for the further professionalisation of PSI, especially for court translators
and interpreters. For instance, the European Legal Interpreters and Translators Association
(EULITA) was responsible for pushing the adoption of Directive 2010/64/EU (on the adop-
tion of this Directive in other European countries, see Giambruno 2014b). The association
has repeatedly called upon the EU to ensure that all Member States have transposed the
Directive into domestic law since the deadline to do so was in 2013. Yet, almost ten years
later, many countries have failed to do so (EULITA 2019).
21
Carmen Valero-Garcés
These discrepancies have been the object of numerous studies, articles and seminars con-
cerning not only interpreters but also the perception that providers and clients have of their
ethics and how language services carry out their role (Gentile 2016).
The principles that guide action in PSI may be influenced by several factors, such as the
society in question, the culture(s) the society is in contact with, the participants’ educational
background and even their personal or private ethics. When two different professions with
respective codes coexist in the same context, there may be overlap, disagreement or mis-
alignment. When it comes to achieving or practising the ethical principles that guide each
profession, ethical conflicts may arise if and when different solutions clash. This tendency is
gaining support in some instances and points to the need to re-examine theories on PSI and
guidelines established in codes and principles (Wallace and Monzó-Nebot 2019: 12).
Research on the professionalisation of PSI illustrates a first era during which the conduit
metaphor (Roy 1993/2002) was taken as an inspiration to guide practice and draft codes of
conduct (Merlini 2015: 28) to claim some authority and prestige. Nowadays, an increasing
number of studies show how interpreters engage their ethics (Bot 2003; Inghilleri 2010;
Valero-Garcés 2017), thus transgressing the requirements of impartiality by mediating, tak-
ing on additional tasks and making decisions unconsciously or n ot – as to when omissions
and additions are used effectively to support an ongoing encounter while also not siding
exclusively with one of the parties (Martin 2000). Interpreters may also perceive power im-
balances and even go a step further to take measures to compensate for inherent asymmetries
(Bancroft 2017: 2 05–10).
Exploring interpreting as social interaction, Wadensjö (1998) finds that in situated en-
counters, mediation is inherent in interpreting. Wadensjö (1998: 1 05–6) writes,
In dialogue interpreting, the translating and coordinating aspects are simultaneously pres-
ent, and the one does not exclude the other. These aspects condition each other. Seen
like this, it is not an empirical question whether interpreters are translators or mediators –
they cannot avoid being both (italics in the original).
22
General issues about public service interpreting
prototypes for interpreters placed along an imaginary continuum according to the degree
of mediation involved. Terms used in the literature to refer to the interpreter’s role include
active participant, assistant, cultural broker, advocate, conciliator middleman, broker, go-
between, gatekeeper, clarifier, explainer, cultural mediator, helpmate or agent. Although
these terms are metaphorical descriptions, they may turn into prescriptive ones. If each term
(or role description) came with an ethical code of its own, that would contribute to creating
confusion rather than a shared professional identity. The diversity of fields and situations PSI
covers may need descriptive but flexible classifications with no clear- cut borders if each set
of circumstances requires different actions and choices by the interpreter (Angelelli 2004:
47). Some examples of the complexity of the role of the interpreter in PSI include the di-
agrammatic tool devised by Zimányi (2009), the notion of Role-Space coined by Lee and
Llewellyn-Jones (2013) and Aguirre’s multi-layered continuum (2019).
One of the characteristics of PSI that triggers the idea that the complexity of the role
can be split into different sub-roles is the diversity of public services involved. Runcieman
(2020), for instance, suggests that the interpreter’s interactive role is more visible and relevant
in some fields than in others. For example, there is greater demand for cultural sensitivity and
understanding of both the source and target cultures in asylum interpreting. Here, he argues,
the interpreter must continually mediate between diverse sociocultural conventions where the
potential for misunderstandings may constantly arise. As an example, Runcieman cites the
application process: the asylum seeker may have no understanding of a different judicial
system or have heightened levels of distrust towards state officials and/or strong suspicions
about the potential abuse of power of the state. In their verbal communication, the asylum
seeker might also have fewer or very different politeness markers or use more elaborate hedg-
ing strategies, which could make them seem too aggressive or too evasive, potentially lead-
ing to an unfavourable outcome in their appeal. Moreover, many asylum interviews elicit
petitioners’ narratives about their lives and experiences, which can be a source of frustration,
incomprehension, or doubts about their veracity for officials due to differences in the cultural
canons of what constitutes a compelling, plausible narrative. For some or all these reasons,
interpreters often intervene or even assume the role of interviewers. At times, they may even
alter the style and register of interviewees’ statements (Pöllabauer 2004; Runcieman 2020).
In court interpreting, Berk-Seligson (2002) and Hale (2004) also indicate that interpreters’
interventions like altering the style and register can make the difference between a defendant
being found guilty or innocent.
In the healthcare setting, there may appear to be a tendency for the interpreter to act
more as a conduit, conveying information from one language to another without a personal/
cultural contribution (Wilcox and Shaffer 2005: 4), particularly, when communicating the
medical practitioners’ prescriptions regarding medicines and curative therapies (but see Gav-
ioli and Merlini, this volume, for a discussion about dealing with facts and emotions in
interpreter-mediated interaction in healthcare). Again, the interpreter acting as a conduit
might not be as straightforward as it seems in the case of emergency room care or doctor-
patient interactions in healthcare centres. Relevant factors include different socio-cultural
and socioeconomic backgrounds with differing levels of education and socioeconomic status,
which, makes some explanations necessary for effective communication (Angelelli 2008).
Who has the responsibility/competence to explain the professional context or concepts in
such situations seems to be a never-ending topic of discussion.
Another reason to look at PSI as specific to sub- areas or settings is that health and legal
settings cover a lot of PSI work. In countries where PSI is more developed, such as Austra-
lia or Canada, healthcare interpreting has gained much attention and court interpreting is
23
Carmen Valero-Garcés
widely recognised as a profession. As Sasso and Malli (2014) suggest, if policy recognition
is desired, then perhaps fragmenting the field is an alternative approach. This is reflected in
the existence of different ISO standards, now specifically elaborated for the healthcare and
legal areas.1 However, Corsellis (2008) also warned about the impact that fragmentation may
have on PSI compared with the advantage of standing together and potentially becoming
stronger as a profession. Evidence has shown that significant milestones have been achieved
as a unified body, including the publication of the first international standard for community
interpreting, the ISO norm Interpreting: Guidelines for community interpreting in 2014. A revision
of these standards has been initiated in 2021.
Directly or indirectly, existing ISO standards may leave room for discussion. Besides,
the differences between countries are so extensive and intricate that PSI could evolve in
many different directions. As Moreno-Rivero (2020) points out, now, the provision of PSI
in legal and healthcare settings in the EU is only regulated by Directive 2010/64/EU. More
specifically:
Directive 2010/64/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 20 October 2010,
on the right to interpretation and translation in criminal proceedings, grants access to
translation of all relevant documentation and court interpreting in c riminal – but not
civil – proceedings to speakers of all EU official languages.
Directive 2011/24/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 9 March 2011,
on the application of patients’ rights in cross-
border healthcare, acknowledges patients’
rights to information. However, each country has the responsibility (and freedom) to
issue its laws to regulate the application of these rules. Consequently, the adoption of
language rules that help manage PSI partly relies on each country individually: there is
a common EU-wide framework, but its implementation (or the lack of it) differs among
countries, as noted in a report by the European Commission (2018).
24
General issues about public service interpreting
resources and translation technologies for LLD (Giambruno 2014a; Balogh, Salaets and Van
Schoor 2016).
The lack of recognition of PSI as a profession implies a lack of professionalisation. To
achieve professionalisation, training is necessary, and the level and length of education no
doubt have an impact on professional status. Nevertheless, at the time of this writing, there
are still no clear directions about whether training should be offered by higher education
institutions or professional training institutions or whether it should be provided by NGOs
and other bodies involved in migration support (De Pedro Ricoy 2010). In practice, PSI is
still struggling with offering well-
trained translators and interpreters in the required (wide)
variety of languages and cultures, and debates are still ongoing about the formats to adopt,
from one-day instruction to periodical seminars or workshops to undergraduate and post-
graduate courses (Valero-Garcés 2019).
The present situation does not mean that PSI has not evolved. Despite the differences
between countries, PSI has gradually advanced towards professionalisation ever since it be-
came the focus of academic and research activity when the well- known 1995 Critical Link
conference was held in Canada. Seemingly, the boundaries between conference interpreting
and PSI are becoming blurred and the differences are starting to fade altogether. Some indi-
cations of this change are seen in the current debate over prestige and by increasing recog-
nition of PSI by other professionals and societies at large. Also, as Mikkelson (1996) argues,
some links between conference interpreting and PSI are quite evident. For instance, while
conference interpreting has contributed to shaping standards in PSI (sometimes a bit blindly
and because there was little else to serve as a model), it is becoming increasingly clear that
PSI can contribute to the development of conference interpreting by increasing practitioners’
perceived status of con-
sensitivity to various layers of contexts. In her analysis of the self-
ference and public service interpreters, Gentile (2014) found that a sense of lower status still
characterises PSI interpreters. At the same time, her data indicate an increasing awareness of
the social role carried out by the interpreting profession as a driving force that motivates PSI
interpreters to follow the path towards full professionalisation.
Searching for models of good practice based on the realisation that quality indeed impacts
equal access to justice and fair trials may also contribute to consolidating the profession-
alisation of PSI. For Corsellis (Valero-Garcés 2014: 10) this will include exploring non-
p rofessional interpreting both in research and training beyond mainstream institutions to
include groups of interpreting practitioners whose positions have been, or still are, rather
peripheral (be it professional, ad hoc, novice, volunteer and/or activist), but who play an
active part in society.
Merging different disciplinary and methodological approaches for the purpose of research
and training could also promote PSI professionalisation (see Krystallidou; Sagli and Skaaden,
this volume). Research indicates that the professionalisation process of PSI is linked also to
complex ideological and social factors. In some areas, the tendency is that PSI interpreting
generally is perceived as no less professionalised than conference interpreting or legal in-
terpreting and translation (LIT), which is recognised as a professional branch of its own in
some countries. No doubt, language service providers, practitioners and their clients must
continue striving for the common ground before PSI is broadly accepted as a profession
(Bancroft 2017).
However, some of the characteristics of PSI already mentioned, such as unstable working
conditions and poor remuneration, also contribute to the de-professionalisation of the trade,
thus compromising, for instance, individuals’ right to basic services or to fair trials. Gentile
(2017) shows how the implementation of nationalist ideologies, including aspects such as the
25
Carmen Valero-Garcés
privatisation and outsourcing of PSI, have hindered the professionalisation of PSI and dam-
aged public perception of the profession.
Gentile (2017) further points out that to achieve full professionalisation and public rec-
ognition, PSI must gain and maintain the general public’s trust. However, the tendency to
outsource and/or cut interpreting services among some national governments in the EU,
particularly after the 2008 economic crisis, has not only transformed PSI into a commodity
but is creating a situation in which the winner is a cheap service rather than a quality one.
Cheaper services may become more widespread after the coronavirus pandemic, which has
boosted the need for interpreting services but created the conditions for a new economic
crisis.
In 2011, Sela-Sheffy and Shlesinger referred to translators and interpreters as ‘an extreme
example of an understudied semi-professional occupation’ (2011: 3). And indeed, it seems
like PSI continues to be a semi-profession, that is, an occupation that has achieved a few
characteristics of professions but does not possess sufficient autonomy to be sociologically
classified as such (Saha and Dworkin 2009).
Problems of professionalisation, financial cuts to translation and interpreting services,
privatisation and outsourcing to external agencies appear to be destroying systems where
service providers were once encouraged to use registered and state-certified interpreters (for
instance, in the United Kingdom and the Netherlands) (Gentile 2017). Gentile (2017) notes
that this illustrates the link between national policies and their impact on professionalisation
processes and interpreters’ perception of their status. In her research on the professional status
of PSI, Gentile provides insights into interpreters’ opinions and comments on three param-
eters: remuneration, perception of status and the social value of interpreting. In her own
words, ‘Despite the discouraging results, a positive aspect that emerges from the survey is
that many interpreters continue to work in this setting because they consider it to be a moral
imperative, a sign of justice towards the most vulnerable’ (Gentile 2014: 204).
This attitude suggests that interpreters include principles of social justice in their actions
to address the ‘democratic deficit’ (Gentile 2017: 83). Therefore, following Gentile, inter-
preters’ codes of ethics should arguably pay attention to the link between PSI and social
justice by specifying that interpreters are actively involved in addressing discrimination in
hospitals, courts, and all other settings where their services are required, an approach that has
proved useful in, for instance, enhancing the professionalisation of other professions, such as
nursing (Cohen and Ezer 2013).
Finally, dialogue between institutions and service providers could prove fruitful to raise
awareness of serious consequences, which may possibly result in the violation of language
rights, incorrect diagnoses and miscarriages of justice. In a world in which migrants are
ghettoised and discriminated against, interpreters are professionals who, ideally, work re-
sponsibly to make sure that human rights are respected. There may thus be a strong need to
listen to the opinions, fears and struggles of these professionals who sustain an ethical duty
that many national governments seem to have forgotten.
26
General issues about public service interpreting
Further reading
Monzó-Nebot, Esther, and Melisa Wallace (2020) Ethics of Non-P rofessional Translation and Interpreting,
Special issue of Translation and Interpreting Studies 15 (1).
This special issue of Translation and Interpreting Studies explores the emergency of new societies, new
values, new demands when mapping non-
professional interpreting and translation, and pay special attention to
issues of justice, trust, accuracy, truth, virtue, and self-care.
Ng, Eva, and Ineke Creeze (2020) Interpreting in Legal and Healthcare Settings: Perspectives on Research and
Training. Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
The book addresses issues related to interpreting in legal and healthcare settings at large, but the variety of
innovative themes it addresses, based on empirical research and real-life experiences from different parts of the
world makes it suitable also for PSI researchers and trainers.
Phelan, Mary, Mette Rudvin, Hanne Skaaden, and Patrick Stefan Kermit (2020) Ethics in Public Service
Interpreting. London/New York, Routledge.
The book explores ethical dilemmas from different perspectives and explains the difference between personal
and professional principles; it also offers ample explanation and discussion of guidelines, clearly illustrated with
examples.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen (2019) “Training public service interpreters and translators: Facing chal-
lenges”, Revista de Llengua i Dret, Journal of Language and Law 71: 88–105. https://doi.org/10.2436/
rld.i71.2019.3262
The article calls attention to the need of education and training for raising the status of PSIT and provides
experiences of PSIT researchers, practitioners and trainers which help highlight challenges and advances in the
PSIT area.
Related chapters
Chapter 7, Public service translation: Critical issues and future directions by Mustafa Taibi
Chapter 16, ‘Interpreter’s mistake’ – Why should other professions care about the professionalization of interpret-
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
Note
1 ISO 13611:2014, Interpreting — Guidelines for community interpreting; ISO 18841:2018, Inter-
preting services — General requirements and recommendations; ISO 20228:2019, Interpret-
ing services — L egal i nterpreting — Requirements; ISO 21998:2020, Interpreting services —
Healthcare i nterpreting — Requirements and recommendations; ISO 20539:2019, Translation,
interpreting and related technology — Vocabulary.
27
Carmen Valero-Garcés
References
Agar, Michael H. (1985) “Institutional discourse”, Text & Talk, Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of
Discourse 5 (3): 147–68.
A guilar-Solano, María (2015) “Non-professional volunteer interpreting as an institutionalized practice
in healthcare: A study on interpreters’ personal narratives”, Translation & Interpreting 17 (3): 132–48.
Aguirre Fernández Bravo, Elena (2019) “Interpreter role (self-)perception: A model and an assessment
tool”, Revista de Llengua i Dret, Journal of Language and Law 71: 62–72.
Anderson, R. Bruce W. (1978) “Interpreter roles and interpretation situations: Cross cutting typol-
ogies”, in Language, Interpretation and Communication, David Gerver and H. Wallace Sinaiko (eds).
New York/L ondon, Plenum Press: 130–217.
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2004) Revisiting the Interpreter’s Role. A Study of Conference, Court, and Medical
Interpreters in Canada, Mexico, and the United States. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
——— ( 2008) “The role of the interpreter in the healthcare setting: A plea for a dialogue between
research and practice”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting. Definitions and Dilemmas, Car-
men Valero-Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 247–65.
——— (2015) Study on Public Service Translation in C ross-B order Healthcare. Final Report for the Euro-
pean Commission, Directorate-General for Translation Ref. DGT/2014/T PS. URL:https://
publications.europa.eu/e n/p ublication- d etail/- /p ublication/6 382f b66- 8 387- 11e5- b 8b7-
01aa75ed71a1 (accessed 1 November 2021).
Baixauli-Olmos, Luis (2013) “A description of interpreting in prisons – Mapping the setting through an
ethical lens”, in Interpreting in a Changing Landscape. Selected Papers from Critical Link 6, Cristina Schäff-
ner, Krzysztof Kredens and Yvonne Fowler (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 4 5–60.
Balogh, Katalin, Heidi Salaets, and Dominique Van Schoor (2016) TraiLLD: Training in Languages of
Lesser Diffusion. Antwerp, Lannoo Publishers.
Bancroft, Marjory (2017) “The voice of compassion: Exploring t rauma-i nformed interpreting”, in
Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development in Public Service Interpreting and Translation, Carmen V alero-
G
arcés and Rebecca Tipton (eds). Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 195–219.
Benhaddou Handi, Elhassane (2012) “La traducción y la interpretación en el Ministerio del Interior”,
in Ensayos sobre la traducción jurídica e institucional, Iciar Alonso Araguás, Jesús Baigorri Jalón and
Helen Campbell (eds). Granada, Comares: 9 1–102.
Berk-Seligson, Susan (2002) The Bilingual Courtroom. Court Interpreters in the Judicial Process. Chicago,
IL, The University of Chicago Press.
Blanchet, Philippe (2016) Discriminations: combattre la glottophobie. París, Textuel.
Bot, Hanneke (2003) “The myth of the uninvolved interpreter interpreting in mental health and the de-
velopment of a three-person psychology”, in The Critical Link 3: Interpreters in the Community, Louise
Brunette, Georges L. Bastin, Isabelle Hemlin and Heather Clarke (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 27–35.
Burdeus-Domingo, Noelia, Suzanne Gagnon, Sophie Pointurier, and Yvan Leanza (2020) “Puentes
y barreras en la formación en interpretación en los servicios públicos: la docencia a intérpretes con
experiencia no profesional”, in The Human Factor in PSIT, Carmen Valero-Garcés (ed). Alcalá de
Henares, Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad: 77–84.
Camayd-Freixas, Erik (2013) US Immigration Reform and Its Global Impact. Lesson from the Postville Raid.
New York, Palgrave Macmillan.
Cohen, John, and Tamar Ezer (2013) “Human rights in patient care: A theoretical and practical frame-
work”, Health and Human Rights 15 (2): 7 –19.
Corsellis, Anne, and Leandro Félix Fernandez (2001) Aequitas: Access to Justice across Language and Cul-
ture in the EU. (Extract from chapter 7), Erik Hertog (ed.). Antwerp, Lessius Hogeschool.
Corsellis, Anne (2002) “Creating a professional context for public service interpreters and translators”,
in Traducción e Interpretación en los Servicios Públicos: Nuevas necesidades para nuevas realidades/New Needs
for New Realities, Carmen Valero-Garcés and Guzmán Mancho (eds). Alcalá de Henares, Servicio
de Publicaciones de la Universidad: 31–38.
——— (2008) Public Service Interpreting. The Firsts Steps. Basingstoke Hampshire, Palgrave.
De Pedro Ricoy, Raquel (2010) “Training public service interpreters in the UK: A fine balancing act”,
The Journal of Specialised Translation 14 (n /a): 1 00–20.
D ysart-Gale, Deborah (2005) “Communication models, professionalization, and the work of medical
interpreters”, Health Communication 17 (1): 91–103.
28
General issues about public service interpreting
29
Carmen Valero-Garcés
Silvana E. Carr, Diana Abraham, and Aideen Dufour (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Ben-
jamins: 21–33.
Phelan, Mary, Mette Rudvin, Hanne Skaaaden, and Patrick Stefan Kermit (2020) Ethics in Public Ser-
vice Interpreting. London/New York, Routledge.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2002) “Getting organized: The evolution of community interpreting”, Interpreting
4 (1): 125–40.
——— (2008) “Interpreting as mediation”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting. Definitions and
Dilemmas, Carmen Valero-Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benja-
mins: 9 –26.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2004) “Interpreting in asylum hearings: Issues of role, responsibility and power”,
Interpreting 6 (2): 143–80.
Prunč, Erich (2012) “Rights, realities and responsibilities in community interpreting”, The Interpreters’
Newsletter 17 (2012): 1–12.
Ross, W. David (1930/2002) The Right and the Good. Oxford, Oxford University Press. (Reprinted
from the original edition with an introduction by Philip Stratton-Lake).
Roy, Cynthia B. (1993/2002) “The problem with definitions, descriptions, and the role metaphors
of interpreters”, in The Interpreting Studies Reader, Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam Shlesinger (eds).
London/New York, Routledge: 344–54.
Runcieman, Alan James (2020) “Community interpreting and the Covid 19 crisis: Present relevancy
and future directions”, Tilburg Papers in Cultural Studies No. 242.
Saha, Lawrence, and Anthony Gary Dworkin (2009) International Handbook of Research on Teachers and
Teaching. New York, Springer.
Sasso, Angela, and Kiran Malli (2014) “Trying to fit a square peg in a round hole: Is community
interpreting just too big for public policy? The Canadian experience a provocation”, FITISPos
International Journal 1 (n /a): 42–50.
Sela-Sheffy, Rakefet, and Miriam Shlesinger (2011). Identity and Status in the Translational Professions.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Skaaden, Hanne (2018) “Invisible or invincible? Professional integrity, ethics, and voice in public ser-
vice interpreting”, Perspectives: Studies in Translatology 27 (14): 1 –14.
Skutnabb-K angas, Tove (1999) “Linguistic human r ights – A re you naive, or what?”, TESOL Journal
8 (3): 6 –12.
Staton, Bethan (2019) “U K’s court interpreters fear cost-cutting is threatening fair trials”, Financial
Times. URL: https://w ww.ft.com/content/55447226-a f9c-11e9-8030-530adfa879c2. (accessed 1
November 2021).
Tseng, John (1992) “Interpreting as an emerging profession in Taiwan – A sociological model”, MA
Thesis. Taipei, Taiwan, Fu Jen Catholic University.
United Nations (2020) “Everyone included: Social impact of COVID-19”. DISD. URL: https://
www.un.org/development/desa/d spd/everyone-i ncluded-covid-19.html. (accessed 1 November
2021).
Valero-Garcés, Carmen (2014) “Interview of Anne Corsellis”, FITISPos International Journal 1 (2014):
92–102.
——— (2017) “Ethical codes impacting in prison communication”, in Ideology, Ethics and Policy Devel-
opment in Public Service Interpreting and Translation, Carmen Valero-Garcés and Rebeca Tipton (eds).
Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 105–30.
——— (2019) “Training public service interpreters and translators: Facing challenges”, Revista de
Llengua I Dret, Journal of Language and Law 71: 8 8–105.
——— (
2020) “ Overcoming language barriers in the Spanish healthcare context”, in Interpret-
ing in Legal and Healthcare: Perspectives on Research and Training, Eva Ng and Ineke Creeze (eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 2 33–41.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen, and Laura Gauthier (2010) “Bourdieu and public service interpreting and
translation: Towards a social theory in PSIT”, MonTI 2 (n /a): 9 7–117.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen, and Bianca Vitalaru (eds) (2014) (Re)Visiting Ethics and Ideology in Situations of
Conflict. Alcalá de Henares, Universidad de Alcalá Publicaciones.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. Harlow, Essex, Addison Wesley Longman Limited.
Wallace, Melissa, and Esther Monzó-Nebot (2019) “La traducció i la interpretació jurídiques en els
serveis públics: definició de qüestions clau, revisió de polítiques i delimitació del públic de la
30
General issues about public service interpreting
traducció i la interpretació jurídiques en els serveis públics”, Revista de Llengua i Dret, Journal of
Language and Law 71: 1–12.
Weiss, Regula, and Rahel Stuker (1998) Interprétariat et médiation culturelle dans le système de soins, rapport
de base. Neuchâtel, Forum suisse pour l’étude des migrations.
Wilcox, Sherman, and Barbara Shaffer (2005) “Towards a cognitive model of interpretation”, in Topics
in Signed Language Interpreting: Theory and Practice, Terry Janzen (ed). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John
Benjamins: 2 – 40.
Zimányi, Krisztina (2009) “On impartiality and neutrality: A diagrammatic tool as a visual aid”,
Translation & Interpreting 1 (2): 55–70.
31
2
THE AMBIGUITY OF
INTERPRETING
Ethnographic interviews with public service
interpreters
Kristina Gustafsson
The public service interpreter quoted above describes his experiences of multilingual en-
counters between welfare professionals and clients in various service settings. In the same
interview, he discusses aspects of ethics, loyalties, power, and responsibilities associated with
interpreting in the public sector. The interpreter is one of 26 key participants interviewed
during two research projects about public service interpreting in Sweden with the joint title
The Interpreter Project (Gustafsson, Norström and Fioretos 2012, 2013; Norström, Fioretos and
Gustafsson 2012).2
For me, as a scholar in ethnology and social work, a starting point for The Interpreter
Project was the perception that although there are thousands of encounters every day in
different welfare service settings between welfare professionals and clients who do not
speak Swedish (the majority language), their knowledge of interpreting and the position
of the interpreter is far from comprehensive. Extensive research has been conducted that
explores issues of multilingualism and language competence in social work (Chand 2005;
Harrison 2006, 2007; Kriz and Skivenes 2010; Tipton 2016; Westlake and Jones 2017; Hall
and Valdiviezo 2018; Gustafsson, Norström and Höglund 2019), in legal and court settings
(Torstensson 2010; Elsrud 2014; Elsrud, Lalander and Staaf 2017; Staaf and Elsrud 2018), in
health and medical care (Gerrish et al. 2004; Kale and Syed 2010; Hadziabdic 2011; Plejert
et al. 2015; Haralambous et al. 2018; Granhagen et al. 2019), and in the area of migration
and asylum investigation (Herlihy and Turner 2007; Kjelsvik 2014; Akin 2017; Puumala,
Ylikomi and Ristimäki 2017). However, public service interpreting is not the main focus
of these studies.
Furthermore, when it is discussed, interpreting is primarily presented as an issue that
might have a negative impact on the work of welfare professionals and the legal rights of
the client. Chand (2005) and Kriz and Skivenes (2010) present several critical aspects of how
neglect and lack of competence regarding language barriers in social work and in health and
medical care result in structural discrimination against clients who do not speak the majority
language in the United Kingdom and Norway. Both authors find that interpreting is often
perceived as a n on-functional tool among welfare professionals and that the use of inter-
preting might therefore actually exacerbate discrimination rather than promote integration
and social justice. Based on the body of previous research in various areas of welfare, three
challenges associated with public service interpreting appear to be particularly prominent.
One problem is that too many public-service interpreters lack training and competence and
this impacts the quality and legal security of the encounter. More strikingly though, most
welfare professionals lack training in how to interact with interpreters. In their research on
the topic, Westlake and Jones (2017) suggest that it is not fruitful to blame interpreters for
a lack of skills if welfare professionals fail to use them properly (see Skaaden this volume).
Thus, on the one hand, the welfare professionals’ lack of familiarity with interpreting may
affect the interpreters’ lack of competence and, on the other hand, explain why there is so lit-
tle demand for better interpreting services. A third problem is a lack of resources. Dominelli
(2018) discusses how the tight budgets of welfare services often become legitimate grounds
for not using interpreting and translation services at all.
The above description reveals a disturbing gap between societal needs and the demand
for highly skilled interpreting and translation services. Another obstacle against equity and
legal security is the low remuneration and low status of public service interpreters, who
often work under quite demanding conditions (Norström, Fioretos and Gustafsson 2012).
Although Sweden and other Nordic countries have extensive experience of migration, mul-
ticulturalism, and multilingualism, these features are treated as temporary situations that can
be met with short-term solutions rather than as inherent features of super-diverse societies
(Meissner and Vertovec 2015; Grzymala-Kazlowska and Phillimore 2018). This gap might
undermine the social rights of non-Swedish speakers in their interactions with the authori-
ties and welfare institutions and might lead to linguistic injustice (Piller 2016).
Although the literature referred to highlights problems and shortcomings, it also shows
the importance of awareness, knowledge, and skills in multilingual communication among
welfare professionals (Harrison 2006, 2007; Hall and Valdiviezo 2020). However, the per-
spectives of public service interpreters on these issues are missing from this body of literature.
Having performed fieldwork and multiple research studies among public service interpreters
for more than a decade, I found that they have gained unique insights into the contemporary
situation in Sweden and that these insights are of value for addressing the above-mentioned
gaps. The experiences of public service interpreters can offer instructive insights into how
the authorities work, about the responses of welfare institutions to global migration, and
the effects of a changing linguistic landscape (Blommaert 2013). Also, more can be learned
about the position of interpreters and how they take part in the negotiation of meaning in
multilingual encounters.
Thus, the aim of this chapter is to explore the potential of ethnographic interviews as a
method for documenting and exploring public service interpreters’ experiences of working
in various welfare settings. In order to present the method, I used my previous research,
including 72 lengthy interviews with 26 different interpreters, all working in the public ser-
vice sector. From this extensive material, two examples were selected, reflecting situations
that made the interpreters discuss in greater detail about what was going on, both in a wider
societal and welfare policy perspective and more narrowly regarding their own position.
This introduction is followed by a section on theories about multilingual encounters in
the context of postcolonial migration, with a special focus on the position of interpreters.
33
Kristina Gustafsson
The concepts of ambiguity and impartiality are presented. The next section includes a de-
scription of ethnographic interviews and methodological considerations. This is followed by
a section on critical issues and topics, in which two selected narratives are analysed. In the
conclusion, I discuss ethnographic interviews as a research method, its prospects, and conse-
quences and highlight some reflections on the idea of adding the interpreter’s perspective to
research in various welfare service areas, as well as in studies of public service interpreting.
Most sections, including this introduction, start with an interview extract. These quotes
demonstrate how theoretical concepts and methodological issues can be used in order to
analyse concrete contexts, namely, interpreters’ experiences and knowledge of multilingual
encounters.
This quote pinpoints the sometimes-overwhelming discrepancy between the different re-
alities that might be found in multilingual encounters. Such discrepancies go far beyond
language barriers and can play a crucial role in the communication and interaction between
clients and welfare professionals. Such diverging realities might constitute the core of a mul-
tilingual encounter in which an interpreter is positioned in the middle. In order to better
understand the constitution and meaning of the multilingual encounter and consequently
also the position of the interpreter, I will use theories of multilingual encounters drawing on
studies of postcolonial migration (Harrison 2006). Within the broad and multidisciplinary
field of migration studies, postcolonial theory and the study of migration and mobility are
increasingly combined (Mains et al. 2013). The addition of postcolonial theory to migration
studies means that contemporary migration and multilingual encounters can be analysed
in terms of the aspects that are significant for colonial encounters as such encounters have
taken place throughout history in different parts of the world, often facilitated by different
kinds of mediators or interpreters (Gustafsson, Norström and Fioretos 2013; de Jong 2015;
Gustafsson 2021).
It is noteworthy that participants in both historical and contemporary encounters some-
times have incompatible worldviews and that the settings are asymmetrical, unequal, and
multilingual (Fur 2017). Historically, these asymmetries were the result of the administra-
tive (a nd military) power of the colonisers. Power asymmetries are produced in a similar
way in contemporary welfare settings that are governed by law, administrative procedures,
authority, and the mandate of the welfare professionals to assess the private lives of their
clients, make decisions and treat them. Furthermore, requiring people to adjust to and use
34
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
the colonisers’ language while forbidding them to use an already existing language has been
one of the most powerful tools of oppression in colonial history (M ignolo and Walsh 2018).
Thus, language has been and still is a ‘war zone’ in many countries, including Sweden (A ngu
2018). Placing contemporary migration and language policies in Sweden within this context
highlights the hierarchies that exist between people speaking different languages. Using this
theoretical framework can potentially reveal how power relations are shaped between wel-
fare professionals and clients who do not speak the majority language and lack knowledge
about administrative procedures or regulatory laws.
Moreover, as stated above and highlighted in the quote, similarities between colonial
encounters in history and in contemporary settings can also be found in the position of me-
diators and interpreters (de Jong 2015, 2016). They operate in frontline encounters between
two participants who are rarely equal or on friendly terms. At the same time, these mediators
are positioned in the margin because they have no formal power to interfere. Nevertheless,
the mediators wield power because only they master both languages spoken and often un-
derstand the different realities and worldviews that participants represent. In this sense, the
mediators have great informal power, which puts them in a delicate position with a high
demand for trust (Gustafsson, Norström and Fioretos, 2013).
In order to build trust, it is considered fundamental that interpreters remain impartial
and keep all information confidential. Interpreters must not add their own opinions about
factual issues or about the other participants. Previous knowledge about the involved par-
ties, the case in question, or other significant information should not be mentioned by the
interpreter during the assignment in any way (K ammarkollegiet/The Legal, Financial and
Administrative Services Agency 2019). In her book Justice and the Politics of Differences, Young
(1990) explores the ideal of impartiality as guided by the ethics of rights and as defined in
relation to the public, the authorities, and welfare institutions. She defines impartiality as
being able to stay outside, detached and dispassionate, and to abstract from the particularities
of a situation and any associated feelings, affiliations or points of view (Young 1990: 97–98).
Young refers to feminist theorists who have criticised this definition and who suggest that
many decisions taken by the authorities demand engagement, empathy and nuanced under-
standings of the context and the particular needs that people have (Young 1990). Further-
more, this critique states that it is not possible for a welfare professional or an authority to
be truly impartial. One obvious reason is that people always speak and act from a position
that represents the sum of their life circumstances, background, and personal experiences,
as well as historical and societal events and situations. Another more prominent reason in
this context is that authorities and welfare professionals act on behalf of and at the discretion
of politically controlled organisations and legislation. If we return to the position of the in-
terpreter during a multilingual encounter, impartiality appears as an act of negotiating the
interpreter’s own, as well as other’s personal opinions and sentiments and participating, but
not siding with anyone in particular, or taking any decisions about the case at hand. In his
book Ambiguities of Witnessing: Law and Literature in the Time of the Truth Commission, Sanders
(2007) writes:
I take ambiguity in a literal sense. Etymologically the word derives from Latin and
combines amb –, meaning “both ways”, and agere, “to drive”. Agere is also the root of the
verb “to act” and the nouns “agent” and “agency”. “A mbiguity” may thus be taken to
mean an acting on both sides. Its implications would then be not purely semantic, but
also pragmatic (Sanders 2007: 5).
35
Kristina Gustafsson
Ambiguity in Sanders’s sense could be seen as embedded in the act of interpreting because
a public service interpreter is supposed to serve all participants in the encounter equally, in-
cluding both welfare professionals and clients. Defined as ‘acting on both sides’, the concept
of ambiguity may enhance our understanding of what is often both a privileged and a chal-
lenging middle position of the interpreter, handling ethical dilemmas, diverging realities,
power asymmetries, unequal relations, and different languages.
The ambiguous position of the interpreter and the function of impartiality is central in
the quote that opened this section. In the interview transcript, the public service interpreter
emotionally described the discrepancies in the interactions that she witnessed between the
welfare professionals and many of the migrant clients coming from war zones and traumatic
backgrounds. Her role is to act on behalf of both participants and facilitate their commu-
nication. But she is also a participant in the encounter and she should remain unbiased, not
interfere or make decisions concerning the case at hand. The ambiguity manifest when she
perceives that the participants misunderstand each other and that the welfare professionals
fail, or do not have sufficient strength to listen to their client; as she witnesses a clash be-
tween different worlds, life experiences, and power asymmetries. Her narrative instructs
our understanding of how unequal power relations and injustices might arise in seemingly
ordinary multilingual encounters in Sweden today, as similar narratives have through-
out colonial history. It also informs about interpreters’ ambiguous positions. Furthermore,
while there is sometimes little that interpreters can do in the hic et nunc of the situation,
apart from enabling participants’ communication, her narrative highlights the challenges
that are part and parcel of public services in a changing, multilingual, global society. In the
next section, I will focus on methodological issues involved when collecting empirical ma-
terial that can allow for investigation of public service interpreters’ experiences, attitudes,
and knowledge.
The interpreter in the quote continued by describing how agonising it was to stand at the
doorstep alongside the police officers. It made him feel like an intruder. In contrast to the
quote in the previous section, this short narrative brings to mind the interpreter’s own back-
ground, from a country afflicted by conflict and war. By stating that he was standing next
to the police officers, the interpreter is pointing at himself in the narrative as it opens up for
further analysis of how he can handle his position.
The duality in being an outsider versus a co-participant on the inside was typical in
the interviews with the 26 key participants. On the one hand, the interpreters described
and reflected on the situation and the actions of the welfare professionals and their cli-
ents. In this respect, they positioned themselves as witnesses of the situations and com-
munication taking place in various welfare settings. On the other hand, the interpreters
also reflected on their own role in the same kinds of situations, as participants rather
than as observers.
36
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
The Interpreter Project mentioned above, from which the examples in this chapter are
drawn, included extensive ethnographic fieldwork in different areas of public service inter-
preting: training, authorisation, procurement, and working conditions. The selection of the
key participants and the design of the interviews were based on two main considerations.
First, inclusion criteria were formulated in order to achieve a broad representation rather
than a representative selection (Davies 2008). Second, the project used a longitudinal design
with the aim of interviewing the 26 interpreters three times over a four-year period in a
total of 78 interviews. In the end, 72 interviews were completed. The idea was that 50% of
the 26 interpreters should be authorised interpreters, 50% of them female and 50% of them
male, and that 50% should be working in the three main Swedish metropolitan areas and
50% should be working in rural areas from the north of Sweden to the south. The selection
should also include interpreters with extensive experience, as well as those who had only
recently started to work. There were no criteria concerning target languages besides the de-
sire to include native Swedes who interpreted in languages other than Swedish and to have a
broad representation of both common and rare migrant languages. During the recruitment
process special focus was given to the languages that were the most common during different
periods of migration to Sweden. It needs to be noted that most of the 5,0 00–6,000 public
service interpreters that are active in Sweden have a migration background.
The first interview comprised three questions: Tell us about how you became an inter-
preter. How would you describe your role as an interpreter? Tell us about when you came to
identify with the profession. In the second interview, the participants were asked to prepare
and describe five encounters when they acted as interpreters. These were then discussed in
detail in the interview. Some participants who had experience of union work and/or pro-
curement procedures as representatives of associations for interpreters in spoken languages
were asked to discuss this. In the third interview, the interpreters were asked to talk more
broadly about interpreting in the multilingual society and also to give their thoughts on
integration.
As noted, The Interpreter Project included extensive fieldwork and, for ethnographers, ob-
serving, interviewing, and listening is an ongoing process (O´Reilly 2012). However, the
interviews with the 26 interpreters were distinct from the rest of the fieldwork and were al-
ways carefully prepared. For example, the interpreters received the topics and the interview
questions in advance. Confidentiality was ensured and interview ethical guidelines were
followed both during the interviews and in the texts that were produced based on the inter-
views. All published examples (including this chapter) have been accurately reproduced and
coded so that no identifiable information can be linked to specific individuals or situations.
The interviews were o pen-ended and the style was collaborative rather than interrogative,
which is significant for ethnographic interviews. Davies (2008) defines ethnographic inter-
views as a method which, in line with ethnographic research, produces documentation of
interactions and contexts of an interview and uses reflexivity as a tool in the critical valida-
tion of the material. I would argue that interviews also may have an inherent ethnographic
quality, in our case, due to the interpreters’ detailed narratives, that is, their fi rst-hand ob-
servations of multilingual welfare service settings. The interviews drawn on in this chapter
included narratives of specific situations, relations, contexts, environments, and political
situations, which reminds of thick descriptions (Emerson, Fretz and Shaw 2011).
A critique of the ethnographic interview method would be that it is not optimal with re-
gard to drawing general conclusions, since people tend to describe situations that are unique
to them and therefore memorable (Holstein and Gubrium 2003). While this is also the
strength of the method (a s will be shown), I might add that the 26 key participants are all
37
Kristina Gustafsson
highly experienced ones so they, taken together, can provide information suitable to ob-
serve patterns of repeated and similar situations. Hence, the ethnographic interviews have a
certain quantitative quality. This suggests that they indisputably provide the opportunity to
identify and analyse conclusions that are more general.
The goal of the interviews was to capture how lived experiences are verbalised and or-
ganised into narratives. It is vital to recognise that such narratives are not simple reconstruc-
tions of personal experiences or past events, but are creative endeavours. Through narratives,
people present and maintain a certain self, make moral distinctions, or suggest and legiti-
mise specific courses of action. Moreover, people’s narratives are never merely personal, but
emanate from specific h istorical-societal contexts and relate to dominating narratives that
penetrate the social world of the individual (R iessman 2008). Thus, by analysing personal
narratives, it is possible to learn something about society, current norms, and specific histor-
ical events. The interpreters in our interviews, it turned out, were able to describe past times
and changes that had taken place in Swedish interpreting services, as well as in public service
in general, migration patterns, and policies over the years. The interpreters’ narratives high-
lighted demanding working conditions and challenges. Most of all, the interviews include
narratives about multilingual encounters and situations that took place in different welfare
service settings. In this way, the interviews became a keyhole through which the researchers
could peek into rooms that were otherwise closed.
Critical issues and topics: the ambiguity of the position of the interpreter
In this section, I will concentrate on narratives selected from interviews with two of the
26 key participants. The main reason for such a narrow selection is that they represent in-
structive examples of the ethnographic quality of the interviews in terms of a detailed and
reflexive thick description (Emerson, Fretz and Shaw 2011). Furthermore, the two narratives
exemplify different perspectives taken by the interpreters – as a witness and outside observer
on the one hand, and as an insider and participant in the encounter on the other.
Being a witness
One significant aspect of the encounters that take place in welfare settings is that they are
asymmetrical, with one party having the administrative power to make decisions about the
other. There is a risk that this asymmetry will be reinforced in multilingual encounters be-
cause of the unequal access to the majority language (Harrison 2006, 2007). In this section,
I analyse this asymmetry in a narrative by a female interpreter. Emigrated to Sweden in the
early 1970s, she was born and raised in a European country where she worked as a secretary
at an international company for most of her working life. At the age of 40, she decided to
retrain to become an interpreter, and when we met, she had completed the national train-
ing programme for interpreters and had more than ten years of experience in public service
interpreting. She describes how she regards assignments at one Swedish authority as particu-
larly challenging, namely, the Swedish Social Insurance Agency. ‘There, people can really be
treated badly, not myself as an interpreter, but the clients. And that also affects me, because
it is a situation that I cannot control’ (8:2, p. 1). Later in the interview, she describes in detail
a recent experience of one such encounter:
It was a small, narrow and asymmetrical room and there were six of us squeezed into it:
The case worker, a psychiatrist from the adult psychiatric rehab centre, a local medical
38
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
practitioner, a former employer, the client, and me. The room was narrow and the at-
mosphere was not much better and the case was awful (8:2, p. 14).
The case was awful in many ways, she states, most likely because the client was in a very dif-
ficult situation. He had been on sick leave for the first time in nine years when he was made
redundant due to lack of work. He now found himself in a vicious circle of contacts with the
health and medical services, the Swedish Social Insurance Agency, and the Swedish Public
Employment Service. Because of recent legislative changes, he was obliged to be actively
seeking work in order to be accepted as a client of the Swedish Social Insurance Agency.
However, he was too ill to work, according to documentation from the health and medical
services. Thus, he could not be accepted as a job seeker by the Swedish Public Employment
Service and had lost his right to receive sickness and unemployment benefits. He had had no
income for eight months, only being able to survive on loans from family and friends. Now
they wanted him to repay them.
The interpreter described how helpful the case worker was but how furious the other
parties became when they realised that the case worker had no authority to do anything
about the case. The client had to appeal to another unit outside the Swedish Social Insurance
Agency. The interpreter recalls the reactions of the other parties when they realised that
no one, neither the Swedish Social Insurance Agency nor the Swedish Public Employment
Service, was going to take responsibility and help the client.
The interpreter describes how it became very difficult to interpret when the local medical
practitioner, the former employer, and the psychiatrist started their own conversation about
how to proceed with the case.
In the end, the case worker listened to the critique and said: ‘Do you want me to call the
unit responsible for appeals and ask them to prioritise this case?’ She was really calm. I
translated. ‘Yes, please do anything you can’, the client replied (8:2, p. 18).
The interpreter explains that the term ‘prioritise’ was a relief because the procedure for ap-
pealing the case would take a long time with all the registration and diary entries ‘a nd, in
the end, maybe nothing would change’, she says (8:2, p . 19). She adds that despite this, the
client was even more upset afterwards and that it was such a comfort for her to know that
he was receiving support from his former employer, the local medical practitioner, and the
psychiatrist. The interpreter ends her story:
He was lucky. I really admired her, the former employer. Becoming involved with
someone who doesn’t work for you anymore. It was wonderful to witness, I really hope
they will sort it out (8:2, p. 19).
39
Kristina Gustafsson
all. She also witnesses a group of welfare professionals and one private actor (t he former
employer) who ultimately all try to operate within the rules of the different welfare au-
thorities. Hence, the power asymmetries of the encounter were negotiated and modified
during the meeting. From her position as an interpreter, she could observe how this was
happening, and she was the facilitator who listened to and assisted all participants in this
process.
This interpreter’s narrative and situation are unique, but her perception of what took
place is similar to the accounts of many of the 26 key participants. They reported witnessing
systematic problems in both welfare institutions and in the authorities, neither of which take
responsibility for their clients’ problems – the kind of systematic administrative problems
that leave the client with no opportunity to receive a rational explanation about how or why
decisions are taken. In the literature, this has been referred to as administrative or bureau-
cratic violence (Abdelhady, Gren and Joormann 2020). In this particular case, the interpreter
was able to manage her position and what she regarded as an unfair situation because other
people were involved in the case. She explains that every time an issue is understood and
resolved by a welfare professional, she feels that she can leave it behind and that the ambi-
guity of serving all participants without siding with any of them in particular becomes less
problematic.
In the next example, another interpreter describes and reflects in more detail on how she
perceived her own ambiguous position in a courtroom, and how she (a lmost) failed to re-
main impartial in relation to the participants she assisted, and how this made her reconsider
what her impartiality entailed in this particular case.
Negotiating impartiality
The interpreter underlines four words written on a piece of paper – ‘i nterpreting for a mur-
derer’ (26:2, p. 21). The interpreter is a m
iddle-aged female who had decided to learn and
study languages already in her youth. After completing university education in her home
country, she has worked as an interpreter and translator for more than twenty years, both in
her home country and in Sweden. In Sweden, she is an authorised interpreter with special
competence in legal interpreting and she often accepts assignments involving criminals.
She describes how courts provide a microphone and PA system in order to ensure good
sound quality. The equipment creates space between her and the parties for whom she is
interpreting. In this particular case, the defendant had been charged with murder and the
interpreter felt that it was good that she was at a distance from him. However, the defendant
did not want this equipment to be used and the interpreter ended up close to him, which
made chuchotage, or whispered interpreting, possible. From where she was sitting, she looked
straight at the plaintiffs, a family that was grieving and who also felt hatred towards the
defendant.
We were in a secure room because the court had decided that there was a high risk that
the family would attack the defendant. Oh, the hate, if you’d seen it. It was awful, you
could almost cut it with a knife. It was so heavy. And there I was, stuck in the middle.
During the break, I became aware that the family felt I was on the defendant’s side. I was
a sort of pariah in their eyes. /---/ I could see how everyone, the solicitors, prison staff
and other staff, kept a distance from the defendant. He was in kind of a bubble. After a
while, I realised that this was all affecting me (26:2, p. 22).
40
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
The interpreter realised that she was really trying to distance herself from the defendant.
Like the others, she detested him. She told herself that if she did not remain impartial, this
would impact his case, and that would be wrong. She recalls:
I thought, okay, nobody is showing him any respect or acknowledging him. I will break
the bubble and try to treat him like a human being. I started saying hello and goodbye
and kept thinking: ‘Yes, you still are a human being.’ This made him calmer. During the
hearings, I learned more about him. He was from another country and all he was facing
was loneliness and isolation. Nobody was allowed to talk to him and nobody wanted to
talk to him. /---/ When the hearings were over I recorded some music from his home
country and asked the solicitor to hand it over to him. The solicitor said: ‘He’s been
charged with murder’. I said: ‘He’s a human being’. The solicitor said: ‘You are strange’
(26:2, p. 23).
In this narrative, the interpreter reveals several layers of considerations, relationships, expec-
tations, and dilemmas. In the moment, she realises that others perceive her as a pariah and
associate her with someone charged with murder. She realises that she, like the plaintiffs, feels
disgust and hatred towards the person charged with murder, implying that she is biased rather
than impartial towards the defendant and the ongoing trial. She explains that in order to repre-
sent all parties equally, even those she might detest, she had to actively re-think her relationship
to the defendant and his situation. This might seem like she is taking sides, but in reality, it
worked the other way around – from being part of dehumanising the defendant to acknowl-
edging his humanity. This was not because she wanted to take a stand in favour of or against
any of the parties, but in order to promote equal participation. From the insider’s perspective,
she discussed her own position and feelings. This meant that the ethnographic interviews
turned out to be a situation in which she, like the other 25 key participants, started to process
her perceptions of interpreting and what it means to defend your integrity as an interpreter.
Moreover, the example shows how complicated it can be for an interpreter to favour
participation on equal grounds. It also illustrates how interpreters who exercise professional
discretion in public service meetings must be mentally prepared to deal with these kinds of
situations every now and then. In our interviews, it emerged that interpreters talk about the
dilemma of supporting equal participation in terms of being biased or remaining impartial.
In order to strive for impartiality, the interpreters meant that they constantly had to negotiate
and process multiple factors such as gender and race issues, expectations about loyalties of
fellow compatriots or, as in the example outlined above, feelings of hatred and detestation –
f rom the other participants towards her, as well as her own feelings about the person charged
with murder.
41
Kristina Gustafsson
link in the welfare state. It is in these encounters that democratic practices are supposed to
take place and where trust in society and its institutions is supposed to be built.
In this chapter, these encounters have been placed in a context of postcolonial migration
characterised by features that are typical of colonial encounters, that is, they are inflicted by
administrative power, they sometimes include different and incompatible worldviews and
they are asymmetrical, multilingual, and unequal. Moreover, an important consideration
is that interpreters have a unique position as both witnesses and participants in these multi-
lingual encounters and that ethnographic interviewing is a method that makes it possible to
document and analyse their experiences in both capacities.
A significant gain with ethnographic interviews is the detailed and reflexive narratives
of fi rst-hand observations and the way the interview itself creates an opportunity to take
a step back and articulate the lived experiences in the process of making them more com-
prehensive, open for interpretation, and abstract (Davies 2008). An o ften-reported problem
with this method is that collecting and analysing a rich and voluminous material is time
consuming and that it is at times difficult to classify information properly. Furthermore, to
present such a rich material in a fair way within the limits of a book chapter is challenging.
This question goes back to the careful and sometimes painful work of writing an accurate
ethnography based on only a few (in this chapter mainly two) selected examples that repre-
sent both the breadth and the depth of the material. This is often the most difficult task in
the entire research process.
Although it has not been possible to analyse all aspects of interpreters’ experiences and
knowledge of multilingual public service encounters within the limits of this chapter, some
layers of knowledge have been highlighted. First, as witnesses, interpreters get unique in-
sights into encounters that take place behind closed doors, and such encounters are reflected
in their narratives. These narratives could be useful for disciplines such as social work, health,
and medical care, and for continuing education of those who work in various welfare insti-
tutions. They provide for a better understanding of linguistic power asymmetries and may
help develop skills in how to use interpreting especially in asymmetric situations (Westlake
and Jones 2017) (see also Radanovic Felberg and Sagli, this volume).
Second, the ethnographic interviews include narratives about how impartiality and the
aim of favouring equal participation can be negotiated and processed during an encounter
depending on various factors that appear both inside and outside the encounter. Such factors
could include interpreters’ critical opinions about administrative procedures and welfare
policies, or their perceptions of the expectations of both welfare professionals and possibly
fellow-compatriot clients on a more personal level. Many interpreters have experiences that
are similar to those of their clients or have a working situation in which they meet the same
clients repeatedly. Another factor that might affect interpreters is prejudice and racist be-
haviour of welfare professionals towards their clients. Hence, it should be possible to learn
more about how impartiality and equal participation is negotiated in practice and in a way
that acknowledges that people always have a starting point and always have prior knowledge.
This is seen in the interpreters’ narratives and is something they addressed when interviewed.
Third, from a societal perspective, we can see how interpreters’ narratives provide us with
valuable insights and information about both positive and negative sides of society. They
give examples of encounters that work out to everyone’s satisfaction, and also accounts of
oppression, dominance, linguistic power asymmetries, and structural discrimination. Inter-
preters who have worked a long time in the public service sector have unique insights also
into the historical development of encounters between public institutions and minorities.
Thus, more robust analyses of the unique – but through ethnographic interviews, also more
42
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
g eneral – perspectives of public service interpreters might be fruitful to add to the otherwise
quite sparse literature on interpreters’ perceptions; of language competences, of their work
in various public institutions, of social work, health and medical care, education, the Police,
and the judiciary system.
Research on the perceptions of public service interpreters provides us with opportuni-
ties to investigate encounters between welfare professionals and clients in the context of
global migration. More specifically, this research allows us to explore the dilemmas that
interpreters face on duty. Both p erspectives – that is, the perspective of interpreters’ as
witnesses, on the one hand, and as participants in these asymmetrical encounters, on the
other – are crucial in understanding contemporary work in multilingual welfare set-
tings. Examples can demonstrate how linguistic asymmetries can be bridged, and how
this might promote linguistic and social justice. To my mind, research on interpreters’
perspectives needs to be further developed in the future, preferably in multidisciplinary
research environments.
Further reading
Davies, Charlotte, A. (2008). Reflexive Ethnography. A Guide to Researching Selves and Others. London,
Routledge.
This book provides the reader with extensive descriptions and examples of how to conduct ethnography,
including observations, ethnographic interviews, the use of visual methods and the internet, as well as ways of
structuring, analysing and writing up.
Hall, Jonathan and Sonia Valdiviezo (2020) “The social worker as a language worker in a multilingual
world. Educating for language competence”, Journal of Social Work Education 56 (1): 17–29.
In this paper the authors situate social work and other public welfare professions in multilingual contexts.
They explore linguistic competences that include using interpreters and ways towards achieving a more conscious
use of languages in welfare settings.
Piller, Ingrid ( 2016) Linguistic Diversity and Social Justice. An Introduction to Applied Sociolinguistics.
Oxford, Oxford University Press.
In this book, Piller explores and explains how linguistic justice can be used as a lens through which power
asymmetries in encounters between individuals, groups and societies might be understood and politically changed.
Young, Iris Marion ( 1997) “ A symmetrical reciprocity: On moral respect, wonder and enlarged
thought”, Constellations 3 (3): 340–63.
In this classical paper, Young critically discusses the ideal of understanding and imagining oneself in the situa-
tion of the other as a way of overcoming power asymmetries. She adds insights into how to address power asym-
metries, not by eliminating them, but by acknowledging them and showing respect to others by listening to them.
Related chapters
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in public service interpreting by Claudio Baraldi
Chapter 11, Vulnerable encounters? Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services for victim survivors
of domestic violence and abuse by Rebecca Tipton
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalisation of interpret-
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
Notes
1 All quotes come from interviews collected within The Interpreter Project, described in this chapter.
They are all in Swedish (my translation here) and the codes show the number of the interviewee
(1–26), the number of the interview (1–3) and the page in the transcript.
2 The Interpreter Project [Swe: Tolkprojektet] included a series of studies on migration and foreign res-
idents’ integration starting in 2008–2011 and was developed through several follow-up studies.
43
Kristina Gustafsson
The researchers involved in the project were Dr Eva Norström, researcher and project leader,
Dr Kristina Gustafsson, researcher and assistant project leader, and Dr Ingrid Fioretos, researcher.
The team also included several administrators and interpreters as consultants at various times, for
example, in the design and distribution of an online survey about the experiences of interpreters.
References
Abdelhady, Dalia, Nina Gren, and Martin Joormann (eds) (2020) Refugees and the Violent Welfare
Bureaucracies in Northern Europe. Manchester, Manchester University Press.
Akin, Deniz (2017) “Queer asylum seekers: Translating sexuality in Norway”, Journal of Ethnic and
Migrations Studies 43 (3): 4 58–74.
Angu, Pineteh (2018) “Disrupting western epistemic hegemony in South African universities. Cur-
riculum, social justice and agency in post-apartheid South Africa”, International Journal of Learner
Diversity and Identities 25 (1–2): 9 –22.
Blommaert, Jan ( 2013) Ethnography, Superdiversity and Linguistic Landscapes. Bristol, Multilingual
Matters.
Chand, Ashok (2005) “Do you speak English? Language barriers in child protection social work with
minority ethnic families”, British Journal of Social Work 35: 807–21.
De Jong, Sara (2015) “Female migrants as mediators between two worlds. S patio-temporal articu-
lations of intersectional positions”, DiGeSt. Journal of Diversity and Gender Studies 2 (1–2): 111–26.
——— (2016) “Cultural brokers in post-colonial migration regimes”, Negotiating Normativity, Rir-
handu M ageza-Barthel, Nikita Dhawan, Elisabeth Fink, and Johanna Leinius (eds). New York,
Springer International Publishing: 4 5–59.
Dominelli, Lena (2018) Antiracist Social Work. New York, Palgrave Macmillan.
Elsrud, Torun (2014) “Othering the ‘Other’. Threats to self-presentation during interpreter assisted
hearings”, International Journal of Law, Language & Discourse 4 (1): 27–68.
Elsrud, Torun, Philip Lalander, and Annika Staaf (2017) “Noise, voice and silencing during immigrant
court case performances in Swedish district courts”, Ethnicities 17 (5): 667–87.
Emerson, Robert M., Rachel I. Fretz, and Linda L. Shaw (2011) Writing Ethnographic Fieldnotes. 2nd ed.
Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.
Fioretos, Ingrid, Kristina Gustafsson, and Eva Norström (2020) Tolkade möten. Tolkningens betydelse för
rättssäkerhet och integration. Lund, Studentlitteratur.
Fur, Gunlög (2017) “Concurrences as a methodology for discerning concurrent histories”, in Con-
current Imaginaries, Postcolonial Worlds: Towards Revised Histories, Diana Brydon, Peter Forsgren, and
Gunlög Fur (eds). Leiden, Brill/Rodopi: 33–57.
Gerrish, Kate, Chui Man Chau, Abi Sobowale, and Elisabeth Birks (2004) “Bridging the language
barrier: The use of interpreters in primary care nursing”, Health and Social Care in the Community
12 (5): 407–13.
Granhagen Jungner, Johanna, Elisabet Tiselius, Klas Blomgren, Kim Lützén, and Pernilla Pergert
(2019) “L anguage barriers and the use of professional interpreters: A national multisite cross-
sectional survey in paediatric oncology care”, Journal Acta Oncologica 58 (7 ): 1015–20.
Grzymala-Kazlowska, Aleksandra, and Jenny Phillimore (2018) “Introduction: Rethinking integra-
tion: New perspectives on adaptation and settlement in the era of super-diversity”, Journal of Ethnic
and Migration Studies 44 (2): 1 79–96.
Gustafsson, Kristina, Eva Norström, and Ingrid Fioretos (2012) “Community interpreter training in
spoken languages in Sweden”, IJIE. International Journal of Interpreter Education 4 (2): 2 4–38.
——— (2013) The interpreter – A cultural broker?”, in Interpreting in a Changing Landscape. Selected
Papers from Critical Link 6, Christina Schäffner, Krzysztof Kredens and Yvonne Fowler (eds).
Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 187–202.
Gustafsson, Kristina (2021) “Child language brokering in Swedish welfare institutions. A matter of
structural complicity?”, in Translating A symmetry – Rewriting Power, Carbonell, Ovidi and Esther
Monzó-Nebot (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 125–4 4.
Gustafsson, Kristina, Eva Norström, and Petra Höglund (2019) “Language interpreting and broker-
ing in Swedish public service institutions: The use of children for multilingual communication”,
Revista de Llengua i Dret, Journal of Language and Law 71: 13–26.
44
Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters
Hadziabdic, Emina (2011) The Use of Interpreter in Healthcare: Perspectives of Individuals, Healthcare Staff
and Families. Växjö, Linnaeus University Press.
Haralambous, Betty, Jean Tinney, Dina LoGiudice, Lee Meng Soo, and Lin Xiaoping ( 2018)
“Interpreted-mediated cognitive assessment: Who wins and who loses?”, Clinical Gerontologist 41
(3): 2 27–36.
Harrison, Gai (2006) “Broadening the conceptual lens on language in social work. Difference, diver-
sity and English as a global language”, British Journal of Social Work 36: 401–18.
——— (2007) “L anguage as a problem, a right or a resource?: A study of how bilingual practitioners
see language policy being enacted in social work”, Journal of Social Work 7: 7 1–92.
Herlihy, Jane, and Stuart Turner (2007) “Memory and seeking asylum”, European Journal of Psychother-
apy and Counseling 9 (3): 267–76.
Holstein, James A., and Jaber Gubrium (2003) Inside Interviewing. New Lenses, New Concerns. London,
SAGE Publications.
Kale, Emine, and Hammad Raza Syed (2010) “Interpreters in health care. Language barriers and the
use of interpreters in the public health services. A q uestionnaire-based survey”, Patient Education
and Counseling 81: 187–91.
Kammarkollegiet 2019 [The Legal, Financial and Administrative Services Agency 2019] God Tolksed:
Kammarkollegiets Råd till Auktoriserade Tolkar. Stockholm, Kammarkollegiet.
Kjelsvik, Björghild (2014) “Winning a Battle, But Losing the War”: Contested identities, narratives,
and interaction in asylum interviews”, Text & Talk 34 (1): 89–115.
Kriz, Katrin, and Marit Skivenes (2010) “L ost in translation: How child welfare workers in Norway
and England experience language difficulties when working with minority ethnic families”, British
Journal of Social Work 40: 1353–67.
Mains, Susan P., Mary Gilmartin, Declan Cullen, Robina Muhammad, Divya, P. Tolia-Kelly, Parvati
Raghuram, and Jamie Winders (2013) “Postcolonial migrations”, Social & Cultural Geography 14
(2): 131–4 4.
Meissner, Fran, and Steven Vertovec (2015) “Comparing super-diversity”, Ethnic and Racial Studies 38
(4): 541–55.
Mignolo, Walter D., and Catherine E. Walsh (2018) On Decolonality. Concepts, Analytics, Praxis. Durham,
NC/L ondon, Duke University Press.
Norström, Eva, Ingrid Fioretos, and Kristina Gustafsson (2012) “Working conditions of community
interpreters in Sweden: opportunities and shortcomings”, Interpreting: International Journal of Research
and Practice in Interpreting 14 (2): 242–60.
O’Reilly, Karen (2012) Ethnographic Methods, 2nd edition. London, Routledge.
Plejert, Charlotta, Eleonor Antelius, Maziar Yazdanpanah, and Rune T. M. Nielsen (2015) “‘There’s a
Letter Called ef ’ on challenges and repair in i nterpreter-mediated tests of cognitive functioning in
dementia evaluations: A case study”, Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology 30: 163–87.
Puumala, Eeva, Riitta Ylikomi, and Hanna Leena Ristimäki (2017) “Giving an account of persecu-
tion: The dynamic formation of asylum narratives”, Journal of Refugee Studies 31 (2): 197–215.
Riessman, Catherine. K. (2008) Narrative Methods for the Human Sciences. Los Angeles, CA, Sage
Publications.
Sanders, Mark (2007) Ambiguities of Witnessing: Law and Literature in the Time of a Truth Commission.
Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.
Staaf, Annika, and Torun Elsrud (2018) “Competent interpreters and enabling working conditions in
court: Crucial prerequisites for a fair trial”, Retfaerd (Nordisk tidskrift) 3 –4: 61–75.
Tipton, Rebecca (2016) “Perceptions of the ‘occupational other’: Interpreters, social workers and in-
tercultures”, British Journal of Social Work 46 (2): 463–79.
Torstensson, Niklas (2010) Judging the Immigrant: Accents and Attitudes. Umeå, Institutionen för språk-
studier, Umeå universitet.
Westlake, David, and Rebecca K. Jones (2018) “Breaking down language barriers: A p ractice-near
study of social work using interpreters”, The British Journal of Social Work 48 (5): 1388–408.
Young, Iris, Marion (1990) Justice and the Politics of Difference. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press.
45
3
AGENCY IN AND FOR
MEDIATING IN PUBLIC SERVICE
INTERPRETING
Claudio Baraldi
Introduction
Studies of interaction have shown that interpreters’ activity definitely has an impact on com-
munication, not least to make the latter possible (e.g. Wadensjö 1998; Angelelli 2004; Gavi-
oli 2015b; Baraldi 2017). Interpreter’s agency is exercised in a number of ways and it is hardly
possible to provide a full account of the complexity of this notion. My focus here will be on
interpreters’ exercise of agency which leads to change in the structures of the interaction,
with a possible consequent impact also on the structure of the social system the interaction
is part of. In other words, interpreters acting in, for example the healthcare system can lead
to change the structure of interaction of the healthcare system with a possible impact on the
healthcare system as a whole. I will concentrate on this view, thus excluding those equally
legitimate conceptualizations which see agency as possibly including ‘agency for neutral-
ity’ (Tipton 2008b). This chapter focuses on a single contextualizing social system, that of
healthcare, which is used to exemplify the types of connections linking up the construction
of agency in the interpreter-mediated interaction and the comprehensive social system in
which such interaction is included. The chapter is not, however, on healthcare interpreting,
whose discussion is dealt with in Gavioli and Merlini, this volume.
In my literature overview, the idea of agency is discussed in reference to any (employed)
bilingual providing interpreting in healthcare interaction, without distinguishing between
those different types of professionalism that may be involved (e.g. interpreters or mediators).
The reason for this choice is that agency is here regarded as a combined product of a social
system and of interactions included in that system, not necessarily connected to the indi-
vidual professional status (see Baraldi and Gavioli 2012). The discussion of the interactional
conditions allowing for the exercise of agency by the participant providing interpreting may
instead have consequences for the development of interpreting skills.
Theoretical considerations concern the meaning of agency in coordinating interpreter-
mediated interaction with reference to the social systems in which individual interactions
are included: the complexity of the conditions in which interpreters’ agency is constructed
is then highlighted. The section on critical issues and topics includes some transcribed audio
recordings of interpreter-mediated interactions in the healthcare system, which are analysed
as examples of the specific conditions of interpreters’ agency within this system.
Literature overview
Several studies on Public Service Intepreting (PSI) show that interpreters do take autono-
mous initiatives in interpreter-mediated interactions, since they have access to and produce
knowledge in different languages. Their initiative in doing so may be labelled as agency.
As shown in the literature, interpreters’ agency is manifested through actions that affect
the process and outcome of interpreter-mediated interactions. The concept of coordination
(Wadensjö 1998) is a key one to understand the meaning of agency in interpreter-mediated
interactions. Wadensjö distinguishes between implicit coordination and explicit coordina-
tion. Implicit coordination shows interpreters’ agency in rendering, particularly through the
choices that are made in selecting among different types of renditions. Explicit coordination
shows interpreters’ agency through autonomous initiatives that are not renditions of utter-
ances. Such are requests for clarification, comments on translations, requests to comply with
the t urn-taking order, invitations to start or continue talking. The concept of explicit coor-
dination through the use of n on-renditions highlights interpreters’ agency as it shows that,
in the interaction, the involvement of participants in making meanings and purposes clear is
necessary to allow for rendering in the other language (see also Davidson 2002).
Inghilleri (2005) defines interpreting as a zone of uncertainty, in which interpreters can
either reproduce existing power relations, by acting in favour of the institution, or create
opportunities for improving understanding of migrants’ expressions. In her view, agency
highlights ‘the potential for interpreters to exert equal or greater control over interpreting ac-
tivity, even where this involves the disruption of p re-established power relations’ (Inghilleri
2005: 76). So, the latter type of interpreter agency has probably a stronger impact: disrupting
and bringing change to pre-established power relations means recognizing migrants as social
agents (Tipton 2008b) and empowering disadvantaged parties (Mason and Ren 2012).
There are studies (Van Langhenove and Harré 1999:24) that have defined agency as the
choice of a specific course of action among various possible ones. While this concept has been
introduced in sociology to show that society is not completely predefined by social structures
(Giddens 1984), it may apply to conditions of interpreter-mediated interaction as well. In
interpreter-mediated interaction, agency is visible through the interpreter’s positioning. The
concept of positioning represents what actually happens in the interaction (Van Langenhove
and Harré 1999). In Winslade and Monk’s view, positioning means that social roles adapt
‘to the subtleties and nuances of m oment-by-moment interaction’ (2008: 98). Positioning
represents the way in which participants’ actions show up as deliberate choice, intelligible
as such in the interaction. Interpreters can consequently exercise agency by positioning in
the interaction. Angelelli (2004), for instance, mentions different types of positioning in
healthcare interpreting which show interpreters’ agency; some are co-constructing mean-
ings, setting communication rules, controlling/fi ltering the information flow, paraphrasing
or explaining terms or concepts, sliding the message up and down the register scale, aligning
with or replacing one of the parties. Agency, however, cannot be considered as the product
of a single individual positioning since it is produced in social interactions. Mason (see 2009
and this volume) accounts for positioning as a structure of interaction in that (a ll) participants
‘position themselves and others and are, in turn, affected by each other’s positionings’ (2009:
59). Being based on such interplay of conversational moves, positioning displays interpreters’
agency in the interaction and impacts on the interaction structures.
Taking a different perspective, Gavioli (2015a) discusses the structure of positioning by
highlighting the ways in which healthcare providers ‘authorize’ interpreters to exercise
agency in exploring migrant patients’ needs and requests, as well as in explaining issues
47
Claudio Baraldi
related to medical consultation. In this case, interpreters’ agency is based on providers’ at-
tribution of epistemic authority to the interpreters, that is attribution of rights and respon-
sibility of access to and production of knowledge in the terms of Heritage and Raymond
(2005). Although attribution of epistemic authority leaves it up to the interpreters to choose
the best way to convey recommendation or reassurance in the patient’s language, in Gavioli’s
perspective, interpreters are not ‘co-diagnosticians’ (Hsieh 2007). Rather they are acknowl-
edged and recognized in their right and responsibility of dealing with bilingual communi-
cation, that is to act in the interaction, trusting that they know how to handle participants’
contents and purposes bilingually. This attribution of rights and responsibilities is not always
explicit, rather it may be the result of implicit attribution of trust in interpreters’ ability to
transfer information in the ways relevant to the purposes of the interaction (see Baraldi and
Gavioli 2007, 2021). In this perspective, in order to be exercised, interpreters’ agency needs
to be acknowledged and legitimized by institutional providers (Tipton 2008a) and also by
the laypeople participating in the interaction.
To sum up, according to the literature, interpreters’ agency is based on structures of posi-
tioning which guide the actions of interpreters, institutional providers and laypeople. These
structures may lead to the attribution (to the interpreters) of their rights of access to knowl-
edge and responsibilities for producing it and thus to the recognition (expectation) of the
interpreters’ possibility to choose ways of acting, that is to exercise agency in coordinating
interpreter-mediated interactions.
Theoretical considerations
a Agency as a form of active participation
Following Wadensjö (1998), the concept of agency highlights the importance of in-
terpreters’ choices for their renditions and non-renditions. By observing the structure of
positioning, however, agency is shown by intelligible choices of actions (Van Langenhove
and Harré 1999), that is actions which clearly manifest the speakers’ autonomous selec-
tion among a range of options. While in a bilingual type of interaction (l ike interpreter-
mediated interaction) not all actions are intelligible by all participants, some are. This is
for instance the case when interpreters shift immediately to the other language (or not)
or when they provide feedback channels positioning themselves as listeners, one inter-
locutor as teller and the other one as (temporary) by-stander. Since positioning involves
each participant, participants are likely to react to their (attributed) positioning, for
example accepting or rejecting it.
While Wadensjö’s concept of coordination first allowed for the possibility of observ-
ing interpreters’ agency in the interaction, it does not distinguish between what is active
participation and what is in fact agency in the terms adopted in this discussion. Active
participation is shown through any turn taken in talk by the interpreter, to take the view
of a w
ell-known interactional approach called Conversation Analysis (Gavioli 2015b).
Does this mean that any active participation of interpreters (a s well as of their interloc-
utors) can be considered as an expression of agency? To answer this question, a problem
needs to be solved that is the relationship between action, which makes participation
visible, and structural change. Although Giddens’ (1984) discussion of agency lends itself
to multiple interpretations (see e.g. Tipton 2008b or Inghilleri 2005), the view I am
maintaining here is that action can be considered as an expression of agency if it leads to
social change. Social change is particularly related to the structure of positioning in the
interaction and may have an impact on the structure of roles in the overarching social
48
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
system. Let us, for instance, take the medical system. Here, the positioning of the doctor
and the interpreter, collaborating (or non-collaborating) to make an explanation clear
may have an impact on the involvement (or non-involvement) of the patient and thus
lead to change in the hierarchy of roles. While the structure of positioning may be visi-
ble to participants in the interaction, social change produced through interpreters’ exer-
cise of agency becomes visible (and intelligible) only through the analysis of interaction.
A view of agency as leading to social change poses an interesting challenge. Agency,
in this view, is not necessarily a synonym of effective encouragement and support of ac-
tive participation. Indeed, agency may prevent patients’ participation and expression, as,
for example shown by Davidson (2000) about interpreters impeding doctors’ access to
patients’ untranslated narratives. Favouring participants’ access to services or allowing
for their expression of worry, however, involves interpreters’ exercise of agency, even
allowing for the possibility that they fail. While effective involvement in participation
may be achieved even without exercising agency, for example by automatically orient-
ing to normativity, effective interpreting and agency are associated when social change
is produced by encouraging and supporting (a ll) participants’ active involvement and
autonomous participation. Largely speaking, effective interpreting is produced when
interpreters’ selection of choice is exercised in a way as to produce a change of structure
of interactions that is beneficial to the accomplishment of the participants’ tasks and rap-
port, by preventing or reducing hierarchical structures of positioning, due to language
barriers and/or ways of communicating. This explains why, as mentioned in the first
section of this chapter, the interactional conditions allowing for the exercise of agency
necessitate development of interpreting skills so strongly.
So, while interpreters are active even in reproducing knowledge, they are agents when
they produce it, that is when they make their choices intelligible in the interaction, show-
ing their epistemic authority. Interpreters’ agency is thus based on the possibility of pro-
duction of ‘k nowledge in communication’ and is made intelligible through (1) modified
renditions, that is expanded, summarized, or reduced renditions (Wadensjö 1998), and
(2) non-renditions which show autonomous elaboration of other participants’ utter-
ances. In other words, interpreters’ agency is made intelligible through reflexive coordi-
nation that is when their actions focus on the ways in which the communication process
is produced (see Luhmann 1995 on the idea of reflexivity and Baraldi 2017, Baraldi and
Gavioli 2016 for its application to interpreter-mediated interaction).
Reflexive coordination means that interpreters’ actions focus on communication,
in whatever way it manifests itself. Reflexivity in interpreters’ renditions and non-
renditions are actions which clarify opportunities or stress problems/doubts concerning
information and actions in communication processes. This happens, in particular, when
interlocutors’ utterances need further investigation, because they include relevant infor-
mation or intentions, because they are complicated or unclear, because they are delicate.
For instance, interpreters may provide expanded renditions which clarify previous in-
formation (e.g. the meaning and purpose of a medical test) or the speaker’s intentions
(e.g. the reasons for the doctor’s prescription), which may sometimes create problems of
acceptance (e.g. the test is delicate or perceived as invasive). Reflexive coordination, as
well as agency, does not necessarily encourage and support the interlocutors’ active par-
ticipation (e.g. an interpreter may stress patients’ problems which are not relevant in the
context of the examination thus contrasting the health care provider’s work). So reflex-
ive coordination is not per se a synonym of good interpreting, but only a presupposition
for (possibly good) interpreters or mediators to exercise their agency.
49
Claudio Baraldi
advance the prior report by finding a point in the prior utterance and thus shifting
its focus, redeveloping its gist, making something explicit that was previously im-
plicit in the prior utterance, or by making inferences about its presuppositions or
implications (1985: 104).
50
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
As discussed above, rendering is, however, not the sole activity of interpreter agents.
Interpreters’ agency can also be exercised through non-relaying, through zero or non-
renditions. Let me focus on non-renditions which can effectively empower interlocu-
tors’ agency. N on-renditions are produced in interactions between the interpreter and
another participant thus producing monolingual sequences, which are known in inter-
preting interactional studies as ‘dyadic sequences’ (Davidson 2002). Dyadic sequences
allow for brief or longish talk about matters which concern the possibility of rendering
in one way or another, thus giving space to the interlocutors to negotiate what they want
to say (together with their right to say so). In studies on monolingual conflict manage-
ment, negotiation includes the ways in which the parties deal with their different or
opposite preferences (Carnevale and Pruitt 1992).
In interpreter-mediated interactions, negotiation applies when the interpreter and
another participant contribute to reach an agreement on shared objectives. Negotia-
tion involves the interpreter and another participant in dyadic sequences of interaction
where language is shared. Negotiation is particularly important when the interpreter
and another participant deal with what is to be rendered to the third participant (Gavioli
2015a). However, negotiation may also include production of small talk between the
interpreter and another participant (e.g. with the function of creating emotional ties and
possibly speak about ‘not-easy’ items more relaxedly, see Penn and Watermeyer 2012),
and organization of new doctor-patient encounters in which the interpreter should be
present to translate (Baraldi and Gavioli 2020). Negotiation has an important funcstion
in contributing to make rendition possible, thus when dyadic sequences involve negoti-
ation as for what needs to be rendered, they cannot be considered side sequences in the
term of Jefferson (1972) and are instead functional to the provision of rendition. Clearly,
such function is accomplished if renditions actually occur following dyadic sequences.
Summing up, PSI as a mediation system includes the combination of two distinc-
tive actions, bilingual transformative relaying (based on renditions) and monolingual
negotiation (based on n on-renditions), both of which require exercise of agency on the
part of interpreters. The combination of mediation and negotiation creates the complex
conditions for interpreters’ agency.
c Conditions for interpreters’ agency
Local dynamics of interpreter-mediated interactions manifest the ways in which the
mediation system is generated and thus highlight the conditions in which reflexive co-
ordination and interpreters’ agency are manifested. Such conditions are established by
the structures of the social systems in which interactions are included (Luhmann 1995).
These social structures are of key importance in establishing conditions of action and
they are based on social expectations. In particular, social roles are the predefined (in
each system) structures allowing for local structures of positioning. Positioning can r e-
d esign roles locally, but it cannot change their structural function in the social system.
The differences among the social systems determine different conditions of inter-
action, including interpreter-mediated interaction. It is not the aim of this chapter to
provide a comparative analysis between different social systems, rather the focus is on
the dynamics in which conditions are established for the production of a system of me-
diation. As mentioned above, here I will concentrate on a single social system, that of
healthcare. Although the healthcare system is not a simple one and it is thus impossible
to represent its internal differentiations in all their aspects, the variety represented shares
the function of providing care for illness (Luhmann 1990), and the same social roles, that
is healthcare providers and patients. Distribution of agency, in the healthcare system,
51
Claudio Baraldi
is based on the possibility of the social role of the healthcare providers to establish the
attribution of the rights of access to medical knowledge and the responsibilities for its
production.
The identification of social roles in medical (interpreter-mediated) interaction ex-
plains the conditions of interpreters’ exercise of agency. The healthcare provider has the
right and responsibility of managing the interaction with patients who are in need or
obligation to get access to the system. The role of patients makes sense of the function
of the system (that is in fact ‘used’ by the patients). Patients provide relevant information
about their health problems, their worries, and the obstacles they encounter in following
medical therapies or in planning future examinations or tests. Without patients produc-
ing knowledge during medical encounters, treatment of illness is impossible. Roles of
provider and patient condition the role of interpreter as the expert in bilingual commu-
nication, in particular interpreter’s rights and responsibility of getting access to and pro-
ducing knowledge bilingually. The interplay between roles of providers, patients, and
interpreters is visible through specific structures of positioning. Participants’ positioning
shows the different distributions of epistemic authority and thus the different conditions
of agency in specific interactions. Thus, the interplay between actions of providers,
patients, and interpreters and the structure of positioning can only be understood by
analysing specific interpreter-mediated events in the healthcare system.
52
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
Preliminary negotiation
The most frequent form of preliminary negotiation includes providers’ indications about
what should be rendered and mediators’ minimal responses which actively show understand-
ing and invite continuation. Gavioli (2012) shows the function of minimal responses in coor-
dinating participation. Mediator’s contribution to this preliminary negotiation is ‘m inimal’
allowing for both the clinician to provide a lengthy, complete explanation and the patient to
‘hold on’ for a lengthy rendition to come.
In extract 1, preliminary negotiation is shown in turns 1–23. The gynaecologist (GYNf, f
stands for female) in this case describes a diet for a waiting mother (PATf ), and the mediator
(M EDf ) provides minimal responses, including repetitions of the doctor’s utterances in turns
4 and 17. There are two long pauses occurring after turns 17 and 21 which are treated by the
mediator as suspensions of the clinician’s talk, a suspension which is made clear by both the
grammatical structures and the intonation used. The mediator then does not take the turn
until completion of the doctor’s long talk.
Extract 1 (consultation 1)
01 GYNf per la gravida:nza (.) è meglio mangiare: eh casomai
for the pregnancy (.) it’s better to eat eh possibly
02 MEDf mm
(0.6)
03 GYNf e non saltare i pasti (.) quindi è meglio far (0.3) cola[zio:ne]
and not to skip meals (.) so it’s better to have (.) bre[akfast
04 MEDf [colazione]
[breakfast]
05 GYNf e poi fare il pra: e poi casomai anche a metà mattina mangiare un frutto (.)
colazione come dicevamo può voler dire (.) una volta: non so
and then have lunch and then possibly also at m id-morning to eat some fruit (.) break-
fast as we were saying can mean (.) one time I don’t know
06 MEDf hm hm
07 GYNf posso prendere: il tè se ti pia:ce oppure un s[ucco opp]ure (.) il la:tte (.) okay? e
you can have some tea if you like it or a j[uice or] (.) some milk (.) okay? and
08 MEDf [hm hm]
09 MEDf hm
53
Claudio Baraldi
Rendition starts at turn 24 (below). Here we have an example of what I defined above as
a ‘formulation’. The mediator develops the provider’s advice (by giving examples, such as
that of breakfast), the types of fruit the patient can eat (pear or apple), and the correct way of
eating (slowly, often, and varying the food types), splitting her rendition so as to seek (a nd
allow for) the patient’s feedback (see, e.g. her use of ‘you know’ before pausing). The patient
provides continuing and acknowledging feedback several times (turns 25, 27) and finally
in turn 29 after a longish pause. The mediator’s minimal response in turn 30 functions as a
signal that translation has been accomplished and passes the turn back to the gynaecologist.
24 MEDf mm mm (0.4) that’s (0.3) you during pregnancy is better not to not to you
know (.) miss (.) eh meals (0.2) not to: leave off (0.2) some meals you know
(.) that’s (0.3) you:r you:r breakfast you have to (0.8) your breakfast you need
to have it even though you may not feel hungry but you can only drink
te:a (0.6) you kno:w o:r milk (0.5) or juice (0.6) uhm not (1.0) something
heavy if you are not feeling hungry (0.4) eh just liquid something like
that little then (0.7) later you can mm: (0.3) know take (0.6) a fruit like that
if it is apple: (0.3) o:r pear (0.3) hm hm take a fruit like that’s (0.4) then
54
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
in the afternoon (0.2) you can (0.4) eat (0.4) the: the: soup (0.2) mm: with
semolina (0.3) with semo (0.4) eat soup with semo then in the evening (0.1)
you may only eat (.) you know soup and meats (0.6) eh eh (0.5) without eat-
ing semo again (0.4) [o:r]
25 PATf [°okay]°
26 MEDf you know (0.4) uh uh i: vary: yo:ur your meals (0.3) and that you know
sma:ll (0.3) little little but often (0.3) eh little at tea time little at tea
time (0.5) [mm]
27 PATf [ok]
(0.4)
28 MEDf and vary it
(0.7)
29 PATf °okay°
30 MEDf mm
31 GYNf okay? pensi sia possibile?
do you think it’s possible?
Extract 1 shows a frequent type of occurrence in the analysed corpus, in which preliminary
negotiation, based on the mediator’s minimal feedback to the healthcare providers, is followed
by a (rendered) formulation for the patient, who eventually acknowledges the rendition.
Another form of preliminary negotiation is based on providers’ explicit authorization
to develop renditions (Gavioli 2015a). This case shows very clearly that the upgrading of
providers’ epistemic authority during preliminary negotiation conditions the following up-
grading of mediators’ epistemic authority, both enhancing reflexive coordination. Extract 2
is introduced by the provider’s authorization to the mediator’s exercise of agency (le spieghi-
amo means ‘let us explain’, you and I together). The gynaecologist invitation to collaborate
initiates a negotiation where a screening test, amniocentesis, is mentioned as a possibility the
patient might want to consider. This negotiation is managed in a way that is very similar to
the negotiation we saw in extract 1, with the doctor telling (more briefly in this case) and
the mediator acknowledging her talk. What is interesting to note here is that at the end of
her brief explanation, the gynaecologist encourages the mediator to try and explain herself
to the patient that amniocentesis is a possibly available screening (t urn 13). This encourage-
ment, together with the fi rst-person plural expressed by the verb le spieghiamo authorizes the
mediator to take charge of expressing the content of the doctor’s contribution in the way the
mediator deems appropriate for the patient and the situation. It thus creates the conditions
for the mediator’s exercise of agency. The mediator’s rendition to the patient (starting at turn
14) is a relevant development of the provider’s hasty summary. This development includes
repeated reassurances that what the doctor is saying (v ia the mediator) does not imply a
necessity to take the screening (an invasive one) nor to put any pressure on acting (see the
items in bold in the rendition). The rendition is concluded with a question about the pa-
tient’s interest in going on with such screening, which is followed by a declination voiced by
the patient’s husband (H USm, m stands for male) (turn 15). Note that the mediator designs
the final question as a yes/no alternative to accept the test, thus making declination equally
possible. The mediator then reports the conclusion about the assigned task to the provider,
including the report that she informed the patient, and the patient’s decision (what’s in God’s
mercy). This conclusion and the provider’s confirmation (turn 18) show the uptake of the
mediator’s right and responsibility for exercising agency.
55
Claudio Baraldi
Extract 2 (consultation 2)
12 GYNf allora adesso le spieghiamo che::
so now let’s explain her that
(t urns 2 -12 omitted)
13 GYNf prova mo’
go on try
14 MEDf w fih bitulik fahs ya’ni biyin’aml li lnisaa smiytu fahs amniocentesi (.) ana
ba’tiha kama’luma kawn ya’ni kul imraa lazim ta’raf hadi l ma’luma kawn
fi Italia biyin’amal w ba’dha al insane huwa hur yi’ml li huwa biyiqarar inu
biti’milih aw ma biti’milih hada lfahs huwa ‘ibara ‘a n fahs amniocentesi illi
huwa ‘ibara ‘an unbub daqiq bidakhluh biqarib min sirra biyakhdu maa min
sail li ‘and tifl w biya’malu ‘a lih analisi tahalil hatta yishufu ida kan tifl mzian
walla monghuli walla mush tabi’i (.) hada lfahs biya’maluh likul nisaa li fuq
khamsa wa talatin sana bidun ma yidfa’u ‘a lih fulus (.) nisaa li ‘amraha taht
khamsa wa talatin sana kama’luma bit’rf ha (.) w fih yalli mumkin bitfakar
innu ta’malu halla’ ta’tina khabar liannu nakhudlha maw’id ma’a tabibat aljinat
almas’ula bihadi lhaja fa antum kama’luma kafahs ya’ni ‘r ftu ‘anu (.) w
lakin habin in ta’mlu had l fahs walla [mtamainin w mtawklin ‘a la li Allah ba’t?]
and then she is saying that there is another c heck-up that they have for pregnant women
it’s called the amniocentesis (.) I give this to you as information because all pregnant
women should be informed about it, because in Italy they take it and then everybody is
free to take it or not, each person can decide if they want to have it, this amniocentesis
consists in a small tube that is introduced near the navel and is used to take some liquid
around the baby, with a diagnostic purpose to see if (the baby) is normal or handicapped
or retarded (.) this is a free test for women over thirty-five (.) to take it women under
thirty-five, just to inform you, have to pay (.) if you are interested in taking it, just tell
us soon so that we can make an appointment with the gynaecologist who is specialized
in this area (.) this is an information that you now have (.) so do you want to take
it [or do you put yourselves at the mercy of God?
15 HUSm [mntamainin ‘ala illi Allah ba’tlina iyah]
mntamainin ‘a la illi Allah ba’tlina iyah
[we put ourselves at the mercy of God] we put ourselves at the mercy of God
16 MEDf illi Allah ba’atu
at the mercy of God
17 MEDf a llora (.) come informazione l’abbiamo avuta poi:: (.) quello che
viene da dio::
okay (.) the information we had it then:: (.) what’s in God’s mercy::
18 GYNf o::kay
A couple of points may, thus, be made on preliminary negotiation and their creating condi-
tions for the mediators’ exercise of agency. First, extracts 1–2 show that preliminary negotia-
tion allows for reflexive coordination. Mediators actively participate in the negotiation with
providers showing both providers and patients that they are understanding and preparing for
the following rendition. Providers manifest their epistemic authority in giving advice mean-
while acknowledging mediators’ own epistemic authority in receiving the explanation and
then choosing how it can best be given in the other language. Second, extracts 1–2 show
that preliminary negotiation is followed by mediators’ renditions developing the providers’
talk by designing it for their patients. These renditions are formulations which function as
56
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
‘transformative relaying’ (Heritage and Clayman 2010), showing the mediators’ exercise of
agency and its impact on the distribution of epistemic authority in the interaction. This ren-
dition includes an option between taking or not taking the test, where the negative option is
rendered through a formulaic expression (‘at the mercy of God’). The formulation of the neg-
ative option is based on the mediator’s experience of possible negative cultural presuppositions
about abortion in Arab families. The mediator avoids the impression of advocating for a choice
evoking abortion. The mediator’s formulation is thus designed to encourage the migrants to
decide autonomously. In this case, the choice is expressed by the patient’s husband. It is rela-
tively frequent in my data that husbands participate in this type of consultation, for several pos-
sible reasons, including the fact that several wives arrive in Italy through family reunion when
they are already pregnant and do not feel like getting in touch with public services they are not
familiar with. Thus, husbands may replace wives in providing information and answers. While
the relationships between women and men are clearly a huge and highly debated problem go-
ing outside the purposes of this study, here suffice to note that (1) when (A rab) women in our
data want to have private information, they use the services alone, if a mediator is available;
(2) language mediation is not advocacy for social justice or women’s emancipation; mediators’
empowerment concerns migrants’ opportunities to s elf-express, as single individuals or couples.
Thus, extracts 1–2 show that reflexive coordination, achieved through mediator’s agency,
may need preliminary negotiations in which providers show primary rights and responsibili-
ties for producing knowledge, but still acknowledge (in the negotiation) the mediators’ rights
and responsibility in re-producing it in the other language. Preliminary negotiations are
rather frequent, but not found in all encounters and they definitely do not represent the only
way in which conditions are created for the exercise of interpreters’ agency. There are clearly
other practices creating conditions for expression of interpreters’ agency and acknowledging
their rights and responsibilities for r e-producing knowledge bilingually. Practices moreover
may be connected to the goals of the interaction. Preliminary negotiations however are one
way in which conditions for interpreters’ agency should thus not be censored a priori.
Embedded negotiation
While preliminary negotiations occur as basically long turns, pre-planning what needs to
be rendered to the other interlocutor, embedded negotiations are normally occasional oc-
currences arising from the interaction, showing, for example that something needs to be
(better) clarified or presented with particular care. Embedded negotiations are again dyadic
sequences occurring with the clinicians or the patient, but they typically occur when pa-
tients’ answers are provided which reveal complicated, unclear, or delicate contents. Let me
show one example.
In extract 3, turn 2, the mediator provides a rendition of the doctor’s question (DOCf
is the female doctor in the transcript) about the patient’s attempts to become pregnant. The
patient provides a partial and indirect answer in which she mentions she has taken the con-
traceptive pill. The mediator then treats the patient’s answer as n on-complete and initiates
an embedded negotiation with a continuer which invites the patient to add information.
Since the patient’s new answer is also indirect, the mediator provides two formulations of
what she understood to be the gist of the meaning of the patient’s answer (turns 6 and 8).
Both formulations are provided in interrogative form thus inviting the patient to possibly
confirm the correctness of the mediator’s understanding. The patient confirms and the me-
diator asks a new detail about how long the patient took the pill (turn 10), then she repeats
the patient’s answer to check understanding (turn 12; confirmed in turn 13) and provides a
57
Claudio Baraldi
third interrogative formulation for further confirmation (t urn 14). This insistence in looking
for confirmation is a consequence of the patient’s indirect and thus uninformative way of
answering the initial question. The mediator’s rendition is a formulation of the gist of the
whole exchange including the embedded negotiation and is composed of two parts: (a) a
summary of the patient’s indirect answers to the mediator’s requests (t urn 16), and (b) a con-
clusive answer to the doctor’s question (t urn 18).
Extract 3 (consultation 3)
01 DOCf da quanto tempo è che la cerca?
how long has she been looking for it?
02 MEDf sh’hal ‘ndk dyal l wqt w nti ka tqlbi thmli?
how long have you been looking for pregnancy?
(1.0)
03 PATf eh, eh knt drt l fanid f sh’har hdash
eh, eh I took the pill during the month of November
04 MEDf mhm
05 PATf w mn sh’har thnash w ana ma bqitsh drtu htta l daba
and since December I haven’t taken it until today
06 MEDf ya’ni mn sh’har hdash w nti bagha thmli?
so it is since November that you have been looking for pregnancy?
07 PATf ah
yes
08 MEDf ya’ni hyitti l fanid f sh’har hdash?
so you stopped the pill in November?
09 PATf ah
yes
10 MEDf w sh’hal w nti ka takhdi l fanid?
and how long had you been taking it?
11 PATf sh’har wahd
one month only
12 MEDf sh’har wahd
one month only
13 PATf ah
yes
14 MEDf hiya sh’har hdash?
so November?
15 PATf ah
yes
16 MEDf allora la signora aveva preso la pillola nel mese di novembre
so the lady took the pill during the month of November
17 DOCf =sì
=yes
18 MEDf =l’ha sospesa, e quindi è da dicembre che lo sta cercando.
=she stopped it, so it’s since December that she is looking for it
The analysis of extract 3 highlights that embedded negotiations like the one shown are based
on mediators’ exercise of agency clarifying communication, therefore achieving reflexive
coordination. First, the mediator treats the information provided in the patient’s answer
58
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
as the beginning of something more complex that is to come (since the detail is not per se
relevant to answer the doctor’s question). Second, the mediator investigates the gist of the
patient’s answer by way of minimal responses, autonomous questions, and interrogative for-
mulations of the patient’s utterances, thus encouraging her to go on and tell the whole story.
Such encouragement (and interactional support to go on) is shown by the mediator’s scru-
pulous checking of what the patient means in turns 3 and 5, which helps the patient respond
to doctor’s question. At the end of the negotiation, the mediator provides a rendition in the
form of a formulation for the provider, who shows trust in the mediators’ work, leaving her
and the patient time for talking and waiting for rendition allowing for the continuation of
the consultation.
Extract 3 shows successful mediation through reflexive coordination, since the mediator
takes the responsibility of exploring and eventually clarifying the meaning of the patient’s
answer and can thus suitably design her rendition for the doctor. This embedded negotiation
is important for coordinating the patient’s answer with the doctor’s question (see Davidson
2002). Mediators’ agency is thus achieved through reflexive coordination both inside the
embedded negotiation and as transformative relaying following such negotiation.
Conclusions
The analysis of extracts 1–3 shows forms of organization of interpreter-mediated interaction,
as an expression of a system of language mediation combining (1) negotiations of informa-
tion and intentions relevant to the context, and (2) mediation based on formulations in the
terms of Heritage (1985), but in the other language, re-presenting the gist of the previous
talk. This analysis is relevant to understand the conditions of mediators’ agency and reflexive
coordination and to show some ways in which mediators’ agency can trigger social change,
by both being given and taking the right and responsibility to deal with (sometimes com-
plex) communicative issues in the bilingual interaction.
Interpreter-mediated interactions may include different ways of distributing epistemic
authority and then agency among participants. In particular, reflexive coordination of em-
bedded negotiation with conclusive formulations in the other language shows mediators’
exercise of agency in interpreting the problems and nuances of communication. In prelim-
inary negotiation, agency is distributed among providers, patients, and mediators. The ac-
knowledgment of both the clinician and the patient of the mediator’s agency in interpreting
what needs to be rendered may lead to a collaborative achievement, possibly changing the
interlocutors’ positioning.
In the overall interaction then, reflexive coordination allows for equal distribution of
participation, mutual empowerment, and mutual sensitivity, showing the existence of a lan-
guage mediation system (Baraldi 2017) and a participants’ orientation to it. In this system,
interpreters’ agency is effective if it leads to the empowerment of all participants – which often
means supporting those who are disadvantaged (Baraldi 2019), thus changing the distribu-
tion of epistemic authority in the interaction and consequently the hierarchical structure of
positioning. This change can impact on the hierarchical structure of roles in the overarching
social system when the language mediation system is widespread in this system.
However, there are at least three factors conditioning the exercise of agency and achieve-
ment of reflexive coordination. First, reflexive coordination in a single i nterpreter-mediated
encounter may be strongly influenced by other dyadic or triadic encounters, mediated or not
mediated, involving the same migrant patient. Second, the way of experiencing agency and
achieving reflexive coordination may be conditioned by a shared spoken language between
59
Claudio Baraldi
the provider and the patient. Third, communication may be very confused, so that the me-
diator’s agency is exercised in difficult conditions.
Related topics
One topic that is clearly evoked in what I discussed above is intercultural mediation. The
meaning of intercultural mediation is ambivalent. First, language mediation may certainly
support mutual acceptance and intercultural adaptation between participants, but it may also
undermine them (Baraldi and Gavioli 2017) when interpreters become agents of essential-
ism. Essentialism presents ‘people’s individual behaviour as entirely defined and constrained
by the cultures in which they live so that the stereotype becomes the essence of who they
are’ (Holliday 2011: 4). Thus, an interpreter’s essentialist approach leads to ignore the im-
portance of language mediation in adjusting cultural assumptions in the interaction. Second,
the meaning of ‘intercultural’ in reference to mediation is at least t wo-faceted: mediation is
intercultural not only when it deals with cultural differences but also by narrowing the cul-
tural gap between migrants’ personal problems and social systems (Baraldi 2018b), e.g. that
between medical and n on-medical culture in the healthcare system.
Another interesting related topic, which was touched only partially in this chapter is
problematic exercise of interpreters’ agency, which can undermine effectiveness of PSI.
Problematic exercise of agency is visible through the anomalous positioning of interpreters
as institutional gatekeepers. In this chapter I mentioned the case in which interpreters select
information they believe to be diagnostically relevant, providing zero or reduced renditions
of some contents in laypeople’s talk (Davidson 2000; Hsieh 2007; see also Bolden 2000). In
this way, interpreters subtract epistemic authority to both providers and seekers (patients in
our case, but this problematic exercise of agency has been observed in other social systems,
see for example Angermeyer 2009; Davitti 2013; Maryns 2013). While this chapter has
maintained that exercise of agency in the (radical) terms I have illustrated here is essential
for the mediation system to trigger change towards more equality and inclusiveness, it needs
to be accompanied by strong interpreters’ expertise, e.g. to handle dyadic (negotiation) se-
quences in order to make relevant, rather than reduce, patients’ views.
Further reading
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2004) “Questioning invisibility” (pp. 7–14); “Finding visibility” (pp. 73–144),
in Medical Interpreting and Cross-cultural Communication, Claudia V. Angelelli (ed). Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
The two chapters, from a book presenting research on healthcare interpreting, highlight several ways in
which interpreters can exercise agency. Interpreters’ agency can have either positive or negative consequences on
interlocutors’ participation: it may produce co-participation and co-construction of meanings, control and filter
in communication and information flow, may be helpful in explaining terms or concepts, may lead to alignment
with or replacement of the parties.
Baraldi, Claudio (2019) “Pragmatics and agency in healthcare interpreting”, in The Routledge Handbook
of Translation and Pragmatics, Rebecca Tipton and Louisa Desilla (eds). London, Routledge: 319–35.
This chapter focuses on the use of the concept of agency to give meaning to interpreters’ activity, in particular
clarifying the ways in which interpreters’ agency affects interpreter-mediated interactions and is in turn affected
by interaction. It analyses the ways in which interpreters’ agency becomes relevant in interactions involving
healthcare professionals and patients, and the ways in which the recipients treat interpreters’ agency.
Inghilleri, Moira (2005) “Mediating zones of uncertainty. Interpreter agency, the interpreting habitus
and political asylums adjudication”, The Translator 11 (1): 69–85.
This article defines the concept of interpreting as a zone of uncertainty, in which interpreters can ex-
ercise agency either reproducing existing power relations, by acting in favour of the institution, or creating
60
Agency in and for mediating in PSI
opportunities for improving understanding of migrants’ expressions. The concept of agency highlights the poten-
tial for interpreters to exert control over interpreting activity.
Tipton, Rebecca (2008a) “Reflexivity and the social construction of identity in interpreter-mediated
asylum interviews”, The Translator 14 (1): 1–19.
This article draws on the sociological theory proposed by Anthony Giddens, exploring the consequences of the
distinction between agency and social structure for interpreting. In this view, interpreters are considered as social agents.
However, interpreters’ agency must be recognized by the institutional providers; this need of recognition gives relevance
to the social context of interpreting, which may establish the practices to which interpreters should commit.
Related chapters
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason
Chapter 11, Vulnerable encounters? Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services for v ictim-survivors
of domestic violence and abuse by Rebecca Tipton
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalisation of interpret-
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
References
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2004) Medical Interpreting and Cross-cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
Angermeyer, Philipp Sebastian (2009) “Translation style and participant roles in court interpreting”,
Journal of Sociolinguistics 13 (1): 3 –28.
Baraldi, Claudio ( 2012) “ Interpreting as dialogic mediation: The relevance of expansions”, in
Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli ( eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 2 97–326.
——— (2017) “L anguage mediation as communication system”, Communication Theory 27: 367–87.
——— (2018a) “Formulations as a way of providing renditions in mediated interactions: A single case
analysis”, Journal of Applied Linguistics and Professional Practice 11 (3): 248–69.
——— (2018b) “Interpreting as mediation of migrants’ agency and institutional support. A case anal-
ysis”, Journal of Pragmatics 125: 13–27.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura. Gavioli (2007) “Dialogue interpreting as intercultural mediation. An
analysis in healthcare multicultural settings”, in Dialogue and Cultures, Marion Grein and Edda
Weigand (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 155–75.
——— (2012) “Introduction”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and
Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 1–21.
——— (2016) “On professional and non-professional interpreting: The case of intercultural media-
tors”, European Journal of Applied Linguistics 4 (1): 33–55.
——— (2017) “Intercultural mediation and ‘(non)professional’ interpreting in Italian healthcare in-
stitutions”, in Non-professional Interpreting and Translation, Rachele Antonini, Letizia Cirillo, Linda
Rossato and Ira Torresi (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 83–105.
——— (2021) “Effective communication and knowledge distribution in healthcare interaction with
migrants”, Health Communication 36 (9): 1059–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1735701
(published online 20 March 2020).
Bolden, Galina B. (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: Interpreters’ in-
volvement in history taking”, Discourse Studies 2 (4): 87–419.
Bush, Robert A. Baruch, and Joseph P. Folger (1994) The Promise of Mediation: Responding to Conflict
Through Empowerment and Recognition. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass.
Carnevale, Peter J., and Dean G. Pruitt (1992) “Negotiation and mediation”, Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy 43: 531–82.
Davidson, Brad (2000) “The interpreter as institutional gatekeeper: The social-linguistic role of inter-
preters in Spanish-English medical discourse”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (3): 3 79–405.
——— (2002) “A model for the construction of conversational common ground in interpreted dis-
course”, Journal of Pragmatics 34: 1 273–300.
61
Claudio Baraldi
Davitti, Elena (2013) “Dialogue interpreting as intercultural mediation: Interpreters’ use of upgrading
moves in parent-teacher meetings”, Interpreting 15 (2): 168–99.
Gavioli, Laura (2012). “M inimal responses in interpreter-mediated medical talk”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 201–28.
——— (2015a) “ On the distribution of responsibilities in treating critical issues in i nterpreter-
mediated medical consultations: The case of ‘le spieghi(a mo)’”, Journal of Pragmatics 76: 159–80.
——— (2015b) “Conversation analysis”, in Researching Translation and Interpreting, Claudia V. Angelelli
and Brian James Baer (eds). London, Routledge: 185–94.
Giddens, Anthony. (1984) The Constitution of Society. Cambridge, Polity Press.
Hepburn, Alexa, and Galina B. Bolden (2017) Transcribing for Social Research. London, Sage.
Heritage, John (1985) “A nalysing news interviews: Aspects of the production of talk for an overhear-
ing audience”, in Handbook of Discourse Analysis, Vol. 3. Discourse and Dialogue, T. van Dijk (ed).
London, Academic Press: 95–117.
Heritage, John, and Steven Clayman (2010) Talk in Action. Interactions, Identities, and Institutions. Chichester,
Wiley-Blackwell.
Heritage, John, and Geoffrey Raymond (2005) “The terms of agreement: Indexing epistemic authority
and subordination in t alk-i n-i nteraction”, Social Psychology Quarterly 68 (1): 15–38.
Holliday, Adrian (2011) Intercultural Communication and Ideology. London, Sage.
Hsieh, Elaine (2007) “Interpreters as co-diagnosticians: Overlapping roles and services between pro-
viders and interpreters”, Social Science & Medicine 64: 924–37.
Jefferson, Geil (1972) “Side sequences”, in Studies in Social Interaction, D.N. Sudnow (ed). New York,
Free Press: 294–33.
Luhmann, Niklas (1990) “Der medizinische code”, in Soziologische Aufklärung 5: Konstruktivistische
Perspektiven, Niklas Luhmann (ed). Opladen, Westdeutscher Verlag: 183–95.
——— (1995) Social Systems. Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.
Maryns, Katrijn (2013) “Disclosure and (re)performance in an interpreter-mediated asylum inter-
view”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 17 (5): 661–86.
Mason, Ian (2006) “On mutual accessibility of contextual assumptions in dialogue interpreting”, Jour-
nal of Pragmatics 38: 359–73.
——— (2009) “Role, positioning and discourse in f ace-to-f ace interpreting”, in Interpreting and Trans-
lation in Public Service Settings. Policy, Practice, Pedagogy, Raquel de Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle A. Perez,
and Christine WL Wilson (eds). Manchester, St. Jerome: 5 2–73.
Mason, Ian, and Wen Ren (2012) “Power in face-to-face interpreting events”, in The Sociological Turn
in Translation and Interpreting Studies, Claudia V. Angelelli (ed). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Ben-
jamins: 234–53.
Penn, Claire, and Jennifer Watermeyer (2012) “W hen asides become central: Small talk and big talk
in interpreted health interactions”, Patient Education and Counseling 88: 391–98.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2008) “Interpreting as mediation”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting.
Definitions and Dilemmas, Carmen Valero-Garces, and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 9 –26.
Tipton, Rebecca (2008b) “Interpreter neutrality and the structure /agency distinction”, in Research and
Practice in Public Service Interpreting and Translation, Carmen Valero-Garces (ed). Alcalá, Universidad
de Alcalá: 183–97.
Van Langenhove, Luk, and Rom Harré (1999) “Introducing positioning theory”, in Positioning Theory,
Rom Harré and Luk van Langenhove (eds). Oxford, Blackwell: 14–31.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
Winslade, John, and Gerald D. Monk (2008) Practicing Narrative Mediation: Loosening the Grip of Conflict.
San Francisco, Jossey-Bass.
62
4
CULTURAL ASSUMPTIONS,
POSITIONING AND POWER
Towards a Social Pragmatics of Interpreting
Ian Mason
Introduction
During the G20 meeting in Osaka in June 2019, the BBC captured a photograph of Donald
Trump shaking hands with Xi Jinping. Trump offers a hearty handshake and a firm grip,
leaning in towards his counterpart and seeking eye contact, as is the American way. Xi
Jinping, on the other hand, offers little eye contact, it being the generally accepted Chinese
cultural assumption that to look someone straight in the eye shows a lack of respect. Here,
in microcosm, is the nub of the matter: the different cultural assumptions that interlocutors
bring to their meetings with others of different cultural backgrounds and the way in which
each party interprets the speech, body language and paralinguistics displayed by others ac-
cording to their own cultural assumptions. Almost automatically we position each other
according to our own cultural assumptions and, once a first impression is formed, we are
seemingly reluctant to shift our point of view. At the same time, we are largely unaware of
our own speech habits, body language and direction of gaze because they are instinctive and
spontaneous – indeed, we attach little importance to them.
The Trump/X i handshake is, however, merely the tip of an iceberg fraught with com-
plication. To begin with, we need to disabuse ourselves of the notion that nation-states are
coterminous with a given culture. Piller (2012, 2017) cogently argues the case for multiple
cultures existing in any society. It is here that the notion of communities of practice makes
sense; as people interact within communities, developing their own discourses and assump-
tions of what needs to be said and what does not, so expectations of common behaviour
begin to form. For example, there is the discourse of laissez-faire capitalism (or ‘neo-l iberal
economics’) or the discourse of Marxism. Or see below under the heading ‘Relevant the-
oretical considerations’, where I comment on the multiplicity of tasks that translators and
interpreters perform. At the m icro-linguistic level, transitivity, agency and cohesion are in-
volved. At a much higher level, discourses, genres and text forms emerge through a semiotic
process. In all of this, we send signs to each other which are either taken up or discarded.
Some voices, however, are stronger than others so that the ability to make oneself heard
varies greatly between the strong and the weak.
In all of these notions, it seems clear that the social context in which communication
takes place impacts the meaning of the language we use quite heavily. In what follows, I
DOI: 10.4324/9780429298202-6 63
Ian Mason
Interpreters can be placed in contradictory positions with respect to their initial and/or
acquired habitus and the norms of specific interpreting contexts and/or the norms of
interpreter training.
Thus, Inghilleri recognises ‘specific interpreting contexts’ and proposes that individual in-
terpreters’ habitus is formed partly in and by their personal background, partly in relation
to settings where they perform as interpreters and partly in their training to become in-
terpreters. (In line with Bourdieu’s theory she notes that individuals have varying habi-
tus). Interpreters may be involved in sign-language interpreting (shift of mode), chuchotage
(simultaneous but whispered), simultaneous conference interpreting (in a soundproof booth),
public service interpreting in a variety of institutional frameworks, each with its own com-
munity of practice. There is a world of difference between, for example, a given country’s
immigration services and its health service. Meanwhile conference interpreters, often mem-
bers of AIIC, have their own community of practice and engage, while interpreting, with
many others. Many would argue that public service interneters also have their own com-
munity of practice. That is definitely the case with those who are professionally trained and
certified, including spoken as well as signed language interpreters.
Translators may be sub-titlers, summarisers, be required to design an effective text to
match a promotional source text or required to adhere strictly to a legal text. It seems to me
that professional translators and interpreters become temporary members of several commu-
nities of practice, during which they either fully espouse the community’s practice or feel
themselves to be temporary and on the margins. Wenger’s theory involves identity, with its
sub-components of identities of participation and identities of non-participation. Broadly, the con-
cept is that we may feel fully aligned to a community of practice or only partly so, a feeling of
being an outsider as it were. For example, through membership of professional associations,
we may feel fully aligned with our profession and participate in discussion boards with other
interpreters. On the other hand, if we receive negative feedback on our interpretation or find
our suggestions rejected, we are bound to feel marginalised, at least to some extent. Positive
feedback, naturally, has the opposite effect. The same goes for interpreters at international
conferences and face-to-face interpreters. Many interpreters at international conferences
have experienced the feeling of being marginalised by conference delegates or treated as
non-insiders. On the other hand if, as a public service interpreter, the immigration or medi-
cal service adopts you as one of their own you rapidly become part of the institution – with,
occasionally, an interpreting stance to match, assuming the position of c o-interlocutor in
interviews (see, e.g. Bolden 2000, Mason and Stewart 2001).
64
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
Less often, it seems, the interpreter will seek to give more ‘voice’ (Blommaert 2005: 68)
to the less powerful party in an exchange, an asylum seeker, for example, or a patient (Barsky
1996; Hale 1997; Angelelli 2004). And it is here that I come to my second key concept: po-
sitioning. By direction of gaze, body posture, pronominal use, facial expression, etc., we as
participants in interaction can position the less powerful party as included or excluded. We
can also position ourselves (‘reflexive positioning’), or position others (‘interactive position-
ing’, Davies and Harré 1990:48). An example from Wadensjö (1998: 179–86) will illustrate
this. The situation is a standard medical c heck-up.
The nurse, Nelly, meets a family, consisting of the seven-year-old child Clara, her new-
born sister, their mother, Marta, father, Felix, and a grandmother, Galia, all recently
arrived in Sweden from the USSR.
(Wadensjö 1998: 180)
The interpreter, Inna, relays a ‘gently-spoken proposal’ from the nurse, Nelly:
Nelly: då ska vi se. ska du få sitta här.
then let us see. you may sit here.
Inna: садись там. (.) и я с тобой сяду.
sit there. (.) and I will sit with you.
The child then silently starts to cry at the sight of a needle. Later on, the interpreter Inna,
again adopting a stance close to the child, says (t ranslating nurse Nelly):
Inna: и знаешь что. когда они тебе уже помазали этим кремом.
and you know what. when they have smeared you with this cream.
когда они будут тебе помазать. тогда совсем ничего не чувствуется.
when they smear you. Then you don’t feel anything at all.
(Adapted from Wadensjö (1998: 179–86)
Thus, the interpreter stays close to the child, sits beside her and adds ‘I will sit with you’ and
‘a nd you know what?’, seeking to create empathy with the child. It is a case of interactive
positioning.
Finally, I come to my third concept: power. Pragmatics-based accounts of interpreting
and conversation analysis accounts appear to assume that each participant in an encoun-
ter enjoys equal power in a speech exchange. Blommaert (2005) is highly critical of both
approaches and also of critical discourse analysis. All three approaches, in his view, fail to
acknowledge the vast inequalities of power among participants: from asylum seekers to
asylum adjudicators, from patients to doctors, from accused persons to judges, the disparity
is patent, but often overlooked. It is here that I would wish to make a distinction between
institutional power and interactive power. The public service interpreter, for example, enjoys
very little institutional power. Rates of pay are generally low, even when the interpreter is
qualified (e.g. the DPSI – Diploma of Public Service Interpreting – in the United King-
dom). Interpreters are often called upon without any prior knowledge of what or for whom
they will interpret. The coaching of users of interpreting services is still in its infancy.
Public services appear to assume that they know best and impose conditions on interpreters
that are far from ideal. Meanwhile, it is expected that court interpreters restrict themselves
to a close translation of what is said and there is little tolerance for interruption when an
interpreter perceives a misunderstanding. Also, asylum interviews are conducted at a brisk
pace with a focus on the end result, and interpreters are expected just to ‘t ranslate’, that is, a
shorthand request addressed to interpreters that assumes translation to be something simple
65
Ian Mason
Literature overview
It will be apparent in what precedes that my analysis rests on the work of various theoreti-
cians from different fields of study. Wenger (1998) is persuasive on communities of practice
while Davies and Harré (1990) offer the notion of positioning which is entirely compatible
with communities of practice and which introduces a dynamic, interactive element into
talk exchanges. Barton and Tusting (2005) adjust Wenger’s theory by bringing the notion of
discourse to the fore (whereas, for Wenger, it had just been a ‘resource’ that ‘reflects’ a com-
munity). Meanwhile, Blommaert (2005) proposes a critique of many discoursal approaches
from the perspective of inequalities of power and the distribution of resources among users.
The notion of ‘voice’ (defined as ‘the way in which people manage to make themselves un-
derstood or fail to do so’, Blommaert 2005: 4) applies to PSI in a very real sense. It will be
central to the analyses that follow. At the same time, I am indebted to all those whose work
has analysed actual decisions made by interpreters, cumulatively offering an ample corpus of
interpreter behaviour with an eye not to criticism of interpreters but rather to a description
of their behaviour: B
erk-Seligson (1990), Hale (1997), Wadensjö (1998), Pym (1999), Bolden
66
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
(2000, 2018), Roy (2000), Davidson (2001), Angelelli (2004), Pöllabauer (2004), Bot (2005),
Gavioli and Baraldi (2011), Baraldi and Gavioli (2012), Pöchhacker (2012).
Critical issues
In what ways do cultural assumptions, positioning and power come together to form an inte-
grated theory? Let us start from the perspective of power. It is widely acknowledged that the
PSI interpreter is almost always involved in disparities of power, both institutional and in-
teractive. How is the interpreter to negotiate these d isparities – both in the interaction itself
and having regard for the availability of linguistic resources to each party to an exchange?
In Mason (2014), I detailed a case of an interpreter in an African context translating from a
formal European speech about a new savings bank for the benefit of African villagers who
might be interested in investing (based on Kouraogo 2001). This may well be about a savings
bank but I would argue that the interpreter was acting as a PSI interpreter. I claimed that
the interpreter had saved the European speech maker by adjusting his output to the villagers’
contextualisation (e.g. by adding interjections (‘ee’, ‘hey’), explaining to the villagers (‘you
need 1000 francs to open your account’, ‘that’s what he said’), encouraging the villagers to
subscribe (‘that’s a lot!’). The same is not very often true in reverse. Few concessions, I would
venture, are made to asylum seekers, whose experience of life is culturally entirely different
from that of well-to-do Europeans.
Examples from a corpus I analysed many years ago (Mason 2006a) suggest an inter-
preter fully aligned with the aims of the UK immigration authorities, whose immediate goal
was to deport illegal immigrants forthwith. The interpreter not only allows herself to ask
supplementary questions that incriminate immigrants but also completely suppresses their
discourse of seeking education in the United Kingdom (cf. also Keselman, Cederborg and
Linell 2010, where those acting as interpreters seem to be supporting what they believed was
the case workers’ aim but in effect are compromising the integrity of the caseworkers and
putting the young asylum seekers in a precarious position). It is important to note, however,
that the interpreters were lay persons, not professionally trained interpreters.
It is apparent that the PSI interpreter stands at the centre of all these issues. What is at
stake and what are the consequences of their actual decisions? From the perspective of cul-
tural assumptions, positioning and disparities of power, I aim to examine data samples that
illustrate the dilemmas involved.
Analysis
An example provided by Maryns (2006: 245) effectively brings together all these separate
strands. A judge, assisted by an interpreter, asks an asylum seeker a question to which he
presumably already knows the answer. This in itself illustrates a power imbalance: the judge
appears to be privy to knowledge that the asylum seeker may or may not know. The point of
the question, therefore, must simply be an attempt to catch the asylum seeker out. Moreover,
as Maryns points out, both the judge and the interpreter are non-native speakers of English.
Nevertheless ‘whereas the judges are highly literate people who developed their competence
at a European university, the interpreter also acquired his English through formal learning
but in a West African learning context’. The sequence I am referring to runs as follows:
67
Ian Mason
I shall now analyse this sequence from the point of view of the context it enacts. The judge
(01) asks the name of the rector, that is the head of a Belgian university. The equivalent
English term (a s reflected in the literal translation) is v ice-chancellor. The interpreter relays
this into English as ‘the director’ and is immediately corrected by the judge, who says ‘the
chancellor’. Now, in English universities this is a largely ceremonial position (attending
graduation ceremonies, for example) and certainly not the chief executive (who is the v ice-
chancellor). The Judge can, of course, be forgiven for not being familiar with the idiosyncra-
sies of English universities but the point here is that he interrupts and corrects the interpreter
and is thus the primary cause of the confusion that ensues. The asylum seeker confidently
asserts that the chancellor ‘at the time when I was in the university’ (06) was professor Rich-
ard Thomas. The judge then self-corrects to ‘v ice-chancellor’ (09).1 The interpreter then
naturally assumes that ‘chancellor’ and ‘vice-chancellor’ are separate positions within the
university (10) and disconcerts the asylum seeker by asking for the name of v ice-chancellor.
This induces the asylum seeker to assert that the v ice-chancellor is in the English section (
11–12), something which cannot possibly be the case.
We can now review this example through the lens of ‘voice’. Having confidently asserted
that the chancellor was professor Richard Thomas, the asylum seeker is now totally confused
(‘I don’t know where you talk’, 11), loses his English and, in an attempt to make sense, utters
a total – and possibly incriminating – invention. The confusion then becomes worse, with
the asylum seeker wondering ‘do we have a vice chancellor in faculty itself. we don’t have’
(not in the transcript). He has lost his ability to make himself heard, his ‘voice’.
Meanwhile, the judge is highly unlikely to be challenged over his equivocation since he
enjoys total power within his own court. He can speak at any moment of his choosing, inter-
rupt or question the veracity of the asylum seeker. He is also the one who asks the questions.
It goes without saying that the asylum seeker can do none of these things.
The example also illustrates the different cultural assumptions made by the judge and the
interpreter. The judge assumes that he is sufficiently acquainted with the English language to
be confident in his nomenclature. The interpreter, on the other hand, has very little confi-
dence: having translated ‘rector’ as ‘d irector’, he is immediately corrected by the judge. Rec-
ognising his lack of familiarity with English cultural assumptions, he has to tread carefully.
Thus, his assumption that ‘chancellor’ and ‘vice-chancellor’ are different positions within a
university is both prudent and rational. He has been positioned as ‘not knowing’ while the
judge positions himself as ‘all-knowing’. In this respect, Blommaert’s notion of ‘language
ideology’ (Blommaert 2005: 171) rings true.
68
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
Cultural assumptions are also involved here. The judge positions the interpreter as a
competent translator but appears totally unaware of the misunderstanding that he personally
has introduced into the encounter, assuming that an interpreter (even one whose native
tongue is not English and who has learned English in an African context) is merely there
to ‘translate’. The interpreter, on the other hand, makes the cultural assumption that the
judge is an authoritative figure, who knows the difference between a chancellor and a v ice-
c hancellor and so translates accordingly. Errors on both sides in social role recognition (see
Vicker 2009) are clear. Thus the interpreter contributes to his own delegitimisation.
Cultural assumptions of a more explicit kind are involved in a study by S pencer-Oatey
and Xing (2008). Admittedly, this is taken from a business interpreting context (where
interpreters are expected to serve their employers), not PSI (where the expectation is that
the interpreter will serve both parties). But the example serves to encapsulate many of
the concepts introduced here. The article describes a Chinese delegation of businessmen
visiting the premises of a UK business. At the initial meeting, the head of the British
team made a speech of welcome to the Chinese guests and then each member of the team
introduced themselves. The head of the British team then invited the Chinese delegation
members to introduce themselves. The head of the Chinese delegation then assumed that
he should make a speech in return and ‘started to express the group’s appreciation to the
hosts.’ The interpreter immediately intervenes to say that the Chinese delegation had been
invited simply to introduce themselves, not to give a return speech. Here it is clear that the
Chinese interpreter, being Chinese himself, feels that he is sufficiently close to the Chinese
delegation (i.e. part of their community of practice) to be able to advise them on protocol
(a n identity of participation). This clearly was an error. S pencer-Oatey and Xing (2008:263)
relate that there followed ‘several minutes of uncomfortable discussion in Chinese by the
visitors’ before each then complied with the invitation to introduce themselves. In follow-
p interviews, the different cultural assumptions are stark. The head of the Chinese dele-
u
gation is reported as saying,
According to our home customs and protocol, speech is delivered on the basis of rec-
iprocity. He has made his speech and I am expected to say something (…) But he had
finished his speech, and he didn’t give me the opportunity, and they each introduced
themselves, wasn’t this clearly implied that they do look down upon us Chinese.
As suggested earlier, the danger with cultural assumptions is that, when we encounter unex-
pected behaviour, we read specific intentions into it (‘they do look down upon us Chinese’).
The British head, on the other hand, was wholly unaware of this, saying that
… they used to have return speeches, but that as the Chinese have become more familiar
with [us], ‘formalities have really eroded and drifted away’.
At stake here are unspoken assumptions. The Chinese delegation may have been trying to
look informal but, as is clear from the quote above, there are expectations of reciprocity and
business meetings in China are nearly always formal. The British head appears to assume
that, because strict formality has all but disappeared from UK business meetings, the Chinese
will be comfortable with this. In turn, the Chinese delegation arrive in the United Kingdom
with an expectation of formality and then feel cheated.
Even the seating arrangements are criticised. The Chinese delegation leader observes
(2008: 260)
69
Ian Mason
It shouldn’t have been that he was the chair and we were seated along the sides of the
table. With equal status, they should sit along this side and we should sit along that side.
The British leader had made the mistake of placing himself at the head of the (rectangular)
table. This is consequently liable to be interpreted within a formal Chinese context as some-
what arrogant and disrespectful. Clearly, business in China and business in the United King-
dom are totally different communities of practice, with different expectations as to what is
appropriate and what inappropriate. The Chinese leader felt that he had been deprived of
voice and therefore of status. The interpreter, meanwhile, had overstepped the mark with
catastrophic consequences, believing that he enjoyed status as a gatekeeper when in fact his
intervention was perceived as a gross intrusion. Thus, insensitivity to cultural assumptions
can be a huge problem for interpreters (see also Wadensjö 2001).
This is, of course, another illustration of the interactional power of the interpreter – in
this case, a negative one. Power, though, can also be exerted by means of gaze, facial ex-
pression or even body posture. The raising of eyebrows for example can, if directed at an
interlocutor, be a cooperative signal, inviting expansion, explication or simply lack com-
prehension. However, if the interpreter looks away from their interlocutor the raising of
eyebrows can cast doubt on what is being interpreted, especially if accompanied by frowning
or lowering the corners of the mouth (see Mason 2012). Meanwhile, a downward gaze can
communicate impartiality or even a refusal to be involved in what is being discussed while
leaning forward and giving eye contact to both other parties in turn can signal involvement
and a determination to assist. The role of gaze in face-to-face interpreting has perhaps not
yet been sufficiently explored (but see Davitti 2015; Davitti and Pasquandrea 2017).
All this, of course, is a matter of pragmatics. I am referring not to linguistic pragmatics
but to a kind of sociolinguistic – or social – pragmatics that has perhaps not yet been fully
explored.2 Part of this is accounted for by a lack of full data. That is, we would need to
be able to follow the speech, intonation and body posture of each individual participant
in exchange. Ideally, we would need cameras trained on each participant, recording their
facial gestures, gaze patterns and body posture. Transcripts would need to include markers
for intonation, emphasis, speech uttered very quietly, speech accompanied by frowns or
smiles and so on. The problem, of course, is that many exchanges where PSI takes place are
confidential. Very often only audio recordings are permitted. Output intended for televi-
sion audiences is just that, that is not intended as a study of interpreting behaviour. Thus, a
camera may cut away at a moment that may prove crucial to the understanding of posture
or gaze of an interpreter.
Social role recognition, turn allocation and conversational repair (Vicker n.d.) are all
pivotal to the work of the interpreter (see also Wadensjö (2015), as are the Gricean maxims
of quantity, quality, relation and manner (Grice 1975) or Sperber and Wilson’s (1995) rel-
evance theory – but here adjusted to take into consideration communication rights, access
to resources and other aspects of inequality of power and control. I would add a further
caveat: relevance theory is useful in its recognition of communicators’ ability to pick up cues
(‘ostension’) about what a speaker means and the further recognition that hearers then infer
on the basis of these cues what a working meaning is. Inference is thus key to understanding
and, for Sperber and Wilson, the hearer will always select the meaning that best matches
the principle of relevance (g reatest cognitive effect for least processing effort). Gutt (2000:
33) adds ‘… and there is never more than one interpretation that fulfils this condition’. This
however closes down the possibility of genuine ambiguity. Interpreters are frequently at a
loss to understand what someone has just said (especially if he or she is a n on-native speaker
70
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
01 Q And during all the time of your detention, did you ask to see a lawyer?
02 A A lawyer.
03 Q Yes, to be assisted by a lawyer?
04 A I wasn’t given that chance.
05 Q But did you ask it?
06 A And I did not make such a request.
07 Q For what reason?
08 A Right from the (x xx) I was denying what they were trying to put on me so I did
not want to get the assistance of a lawyer to substantiate. To gain the requisition
of a lawyer to be personal means confirmation of their intention so I did not do
that to substantiate what they were thinking.
09 Q And did you have the right to be in communication with your family?
10 A In fact, my idea of joining this thing from the very beginning has not been in the
knowledge of my wife because she wouldn’t like it, so I kept all the activities that
I was doing with this movement quite secret from my wife.
The asylum adjudicator (in Canada) is clearly working on very western cultural assumptions:
that it is normal to ask for the assistance of a lawyer when being questioned by police and that
you would surely share your concerns with your nearest-and-dearest. In an African context,
71
Ian Mason
this might in some countries be very dangerous. You would not want your wife to know
about your activities because then she might be arrested and tortured in turn. Similarly,
you might not want to avail yourself of a lawyer employed by the government because the
government is your oppressor. The union movement in which the asylum seeker was active
no doubt conducted all its activities in secret. This, of course, is totally foreign to Europe,
the United States and Canada, where accused persons always ask for a lawyer so that this
dialogue works on diametrically opposed cultural assumptions. The dialogue itself did not
involve an interpreter but it is easy to see the problems that would have been involved if the
asylum seeker had demanded the services of an interpreter and expressed himself in his na-
tive language. In a dialogue where mutually opposing cultural assumptions are being made,
the interpreter would have to protect her/h imself from misunderstandings based on cultural
assumptions. Whether she/he would be given the opportunity to do so would depend on
circumstances. Under some jurisdictions, she/he might, under others – especially
in a legal
context – she/he might not. From the perspective of the interpreter, if you are prosecuted by
the legal system, you have no legal escape (like asking for a lawyer). Thus, interpreting can
contribute to social pragmatics by adding/suggesting new assumptions.
Related topics
One of the many issues raised here is social pragmatics. There are a host of web-based
sources for social pragmatics but they are largely confined to the field of language disorders
or impairments or of language learning. Beverley Vicker points the way with ‘Meeting the
challenge of social pragmatics with students on the autism spectrum’. For communication in
children’s development, see ‘W hat is social communication (Pragmatics)?’ (K idsense n.d.). A
search for ‘social pragmatics theory’ will reveal many more articles. A true social pragmatics
for interpreting however is yet to be written. Above, I have endeavoured to suggest what
such a pragmatics might include.
For Gricean pragmatics, see Grice (1975) and for its application to translation and inter-
preting see Hatim and Mason (1990) or Gutt (2000). Sperber and Wilson (1986) update and
correct Grice (1975), offering a plausible account of c ommunication – subject to the caveat
expressed above.
Another topic is indeterminacy (or under-determinacy) of meaning. On this see Robyn
Carston (2002) – from a linguistic pragmatics point of v iew – or Gumperz (1992) from a
more interactional sociolinguistics angle. For Carston,
where ‘linguistic meaning’ is the lexical-syntactic form of the sentence and ‘what is said’ is
taken to be the ‘the thought or proposition which it is being used to express.’ This is to be
distinguished from ‘what is meant’ since, as previously suggested, speakers make judgements
about how much hearers already know and adjust their speech according to the principle of
relevance: greatest contextual effect for minimal processing effort. In other words, you don’t
need to say more than is necessary to ensure understanding – a principle we all adhere to.
Finally, I return to Piller’s observation that cultures are not coterminous with n ation-
states. A phrase that is frequently used, for example is ‘in Chinese culture’. Yet there are
72
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
so many different cultures in China. Sichuan is entirely different from Beijing and Shang-
hai (w ith a dialect to match), while Guangdong is entirely different from Northern China
(again, with a d ialect – some would say language – to match, Cantonese). Then, there are
the Muslim cultures of Xinzhang and the Uighur people, to say nothing of, for example
dissident cultures. There are as many cultures in China, in fact, as there are communities
of practice. On these notions, see Wenger (1998) and Piller (2012, 2017). Piller prefers to
describe ‘culture’ not as a noun but as a verb, ‘something people do’ (2017: 9) and asks ‘who
makes culture relevant to whom, in which context for which purposes’? (2017: 67). Such
notions are highly pertinent to the cultural turn in interpreting studies and assist in relating
cultural assumptions to communities of practice, positioning and power. In so doing, they
contribute to a social pragmatics of interpreting.
Further reading
Blommaert, Jan (2005) Discourse. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
The volume focuses on how language can offer a crucial understanding of often underestimated aspects of
power relations, maintaining that studies of discourse should highlight what power does to people, groups, and
societies, and how this impact comes about.
Carston, Robyn ( 2002) Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication. Oxford,
Blackwell.
The volumes discusses the ‘underdeterminacy’ thesis and provides for reflection on a distinction between
what is explicitly and implicitly communicated.
Gumperz, John J. (1992) “Contextualisation and understanding”, in Re-thinking Context: Language as
an Interactive Phenomenon. Studies in the Social and Cultural Foundations of Language, vol. 11, Alessandro
Duranti and Charles Goodwin (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 229–52.
This seminal paper discusses the relevance of context and the ways it is ‘talked about’ making, and referring
to, assumptions that impact heavily on the interlocutors understanding of meaning.
Piller, Ingrid (2017) Intercultural Communication: A Critical Introduction. 2nd ed. Edinburgh, Edinburgh
University Press.
The volume deals with intercultural communication as a discursive construction. It highlights the use and
prestige of different languages and language varieties as well as the varying access that speakers have to them,
moving debate on what the idea of ‘culture’ is all about.
Wenger, Etienne (1998) Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning, and Identity. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
The book presents a theory of learning based on the idea that engagement in social practice is the fundamen-
tal process by which we learn and so become who we are. The starting point of this process are the ‘communities
of practice’ that people form as they pursue shared enterprises over time. The theory explores the intersection of
issues of community, social practice, meaning, and identity
Related chapters
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in public service interpreting by Claudio Baraldi.
Chapter 11, Vulnerable encounters? Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services for victim survivors
of domestic violence and abuse by Rebecca Tipton
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalisation of interpret-
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
Notes
1 At least, we assume he self-corrects. One drawback of Maryns’s data is that we have no marking
for intonation. It could be simply a confirmation of the previous line, ‘yes, professor Thomas’ (08).
2 Social pragmatics has been applied to language learning and to language disorders but not, as far
as I am aware, to interpreting theory.
73
Ian Mason
References
Angelelli, Claudia (2004) Medical Interpreting and Cross-cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
Baker, Mona (2006) Translation and Conflict: A Narrative Account. London, Routledge.
Baraldi, Claudio and Laura Gavioli ( eds) ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, Benjamins.
Barsky, Robert F. (1994) Constructing a Productive Other. Discourse Theory and the Convention Refugee
Hearing. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, Benjamins.
Barton, David and Karin Tusting (eds) (2005) Beyond Communities of Practice. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
Berk-Seligson, Susan (1990) The Bilingual Courtroom. Court interpreters in the Judicial Process. Chicago/
London, The University of Chicago Press.
Bolden, Galina (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: Interpreters’ involve-
ment in history taking”, Discourse Studies 2 (4): 387–419.
——— (2018) “Understanding interpreters’ actions in context”, Communication & Medicine 15 (2):
135–49.
Bot, Hanneke (2005) Dialogue Interpreting in Mental Health. Amsterdam/New York, Rodopi.
Davidson, Brad (2001) “Questions in cross-linguistic medical encounters: The role of the hospital
interpreter”, Anthropological Quarterly 74 (4): 170–78.
Davies, Bronwyn, and Rom Harré (1990) “Positioning: The discursive production of selves”, Journal
for the Theory of Social Behaviour 20 (1): 43–63.
Davitti, Elena (2015) “Interpreter-mediated parent-teacher talk”, in Linking Discourse Studies to Profes-
sional Practice, Grujicic-A latriste, Lubie (ed). Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 176–200.
Davitti, Elena and Sergio Pasquandrea (2017) “Embodied participation: What multimodal analysis can tell
us about interpreter-mediated encounters in pedagogical settings”, Journal of Pragmatics 107: 105–28.
Gavioli, Laura and Claudio Baraldi (2011) “Interpreter-mediated interaction in healthcare and legal
settings”, Interpreting 13 (2): 205–33.
Grice, H. Paul (1975) “L ogic and conversation”, in Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts, Peter Cole and
Jerry L. Morgan (eds). New York, Academic Press: 4 1–58.
Gutt, Ernst-August (1991/2000) Translation and Relevance: Cognition and Context. Oxford, Blackwell/
M anchester: St. Jerome Publishing.
Hale, Sandra (1997) “The interpreter on trial: pragmatics in court interpreting”, in The Critical Link:
Interpreters in the Community, Silvana E. Carr, Roda P. Roberts, Aideen Dufour and Dini Steyn
(eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 201–11.
Hatim, Basil, and Ian Mason (1990) Discourse and the Translator. London, Routledge.
Inghilleri, Moira (2005) “Mediating zones of uncertainty. Interpreter agency, the interpreting habitus
and political asylum adjudication”, The Translator 11 (1): 69–85.
Keselman, Olga, A nn-Christine Cederborg, and Per Linell (2010). “‘That is not necessary for you to
know!’: Negotiations of participation status of unaccompanied children in interpreter-mediated
asylum hearings”, Interpreting 12 (1): 83–104.
Kidsense n.d. “W hat is social communication ( Pragmatics)?” https://childdevelopment.com.au/a reas-
of-concern/play-a nd-social-skills/social-communication-pragmatics/ (accessed 28 January 2021).
Kouraogo, Pierre (2001) “The rebirth of the king’s linguist”, in Triadic Exchanges. Studies in Dialogue
Interpreting, Ian Mason (ed). Manchester, St. Jerome Publishing.
Lave, Jean, and Etienne Wenger (1991) Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge,
Cambridge University Press.
Maryns, Katrijn (2006) The Asylum Speaker: Language in the Belgian Asylum Procedure. Manchester,
St. Jerome Publishing.
Mason, Ian (2006a) “On mutual accessibility of contextual assumptions in dialogue interpreting”,
Journal of Pragmatics 38: 359–73.
——— (2006b) “Ostension, inference and response: Analysing participant moves in community in-
terpreting dialogues”, Taking Stock: Research and Methodology in Community Interpreting, Linguistica
Antverpiensia 5: 103–20.
——— (2012) “Gaze, positioning and identity in i nterpreter-mediated dialogues”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
Benjamins: 177–99.
74
Cultural assumptions, positioning and power
——— (2014) “Discourse and translation – a social perspective”, in Translation: A Multidisciplinary
Approach, Juliane House (ed). Basingstoke, Palgrave Macmillan: 36–55.
Mason, Ian and Miranda Stewart (2001) “Interactional pragmatics, face and the dialogue interpreter”,
in Triadic Exchanges. Studies in Dialogue Interpreting, Ian Mason (ed). Manchester, St. Jerome Pub-
lishing: 50–70.
Piller, Ingrid (2012) “Intercultural communication: an overview”, in The Handbook of Intercultural Dis-
course and Communication, Christina Bratt Paulston, Scott F. Keisling and Elizabeth S. Rangel (eds).
Oxford, Blackwell: 3 –18.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2012) “Interpreting participation: Conceptual analysis and illustration of the in-
terpreter’s role in interaction”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi,
and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 45–69.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2004) “Interpreting in asylum hearings: Issues of role, responsibility and power”,
Interpreting 6 (2): 143–80.
Pym, Anthony (1999) “‘Nicole slapped Michelle’: Interpreters and theories of interpreting at the
O.J.Simpson trial”, The Translator 5 (2): 265–83.
Roy, Cynthia (2000) Interpreting as a Discourse Process. New York/Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Spencer-Oatey, Helen, and Jianyu Xing (2008) “Issue of face in a Chinese business visit to Britain”,
in Culturally Speaking. Culture, Communication and Politeness Theory, 2nd ed., Helen Spencer-Oatey
(ed). London/New York, Continuum: 258–76.
Sperber, Dan, and Deirdre Wilson (1986) Relevance. Communication and Cognition. Oxford, Blackwell.
Vicker, Beverley “Meeting the challenge of social pragmatics with students on the autism spectrum”,
Indiana Resource Center for Autism. https://w ww.printfriendly.com/p/g/5s35kL (accessed 29
October 2021).
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (2001) “Interpreting in crisis: The interpreter’s position in therapeutic encounters”, in Triadic
Exchanges. Studies in Dialogue Interpreting, Ian Mason (ed). Manchester, St Jerome Publishing: 71–85.
——— (2015) “Interpreter-mediated interaction” in Handbook of Pragmatics Online. [IPrA Research
Center] Jef Verschueren and Jan-Ola Östman (eds) Amsterdam, John Benjamins. https://doi.org/
10.1075/hop.19.int14
Widdowson, Henry G. (2004) Text, Context, Pretext. Malden, Blackwell.
75
5
CORPUS-BASED STUDIES
OF PUBLIC SERVICE
INTERPRETING1
Bernd Meyer
Introduction
As Kennedy (1998) states in his book An introduction to corpus linguistics, any systematic collec-
tion of language data is a corpus, that is ‘a body of written texts and/or transcribed speech
which can serve as a basis for linguistic analysis and description’ (ibid.: 1). Corpora may vary
in size and s cope – the amount of data in a collection depends on many factors and does not
necessarily say anything about the quality or the usability of a corpus. Rather, aspects such as
research questions and framework, the quality of data, or its rarity tell much more about the
significance of a corpus and its possible reuse.
Collections of linguistic data have always been part of methodologies in almost all sci-
entific fields in the humanities, and today’s digital technologies offer new possibilities for
data collection, storage, analysis, and presentation (Borgman 2010, 2015). The disruptive
character of digital technologies potentially changes the status of corpora in many fields,
and it does so also in public service interpreting (PSI)-related research. Nevertheless, the use
of digital technologies is a relatively new trend, gaining momentum in interpreting studies
only recently.
Today, corpora of spoken language in general and in PSI research specifically are still
rare. However, more and more research institutions host spoken language corpora because
of a growing interest in different aspects of language use (Caines, McCarthy, and O’Keeffe
2016) or in language documentation (Newman, Baayen and Rice 2011). Spoken language
corpora may vary in size and type of discourse they refer to, but usually they come enriched
with annotations and metadata on contextual aspects of discourse, such as information about
the time and place of the communicative event, or information about specific properties of
participants, like kinship relations, age, gender, professional status, and so forth.
Goffman’s analyses of human interaction (Goffman 1967). These approaches led to numerous
theoretical and methodological innovations regarding the analysis of face-to-face encounters.
The pragmatic orientation towards the situatedness of linguistic action also inspired early
researchers working with audio recordings from interpreter-mediated interactions, such as
Lang (1978), Kaufert and Koolage (1984), or Knapp and K napp-Potthoff (1986). However,
the study of such interactions was based on analogue data (tape recordings and hand-or
type-written transcripts), simply because digitisation, storage and computer-assisted tran-
scription of oral data were not possible or far too difficult at that time.
The explicit call for corpus-linguistic methodologies in interpreting studies began with
Shlesinger (1998), who developed her argument against the background of corpus-based
translation studies, that is the investigation of structural properties of translated texts, and
with the intention of strengthening the methodological rigour in interpreting studies as an
emerging discipline. Thus, much of Shlesinger’s argument was about the possibilities that
may emerge from new technologies and, furthermore, text-analytical methodologies that
have proven to yield interesting insights in translation studies. While some of her proposals
and visions have already been put into practice (such as the design of corpora expressly for
the study of interpreting, or the creation of parallel corpora of interpreting data), others seem
to be far too optimistic (‘computerized comparisons between oral and written translations’,
Shlesinger 1998: 4, or ‘a controlled examination of the large number of variables involved in
interpreting’ 1998: 5). However, Shlesinger’s intention was obviously not to design a specific
research project in detail, but to highlight the potentials of using c orpus-linguistic methods
and technologies.
Making use of such approaches was hampered initially not only by technical limitations,
but also by a general tendency in translation studies towards idiosyncratic theory-building,
and limited exchange with theoretical and methodological frameworks of neighbouring sci-
ences (see, e.g. Reiß and Vermeer 1984/2014 on ‘skopos theory’). Furthermore, researchers
from interpreting studies are often working in translation and interpreting departments,
where academic training for conference interpreting is provided and other forms of inter-
preting are often considered to be peripheral or simply not part of the curriculum. Thus, PSI
researchers who feel comfortable with the theoretical and methodological frameworks from
pragmatics or sociolinguistics often come from neighbouring disciplines, such as sociology,
sociolinguistics, and pragmatics.
A typical feature of PSI corpora is the preference for authentic (i.e. non-experimental)
data and the small size of samples, due to remaining difficulties in data collection and field
access. Public service institutions, such as administrations, medical or educational institu-
tions and social services, usually have to comply with strict privacy regulations. Clients
and institutional agents are dealing with private matters and do not easily grant access to
researchers, or feel uncomfortable with the documentation of events by audio and video
recordings. Moreover, PSI research is usually carried out in qualitative research frameworks,
which primarily focus on understanding how people establish social order, or on their beliefs
and linguistic routines, rather than analysing large data collections quantitatively. While
corpus linguistics usually tries to describe patterns of language use in large data sets by using
quantitative methods, a considerable part of PSI research aims at understanding particular
or general aspects of interpreter-mediated interactions by looking at the use of language
and other communicative resources in detail. Instead of making normative judgments about
interpreter performance, the general aim of this body of PSI research is to investigate how
people in reality interact, using various communicative resources, and how their commu-
nicative practices shape the course and the outcome of interactions. Researchers therefore
77
Bernd Meyer
often draw their conclusions on the basis of single cases or examples that serve to illustrate
or highlight particular aspects of interpreter-mediated interactions. These qualitative ap-
proaches developed historically without intense contact to corpus linguistic approaches and
their focus on quantification.
However, with the rapid development of corpus technologies, working with corpora
is becoming part of the PSI toolkit. The interest in methodological reflection and digital
technologies has also reached the field of PSI studies, as Niemants (2012) shows. The combi-
nation of ethnographic or situational metadata, data from participant observation, together
with quantitative and qualitative analyses of interactions or texts raise the validity of con-
clusions and reveal communicative aspects that otherwise would not have come to the fore.
PSI research focuses on interactions between three or more participants using differ-
ent languages, mostly in institutional contexts. This places certain requirements on corpus
technologies that text-oriented corpus linguistics often does not have to meet (Schmidt and
Wörner 2009). This includes the focus on multi-party interactions, multi-modal analyses
considering non-verbal actions (smiling, laughing, gestures, and so on), or paralinguistic
phenomena such as pausing, prosody, and so forth. Thus, corpus technologies for the analysis
of interaction data need to allow for different layers of annotations and should provide mea-
sures for the collection, storage, and representation of metadata on social properties of partic-
ipants, and on the situational context in which the interaction takes place. Furthermore, such
data collections need to maintain the simultaneity and reciprocity of linguistic actions, that
is how discourse unfolds during the communicative event, taking into account the temporal
dimensions of speech. Another aspect is the use of special characters and signs to capture
phenomena of spoken language, such as false starts, prosodic patterns, pausing, hesitations,
and interjections. For multilingual data, the list of requirements is even longer because it
may be necessary to provide discourse data together with back translations of original ut-
terances (Rehbein et al. 2004; Belzyk-Kohl 2016), and to accommodate language-specific
features and scripts that differ from Latin, such as Arabic (Farag 2019).
Although such technologies are available in principle, only a small number of researchers
explore the whole spectrum of possible uses. Still, the combined representation of con-
textual information, images, videos, sound recordings, and transcripts is an expensive,
t ime-consuming, and challenging enterprise that often goes beyond the limits of individual
research projects. However, the influence of the observation also affects collections of au-
thentic discourse data and poses methodological questions. As Ochs puts it, data gathering
and transcription reflect ‘theoretical goals and definitions’ (Ochs 1979: 44). Transcription,
recording, and other steps of data collection and representation imply selective choices of
the researcher and, thus, influence the outcome in the sense of selective authenticity which
may even have political implications (Buchholtz 2000). Similarly, the presentation of data
from lesser used languages, minority languages, or n on-standard varieties poses challenges
for research communities (Egbert, Yufu and Hirataka 2016). Thus, making use of the full
range of advancements in corpus technologies may also be a way to partly bridge the gap
between the original communicative event and its digital representations in a corpus (sound
files, videos, images, transcripts).
The typical methodological approach in PSI research is the analysis of talk, more or
less explicitly transcribed on the basis of Jeffersonian transcription conventions (Hepburn
and Bolden 2013). Analytical categories are often taken from conversation analysis (turn,
turn-taking, sequence), and combined with Wadensjö’s concept of rendition types, or her
concept of different coordination moves. Although Setton (2011: 48) maintains that samples
used in such studies are generally ‘too small for normal science’, the impact of these studies
78
Corpus-based studies of PSI
79
Bernd Meyer
contributions in an edited volume by Baraldi and Gavioli (2012). Pasquandrea (2011) goes
beyond these analyses of the participation of interpreters by integrating multi-modal aspects
such as gaze and other body movements by which, for example, a participant may signal his
or her involvement in the o n-going interaction, which, in turn, may trigger or at least sup-
port certain types of participation by the interpreter.
As one of the precursors to Wadensjö’s analysis of the interpreters’ footing, Müller (1989)
uses a small corpus of ethnographic interviews with Italian migrants in Germany to dis-
cuss how the involved interpreters, being migrants themselves, shift their participation from
translating towards other forms of participation in these settings. He shows that the inter-
preting mode is constantly being negotiated and adapted to the needs of the interaction.
His claim is that this shifting between different modes of bilingual interaction and different
participation frameworks is at least partly due to the permeability of the language barrier
in communication with migrants. He describes talk in a foreign language as a scale with
opaque linguistic means, on the one hand, and transparent, that is intelligible, means on
the other. Bilingual constellations, thus, may be characterised by the mutual transparency
or opaqueness of linguistic means for interlocutors. This aspect has been taken up by Meyer
(2012), who perceives this as a systematic feature of ad hoc interpreting in hospitals and other
community settings, and Angermeyer (2015), who analyses legal settings and shows how
monolingual institutional norms stand in stark contrast to the multilingual competencies of
different types of participants.
Another line of research has been established by Sabine Braun, who conducted several
research projects on remote interpreting, and, more specifically, compares on-site and re-
mote video interpreting in legal settings and police interrogations. In a recent study, Braun
(2017) investigates the functions of additions and expansions of original utterances by in-
terpreters using a c onversation-analytic approach. One special feature of her corpus is that
it combines the advantages of simulations (controlled variables) and of authentic discourse
(fewer artefacts, natural behaviour) because role plays were not read out verbatim, based on
scripts, but rather acted out on the basis of transcripts taken from authentic interviews. An
interesting result of this study is that content-related deviations from the original (such as
additions or omissions) increased in remote interpreting and were even more frequent when
subjects became familiar with this interpreting mode. It may be seen as a shortcoming of the
analysis, however, that the transcripts presented in the article only vaguely account for the
simultaneity of discourse, and that the role of gaze and body movements is discussed without
presenting the respective data (Braun 2017: 171).
Angermeyer and Meyer (2021) investigate the potential of qualitative and quantitative
methods for the analysis of non-renditions (Wadensjö 1998:108; interpreters’ utterances
which do not translate source talk by other participants). The c orpus-based approach allows
for a comparison of interpreter performance across different settings, particularly medical
and court interpreting with ad hoc lay interpreters and experienced professional interpret-
ers, respectively. Non-renditions are found to be used at higher rates by ad hoc interpreters,
yet interpreters in all settings are more likely to produce them when speaking the language
of the institution than when speaking a minority language. The comparison also permits
the identification of exceptional cases in which the interpreter’s performance differs con-
siderably from other cases of the same context. These cases are then investigated further,
combining quantitative and qualitative approaches, showing that comparatively high rates of
non-renditions correlate with code-switching by other participants. In other words, when
flexible language strategies result in a convergence of language choice among the primary
interlocutors (w ith minority language speakers switching to the institutional language), this
80
Corpus-based studies of PSI
Metadata
Metadata are often the stepchild of data collection. Not all studies provide information about
the participants of the interpreter-mediated event, their age, gender, linguistic competen-
cies, relationships, professional roles, and so forth. The same is true for the event itself – the
date, time and place, type of discourse, the languages used, its duration, and so on. Research-
ers may think that detailed information is sometimes not necessary for specific research
purposes. However, for a follow-up study, it might be interesting. Thus, researchers should
be keen to collect metadata systematically in order to provide a basis for a sustainable data
collection.
81
Bernd Meyer
In most cases, the collection of spoken language data requires research protocols which
account for the private character of such data. Therefore, researchers have to stick to data
protection laws and regulations, and usually ask participants to consent to the recording of
their voice and face. Thus, metadata usually entail anonymisation of proper names of per-
sons and places by using fictitious names, abbreviations, or numbers instead of the original
names. Details of anonymisation procedures depend on the research site and context. As a
rule of thumb, anonymisation should make it impossible for third parties to identify individ-
uals who have been recorded, including the exact time, place, and context of the recording.
Furthermore, procedures need to be documented to reveal their underlying systematics so
that other researchers can reconstruct general aspects of subjects in the corpus, such as their
professional role, their position in an institution, and so on. For the publication and reuse
of data, it is also necessary to document that consent has been granted. Furthermore, tran-
scripts need to be anonymised in the same way as metadata. This also refers to audio or video
recordings if there is an intention to publish them as well.
Transcription
An important feature of data curation is a transparent system of transcription conventions.
As previously mentioned, any transcription represents only certain aspects of interactions,
not the whole event as it happened in reality. Therefore, the use of transcription conventions
needs to be documented and explicitly mentioned in the metadata. Moreover, transcribing
according to certain conventions should be considered an integral part of the research process,
as the result strongly influences the analysis. Transcriber training and integration of tran-
scribers into team discussions are possible measures to achieve high quality for transcriptions.
Another aspect of transcriptions is their readability. While text corpora usually follow
standards of orthography, spoken language corpora try to grasp features of orality, such as
hearer signals or false starts, repetitions, aborted utterances, dialects, insecure transcriptions
and prosodic cues. Thus, transcription conventions need to account for phenomena that
are not part of standard orthography, such as hearer signals (‘a h!’), or indications of mental
planning (‘…ehm…’). Furthermore, transcriptions from PSI settings have to integrate dif-
ferent languages, potentially with different writing systems. Writing systems such as Arabic,
which goes from right to left, create temporal misalignment if used in combination with
Latin script. If Arabic is written with Roman characters, a systematic romanisation of spo-
ken Arabic is needed, that is a convention that allows transcribers to transcribe features of
spoken Arabic, including vernaculars and dialects, by using Roman characters. Otherwise,
temporal dimensions of speech would not be displayed in the transcription, due to different
directions of the flow of the scripts, and readability of the transcript will be hampered (Farag
2019).
Figure 5.1 gives an example of what the readability of a G erman-Arabic transcript could
look like. The format is that of a musical score in which reading is from left to right and
utterances of different speakers appear in their temporal order in separate tiers. The verbal
tiers [v] of the interpreter (T D2) and the client (K 3), a Syrian refugee, are complemented
by suprasegmental tiers [sup] and idiomatic back translations into English [en]. While the
suprasegmental tiers contain information about prosodic features, such as ‘soft’ and ‘a ngry’,
a commentary outside the score frame [k] provides transcribers’ comments about features of
discourse that are not attached to individual participants. This type of visualisation of tran-
scribed oral data (Schmidt and Wörner 2009, 2014) enables readers with different linguistic
competencies to use the transcript, albeit not to the same degree: a person with knowledge of
82
Corpus-based studies of PSI
Figure 5.1 E xtract from a corpus of telephone interpreting (German-Arabic, Farag 2019: 21)
Arabic and German will be able to carry out analyses that are more sophisticated than those
of a person with English only.
Annotations
Annotations are a dd-on information in the transcript that serve to facilitate the analysis by
categorising, describing, or paraphrasing verbal or non-verbal activities. This may include
translations of single words or utterances, or glosses with information on grammatical fea-
tures, such as tagging parts of speech. In the following, I shall give examples of annotations by
referring to the Community Interpreting Database (ComInDat). ComInDat (A ngermeyer,
Meyer and Schmidt 2012) is designed to facilitate data sharing among researchers in inter-
preting studies and other disciplines. It combines data from three distinct previously existing
corpora. While these corpora represent data from PSI contexts, they differ with respect
to many parameters, including the type and setting of the interactions (e.g. hospitals or
courtrooms), the type of recording, the transcription and annotation conventions, and the
language dyads. At the same time, all interactions included in the corpus share annotations
of the translation status and language choice of individual utterances, which present oppor-
tunities for a comparative analysis.
The component data sets of ComInDat differ in their use of annotation and transcription
software (EXMARaLDA or ELAN) and of transcription systems (HIAT, or based on Chat).
Nonetheless, ComInDat presents these data in a common online platform that facilitates
the viewing and querying of data. T ime-aligned transcripts are displayed in a musical score
format, where users can select which annotation components they would like to see or query.
Two types of annotation are included for all three types of data, as each utterance is anno-
tated for language and translation status.
83
Bernd Meyer
84
Corpus-based studies of PSI
the study to comparing interpreter utterances and preceding primary party utterance(s) (or
absence of utterance). Thus, this model opens up the possibility to explore talk as (t ranslated)
text, on the one hand, and talk as reciprocal activity of different participants, on the other.
The translation status, then, is seen as a text-related phenomenon.
To overcome the problem of annotator variation, utterances in the database are annotated
for translation status in a simple way that is not t heory-dependent and consider only a t op-level
categorisation (A ngermeyer, Meyer and Schmidt 2012). Utterances by interpreters are clas-
sified as renditions if they contain propositional elements that can be related to propositional
elements previously pronounced in the source language. Otherwise, they are categorised as
non-renditions. Similarly, utterances by other speakers are classified as source if they corre-
spond in one way or the other to an utterance by the interpreter, and as non-source if they
do not. This basic annotation of translation status enables researchers to quickly identify un-
translated talk as well as utterances in which interpreters do not engage in rendering talk but
interact with other participants in other ways. Moreover, it becomes possible to quantify the
rate of n on-renditions or non-sources for each speaker and each interaction, and thus to com-
pare types of interpreter-mediated interaction by this measurement. However, the annotation
for ‘non-rendition’ refers to utterances of interpreters that are formally and functionally quite
distinct: from subjective comments, side-talk, clarification sequences, and greetings to hearer
signals and minimal responses. Thus, any analysis has to consider that the categories ‘rendition’
and ‘non-rendition’ in this corpus are covering a whole range of different communicative acts.
Further reading
Borgman, Christine L. (2010) “The digital future is now: A call to action for the humanities”, Digital
Humanities Quarterly 4 (3): 1–30.
85
Bernd Meyer
This essay by Borgman elaborates on what the humanities need to do in order to catch up with other sciences
and outlines fields of action for digital humanities.
Schmidt, Thomas, Christian Chiarcos, Timm Lehmberg, Georg Rehm, Andreas Witt, and
Erhard Hinrichs (2 014) Avoiding Data Graveyards: From Heterogeneous Data Collected in Multi-
ple Research Projects to Sustainable Linguistic Resources. Institut für Deutsche Sprache, Bibliothek
(online).
In this online source, Schmidt and his colleagues discuss general aspects of storing and reusing spoken
language corpora. The article draws on experiences from three different research initiatives in Germany and
sketches areas of open questions with regard to sustainable data handling.
Ruhi, Şukriye, Michael Haugh, Thomas Schmidt and Kai Wörner (eds) (2014) Best Practices for Spoken
Corpora in Linguistic Research. Newcastle upon Tyne, Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
This edited volume provides corpus descriptions as well as corpus-based analyses using spoken language
corpora. It highlights best practices from both the perspective of assembling, annotating and linguistic analysis
of spoken language corpora, as well as from the perspective of processing, archiving and disseminating spoken
language.
The EXMARaLDA website (https://exmaralda.org/en/)
A practical introduction to using transcription software, including tutorials, is provided on the
EXMARaLDA website, managed by Thomas Schmidt (Institute for German Language) and Kai Wörner
(Hamburg University).
Related chapters
Chapter 6, Technology use in language-discordant interpersonal healthcare communication by Sabine Braun,
Khetam Al Sharou and Özlem Temizöz
Chapter 8, Public service interpreting in court – face-to-face interaction by Philipp Angermeyer
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences by Christian Licoppe
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: how authentic data meet simulations for interpreter train-
ing by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe
Note
1 This chapter is partly based on Meyer (2019) and Angermeyer and Meyer (2021). Remaining
shortcomings are mine.
References
Ajsic, Adnan, and Mary McGroarty (2015) “Mapping language ideologies” in Research Methods in
Language Policy and Planning: A Practical Guide 5 (1), Francis M. Hult and David C Johnson (eds).
Hoboken, NJ, John Wiley & Sons: 181–92.
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2004) Medical Interpreting and Cross-Cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
Angermeyer, Philipp S. (2009) “Translation style and participant roles in court interpreting”, Journal
of Sociolinguistics 13 (1): 3 –28.
——— (2015) Speak English or What? Code-Switching and Interpreter Use in New York City Courts. Oxford,
Oxford University Press.
Angermeyer, Philipp S., Bernd Meyer and Thomas Schmidt. (2012) “Sharing community interpreting
corpora: A pilot study”, in Multilingual Corpora and Multilingual Corpus Analysis, Thomas Schmidt
and Kai Wörner (eds). Hamburg Studies in Multilingualism 14, Amsterdam, John Benjamins:
275–94.
Angermeyer, Philipp S., and Bernd Meyer (2021) “Forms and functions of non-renditions in commu-
nity interpreting: A corpus-based study”, The Translator 27 (1): 1 19–36.
Austin, John L. (1962) How to Do Things with Words. The William James Lectures Delivered at Harvard
University in 1955, James O. Urmson (ed). Oxford, Clarendon Press.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
eds) (
Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
86
Corpus-based studies of PSI
Belzyk-Kohl, Yvonne (2016) “Some remarks on transcript translation in discourse analysis”, European
Journal of Applied linguistics 4 (1): 139–64.
Berk-Seligson, Susan (1990) The Bilingual Courtroom. Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.
——— (2009) Coerced Confessions: The Discourse of Bilingual Police Interrogations. Berlin, de Gruyter.
Bolden, Galina B (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: Interpreters’ in-
volvement in history taking”, Discourse Studies 2 (4): 387 ̶ 419.
Borgman, Christine L. (2015) Big Data, Little Data, No Data. Scholarship in the Networked World. Cam-
bridge, MA, MIT Press.
Braun, Sabine (2017) “W hat a m icro-analytical investigation of additions and expansions in remote
interpreting can tell us about interpreters’ participation in a shared virtual space”, Journal of Prag-
matics 107: 165–77.
Buchholtz, Mary (2000) “The politics of transcription”, Journal of Pragmatics 32 (10): 1439–65.
Caines, Andrew, Michael McCarthy, and Anne O’Keeffe (2016) “Spoken language corpora and ped-
agogical applications”, in The Routledge Handbook of Language Learning and Technology. Abingdon,
Routledge: 348–61.
Community Interpreting Database: https://corpora.uni-hamburg.de/hzsk/de/islandora/object/spoken-
corpus:comindat
Egbert, Maria, Mamiko Yufu, and Fumiya Hirataka (2016) “A n investigation of how 100 articles in
the Journal of Pragmatics treat transcripts of English and non-English languages”, Journal of Prag-
matics 94: 98–111.
Farag, Rahaf (2019) “Conversation-a nalytic transcription of A rabic-German t alk-i n-i nteraction”,
Working Papers in Corpus Linguistics and Digital Technologies: Analyses and Methodology 2: online.
Garfinkel, Harold. (1967/1984) Studies in Ethnomethodology. Malden, MA, Polity Press/ Blackwell
Publishing.
Goffman, Erving (1967) Interaction Ritual: Essays in Face-to-Face Behavior. Chicago, IL, Aldine.
Hepburn, Alexa, and Galina B. Bolden (2013) “The conversation analytic approach to transcription”,
in The Handbook of Conversation Analysis, Jack Sidnell and Tanya Stivers (eds). Chichester, Wiley-
Blackwell: 57–76.
Hymes, Dell (1962) “The ethnography of speaking”, in Anthropology and Human Behaviour, Thomas
Gladwin and William C. Sturtevant (eds). Washington, DC, Anthropological Society of America:
1 3–53.
Kaufert, Joseph M., and William Koolage (1984) “Role conflict among culture brokers: the experience
of native Canadian medical interpreters”, Social Sciences and Medicine 18 (3): 283–86.
Kennedy, Graeme (1998) An Introduction to Corpus Linguistics, London, Routledge.
Knapp, Karlfried, and K napp-Potthoff, Annelie (1986) “Interweaving two d iscourses – The diffi-
cult task of the n on-professional interpreter”, in Interlingual and Intercultural Communication, Juliane
House and Shoshana Blum-Kulka (eds). Tübingen, Gunter Narr: 450–63.
Lang, Rainer (1978) “Orderlies as interpreters in Papua New Guinea”, Papua New Guinea Medical
Journal 18 (3): 172–77.
Linell, Per (2004) The Written Language Bias in Linguistics: Its Nature, Origins and Transformations. Lon-
don, Routledge.
Meyer, Bernd (2004) Dolmetschen im medizinischen Aufklärungsgespräch. Eine diskursanalytische Untersu-
chung zur Arzt-Patienten-Kommunikation im mehrsprachigen Krankenhaus. Münster, Waxmann.
——— (2012) “Ad hoc interpreting for partially language-proficient patients: Participation in mul-
tilingual constellations”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and
Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 99–113.
——— (2016) “Case studies”, in Researching Interpreting and Translation, Claudia Angelelli and Brian
Baer (eds). London/New York, Routledge: 1 77–84.
——— (2019) “Corpus based studies on interpreting and pragmatics”, in The Routledge Handbook on
Pragmatics and Translation, Rebecca Tipton and Louise Desilla (eds). Abingdon/New York, Rout-
ledge: 75–92.
Müller, Frank E. (1989) “Translation in bilingual conversation: Pragmatic aspects of translator inter-
action”, Journal of Pragmatics 13: 713–39.
Newman, John, Harald Baayen, and Sally Rice (eds) (2011) Corpus-Based Studies in Language Use, Lan-
guage Learning, and Language Documentation. Amsterdam/New York, Rodopi.
Niemants, Natacha (2012) “The transcription of interpreting data”, Interpreting 14 (2): 1 65–91.
87
Bernd Meyer
Ochs, Elinor (1979) “Transcription as theory”, in Developmental Pragmatics, Elinor Ochs and Bambi B.
Schieffelin (eds). New York, Academic Press: 4 3–72.
Pasquandrea, Sergio (2011) “Managing multiple actions through multimodality: Doctors’ involvement
in i nterpreter-mediated interactions”, Language in Society 40 (04): 455–81.
Rehbein, Jochen, Thomas Schmidt, Bernd Meyer, Franziska Watzke, and Annette Herkenrath (2004)
Handbuch für das computergestützte Transkribieren nach HIAT (A rbeiten zur Mehrsprachigkeit: Folge B
56). Hamburg: Sonderforschungsbereich 538 Mehrsprachigkeit.
Schmidt, Thomas and Kai Wörner (2009) “EXMARaLDA – Creating, analysing and sharing spoken
language data for pragmatic research”, Pragmatics 19 (4): 565–82.
——— (2014) “E XMARaLDA”, in The Oxford Handbook of Corpus Phonology, Jacques Durand, Ulrike
Gut and Gjert Kristoffersen (eds). Oxford, Oxford University Press: 4 02–19.
Setton, Robin (2011) “Corpus-based interpreting studies (CIS): Overview and prospects”, in Corpus-
Based Translation Studies: Research and Applications, Alet Kruger, Kim Wallmach, and Jeremy Munday
(eds). London, Bloomsbury Publishing: 33–75.
Shlesinger, Miriam (1998) “Corpus-based interpreting studies as an offshoot of c orpus-based translation
studies”, Meta: journal des traducteurs/Meta: Translators’ Journal 43 (4): 486–93.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1992) Interpreting as Interaction: On D ialogue-Interpreting in Immigration Hearings and
Medical Encounters. PhD dissertation, Linköping University.
——— (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
88
6
TECHNOLOGY USE IN
LANGUAGE-DISCORDANT
INTERPERSONAL HEALTHCARE
COMMUNICATION
Sabine Braun, Khetam Al Sharou and Özlem Temizöz
Introduction
The optimal approach to establishing communication between healthcare providers and
patients who do not share the same language has been discussed for quite some time; the
debates have ultimately served to highlight the crucial role that linguistically and culturally
competent human interpreters play in these instances of language-discordant interpersonal
healthcare communication.
However, there are many barriers to accessing human interpreters in healthcare settings,
including waiting times, lack of financial resources, restricted local availability of qualified/
trained interpreters in some languages and, at times, a lack of practicability (for instance,
spontaneous communication on a hospital ward). Alternative approaches to accessing hu-
man interpreters and approaches to providing language support without the need for an
interpreter have been developed, which involve drawing on and interacting with different
types of technology. They range from the use of audio and video communication tools en-
abling remote interpretation to volunteer language support through crowdsourcing via dig-
ital platforms, machine translation (MT) and bespoke translation tools/apps. Some of these
approaches were initially developed for situations of medical emergency and/or humanitar-
ian crisis but have subsequently been explored in other settings of interpersonal healthcare
communication, shifting the patterns of interaction in these settings from h uman-to-human
interaction towards increasing involvement of human-machine interaction.
This chapter will review technology-
assisted approaches to facilitating language-
d iscordant interpersonal healthcare communication and explore the extent to which they
are currently capable of meeting patients’ and healthcare providers’ communication needs.
The focus is on situations of dialogic communication and interaction normally involving an
interpreter, a patient and/or caregiver and one or more healthcare providers. Technology-
a ssisted methods of translating written texts are not included (on the latter, see Dew et al.
2018; Haddow, Birch and Heafield 2021; Vieira, O’Hagan and O’Sullivan 2021). Following
a brief epistemological consideration of technology use in interpersonal healthcare settings,
each approach will be considered in a separate section, beginning with a review of the rele-
vant scholarly literature and main practical developments, followed by a discussion of critical
issues and challenges arising.
DOI: 10.4324/9780429298202-8 89
Sabine Braun et al.
Technology use as h
uman-machine interaction:
epistemological considerations
Although interpreter-mediated interaction is different from direct interaction between in-
terlocutors who speak the same language, for example in terms of t urn-taking organisation
(Wadensjö 1998; Baraldi and Gavioli 2012; Davitti 2013), it has traditionally developed as
a form of human interaction, particularly in institutional settings. In the classic triangular
relationship of dialogue interpreting, the interaction takes place both between the primary
interlocutors and between each of them and the interpreter. Linguistic and cultural media-
tion is negotiated and established between all participants involved.
The use of various technologies to assist, complement and/or replace human interpreters
has begun to expand and reshape the interaction in different ways. Most notably perhaps,
technology-mediated interpreting has evolved using audio or video communication tech-
nology either to connect a professional interpreter who is offsite to a healthcare provider
and patient who are co-located in the same space (remote interpreting, see Niemants, Stokoe
and Hansen this volume) or to facilitate language-discordant healthcare consultations which
take place over the phone or by video link (interpreting in telehealth consultations). In these mo-
dalities of interpreting, the d ual – l inguistic-cultural and technological – mediation (Braun
and Taylor 2012) entails multiple layers of interaction: not only do the healthcare provider
and the patient (that is the primary interlocutors) interact with each other through an in-
terpreter, but all participants also interact with the relevant communication technology,
namely screens, cameras and/or microphones and potentially other devices, as well as with
each other through this technology. The section entitled Technology-mediated interpreting in
the present chapter will give an overview of technology-mediated healthcare interpreting
and review emerging issues with these modalities of interpreting, including the impact of
technological mediation on participant satisfaction, interpreting quality and interaction.
Similar communication technologies have also been used to provide a different type of
language support, especially in situations where professional interpreters are not available or
where their involvement is not practicable. In such cases, real-time crowdsourcing of non-
professional language mediators has been adopted to enable access to the language support
available. Digital platforms used for this purpose support audio or video connections, or in
some cases a text chat. Similar to technology-mediated interpreting, this requires partici-
pants to interact with each other at a distance, through communication technology. The
potential ‘conversion’ of a traditionally spoken-language interaction into written interaction
creates additional questions, especially with regard to the effectiveness and coordination of
the interaction. This type of technology-mediated language support will be reviewed in
Real-time crowdsourcing translation and interpreting below.
Still a different approach is provided with the use of MT, either by generic tools such as
Google Translate or bespoke solutions (Dew et al. 2018; Haddow, Birch and Heafield 2021;
Vieira, O’Hagan and O’Sullivan 2021). In this case, the translation is taken over by an MT
engine, and the patient and healthcare provider interact directly with each other, and also
with and through a device and an interface that gives access to the MT software. This occurs
either in situations where the patient and provider are c o-located, using a smartphone or
tablet to access the translation software, or where they are connected through communica-
tion technology; this is for instance the case of telehealth consultations using an integrated
translation tool (Skype Translator or the like). The section called Machine Translation will be
dedicated to the different dimensions of the largely unexplored, complex human-human and
human–machine interaction arising in these settings.
90
Technology use in language-discordant communication
Technology-mediated interpreting
Technology-mediated modalities of interpreting have been utilised in healthcare commu-
nication for many years, initially in the form of telephone interpreting, but over the past
decade, video links have begun to replace telephone-based interpreting (see for instance
Marshall et al. 2019). Before the C ovid-19 pandemic, the most common use of both tele-
phone and video links in interpreter-assisted healthcare communication was to enable re-
mote interpreting, to connect an interpreter who is offsite to d octor-patient encounters in
which the doctor and the patient are c o-present in the same room (Rosenberg 2007; Lázaro
Gutiérrez 2019). With the rise of telehealth consultations by phone or video link during
the pandemic, it has also become more common to integrate interpreters in telehealth con-
sultations. In this configuration, the interpreter is either c o-located with one of the inter-
locutors or participates from a separate location, leading to a three-way communication
link (Rosenberg 2007; Schulz et al. 2015; Zhang, in preparation). While research on this
configuration is still scarce, the available evidence from research on remote interpreting can
provide transferable insights, given that all configurations share an element of remote work-
ing and remote interaction.
Literature overview
In relation to remote interpreting, the gradual replacement of the telephone with video links
has stimulated research comparing onsite interpreting with telephone and video remote in-
terpreting in terms of user preferences and user satisfaction with the communication and/or
care received ( Joseph, Garruba and Melder 2017). This research shows that patients or their
caregivers find all modalities of interpreting satisfactory (Locatis et al. 2010; Price et al. 2012;
Lion et al. 2015; Anttila et al. 2017), although there is some evidence that onsite interpreting
was preferred to the technology-mediated modalities, with video being preferred to tele-
phone interpreting (Lion et al. 2015; Anttila et al. 2017). Where healthcare providers and/or
interpreters were surveyed, they generally preferred onsite interpreting, and, among the
technology-mediated modalities, video to telephone (A zarmina and Wallace 2005; Locatis
et al. 2010; Price et al. 2012). Furthermore, in a study comparing different modalities of
delivery as well as different modes of interpreting, remote simultaneous interpreting via
telephone, which was a new service offered by some US hospitals at the time of the study,
yielded higher levels of patient satisfaction than onsite interpreting and remote consecu-
tive interpreting via telephone (Gany et al. 2007). Two surveys of telephone interpreters
91
Sabine Braun et al.
92
Technology use in language-discordant communication
93
Sabine Braun et al.
modality compared to both onsite and telephone interpreting requires greater awareness of
the importance of spatial arrangements (Davitti and Braun 2020; Klammer and Pöchhacker
2021). Although the relationship between the interactional dimension and other dimensions
of technology-mediated interpreting is under-explored, the detailed analyses of interactional
issues in remote interpreting partially call into question the findings from user satisfaction/
experience studies, which suggest that v ideo-mediated interpreting has more similarities
with onsite interpreting. The i nteraction-focussed studies thus highlight the need for further
in-depth investigations of technology-mediated interpreting as modalities in its own right.
One aspect that interaction- focused studies of technology-mediated interpreting in
healthcare (a nd other) settings have been able to highlight is the potential for adaptation to
the novel modalities. For instance, these studies have identified adapted interactional strat-
egies employed by interpreters (e.g. more explicit coordination; Braun 2007; Davitti and
Braun 2020) and by healthcare providers (e.g. chunking the source speech for better control
of the interaction; Klammer and Pöchhacker 2021). The C ovid-19 pandemic brought a
sudden shift to much more technology-mediated interpreting including a diversification in
the configurations and greater hybridity and complexity in the use of communication media
and devices. The potential for adaptation is therefore an area that needs to be pursued in re-
search and practice, including learning to adapt through education and training (see Amato,
Spinolo and Rodriguez 2018).
Literature overview
Crowdsourcing translation is a form of online collaboration established to respond to urgent
or otherwise unmet communication needs, fulfilled voluntarily by a network of people with
differing profiles and levels of language proficiency. It can be paid or unpaid, and has also
been described as ‘volunteer’, ‘collaborative,’ ‘social’, ‘non-professional’ and ‘community’
translation (O’Brien 2011; Jiménez-Crespo 2017). Crowdsourcing translation has become
an integral part of responding to natural or human-instigated disasters, violent political
upheaval, or health crises, especially when the need arises to carry out aid/rescue work in-
stantly due to the urgency of saving lives and when this need cannot be met by drawing on
professional interpreting services. In these situations, support for providing humanitarian aid
and healthcare often overlap, including emergency medical services, trauma counselling, and
rescue operations. Crowdsourcing platforms that connect volunteer translators/interpreters
and non-governmental organisations (NGOs) have facilitated communication in various
settings. For example, during the 2010 earthquake in Haiti, Twitter and short message ser-
vice (SMS) communications were facilitated using a crowdsourcing platform with an inte-
grated map, which enabled “volunteers from within the Haitian Creole and F rench-speaking
94
Technology use in language-discordant communication
95
Sabine Braun et al.
their impact on the user experience and the translated outcome would be a useful step in
future research.
Machine translation
A different approach to meeting communication needs through technology has been to use
MT. This approach was triggered by the increasing availability of MT systems, including
both generic systems and customised systems. Generic MT tools such as Google Translate
offer free access to an increasing number of language pairs, but the translations they produce
are unreliable and raise issues about liability and data privacy. Customised MT systems focus
on increasing accuracy by training the MT engine for a specific domain. A growing body
of studies explores the use of MT tools for written communication in healthcare contexts
(Turner et al. 2015; Chen and Acosta 2019). However, the present overview focuses on the
use of MT tools in dialogic interpersonal communication. One aspect that is particularly im-
portant in this context is therefore the evolution and use of MT systems capable of processing
and/or generating speech output.
Literature overview
Speech-to-text and speech-to-speech MT solutions have a long history but have remained
experimental for a long time (Seligman, Waibel and Joscelyne 2017). MT systems that pro-
cess and/or generate spoken output are normally a concatenation of automatic speech recog-
nition (ASR) to process spoken source-language input, an MT engine to achieve the actual
language transfer and an optional speech synthesis system to create spoken target-language
output. The processing of spoken language brings additional challenges, because ASR is
at different stages of development for different languages and generally still has difficulty
coping with features that are characteristic of interpersonal communication (for instance,
overlap between speakers and background noise in the environment).
Publicly available generic MT tools have improved over the past decade, and translation
apps such as Google Translate and Microsoft Translator offer translation of written and
spoken input into speech or text output in n ear-real time in a growing number of language
pairs. Evidence of the use of generic MT tools in daily clinical practice emerged recently
as a by-product of a study examining attitudes to vaccination among Polish and Romanian
communities in England (Moberly 2018a). A large proportion of healthcare professionals
involved in delivering vaccines to these communities reported relying on free MT tools.
While official guidance in the UK does not endorse their use in medical consultations,
the healthcare workers believed that the tools were more accessible than professional inter-
preting services, particularly during t ime-pressured appointments. Reacting to this finding,
medical advisers pointed out that tools such as Google Translate should not be used in ev-
eryday clinical practice because of the risk of introducing communication errors and under-
mining patient safety, leaving doctors vulnerable to legal action (Moberly 2018b). However,
the advisers contended that MT tools may have a limited role in an emergency or in other
exceptional circumstances.
Research systematically trialling the use of generic MT tools as a potential alternative
to interpreting services in interpersonal healthcare communication started from a similar
premise and has explored the quality of MT in situations where time constraints make it
difficult to access interpreters, especially emergency situations, and/or where there is a lack
of interpreters for specific language pairs. For example, Nguyen-Lu, Reide and Yentis (2010)
96
Technology use in language-discordant communication
evaluated the use of Google Translate for translating common anaesthetic questions in acute
emergencies, while Haith-Cooper (2014) tested its use in maternity services. Both studies
concluded that the support provided by Google Translate was limited. Turner et al. (2019)
simulated a medical emergency situation to compare the use of Google Translate with a
fi xed-sentence translation tool, finding, however, that the usefulness of Google Translate in
this situation was limited due to translation errors and difficulties responding to questions,
i.e. interactional issues (see also Bouillon et al. 2017).
In assessments of the use of Google Translate to translate emergency department dis-
charge instructions from English into Spanish and Chinese, Khoong et al. (2 019) found
that a high proportion of sentences were translated accurately (92% for Spanish and 81%
for Chinese) but that 2% of Spanish and 8% of Chinese sentence translations had potential
for significant/life threatening harm, for instance due to errors in word disambiguation
problems. In a similar study analysing further language pairs, Taira et al. (2 021) found
the performance of Google Translate to be inconsistent across seven language pairs, with
accuracy rates ranging from 55% to 94%. Testing the capability of another generic transla-
tion app, iTranslate, to translate questions that diabetes patients commonly ask clinicians,
Chen, Acosta and Barry (2 017) found the MT output to be comparable to human trans-
lation in terms of accuracy for simple sentences but e rror-prone for complex sentences.
None of the studies referenced here has addressed questions of how the users interact with
the apps.
As generic MT systems do not provide translation of good quality, a different approach
has been used to develop customised MT systems to cater for the needs in specific contexts/
domains. Several projects have focused on the development of customised one-way speech-
to-speech translation systems. Their primary interest has been the quality that can be
achieved with a customised MT system, although the findings are also interesting from an
interactional perspective. For example, SRI International developed MedSLT, a system that
translates doctor’s diagnostic questions to patients, who can answer with yes or no, or with
body language. While simulated doctor–patient encounters using this tool showed that this
approach is workable from an interactional point of view, despite the limitations it imposes
on the interaction, the system was found to produce translation errors that can cause diag-
nostic errors (Starlander and Estrella 2009). Another MT system, which was developed to
assist English-speaking patients in a South Korean hospital to describe their symptoms to
Korean healthcare professionals (Shin et al. 2015), was integrated into a humanoid robot
to improve the user’s interaction with it. In this case, the MT was evaluated on sentences
with limited vocabulary and structures and was found to be 84% accurate for native English
speech and 64% for non-native English speech. The robot was able to move around the hos-
pital to assist patients in need, and the interaction with it was deemed easy and not requiring
specific operating knowledge.
An example of a two-way speech-to-speech system is Converser for Healthcare, which
translates conversations between English-speaking healthcare professionals and Spanish-
speaking patients in hospital in-patient nursing situations (Seligman and Dillinger 2015).
This system applies interactive verification and other techniques such as backtranslation and
correction of the MT output to mitigate the issues arising in the process of speech recogni-
tion and/or in the MT component. The translation quality of the system was not evaluated.
In a user experience study, hospital staff perceived the MT quality as good enough, although
most patients reported that they prefer a human interpreter. In addition, the user interface of
the tool was deemed to be complex, and users found it hard to use the tablet on which the
tool was running in hospital (Seligman and Dillinger 2015).
97
Sabine Braun et al.
98
Technology use in language-discordant communication
is likely to remain problematic. For example, Haith-Cooper (2014), who concluded that the
use of Google Translate in maternity services is inadequate, recommended the development
of sets of midwifery terminology in different languages and their use in an app to facilitate
multilingual communication between midwives and those who receive maternity services.
This approach will be reviewed in the next section.
Literature overview
Some phrasebook apps allow users to select only from the p re-loaded phrases in the app.
Other apps support f ree-text or speech input and use speech recognition and other language
technologies to match the phrase entered by the user to the pre-loaded phrases, enabling
healthcare professionals and patients to ask questions or describe symptoms in their own
words. An example of a text-based tool is Xprompt (A lbrecht et al. 2013), which draws on a
set of 800 phrases available in over 20 languages. Both patients and healthcare professionals
can search for relevant phrases in their respective source languages in text format. The target
language equivalents are available in text format, audio and as video clips with sign-language
versions. A speech-enabled tool is BabelDr (Bouillon et al. 2017), which ‘l istens’ to questions
asked by healthcare professionals and retrieves a p re-loaded canonical version of the same
questions in advance of retrieving the target language version, which can be shown as text
or played as an audio message. BabelDr uses closed questions to which patients are expected
to respond non-verbally, by nodding or pointing.
A recent study evaluated 15 apps with regard to the extent to which they support every-
day healthcare communication needs (Panayiotou et al. 2019). Only two (CALD Assist and
Talk To Me) were deemed suitable for everyday interpersonal healthcare communication.
At the time of the evaluation, both apps were limited to text input and mainly used closed
questions, that is operated at the most restrictive level of support that such apps provide,
while minimising risks from mismatching speech input and/or free-text input. They were
also the only apps that included an explicit disclaimer about their limitations and emphasised
the importance of using professional interpreters where possible.
In a further study, Turner et al. (2019) tested Google Translate and QuickSpeak, a
phrase-based translation tool, with providers of emergency medical services and Spanish and
Chinese-speaking patients, using simulations and post-session questionnaires. QuickSpeak
received higher usability scores than Google Translate, and patients preferred QuickSpeak
over Google Translate in terms of understanding the questions asked by the emergency
personnel. Both the emergency staff and the patients positively highlighted the flexibility of
Google Translate but emphasised that its usefulness was limited by multiple translation errors
and difficulty in responding to questions.
An approach that has similarities with the use of phrase-based translation tools is Aug-
mentative and Alternative Communication (A AC), which was originally developed to assist
99
Sabine Braun et al.
people with severe speech and language impairments to communicate their needs and feel-
ings (Enderby et al. 2013). Some AAC tools offer picture-based symbols matched with words
to support communication. While they were originally designed to support patients in inten-
sive care units, Somers and Lovel (2006) trialled their use with linguistic-minority patients.
Their findings suggest that AAC tools can improve the prospects of communication in the
absence of a professional interpreter. Similar tools have also been developed to provide basic
communication support in humanitarian crisis situations. For example, ICOON for refugees
(Lloyd 2019), which was developed in cooperation with aid organisations, includes 1,200
symbols and photos to help refugees overcome language barriers and make themselves under-
stood quickly in different situations. A multimodal open-source app called Translation Cards,
developed in a collaboration between UNHCR, Google, Mercy Corps, ThoughtWorks and
Translators Without Borders in response to the 2015 refugee crisis, offers audio-visual flash-
cards for communication. It allows refugees to read and listen to pre-translated phrases in
their own language. Aid organisation can create different sets of card decks to include, for
example translations of basic questions that can be answered with yes/no, non-verbally, or by
showing a document, as well as p re-translated answers to questions that refugees frequently
ask (Drew 2016).
Other tools that focus on healthcare needs have gone beyond translation functionality or
have complemented it. For example, HABABY, an app for prenatal and postnatal care for
refugee women provides country, symptom and medication information in multiple lan-
guages while also offering an anonymous message board and the option for live chat with a
healthcare professional and other features (Federici and O’Brien 2019).
Conclusion
The need to reduce language barriers in interpersonal healthcare communication is grow-
ing in many countries due to global migration. Professional interpreting services, as the
gold standard, can be extended through the use of communication technology, especially
100
Technology use in language-discordant communication
Further reading
Dew, Kristin N., Anne M. Turner, Yong K. Choi, Alyssa Bosold, and Katrin Kirchhoff (2018)
“Development of machine translation technology for assisting health communication: A system-
atic review”, Journal of Biomedical Informatics 85 (September): 56–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jbi.2018.07.018.
This systematic review explores the extent to which machine translation can be used in language-discordant
healthcare communication and the steps that need to be taken to facilitate its adoption and improve its
effectiveness.
Joseph, Corey, Marie Garruba, and Angela Melder (2017) “Patient satisfaction of telephone or video
interpreter services compared with In-person services: A systematic review”, Australian Health
Review 42 (2): 168–77. https://doi.org/10.1071/A H16195.
This review article synthesises previous findings regarding patient satisfaction with the three modalities of
interpreting service delivery: telephone, video-mediated and in-person interpreting.
Klammer, Martina, and Franz Pöchhacker (2021) “Video remote interpreting in clinical communi-
cation: A multimodal analysis”, Patient Education and Counselling 104 (12): 2867–76. https://doi.
org/10.1016/J.PEC.2021.08.024.
The article provides an in-depth analysis of remote interpreting via video link in a doctor-patient encoun-
ter, illustrating the challenges and affordances of this modality of interpreting for the interpreter and the main
interlocutors.
Panayiotou, Anita, Anastasia Gardner, Sue Williams, Emiliano Zucchi, Monita Mascitti-Meuter, Anita
M.Y. Goh, Emily You, Terence W.H. Chong, Dina Logiudice, Xiaoping Lin, Betty Haralambous,
and Frances Batchelor (2019) “Language translation apps in health care settings: Expert opinion”,
JMIR MHealth and UHealth 7 (4): e11316. https://doi.org/10.2196/11316.
This article evaluates several phrasebook apps to assess their suitability for facilitating everyday conversations
in healthcare settings. It includes an analysis of the apps’ features and a set of evaluation criteria.
101
Sabine Braun et al.
Vieira, Lucas Nunes, Minako O’Hagan, and Carol O’Sullivan (2 021) “Understanding the socie-
tal impacts of machine translation: A critical review of the literature on medical and legal use
case”, Information, Communication and Society 24 (11): 1515–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.
2020.1776370.
Based on a critical review of official documents and published research, this article reflects on the consequences
of uninformed uses of machine translation in high-risk domains including healthcare settings.
Related chapters
Chapter 5, Corpus-based studies of public service interpreting by Bernd Meyer
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences by Christian Licoppe
Chapter 20, Blended learning is here to stay! Combining on-line and on-campus learning in the education of public
service interpreters by Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: how authentic data meet simulations for interpreter train-
ing by Natacha Niemants, Jessica P. B. Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véro-
nique Traverso and Emilie Jouin
References
Agarwal, Divyansh, Yuta Baba, Pratik Sachdeva, Tanya Tandon, Thomas Vetterli, and Aziz Alghu-
naim (2020) “Accurate and scalable matching of translators to displaced persons for overcoming
language barriers”, Paper presented at NeurIPS 2020 Workshop on Machine Learning for the
Developing World, online, 12 December 2020. https://doi.org/10.48550/a rxiv.2012.02595.
Albrecht, Urs-Vito, Marianne Behrends, Herbert K. Matthies, and Ute von Jan (2013) “Usage of
multilingual mobile translation applications in clinical settings”, JMIR MHealth UHealth 1 (1): e4.
https://doi.org/10.2196/m health.2268.
Amato, Amalia, Nicoletta Spinolo, and Maria José Rodriguez ( 2018) Handbook of Remote
eds) (
Interpreting -SHIFT in Orality. University of Bologna: AMS Acta -Open access repository https://
doi.org/10.6092/u nibo/a msacta/5955.
Anastasiou, Dimitra, and Rajat Gupta (2011) “Comparison of crowdsourcing translation with machine
translation”, Journal of Information Science 37 (6): 637–59.
Anttila, Ashley, David I. Rappaport, Johan Tijerino, Nusrat Zaman, and Iman Sharif ( 2017)
“Interpretation modalities used on f amily-centered rounds: Perspectives of S panish-speaking fam-
ilies”, Hospital Pediatrics 7 (8): 4 92–98. https://doi.org/10.1542/H PEDS.2016-0209.
Azarmina Pejman, and Paul Wallace (2005) “Remote interpretation in medical encounters: A sys-
tematic review”, Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare 11 (3), 140–45. https://doi.org/10.1258/
1357633053688679. PMID: 15901441.
Balkul, Halil İbrahim (2018) “A comparative analysis of translation/interpreting tools developed
for the Syrian refugee crisis”, International Journal of Language Academy Göç 6: 32–4 4. https://doi.
org/10.18033/ijla.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
eds) (
Amsterdam, John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/ BTL.102.
Bouillon, Pierrette, Johanna Gerlach, Hervé Spechbach, Nikolaos Tsourakis, and Ismahene Sonia
Halimi Mallem (2017) “BabelDr vs Google translate: A user study at Geneva university hospi-
tals (H UG)”, in 20th Annual Conference of the European Association for Machine Translation (E AMT).
Prague, Czech Republic. https://a rchive- ouverte.unige.ch/u nige:94511.
Bowker, Lynne, and Jairo Buitrago Ciro ( 2019) Machine Translation and Global Research: Towards
Improved Machine Translation Literacy in the Scholarly Community. Bingley, Emerald Publishing Lim-
ited. https://doi.org/10.1108/9781787567214.
Braun, Sabine (2007) “Interpreting in small-group bilingual videoconferences”, Interpreting 9 (1): 21–46.
https://doi.org/10.1075/intp.9.1.03bra.
Braun, Sabine, and Judith L. Taylor (2012) Videoconferencing and Interpreting in Criminal Proceedings.
Antwerp, Intersentia.
102
Technology use in language-discordant communication
Chen, Xuewei, Sandra Acosta, and Adam E. Barry (2017) “Machine or human? Evaluating the quality
of a language translation mobile app for diabetes education material”, JMIR Diabetes 2 (1): e13.
https://doi.org/10.2196/d iabetes.7446.
Chen, Xuewei, and Sandra Acosta (2019) “Is machine language translation a viable tool for health
communication?”, in Cross-Cultural Health Translation, Meng Ji (ed). New York, Routledge: 163–
75. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429054402-10.
Davitti, Elena (2013) “Dialogue interpreting as intercultural mediation”, Interpreting 15 (2): 168–99.
https://doi.org/10.1075/i ntp.15.2.02dav.
Davitti, Elena, and Sabine Braun (2020) “A nalysing interactional phenomena in video remote inter-
preting in collaborative settings: Implications for interpreter education”, The Interpreter and Transla-
tor Trainer 14 (3): 279–302. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750399X.2020.1800364.
De Boe, Esther (2020) Remote Interpreting in Healthcare Settings: A Comparative Study on the Influence of
Telephone and Video Link Use on the Quality of Interpreter-Mediated Communication. Antwerp, Univer-
siteit Antwerpen. https://hdl.handle.net/10067/1682650151162165141.
Downie, Jonathan, and Angela Dickson (2019) “Unsound evaluations of medical machine transla-
tion risk patient health and confidentiality”, JAMA Internal Medicine 179 (7 ): 1001. https://doi.
org/10.1001/JAMAINTERNMED.2019.1856.
Drew, Katie (2016) “Making sure refugees aren’t lost in t ranslation – with one simple app”, UNHCR
open access repository https://w ww.unhcr.org/i nnovation/m aking-sure-refugees-a rent-lost-
t ranslation- one-simple-app/.
Enderby, Pam, Simon Judge, Sarah Creer, and Alexandra John (2013) Beyond the Anecdote: Examining
the Need for, and Provision of, AAC in the United Kingdom, Research Report. Communication Matters.
Edinburgh EH, Communication Matters. https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/76406/1/2013_AAC_
Evidence_Base_Beyond_the_Anecdote.pdf.
Federici, Federico M., and Khetam Al Sharou (2018) “Moses, time, and crisis translation”, Translation
and Interpreting Studies 13 (3): 486–508. https://doi.org/10.1075/t is.00026.fed.
Federici, Federico M., and Sharon O’Brien (eds) (2019) Translation in Cascading Crises. New York,
Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429341052.
Fernández Pérez, Maria Magdalena (2017) “Interaction management skills in telephone interpreting”,
Revista Canaria de Estudios Ingleses 75: 103–17. http://r iull.ull.es/x mlui/handle/915/6968.
Fiedler, Jonas, Susanne Pruskil, Christian Wiessner, Thomas Zimmermann, and Martin Scherer
(2022) “Remote interpreting in primary care settings: A feasibility trial in Germany”, BMC Health
Services Research 22 (1): 1 –12. https://doi.org/10.1186/S12913-021-07372-6/TABLES/6.
Gany, Francesca, Jennifer Leng, Ephraim Shapiro, David Abramson, Ivette Motola, David C. Shield,
and Jyotsna Changrani (2007) “Patient satisfaction with different interpreting methods: A ran-
domized controlled trial”, Journal of General Internal Medicine 22 (SUPPL. 2): 312–18. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11606-0 07-0360-8.
Haddow, Barry, Alexandra Birch, and Kenneth Heafield (2021) “Machine translation in healthcare”,
in The Routledge Handbook of Translation and Health, Şebnem Susam-Saraeva and Eva Spišiaková
(eds). New York, Routledge: 108–29. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003167983-10.
Haith- Cooper, Melanie (2 014) “Mobile translators for Non-English- speaking women accessing
m aternity services”, British Journal of Midwifery 22: 795– 8 03. https://doi.org/10.12968/ bjom.2014.22.
11.795.
Hansen, Jessica Pedersen Belisle (2020) “Invisible participants in a visual ecology: Visual space as
a resource for Organising v ideo-mediated interpreting in hospital encounters”, Social Interaction.
Video-Based Studies of Human Sociality 3 (3). https://doi.org/10.7146/si.v3i3.122609.
Hornberger, John C., Count D. Gibson, William Wood, Christian Dequeldre, Irene Corso, Barbara
Palla, and Daniel A. Bloch (1996) “Eliminating language barriers for Non-English-speaking pa-
tients”, Medical Care 34 (8): 845–56. https://doi.org/10.1097/0 0005650-199608000-0 0011.
Hsieh, Elaine (2006) “Understanding medical interpreters: Reconceptualizing bilingual health com-
munication”, Health Communication 20 (2): 177–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc2002_9.
Jiménez-Crespo, Miguel A. (2017) Crowdsourcing and Online Collaborative Translations. Amsterdam, John
Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/btl.131.
Khoong, Elaine C., and Alicia Fernandez (2019) “Unsound evaluations of medical machine translation
risk patient health and confidentiality -reply”, JAMA Internal Medicine 179 (7 ): 1 001–2 . https://doi.
org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2019.1878.
103
Sabine Braun et al.
Khoong, Elaine C., and Jorge A. Rodriguez (2022) “A research agenda for using machine translation
in clinical medicine”, Journal of General Internal Medicine, 37: 1275–77. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11606-021-07164-y.
Khoong, Elaine C., Eric Steinbrook, Cortlyn Brown, and Alicia Fernandez (2019) “A ssessing the use of
Google translate for Spanish and Chinese translations of emergency department discharge instruc-
tions”, JAMA Internal Medicine 179 (4): 580. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.7653.
Krystallidou, Demi, and Sabine Braun (2022) “R isk and crisis communication during COVID-19 in
linguistically and culturally diverse communities: A scoping review of the available evidence”, in
The Language(s) of Covid-19: Implications for Global Healthcare, Steven Wilson and Piotr Blumczynski
(eds). New York, Routledge: forthcoming.
Lázaro Gutiérrez, Raquel (2019) “Telephone interpreting and roadside assistance”, Translation and
Translanguaging in Multilingual Contexts 5 (3): 2 15–40. https://doi.org/10.1075/t tmc.00033.laz.
Lázaro Gutiérrez, Raquel, and Gabriel Cabrera Méndez (2019) “Context and pragmatic meaning in
telephone interpreting”, in Technology Mediated Service Encounters, Pilar Garcés-Conejos Blitvich,
Lucía F ernández-Amaya, and María de la O H ernández-López (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins:
45– 68. https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.300.02laz.
Lee, Jieun (2007) “Telephone i nterpreting — seen from the interpreters’ perspective”, Interpreting 9 (2):
231–52. https://doi.org/10.1075/i ntp.9.2.05lee.
Lion, K. Casey, Julie C. Brown, Beth E. Ebel, Eileen J. Klein, Bonnie Strelitz, Colleen Kays Gutman,
Patty Hencz, Juan Fernandez, and Rita Mangione-Smith (2015) “Effect of telephone vs video in-
terpretation on parent comprehension, communication, and utilization in the pediatric emergency
department”, JAMA Pediatrics 169 (12): 1117. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2630.
Lloyd, Armağan Teke (2019) Exclusion and Inclusion in International Migration: Power, Resistance and Iden-
tity. London, Transnational Press.
Locatis, Craig, Deborah Williamson, Carrie Gould-Kabler, Laurie Zone-Smith, Isabel Detzler, Jason
Roberson, Richard Maisiak, and Michael Ackerman (2010) “Comparing in-person, video, and
telephonic medical interpretation”, Journal of General Internal Medicine 25 (4): 345–50. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11606-0 09-1236-x.
Marshall, Lori C., Amani Zaki, Marlon Duarte, Avelino Nicolas, Jennifer Roan, Anna Fischer Colby,
Allison L. Noyes, and Glenn Flores (2019) “Promoting effective communication with limited
English proficient families: Implementation of video remote interpreting as part of a comprehen-
sive language services program in a children’s hospital”, The Joint Commission Journal on Quality and
Patient Safety 45 ( 7 ): 509–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JCJQ.2019.04.001.
Moberly, Tom (2018a) “Doctors choose google translate to communicate with patients because of easy
access”, The British Medical Journal 362: k3974. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.k3974.
——— (2018b) “Doctors are cautioned against using Google Translate in consultations”, The British
Medical Journal 363: k4546. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.k4546.
Munro, Robert (2013) “Crowdsourcing and the crisis-affected community: Lessons learned and
looking forward from mission 4636”, Information Retrieval 16 (2): 210–66. https://doi.org/10.
1007/S10791-012-9203-2/TABLES/10.
Nguyen-Lu, Nhathien, Peter Reide, and Steve Yentis (2010) “‘Do you have a stick in your mouth?’–
use of Google Translate as an aid to anaesthetic p re-assessment”, Anaesthesia 65 (1): 96–97. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2044.2009.06184_4.x.
O’Brien, Sharon (2011) “Collaborative translation”, in Handbook of Translation Studies, Yves Gambier
and Luc Doorslaer (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 17–20.
Price, Erika Leemann, Eliseo J. Pérez-Stable, Dana Nickleach, Monica López, and Leah S. Karliner
(2012) “Interpreter perspectives of in-person, telephonic, and videoconferencing medical inter-
pretation in clinical encounters”, Patient Education and Counseling 87 (2): 226–32. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.pec.2011.08.006.
Rosenberg, Brett Allen (2007) “A data driven analysis of telephone interpreting”, in The Critical Link
4 Professionalisation of Interpreting in the Community. Selected Papers from the 4th International Conference
on Interpreting in Legal, Health and Social Service Settings, Stockholm, Sweden, 20–23 May 2004, Cecilia
Wadensjö, Birgitta Englund Dimitrova, and A nna-Lena Nilsson (eds). Amsterdam, John Benja-
mins: 65–76. https://doi.org/10.1075/ btl.70.09ros.
Ruiz Mezcua, Aurora (ed) (2018) Approaches to Telephone Interpretation: Research, Innovation, Teaching and
Transference. Bern, Peter Lang. https://doi.org/10.3726/b13326.
104
Technology use in language-discordant communication
Schulz, Thomas R., Karin Leder, Ismail Akinci, and Beverley Ann Biggs (2015) “Improvements
in patient care: Videoconferencing to improve access to interpreters during clinical consulta-
tions for refugee and immigrant patients”, Australian Health Review 39 (4): 395–99. https://doi.
org/10.1071/A H14124.
Seligman, Mark, and Mike Dillinger (2015) “Evaluation and revision of a speech translation system for
healthcare” in Proceedings of the International Workshop on Spoken Language Translation (I WSLT), 209–
16. http://workshop2015.iwslt.org/d ownloads/ I WSLT_2015_RP_4.pdf %0Ahttp://w ww.m t-
a rchive.info/15/I WSLT-2015-seligman.pdf.
Seligman, Mark, Alex Waibel, and Andrew Joscelyne (2017) “TAUS speech-to-speech translation tech-
nology report” https://isl.anthropomatik.kit.edu/downloads/S2STranslationTechnologyReport.
final.pdf.
Shin, Sangmi, Eric T. Matson, Jinok Park, Bowon Yang, Juhee Lee, and Jin- Woo Jung ( 2015)
“Speech-to-speech translation humanoid robot in doctor’s office” in 2015 6th International Con-
ference on Automation, Robotics and Applications (ICARA), 4 84–89. IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/
ICARA.2015.7081196.
Somers, Harold, and Hermione Lovel (2006) “Can AAC technology facilitate communication for pa-
tients with limited English?” ESRC: Final Report. https://personalpages.manchester.ac.uk/staff/
harold.somers/ESRCfinal.pdf
Starlander, Marianne, and Paula Estrella (2009) “Relating recognition and translation quality with us-
ability of two different versions of MedSLT”, in Proceedings of the Twelfth Machine Translation Summit
(M T Summit XII), Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: 324–31. https://w ww.issco.unige.ch/pub/Starlander_
NLPCS_cameraready2.pdf.
Sutherlin, Gwyneth (2013) “A voice in the crowd: broader implications for crowdsourcing translation
during crisis”, Journal of Information Science 39 (3): 397–409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165551512471593.
Taira, Breena R., Vanessa Kreger, Aristides Orue, and Lisa C. Diamond (2021) “A pragmatic assess-
ment of google translate for emergency department instructions”, Journal of General Internal Medicine
36 (11): 3361–65. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-021-06666-z.
Tanner, Lidia, and Alice Obrecht (2015) “Words of relief: Translators without borders’ local lan-
guage translation for emergencies”, HIF/A LNAP Case Study. London: ODI/A LNAP. https://
t ranslatorswithoutborders.org/w p- content/uploads/2 016/0 5/a lnap-i nnovation-wordsofrelief-
case-study.pdf.
Turner, Anne M., Megumu K. Brownstein, Kate Cole, Hilary Karasz, and Katrin Kirchhoff (2015)
“Modeling workflow to design machine translation applications for public health practice”, Journal
of Biomedical Informatics 53 (February): 136. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JBI.2014.10.005.
Turner, Anne M., Yong K. Choi, Kristin Dew, M ing-Tse Tsai, Alyssa L. Bosold, Shuyang Wu, Dona-
hue Smith, and Hendrika Meischke (2019) “Evaluating the usefulness of translation technologies
for emergency response communication: a scenario-based study”, JMIR Public Health and Surveil-
lance 5 (1): e11171. https://doi.org/10.2196/11171.
Wadensjö, Cecilia ( 1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman. https:// w ww.routledge.
com/Interpreting-A s-Interaction/ Wadensjo/p/book/9780582289109.
Wang, Jihong (2018) “‘It keeps me on my toes’. Interpreters’ perceptions of challenges in telephone
interpreting and their coping strategies”, Target. International Journal of Translation Studies 30 (3):
439–73. https://doi.org/10.1075/t arget.17012.wan.
Whitaker, Katriina L., Demi Krystallidou, Emily D. Williams, Georgia Black, Cecilia Vindrola-Padros,
Sabine Braun, and Paramjit Gill (2022) “Addressing language as a barrier to healthcare access and
quality”, British Journal of General Practice 72 (714): 4 –5. https://doi.org/10.3399/BJGP22X718013.
Zhang, Wei (in preparation) Perspectives on remote medical interpreting: A m ixed-methods ap-
proach. Doctoral Thesis, University of Surrey.
105
7
PUBLIC SERVICE TRANSLATION
Critical issues and future directions
Mustapha Taibi
Introduction
Public service translation (PST) plays a key role in facilitating communication between
public services and the general public, particularly in situations where the two sides do not
share the same language. On the one hand, PST allows government departments, agencies
and services (a s well as the non-government sector) to reach out to all members of a society.
On the other, it allows speakers of minority or minoritised languages access to public texts
that would normally be inaccessible due to language barriers.
PST has attracted some research attention in the last two decades. The International Com-
munity Translation Research Group was founded towards the end of 2013. A year later, the
First International Conference on Community Translation was organised at Western Sydney
University. In 2019 the second conference was organised at RMIT University, Melbourne.
A few publications have come out (e.g. Taibi 2011; Lesch 2012; Córdoba Serrano 2016; Taibi
and Ozolins 2016; Tomozeiu 2016; García 2018; Kelly 2018; Määttä 2020). However, PST is
still a developing area of Translation Studies. An indication of its early developmental stage
is the conceptual and terminological variation surrounding it. PST is also known as Com-
munity Translation. Community Translation, in turn, is used to refer to different types of
translation: professional PST (e.g. Taibi and Ozolins 2016; Taibi 2018) or volunteer (crowd-
sourced) translation for online communities (O’Hagan 2011; García 2018). This variation
does not only indicate the terminological diversity that characterises language and communi-
cation disciplines but also denotes different assumptions about professional standards. García
(2018) suggests that the two notions and practices of Community Translation can work hand
in hand, in such a way that h igh-stake translation materials are assigned to professional trans-
lators, and non-critical ones are undertaken by bilingual volunteers or amateurs.
In this chapter, the focus is on PST or Community Translation as a professional service
consisting of written translation intended to bridge the communication gap between public
services and relevant organisations on the one hand and speakers of minority or minoritised
languages on the other. ‘Professional service’ is understood as ‘a paid occupation which requires
a formal qualification’ (Schäffner 2020: 64; see also Skaaden, this volume). Formal qualification
can consist of a relevant education degree, professional certification or any other adequate form
of credentialing. In this sense, it contrasts with non-professional translation, which is offered by
‘individuals not only without formal training in linguistic mediation but also working for free’
(Pérez González and Susam-Sarayeva 2012: 151). However, while the focus is on PST as a pro-
fessional activity, reference is occasionally made to non-professional translation where relevant.
The chapter starts with theoretical considerations and a literature review covering recent
contributions, then discusses some of the most prominent issues in PST (quality assurance,
translation accessibility, and dissemination methods), and finally looks at future directions,
with a focus on multimodal dissemination methods and potential interdisciplinary collabo-
ration with scholarly areas such as social marketing.
Translator agency
Given the social mission of PST and the social contexts where it is usually offered (or
needed), translator agency is central to this area of translation practice. Translation in general
107
Mustapha Taibi
is a social activity performed by social actors to fulfil certain social needs (Tyulenev 2014:
126). As Way (2016: 1018) affirms, ‘translators are social agents who participate not only in
the communication process but in society as a whole’. Among other senses, the notion of
‘agency’ is understood as ‘human action… which makes a difference to a human relationship
or behaviour’ ( Jary and Jary 2000), and as such it applies more specifically to PS translators.
To empower communities, PS translators, as a first step, take into consideration the socio-
linguistic situation of their community, the o ften-asymmetrical relationships between social
groups and between these and institutions, and particularly the relative social status of public
service staff and the target readers of PST. As a second step, they are expected to assume an
active role in enabling access to information, literacy and, ultimately, societal and economic
change (Taibi and Ozolins 2016: 70–71).
In the last two decades or so, translator agency has been a major question in Translation
Studies, probably ‘a s a reaction to a more established tradition of studying norms and systems
in translation’ (Paloposki 2007: 336) but, most importantly, as part of growing interest in the
social role of translators, and the political, ideological and intercultural consequences of their
work. Translator agency is the translator’s ability and power to produce change in societies
through the texts they choose to translate and how they translate them (Tymoczko 2007:
200–16). It can also be associated with activism when it relates to international conflicts and
ideological struggles (see e.g. Baker 2006; Fernández and Evans 2018). In the case of PST,
translator agency is not about activism, but rather about taking a translation approach that
enhances the social role of PST. PS translators, as discussed here, are not activists in the
sense outlined, for instance, by Baker (2013): a group of (volunteer) translators who put their
language and translation skills at the service of social or political movements. Instead, PS
translators are (ideally) adequately trained professionals who offer a language service for a fee
or a salary and, while doing so, exercise agency by taking an active, functional and culturally
sensitive approach to translation, which aims to inform, educate and empower linguistically
disempowered communities. Their agency does not consist of direct advocacy, lobbying or
resistance, but manifests itself in the impact of their translations and translation approach on
society or certain groups within it (See below).
Translator agency implies translator power: that translators have the necessary power and
influence to trigger or contribute to change (Paloposki 2007: 337; Marais 2011: 192). Marais
(2011: 198) notes that the debate on translator agency and power has focused mostly on lit-
erary translation, which raises questions about the applicability of statements about agency
and power to the translation of pragmatic texts. In the case of PS translators, one can hardly
speak of power as it is generally understood: the capacity to influence individuals or groups
and affect their behaviours by actually or potentially offering them or depriving them from
desiderata (D ye 1990: 4). First, PS translators (a s well as interpreters) do not generally enjoy
a high status in society. Second, they do not choose what texts to translate or what languages
to translate them into. This is usually the decision of authorities, public services and, in some
cases, non-government organisations. The decision is political and is often in line with im-
migration policies, language policies, and attitudes towards multilingualism and minority
languages (Córdoba Serrano 2016). However, PS translators do have a role and a degree of
power in terms of the approach they take when translating for local communities. Their
power lies in the extent to which they, consciously or unconsciously, enable access to infor-
mation and education, not through tokenistic language transfer, but through effective com-
munication of contents and mediation between public services and community members.
Thus, PS translators may not have a say in relation to translation policy, funding available for
108
Public service translation
PST, or production and selection of PS texts, but they can exercise agency in their translation
choices by making them consistent with the needs of disempowered audiences.
Translation approach
The above sense of agency goes hand in hand with a full understanding of the role of PST
in society and of the type of translation approach required in each social and translational
context. Referring to the translation of administrative documents, Way (2006: 581) stresses
the role of PS translators as intercultural mediators, and advocates for ‘a more active role for
the translator’, a translator who actively strives to mediate between cultures and overcome the
issues surrounding this mediation. PST is offered (or needed) in local contexts that are char-
acterised by linguistic and cultural diversity, not only in terms of different ethnic, religious
or cultural groups but also in the sense of differences between the cultural assumptions and
(specialist) discourse practices of institutions on the one hand and those of (lay) PST users on
the other. In line with the active mediation required of the PS translator in such diversity,
Lesch (2018) conceives of quality in PST as a matter of adaptation, appropriateness and acces-
sibility for the target readership. Taking as a starting point the work of generalist translation
scholars such as Chesterman and Toury, he suggests a set of functional translation principles,
which revolve round the idea that effective communication through PST requires plain or
simplified language, explication strategies when necessary, and accessible text organisation
(see ‘Accessibility’ below). Taibi and Ozolins (2016: 70) also suggest that ‘functionalist ap-
proaches to translation would be the most attractively suitable for community [PS] transla-
tors in their capacity as active social agents’. Framed within a sociological understanding of
the PS translator’s work, a functionalist approach (1) empowers translators by bringing their
status closer to that of authors (in terms of text production, not socio-economic standing);
(2) places the intended function of the text/translation as the paramount criterion to guide
the translator’s decisions; (3) acknowledges differences between text types and the translation
strategies appropriate for each; and (4) allows ‘for a wide range of renderings and translato-
rial actions’, such as adaptations, transcreations and summary translations, depending on the
translation context and needs (Taibi and Ozolins 2016: 70).
Tomozeiu (2 016) draws on Suojanen, Koskinen and Tuominen’s (2 015) User-Centered
Translation to propose three methods to assist PS translators in visualising the target audi-
ence needs and making informed decisions in the translation process. These are ‘personas’,
‘implied readers’ and ‘audience design’. ‘Personas’ are typical user profiles within the
target community which a PS translator can outline based on their knowledge of and
experience with the community (Tomozeiu 2016: 197, 202). One risk in the creation of
personas, Tomozeiu (2 016: 198) points out, is that they may be centred on some stereo-
typical elements and features of the target community and leave out others, thus under-
mining the inclusiveness aim of PST. ‘I mplied readers’ refers to ‘t he audience based on the
analysis of the text’ (Tomozeiu 2016: 199) rather than previous knowledge of the target
group. In other words, while reading the source text, the translator infers the character-
istics of its target audience, which they can subsequently compare and contrast with those
of PST personas. Finally, ‘audience design’ serves to identify not only primary readers
(addressees), but also secondary ones (auditors, overhearers, eavesdroppers and referees).
Once identified, the PS translator will be in a position to make translation choices that
address these different audiences and, where necessary, prioritise the needs of some of
them ( Tomozeiu 2016: 2 00–1).
109
Mustapha Taibi
Professional-non-professional complementarity?
Within the context of what is desirable vs. what is available, García (2018) identifies areas
where non-professional, crowd-sourced translation can co-exist with and complement pro-
fessional PST. His argument is as follows: (1) both PST and g rass-root volunteer translation,
available mainly on Internet platforms, share the social inclusion ethos; (2) not all con-
tent that needs translation has the same value: some texts are ‘critical and enduring’, while
others are ‘inconsequential and ephemeral’ (2018: 105); (3) therefore, in critical situations
(t ranslations involving high risk) PST needs to be assigned to professional translators, while
low-risk translations can be done by n on-professionals. An example of n
on-critical task is the
translation of ‘peer-to-peer support pages’ for newly arrived migrants (2018: 103). H igh-r isk
translations would include instances such as official information about social security, health-
care, or administrative procedures. Pym (2021: 26) takes up this argument with reference to
both translation and interpreting: ‘The basic argument […] is that, when and where mistakes
can be costly, it is worth paying for highly trained professional translators and interpreters
so that error can be avoided’. Pym (2021: 26) cites findings such as Canfora and Ottmann’s
110
Public service translation
(2018) on damages (partly) attributable to translation errors and concludes: ‘From one or two
disasters, it certainly does not follow that all mistranslations are costly. Some are h igh-stakes,
most are low-stakes, and the difference is important’.
While it is evident that in translation, as in life, there are h igh-stakes and low-stakes sit-
uations, García’s and Pym’s points above raise several questions:
1 Given that ‘high-stakes’ and ‘low-stakes’ are relative and blurry, who would be enti-
tled to make decisions in relation to the classification of translation needs, areas and
situations?
2 What types of ‘cost’ or ‘d amage’ will be quantified and how? There are translation
production and dissemination costs and social costs arising as a result of inexistent or
inadequate PST; there are risks relating to the reputation and legal responsibilities of
public service organisations and risks relating to the lives, livelihoods and welfare of
PST users; there is individual damage and collective damage, and there are quantifiable
and unquantifiable costs and damages.
3 Inadequate PST is not only about translation errors, which may lead to physical dam-
age or safety incidents; it is also about inadequacies such as complete lack of PST in
all or some local languages, insufficient PST in some key sectors such as healthcare or
employment, or less effective PST as a result of inappropriate translation approaches or
dissemination strategies.
Quality
For PST to achieve its social aims, it needs to meet quality requirements. The first require-
ment is professional qualifications; however, in the case of PST (a nd translation in general),
translator qualifications are only one step towards ensuring quality; others rely on the role of
other stakeholders. Taibi (2018) outlines a m ulti-layered quality assurance framework with
four main levels: (1) societal obligations (what the society in question needs to do to ensure
quality in multilingual public messaging), (2) inter-professional collaboration (how profes-
sionals from different relevant fields can contribute to translation quality before translation
takes place), (3) the translation stage and translator’s role (what the translator needs to do and
how they need to do it to ensure their translations are appropriate and effective), and (4) p ost-
translation quality assurance (what needs to happen after a translation has been submitted:
111
Mustapha Taibi
rigorous checking and e nd-user feedback). The levels or steps below (d iscussed in Taibi 2018)
are to be taken as conditions for PST to be at optimal quality standards. Naturally, only the
translation stage below applies to translators. The other levels show where other stakeholders
or actors can make a contribution.
112
Public service translation
113
Mustapha Taibi
community feedback, commissioners (and funders) can find out which translation ef-
forts and approaches reduce cost (by effectively addressing social, educational, environ-
mental or health issues), and those that only end up producing ineffective parallel texts
in minority languages.
Accessibility
PST cannot achieve its social mission unless it is accessible. Accessibility is a general term
used in different fields to refer to easy access, ability to function in a given environment, and
ability to use a product, process or system. Suojanen, Koskinen and Tuominen (2015: 49) de-
fine ‘accessibility’ as ‘efforts making products and services available to all people, regardless
of their individual abilities or backgrounds’. In the context of communication, accessibility
includes ‘intralingual subtitles for the hard of hearing, Braille translations, simplified texts
and the use of plain language for those with cognitive challenges or limited language skills,
and so on’ (I bid: 57). In the particular context of PST, it refers to translation strategies and
adaptations intended to make multilingual resources readable, comprehensible and effective.
These often consist of accommodation shifts such as simplification of specialised terms, usage
of verbs instead of nominalisations, active constructions rather than passive voice, breaking
down complex sentences, and avoidance of archaic language and constructions (Cornelius
2010; Määttä 2020).
Accessibility is crucial for translations, ‘particularly user-centred translation where the
central purpose is to direct texts to their specified user groups and the needs of these groups’
(Suojanen, Koskinen and Tuominen 2015: 57). At the same time, making texts accessible de-
pends on a number of variables, including the norms and conventions of the source and target
languages, literacy levels in the target community of readers, and the extent to which institu-
tional discourse in a given country or organisation favours clarity and access to information
or institutional power and prestige (K atan and Taibi, 2021: 296–324). Translators with the
mindset of ‘translation as a public service’, ‘translation as a social mission’ or ‘translation as
empowerment’ work within the possibilities and constraints of their working languages, the
socio-educational levels of their readerships, and the requirements or expectations of the
institutions and organisations generating content and commissioning translations.
First, in terms of language, while certain textual features may enhance readability and
accessibility across languages (absence of specialised terms, shorter sentences, clear links be-
tween ideas and so forth), languages also vary in their tolerance for complex structures (for
instance, embedded and conjoined clauses) and in their internal hierarchy of formality. A
good example is diglossic languages such as Arabic (Taibi and Ozolins 2016: 48–49), where
the gap between spoken (colloquial) and written language (Modern Standard Arabic) is far
greater than in n on-diglossic languages. The implication is that as soon as the mode of com-
munication changes from spoken to written, the accessibility of the information is reduced
significantly. This applies to other languages as well, but the case of diglossic languages is a
pretty clear one.
Some languages also have additional sources of complexity and may, therefore, require ad-
justable readability metrics. To take Arabic as an example again, there are types of ‘l inguistic
complexity that would not be captured by currently available automated readability analysis’
(Malik, El-Haj and Paasche-Orlow 2019: 171). One such source of complexity is the absence
of diacritics (El-Haj and Rayson 2016; Malik, El-Haj and Paasche-Orlow 2019). Although
diacritics are not usually used in texts for highly literate adults, they can improve readability
114
Public service translation
significantly, especially in the case of homographs and potential ambiguity (Malik, El-Haj
and Paasche-Orlow 2019).
In terms of literacy level, it is well known that literacy rates vary internationally and
that lower socio-economic groups tend to have lower literacy (Reardon et. al. 2013; Lesch
2018). Minoritised language groups have historically had less access to education than
dominant groups (e.g. Lesch 2012, 2018). Migrants and refugees may come from different
socio-economic backgrounds, but at least some of them face literacy challenges in their own
mother tongue (or official language of their home country). Syrian nationals, for example
have traditionally had high literacy levels (at least prior to the 2011 civil war). Still, although
Syrian refugee statistics in Europe confirm this in comparison with some other nationalities,
they also show that approximately a third of these Syrian refugees arrive with a low literacy
level. German data, for example show that ‘24% attended only primary school and 11% had
not attended school at all’ (OECD 2015: 8). What this shows is that, while aiming for com-
prehensible translation, PS translators need to have updated knowledge of the sociolinguistic
and literacy backgrounds of their readers. It also means that alternative dissemination media,
such as video in colloquial language, could be considered (see next section).
As for the relation between institutional discourse and public service users, the function
of institutional language and discourse may vary: texts are sometimes used to communicate
and other times to regulate and maintain institutional status and control. Accessibility of
information is not always the focus of institutional communication. An organisation may
produce texts to inform the public, to comply with legal requirements or, as Barron (2012:
50) notes, ‘to increase the status of this organisation’. Public messaging usually consists of
one-way communication from more powerful institutional agents to less powerful masses
(Barron 2012: 66–67), and the power differential is often reflected in language. The infor-
mation in public messages is usually based on legal or institutional regulations.
Accessibility, then, needs to be addressed at the source (when drafting public service texts
intended for the general public). This was recognised as early as the 1970s, and especially in
the 1990s, with the Plain Language Movement in countries such as Australia, New Zealand,
the United States (Adler 2012: 69), South Africa (Cornelius 2010) and the European Union
(Fight the Fog). This movement has later spread to other countries as well, including Japan
and China (Adler 2012: 69). The movement advocates the presentation of public information
in a plain and precise language and accessible design to minimise the receiver’s effort and
maximise their understanding (Adler 2012: 68). In healthcare, for instance, Schipper et al.’s
(2016) systematic review shows that the preparation of a lay version of healthcare information
enables patients to understand it better. When such lay versions are unavailable, PS transla-
tors have a major role in providing accessible multilingual information. The extent to which
this is possible will naturally depend not only on their agency and knowledge but also on
institutional practices and expectations.
Dissemination media
Dissemination methods are closely linked to accessibility. As Burke (2018) points out, so-
ciolinguistic variation in situations where PST is needed poses serious challenges for both
translation commissioners and translators. As mentioned above, PST is a written translation.
Writing in many language communities is inseparably linked to the standard variety of the
language, which is only accessible to certain socio-educational layers. This calls for PST
hand in hand with an effective communication strategy. Public services, translators and re-
searchers need to determine the dissemination media that are most appropriate and effective
115
Mustapha Taibi
for each audience (age group, regional language varieties, literacy level and so forth). Taibi,
Liamputtong and Polonsky (2019: 147), for example found that older Arabic speakers in Aus-
tralia had different preferences: some preferred reading health awareness materials because
they can be referred back to whenever needed, while others preferred audio-visual media,
due to literacy and eyesight challenges, among other reasons.
Suojanen, Koskinen and Tuominen’s (2015: 9 3–110) ‘user-centred translation’ calls for
empirical studies with real translation users to determine their preferences. This, the au-
thors note, can be done through usability testing by means of questionnaires, focus group
discussions, interviews, think-aloud protocols or eye tracking. Usability testing can generate
a wealth of data that would assist in better catering for the needs of real users. However,
while usability testing can be applied to study the usability of different dissemination media
separately (print translation, video, website, and so forth), proactive research needs to be
undertaken to determine user preferences before actual production of (translated) dissemi-
nation material. Here, questionnaires, focus groups and interviews can also be useful. This,
naturally, is the remit of public services and researchers, not translators.
Public services have a lot to learn from healthcare dissemination research and practice.
The (Australian) National Health and Medical Research Council’s (2019) Guidelines for
guidelines: Dissemination and communication, for example, include advice on communication
needs assessment among the target audience or segment of society, message design, dissem-
ination methods, as well as assessment of effectiveness. The Guidelines also cite literature
pointing out that leaflet or brochure campaigns are less effective than active dissemination
methods such as face-to-face education campaigns and workshops. However, the fact that
one dissemination medium is found to be more preferred or impactful than others does not
mean that it should be used exclusively. As Schipper et al.’s (2016) systematic review shows,
a combination of different approaches can enhance the effectiveness of messaging among
target audiences.
Crisis translation
The International Network on Crisis Translation (see also Federici and O’Brien 2020)
defines ‘crisis translation’
‘as the translation of written information from one linguistic and cultural system to
another in the context of a crisis scenario, with a view to enabling affected communities
and responders to be prepared for crises, improve resilience and reduce the loss of lives’
(INTERACT 2019)
Although crisis translation focuses on emergency situations such as natural disasters and
wars, much of its scope can be subsumed under PST, more specifically under ‘translation
for temporary communities’ (Taibi and Ozolins 2016: 95–102). What Taibi and Ozolins
(2016: 95) mean by ‘temporary communities’ are ‘various types of conglomerations that may
occupy a local space and form a multilingual microcosm during a limited time’ (d isplaced
populations in a bilingual or multilingual space, seasonal migrants and pilgrims).
A crisis, however, can affect both established communities and emerging/temporary
ones, as has been demonstrated recently during the C OVID-19 pandemic. This crisis has
brought the notions of PST and crisis translation closer to each other, as health authorities in
116
Public service translation
each country have had to use the existing translation services or enact makeshift solutions to
address awareness needs during the pandemic.
Much of crisis translation is provided by volunteers collaborating with NGOs (see Elrha,
n.d. on translation in health crises in Africa; Munro 2013 on the 2010 Haiti earthquake).
The situation of PST in many countries is not much better. As is shown in Gigliotti’s (2017)
study of a small corpus of translations between Spanish, Italian and English, translations pro-
vided by non-qualified volunteers raise a number of quality concerns. While crisis situations
are difficult to anticipate and plan for, more stable sociolinguistic situations requiring PST
(a nd interpreting) allow for ample planning, including planning for professional language
services. It is just a matter of political will and resources.
Social marketing
The point was made above that PST would be more impactful if combined with an effective
communication strategy through appropriate and r esearch-supported dissemination methods.
PST is part of public service communication campaigns, which, as Barron (2012: 55) states,
‘are concerned with knowledge transfer and attitude and behaviour change’. Influencing
attitudes and behaviours is an intersection point for PST and social marketing, which ‘seeks
to develop and integrate marketing concepts with other approaches to influence behaviours
that benefit individuals and communities for the greater social good’ (International Social
Marketing Association 2017). Like P ST – but
from a completely different p erspective – social
marketing seeks to guide social transformation through an effective and equitable application
of marketing principles (Saunders, Barrington and Sridharan 2015: 165; International Social
Marketing Association 2017). What social marketing has in common with PST are as follows:
• ‘the overarching goal of greater social good’ (Saunders, Barrington and Sridharan 2015:
164);
• Concern with both impact (effectiveness) and process (e.g. equity, participation of dif-
ferent stakeholders and fairness);
• Recognition of the right of communities and community members to act, decide and
contribute to change (Saunders, Barrington and Sridharan 2015: 165);
• A need for collaboration among different stakeholders, and a recognition of audiences
as ‘active participants and collaborators that ultimately transform and shape their own
lives’ (Saunders, Barrington and Sridharan 2015: 164; See also Blocker et al., 2013)).
Social marketing also has a community-based strand, which has significant insights to offer
PST. McKenzie-Mohr (2011: 3 –5), for instance, cites several studies in countries such as
the United States, the Netherlands and Switzerland showing that most programs aiming
to change behaviours rely on mass information campaigns (for instance leaflets, newsletters
and TV ads). Community-based social marketing offers alternatives including careful iden-
tification of the behaviour to be promoted, identification of barriers and benefits, design of
behaviour-changing strategies, piloting the strategies with a population sample and evalua-
tion of impact (McKenzie-Mohr 2011: 8 –10).
As in information-intensive social marketing, the assumption in PST has been that ac-
cess to information will lead to social change and community empowerment. While this
assumption appears reasonable at face value, it still needs to be supported through research,
especially with interdisciplinary insights and contributions from areas such as c ommunity-
b ased social marketing,
117
Mustapha Taibi
Future directions1
The nature and social role of PST call for interdisciplinary research into a number of areas.
Four key investigation areas can be identified: (1) training and socialisation, (2) quality
and professional standards, (3) dissemination media and (4) the impact of PST on different
aspects of society, including healthcare literacy and other literacies, and the social and po-
litical participation of migrants, refugees and indigenous minorities. While some research
has been undertaken in countries such as Australia, South Africa, Spain and the United
Kingdom, further research is needed, particularly in other multilingual national and local
contexts.
To speak of a translation activity with a positive social impact, first we need to investigate
the extent to which adequate training is available, the types of training and socialisation
to which PS translators have access, as well as the correlation between type and amount of
training on one hand and PS translator performance on the other. Where training is unavail-
able, it needs to be ascertained what alternative induction and socialisation processes are in
place and how effective they are.
PST training is a worthwhile research focus, not only in terms of translator skill devel-
opment but also in relation to the socialisation of future translators as engaged citizens and
active agents for social change. Washbourne (2019: 601) notes, ‘Translator and interpreter
training and education has been slow to engage with educational philosophies such as social
reconstructionism – the prioritising of social betterment as an aim of education’. In transla-
tion and interpreting pedagogy, there is probably no more suitable domain than PST (a nd
interpreting) for a social reconstructionist approach. Curriculum components that involve
engaging with PS texts, translating for local communities, and service-learning placements
offer students opportunities to develop translation and intercultural skills as well as civic
responsibility (Rueda-Acedo 2018: 62–63) and commitment to social advancement.
Whether existent or not, PST training needs to be studied in conjunction with quality
and professional standards. This is a broad and rich area of enquiry which covers, among
other research interests, quality assurance arrangements and practices; collaboration between
PS translators and other stakeholders to ensure quality; assessment of PST quality in different
countries, institutions and language combinations; correlation between translation quality
and end-user trust and perception (of the content, the public service providing it, and trans-
lators). As would be expected in research into a community-based activity, the views of end
users are an essential part of PST quality assessment.
In line with a social reconstructionist approach to education and community-based
quality assessment, a key future direction in PST research is interdisciplinary collaboration
with scholarly areas such as social marketing, mass communication, and healthcare to study
the opportunities that multimodal messaging offers and the impact of each dissemination
method on PST audiences. One main challenge in PST is the linguistic, sociolinguistic
and cultural diversity of audiences. This diversity can be addressed to some extent through
translator expertise, discernment and translation strategies, but much of the challenge is
related to the dissemination medium and the overall communication strategy of the public
service in question. PS translators may produce appropriate or acceptable translations, but if
these are not disseminated through an effective channel (for the relevant community), the
social impact of PST cannot be guaranteed. In this regard, social marketing, mass commu-
nication and healthcare communication in particular have a lot to offer. PST researchers can
also contribute with intercultural, language-specific and t ranslation-related insights in such
interdisciplinary research.
118
Public service translation
Conclusion
PST is socially oriented translatorial action that aims to serve minority and minoritised
language groups by enabling them to access public service contents. As such, this area of
translation practice offers an interesting field for researchers and other social actors, not only
those interested in mediation between languages and cultural systems, but also those con-
cerned with language policies, social equity, democracy and community participation, mass
communication, and public service messaging. In this translation context, ‘t ranslation as em-
powerment’, ‘t ranslator agency’, ‘accessibility’, ‘t ranslation quality’, ‘effective dissemination’
and ‘social impact’ emerge as central notions.
Translation, by nature, requires the collaboration of different stakeholders: authors, com-
missioners, translators, translation checkers/copyeditors and publishers. PST, because of its
social mission and the pragmatic nature of its texts, requires interventions by various social
agents, from the government level (language policy, access to information and funding) to the
level of the translation team (translator, checker and team/service manager), going through
other professionals, professional bodies and public services (translator certification, selection of
translators, briefing translators, writing for multilingual and multicultural audiences).
Given the nature of PST, translation practices and products within it cannot be studied
in terms of language transfer and c ross-linguistic text analysis alone, but they also need to
be researched in terms of their relevance to the lives and interests of PST audiences, and the
social impact they have on them. In other words, both production and reception studies
are needed. PST practices and products also need to be studied and understood as part of a
broader public service communication strategy, rather than isolated instances of communi-
cation between author and reader through a translator. For the above reasons, collaboration
between researchers in PST and relevant areas such as social marketing and mass communi-
cation is likely to be mutually beneficial and productive.
Further reading
Córdoba Serrano, María Sierra (2016) “Translation policies and community translation: The U.S., a
case study”, New Voices in Translation Studies 14: 122–63.
This paper addresses the connection between translation policies and PST, taking the situation in the USA
as an illustrative case. The paper shows how language and translation policies lead to different levels and stan-
dards of PST provision.
Taibi, Mustapha, and Ozolins, Uldis (2016) Community Translation. London, Bloomsbury.
This book introduces PST as a socially oriented translation activity. It discusses different aspects of this
niche area, including its empowering role, the socio-cultural issues facing translators, translation approaches
and quality assurance.
Taibi, Mustapha (ed) (2018) Translating for the Community. Bristol, Multilingual Matters.
This edited volume includes contributions from different countries on the social role and challenges of PST.
Among other topics, it covers PST training, quality assurance and the relationship between PST as a profes-
sional activity and crowd-sourced translation.
Related chapters
Chapter 1, General issues of PSI: Institutions, codes and norms, professionalisation by Carmen Valero-Garcés
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason
Chapter 5, Corpus-based studies of public service interpreting by Bernd Meyer
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
hapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalisation of interpret-
C
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
119
Mustapha Taibi
Note
1 I would like to thank Anne Beinchet (University of Moncton, Canada) for her input into this
section.
References
Adler, Mark (2012) “The plain language movement”, in The Oxford Handbook of Language and Law,
Peter Tiersma and Lawrence Solan (eds). Oxford, Oxford University Press: 6 7–86.
Antonini, Rachele, Letizia Cirillo, Linda Rossato, and Ira Torresi (2017) Non-professional Interpreting
and Translation: State of the Art and Future of an Emerging Field of Research. Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins.
Baker, Mona (2013) “Translation as an alternative space for political action”, Social Movement Studies
12 (1): 23–47.
——— (2006) Translation and Conflict: A Narrative Account. London, Routledge.
Barron, Anne (2012) Public Information Messages: A Contrastive Genre Analysis of State-Citizen Communi-
cation. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Bielsa, Esperanza (2016) “News translation: Global or cosmopolitan connections?” Media, Culture &
Society 38 (2): 196–211.
Blocker, Christopher P., Julie A. Ruth, Srinivas Sridharan, Colin Beckwith, Ahmed Ekici, Martina
Goudie-Hutton, José Antonio Rosa, Bige Saatcioglu, Debabrata Talukdar, Carlos Trujillo, and
Rohit Varman (2013) “Understanding poverty and promoting poverty alleviation through trans-
formative consumer research”, Journal of Business Research 66 (8), 1195–202.
Burke, Jean (2018) “Linguistic diversity among Swahili-speakers: A challenge for translation in Aus-
tralia”, in Translating for the Community, Mustapha Taibi (ed). Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 156–73.
Canfora, Carmen, and Angelika Ottmann (2018) “Of ostriches, pyramids, and Swiss cheese. Risks in
safety-critical translations”, Translation Spaces 7 (2): 167–201.
Córdoba Serrano, María Sierra (2016) “Translation policies and community translation: The U.S., a
case study”, New Voices in Translation Studies 14: 122–63.
Cornelius, Eleanor (2010) “Plain language as alternative textualization”, Southern African Linguistics and
Applied Language Studies 28 (2): 171–83.
Corsellis, Ann (2008) Public Service Interpreting. The First Steps. Basingstoke, Palgrave Macmillan.
Cronin, Michael (2003) Translation and Globalization. London, Routledge.
Dye, Thomas (1990). Power and Society: An Introduction to the Social Sciences. California, Brooks/Cole
Publishing Company.
Díaz Cintas, Jorge, Pilar Orero and Aline Remael (2007) ”Media for all: A global challenge” in Media
for All: Subtitling for the Deaf, Audio Description, and Sign Language, Díaz Cintas, Jorge, Pilar Orero
and Aline Remael (eds). The Hague, Brill: 11–20.
E l-Haj, Mahmoud, and Paul Rayson (2016) “OSMAN: A novel arabic readability metric”, in Proceed-
ings of the Language Resources and Evaluation Conference 2016, Nicoletta Calzolari, Khalid Choukri,
Thierry Declerck, Sara Goggi, Marko Grobelnik, Bente Maegaard, Joseph Mariani, Hélène Mazo,
Asunción Moreno, Jan Odijk and Stelios Piperidis (eds): 250–55. URL: https://w ww.lancaster.
ac.uk/staff/elhaj/docs/elhajlrec2016Arabic.pdf (Accessed 15 January 2021).
Elrha (n .d.) “W hen translation saves lives”. URL: https://w ww.elrha.org/project-blog/when-
t ranslation-saves-l ives (accessed 29 June 2021).
Federici, Federico, and Sharon O’Brien (eds) (2020) Translation in Cascading Crises. Abingdon, Routledge.
Fernández, Fruela, and Jonathan Evans (eds) (2018) The Routledge Handbook of Translation and Politics.
Abingdon, Routledge.
García, Ignacio (2018) “Volunteers and public service translation”, in Translating for the Community,
Mustapha Taibi (ed). Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 98–109.
Gigliotti, Giulia (2017) “The quality of Mercy: A corpus-based analysis of the quality of volunteer trans-
lations for Non-Profit Organizations (N POs)”, New Voices in Translation Studies 17: 52–81, URL:
https://w ww.iatis.org/i mages/stories/publications/ new-voices/Issue_16–2017/A rticles/Gigliotti_
Final.pdf (accessed 15 January 2021).
INTERACT (INTERnAtional network on Crisis Translation) (2019) “Factsheet”, https://cordis.eu-
ropa.eu/project/rcn/207037/f actsheet/en (accessed 15 January 2021).
120
Public service translation
121
Mustapha Taibi
Schäffner, Christina (2020) “Translators’ roles and responsibilities”, in The Bloomsbury Companion to
Language Industry Studies, Erik Angelone, Maureen Ehrensberger-Dow and Gary Massey (eds).
London, Bloomsbury: 63–90.
Schipper, K., M. Bakker, et al. (2016) “Strategies for disseminating recommendations or guidelines to
patients: A systematic review”, Implement Science 11 (1): 82.
Suojanen, Tytti, Kaisa Koskinen, and Tiina Tuominen (2015) User-Centered Translation. London,
Routledge.
Susam-Sarayeva, Sebnem, and Luis Pérez González (2012) (eds) Non-professionals Translating and Inter-
preting: Participatory and Engaged Perspectives. Special Issue of the Translator 18 (2): 1 49–65.
Taibi, Mustapha (2011) “Public service translation”, in The Oxford Handbook of Translation Studies,
Kirsten Malmkjær and Kevin Windle (eds). Oxford, Oxford University Press: 214–27.
——— (2018) “Quality assurance in community translation”, in Translating for the Community, Musta-
pha Taibi (ed). Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 7–25.
Taibi, Mustapha, Pranee Liamputtong, and Michael Polonsky (2019) “Impact of translated health
information on CALD older people’s health literacy: A pilot study”, in Multicultural Health Trans-
lation, Interpreting and Communication, Meng Ji, Mustapha Taibi and Ineke Crezee (eds). London,
Routledge: 138–58.
Tomozeiu, Daniel (2016) “Defining ‘community’ for community translation”, New Voices in Translation
Studies 14: 190–209.
Tymoczko, Maria (2007) Enlarging Translation, Empowering Translators. Manchester, St Jerome.
——— (2000) “Translation and political engagement”, The Translator, 6 (1): 2 3–47.
Tyulenev, Sergey (2014) Translation and Society. London, Routledge.
Washbourne, Kelly (2019) “Training and education, theory and practice”, in Routledge Encyclopedia of
Translation Studies, Mona Baker and Gabriela Saldanha (eds). London, Routledge: 597–602.
Way, Catherine (2016) “The challenges and opportunities of legal translation and translator training
in the 21st century”, International Journal of Communication 10, 1 009–29.
——— (2006) “Translating for the authorities: The role of the translator”, in Studi In Ricordo di Carmen
Sánchez Montero, Graciano Benelli and Giampaolo Tonini (eds). Trieste, Università degli Studi di
Trieste: 579–88.
Wolf, Michaela (2007) “Introduction: The emergence of a sociology of translation”, in Constructing a
Sociology of Translation, Michaela Wolf and Alexandra Fukari (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John
Benjamins: 1–3.
122
PART 2
Introduction
Court interpreting, also called judicial or judiciary interpreting, is a form of public service
interpreting that takes place in courts as part of the judicial process to mediate interaction
between legal professionals and lay participants who speak a language other than the court’s
procedural language. Court interpreting differs from other forms of public service interpret-
ing in various ways. Most notably it tends to have a high degree of professionalization, with
specialized processes of training, certification, and employment for interpreters, as well as
dedicated professional organizations. It is generally subject to legal regulations with regard
to the rights of participants to interpreting, and also to the obligations of interpreters, often
with explicit guidelines that address the ethics of interpreting as well as specific translational
norms (e.g. mandating the use of first person to refer to the source speaker). Court interpret-
ing involves routinized genres of legal-lay interaction, such as the questioning of witnesses or
the announcement of a verdict by a judge, and these pose specific interactional challenges for
interpreters, as does the use of specialized professional registers of legal language. Like other
forms of public service interpreting, court interpreting is generally bidirectional, but court
interpreters are also expected to perform different modes of interpreting, typically both short
consecutive interpreting and simultaneous interpreting, as well as sight translation. Court
interpreting is also increasingly done remotely, via video link, which is addressed in Licoppe,
this volume. Due to its ubiquity as well as its importance in the legal process, court inter-
preting is among the most widely researched areas of public service interpreting, with con-
tributions from scholars in translation studies (including practitioners of court interpreting
and scholars involved in interpreter training), but also from legal scholars and from linguists,
anthropologists or sociologists interested in the relationship between multilingualism, social
power and human rights.
to understand the charges and to participate in their defense. This is stated, for example in
the European Convention on Human Rights (A rticle 6.3e) and the Court Interpreters Act
of 1978 in the United States (Pousada 1979). Many jurisdictions have extended the use of
court interpreters to other participants, whether in practice or by right, particularly courts
in societies with multiple official languages, or with high degrees of linguistic diversity.
Nonetheless, the existence of formalized rights does not guarantee the consistent provision
of effective interpreting services, as noted by Morris (1999), Ozolins (2016) and others. As
a consequence, scholars have often felt the need to advocate for higher and more consistent
standards of interpreting, as have professional organizations of court interpreters, partic-
ularly in the face of budget cuts (Schweda-Nicholson 1994; Fowler 2016). Despite such
efforts, court interpreting practices often vary significantly by language, partly because of
differences in demand and interpreter availability, but also because of legal differences, such
as in cases of languages with a special legal status. For example, in some jurisdictions, the
use of signed languages is mandated by a nti-discrimination laws, and so sign language in-
terpreting may be offered to categories of court users who would otherwise not have a right
to interpreting, such as jurors (Hale et al. 2017). Standards of interpreting may also become
the subject of legal disputes, such as when judicial decisions are appealed on the claim that
a person’s due process rights were infringed. Such disputes often involve assessments of the
accused’s proficiency in the institutional language, most commonly concerning p re-trial
phases of the process, such as police interrogations (see especially Berk-Seligson 2000; Eades
and Pavlenko 2016).
The conditions for court interpreting in a particular jurisdiction are thus shaped by the
sociolinguistic context and history of a given society. While public service interpreting
in court is often conducted to enable the participation of speakers of minority languages
who are not fluent in the majority language, it is also used in other situations. For example,
countries with multiple official languages may grant special rights of access to interpreting
for speakers of c o-official languages that are independent of a person’s proficiency in the
procedural language (see, e.g. Morris 2008 on interpreting in Canada and Israel). In p ost-
colonial countries, the former colonial language may be the primary institutional language
in court even if it is not spoken natively by most (or even any) of the participants, so that
interpreting is done for the majority of lay court users (see, e.g. Ng 2009, 2018 on Hong
Kong; and Moeketsi 1999 on South Africa). In such settings, the interpreter’s performance
may be scrutinized by other bilingual speakers in the courtroom, in contrast to settings of
interpreting for minority language speakers, when the interpreter may be the only bilingual
participant.
The judicial settings that court interpreters work in can be divided into two major types,
adversarial and inquisitorial. In adversarial proceedings, such as most trials in the common
law system that is prevalent throughout the English-speaking world and in former British
colonies, prosecution and defense present conflicting theories to the court and take turns
questioning witnesses, while the role of judges is limited to that of an impartial procedural
referee. Interpreters in adversarial proceedings are thus regularly required to interpret in
highly confrontational situations, mediating between participants who have contradictory
goals. For example, in cross-examination, attorneys ask questions that are designed to coerce
witnesses into making responses that support the opposing side’s theory. This requires inter-
preters to pay close attention to the pragmatic force of questions, as attorneys design ques-
tions to control and restrict the range of possible answers that a witness may give (Hale 1999;
2004; Rigney 1999). Similarly, court interpreters also need to attend to nuanced meaning
differences in a series of near-repetitive questions (P ym 1999).
126
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
127
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
the interpreter to keep up with individual turns, and for the recipient to distinguish who said
what. Alternatively, interpreters may use reported speech or summarize question and answer
sequences into single renditions (see Angermeyer 2015: 132–35).
This distribution of interpreting modes is common in most jurisdictions and is often
taken for granted without questioning its impact on courtroom discourse, interpreter perfor-
mance, or the fairness and efficiency of proceedings. For example, simultaneous interpret-
ing (w ithout the use of equipment) is more likely to lead to omissions, thus disadvantaging
listeners who depend on it for information about the proceedings ( Jacobsen 2012; Anger-
meyer 2015). Furthermore, Ng (2018: 43) points out that chuchotage is not suitable for p ost-
c olonial settings such as in Hong Kong, where jurors or audience members also need to be
able to hear the interpreter because they are not sufficiently proficient in the language of the
court (in this case English).
In contrast to these common courtroom contexts, simultaneous interpreting with tech-
nological aids is common in international courts and tribunals, such as, famously the Nurem-
berg trials and subsequent trials of war criminals (Morris 1990; Takeda 2008; Elias-Bursać
2012). These will not be addressed further in this chapter as the situation is perhaps more
akin to conference-interpreting than public-service interpreting.
Literature overview
As noted above, court interpreting (or more widely defined legal interpreting) has been a topic
of research in a number of different disciplines besides translation studies, including linguis-
tics, anthropology, sociology, and law. Besides being the subject of many journal articles, it
has been examined in monographs, textbooks and special issues of journals in translation
studies (Shlesinger and Pöchhacker 2008) as well as language and law (Eades, Hale and
Cooke 1999; Coulthard and Hale 2016). Research in translation studies has focused espe-
cially on aspects of the interpreters’ performance, investigating the role interpreters play in
mediating courtroom interaction, while also comparing source and target speech to identify
differences in meaning and assess translation quality in a broad sense. Scholars have also
explored ways in which structural differences between specific languages impact interpreta-
tion, as well as how i nterpreter-mediated talk differs pragmatically from s ame-language talk.
Such empirical studies of court interpreting depend on access to data in the form of audio-
recorded courtroom interaction, yet jurisdictions vary greatly in the extent to which they
grant researchers access. Some courts do not permit any recordings at all (for instance, most
courts in the United Kingdom or in Canada), thus effectively preventing empirical research
on courtroom interaction and restricting researchers to working with data from observations
or interviews with interpreters and legal professionals (Fowler 2016), or to analyses of writ-
ten court records or legal opinions (Morris 2008). Other jurisdictions do allow researchers
access, or even go so far as to permit the broadcasting of h igh-profile trials, as some courts
in the United States have done, which greatly facilitates access for researchers interested in
courtroom discourse (see, e.g. Pym 1999; Rigney 1999; for an overview of issues relating to
data collection, see Angermeyer 2013).
In a g round-breaking early b ook-length study, B erk-Seligson (1990) analyzed more
than one hundred hours of audio-recorded data from Spanish-English court interpreting
in different courts in the United States. In addition to providing a survey of the legal and
institutional context and an ethnographic description of interpreters’ working conditions,
Berk-Seligson investigated various ways in which the interpreter’s speech affected the inter-
action. Specifically, her linguistic analysis focused on comparing interpreters’ renditions to
128
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
their corresponding source utterances with regard to their pragmatic force and their speech
style. For example, she found that interpreters could downplay speaker agency and blame
attribution by using agentless passives or other impersonal constructions where the source
speaker had used an active form. To assess the impact of such and other practices on the le-
gal process, Berk Seligson also conducted experiments with mock jurors and found that the
speech style of interpreters (for instance whether they used markers of hesitation and hedg-
ing) could influence the perception of a witness’s credibility.
The effect of interpreters’ choices on the interaction is also investigated by Hale (2004),
drawing on data from Spanish-English court interpreting in Australia. She finds that in-
terpreters may alter the pragmatic force of lawyer’s questions during cross-examination,
by omitting discourse markers or altering the question type. For example, she argues that
interpreters may make cross-examination less coercive and confrontational when attorney’s
declarative statements like ‘I put it to you that…’ are rendered as interrogatives. When in-
terpreting witnesses’ responses from Spanish to English, interpreters are also found to make
omissions, for example of markers of so-called ‘powerless’ speech (O’Barr 1982), but also of
parts of utterances that don’t directly respond to or even challenge the preceding questions.
Based on her findings, Hale argues for enhanced training for court interpreters that in-
cludes attention to pragmatics and that encourages interpreters to mimic the source speakers’
speech style, that is ‘faithfulness of content and manner of speech’ (Hale 2004: 239). Inter-
preting in Australian courts has also been examined by Lee (2009a, 2009b), focusing on the
impact of structural, pragmatic and cultural differences on interpreting between English
and Korean, and Cooke (1996) on a case study of stand-by interpreting for an Aboriginal
Australian defendant.
Another post-colonial common law context that has seen multiple studies of court inter-
preting is the Chinese-English bilingual court system of Hong Kong. In an ethnographic
monograph, Kwai Hang Ng (2009) examines the pragmatic and legal consequences of lan-
guage choice, finding that adherence to the juridical formalism of the British common law
system is reduced when trials are conducted in Cantonese rather than English. When trials
are conducted in English, use of court interpreters is widespread, though many of the legal
professionals may, in fact, be bilingual. Kwai Hang Ng (2009: 161) documents that litigants
in English trials are frustrated with the interruptions caused by consecutive interpreting and
this may prompt them to opt for C hinese-language trials instead. In another recent mono-
graph, Eva Ng (2018) focuses more specifically on court interpreting practices and explores
the ways in which they are influenced by other participants’ proficiency in the two languages
(or lack thereof ). She examines particularly trials that are conducted in English, where jurors
may not be able to understand English-language testimony because chuchotage interpreting
is not audible to them, while on the other hand, bilingual attorneys may conduct examina-
tions in Cantonese without waiting for interpretation. Court interpreting in Hong Kong has
also been examined by Cheung (2012, 2014, 2017), focusing in particular on interpreters’
non-adherence to guidelines (e.g. using reported speech or producing non-renditions), and
Leung and Gibbons (2008, 2009), focusing on interpreters’ marking of the participation
status and their translation of Cantonese u tterance-final particles into English. Interpreting
between English and local languages in p ost-colonial common law contexts has also been
investigated in Ghana (A muzu, Campbell and Ofori 2020), Malaysia (Powell and Hashim
2011) and South Africa (Moeketsi 1999; Thetela 2003), among others. However, as shown by
Powell (2008), interpreters are not always available in such jurisdictions, disadvantaging lay
participants with limited understanding of English, while their testimony in a local language
is understood by legal professionals.
129
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
130
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
problems were not taken seriously by the judge who appeared to assume that all Mexican
citizens speak Spanish. The accused, later found to be innocent, was convicted and spent
several years in prison before the verdict was overturned.
In addition to requiring the recognition of named languages, court interpreting presupposes
that utterances can be translated without loss or change of meaning. Haviland (2003: 772)
refers to this as the ideology of referential transparency, in which ‘substituting one language’s
words for another’s’ can be done ‘as though the word, or code, is merely an exotic costume
for a shared meaning.’ Researchers have increasingly challenged this assumption, either by
pointing out that interpreter-mediated interaction is inherently different from same-language
interaction or by investigating structural differences between languages. For example, lan-
guages differ with regard to the marking of agency, and so speakers of one language may be
able to avoid an attribution of blame in ways that speakers of another cannot (see Lee 2009a
on the use of subject ellipsis in Korean; or B erk-Seligson 1990 on the use of reflexive passives
and datives of interest in Spanish; also Mason 2015). D iscourse-specific features of languages,
such as u tterance-final particles in Cantonese, or discourse markers in other languages, are
notoriously difficult to translate and often end up being omitted by interpreters, in ways that
may not change the propositional content of testimony, but affect its overall coherence (Hale
1999, 2004; Leung and Gibbons 2009). Coherence is also affected by consistency in lexical
choice (Pym 1999), and by the pauses required for consecutive interpreting, which cause frag-
mentation of extended turns (D’hondt 2004; Ng 2009; Angermeyer 2015). As noted above,
scholars have also examined ways in which interpreters may change the register or speech style
of testimony, with potential consequences for how credible or likeable they are perceived to be
by legal decision-makers (Berk-Seligson 1990; Hale 2004; Lee 2011). For example, Gallez and
Maryns (2014: 7 4–75) show how an interpreter renders a defendant’s informal and affective,
‘a lmost childish’ vernacular speech into a more formal and institutionally appropriate register,
thereby giving a different impression of the defendant’s character and preparedness, with po-
tential consequences for the evaluation of ‘his moral and legal responsibility.’
Compared to other institutions, courts have been shown to be less flexible about the lan-
guage choice of multilingual speakers. In particular, lay participants who have an interpreter
available to them may be discouraged or even forbidden from using their second language
proficiency in the institutional language (A ngermeyer 2008, 2015). Such a ll-or-nothing pol-
icies ignore the fact that second language acquisition is dynamic and not uniform, so that a
person may be able to narrate their recollection of an event in a second language, but may
not be able to understand legal arguments or the nuances of complex questions in cross-
examination. Stand-by interpreting, which is common with non-professional interpreters
in other contexts, would be an option for lay participants with some proficiency in the
official language, and its use has been observed in some jurisdictions (see e.g. Kadric 2001:
218). Cooke (1996) reports on the trial of an Aboriginal Australian defendant who spoke
English but also received assistance from an interpreter. Focusing on the defendant’s testi-
mony, Cooke notes,
access to an interpreter at the trial was a significant aid in her narration … The fact
that she spoke mainly English does not detract from the linguistic empowerment that
interpreting assistance gave her at those points when deficiency in her second language
blocked her expressive capacity (Cooke 1996: 286).
The practice of stand-by interpreting points to the fact that the linguistic boundaries be-
tween the participants in the courtroom are not always as impermeable as is often assumed.
131
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
Even lay participants with low proficiency in the court’s language may actively code-switch
into this language rather than rely exclusively on the interpreter, for example to respond to
a yes/no interrogative (A ngermeyer 2015). Alternatively, they may understand a question
in the original and respond in their preferred language before it has been translated. Such
practices can be understood as cases of translanguaging, a concept that has gained currency
in bilingualism and second language acquisition studies in recent years (Li Wei 2011; Oth-
eguy, García and Reid 2019). The term translanguaging describes language practices in
which speakers make use of the full repertoire of their linguistic knowledge, rather than
treating languages as separate and using only one language at a time, as if knowledge of the
other could be turned off (Rock 2017). Legal professionals may similarly make use of their
proficiency in a n on-official language. This is especially common in p ost-colonial contexts,
such as Hong Kong, where Ng (2018) argues that bilingual counsels have an advantage over
monolingual participants because they assume a role of ‘non-ratified overhearer of the ver-
sion of the talk not originally intended for them’ (2018: 70). However, translanguaging prac-
tices are also found in the global north when lay participants use a language that is widely
known (for instance English in continental Europe, or Spanish in certain parts of the United
States), or when attorneys speak their client’s language.
132
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
Figure 8.1 Grammatical person used for participants in source-and target-centered interpreting re-
spectively (i.e. direct translation vs. reported speech; Angermeyer 2015: 85)
translating into languages that grammatically mark the gender or social status of the speaker
or addressee, misunderstandings may also be avoided in direct translation, such as if first
person forms are marked for the gender of the source speaker rather than the interpreter, or
if informal second person forms are used instead of polite address forms (A ngermeyer 2005).
Building on earlier analyses of the use of fi rst-and second-person reference (A ngermeyer
2005; Angermeyer 2009), I have argued in Angermeyer (2015) that interpreter styles in court
interpreting can be understood as source-centered or target-centered (see Figure 8.1). In
source-centered interpreting, interpreters maintain the person deixis of the source speech,
including the use of first person to refer to the source speaker and second person to refer to
the addressee of the source. By contrast, t arget-centered interpreting shifts the deictic center
so that the recipient is treated as addressee (referred to by second person) and first person is
reserved for the interpreter. This distinction between interpreter styles is inspired by Waden-
sjö’s (1998: 246–47) distinction between ‘relaying by replaying’ and ‘relaying by displaying’
as two endpoints on a continuum of interpreting styles, based on how interpreters constantly
and variously mark (limited) responsibility for what they utter, not only by means of choice
of personal deixis but by a range of communicative resources such as changes in vocal pitch,
gaze direction, gestures and so forth.
The choice between source-centered and target-centered interpreting styles may be due
to the interpreter’s preferences, their training and professional affiliation or identity, as well
as a result of accommodating to the preferences or usage of recipients. For example, some
legal professionals may ask the interpreter to ask a witness, rather than asking the witness
themselves directly (see Berk-Seligson 1990: 61; Fowler 1997; Christensen 2008). At the
same time, it may be conditioned by a number of contextual factors, such as the need to
disambiguate between competing source speakers, or the identification of an interactional
trouble source (A ngermeyer 2009). While court interpreting norms are posited as applying
independently of the direction of interpreting, several studies show an asymmetrical distri-
bution of these styles, in that the interpretation of witness testimony into the institutional
language is done consistently in source-centered interpreting style, but interpretation in
the other direction varies between the two styles, with some interpreters favoring a target-
c entered approach throughout and others using it for specific pragmatic reasons. For ex-
ample, Berk-Seligson (1990: 115) notes that many interpreters ‘systematically avoid these
pronouns when the judge is declaring the sentence’ such as by using passive forms (see also
133
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
Angermeyer 2009). Similarly, Ng (2018: 168) finds that in Hong Kong courtrooms, court
interpreters consistently use first person for interpreting from Cantonese into English, but
third person when translating from English into Cantonese, and she observes that this prac-
tice appears to be encouraged by their supervisors. Drawing on a survey answered by inter-
preters, she attributes this practice to a reluctance of interpreters to assume the voice of legal
professionals (whose speech they are translating when interpreting from English). Cheung
(2012) has similar findings and argues that the use of reported speech when translating into
Cantonese can facilitate communication and prevent misunderstandings.
While the use of first person and a source-centered interpreting style is often described
as a practice that distinguishes professional interpreters in courts, its comprehension by lis-
teners is also a marker of professionalism among the other participants in court. Legal pro-
fessionals may expect and understand that first person pronouns refer to the source speaker
rather than to the interpreter, but, as mentioned above, lay participants may find this practice
confusing, and this may induce interpreters to avoid it when they are interpreting for them.
In fact, scholars have increasingly argued that t arget-centered deictic adaptations of the par-
ticipant framework, but also of references to space and time, may enhance lay participants’
ability to understand the interpreter’s renditions (Cheung 2012; Gallez and Maryns 2014).
Defrancq and Verliefde (2017) describe a particularly striking contrast between the partici-
pation frameworks of professional courtroom discourse and the interpreter’s rendition style.
In the inquisitorial system of the Belgian Correctional Court from which they draw their
data, legal professionals employ a ‘paternalistic’ (2017: 210) style where the defendant is the
main topic of conversation but is not being addressed directly. Defranq and Verliefde show
that the court interpreter consistently transforms this paternalistic style in her interpretation
by ‘converting all third person references to the defendant into second person form’ (2017:
221), thus treating the defendant as the addressee rather than as an overhearer. At the same
time, she maintains the paternalistic style when interpreting the defendant’s speech for the
judge (2017: 223–24).
While such deictic shifts may thus play a role in coordinating the interaction and fa-
cilitating understanding, they have perhaps more often been analyzed in relation to the
interpreters’ pursuit of remaining neutral, their identity and ideology (Berk-Seligson 1990;
Shlesinger 1991; Morris 1995; Hale 2004). For example, Leung and Gibbons (2008) show
in their analysis of data from two trials in Hong Kong how the court interpreters’ shifts in
person deixis coincides with other ways in which they mark their own stances v is-à-vis other
participants, such as, for example modifying the pragmatic force of hostile questions posed
by a defense lawyer in a rape trial. However, while deictic shifts permit interpreters to speak
for themselves, they do not automatically mean that the interpreter is perceived as taking
side. In an experimental study, Cheung (2014) found that lay participants did not perceive
the use of reported speech per se as a sign of the interpreter’s lack of neutrality. However, if
reported speech was introduced with a title (‘the judge said…’), interpreters were more likely
to be perceived as aligned with the legal professional, whereas use of a pronoun (‘he said…’)
led to the perception of an alignment with the lay recipient.
Related topics
In recent years, scholars in the field of language and law have increasingly treated court-
room interaction as part of an intertextual chain in the judicial process, investigating
in particular how police records and other documents are recontextualized in criminal
trials (Heffer, Rock and Conley 2013; D’hondt 2014). The impact of intertextuality on
134
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
court interpreting has not yet been examined to the same extent, however. While it has
often been noted that court interpreters may need to engage in sight translation or may
need to interpret complex written texts that are read out loud to them, less attention has
been paid to how those written documents may themselves be the product of processes of
translation, for example in the case of a suspect’s written statement to the police that may
have been produced with or without the aid of an interpreter. As shown by B erk-Seligson
(2 000, 2009) and Morris (2 008), among others, interpreting in police interviews is often
conducted in a more informal and a d-hoc manner than interpreting in court, and this pro-
cess may itself become the subject of a legal argument in trial or on appeal. B erk-Seligson
(2 009) conducts detailed analyses of recordings of police interviews with S panish-speaking
suspects in the United States, showing how bilingual police officers may blur the roles
of interpreter and interrogator. She also shows how suspects’ statements are drafted in
English by police officers, and then given to suspects to sign, often without providing an
accurate backtranslation. When such statements are presented in court, court interpreters
may have to translate them back into the defendant’s language, presenting them as the
defendant’s own words, only for the defendant to deny having said them. Marszalenko
(2 014) compares the working conditions of legal interpreters at three stages of the judicial
process in Japan, that is, during police interrogation, interviews by a public prosecutor and
testimony at trial. Drawing on personal experience as interpreter rather than data from
recorded interaction, Marszalenko notes that it is more difficult for interpreters to remain
neutral during the police interview than at the trial, and argues, like B erk-Seligson, that
the production of the suspect’s written statement by police is problematic. As interpreting
for police becomes a more widely researched topic, the importance of intertextuality for
court interpreting is likely to be explored further in future studies.
Intertextuality also represents a challenge for the translation of legal terms. As noted
above, court interpreting requires interpreters to be familiar with specific legal registers,
which are tied to the distinct legal traditions of their respective jurisdictions. Specialized
legal terminology refers to concepts that have developed highly specific meanings through
legal statutes and traditions of precedent. As a result, such legal terms of art generally do not
have translation equivalents in languages that do not share a similar legal tradition. In such
cases, interpreters may be found to produce lay reformulations of legal concepts that reduce
the institutional character of their rendition. Such challenges point to the limitations of legal
translation, and ultimately to the fact that languages are not neutral vehicles of communi-
cation (Duranti 2011). Even in bilingual legal systems, where participants have the choice
between languages, scholars have shown that this choice may have legal implications. For
example, in his analysis of trials in Hopi Tribal Court, Richland (2008) shows that appeals
to traditional Hopi concepts of justice are more likely to be successful if they are made in
Hopi rather than in English. Similarly, Ng (2009) shows how in Hong Kong, common law
trials become less formal and bound by tradition when trials are conducted in Cantonese
rather than English. In a broader sense, this points to what linguistic anthropologists and
sociolinguists call the social indexicality of language, namely that linguistic forms and codes
are socially meaningful in that they shape how participants perceive each other. A request for
a court interpreter or the act of speaking a non-official language may by itself be evaluated
negatively by other participants and evoke negative social stereotypes, or what Haviland
(2003: 771) calls ‘l inguistic paranoia’, that is a belief that speakers use an unfamiliar language
to hide information from others (see also Fowler 1997; Angermeyer 2015: 10). Court in-
terpreting is thus an area of public service interpreting where the participants’ beliefs about
language and translation may have a considerable impact on the rights of individuals. In this,
135
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
Further reading
Haviland, John B. (2003) “Ideologies of language: Reflections on language and U.S. law”, American
Anthropologist 105: 764–74.
Drawing on court records from two controversial legal decisions in the United States, the author insightfully
examines the language ideologies that underlie judicial attitudes towards translation and constrain how court
interpreting is provided.
Karrebæk, Martha Sif and Solvej Sørensen (2021) “Interpreting as creating a potential for understand-
ing: Insights from a Danish courtroom”, International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law 28 (1):
59–97.
Examining a corpus of audio-recorded court proceedings in Denmark, the authors explore how interpreters
work to facilitate lay participants’ understanding of institutional processes when the corresponding source talk
by legal professionals is implicit and abbreviated.
Ng, Eva N.S. (2018) Common Law in an Uncommon Courtroom: Judicial Interpreting in Hong Kong.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
The author provides a highly detailed linguistic analysis of E nglish-Cantonese interpreting in the bi-
lingual context of Hong Kong, where some courtroom participants have bilingual proficiency while others
do not.
Related chapters
Chapter 5, Corpus-based studies of public service interpreting by Bernd Meyer
Chapter 9, Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews by Sonja Pöllabauer
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences by Christian Licoppe
Notes
1 See for example the Code of Ethics of the National Association of Judicial Interpreters and Translators
in the United States: https://najit.org/w p-content/uploads/2016/09/NAJITCodeof EthicsFINAL.
pdf
2 As noted by Gallez and Maryns (2014), court interpreters may also whisper during witness inter-
pretation, when interpreting into the witness’s language.
References
Amuzu, Evershed K., Akua Campbell, and Seth Ofori (2020) “‘That’s not my understanding’: Inter-
pretation in the Ghanaian multilingual court”, Language & Dialogue 10 (3): 3 89–421.
Angermeyer, Philipp Sebastian (2005) “W ho is “you”? Polite forms of address and ambiguous partic-
ipant roles in court interpreting”, Target: International Journal of Translation Studies 17 (2): 2 03–26.
——— (2008) “Creating monolingualism in the multilingual courtroom”, Sociolinguistic Studies 2 (3):
385– 403.
——— (2009) “Translation style and participant roles in court interpreting”, Journal of Sociolinguistics
13 (1): 3 –28.
——— (2013) “Adapting existing data sources: Language and the law”, in Data Collection in Sociolin-
guistics: Methods and Applications, Christine Mallinson, Becky Childs and Gerard Van Herk (eds).
London/New York, Routledge: 183– 86.
——— (2015) Speak English or What?: Codeswitching and Interpreter Use in New York City Courts. New
York, Oxford University Press.
——— (2016) “Promoting litigant consent to arbitration in multilingual small claims court”, in Dis-
cursive Constructions of Consent in the Legal Process, Susan Ehrlich, Diana Eades, and Janet Ainsworth
(eds). New York, Oxford University Press: 163–85.
136
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
Arumí, Marta and Mireia Vargas-Urpi (2018) “A nnotation of interpreters’ conversation management
problems and strategies in a corpus of criminal proceedings in Spain”, Translation & Interpreting
Studies 13 (3): 421–41.
Berk-Seligson, Susan (1990) The Bilingual Courtroom: Court Interpreters in the Judicial Process. Chicago,
IL, University of Chicago Press.
——— (2000) “Interpreting for the police: Issues in pre-trial phases of the judicial process”, Forensic
Linguistics 7 (2): 212–37.
——— (2009) Coerced Confessions: The Discourse of Bilingual Police Interrogations. Berlin, Mouton de
Gruyter.
Brown-Blake, Celia (2008) “The right to linguistic non-discrimination and Creole language situa-
tions: The case of Jamaica”, Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 23 (1): 3 2–74.
Carstensen, Gunilla, and Leif Dahlberg (2017) “Court interpreting as emotional work: A pilot study in
Swedish law courts”, No Foundations: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Law and Justice 14: 45–64.
Cheung, Andrew K. F. (2012) “The use of reported speech by court interpreters in Hong Kong”,
Interpreting 14 (1): 73–91.
——— (2014) “The use of reported speech and the perceived neutrality of court interpreters”, Inter-
preting 16 (2): 191–208.
——— (2017) “Non-renditions in court interpreting”, Babel 63 (2): 1 74–99.
Christensen, Tina Paulsen (2008) “Judges’ deviations from n orm-based direct speech in court”, Inter-
preting 10 (1): 99–127.
Colin, Joan and Ruth Morris (1996) Interpreters and the Legal Process. Winchester, Waterside Press.
Cooke, Michael (1996) “A different story: Narrative versus ‘question and answer’ in Aboriginal evidence”,
Forensic Linguistics 3 (2): 273–88.
Coulthard, Malcolm, and Sandra Hale (2016) “Guest editor’s introduction” [Special issue on legal in-
terpreting], Language and Law=Linguagem e Direito 3 (2). https://ojs.letras.up.pt/i ndex.php/LLLD/
issue/v iew/134.
De Jongh, Elena M. (1992) An Introduction to Court Interpreting: Theory and Practice. Lanham, University
Press of America.
——— (2012) From the Classroom to the Courtroom: A Guide to Interpreting in the U.S. Justice System.
Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
Defrancq, Bart, and Sofie Verliefde (2017) “Interpreter-mediated ‘paternalistic’ interaction in a judge-
centered courtroom”, Interpreting 19 (2): 209–31.
D’hondt, Sigurd (2004) Interculturele communicatie in rechtbanken. Brussels, Politeia.
——— (2014) “Quoting from the case file: How intertextual practices shape discourse at various stages
in the legal trajectory”, Language & Communication 36: 1– 6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.
2013.12.008.
Duranti, Alessandro (2011) “Linguistic anthropology: The study of language as a n on-neutral me-
dium”, in The Cambridge Handbook of Sociolinguistics, Rajend Mesthrie (ed). Cambridge and New
York, Cambridge University Press: 28–46.
Eades, Diana, Sandra Hale, and Michael Cooke (1999) “Introduction” [Special issue on legal interpret-
ing]. International Journal of Speech Language and the Law 6 (1): 1–5.
Eades, Diana and Aneta Pavlenko (2016) “Translating research into policy: New guidelines for com-
municating rights to non-native speakers”, Language and Law=Linguagem e Direito 3 (2): 45–64.
Edwards, Alicia Betsy (1995) The Practice of Court Interpreting. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, Benjamins.
Elias-Bursać, Ellen (2012) “Shaping international justice: The role of translation and interpreting at
the ICTY in The Hague”, Translation and Interpreting Studies 7 (1): 34–53.
Fowler, Yvonne (1997) “The courtroom interpreter: Paragon and intruder?”, in The Critical Link:
Interpreters in the Community, Silvana E. Carr, Roda Roberts, Aideen Dufour and Dini Steyn (eds).
Amsterdam, Benjamins: 191–200.
——— (2016) “Court interpreting in England: What works? (a nd for whom)? How interpreted prison
video link impacts upon courtroom interaction”, Language and Law=Linguagem e Direito 3 (2):
135–68.
Gallez, Emmanuelle and Katrijn Maryns (2014) “Orality and authenticity in an interpreter-mediated
defendant’s examination: A case study from the Belgian Assize Court”, Interpreting 16 (1): 49–80.
Gallez, Emmanuelle and Anne Reynders (2015) “Court interpreting and classical rhetoric: Ethos in
i nterpreter-mediated monological discourse”, Interpreting 17 (1): 64–90.
Goffman, Erving (1981) Forms of Talk. Oxford/Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania Press.
137
Philipp Sebastian Angermeyer
Hale, Sandra (1999) “Interpreters’ treatment of discourse markers in courtroom questions”, Forensic
Linguistics 6 (1): 5 7–82.
——— (2004) The Discourse of Court Interpreting: Discourse Practices of the Law, the Witness and the Inter-
preter. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, Benjamins.
——— (2015) “Approaching the bench: Teaching magistrates and judges how to work effectively with
interpreters”, MonTI. Monografías de Traducción e Interpretación 7: 163–80.
Hale, Sandra, Mehera San Roque, David Spencer and Jemina Napier (2017) “Deaf citizens as jurors
in Australian courts: Participating via professional interpreters”, International Journal of Speech, Lan-
guage and the Law 24 (2): 151–76.
Heffer, Chris, Frances Rock and John Conley (eds). (2013) L egal-Lay Communication: Textual Travels in
the Law. New York, Oxford University Press.
Jacobsen, Bente (2012) “The significance of interpreting modes for question–answer dialogues in
court interpreting”, Interpreting 14 (2): 2 17–41.
Kadric, Mira (2001) Dolmetschen bei Gericht: Erwartungen, Anforderungen, Kompetenzen. Vienna, WUV,
Universitätsverlag.
Lee, Jieun (2009a) “Interpreting inexplicit language during courtroom examination”, Applied Linguis-
tics 30 (1): 93–114.
——— (2009b) “W hen linguistic and cultural differences are not disclosed in court interpreting”,
Multilingua 28 (4): 379–401.
——— (2011) “Translatability of speech style in court interpreting”, International Journal of Speech,
Language and the Law 18 (1): 1–33.
Leung, Ester and John Gibbons (2009) “Interpreting Cantonese utterance-final particles in bilingual
courtroom discourse”, Interpreting 11 (2): 190–215.
Leung, Ester S. M., and John Gibbons (2008) “W ho is responsible? Participant roles in legal interpret-
ing cases”, Multilingua 27 (3): 177–91.
Li Wei (2011) “Moment analysis and translanguaging space: Discursive construction of identities by
multilingual Chinese youth in Britain”, Journal of Pragmatics 43 (5): 1222–35.
Licoppe, Christian, and Maud Verdier (2013) “Interpreting, video communication and the sequential
reshaping of institutional talk in the bilingual and distributed courtroom”, International Journal of
Speech Language and the Law 20 (2): 247–75.
Marszalenko, Jakub E. (2014) “Three stages of interpreting in Japan’s criminal process”, Language and
Law=Linguagem e Direito 1 (1): 174–87.
Martinsen, Bodil, and Friedel Dubslaff (2010) “The cooperative courtroom: A case study of interpret-
ing gone wrong”, Interpreting 12 (1): 21–59.
Mason, Marianne (2015) “The role of interpreters in adjudicating blame: An examination of clitics and
active-passive voice in a Spanish-English bilingual criminal trial”, Translation & Interpreting Studies
10 (2): 187–202.
Matthews, Gladys, and Enrica J. Ardemagni (2013) “Judicial interpretation education in U.S. colleges
and universities: The path to academic recognition”, Translation & Interpreting Studies 8 (1): 73–93.
Mikkelson, Holly (1998) “Towards a redefinition of the role of the court interpreter”, Interpreting 3
(1): 21–45.
——— (2000) Introduction to Court Interpreting. Manchester, St. Jerome Publishing.
Moeketsi, Rosemary (1999) Discourse in a Multilingual and Multicultural Courtroom: A Court Interpreter’s
Guide, Hatfield, Pretoria, J.L. van Schaik.
Morris, Ruth (1990) “Interpretation at the Demjanjuk trial”, in Interpreting – Yesterday, Today and
Tomorrow, vol. 4, David Bowen and Margareta Bowen (eds). Binghamton, NY, State University of
New York: 101–7.
——— (1995) “The moral dilemmas of court interpreting”, The Translator 1: 25–46.
——— (1999) “Plus ça change…? Community interpreters at the end of the twentieth century”, in
The Critical Link 2: Interpreters in the Community, Roda P. Roberts, Silvana E. Carr, Diana Abraham
and Aideen Dufour (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 243–64.
——— (2008) “M issing stitches: An overview of judicial attitudes to interlingual interpreting in the
criminal justice systems of Canada and Israel”, Interpreting 10 (1): 34–64.
Nakane, Ikuko and Makiko Mizuno (2019) “Judgments on court interpreting in Japan: Ideologies and prac-
tice”, International Journal for the Semiotics of Law – Revue internationale de Sémiotique juridique 32: 773–93.
Ng, Kwai Hang (2009) The Common Law in Two Voices: Language, Law, and the Postcolonial Dilemma in
Hong Kong. Stanford, Stanford University Press.
138
Public service interpreting in court: face-to-face interaction
O’Barr, William M. (1982) Linguistic Evidence: Language, Power, and Strategy in the Courtroom. New
York, Academic Press.
Otheguy, Ricardo, Ofelia García and Wallis Reid (2019) “A translanguaging view of the linguistic
system of bilinguals”, Applied Linguistics Review 10 (4): 625–51.
Ozolins, Uldis (2016) “The long road to effective court interpreting: International perspectives”, Lan-
guage and Law=Linguagem e Direito 3 (2): 9 8–115.
Pérez González, Luis ( 2006) “
Interpreting strategic recontextualization cues in the courtroom:
Corpus-based insights into the pragmatic force of non-restrictive relative clauses”, Journal of Prag-
matics 38 (3): 390–417.
Pousada, Alicia (1979) “Interpreting for language minorities in the courts”, in Language in Public Life,
James Alatis and G. Richard Tucker (eds). Washington, DC, Georgetown University Press: 1 86–208.
Powell, Richard (2008) “Bilingual courtrooms: In the interest of justice?”, in Dimensions of Forensic
Linguistics, John Gibbons and M. Teresa Turell (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins:
131–59.
Powell, Richard, and Azirah Hashim (2011) “Language disadvantage in Malaysian litigation and arbi-
tration”, World Englishes 30 (1): 92–105.
Pym, Anthony (1999) “‘Nicole slapped Michelle’: Interpreters and theories of interpreting at the O.J.
Simpson trial”, The Translator 5 (2): 265–83.
Richland, Justin B. (2008) Arguing with Tradition: The Language of Law in Hopi Tribal Court. Chicago,
University of Chicago Press.
Rickford, John R., and Sharese King (2016) “Language and linguistics on trial: Hearing Rachel
Jeantel (a nd other vernacular speakers) in the courtroom and beyond”, Language 92 (4): 948–88.
Rigney, Azucena C. (1999) “Questioning in interpreted testimony”, Forensic Linguistics 6: 83–108.
Rock, Frances (2017) “Shifting ground: Exploring the backdrop to translating and interpreting”, The
Translator 23 (2): 217–36.
Schweda-Nicholson, Nancy (1994) “Professional ethics for court and community interpreters”, in Pro-
fessional Issues for Translators and Interpreters, Deanna L. Hammond (ed). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 79–98.
Shlesinger, Miriam (1991) “Interpreter latitude vs. due process: Simultaneous and consecutive inter-
pretation in multilingual trials”, in Empirical Research in Translation and Intercultural Studies, Sonja
Tirkkonen-Condit (ed). Tübingen, Günter Narr: 1 47–55.
Shlesinger, Miriam, and Franz Pöchhacker (2008) “Introduction: Doing justice to court interpreting”,
Interpreting 10 (1): 1–7.
Takeda, Kayoko (2008) “Interpreting at the Tokyo War Crimes Tribunal”, Interpreting 10 (1): 65–83.
Thetela, Pulie (2003) “Discourse, culture and the law: The analysis of crosstalk in the Southern Afri-
can bilingual courtroom”, AILA Review 16: 78–88.
Vargas-Urpi, Mireia (2019) “W hen non-renditions are not the exception: A corpus-based study of
court interpreting”, Babel 65 (4): 478–500.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London/New York, Longman.
139
9
RESEARCH ON INTERPRETER-
MEDIATED ASYLUM
INTERVIEWS
Sonja Pöllabauer
Introduction
This contribution outlines epistemological, theoretical and methodological considerations,
and strands of topics in research on interpreter-
mediated communication in asylum contexts.
The focus lies primarily on research that uses and explores authentic communication data,
that is instances of naturally occurring talk, taking place in different administrative stages
of the asylum adjudication procedure, recorded and subsequently transcribed. Research that
predominantly addresses users’ or interpreters’ perspectives is not included in this review
(see Gustafsson, this volume, for a discussion of such work). All of the studies under review
present some form of ‘interactional data’ (Vuori and Hokkanen 2020: 110). The studies un-
der review present data on spoken-language interpreting only; at present, no studies report
on corpora of naturally occurring sign language interpreting (SLI) in an asylum context
(but see Slettebakk Berge and Stone et al., this volume, on problems related to sign language
interpreting).
The thematic focus of this chapter lies on communication in asylum proceedings, which
often requires interpreters to assist in the process of determining whether individuals in fear
of persecution are to be granted refugee status (asylum). In most industrialised countries, the
asylum adjudication system is governed by international humanitarian law, with the 1951
Geneva Convention relating to the Status of Refugees being one of the key instruments,
together with national asylum-
specific legislation. While the structure of the proceedings
may vary nationally, most countries provide several levels of adjudication, often with an
appeal instance. The number of refugees worldwide and dominant source and host countries
are influenced by current geopolitical developments and subject to rapid change (relevant
statistics are available from key players in refugee protection such as the UN International
Organisation for Migration (IOM) or the UN Refugee Agency (UNHCR)). Due to lack
of documentary evidence, most countries rely on in-person oral interviews to establish the
validity of applicants’ claims. These interviews generally have a prototypical structure and
format, with the burden of proof lying on the applicants who are required to disclose rel-
evant facts and present their claims in the best possible way, and where contradictions may
harm their credibility. The applicants’ oral accounts of events are then remoulded and pre-
served in written records (transcripts) of the interviews which are the basis for the asylum
officials’ decisions. Interpreters play a crucial role in this system as both sides need to rely on
the interpreter and trust in him or her to render what is said fully and correctly.
The literature review presents a chronological outline of the development of this field,
which is followed by a discussion of central epistemological, theoretical and methodological
considerations. A discussion of salient strands of topics follows, and an outline of work in
related fields concludes the chapter.
Literature overview
Among the first publications on interpreter-mediated interaction in an asylum context, dat-
ing back to the late 1980s and early 1990s, are contributions that are not data-
driven. Kälin’s
(1986) and Monnier’s (1995) study of the structure of asylum interviews and cross-cultural
misunderstandings in (Swiss) asylum proceedings are among the most influential of those
early conceptual-theoretical publications.
One of the pioneering authors of the 1990s is Barsky (1993, 1994, 1996) who, based
on official transcripts of Canadian Refugee Convention hearings, provides a profound and
critical discussion of the routines and the intrinsic bias inherent to such encounters. With a
focus on the US, Anker (1991) also zooms in on data from the 1980s. Though Anker’s study
does not strictly meet the criteria for inclusion in the body of literature under review, as
her corpus does not include recordings of authentic interviews, it is still worth mentioning
as this two-year observational study reports on one of the largest corpora in the pertinent
literature with a sample of 193 hearings that were observed. Like other authors who first
drew attention to this then marginal field of research, Anker points to ‘major problems in
foreign language interpretation’ (1991: 272) such as faulty renditions, no training and no
standardised selection criteria for interpreters. Following and in part inspired by these first
t hought-provoking forays into this field, the 2000s saw a surge in more d ata-d riven research
(see Pöllabauer 2006b: 235), a trend that may be explained not only by broader geopolitical
developments which sparked greater interest in migration-
related topics but also by the fact
that with the new millennium, PSI research had gained momentum. It seems neither feasible
nor useful, however, to present a full chronological timeline of more recent research in this
section; publications that have proven particularly fruitful are discussed in more detail under
the sections below.
141
Sonja Pöllabauer
(2008) on Sweden and finally, Lee (2014) analyses the situation in South Korea. Interestingly,
Australia 2 does not appear on this map, though it has been pinpointed as one of the pioneer-
ing countries as regards PSI practice standards and training. One explanation might be Aus-
tralia’s restrictive offshore asylum policy which might impede researchers’ access to the field
to an even greater degree than in other countries. What is also apparent is the lack of data
on central European countries that have been under former Communist rule (exceptions
are Tryuk’s (2017) survey on Polish interpreters’ views, and recent ethnographic work by
Tužinská (2019) on the Slovak asylum context.
Data-driven research
Most of the studies under review adopt a qualitative design and a phenomenological or
constructivist approach, though elements of quantitative methods are occasionally used to
describe specific data (sub)sets (Keselman et al. 2008, 2010b; van der Kleij 2015). The tag-
ging of research designs is not clear-
cut, yet most of the studies in the literature under review
can broadly be described as case studies, in that they combine different methods to explore
a communicative phenomenon and interactants’ perspectives within its context. Some can
be classified as typical examples of ethnographic research, and also use this descriptor, while
others do not use this label yet adopt an ethnographic multi-
data design. Almost all use
some form of discourse analysis, which is a blurry concept per se in that it is sometimes used
142
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
broadly and unsystematically to describe the analysis of language in use, and sometimes is
associated with a particular methodological-theoretical framework (Hale and Napier 2013:
119). Many underline the problems in getting access to the data, given the sensibility of the
situations involved.
143
Sonja Pöllabauer
144
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
also combine those with a document analysis of the official records that were taken during
these interviews. Like Lee’s (2014) ‘case study’ of ad-hoc-interpreting by untrained lay in-
terpreters in South Korean asylum appeal hearings, they do not narrow down their approach
to a specific school of discourse analysis but conduct a broad analysis that combines different
elements of the studies mentioned above.
Van der Kleij’s (2015) study on i nterpreter-mediated asylum interviews in the Netherlands
is fittingly labelled a ‘corpus analysis’ as it is the only study in the literature under review that
presents a predominantly quantitative analysis of shifts and modifications in the interpreters’
renditions. The author combines different theoretical constructs, such as Wadensjö’s (1998)
model of renditions and Goffman’s ( 1981) participation framework, with Chesterman’s
(1993) translational norms and van Leuven-Zwart’s (1989, 1990) notion of translation shifts.
Multimodal data
A very specific set of multimodal data is used by Mason and Stewart (2001), Tillman (2009)
and Mason (2012), who explore fragments of TV documentaries on asylum interviews that
feature instances of authentic hearings. While Mason and Stewart (2001), who focus on face,
and Tillman (2009), who conducts a quantitative analysis of modal particles, do not compre-
hensively describe their corpus and its limitations, Mason (2012) discusses the usability and
limitations of such multimodal data in more depth. His fine-grained analysis of gaze patterns
shows that the interplay of gaze and nonverbal signals is closely linked to issues of status and
role. While the data used by Mason and Stewart and Tillman are rather small fragments of
video-recorded data that were recorded and preprocessed for a different purpose (use in TV
documentaries), Licoppe and Veyrier’s (2020) work on asylum appeal hearings in France is
currently the only large-scale a sylum-specific study that is based on a corpus of 150 hours
of multimodal courtroom data (see also Licoppe and Verdier 2013, and Licoppe and Veyrier
2017, and Licoppe this volume, on aspects of courtroom interpreting that also bear relevance
to asylum interpreting). Licoppe and Veyrier (2020) adopt an explicit ethnographic approach
by combining video recordings with observation and interviews, and specifically focus on
chunking and turn management in stretches of extended answers in remote consecutive.
Their theoretical framework is drawn from the literature on institutional talk and power and
identity, and Wadensjö’s (1998) notion of explicit or implicit coordination.
145
Sonja Pöllabauer
considerably and is not comparable. Some provide information on the number of cases, rang-
ing between one (Gómez Díez 2010) and 390 (Gill et al. 2016), while others measure their
corpus in minutes or hours, ranging between a few minutes (Tillman 2009; Mason 2012)
and 150 hours (Licoppe and Veyrier 2020).
146
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
be heard, partially also due to his lack of knowledge about the applicants’ multilingual and
multicultural background. Much of the applicant’s narrative does not ‘survive the interpret-
er’s filter’ (2010: 353), who assumes tasks that would in fact be the official’s duty. This is also
an example of how unawareness or ignorance on the part of the authorities as regards the
selection and recruitment of suitable interpreters may exacerbate the intrinsic bias inherent
to the system. In a different geographical context, though similar as regards the degree (or
lack) of awareness by official stakeholders and interpreters, Lee (2010) shows in a case study
of South Korean appeal hearings how untrained interpreters assume discrepant roles that
may also be detrimental to the applicants’ claims. Her examples also provide ample evidence
of how content may be mistranslated. Lee’s (2014: 66) is also one of the few studies in the
literature under review where the complexities of relay interpreting in such a context are
addressed (see also Maryns 2006: 238–39 on a case of multilanguage translation).
Narrative inequalities that are intrinsic to the system and may hamper applicants’ chances
of being heard (Barsky 1994; see also Doornbos 2005 on evidentiary assessment) are dis-
cussed extensively by Blommaert (for instance 2001) and Maryns and Blommaert (2002).
The complex entextualisation process that is inherent to the process of drafting a formal
record (Blommaert 2001) and applicants’ dense and complex ‘home narratives’ that are used
to narrate and contextualise ‘displaced lives’ (2001: 63), as well as ‘deterritorialised’ (Maryns
and Blommaert 2001: 63) and ‘transidiomatic’ (ibid.) language practices and forms of shifting
and mixing different codes which are part of applicants’ language repertoire, make interpret-
ing in such a context a difficult task. Deterritorialised and transidiomatic language practices,
terms that were coined by Jacquemet (2000), describe linguistic resources and language use
that are ‘out of place’ (Maryns and Blommaert 2001: 63) in that they are not usually associ-
ated with the context (space) in which they are used, and, in the case of transidiomacy, re-
sources that are ‘not associated with a (perceived) linguistic community’ (ibid.: 64). Maryns
(2006, 2013), for instance, presents a wealth of examples of interpreters’ influence on such
complex discursive processes and shows how interpreters’ and officials’ lack of awareness of
these processes may result in misunderstandings and faulty renderings that can affect the de-
termination of credibility. Doornbos (2005) also draws attention to the fact that interpreters
are not always impartial, as stipulated by codes of ethics, and sometimes assume an expert
role by providing background information that may be used by officials to contest claimants’
arguments and credibility (see also Scheffer 2001: 139–87 on interpreters’ role in ‘credibility
testing’ routines; my translation).
The role of interpreters is also a transversal topic straddling most of the studies under
review, as almost all, in one way or the other, discuss interpreters’ agency and positioning
within that complex discursive system as outlined above. Barsky, for instance, pleads for a
distinctively active role of interpreters as ‘intercultural agents’ (1996), who in his view should
be allowed to articulate and even embellish applicants’ claims to counterbalance the intrinsic
bias that is inherent to the asylum determination system. Justified as such a claim may be,
examples of more recent data-based studies suggest that in the face of a lack of trained inter-
preters and little awareness among principal stakeholders of the complexity of the tasks to be
assumed by interpreters, such a claim may remain an ideal. With reference to Prunč’s (1997)
notion of ‘translation culture’ as the variable set of norm, conventions and expectations fram-
ing interactants’ behaviour in a given field, Pöllabauer (2006a), for instance, shows that along
a continuum of different forms of agency, interpreters often assume discrepant roles that
hold a potential for manipulation. Prunč’s construct of translation culture is influenced by
issues such as loyalty, cooperation and transparency, which were used by Pöllabauer (2006a)
to analyse the positioning of the interpreters in her corpus. Her data suggest that there is a
147
Sonja Pöllabauer
considerable degree of cooperation between officers and interpreters, and shifting degrees
of loyalty towards the asylum applicants, for whom the entire system is not very transparent.
Pöllabauer’s data suggest that interpreters try to coordinate talk to produce translations that
comply with official language ideologies, and in such a role position themselves as allies and
helpers of the administrative officials. They seek to solve obstacles to communication in a
cooperative way, while at the same time, though not always, they try to uphold rapport with
applicants. In such a function, they also provide meta-
comments and, in a role as ‘auxiliary
police officers’ (2004: 157), may even conduct their own subhearings in ‘internal rounds of
talk’ (2004: 160). Pöllabauer’s (2004) examples also support the assumption that the linguistic
diversity and the specific languages repertoires that are used in this context (an issue also
raised by Maryns and Blommaert 2001), including also the use of a lingua franca, a topic
explored by Maryns (2006) and Gómez Díez (2010), cannot always be rendered adequately
by interpreters who sometimes have limited linguistic skills (Gómez Díez 2010; Lee 2014) or
little awareness of the (super)diverse backgrounds of applicants ( Jacquemet 2011). Similarly,
Maryns (2013) suggests that there is a serious discrepancy between what interpreters should
and can do. Her specific case study of an appeal case involving a Krio-speaking applicant and
an English (not Dutch) interpreter, with the asylum official himself translating the English
utterances and re-entextualising them in the Dutch record, shows that interpreters may also
tend to make utterances more ‘palatable’ (2013: 681) by ‘flagging the inexpressible’ (2013:
682). Her study is also one of the few examples among those studies under review which
discusses interpreters’ influence on the disclosure and ‘re-performance’ (2013: 661) of gender
issues and gender-based evidence.
The negotiation of the participation status of unaccompanied children, as a specifically
vulnerable group, is addressed by Keselman et al. (2008; Keselman, Cederborg and Linell
2010a; Keselman et al. 2010b) who show that interpreters’ involvement may challenge mi-
nors’ participation status and their rights to be heard and that interpreters may even ‘exclude,
distort, discredit and guide the voices of children’ (2010a: 83). Their data also suggest that
interpreters, often unknowingly, change the question format (2010b) and change open ques-
tions that are aimed at enabling applicants to disclose information and are an important
element of the free narrative phase of interviews, into more focused question types (2008).
In her pragmatic microanalysis of modal particles, Tillman (2009) arrives at a similar conclu-
sion: while modal particles may be used to reduce distance and establish rapport, a change of
such particles through interpreters may turn questioning strategies into less open and more
forceful formats, which ultimately may have an influence on power relations and rapport.
Power within the asymmetrical system of asylum hearings is also the focus of Mason’s
(2012) study of gaze patterns. He shows that gaze is not only used to regulate participation
frames but is also ‘closely bound up with role and status’ (2012: 177). Interpreters also use
gaze to coordinate turn-management, and he also concludes that ‘different gaze behaviour’
(2012: 197) may be considered inconsistent with the dominant ‘norm in this community of
practice’ (2012: 197). Issues of power are also linked to facework. The idea that interpreters
are aware of their own face is discussed by Pöllabauer (2004, 2005) who analyses interpreters’
strategies for saving their own but also the other interactants’ face and shows that interpreters
may sometimes challenge applicants’ face (see also Dahlvik 2018 on examples of disrespectful
and biased behaviour on the part of interpreters and officials). While interpreters generally
opt for direct forms of address, they sometimes indicate the authorship of a statement if the
officers’ or applicants’ face is in danger by, for instance, transforming the deictic structure of
utterances (‘He said that…’), or they omit the face-threatening comment (Pöllabauer 2004:
163). If their own face is endangered by an applicant’s statement, they may also opt for a
148
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
comment, which, however, need not necessarily be made transparent to the applicant (ibid:
167). In a similar vein, Gómez Díez also studies applicants’ reactions to face-threatening acts
and argues ‘[…] that the interpreter makes some alterations that weaken the defensive force
of the applicant’s arguments’ (2010: 351).
Interpreter education is another of the (sub)strands that can be inferred from a closer
reading. The studies under review show that non-t rained ( L ee 2014) and ad-hoc interpret-
ers (Gómez Díez 2010) are still assigned to work as interpreters. And while training may not
be top on the asylum authorities’ list of priorities, data in the studies under review suggest
that specific training is much needed (Keselman et al. 2008; Lee 2014). Even if trained
interpreters are used (see Pöllabauer 2005; Kolb and Pöchhacker 2008), and these are often
simply not available for specific language combinations (see Maryns 2013; van der Kleij
2015; Dahlvik 2018), the outcome might not necessarily be much better, which would be
an extra argument for more specific training. While interpreters’ qualifications and (lack
of ) training are often mentioned, sometimes only in passing as often little is known on the
interpreters’ backgrounds, only few authors focus specifically on training interpreters for
an asylum context (see Bergunde and Pöllabauer 2019; also, see Ticca, Traverso and Jouin,
this volume).
A specific strand of focus, first taken up by Scheffer (2001) and also discussed by Pölla-
bauer (2004, 2005), is that interpreters serve as co-producers of the official written record
and as such try to ‘interpret for the record’ (Pöchhacker and Kolb 2009: 26) by reformulating
content and adapting it to the conventions of interview records. They play a considerable
role in the trajectory and reformulation of narratives into a written record that complies
with the official expectations of this specific text genre (Blommaert 2001). This topic is
dealt with by Kolb and Pöchhacker (2008) and Pöchhacker and Kolb (2009) who show that
‘interpreters tend to adjust to a striking degree to the needs of the record’ and in doing so
assume considerable responsibility for ‘the legally relevant manifestations of the interview’
(2009: 119). Like Pöllabauer (2005: 281–83), they present instances of talk where interpreters
seem to feel responsible for cooperatively and jointly solving problems such as a lack of co-
herence (Pöchhacker and Kolb 2009: 1 29–31).
One last strand of topics, which has not been discussed in much detail above, is found
in Licoppe and Veyrier (2020)3. In a recent large-
scale study on remote interpreting in a
‘distributed’ (Licoppe and Verdiere 2013: 247) spatial arrangement, they examine the man-
agement of extended answers, which are a typical element of the free narrative phase of
in-depth hearings (also see Keselman et al. 2008 on question types that are prone to elicit
more productive answers in the free narrative phase). They discuss chunking and turn-
t aking as well as the use of implicit (body signals, gaze) and explicit coordination techniques.
Their data, which include examples of two forms of site organisation (applicant and inter-
preter in one site, or interpreter and judge in one site) clearly show that problems in turn-
m anagement, in other words, who speaks when and who allocates and controls turns, may
be detrimental to communication.
149
Sonja Pöllabauer
focus on his notion of doxa, the authors analyse Red Cross and UNHCR interpreters’ po-
sitionality in such a field, drawing up a ‘paradigmatic interpreter profile’ (ibid.), based on
training notes and interpreters’ written personal résumés. They suggest that interpreters
working for the Red Cross and UNHCR view themselves first and foremost as humanitar-
ian actors.
In a local context, applicants also need to communicate in medical, police or court set-
tings. Interpreter-
mediated communication in such fields has drawn ample attention in re-
search (see chapters by Angermeyer, Licoppe and Pöllabauer, this volume). Less information,
however, is available on how applicants communicate in refugee reception facilities or at
non-governmental and municipal institutions which provide advice on a sylum-related mat-
ters. One case in point is Merlini’s (2009) study of interpreting at a ‘foreigners’ advice bu-
reau’ in Italy. Based on transcripts of such encounters, Merlini shows that interpreters act
as ‘cultural mediators’ and ‘co-providers’ of service, trying to help turn ‘narrative chaos in
linear form’ (2009: 85).
Killman (2019), in a non-empirically based contribution, also addresses interpreters’
agency and positioning and the specific dynamics and rapport-building in meetings between
applicants, their US attorneys and interpreters. This specific context, which is also largely
under-researched, entails a power structure that differs considerably from that of official
hearings: applicants are given more voice, and interpreters may have to provide services
different from those in strictly legal encounters.
Another under-researched topic is the communication needs of specifically vulnera-
ble groups of applicants such as those with physical or cognitive disabilities or the elderly.
Sivunen (2019), for instance, addresses the communication needs of Deaf refugees. In an
ethnographic study of communication in a Finnish reception centre, she reveals the range
of social and linguistic challenges which hamper Deaf applicants’ access to information and
social participation. Sign language interpreting, including the use of Deaf interpreters, may
also be required in asylum hearings, though no authentic data on such encounters have been
explored so far to the author’s knowledge.
One last strand of publications to be mentioned under this section are studies which focus
on encounters where no interpreters are involved, even in cases where one could have ex-
pected one. Maryns and Blommaert (2002) and Maryns (2005), for instance, show that the
imposition of a monolingual code in asylum interviews, for example the use of English as a
lingua franca, may serve as a filter and have detrimental effects on the ‘entextualisation of
the case’ (Maryns 2005: 299) and ultimately the credibility of applicants’ claims (Maryns and
Blommaert 2002: 18–19). This is part of a monolingual language ideology, based on the idea
that one dominant (national) language should be used in institutional (and other encoun-
ters), and where the ideal model of society should be ‘as uniform and homogenous as possible’
(Blommaert and Verschueren 1998: 117; italics in original).
Further reading
Barsky, Robert F. (1994) Constructing a Productive Other. Discourse Theory and the Convention Refugee
Hearing. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
This monograph is one of the pioneering publications on interpreting in an asylum context and offers a rich
and theoretically inspiring insight into the subject.
Inghilleri, Moira (2005) “Mediating zones of uncertainty. Interpreter agency, the interpreting habitus
and political asylum adjudication”, The Translator 11 (1): 69–85.
This article uses a Bourdieusian approach to discuss the agency and positionality of interpreters in an asylum
context.
150
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
Jacquemet, Marco (2011) “Crosstalk 2.0: Asylum and communicative breakdowns”, Text & Talk 31
(4): 475–97.
Based on Gumperz’ construct of contextualisation cues, this article outlines the communicative and social
complexity of asylum interviews as well as challenges and pitfalls of interpreter-
mediated communication in
this context.
Maryns, Katrijn (2006) The Asylum Speaker. Manchester, St. Jerome.
This monograph draws on a wealth of ethnographic data to examine the discursive processes in asylum
proceedings and how the work of interpreters may impact the outcome of these processes and the assessment of
asylum claims.
Related chapters
Chapter 2, The ambiguity of interpreting: Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters by Kristina
Gustafsson
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in public service interpreting by Claudio Baraldi
Chapter 5, Corpus-based studies of public service interpreting by Bernd Meyer
Chapter 8, Public service interpreting in court – face-to-face interaction by Philipp S. Angermeyer
Chapter 11, Vulnerable encounters? Investigating vulnerability in i nterpreter-mediated services for victim-survivors
of domestic violence and abuse by Rebecca Tipton
Notes
1 The list is ordered, first, alphabetically by countries, and second, chronologically by authors. It is
not exhaustive and includes only one study with authors with more than one relevant publication;
for more references, see the references section in this chapter.
2 New Zealand practices and interpreters’ perspectives are tackled in a survey by Fenton (2001).
3 Remote interpreting in an asylum context has been discussed in other studies (for instance Ellis
2004; Federman 2006; Mollo 2006), but not based on authentic discourse data. For an example
of phone interpreting, which shows how communication problems may be amplified by technical
problems, see Gibb (2019).
References
Anker, Deborah (1991) “Determining asylum claims in the united states: executive summary of an
empirical study of the adjudication of asylum claims before the immigration court”, in Refugee
Policy: Canada and the United States, Howard Adelman (ed). Toronto, York Lanes Press, 268–81.
Barsky, Robert F. (1993) “The interpreter and the Canadian convention refugee hearing: crossing the
potentially l ife-threatening boundaries between ‘Coccode-e -Eh’, ‘Cluck-Cluck’, and ‘Cot-Cot
Cot’”, Traduction, Terminologie, Rédaction 6 (2): 131–56.
——— (1996) “The interpreter as intercultural agent in convention refugee hearings”, The Translator
2 (1): 45–63.
——— (2000) Arguing and Justifying: Assessing the Convention Refugees’ Choice of Moment, Motive and Host
Country. London/New York, Routledge.
Bauman, Richard, and Charles L. Briggs (1990) “Poetics and performance as critical perspectives on
language and social life”, Annual Review of Anthropology 19: 59–88.
Bergunde, Annika, and Sonja Pöllabauer (2019) “Curricular design and implementation of a training
course for interpreters in an asylum context”, Translation & Interpreting 11 (1): 1–21.
Bernstein, Basil (1990) The Structuring of Pedagogic Discourse. Class, Codes and Control IV. London,
Routledge.
Blommaert, Jan (2001) “Investigating narrative inequality: African asylum seekers’ stories in Belgium”,
Discourse & Society 12 (4): 413–49.
Blommaert, Jan, and Jef Verschueren (1998) Debating Diversity. Analysing the Discourse of Tolerance. London,
Routledge.
Bourdieu, Pierre (1977) Outline of a Theory of Practice. London, Sage.
Brown, Penelope, and Stephen C. Levinson (1987) Politeness. Some Universals in Language Use. Cambridge,
Cambridge University Press.
151
Sonja Pöllabauer
Chesterman, Andrew (1993) “From ‘Is’ to ‘Ought’: Laws, norms and strategies in translation studies”,
Target 5 (1): 1–20.
Dahlvik, Julia (2018) Inside Asylum Bureaucracy: Organizing Refugee Status Determination in Austria. Berlin,
Springer Open.
Delgado Luchner, Carmen, and Leïla Kherbiche (2018) “Without fear or favour? The positionality of
ICRC and UNHCR interpreters in the humanitarian field”, Target 30 (3): 4 15–38.
Doornbos, Nienke (2005) “On being heard in asylum cases: Evidentiary assessment through asylum
interpreters” in Proof: Evidentiary Assessment and Credibility in Asylum Procedures, Gregor Noll (ed).
L eiden-Boston, Martinus Nijhoff: 103–22.
Ellis, S. Ronald (2004) Report to the immigration and refugee board audit and evaluation committee,
immigration and refugee board response to the report on videoconferencing in refugee hearings.
URL: http://w ww.irbcisr.gc.ca/Eng/t ransp/ReviewEval/Pages/Video.aspx (accessed 4 Novem-
ber 2021).
Fairclough, Norman, and Ruth Wodak (1997) “Critical discourse analysis”, in Discourse as Social Inter-
action. Discourse Studies: A Multidisciplinary Introduction, Teun A. van Dijk (ed). London, Sage: 2 58–84.
Federman, Mark (2006) “On the media effects of immigration and refugee board hearings via video-
conference”, Journal of Refugee Studies 19 (4): 433–52.
Fenton, Sabine (2001) Expressing a well-founded fear: Interpreting in convention refugee hearings.
URL: http://w ww.refugee.org.nz/Reference/Sabine.html (accessed 4 November 2021).
Gibb, Robert (2019) “Communicative practices and contexts of interaction in the refugee status deter-
mination process in France” in Asylum Determination in Europe, Nick Gill, and Anthony Good (eds).
Cham, Switzerland, Palgrave McMillan: 155–74.
Gibb, Robert, and Anthony Good (2014) “Interpretation, translation and intercultural communica-
tion in refugee status determination procedures in the UK and France”, Language and Intercultural
Communication 14 (3): 385–99.
Giddens, Anthony (1984) The Constitution of Society. Cambridge, Polity Press.
Gill, Nick, Rebecca Rotter, Andrew Burridge, Jennifer Allsopp, and Melanie Griffiths ( 2016)
“Linguistic incomprehension in British asylum appeal hearings”, Anthropology Today 32 (2): 18–21.
Goffman, Erving (1967) Interaction Rituals: Essays in Face-to-Face Behaviour. Chicago, IL, Aldine Pub-
lishing Company.
——— (1981) Forms of Talk. Pennsylvania, University of Pennsylvania Press.
Gómez Díez, Isabel (2010) “The role of the interpreter in constructing asylum seeker’s credibility: A
hearing at the Spanish asylum and refugee office”, Sociolinguistic Studies 4 (2): 333–70.
Gumperz, John, Thomas Jupp, and Celia Roberts (1979) Crosstalk: A Study of Crosscultural Communi-
cation. Background Material and Notes to Accompany the B.B.C. Film. London, National Center for
Industrial Language Training in Association with the BBC.
Hale, Sandra, and Jemina Napier (2013) Research Methods in Interpreting. London, Bloomsbury.
Inghilleri, Moira (2003) “Habitus, field and discourse. Interpreting as a socially situated activity”,
Target 15 (2): 243–68.
——— (2012) Interpreting Justice. Ethics, Politics, and Language. London/New York, Routledge.
Jacquemet, Marco (2000) Beyond the Speech Community. Paper, 7th International Pragmatics Con-
ference. Budapest, July 2000.
——— (2009) “Transcribing refugees: The entextualization of asylum seekers’ hearings in a transidi-
omatic environment”, Text & Talk 29 (5): 525–46.
——— (2010) “The registration interview. Restricting refugees’ narrative performance” in Critical
Readings in Translation Studies, Mona Baker (ed). London/New York, Routledge: 133–51.
——— (2014) “Asylum and superdiversity: The search for denotational accuracy during asylum hear-
ings”, Language & Communication 44: 72–81.
Jefferson, Gail (1972) “Side sequences”, in Studies in Social Interaction, David Sudnow (ed). New York,
The Free Press: 2 95–31.
Kälin, Walter (1986) “Troubled communication: C ross-cultural misunderstandings in the a sylum-
hearing”, International Migration Review 20 (2): 2 30–41.
Keselman, Olga, A nn-Christin Cederborg, Michael E. Lamb, and Örjan Dahlström (2008) “Mediated
communication with minors in a sylum-seeking hearings”, Journal of Refugee Studies 21 (1), 1 03–16.
Keselman, Olga, Ann-Christin Cederborg, and Per Linell (2010a) “‘That is not necessary for you to
know!’. Negotiation of participation status of unaccompanied children in interpreter-mediated
asylum hearings”, Interpreting 12 (1): 83–104.
152
Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews
Keselman, Olga, A nn-Christin Cederborg, Michael E. Lamb, and Örjan Dahlström (2010b) “A sylum-
seeking minors in interpreter-mediated interviews: What do they say and what happens to their
responses?”, Child & Family Social Work 15: 325–34.
Killman, Jeffrey (2019) “Interpreting for asylum seekers and their attorneys: The challenge of agency”,
Perspectives 28 (1): 73–89.
Kleij, Susanne van der (2015) Interaction in Dutch Asylum Interviews. A Corpus Study of Interpreter-Mediated
Institutional Discourse. Utrecht, LOT.
Kolb, Waltraud, and Franz Pöchhacker (2008) “Interpreting in asylum appeal hearings: Roles and
norms revisited” in Interpreting in Legal Settings, Debra Russell, and Sandra Hale (eds). Washington,
DC, Gallaudet University Press: 2 6–50.
Lee, Jieun (2014) “A pressing need for the reform of interpreting service in asylum settings: A case
study of asylum appeal hearings in South Korea”, Journal of Refugee Studies 27 (1): 62–81.
L euven-Zwart, Kitty M. van (1989) “Translation and original, similarities and dissimilarities I”, Target
1 (2): 151–81.
——— ( 1990) “Translation and original, similarities and dissimilarities II”, Target 2 (1): 69–95.
Licoppe, Christian, and Clair-Antoine Veyrier (2017) “How to show the interpreter on screen? The
normative organization of visual ecologies in multilingual courtrooms with video links”, Journal
of Pragmatics 107: 1 47–64.
——— (2020) “The interpreter as a sequential coordinator in courtroom interaction. ‘Chunking’ and
the management of turn shifts in extended answers in consecutively interpreted asylum hearings
with remote participants”, Interpreting 22 (1): 5 6–86.
Licoppe, Christian, and Maud Verdier (2013) “Interpreting, video communication and the sequential
reshaping of institutional talk in the bilingual and distributed courtroom”, The International Journal
of Speech, Language and the Law 20 (2): 247–75.
Maryns, Katrijn (2004) “Identifying the asylum speaker: Reflections on the pitfalls of language analy-
sis in the determination of national origin”, Speech, Language and the Law 11 (2): 241–60.
——— (2005) “Monolingual language ideologies and code choice in the belgian asylum procedure”,
Language and Communication 25 (3): 299–14.
——— (2013) “Disclosure and (re)performance of gender-based evidence in an i nterpreter-mediated
asylum interview”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 17 (5): 661–86.
Maryns, Katrijn, and Jan Blommaert (2001) “Stylistic and thematic shifting as a narrative resource:
assessing asylum seekers’ repertoires”, Multilingua 20 (1): 61–84.
——— (2002) “Pretextuality and pretextuality gaps: On de/refining linguistic inequality”, Pragmatics
12 (1): 11–30.
Mason, Ian (2012) “Gaze, positioning and identity in i nterpreter-mediated dialogues”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi, and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 177–99.
Mason, Ian, and Miranda Stewart (2001) “Interactional pragmatics, face and the dialogue inter-
preter”, in Triadic Exchanges. Studies in Dialogue Interpreting, Ian Mason (ed). Manchester, St.
Jerome: 51–70.
Merlini, Raffaela (2009) “Seeking asylum and seeking identity in a mediated encounter. The projec-
tion of selves through discursive practices”, Interpreting 11 (1): 5 7–92.
Mollo, Eugenio (2006) “The expansion of video conferencing technology in immigration proceed-
ings and its impact on venue provisions, interpretation rights, and the Mexican immigrant com-
munity”, Gender Race & Justice 9 (3): 6 89–12.
Monnier, Michel Acatl (1995) “The hidden part of the asylum seekers’ interviews in Geneva, Switzer-
land: Some observations about the socio-
political construction of interviews between gatekeepers
and the powerless”, Journal of Refugee Studies 8 (3): 305–25.
Nikolaidou, Zoe, Hanna Sofia Rehnberg, and Cecilia Wadensjö (2019) “Negotiating access with
public authorities in research on asylum”, Working Papers in Urban Language and Literacies 262: 1–17.
Pöchhacker, Franz, and Waltraud Kolb (2009) “Interpreting for the record. A case study of asylum
review hearings”, in The Critical Link 5. Quality in Interpreting – A Shared Responsibility, Sandra Hale,
Ludmila Stern, and Uldis Ozolins (eds). Amsterdam & Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 119–34.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2004) “Interpreting in asylum hearings: Issues of role, responsibility and power”,
Interpreting 6 (2): 143–80.
——— (2005) “I Don’t Understand Your English, Miss”. Dolmetschen bei Asylanhörungen. Tübingen,
Gunter Narr.
153
Sonja Pöllabauer
——— (2006a) “‘Translation culture’ in interpreted asylum hearings”, in Sociocultural Aspects of Trans-
lating and Interpreting, Anthony Pym, Miriam Shlesinger, and Zuzana Jettmarová (eds). Amsterdam/
Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 151–62.
——— ( 2006b) “’During the interview, the interpreter will provide a faithful translation.’ The poten-
tials and pitfalls of researching interpreting in immigration, asylum, and police settings: Method-
ology and research paradigms”, Linguistica Antverpiensia NS5: 229–4 4.
Prunč, Erich (1997) “Translationskultur (Versuch einer konstruktiven Kritik des translatorischen
Handelns)”, TEXTconTEXT 11=NF 1 (2): 99–127.
Rienzner, Martina (2011) Interkulturelle Kommunikation im Asylverfahren. Frankfurt am Main, Peter
Lang.
Rousseau, Cécile, François Crépau, Patricia Foxen, and Francé Houle (2002) “The complexity of de-
termining refugeehood: A multidisciplinary analysis of the decision- making process of the Cana-
dian immigration and refugee board”, Journal of Refugee Studies 15 (1): 43–70.
Scheffer, Thomas (2001) Asylgewährung. Eine ethnographische Verfahrensanalyse. Stuttgart, Lucius & Lucius.
Sivunen, Nina (2019) “An ethnographic study of deaf refugees seeking asylum in Finland”, Societies 9
(1). URL: https://doi.org/10.3390/soc9010002 (accessed 4 November 2021).
Strauss, Anselm L., and Juliet Corbin (1991) Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory Procedures
and Techniques. Newbury Park, Sage.
Tillmann, Maria (2009) “The pragmatic significance of modal particles in an interpreted German
asylum interview” in Interpreting and Translating in Public Service Settings. Policy, Practice, Pedagogy,
Raquel de Pedro Ricoy, Christine Wilson, and Isabelle Perez (eds). Manchester, St. Jerome: 156–70.
Tipton, Rebecca (2008) “Reflexivity and the social construction of identity in i nterpreter-mediated
asylum interviews”, The Translator 14 (1): 1–19.
Toury, Gideon (1980) In Search of a Theory of Translation. Tel Aviv, The Porter Institute for Poetics and
Semiotics.
Tryuk, Malgorzata (2017) “Conflict. Tension. Aggression: Ethical issues in interpreted asylum hear-
ings at the office for foreigners in Warsaw”, in Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development in Public Service
Interpreting and Translation, Carmen Valero-Garcés, and Rebecca Tipton (eds). Bristol, Multilingual
Matters: 179–94.
Tužinská, Helena (2019) “Doing things with questions. Interpreting in asylum settings”, Lud 103: 81–99.
Vertovec, Steven (2007) “Super-d iversity and its implications”, Ethnic and Racial Studies 30 (6): 1024–54.
Vuori, Jaana, and Sara Hokkanen (2020) “Empirical design in studies of public service interpreting and
translation”, FITISPOS International Journal 7 (1): 110–37.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London/New York, Longman.
154
10
CONSECUTIVE INTERPRETING
AND MULTIMODAL SEQUENCES
Christian Licoppe
EM/CA; see Schegloff 2007; Sidnell and Stivers 2014), the last stream of research looks at
the organization of multilingual talk in legal settings as a sequential phenomenon, with an
interest in phenomena such as the management of turn-taking (Wadensjö 1998) and pauses
(Nakane 2011, 2014), various forms of repair, repetition and recycling (Wadensjö 1997),
code-switching (A ngermeyer 2015) and so on. Recent research focusing on interaction has
started to highlight the importance of gaze (Mason 2012; Davitti 2015) and more generally
the role of multimodal resources in the sequential coordination of interpreted talk (see for
instance Pasquandrea 2011; Davitti and Pasquandrea 2017; Vranjes et al. 2019), a point I will
elaborate later.
One important aspect of this last direction of research is the way in which it allows a radical
respecification of the notion of ‘modes of interpreting’, and in particular ‘consecutive inter-
preting’, which is the most common mode of interpreting in legal settings. In the more tra-
ditional (i.e. not EM/CA-inspired) interpreting literature, consecutive interpreting is usually
defined as “the process of interpreting after the speaker or signer has completed one or more
ideas in the source language and then pauses while the interpreter transmits that information”
(Russell and Takeda 2015). By considering turn-taking as an emergent phenomenon and a
locally managed ‘members’ issue’, CA invites us to consider consecutive interpreting not just
as an abstract definition or as a prescription for the interpreter to speak and provide a rendition
‘after’ the previous speaker, but as a practical accomplishment in which such successiveness
(‘after’), has to be collaboratively achieved. For instance, transition-relevant places (TRP) in
ongoing, consecutively interpreted courtroom talk must be constantly scrutinized by partici-
pants for their relevance to the start of an interpreted rendition. The actual start of a rendition
is locally managed and collectively negotiated. The idea that the interpreter starts ‘after’ a
previous speaker has completed a turn in the target language and offered a pause becomes a
problem for participants and analysts alike. When one respecifies consecutive interpreting as a
conversational process and accomplishment, the interpreter’s competence and agency regard-
ing sequential concerns emerge as a crucial aspect of interpreting as an activity in legal settings
(and more generally in any setting), as I will try to show in the next section.
156
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
The micropause after “cette année-là” in Lines 1 –2 marks such a moment. What comes before
could be understood as a grammatically complete wh-question. In the ‘mechanistic’, third-
party view of consecutive interpreting, the interpreter should thus start there and interpret the
previous ‘question’. From a CA perspective, however, this constitutes a TRP. The institutional
character of a setting usually manifests itself through constraints on turn type and t urn-taking
(Drew and Heritage 1992). At the kind of sequential juncture we have here, only two op-
tions are oriented to t urn-taking by the interpreter (to provide a rendition), and self-selection
by current speaker (the judge going on). In Extract 1, option 2 is made operative: the judge
self-selects to transform the initial open w
h-question into a Y/N question with a candidate
answer.
Extract 2 shows how both judges and interpreters can simultaneously orient to turn-
t aking as relevant in such a sequential environment.
Extract 2.
1. J. la première question à: à madame/ combien de personnes
the first question for madam how many persons
2. de sa famille: euh sont en Guyane (.)
from her family are in (French) Guiana
3. et à [quel titre-
and for which reason
4. I. [.h cuántas personas de su familia están en Guyana/
How many persons from your family are in Guiana
Here, the pause at the end of Line 2 marks a grammatically completed question and a potential
TRP. Both parties orient towards its relevance to t urn-taking, as shown by the occurrence of
an overlap. The judge self-selects to elaborate the question (a nd eventually relinquishes the
floor), while the interpreter starts her rendition. The fact that it is eventually that question
which is asked and interpreted appears clearly as a collaborative accomplishment in which
the judge and the interpreter have both been agentive.
Such TRPs may be treated explicitly as equivocal, as in Extract 3 below.
Extract 3.
1. J2. merci madame la présidente (.) madame est-ce que: votre compagnon
thank you madam president madam had your partner
2. avait été violent également euh à l’égard des enfants/
also been violent towards the children?
157
Christian Licoppe
Here, the interpreter starts her rendition at the first opportunity, after the grammatical com-
pletion of the question. However, she breaks her rendition to inquire whether the question
was actually finished (w ith an initial-positioned apology), thus initiating a potential repair
sequence. She thus makes explicit the relevance of such sequential configurations for both
judges and interpreters, and their equivocality.
All this shows how (a) ‘questions’ are emergent phenomena in the course of the court-
room interaction; (b) ‘questions’ are collaborative accomplishments whose outcome depends
on the way participants orient to t urn-taking concerns at TRPs; (c) through such a ‘purely’
sequential involvement, for which there is no time out, the interpreter bears directly on what
is said and interpreted in the courtroom. Beyond the example of the judge’s questions, all this
remains true at any point of the multilingual courtroom interaction. The EM/CA perspec-
tive allows us to unpack the deceptive simplicity of the traditional perspective on consecu-
tive interpreting which requires that the interpreter start her rendition ‘a fter’ a turn in the
source language. It allows us to show the agency of the interpreter in courtroom talk as a co-
ordinated activity, and to move away from a s emantics-centred view of ‘interpreting-a s-text’
to the action-centred perspective of ‘interpreting-a s-activity’ (Wadensjö 1998). Consecutive
interpreting has to be understood as a joint practical accomplishment.
158
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
this raises a new issue for the courtroom interpreter-as-a-member, which is to determine at
which TRP it would be proper for him/her to start his/her rendition.
The expansive character of the source talk in legal settings thus raises specific issues for
interpreters (Wadensjö 2010; Licoppe 2020). Their dilemma concerns when exactly to begin
their rendition when interpreting consecutively: they should start their rendition after some
bit of talk has been elaborated ‘enough’ for local practical purposes, but if they let the source
speaker go on for too long, the kind of work they will have to do may change from con-
secutive interpreting to ‘long consecutive interpreting’, and all the way down to ‘summary
interpreting’, with its implied loss of accuracy in the rendition. One way out of this is for the
source speaker and the interpreter to collaborate in avoiding an utterance extending beyond
too many minutes of talk and to ‘chunk’ long expansions into smaller utterances which
can be interpreted successively in a more manageable way. The phenomenon of ‘chunking’
has also been observed in therapeutic talk, but it is particularly relevant to interpreted legal
settings. It happens in police interventions for instance, when the police read their rights
to people about to be arrested (Russell 2000; Nakane 2007), but also in court proceedings
(Licoppe and Verdier 2013) and in asylum proceedings (Licoppe and Veyrier 2020).
Chunking is a sequential and collaborative phenomenon, characteristic of consecutive
interpreting, where the agency of the interpreter plays an important part. It involves two
sequential issues. One is its initiation: collaboration is necessary to allow the interpreter to
launch his/her rendition in such a way that it can be understood that the source utterance
has not yet come to an end. Multimodality is a crucial resource there. Gaze, but also body
reorientations, interruptive gestures, controlling touch, inbreaths and vocalizations, all the
way up to explicit verbal requests and apologies, are commonly used resources for managing
t urn-taking and potentially initiating chunking (Licoppe and Veyrier 2020; Vranjes and Bot
2021). In legal settings, there is an expectation on the part of the judges that the interpreter
should manage chunking on his/her own, which may be made explicit by judges in instances
of trouble (Licoppe and Veyrier 2020). The second significant issue regarding sequential
juncture concerns what may happen at potential ‘endings’ of the interpreter’s rendition after
the initiation of chunking.
159
Christian Licoppe
asylum seeker, while verbally instructing her to go on with her story. Gaze is a powerful
multimodal resource to manage recipiency in conversation in general and in a consecutively
interpreted talk in particular (Mason 2012; Vranjes et al. 2019). Moreover, and in this ex-
ample, the verbal instruction made clear what the gaze shifts meant. The story was chunked
several times in this way with the interpreter looking back to the asylum seeker after each
rendition, thus providing her with a strong opportunity to go on with her story (w ith the
judge displaying his alignment with such management of recipiency by not talking). Even-
tually, the asylum seeker, who spoke Haitian Creole, came to the point in her story in which
she told the circumstances of her husband’s death (Extract 4 below).
Extract 4.: AS. is the asylum seeker. I. is the interpreter. J. is the judge.
01. AS. lè mwen rive bò stad sylvio katò
when I arrived near the stadium Sylvio Cator
02. mwen jwenn yo bat li yo tiye l atè a
I found him beaten up he had been killed on the ground
03. (.)
04. AS. [li benyen nan san atè a
he was lying in his blood on the ground
05. I. [ehh
06. AS. (
0.5) ((
cries and sneezes))
07. I. hmm (0.2)
¤et donc heu:: il nous avait dit qu'il revenait and
so er he had told us he would be back
¤Image 2.3
160
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
20. (1.4)
21. J. d'accord donc heu: ( 0.5) ensuite heu les les personnes
22. okay so er then er the the persons
23. qui ont menacé ( 0.4) madame après le décès de son:: mari
24. who threatened Madam after the death of her husband
25. (
0.4) ce sont les mêmes que celles qui:::: ont fait des
26. are the same persons who made
27. menaces avant/
28. threats before
The asylum seeker’s story comes to a dramatic culmination in which she tells of the death of
her husband (Line 2). The interpreter orients to the TRP projected by this announcement
as an opportunity to interpret and starts to speak then; however, the asylum seeker goes on
with a reformulation of her sad discovery. There is therefore an overlap, and the interpreter
relinquishes the turn (Lines 4 –5). At the next TRP, the asylum seeker breaks down, cries and
sneezes (Image 2.3). The interpreter treats the sequential juncture and the emotional display as
a cue for her to start interpreting. Of particular interest here, is the way the interpreter actively
designs the end of her rendition. After the end of the second clause in Line 16 (which provides
a possible TRP), she stops talking, thus conveying a sense that she has possibly finished her
interpreting. However, during the lengthy pause that ensues, and contrary to what she previ-
ously did at several successive and similar sequential junctures, instead of looking at the asylum
seeker, she keeps her gaze fixed on the screen and therefore on the presiding judge (Image 2.4).
Through such an embodied conduct, she appears to orient toward the judge as the potential
next speaker. In so doing, she also displays her understanding that this particular juncture is a
possible completion point for the s tory-so-far (indeed the death of the husband is recognizable
as a kind of dramatic culmination, and therefore as a possibly adequate ending to the story),
and that the story-so-far can now be treated as a response to the initial question. The judge
161
Christian Licoppe
seems to align with the sequential opportunity the interpreter’s embodied actions project for
him, for he starts a new question, on a different topic (Line 23–29). Through her behaviour
at such sequential positions, the interpreter is therefore active in the production of the story as
such, both in conveying a sense that it is not over yet and allowing it to flow or in conveying
that a story or a turn has reached a potential ending, thus providing a slot for a new question.
These are not ‘just’ sequential concerns, for this connects to issues of power in the asylum
court in two different ways. First, because there are inequalities in the resources participants
may deploy there. In particular, unlike the judge, the asylum seeker usually does not under-
stand the rendition and cannot anticipate its ending, which makes it very difficult for her to
claim the turn, should the interpreter elicit to look at the judge at the end of her rendition
(thus giving her the opportunity to ask a new question). Second, our analysis points towards
two distinct potential trajectories for consecutively interpreted narratives in courtroom in-
terrogations. Should the judge emerge as the next speaker (generally to ask a new question
within that institutional organization of talk), then the asylum seeker’s answer/story-in-
progress runs the risk of remaining truncated (for its progression has been recognizably
interrupted earlier). This is a scenario which is often preferred by judges, who usually like
to control the proceedings through successive questions rather than leave witnesses with
the liberty to talk at length. On the other hand, should the turn be made available to the
asylum seeker at this point, then he/she may get a chance to complete his/her answer/story.
So, in the case of expansive answers that are produced in successive interpretable chunks,
the management of t urn-taking after the interpreter’s rendition of one of these chunks may
either lean toward ‘control’ (by the judge) or ‘voice’ (when the asylum seeker is allowed to
continue his/her tale). Sequential concerns and power effects are tightly interwoven in the
management of consecutively interpreted courtroom interaction.
162
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
proceedings investigated for themselves, rather than engaging from the start in a comparison
with face-to-face settings.
I will use the extensive recordings my colleagues and I made of courtroom asylum
proceedings in France to illustrate this point by investigating the way a purely sequential
achievement (the chunking of long answers for the sake of consecutive interpreting) may
be affected by video link configurations. Video links open up new possibilities of spatial
distribution for participants, and particularly for interpreters. In our case, the interpreter
would appear either sitting beside the asylum seeker and away from the court (this was the
case in Section 2, and it is the institutionally preferred configuration), or, in the case of rare
languages where no interpreter is available in the remote site, in court and away from the
asylum seeker. In the latter case, as happens in the coordination of talk in telephone inter-
preting (Wadensjö 1999), one would expect that the interpreter would have less embodied
resources available to coordinate the flow of the asylum seeker’s talk. How is the chunking
of expansions and narrative actually done in v ideo-mediated settings?
In the following extract, where the asylum seeker speaks Mandinka, the judge has just
asked about the circumstances of the death of the (m ale) asylum seeker’s father. Such a ques-
tion projects a narrative as an answer, and indeed, the asylum seeker has started relating the
events that led up to this fateful incident. As he describes his father’s arrest, the interpreter
initiates a clarification sequence about the place where the father was taken (which breaks
the flow of the narrative) and starts to interpret immediately afterwards what appears then as
the ‘beginning’ of the story. It is important also to note that the video frames the interpreter
and the judge, with the interpreter on the far left of the field and appearing relatively small
on the image. Extract 5 begins as the interpreter is interpreting this particular sequence into
French.
04. *¤(
0.8)
05. *¤((
looks up at the screen))
163
Christian Licoppe
¤ Image 3.2
06. I. [apéré
après
and then ?
07. AS. [kàn-laaji komisariya polisi
Camp Laadji a police station
08. I. *donc c’est à la police [qu’ils l’ont ( mis)
so they brought him to the police
09. *¤((
looks at the judge))- -
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
10. J. [d’accord
okay
¤ Image 3.3
11. I. *¤ (
0.5) then/
*¤((
looks up at the screen))
¤ Image 3.4
164
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
12. AS.
ìyoo k
àn-
laaji (.)
kábiriŋ à bé jée ì kà’à bùutee jée kà
Yes in Camp Laadji. During his imprisonment there they beat him
repeatedly
At the end of his rendition, the interpreter is looking down at his notes. The judge is tak-
ing notes and not looking at him (Image 3.1). The interpreter then looks up at the screen.
There is a micropause, and the interpreter and the asylum speaker start to speak in overlap.
The interpreter provides an invitation for the asylum seeker to go on (Line 6, Image 3.2),
thus showing both that his earlier gaze was projecting the asylum seeker as the next speaker,
and that he treats the lack of immediate response as a potential problem. In the overlap, the
asylum seeker speaks (thus showing that he has understood the sequential meaning of the
interpreter’s gaze shift) but only to provide a correction to his previous utterances (the name
of the camp, Line 7). The interpreter provides a rendition of that correction (Line 8) and
looks at the judge (Line 9, Image 3.3). The judge treats the gaze as offering a slot for him to
talk, and he provides an agreement token (Line 10). By passing such an opportunity to ask a
new question he orients to the asylum seeker’s narrative-so-far as unfinished. The interpreter
then looks at the screen. There is again a silence, and the interpreter provides verbal instruc-
tion for the asylum seeker to go on (“then/”, Line 11). Such an upgrade again orients to the
possibility that his looking up at the screen may not have been understood as a floor-giving
move. Only then does the asylum speaker resume his narrative.
This short sequence allows me to make several analytic points. First, it shows that when
the interpreter is away from the asylum seeker, his range of resources for managing the lat-
ter’s talk, and in particular for giving the floor back to him, is reduced. He cannot rely on
embodied cues such as body reorientations or touch, nor on audible ones that might not be
heard, such as inbreaths. The interpreter must rely mostly on gaze shifts and explicit verbal
and non-verbal instructions. Thus, the interpreter’s range of resources for managing the
sequential flow of t alk-in-interaction appears reduced when compared to co-present config-
urations. Second, through his verbal upgrades, the interpreter seems to internalize the frailty
of the v ideo-mediated interactional frame and to display a particular sensitivity to the fact
that his gaze shift may not have been noticed or recognized as providing an opportunity for
the asylum seeker to talk next, and therefore to the fact that more work may have to be done
by him with respect to next speaker selection. In other words, it shows an orientation on the
part of the interpreter towards the possibility that the gaze may lose its force as a resource to
select the next speaker, in line with early studies of video communication which showed that
the gaze may lose its performative power in interaction in v ideo-mediated settings (Heath
and Luff 1992).
The video medium may have only indirect effects on the semantic work involved in the
interpreting. It is only through problems in quality of audio and video or delays, and the po-
tential fatigue associated with screen-based activities that renditions may be affected. How-
ever, regarding the sequential management of courtroom interaction, even when technical
conditions are good and fatigue has not settled in, the v ideo-mediated courtroom ecologies
may affect the sequential management of the proceedings. This has direct implications on
the m icro-politics of narrative production in asylum proceedings. By making it slightly more
difficult for the courtroom-located interpreter to give the floor back to the remote asylum
seeker, asylum proceedings in distributed courtrooms are more prone to support ‘control’
rather than to allow the unimpeded flow of ‘voice’, that is for the asylum seeker to tell his/her
story rather than answering a volley of questions by the judges. It also shows the importance
of strictly enforcing the prescription that requires interpreters to sit next to the person who
165
Christian Licoppe
does not speak the language of the court and, whenever that is not possible, for them to be
visible on screen when they talk (on this latter point, see Licoppe and Veyrier 2017).
Extract 6
DJ. is the (m ale) questioning (Deputy) judge, I. is the (female) interpreter, AS. is the (m ale)
asylum seeker.
#
01. DJ. vous voyez/ £(
0.5)#ce qu'y a écrit (.) sur la carte#
you see what is written on the card
£ad £points
#camC #-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
zoom
in-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-#
166
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
# Image 1
#Image 4.1
02. #(0.8) #
#camC #zoom out#
#
03. I. #uhh #
#camC #zoom in #
#
04. (0.4)µ (1.7) µ (0.3)
µgre µ sits straighterµ
05. I. $pol-[pulaire ]
polpular
06. AS. [popilè] (
0.6)# $là sè $ popilè (.)&
popular there it is popular
$req $points $points $hand gesture-
>
#Image 4.2
167
Christian Licoppe
13. (0.9)
14. AS. ah bon $#l’la
ah well there
$req $points with left hand
15. I. $voilà c'est ça
right it’s that
$req $points
16. $µ (1.4)$
$req $home position
µgre µ sits
17. AS. ¤ ( ) ¤
¤int ¤leans backward
18. (0.7)
19. DJ. c'est étonnant (
0.5) qu'ils se soient
It is surprising that they made
20. £#trompés en imprimant les cartes
a mistake when printing the cards
£ad £turns head towards Pr
#Image 4.3
21. (1.8)
22. AS. ba:h euh: bon m’pa (.) m’pa: janm gadé sa
kom-
I didn’t I didn’t ever look at that
like-
23. sa nan bon kondisyon,
it’s in good condition
After manipulating the card for a while, the deputy judge (DJ), who is leading the inter-
rogation in this sequence, eventually asks the asylum seeker if he can see what is written
on the card (Line 1), while pointing at the screen (I mage 4.1), thus displaying his under-
standing that it is currently visible enough for the purpose at hand. In view of the initial
discussion, this can be heard as a request for the asylum seeker to recognize the misspelling
(he initially denied noticing it) and to discuss it. The interpreter (who is experienced and
works in this court on a regular basis) does not provide a rendition of this turn. After a
significant silence, she provides an identification of the misspelling as something she can
see on screen (Line 5), thus enacting herself as a recipient of the showing. This is uttered
168
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
in overlap with a contradictory statement by the asylum seeker asserting that it is written
“popular” instead (Lines 6–7 ), while pointing at the screen (I mage 4.2). Then, the inter-
preter initiates a sequence that aims at the clarification of this discrepancy. She reaffirms
her point in third position (her assertion Line 12 that it is written “polpular”), thus pro-
viding for the emergence of a potential argument (A ntaki 1994). It is defused by the next
turn of the asylum seeker who agrees with her, as if it were news to him (the “ah bon” in
initial position, Line 14), so that he seems to orient to what she said rather than to what
he claimed to see. Eventually, the interpreter provides an argument closing confirmation
turn ( Line 15).
In her use of verbal (speaking Haitian creole, referring to what is visible on screen) and
non-verbal communication (looking alternately at the screen and at the asylum seeker), the
interpreter does two things. First, she constitutes herself as a joint recipient of the showing
by displaying in her embodied conduct that she is trying to see what is shown on screen
( Line 5). Such a stance is allowed by the grammatical design of the showing initiation by
the judge, and by the local ecology in which the interpreter and the asylum seeker are fac-
ing the screen side by side. Second, she engages in a collaboration with the asylum seeker
to produce a joint visual recognition of what has been framed as viewable, that is that the
card contains a misspelling. Since the asylum seeker had hitherto contested this, she can be
seen to remain within the provisions of her institutional mandate, which requires her to
provide accurate consecutive interpreting but also allows her some discretion in engaging in
clarification sequences when needed. This is achieved in her reasserting what she has seen
in Line 12, which initiates an argumentative sequence, and a particular kind of ‘schisming’
( Egbert 1997), in which interpreter and asylum seeker are engaged together in a separate
conversation.
The questioning deputy judge seems sensitive to that participation framework, for, turn-
ing towards the judge sitting next to him (Image 4.3), he addresses a comment to the latter,
designed as an expression of surprise regarding the misspelling (Lines 19–20). In the context
of asylum proceedings where judges are focused on and usually suspicious of the credibility
of asylum seekers, such a publicly available comment (it is hearable by all) on the current case
can be heard as highly detrimental to the case. Judges are not supposed to do that, and they
generally refrain from doing so in co-present settings. What makes it possible here? First, the
kind of schisming enacted by the sequence between the interpreter and the asylum seeker
regarding what they can jointly see opens up the possibility of further schisming through
the initiation of a conversational thread between the judges. Second, not only does the t wo-
sited, v ideo-mediated ecology reinforce that possibility, but, as is common for participants
in v ideo-mediated communication, it can lead the judges to assume that since the remote
others are ‘not there’, they won’t hear any asides. This is probably what has happened here
and explains why the judge made a publicly available comment on the case under way, some-
thing which he certainly would have avoided in a co-present hearing (whenever asides occur
there, it is usually in the form of whispers). In line with the idea that expectations regarding
consecutive interpreting are suspended here, the interpreter does not provide a rendition
of this assessment, even though it is highly relevant and detrimental to the case. She is still
oriented towards the asylum seeker who is initiating an account for his being unaware of the
misspelling (Lines 22–23).
In this multimodal showing-oriented sequence we see how v ideo-mediated courtroom
environments may be conducive to situations in which the interpreter may engage in a dif-
ferent participation framework (here, the interpreter is a recipient of the showing herself,
striving to ‘see-together’ with the asylum seeker) and in which the orientation to consecutive
169
Christian Licoppe
interpreting may be suspended in an emergent fashion. Not only may this have significant
consequences regarding the judicial activity underway (here, allowing for both the produc-
tion of a publicly audible negative assessment done as an aside between judges and the lack
of any rendition of it to the asylum seeker), but such a troubled multimodal sequence also
reveals how much our usual view of consecutive interpreting and its very possibility rely on a
monomodal view of t alk-in-interaction where utterances can be understood and interpreted
on the basis of how they can be heard. However, highly multimodal sequences put such
a presupposition to the test by making the achievement of consecutive interpreting more
difficult to accomplish, with v ideo-mediated configurations amplifying potential problems.
Conclusion
I have first used the perspective of EM and CA to provide a respecification of consecutive
interpreting in legal settings (and also in general), then as a framework to understand the way
in which video links may affect consecutively interpreted courtroom interaction. Rather
than a mode or a process which could be described from a third-party perspective (a s in ‘the
interpreter starts to interpret after the question or a previous turn’), consecutive interpret-
ing appears as an unfolding and joint accomplishment. Questions (a nd answers) emerge as
collaborative achievements. They are the outcomes of the way turn-taking is managed by
participants. Besides any consideration of content and semantic accuracy, interpreters are
therefore unavoidably agentive in the sequential organization of consecutively interpreted
interaction. I have discussed how such a ‘sequential’ agency is consequential in courtroom
interaction, first by showing how ‘questions’ are c o-produced, and second, by identifying a
sequential phenomenon which is characteristic of the kind of sequential work done to sup-
port consecutive interpreting, that is the ‘chunking’ of narratives and expansive turns. I have
also shown how the management of long turns by chunking is not simply a practical inter-
actional matter but involves issues of power and control in courtroom interaction in the way
it opens the latter to different trajectories for interpreted narratives. According to the way
t urn-taking is managed as the rendition of chunked narrative proceeds, the floor can either
be given back to the speaker to continue telling his/her story or to the judge to ask further
questions, even though the answer may have been marked as incomplete.
Regarding the analysis of the elusive effects of the introduction of video links and re-
mote participants in the courtroom, an understanding of these sequential issues is critical,
because they are affected by technological mediations and the transformations these bring
to courtroom ecologies. For instance, the presence of video links opens up two possibilities
for the interpreter where remote witnesses are involved: to sit next to the remote witness in
the remote site or to sit away from the witness and in court. In the latter case, the embodied
resources available to the interpreter to manage the flow of talk are more limited and con-
strained than in c o-present configurations with respect to a previous speaker who is only
perceptible on screen. For example, I have shown how an interpreter in court has to work
harder to initiate the chunking of a narrative produced by a remote asylum seeker because
touch could not be used and the latter was much less aware of the embodied behaviour of
the interpreter. This can also make it more difficult for the interpreter to give the asylum
seeker the floor back. Such constraints on interactional resources when interpreter and asy-
lum seeker are talking through a video link may allow greater control of courtroom inter-
action by judges, thus bearing on issues of power in consecutively interpreted courtroom
proceedings.
170
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
Finally, I have shown how video links may occasion the development of complex mul-
timodal sequences, such as showing a document to a remote witness (a nd interpreter). In
the case here demonstrated, this led to a suspension of consecutive interpreting, with the
development of a participation framework in which the interpreter was jointly engaged with
the asylum seeker in scrutinizing the document. I have also shown how this may be conse-
quential to the proceedings themselves through the possibility of schisming, how the pro-
duction of case-relevant comments is made possible and how they may be left uninterpreted.
Not only are multimodal sequences particularly complex achievements when done through
video links (something which definitely merits more research), but in breaking the organiza-
tion of consecutive interpreting, such object-centred sequences also demonstrate how much
the traditional view of consecutive interpreting relies on the assumption that it operates on
‘monomodal’ talk, intelligible simply on the basis of the way utterances can be heard.
Further reading
Braun, Sabine (2017) “W hat a m icro-analytical investigation of additions and expansions in remote
interpreting can tell us about interpreters’ participation in a shared virtual space”, Journal of Prag-
matics 107(2017): 165–77.
This article discusses the issues expansions and additions in answers may raise for interpreters, and which
constitute the background for the ‘chunking’ phenomenon.
Gallez, Emmanuelle, and Katrijn Maryns (2014) “Orality and authenticity in an interpreter-mediated
defendant’s examination”, Interpreting 16(1): 49–80.
This article provides an analysis of interpreted asylum court proceedings from an interactional perspective.
Licoppe, Christian (2017) “Showing objects in Skype v ideo-mediated conversations. From showing
gestures to showing sequences”, Journal of Pragmatics 110 (2017): 63–82.
This article introduces readers to the sequential organization of the showing of objects, with a focus on
showing sequences.
Napier, Jemina, Robert Skinner, and Sabine Braun (2018) Here or There. Research on Interpreting via
Video Link. Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press.
This book gathers state-of-the-art research on remote interpreting and interpreting through video-links from
a practice-based perspective.
Vranjes Jelena, and Hanneke Bot (2021) “A multimodal analysis of t urn-taking in i nterpreter-mediated
psychotherapy”, Translation & Interpreting 13 (1): 101–17.
This article discusses the phenomenon of chunking in a different (medical) institutional setting.
Related chapters
Chapter 6, Technology use in language-discordant interpersonal healthcare communication by Sabine Braun,
Khetam Al Sharou and Özlem Temizöz
Chapter 9, Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews by Sonja Pöllabauer
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véro-
nique Traverso, and Emilie Jouin
Transcription key
°yes° segment produced very softly
YES segment produced more loudly
: sound elongation;
:::: longer sound elongation
- sound cut-off
(1.4) silence expressed in seconds
171
Christian Licoppe
References
Angermeyer, Philipp S. (2015) Speak English or What? Codeswitching and Interpreter Use in New York City
Courts. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Antaki, Charles (1994) Explaining and Arguing. The Social Organization of Accounts. London, Sage.
Balogh, Katalin, and Heidi Salaets (2018) “A comparative study of simulated and real-life settings”,
in Here or There. Research on Interpreting via Video Link, Jemina Napier, Robert Skinner, and Sabine
Braun (eds). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 264–96.
——— (2019) “The role of non-verbal elements in legal interpreting: A study of a cross-border
interpreter-mediated videoconference witness hearing”, in The Routledge Handbook of Translation
and Pragmatics, Rebecca Tipton and Louisa Desilla (eds). London, Routledge: 394–429.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Berk-Seligson, Susan (1990) The Bilingual Courtroom. Chicago, IL, Chicago University Press.
——— (2009) Coerced Confessions: The Discourse of Bilingual Police Interrogations. New York, Walter de
Gruyter.
Braun, Sabine (2007) “Interpreting in small group bilingual videoconferences: Challenges and adap-
tation”, Interpreting 9 (1): 21–46.
Braun, Sabine, and Judith Taylor (2012) Videoconference and Remote Interpreting in Legal Proceedings. Ant-
werp, Belgium, Intersentia Publishing Ltd.
Conley, John, and William O’Barr (1998) Just words. Law, Language and Power. Chicago, IL, University
of Chicago Press.
Davitti Elena (2015) “Gaze” in Routledge Encyclopedia of Interpreting Studies, Franz Pöchhacker, Nadja
Grbic, Peter Mead, Robin Setton (eds). London, Routledge: 168–69.
——— (2018) “Methodological explorations of i nterpreter-mediated interaction: Novel insights from
multimodal analysis”, Qualitative Research 19 (1): 7–29.
Davitti Elena, and Pasquandrea Sergio (2017) “Embodied participation: What multimodal analysis
can tell us about interpreter-mediated encounters in pedagogical settings”, Journal of Pragmatics 107
(2017): 105–28.
Drew, Paul (1992) “Contested evidence in courtroom cross-examination: The case of a trial for rape”,
in Talk at Work, Paul Drew and John Heritage (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press:
470–520.
Drew, Paul, and John Heritage (1992) “A nalyzing talk at work”, in Talk at Work, Paul Drew and John
Heritage (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 3 –65.
Egbert, Maria (1997) “Schisming: The collaborative transformation from a single conversation to
multiple conversations”, Research on Language and Social Interaction 30 (1): 1–51.
Fowler, Yvonne (2013) Non-English-Speaking Defendants in the Magistrates Courts: A Comparative Study
of Face-to-Face and Prison Video Link Hearings in England. PhD dissertation, Aston University, UK.
Galatolo, Renata, and Paul Drew (2006) “Narrative expansions as defensive practices in courtroom
testimony”, Text & Talk 26 (6): 661–98.
Gavioli, Laura, and Baraldi, Claudio (2011) “Interpreter-mediated interaction in healthcare and legal
settings: Talk organization, context and the achievement of intercultural communication”, Inter-
preting 13 (2): 205–33.
Goodwin, Charles (2007) “Environmentally coupled gestures”, in Gesture and the Dynamic Dimension
of Language, Susan Duncan, Justine Cassell and Elena Levy (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John
Benjamins: 195–212.
172
Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences
——— (2013) “The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge”,
Journal of Pragmatics 46 (2013): 8 –23.
Hale, Sandra (1997) “Clash of world perspectives: The discursive practices of the law, the witness and
the interpreter”, International Journal of Speech Language and the Law 4 (2): 1 97–209.
——— (2001) “How are courtroom questions interpreted? An analysis of Spanish interpreters’ prac-
tices”, in Triadic Exchanges: Studies in Dialogue Interpreting, Ian Mason (ed). Manchester, St Jerome
Publishing: 21–50.
——— (2004) The Discourse of Courtroom Interpreting: Discourse Practices of the Law, the Witness, and the
Interpreter. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Heath, Christian, and Luff, Paul (1992) “Media space and communicative asymmetries. Preliminary
observations of video mediated interactions”, Human Computer Interaction 7 (3): 315–46.
Inghilleri, Moira (2012) Interpreting Justice: Ethics, Politics, and Language. London, Routledge.
Jakobsen, Bente (2007) “Pragmatic meaning in court interpreting. An empirical study of additions in
consecutively-i nterpreted question-a nswer dialogues”, International Journal of Speech Language and
the Law 11 (1): 165–69.
Krouglov, Alexander (1999) “Police interpreting: Politeness and sociocultural context”, The Translator
5 (2): 285–302.
Lee, Jieun (2015) “Court interpreting”, in The Routledge Handbook of Interpreting, Holly Mikkelson and
Renée Jourdenais (eds). London, Routledge: 1 86–201.
Licoppe, Christian, and Clair-Antoine Veyrier (2017) “How to show the interpreter on screen? The
normative organization of visual ecologies in multilingual courtrooms with video links”, Journal
of Pragmatics 107, 1 47–64.
——— (2020) “The interpreter as a sequential coordinator in courtroom interaction. ‘Chunking’ and
the management of turn shifts in extended answers in consecutively interpreted asylum hearings
with remote participants”, Interpreting 22 (1): 5 6–86.
Licoppe, Christian, and Maud Verdier (2013) “Interpreting, video communication and the sequential
reshaping of institutional talk in the bilingual and distributed courtroom”, The International Journal
of Speech, Language and the Law 20 (2): 247–76.
Licoppe, Christian, Maud Verdier, and C lair-Antoine Veyrier (2018) “Voice, power and turn-taking
in multi-lingual, consecutively interpreted courtroom proceedings with video links”, in Here or
There. Research on Interpreting via Video Link, Robert Skinner, Jemina Napier and Sabine Braun (eds).
Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 299–322.
Mason, Ian (2012) “Gaze, positioning and identity in i nterpreter-mediated dialogues”, in Coordinating
Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 177–200.
Matoesian, Gregory, and Kristina Enola Gilbert (2018) Multimodal Conduct in the Law. Language, Ges-
ture and Materiality in Legal Interaction. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Merlini, Sara (2009) “Seeking asylum and seeking identity in a mediated encounter: The projection of
selves through discursive practices”, Interpreting 11 (1): 57–92.
Mondada, Lorenza (2016) “Challenges of multimodality: Language in the body in social interaction”,
Journal of Sociolinguistics 29 (3): 336–66.
Nakane, Ikuko (2007) “Problems in communicating the suspect’s rights in interpreted police inter-
views”, Applied Linguistics 28 (1), 87–112.
——— (2011) “The role of silence in interpreted police interviews”, Journal of Pragmatics 43 (9):
2317–30.
——— (2014) Interpreter-Mediated Police Interviews: A D iscourse-P ragmatic Approach. New York, Palgrave
Mac Millan.
Napier, Jemina, Skinner, Robert, and Sabine Braun (eds) (2018) Here or There. Research on Interpreting
via Video Link. Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press.
Ng, Eva (2013) “W ho is speaking? Interpreting the voice of the speaker in court”, in Interpreting in a
Changing Landscape: Selected Papers from Critical Link 6, Christina Schäffner, Krzystof Kredens, and
Yvonne Fowler (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 2 49–66.
Ostos, Sarai (2019) Discours évangéliques interprétés: analyse de l’organisation interactionnelle. PhD dissertation,
Université Paul Valéry, Montpellier.
Pasquandrea, Sergio (2011) “Managing multiple actions through multimodality: Doctors’ involve-
ment in i nterpreter-mediated interactions”, Language in Society 40 (4): 455–81.
173
Christian Licoppe
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2004) “Interpreting in asylum hearings: Issues of role, responsibility and power”,
Interpreting 6 (2): 143–80.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2012) “Interpreting participation”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpret-
ing, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 45–70.
——— ( 2020) “ Going video. Mediality and multimodality in interpreting”, in Linking up with
Video: Perspectives on Interpreting Practice and Research, Heidi Salaets and Geert Brône ( eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 13– 45.
Russell, Debra (2000) “‘L et me put it simply …’: The case for a standard translation of the police cau-
tion and its explanation”, International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 7 (1): 26–48.
Russell, Debra, and Kayoko Takeda (2015) “Consecutive interpreting”, in The Routledge Handbook of
Interpreting, Holly Mikkelson and Renée Jourdenais (eds). London, Routledge: 96–111.
Salaets, Heidi, and Geert Brône (2020) Linking up with Video. Perspectives on Interpreting Practice and
Research. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Schegloff, Emmanuel (2007) Sequence Organization in Interaction. A Primer in Conversation Analysis.
Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Sidnell, Jack, and Tania Stivers (eds) (2014) Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Oxford, Wiley-Blackwell.
Tuncer, Sylvaine, Christian Licoppe, and Pentti Haddington (2019) “W hen objects become the focus
of human action and activity: Object-centred sequences in social interaction”, Gesprächsforschung:
O nline-Z eitschrift zur verbalen Interaktion 20: 384–98.
Vranjes, Jelena, and Geert Brône (2020) “Eye-tracking in interpreter-mediated talk: From research to
practice”, in Linking up with Video: Perspectives on Interpreting Practice and Research, Heidi Salaets and
Geert Brône (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 203–34.
Vranjes, Jelena, Hanneke Bot, Kurt Feyaerts, and Geert Brône (2019) “A ffiliation in interpreter-mediated
therapeutic talk: On the relationship between gaze and head nods”, Interpreting 21 (2): 220–44.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1997) “Recycled information as a questioning strategy. Pitfalls in interpreter-
mediated talk”, in The Critical Link: Interpreters in the Community, Silvana E. Carr, Roda P. Roberts,
Aideen Dufour, and Dini Stern (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins: 35–54.
——— (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (1999) “Telephone interpreting and the synchronization of talk in social interaction”, The
Translator 5 (2): 247–64.
——— ( 2010) “ On the production and elicitation of expanded answers to yes/ no questions in
i nterpreter-mediated trials”, in Text and Context. Essays on Translation and Interpreting in Honor of
Ian Mason, Mona Baker, Maeve Olohan, and María Calzada-Pérez (eds). Manchester, St Jerome
Publishing: 1–26.
174
11
VULNERABLE ENCOUNTERS?
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated
services for victim-survivors of domestic violence
and abuse
Rebecca Tipton
others, as family-based violence, gender-based violence and intimate partner-v iolence, re-
flecting the many social and relational dynamics of situations in which individuals are subject
to forms of abuse and/or violence in the domestic setting. Responding to Brown, Ecclestone
and Emmel’s (2017: 506) criticism of the emphasis in ‘v ulnerability studies’ on theoretical
debates and policy critiques to the neglect of empirical investigation, this chapter makes a
modest contribution to knowledge on how vulnerability and related phenomena of risk and
autonomy operate in particularised service settings.
Despite growing interest, the number of studies addressing language and communi-
cation in these settings remains small and largely limited to preliminary investigations of
courts and specialist perpetrator programmes (Abraham 1998; Abraham and Oda 2000;
Oda and Joyette 2003). More systematic approaches have emerged in the past decade, par-
ticularly in Spain, oriented towards improvements in interpreter education and training,
and knowledge exchange between statutory support services in legal, social service and
healthcare settings (for instance Toledano Buendía and Del Pozo Triviño 2014; Valero-
G arcés 2015). However, the emphasis on structural and perceptual phenomena, which is a
necessary stage in mapping practice in settings and identifying research agendas, leaves am-
ple scope for investigation of matters concerning vulnerability autonomy and risk, among
many others.
Through ethnographic work (observations and interviews) and close analysis of au-
thentic interpreter-mediated police interviews with v ictim-survivors, my research has
sought to broaden the scope of investigation. In particular, I draw attention to charity
sector victim support services which take place in complex, m ulti-sited research settings
where professional and n on-professional interpreting commonly intersect. To date my
research has explored themes relating to social contracts in interpreter mediation for
v ictim-survivors (Tipton 2017a), the concepts of accountability and responsibility in a
third sector setting (Tipton 2017b), the social trajectories of v ictim-survivors through
a third sector organisation and variability in English language proficiency (Tipton
2018), question forming and code-switching in police interviews with v ictim-survivors
(Tipton 2019a), and interpreting as a biopolitical endeavour that supports the achieve-
ment of political community in these settings (Tipton 2021). The work has also sup-
ported practitioner workshops, leading to the development of guidelines for interpreters
and staff working in a charity organisation in the city of Manchester (Tipton 2020a,
2020b).
This chapter revisits findings from this extended period of inquiry to develop a more
critically-informed perspective regarding the forms of vulnerability that are present in
interpreter-mediated encounters, and how they are perceived and attended to by the various
parties involved in different service settings. In the first part of this chapter, I examine the-
orisations of vulnerability before exploring forms of vulnerability in interpreter-mediated
encounters specifically. The second section revisits examples from police interview data
reported in Tipton (2019a) to illustrate ways in which v ictim-survivors and interpreters
generate layers of situational and linguistic vulnerability which can impact on epistemic
vulnerability. In the final section, I present new findings from re-coded interview data with
interpreters working in both statutory and charity settings to evaluate discursive construc-
tions of what I term ‘professional vulnerability’. The following main questions underpin
what follows:
1 how might vulnerability be theorised in service encounters involving service users with
limited or no majority language proficiency?
176
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
2 what relationship between vulnerability and autonomy can be discerned through anal-
ysis of authentic interpreter-mediated encounters?
3 how do interpreters discursively construct vulnerability and what is the significance of
such constructions for our understanding of the interpreter as a professionally vulnerable
subject?
177
Rebecca Tipton
Epistemic vulnerability
The example from an interpreter-mediated asylum interview given in the previous section
is a reminder that, as Johnson (2020: 684) writes, ‘[w]hen we need other people to help us
meet our epistemic need, we are vulnerable to them’. Määttä, Puumala and Ylikomi’s study
shows how linguistic vulnerability (i.e. vulnerability that arises as a consequence of language
interpretation and ideologies about language) in conjunction with psychological vulnerabil-
ity, can lead to epistemic vulnerability and therefore be highly consequential in institutional
178
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
processes that are anchored around truth and credibility, such as an asylum procedure. How-
ever, as Määttä, Puumala and Ylikomi assert, epistemic vulnerability can also be generated
by particular institutional dispositions to knowledge which ‘[affect] what kind of informa-
tion becomes valued as knowledge’ (2021: 48).
For v ictim-survivors of domestic violence and abuse, linguistic and psychological forms
of vulnerability are present in encounters with police and charity support services for the
reasons outlined in Määttä, Puumala and Ylikomi’s work. In police settings, epistemic
vulnerability is an inherent quality of evidential account taking and is influenced by the
disposition of the interviewing officer to knowledge of evidential value and, in court set-
tings, from the verifiability of the victim account. In charity settings, by contrast, although
some account-taking does take place, much of the interactional work centres around an
individual coming to terms with a situation and taking action to move forward and self-
determine. In this sense, knowing the self becomes a priority, which means that the organ-
isation’s disposition to knowledge is chiefly concerned with how it is validated rather than
how it is valued.
In other words, the truth-value of v ictim-survivor discourse is not in question in such
circumstances; epistemic interdependence in c harity-led encounters is often shaped by the
belief shown in a victim’s story, which places the interpreter in a different set of interac-
tional parameters to other institutional interactions with v ictim-survivors. This setting-
specific distinction is therefore salient and has implications for interpreter practice, and the
way such practice and its outcomes are evaluated. Displaying believability towards victims,
however, requires careful management. In my study, charity support workers stressed that
(interpreters’) efforts to be empathic, for example through verbalisations such as ‘I cannot be-
lieve you put up with that’ or ‘that he did that to you’ (Tipton 2020b: 14), can be interpreted
by v ictim-survivors as though they are being blamed for creating the situation themselves,
potentially impacting on the decision taken whether or not to return to the family home.
An interpreter’s disposition to knowledge in such settings therefore requires specific training
interventions to prevent such inadvertent v ictim-blaming.
179
Rebecca Tipton
provisions. The Code of Practice for Victims of Crime in England and Wales (revised 2015),
sets out various entitlements to interpreting and translation provisions, but these are strictly
limited to procedural matters (evidence-gathering, court attendance), and do not extend, for
example to other police victim support services that may be provided through victim hubs1.
Under the current Section 16 definition 2, people with limited English language profi-
ciency are not characterised as vulnerable witnesses in England and Wales; however, this is
not the case in all jurisdictions where English is the majority language of the criminal justice
system as highlighted by Asquith, Bartkowiak-Théron and Roberts (2017). These authors
cite The New South Wales Law Enforcement (Powers and Responsibilities) Act (2002) and
the 2005 Regulation, which lists five categories of vulnerability and includes individuals
from non-English speaking backgrounds. Interestingly, these provisions strictly apply to of-
fending only and are not operationalised to account for victim or witness vulnerability. The
question as to whether a n on-English speaking background should be considered as a named
characteristic of vulnerable witnesses and victims in England and Wales is beyond the scope
of this chapter to address. However, based on earlier discussions, it may be argued that a per-
son is not inherently vulnerable due to language proficiency or background though they may
experience situational vulnerabilities, thereby justifying its exclusion from the normative list
of what constitutes a vulnerable witness.
In England and Wales, police guidance at the time of writing offers LEP individuals
(whether suspects, victims or witnesses) the opportunity to select their preferred or ‘best
language’ for interview purposes (A ssociation of Chief Police Officers 2012: 227). The
guidance is underpinned by several key assumptions that arguably underplay the nature of
vulnerability and its subsequent handling in the interview: (1) that a single language will
be selected, one in which the individual is fully proficient; and (2) that ‘monolingualising
practice’ (A ngermeyer 2015: 8), characteristic of many bureaucracies, will prevail, according
to which an individual either only speaks in the majority language or not at all, leading to a
participation framework in which the interpreter takes every other turn. Individual reper-
toire development (Blommaert, Collins and Slembrouck 2005) and emerging competence in
the majority language which an individual may or may not seek to employ in the interaction
are not factored into the guidance, and yet it is increasingly being recognised in research
on public service interpreting (A ngermeyer 2008; Tipton 2019b; Monteoliva-García 2020).
As Angermeyer (2015) observes in relation to non-English speaking litigants in a small
claims court in New York City, many individuals try to resist monolingualising practices
that foreclose their attempts to draw on their repertoire but are often challenged in so doing.
In Monteoliva-García’s (2020) study on police interpreting in Scotland, by contrast, the
interviewing officers do not follow the anticipated monolingualising approach and actively
encourage two S panish-speaking suspects to use their limited English language repertoires,
thereby relegating the interpreter to a ‘stand by’ position. This places pressure on the inter-
preter to know how, when and to what extent to intervene in the police interview, with
many interventions appearing to hinge on the interpreter’s perceived vulnerability of the
suspects in light of their lack of institutional expertise. The officers’ decision in this case
generates a different type of situated vulnerability for the interpreter and the suspects to that
evidenced in my research, highlighting variability in current interpreter-mediated police
interview practices in the United Kingdom.
Resistance to institutional monolingualising practice is, however, evidenced in one of the
interviews that formed part of my study. In this case, a Russian-speaking victim makes sev-
eral attempts to bypass the interpreter and speak in English over the course of the evidential
interview. The vast majority of such attempts were curtailed by the interviewing officers as
180
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
00:06:13 PO1 1 so what I’d like you to do:: {name} if you could just go ba:ck
[to that day]
00:06:15 V 2 [uh hu]
00:06:15 PO1 3 to all the events till before you left the house
00:06:17 INT 4 [хочу вернуться чтоб вы] вернулись в тот день нанана прежде
этих событий перед тем как вы ушли из дома
[I want to come back that you] come back to that day nanana before the
events before that when you left the house
00:06:27 PO1 5 but please remember if you ca:n’t {name}
V 6 я могу сейчас объяснить что случилось
I can explain now what happened
00:06:40 INT 7 I can tell you now what has happened
00:06:42 V 8 I remember this I remember
00:06:47 V 9 {name} come from jail he was arrested
00:06:50 PO1 10 stop stop stop
00:06:52 INT 11 по-русски говорите speak in Russian
speak in Russian
Key to speaker roles: PO1: Interviewing officer 1; V: Victim; INT: Interpreter
Transcription key: [ ] overlapping talk; o:: long vowel; italics back translation from Russian; this
stress; {name} redacted information
the victim’s level of proficiency falls way short of what is needed in an evidential interview,
as observed in Extract 1 above. The interviewing officers’ decision in this case can be seen as
oriented to their understanding rather than as normative or ideological, and as performing
two key functions: maximising the quality and detail of the evidence and safeguarding the
victim.
Research also shows that the concept of ‘best language’ can and does vary, both in
terms of the choice made ahead of the police interview and in terms of the language(s) em-
ployed in the interview itself, sometimes with procedural consequences (for instance Gallai
2013). In short, if an individual selects a language for interview in which they have limited
proficiency – whether for political, ideological or other reasons – they are in effect making
all parties present in the encounter vulnerable to some extent. This raises important ques-
tions as to the nature and level of interpreter intervention in negotiating and/or making vis-
ible issues arising from limited language proficiency. A second interpreter-mediated police
interview (Tipton 2019a) discussed below helps to illustrate this situation.
The extracts are taken from an interview involving a female police interviewer, an ex-
perienced female interpreter and native speaker of Italian, and a female v ictim-survivor of
domestic abuse who chooses to be interviewed in Italian, which is not her first language.
The reason for this decision and details of the victim’s first language were not made available
to the researcher, although there are several indications that it may be Arabic. The extracts
are taken from an account-giving sequence lasting t wenty-two minutes in which the victim
and interpreter take turns, punctuated by several response tokens ‘uh hu’ on the part of the
181
Rebecca Tipton
interviewing officer. The sequence is prompted by the initial question: ‘Okay. So, if you can
then, in as much detail, tell me what happened on Sunday the 6th of November from about
2 o’clock onwards?’.
Extract 2, taken from just after four minutes into the interview, shows the interpreter
seeking clarification with regard to the victim’s use of possessive pronouns in Italian. The
interpreter’s interjection in line 4 is reported to the interviewing officer as a request for a
‘repetition’, which does not clearly signal an issue of language proficiency that may require
negotiation as part of the interview process. Although this is followed up in lines 13–14 with
confirmation that the discussion was not about substantive issues, the interpreter does not
make the reason for the clarification explicit.
The extract draws attention to the situational vulnerability generated through the vic-
tim’s choice of interview language and raises the question as to what benefits accrue (to all
Extract 2
00:04:37 V 1 E: non lo so cosa (.) ch-(.) cosa hanno detto perché io: io non ho chie-
2 sto lui cosa ha detto, lui mi ha detto che: mi ha chiesto solo pe:r (.) per
3 la sua salute e basta.
A:nd I don’t know what (.) wh-(.) what they said because I: I didn’t ask him
what he has said, he told me tha:t he asked me only abou:t (.) about his/her
health and that’s it.
00:04:50 INT 4 Erm, the interpreter is gonna ask for=a=repetition (.) Chiedo scusa
5 signora, (.) l’ultima cosa che ha detto:? Erm… Mi ha chiesto per la
sua: …?=
I’m sorry Madam, (.) the last thing you said:? Erm… He asked me about
his/her…?
00:04:57 V 6 =Per sua salute.
For his/her health.
00:04:58 INT 7 Lei [s-?=
You s -?
00:05:00 V 8 =[No no io. La sua fi:glia [ ] gli ha chiesto a lu:i: per la sua salute.
No not me. His daug:hter asked him about his /her health.
00:05:01 INT 9 [Ah… ]
00:05:04 INT 10 La salute di lei signora..?=
The health of you madam…?
00:05:05 V 11 =No no la salute [suo]
No no the health of his…
00:05:06 INT 12 [Ok].
00:05:07 INT 13 Erm... I didn’t ask any details erm… erm... of the conversation (.) what
14 it was about mh… I just know that: at one point she a:s-((throat clear-
15 ing)) asked my husband about his health.
Key to speaker roles: V = Victim; INT = Interpreter
Transcription key: (.) micro pause; italics back translation from Italian; [ ] overlapping talk; =
no gap between the two lines; (( )) non-verbal feature; a: long vowel.
182
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
Extract 3
parties) from an interpreter making the issue explicit to the interviewing officer. Specifically,
the exchange raises questions as to the impact, if any, such information could have on the
officer’s disposition to knowledge in the a ccount-giving process. It also raises questions as
to the potential implications of the negotiation of meaning at this early point in the inter-
view for the way in which the interpreter subsequently handles the linguistic and epistemic
vulnerability arising from the selected interview language. Extracts 3 and 4 provide some
insight into both issues.
Extract 3 is taken from a short time later in the interview in which the victim experi-
ences difficulties in conjugating the verb ‘to die’ in line 3 (i.e. ‘muoi’ rather than ‘muori’).
The victim opts for a different construction, ‘it is better to die’, which the interpreter clearly
understands and, in seeking an idiomatic rendering (‘it might as well be …’), mitigates the
illocutionary force of the utterance. The fact that the threat was not taken up again later in
the account-probing phases of the interview raises questions as to whether the overall level
of risk facing the v ictim-survivor was fully assessed and whether the officer’s disposition to
knowledge was impacted by the mitigation.
Extract 4 shows the interpreter generating an instance of linguistic vulnerability. In line
4, the interpreter initiates an intervention to alert the interviewing officer to the need for
clarification and follows up after the short monolingual exchange (lines 4 –5) in line 7 with
an explanation that signals the v ictim-survivor’s linguistic vulnerability to the interviewing
officer, 52 minutes into the interview. The lack of m eta-commentary by the interpreter at
other points in the interview for the benefit of the interviewing officer suggests that the in-
terpreter does not pay attention to the fact that lack of mutual understanding may jeopardise
the v ictim-survivor’s trust in the police officer and consequently in the public institution.
The two interviews show respectively an example of a v ictim-survivor using a limited
repertoire in English and a second who uses a language in which proficiency was limited.
These decisions to speak in a language of limited proficiency were ostensibly designed to
enable these individuals to maintain some autonomy over the situation and alleviate personal
suffering, but the risk of inflicting harm on the self and increasing situational vulnerability
is evident. The extracts also show how the interviewing officers’ disposition to knowledge
can be impacted and the extent to which their (lack) of understanding leads to action in
mitigation. In the first interview, the frequent (re-)a ssertion of the anticipated participation
framework was to protect the victim from herself, for the PO to protect himself from the
183
Rebecca Tipton
Extract 4
00:51:45 P 1 and, can you describe how he.. he got hold of your hair?
00:51:51 INT 2 può descrivere come le ha afferrato i capelli?
can you describe how he grabbed your hair?
00:51:55 V 3 come afferrato? aperto? afferrato?
what do you mean by grabbed? opened? grabbed?
00:52:00 INT 4 erm (.) the interpreter needs to (.) n o-non sa cosa significa afferrato in
italiano?
5 don’t you know what ‘afferrato’ means
in Italian?
00:52:05 V 6 no.
00:52:07 INT 7 Erm.. {name} is not sure what the word (.) the Italian word for ‘d rag’
8 means. ‘a fferrare:’ e’… prendere con la mano cosi’…
dra:g is…take with your hand like this… ‘to drag’ is to do like that with
9 the hand like this.
10 quindi, come-come le ha preso i capelli?
then, how-how did he take hold of your hair?
00:52:26 V 11 con.. con.. tutte e due le mani.
with..with.. all and both of the hands
Key to speaker roles: P = Police officer; INT = Interpreter; V = Victim
Transcription key: (.) micro pause; {name} redacted information; italics back translation from
Italian
effects of misunderstanding and maximise the clarity and detail of the evidence provided.
In the second, the interpretation and lack of transparency over language choice contributed
to shaping the interviewing officer’s positive impression of the v ictim-survivor’s ability to
provide a detailed account in Italian.
It is important to stress that the observations regarding the lack of m eta-commentary in
the second interview are not intended as a criticism of the interpreter’s approach. However,
the selected extracts do raise important questions about the m ulti-layered nature of vul-
nerability in interpreter-mediated victim interviews and its impact on officers’ disposition
to knowledge. At the time of the research, anecdotal evidence suggested that interpreter-
mediated police interviews were less likely to be categorised as high risk, resulting in some
individuals not being monitored as a priority. The small sample of interviews made available
for my study did not allow for corroboration of this point, nor was I given access to the in-
terviewing officer and interpreter as originally planned in the research design to talk through
the decision-making processes. However, the interviews do highlight different ways that
vulnerability manifests in interpreter-mediated victim interviews, providing scope for their
systematic analysis across a much larger sample, opening up questions with regard to the
nature and importance of interpreter meta-commentary within a future-oriented approach
to the interview process.
184
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
experience within and across domestic abuse service settings. It draws on 15 s emi-structured
interviews with spoken language interpreters: eight from a police constabulary in the East of
England and seven from a city council service in the North West of England, two males and
13 females, with experience ranging between 5 and 27 years. Each interview lasted between
one and one and a half hours. Thirteen interviewees gave permission for the interview to be
recorded and transcribed, and in the other two, the author took notes. Interviewees from the
police constabulary were recruited through a survey, and the city council interpreters were
recruited through recommendation by the service manager, all following ethical approval
for the study from the University of Manchester.
The interviews covered topics such as the extent of domestic abuse-related interpret-
ing experience, interpreting in risk assessments, rapport and trust building, assignments at
refuges, emotional impact of domestic v iolence-related work, domestic v iolence-specific
interpreter training (availability/impact on practice), service user repertoire use in English
during encounters, language challenges around domestic abuse, pre- interview briefing
(police), understanding of victim interview process (police), experience of working with the
same v ictim-survivor across services (in statutory and charity sectors) and available support
for interpreters. Although the concept of vulnerability was not a focus of the interviews
and a keyword search of the data confirms that it was not mentioned by the interviewer
or respondents directly, using a thematic approach to re-analyse the d ata – as suggested by
Braun and Clarke (2006) working with qualitative research in psychology – the theme of
‘professional vulnerability’ emerges as salient through indirect references identified through
the sub-themes of disclosure, challenges to interpreter impartiality, personal safety, service
user desire for autonomy and freelancer precarity.
In agential terms, ‘professional vulnerability’ denotes actions taken that result in increased
vulnerability for the interpreter and/or for his/her interlocutors, and actions taken by others
that impact on the level of interpreters’ perceived and actual vulnerability. The examples dis-
cussed in the previous section of the different kinds of vulnerability in interpreter-mediated
interaction are thus complemented in this section through a focus on the broader contextual
factors shaping the interpreter’s experience of vulnerability, opening up critical points for
commissioners of interpreting services and interpreter educators.
Extract 5 is one of several from the data that draw attention to interpreters being viewed
as knowledge holders who may be relied upon for information to support continuity of ser-
vice within a single service or across services. The interpreter in this extract does not appear
to perceive the self as vulnerable when such requests arise, nor does s/he suggest that such
requests are resisted. The account, nevertheless, highlights several situational vulnerabilities
Extract 5
The victim needs to be taken into some home, obviously if they’re not going back to their
house, and then it’s handed from one service to the other service. Within that hand [over],
although I follow it all the way through, the same officer doesn’t follow it all the way
through…these new people, I would say, don’t have much about the case. And if there is
not a decent brief being given to these people, they ask the interpreter because I’ve been
there all the way though (Police Interpreter Interview 1).
185
Rebecca Tipton
that may be of some consequence. These include the potential for increased epistemic vul-
nerability (of primary service provider and v ictim-survivor) due to incomplete or inaccurate
information relayed by the interpreter, and a diminished sense of control (a nd, hence, in-
creased ‘psychological vulnerability’ (Määttä, Puumala and Ylikomi 2021)) for the v ictim-
survivor in being excluded from discussions.
A second interview with a city council interpreter also on the theme of handovers (this
time within the same service) highlights an agential response that shows sensitivity to the
v ictim-survivor’s psychological vulnerability and awareness of the potential for the institu-
tion to generate linguistic vulnerability. In this case, the interview notes show the interpreter
recognised the risk of re-traumatisation posed by a new case worker by asking the service
user to talk about experiences that had been recounted several times before. The interpreter
reported asking the new case worker to start from the most recent section of the notes to
avoid emphasising issues not relevant to that day’s service encounter. This draws attention
to the impact of the interpreter’s familiarity with a service user’s institutional trajectory on
her/h is disposition to knowledge and moral sensitivity to the v ictim-survivor’s experience
and vulnerability to distress being triggered. The interpreter’s actions can be described as
future-oriented and motivated by professional interest in the outcome of that day’s interac-
tion for the service user.
It is not the goal of the analysis to evaluate the rights or wrongs of such intervention by
the interpreter. However, it is important to note that approaches inevitably vary among in-
terpreters, and for those who adopt a minimal interventionist approach to interpreting, such
future orientation will be precluded. In this case, the avoidance of harm (t riggering distress)
seems to have been a motivating factor for the interviewed interpreter. However, it may be
argued that the interpreter’s decision to act as reported above was based on her own perceived
vulnerability only and not on professional expertise and, hence, that the intervention is risky
and needed to have been negotiated with the primary service provider.
Extract 6 shows the interpreter experiencing professional vulnerability within a single
encounter due to issues of perceived physical and financial risk. Faced with an uncertain
situation, alone with two v ictim-survivors who are apparently in touch with the alleged
perpetrator, the interpreter has to wait for a manager to sign the job card in order to receive
payment. The lack of autonomy to physically leave the situation brought about by the ad-
ministrative needs associated with freelancer status highlights the diminished agency of the
Extract 6
Three victims, and I, being left at this house’s living room. Two hours waiting for this
manager to turn up. So, we were alone for two hours. I had no professional next to me.
What we’re doing in two hours? We’re talking. I’m not talking, they are talking. Because
they are so worried, “a re we staying here? Is there food? I don’t have money, what are
we going to do? Is he going to find me? What’s happening?”. And for some reason, the
victims’ phones were with them, the same numbers and everything else. So, I don’t know
if perhaps it’s their choice, I don’t know. But so, it kept ringing. I can hear the suspect
calling them. What do you do? You walk out? I could’ve walked out. And also, I need to
get signed to get paid. There is no one to sign me (Police Interpreter Interview 1).
186
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
Extract 7
…a sexual assault happened, and the person left on bail. It was chucking it down, I didn’t
realise the person was hanging around, the suspect was hanging around. I went into my
car and then he just appeared next to me saying “can you give me a lift to town?” (Police
Interpreter Interview 8).
interpreter, especially in assignments that stretch beyond the usual working day in which in-
terpreters are left with no back-office support. Extract 7 is a further articulation of perceived
physical risk; this interpreter account (and others in the data set) was set against a backdrop
of government funding cuts to police services and the lack of funding to pay for taxis or
accommodation if a police interview finished late at night.
These situations, along with the example above of the interpreter providing continuity of
service between institutional handovers are, at this time of writing, seldom highlighted in
academic research on public service interpreting. Yet these interstitial experiences (i.e. non-
interpreter mediated encounters in between interpreter-mediated encounters) are clearly
a source of professional vulnerability that interpreters appear to have little control over. It
raises the question of institutional accountability to interpreters in such moments and the
extent to which this accountability is foregrounded in commissioning interpreting services.
Another category of professional vulnerability in the data concerns what I term challenges
to interpreter impartiality where impartiality is understood primarily as non-involvement
in matters of substance in an encounter rather than non-involvement tout court. At stake is
the porousness of social interaction and the over-estimation on the part of many interpreters
that others automatically share a consensus as to where the interpreter’s boundaries of in-
volvement lie as a professional. The experience of boundary encroachment can be a source
of professional discomfort and perceived vulnerability for interpreters.
The data contains several examples of such encroachments. Two city council interpret-
ers report side conversations initiated by v ictim-survivors prior to the interpreter-mediated
encounter or during the encounter. The first involved disclosure of a level of violence perpe-
trated against the victim that the interpreter found very difficult to handle; she subsequently
struggled to maintain her composure during the main encounter. The second concerns ap-
parently regular requests from v ictim-survivors for the interpreter to embellish their stories.
The interpreter in question had no difficulty in asserting her professional boundaries and
deny such requests, but the unexpected infringement on her professional role boundaries
was striking and highlights the challenges of interpreters being coerced into conversations
against their professional will: ‘Many times, say 85% of my clients say ‘what’s your personal
view?’. I keep saying ‘I can’t’’ (City Council Interpreter Interview 7).
Extract 8 contains another example of boundary encroachment. Here the interpreter re-
ports refusing to take sole responsibility for taking a victim personal s tatement – a document
that informs a court of the harm suffered by a victim of crime and that may be regarded
when a sentence is decided – out of concern that her status as interpreter would impact on
the document’s admissibility in court and on the victim’s ability to fully exercise her/h is
rights. The interpreter asserts her professional vulnerability on the basis that going along
with the officer’s w ish – that
she takes a statement on her own, outside of her usual profes-
sional m andate – would lead to an inappropriate public record and potentially impact on the
187
Rebecca Tipton
Extract 8
…here is a victim, and the written statement needs to be taken… there is something called
VPS “v ictim’s personal statement”, and I get this [question] “can you take a statement?”,
and [the police officer] tries to walk out. So many times. And then I go “well, no I can’t”,
and then [he says] “well the other interpreter did it” (Police Interpreter Interview 8).
outcome of the case for the v ictim-survivor. The officer’s reported response suggests that
the level of epistemic vulnerability is not fully acknowledged or downplayed for reasons of
procedural expediency or his own convenience; yet, it is not an unusual occurrence in police
settings. Mayfield’s (2016) research reveals that interpreters who are anxious about maintain-
ing good links with the service and mitigate precarity of their freelancer status will often go
along with an officer’s wishes without taking full account of the epistemic vulnerability that
may ensue for the limited (English) language proficient speaker.
Conclusion
This chapter draws on research conducted over a five-year period (2015–20), revisiting find-
ings and presenting new ones in the context of an investigation of vulnerability in settings
involving v ictim-survivors requiring spoken language interpreting. The discussion high-
lights that what is understood by the ‘v ulnerable subject’ by parties to encounters varies,
generating dispositions to both knowledge and interaction that are often neglected in pub-
lic service interpreting research. There is clearly scope to further investigate the extent to
which some interpreter decision-making is triggered by perceptions of vulnerability relat-
ing to encounter-specific and wider social factors and its impact on service user outcomes
to deepen understanding of the relationship between interpreter agency and risk, and the
nature of epistemic interdependence in interpreter-mediated encounters. The proposed
‘future-oriented’ approach to interpreting offers a useful analytical category for the pursuit
of such questions in ways that foreground relational approaches to vulnerability and the im-
portance of fostering service user autonomy.
While the category of professional vulnerability emerges as salient in the re-analysis of in-
terviews with interpreters, caution is needed against viewing interpreters as inherently more
vulnerable than other professionals. Nevertheless, the contextual lens of domestic abuse ser-
vices, both statutory and non-statutory, has made it possible to identify a range of situational
vulnerabilities that merit further attention from commissioners of services and interpreter
educators, to enhance protection and support for interpreters where it is needed, and to de-
velop the foundations for interpreter strategy development.
Further reading
del Pozo Trivino, Maribel (2018) “Immigrant women in situations of gender violence: Towards im-
proving communication with public providers through interpreters”, in Galician Migrations: A Case
Study of Emerging Super-D iversity, Renée DePalma, Antía Pérez-Caramés (eds). Cham, Switzerland,
Springer: 265–78.
188
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
This chapter describes findings from a questionnaire conducted under the SOS-VICS study among agents with
experience of assisting foreign female victims of gender-based violence and how these supported the development
of training resources for key service providers.
Schröttle, Monika, Ravi K. Thiara, and Stephanie A Condon (2011) Violence Against Women and
Ethnicity: Commonalities and Differences across Europe. Leverkusen-Opladen, Verlag Barbara Budrich.
This book addresses issues of ethnicity and violence against women within and across national contexts and pro-
vides an excellent background resource for interpreting studies scholars interested in understanding how women
from different ethnic, racial and linguistic minorities are confronted with different legal, cultural and economic
challenges depending on which (European) country they live in.
Related chapters
Chapter 14, Public service interpreting in social care by Dorien Van De Mieroop, Antoon Cox, and Koen
Kerremans
Chapter 16, ‘Interpreter’s mistake’ – Why should other professions care about the professionalization of interpret-
ers? by Hanne Skaaden
Notes
1 The author’s correspondence with the head of a victim hub described how in the local area bilin-
gual volunteers had been recruited to support service users to mitigate language issues.
2 Section 16 of the Ministry of Justice Guide (2011) https://lemosandcrane.co.uk/resources/Vulnerable
%20and%20intimidated%20witnesses.pdf
References
Abraham, Diane (1998) Final Report: Pilot Project Cultural Interpreter Services in the Domestic Violence
Courts. North York/Toronto, Ministry of Citizenship, Ontario Women’s Directorate.
Abraham, Diane and Melanie Oda (2000) “The cultural/community interpreter in the domestic vio-
lence court – a pilot study”, in The Critical Link 2: Interpreters in the Community. Selected Papers from
the Second International Conference on Interpreting in Legal, Health and Social Service Settings, Vancouver,
BC, Canada, 19–23 May 1998, Roda P. Roberts, Silvana E. Carr, Diane Abraham, and Aideen
Dufour (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 1 65–90.
Angermeyer, Philipp S. (2008) “Creating monolingualism in the multilingual courtroom”, Sociolin-
guistic Studies 2 (3): 3 85–403.
——— (2015) ‘Speak English or What?’ Codeswitching and Interpreter Use in New York City Courts. Oxford,
Oxford University Press.
Asquith, Nicole, L., Isabelle Bartkowiak-Théron, and Karl A. Roberts (2017) “Vulnerability as a
contemporary challenge for policing”, in Policing Encounters with Vulnerability, Nicole, L. Asquith,
Bartkowiak-Théron, Isabelle, and Karl A. Roberts (eds). Cham, Palgrave Macmillan: 1–24.
Association of Chief Police Officers (now NPCC) (2012) Practice Guidance on European C ross-Border
Investigations. London, National Policing Improvement Agency.
Barsky, Robert, F. (1994) Constructing a Productive Other: Discourse Theory and the Convention Refugee
Hearing. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Blommaert, Jan, James Collins and Stef Slembrouck (2005) “Spaces of multilingualism”, Language &
Communication 25: 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2005.05.002
Braun, Virginia and Victoria Clarke (2006) “Using thematic analysis in psychology”, Qualitative
Research in Psychology 3 (2):77–101.
Brown, Kate, Kathryn Ecclestone, and Nick Emmel (2017) “The many faces of vulnerability”, Social
Policy and Society 16 (3): 497–510.
Emirbayer, Mustafa and Ann Mische (1998) “W hat is agency?” American Journal of Sociology 103 (4):
962–1023.
Fineman, Martha, A. (2008) “The vulnerable subject: Anchoring equality in the human condition”,
Yale Journal of Law and Feminism 20 (1): 1–23.
——— (2010) “The vulnerable subject and the responsive state”, Emory Law Journal 60 (2): 251–75.
189
Rebecca Tipton
Gallai, Fabrizio (2013) “‘I’ll Just Intervene Whenever He Finds It a Bit Difficult to Answer’: Exploring
the myth of literalism in interpreted interviews”, Investigative Interviewing: Research and Practice (II-
R P) 5 (1): 57–78.
Gilson, Erin (2014) The Ethics of Vulnerability: A Feminist Analysis of Social Life and Practice. New
York/Abingdon, Routledge.
Goodin, Robert, E. (1985) Protecting the Vulnerable: A Reanalysis of Our Social Responsibilities. Chicago,
IL, University of Chicago Press.
Johnson, Casey Rebecca (2020) “Epistemic vulnerability”, International Journal of Philosophical Studies
28 (5): 677–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/09672559.2020.1796030
Määttä, Simo, K., Eeva Puumala, and Riitta Ylikomi, (2021) “Linguistic, psychological and epistemic
vulnerability in asylum procedures: An interdisciplinary approach”, Discourse Studies 23 (1): 46–66.
Mackenzie, Catriona (2013) “The importance of relational autonomy and capabilities for an ethics
of vulnerability”, in Vulnerability: New Essays in Ethics and Feminist Philosophy, C. Mackenzie, W.
Rogers and S. Dodds (eds). New York, Oxford University Press: 33–59.
Mackenzie, Catriona, Wendy Rogers, and Susan Dodds (eds) (2014) Vulnerability: New Essays in Ethics
and Feminist Philosophy. New York, Oxford University Press.
Matras, Yaron, Rebecca Tipton, and Leonie Gaiser (2023) “A gency and multilingualism in public
health care: Practitioner statements on local practice and policy”, in Understanding the Dynamics
of Language and Multilingualism in Professional Contexts: Advances in Language-Sensitive Management
Research, Betty Beeler, Claudine Gaibrois, Philippe Lecomte, and Mary Vigier (eds). Cheltenham,
Edward Elgar Publishing.
Mayfield, Katrina (2016) The Issues and Challenges Surrounding Interpreter-Assisted Investigative Interviews
of Victims and Witnesses. Unpublished MA Dissertation. London Metropolitan University.
Monteoliva-García, Eloísa (2020) “The collaborative and selective nature of interpreting in po-
lice interviews with stand-by interpreting”, Interpreting 22 (2): 2 62–87. https://doi.org/10.1075/
intp.00046.mon
Oda, Melanie and Donna Joyette (2003) “Interpreting for the perpetrator in the partner assault re-
sponse program – the selection and training process”, in The Critical Link 3: Interpreters in the Com-
munity, Louise Brunette, Georges Bastin, Isabelle Hemlin and Heather Clarke (eds). Amsterdam
and Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 147–62.
Peroni, Lourdes and Alexandra Timmer (2013) “Vulnerable groups: The promise of an emerging
concept in European human rights convention Law”, International Journal of Constitutional Law 11
(4): 1056–85.
Postema, Gerald, J. (1980) “Moral responsibility in professional ethics”, New York University Law Review
55 (1): 63–89.
Pym, Anthony (2015) “Translation as risk management”, Journal of Pragmatics 85: 67–80.
Tipton, Rebecca (2017a) “Contracts and capabilities: Public service interpreting and third sector do-
mestic violence services”, The Translator (Special Issue on Translation, Ethics and Social Responsi-
bility) 23 (2): 237–54, https://doi.org/10.1080/13556509.2017.1280875
——— (2017b) “Interpreting-as-conflict: PSIT in third sector organisations and the impact of third
way politics”, in Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development in Public Service Interpreting and Translation,
Carmen Valero-Garcés and Rebecca Tipton (eds). Buffalo, Toronto, Bristol, Multilingual Matters:
38–62.
——— (2018) “Translating/ed selves and voices: Language support provisions for victims of domestic
violence in a british third sector organization”, Translation and Interpreting Studies 13 (2): 1 63–84.
——— (2019a) “‘Yes I understand’: Language choice, question formation and code-switching in
i nterpreter-mediated police interviews with v ictim-survivors of domestic abuse”, International Jour-
nal Police Practice and Research. https://doi.org/10.1080/15614263.2019.1663733
Tipton, Rebecca (2019b) “Exploring the E SOL-PSIT relation: Interpellation, resistance and resil-
ience”, Language & Communication 67: 16–28. https://doi- org.manchester.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.
langcom.2018.12.004
——— (2020a) Guide for staff at women’s aid on working with interpreters. http://m lm.humanities.
manchester.ac.uk/w p-content/uploads/2020/05/ Blog-Post- Guide.pdf
——— (2020b) Guide for spoken language interpreters working with adult survivors of domes-
tic abuse. http://m lm.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/w p-content/uploads/2020/05/ Blog-post-
Interpreter-Guidelines.pdf
190
Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services
——— (2021) “Biopolitics, complicity, and community in domestic abuse support settings: Implica-
tions for interpreter guidance”, in Empirical Studies of Translation and Interpreting: The Post-Structuralist
Approach, Caiwen Wang and Binghan Zheng (eds). New York and London, Routledge: 9 –28.
Toledano Buendía, Carmen, and Maribel del Pozo Triviño (eds) (2014) La interpretación en contextos de
violencia de género. Valencia, Tirant lo Blanch.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen (2015) “Insights from the field: Perception of the interpreter(s) in cases of
gender-based violence”, Cultus 8: 76–95.
191
12
PUBLIC SERVICE INTERPRETING
IN HEALTHCARE
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
Introduction
Together with legal services, healthcare is one of the traditional areas in which the need for
interpreting services is more than obvious. Providing adequate assistance in the absence of a
shared language has been a chief preoccupation on two grounds: the need for hospitals and
clinics to guarantee a basic human right, such as that of access to healthcare; and the high
costs incurred by services when this access is not provided (Ingleby et al. 2012; Hsieh 2015;
Hohenstein and L évy-Tödter 2020). The challenge has acquired new urgency as migration
flows, along with massive c ross-border movements of people travelling abroad for business or
tourism, have heavily changed the patients’ panorama in most of the world.
For a number of reasons, including funding, service organisation, language availability,
and the need to cope with emergencies, healthcare interpreting has historically been provided
by a variety of bilinguals ranging from qualified interpreters to experienced practitioners, to
quickly trained mediators, and to family members or healthcare personnel performing for
the occasion. As a consequence, possibly more than other settings, healthcare interpreting
seems characterised by a considerable diversity in types of abilities and experience of the bi-
lingual participants performing as interpreters. Since the beginning of the new century, this
much diversity has prompted increased interest in research and education.
doctors to deliver correct diagnoses and treatment (Heritage and Maynard 2006; Gill Teas,
Pomerantz and Denvir 2009). Yet, this participation is not easily accomplished even when
language obstacles are not there (Ruusuvuori 2007; Heritage and Lindström 2012). From the
1990s onwards, dialogue interpreting studies of empirical data (y ielded by both interviews/
focus groups and recorded/transcribed interpreted interactions) have exposed the problem
of involving patients and enabling them to contribute, in relevant ways, to their interaction
with clinicians (see Angelelli 2004). Interpreting means helping others to communicate
across language barriers, and this requires profound social and linguistic competencies which
are often overlooked or neglected, especially by service providers or naive commentators.
Eliciting patients’ talk, showing sympathy, asking for or giving clarifications was sometimes
necessary to put the interlocutors in touch (Metzger 1999). Moreover, negotiation of the
interlocutors’ intended meaning seemed often necessary to make sense of the participants’
utterances (Davidson 2002).
Wadensjö’s seminal work (1998) was probably the first to extensively clarify the new
approach. At the beginning of the 1990s, many aspects of interpreting in face-to-face inter-
action were new because the traditional way of looking at interpreting was from a monologic
rather than a dialogic perspective (ibid.: 43–44). The latter view uncovered a full range of
actions which were not normally expected to be performed (and not even considered as
acceptable) in court and conference interpreting – such as coping with features of extempo-
rary talk, repair moves, para-and non-verbal items, implicit requests or unclear statements.
Thus, interpreting was seen to involve two different and yet intersecting activities, namely
translating and coordinating (ibid.: ch.6).
For the healthcare setting, this may mean that interpreters supply additional items to
put the patients at their ease (ibid.: 179–84) or talk briefly to the patients, in their language,
to help them say what they are trying to say (Englund Dimitrova 1997). Simplification of
technical jargon is often involved in rendering clinicians’ talk, and clinicians sometimes ask
interpreters how something may be said or explained in the other language (Gavioli 2015a).
Patients are guided by interpreters to provide details which are likely to be found relevant by
the clinicians, and interpreters take up an active listening role to help patients produce their
stories of illness (see Angelelli 2014; Merlini 2015, and our discussion below). While this be-
haviour may be debatable given the risk of excluding one participant from the conversation
(Valero-Garcés 2007), it highlights a central issue in interpreter-mediated healthcare talk,
namely the need to provide interactional space in particular for the patients to express their
emotions and concerns.
193
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
Literature overview
The literature on public service interpreting in healthcare is by now bulky; the more recent
studies can however be broadly divided into those using data derived from interviews or
focus groups with interpreters, clinicians, and to a much lesser extent m inority-language
patients, and those using recordings of naturally occurring interpreter-mediated encounters.
While the former look into how interpreting is perceived by those who work in/w ith it,
the latter sheds light on the actual practices adopted in communicating via interpreters, al-
though the two approaches can be and often are combined (see e.g. Hsieh 2010; Krystallidou
2013). For the sake of epistemological clarity however, we deal with them separately in our
discussion below.
194
Public service interpreting in health care
both interpreting professionals and healthcare providers still reveal concern for perceived
clashes between what interpreters do and precisely the roles they feel they are expected to
adhere to. Critical issues fall into two categories: (a) conflicting feelings between acting
‘like conduits’ (cf. Roy 1993/2002) versus showing empathy and involvement and (b) the
management of issues related to the medical world versus issues related to the world of the
patients (M ishler 1984). It is worth noting that by far the largest amount of research on per-
ceived contrasting expectations comes from nursing and healthcare journals, possibly show-
ing that such clashes are of particular concern to healthcare organisations.
195
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
these two ‘voices’ is not the doctor’s alone, and clearly involves interpreters’ contribution
in making medicine clear for the patients (A mato 2012: 43). Doctors and interpreters are
thus inevitably called upon to cooperate, and this has been one further object of inquiry in
interviews and focus groups.
One of the earliest accounts of interpreters’ perceptions is found in Angelelli (2004: ch.7).
Her interviews with interpreters highlight a perceived need to both simplify the doctors’
language and their ‘rushed’ instructions (ibid.: 111), and mitigate the impact of patients’
storytelling (ibid.: 109). Interpreters believe that both institutional awareness and medical
knowledge need to be rearranged for the patients to get equal care. For instance, they find
that it may be relevant to inform patients about services available in the hospital, or suggest
that they ask questions since they are often unaware they can (Hsieh 2008). Interpreters may
also support healthcare services by explaining what is involved in prevention or other med-
ical protocols; Madlon-Kay and Smith (2019), for example show that interpreters of Somali
communities in the US often go through the benefits of vaccination to contrast the idea that
it provokes autism.
These findings highlight the participants’ perception that interpreters’ knowledge of
medicine and of the medical system is an important factor, often determining the accuracy
and effectiveness of diagnostic assessment (see also Haralambous et al. 2018). At the same
time, doubts have been raised as to what may be considered distinctive of the clinician’s and
the interpreter’s contributions respectively, thus stirring debate on the extent and scope of
collaboration between the two (Hsieh 2010).
196
Public service interpreting in health care
history taking ( Gavioli 2015b; Gavioli and Wadensjö 2021), and doctors’ explanations
(A ngelelli 2018; Baraldi and Gavioli 2021a, 2021b).
Gavioli and Wadensjö (2021) have shown that when very precise details are sought, cli-
nicians may repeat the same question several times in slightly different ways so as to help
the interpreter find the most effective formulation in the other language. In these triadic
sequences, patients sometimes show they can understand the doctors’ (repeated) question
directly, and interpreters are seen to just support direct understanding by confirming or
repairing occasional troubles. Patients’ ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answers, or simple answers accompanied
by gestures, can similarly be understood by doctors directly, thus only requiring interpreters’
monitoring the achievement of understanding.
Other studies have instead analysed monolingual dyadic sequences where one partici-
pant produces an extended turn. Here, data have shown that these sequences need expert
handling to ensure that all participants are kept on track (Baraldi and Gavioli 2007). While
a doctor may not (and most likely does not) understand the content of the patient’s story
in another language, s/he can probably understand that it is a story told to an interlocutor,
the interpreter, who displays that s/he is listening, by either keeping eye-contact, nodding
or producing continuers. The interpreter’s feedback may thus be considered to perform a
double function: eliciting the patient’s story, and putting the doctor ‘on hold’ waiting for
a rendition of the story to come immediately afterwards (Gavioli 2012: 213). The coordi-
nating and monitoring function of paralinguistic features, including gaze and gestures, in
the management of monolingual sequences, may assume high relevance (Pasquandrea 2011;
Gerwing and Li 2019) and suggest further means of effective coordination.
In parallel with interpreters’ perception that medical language needs simplifying, some
most recent studies focus on the treatment of specialised terminology. Watermeyer, Thwala
and Beukes (2021) observe that some frequently used practices include naming illnesses in
one and then the other language, and providing a lay explanation (e.g. ‘contractions is when
your belly becomes hard’, ibid.: 7). Bolden (2018) analyses two cases. In the first one, the
interpreter displays that she does not know how to translate a technical item and negotiates
the solution with the patient who supplies the item using the medical term in Latin. In the
second case, the interpreter helps the doctor position the patient’s head correctly in the
course of an ophthalmologic test and does so by synchronising her rendition with the doc-
tor’s actions. The collaboration between doctor and interpreter thus succeeds in making the
doctor’s handling of the diagnostic instrument meaningful for the patient.
197
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
198
Public service interpreting in health care
patients’ statements of emotion might lead to the patients’ lifeworld being muted, and to
empathic communication being severely compromised.
199
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
like ‘tell him/her’ or ‘let us explain to him/her’. These devices are seen to recurrently in-
troduce complex or delicate clinicians’ explanations or requests, and seem to acknowledge
the interpreters’ competence in redesigning the service provider’s contribution in ways they
deem appropriate. This provides further evidence that interpreters’ supplementary contri-
butions are often not the result of autonomous choice, and may well be triggered by other
participants’ conversational actions.
These findings suggest that clinicians and patients do not simply offer their contributions
for interpreters’ rendering, they can also elicit particular types of renditions by display-
ing that something is difficult to explain and may be easily misunderstood. In this sense,
interpreters are given and take up responsibility for interpreting by exercising (a nd being
expected to exercise) professional discretion. The rights and onus of such discretion become
part of the activity of talk, as they are negotiated and acknowledged by all participants in
interaction (see also Gavioli and Wadensjö 2021).
200
Public service interpreting in health care
being the most fundamental to unravel the complex interplay between r apport-building and
other motives for action. Indeed, the most interesting finding was that students learnt to
identify where and how empathy works may run counter to the principle of i mpartiality – as
in those cases in which student interpreters empathised with one party while openly d is-
a ligning with the other for (covert) ideological reasons.
In light of the above, it has been argued that developing healthcare student interpreters’
understanding of their own and others’ feelings may push professionalisation a step further.
In an inspiring paper, Niemants (2013) applied the theoretical distinction between an ‘ethics
of conviction’ and an ‘ethics of responsibility’ to the analysis of simulated and authentic
medical interactions. Whereas in class students tend to adhere to an idealised model of in-
terpreting conduct, in real life professional interpreters are seen to take responsibility for
the outcome of the interaction, departing from theoretical guidelines when this is deemed
necessary to achieve overarching communication goals. Hence, the question raised by Nie-
mants is how students can be helped bridge the gap between ‘playing’ roles in the classroom
and ‘taking’ roles in the real world. One answer may be found in the social constructivist
concept of ‘situated learning’ (K iraly 2000; González-Davies and Enríquez-Raído 2016);
by observing, reflecting, and providing feedback on their own and others’ performances,
students will actively contribute to shaping (and transforming) the competencies of their
communities of practice.
Situated learning theories show evident connections with the conceptual transition from
‘interpreter training’ to ‘interpreter education’, as spearheaded by Angelelli (2008), and
expanded upon in Tipton and Furmanek (2016) and Cirillo and Niemants (2017). Once
the narrow confines of instrumentally conceived practical-only training are overcome, the
wider educational goal may realistically become what Skaaden (2019) calls ‘the exercise of
discretion’, and Danou (2007: 56) ‘the art of improvisation’ as reasoned opposition to the
linearity of habit through a long and arduous reflection on one’s professional practice.
Conclusions
While effective medical treatment and humane care clearly go hand in hand, achieving a
successful balance between the two components requires complex communication compe-
tencies. In interpreter-mediated interaction, this complexity is even more manifest because
there is a participant, that is the interpreter, whose task in interaction is to attend precisely
and pre-eminently to communication.
The above discussion has shown that active agency calls for responsible coordination
on two fronts. On the one hand, interpreters need to be acknowledged in their ability to
understand and render appropriately what is said by the other participants (see Baraldi, this
volume). On the other hand, to be recognised as competent and skilled collaborators, inter-
preters must be able to provide suitable solutions which work, visibly and successfully, for all
the participants in interaction (Wadensjö 2018).
In conclusion, the literature on interpreting in healthcare documents how interpreters’
collaboration with clinicians and patients does, in fact, go far beyond a reductive view of
interpreters’ participation. Hsieh (2014: 75–76) provokingly argues that the ‘interpreter-
as-conduit’ metaphor need not be discarded if, and only if, the conduit role is understood
‘not as a passive [one] that simply relays the voices of others but as a means of reaffirming
the primary relationship between healthcare provider and patient. Evidence shows that this
goal can indeed be achieved through interpreters’ active and knowledgeable monitoring
of participants’ talk contributions, cognitive assumptions and emotional perspectives (even
201
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
when these are not verbally expressed), as well as through their making real-time strategic
judgments about whether, when, and how to interpret.
Further Reading
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (eds) (2021a) “W hen clinicians and patients do not speak the same
language”, Health Communication 36 (9). Special issue.
Sarangi, Srikant (ed) (2018) “Interpreter-mediated healthcare encounters”, Communication & Medicine
15 (2). Special Issue.
Both special issues are publications in research journals of communication in health care and are thus of equal
interest to clinicians, providers of interpreting services, and interpreting professionals. At the time of writing,
they offer the most up-to-date insight into interpreter-mediated clinician-patient interaction.
Heritage, John, and Douglas Maynard (2006) Communication in Medical Care. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
A milestone volume in studies of doctor-patient interaction in monolingual talk, highlighting the different
phases of clinical consultations and their main interactional features.
Hsieh, Elaine, and Brenda Nicodemus (2015) “Conceptualizing emotion in healthcare interpreting: A
normative approach to interpreters’ emotion work”, Patient Education and Counseling 98: 1474–81.
The study identifies a number of motives behind interpreters’ emotion work (or lack of it), warns against
concrete risks of emotional mismanagement (especially in the context of diverging sociocultural systems), and
proposes an original normative model to help interpreters evaluate the functions of interlocutors’ expression of
emotions, and choose which therapeutic and interpersonal goals to prioritize in any given situation.
Theys, Laura, Demi Krystallidou, Heidi Salaets, Cornelia Wermuth, and Peter Pype (2020) “Emotion
work in interpreter-mediated consultations: A systematic literature review”, Patient Education and
Counseling 103 (1): 33–43.
Focusing on rapport-building work, the paper highlights the dearth of scientific evidence on verbal and,
particularly, nonverbal semiotic resources – such as gaze, gestures, body orientation, and so on – through which
participants in medical interactions co-construct emotional communication.
Related chapters
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in public service interpreting by Claudio Baraldi
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences: showing evidence at a distance in a video-
mediated courtroom setting by Christian Licoppe
Chapter 13, Challenges and remedies for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments by Charlotta Plejert
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: how authentic data meet simulations for interpreter train-
ing by Natacha Niemants, Jessica P. B. Hansen, and Elizabeth Stokoe.
202
Public service interpreting in health care
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véro-
nique Traverso, and Emilie Jouin
Chapter 23, Interprofessional education … interpreter education: in or and? Taking stock and moving forward by
Demi Krystallidou
References
Amato, Amalia (2012) L’interprete nel contesto medico. Bologna, CLUEB.
Angelelli, Claudia Viviana (2004) Medical Interpreting and C ross-Cultural Communication. Cambridge,
Cambridge University Press.
——— ( 2006) “ Validating professional standards and codes: Challenges and opportunities”,
INTERPRETING: International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 8 (2): 175–93.
——— (2008) “The role of the interpreter in the healthcare setting. A plea for a dialogue between re-
search and practice”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting: Definitions and Dilemmas, Carmen
Valero Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 147–63.
——— (2014) “U h I am not understanding you at all: Constructing (M is)u nderstanding in provider/
patient-interpreted medical encounters”, in Investigations in Healthcare Interpreting, Brenda Nicode-
mus and Melanie Metzger (eds). Washington, Gallaudet University Press: 1–31.
——— (2018) “W ho is talking now? Role expectations and role materializations in interpreter-
mediated healthcare encounters”, Communication & Medicine 15 (2): 123–34.
Angelelli, Claudia Viviana, Niels A gger-Gupta, Carola Green, and Linda Okahara (2007) “The Cali-
fornia standards for healthcare interpreters: Ethical principles, protocols and guidance on roles and
intervention”, in The Critical Link 4: The Professionalization of Interpreting in the Community, Cecilia
Wadensjö, Birgitta Englund Dimitrova and A nna-L ena Nilsson (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 167– 80.
Baraldi, Claudio (2012) “Interpreting as dialogic mediation: The relevance of expansions”, in Coordinating
Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 297–326.
——— (2015) “Coordination and mediation of doctors’ questions and patients’ answers in bilingual
interactions”, in Plurilingualism in Healthcare: An Insight from Italy, Sara Vecchiato, Sonia Gerolimich
and Nickolas Komninos (eds). London, Bica Publishing: 31–56.
——— (2019) “Pragmatics and agency in healthcare interpreting”, in Routledge Handbook of Translation
and Pragmatics, Rebecca Tipton and Louisa Desilla (eds). London, Routledge: 319–35.
Baraldi, Claudio and Laura Gavioli (2007) “Dialogue interpreting as intercultural mediation: An anal-
ysis in healthcare multicultural settings”, in Dialogue and Culture, Marion Grein and Edda Weigand
(eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 155–75.
——— (2021) “Effective communication and knowledge distribution in healthcare interaction with
migrants”, Health Communication 36 (9): 1 059–67.
Barbieri, Viola (2009) “La traduzione della diversità culturale nell’interazione mediata”, in La media-
zione linguistico-culturale: una prospettiva interazionista, Laura Gavioli (ed). Perugia, Guerra: 203–30.
Bolden, Galina (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: Interpreters’ involve-
ment in history taking”, Discourse Studies 2 (4): 387–19.
——— (2018) “Understanding interpreters’ actions in context”, Communication & Medicine 15 (2):
135–49.
Butow, Phyllis, Elisabeth Lobb, Michael Jefford, David Goldstein, Maurice Eisenbruch, Afaf Girgis,
Madeleine King, Ming Sze, Lynley Aldridge and Penelope Schofield (2010) “A bridge between
cultures: Interpreters’ perspectives of consultations with migrant oncology patients”, Support Care
Cancer 20: 235–4 4.
Bylund, Carma and Gregory Makoul (2005) “Examining empathy in medical encounters: An obser-
vational study using the empathic communication coding system”, Health Communication 18 (2):
123–40.
Cirillo, Letizia and Natacha Niemants (eds) (2017) Teaching Dialogue Interpreting. Research-Based Proposals
for Higher Education. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Cox, Antoon (2015) “Do you get the message? Defining the interpreter’s role in medical interpreting
in Belgium”, Monographs in Translation and Interpreting 2: 1 61–84.
Danou, Gérard (2007) Langue, Récit, Littérature dans l’Éducation Médicale. Limoges, L ambert-Lucas.
203
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
Davidson, Brad (2000) “The interpreter as institutional gatekeeper: The social-linguistic role of inter-
preters in Spanish–English medical discourse”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (3): 379–405.
——— (2001) “Questions in cross-linguistic medical encounters: the role of the hospital interpreter”,
Anthropological Quarterly 74 (4): 1 70–78.
——— (2002) “A model for the construction of conversational common ground in interpreted dis-
course”, Journal of Pragmatics 34: 1 273–300.
Delizée, Anne (2018) Du rôle de l’interprète en santé mentale: analyse s ocio-discursive de ses positions subjectives
au sein de la triade thérapeute-patient-interprète, PhD Thesis, University of Mons.
D ysart-Gale, Deborah (2005) “Communication models, professionalization, and the work of medical
interpreters”, Health Communication 17 (1): 9 1–103.
Englund Dimtrova, Birgitta (1997) “Degree of interpreter responsibility in the interaction process in
community interpreting”, in The Critical Link: Interpreters in the Community, Silvana E. Carr, Roda P.
Roberts, Aideen Dufour and Dini Steyn (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 147–64.
Falbo, Caterina (2013) “‘Interprete’ et ‘mediatore linguistico-culturale’: Deux figures professionnelles
opposées?”, in Plurilinguisme et monde du travail: Professions, opérateurs et acteurs de la diversité linguis-
tique, G. Agresti and C. Schiavone (eds). Roma, Aracne: 253–70.
Farini, Federico (2013) “The pragmatics of emotions in interlinguistic healthcare settings”, Research in
Language, 11 (2): 1 63–87.
Felberg, Tatjana and Hanne Skaaden (2012) “The (de)construction of culture in interpreter-mediated
medical discourse”, Linguistica A ntverpiensia – New Series Themes in Translation Studies 11: 95–112.
Fernández, Emilia Iglesias (2010) “Verbal and nonverbal concomitants of rapport in health care
encounters: Implications for interpreters”, The Journal of Specialised Translation 14: 216–28.
Gavioli, Laura (2012) “M inimal responses in interpreter-mediated medical talk”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 201‒28.
——— ( 2015a) “On the distribution of responsibilities in treating critical issues in i nterpreter-
mediated medical consultations: The case of ‘le spieghi(a mo)’”, Journal of Pragmatics 76: 169–80.
——— (2015b) “Doctors’ acknowledgment of patients’ answers: Rendition patterns in interpreter-
ediated, h istory-t aking sequences”, in Plurilingualism in Healthcare: An Insight from Italy, Sara Vec-
m
chiato, Sonia Gerolimich and Nickolas Komninos (eds). London, Bica Publishing: 57–80.
Gavioli, Laura and Cecilia Wadensjö (2021) “Reflections on doctor q uestion – Patient answer sequences
and on lay perceptions of close translation”, Health Communication 36 (9): 1080–90.
Gerwing, Jennifer, and Shuangyu Li (2019) “Body-oriented gestures as a practitioner’s window into
interpreted communication”, Social Science & Medicine 233: 171–80.
Gill Teas, Virginia, Anita Pomerantz, and Paul Denvir (2009) “Pre-emptive resistance: Patients’ par-
ticipation in diagnostic sense-making activities”, Sociology of Health and Illness 32 (1): 1 –20.
González-Davies, Maria, and Vanessa Enríquez-Raído (2016) “Situated learning in translator and inter-
preter training: Bridging research and good practice”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 10 (1): 1–11.
Haralambous, Betty, Jean Tinney, Dina LoGiudice, Sook Meng Leed and Xiaoping Lin ( 2018)
“Interpreter-mediated cognitive assessments: Who wins and who loses?”, Clinical Gerontologist 41
(3): 227–36.
Heritage, John (2012) “Epistemics in action: Action formation and territories of knowledge”, Research
on Language and Social Interaction 45: 1–29.
Heritage John and Douglas Maynard (2006) Communication in Medical Care: Interactions between Primary
Care Physicians and Patients. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Heritage, John and Anna Lindström (2012) “K nowledge, empathy and emotions in a medical encoun-
ter”, in Emotion in Interaction, Anssi Peräkylä and M arja-Leena Sorjonen (eds). Oxford, Oxford
University Press: 2 56–73.
Hohenstein, Christiane, and Magdalène L évy-Tödter (eds) (2020) Multilingual Healthcare: A Global
View on Communicative Challenges. Wiesbaden, Springer Gabler.
Hsieh, Elaine (2008) “I am not a robot! Interpreters’ views of their roles in health care settings”, Qual-
itative Health Research 18 (10): 1 367–83.
——— (2010) “Provider–interpreter collaboration in bilingual health care: Competitions of control
over i nterpreter-mediated interactions”, Patient Education and Counseling 78: 154–59.
——— (2014) “Emerging trends and the corresponding challenges in bilingual health communi-
cation”, in Investigations in Healthcare Interpreting, Brenda Nicodemus and Melanie Metzger (eds).
Washington, Gallaudet University Press: 70–103.
204
Public service interpreting in health care
——— (2015) “Healthcare interpreting”, in Routledge Encyclopedia of Interpreting Studies, Franz Pöch-
hacker (ed). London/New York, Routledge: 177–82.
Hsieh, Elaine, and Eric Mark Kramer (2012) “Medical interpreters as tools: Dangers and challenges
in the utilitarian approach to interpreters’ role and functions”, Patient Education and Counseling 89
(1): 158–62.
Hsieh, Elaine, and Brenda Nicodemus (2015) “Conceptualizing emotion in healthcare interpreting: A
normative approach to interpreters’ emotion work”, Patient Education and Counseling 98: 1474–81.
Ingleby, David, Antonio Chiarenza, Walter Devillé, and Ioanna Kotsioni (eds) (2012) Inequalities in
Health Care for Migrants and Ethnic Minorities. Antwerp, Garant.
Kiraly, Donald (2000) A Social Constructivist Approach to Translator Education. Manchester, St. Jerome.
Krystallidou, Demi (2013) The Interpreter’s Role in Medical Consultations as Perceived and as Interac-
tionally Negotiated, PhD dissertation, University of Ghent, [Online]. https://w ww.researchgate.
net/publication/2 66839166_The_interpreter’s_role_in_medical_consultations_as_perceived_
and_as_interactionally_negotiated_a_study_of_a_Flemish_hospital_setting_using_interview_
data_and_video_recorded_interactions (accessed 1 January 2017).
Krystallidou, Demi, Aline Remael, Esther. de Boe, Kristin Hendrickx, Giannoula Tsakitzidis, Sofie
van de Geuchte, and Peter Pype (2018a) “Investigating empathy in i nterpreter-mediated simulated
consultations: An explorative study”, Patient Education and Counseling 101 (1): 33–42.
Krystallidou, Demi, Celine Van De Walle, Myriam Deveugele, Evangelia Dougali, Fien Mertens,
Amélie Truwant, Ellen Van Praet and Peter Pype (2018b), “Training ‘doctor-minded’ interpreters
and ‘interpreter-minded’ doctors. The benefits of collaborative practice in interpreter training”,
Interpreting 20 (1): 126–4 4.
Krystallidou, Demi, Carma Bylund, and Peter Pype (2020) “The professional interpreter’s effect on
empathic communication in medical consultations: A qualitative analysis”, Patient Education and
Counseling 103 (3): 521–29.
Leanza, Yvan (2005) “Roles of community interpreters in pediatrics as seen by interpreters, physicians
and researchers”, Interpreting 7 (2): 167–92.
Leanza, Yvan, Isabelle Boivin, and Ellen Rosenberg (2013) “The patient’s lifeworld: Building mean-
ingful clinical encounters between patients, physicians and interpreters”, Communication & Medicine
10 (1): 13–25.
Leanza, Yvan, Elias Rizkallah, Thomas M ichaud-Labonté, and Camille Brisset (2017) “From con-
cern for patients to a quest for information: How medical socialization shapes family physicians’
representations of interpreters”, Interpreting 19 (2): 232–59. https://doi.org/10.1075/i ntp.19.2.04lea
Li, Shuangyu, David Pearson, and Sarah Escott (2018) “Language barriers within primary care con-
sultations: an increasing challenge needing new solutions”, Education for Primary Care 21: 385–91.
M adlon-Kay, Diane, and Emily Smith (2019) “Interpreters’ knowledge and perceptions of childhood
vaccines: Effect of an educational session”, Vaccine 38: 1216–19.
Martin, Anne and Isabel Abril. Martí (2008) “Community interpreter self-perception: A Spanish case
study”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting, Carmen Valero Garcés and Anne Martin (eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 203–30.
Mason, Ian (2006) “On mutual accessibility of contextual assumptions in dialogue interpreting”, Jour-
nal of Pragmatics 8 (3): 359–73.
——— (2009) “Role, positioning and discourse in f ace-to-f ace interpreting”, in Interpreting and Trans-
lating in Public Service Settings, Raquel de Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle Perez and Christine Wilson (eds).
Manchester, St Jerome: 52–73.
Meeuwesen, Ludwien, Fred Tromp, Barbara C. Schouten, and Johannes A.M. Harmsen ( 2007)
“Cultural differences in managing information during medical interaction: How does the physi-
cian get a clue?”, Patient Education and Counseling 67: 183–90.
Merlini, Raffaela (2009a) “Seeking asylum and seeking identity in a mediated encounter: The projec-
tion of selves through discursive practices”, Interpreting 11 (1): 57–92.
——— (2009b) “Interpreters in emergency wards: An empirical study of doctor-interpreter-patient
interaction”, in Interpreting and Translating in Public Service Settings, Raquel de Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle
Perez and Christine Wilson (eds). Manchester, St Jerome: 89–114.
——— (2015) “Empathy: A ‘zone of uncertainty’ in mediated healthcare practice”, Cultus: The Journal
of Intercultural Mediation and Communication, 8: 27–49.
——— (2019) “Through the looking-glass: Reflections of/on empathy in healthcare interpreter edu-
cation”, Cultus Journal 12: 220–45.
205
Laura Gavioli a nd Raffaela Merlini
——— (2020) “Dialogue interpreting”, in Encyclopedia of Translation Studies. 3rd ed., Mona Baker and
Gabriela Saldanha (eds). London/New York, Routledge: 147–52.
Merlini, Raffaela, and Roberta Favaron (2005) “Examining the ‘voice of interpreting’ in speech pa-
thology”, Interpreting 7 (2): 263–302.
Merlini, Raffaela, and Mariadele Gatti (2015) “Empathy in healthcare interpreting: Going beyond the
notion of role”, The Interpreter’s Newsletter 20: 139–60.
Metzger, Melanie (1999) Sign Language Interpreting: Deconstructing the Myth of Neutrality. Washington,
Gallaudet University Press.
Mishler, Elliot George. (1984) The Discourse of Medicine: The Dialectics of Medical Interviews. Norwood, Ablex.
Niemants, Natacha (2013) “From role-playing to role-taking. Interpreter role(s) in healthcare”, in
Interpreting in a Changing Landscape, Christina Schäffner, Krzysztof Kredens and Yvonne Fowler
(eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 305–19.
Ochs, Elinor (1989) “Introduction”, The Pragmatics of Affect [Special issue], Text 9 (1): 1–5.
Ozolins, Uldis (2016) “The myth of the myth of invisibility?”, Interpreting 18 (2): 273–84. https://doi.
org/10.1075/i ntp.18.2.06ozo
Pasquandrea, Sergio (2011) “Managing multiple actions through multimodality: Doctors’ involvement
in i nterpreter-mediated interactions”, Language in Society 40: 455–81.
Penn, Claire, and Jennifer Watermeyer (2012) “W hen asides become central: Small talk and big talk
in interpreted health interactions”, Patient Education and Counseling 88: 391–98.
Pittarello, Sara (2009) “Interpreter-mediated medical encounters in North Italy: Expectations, per-
ceptions and practice”, The Interpreters’ Newsletter 14: 59–90.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2008) “Interpreting as mediation”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting, Valero
Carmen Valero Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 9 –26.
Raymond, Chase Wesley (2014a) “Epistemic brokering in the interpreter-mediated medical visit:
Negotiating ‘patient’s side’ and ‘doctor’s side’ knowledge”, Research on Language and Social Interaction
47 (4): 426–46.
——— (2014b) “Conveying information in the interpreter-mediated medical visit: The case of epis-
temic brokering”, Patient Education and Counseling 97: 38– 46.
Roy, Cynthia (1993/2002) “The problems with definitions, descriptions and the role metaphors of
interpreters”, in The Interpreting Studies Reader, Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam Shlesinger (eds).
London, Routledge: 345–53.
Ruusuvuori, Johanna ( 2007) “ Managing affect: Integration of empathy and problem- solving in
healthcare encounters”, Discourse Studies 9 (5): 597–622.
Skaaden, Hanne (2019) “Invisible or invincible? Professional integrity, ethics, and voice in public
service interpreting”, Perspectives 27 (5): 704–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/0907676X.2018.1536725
Sturman, Nancy, Rebecca Farley, Fernanda Claudio and Patricia Avila (2018) “Improving the effec-
tiveness of interpreted consultations: Australian interpreter, general practitioner and patient per-
spectives”, Health and Social Care in the Community, 26: 233–40.
Tebble, Helen (2012) “Interpreting or interfering?”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting,
Claudio Baraldi and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 2 3–44.
Tipton, Rebecca and Olgierda Furmanek (2016) Dialogue Interpreting: A Guide to Interpreting in Public
Services and the Community. London/New York, Routledge.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen (2007) “Doctor-patient consultations in dyadic and triadic exchanges”, in
Healthcare Interpreting, Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam Shlesinger (eds). Amsterdam, Benjamins: 35–51.
Vargas Pelaez, Alavaro, Sarah Ramirez, Chavely Valdes Sanchez, Shady Piedra Abusharar, Jose
Romeu, Connor Carmichael, Soraya Bascoy, Rose Baron, Ariana P ichardo-Lowden, Nathalia
Albarracin, Claire Jones, and Patricia Silveyra (2018) “Implementing a medical student interpreter
training program as a strategy to developing humanism”, BMC Medical Education 18: 141. https://
doi.org/10.1186/s12909-018-1254–7
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (2018) “Involvement, trust and topic control in interpreter-mediated healthcare encounters”,
Communication & Medicine 15(2): 165–76. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.38681
Watermeyer, Jennifer, Zamokuhle Thwala, and Johanna Beukes (2021) “Medical terminology in in-
tercultural health interactions”, Health Communication 36 (9): 1115–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10410236.2020.1735700
Zorzi, Daniela, and Laura Gavioli (2009) “La gestione dell’affettività in conversazioni mediate da
interprete. Alcune riflessioni su attività di coordinamento e traduzione”, in La mediazione linguistico-
culturale: una prospettiva interazionista, Laura Gavioli (ed). Perugia, Guerra: 171–202.
206
13
CHALLENGES AND REMEDIES
FOR INTERPRETER-MEDIATED
DEMENTIA ASSESSMENTS
Charlotta Plejert
Introduction
War and conflict, as well as globalization generally, have resulted in many societies becom-
ing increasingly linguistically and culturally complex. Whereas this development primarily
has many advantages, it also poses challenges for public sectors, such as health-care services,
particularly when changes take place rapidly. One case-in-point is the dramatic migration to
Europe from the Middle East in and around the year 2015, which has put language and cul-
ture into the limelight. In countries that have often been perceived as largely culturally and
linguistically homogeneous, such as the northern countries of Europe (note, however, in-
digenous populations in these countries), the t wenty-first-century political crisis was not the
only challenge to impact on the health-care sector. People who immigrated to these coun-
tries in the 1960s are currently becoming old and are increasingly in need of help for age-
r elated conditions. One of these conditions is dementia. Dementia, in its most common form
Alzheimer’s disease, is characterized by successive memory loss, but also by other symptoms,
like confusion, anxiety, and a gradual loss of language and motor skills (A lzheimer’s Society
2021). Despite the fact that many immigrants over time become proficient second language
speakers of the majority language (by majority language, an official language of a country
is intended here, for example, Swedish in Sweden), very many require the assistance of an
interpreter in settings in which it is important that the degree of understanding between in-
terlocutors is adequate, for example, when seeking care. In addition, in the case of dementia,
there is some evidence that suggests that languages acquired later than one’s first language/s
will be negatively affected by the degenerative disease earlier than the mother tongue/s
(De Bot, Plejert and Simonsen 2020). This means that also in cases of persons who used to
be proficient second language speakers, communication may be enhanced if an interpreter
mediates the encounter or is accessible as a support at points of communicative difficulties.
In many societies, there is a general lack of interpreters, and not the least a shortage of
interpreters with adequate training when it comes to mediating quite specific clinical tasks,
such as tests of cognitive functioning, or tests of speech and language skills. In addition, de-
spite ethnocultural diversity in many countries, clinical assessment tools for adults are often
based on populations who are reasonably educated. Lack of adaptations of clinical tools to
levels of literacy, and/or cultural aspects is an issue that might affect the ease by which a test is
208
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
Literature overview
Even if there exists a fair number of studies on interpreted encounters in which interactional
approaches have been employed (e.g. Wadensjö 1998, 2004, 2018; Bolden 2000; Angelelli
2004; Hsieh 2008; Bridges et al. 2011; Raymond 2014), there is a dearth in research in which
formal clinical evaluations and measurements involving people with cognitive or language
problems are in focus. Work on interpreting in psychotherapy and mental health is, however,
an exception, where the role, behaviour, and impact of the interpreter, and the relationship
between therapist, patient, and interpreter have been explored (e.g. Bot 2003, 2005, 2014;
Miller et al. 2005). The work by Langdon and Saenz (2016) and Saenz and Langdon (2019)
on interpreting within speech and language pathology and audiology offers h ands-on advice
for training interpreters as well as clinicians when it comes to how to prepare for meditated
test administration. This work will be returned to in the discussion part of this chapter.
A handful of studies report how clinicians find it challenging to implement cognitive
tests using interpreters, while at the same time maintaining the professional quality of the
diagnostic process. By and large, empirical information about psychometric validity and
reliability of tests when administered with the help of an interpreter is lacking (e.g. Plejert
et al. 2015; Majlesi, Antelius and Plejert 2017; Majlesi and Plejert 2018; Migrationsskolan
2019). Van De Mieroop, Bevilacqua and Van Hove (2012) reported how an interpreter,
during the administration of the Mini Mental State Examination (M MSE), a commonly
used screening tool in dementia assessments, made additions to the occupational therapist’s
initial test questions. For example, when the occupational therapist asked what season it was,
the interpreter turned the initial open question into a close-ended question with optional
answers, providing the patient the opportunity to choose between spring, summer, autumn,
and winter, despite the fact that this was not part of the original test question. The modified
rendition by the interpreter thus made the task easier for the patient, potentially influencing
the patient’s answer and how it was assessed. Three further studies investigate interpreting
in relation to cognitive screening for dementia (Plejert et al. 2015; Majlesi et al 2017; Majlesi
and Plejert 2018), the latter of which focuses on participants’ (not least the interpreter’s) n
on-
verbal conduct and its potential impact on test results (Majlesi and Plejert 2018).
Many of the reviewed studies above point out that interpreters very often lack knowledge
and understanding of the bases for neuropsychological tests; why a task has a certain form
(for instance, why ask someone to mention as many fruits as possible within 60 seconds?),
and why it must be administered in sometimes quite restricted ways (for example, why not
modify the formulation of a question just slightly when a patient does not seem to under-
stand?). This kind of challenge has been discussed previously in Plejert et al. (2015), and
Majlesi and Plejert (2018) and will also be illuminated in the current chapter. Another issue
that adds to the complexity of formal assessments deals with the lack of suitable tests in all
languages needed. In memory-, as well as in speech and language clinics, it is not uncommon
that interpreters are asked to translate texts (prima vista, or sight interpreting) from tests not
available or validated in the patient’s best language (Langdon and Saenz 2016). There are
many reasons for clinicians to avoid asking for this service. For example, words in different
languages do not generally have exact equivalents in another language. There may also be
words to be tested that refer to a common object within one culture, but the item is uncom-
mon in another one (so the task is culturally biased in a way that makes it difficult for the
patient to respond appropriately).
There may also be differences between dialects, and often words have many synonyms, so
if one interpreter on one occasion translates a piece of text, or names an object in a picture in
209
Charlotta Plejert
a test in a specific way, and a new interpreter is enrolled at another point for the same test and
patient, it is quite unlikely that the material is translated in the same way, which of course
may heavily affect what conclusions can be drawn from the patient’s test results from one
time to the next. Interpreters are sometimes not informed beforehand that the appointment
might involve sight interpreting, and very many are not trained for that task. This may put
the interpreter in an uncomfortable position of either accepting a task, which goes against
their professional oath of conduct, or telling the clinician that the task cannot be carried out,
which might then become a dilemma for the patient.
Thus, lack of clinician training in working with interpreters, and the fact that they may
have a simplified view upon important lexical and structural differences between languages
are crucial, too, not just interpreter training. In a report from Migrationsskolan (2019), a
centre in southern Sweden focusing largely on health issues in relation to elderly immigrants,
several challenges are revealed connected to lack of interpreter training, biased test materials,
and limited clinician training in working with interpreters. The findings and conclusions
from that report are quite relevant for this chapter and will be discussed in terms of implica-
tions for h ealth-care professionals and interpreters concerning training, but also of the estab-
lishment of routines along the entire diagnostic pathway, from booking an interpreter, the
mediated encounter proper, to debriefing and evaluation of an interpreted event (Langdon
and Saenz 2016). Some hands-on suggestions for improvements for interpreter-mediated,
formal clinical assessments will therefore be brought forth at the end of this chapter.
210
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
and delayed recall of five nouns); visuospatial abilities (clock-drawing and three-dimensional
cube copy); executive functions (adapted Trail Making B, phonemic fluency and verbal
abstraction); attention, concentration, and working memory (target detection, serial sub-
traction, digits forward and backward); language (confrontation naming, repetition of two
syntactically complex sentences, and phonemic fluency); and orientation to time and place.
The data is part of a larger corpus for a study of memory assessments involving ethnic
minority persons with suspected dementia, carried out at Centre for Dementia Research
(CEDER) at Linköping University, Sweden, during the years 2 011–16. All participants were
informed about the study and gave their consent concerning participation, which included
the permission to record their interaction. For anonymity, the terms used here are patient,
interpreter, and clinician. The project was ethically approved by a regional board for ethical
vetting.
The method used for this chapter is multimodal interaction analysis (Mondada 2006).
This method derives from CA (Sidnell and Stivers 2013) and has the m icro-level of inter-
actional engagement in focus. It aims to diverge from an observer’s perspective, in favour of
an emic one, in which evidence of what people do in interaction (video-recorded) can only
be determined based on the relationship between an initiative and how it is responded to
in interaction from the interlocutors’ perspective. Multimodal means that any, ever so subtle
communicative cues may be of relevance to participants, so additionally to verbal exchanges,
gesture, gaze, body posture, and prosody are all of potential interest. To give a very simple
example: If Ada meats her friend Bob in the street, she will most likely say ‘Hello how are
you?’, and Bob will most likely respond something like ‘Great thanks – how are you?’. At
this point, if recorded, analysts can understand that there is a greeting exchange going on
that both participants orient to, and the interaction is likely to proceed with a new initiative
taken by one of the participants, for example ‘W hat’s up?’. Had Bob’s response, however,
been absent (i.e. no response at all, verbal or n
on-verbal), it would clearly have been notably
absent and signalled some kind of problem (for instance of hearing, or perhaps of a social na-
ture). The analytical process is performed during repeated and very careful watching, coding
and highly detailed transcriptions of video sequences of interaction. Relevant categories that
emerge through the analytical process are subsequently routinely displayed in data-sessions,
in which experienced analysts collaboratively discuss them; a kind of qualitative validation
forum, one might say.
211
Charlotta Plejert
212
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
25 (0.5)
26 PAT: ch[ane]
how [much]
27 INT: [mez]aka (0.3)aw fanna beka ( 0.7) agar
28 zhenaftitay kh[o]↑
[do] (
0.3) like this (( knocks on table)) if you (
0.7) heard
it ok[ey]
29 PAT: [a]
30 (0.9)
31 INT: harf (0.4)å s- du [säger]
letter (0.4) s- you [say]
32 PAT: [a]
translation into idiomatic English in italics. Minimal responses (generic to many languages)
are not translated. N on-verbal practices of relevance for analysis are provided within double
parentheses in the English translation.
In line 01, the occupational therapist gives a part of the formal instructions as stated in
the material at hand, but her account is not perceived by the interpreter as information to
be rendered to the patient straight on at this point. This is possibly due to the fact that the
instruction is immediately followed by a comment about conducting the task in Swedish
(line 02), and a reservation let’s see (line 05), and this talk is not clearly directed to any of
the interlocutors, albeit the occupational therapist is looking at the patient. The patient
precedes the interpreter, wondering what the occupational therapist is saying in Swedish.
In lines 07 and 08, the occupational therapist elaborates on her instruction, and this elab-
oration is prosodically and syntactically tied to the initial information (line 01) beginning
with and (and as soon as I say the letter a: etc. line 07). The talk is now clearly directed towards
the patient by means of gaze as well as the use of you (I want you to knock on the table lines
07, 08). The occupational therapist also accompanies her utterance with a bodily enactment
(K indell et al. 2013), knocking with her right hand on the table within the visual space of
the patient. The patient again initiates repair in terms of a clarification request, directed to
the interpreter (what does she say line 12). This may be due to the time passing without any
rendition from the interpreter (line 04, 0.4s pause, lines 09–11, two pauses and a minimal
response from the interpreter, but no rendition). In line 14, the interpreter starts rendering
the instructions previously given in Swedish by the occupational therapist. In the continua-
tion of the excerpt (lines 14–32), there is a negotiation between the interpreter and the pa-
tient of what is to be done. Of particular interest is the interpreter’s comment in line 21 that
a is a letter, preceded by the patient’s query concerning the choice of language (in Swedish↑)
with a rise. The interpreter then exemplifies what is expected from the patient (line 24),
accompanied by a similar movement as the one previously used by the occupational thera-
pist (line 08), knocking with his right hand on the table. Again, the patient initiates repair
(l ine 26) and the interpreter repeats his prior verbal and n on-verbal instruction (k nocking);
however, this time not defining the sound [ɑ:], but stating it signalling established shared
knowledge.
It is not just to the patient that the task at hand might seem somewhat unclear. The inter-
preter in this particular case has not received any prior information about how the task is to
213
Charlotta Plejert
be carried out – and for what purpose. What language to use has also been an issue several
times in the talk between the occupational therapist and the interpreter in relation to differ-
ent sub-tasks in the test (see 1b, lines 02, 05). This may explain why some negotiations are
necessary between the interpreter and the occupational therapist before the interpreter can
make any renditions to the patient. This is illustrated in excerpt 1b:
Excerpt 1b
As can be observed in line 33, the interpreter asks for clarification about the expected conduct
of the patient. Again, the language to be used is an issue. This is of particular interest in relation
to the matter of letters contrasted with sounds, as the occupational therapist will produce letters
in a string of speech as they are pronounced in Swedish. Since alphabets are pronounced dif-
ferently in distinctive languages, this might potentially mean that the interpreter is not certain
whether or not he should also, in fact, render the occupational therapist’s string of letters. The
fact that the patient is simply to knock is confirmed by the occupational therapist in a partly
completed explanation of the procedure of saying aa out loud (line 35). Due to recording qual-
ity, it has not been possible to disentangle exactly what is said by the interpreter in line 36. The
issue that the letters are to be produced in Swedish is picked up by the interpreter as important
information for the patient. In his subsequent rendition, despite the occupational therapist not
saying what language she is using, he adds that the letters will be said in Swedish. This is illus-
trated in Excerpt 1c, when the occupational therapist eventually provides the patient with the
first complete instruction (lines 37–39), followed by the rendition (lines 40–41):
As can be noticed, in this episode, the patient uses a: as a minimal, confirmatory response
(lines 42 and 46). Her confirmations, however, do not necessarily mean that she has under-
stood the instructions rendered by the interpreter. The evidence for this claim is found in
the way the interpreter repeats (and elaborates) his renditions over and over again, so some
semiotic cues, for example, the patient’s facial expression and/or gaze, are treated by the
interpreter as signalling that clarification is needed, even if the patient’s confirmatory verbal
response would typically be perceived as conveying understanding. As before, the clinician
as well as the interpreter use a knocking gesture to illustrate the requested non-verbal rec-
ognition of the letter from the part of the patient. At the point when the clinician knocks
(line 39), the patient, however, is looking at the interpreter and possibly does not take notice
of her gesture, whereas the interpreter’s subsequent knock (line 44) is produced within the
patient’s visual space. 20 turns at talk are omitted after line 47 in the above excerpt. What
happens in those turns is continued explanation work by the interpreter, primarily consisting
of repetitions of the nature of the task and how the patient is expected to act. None of it is
rendered in Swedish to the occupational therapist. Neither does the occupational therapist
interfere. Eventually, the interpreter assesses the patient to be ready to conduct the task and
proclaims to the occupational therapist to go on with the task proper:
214
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
Excerpt 1c
Excerpt 1d
68 INT: varsågod
go ahead
69 (0.4)
70 OT: så nu börjar [( x)]
so now starts[( x)]
71 INT: [alan] bezhnava!
[now] listen!
72 OT: .hh ef::: ( 0.6) bi::↑ (0.4) a:::↑ (
0.9) si::↑ (0.5)
73 em:::(0.7) en::↑ (0.7) a::↑ (
1.0) a::↑ (
0.9) .t .hh=
74 INT: =zhenaftet
= did you hear?
75 (0.3)
215
Charlotta Plejert
The perhaps most noteworthy aspect of Excerpt 1d are the many silences between the sounds
in the occupational therapist’s turn (lines 72–73), several of which are longer than half a sec-
ond, and the ones following the sound /ɑ:/ are particularly extended. This, in addition to
prolonging the vowels in general, provides the patient with ample opportunities to respond
and knock on the table when /ɑ:/ is produced towards the end of the turn. However, what
cannot be known at this point is whether the lack of response is due to the patient perhaps
having cognitive difficulties is hard of hearing, or is a result of not understanding the task at
hand. The issue of hearing is addressed by the interpreter (line 74), but his check does not
receive any confirmatory or rejecting response from the patient, so the interpreter is not
provided any input on whether the patient cannot hear or has difficulties understanding the
task. The occupational therapist also acts as if she is not entirely certain that the patient has
really understood what to do, and, whether cognitive decline potentially affects her short-
term ability of remembering instructions. This is evident in Excerpt 1e:
Excerpt 1e
Anecdotal evidence suggests that it is not uncommon that people with dementia are asked
questions that explicitly refer to their memory abilities, such as do you remember [X] like the
occupational therapist does in excerpt 1e (line 76). In this clinical setting, it may be a way for
216
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
the occupational therapist to check if it is the patient’s memory that affects her ability to carry
out the task, or whether it is a matter of finding the task difficult to understand. It is, however,
noteworthy, that this question is not rendered to the patient. Instead, the interpreter trans-
forms the occupational therapist’s question and instruction (line 76) into a directive (line 78),
followed by a repetition of the instruction and an embodied demonstration once more, as he
knocks on the table (l ine 84). After confirmatory tokens by the patient (l ine 86) as well as the
occupational therapist (l ine 88), the interpreter d ouble-checks that the patient has understood
(line 90), which she confirms, this time not just using a, but the word yeah (line 91). After a
few more lines (92–107 omitted), the occupational therapist makes a new try.
Excerpt 1f
217
Charlotta Plejert
The occupational therapist starts producing the letters with vowel prolongation, this time
not leaving quite as long silences in between sounds as previously (lines 108–9). The first
reaction comes after the second letter, where the patient, after a pause a bit longer than half a
second, conducts a gesture which is not a knock, but she puts the palm of her right hand flat
down on the table in front of her in a tapping fashion (line 110), and then withdraws it to its
original position of both arms crossed in front of her chest. This gesture does not seem to be
acknowledged by the occupational therapist, since she is looking down at the test material in
front of her and she continues producing the letters in a prosodically successive way as a list.
The interpreter does not render anything at this point, although there is a possible place for
a rendition (line 113) before the occupational therapist continues with the list. The patient
provides a minimal verbal response in the turn following the occupational therapist (lines
114–15).
Subsequently, an interesting change takes place, as the patient tries out a new way of par-
ticipating in the task. In her turns following the occupational therapist, she herself provides
the sound [ɑ:], with prolongation and a rise in a similar fashion as the occupational therapist
(line 119); a kind of prosodic accommodation to signal orientation towards a common ac-
tivity (Szczepek Reed 2006). The patient also explicitly points out that she identifies a: in a
response to the occupational therapist’s production of the sound (line 121–22), and, again,
the occupational therapist refrains from taking the turn (cf. the 1.2s pause in line 123). At this
point, the interpreter could of course render what the patient said, but instead, he explains to
the patient that this is the point for her to knock on the table (line 124). In fact, the patient
once more produces a gesture that is a light tap in front of her (l ine 126), but on the one hand,
the occupational therapist is not fully aware of what the patient and interpreter are negotiat-
ing, on the other, her gaze is directed downwards at the test materials. It is thus not possible
for her to attend to the patient’s presumably successful response to the task. As the occupa-
tional therapist provides the pronunciation of yet another letter (line 127), the interpreter
218
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
initiates repair of the patient’s gesture (the flat palm and too light a tap), producing a copy
of the way the occupational therapist had illustrated the knock; a firm and hard tap with his
fist (fi ngers bent) on the surface of the table (line 129). Now, the exercise continues in the
modified form, with the patient displaying an understanding of repeating similar sounds as
the occupational therapist before her, with vowel prolongation and a rise (lines 132–45),
with a slight break in the pattern when the patient responds to the [ɑ:] provided (line 136)
with a short response token (line 137). This is appreciated by the interpreter as a recognition
of the letter, and he advices the patient to knock (line 139), but before finishing his turn,
the occupational therapist continues producing sounds (line 140). Again, the patient reacts
to the sound [ɑ:] (line 141), but she does not knock on the table, and the interpreter does
not act this time. In the continuation, the patient returns to producing prolonged sounds
in a similar fashion as the occupational therapist, and eventually, the occupational therapist
terminates the activity.
219
Charlotta Plejert
the task, there is also proof of points where she, in fact, does manage to produce the asked
for action, that is, to knock on the table when noticing the target sound. However, since her
knock is produced rather slowly so that the occupational therapist oftentimes has started to
produce the sound of the next letter on her list, the patient’s adequate response does not seem
to be noticed by the occupational therapist. The way she knocks also does not conform to the
gesture initially modelled by the occupational therapist. This is picked up by the interpreter,
who not only refrains from rendering some verbal information that might have assisted the
clinician in acknowledging the patient’s recognition of the target sound, but he also conducts
repair on the way the gesture is executed. This is potentially due to his noticing that the oc-
cupational therapist does not seem to acknowledge the patient’s gesture when she conducts it
with a flat hand, and too lightly. Maybe, therefore, he focuses on assisting the patient in how
to produce a proper knock rather than rendering her existing verbal and non-verbal responses.
All of this, in turn, can be related to the interpreter’s (a s well as the patient’s) unfamiliarity
with the purpose of the task at hand; something which will be discussed next.
220
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
test at hand. This also goes for his not pointing out that the patient, in fact, was providing a
tapping gesture as a response to the sound, when the occupational therapist appeared not to
see this (probably due to her often looking down at the test material).
It is not intended here to put any blame on the interpreter, nor on the occupational thera-
pist. If any person without a special knowledge about the purpose of the test in question pon-
ders a bit upon the task, it probably seems rather alien to most. The interpreter had worked in
appointments for this patient before, since he was proficient in her specific language variety
(which is ideal), but those appointments had comprised medical h istory-taking, with a bilin-
gual relative also present. Medical h istory-taking is more like an informal interview, where
the clinician asks questions about the patient’s experiences and symptoms, and the patient or
a relative/peer answers. It may also be easier for the clinician to avoid using complex medical
terms in that activity.
For the formal assessment in this case, practical circumstances affected the possibility to
meet before the appointment to discuss tasks with the occupational therapist, neither did the
interpreter know in advance that test materials in Swedish were to be used. The conditions
for him to perform at his best were therefore also negatively affected in several ways.
221
Charlotta Plejert
the occupational therapist in the showcase was trained in working with an interpreter, being
careful always gazing at the patient, addressing her using the first person, and speaking in
reasonably short utterances and at a slow pace, to ease the work of the interpreter. However,
the contextual affordances related to the educational bias of the test, and other circum-
stances, hampered the outcome of the encounter in several ways. Some issues about how to
deal with such confounding factors are dealt with below.
Briefing-Interaction–Debriefing (BID)
As mentioned several times throughout this chapter, applying the BID routine when an
appointment that involves formal, clinical assessment, is to be mediated, would be one fairly
simple way of enhancing the situation for interpreters, clinicians as well as patients. That
way, interpreters would gain an insight into what materials are to be used, their purposes,
and whether or not the interpreters are willing to conduct any translations of texts (even if
this should generally be avoided), or if they are trained in prima vista interpreting. However,
this is still an issue when it comes to assessment material that preferably should have been
validated clinically, for example, concerning language, age, and cognitive status of patients,
so a language translation by an individual interpreter before a single appointment, is from a
clinical perspective not sufficient in terms of being evidence based, despite its perhaps being
more easily managed in a mediated situation.
In memory clinics in Sweden, there is often an ambition to try to achieve a p re-appointment
talk with interpreters, generally a little bit before the patient arrives (Plejert et al. 2015). How-
ever, clinicians attest that there are often practical, such as interpreters needing to catch a
certain bus, or organizational circumstances, like large number of patient appointments, that
come in the way, and sometimes also economic factors, such as increased expenses for a lon-
ger interpreter assignment (Plejert et al. 2015). For the same reasons, it is not always possible
to have any d e-briefing where the clinician and interpreter go through different aspects of
the interpreted event in retrospect. In lack of specifically trained memory interpreters, as in the
case of the programme of Migrationsskolan (2019) described above, or, of course assessments
performed by a skilled bilingual clinician, the BID routine would nonetheless be an improve-
ment in contrast to insufficiently trained or unprepared interpreters, and clinicians relying on
perhaps biased test materials (which might not be revealed due to lack of renditions), and an
unrealistic trust in the competence of the interpreter.
Further reading
Alzheimer Europe (2018) The Development of Intercultural Care and Support for People with Dementia from
Minority Ethnic Groups. https://w ww.alzheimereurope.org/Ethics/Ethical-issues-i n-practice/2018-
Intercultural-care-a ndsupport/(language)/eng-GB (accessed 19 January 2022).
This report provides an overview of challenges and prospects related to ethnic minority persons with dementia
from a European perspective. It covers issues concerning cultural and educational bias in relation to dementia
assessment processes, including challenges relating to interpreter-mediated encounters in that setting.
Langdon, W. Henriette, and Terry I. Saenz (2016) Working with Interpreters and Translators: A Guide for
Speech-L anguage Pathologists and Audiologists. San Diego, Plural Publishing.
This guide provides a comprehensive overview of the B riefing-Interaction-Debriefing procedure for inter-
preted encounters within speech and language pathology and audiology. However, its content is clearly relevant
for areas beyond these fields.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
This book is seminal for anyone with a theoretical and methodological interest in a dialogical approach to
interpreting.
222
Challenges for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments
Related chapters
Chapter 2, The ambiguity of interpreting: Ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters by Kristina
Gustafsson.
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason.
Chapter 8, Public service interpreting in court – f ace-to-face interaction, by Philippe S. Angermeyer.
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences by Christian Licoppe.
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli, and Raffaela Merlini.
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen, and Elizabeth Stokoe.
Transcription conventions
really ↑: rise
ye:::a : prolonged sounds
-: cut off word
=: speech immediately latched on to the previous utterance
°mhm°: word or utterance pronounced quietly or soft
.hh: i n-breath
(.): m icro-pause (less than 0.2 sec.)
(0.4): pause
[yea]
[mm]: overlapping speech
((k nocks)): non-verbal action
!: indicates directive
Italics: Idiomatic translation into English
References
Alzheimer’s Society (2021) The Progression of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. https://w ww.alzhei
mers.org.uk/about- dementia/s ymptoms- a nd- d iagnosis/how- dementia-progresses/progression-
a lzheimers-d isease-dementia (accessed 19 January 2022).
Angelelli, V. Claudia (2004) Medical Interpreting and C ross-Cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
Bolden, Galina (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: interpreter’s involve-
ment in history taking”, Discourse Analysis 2 (4): 387–19.
Bot, Hanneke (2014) “Role models in mental health interpreting”, in Interpreting and Translating in
Public Service Settings. Policy, Practice, Pedagogy, Raquel De Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle Perez and Christine
Wilson (eds). New York/Routledge: 115–26.
——— (2005) Dialogue Interpreting in Mental Health. Amsterdam, Rodopi Publishers.
——— (2 003) “T he myth of the uninvolved interpreter in mental health and the development
of a t hree-person psychology”, in The Critical Link 3. Interpreters in the Community, Louise
Brunette, Georges Bastin, Isabelle Hemlin, and Heather Clarke (e ds). Amsterdam, John Ben-
jamins: 37–47.
Bridges, M. Susan, Cynthia K. Y. Yiu, and Coleman, P. McGrath (2011) “Multilingual interactions
in clinical dental education: A focus on mediated interpreting”, Communication and Medicine 8 (3):
197–10.
Corsaro, A. William (1982) “Something old and something new. The importance of prior ethnogra-
phy in the collection and analysis of audiovisual data”, Sociological Methods & Research 11 (2): 145–66.
De Bot, Kees, Charlotta Plejert, and Hanne Gram Simonsen (eds) (2020) Multilingualism and Ageing. An
Overview. The Hague, Brill Academic Publishers.
Hsieh, Elaine (2008) “‘I am Not a Robot! Interpreters’ views of their roles in health care settings”,
Qualitative Health Research 18: 1367–83.
Jefferson, Gail (2004) “Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction”, in Conversation Analysis:
Studies from the First Generation, Gene H. Lerner. (ed). Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 13–31.
223
Charlotta Plejert
Kindell, Jacquline, Karen Sage, John Keady, and Ray Wilkinson (2013) “Adapting to conversation
with semantic dementia: Using enactment as a compensatory strategy in everyday social interaction”,
International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 48 (5): 497–507.
Kammarkollegiet. https://w ww.kammarkollegiet.se/engelska/start (accessed 19 January 2022).
Majlesi, Ali Reza, and Charlotta Plejert (2018) “Embodiment in tests of cognitive functioning: a study
of an i nterpreter-mediated dementia evaluation”, Dementia-the International Journal of Social Research
and Practice 17 (2): 138–63.
Majlesi, Ali Reza, Eleonor Antelius, and Charlotta Plejert ( 2017) “ Epistemic negotiations in
interpreter-mediated dementia evaluations: The co-operative role of patients’ relatives”, in Multi-
lingual Interaction and Dementia, Charlotta Plejert, Camilla Lindholm and Robert W. Schrauf (eds).
Bristol, Multilingual Matters: 74–102.
Migrationsskolan (2019) Rapport [report]. Inga om men eller varför: att främja säker och jämlik kognitiv
utredning genom tolk, Region Skåne 2019.
Miller, E. Kenneth, Zoe L. Martell, Linda Pazdirek, Melissa Caruth, and Diana Lopez (2005) “The
role of interpreters in psychotherapy with refugees: an exploratory study”, American Journal of Ortho-
psychiatry 75 (1): 27–39.
Mondada, Lorenza (2006) “Video recording as the reflexive preservation and configuration of phe-
nomenal features for analysis”, in Video Analysis: Methodology and Methods, Hubert Knoblauch, Bernt
Schnettler, Jurgen Raab and Hans-Georg Soeffner (eds). Frankfurt, Peter Lang: 51–68.
Nasreddine, S. Ziad (2003–2014) The Montreal Cognitive Assessment MoCA. www.mocatest.org (accessed
19 January 2022).
Nielsen, T. Rune, Kurt Segers, Valérie Vanderaspoilden, Ulrike Beinhoff, Lennart Minthon, Anna
Pissiota, Peter Bekkhus-Wetterberg, Guro Hanevold Bjørkløf, Magda Tsolaki, Mara Gkioka, and
Gunhild Waldemar (2019) “Validation of a European Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test Bat-
tery (CNTB) for evaluation of dementia”, International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 34 (1): 144–52.
Nielsen T. Rune, and Kasper Jørgensen (2013) “Visuoconstructional abilities in cognitively healthy
illiterate Turkish immigrants: A quantitative and qualitative investigation”, The Clinical Neuropsy-
chologist 27 (4): 681–92.
Plejert, Charlotta, Eleonor Antelius, Maziar Yazdanpanah, and T. Rune Nielsen (2015) “There’s a
letter called Ef. On challenges and repair in interpreter-mediated tests of cognitive functioning in
dementia evaluations: A case study”, Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology 30 (2): 163–87.
Raymond, W. Chase (2014) “Conveying information in the interpreter-mediated medical visit: The
case of epistemic brokering”, Patient Education and Counselling 97: 38– 46.
Roger, Peter, and Chris Code (2011) “L ost in translation? Issues of content validity in interpreter-
mediated aphasia assessments”, International Journal of S peech-Language Pathology 13 (1): 61–73.
Saenz, I. Terry, and Henriette W. Langdon (2019) “Speech-language pathologists’ collaboration with
interpreters: Results of a current survey in California”, The International Journal for Translation &
Interpreting Research 11 (1): 43–62.
Sidnell, Jack, and Tanya Stivers (2013) The Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Vol. 121. Chichester,
West Sussex, John Wiley & Sons.
Szczepek Reed, Beatrice (2006) Prosodic Orientation in English Conversation. Basingstoke, Palgrave
Macmillan.
Van De Mieroop, Dorien, Giovanni Bevilacqua, and Lotte Van Hove (2012) “Negotiating discursive
norms: Community interpreting in a Belgian rest home”, Interpreting 14 (1): 23–54.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (2018) “Involvement, trust and topic control in interpreter-mediated healthcare
encounters”, Communication & Medicine 15 (2): 165–76.
——— (2004) “Dialogue interpreting. A monologising practice in a dialogically organised world”,
Target 16 (1): 105–204.
224
14
PUBLIC SERVICE INTERPRETING
IN SOCIAL CARE
Dorien Van De Mieroop, Antoon Cox and Koen Kerremans
Introduction
id-September 2019 a Belgian newspaper reports that the yearly budget allocated to public
M
service interpreting (PSI) has been exhausted and that counter to tradition, no additional
funds will be provided by the government. The newspaper article quotes a tragic case of a
victim of domestic violence who had to rely on a friend of her husband, the alleged perpetra-
tor of the abuse, to act as an interpreter during her conversation with a social service provider
(Beel 2019). This painful example is just one of the many situations in which the assistance
of a professional interpreter would have been desirable. This is of course not a solely Belgian
problem, nor a problem specific of the social care context, as the n on-availability of profes-
sional interpreters is a ubiquitous phenomenon that is often caused by general reasons such
as a lack of financial resources and of local interpreter availability, depending on the specific
language for which an interpreter is sought (Pöllabauer 2012; Pomeroy and Nonaka, 2013).
Nevertheless, in social care this may sometimes even be more problematic. More than three
decades ago, Freed already pointed at ‘the need for social workers to work with interpreters
as professional teams so that the social workers understand not only the words but also the
nuances of the language and culture of non-English-speaking clients’ (Freed 1988). Yet,
recent research still draws attention to the fact that social workers may sometimes not only
be insufficiently aware of professional interpreters’ added value compared to alternative op-
tions (Pöllabauer 2012; Pomeroy and Nonaka, 2013), but also that they sometimes view the
presence of interpreters in a negative way, for example, perceiving them as potential barriers
in relationship-building processes with their clients (K riz and Skivenes, 2010). This chapter
aims to discuss these and other aspects surrounding interpreting in social care contexts, of
which we first delineate the scope (see “Epistemological considerations”). In the Literature
review section, we review the state-of-the-a rt literature on interpreting in these contexts,
highlighting its main findings. Then we address some of the critical issues that constitute
potential avenues for future research (see section “Critical issues and topics”) and end with a
few concluding remarks.
Epistemological considerations
Delimiting the area of social care is complex due to its coverage of a wide spectrum of differ-
ent activities, with vague boundaries and a fragmented governance structure. In this chapter,
we consider social care as the provision of help, care and protection from harm to people
who need additional support. These people may range from children to adults who may be
vulnerable due to factors such as physical or mental disability or (chronic) illness, migration
or socio-economic background, or age. The types of support offered to these people may be
w ide-ranging, going from offering physical protection and safety to help with homework,
personal care, administrative matters and so on. For example, people experiencing poverty
may interact with social workers in social welfare centres to discuss financial issues. They
may engage in interactions concerning work permits with community counsellors or con-
cerning job opportunities with consultants at public employment services. They may seek
solutions for school bills with school counsellors and care coordinators or explore housing
options with the local housing department and so on. All these different types of interactions
constitute different ‘activity types’ (Levinson 1992), each with their own particular goals,
level of formality, set of structural properties, expectations regarding allowable contributions
etcetera. Yet, despite this high variability, the provision of help to people in need is central
in social care interactions. Importantly, this provision of help is largely language-based, as is
also the case in many other public service settings (for instance, medical or legal settings). As
Hall and Valdiviezo (2020: 17) argue, social workers are language workers in the sense that
they operate ‘in a community created by language and are immersed in language, which is
their principal medium for doing their work’. Thus, a social worker’s language competence
‘can make a significant difference in building an effective working alliance with a client of a
different culture, background, and demographics’ (Hall and Valdiviezo 2020: 18).
While a high proportion of social care is provided informally and unpaid by relatives, friends
or neighbours, this chapter will primarily consider social care that takes place within institutional
contexts. However, it is important to note that we do not limit ourselves only to social workers as
providers of social care, as a large portion of the wide spectrum of different social care activities
is carried out by various types of professionals (as the examples below will illustrate). Moreover,
we will especially zoom in on situations in which care provision is challenged by the presence of
language barriers or the lack of sufficient language competences. Perhaps because interpreting in
social care covers such a wide variety of contexts, there are countries in which it seems to remain
less regulated than interpreting in other settings. For instance, in the UK, interpreters working
in the justice sector must be registered with a state-regulated register of PSIs (Lucas 2020). In
the EU, Directive 2010/64/EU establishes the right to interpretation and translation in criminal
proceedings. In US healthcare institutions, the provision of language assistance free of charge to
foreign-speaking patients has been mandatory and regulated since 2000. And while in Sweden,
interpreting in public service (including social care) is regulated (cf. The Administrative Pro-
cedure Act (2017:900) 2017)), interpreters can only be certified as legal or medical interpreters,
but not in the domain of social care interpreting. Hence, in the social care sector, the regulation
of interpreting is often a complicated matter, which, in practice, often results in care providers
having to rely on informal interpreters without formal qualifications.
Literature review
Pöllabauer (2012: 214) observes that the interpreting studies literature lags behind in studies
on social service and welfare institutions as compared to studies on interpreting in medical
226
Public service interpreting in social care
and legal settings. Moreover, given the wide variety of types of social care, as discussed
above, the – indeed
relatively scarce – literature
is dispersed over a broad range of social
care settings, such as housing and municipal services (Pöllabauer 2012), care for the elderly
(Freed 1988; Van De Mieroop, Bevilacqua and Van Hove 2012) or other vulnerable people
(Berthold and Fischman 2014), child welfare (K riz and Skivenes 2010; Lindsay et al. 2014;
Maiter et al. 2017; Westlake and Jones 2018) and child protection work (Chand 2005; Hum-
phreys, Atkar and Baldwin 1999; Sawrikar 2015; Alaggia, Maiter and Jenney 2017). These
studies usually draw on in-depth interviews with social workers, focus group sessions, and
in some cases direct observations of counselling sessions. They usually take the perspective
of social service workers and emphasize the problems they encounter when working with
non-native speaking clients, rather than looking at the experiences of service users requiring
interpretation services. This reflects the broader discourse on language policy in social care
that often looks at linguistic diversity as a ‘problem’ rather than a ‘resource’, or at language
support policies as a tool to overcome a n on-native speaker’s ‘personal deficit’ rather than as
an affirmation of a ‘human right to equal treatment’ (Harrison 2007).
In particular, the narrative around the use of interpreters is often a problematizing one,
which highlights, above all, the challenges related to poor-quality interpreting (Westlake
and Jones 2018; Lucas 2020). For example, drawing on interviews with child welfare workers
in Norway and England, Kriz and Skivenes (2010: 1358) report that ‘language barriers and
the use of interpreters result in information deficits’, but also other disadvantages are cited,
such as ‘more curtailed relationship-building processes, feelings of mistrust and practical
obstacles’ (on the issue of relationship-building processes, see section below). Along similar
lines, a study by Gustafsson, Norström and Höglund (2019: 22–23) of Swedish social services
and health centres based on an online survey of public service staff, supplemented with data
obtained from group interviews, revealed that although the service providers involved in the
study assumed that professional interpreters were trained, had special language skills and fol-
lowed a code of ethics, they also expressed ‘d istrust towards interpreting services, claiming
that they often find the interpreting conducted by children and relatives to be more reliable
and competent than that of professional interpreters’.
Other studies also highlight interpreters’ shortcomings and how these should be addressed.
For example, drawing on qualitative interviews with clients and service providers, Sawrikar
(2015) highlights a range of challenges related to the use of interpreters, distinguishing be-
tween problems at the level of interpreters, of service providers, of clients and of resources.
Humphreys, Atkar and Baldwin (1999) carried out semi-structured interviews with child
protection workers, case file analysis and direct observation of interpreter-mediated counsel-
ling sessions with Asian families, which led them to identify problems at the level of planning
and availability of professional interpreters (particularly for home visits), their effectiveness
(w ith professional interpreters sometimes acting as gatekeepers), and their suitability (for in-
stance, in situations where the gender of the interpreter plays a role). Chand (2005) arrives at
similar conclusions based on a review of the literature on the use of professional interpreters
in child protection work. Each of these three papers emphasizes the key role of interpreter
training as well as of the training of service providers to work with interpreters.
With regard to training of interpreters, there is some – though fairly limited – literature
explaining which standards professional interpreters in social care need to prepare for. Berthold
and Fischman (2014) propose specific elements for interpreter training beyond language flu-
ency, including attention for contextual elements (for example, how to recognize symptoms of
clients suffering from trauma and mental illness) in settings of c ross-cultural communication.
They further recommend that interpreters working in social care should adhere to a similar
227
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
code of ethics as used in the (mental) health domain, focusing on maintaining professionalism,
impartiality, boundaries and confidentiality; and avoiding taking up conflicting roles. Pölla-
bauer (2012) illustrates the importance of such standards based on recordings of interpreter-
mediated encounters and in-depth interviews with social workers. She documents a situation
in which a special pool of interpreters was set up to serve refugees in Austria in the context
of an EU-funded project. Interpreters received training that was limited in duration and did
not include language-specific training or interpreting-specific elements regarding professional
roles and interpreting ethics. While social service providers working with those interpreters
considered the project as successful, they nevertheless pointed at shortcomings in terms of im-
partiality and undesirable interventionist attitudes of interpreters, leading to concerns about in-
terpreters acting as ‘gatekeepers’ and hampering social service providers’ access to information.
While appropriate training of interpreters is critical for service quality, the literature has
also considered the lack of attention for working with interpreters in social work training
(Tipton 2016). Based on a review of the literature, for instance, Berthold and Fischman (2014)
emphasize the critical need for effective collaboration between social workers and interpret-
ers in contexts where social workers interact with survivors of severe human-perpetrated
trauma speaking another language. While the authors propose a set of curriculum compo-
nents for interpreters working with trauma survivors, they at the same time argue that social
workers need to ‘understand and manage the dynamics of working with an interpreter, be
aware of the appropriate and inappropriate use of interpreters, and be informed about inter-
preter selection criteria’ (Berthold and Fischman 2014: 3). A related study by Westlake and
Jones (2018), based on audio recordings of home visits by social workers, finds that there is
a set of good practices that social workers can draw on to improve the quality of communi-
cation in interpreter-mediated service interactions. Better communication outcomes were
underpinned by an assertive approach to working with interpreters, which ‘enabled workers
to structure sessions in ways that involved the client more fully in conversation’ (Westlake
and Jones 2018: 1406). Their study shows that – rather than ‘up-skilling’ interpreters –
‘identifying elements of good practice that social workers have more control over may hold
potential for improving the quality of service for non-native speakers’ (ibid.). Finally, based
on focus group sessions with workers in the child and women protection sectors, Alaggia,
Maiter and Jenney (2017) provide some practical examples of the consequences of service
providers’ lack of training on working with interpreters, and in particular of their difficulties
to manage concerns relating to confidentiality and gender-and culturally sensitive issues.
Even though the challenges in terms of interpreting quality and the training of
interpreters – as well as of social workers to work with interpreters – are important, we will
not discuss these further in this chapter as these are general issues that are relevant beyond
social care contexts for which we refer the reader to the first and last part of this volume
containing more general discussions of these topics. Instead, in the next section, we pick up
four critical issues that have been touched upon in the literature on current practices in inter-
preted social care interactions, namely professional interpreting versus alternative solutions,
professionals combining interpreting and social work, rapport-building across language bar-
riers and, in conclusion, interpreters as potential gatekeepers in social care interactions.
228
Public service interpreting in social care
- Fragments 1 and 2 are both social care interactions that were recorded ‘at the margins’
of two medical interactions between Russian-speaking patients and Flemish specialist
doctors. These doctor-patient interactions were interpreted by a multilingual profes-
sional who worked full-time at the hospital’s support service for ‘language assistance
and intercultural mediation’. This service involves on the one hand social care work, for
instance helping patients with the hospital-related paperwork, and on the other hand
language support, for instance interpreting d octor-patient interactions. In line with
how she labels herself, we will refer to the multilingual professional of this service as
‘intercultural mediator’, but it is important to emphasize that part of her job consists of
interpreting and that she thus has a double role in the context of this hospital.
- Fragments 3 and 4 come from one interpreted Italian-Flemish encounter in a Belgian home
for the elderly. In this interaction, the – recently arrived – resident and the head nurse engage
in a general discussion that focuses on the social side of this new resident’s stay at the rest
home, rather than on medical issues. In particular, the resident’s expectations regarding all
sorts of aspects of his stay at the rest home are discussed. The interaction is interpreted by
a bilingual social worker who works both at the current rest home and the new resident’s
previous rest home. He also has a double role at these rest homes, namely of social worker
and of interpreter, but we will refer to him as ‘interpreter’ in this chapter, as he emphasized
himself that this was the role he was taking up in these interactions.
We analyse these extracts using a discourse analytical approach, in which attention is paid to
the discursive as well as sequential features of the conversations, as we hope that this will help
to provide a critical, nuanced and multifaceted perspective on the topics that are broached in
the subsections below. It is important to note that our approach is descriptive; we do not aim
to provide clear-cut advice on what the ‘best’ way is to deal with the targeted challenges, first,
because the pros do not always easily outweigh the cons; second, because what may be a pro in
one social care context may be a con in another and finally, because pros and cons cannot be
straightforwardly linked to certain interactional circumstances, conditions or distinctions be-
tween categories, such as the use of professional or non-professional interpreters. This is largely
because of the extreme variability of encounters taking place in social care contexts, as sketched
above, for which general advice would entail making problematic simplifications. Instead, our
aim is to shed a nuanced and multifaceted light on these issues separately, but also on how these
may often – but not always – be interrelated, sometimes in contradictory ways.
229
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
be made quickly and in an ad hoc way. Given the often impromptu character of arranging
interpreting assistance, it is not surprising that service providers regularly prefer alternative
solutions that are easy to resort to as they are readily available (Humphreys, Atkar and Bald-
win 1999). One type of alternative solution is the use of technological bridging tools such
as Google Translate. In a case study of service providers’ practices and beliefs during mul-
tilingual service encounters in the city of Brussels (Belgium), the public service providers
indicated that while they consider professional interpreters to be essential bridging persons
during interactions with non-native speakers, they also confirmed that they use tools like
Google Translate to support interactions between social care providers and service users
(Kerremans et al. 2018). They believe technology to be a successful complementary language
aid in cases where speakers were to a certain level able to communicate with each other using
a lingua franca. Social care providers also reported on asking informal interpreters such as
family members or friends of the service seeker to provide assistance during a multilingual
service encounter (Kerremans et al. 2018), a practice that is also discussed in other studies
(Lucas 2016).
Notably, these ‘solutions’ are often not only regarded as practical but also as an advantage.
Especially in relation to social care contexts, the practice of relying on informal interpreters
is often evaluated positively (cf. Gustafsson, Norström and Höglund 2019). In an editorial
written for the journal Social Work, Pomeroy and Nonaka (2013) argue that this positive eval-
uation of using informal interpreters may be related to the specific goal or nature of social
care – namely ‘to provide a safe environment and to build a trusting relationship despite lin-
guistic differences’ – due to which family members or friends of the client may seem ideally
suited to act as interpreters (Pomeroy and Nonaka 2013: 103). It is thus not so surprising that
Pollock (2020) finds that patients and service users consistently seem to prefer an informal
interpreter. At the same time, however, these authors also point to the disadvantages of this
practice. For example, Pomeroy and Nonaka (2013) refer to confidentiality issues – of which
the example cited at the very start of this chapter is a case in point. Also the potentially
limited command of the majority language by these informal interpreters is mentioned as a
disadvantage (Pollock 2020), especially as it may go unnoticed by the primary interlocutors.
This can then lead to so-called ‘false fluency’ situations where service providers mistakenly
believe that the other party understood their message (Cox et al. 2019).
Overall, we believe that whether to use professional interpreters or draw on a lternatives –
a nd, in the latter case, which a lternative – is not a question that has one straightforward an-
swer given the variability of types of interactions, interactional goals and specific constraints
and affordances of the different contexts and types of social care in which the creation of
trust and the maintenance of social relations is, in some cases, a delicate issue that is of the
utmost importance. Yet, what seems unequivocally clear is that it is important to raise aware-
ness regarding the consequences and potential pitfalls of the choices that are being made by
social care providers in this respect.
230
Public service interpreting in social care
231
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
interactions, such a window of opportunity may also be ‘too good’ for (relationally oriented)
small talk, as it allows for discussions that are much more goal-oriented than would be
possible for multi-tasking interlocutors in non-interpreted interactions (Van De Mieroop
2016: 309, emphasis in the original). This is exactly what we see in fragment 1. As soon as
the doctor starts orienting towards the phone call, this results in the emergence of two c o-
occurrent conversations (‘schisming’, see Egbert, 1997). Of interest to us here is that during
the doctor’s phone call, the patient initiates a monolingual ‘social care’ interaction with the
intercultural mediator.
In the first lines, the doctor announces the need for an urgent medical examination of the
patient and then starts orienting to making arrangements for this ‘eye fundus’ via the phone.
This is clear in line 3, in which she first d ouble-checks the phone number (‘seventy eighty’),
then starts clicking the phone keys and finally voices a ‘to do’-list for this in a lower voice. All
these activities clearly mark that the medical interaction is temporarily put on hold. As these
signals are understandable non-verbally, the patient does not wait for a translation of the doc-
tor’s turn. Instead, he immediately s elf-selects and initiates a topic that is not directly related to
the medical interaction at hand but is rather a topic that could emerge in a ‘social care’ interac-
tion, that is the potential extension of his term in Belgium. While he initially formulates this
issue in a relatively low v oice – hence
the many unclear formulations marked as (x xx) in line
5 – and asks a general question (‘how will it be’, line 5), he subsequently reformulates the topic
in more specific terms (cf. ‘the thirty-first’, line 6) and also suggests a potential solution for the
issue (‘they always prolong it’, line 7). The intercultural mediator then confirms the need for
this extension (line 8) and further accounts for this in lines 1 0–12. This interaction continues
beyond this fragment (not shown here for reasons of space), but it is clear from the part that
we have shown here that this is not merely small talk between the intercultural mediator and
the patient while they are waiting for the doctor to finish making arrangements. Rather, it
presents an example of how the p atient – who knew the intercultural mediator from various
previous e ncounters – prompted the latter to switch from one professional role to another,
and we could observe here, in particular in line 8, that this shift took place in a seamless way.
This is not at all a unique example in our data. It is not surprising that c are-receivers use
each potential window of opportunity to make the most of the time in which there is an
intercultural mediator present. This, of course, may sometimes pose a challenge to all the
interlocutors involved, as it may become quite confusing to distinguish which role is taken
up at what particular time and which information is relevant when and for whom. We could
observe an example of this kind of role mixing in the following fragment, which occurs in a
situation highly similar to the previous one, namely while the doctor – a liver specialist this
time – is on the phone. In this case, the patient wants the intercultural mediator to ask the doc-
tor why he is still not on the waiting list for a kidney transplant after having been in treatment
for three years. While this issue is clearly medically relevant, the patient adds to this that he
needs to be put on the waiting list to solve issues with his passport.2 Thus, while the former –
medical – issue clearly addresses the intercultural mediator in her role as interpreter, the latter
aspect of the patient’s turn – the implications for his passport – is oriented to the social care
part of her job. When the doctor puts down the phone, the intercultural mediator immediately
self-selects and starts translating the patient’s question as we see in the first line of fragment 2.
We can observe that in this translation of the patient’s question, the intercultural media-
tor only selects the medical information, namely whether this patient is eligible for a kidney
transplant and whether he has now been put on the waiting list. So, in this translation, there
is no trace of the other part of the patient’s preceding turn, which was about the implications
of this medical issue for his passport, and we can thus observe here that the intercultural
232
Public service interpreting in social care
Fragment 2 –
Encounter 2, interaction between doctor (
Dr) and
intercultural mediator (IM)
mediator triages the information that the patient provides in relation to the role that she is en-
acting at a particular moment in time. This ‘information triage’ strategy may be considered as
a way to deal with the duality of her professional role as intercultural mediator.
Finally, these fragments also demonstrate that ‘social care’ interactions do not necessarily
always take place in w ell-defined interactional environments that are strictly oriented to
social c are-goals, but rather that many of these interactions occur at the fringes of other
interactions, in which the time that is available may be used to the maximum extent. One
can easily imagine that such interactions occur in the waiting room of a medical practice or
when moving from one scheduled meeting to another, but also, as we observed here, even
while a phone call by a doctor briefly suspends the medical interaction in which the partic-
ipants are involved. This illustrates that the reality of social care may sometimes be highly
fragmented. On the one hand, this may be regarded as an efficient use of available resources,
but on the other hand, it poses an additional challenge for multilingual professionals who
thus face the dilemma of dealing with the input they receive either ‘a s social care providers’
233
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
(thus focusing on the content of the patient’s questions) or ‘a s interpreters’ (thus focusing on
rendering this content).
Fragment 3 – Encounter
3, interaction between head nurse (
HN),
interpreter (I) and resident (R)
234
Public service interpreting in social care
We see in the initial lines the end of the head nurse’s turn in which she explains the rules
for smoking on the premises. In line 4, the interpreter first acknowledges the importance
of these rules and then starts interpreting the head nurse’s turn. This is quite a lengthy turn
(lines 5 –11), which ends with a word search for the correct ending of the word polmone
(‘lungs’, see line 18). While this turn is fairly technical and clearly oriented to the goal of
235
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
the interaction, namely making sure that this new resident knows the rest home’s rules, the
latter responds to this explanation by initiating a personal story about his daughter’s aversion
to smoking indoors. While this is of course topically related to the preceding talk (both
referring to smoking), it is unrelated to the rest home context and thus the resident’s talk is
not relevant for furthering the task-related goals of the interaction. Nevertheless, the inter-
preter produces a turn in which he aligns with this shift to a personal story and even adds
a – partially unintelligible – account for the behaviour of the resident’s daughter (see line 21:
‘because…’). In the next turn, the resident aligns with this and finishes the interpreter’s sen-
tence, after which the latter starts translating the resident’s story (see line 27). Furthermore,
he ends his turn with a hypothetical factual statement about the resident, namely that he will
‘a lways have smoked’. This is acknowledged by the head nurse, but before she can add any-
thing else, the interpreter continues in this small talk frame and asks the resident whether this
hypothetical statement is correct, as we see in line 30, after which the small talk continues a
bit longer (not shown here for reasons of space).
So, in this fragment, we first of all observed a switch to more relationally oriented talk,
which – even though it is topically related – does not further the transactional goals of the
interaction. In particular, this small talk is initiated by the resident and encouraged by the
interpreter (see his initiation of an account in line 21 and his follow-up question in line 30).
Hence, in this case, we could observe that the interpreter not only facilitates the establish-
ment of rapport between all the interlocutors involved by interpreting the resident’s story, in
spite of its non-task related nature, but that he also stimulates the continuation of this type of
non-task-oriented talk, by aligning with it and asking a follow-up question. Thus, while in
some studies (K riz and Skivenes 2010; Lindsay et al. 2014), attention is drawn to interpreters’
presence as hindering the establishment of rapport in social care interactions, we have shown
with this extract from our data that the opposite is also possible. In particular, we illustrated
that interpreters may not only facilitate rapport, but that they may also contribute actively to
establishing and maintaining social ties. As they sometimes know many details of the care-
receiver’s personal situation, they may actually act as catalysts of the establishment of rapport
which is of particular importance to social care interactions (see also the recommendation by
Westlake and Jones 2018: 1405). In this subsection, we thus offered a nuanced perspective on
the potential influence of the presence of a third party on r apport-building.
Interpreters as gatekeepers
That participants taking up the role of interpreters may act as gatekeepers who, for example,
omit a particular piece of information in their translation, is a well-documented fact (for an
overview of studies on interpreters as gatekeepers, see Pöllabauer 2012). An example of this
was presented in fragment 2 above, in which the intercultural mediator did not translate the
patient’s reason for his request about being put on the waiting list for a kidney transplant.
Above, we explained this as a triage strategy to deal with the double role that this hospital’s
employee of the ‘language assistance and intercultural mediation’-service had, but this omis-
sion may also be viewed as a gatekeeping strategy. Such omissions are quite common in our
data and many of them seem to be oriented to efficiency improvement, namely that the –
often limited – time that is available for a particular encounter is only used for the transmis-
sion of information that is deemed relevant for the interaction’s specific t ask-related goals.
While this type of gatekeeping is of course most well-known, gatekeeping may also work
in the other direction. Because interpreters are g o-betweens between the different l anguage-
r elated parts of the interaction, they may also add information that they perceive as missing
236
Public service interpreting in social care
Fragment 4 – Encounter
3, interaction between head nurse (HN) and
interpreter (I)
1 I maar hij hij als ik het goed begrijp hij heeft dus echt
but he he if i understand it correctly so he really has
2 hij is vroeg wakker [maar dat krijg ik hem niet uitgelegd
he is awake early [but i can’t manage to explain that to him
3 HN [ja: ja
[ye:s yes
4 I inderdaad dat hij om vijf uur
indeed that he at five o’clock
5 en als hij alleen zou gaan douchen
and if he would go take a shower alone
6 ‘
k=ben ‘
k=ben zelf iets aan ‘ t voorstellen
i=am i=am proposing something myself
7 HN ma daarbij weet ik ni of hij [da kan
but next to that i don’t know if he [can do that
8 I [weetnie of hij da kan (.)
[don’t know if he can do that (.)
9 misschiens kan hij dat (.) nee ‘ k=denk het niet
maybe he can do that (.) no i=don’t think so
10 allora c’è un’ altra possibilità luigi
so there is another possibility luigi
in one of these parts. Moreover, due to their often multiple roles, they may have knowledge
that none of the other interactional participants have. We see an example of this in the
following fragment, which was extracted from the same intake interview with a new rest
home resident as discussed above. At this point in the conversation, they are talking about a
problem that has arisen in the first couple of days, namely that the resident would really like
to bathe before breakfast, but that there are no spots available for early morning bathing as-
sistance. After a long discussion about this, the talk ends up in an impasse (prior to this frag-
ment). Then the interpreter proposes a potential solution himself, as we see in fragment 4.
In lines 1–4, the interpreter formulates a summary of the issue, as such ensuring a cor-
rect understanding of the situation (see line 1). Then, in line 4, he breaks off his sentence
and in the subsequent line formulates a potential solution, namely that the resident would
shower on his own in the morning. Interestingly, he then explicitly marks this as a proposal
of ‘h imself ’ (lines 6) and by means of this metacomment, he hints at the unusual nature of
this proposal in his current role as interpreter. In the subsequent turn, the head nurse does
not explicitly pick up on this final part of the interpreter’s turn (line 6) but reacts to the in-
terpreter’s proposal and formulates a counterargument (‘I don’t know if he can do that’, line
7). The interpreter first mirrors this formulation in overlap and then reflects on this out loud
(line 9). While he concludes this reflection with a negative evaluation (‘I don’t think so’, line
9), he nevertheless continues and embarks on a translation of his proposal to the resident (of
which we see the start in line 10, the rest is omitted for reasons of space but it may be relevant
237
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
to know that the discussion ends with the decision to try out the interpreter’s suggestion).
So, even though he initially oriented to the marked nature of his own contribution – see the
metacomment in line 6 – and in spite of an important counterargument that was raised by
the head nurse, the interpreter does not wait for a ratification by the nurse before formulating
this proposal to the resident.
Hence, it is due to the interpreter’s contribution that a potential solution is found. Impor-
tantly, it is because of the fact that this interpreter is actually a social worker acting as an
interpreter, that he has knowledge of many aspects of the issue (that is to say his knowledge
of the care home’s facilities, of the resident’s potential abilities, of the previous arrangements
with this resident in another care home and of the rules and options of the current care
home). We have thus seen that the presence of this interpreter/social worker was crucial
for the furthering of the interactional goals and that his contribution had an empowering
effect for the resident who is thus able to try out a solution that is more tailored to his
needs. Overall, we can conclude that especially in social care interactions, in which dou-
ble roles are quite common and interpreters are often highly knowledgeable parties in the
interaction, gatekeeping may occur in several directions. Next to selecting information as
a way of doing recipient design (see fragment 2), interpreters may also contribute to the
interaction, such as by adding information or, as is the case here, by suggesting ways to
reach t ask-related goals based on their extensive knowledge of various aspects surrounding
the interaction, ranging from the personal situation of the care-receivers to the rules and
regulations of various institutions.
Concluding remarks
This chapter has pointed out that the domain of social care provides a vast array of different
contexts with a similarly wide range of different types of interactions and communication
means. Moreover, interpreting in social care is much less studied than, for instance, inter-
preting in medical and legal settings. Nevertheless, we believe that social care contexts offer
many promising avenues for future research. While some of the critical issues discussed in
this chapter raise potential policy concerns that are very much in line with those observed
in other areas of PSI, including in the healthcare sector, some issues can be considered as
related to the nature of social care in which the creation of a safe environment and the es-
tablishment and maintenance of trusting relationships are central (cf. Pomeroy and Nonaka
2013: 103). Social care contexts thus require careful reflections on the implications of a va-
riety of decisions that have to be made each time, such as those referred to above: what the
implications are of deciding to assign professional interpreters or informal interpreters, or
even go for another alternative; what the effects are of the double roles of n on-professional
interpreters or of acting as an interpreter oneself; how to foster the establishment of rapport
that tends to be so important in social care context (cf. Westlake and Jones 2018); and how
to negotiate the implications of gatekeeping. Each of these decisions entails its own fluid
set of advantages and disadvantages, which have to be carefully weighed depending on the
particularities of each social care situation. For example, while informal interpreters likely
increase the prevalence of role confusion, they can also help care-receivers to feel more at
ease and support the establishment of rapport between the care provider and receiver, which
tends to be especially important in social care. Yet, this is not always the case, which is im-
mediately clear when rehashing the example at the start of this chapter, of the case in which a
victim of domestic violence had to rely on a friend of her husband, the alleged perpetrator of
238
Public service interpreting in social care
the abuse, for interpreting assistance during her conversation with a social service provider.
This illustrates the delicate balancing acts these decisions present, which would benefit from
further academic scrutiny. In sum, there are still many questions in relation to the domain of
interpreting in social care contexts, which offer exciting new research opportunities render-
ing insights that are set to be important for, but also beyond, social care.
Further reading
Gustafsson, Kristina, Eva Norström, and Petra Höglund (2019) “Language interpreting and broker-
ing in Swedish public service institutions: The use of children for multilingual communication”,
Revista de Llenguia i Dret, Journal of Language and Law 71: 13–26.
This article investigates the issue of children as language brokers in social services. It does so by collecting
the experiences of both service providers and former child brokers. One of the main findings is that there is a
gap between the perceptions of service providers on their work with child interpreters on the one hand and the
experiences of the child interpreters on their situation on the other hand.
Lucas, Siân (2020) “Spoken language interpreters in social work.” 52. Insight. A series of evidence
summaries. Glasgow: Institute for Research and Innovation in Social Services (IRISS). URL:
https://w ww.iriss.org.uk/resources/i nsights/spoken-language-i nterpreters-social-work (accessed
22 November 2021).
This is a recent and holistic review on interpreting in social care. It discusses interpreting in social care from
the perspectives of the different forms of interpreting provision, the legislative and policy contexts as well as
research and practical issues.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2012) “Gatekeeping practices in interpreted social service encounters”, Meta: Journal
Des Traducteurs/Meta: Translators’ Journal 57 (1): 213–34.
This article offers a broad review on the concept of gatekeeping in institutional encounters. It also presents
a case study of gatekeeping practices by recently trained interpreters and its impact on impartiality during social
and welfare counseling sessions.
Westlake, David, and Rebecca K. Jones (2018) “Breaking down language barriers: A p ractice-near
study of social work using interpreters”, British Journal of Social Work 48 (5): 1388–408.
This article investigates how social workers could better communicate and collaborate with interpreters during
counseling sessions. It does so through an analysis of audio recordings of interpreter-mediated meetings between
service providers and families and focus group sessions with service providers.
Related chapters
Chapter 11, Vulnerable encounters? Investigating vulnerability in interpreter-mediated services for v ictim-survivors
of domestic violence and abuse by Rebecca Tipton
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in Health Care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalization
of interpreters? by Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to communicate via interpreter by Tatjana R. Felberg and
Gry Sagli
Notes
1 As the speech is unclear (indicated by the fact that it is written between brackets), it is not certain
what is said here.
2 He literally says: ‘потом сказал (x x) что ты в очереди не стоишь я три года здесь как на, в
очереди не стою (x x) три года здесь и он не ничего не сделал для меня, мне бумаги надо
быть что паспорт поменять, там сказали принеси из госпиталя бумаги что’ – ‘then he said
(x x) that you weren’t standing in line, I’m not standing in line for three years, I don’t stand in line
for three years (x x) three years here and he didn’t do anything for me, I need to change my pass-
port, there they said bring the papers from the hospital’.
239
Dorien Van De Mieroop et al.
References
Alaggia, Ramona, Sarah Maiter, and Angelique Jenney (2017) “In whose words? Struggles and strat-
egies of service providers working with immigrant clients with limited language abilities in the
violence against women sector and child protection services”, Child & Family Social Work 22 (1):
472–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.12266.
Beel, Veerle (2019) “Geld voor sociaal tolken is op”, De Standaard, 15 September. URL: https://w ww.
standaard.be/cnt/d mf20190915_04610094 (accessed 22 November 2021).
Berthold, S. Megan, and Yael Fischman (2014) “Social work with trauma survivors: Collaboration
with interpreters”, Social Work 59: 103–10. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/swu011.
Chand, Ashok (2005) “Do you speak English? Language barriers in child protection social work with
minority ethnic families”, British Journal of Social Work 35 (6): 807–21.
Cox, Antoon, and Raquel Lázaro Gutiérrez (2016) “Interpreting in the emergency department:
How context matters for practice”, in Mediating Emergencies and Conflicts: Frontline Translating and
Interpreting, Federico M. Federici ( ed). London, Palgrave Macmillan UK: 33–
58. https://
doi.
org/10.1057/978[[sbn]]1[[sbn]]137[[sbn]]55351[[sbn]]5_2.
Cox, Antoon, Ellen Rosenberg, A nne-Sophie Thommeret-Carrière, Luc Huyghens, Phillippe Hum-
blé, and Yvan Leanza (2019) “Using patient companions as interpreters in the emergency depart-
ment: An interdisciplinary quantitative and qualitative assessment”, Patient Education and Counseling
102 (8): 1439–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2019.03.004.
Egbert, Maria M. ( 1997) “ Schisming: The collaborative transformation from a single conversa-
tion to multiple conversations”, Research on Language & Social Interaction 30 (1): 1–51. https://doi.
org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi3001_1.
Freed, Anne O. (1988) “Interviewing through an interpreter”, Social Work 33 (4): 315–19. https://doi.
org/10.1093/sw/33.4.315.
Hall, Jonathan, and Sonia Valdiviezo (2020) “The social worker as language worker in a multilingual
world: Educating for language competence”, Journal of Social Work Education 56 (1): 17–29. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10437797.2019.1642275.
Harrison, Gai (2007) “Language as a problem, a right or a resource? A study of how bilingual practi-
tioners see language policy being enacted in social work”, Journal of Social Work 7 (1): 71–92.
Humphreys, Catherine, Sandeep Atkar, and Norma Baldwin (1999) “Discrimination in child protection
work: Recurring themes in work with Asian families”, Child & Family Social Work 4 (4): 283–91.
Kerremans, Koen, Laurent-Philippe De Ryck, Vanessa De Tobel, Rudi Janssens, Pascal Rillof, and
Marianne Scheppers (2018) “Bridging the communication gap in multilingual service encounters:
A Brussels case study”, The European Legacy 23 (7–8): 757–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/10848770.
2018.1492811.
Kriz, Katrin, and Marit Skivenes (2010) “L ost in translation: How child welfare workers in Norway
and England experience language difficulties when working with minority ethnic families”, British
Journal of Social Work 40 (5): 1353–67.
Levinson, Stephen C. (1992) “Activity types and language”, in Talk at Work -Interaction in Institutional
Settings, Paul Drew and John Heritage (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 6 6–100.
Lindsay, Sally, Sylvie Tétrault, Chantal Desmaris, Gillian King, and Gènevive Piérart (2014) “Social
workers as ‘cultural brokers’ in providing culturally sensitive care to immigrant families raising a
child with a physical disability”, Health & Social Work 39 (2): 10–20.
Lucas, Siân (2016) “Interpreting: One size fits all? English language as an essential component of social
work”, in Social Work in a Diverse Society: Transformatory Practice with Black and Minority Ethnic Individ-
uals and Communities, Charlotte Williams and Mekada Graham (eds). Bristol, Policy Press: 91–108.
Maiter, Sarah, Ramona Alaggia, Adrienne S. Chan, and Bruce Leslie (2017) “Trial and error: Attend-
ing to language barriers in child welfare service provision from the perspective of frontline work-
ers”, Child & Family Social Work 22 (1): 165–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.12214.
McCarthy, Michael (2000) “Mutually captive audiences: Small talk and the genre of close-contact ser-
vice encounters”, in Small Talk, Justine Coupland (ed). Harlow, Pearson Education Limited: 84–109.
Pollock, Sarah (2020) “More than words can say: Why health and social care policy makers should
reconsider their position on informal interpreters”, Critical Social Policy 0 (0): 1–20. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0261018320911819.
Pomeroy, Elizabeth C., and Angela Nonaka (2013) “Language and social work: Are we really commu-
nicating effectively?”, Social Work 58 (2): 101–4. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/swt008.
240
Public service interpreting in social care
Raevaara, Liisa (2011) “Accounts at convenience stores: Doing dispreference and small talk”, Journal of
Pragmatics 43 (2): 556–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.01.020.
Sawrikar, Pooja (2015) “How effective do families of Non–English-Speaking Background (N ESB)
and child protection caseworkers in Australia see the use of interpreters? A qualitative study to help
inform good practice principles”, Child & Family Social Work 20 (4): 396–406.
Sheneman, Naomi (2017) “Extralinguistic knowledge: Why is it so important in interpreting? (N IS)”
Network Interpreting Service (blog). July 17, 2017. URL: https://networkinterpretingservice.com/
2017/07/17/extralinguistic-k nowledge-why-is-it-so-i mportant-i n-i nterpreting/ (accessed 22
November 2021).
Spencer-Oatey, Helen (2000) “Rapport management: A framework for analysis”, in Culturally Speaking.
Managing Rapport through Talk across Cultures, Helen Spencer-Oatey(ed). London, Continuum: 11–46.
The Administrative Procedure Act (2017:900) 2017. The government of Sweden. URL: https://w ww.
government.se/49855a/contentassets/3c56d854a4034fae9160e12738429f b8/t he- administrative-
procedure-act-2017900 (accessed 22 November 2021).
Tipton, Rebecca (2016) “Perceptions of the ‘occupational other’: Interpreters, social workers and in-
tercultures”, The British Journal of Social Work 46 (2): 463–79.
Torralba, Karina D., Lawrence K. Loo, John M. Byrne, Samuel Baz, Grant W. Cannon, Sheri A.
Keitz, Annie B. Wicker, Steven S. Henley, and T. Michael Kashner (2016) “Does psychological
safety impact the clinical learning environment for resident physicians? Results from the VA’s
learners’ perceptions survey”, Journal of Graduate Medical Education 8 (5): 699–707. https://doi.
org/10.4300/JGME-D -15- 0 0719.1.
Van De Mieroop, Dorien (2016) “Small talk in interpreted interactions in a medical setting”, Language
and Intercultural Communication 16 (2): 292–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/14708477.2015.1117483.
Van De Mieroop, Dorien, Giovanni Bevilacqua, and Lotte Van Hove (2012) “Negotiating discursive
norms: Community interpreting in a Belgian rest home”, Interpreting 14 (1): 23–54.
241
15
A SHARED RESPONSIBILITY
FOR FACILITATING INCLUSION
IN SCHOOL SETTINGS
WHERE SIGN-LANGUAGE
INTERPRETING IS PROVIDED
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
Introduction
This chapter examines interpreted-mediated education and how teachers and interpreters
can collaborate on facilitating inclusion when students with signed and spoken language
are in the same class. School programs for sign-language students have changed in step with
the development of legislative mandates for inclusion in public education (U NESCO 1994).
Educational interpreting has therefore become more and more common (Smith 2015). For
instance, it has been estimated that 75% of all deaf and hard-of-hearing students in the USA
depend on sign-language interpreters to access social and academic discourses in schools
(Marschark et al. 2005). While there are no exact figures in Norway indicating how many
students are using interpreter services in primary and upper secondary school, we know that
this is becoming more common at all school levels.
In specialized literature, inclusion is understood as a general approach to teaching and
organization that enhances the learning outcome and sense of belonging for all students
(M itchell and Sutherland 2020). To ensure positive learning outcomes, the teacher can adapt
tasks and activities to each student’s capabilities and their zone of proximal development
(Vygotsky 1978). To ensure a sense of belonging, the teacher can endeavor to establish a social
learning environment where the students recognize and acknowledge their dissimilarities and
know how to deal with them (Berg 2013). This chapter explores possibilities for inclusion in
interpreted-mediated education. In such a setting, inclusion can mean that s ign-language stu-
dents experience belonging, can participate in social and academic activities, and have access
to the same meaning content as the other students. Access and participation promote learning
and development: Sociocultural theory posits that learning requires both individual and social
processes (Wertsch 1991). Students must mentally categorize the subject matter, but they must
also explore the meaning potentials of the subject in question with other students. Mediating
artifacts used in these processes may be books, computers, and models. However, the most
important artifact is ‘language’ as concepts contribute to developing mental schemata, and
‘languaging’ contributes to social interaction and exploration of content (Linell 2009).
The understanding teachers and interpreters have of their roles and responsibilities im-
pacts their professional practice and how they act in relation to the students and each other
(Berge 2016). Roughly speaking, the teacher first and foremost is responsible for planning,
implementing, and assessing the teaching program. The responsibility of the interpreter is
primarily language mediation. However, teaching and interpreting are complex and situated
activities and it is difficult to anticipate all the language barriers that may arise. Both teachers
and interpreters can contribute with insight into how to adapt different c lass-talk activities so
participation becomes accessible to deaf students via interpreting. Research on interpreting
has pointed out how all involved participants in interpreted events share the responsibility
for quality. Ozolins and Hale (2009: 3) argue that: ‘Each speaker needs to assume responsi-
bility for what they say and how they say it and it is critical for all participants in interpreted
encounters to have a mutual understanding of each other’s roles and needs’. To take the
responsibility of adaption, both teachers and interpreters must be aware of the language bar-
riers that may arise in various c lass-talk activities, how they impact deaf students’ learning
possibilities, and how they can contribute to reducing the barriers within the scope of their
professional expertise (Harrington and Turner 2001). In the analyses below, I will show how
collaboration between the professions can facilitate inclusion.
243
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
students’ participation may be marginalized. Teachers may thus not know how to adapt
their practice, or they may be unaware of their responsibilities. A familiar assumption in the
literature on interpreting is that ‘the interpreter creates an illusionary access to learning and
inclusion’ (Winston 2004: 132). The teachers believe that their teaching is accessible for all
students when the interpreter is in the classroom. Therefore, they overlook the need deaf
students might have to receive subject and language support (A ntia, Stinson and Gaustad
2002; Thoutenhoofd 2005). Unfortunately, as a group deaf students have poorer subject-
related achievements and a lower sense of belonging than hearing classmates (A ntia and
Kreimeyer 2001; Schick 2004; Hendar 2012; Kermit 2018). One reason might be lack of
bilingual support.
Deaf students are bilingual learners with some different language experiences compared
to those of hearing students (Smith 2015: 269). While most hearing children are exposed
to the language of their parents from the moment they are born, most deaf children have
parents who do not know sign-language and are not able to serve as fluent language models
during the critical years of language acquisition. While most hearing children are members
of a rich language environment with a constant and consistent exposure to a variety of lan-
guages, most deaf children have very few language models at home and at school. This also
influences their access to incidental learning which takes place by overhearing other people’s
talk (Marschark et al. 2008). It is therefore important that both teachers and interpreters
have insight into the language barriers that deaf students can experience. As Smith (2015:
269) argues:
All these essential challenges are identified and highlighted in earlier research on educational
interpreting.
Literature overview
244
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
Routledge Handbook of Interpreting (Smith 2015). A recent Norwegian volume has two chapters
on educational interpreting: Ringsø and Agerup (2018) examine interpretation for children
in primary and lower secondary school, underlining the importance of collaboration, partic-
ularly when it comes to preparation. Kermit and Berge (2018) use various field observations
to describe situations where educational interpreters avoid taking part in the work to facil-
itate inclusion. Both chapters argue that the responsibility of educational interpreters must
be part of a new discussion in the field. Other publications are found in various periodicals,
where the Journal of Deaf and Deaf Education is an important publication channel.
245
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
more accessible when the interpretation was literal. Other times it was more accessible when
the interpreter liberated herself from the words in the source text and used v isual-gestural
features to illustrate the content. This mediation strategy might also support deaf students’
learning and access. Marschark et al. (2005, 2008) have used v ideo-recordings to study ac-
cessibility in interpreted-mediated lectures, finding that it is of little significance for univer-
sity student learning outcome when it comes to which translation style is used. What is most
important is that the students experience the mediation as meaningful.
Visual accessibility
The visual accessibility of sign-language students is the main focus of several studies. Berge
and Thomassen (2015), Hansen (2005), Harrington (2005), Mather (2005), and Winston
(2004) have similar findings: Visual accessibility is challenged in interpreted-mediated ed-
ucation because the teaching is performed according to oral traditions. Key issues here are
physical placement, talk tempo, t urn-taking, and use of the blackboard. Another issue is that
information is provided from several information sources at the same time. For instance,
when the teacher is talking and showing a model to the students, hearing students can both
listen to the teacher’s voice and look at the model, while deaf students must choose to either
look at the model or the interpreter. When they look at one source, they lose access to in-
formation from the other. Focusing one’s gaze on the interpreter over extended periods of
time may also be experienced as demanding for the students’ concentration, and i nterpreted-
mediated education might be experienced as heavier than teaching where teachers and stu-
dents are using the same language (Hansen 2005).
246
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
who understood their needs as bilingual and deaf learners, acted with professionalism, had
fluent language skills, and had a ‘good attitude’. These expectations are similar among Nor-
wegian upper secondary and tertiary-education students, as shown in a few, relevant inter-
view studies (Hansen 2005; Berge and Ytterhus 2015).
Talk tempo
Facilitating teachers’ talk tempo is important in terms of how teachers and interpreters can
collaborate on making content accessible. If the teacher speaks too fast, the interpreters
struggle to keep up to the detriment of the interpretation quality (Kurz and Langer 2004:
19). The interpreter in the excerpt below points out the opposite: If the teacher speaks too
slowly, may also be a problem. Therefore, the interpreter points to the importance of speak-
ing normally:
The interpreter advises the teachers to talk the way they normally do. However, this can
be a double-edged sword: While the teachers need to talk naturally, they must also adapt for
interpreting. Meaning, they cannot talk and teach like they normally do. The teachers have
obviously learnt an effective strategy for adapting their tempo, taking a break after some
sentences so the interpreter manages to keep up (Teacher 2). Another issue in the excerpt is
that the teachers want the interpreter to tell them if they are unable to keep up. They have
little insight into the language processes interpreters are dealing with. Therefore, teachers
247
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
must trust that the interpreter will stop them, and the interpreter must trust the teachers to
take time to wait, repeat, and clarify the content of their utterances.
Visual orientation
Facilitating breaks for textual and visual processing is an important part of the collaboration
between teachers and interpreters that will make the content more accessible. The excerpt
below is from a class where a teacher is presenting technical programing software (Berge and
Thomassen 2015). He enters commands on his computer that are displayed on a big screen
and describes what these commands do. At times, he points to specific sections on the screen.
He expects the students to follow what he is telling them, see where he is pointing, and per-
form the same actions on their own computers. In this situation, sign-language students must
orient their gaze between several sources of information.
The interaction between interpreter and teacher has a time lag that influences deaf stu-
dents’ access to the content and to participate in the learning activity: In line 4 (Figure 15.1),
we notice that when the teacher says ‘Now that’s done. Now you see that it has changed
colors’ while pointing at the blue rectangle on the screen. The teacher’s pointing can be
understood as a situated gesture (Goodwin 2000), and it establishes a reference in his speech
which informs the students about the functions in the software. Access to this information
is vital for mastering the task. The interpreter also points to the screen (Figure 15.2) and
coordinates her interpretation to emphasize the message. However, the teacher and the in-
terpreter are not pointing at the same time, and the interpreter’s pointing does not indicate
the rectangle that is being referred to. The teacher’s and the interpreter’s actions are not syn-
chronized, neither in time nor space, and the meaning potential in the i nterpreted-mediated
text becomes less informative.
Excerpt 2 also illustrates that s ign-language students must orient their gaze between several
simultaneously used information sources: The screen, the computer, the teacher’s gestures,
and the interpreter’s mediation are all artifacts of information (Figure 15.3). However, when
the students are watching one of the sources, they lose information from another (Berge and
Thomassen 2015). The expectation that they should carry out the task while keeping pace
with the teacher is hard to satisfy. Most likely they will lag behind the progression of their
248
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
Figure 15.1 T
he teacher points to a specific sec- Figure 15.2 The interpreter interprets the utter-
tion on the screen while talking ances, pointing somewhat later at
the screen
classmates. This may impact their learning experience and undermine their engagement in
the activity (Vygotsky 1978). The teacher’s teaching practice is hardly planned to provide
breaks for time lag and visual accessibility. After watching v ideo-recordings of the situation,
the teacher became more aware of possible drawbacks and said that he could have split the
activity into sequences: First he could have presented the software then the students could
have worked with their tasks. He also thinks that this adaption could have benefited all the
students in the class. In the next excerpt, I will look closer at the relationship between visual
accessibility and physical facilitation.
249
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
Teacher: I have asked the interpreters many times ‘W here should I stand?’ and ‘How is
this here?’, and then I think it would be very nice if I could get a clear answer.
Because I have often been told that ‘Just do what you usually do, and we adapt
to you’. I would rather like them to tell me actually what they want me to do,
like ‘If you stand there that would be practical’. I’m asking because I’m unsure,
so I would really like an answer.
Interpreter: Mm. It seems to persist, this idea that we should be the fly on the wall,
not visible at all. We simply follow after the teacher, and then we simply
interpret. We don’t quite manage this way of collaborating.
The excerpt above captures an interpreter’s ambiguity. Even if she seems to be question-
ing the view that an interpreter should be like ‘the fly on the wall’, she acknowledges that
she and the others in the interpreter team at this school do not really want to answer the
teacher’s questions relating to adapting the physical set-up. There may be many reasons for
this: They may not want to distract the teacher’s attention from the teaching, to evaluate
the teacher’s practice or assume responsibility for more tasks than the interpreting itself. It
seems like the interpreter’s understanding of professional role and responsibility limits her
way of collaboration with the teacher. This is why role expectations that highlight concepts
like ‘neutrality’ or ‘invisibility’ may not be relevant for interpreters working in school. This
is particularly the case if these ideas establish a practice that impedes deaf students’ access to
learning and teachers’ access to adapt their teaching (A ntia and Kreimeyer 2001; Thouten-
hoofd 2005; Smith 2015). However, the excerpt shows that there is a certain degree of open-
ness for changing attitudes and practices when it comes to how interpreters and teachers can
work together.
Bilingual support
Facilitating for a bilingual education program is important when teachers and interpreters
are to collaborate (Smith 2013). An issue they must resolve is how they both can apply their
professional knowledge in establishing a bilingual teaching practice. One question is how
they can divide the task of answering students’ requests for explanations of signs and words
(Berge and Ytterhus 2015):
Excerpt 5 shows that the collaboration between interpreter and student is challenged
because the interpreter does not want to answer the students directly. In Norway, there
has been an ongoing debate concerning whether interpreters should involve themselves in
explaining words to sign-language students (Berge 2003: 67; Berge and Ytterhus 2015).
Some believe that this is a task for teachers only because they have insight into the students’
learning process and expert knowledge to explain the word correctly. Others believe that
interpreters have a bilingual competence that provides them with insight into the linguis-
tic differences the students encounter in various texts, and therefore they also have expert
knowledge to explain words and signs. Students’ questions may also relate to clarifying the
interpreting, which the teacher does not have insight into. Nancy, the student in excerpt 5,
shows that she wants the teacher and the interpreter to collaborate, acknowledging that the
250
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
division of their responsibilities may be situated and text-dependent. At times, she wants to
discuss words and signs with the interpreter, other times, when she needs a more elaborate
explanation, she wants support from the teacher. This means that both the interpreter and
the teacher have professional knowledge students can make use of to master a bilingual ed-
ucational setting. This also suggests that taking the student’s perspective may though some
light on this lengthily debated problem.
251
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
Interviewer What do you think about t urn-taking and communication in the class,
knowing when you can start to say something?
Nancy Yes. I’m aware of this. This is something I think about. It feels like: Is it
correct for me to talk now, or what? Many times I just don’t say anything.
Interviewer So, there’s a difference between the ways hearing and deaf students do things
when talking together?
Toril Yes. We, the deaf, we use our gaze before we start to say something.
Nancy Those who hear, they just turn their face away and look at other things when
they talk. Their faces and eyes are not still. We don’t do that. I look at the
mouth and face of the person I’m talking to.
Interviewer I was wondering, the information given to hearing students about this, is it
good enough?
Nancy No. Most people don’t know much about being deaf. We want to make an
information video about what it means to be deaf. Then we don’t have to tell
them: I’m so tired of those who hear asking questions.
Interviewer What can the teacher do to make your interaction easier?
Nancy Information.
Toril Information.
Excerpt 6 is an example of how deaf students can experience inclusion. In their class,
the participants have different languages and cultural norms for connecting, maintaining
the talk, and coordinating turn-taking. The deaf students find it difficult to know when to
start speaking, that often leads them to shy away from participating. Their sense of inclusion
and belonging seems marginalized. In addition, the two students assume responsibility for
informing classmates about sign-language and being deaf. In a way, they are taking on the
teachers’ responsibilities. They actually would like some more help with informing the class.
To do so, teachers need knowledge. They can acquire this through reading books, training,
and guidance. The interpreters’ professional expertise can also be useful. This is especially
important when bearing in mind that there is scarce literature on the topic of interpret-
ing and teaching students with sign language in mainstream classrooms. The mutual goal
of establishing an inclusive learning environment can motivate interpreters, teachers, and
students to have regular meetings with each other (A ntia and Kreimeyer 2001; Seal 2004;
Smith 2015; Kermit and Berge 2018). With insight into each other’s perspectives, they can
promote an inclusive learning environment where all participants know about each other’s
preferences and particularities, and how to deal with them (Berg 2013).
Summary
This chapter has presented several topics where teachers and interpreters can collaborate on
facilitating academic and social inclusion. In summary, we can see that:
When it comes to talk tempo, structure, and time-lag, the teacher can pause between sen-
tences, be precise with topics and focus, wait for a response, and prepare interpreters
252
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
and students. The interpreters can ask for repetitions and clarifications, inform the
teacher about strategies for adapting talk tempo, and be prepared before the interpreting
commences.
When it comes to bilingual support, the teacher can be sensitive about the need to explain
concepts and be interested in the interpretation process and the language differences.
The interpreter can inform about language differences, explain concepts and signs, and
discuss translation styles and sign choices with deaf students.
When it comes to visual accessibility, the teachers can facilitate placement of the students,
pause for visual processing, and sequence their teaching practice. The interpreter can
time their mediation for visual processing. They can also discuss solutions for how to
organize the physical environment, and when processing breaks are needed throughout
the activity.
When it comes to g roup-work activities, the teachers can be sensitive to group size and
composition, and they can frame the turn-taking in question-answer activities. They
can also frame informal g roup-work situations which develop between the students
when they are working with a task and give instructions for when the students can talk
with one another and when they should work in silence. The interpreter could facilitate
signals for t urn-taking, inform students if they missed rendering talk due to overlap or
unclear turn-taking, and inform the class about the importance of adapting their talk
and seating arrangements.
When it comes to the social learning environment, the teachers must be aware of their role
as a model for the class and their attitude when it comes to preserving diversity and
inclusion for all students. One solution can be to inform the class so that they have
knowledge about how to handle the different language cultures. The teaching must also
be differentiated so all students can experience learning. The interpreter can assist peer
interaction with natural behavior and small talk and inform teachers and the class about
how to adapt their speech in interpreted-mediated communication.
253
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
recognize their responsibility for teaching all students and act as collaborative partners with
the interpreters working in their class.
Judging from current research on interpreted-mediated education, it appears that in-
terpreters do not have w ell-established professional practices of collaborating closely with
students and teachers. Rather it may appear that they expect that teachers should assume the
total responsibility for establishing an inclusive teaching practice (Kermit and Berge 2018).
However, in the interpreting field, there is a certain degree of openness for a change in atti-
tudes and practices. Several researchers have for instance highlighted roles and responsibili-
ties of educational interpreters in relation to satisfying the actual conditions in the classroom
and the overall goal of inclusion (A ntia and Kreimeyer 2001; Harrington and Turner 2001;
Seal 2004; Thoutenhoofd 2005; Russel and Winston 2014; Berge 2016; Ringsø and Agerup
2018). An important theme in future research is to gain more insight in how different teach-
ers and interpreters share the responsibility for facilitating inclusion in school settings where
sign-language interpreting is provided.
It takes time to adjust practices, to share professional wisdom, and to explore possible
solutions. To ensure collaboration between teachers and interpreters, the school leaders must
give them resources to meet outside the teaching hours. Teachers and interpreters need to
establish arenas where they can meet as equal partners. Up to now, it seems that teachers
and interpreters have had an asymmetric relationship. Cooperation has mainly involved the
teacher informing the interpreter about the topic and terms of the day. Of course, this kind
of preparation improves the language interpretation, but as this chapter has shown, several
other areas also need to be addressed when it comes to how they can work together to adapt
the teaching and interpreting practice.
In a larger societal perspective, Dean and Pollard (2011: 158) find that sign-language in-
terpretation services are about ‘doing good’ in the sense of contributing to greater equality
between deaf and hearing participants. One way of ‘doing good’ might be to establish new
practices for co-working between interpreters and students. From earlier studies we know
that the interpreting practice needs to be in balance: On the one hand, the autonomy of deaf
students must not be restricted by the interpreters working in school. On the other, deaf
students must not be prevented from experiencing academic and social inclusion because the
teacher and the interpreter are unable to figure out how to collaborate. The need for and
wish to have access to interpreters’ professional knowledge obviously differ from one student
to the next due to their different backgrounds, education level, and personal preferences.
Moreover, student diversity is becoming more and more complex in terms of cultural back-
grounds, use of technical hearing-aid solutions, language contact, and choices of education
programs. For further insight, several researchers can explore this diversity and help us to
better understand what deaf and hearing students think could help to establish academic and
social inclusion.
Further reading
Linell, Per (2009) Rethinking Language, Mind, and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories
of Human Sense-Making. Charlotte, NC, Information Age Publishing.
Vygotsky, Lev S. (1978) Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge,
MA, Harvard University Press.
Both Linell and Vygotsky offer knowledge on basic structures of language, languaging, learning, and human
interaction. They can help to establish a common theoretical platform for interpreters and teachers, which may
help them to focus on their professional practice.
254
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
Marschark, Marc, Sapere, Patricia, Convertino, Carol, and Seewagen, Rosemarie (2005) “Educational
interpreting: Access and outcomes”, in Sign Language Interpreting and Interpreter Education: Directions
for Research and Practice, Marc Marschark, Rico Peterson and Elizabeth A. Winston (eds). New
York, Oxford University Press: 57–85.
This chapter addresses sign-language interpreting and the need to adapt interpreter-mediated education to
ensure that deaf students actually have access to education and learning.
Smith, Melissa Beth (2013) More Than Meets the Eye: Revealing the Complexities of an Interpreted Education.
Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press.
This work provides knowledge on how educational interpreters get involved and take responsibility for four
tasks: visual accessibility, promoting language and learning, cultivating opportunities for social and academic
participation, and establishing inclusive learning environments. It promotes discussions on division of responsi-
bility and addresses the assumption that educational interpreters should only act as invisible actors.
Related chapters
Chapter 2, The ambiguity of interpreting: ethnographic interviews with public service interpreters by Kristina
Gustafsson
Chapter 17, Training sign language interpreters for public service interpreting by Christopher A. Stone, Cyn-
thia B. Roy and Jeremy L. Brunson
Chapter 23, Interprofessional education … interpreter education: in or and? Taking stock and moving forward,
by Demi Kristallidou
References
Antia, Shirin D., and Kathryn H. Kreimeyer (2001) “The role of interpreters in inclusive classrooms”,
American Annals of the Deaf 146 (4): 355–65. https://doi.org/10.1353/a ad.2012.0142.
Antia, Shirin D., Michael S. Stinson, and Martha G. Gaustad (2002) “Developing membership in the
education of deaf and h ard-of-hearing students in inclusive settings”, The Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education 7 (3): 214–29. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/7.3.214
Berg, Kari (2013) “Læringsledelse i møte med elevmangfoldet [Classroom management and meeting
with pupils’ diversity]”, in Klasseledelse for elevenes læring, Gunnar Engvik, Tine A. Hestbek, Torlaug
L. Hoel and May Britt Postholm (eds). Trondheim: Akademika: 199–216.
Berge, Sigrid S. (2003) “Tegnspråktolkens handlinger [Sign language interpreters’ actions]”, in Skriftse-
rien for klasseromsforskning, Ragnheidur Karlsdottir and Sigrun Gudmundsdottir (eds). Trondheim,
Tapir Akademisk Forlag.
——— (2016) Tolkemediert undervisning for tegnspråklige elever i videregående skole: En analyse av under-
visningstolkers rollesett [Interpreter-Mediated Education for Sign Language Students in High School: Analysis
of Educational Interpreters’ Roleset [Doctoral thesis]. Norges teknisk-naturvitenskapelige universitet.
https://ntnuopen.ntnu.no/ntnu-x mlui/handle/11250/2427493.
——— (2018) “How sign language interpreters use multimodal actions to coordinate t urn-taking in
g roup-dialogues between deaf and hearing upper secondary school students”, Interpreting: Interna-
tional Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 20(1): 96–125. http://hdl.handle.net/11250/2427493
——— ( 2020) “ Læreres forventninger til tegnspråktolkers yrkesrolle i videregående opplæring
[Teachers expectations for the role of sign language interpreter in h igh-school]”, Spesialpedagogikk
03 (06): 47–61.
——— (2021) “Tolkemedierte gruppearbeid mellom elever med talt og tegnet språk [Interpreter-
mediated g roup-work among spoken and sign language pupils]”, Nordisk tidsskrift for utdanning og
praksis 15 (1): 1–17. https://doi.org/10.23865/up.v15.2216.
Berge, Sigrid S., and Patrick S. Kermit (2018) “Deaf students’ access to informal g roup-work activ-
ities seen in light of the educational interpreter’s role”, in Marginalization Processes across Different
Settings: Going beyond the Mainstream, Sangeeta B agga-Gupta (ed). Newcastle, Cambridge Scholars
Publishing: 106–28.
Berge, Sigrid S., Eli Raanes, and Marianne P. Nyhus (2018) “Tolking med bruk av tegn som støtte for
munnavlesning-TSS [Literal interpreting]”, in Tolking, språkarbeid og profesjonsutøvelse, Hilde Haua-
land, A nna-Lena Nilsson and Eli. Raanes (eds). Oslo, Gyldendal: 180–201.
255
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge
Berge, Sigrid S., and Gøril Thomassen (2015) “Visual access in interpreter-mediated learning situa-
tions for deaf and hard-of-hearing high school students where an artifact is in use”, Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education 21 (2): 187–99. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/env057.
Berge, Sigrid S., and Borgunn Ytterhus (2015) “Deaf and hearing h igh-school students’ expecta-
tions for the role of educational sign language interpreter”, Society, Health and Vulnerability 6: 1–26.
https://doi.org/10.3402/shv.v6.28969.
Davis, Jeffrey E. (2005) “Code choices and consequences: Implications for educational interpreting”,
in Sign Language Interpreting and Interpreter Education, Marc Marschark, Rico Peterson and Elizabeth
A. Winston (eds). Oxford, Oxford University Press: 112–41.
Dean, Robyn K., and Robert Q. Pollard (2011) “Context-based ethical reasoning in interpreting: A
demand control schema perspective”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 5 (1): 155–82.
Goodwin, Charles (2000) “Action and embodiment within situated human interaction”, Journal of
Pragmatics 32 (10), 1489–522. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378–2166(99)0 0096-X.
Hansen, Aase L. (2005) Kommunikative praksiser i visuelt orienterte klasserom: En studie av et tilrettelagt
opplæringstilbud for døve lærerstudenter [Communicative Practices in Visually Oriented Classrooms: A
Study of an Adapted Teaching Program for Deaf Teacher Students] [Doctoral thesis]. Norges teknisk-
naturvitenskapelige universitet.
Harrington, Frank J. (2000) “Sign language interpreters and access of deaf students to university Cur-
ricula”, in The Critical Link 2: Interpreters in the Community, Roda P. Roberts, Sivana E. Carr, Diana
Abraham and Aideen Dufour (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins, 219–42.
——— (2005) “A study of the complex nature of interpreting with deaf students in higher education”,
in Attitudes, Innuendo, and Regulators: Challenges of Interpretation, Melanie Metzger and Earl Fleet-
wood (eds). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 162–87.
Harrington, Frank. J., and Graham Turner (2001) Interpreting Interpreting. Studies and Reflections on Sign
Language Interpreting. Coleford, Douglas McLean.
Hendar, Ola (2012) Elever med hørselshemming i skolen. En kartleggingsundersøkelse om læringsutbytte [Deaf
and Hard-of-Hearing Pupils Learning Outcome]. Oslo.
Kermit, Patrick S. (2018) Hørselshemmede barn og unges opplæringsmessige og sosiale vilkår i barnehage og
skole. Kunnskapsoversikt over nyere nordisk forskning [Educational and Social Conditions in Kindergartens
and Schools for Children and Youth with Hearing Impairments. A Review of Recent Nordic Research].
Trondheim, NTNU Samfunnsforskning.
Kermit, Patrick S., and Sigrid S. Berge (2018) “Tegnspråktolking i videregående skole: Inkludering
og skoletolkens yrkesansvar [Sign-language interpreting in secondary education: Inclusion and
responsibilities of educational interpreters]”, in Tolking, språkarbeid og profesjonsutøvelse [Interpreting,
Language Work and Professional Conduct] Hilde Haualand, A nna-Lena Nilsson and Eli Raanes (eds).
Oslo, Gyldendal: 3 53–71.
Kurz, Kim B., and Elizabeth Caldwell Langer (2004) “Student perspectives on educational interpreting:
Twenty deaf and hard-of-hearing students offer insights and suggestions”, in Educational Interpreting:
How It Can Succeed, Elizabeth A. Winston (ed). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 9 –47.
Marschark, Marc, Patricia Sapere, Carol Convertino, and Jeff Pelz (2008) “L earning via direct and
mediated instructions by deaf students”, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13 (4): 546–61.
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enn014.
Mather, Susan. M. (2005) “Ethnographic research on the use of visually based regulators for teachers
and interpreters”, in Attitudes, Innuendo, and Regulators: Challenges of Interpretation, Melanie Metzger
and Earl Fleetwood (eds). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 136–61.
Mitchell, David, and Dean Sutherland (2020) What Really Works in Special and Inclusive Education: Using
Evidence-Based Teaching Strategies. London, Routledge.
Napier, Jemina (2002) “University interpreting: Linguistic issues for consideration”, Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education 7 (4): 2 81–301. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/7.4.281.
——— (2005) “Linguistic features and strategies of interpreting: From research to education to prac-
tice”, in Sign Language Interpreting and Interpreter Education, Marc Marschark, Rico Peterson and
Elizabeth A. Winston (eds). Oxford: Oxford University Press: 84–111.
——— (2015) “Comparing signed and spoken language interpreting”, in The Routledge Handbook of
Interpreting, Holly Mikkelson and Renée Jourdenais (eds). London, Routledge: 129–40.
Napier, Jemina, and Roz Barker (2004) “Accessing university education: Perceptions, preferences,
and expectations for interpreting by deaf students”, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 9 (2):
228–38. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enh024.
256
Facilitating inclusion through sign-language in schools
Ozolins, Uldis, and Sandra Hale (2009) “Quality in interpreting: A shared responsibility”, in The Crit-
ical Link 5: Quality in interpreting – A Shared Responsibility, Sandra Hale, Uldis Ozolins and Ludmila
Stern (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 1–10.
Ringsø, Torill and Charlotte H. Agerup (2018) “To profesjoner i et klasserom [Two professions in a
classroom]”, in Tolking, språkarbeid og profesjonsutøvelse [Interpreting, Language Work and Professional
Conduct], Hilde Haualand, A nne-Lena Nilsson and Eli Raanes (eds). Oslo, Gyldendal: 336–52.
Roy, Cynthia B. (2000) Interpreting as a Discourse Process. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Russel, Debra, and Betsy Winston (2014) “TAPing into the interpreting process: Using participant
reports to inform the interpreting process”, Translation and Interpreting 6 (1). http://w ww.t rans-i nt.
org/i ndex.php/t ransint/a rticle/v iew/315/153.
Schick, Brenda (2004) “How might learning through an educational interpreter influence cognitive
development?”, in Educational Interpreting: How It Can Succeed, Elizabeth A. Winston (ed). Washing-
ton, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 73–88.
Seal, Brenda C. (2004) Best Practices in Educational Interpreting. 2nd ed. Boston, MA, Pearson.
Smith, Melissa B. (2015) “Interpreting in education”, in The Routledge Handbook of Interpreting, Holly
Mikkelson and Renée Jourdenais (eds). London, Routledge: 265–79.
Thoutenhoofd, Ernst (2005) “The sign language interpreter in inclusive education: Power of authority
and limits of objectivism”, The Translator 11 (2): 2 37–58.
UNESCO (1994) The Salamanca Statement and Framework for Action on Special Needs Education. http://
www.unesco.org/education/pdf/salama.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
Wertsch, James V. (1991) Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action. Cambridge, MA,
Harvard University Press.
Winston, Elizabeth A. (2004). “Interpretability and accessibility of mainstream classrooms”, in Edu-
cational Interpreting: How It Can Succeed, Elizabeth A. Winston (ed). Washington, DC, Gallaudet
University Press: 132–67.
257
PART 3
Hanne Skaaden
Introduction
Interpreters’ professionalization in the community or public service setting lags behind
other occupational groups (M ikkelson 1996; Wadensjö 1998, 2007; Hale 2007, 2008;
Ozolins 2010; Prunč 2012; Grbić 2015; Tipton and Furmanek 2016; Cirillo and Nie-
mants 2017; Gentile 2017). As a result, high status professionals of medicine and law, in
encounters where they face a language barrier in communication with their patients or
clients, are served by lay persons of interpreting (Skaaden 2018, 2020). In the light of
a general model of professionalization, this chapter revisits some central concepts and
conf licts associated with the interpreter function as performed in institutional encounters.
That is, encounters where ‘one person who represents an institution encounters another
person seeking its services’ (A gar 1985: 147). Interpreting in institutional encounters as
def ined here entails encounters between doctors or lawyers and their patients or clients
also in the realm of these occupations’ private practices, where they still represent the
‘institution’ of their respective professions. In other words, the concern is interpreting
as practiced in the community or public service setting. The term community inter-
preting was coined in the process of professionalizing courtroom interpreters. Thus,
courtroom interpreters used the label to distance themselves from colleagues working
in other settings, as had the conference interpreters done before them, Mikkelson (1996:
126) notes. With setting, in the sense of arena or scene of performance, as the basic cri-
terion of categorization, the three concepts ‘conference’, ‘courtroom’ and ‘community’
interpreting soon came to signify a status hierarchy in interpreting. Today, the labels
‘legal interpreting’ and ‘medical interpreting’ appear as remnants of this hierarchy, al-
though they essentially refer to the same core a ctivity – at least when ‘legal’ interpret-
ing is performed outside the courtroom itself, as it typically is, in asylum and police
interviews or the social services. At the same time, an impression is often created that
interpreting in the community equals ‘d ialogue interpreting’ (see for instance, Tipton
and Furmanek 2016). Although the interpreter’s presence at the table with the parties
is common in institutional encounters, the f ace-to-face mode is not inextricably linked
to setting, nor is the basic method of short sequence consecutive interpreting in both
language directions. The mode may also occur in the conference and courtroom set-
tings (A lexieva 2002: 220). In line with the European tradition, I shall henceforth use
the term public service interpreting (P SI) instead of ‘community interpreting’. Following
Wadensjö (2 007: 1), PSI here encompasses ‘interpreting performed in legal, health and
social service settings’.
Fragmentation, as illustrated by the field’s multitude of labels, is but one of the factors
hampering interpreters’ professionalization. PSI is still ‘often used in juxtaposition to the
more prestigious conference interpreting’, as Pöllbauer (2013: 5) notes. Despite its low sta-
tus within the interpreter professions’ internal hierarchy, the interpreting of institutional
dialogues is in many ways the most complex form of interpreting, due to the relational and
interactional factors that influence the interpreter’s work conditions in this arena of perfor-
mance as demonstrated by Wadensjö’s (1998) seminal work. Concurrently, the PSI setting is
characterized by its encounters’ sensitive topics, strict confidentiality and interpreters work-
ing a lone – factors
that make quality control difficult compared to the conference and court-
room settings (Gentile 1997: 113). In concert, these factors have an impact on the process of
professionalization and add to conflict (Skaaden 2020: 190–91). Interestingly, PSI is also the
setting where the ‘code of ethics’ and concepts of ‘accuracy’, ‘agency’ and ‘role’ are mostly
debated. What is the core of the conflict(s) and how does conflict and fragmentation impact
the process of professionalization?
262
The professionalization of interpreters
and even has repercussions for interpreting research and the building of the profession’s
knowledge base.
A premise in the following is that a model of the professionalization of PSI must take
into consideration both the m icro-and m acro-levels of the activity and accordingly include
the interpreter’s discursive role as well as societal role. That is the interpreters’ rendering
as well as their coordinating the dialogue at the discursive level – and, at the societal level,
the enabling of communication across linguistic barriers, thus, the clients’ duties and rights.
With a holistic vantage point, the aim is to address the following questions: What may hin-
der the process of professionalization in PSI? What is at the core of the conflicts? What are
the implications for the clients, for interpreting research and the field’s development of its
knowledge base?
263
Hanne Skaaden
(2 012: 3) notices. In part, this situation has to do with the profession’s internal hierar-
chy, and what was def ined as ‘worthy’ of the researchers’ attention. Around the time
that PSI started gaining recognition as a f ield of research, the researchers’ focus also
shifted from cognition to interaction (M ikkelson 1996; Wadensjö 1998; Pöchhacker
2008). PSI has since developed into a productive f ield of research (e.g., Kainz, Prunč
and Schlöger 2011; Vargas Urpi 2012; Tipton and Furmanek 2016; V alero-Garcés and
Tipton 2017), paying attention particularly to the interactional aspect. However, the
divide remains along the lines identif ied by Hale (2 007, 2008). Furthermore, PSI’s lack
of education has bearing on its knowledge base (Cirillo and Niemants 2017: 2; Skaaden
2020: 187–8 8).
Different models of professionalization are applied in analyses of the activity of inter-
preting. Inghilleri (2 005a, [2005b] 2014) in her analysis of interpreted asylum interviews
applies Bourdieu’s (1977) ‘theory of practice’, wherein (somewhat simplified) an activity
relates to a ‘field’ in social space where individuals interact in negotiating material and
symbolic goods and power according to a certain ‘h abitus’ that is based on their expecta-
tions consistent with their previous experiences. (O n Bourdieu’s model and PSI, see also
Valero-Garcés and Gauthier 2010.) Within Bourdieu’s framework, Inghilleri ([2005b]
2014) finds that the activity of public service interpreters occupies ‘a zone of uncertainty’.
Such zones in social space occur ‘where problematic gaps emerge between individual
expectations and actual experience’ (ibid. 3). Referring to a m acro-level of interaction
or experience, Inghilleri ([2005b] 2014: 1 3–14) concludes that ‘interpreters and the in-
terpreting profession must emerge from their position of uncertainty in the wider social
order’. Accordingly, she suggests that members of the interpreter profession in order to
obtain ‘a more stable interpreting habitus’ must come to agreement with respect to “who
they are”’ (loc cit.).
Also exploring asylum interviews, Tipton’s (2 008) interest is placed at the m icro-
level of interaction. Drawing on Giddens’ (1984) notion of ‘r eflexive conduct’ whereby
we, as knowledgeable agents in human interaction, constantly monitor self and others,
Tipton (2 008: 4) observes that the ‘authentic’ voice of the interlocutors is often obscured
in i nterpreter-mediated interaction. Accordingly, reflexivity is ‘t runcated’ thus limiting
each interlocutor’s ability to act as ‘a reflexive agent’, Tipton (2 008: 14) finds and sug-
gests that in compensation the interpreter should act as ‘k nowledgeable cultural agent’.
A general model of the process of professionalization as described by a group of
Scandinavian sociologists (G rimen 2008a, 2008b, 2008c; Grimen and Molander 2008,
Molander and Terum 2008, henceforth, ‘the Scandinavian model’) allows for a m eta-
v iew on some of the aspects hampering the process of professionalization in PSI. The
Scandinavian model sees ‘professionalization’ as a process that integrates a performa-
tive and an organizational aspect. The performative aspect depicts ‘“practice” in the
sense of professional activity’ and describes the ‘professional’ as (1) offering a service
(2) for clients who depend upon the professional’s specialized skills (3) to solve a ‘how
to’ problem, by (4) applying specialized skills in unique situations that are difficult to
standardize. Hence, (5) the professional exercises discretion (Molander and Terum 2008:
18–20). The activity of interpreting fulfils the criteria of the performative aspect just
listed with good margin as displayed in Table 16.1. Hence, interpreting can be regarded
as professional practice.
Consequently, interpreters, like other professionals, exercise discretion. What differs is the
domain for which they are responsible – their area of expertise. Moreover, unique for the
interpreter profession is the fact that there are always two c lients – speaker and listener alike.
264
The professionalization of interpreters
Table 16.1 The general and the interpreter-specific ways of exercising professional discretion (a fter
Skaaden 2020: 151a)
This aspect places the interpreter in a position of double a llegiance – an aspect I return to in
the subsection on the interpreters’ clients.
In its essence, the Scandinavian model reaches back to the sociologist Talcott Parsons’
(1978) definition of a profession, stating that professions are ‘occupational groups that per-
form certain rather specialized functions for others (“laymen”) in the society on the basis of
h igh-level and specialized competence, with the attendant fiduciary responsibility’ (Parsons
1978: 40). An additional criterion according to Molander and Terum (2008: 18–20) is the ex-
istence of a codified knowledge base. This means that professionals in their exercise of discretion,
in addition to the guidelines found in their professional ethics, ‘apply a systemized amount of
knowledge to unique cases’ (Molander and Terum 2008: 19. My translation from Norwegian,
italics in original). Obviously, the building of a knowledge base is a process, and for PSI one
that has started only recently.
The professionalization of interpreting in the community or public service setting was
the topic of the Stockholm Critical Link in 2004. In her foreword to the conference
proceedings, Wadensjö (2 007: 2 –3) underlines that professionalization is a process that
depends on several factors. Wadensjö (2 007: 3) names three that are labelled by Parsons
(1968) of primary importance: First, according to the factor of ‘u niversalism’, the profes-
sional treats all clients equally. Second, according to the factor of ‘functional specificity’,
the professional carries out a specified task for the collective good and not (primarily)
for personal gain. Third, according to the factor of ‘achieved competence’, the profes-
sional gains competence through training or education and not through heritage. Parsons,
who fathered the sociology of professions through his studies of professionalization within
medicine, has later been criticized for his essentialist distinction between professions and
non-professions (for instance, Freidson 2001). Since Parsons’ time, sociology has further
characterized professionalization in terms of ‘struggles between conflicting interests of
groups and societies’, Wadensjö (2 007: 2 –3) notes. She points to conflicts within the inter-
preter profession, for instance, conflicting conceptualizations of the activity, conflicting
definitions of the occupational role and conflicts pertaining to the profession’s ethics and
its abovementioned internal hierarchy.
265
Hanne Skaaden
In sum, based on his studies of the process within medicine and law, Parsons (1968,
1978) emphasized the significance of three factors for professionalization to take place:
the existence of (i) a clearly defined task, (ii) a set of ethical principles upon which there
is general consent, and (iii) organized education to guide the professionals in their ex-
ercise of discretion. The three interrelated factors are far from settled for the interpreter
profession. Nearing two decades after the Stockholm Critical Link, education for practi-
tioners of PSI is still underdeveloped (Cirillo and Niemants 2017; Valero-Garcés and Tip-
ton 2017; Pokorn, Viezzi and Felberg 2020). Moreover, issues pertaining to ‘functional
specificity’ and the interpreters’ occupational role as well as disputes of the value of the
profession’s code of ethics remain unresolved (H ale 2008; Tipton and Furmanek 2016;
Phelan et al. 2020).
266
The professionalization of interpreters
It follows from the necessary exercise of discretion that all professional activity is in its
nature fallible (Grimen and Molander 2008: 188; Molander and Terum 2008: 20). At this
point, the interpreters’ profession is not different in principle from those professions they
serve. What differs is the domain over which they exercise discretion. In line with the
general nature of professional ethics, the accuracy principle provides guidelines for the
practitioners’ exercise of discretion in real life situations and delineates the extension of
their domain of expertise.
The idea that the accuracy principle entails room for the exercise of discretion is not new.
In fact, Cicero depicted the exercise of discretion, when stating that the translator should not
‘count’ the words but rather ‘weigh them out’ to the reader (Venuti 2000: 23 after Jerome).
It goes without saying that reaching accuracy is challenging for interpreters, who have only
split seconds to weigh out their renditions.
Exercise of discretion in general concerns the professionals’ ability to discern between a
‘good’ and ‘bad’ solution to a problem that occurs within their area of expertise (Grimen
and Molander 2008: 180). A short dialogue between prosecutor (speaking Norwegian) and
witness (speaking Bosnian) serves to illustrate the interpreters’ exercise of discretion:
267
Hanne Skaaden
The brief dialogue gives a glimpse into the judgements that interpreters must make when-
ever indeterminacy presents itself within the realm of linguistic convention. Here, a problem
occurs when the witness’ nickname, Dada, in line 3 coincides phonetically with a more fre-
quent unit in the source language convention [da:da] (meaning ‘yes, yes’). The interpreter first
discerns the latter option as the witness’ response. Since the highly frequent unit is also a logical
answer (expressing agreement) to the prosecutor’s question ‘Did you have a nickname?’ (lines
1–2), the interpreter’s initial response (line 4) is understandable. The coincidental homonymy is
difficult to anticipate. The interpreter’s swift realization of the alternative option reveals to the
observer the interpreter’s exercise of discretion. Medical and legal professionals must exercise
discretion because the material of their work (symptoms and evidence, respectively) is often
ambiguous. The Dada example illustrates how the material handled by the interpreter – oral
utterances situated in a specific c ontext – is also characterized by indeterminacy. The brief ex-
change, including the interpreters backtracking and repair, shows how interpreters apply their
agency to exercise discretion within a narrow, yet extremely complex, domain.
Obviously, the interpreter is neither a ‘m achine’ nor ‘invisible’, although these metaphors
were originally introduced to signify the accuracy principle as a token of the interpreters’
area of expertise in the early phases of sign language interpreters’ professionalization. The
aim was to rid the occupation of the image of ‘helper’ for the deaf person (Roy 2002:
349). Analysing the concept of cultural mediation, Pöchhacker (2008: 19–24) observes that
‘mediation’ has a dual meaning and may in some languages imply intervention in a con-
ciliatory sense. Mediation’s conciliatory meaning expands the interpreter’s power domain
towards ‘helper’ in ways that may restrict the clients’ voice and integrity, however (Skaaden
2018). Assigning the task of ‘cultural mediation’ to the interpreter augments the problem, Fel-
berg and Skaaden (2012) observe, as ‘culture’ is in itself an extremely complex phenomenon,
harbouring diverse aspects such as age, class, gender, professional, religious and regional af-
filiation. The observation is in line with Inghilleri’s ([2005b] 2014: 9) that cultural mediation
results in a ‘cultural conduit’, with ‘the interpreter as the channel that enables the two sides
to discursively “connect”’. In sum, the cultural conduit is as problematic as the language
conduit. The notion of accuracy in rendition is indeed difficult to define – and live up to.
However, cultural mediation raises serious ethical issues as well. This aspect becomes evi-
dent when in the next paragraphs I shall further delineate the extension of the interpreter’s
domain for the exercise of discretion and identify the interpreters’ clients.
268
The professionalization of interpreters
defining an agent’s participation status on the ‘stage’ of the dialogue. The distinction, made
by Erving Goffman (1974: 128–29), is emphasized by Wadensjö (1998: 86) in her analyses
of interpreters’ coordinating strategies. Unfortunately, the distinction is often overlooked
when promoters of cultural mediation refer to Wadensjö’s model in defence of an extended
occupational role (Skaaden 2020: 169). The two meanings of ‘role’ may relate to Inghilleri’s
([2005b]2014: 5) distinction between m acro-and micro orderings of experience, wherein
the ‘interpreting habitus is thus, like habitus more generally, by its very nature, contingent
upon macro and micro orderings of experience’ and depends on the agents’ positioning, in-
cluding ‘the positioning of the interpreter within the interpreted event’ (ibid. 13).
Wadensjö’s (1998) model of the interpreted dialogue is first and foremost concerned with
the interpreters’ display of agency on the m icro-level of ordering, that is role in the sense of
‘part’ within the micro cosmos of the dialogue. Performed at this m icro-level the interpreter’s
agency involves both the rendering and coordinating of other speakers’ talk, Wadensjö explains:
As a matter of fact, these aspects condition each other. Seen like this, it is not an empir-
ical question whether interpreters are translators or m ediators – they
cannot avoid being
both. However, the coordinating and the translating functions are foregrounded at par-
ticular moments, sometimes supporting, and sometimes disturbing one another. The
two aspects of interpreting – translation and coordination – are in practice inseparable,
but it is possible and indeed fruitful theoretically to distinguish between them and use
them as analytical concepts (Wadensjö 1998: 106).
Coordination is necessitated by the fact that interpreted dialogues differ in structure from
‘normal’ dialogues (Goffman 1974: 224). The interpreter’s coordinating becomes necessary
because the ‘g lue’ of dialogues, normally manifested through subtle feedback signals, such
as gaze, sighs, hand or body movements, has altered effect in interpreted discourse. Thus,
coordinating refers to the interpreters’ positioning of the professional self within the context
(or ‘field’) of the dialogue, including strategies for t urn-taking, pronoun choice and requests
for clarification. As witnessed in the above example (lines 2, 5 and 7, respectively), the in-
terpreter’s coordinating strategies are both implicit and explicit. Managing the coordination
and rendering of the interlocutors’ utterances refer to the interpreters’ ‘rather specialized
functions’ that they as professionals ‘perform for others (laymen)’, their clients, as Parsons
(1978) defined professional activity in the above quote. These strategies should receive atten-
tion in education as well as research.
Laymen interpreters typically fail to position themselves within the m icro-level of the
dialogue, for instance, by lacking awareness of pronoun choice (Bot 2005: 181). A result of
failed coordination may be the ‘fragmentation of reflexive conduct’ whereby the interloc-
utors while waiting to speak during the interpreter’s rendition ‘a re linguistically debarred
from following the conversation’, Tipton (2008: 11) notes in her observed asylum interviews.
Drawing on Giddens’ notion of interlocutors as ‘k nowledgeable agents’, Tipton labels the in-
terpreter a ‘k nowledgeable cultural agent’. The suggestion hinges the complexity of ‘culture’
mentioned above, as the approach turns the interpreter into a potentially omnipotent agent,
a cultural conduit.
Giddens’ proposal (1984: 9) offers possibility to restrict agency, however, by emphasiz-
ing that ‘structural constraints’ may limit a knowledgeable agent’s capacity to act. What
may limit the interpreters’ agency is their occupational function as expressed by the pro-
fession’s moral. Moreover, their bilingual and interactional skills as well as interpreting
strategies may limit or enable them to remain within their area of expertise, so as ‘to
269
Hanne Skaaden
The attitude that only the minority speaker needs interpreting opens for the cultural media-
tor to represent ‘them’ who are different from ‘us’. If not emanating from it, the perspective
certainly results in stereotyping: ‘the process by which all members of a group are asserted
to have the characteristics attributed to the whole group’ (Scollon and Scollon 2001: 168).
Thus, preventing the individual to be heard and seen. Using culture as an explanatory tool
in contact between individuals may result in what the philosopher Emmanuel Lévinas la-
belled othering, a division between ‘us’ and ‘them’, that reduces ‘them’ into ‘a categorically
abstract otherness’ (Finkielkraut [1984] 1997, x iv–xv) and subsequent exclusion in the form
of discrimination.
The image of cultural mediator is enthusiastically seized by some practitioners of PSI,
possibly because a more productive societal function may seem to enhance their professional
status, Inghilleri ([2005b]2014: 13) observes. ‘Configurations of the interpreting habitus that
270
The professionalization of interpreters
establish interpreters as “interactionally equal” participants, though they may empower in-
terpreters professionally, do not guarantee that applicants’ claims for political asylum will be
more adequately or justly represented’, she (ibid. 13) concludes, however.
The fact that in institutional encounters the interpreters’ clients on one side of the table
themselves fulfil a professional function limits the interpreters’ occupational role. From this
perspective, an additional problem arises for the cultural mediator: an extended role allows
the interpreter agency to act in ways that may undermine the professional integrity of the
medical or legal workers in charge and infringe on their domain for the exercise of discre-
tion (Skaaden 2018: 11–12). In defining the societal function of interpreters in institutional
encounters, Norwegian authorities accordingly emphasize that PSI ‘enables professionals
and officials to inform, guide and hear the parties in the case of language barrier’ (NOU
2014: 18, author’s translation from Norwegian). In line with legal and professional standards,
the definition assigns the responsibility to communicate with their patients and clients to
the professional in charge of the institutional encounter. It follows that when doctors and
lawyers allow for lay persons of interpreting to handle their communication, they risk their
own professional integrity and the opportunity to exercise discretion within their own area
of expertise.
In democracy, where duties and rights are acknowledged as two sides of the same coin,
an attitude ‘that people in the country speaking another language – foreigners, immigrants
and asylum seekers – are themselves responsible for communicating successfully with the
authorities’ (Prunč 2012: 3) implies violation of basic human rights (Viezzi 2015: 512). Yet,
economic crisis and ruling populistic ideologies may set the latter principles under attack
even in countries like the Netherlands and the UK (Gentile 2017: 63). Obviously, the trend
has implications not only for the interpreter profession, but also for the professions they
serve in institutional encounters. In sum, awareness should be raised among occupational
groups in charge of institutional encounters about the language barrier’s impact on their
own exercise of discretion and professional integrity. At the same time, the realization that
interpreters engage in a fallible activity through the exercise of discretion exposes the need
for interpreter education.
271
Hanne Skaaden
Prunč and Schlöger 2011; Cirillo and Niemants 2017; Sagli and Skaaden, this volume). On
a general note, it is worth observing that extensive and fluid delineations of the interpreters’
occupational role add difficulties to the development of functional interpreter education.
If the interpreter is to act as advocate or co-diagnostician, interpreter education must in
theory – and p ractice – prepare
the students to perform as knowledgeable agents within the
cultures of all professions they may serve in their future practice. Consequently, interpreter
education will become very expensive and lengthy.
Fragmentation of the profession is an obstacle to professionalization and certainly to
establishing education. Following Inghilleri ([2005b] 2014: 5), the profession’s members
must establish ‘a more stable interpreting habitus’ that can ‘a llow all participants to operate
with a shared understanding regarding interpreting activity’. At the same time, Inghilleri
draws attention to the disruption of accustomed power relations caused by interpreting.
The circumstance, that the structure and reflexive conduct of interpreted events differ from
non-interpreted institutional encounters, underscores the necessity to also educate the pro-
fessionals in charge of the institutional encounter on how to communicate via interpreters
(Felberg and Sagli, this volume, Skaaden 2017: 339).
To sum up, the field’s fragmentation serves to reiterate the shortage of educational oppor-
tunity for interpreters. In turn, lack of education has negative repercussions even for research
and the building of the profession’s knowledge base, as I shall argue next.
272
The professionalization of interpreters
A virtue of empirical research is to describe ‘what is’ and not ‘what ought to be’ (Inghilleri
2005a: 142). Yet, the researcher’s findings are invalid if the object of study is ill defined. An
analogy with the medical profession serves to substantiate my point: One would not observe
lay persons performing surgery and subsequently let the findings model the future practice
of medicine.
To conclude, PSI research may gain from reconstructing its research habitus in order
to avoid (re)producing knowledge that may lead to unintended practices. With its shift
from cognition to interaction, interpreting studies seem to have lost sight of its core activ-
ity and the fact that the activity of interpreting is based on both cognition and interaction.
Wadensjö (1998: 106) emphasizes that ‘it is not an empirical question whether interpreters are
translators or mediators – they cannot avoid being both’. The profession’s knowledge base
must include the study of language as a sense-making activity in interaction, on the level of
micro ordering, as well as the activity of interpreting as societal function, on the level of macro
ordering. As argued here, expanding the interpreters’ occupational role into ‘advocate’ and
‘co-diagnostician’ has undesired implications for the macro ordering of societal functions.
Before the field of research on PSI concludes on the extension of the interpreter’s domain for
the exercise of discretion, the interpreters’ positioning and coordination within the micro
orderings of the interpreted event should be explored in more detail and fed back into prac-
tice through education before being subject to further future research.
Further reading
Cirillo, Letizia, and Natacha Niemants (eds) (2017) Teaching Dialogue Interpreting. Amsterdam, John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
The book presents research based contributions to interpreter education, thereby displaying various as-
pects of dialogue interpreting and the interpreters’ area of expertise, thus, their domain for the exercise of
discretion.
Molander, Anders, and Harald Grimen (2010) “Understanding professional discretion”, in Sociology
of Professions. Continental and Anglo-Saxon Traditions. Lennart G. Svensson and Julia Evetts (eds).
Gothenburg, Daidalos: 1 67–87.
The article explains the nature and structure of the ‘exercise of discretion’ in professional practice in general.
Phelan, Mary, Mette Rudvin, Hanne Skaaden. and Patrick Stefan Kermit (2020) Ethics in Public Service
Interpreting. London, Routledge.
The book discusses the difference between personal and professional ethics, explores the link between eth-
ics and professional development in interpreting and provides a theoretical basis for the field of PSI’s ethical
dilemmas.
Related chapters
hapter 1, General issues of PSI: Institutions, codes and norms, professionalization by Carmen Valero Garcés
C
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in Public Service Interpreting by Claudio Baraldi
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to communicate via interpreter by Tatjana R. Felberg and
Gry Sagli
References
Agar, Michael H. (1985) “Institutional discourse”, in Text & T alk -Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study
of Discourse 5 (3): 147–68.
Alexieva, Bistra (2002) “A typology of interpreter-mediated events”, in The Interpreting Studies Reader,
Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam Shlesinger (eds). London, Routledge: 218–34.
273
Hanne Skaaden
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2004) Medical Interpreting and C ross-Cultural Communication. Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
Bot, Hanneke (2005) Dialogue Interpreting in Mental Health. Amsterdam, Rodopi.
Bourdieu, Pierre (1977) Outline of a Theory of Practice, transl. Richard Nice. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
D’Hayer, Danielle. (2012). “Public service interpreting and translation: Moving towards a (v irtual)
community of practice”, Meta 57 (1): 235–47. https://doi.org/10.7202/1012751ar (accessed 12
November 2021).
Dworkin, Ronald (1978) Taking Rights Seriously. Harvard, Harvard University Press.
Felberg, Tatjana R., and Hanne Skaaden (2012) “The (de)construction of culture in interpreter-mediated
medical discourse”, Linguistica Antverpiensia, New Series. Themes in Translation Studies 11: 67–84.
Finkielkraut, Alain (1997) The Wisdom of Love. Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.
Freidson, Eliot (2001) Professionalism. The Third Logic. Cambridge, Polity Press.
Gentile, Adolfo (1997) “Community interpreting or not? Practices, standards and accreditation”, in
The Critical Link: Interpreters in the Community. Papers from the First International Conference on Interpret-
ing in Legal, Health and Social Service Settings, Silvana E. Carr, Roda Roberts, Aideen Dufour, and
Dini Steyn (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 109–19.
Gentile, Paola (2017) “Political ideology and the de-professionalization of public service interpreting:
The Netherlands and the United Kingdom as case studies”, in Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development
in Public Service Interpreting and Translation, Carmen Valero-Garcés and Rebecca Tipton (eds). Bris-
tol, Multilingual Matters: 63–84.
Giambruno, Cynthia (2014) Assessing Legal Interpreter Quality through Testing and Certification: The Qual-
itas Project. Alicante, Publicaciones de la Universidad de Alicante.
Giddens, Anthony (1984) The Constitution of Society. Cambridge, Polity Press
Gile, Daniel (2009) Basic Concepts and Models for Interpreter and Translator Training. Revised edition.
Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Goffman, Erving (1974) Frame Analysis. An Essay on the Organization of Experience. Cambridge, MA,
Harvard University Press.
Grbić, Nadja (2015) “Profession”, in The Routledge Encyclopedia of Interpreting Studies, Franz Pöchhacker
(ed). London, Routledge: 321–26.
Grimen, Harald (2008a) “Profesjon og tillit” [Profession and trust], in Profesjonsstudier [The Sociology of
Professions], Anders Molander and Lars I. Terum (eds). Oslo, Universitetsforlaget: 197–215.
——— (2008b) “Profesjon og profesjonsmoral” [Profession and professional moral], in Profesjonsstud-
ier, [The Sociology of Professions], Anders Molander and Lars I. Terum (eds). Oslo, Universitetsfor-
laget: 144– 60.
——— (2008c) “Profesjon og kunnskap” [Profession and knowledge], in Profesjonsstudier, [The Sociol-
ogy of Professions], Anders Molander and Lars I. Terum (eds). Oslo, Universitetsforlaget: 71–87.
Grimen, Harald, and Anders Molander (2008) “Profesjon og skjønn” [Profession and discretion], in
Profesjonsstudier, [The Sociology of Professions], Anders Molander and Lars I. Terum (eds). Oslo, Uni-
versitetsforlaget: 179–96.
Hale, Sandra (2007) Community Interpreting. London/New York, Palgrave Macmillan.
——— (2008) “Controversies over the role of the court interpreter”, in Crossing Borders in Community
Interpreting. Definitions and Dilemmas, Carmen V alero-Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam,
John Benjamins: 99–123.
Inghilleri, Moira (2005a). “The sociology of Bourdieu and the construction of the “object” in trans-
lation and interpreting studies”, The Translator 11 (2): 125–45.
——— ([2005b] 2014) “Mediating zones of uncertainty: Interpreter agency, the interpreting habitus
and political asylum adjudication”, The Translator 11 (1): 69–85.
Kainz, Claudia, Erich Prunč, and Rafael Schlöger (eds). (2011) Modelling the Field of Community Inter-
preting. Questions of Methodology in Research and Training. Wien, LIT Verlag.
Langacker, Ronald W. (1994) “Culture, cogition, and grammar”, in Language Contact and Language
Conflict, Martin Pütz (ed). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 2 5–53.
Linell, Per (2009) Rethinking Language, Mind, and World Dialogically. Interactional and Contextual Theory
of Human Sense-Making. Charlotte, Information Age Publishing.
Mikkelson, Holly ( 1996) “ Community interpreting: An emerging profession”, Interpreting 1 ( 1):
125–31.
274
The professionalization of interpreters
Molander, Anders, and Lars I. Terum (2008) “Profesjonsstudier – en introduksjon” [The sociology of
professions, an introduction], in Profesjonsstudier. [The Sociology of Professions], Anders Molander and
Lars I. Terum (eds). Oslo, Universitetsforlaget: 13–27.
Morris, Ruth (2008) “M issing stitches: An overview of judicial attitudes to interlingual interpreting
in the criminal justice systems of Canada and Israel”, Interpreting 10 (1): 34–65.
NOU (2014) Tolking i offentlig sektor. Et spørsmål om rettsikkerhet og likeverd. [Interpreting in the Public Sector.
A Question of Legal Safeguard and Equality]. Oslo, Norges Offentlige Utredninger.
Ozolins, Uldis (1991) Interpreting, Translating and Language Policy. Melbourne, National Languages
Institute of Australia.
——— (2010) “Factors that determine the provision of public service interpreting: Comparative per-
spectives on government motivation and language service implementation”, The Journal of Special-
ised Translation. JosTrans, July 2010, 14: 194–215.
Parsons, Talcott (1968) “Professions”, in International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, David L. Sills
(ed). Vol. 12. New York, The Free Press: 536–47.
——— (1978) Action Theory and the Human Condition. New York, The Free Press.
Pokorn, Nike K., Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatjana R. Felberg (eds). (2020) Teacher Education for Community
Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation: Selected Chapters. Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press, Fac-
ulty of Arts and National Institute of Public Health.
Prunč, Erich (2012) “R ights, realities and responsibilities in community interpreting”, The Interpreters’
Newsletter 17: 1–12.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2004) Introducing Interpreting Studies. London, Routledge.
——— (2008) “Interpreting as mediation”, in Crossing Borders in Community Interpreting. Definitions
and Dilemmas, Carmen Valero-Garcés and Anne Martin (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 9 –26.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2013) “Community interpreting”, in The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, Carol A.
Chapelle (ed). Blackwell Publishing Ltd: 1–7.
Roy, Cynthia (2002) “The problem with definitions, descriptions, and the role metaphors of inter-
preters”, in The Interpreting Studies Reader, Franz Pöchhacker and Miriam Shlesinger (eds). London,
Routledge: 344–54.
Scollon, Ron, and Suzanne W. Scollon (2001) Intercultural Communication. Malden, MA, Blackwell
Publishing.
Skaaden, Hanne (2013) Den topartiske tolken. Lærebok i tolking. [The Interpreter’s Double Allegiance. Text-
book on interpreting.] Oslo, Universitetsforlaget.
——— (2016) “Professionalization and trust in public sector interpreting”, in International Journal of
Language Translation and Intercultural Communication. Special Issue, Eleftheria Dogoriti and Theodoros
Vyzas (eds). 5: 56–66.
——— (2 017) “‘T hat we all behave like professionals’. An e xperiential–dialogic approach to inter-
preter education and online learning”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting. R esearch-Based Proposals
for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (e ds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins:
324– 4 0.
——— (2018) “Invisible or invincible? Professional integrity, ethics, and voice in public service in-
terpreting”, in Perspectives. Studies in Translation Theory and Practice 27 (5): 7 04–17. https://doi.org/
10.1080/0907676X.2018.1536725 (accessed 11 December 2021).
——— (2020) “Ethics and profession”, in Ethics in Public Service Interpreting, Mary Phelan, Mette Rud-
vin, Hanne Skaaden, and Patrick Stefan Kermit. London, Routledge: 147–201.
Tipton, Rebecca (2008) “Reflexivity and the social construction of identity in interpreter-mediated
asylum interviews”, The Translator 14 (1): 1–19.
Tipton, Rebecca, and Olgierda Furmanek (2016) Dialogue Interpreting. A Guide to Interpreting in Public
Services and the Community. London, Routledge.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen. (2019) “Training public service interpreters and translators: Facing chal-
lenges”, Revista de Llengua i Dret 71: 88–105.
Valero-Garcés, Carmen, and Laura Gauthier. (2010). “Bourdieu and public service interpreting and
translation: Towards a social theory in PSIT”, MonTI 2: 97–117.
Valero-Garcés Carmen, and Rebecca Tipton (eds) (2017) Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development in Public
Service Interpreting and Translation. Bristol, Multilingual Matters.
Vargas Urpi, Mireia (2012) “State of the art in community interpreting research: Mapping the main
research topics”, Babel 58 (1): 50–72.
275
Hanne Skaaden
Venuti, Lawrence (2000) “Jerome: Letter to Pammachius” trans. Kathleen Davis, in The Translation
Studies Reader, Lawrence Venuti (ed). London, Routledge: 21–30.
——— (2000) “Translation, community, Utopia”, in The Translation Studies Reader, Lawrence Venuti
(ed). London, Routledge: 4 82–502.
Viezzi, Maurizio (2015) “Linguistic pluralism, multilingualism and plurilingualism in the EU”, Annu-
ario di Diritto Comparato e di Studi Legislativi 5: 503–20.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (2007) “Foreword. Interpreting professions, professionalization and professionalism”, in Crit-
ical Link 4: Professionalisation of Interpreting in the Community, Cecilia Wadensjö, Birgitta Englund
Dimitrova, and Anna Lena Nilsson (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 1 –8.
276
17
TRAINING SIGN LANGUAGE
INTERPRETERS FOR PUBLIC
SERVICE INTERPRETING
Christopher A. Stone, Cynthia B. Roy and Jeremy L. Brunson
Introduction
There are similarities and differences between public service interpreting (PSI) for spoken
language users and for sign language users (de Meulder and Haualand 2019). Often, while
settled immigrant communities may become fluent in local/national languages, deaf peo-
ple may already be fluent but unable to hear the local/national language. Spoken language
communities’ rights to interpreters stem from linguistic access, whereas for deaf people in-
terpreter access is often granted due to disability laws (Mathers 2006; Brunson 2007; de
Meulder 2017). One of the consequences of this is that PSI typically includes social services,
legal, medical, but for sign language interpreters it also regularly includes primary, second-
ary, and tertiary education as well as interpreting in the workplace/workforce and enter-
tainment (for example, theatrical performances). Globally we see many, if not all, of these
settings are paid by the public purse, often with the provision of professional interpreters
as outlined in the UN convention on the rights of people with disabilities (CRPD) (Stone
2013) and in the US in the disability act (A DA) (Brunson 2011).
Historically sign language interpreting (SLI) is something that has been provided by
members of deaf communities with an early Western example being in British North Amer-
ica in the seventeenth century (Carty, Macready and Sayers 2009). Professionalisation has
only really happened in the twentieth century with systemic consequences, including the
institutionalisation of interpreter education. The breadth of PSI settings for SLI has meant
that typically those raised in the community by sign language using parents have become
interpreters and interpreter trainers with very little if any formal interpreter education or
interpreter educator education. Initially, these heritage users provided informal training in
many countries, via workshops and collective action. This training was often in response to
satisfying an assessment of being safe to practice so that interpreters can be listed on a direc-
tory or register and then be eligible to be paid.
As Deaf communities better engaged with governments, and sign language interpreters
professionalised, we started to see the evolution of short-term courses via Deaf associations
or, after the creation of interpreting associations, these developments in collaboration with
interpreters. These often then led to more formalised training within community colleges
or universities (depending on the local context) beginning with training programs as short
Educational preparation
There has been an epistemological move in sign language interpreter education towards the
belief that the performance of interpreting should be the making of intentional, informed
choices when crafting an interpretation. To engage in this intentional activity requires stu-
dents to learn how the language communities will behave, be that linguistically, socially,
psychologically, historically, or cognitively (Roy, Brunson and Stone 2018). Experience tells
us that students should know the diverse sociolinguistic and cultural landscapes of the peo-
ple involved in interpreter-mediated interaction, and the impact of broader social forces on
everyone involved in an interpreter-mediated event.
Sign language interpreting preparation programs have long been conceptualised as train-
ing. In fact the naming convention for these programs, until recently, was Interpreter Train-
ing Programs or Interpreter Preparation Programs. Only recently do we see a change in this
nomenclature to IEPs. Training interpreters often implies a focus on technical skills such as
how to render a spoken word effectively at a phrasal or sentential level, without considering
the broader context within which the interpreter-mediated event is occurring. We suggest
278
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
that given that interpreting is a social phenomenon, embedded in social relations, education
is a more appropriate term as this not only focuses on the technical skills but also a more
comprehensive liberal education that better frames the work that PSIs undertake.
Furthermore, training can be understood as being completed when an interpreter gradu-
ates from a program, while completing an educational program implies the preparedness to
learn more. The change in nomenclature has included a move towards a discoursal and sys-
temic level of understanding of interpreter-mediated activity. Although there is conflation
between discourse as a linguistic phenomenon and discourse as a sociological phenomenon,
we would argue that interpreters need to engage in studies of both types of discourse and to
see their work within a sociohistorical context.
There are huge disparities in programs that could be a result of the lack of formal inter-
preter educator education. Many IEPs focus on (textual) equivalence (Wadensjö 1998), that
is, say exactly what they said, while a few programs focus on discourse and systemic-level de-
cisions. As mentioned above we are starting to see the further discussion of SLI PSI as being
part of systems and as such, the responsibilities placed on interpreters at a systemic level are
what we need to be mindful of when working as interpreters within public services. This
involves interpreters understanding that the interpretations they craft happen in a context
with real people and they need to be sensitive to the moments and systems in which they
find themselves.
Furthermore, research has examined the systemic instantiation of sign language inter-
preters’ PSI in video relay interpreting (Brunson 2011), health (Shaffer 2018), secondary
education (Brunson and Stone 2021), to date. These studies shone a light on these aspects of
social justice and suggest a need for interpreters to be much more aware of their agency when
working as interpreters; deaf people are often in an asymmetric power relationship with (
non-deaf ) service providers and the facilitation an interpreter can engage in for both parties
during an interpreter-mediated event moves beyond linguistic work.
SLI educators have been able to draw upon the literature of both spoken and sign lan-
guage IEPs, curricula, and initiatives (such as the MediSign or JustiSign projects funded by
the EU). This has meant that many programs around the world have robust role-models
when establishing programs. Should these programs be established in universities then the
universities often also have language programs and IEPs that enable c ross-fertilisation. How-
ever, Winston (2005) suggests that one of the disadvantages of programs being established
in universities is the lack of ensuring that sign language using deaf people are involved in all
aspects of the programs such that students are given appropriate r ole-modelling of linguistic
and cultural behaviours within the classroom environment. And so interpreter education
needs to ensure that the thought worlds, as in the attitudes, belief systems, assumptions of
both societal groups and individuals – the stakeholders, are included in the classroom in a
meaningful way.
279
Christopher A. Stone et al.
expectations of who the service user will be and the type of account they will give. There is
a need for interpreters in PSI to understand the questions being asked and the consequences
of the decisions made for those using interpreting services; this promotes a better under-
standing of the agency that interpreters can engage in and its limits. And for more elegant
interpreting decisions.
While these issues are important and necessary, some scholars (Pöchhacker 2016; Roy,
Brunson and Stone 2018) have suggested that the field of interpreting needs a broader con-
ceptualisation, that is, that there needs to be an integration of all of these foci to better rep-
resent interpreting from multiple perspectives and multiple levels of analysis. This drawing
together of multiple levels of analysis is perhaps the most critical issue facing interpreter
education worldwide. And it is rare to see multidisciplinary teams engaged in educating
interpreters.
As educators and curriculum designers, we rely on a conceptualisation of meaning as be-
ing created and determined m inute-by-minute within the participants’ exchange (Wadensjö
1998). This conceptualisation is essential for interpreting students to grasp and facilitate the
development of the threshold foundational interpreting skills of interaction and language
management. That said, we also suggest that a curriculum needs to include issues of so-
cial justice, identity politics, power relations, and more. Our aim must be that we devise a
curriculum that can educate students about the world in which they will be working upon
graduation.
Literature overview
Sign language interpreter education has suffered from a dearth of research into effective
educational strategies. As with the professionalisation of sign languages interpreters, there
has been a professionalisation of SLI educators which has had a commensurate impact on
curricula. Initially we saw those who worked as interpreters were those training other inter-
preters. Gradually we have seen courses develop to educate interpreter educators in teaching
and learning, and ‘andragogy’, that is, adult learning theory (K nowles, Holton and Swanson
2012), to ensure those educators could create appropriate learning frameworks for their
students, although this is something that is still not systemically done. This emerging devel-
opment has been supported by transnational work on program guidelines. More recently we
have seen work on situated learning (Stone and Hughes 2020) and cognitive apprenticeship,
both of which have contributed at improving education of sign language interpreters for PSI.
Below we will deal first with the international program guidelines and then we will discuss
some innovations as presented in the literature.
Program guidelines
Devising curricula involves putting into practice a set of beliefs concerning how people learn,
what they should be learning, and the contexts that will support learning. Within Europe
the European forum of sign language interpreters (efsli) coordinated a project (f unded by the
European Commission) that led to the development of assessment guidelines for IEPs (efsli
2013a) and learning outcomes for a three-year program (efsli 2013b). The project brought
together experts from across Europe who had experience delivering interpreter education in
university settings via efsli’s committee of experts. The aim was to create a standard across
the European Union with a focus on different approaches to assessing interpreters and iden-
tifying outcomes these assessments should measure.
280
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
Most interpreter training programs in Europe use virtual learning environments (V LEs),
and technology has allowed for the easy use of video recordings of classroom activities that
can support further reflection and analysis of one’s own interpreting work. VLEs and video
tech enable the collection of a dossier or portfolio of different types of assessments such as
reflective journals, practice exercises, and situated learning and situated assessments. These
dossiers support the adult learners to better understand their strengths and weaknesses.
As recommended, marking matrices allow for a transparency of assessment and enable
students to better gauge which aspects of performance they have demonstrated well. The
learning outcomes (efsli 2013b) support the development of programs that are comprehen-
sive, including intercultural competence and sociologically structured knowledge of relevant
deaf communities and of relevant (public service and other) institutions in mainstream soci-
ety. Nevertheless, since signed languages have limited diffusion, programs need to include
the learning and mastery of a signed language which is often how SLI programs differ from
their spoken language counterparts.
In 2019 the Commission of Collegiate Interpreter Education (CCIE) for interpreter ed-
ucation within the United States and Canada established ten standards for designing pro-
grams, evaluating, and analysing postsecondary professional IEPs. The standards address
institutional practices, faculty governance, curriculum development, practicum supervision,
and assessment of interpreter education to provide a common set of expectations about what
basic knowledge and competencies interpreting students should acquire.
CCIE also emphasises the need to involve deaf faculty in teaching interpreting. Although
there has been a long-standing political stance to support signed language teaching by
deaf people, the active involvement of deaf faculty in the teaching of interpreters has oc-
curred alongside the call for a more generally diverse faculty demographic and the exposure
within situated learning to diverse experiences, including deaf blind and deafened people
(Erlenkamp et al. 2014). This emphasis falls very much in line with the growing understand-
ing of the need to embed social justice within the curriculum and that a diverse faculty is
more likely to attract a diverse student body that will then graduate to ensure that the inter-
preting profession reflects the society within which it is found.
Our literature overview of educational programs focuses on three institutions: Mac-
quarie University (Sydney), Northeastern University (Boston), and Gallaudet University
(Washington DC), which have scholars at the forefront of educational developments in SLI
and with a long tradition in SLI training and education. Our discussion concerns two main
areas: innovative curricula, and innovative teaching practices.
Innovative curricula
At Macquarie University in Sydney, Australia, Napier (2006) developed a program aimed at
supporting working interpreters who sought further professional development by introduc-
ing a blended approach. Her concept of blending includes three aspects: (1) blending sign lan-
guage students with spoken language interpreting students in the same course; (2) blending
interpreting and discourse theories to provide a theoretical framework for the development
of skills; and (3) blending online and face-to-face teaching.
The proposal in Napier (2006) was innovative because up until that point, sign language
interpreters were typically educated separately from their spoken language interpreter peers.
Napier’s blended approach enabled interaction between interpreters using different modal-
ities, thus allowing for a common understanding of the similarities of PSI irrespective of
modality (that is spoken or signed languages) and for spoken and signed language interpreters
281
Christopher A. Stone et al.
to observe colleagues working within situated interpreter-mediated training tasks, like, for
instance, role plays.
Napier’s approach maintained a focus on interpreters working in the community at the
interface between citizens and public institutions. It also enabled a sharing of experiences
and strategies between interpreters working with different language combinations, and with
different language communities, thanks principally to online and face-to-face blended in-
struction. Moreover although the discourse analysis of monologues was included, the pro-
gram shifted its focus to the key concern in PSI, that is dialogue and multiparty discourse.
Blending is an approach that has been championed by other work as well. Since speech
and sign occur in different modalities, a conference norm of simultaneous interpreting is of-
ten expected in SLI. However, given that SLI takes place predominantly as PSI, such simul-
taneous norm is not necessarily followed. For example, Russell (2002) demonstrated how a
blended use of simultaneous and consecutive interpreting in legal contexts resulted in greater
accuracy and benefits those disadvantaged by an unfamiliar environment.
At Northeastern University (Boston, MA) changes happened in light of a survey of the
work assignments of interpreters, including those who had recently graduated from an IEP
(Cokely 2005). The survey found that within their first two years of work more than 60%
of graduates’ work was dialogic assignments, falling to 50% of the work of more senior
interpreters. The work assignments were ranked according to what seemed their main in-
teractional functions. In order of frequency, the top four interactions were: inquiry, where
questions and answers are exchanged; narrative, an everyday telling of what happened; ex-
pository, where information is given or exchanged; and, persuasive, where one speaker is
trying to persuade others to do or believe something (Cokely 2005: 11).
Based on this finding, Cokely revised the curriculum so that students practiced working
in these types of interaction in four sets (one per function) of 1 5-week skills development
courses. Within this program students would not only learn about how to deal with these
interactional functions but would also learn how to determine when or whether to apply
translation (a s in rendering an English medical form into ASL and the ASL responses into
written English), consecutive, or simultaneous techniques while interpreting.
At Gallaudet University innovative curricula focusing on dialogue and interaction were pro-
posed for both the BA and the MA levels (Shaw, Collins and Metzger 2006). Initially the students
took a course labelled discourse analysis and examined spoken English dialogues, American
Sign Language dialogues, and spoken and signed interpreter-mediated dialogues, to understand
question-answer pairs, overlapping, prosody, and other characteristics of communication. This
is done by students’ use of video recordings and then annotating the features of interaction they
found in it, potentially using a video annotation freeware such as ELAN (Version 6.2) (2021).
This knowledge was then transferred and expanded in ensuing courses for interpreting in differ-
ent settings (medical, education, business, and government). The analysis of discourse was to be
integrated and practiced throughout skills development, internships, and practicums, as well as
infused into every assessment product and process. At the MA level the expectations increased
with respect to reading the professional literature, writing essays, and research projects.
Discourse mapping
Winston and Monikowski (2000, 2005) created the activities of discourse mapping as a
way of introducing the practice of discourse analysis; that is, a way to give students time to
282
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
identify the linguistic structures, patterns, and strategies that are used to communicate in any
language, and a vocabulary to talk about their findings. Discourse mapping offers a system-
atic approach for analysing any text be it written, spoken, or signed, monologue, dialogue,
or multiparty interaction. Developing the ability to analyse discourse structures supports
students’ ability to create and recognise successful, effective, and appropriate interpretations.
When infused into interpreting courses, discourse mapping sparks critical thinking about
different aspects of interpreting. These include the comprehension and production of lan-
guage within a discourse context. It also provides a framework for self-analysis to examine
the original utterance and/or target language renditions. Learners develop the skills they
need to assess their own interpretations, to review their analyses of the communication, and
to gauge the effectiveness of their own work. They can also identify where discourse goals
were skewed in an interpreter-mediated event.
Discourse mapping includes a rich series of flexible activities designed to help educators
encourage analytical thinking and critical assessment. Its goal is that SLI shifts from simple
lexical equivalence to an improved adherence to interlocutors’ communicative goals. This
has been influential in the teaching of signed language interpreters since 2000 within aca-
demic programs. In many ways thinking of interpreting as a discourse process (Roy 2000)
is today so embedded in our profession that discourse mapping comes as an almost natural
activity in the foundation of interpreter education.
Learning within this community of practice exposes students to the multimodal nature
of sign language interpreter-mediated interaction, including co-speech and no-speech
283
Christopher A. Stone et al.
The authors v ideo-recorded student role plays and found examples when student interpreters
were driven by notions of language fidelity, rather than communication fidelity. Sign lan-
guage cultures are gesture-rich cultures (Kusters et al. 2017) and when interacting with non-
signing people, deaf people will use language as well as other communication strategies to try
and engage in direct communication; strategies which should not be overlooked or ignored.
The multimodal nature of the interaction within the situated learning environment facili-
tated the students’ exposure to and learning of situationally driven interpreting choices. This
technical environment thus allowed the practice of interpreting to model r eal-world interac-
tive settings and provided for more authentic data for the analysis of interpreted interaction.
For example, when the nurse named a suggested remedy, the deaf patient used the sign for
drink (that is synonymous with the UK gesture for drink) which the nurse identified. The
student interpreter continued to render the spoken and signed originals without acknowl-
edging that direct interaction had occurred. Debriefing after the situated learning event, and
viewing the video recorded interaction, enabled discussions with the senior interpreter (who
was teaching), senior healthcare professional, and Deaf community member (the patient in
this scenario) about the need to pay attention to multimodal communication both for opti-
mising communication and to improve rapport building.
Technological equipment like w all-mounted video cameras and c eiling-mounted micro-
phones contributed to a fundamental shift in the analysis of sign language interaction since
multimodality is a constitutive feature of this type of communication, so role plays taking
place within a situated learning framework like the one described above are going to pos-
itively influence SLI PSI research. Such technology will probably be used increasingly and
is already more and more frequently employed in practicum, which we shall now discuss.
Practicum
The training of SLI, as with any service field, has always attempted to include moments
when students were expected to learn outside the classroom, observing or working alongside
more senior colleagues, and being better exposed to ‘live engagement’ in PSI. This takes the
form of in/externships, work placement, practicums, and volunteer work during courses and
at the end of courses to support the transition of graduates into the world of work. There
has long been a call for a greater emphasis being ‘g iven to the relationship between what is
learned in the classroom and what is needed outside the classroom, and this has been a valu-
able contribution of the situated learning movement’ (A nderson, Reder and Simon 1996: 5).
This not only focuses on service learning to improve technical skills but also to provide a
better context for the delivery of interpreting service. For most SLI programs there is a need
for students to better understand deaf community structures, institutionalised interpret-
ing provision, developing professional networks, and better understand collegiality in the
field/on the job (Monikowski and Peterson 2005; Shaw 2013).
Historically we have examples in the UK of SLI training as part of training for Church of
England clerics for the deaf (as part of a multi-professional role, Pym 2014). This training in-
volved in-service training within Deaf ‘Missions’ – welfare centre and social clubs for deaf peo-
ple (Simpson 2007). And this ‘earlier provision required the trainee missioner/welfare officers
to spend much of their time in the company of, and interpreting for, deaf people with a conse-
quential emphasis on good communication/interpreting skills’ (Simpson 1991: 217). Gradually
284
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
with the shift away from those born into the community providing interpreting to the profes-
sionalisation and institutionalised training of interpreters, there was a move away from training
embedded within sign language communities in many Western/Global North contexts.
This has now come full circle with a recognition of the need for both service training for
language development and cultural awareness earlier in SLI education programs, and train-
ing internships specifically focused on observing and junior practice as sign language inter-
preters within community settings supporting transitioning from training to employment.
This can be hard to ensure within u niversity-based courses, where work needs to take place
to ensure, one, there is a network of community places and spaces for language immersion,
and two, there is a network of working professional interpreters appropriately trained to
support and mentor student interpreters within real workplace settings.
Professional interpreters sometimes form part of universities’ extended graders/m arkers of
the student interpreters’ work in the field (see efsli 2013a for some workplace learning rubric
templates). In some institutions these workplace assessments include fitness to practice assess-
ments, that is, to ensure that potential graduates have the appropriate professional integrity
to work as public service interpreters. One criticism of this is that often Deaf community
members are not included in the assessment of interpreters and yet they are the stakeholders
typically most disadvantaged by poor-quality interpreting services.
Sociological issues
Every interpreted event is bound by the social relations in which it occurs. Interpreters and
the interlocutors for whom they are interpreting must contend with their own embedded-
ness in these social relations. Issues of power that are shaped by marginalised identities come
to bear on the interpretation. Interpreters must recognise that their work involves more than
language work. They are also responsible for transmitting culture and context.
In line with greater levels of education of deaf people in society, more deaf people with
formal academic qualifications are now teaching interpreter students and their institutional
status is equal to their non-deaf interpreter educator peers. This also enables greater levels
of sign language medium education, with deaf people actively engaged in conversations,
including cultural sensitivity, different sensory orientations, and cultural differences in the
understanding and application of professional ethics. This also means that the education of
interpreters cannot solely focus on learning to interpret. To provide effective interpretations,
practitioners must be versed in a myriad of topics in which service recipients are competent.
Indeed, an interpreter working at the United Nations must understand geopolitics while
an interpreter working with a philosophy lecturer in a university class on d ecision-making
must have more than a rudimentary understanding of concepts such as consequentialism,
deontology, and virtue ethics to render an effective interpretation. An interpreter focusing
merely on the languages they are interpreting between will likely miss the overall goal (Gish
1987), which will make the service recipient confused or require them to figure out where
the conversation or presentation is going.
Crowded curricula
In broader terms there are some critical issues that IEPs are facing that many institutions
struggle with, and at the time of this writing, with the world economy hit by a pandemic
285
Christopher A. Stone et al.
might become more exacerbated. As discussed above, there are many issues that BA and
MA programs wish to cover leading to a very crowded curriculum. Juggling the appropriate
teaching of technical skills development, the professionalisation of working languages, and
intercultural interaction management is already a mammoth task. And yet there is an ever-
stronger desire to ensure that there is a better understanding of the role of the interpreting/
interpreter cog within a much larger systemic machine. Perhaps integrated MA programs
(five years BA+MA) might be a preferred format, but these can be hard to establish in many
contexts as there is no tradition for them, and it limits the flexibility of recruiting students
from different demographics with different educational needs.
Financial viability
We also see that generally there are increasing financial pressures on universities in both the
UK and the US to make money. Most IEPs have smaller class sizes than larger p rofit-making
subjects. Class sizes need to be smaller to ensure good-quality language teaching, appropriate
technical skills development, and meaningful discussions on the role and place of interpreting
within society. Many universities emphasise the retention of recruited students. When stu-
dents are not successful in lower-level classes, faculty might be encouraged to give them pass
marks so as to avoid losing a potential paying student. Furthermore, when too few students are
progressing through the program, the classes can become too small to justify running a class.
Deaf interpreters
One area of import is the e ver-increasing number of deaf people who are practising as profes-
sional interpreters within PSI contexts. The U S-based Deaf Interpreter Institute (DII 2021)
engages a team of experts under the National Consortium of Interpreter Education Centers
(NCIEC). Just as efsli and the CCIE the DII has outlined competencies that deaf interpreters
need, such as identifying the lived experience of being deaf and the communicative experi-
ence that deaf people bring to their professional interpreting. These competencies are a result
of a 2006 survey of deaf interpreters or DIs in the US and so predate both efsli and the CCIE.
The Danish Deaf Association (DDL) led a project to identify and create curriculum
guidelines for deaf people to be educated as interpreters. This was funded by the European
Commission and focused on developing a curriculum as a European standard. Using the efsli
learning outcomes document (efsli 2013b) as a template (w ith efsli being one of the project
partners), a curriculum was devised with experts from European universities with experience
of training deaf people to work as interpreters in a variety of settings. These settings included
PSI either between different sign languages for deaf migrants (either voluntary or through
involuntary displacement, see Mobile Deaf (2017–23), or for language-deprived individuals
with idiosyncratic language use and in need of language adaptation and modification. Some
of the issues for deaf interpreters in IEPs are not dissimilar to hearing heritage language
signers; as programs are typically focused on teaching those wishing to be sign language
interpreters but still needing extensive language teaching rather than those raised using a
signed language and/or using a signed language for primary face-to-face communication.
286
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
Working in VMI presents interpreters with a wider range of linguistically and culturally
diverse customers, topics, and prolonged periods of work than is found in any other in-
terpreting setting. Interpreters are assigned to calls as requests come in, and most calls are
handled with little or no advance preparation to minimise wait time. Work in VMI settings
is physically and mentally stressful, and signed language interpreters may be expected to
work with intimate register (for example, calls to family members), unfamiliar vocabu-
lary, and subject matter outside their level of education, experience, or comfort. There are
different models of VMI across the globe. The one available in Sweden, for example (see
Warnicke and Plejert 2016), is quite different from the US models of service delivery. Even
so, interpreters do report the lack of decision latitude causes additional stress (Dean and
Pollard 2001), a factor that, to our knowledge, is only just being considered in IEPs and is
yet to be embedded in curricula.
Furthermore, interpreters sometimes encounter unfamiliar regional or cultural variations
of signed languages used by Deaf callers, and unfamiliar regional and cultural variations
of spoken languages used by non-deaf callers. The number of deaf individuals from ethnic
and racial minority and immigrant populations using VMI has increased, as has the number
of deaf callers who may use sub-culture or idiosyncratic sign language. Some call centres
employ interpreters with multilingual competence that field international calls. For exam-
ple, in the US, American Sign Language/ Spanish/ English interpreters often serve callers
from Spanish-speaking countries. Even so, there are widely varying regionalisms, and higher
risks of misunderstanding, due to the plethora of cultures and signed languages. In the US,
the idea of being a sign language interpreter with multiple languages is relatively new but
in countries such as New Zealand, where, for example, Maōri is an official language, SLI
IEPs focus on dominant languages while hoping to ensure that SLI can deal with formulaic
greetings typically used in community settings (like the use of Welsh in Wales; Spanish in
the US; French in Canada, and so on).
In VMI call centres, specifically established to ensure accessible telephone calls (k nown
as video relay services – VRS) interpreter skills may vary widely. Unskilled interpreters no
doubt place a large onus on callers to manage their own access. Brunson (2010) describes
how Deaf callers sometimes have to call several times before they are matched with an in-
terpreter they can understand, and who can understand them, a phenomenon he refers to as
‘calculated consumer labour’. Apart from training for SL interpreters, to better adapt their
language for deaf callers, training is also needed for communication facilitators, who can
copy what the VRS interpreter signs on the screen for Deaf Blind individuals, who use tactile
sign language (see Sforza 2014 for a broader description of SLI for Deaf Blind individuals).
IEPs in SLI also rarely focus on the skills needed to provide quality renditions when inter-
preters work into and out of multiple languages in a language chain.
VMI service in public settings, particularly in hospitals, police stations, and prisons, is
fast becoming a default way of providing interpreting irrespective of its appropriateness. It
can, however, be hindered by technical and logistical problems that compromise the intel-
ligibility of the interpretation and the comfort of the deaf individual especially if this is via
mainstream platforms rather than bespoke SLI platforms. Mainstream technology is fraught
with problems attributable to poor quality transmission, equipment function, and issues
related to bandwidth, firewalls, lost connections, and staff’s lack of familiarity with the
technology. Unseen and unheard participants, procedures, and interactions within the room
in which the Deaf person is situated can all influence the effectiveness of the interpretation
because the interpreter is in a remote location. The Deaf individual cannot see who else, if
anyone, is in the room with the interpreter, which can create discomfort and raises concerns
287
Christopher A. Stone et al.
about confidentiality, especially in medical and legal settings (see Licoppe this volume, for a
detailed description of spoken-language interpreting via video in court proceedings).
Educating sign language interpreters in how to use available video technique can include
how to ensure the ‘pinning’ or ‘spotlighting’ of interpreters for deaf people to see, and how
to optimise the visual orientation of the deaf service user (Stone and Köhring 2021). Train-
ing in how to collaborate with a non-rendering interpreter-technician may also be needed.
If the Deaf participant is in crisis – frightened, ill, medicated, disoriented, or experiencing
vision d ifficulties – a two-dimensional screen and the lack of full-spectrum view of the
other participants’ room may further hinder access to immediate and accurate information.
Obviously, the video screen is not a viable option for individuals who are Deaf Blind. For in-
dividuals with certain mental health issues, the usefulness of video technique may be limited
too. In a p ost-COVID-19 world interpreters no doubt need to have further training in the
negotiation of VMI-service delivery.
Further reading
Ehrlich, Suzanne, and Jemina Napier (eds) (2015) Interpreter Education in the Digital Age. Washington,
D.C., Gallaudet University Press.
This collection brings together innovative research and approaches for blended learning using digital technol-
ogy in interpreter education for signed and spoken languages.
Napier, Jemina (ed) (2009). International Perspectives on Sign Language Interpreter Education. Washington,
D.C., Gallaudet University Press. (updated volume expected 2022).
This collection provides an international overview on interpreter training from experts in Austria, Australia,
Brazil, Canada, Fiji, Finland, Ireland, Japan, Kenya, Kosovo, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Scotland,
Sweden, and the United States.
Winston, Elizabeth A. (2005) “Designing a curriculum for American sign language/English inter-
preting educators”, in Sign Language Interpreting and Interpreter Education: Directions for Research and
288
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
Practice, Marc Marschark, Rico Peterson and Elizabeth A. Winston (eds). Oxford, Oxford Univer-
sity Press: 208–34.
This chapter considers the learning requirements of interpreters wishing to become interpreter educators
within a United States context. It explores course content and the rationale behind the type of education edu-
cators require.
Related chapters
Chapter 1, General issues of PSI: Institutions, codes and norms, professionalisation by Carmen Valero-Garcés.
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason.
Chapter 15, A shared responsibility for facilitating inclusion where signed language interpreting is provided by
Sigrid Slettebakk Berge.
Chapter 18, Role play as a means of training and of testing PSI by Magnus Dahnberg.
Chapter 21, The Conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica P. B. Hansen and Elisabeth Stokoe.
Chapter 25, Education and training of public service interpreter teachers by Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
References
Anderson, John R., Lynne M. Reder, and Herbert A. Simon (1996) “Situated learning and education”,
Educational Researcher 25 (4): 5 –11. https://doi.org/10.3102/0 013189X025004005.
Brunson, Jeremy. L. (2007) “Your case will now be heard. Sign language interpreters as a problematic
accommodation in legal interactions”, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13 (1): 77–91.
——— (2010) “Visually experiencing a phone call: The calculated consumer labor deaf people per-
form to gain access through video relay services”, Disability Studies Quarterly 30. http://d x.doi.
org/10.18061/d sq.v30i2.1245.
——— (2011) Video Relay Service Interpreters: Intricacies of Sign Language Access. Washington, DC, Gal-
laudet University Press.
Brunson, Jeremy. L., and Christopher Stone (2021) “The sociological organization of K-12 educational
interpreting by the IEP”, in Advances in Educational Interpreting, Elizabeth. A. Winston and Stephen.
B. Fitzmaurice (eds). Washington DC, Gallaudet University Press: 250–93.
Carty, Breda, Susannah Macready, and Edna E. Sayers (2009) “‘A grave and gracious woman’: Deaf
people and signed language in colonial New England”, Sign Language Studies 9 (3): 287–323.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.0.0018.
CCIE (2019) Commission of Collegiate Interpreter Education website. http://w ww.ccie-accreditation.
org (accessed 26 November 2021).
Cogan, Cathy, and Dennis Cokely (2015) Preparing Interpreters for Tomorrow: Report on the Study of Emerg-
ing Trends in Interpreting and Implications for Interpreter Education. Boston, Massachusetts, National
Interpreter Education Center.
Cokely, Dennis (2005) “Curriculum revision in the t wenty-first century: Northeastern’s experience”,
in Advances in Teaching Sign Language Interpreters, Cynthia Roy (ed). Washington DC, Gallaudet
University Press: 1–21.
Dean, Robyn K., and Robert Q. Pollard (2001) “Application of d emand-control theory to sign lan-
guage interpreting; Implications for stress and interpreter training”, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Education 6 (1): 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/6.1.1.
de Meulder, Maartje (2017) “The influence of deaf people’s dual category status on sign language
planning: The British sign language (Scotland) Act (2015)”, Current Issues in Language Planning 18
(2): 215–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/14664208.2016.1248139.
de Meulder, Maartje and Hilde Haualand (2019) “Sign language interpreting services: A quick fix for
inclusion?”, Translation and Interpreting Studies 16 (1): 19–40 https://doi.org/10.1075/t is.18008.dem.
DII (2021) Deaf Interpreter Institute website. http://w ww.diinstitute.org. (accessed 26/11/2021).
ELAN (Version 6.2) [Computer software]. (2021). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguis-
tics, The Language Archive. ULR: https://a rchive.mpi.nl/t la/elan (accessed 23/02/2022).
Erlenkamp, Sonja, Guri Amundsen, Sigrid S. Berge, Trine Grande, Odd Morten Mjøen, and Eli Raanes
(2014) “Becoming the ears, eyes, voice and hands of someone else: Educating generalist interpreters
289
Christopher A. Stone et al.
290
Training sign language interpreters for PSI
Sforza, Stephanie (2014) “DI(2)= Team Interpreting”, in Deaf Interpreters at Work: International Insights,
Robert Adam, Christopher Stone, Steven D. Collins, and Melanie Metzger (eds). Washington,
DC, Gallaudet University Press: 19–28.
Stone, Christopher (2013) “The UNCRPD and ‘professional’ sign language interpreter provision”, in
Interpreting in a Changing Landscape: Selected Papers from Critical Link 6, Christina Schäffner, Krzysztof
Kredens, and Yvonne Fowler (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 8 3–100.
https://doi.org/10.1075/btl.109.08sto.
Stone, Christopher, and Thaïsa Hughes ( 2020) “
Facilitating legitimate peripheral participation
for student sign language interpreters in medical settings”, in Handbook of Research on Med-
ical Interpreting, Izabel E. T. de V. Souza and Effrossyni Fragkou (eds). IGI Global. https://doi.
org/10.4018/978-1-5225-9308-9.
Stone, Christopher, and Jenny Köhring (2021) “Sensory ecologies and semiotic assemblages during
British sign language interpreted weather forecasts”, International Journal of Multilingualism 18 (2):
1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790718.2020.1867149.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
Warnicke, Camilla, and Charlotta Plejert (2016) “The positioning and bimodal mediation of the inter-
preter in a Video Relay Interpreting (V RI) service setting”, Interpreting 18 (2): 198–230. https://doi.
org/10.1075/i ntp.18.2.03war.
Winston, Elizabeth A., and Christine Monikowski (2000) “Discourse mapping: Developing textual
coherence skills in interpreters”, in Innovative Practices for Teaching Sign Language Interpreters, Cynthia
B. Roy (ed). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 15–59.
——— (2005) “Translation: The gaps of discourse mapping”, in Advances in Teaching Sign Language
Interpreters, Cynthia B. Roy (ed). Washington, DC, Gallaudet University Press: 49–77.
291
18
ROLE PLAY AS A MEANS OF
TRAINING AND TESTING
PUBLIC SERVICE INTERPRETING
Magnus Dahnberg
Introduction
The simulated role play exercise is, among other things, a kind of bread-and-butter activity in
interpreter training; a method that in its simplest form (involving only interpreter candidates)
does not require much more than creativity and engagement from peers and could even be
used on a d ay-to-day basis. With teachers, instructors, or assessors engaged as role players and
observers, role play seems to work well to practice and test interpreting skills in a structured
way. And when it is possible to involve professional service providers – medical doctors, police
officers, social workers, and so o n – in simulated, interpreter-mediated institutional talk, this
adds another dimension, which is both interactional and professional, to the test or exercise.
Since a simulation is per definition not an authentic situation, there will always be at least
some elements in a role play that reflect real life to a very small degree or do not reflect it
at all. A classroom does not necessarily reflect the atmosphere of a health care centre or a
police station. An interpreter trainer, trying their best to act like a medical doctor in a role
play, does not necessarily pose questions or talk to the patient in the same way as any of the
real-life doctors that the interpreter student will ever meet in the future. A mock police of-
ficer and a mock interrogee, who are busy reading all of their utterances in a role play from
a script, do not necessarily react to the interpreter’s requests for clarification in a way that
might be frequently seen in the city’s real police stations. And a professional social worker,
who takes part in a role play exercise acting their professional self in a mock situation, does
not necessarily act in the same way as they normally would do at work. Different things are
at stake for participants in authentic interaction and in the role plays. There is good reason to
retain some scepticism about the idea that a role play could fully resemble reality.
The reality of the professional world, though, is complex and the task of pedagogy is to
help students to deal with it little by little. Thus, there is all the more reason to discuss which
skills can really be trained and tested in different types of role play in order to achieve the
desired learning or assessment objectives. There is no universal approach that allows us to
practice or test everything, and different arrangements provide different possibilities and
limitations, as will be shown in the following.
The examples from role plays given in this chapter are from video recordings of Swedish
state authorisation tests for Public Service interpreters, lasting 88 minutes in total, and audio
recordings of training exercises for Swedish military interpreters, lasting 134 minutes in total
( Dahnberg 2015).
Literature overview
When setting up a role play, you create a simulated situation with a high or at least a certain
resemblance to a real-life setting, for example, for training purposes (Crookall and Saunders
1989). It can be said to be a part of the concept of situated learning (Gonzalez-Davies and
Enríquez Raído 2018), even though situated learning comprises training in authentic situa-
tions as much as in simulated ones. Tipton and Furmanek (2016) see role play also as a means
of continued professional development for professional interpreters. At the same time, role play as
a method is also being questioned. A critical approach to role playing for training purposes
questions whether a traditional role play can be said to have any useful resemblance at all to
authentic situations (Stokoe 2011; Niemants and Stokoe 2017; and see also Niemants et al.,
this volume).
On the basis of dialogical theory, different contexts of interpreter-mediated talk can be
classified as different communication activity types (Linell 2010), or genres (Tebble 2014), which
seems to be a fruitful approach to creating role plays in order to train or test for different
Public Service Interpreting settings. The concepts of both communicative activity types and
genre focus on how, for example, a medical consultation or a police interrogation usually unfolds,
what happens in a typical situation, what are usually the overall and individual conversational
goals of the participants, and how they are reached. In order to get a high degree of resemblance
to a medical consultation, interpreting students can be trained in role play together with med-
ical students – a n exercise that will also benefit the medical students who then get the chance to
practice situations that they may encounter in their own future practice (K rystallidou et al.
2018). One may also use transcripts based on authentic PSI encounters as study material or as
a starting-point for role play training (Tebble 2014). Authentic data in itself, however, does
not necessarily provide a high degree of authenticity or usefulness for a role play based on
293
Magnus Dahnberg
these data, since such benefits depend also on whether the role play design is successful or
not (Gavioli 2018).
Analysing what should be trained and tested, that is the interpreting skills needed for
professional work, is crucial (Hale 2007). Skills that have been identified as vital are, among
others, complex interactional skills and code-switching (Skaaden and Wadensjö 2014), t urn-
taking (Frøili 2001), knowledge of professional ethics, language competence, listening and
comprehension, memory, public speaking, note-taking, conversational management, cross-
c ultural awareness, and theoretical knowledge (Hale 2007: 1 77–78). A general description of
the interpreter’s work says that it is to render and coordinate the primary participants’ talk. The
interpreter implicitly coordinates the conversation by regularly taking the floor to render a
previous utterance, and explicitly by, for example, requesting clarifications (Wadensjö 1998).
The coordinating function of interpreters has received increased attention in recent years
and is now well-established by research (Wadensjö 1998; Baraldi and Gavioli 2014; Krys-
tallidou 2016; Slettebakk Berge 2018, among others). When testing and assessing, a rubric
system for noting assessment may be used (Hale 2007; Hlavac et al. 2016). The testers need
to have a high level of knowledge about a broad range of subjects, from language standards
and understanding the complexity of sub-skills needed in interpreting, to test design and
advantages and disadvantages of different tests (Skaaden and Wadensjö 2014).
294
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
Scripts
Role plays based on scripts are called closed role plays. Such scripts, where the intended utter-
ances of the primary participants are written as in a script for a theatre play, may be based
on a script-writing author’s own ideas and general experience, or on transcripts from an
authentic PSI encounter.
In the first case, using a script obviously gives a (script-writing) trainer or test organiser
the possibility of working on comprehension and rendition of lexical items, by including any
words or phrases that they would like their students or examinees to use and interpret. Using
a script will also to some extent ensure a certain degree of reliability of a test, since one may
expect the same subject matter and formulations to occur in each and every test based on
the same script, which in its turn could be expected to facilitate the comparison of different
test-takers’ performance. However, the script in itself does not regulate the way it is read out
by the participants when it comes to speed, voice volume, pauses, possible misreading, and so
on. The level of resemblance to authentic PSI encounters will depend strongly on the script
author’s ability to create a base for the role players to achieve such a resemblance when acting.
Scripts based on transcripts from authentic PSI encounters, however, provide fewer pos-
sibilities for the trainer or test organiser to test specific words and phrases (for instance those
which were taught in the course) but can be expected to give a higher degree of resem-
blance to a naturally occurring situation concerning both vocabulary and overall discourse
structure.
Scripts may be written in one language only, mostly with the lines of both the majority
language speaker and the minority language speaker written in the majority language. In
such case, the persons playing the roles of minority language speakers will have to either
sight-translate their lines into the minority language while role playing or translate them in
advance and then read the translation out during the role play. Scripts may also be bilingual,
with each person’s lines written in their own language, and the interpreter being the only
one reacting extemporarily. A proposed rendition for the interpreter to check out afterwards
may also be included in the script.
There are strong indications that the presence of a script in itself has an impact on both
turn-taking and repair sequences in an interpreter-mediated dialogue, carried out as a role
play (Dahnberg 2015). Open role plays (see next section) show quite a complex pattern of
interaction around interpreters’ coordinating activities, such as giving and taking the floor
or asking for clarification, which scripted role plays do not show or show only to a limited
extent (ibid.). This means that when it comes to t urn-taking and the coordinating function
of the interpreter, role plays based on scripts can in general be expected to be less suitable for
295
Magnus Dahnberg
training and testing than open role plays (and, indeed, have a lower level of resemblance to
real-l ife encounters).
The following example is from a scripted training role play, where a professional inter-
preter gets specialised training at the Swedish Armed Forces Language School before being
deployed abroad. It is a mock interview where Julia, who plays the role of a journalist, in-
terviews Andrea, who plays the role of a Swedish military attaché. Ingvar is the interpreter
being trained.
Example 1
1 Julia а ну спасиббольшое (.) итак Андреа ↓ э как вы уже знаете я
well then thanks a lot (.) so Andrea eh as you already know I
заинтересовалась шведским взглядом на вопросы обороны в связи с
тем что
have become interested in the Swedish view of defence issues due to the fact that
в последнее время в средствах массовой информации обращалось
внимание
recently mass-media has paid attention
на высказывание шведского г л-*лавнокомандующего ◊
to the statement of the Swedish sup-*preme commander
2 Ingvar да ээм
yes eehm
3 Ingvar då så jag eh eeh e intresserad av (1.0) ehm den eeh (.) svenska eh eeh svenska eh
well so I eh eeh am interested of (1.0) ehm the eeh (.) Swedish eh eeh Swedish eh
försvars eh politiken på grund av att de eh i media nyligen har uppmärksammats
defence eh policy because media recently paid attention to
eh uttalanden från svenska eh överbefälhavaren
eh statements from the Swedish supreme commander
4 Andrea m
5 Julia ой (.) э да ↓ (.) вы вот меня перебили когда я говорила что /…/
oh (.) eh yes ↓ (.) now you interrupted me when I was saying that
(Dahnberg 2015: 100–3)
Julia is reading from her script (1). Ingvar takes the turn (2) and starts rendering her ut-
terance (3). Julia tells him he interrupted her (5), although there was no overlapping talk or
other signs of a turn-taking struggle, apart, possibly, from Julia’s continuation intonation
towards the end of turn 1. So, Julia’s comment about being interrupted by Ingvar is probably
due to the fact that she has not read through all of her lines yet. Playfully challenging, as it
were, Ingvar’s task, she is orienting to the script where there are still some words to read out
before Ingvar’s supposed rendition and Andrea’s answer to Julia’s question.
296
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
examination, diagnosis, recommended treatment, closure), and the individual goals of each
participant (correct diagnosis and treatment and getting rid of the pain in the back, respectively).
Since instructions for open role plays do not contain any lines to be read out verbatim by
the role players, the role players have more possibilities to improvise and speak spontaneously
than in closed role plays. Thus, role plays with role cards, in comparison to scripted role
plays, focus more on conversational goals and on the coordinating task of the interpreter, as
is seen in the next example below.
In Example 2, Julia is still acting the part of a journalist, but in this unscripted role play
she is having a conversation with Adam, a young Swede, about his life and work. Adam, who
has worked in a museum, happens to mention Greek mythology:
Example 2
1 Adam eh ja Minerva å Athena e ju samma sak fast °den ena e grekisk å den andra e
eh yes Minerva and Athena are the same thing but one is Greek and the other is
romersk tror jag° de e ju (.) a visst e hon visdomens [a fi-]
Roman I think it is of course (.) she is wisdom’s right [a fi-]
2 Julia [a]
[and]
3 Julia а Афина она же ещё отвечала (.) °она не была богиней войны
случайно°?
and Athena she also answered (.) wasn’t she the goddess of war by any chance
да да
that’s right
4 Ingvar [э так по-по-]»
eh so wa- wa-
5 Julia [она же была покровительница Афин]
she was the patroness of Athens
6 Ingvar »[подождите]»
wait a moment
7 Julia [m]
8 Ingvar »пожалуйста а (.) вот (.) Афина и Минерва это же э
please and (.) well (.) Athena and Minerva that is eh
9 Julia [m]
10 Ingvar [такая же]»
the same
11 Ingvar »богиня только Минерва (.) в Риме
goddess only Minerva (.) is in Rome
12 Julia [mhm]
13 Ingvar [a]»
and
14 Ingvar »в Гре- (.) там в Гре- в Греции э (.) э Афина
in Gre-there in Gre-in Greece eh is Athena
15 Julia mhm
16 Ingvar m
17 (1.0)
18 Julia aha хорошо я поняла↓
okay good I see
297
Magnus Dahnberg
In this example, Julia, who actually understands Swedish, seems to forget to wait for
Ingvar to render Adam’s utterance (1), and so she answers Adam directly (2 , 3, 5) in Russian.
Her talk is then coordinated by Ingvar: he interrupts Julia (4, 6) and explicitly asks her to
wait (6, 8), as it seems, for him to render Adam’s utterance (1). Unlike Example 1, where Julia
tells Ingvar he interrupted her, she now just gives a signal of approval (7). Since there is no
script in this role play, and thus, no lines to be read out, the interpreter seems to be given a
more active role in coordinating Julia’s and Adam’s talk. Even more visible is the interpreter’s
coordinating function in 1 8–21: when Julia in 18 seems to finish the sequence about Minerva
and Athena, not proceeding with the question she initiated in turn 3, Ingvar explicitly puts
Julia back on track (19, 21).
298
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
interprets from a written text and not from the sound of an utterance (Radanovic Felberg
2015). In order for the students to get acquainted with the various modalities and demands of
PSI interpreting settings (and to increase the resemblance of role plays to real-life situations),
there are reasons for introducing sight interpretation as part of the role play. From this point
of view, script use could well be limited to prima vista interpretation of, for instance, drug
prescriptions, or minutes from a police interrogation or an asylum interview.
Participants
In public service interpreter training and testing, the prototypical set of participants in role
plays includes three persons: one primary participant acting as a public service provider
(medical doctor, police officer, immigration services officer, etc.), one primary participant
acting as a layperson (patient, crime witness, asylum seeker, etc.), while the third person is
the interpreter being trained or tested. The participant acting out the role of public service
provider needs to display to some extent professional knowledge of the subject being dis-
cussed in the role play, and also of the type of institutional talk that is being simulated.
For that reason, I have chosen to use a role play taxonomy based on the composition of
participants when it comes to the above-mentioned level of professional knowledge: in profes-
sional role plays, the part of the service provider is acted out by a r eal-life professional service
provider, in semi-professional role plays the part of the service provider, and perhaps also the
part of the layperson, is played by a teacher, instructor, or actor, while in non-professional role
plays both of the primary participants are played by interpreter students.
The name professional refers here to a service provider’s supposed knowledge of their pro-
fessional field and of the communicative activity type in question, and not to their ability
within the field of role playing. Moreover, the term semi-professional is not intended to reduce
the teachers’ professional abilities in the field of teaching, using role play, which can actually
be expected to be higher than those of a service provider. But a PSI teacher, who usually is
not a professional within medicine, social service, or law, will still in most cases have a higher
degree of knowledge of the professional field of PSI and of the communicative activity type
in question than their students.
299
Magnus Dahnberg
course over a weekend. In this role play, Daniel, who is also a professional doctor, examines
a patient who complains of ear pain. Isidor interprets.
Example 3
1 Daniel har du haft nåra andra symtom ifrån halsen eller från nasofarynx
have you had any other symptoms in the throat or in nasopharynx
2 Isidor ээ а у вас какие то э другие ну >проблемы или симптомы< ээ в горле
или
eeh so do you have any eh other well >problems or symptoms < eh in the throat or
3 Isidor va-va sa du sist
what-what did you say in the end
4 Daniel nasofarynx
nasopharynx
5 Isidor эи
eh and
6 Daniel nässvalget
nose cavity and upper part of the throat
When Isidor gives signals (3, 5) that he faces some difficulty in rendering the term naso-
pharynx, Daniel, being a professional in medicine, immediately provides him with a more
colloquial term for the same thing.
The primary participants’ general knowledge and experience of the communication activ-
ity type trained or tested in a role play can also be expected to have a strong impact on their
interaction, and thus, on the resemblance of the role play to a naturally occurring situation.
This has been noted by several authors. Landqvist (2006) compares how experienced and
unexperienced negotiators interact in a series of unscripted, interpreter-mediated role plays:
Results show that competence is generally indicated by the special ways professional
negotiators use communicative projects, create phase structures, and organise topics
in local turns-of-talk and globally in the talk. Competence is also indicated by m
eta-
communication, which is used for constructing participant roles and authority, as well as
global coherence, all of which contribute to a formal level of interactive style (Landqvist
2006:abstract).
Bradford (2017: 5 –6) reflects on the use of professional service providers as primary partici-
pants in training interpreter students via role plays:
A potentially more attractive a lternative – whilst prey to the same issues as in having
two teachers play the roles – is to involve real service providers. Establishing links with
professional communities outside the university would be progressive and mutually
beneficial, as Hale suggests. However, the barrier here might be financial or due to the
workloads of such service providers. That said, if only one ‘real’ service provider were
to participate somehow in one seminar per term/semester, all parties concerned would
be enriched and gain new insights.
Krystallidou et al. (2018) also note the need for interpreter training to account for real-life
professional practice of medical doctors, reporting on a joint training intervention where in-
terpreting and medical students role played together as a means of interprofessional education.
300
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
The main point with the joint training is that each to-be practitioner (interpreter student
or medical student) gets to know about the work and practices of a professional group with
which they will collaborate in the future. In addition, the level of resemblance of the role
play encounter to a real-life situation is likely to be high for both doctors and interpreters.
Example 4
1 Ingvar hej jag heter ingvar ahlgren jag e tolk (.) jag e neutral opartisk har tystnadsp-
likt jag
kommer att tolka allt som säjs här och jag tolkar i första person↓
hi i am ingvar ahlgren i am an interpreter (.) i am neutral impartial have a duty of
confidentiality i will interpret everything said here and i interpret in the first person↓
2 Ingvar varsågoda
please
3 Ingvar пожалуйста
please
4 Peter bra tack (.) å så e de bra att du talar så starkt å tydlit också som du gör här så
vi får mä allting
fine thanks (.) and it’s good that you speak loudly and clearly as you do so that
we get everything
When Ingvar has finished his introduction (1) and asks the primary parties to start talking
(2 , 3), Peter makes a positive comment on Ingvar’s way of speaking (4). It is not quite clear
whether Peter makes this comment in his role of police officer, in his capacity as test or-
ganiser, or as a professional actor. But he seems to at least partly orient towards the testing
situation, making sure that it will be easy to hear and assess the interpreter’s renditions. In
a similar way, teachers as primary participants can make sure that their training session is
conducted so that its didactic goals are reached.
301
Magnus Dahnberg
or extra teachers acting out the role play parts and so may be used quite extensively during
a training programme. But interpreter students cannot be expected to have the professional
knowledge of real service providers, or the pedagogical abilities of their teachers. Thus, the
students will probably need detailed instructions about how to practise and what to practise
in a specific role play. In order to make the students practice special terminology, standard
phrases, or cultural references, the role play organiser can provide the students with scripts
(see Scripts above) where the needed phrases or terms are included.
Tebble (2014), in her genre-based approach to teaching interpreting, underlines the sim-
ilar structure and framework content (genre) of different medical consultations, suggest-
ing that interpreting students can benefit from role playing typical interactional sequences
(genre elements) in a d octor-patient encounter, if they first thoroughly study transcripts
from real PSI encounters and then themselves create scenarios based on these. Thus, prior
to role play, students would study and analyse the communication activity type in question,
including both typical genre elements such as greetings, stating the problem, diagnosing the
facts, and so on, and typical formulations and phrases occurring in these moments or phases.
In any case, fellow students role playing together can be expected to orient both towards
the mock institutional talk, the classroom situation, and their interpersonal relations. In
Example 5, Dennis, a (mock) doctor, examines Pavel, who (in the role play) suffers from
angina pectoris. Ivar interprets.
Example 5
1 Dennis a de e ju så för oss män att eh att fettet sätter sej (.) på magen de måste man
akta sej för
well it’s the case for us men that eh that the fat settles (.) on the stomach you have to
beware of that
2 Ivar /…/
3 Pavel э надо сознаваться у меня довольно большое брюшко
eh must admit I have a rather big belly
4 Ivar ja eh å eh jag har ju en rätt stor eh (.) mage
yeah eh and eh I have quite a big eh (.) belly
5 Dennis ja SKRATTAR
yes LAUGHS
Pavel claims to be fat (3) and Ivar interprets this (4). Dennis bursts into laughter (5), proba-
bly due to the contradiction between Pavel’s words in the role play and his slim real-life figure.
This form of role play, involving only interpreter candidates working on the basis of scripts,
might end up low on a resemblance scale if measured against a professional encounter. How-
ever, it can be used as a kind of everyday quantity training that does not require much prepara-
tion and may still help the students broaden their repertoire of common phrases and specialised
terminology in both their working languages (or, as in Example 5, help them deal with cre-
ativity and irony, which may introduce elements of unexpectedness into the scripted role play).
302
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
communicative activity type trained (or tested) can be expected to be higher in role plays
based on role cards or scenarios than in role plays based on scripts. The more the primary
parties have to improvise during the role play, the more their understanding of the intended
situation, and their professional knowledge of the communicative activity type trained or
tested, affects the way the interaction will unfold.
Example 6
1 Daniel men nu är den inte pärlemorfärgad utan röd och den buktar ut
but now it is not pearly white but red and it bulges out
/…/
2 Isidor сейчас он красноватый и и ээ чуть (.) щас ээм (1.0) ну он
now it is reddish and and er a little (.) well eerm (1.0) well it
просто (3.0) как сказать (1.0) ээ (1.0) ну не знаю э
er just (3.0) how do you say (1.0) eer (1.0) well I don’t know er
3 Patrick н
е знаете? я думаю что это ваша ваша работа знать (.) *ну правда*
SKRATT
don’t know? I think it’s your job to know *well really* LAUGHTER
Isidor signals (2) that it is difficult for him to render Daniel’s “it bulges out” (1), which is met
by a joking remark (3) from Patrick (these words are not included in the script), after which all
three of the role players start laughing. The individual objective of having a social gathering
with friends and colleagues is shown by the informal atmosphere of joking and laughing, as
well as the focus on brushing up language skills, as seen in Patrick’s utterance 3 (“I think it’s
303
Magnus Dahnberg
your job to know”). Patrick’s joke in 3 may also refer to Daniel being a medical doctor in real
life. Later in the dialogue, Patrick helps Isidor with finding an appropriate word in Russian.
This can be compared to the formal atmosphere in the next example from a state certi-
fication test. Paul plays the role of a police officer, interrogating a suspect on the matter of
goods smuggling. Paul is also part of the authorising commission which after the role play
will make a formal a ssessment – a summative one – of the interpreter’s skills. Inga interprets.
The results of her performance will decide whether she will receive the status of authorised
interpreter or not.
Example 7
1 Paul /…/ Liselott klinga→ har avgivit en fullständig bekännelse↓
Liselott Klinga has given a full confession
/…/
2 Inga eh (.) skulle du upp[repa]
er (.) would you re[peat]
3 Paul [hon har avgivit en] fullständig bekännelse
[she has given a] full confession
4 (3.0)
5 Paul hon har alltså erkänt allt
so she admitted everything
(Dahnberg 2015: 97)
No jokes are made, and no laughter is heard. When Inga asks for repetition (2), Paul repeats
the words from his line in the script more or less word by word (1, 3). Since the result of this
is silence (4), Paul again says, and now in his own words in a more colloquial style (these
words are not included in the script), that the suspect’s mate has made a full confession (5).
For Inga, what is at stake is her possible status as an authorised interpreter. It seems that
this makes her unwilling to take risks and make mistakes, or confess that she does not know
how to render Paul’s utterances. Instead, she asks for repetition or simply keeps quiet. Paul,
in turn, is both a primary participant in the talk and an assessor of Inga’s performance. For
him, what is at stake is, among other things, his contribution to a correct assessment, which
seems to make him want to stick to the script as strictly as possible. According to his instruc-
tions, he is supposed to act as naturally as possible, on the one hand, and on the other to make
sure that what he says matches the text in the script.
So, the interaction in a test role play can be expected to differ from the interaction in a
training one. At the same time, the participants’ individual objectives and goals also have
an impact on the interaction and thus, on how closely it resembles the authentic situation.
This means that a role play organiser should bear in mind the purposes for which a role
play is set up, since the participants’ interaction shows their orientation to these purposes.
Training and/or testing such different skills as general professional behaviour, interaction
(coordination, turn-taking), or rendition (accuracy, terminology, language treatment) are
likely to require different types of role play instructions and participants.
304
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
up is likely to affect how the interaction between the primary participants and the interpreter
will unfold, which means that a trainer or a tester should be able to choose an appropriate
setup for the purpose of each role play. If, for example, the purpose is to practice rendering
standard phrases or specialised terms, it would be appropriate to set up a scripted role play
where these phrases or terms are included in the script and occur there frequently. For such
a purpose, it seems unnecessary to hire a real public service provider just to read the lines
of the script – the students should probably be able to read the lines to each other. If, how-
ever, a role play is set up in order for students to practice the coordinating function of the
interpreter, it would be appropriate to set up a role play based on a scenario or on role cards,
where the students speak freely within the framework of the scenario. In order to allow the
interpreter students to become acquainted with a certain communicative activity type, it
seems appropriate to set up (unscripted) role plays that, as far as possible, involve participants
who are professionally specialised in the fields or institutions in question.
Thus, an interpreter trainer will benefit f rom – as a m inimum – knowing to what extent
one or the other role play setup can be expected to meet the trainer’s requests for their inter-
preter students to practise one or the other training element in their syllabus.
A very free scenario is provided by the Conversation Analysis Role Play Method (CARM)
(Stokoe 2011; Niemants and Stokoe 2017), described in another chapter of this volume.
Within the CARM framework, a short sequence of authentic interaction is shown to the
PSI students for them to analyse and discuss. Then the students take the roles of participants
in the sequence and act out its continuation. This method is intended on the one hand to
develop candidates’ theoretical understanding of interpreter-mediated interaction and, on
the other, it provides a scenario where only the beginning is set out, leaving the participants
free to imagine possible continuations. Hence, it can be used as an efficient part of education,
particularly for training t urn-taking and coordination techniques, but hardly replaces any of
the other kinds of role play training methods described in this chapter.
For testing, a constant problem is achieving validity (a test measures what it is purported
to measure) while securing reliability (everyone is tested equally). Arranging a series of role
plays for the purpose of testing and assessing the interpreting skills of several persons pres-
ents one with the problem of making the role plays such that they trigger skills necessary in
authentic situations, on the one hand, and making them equal, or at least equally difficult
(or easy), for each person undergoing the test, on the other. At first glance, using a script
with lines to be read out by the primary participants may seem to solve the reliability issue,
provided the lines will be read out in more or less the same way for every test. However,
this may not actually happen. On the contrary, different actors can be expected to read these
lines with different speed, intonation, pauses, and so on. Moreover, different interpreters
will most certainly render these lines differently and will regularly ask for clarifications
and repetitions, which will change the dynamics of each role play. To achieve a high level
of validity and authenticity, it would probably be appropriate to arrange a scenario where
specialists in the field play the role of their professional self, which at the same time most
certainly will make the role plays unfold differently each time, thus potentially jeopardising
reliability.
However, a role play based on a scenario (role cards), where the primary participants act
freely, but strictly within the frames of the intended communicative activity type and the
given scenario, can be expected to give the assessors input to assess the interpreter’s coordi-
nating skills as well as their rendering, language proficiency, and knowledge of specialised
terminology. This is despite the fact, or one might even say thanks to the fact, that such
role plays will most surely differ from one another at the level of single utterances. The
305
Magnus Dahnberg
Further reading
Bancroft, Marjory A., Sofia García-Beyaert, Katharine Allen, Giovanna Carriero-Contreras, and
Denis Socarrás-Estrada (2015) The Community Interpreter®: An International Textbook. Columbia,
Culture & Language Press.
306
Role play as a means of training and testing PSI
Bancroft, Marjory A., Sofia G arcía-Beyaert, Katharine Allen, Giovanna C arriero-Contreras, Denis
Socarrás-Estrada, and Hank Dallmann (2015) The Community Interpreter®: An International Work-
book on Activities and Role-Plays for Medical, Educational and Social Service Interpreters. Columbia, Cul-
ture & Language Press.
These two books, a textbook and a workbook, adopt an interpreter-oriented perspective on teaching commu-
nity interpreting, with a special focus on medical interpreting, including role play exercises.
Leaman, Lori Hostetler, and Toni Michele Flanagan (2013) “Authentic role-playing as situated learn-
ing: Reframing teacher education methodology for h igher-order thinking”, Studying Teacher Edu-
cation 9 (1): 4 5–61.
This article proposes a kind of training role play where the action can be paused and discussed by the students
and their instructors, and then start again from where it stopped. It is called Authentic Role Play as Situated
Learning (A RSL), and while the article describes teacher education and not interpreter education, it still gives
food for thought on how to develop the role play as a training method.
Related chapters
Chapter 4, Cultural assumptions, positioning and power: Towards a social pragmatics of interpreting by Ian
Mason
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 13, Challenges and remedies for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments by Charlotta
Plejert
Chapter 23, Interprofessional Education …. Interpreter Education in or and? Taking stock and moving forward
by Demi Krystallidou
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to communicate via interpreter by Tatjana R. Felberg and
Gry Sagli.
Note
1 This might be comparable to respeaking, when speech is to be transferred to writing (in the same
language), namely when interpretations are provided in writing for the deaf and hard of hearing,
and where the respeaking becomes a dictation with a written structure and where punctuation is
explicitly given (Eugeni 2008).
References
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (2014) “A re close renditions the golden standard? Some thoughts
on translating accurately in healthcare interpreter-mediated interaction”, The Interpreter and Trans-
lator Trainer 8 (3): 336–53.
Bradford, Terry J. (2017) “Generating and using scripted role-plays in the teaching of interpreting and
language”, The Language Scholar Journal 2: 18–36.
Carter, Ronald (2004) “Grammar and spoken English”, in Applying English Grammar: Corpus and
Functional Approaches, Caroline Coffin, Ann Hewings, and Kieran O’Halloran (eds). London/New
York, Routledge: 50–64.
Cirillo, Letizia, and Maura Radicioni (2017) “(Role-)playing fair(s)”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting:
Research-Based Proposals for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amster-
dam, John Benjamins: 120–35.
Crookall, David, and Danny Saunders (eds) (1989) Communication and Simulation: From Two Fields to
One Theme. Philadelphia, Clevedon.
Dahnberg, Magnus (2015) Tolkmedierade samtal som rollspel [Interpreter-Mediated Conversations as
Role Play] (d iss.). Uppsala, Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis.
Eugeni, Carlo (2008) “Respeaking the TV for the deaf: For a real special n eeds-oriented subtitling”,
Studies in English Language and Literature 21: 37–47.
Frøili, Jorunn (2001) “Signals at the transition place: The interpreter’s turn-taking in dialogues”, in
Meetings at the Crossroads: Studies of Multilingualism and Multiculturalism in Oslo and Utrecht, Anne
Hvenekilde and Jacomine Nortier (eds). Oslo, Novus Press: 136–57.
307
Magnus Dahnberg
Gavioli, Laura (2018) “Do authentic data mean authentic learning? On the use of authentic sam-
ples and (in)authentic activities in teaching and learning dialogue interpreting”, in Translation and
Interpreting for Language Learners (TAIL). inTRAlinea Special Issue, Laurie Anderson, Laura Gavioli,
and Federico Zanettin (eds). URL: http://w ww.intralinea.org/specials/a rticle/2292 (accessed 29
January 2021).
Gonzalez-Davies, Maria and Vanessa Enríquez Raído (eds). (2017) Situated Learning in Translator and
Interpreter Training: Bridging Research and Good Practice. London, Routledge.
Hale, Sandra (2007) Community Interpreting. London, Palgrave Macmillan.
Han, Chao (2016) “Building the validity foundation for interpreter certification performance testing”
(thesis abstract), The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 10 (2): 248–49.
Hlavac, Jim, Rita Pierina Wilson, Marc Orlando, Helen Anne Tebble, Shani Lyn Tobias, and Jonathan
Andrew Beagley (2016) INT (Improvements to NAATI Testing) 2: Options for Interpreter Assessment
Delivery Including Specialisations. Melbourne, Monash University Publishing.
Kasper, Gabriele (2000) “Data collection in pragmatics research”, in Culturally Speaking: Managing Rap-
port through Talk across Cultures, Helen Spencer-Oatey (ed). London/New York, Continuum: 316–41.
Kasper, Gabriele, and Merete Dahl (1991) “Research methods in interlanguage pragmatics”, Studies in
Second Language Acquisition 13 (2): 215–47.
Krystallidou, Demi (2016) “Investigating the interpreter’s role(s). The A.R.T. framework”, Interpreting
18 (2): 172–97.
Kristallidou, Demi, Céline Van De Walle, Myriam Deveugele, Evangelia Dougali, Fien Mertens,
Amélie Truwant, Ellen Van Praet, and Peter Pype (2018) ”Training ‘doctor-minded’ interpreters
and ‘interpreter-minded’ doctors – The benefits of collaborative practice in interpreter training”,
Interpreting 20 (1): 126–4 4. https://doi.org/10.1075/i ntp.00005.kry.
Lave, Jean, and Etienne Wenger (1991) Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge,
Cambridge University Press.
Linell, Per (2009) Rethinking Language, Mind, and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories
of Human Sense-making. Charlotte, N.C, Information Age Publishers.
——— (2010) “Communicative activity types as organizations in discourses and discourses in organi-
zation”, in Discourses in Interaction, Sanna-K aisa Tanskanen, M arja-Liisa Helasvuo, Marjut Johans-
son, and Mia Raitaniemi (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 33–59.
McNamara, Tim (2000) Language Testing. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Niemants, Natacha, and Elizabeth Stokoe (2017) “Using the conversation analytic role-play method in
healthcare interpreter education”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: Research-Based Proposals for Higher Edu-
cation, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 294–321.
Radanović Felberg, Tatjana (2015) “Utfordringer ved prima vista tolking i asylintervjuer” [Challenges
with prima vista interpreting in asylum interviews], FLEKS -Scandinavian Journal of Intercultural
Theory and Practice 2 (1): 1–17. https://doi.org/10.7577/fleks.1297.
Schildkamp, Kim, Fabienne M. van der Kleij, Maaike C. Heitink, Wilma B. Kippers, and Bernard
P. Veldkamp (2020) “Formative assessment: A systematic review of critical teacher prerequisites
for classroom practice”, International Journal of Educational Research 103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ijer.2020.101602 (accessed 31 January 2022).
Skaaden, Hanne and Cecilia Wadensjö (2014) “Some consideration on the testing of interpreting
skills”, in Assessing Legal Interpreter Quality through Testing and Certification: The Qualitas Project, Cyn-
thia Giambruno (ed). Alicante, Universidad de Alicante: 17–26.
Slettebakk Berge, Sigrid (2018) “How sign language interpreters use multimodal actions to coordinate
t urn-taking in group work between deaf and hearing upper secondary school student”, Interpreting
20 (1): 96–125.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2011) “Simulated interaction and communication skills training: The ‘conversation-
a nalytic role-play method’”, in Applied Conversation Analysis: Intervention and Change in Institutional
Talk, Charles Antaki (ed). London, Palgrave Macmillan: 119–39.
Tebble, Helen (2014) “A genre-based approach to teaching dialogue interpreting: The medical consul-
tation”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 418–36.
Tipton, Rebecca, and Olgierda Furmanek (2016) Dialogue Interpreting. A Guide to Interpreting in Public
Services and the Community. London/New York, Routledge.
Van Besien, Fred, and Chris Meuleman (2014) “Dealing with speakers’ errors and speakers’ repairs in
simultaneous interpretation”, Translator 10 (1): 59–81.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
308
19
MONITORING IN DIALOGUE
INTERPRETING
Cognitive and didactic perspectives
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
Introduction
This chapter explores the concept of monitoring and how it can be understood as a central
cognitive process in dialogue interpreting, one of the main interpreting techniques used in
public service. We use the term dialogue interpreting for short consecutive interpreting, in both
language directions, utterance by utterance, in dialogues between two or more primary par-
ties. The interpreter may use notes but in many cases does not. Dialogue interpreting in this
sense is used not only in public service interpreting, but also in other settings such as business
meetings or diplomatic encounters.1
Dialogue interpreting has so far mainly been investigated from a sociological and inter-
actional perspective. This is not surprising because, in most encounters in the public sector,
the spoken language interpreter works in absolute proximity to the primary participants,
which has tended to foreground social and interactional aspects in research. After Waden-
sjö’s (1992) seminal work, many studies have investigated dialogue interpreting in various
settings (Roy 1993; Straniero Sergio 1999; Angelelli 2004; Tebble 2009; Baraldi and Gavioli
2012a are cases in point) elucidating the constraints and challenges of different contexts from
an interactional perspective. As Wadensjö (1992, 1998) has shown, dialogue interpreters not
only translate but also coordinate talk in social interaction. It can therefore be assumed that
the dialogue interpreter functions differently in the interpreted event as compared to, for
instance, conference interpreters working in simultaneous mode. Interaction is an inherent
feature of dialogue interpreting.
However, understanding also the cognitive (mental) processes underlying dialogue in-
terpreting and the interpreting performance is both theoretically important and crucial
from an applied perspective, not the least in the context of education and professionaliza-
tion of interpreters in the public sector. Traditionally, cognitive research on interpreting
has focused on the simultaneous or the long consecutive techniques (for an overview see
Liu 2008), which are typically used in conference interpreting. Thus, most cognitive re-
search in interpreting has focused on conference interpreting. There are also some studies
about sight translation (for example, Shreve, Lacruz and Angelone 2010), but very few
cognitive research studies have focused on dialogue interpreting as used in the public
service sector (for an overview, see Tiselius and Albl Mikasa 2019). In his conceptual
analysis of the interpreter’s role in interaction, Pöchhacker (2 012: 67) points to ‘a need to
apply a more distinctly cognitive perspective to the sociolinguistic analysis of participation
in discourse’. Answering this need, this chapter addresses the complexities of cognition in
dialogue interpreting through the concept of monitoring. We will show that monitoring
is a multifaceted concept which can be fruitfully applied to research on dialogue inter-
preting from several theoretical perspectives. We will also demonstrate that a more general
understanding of monitoring can be a useful pedagogical concept to apply in interpreter
education.
310
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
In the words of Risku (2014: 339), ‘each research endeavour will focus on specific research
questions and naturally cannot include the whole complexity of human cognition.’ There-
fore, in our view, a research study can have its main focus on one element in the event, such
as the cognitive processes of the interpreter. Such a focus can also be of use for a number of
didactic purposes.
Interpreters, in our understanding, monitor not only themselves, but also the other par-
ticipants. Therefore, it seems natural to assume that dialogue interpreting is characterized by
distributed cognition (Hutchins 1995). Following Kirsh (2006) and Sutton (2006), distrib-
uted cognition is not only interpersonal but indeed comprises all components which con-
tribute to a system performing its task. In dialogue interpreting, the cognition is distributed
over the participants, but also artefacts (such as notepad and pen, computer, telephone, dic-
tionaries, brochures, and forms) typically present in an interpreted event, which contribute
to the fulfilment of the task.
In translation studies, the notion of distributed cognition has been taken up by, for
instance, Ehrensberger-Dow (2 014) and Risku (2 014). Risku (2 014: 335) points out the
need to investigate the translation process using both experimental and ethnographic ap-
proaches, as research suggests that cognitive processes are c ontext-dependent (see, for ex-
ample, Clark and Chalmers 1998; Suchman 2007; Clark 2008). Dialogue interpreting is
very much context-dependent (Wadensjö 1998), and the cognitive processes of dialogue
interpreters can be assumed to both have impact on and be affected by the interpreted
event.
Sutton (2 006) and Michaelian and Sutton (2 013) stress that coordination is an im-
portant feature of distributed cognition, since all the components potentially involved in
task fulfilment have to be coordinated for a successful outcome. Wadensjö (1998) has also
pointed to the interpreter’s work of coordinating the exchange in an interpreted event.
As we will show below, monitoring is a crucial cognitive factor in the interpreter’s co-
ordination work.
Monitoring – a
multifaceted theoretical concept
The M erriam-Webster dictionary (2019) defines monitoring as the act ‘to watch, keep track
of, or check, usually for a special purpose.’ We assume that all cognitive processes are moni-
tored to certain degrees; however, space limitations prevent us from offering a fuller account
of the cognitive processes monitored in dialogue interpreting.
Several disciplines relevant to the study of dialogue interpreting have proposed con-
cepts of monitoring. In the three consecutive sub-sections below, we review three partly
different notions of monitoring, proposed within different theoretical frameworks, all po-
tentially relevant for the study of dialogue interpreting and the education of dialogue
interpreters. To illustrate the theoretical concepts, we give examples from our own re-
search. The examples in the text come from material collected for the project The invisible
process – Cognition and working memory in dialogue interpreting ( V R 2 016- 01118), funded by
the Swedish Research Council. The data consists of interpreted scripted role plays between
a Swedish job counselor (C) and a newly arrived French-, Spanish-, or Polish- speaking job
seeker ( JS), both actors, professional or amateurs. Although the role plays were scripted,
which means that the study is based on elicited data, the interpreter-participants (students
and experienced interpreters) had not seen the scripts and had only been briefed on the
overall topic of the encounter (for more information about the data, see Tiselius and
311
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
Englund Dimitrova 2019, 2021; Tiselius and Sneed 2020). In the subsequent section, Mon-
itoring in dialogue interpreting, we introduce our understanding of monitoring. Finally, in
the section called Interpreting practice and education, we discuss the ideas and data presented
from a didactic perspective.
312
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
David said during the post-task interview that based on the JS’s European Spanish accent,
he was taken by surprise when she mentioned ‘fleeing’ (turn 51). This example shows how
David’s monitoring of his own understanding, quick evaluation, and action avoids a misun-
derstanding which could have challenged the participants’ mutual understanding.
313
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
The type of monitoring (Levelt 1980; Laver 1983) discussed above and exemplified in
Example 2 stems from theories of speech disfluency and is thus restricted to speech production.
It covers the interpreters’ listening phase (assuming that their speech planning starts during the
phase of listening to the primary parties), and also their production phase when they monitor
their own utterance. This view is, quite clearly, of importance for dialogue interpreting, but,
as this understanding of monitoring only covers self-monitoring of speech planning and pro-
duction, it is not sufficiently broad to cover all relevant aspects of dialogue interpreting. Levelt’s
and Laver’s understanding of monitoring is close to Toury’s monitor model.
314
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
primary party’s utterance, in this case stopping them from intervening, but constraints in
her cognitive capacity has not allowed her to plan her production. Therefore, she has to use
a moment at the beginning of her utterance for planning.
315
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
listening and three when the interpreter is speaking. When one of the primary parties speaks
and the interpreter listens, the interpreter monitors: (a) their own comprehension of that ut-
terance, as illustrated by Example 1 above; (b) the relation of that utterance to the interpreter’s
recent rendition (was it understood as intended?); and (c) their own memory and processing
capacity (ready to take the turn if necessary; Example 3 above). When the interpreter speaks,
the interpreter monitors: (d) their own utterance (in relation to the given language; Exam-
ple 2 above); (e) the relation between their own utterance and the primary party’s previous
utterance (in those cases when the interpreter’s utterances are renditions of a primary party’s
utterances); and (f ) verbal and non-verbal reactions of the primary parties. Carrying out these
processes would entail particular ways of cognitive processing, as detailed below.
From our perspective, monitoring includes both self-monitoring – as described above with
references to Toury (2012), Levelt (1983) and Laver (1981) – and monitoring of other partic-
ipants, that is, understanding their utterances, the relationship to other utterances, and non-
verbal reactions, similar to the mutual attention described by Goodwin (1980). As shown by
our examples, we explore monitoring primarily as an element of the interpreter’s cognitive
processes when performing in face-to-face encounters. In doing this, we do not see the in-
terpreter as an isolated individual, but as a participant solicited in the context of an event and
thereby contributing in co-constructing meaning. Monitoring, in our view, is a necessary,
pervasive, and continuous element of the dialogue interpreter’s all cognitive processes.
316
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
completed a questionnaire and performed a retrospection task. Arumí and V argas-Urpi ana-
lyzed how the interpreters handled potential problems (lexical, pragmatic, cultural, conver-
sation management, impromptu speech, pronunciation/expression, and ethical dilemmas)
in the interpreted role plays. One of their findings was that information management strat-
egies were used to segment information, emphasize important segments of the rendition,
clarify, and explicitate meaning. These information management strategies can be seen as
responses to issues identified through monitoring. They also found that turn-taking man-
agement serves – among other functions – as a strategy to handle the interpreter’s cognitive
load (compare with Example 3 above).
Herring (2018, 2019) studied dialogue interpreters (novices n = 3, and experienced n =
8) between Spanish and English as they interpreted a simulated encounter where the par-
ticipants had been instructed as to what issues they would discuss and what terminology to
use but had no script. The interpreters also participated in p rocess-tracing retrospection after
the task. She investigated the interpreters’ online self-regulation (cf. Boekaerts, Zeidner and
Pintrich 1999), which she describes as actions to (a mong other things) modify performance
‘to create or maintain alignment between current and goal states’ (2019: 286). In contrast
to our proposed understanding of monitoring, Herring distinguishes between monitoring
(the observation) and control (the action; cf. Nelson and Narens 1990). Herring found that
interpreters monitor their own knowledge/understanding of various aspects of the situa-
tional context, the backstory, the identities of and relationships between the parties, and
external factors impacting the encounter or interpreting (2019: 296). Herring also stresses
turn-taking as a strategy to handle cognitive load, especially in emotionally charged inter-
actions (2019: 290).
In our project, we found that, with experience, the interpreter’s professional self-concept
changes and strengthens. This, in turn, contributes to more effective monitoring, since the
interpreter will know what action to take (Englund Dimitrova and Tiselius 2016). We have
also found indications that different levels of language competence may increase or decrease
cognitive load. A change in cognitive load may, in turn, require extra monitoring effort
(Thomsen 2018, Tiselius and Englund Dimitrova 2019).
Monitoring needs processing capacity and is therefore dependent on working memory
(WM). In a general study (not related to interpreting) of WM, Miller, Watson, and Strayer
(2012) suggest that individuals with greater WM capacity also show greater monitoring
capacity. Research on interpreters’ WM has mostly focused on conference interpreters but
findings are relevant to dialogue interpreting too. M eta-analyses of studies on interpreters’
WM by Wen and Dong (2019) and Mellinger and Hanson (2019) have shown that the WM
capacity of experienced interpreters is larger than that of non-interpreters. Timarová, Drag-
sted, and Hansen (2011: 122) established experimentally that in simultaneous interpreting,
the ear-voice span, or lag, is approximately between two and five seconds, and not going
beyond ten seconds. Ear-voice span is clearly correlated to the interpreter’s WM capacity and
can be seen as an analogue to what we label processing span in dialogue interpreting (Tiselius
and Englund Dimitrova 2021). In our study on temporal aspects of dialogue interpreting,
we found that there may be a universal cognitive constraint limiting the length of turns in
dialogue interpreting.
In neuroscience, researchers have proposed that the anterior cingulate cortex is the area
of the brain which monitors conflicts and detects errors in cognitive tasks. (M iller, Watson
and Strayer 2012). This is also the area of the brain where Hervais-Adelman and Babcock
(2020) found differences between interpreters and n on-interpreters in their overview of
fMRI studies on the interpreter’s brain. Monitoring, thus, is a central cognitive process, and
317
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
the interpreter’s brain seems to develop accordingly, suggesting that monitoring is what helps
interpreters regulate cognitive effort in order to handle cognitive load.
318
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
animated audio or video recordings, see Niemants, Hansen and Stokoe, in this volume) has
also been introduced.
There are many aspects of the interpreted dialogue which are difficult to highlight in a
roleplay. Several of these aspects can be identified and discussed using CARM, but they also
need to be understood, practiced, and hopefully automatized through the course of an inter-
preter education. In short, students need to develop their meta-cognitive awareness and learn
how to steer their cognitive capacities. They need to be able to visualize and describe their
cognitive processes. We argue that this is where an understanding of the various processes of
monitoring comes into play.
To manage a dialogue interpreting encounter, interpreters need to learn to recognize and
handle their own cognitive constraints, identify cognitive demands, and manage cognitive
effort. This can be done systematically in interpreter education by focusing on the described
six cognitive processes. Many students learn to do it intuitively but, by identifying the need
for monitoring and making students aware of techniques interpreters use, students are likely
to do so more effectively, and perhaps more of them can learn to use relevant techniques.
Furthermore, it will also help both students and interpreters identify ineffective actions or
contradictory signals. When students acquire meta-cognitive awareness, they are given tools
and also a language to talk about their cognitive experience and resources.
In order to foster the development of meta-cognitive awareness and of learning how
to steer one’s own cognitive resources for dialogue interpreting, we suggest a combined
theoretical and experience-based approach. First, the students will be guided through the
theoretical background of the monitoring models we introduced above. Ideally, the students
would read the relevant scholarly literature but, if that proves too abstract, the teacher can
support learning through, for instance, short v ideo-recorded overviews of each of the ar-
ticles, which will allow for students to go back to the recorded overviews when they are
reading on their own. The articles can also be discussed in class.
Second, transcribed examples illustrating each process are reviewed and discussed in class.
In this step, the examples we provide in the text can be used as a point of departure. In the
next step, we would also encourage teachers to use their own examples, whether found in
the literature or the instructor’s own. These examples can be presented in recorded and/or
transcribed formats (similar to those used in CARM). It is important that these are also
discussed in class.
The aspects we suggest to highlight in the examples, so as to enhance students’ under-
standing of monitoring are:
• urn-taking, and how it impacts communication and information transfer. This can be
T
done by observing individual strategies for handling t urn-taking, used by the interpret-
ers in the examples, as well as by their student peers in exercises, in order to test them
later. Also, students can observe how problems with t urn-taking may result in overlap-
ping talk, loss of information or misunderstandings.
• Gazing, its impact, and how it is handled by interpreters. Understanding the impact
of gaze direction and gaze aversion gives interpreters a strong tool to coordinate the
conversation (compare Vranjes, Brône and Feyaerts 2018 and Davitti 2013). By under-
standing the impact of gaze, the interpreter student learns how to use gaze to take action
when monitoring and, for instance, regulate turn-taking for the benefit of their own
or the participants’ comprehension, or for coping with their own cognitive constraint.
• Gesturing, and how body and gaze can be powerful resources in the interpreting pro-
cess. This can be done by practicing the use of bodily resources as a tool in t urn-taking,
319
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
an often u
nder-explored area in training. Furthermore, in relation to monitoring, it is
important to master one’s own bodily expressions so as not to confuse the participants’
understanding.
Third, after the theoretical and observation steps, students can record themselves and their
classmates when interpreting and discuss and analyze their own performance.
Finally, the students need to be offered the opportunity to discuss the implications of
each and all monitoring of the different processes and how important each one of them is (in
relative terms) to the interpreting process.
Students may also be further invited to investigate themselves in smaller research s tudies –
for example, for a final course paper or a final program thesis – where they use their own
recorded interpreting as data and investigate them from a cognitive perspective.
We believe that interpreting exercises should focus not only on the linguistic aspects of
the students’ renditions, as is sometimes the case, but must take into account the full range of
cognitive resources in the process. Through the steps we suggest, future dialogue interpret-
ers are trained through combined theoretical and practical methods to understand monitor-
ing. Students will, thus, learn how their own monitoring w orks – through video recordings
and transcription exercises of their own performance – and will also (better) understand
their own cognitive constraints, also through video recordings and transcriptions.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we see the application of monitoring to research and education as a road to
improve interpreter students’ performance through their conscious access to their own cog-
nitive processes. In this chapter, we hope to have shown that monitoring is crucial in the
dialogue interpreter’s cognitive processing. We have also given examples of how monitoring
can be used in a cognitive approach to teaching dialogue interpreting. Finally, we argue that
more research on cognition in dialogue interpreting in general, and research on monitoring
in dialogue interpreting in particular, is sorely needed.
Further reading
Angelone, Eric (2010) “Uncertainty, uncertainty management and metacognitive problem solving
in the translation task”, in Translation and Cognition, Gregory M. Shreve, and Eric Angelone (eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 17–40.
The article presents a study looking into the metacognition of translation students and a translation teacher,
that is how participants think about their process. Through think-aloud protocols and screen recordings, the
author investigates how inexperienced and experienced participants handle situations when they are not certain
about how to solve a translation problem. Furthermore, the author discusses monitoring as one component of
metacognition.
Fernández Bravo, Elena Aguirre, (2019) “Metacognitive self-perception in interpreting”, Translation,
Cognition & Behavior 2 (2): 147–64.
This article reports on the development of a tool for students to identify learning processes and development
of interpreting skills. The aim of the instrument is to help students achieve a metacognitive understanding of
their interpreting (and learning) process. The authors suggest that the tool can be used for diagnostic and for-
mative assessment and that the scale proposed can be useful for further competence development of professional
interpreters.
Orlando, Marc ( 2012) “ Training of professional translators in Australia: Process-oriented and
product-oriented evaluation approaches”, in Global Trends in Translator and Interpreter Training,
Séverine Hubscher-Davidson and Michał Borodo (eds). London, Continuum: 1 97–216.
320
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
In this article the author describes the translator education curriculum at Monash university in Melbourne,
Australia. An important part of the training is the integration of “the integrated translator’s diary” (see also
Gile 2009) aiming at developing students’ metacognitive awareness. The article is further relevant (and appli-
cable to interpreting) as it also discusses how trainers can approach the assessment of the process of translation
both from a formative and a summative perspective.
Related chapters
Chapter 10, Consecutive interpreting and multimodal sequences by Christian Licoppe.
Chapter 18, Role play as a means of training and of testing public service interpreting by Magnus Dahnberg.
Chapter 21, The Conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe.
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véro-
nique Traverso and Emilie Jouin.
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to communicate via interpreter by Tatjana R. Felberg and
Gry Sagli.
Chapter 25, Education and training of public service interpreter teachers by Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer.
Notes
1 In public service interpreting different interpreting techniques are used. Public service interpret-
ers interpret both simultaneously and consecutively, and they do sight translation from a written
text (for an overview of the skills needed for, and teaching demands of dialogue interpreting, see
for instance Tipton and Furmanek 2016).
2 Pöchhacker (2012: 60, 62) mentions interpreter’s monitoring too, but in a more general sense.
References
Angelelli, Claudia (2004) Revisiting the Interpreter’s Role: A Study of Conference, Court, and Medical Inter-
preters in Canada, Mexico and the United States. Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
Arumí Ribas, Marta, and Mireia V argas-Urpi (2017) “Strategies in public service interpreting”, Inter-
preting 19 (1): 118–41.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (eds) (2012a) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins Publishing.
——— (2012b) “Understanding coordination in i nterpreter-mediated interaction”, in Coordinating Par-
ticipation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi, and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins Publishing: 1 –22.
Boekaerts, Monique, Moshe Zeidner, and Paul R. Pintrich (eds) (1999) Handbook of Self-regulation.
New York, Academic Press/Elsevier.
Chesterman, Andrew (2015) “Models of what processes?”, in Describing Cognitive Processes in Translation:
Acts and Events, Maureen Ehrensberger-Dow, Birgitta Englund Dimitrova, Severine Hubscher-
Davidson, and Ulf Norberg (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 7–20.
Clark, Andy (2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford, Oxford
University Press.
Clark, Andy, and David J. Chalmers (1998) “The extended mind”, Analysis 58 (1): 7–19.
Davitti, Elena (2013) “Dialogue interpreting as intercultural mediation: Interpreters’ use of upgrading
moves in parent–teacher meetings”, Interpreting 15 (2): 168–99.
Doherty-Sneddon, Gwyneth, and Fiona G. Phelps (2005) “Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or
social difficulty?”, Memory & Cognition 33: 727–33. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195338.
Ehrensberger-Dow, Maureen (2014) “Challenges of translation process research at the workplace”,
MonTI Monographs in Translation and Interpreting 15: 355–83. URL: https://r ua.ua.es/d space/
bitstream/10045/43732/1/MonTI_2014_Special_Issue_13.pdf (accessed 29 June 2021).
Englund Dimitrova, Birgitta, and Elisabet Tiselius (2016) “Cognitive aspects of community interpret-
ing: Toward a process model”, in Reembedding Translation Process Research, Ricardo Muñoz Martín
(ed). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 195–214.
321
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
Gile, Daniel (2009) Basic Concepts and Models for Interpreter and Translator Training. Amsterdam/Philadelphia:
John Benjamins.
Goodwin, Charles (1981) Conversational Organization: Interaction between Speakers and Hearers. New
York, Academic Press.
Goodwin, Marjorie (1980) “Processes of mutual monitoring implicated in the production of descrip-
tion. Sequence”, Sociological Inquiry 50 (3 –4): 303–17.
Herring, Rachel (2018) ‘I Could Only Think About What I Was Doing, and That Was a Lot to Think
About’: online self-regulation in dialogue interpreting, PhD diss., University of Geneva, URL:
https://a rchive-ouverte.unige.ch/u nige:108626 (accessed 29 June 2021).
——— (2019) “‘A lot to think about’: Online monitoring in dialogue interpreting”, Translation, Cog-
nition & Behaviour 2 (2): 283–304.
Hervais-Adelman, Alexis, and Laura Babcock (2020) “The neurobiology of simultaneous interpret-
ing: Where extreme language control and cognitive control intersect”, Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition 23 (4): 7 40–51. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000324.
Hutchins, Edwin (1995) Cognition in the Wild. Massachusetts, MIT press.
Kirsch, David (2006) “Distributed cognition: A methodological note”, Pragmatics & Cognition 14 (2):
249–62.
Laver, John D. (1980) “Monitoring systems in the neurolinguistic control of speech production”, in
Errors in Linguistic Performance: Slips of the Tongue, Ear, Pen, and Hand, Victoria A. Fromkin (ed). New
York, Academic Press. 287–305.
Levelt, Willem J. (1983) “Monitoring and s elf-repair in speech”, Cognition 14 (1): 41–104.
Liu, Minhua (2008) “How do experts interpret? Implications from research in Interpreting Studies
and cognitive science”, in Efforts and Models in Interpreting and Translation Research, Gyde Hansen,
Andrew Chesterman, and Heidrun Gerzymisch-Arbogast (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John
Benjamins: 159–78.
Mellinger, Christopher D., and Thomas A. Hanson (2019) “Meta-analyses of simultaneous interpret-
ing and working memory”, Interpreting 21 (2): 165–95.
Merriam-Webster (2019) “Definition of MONITORING”. URL: https://w ww.merriam-webster.
com/d ictionary/monitoring (accessed 19 November 2019).
Michaelian, Kourken, and John Sutton (2013) “Distributed cognition and memory research: History
and current directions”, Review of Philosophy and Psychology 4 (1): 1–24.
Miller, A. Eve, Jason M. Watson, and David L. Strayer (2012) “Individual differences in working mem-
ory capacity predict action monitoring and the error-related negativity”, Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 38 (3): 757– 63. http://d x.doi.org.ezp.sub.su.se/10.1037/
a0026595.
Mondada, Lorenza (2018) “Transcription in linguistics”, in Research Methods in Linguistics, Lia Litos-
seliti (ed). London, Bloomsbury: 85–115.
Muñoz, Ricardo Martín (2016) “Processes of what models?: On the cognitive indivisibility of transla-
tion acts and events”, Translation Spaces 5 (1): 145–61.
Nelson, Thomas O., and Louis Narens (1990) “Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new find-
ings”, in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory Vol. 26, Gordon
H. Bower (ed). San Diego, CA, Academic Press: 125–73.
Norrby, Catrin (2014) Samtalsanalys: så gör vi när vi pratar med varandra. [Conversation analysis: This is
what we do when we talk to each other]. Lund, Studentlitteratur.
Nozari, Nazbanou, and Jared Novick (2017) “Monitoring and control in language production”, Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science 26 (5): 403–10.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2012) “Interpreting participation”, in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpret-
ing, Claudio Baraldi, and Laura Gavioli (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 45–70.
Risku, Hanna (2014) “Translation process research as interaction research: From mental to socio-
cognitive processes”, MonTI Monographs in Translation and Interpreting 1: 331–53. URL: https://
rua.ua.es/d space/bitstream/10045/43731/1/MonTI_2014_Special_Issue_12.pdf (accessed 29 June
2021).
Roy, Cynthia B (1993) “A sociolinguistic analysis of the interpreter’s role in simultaneous talk in in-
terpreted interaction”, Multilingua 12 (4): 341–63.
Shreve, Gregory M., Isabel Lacruz, and Eric Angelone (2010) “Cognitive effort, syntactic disruption,
and visual interference in a sight translation task”, in Translation and Cognition, Gregory M. Shreve,
and Eric Angelone (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 63–84.
322
Monitoring in dialogue interpreting
Straniero Sergio, Francesco (1999) “The interpreter on the (talk) show: Interaction and participation
frameworks”, The Translator 5 (2): 303–26.
Suchman, Lucy (2007) Human-Machine Reconfigurations. Plans and Situated Actions. Cambridge, Cam-
bridge University Press.
Sutton, John (2006) “Distributed cognition: Domains and dimensions”, Pragmatics & Cognition 14 (2):
235–47.
Tebble, Helen (2009) “W hat can interpreters learn from discourse studies?”, in The Critical Link 5:
Quality in Interpreting. A Shared Responsibility, Sandra B. Hale, Uldis Ozolins, and Ludmila Stern
(eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 2 01–19.
Thomsen, Thomas ( 2018) Tolkningsstrategier i ljuset av språkkompetens, tolkningsriktning och tolk-
erfarenhet. [Interpreting strategies in the light of language competence, directionality and
interpreting experience] Master’s diss. Stockholm University. URL: http:// su.d iva-
portal.org/
smash/get/d iva2:1217466/F ULLTEXT02.pdf (accessed 9 January 2021).
Timarová, Šárka, Barbara Dragsted, and Gyde Hansen (2011) “Time lag in translation and interpreta-
tion: a methodological exploration”, in Methods and Strategies of Process Research: Integrative Approaches
in Translation Studies, Cecilia Alvstad, Adelina Hild, and Elisabet Tiselius, Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins: 1 21–46.
Tipton, Rebecca, and Olgierda Furmanek (2016) Dialogue Interpreting: A Guide to Interpreting in Public
Services and the Community. London, Routledge.
Tirkkonen-Condit, Sonja (2005) “The monitor model revisited: Evidence from process research”,
Meta 50 (2): 405–14. https://doi.org/10.7202/010990ar.
Tiselius, Elisabet, and Michaela A lbl-Mikasa (2019) “Cognitive processes in dialogue interpreting:
Introduction”, Translation, Cognition & Behaviour 2 (2): 233–39. https://doi.org/10.1075/tcb.00027.
tis.
Tiselius, Elisabet, and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova (2019) “A symmetrical language proficiency in dia-
logue interpreters: Methodological issues”, Translation, Cognition & Behaviour 2 (2): 305–22. https://
doi.org/10.1075/tcb.00031.tis.
——— (2021) “Turn-taking in dialogue interpreting – coping with cognitive constraints”, Cognitive
Linguistic Studies 8 (2): 328–55.
Tiselius, Elisabet, and Kayle Sneed (2020) “Gaze and eye movement in dialogue interpreting: An
eye-t racking study”. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, Special issue 4, Interpreting: A Window into
Bilingual Processing 23: 7 80–87.
Toury, Gideon (1995/2012) Descriptive Translation Studies and Beyond. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John
Benjamins Publishing.
Vranjes, Jelena, Geert Brône, and Kurt Feyaerts (2018) “On the role of gaze in the organization of
t urn-t aking and sequence organization in i nterpreter-mediated dialogue”, Language and Dialogue
8 (3): 439–67.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1992) Interpreting as interaction: On dialogue-interpreting in immigration hearings and
medical encounters. PhD diss., University of Linköping.
——— (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London/New York, Longman.
Wen, Hao, and Yanping Dong (2019) “How does interpreting experience enhance working memory
and short-term memory: A meta-a nalysis”, Journal of Cognitive Psychology 31 (8): 769–84.
323
Elisabet Tiselius and Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
324
20
BLENDED LEARNING IS HERE
TO STAY!
Combining on-line and on-campus learning in
the education of public service interpreters
Introduction
Blended learning refers to educational approaches that combine on-campus and on-line learn-
ing activities (Bersin 2004; Bonk and Graham 2006; Skrypnyk et al. 2015; Stein and Graham
2020). This chapter addresses blended learning in the education of practitioners of public
service interpreting (PSI).
The teaching of interpreting is traditionally organized by means of o n-campus activities
(Sandrelli and Jerez 2007; Pöchhacker 2013, 2016). To the extent training options exist for
practitioners of PSI, the training likewise appears to be predominantly on-campus–based
(Driesen 2012). The on-site organization of learning activities is an obvious choice be-
cause interpreting is a skill-based activity, and the learning of skills requires options for
practical e xercise – that the traditional classroom setting naturally offers. Despite this basic
premise, the utilization of on-line activities and digital technology has recently increased
in interpreter education (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007; Skaaden and Wattne 2009; Sandrelli
2015; Pöchhacker 2016; Skaaden 2016, 2017; Kim 2017; Fantinuoli 2018a). From 2020,
due to the Covid-19 pandemic, the digitalization of education – including interpreter
education – exploded.
Our focus is the education of students preparing to practice in PSI, that is, the practice of
interpreting in various institutional encounters. Following Agar (1985: 147), an institutional
encounter is an ‘encounter between a representative of some institution and another person
seeking its services’. From this vantage point, PSI refers to interpreting that ‘enables profes-
sionals and officials to inform, guide and hear the parties in the case at hand despite language
barriers’ (Skaaden 2017: 323, with reference to Norwegian legal documents).
Against this backdrop, this chapter seeks to examine the following questions: What is
blended learning and what purposes does it serve? What is the state of blended learning in
the education of students preparing for practice in PSI? How can blended learning advance
the education of PSI practitioners? Given that interpreting is a practical skill, a critical issue
is how on-line learning activities can possibly facilitate students’ mastery of interpreting.
What learning aims can be acquired on-line and which must be addressed on-site? In order
to elaborate on this final question, we shall use examples from blended learning organized
as part of a Bachelor’s (BA) program in PSI at Oslo Metropolitan University (OsloMet).
We shall argue that didactic approaches that stimulate student interactivity and the forming
of community in knowledge building are essential in creating opportunity for learning,
whether o n-site or on-line (Kolb 1984; Cherny 1999; Herring 2004; Holmer 2008; Skaaden
2017). A basic criterion is that the learning aims are appropriately matched with learning
activity and channel of communication (Palloff and Pratt 1999, 2001; Osman and Herring
2007).
326
Blended learning is here to stay!
More recent studies indicate that blended approaches’ positive effects on students’ learn-
ing are to a large extent fulfilled. A summary of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of the
effectiveness of blended learning found that the students’ academic achievements in blended
approaches were higher than in fully face-to-face approaches or fully on-line learning modes
(Skrypnyk et al. 2015: 71). However, as the included studies lacked consistency in forms of
blended learning environments and learning outcomes compared, ‘the research offers lim-
ited evidence as to what aspects of BL [blended learning] pedagogy and technology influence
learning outcomes’, Skrypnyk et al. (2015: 74) conclude.
Notwithstanding the promising effects of blended learning, the design and contents of
the blend are of importance for success, as well as the match between learning aims and ac-
tivities. In sum, to succeed in organizing blended learning, the type of knowledge or skills
to be mastered must be combined with the appropriate learning activities and channels of
communication – whether o n-line or on-campus (Palloff and Pratt 2001: 52; Osman and
Herring 2007; Skaaden 2017). Before we return to this issue, a brief literature review follows.
Literature overview
What does the literature tell us about the status of blended learning in interpreter education?
What are the major themes addressed? Most importantly, to what extent is blended learning
practiced in the education of interpreters in general and in the training of students of PSI in
particular?
The academic interest for the general topic of new technologies in interpreting has in-
creased (Fantinuoli 2018a, 2018b; Pokorn and Mellinger 2018; Braun 2019). C omputer-
a ssisted interpreter training, including blended learning, is, together with remote interpreting
and computer-assisted software, the areas that have received most researchers’ attention
(Fantinuoli 2018a: 157).
However, literature with specific focus on blended learning in the education of students
of PSI is still scarce (but see Skaaden and Wattne 2009; Skaaden 2016, 2017). Publications
are more abundant if the scope is extended to include associated topics, such as blended
learning in the education of interpreters working in other settings than PSI (see for instance
Moser-Mercer 2008; Şahin 2013; Chan 2014; Sandrelli 2015; Motta 2016; Deysel and Lesch
2018; Stengers, Kerremans and Segers 2018), new technology and interpreting (for exam-
ple, Braun et al. 2013; Carl and Braun 2018; Fantinuoli 2018a, 2018b; Skaaden 2018; Braun
2019; Kerremans et al. 2019; Eser, Lai and Saltan 2020), and blended learning in the broader
field of education (for instance, Skrypnyk et al. 2015; Ma’aro and Embi 2016; Alizadeh et al.
2019). Within the current frame, we cannot, however, provide a comprehensive review of
the entire field of blended learning.
In what follows, we concentrate on issues considered central to the education of PSI stu-
dents, and where relevant for this purpose, also include research from other fields of knowl-
edge. Blended learning in this context means that at least three major strands of research
meet: interpreting research, pedagogical research and research on C omputer-Mediated
Communication (on CMC, see for instance Cherny 1999; Herring 2004). Furthermore, it is
worth stressing that great variation in the terminology used in the literature on interpreting
and interpreter education complicates internet literature searches. Despite such challenges,
we have identified some issues that are recurrently discussed in the literature. The issues
sort along the three main strands that will also be followed in the below presentation: (A)
increased cost-effectiveness, access and flexibility; (B) technological innovations; and (C)
pedagogical and didactic approaches.
327
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
B Technological innovation
Evolving technology is a foundation for blended learning, and available technolo-
gies may expand or restrict the range of blended learning practices that are realizable.
Descriptions of various technologies accordingly make up another major issue in the
relevant research literature. However, as technological development is immense, articles
that date more than a decade back may have only historical interest.
While a wide range of information and communication technologies have been avail-
able for decades, it was not until the m id-1990s that a number of researchers and teachers
in different countries began to test computer-assisted approaches in interpreter training
(Sandrelli and Jerez 2007: 270). Sandrelli and Jerez (2007), whose primary interest is in
the conference setting, provide a systematic overview of the initial phase of development
of computer-assisted interpreter training, including blended learning, and they present a
description of some main technological efforts of the early phases. First, r epository-based
initiatives with IRIS, The Interpreter’s Resources Information System, as an example, were cre-
ated in the m id-1990s. IRIS aimed at providing teachers and students access to a database
328
Blended learning is here to stay!
of written, audio and video material in different languages (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007:
278–86). Second, authoring programs (or dedicated software programs) for interpreter
training were developed and employed. Interpr-It was such an authoring program, aimed
at improving students n ote-taking techniques through a guided approach to discourse
analysis (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007: 286, see also Gran, Carabelli and Merlini 2002: 277).
Similarly, Interpretation (produced between 1999 and 2002) was an authoring program that
‘would make it possible to create language-independent teaching material for interpret-
ing’ (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007: 287). Finally, e-learning platforms for interpreter training
were developed. Here, one of the first examples is The Geneva e-learning Portal with its
‘integrated VLE (Virtual Learning Environments) developed specifically to meet the re-
quirements of interpreter trainers and trainees’ (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007: 292). A later de-
velopment along this trend is The Geneva Virtual Institute, which employs blended learning
as it ‘involves a mixture of f ace-to-face and online activities, structured and unstructured
activities, and individual and collaborative tasks’ (Sandrelli and Jerez 2007: 293).
In sum, technology has obviously made quantum leaps in the past decades. As far as
technology goes, the only conclusion to be drawn with certainty is that its development
is continuous.
329
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
patterns, describes for instance synchronous chats as a ‘medium [that] is far from ideal for
formal lectures or debates; without the n on-intrusive visual feedback, it is disconcert-
ing to speak for any length to an audience that does not respond’ (Cherny 1999: 177).
Apparently, to allow for the social construction of knowledge to emerge, it is central
that community is established in the sense delineated by Herring (2004). Herring (2004:
19), whose interest lies in CMC and learning, identifies five criteria that indicate virtual
community: the occurrence of structure (for example, by reference to group, ‘we/they’);
the negotiation of meaning; interactivity through reciprocity; expression of solidarity/
conflict management; and participation over time.
Synchronous text-only-chats are discursive events where students, via their home-
computers’ keyboard, are able ‘to “talk” to each other at the same time in m ulti-
party dialogue or polylogue while being physically distant’ (Holmer 2008: 1). In
their study of synchronous chats as a channel of learning activities, Osman and Her-
ring (2 007: 127) analyze the construction of knowledge in terms of five types of
observable behavior (1) sharing and comparing of information, (2) discovery and
exploration of dissonance, (3) negotiation of meaning, (4) testing and modifica-
tion of proposed construction, and (5) agreement statements/applications of new
constructed meaning. They show that despite the chat discourse’s fragmented ap-
pearance, virtual community can be observed, thus, providing opportunity for the
social construction of knowledge. In sum, it seems that the o n-line learning environ-
ment favors approaches that utilize student involvement and stimulate interactivity,
both student-teacher and among peers (Palloff and Pratt 1999, 2001; Herring 2004;
Osman and Herring 2007).
‘Technology is not the “be all and end all” of the online course. It is merely the
vehicle for course delivery’, Palloff and Pratt (2001: 49) accentuated at the turn of the
twenty-first century. Twenty years later, the progress of o n-line communication is
massive. Smartphone applications, allowing for real-time audio-visual contact via con-
stantly developing channels, are changing the options for o n-line collaborative learning
considerably. The question is, thus, no longer whether o n-line channels should be part
of education, the question is how. In sum, the collaborative building of knowledge and
student involvement in the process of learning are of importance for o n-line learning
to succeed (Palloff and Pratt 1999: 2 8–32) and become essential topics in the further
development of o n-line classrooms and blended learning.
330
Blended learning is here to stay!
do with the building of confidence, Grimen (2008: 72) stresses. It follows that, in interpreter
education, where learning aims involve complex practical skills and the exercise of discre-
tion, the complexity of knowledge types must be taken into consideration when organizing
learning activities in the blended classroom.
As described above, the literature highlights that collaborative and student-centered ap-
proaches are key concepts for the social building of knowledge to emerge whether o n-
c ampus or on-line (Palloff and Pratt 1999; 2001; Herring 2004; Osman and Herring 2007).
Palloff and Pratt (1999: 32) underscore the following general indicators that o n-line com-
munity is forming:
i Professional ethics, integrity and status: introduction to the profession and its knowledge
base; drawing the boundaries of the interpreters’ area of expertise and domain for their
exercise of discretion.
ii Language: introduction to the nature of language and bilingualism as phenomena; rais-
ing awareness of the students’ bilingual and personal competencies.
331
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
iii Situated knowledge and interpreting techniques: characteristics of institutional and interpreted
discourse; how to employ interpreting methods and strategies in different contexts.
iv Specific context knowledge: general strategies for entering new language domains; how to
acquire specific terminology and context knowledge.
The general learning aims summarized under i.-iv. are addressed from different angles
throughout the BA’s courses.
The next paragraphs briefly illustrate how the blending of on-line and on-campus ac-
tivities may create learning opportunities for the learning of different knowledge types and
skills, using examples from the BA’s first year course on consecutive dialogue interpreting
and its third-year course on video remote interpreting (V RI). During the first year, on-
campus weekend gatherings take place three times each semester, while mandatory on-line
activities occur weekly. In the third year course on VRI, all activities occur on-line after a
kickoff gathering on campus (Skaaden 2017, 2018).
In our blended courses, the learning activities on campus involve both group and ple-
nary sessions and the on-line activities involve asynchronous and synchronous channels of
communication. Depending on their aims, learning activities are organized in language-
specific groups or mixed language groups. To exemplify, gathering in their language groups’
synchronous chat channel, the students approach bilingual terminology and context knowl-
edge (that is the above learning aims ii. and iv.) This activity is illustrated in (1) below with
an excerpt from the German group. In chats with students from various language groups
(example (2) below) issues of professional ethics, integrity and status (learning aim i.) are
discussed on-line. Furthermore, turn-taking strategies in dialogue interpreting (learning
aim iii.) are addressed in on-campus roleplay sessions with students from various language
groups. Finally, the latter topic is revisited in o
n-line chats later in the BA program, as illus-
trated in example (3) below.
332
Blended learning is here to stay!
discussed, the students here chat in their mother tongue, German. For the sake of brevity, the
excerpt from the chat log is in (1) rendered in the authors’ English translation:
Example (1)
1. Anna 19:00> I just throw my first word into the ring: overføringsflyktning [Norwegian for
‘resettlement refugee’]
2. Bibi 19:00> I still have not found a good word here
3. Cora 19:00> quota refugee?
4. Cora 19:01> or:
5. Dina 19:01> Thank you for the many links [facilitator’s name] the sites were really in-
formative and useful
6. Cora 19:01> Refugee within the frames of a declaration for takeover from the Federal
Ministry of the Interior
7. Dina 19:01> Super, I just googled it, but did not come across that [one]
8. Cora 19:02> Do you, Bibi, find that both [suggestions] work?
9. Bibi 19:03> Cora, [I] believe yours is not the same and too long (Skaaden 2016: 56)
In posting 1, the ‘r ing’ used as a metaphorical description of their joint activity is an indica-
tor of the participants experiencing community. The interactions in example (1) are all student to
student. An exception is posting 5 that is directed at the facilitator, who in line with principles
of collaborative learning takes on a secondary role in the chat discourse. However, as posting
5 reveals, the facilitator has prior to the r eal-time chat shared links on the topic to be discussed
via the learning platform. Simultaneously, the personal nature of Dina’s feedback to the facili-
tator is another indicator of community. Along with the reference to Google in posting 7, post-
ing 5 also indicates how the students utilize o n-line resources in their strategies for accessing
constantly new language domains – quite in line with the general learning aims sketched in iv.
above. Finally, example (1) shows how students, such as Bibi and Cora, take on different roles
in their language group collaborations. The excerpt, thus, illustrates how group diversity offers
opportunity for the individual student to develop bilingual registers and bilingual sensitivity.
In the synchronous chat discussions of mixed language groups, the students’ diverse back-
grounds represent a resource in the learning process as well. In these chats, students with
different working languages communicate in their common language Norwegian, which
is also most students’ second language. Again, for the sake of brevity, the excerpts are here
represented in the authors’ English renditions only. In the mixed language group chats, issues
of professional ethics, professional integrity and status are addressed in accordance with the
learning aims mentioned under (i.) above. This is witnessed in example (2). Here, five students
of Lithuanian, Somali, Sorani and Spanish discuss the interpreters’ own health and work sit-
uation in response to a curricular text on stress management and the danger of burnout. The
exchanges in example (2) occur in response to the facilitator’s open-ended question (posting
1) that refers to a specific quote from a curricular text on psychological burnout effects:
Example (2)
1. Facilitator 19:39> ‘The fuzzy boundaries of your responsibilities’ is included in the lit-
erature as one element [that may lead to burnout]. How does this [element] apply to the
interpreter?
333
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
2. Sonia 19:40> All users [of the interpreter’s services] do not understand in the same way
what an interpreter can do and cannot do and therefore the boundaries of the interpret-
er’s area of responsibility appear unclear
3. Leja 19:41> if someone is uncertain about what [type of ] tasks an interpreter should
actually take on
4. Leja 19:41> ito [‘in terms of ’] ‘extra’ tasks that suddenly pop up during an assignment
5. Sara 19:42> Fuzzy boundaries would be if s/he [the interpreter] is given tasks which
are outside the interpreter’s boundaries [of responsibility], and carries them out without
clearly signalling that this is not the interpreter’s responsibility
6. Seza 19:44> Vague boundaries may also mean being unclear about your own skills and
abilities as an interpreter
7. Sofia 19:44> and it may also be that users ask the interpreter to breach her/h is own pro-
fessional ethics, like client confidentiality? (That a user asks the interpreter about how
his case is coming along, like in the video we watched at our [on-campus] gathering?)
(Skaaden 2017: 333)
The students’ reflections here indicate community by the forming of a ‘we’ as opposed to
‘them’ in their reference to their clients’ expectations (postings 2 –5). Reciprocity is also
emerging from the students’ incremental associations to their own work conditions (postings
4 –6). Furthermore, the link made between professional ethics and the negative effects that
the vague role boundaries have on their professional integrity (6 and 7) signals interactive
reflections over course content, in line with the abovementioned community indicators.
Finally, in posting 7, the student’s mention of ‘the video we watched at our [on-campus]
gathering’ demonstrates the course’s topical integration of learning aims addressed o n-line
and those addressed on-campus – and the student’s recognition of such integration.
All in all, the chat logs indicate community in terms of interaction student to student,
socially constructed meaning and the sharing of resources, negotiations, questions and ex-
pression of (d is)agreement. As examples (1) and (2) briefly illustrate, the facilitator’s role is to
inspire student-to-student interaction and reflections by posing open-ended questions rather
than providing a set answer. The excerpts show that with this approach, even technologically
primitive channels like text-only-chats provide opportunities for learning through mean-
ingful joint reflections.
334
Blended learning is here to stay!
on-site interpreting of dialogues, where coordinating and interactional skills play a promi-
nent role in enabling the interpreter’s rendering (Wadensjö 1998). In our fi rst-year course,
roleplayed interpreting exercises are organized on campus in mixed language groups, where
the students play the parts of patient/client and professional based on role cards and scripts
and take turns playing the part of the interpreter. The students who are not assigned a part in
the roleplay itself take on the task of observers and subsequently provide peer feedback to the
student playing the part of interpreter, as illustrated in the photo image.
Each roleplay exercise is video recorded and is subsequently fed back to the student playing
the part of interpreter via the on-line learning platform. This enables each student to further
reflect upon own performance after returning home from the o n-campus sessions. However,
the main purpose of the roleplays is for each session to serve as a tableau for the group’s joint
observations of both translational and interactional strategies. The facilitator who leads the
subsequent reflections stimulates peer feedback by posing open-ended questions on the strat-
egies applied, such as pronoun and register choice, turn-taking and clarification strategies
(Felberg and Skaaden 2020: 98–100). While Palloff and Pratt (1999: 31) underscore that
open-ended questions serve to stimulate student interaction in the o n-line classroom, in
our experience, the questioning strategy is a resource also in on-campus sessions. Here, the
open-ended questions serve to elicit subsequent reflections over each roleplay situation – a
strategy well known from professional training in general (see for instance Kolb 1984).
In an e xperiential-dialogic approach, the aim is to stimulate collaborative knowledge build-
ing through joint experience, observation and subsequent group reflection (Skaaden 2017:
335
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
336). In a collegial chat session organized for facilitators of roleplayed exercises, the question
is raised how one may learn from observing someone else’s mistakes. In their response, the
facilitators accentuate the advantages of student diversity in the approach and conclude that
both the novice and the experienced [interpreters] learn, because it is limited how much
interpreting even the experienced ones have actually observed – so observing others is
a way of observing variations of yourself […] And they are not used to getting feedback
from someone who is able to “control” their target renditions and interpreting perfor-
mance (Skaaden 2017: 338).
The aim of the roleplay activity is, as indicated by the facilitator’s comment, for the group to
explore, through observation and reflection, different choices and their consequences for the
interpreted dialogue. Thus, the students, much in line with Kadrić’ (2017: 290) description,
discover that ‘there is an alternative to every action. Every word, gesture or decision could
have been different’. In this manner, the observations and joint reflections may add to the
students’ professional confidence and ability to apply their ‘tool’ in real-life situations, thus,
develop confidence in their application of interpreting and interactional strategies, in line
with the abovementioned learning aim iii.
In our experience, an important effect of observing interpreting in a working language
other than your own is that it serves to unveil aspects of interpreting that are independent of
the specific pair of working languages. For instance, the on-site consecutive interpreting of
dialogues requires the intentional application of subtle feedback signals such as breath-taking,
gaze or hand and body movements to coordinate the turn-taking. In the joint reflections’
phase following each roleplay, the facilitators draw attention to such features whereby the
students develop awareness for how the subtle feedback signals can be applied strategically
for turn-taking. In particular, the observation of peers adds to the understanding of these
tools’ importance in positioning the interpreter’s professional self in the interpreted dialogue.
Evidence from several studies reveals that t urn-taking in video remote interpreting (VRI)
is problematic (see for instance Braun and Taylor 2012). The students in our third year BA
course on VRI also pinpoint problems associated with t urn-taking during VRI after having
practiced roleplay exercises ‘on the screen’. In excerpt (3), students from three different lan-
guage groups (A rabic, Polish, Sorani) describe their experiences with on screen (V RI) inter-
preting as opposed to the accustomed on-site interpreting and how the medium affects their
turn-taking signals. Again, the students communicate in their common language, Norwe-
gian, but their postings are here represented in the authors’ English renditions and aliases are
assigned for anonymization.
Example (3)
1. Petra [19:47:56]> You don’t know whether they are looking at the interpreter, right. I
mean, difficulties with visual signals
[…]
2. Sefu [19:50:00]> [you] have to use more words to manage it [the turn-taking] instead of
a simple hand gesture
[…]
3. Aisha [19:52:20] > Eye contact does not function the same way as in on-site interpreting
(Skaaden 2018: 845)
336
Blended learning is here to stay!
Observing that the effects of accustomed feedback signals are altered in channels like
Skype, Teams or Zoom, the students here link the experienced difficulties with t urn-taking
during VRI to how they utilize feedback signals in coordinating on-site interpreted dia-
logues. In this manner, example (3) serves to illustrate that certain skills and strategies are less
suitable for acquisition o
n-line. The subtle t urn-taking strategies applied in o n-site dialogue
interpreting exemplify a learning aim that is difficult to acquire via o n-line channels. This
speaks in favor of a blended approach in interpreter education also in the future.
Concluding remarks
This chapter has demonstrated how some general principles of on-line and blended learning
can benefit courses on PSI – where education is scarce and flexible learning opportunities
are much needed. In the case demonstrated, all learning activities, whether on-line or on-
campus, lean on principles of collaborative learning. This means that student involvement
and interaction is central, whether the focus of attention is the students’ joint observations
and reflections over on-campus roleplays or their reflections in on-line chats over profes-
sional ethics or bilingual terminology and context knowledge. Not all learning aims can be
achieved through on-line communication, however. As elaborated on above, the interac-
tional and coordinating skills required in the performance of on-site, consecutive interpret-
ing of dialogues, serves to remind us of this fact.
The outburst of the pandemic in 2020 created urgent need for digitalization of most uni-
versity courses. Still, as stated by Graham (2006: 6 –7): ‘it is imperative that we understand
how to create effective blended learning experiences that incorporate both face-to-face and
computer-mediated (CM) elements.’ This remains a principle to be r emembered – although
technological development is continuous. In organizing blended learning, it is imperative to
address which learning aims and activities are suited for o n-line communication and which
activities can best be dealt with face-to-face when the students gather on campus.
Further reading
Cirillo, Letizia, and Natacha Niemants (eds) (2017) Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: R esearch-based Propos-
als for Higher Education. Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company.
The book provides research based contributions to interpreter education, illustrating various pedagogical and
didactic approaches and addressing epistemic issues of interpreter education. It includes a chapter on epistemic
issues of PSI education in the context of blended learning.
Fantinuoli, Claudio (ed) (2018b) Interpreting and Technology. Berlin, Language Science Press.
This book explores core issues, approaches and challenges important for interaction between interpreting and
technology in various settings.
Pokorn, Nike K., Mauricio Viezzi, and Tatjana R. Felberg (eds) (2020) Teacher Education for Community
Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation. Selected Chapters. Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press/N IJZ.
The book addresses the need for educating the trainers and teachers of ( future) practitioners of PSI, presenting
different pedagogical approaches, including blended learning.
Siemens, George, Dragan Gašević, and Shane Dawson (eds) (2015) Preparing for the Digital University: A
Review of the History and Current State Of Distance, Blended, and Online Learning. Athabasca, Athabasca
University. URL: https://researchmgt.monash.edu/ws/portalfiles/portal/256525723/256524746_
oa.pdf (accessed 1 December 2021).
This publication presents a series of articles offering an overview of research on distance, on-line and blended
learning and positions blended learning in the wider complex domain of digital learning.
Skaaden, Hanne (2017) “‘That we all behave like professionals’: An experiential–dialogic approach to
interpreter education and online learning”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: Research-based Proposals
337
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Pub-
lishing Company: 323–40.
This is one of the r esearch-based chapters in the a bove-mentioned book. It explores epistemic issues of PSI education
in the context of blended learning. A qualitative analysis of chat logs shows how the applied didactic approach
allows for professional identity and knowledge to develop.
Related chapters
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalization of inter-
preters? by Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 18, Role play as a means of training and of testing public service interpreting by Magnus Dahnberg
Chapter 19, Monitoring in dialogue interpreting – cognitive and didactic perspectives by Elisabet Tiselius and
Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
Chapter 21, The Conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elisabeth Stokoe.
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia
Chapter 23, Interprofessional Education …. interpreter education: In or and? by Demi Krystallidou.
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to communicate via interpreter by Tatjana R. Felberg and
Gry Sagli.
Chapter 25, Education and training of public service interpreter teachers by Mira Kadrić and Sonja
Pöllabauer.
References
Agar, Michael (1985) “Institutional discourse”, Text – Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of Discourse
5 (3): 147–68.
Alizadeh, Mehrasa, Parisa Mehran, Ichiro Koguchi, and Haruo Takemura ( 2019) “Evaluating a
blended course for Japanese learners of English: Why quality matters”, International Journal of Edu-
cational Technology in Higher Education 16 (1): 1–21.
Balogh, Katalin, and Heidi Salaets (eds) (2016) TraiLLD: Training in Languages of Lesser Diffusion. Train-
ing of Legal Interpreters in LLDs. Leuven, Lannoo Campus.
Bersin, Josh (2004). The Blended Learning Book: Best Practices, Proven Methodologies, and Lessons Learned.
San Francisco, CA, Pfeiffer.
Bonk, Curtis J., and Charles R. Graham (2006) The Handbook of Blended Learning: Global Perspectives,
Local Designs. San Francisco, CA, Wiley.
Braun, Sabine (2019) “Technology and interpreting”, in The Routledge Handbook of Translation and Tech-
nology, Minako O’Hagan (ed). New York, Routledge: 271–88.
Braun, Sabine, and Judith Taylor (eds) (2012) Videoconference and Remote Interpreting in Criminal Proceed-
ings. Cambridge, Intersentia.
Braun, Sabine, Cathrine Slater, Robert Gittins, Panagiotis Ritsos, and Jonathan Roberts (2013)
“Interpreting in virtual reality: Designing and developing a 3D virtual world to prepare interpret-
ers and their clients for professional practice”, in New Prospects and Perspectives for Educating Language
Mediators, Donald C. Kiraly, Silvia Hansen-Schirra, and Karin Maksymski (eds). Tübingen, Gunter
Narr: 93–120.
Carl, Michael, and Sabine Braun (2018) “Translation, interpreting and new technologies”, in The
Routledge Handbook of Translation Studies and Linguistics, Kirsten Malmkjær (ed). London, Routledge:
374–90.
Chan, Clara Hoyan (2014) “Building an online library for interpretation training: Explorations into an
effective blended-learning mode”, Computer Assisted Language Learning 27 (5): 454–79.
Cherny, Lynn (1999). Conversation and Community: Chat in a Virtual World. Stanford, CSLI Publications.
Cirillo, Letizia, and Maura Radicioni (2017) “(Role-)playing fair(s)”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting:
Research-based Proposals for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amster-
dam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 119–37.
Cross, Jay (2006) “Foreword” in The Handbook of Blended Learning: Global Perspectives, Local Designs,
Curtis J. Bonk and Charles R. Graham (eds). San Francisco, CA, Wiley: x vii–xxiii.
338
Blended learning is here to stay!
Dahnberg, Magnus (2015) “Tolkemedierade samtal som rollspel.” [“Interpreter mediated dialogues as
roleplays”], PhD diss., Uppsala, Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis
Deysel, Elizabeth, and Harold Lesch (2018) “Experimenting with c omputer-assisted interpreter train-
ing tools for the development of self-assessment skills: National Parliament of RSA”, in Interpreting
and Technology, Claudio Fantinuoli (ed). Berlin, Language Science Press: 6 0–90.
Driesen, Christiane (2012) “Die Tandem-Lehrmethode zur Qualifizierung von Dolmetschern in
seltenen Sprachen entsprechend der EU-Richtlinie 2010/64”, in Dolmetschqualität in Praxis, Lehre und
Forschung: Festschrift für Sylvia Kalina, Barbara Ahrens, Michaela A lbi-Mikasa, and Claudia Sasse
(eds). Tübingen: Gunther Narr Verlag: 149–62.
Eser, Oktay, Miranda Lai, and Fatih Saltan (2020) “The affordances and challenges of wearable tech-
nologies for training public service interpreters”, Interpreting 22 (2): 288–308.
Fantinuoli, Claudio (2018a) “Computer-assisted interpreting: Challenges and future perspectives”, in
Trends in e -Tools and Resources for Translators and Interpreters, Isabel D
urán-Muñoz and Gloria Corpas
Pastor (eds). Leiden, Brill: 153–74.
Fantinuoli, Claudio. (ed) (2018b) Interpreting and Technology. Berlin, Language Science Press.
Felberg, Tatjana R., and Hanne Skaaden (2020) “A blended approach to interpreter education: online
and onsite learning activities in concert”, in Teacher Education for Community Interpreting and Intercul-
tural Mediation. Selected Chapters, Nike K. Pokorn, Mauricio Viezzi, and Tatjana R. Felberg (eds).
Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press/N IJZ: 112–35.
Gee, James Paul, and Judith L. Green (1998). “Discourse Analysis, Learning, and Social Prac-
tice: A Methodological Study.” Review of Research in Education, 23(1), 119–169. doi: 10.3102/
0091732X023001119
Gentile, Adolfo (1995) “Translation theory teaching. Connecting theory and practice”, in Teaching
Translation and Interpreting 3, Cay Dollerup and Vibeke Appel (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins
Publishing Company: 55–63.
Giambruno, Cynthia (2014) “Dealing with languages of lesser diffusion”, in Assessing Legal Interpreter
Quality through Testing and Certification: The Qualitas Project, Cynthia Giambruno (ed). Alicante,
Publicaciones Universidad de Alicante: 83–107.
Graham, Charles R. (2006) “Blended learning systems: Definition, current trends, and future direc-
tions”, in The Handbook of Blended Learning: Global Perspectives, Local Designs, Curtis J. Bonk and
Charles R. Graham (eds). San Francisco, CA, Pfeiffer: 3 –21.
Gran, Laura, Angela Carabelli, and Raffaela Merlini (2002) “Computer-assisted interpreter training”,
in Interpreting in the 21st Century: Challenges and Opportunities, Guiliana Garzone and Maurizio
Viezzi (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 2 77–94.
Grimen, Harald (2008) “Profesjon og kunnskap [Profession and knowledge]”, in Profesjonsstudier [‘Sociol-
ogy of Professions’], Anders Molander and Lars Inge Terum (eds). Oslo, Universitetsforlaget: 71–87.
Hansen, Inge G., and Miriam Shlesinger (2007) “The silver lining: Technology and self-study in the
interpreting classroom”, Interpreting 9 (1): 95–118.
Herring, Susan C. (2004) “Computer-mediated discourse analysis: An approach to researching online
communities”, in Designing for Virtual Communities in the Service of Learning, Sasha Barab, Rob Kling,
and James H. Gray (eds). New York, Cambridge University Press: 338–76.
Holmer, Torsten (2 008) “Discourse structure analysis of chat communication”, Language@Internet 5:
1–17. URL: https://w ww.languageatinternet.org/a rticles/2 008/1633 (accessed 27 October 2021).
IMDi and NHO (The Directorate of Integration and Diversity (IMDi) and Norwegian Federation
of Service Industries and Retail Trade (N HO) (2018) “Behovet for tolk i 2017” [The need for
interpreters in 2017]. URL: https://w ww.imdi.no/contentassets/9471e477a7494d11826d63cf9af2
1ac1/behovet-for-tolk-i-2017 (accessed 27 October 2021).
Kadrić, Mira (2017) “Make it different! Teaching interpreting with theatre techniques”, in Teaching
Dialogue Interpreting: Research-based Proposals for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and N. Niemants
(eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 275–93.
Kerremans, Koens, Raquel Lázaro Gutiérrez, Helene Stengers, Antoon Cox, and Pascal Rillof (2019)
“Technology use by public service interpreters and translators: The link between frequency of use
and forms of prior training”, FITISPos International Journal 6 (1): 107–22.
Kim, Dohun (2017) “F lipped interpreting classroom: Flipping approaches, student perceptions and
design considerations”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 11 (1): 38–55.
Kinsella, Elizabeth Anne, and Allan Pitman (eds) (2012) Phronesis as Professional Knowledge: Practical
Wisdom in the Professions. Rotterdam, Sense Publishers.
339
Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
Kolb, David (1984) Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall.
Lim, Lily (2013) “Examining students’ perceptions of training”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 7
(1): 71–89.
Ma’arop, Amrien Hamila, and Mohamed Amin Embi (2016) “Implementation of blended learning in
higher learning institutions: A review of the literature”, International Education Studies 9 (3): 41–52.
Moser-Mercer, Barbara (2008) “Skill acquisition in interpreting: A human performance perspective”,
The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 2 (1): 1 –28.
Motta, Manuela (2016) “A blended learning environment based on the principles of deliberate practice
for the acquisition of interpreting skills”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 10 (1): 133–49.
Osman, Gihan, and Susan Herring (2007) “Interaction, facilitation, and deep learning in cross-
c ultural chat: A case study”, The Internet and Higher Education 10 (2): 125–41.
Ozolins, Uldis (2010) “Factors that determine the provision of public service interpreting: Compar-
ative perspectives on government motivation and language service implementation”, The Jour-
nal of Specialised Translation 14: 194‒215. URL: https://w ww.jostrans.org/issue14/a rt_ozolins.pdf
(accessed 1 December 2021).
Palloff, Rena M., and Keith Pratt. (1999) Building Learning Communities in Cyberspace. Effective Strategies
for the Online Classroom. San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Palloff, Rena M., and Keith Pratt (2001) Lessons from the Cyberspace Classroom. The Realitites of Online
Teaching. San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2013) “Teaching interpreting/Training interpreters”, in Handbook of Translation
Studies, Volume 4, Yves Gambier and Luc van Doorslaer (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Pub-
lishing Company: 174–80.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2016) Introducing Interpreting Studies, 2nd ed. London, Routledge.
Pokorn, Nike K., and Christopher D. Mellinger (2018) “Introduction: Community interpreting,
translation, and technology”, Translation and Interpreting Studies 13 (3): 337–41. URL: https://doi.
org/10.1075/t is.00019.int (accessed 10 December 2021).
Pokorn, Nike K., Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatjana Radanović Felberg (eds) (2020) Teacher Education for Commu-
nity Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation. Selected Chapers. Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press/NIJZ.
Rasheed, Abubakar Rasheed, Amirrudin Kamsin, and Nor Aniza Abdullah (2 020) “Challenges in
the online component of blended learning: A systematic review”, Computers and Education 144,
103701: 1–17. URL: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103701 (accessed 28 October 2021).
Şahin, Mehmet (2013) “Virtual worlds in interpreter training”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 7
(1): 91–106.
Sandrelli, Annalisa (2015) “Becoming an interpreter: The role of computer technology”, MonTI.
Monografías de Traducción e Interpretación, MonTI, Special Issue 2: 111–38.
Sandrelli, Annalisa, and Jesus de Manuel (2007) “The impact of information and communication
technology on interpreter training”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 1 (2): 269–303.
Schön, Donald A. (1983) The Reflective Practitioner. How Professionals Think in Action. New York, Basic Books.
Skaaden, Hanne (2016) “Training interpreters online. An experiential dialogic approach”, in TraiLLD:
Training in Languages of Lesser Diffusion, Katalin Balogh, Heidi Salaets, and Dominique Van Schoor
(eds). Leuven, Lannoo Campus: 46–69.
Skaaden, Hanne (2017) “‘That we all behave like professionals’: An experiential–dialogic approach to
interpreter education and online learning”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: R esearch-based Proposals
for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Pub-
lishing Company: 323–40.
Skaaden, Hanne (2018) “Remote interpreting: Potential solutions to communication needs in the
refugee crisis and beyond”, The European Legacy. Towards New Paradigms. Special Issue: A Question of
Communication: The Role of Public Service Interpreting in the Migrant Crisis, 23 (7–8): 837–56. URL:
https://doi.org/10.1080/10848770.2018.1499474 (accessed 10 December 2021).
Skaaden, Hanne, and Maria Wattne (2009) “Teaching interpreting in cyberspace. The answer to all
our prayers?”, Interpreting and Translating in Public Service Settings. Policy, Practice, Pedagogy, Raquel De
Pedro Ricoy, Isabelle Perez, and Chrisine Wilson (eds). Manchester, St. Jerome Publishing: 74–88.
Skrypnyk, Oleksandra, Srećko Joksimović, Vitomir Kovanović, Shane Dawson, Dragan Gašević, and
George Siemens (2015) “The history and state of blended learning”, in Preparing for the Digital
University: A Review of the History and Current State of Distance, Blended, and Online Learning, George
340
Blended learning is here to stay!
Siemens, Dragan Gašević, and Shane Dawson (eds). Athabasca, Athabasca University: 55–92.URL:
https://researchmgt.monash.edu/ws/portalfiles/portal/256525723/256524746_oa.pdf (accessed 28
October 2021).
Stein, Jared, and Charles R. Graham (2020) Essentials for Blended Learning: A Standards-based Guide.
London, Routledge.
Stengers, Hélène, Koen Kerremans, and Jeroen Segers (2018) “Integrating virtual conference tools in
interpreter and translator training”, Trans-kom 11 (2): 220–37.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (2014) “Perspectives on role-play: Analysis, training and assessment”, The Interpreter
and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 4 37–51.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
341
21
THE CONVERSATION ANALYTIC
ROLE-PLAY METHOD
How authentic data meet simulations for
interpreter training
Introduction
Developed by Stokoe (2011a) to train professionals working in communication-rich insti-
tutions (for instance, healthcare, police interviews, classrooms, sales, crisis negotiation), the
Conversation Analytic Role-Play Method (CARM) uses anonymised extracts from authen-
tic conversations, recorded in situ as part of the daily work of the organisation, to enable
trainees to learn from what actually happens in encounters. Having seen and heard a se-
quence of turns whose transcript is synchronised with the audio/v ideo recording, trainees
discuss possible interactional difficulties and suggest candidate next turns. The original next
turn of the conversation is revealed, and trainees consider its likely effectiveness. Starting
from Stokoe’s observation that even trainees with extensive experience have problems in
predicting what works, as well as articulating ‘tacit knowledge of their own practices, de-
spite the fact that “they can do it” in real situations’ (2011b), the CARM method appears
particularly relevant for the professionalisation of Public Service Interpreting (PSI) and for
training four possible targets.
First, CARM can be used to train practising PSI interpreters. While they are profes-
sionals in their own right, interpreters work across settings in which they are not members
(Garfinkel 1967). For example, PSI interpreters may not have equivalent epistemic access to
the work of medicine and the interactional actions that comprise it, as argued already in the
seventies by Labov and Fanshel (1977). Interpreters may therefore benefit from observing the
building blocks of the kinds of medical or other conversations they may interpret, so as to
adapt their skills to different ‘communities of practice’ (Wenger 1998).
Second, CARM may be applied to the training of student-interpreters (K rystallidou
2014; Dal Fovo 2016, 2018; Niemants and Stokoe 2017), allowing them to engage with au-
thentic contexts of a future profession they may have seldom (or never) experienced before,
which may make it difficult for them to reliably simulate interactions in traditional r ole-play
exercises. In contrast to role-plays presented by other authors (see, for example, Dahnberg
this volume), which presume trainees already understand the interpreting process and the
settings sufficiently to simulate i nterpreter-mediated encounters and/or are used to train and
343
Natacha Niemants et al.
out in different ways in interpreter training, ranging from ‘closed’ role-plays where primary
speakers are enacted by following a script and trainees interpret their pre-determined t urns-
at-talk, to more ‘open’ role-plays making use of cue cards and/or short role descriptions
where primary speakers, possibly played by professionals from that field, enact their role
according to what they believe they would say in that given situation (see Dahnberg this
volume). Irrespective of the method chosen, role-play activity is complex in two ways: as
Francis (1989) and Linell and Thunqvist (2003) underline, the framing activity involves actors
(typically trainers and trainees) pretending to be different characters, and the framed activity
involves the characters being acted (for instance, a doctor, a patient, and an interpreter).
Analyses of simulated encounters have shown that trainees tend to orient more to the fram-
ing than to the framed activity (De la Croix and Skelton 2009). Since trainees primarily
design their turns for the purpose of evaluation by the trainers rather than for the purpose
of communication between the primary participants (Niemants 2014), it is ‘hard to support
a claim that participants in role-play are oriented to the same interactional contingencies
as they would be in the actual settings’ (Stokoe 2011a). Even where r ole-plays are based on
authentic data, or scripts reproduce precise words spoken by real participants, what is at stake
to those participants will not be at stake to the trainers and trainees who simulate it, since
‘reality does not travel with the text’ (Widdowson 2003: 711) and authentic data do not nec-
essarily entail authentic activities (see Seidlhofer 2003; Boulton and Tyne 2014). As shown
in Stokoe (2013b), Niemants (2015), and Stokoe et al. (2020), a second level of inauthenticity
occurs since the participants in r ole-plays may use different communicative strategies to the
real settings, either with the aim of reproducing assessable talk or by invoking (stereotypical)
category-based knowledge of roles (see Francis 1989: 59).
However, empirical studies of authentic (interpreter-mediated) interactions have under-
mined many of the conventional arguments about r ole-played interaction, showing that:
- we cannot trust c ommon-sense intuitions since they tend to caricature what really hap-
pens (a s also pointed out by Schegloff 1996: 166–69 and Speer 2005: 54);
- translations are not necessarily provided on a t urn-by-turn basis as ‘conversation unfolds
through t urn-taking, with options at each point’ (Hepburn, Wilkinson and Butler 2014:
248), and interpreters may choose to negotiate their own understanding of the talk as
well as that of others before attempting to translate (Wadensjö 1998; Bolden 2000;
Davidson 2000, 2002; Baraldi and Gavioli 2012);
- interpreters do not only respond to primary speakers’ actions but also make ‘sequence-
initiating actions’ (Davitti and Pasquandrea 2014) – and do so at particular ‘choice
points’ (Hepburn, Wilkinson and Butler 2014: 248);
- interpreting is not just about verbal communication but also involves non-verbal com-
munication (Niemants, Ticca and Traverso 2021), whose importance is particularly vis-
ible when participants cannot share the same physical space and communicate through
telephone (Castagnoli and Niemants 2018; Russo, González Rodríguez and Iglesias
Fernández 2019) or through video interpreting platforms (Hansen 2020; Licoppe and
Veyrier 2020; Licoppe this volume) that appear on the increase because of the pandemic.
Such studies have started to pay much closer attention to ‘how people actually talk (a nd re-
spond) when interpreting and being interpreted’ (Turner and Merrison 2016: 138), thereby
questioning textual evaluations of interpreting as more or less accurate renditions of source
text into target texts, and highlighting ways in which apparently imperfect practices (such as
breaches of tenets of faithfulness and neutrality) can have effects on the coordination of PSI
344
The conversation analytic role-play method
as a social interaction (Baraldi 2012: 323), be it onsite or remote. As shown by Shaw et al.
(2016) and Stokoe et al. (2018), sometimes best practice, as identified by CA research, con-
tradicts existing guidance for practitioners. The implication is that when people turn guid-
ance into talk, it might not work, and that o ver-riding objectives and primary speakers’ first
moves may create unpredictable contingencies of interaction (that is, professional dilemmas
or choice points) which call for situated responses.
while the participant interacts with the text as an intended recipient, the observer
views this interaction from the outside, adopting a critical analytical perspective.
Observer as well as participant roles can allow learning: observation allows strategies
of interaction to be noticed, while participation allows such strategies to be tested
(2 001: 241).
As we will see in the following section, the CARM method enables learners to observe
authentic interactions as they unfold as well as to participate by responding to participants’
ongoing talk, thereby providing two ways of preparing to the complexity of PSI in different
settings and language combinations.
We illustrate the technique developed by Stokoe via authentic (interpreter-mediated)
healthcare audio data in an Italian hospital and video data of interpreting in Norwegian
hospital encounters, with the aim to train both (practising and would be) interpreters and
healthcare professionals. However, CARM can readily be adapted to fit other languages, do-
mains, and target audiences, provided that the trainer bases their workshops on conversation
analytic research.
Literature overview
Over the last decade, CARM has been used to address some of the shortcomings of closed and
open role-plays in different training settings and for different talk-based institutions (Stokoe
2014). CARM prompts workshop participants to analyse naturally occurring talk ‘in the
345
Natacha Niemants et al.
wild’ (Stokoe 2020: 333) and to investigate why a specific action is accomplished at a partic-
ular moment (why that now? being the quintessential CA question) then encourages discussion
of possible next turns. Although CARM almost always matches the data setting to the partic-
ipants (mediators see mediation, police see investigative interviews, salespeople see sales calls,
and so forth), workshops also include practices from other settings. For example, mediators
encounter crisis negotiation and crisis negotiators encounter sales calls, partly to show how the
workshop will run via clips from another setting, and also because many (in)effective practices
are shared across settings. In Church and Bateman (2019), for instance, trainers are confronted
with classroom recordings, and the ‘forensic reflection on practice’ CARM stimulates can
be valuable in informing trainers, that is early childhood teachers, and show that rather than
rely on hypothetical scenarios or role-plays, workshop participants can discuss what actually
happens in classrooms and how teachers respond to children, thereby stimulating reflection
on strategies for intervening in disputes (which are a productive site for learning). So CARM
provides them with an opportunity to gradually observe what is happening immediately prior
to a teacher intervention, to hypothesise about its likely trajectory, and assess whether the
teacher did manage to effectively intervene in the dispute. Halfway between those who advise
that children should resolve conflicts themselves and those who encourage direct intervention
by teachers, CARM here shows how practices are contingent to prior actions and how teach-
ers can locally respond to the children’s needs and concerns on a turn-by-turn basis, rather
than aprioristically adopt one position or the other. Quoting Pomerantz and Fehr (1997: 184),
the authors argue that this method ‘involves moving away from discussing what particular
people did on the occasion, to considering what people need to know and do in order to ap-
propriately perform the action in any new situation they encounter’.
The reach of CA, and thus of CARM (Stokoe 2020), is expanded by its openness to work
across disciplines and languages and makes of it a perfect candidate to deal with interpreter-
mediated interactions in different institutional settings, where just like in any other setting,
every word matters.
While CA-based activities have been used for about 20 years in interpreter training (for
instance by Merlini 2007; Zorzi 2008; Bührig et al. 2012; Davitti and Pasquandrea 2014;
Baraldi and Gavioli 2020), to the best of our knowledge the first attempt to use CARM in
PSI interpreting dates back to 2011, when, within the framework of her PhD project, Nie-
mants was comparing role-played vs. actual interpreter-mediated encounters in healthcare
and had identified CARM as a viable solution to avoid the two layers of inauthenticity she
has found in her corpus (see above). Since then, other trainers have used CARM, or variants
thereof, in interpreting classes at graduate level (Dal Fovo 2018), in sessions for postgraduate
students (K rystallidou 2014; Dal Fovo 2016), as well as in sessions for interpreters’ trainers
(Wadensjö 2014), and CARM is an entry of the Dictionary of Education and Assessment in
Translation and Interpreting Studies (Dastyar 2019).
346
The conversation analytic role-play method
As with any work involving use of recordings, training data must conform to the ethical
consent permissions with which they were collected. In particular, one must be aware that
even with voice pitching and blurred video clips, participants, especially interpreters, may
be identifiable based on for instance regional dialects. In general, it is important to encour-
age trainees to consider respectful treatment of participants whose actions we are discussing
before the data are presented, as suggested by Parry et al. (2016: 1281) in their recommen-
dations for good practice in video recording and especially in dissemination, reporting, and
use of the research findings.
Figure 21.1 S lide introducing questioning in men’s health consultations with Italian patients
347
Natacha Niemants et al.
Figure 21.2 E
xample of questioning in men’s health consultations with foreign patients (a)
348
The conversation analytic role-play method
Figure 21.3 E xample of questioning in men’s health consultations with foreign patients (b)
likely to produce different types of renditions and possibly some requests for clarifications,
while doctors will probably produce candidate answers by the patient) and the trainer finally
shows what follows (Figure 21.3).
In this second part of the excerpt, participants show that meaning may need clarification
even when patients and doctors communicate directly, because it is not in the utterance
itself (in this case quanti anni ha?) but is the result of a joint construction between speaker
and listener who, by reacting, show what they are making of previous turns. As such, this
non-mediated interaction may be fruitfully used both with (practising and would be) inter-
preters and with doctors, to raise their awareness of the machinery and sequential structure
of conversation beginnings in this specific medical context. Trainers may possibly comment
on the fact that in line 10, ANDm shows he is aware of a possible misunderstanding, then in
12 he shows his own understanding, and in 14 he utters something in a low volume, thereby
showing that he addresses his colleague who is writing down the medical report and tran-
scribing what is jointly negotiated in this excerpt, that is that the patient and his partner are
about the same age.
In the CARM workshop focussing on a men’s health context, trainers may venture as
far as showing what may happen if and when the patient’s partner is not only talked about
but also present in the consultation, which often happens in the case of infertility problems,
where partners can react to questions like quanti anni ha? and take some interactional space.
Once trainees have entered infertility consultations and realised that initial questions may not
be addressed to the patient but to relatives, trainers may move on to an interpreter-mediated
consultation where the same quanti anni ha? question appears. This final example aims to
raise trainees’ awareness of how/when interpreters talk in such consultations, demonstrating
how the interpreters’ participation is locally negotiated in collaboration. From the trainers’
viewpoint, there are no added constraints for the use of CARM with interpreter-mediated
data, which still make use of anonymised audios and synchronised transcripts. While from
the trainees’ viewpoint, be they practising or future interpreters and/or doctors, the two Fig-
ures below show that mediated interactions are not just the sum of original turns plus their
349
Natacha Niemants et al.
(immediate) renditions into the other language, but a different and co-constructed object,
where all participants – a nd especially doctors and interpreters – contribute on a t urn-by-
t urn basis (Figure 21.4).
In the excerpt above, there is no need for the pragmatic context to disambiguate the
quanti anni ha? question, since the physician makes the referent explicit in his turn 3. How-
ever, the trainer may want to comment on the fact that the doctor does not only produce
the routine question about age, as he did when interacting with the patients directly, but
he packs it with two other questions (one about how long the couple has been looking for
a child and the other about their having other children already). By blocking the transcript
synchronised with the audio after the 0.6 silence at line 6, trainers may additionally invite
trainees to reflect on its meaning, before asking them to produce candidate next turns and
playing what really happened in this authentic consultation, where the E nglish-speaking
woman called to interpret shows visible/audible troubles in rendering the series of questions
(Figure 21.5).
If used with experienced and in-training interpreters, these last two excerpts can show
that PSI is not just about the interpreter, since here it is the doctor himself who changes the
way in which he asks the same routine question (a s a standalone vs. in a series of questions).
This practice is not necessarily functional in interpreter-mediated interactions, where the la-
tency increases after long turns and series of questions, which both seem to project dyadic se-
quences in the foreign language where the I talian-speaking doctor is temporarily excluded.
Interpreters may thus be called to reflect on the need to intervene in medical consultations
where doctors’ first moves may determine non-functional conversational mechanisms, to
create some space for their translating and coordinating activities, and to ensure more effec-
tive communication.
If used with doctors, the very same excerpt can lead them to conclude that asking ques-
tions one at a time, and making their referent explicit, may possibly be more functional in
interpreter-mediated interactions, where interpreters need to easily understand and then
render jointly constructed meaning. Trainers may show doctors alternative examples of ex-
plicit standalone questions present in the corpus (like la sua compagna quanti anni ha?, quanti
350
The conversation analytic role-play method
Figure 21.5 E
xample of questioning in interpreter-mediated men’s health consultations (b)
anni ha sua moglie?, where the partner and the wife are explicitly referred to) and, in case
video recordings were present, let them examine the gazes and gestures that accompany
and/or substitute turns at talk or parts thereof, thereby further raising their awareness of
non-verbal communication.
351
Natacha Niemants et al.
Figure 21.6 First slide in training path, image for introduction to the setting
(Figure 21.7). The extract demonstrates how the MP poses a question directed to Paulina
through physical orientation, gaze and by naming her, and as such explicitly allocates her as
the next speaker. Since Paulina and the MP do not speak the same language, interpreting is
treated as relevant by all participants, and the interpreter produces the next turn. Although
the interpreter does not have available all the same resources for directing the utterance to
Paulina (interpreter’s gaze and bodily orientation from a remote location will not have the
352
The conversation analytic role-play method
Figure 21.8 Slide with images from both sites in the interaction
same referential meaning for the participants at the ward), the MP’s use of name makes it
possible for the interpreter to direct the utterance in Polish, which in turn makes relevant a
response by Paulina. If it was not already clear to all participants who were addressed by the
question based on prior speech and local context, the question’s design makes it clear to all
participants who are addressed. From a conversation analytic point of view, the long silence
following the interpreter’s rendition (line 4) could indicate trouble responding to the ques-
tion. However, time lag may occur in v ideo-mediated interaction changing temporality of
the interaction (Rusk and Pörn 2019). So, while the extract is seemingly simple and only dis-
plays an interpreted extract of a question-answer sequence, it is still rich in discussion points.
The next extract shows a similar learning point, however the example is more complex.
The slide illustrated in F igure 21.8 contains images from both sites in the interaction, both
the interpreter’s point of view (to the left) and the ward’s point of view (to the right). The in-
terpreter has only partial visual access to the doctor and cannot see the patient as the patient
is seated beyond the scope of the camera. After introducing the seating arrangements and
visual affordances to the trainees, trainers may play the first part of the excerpt, where the
doctor and patient have begun to speak directly in Norwegian. For this example, we suggest
showing the video as recorded from the interpreter’s point of view first (that in the left-hand
side of Figure 21.8 above).
The doctor asks questions and the patient responds without leaving space for interpreting
(line 1). The interpreter produces an audible i n-breath (line 2), which due to time lag is only
audible in the ward during the patient’s response (line 3). The doctor treats the patient’s
response as adequate and moves on to a next question (line 9). After the patient’s minimal
response (line 11), the doctor expands his question, specifying ‘w ith the medication’ (line
12). Again, the interpreter produces an audible in-breath that is only audible in the ward
during the doctor’s utterance. After the doctor’s utterance, follows a long silence. This would
be a place to stop the video and invite trainees to discuss what the next move would be and
why (Figures 21.9 and 21.10).
353
Natacha Niemants et al.
Figure 21.9 S lide showing second example in the training path up until focus point
What happens next is that the doctor verbally invites the interpreter to interpret (see
Hansen 2020 for an in-depth analysis). The example demonstrates how the relevance of
interpreting, and thus the interactional trajectory, is locally negotiated by all participants.
This last example confirms, although from a different perspective, that translation is jointly
negotiated: while in Figures 21.4 and 21.5 it was the doctor’s first move that affected the
interpreter’s rendition, here it is the patient responding directly that makes interpreting a
matter of negotiation (and again far from the ideal pattern where interpreters closely translate
after every single turn at talk).
Showing the same sequence with the video from the ward’s point of view will demonstrate
to the trainees how participants in interaction may rely on embodied resources in interaction,
354
The conversation analytic role-play method
which are not available to participants at other sites. This training trajectory has demon-
strated how both the interactional space afforded by the visual ecology, an interactional space
participants-in-interaction create and shape by using the technology, and primary partici-
pants’ actions may affect interpreters’ understanding and actions. This training path, with
escalating complexity, enables to observe embodied conduct and how interaction and as such
interpreting is organised as a complex set of actions, both verbal and embodied. In their un-
derstanding of what is (a) the meaning of a primary participants’ utterance, and (b) what is a
relevant next action to this, interpreters do not only draw on the verbal conduct, but also on
the complex multimodal utterances situated within a specific setting (Hansen 2020).
355
Natacha Niemants et al.
For the sake of convenience, the above training paths have been exemplified using au-
thentic (interpreter-mediated) healthcare data and focussing on just one building block of
medical encounters (the q uestion-answer pair). However, CARM may readily be adapted
to fit any language and PSI domain (police interviews, asylum interviews, school settings,
political interviews, and so forth), provided that the trainer has (a) a collection of audio-
and/or v ideo-recorded interactions available or can use data collected and analysed by other
researchers, and (b) a thorough understanding of CA enabling the identification of the same,
or other, conversational patterns. A first related topic to this chapter is therefore the prob-
lem of data availability. While both text and multimodal corpora are increasingly available
through archives, still not much data are available in the area of interpreter-mediated inter-
action and trainers may find it difficult to obtain the necessary data. When data are available
and transcribed, trainers may need support for analysing and presenting them. They may
thus benefit from attending a CARM workshop (https://w ww.carmtraining.org) as well as
from reading CA studies applied to interpreting in different institutional settings, possibly
becoming a CARM affiliate (https://w ww.carmtraining.org/a ffiliate).
A CARM workshop may be carried out with smaller groups of 15–35 participants, which
is perhaps more common, to larger groups of as many as 1 00–50. The workshop may be car-
ried out as a short session of a couple of hours, a series of sessions of a range of time, or longer
workshops lasting for a day or two. The CARM trainer chooses a number of ‘trainables’ and a
number of examples to demonstrate the trainable depending on the duration of the session and
the purpose of the workshop. The latter can be carried out as a live session with all participants
physically co-present at one location, it can take place remotely using video-technologies or it
may involve both on-site and remote participants in synchronous blended mode. Irrespective
of the mode and of the number of trainables and trainees, each example is framed and intro-
duced for the participants. The extract is played for them using audio or video recordings and
synchronised transcripts. The script is stopped at a specific point in the interaction and the par-
ticipants are invited to discuss what the next move should be. This is then generally discussed
in smaller groups of three to six participants. The trainer then shows the participants what was
the next move in this specific setting. The groups proceed to discuss what the practitioner/s
did in the extract. The trainer moves on to a next example, scaffolding the participants to iden-
tify effective (and less effective) practice from the evidence laid out to them. Many different
topics may be brought up during the workshops depending on the chosen trainables, on the
extracts presented, and on the discussions brought up by workshop participants (professional
ethics, professional practice, or interactional practices, to mention just a few). As exemplified
in what follows, the workshops may also possibly prepare the ground for some role-playing,
where the trainees can act out the conversations they have observed and participated in.
Although CARM has so far mainly been used in language-specific interpreting courses
involving single language pairs such as those in the experiences dealt with in this chapter, we
would argue that it may also be suitable in general interpreter training. One general course
of this type was experimented in a master interpreting program held at the University of
Bologna/Forlì in the academic years 2020/21, 2021/22 and 2022/23. This 30-hour mod-
ule presented 32 Italian-speaking students having five different language combinations (Ita
lian<>English/French/German/Russian/Spanish) with authentic data recorded in a range
of languages and specialised settings. The aim was to foster discussion on relevant theory
starting from the practice of interpreters and mediators in, for example, healthcare, a sylum-
seeking interviews, business meetings, and TV talk-shows (see Niemants 2021b for further
details). While dialogue interpreting classes in Forlì are traditionally taught in c o-presence
by two university trainers at the same time, one for each language, CARM could here
356
The conversation analytic role-play method
make ‘a language-specific cohort dependent on one teacher’ (Hale and Ozolins 2014: 7),
thereby preparing the theoretical ground of all first year students (the module was in blended
synchronous mode with most of the students in the classroom and some at home following
and interacting via Microsoft Teams in 2020/21 and 2021/22, while it was entirely onsite
in 2022/23) before they started their additional 20-hour traditional role-play exercises per
language pair. We would thus venture as far as stating that such a language-independent use
of CARM proved suitable to respond to cost-cutting pressures, which over the last years
have affected a number of courses not only at Bologna/Forlì, as well as to address remote
teaching/learning needs, such as those required during current pandemics.
Further reading
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (eds) (2021) “W hen clinicians and patients do not speak the same
language – Interpreting in health care”, special issue of Health Communication 36 (9).
The seven papers published in this special issue of Health Communication inquire what happens when a
bilingual interpreter and/or mediator participates in talk between health care professionals and patients who
speak different languages, thereby providing a good basis to identify some possible trainables in view of a setting-
specific CARM workshop.
Hansen, Jessica, and Jan Svennevig (2021) “Creating space for interpreting within extended turns at
talk”, Journal of Pragmatics 182: 144–62.
This article explores multimodal practices employed by medical professionals and interpreters to collab-
oratively manage the temporary suspension of medical professionals’ longer turns in progress, which allows
interpreters to begin interpreting. It also addresses how video-technology provides a complex interactional space
challenging the fine-tuned moment-by-moment negotiation of turns.
Niemants, Natacha, and Anne Delizée (eds) (2021) The Interpreters’ Newsletter 26.
This thematic issue of The Interpreters’ Newsletter focuses on dialogue interpreting methodology and the
eleven contributions it contains are based on authentic data in different communicative contexts, thereby proving
useful to identify trainables across languages-cultures and settings, as well as to raise awareness of the wide
range of possible analytical lenses.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2018) Talk. The Science of Conversation. London, Little Brown Book Group.
The book describes key domains of conversation analytic research and challenges much of what we think we
know about talk, including many communication myths. It was written as a result of the public engagement
generated by CARM workshops so that the multiple non-academic stakeholders of CA research, and anyone
who is new to this method and approach, could learn about it.
Related chapters
Chapter 1, General issues of PSI: institutions, codes and norms, professional quality by Carmen Valero-Garcés
Chapter 5, Corpus-based studies of PSI by Bernd Meyer
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in healthcare by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 16, ‘Interpreter’s mistake’ — Why should other professions care about the professionalization of inter-
preters? by Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 18, Roleplay as a means of training and of testing public service interpreting by Magnus Dahnberg
Chapter 23, Interprofessional education … interpreter education: in or and? Taking stock and moving forward by
Demi Krystallidou
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to work with interpreters by Tatjana Radanovic Felberg
and Gry Sagli
Notes
1 The data consist of three sub-corpora of men’s health consultations: the first with Italian patients,
the second with foreign patients speaking Italian, and the third with foreign patients communi-
cating through an interpreter-mediator. The data were collected as part of an interdisciplinary
project financed by the University of Modena and Reggio Emilia and gathering andrologists,
357
Natacha Niemants et al.
sociologists, and linguists (FAR 2017, Claudio Baraldi, PI, together with Federico Corradini and
Laura Gavioli as linguists, as well as with Antonio Granata, Michela Locaso, Vincenzo Rochira,
Daniele Santi, and Giorgia Spaggiari as andrologists).
2 This work was partly supported by the Research Council of Norway through its Centres of Ex-
cellence funding scheme, project number 223265.
References
Anderson, Laurie (2012) “Code-switching and coordination in i nterpreter-mediated interaction”,
in Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi, and Laura Gavioli ( eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 115–48.
Baraldi, Claudio ( 2012) “ Interpreting as dialogic mediation: The relevance of expansions”, in
Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting, Claudio Baraldi, and Laura Gavioli ( eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 297–326.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
eds) (
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (2019) La mediazione linguistico-culturale nei servizi sanitari: interazi-
one ed efficacia comunicativa. Milano, Franco Angeli.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli (2020). “Authentic mediated interactions for training healthcare
mediators”, in Teacher Education for Community Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation: Selected Chap-
ters, Nike Pokorn, Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatiana Radanovic Felberg (eds). Ljubljana, Ljubljana
University Press: 176–99.
Bolden, Galina (2000) “Toward understanding practices of medical interpreting: Interpreters’ involve-
ment in history taking”, Discourse Studies 2 (4): 387–419.
Boulton, Alex, and Henry Tyne (2014) Des documents authentiques aux corpus. Paris, Didier.
Bührig, Kristin, Ortrun Kliche, Bernd Meyer, and Birte Pawlack (2012) “The corpus ‘Interpreting in
Hospitals’: Possible applications for research and communication training”, in Multilingual Corpora
and Multilingual Corpus Analysis, Thomas Schmidt, and Kai Wörner (eds). Amsterdam & Philadel-
phia, John Benjamins: 305–17.
Castagnoli Sara, and Natacha Niemants (2018) “Corpora worth creating: A pilot study on telephone
interpreting”, InTRAlinea. URL: http://w ww.intralinea.org/specials/a rticle/corpora_worth_cre-
ating_a_pilot_study_on_telephone_interpreting (accessed 20 December 2021).
Chiarenza, Antonio (2020) “Practice-based training for intercultural mediators in healthcare ser-
vices”, in Teacher Education for Community Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation: Selected Chapters,
Nike Pokorn, Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatiana Radanovic Felberg (eds). Ljubljana, Ljubljana Univer-
sity Press: 200–26.
Church Amelia, and Amanda Bateman (2019) “Methodology and professional development: Conver-
sation Analytic Role-play Method (CARM) for early childhood education”, Journal of Pragmatics
143: 242–54.
Clark, Herbert, and Susan Brennan (1991) “Grounding in communication”, in Perspectives on Socially
Shared Cognition, Lauren Resnick, John Levine, and Stephanie Teasley (eds). Washington, DC,
American Psychological Association: 127–48.
Dal Fovo, Eugenia (2016) “The interpreter’s role in dialogue interpreting on television: A training
method”, The Interpreters’ Newsletter 23: 83–113.
Dal Fovo, Eugenia (2018) “The use of dialogue interpreting corpora in healthcare interpreter training:
taking stock”, in Pragmatic Issues in Specialized Communicative Contexts, Francesca Bianchi, and Sara
Gesuato (eds). Leiden & Boston, Brill Rodopi: 48–68.
Dastyar, Vorya (2019) Dictionary of Education and Assessment in Translation and Interpreting Studies. New-
castle upon Tyne, Cambridge Scholars Publishing 67.
Davidson, Brad (2000) “The interpreter as institutional gatekeeper: The social-linguistic role of inter-
preters in Spanish-English medical discourse”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (3): 379–405.
Davidson, Brad (2002) “A Model for the construction of conversational common ground in inter-
preted discourse”, Journal of Pragmatics 34: 1273–300.
Davitti, Elena, and Sergio Pasquandrea (2014) “Enhancing r esearch-led interpreter education: An ex-
ploratory study in applied conversation analysis”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 374–98.
De la Croix, Anne, and John Skelton (2009) “The reality of role-play: Interruptions and amount of
talk in simulated consultation”, Medical Education 43: 695–703.
358
The conversation analytic role-play method
Falbo, Caterina, and Eugenia Dal Fovo (2019) “How to design and prepare role-plays”. Presentation
given at the TRAMIG project workshop 3, October, 30, University of Trieste.
Falbo, Caterina, and Natacha Niemants (2020) “Œuvrer pour se comprendre: de la responsabilité de
l’interprète et des autres participants”, in Regards sur les médiations culturelles et sociales. Acteurs, dis-
positifs, publics, enjeux linguistiques et identitaires, Jean Paul Dufiet, and Elisa Ravazzolo (eds). Trento,
Collana Labirinti n. 186, Università di Trento: 39–66.
Francis, David (1989) “Game identities and activities: Some ethnomethodological observations”, in
Communication and Simulation: From Two Fields to One Theme, David Crookall, and Danny Saunders
(eds). London, Multilingual Matters: 53–68.
Garfinkel, Harold (1967) Studies in Ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall.
Gavioli, Laura (2015) “On the distribution of responsibilities in creating critical issues in interpreter-
mediated medical consultations: The case of ‘le spieghi(a mo)”, Journal of Pragmatics 76: 169–80.
Gavioli, Laura, and Guy Aston (2001) “Enriching reality: Language corpora in language pedagogy”,
ELT Journal 55 (3): 238‒46.
Gutiérrez, Raquel Lázaro (2013) “Natural interpreters’ performance in the medical setting”, in The
Critical Link 6. Interpreting in a Changing Landscape, Chrstina Schäffner, Krzysztof Kredens, and
Yvonne Fowler (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 165–85.
Hale, Sandra, and Uldis Ozolin (2014) “Monolingual short courses for language-specific accredita-
tion: Can they work? A Sydney experience”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 8 (2): 1–23.
Hansen, Jessica (2020) “Invisible participants in a visual ecology: Visual space as a resource for or-
ganising v ideo-mediated interpreting in hospital encounters”, Social Interaction. Video-Based Studies
of Human Sociality 3 (3). URL: https://t idsskrift.dk/socialinteraction/a rticle/v iew/122609/169758
(accessed 20 December 2021).
Hepburn, Alexa, Sue Wilkinson, and Carly Butler (2014) “Intervening with conversation analysis in
telephone helpline services: Strategies to improve effectiveness”, Research on Language and Social
Interaction 47 (3): 239–54.
Heritage, John (2010) “Questioning in medicine”, in “W hy Do You Ask?”: The Function of Questions in
Institutional Discourse, Alice Freed, and Susan Ehrlich (eds). New York, Oxford University Press: 4 2–68.
Heritage, John (2011) “Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: Empathic moments in in-
teraction”, in The Morality of Knowledge in Conversation, Tanya Stivers, Lorenza Mondada, and Jakob
Steensig (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 159–83.
Heritage, John, and Douglas Maynard (2006) Communication in Medical Care. Cambridge, Cambridge
University Press.
Krystallidou, Demi (2014) “Gaze and body orientation as an apparatus for patient inclusion into/
exclusion from a patient-centred framework of communication”, The Interpreter and Translator
Trainer 8 (3): 399–417.
Labov, William, and David Fanshel (1977) Therapeutic Discourse: Psychotherapy as Conversation. New
York, Academic Press.
Licoppe, Christian, and C laire-Antoine Veyrier (2020) “The interpreter as a sequential coordinator
in courtroom interaction ‘Chunking’ and the management of turn shifts in extended answers in
consecutively interpreted asylum hearings with remote participants”, Interpreting 22 (1): 56–86.
Linell, Per, and Daniel Thunqvist (2003) “Moving in and out of framings: Activity contexts in talk
with young unemployed people within a training project”, Journal of Pragmatics 35: 409–34.
Mason, Ian (2006) “On mutual accessibility of contextual assumptions in dialogue interpreting”, Jour-
nal of Pragmatics 38: 3 59–73.
Merlini, Raffaela (2007) “Teaching dialogue interpreting in higher education: A research-driven, pro-
fessionally oriented curriculum design”, in Tradurre: Professione e Formazione, MariaTeresa Musac-
chio, and Geneviève Henrot (eds). Padova, CLEUP: 2 77–306.
Niemants, Natacha (2014) “La voix de l’interprète dans l’entretien clinique réel et simulé”, Repères-Dorif
Traduction, médiation, interprétation 2. URL: https://w ww.dorif.it/reperes/natacha-s-a-n iemants-la-
voix-de-l interprete-d ans-lentretien-clinique-reel-et-simule/ (accessed 20 December 2021).
Niemants, Natacha (2015) L’interprétation de dialogue en milieu médical: Du jeu de rôle à l’exercice d’une
responsabilité. Roma, Aracne.
Niemants, Natacha (2019) “La voix des patient.e.s en santé reproductive: pour une interprétation des
(non)-dits”, mediAzioni 26. URL: https://mediazioni.sitlec.unibo.it/index.php/no-26-special-issue-
2019/119-articoliarticles-no-26-2019-/3 92-2020-02-22-10-47-35.html (accessed 20 December
2021).
359
Natacha Niemants et al.
Niemants, Natacha (2021a) “La participation des migrants en santé reproductive: le cas des consulta-
tions d’andrologie”, ELA 202: 207–18.
Niemants, Natacha (2021b) “Teoria e prassi dell’interpretazione dialogica”, in Interpretare da e verso
l’italiano: didattica e innovazione per la formazione dell’interprete, Mariachiara Russo (ed). Bologna,
BUP: 41– 60.
Niemants, Natacha, and Elizabeth Stokoe (2017) “Using CARM in healthcare interpreter education”,
in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: Research-Based Proposal for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo, and
Natacha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 293–321.
Niemants, Natacha, Anna Claudia Ticca, and Véronique Traverso (2021) “Patients’ disalignment in
two different healthcare settings”, Health Communication 36 (9): 1068–79.
Ozolins, Uldis (2017) “It’s not about the interpreter: Objectives in dialogue interpreting teaching”, in
Teaching Dialogue Interpreting: R esearch-Based Proposal for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo, and Nata-
cha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam & Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 45–62.
Parry, Ruth, Marco Pino, Christina Faull, and Luke Feathers (2016) “Acceptability and design of
v ideo-based research on healthcare communication: Evidence and recommendations”, Patient Edu-
cation and Counselling 99 (8): 1 271–84.
Phelan, Mary, Mette Rudvin, Hanne Skaaden, and Patrick Stefan Kermit (2020) Ethics in Public Service
Interpreting. London/New York, Routledge.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2004) Introducting Interpreting Studies. London/New York, Routledge.
Pomerantz, Anita, and Barbara Fehr (1997) “Conversation analysis: An approach to the study of social
action as sense making practices”, in Discourse as Social Interaction: Discourse Studies: A Multidisci-
plinary Introduction, Teun van Dijk (ed). London, Sage. 64–91.
Raymond, Chase Wesley (2014) “Epistemic brokering in the interpreter-mediated medical visit:
Negotiating “patient’s side” and “doctor’s side” knowledge”, Research on Language and Social Inter-
action 47 (4): 426–46.
Rudvin, Mette, and Elena Tomassini (2011) Interpreting in the Community and the Workplace. New York
et al., Palgrave Macmillan.
Rusk, Fredrik, and Michaela Pörn (2019) “Delay in L2 interaction in v ideo-mediated environments in
the context of virtual tandem language learning”, Linguistics and Education 50: 5 6–70.
Russo Mariachiara, María Jesús González Rodríguez, and Emilia Fernández Iglesias (eds) (2019). Tele-
phone Interpreting. The Impact of Technology on Dialogue Interpreting/L’interpretazione Telefonica. L’impatto
della tecnologia sull’interpretazione dialogica. Bologna, BUP.
Schegloff, Emanuel (1996) “Confirming allusions: Toward an empirical account of action”, American
Journal of Sociology 102 (1): 161–216.
Schegloff, Emmanuel (2007) Sequence Organization in Interaction. A Primer in Conversation Analysis, Vol.
1. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Seidlhofer, Barbara (eds) (2003) Controversies in Applied Linguistics. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Seleskovitch, Danica, and Marianne Lederer (2001) Interpréter pour traduire. Paris, Didier.
Shaw, Chloe, Elizabeth Stokoe, Katie Gallagher, Narendra Aladangady, and Neil Marlow (2016)
“Parental involvement in neonatal critical care d ecision-making”, Sociology of Health & Illness 20
(20):1–26.
Speer, Susan (2005) Gender Talk: Feminism, Discourse and Conversation Analysis. London/New York,
Routledge.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2011a) “Simulated interaction and communication skills training: The ‘conversation
analytic role-play method’”, in Applied Conversation Analysis, Charles Antaki (ed). New York, Pal-
grave Macmillan: 1 19–39.
Stokoe, Elizabeth ( 2011b) “ O vercoming roadblocks to mediation: Training mediators using the
‘Conversation Analytic Role-Play Method’”, Mediation Digest Online.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2013a) “Applying findings and creating impact from conversation analytic studies
of gender and communication”, Economic and Industrial Democracy 34 (3): 537–52.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2013b) “The (in)authenticity of simulated talk: Comparing r ole-played and actual
interaction and the implications for communication training”, Research on Language and Social Inter-
action 46 (2): 1 65–85.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2014) “The Conversation Analytic R ole-play Method (CARM): A method for
training communication skills as an alternative to simulated role-play”, Research on Language and
Social Interaction 47 (3): 255–65.
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2020) “Psychological matters in institutional interaction: Insights and interventions
from discursive psychology and conversation analysis”, Qualitative Psychology 7 (3): 331‒47.
360
The conversation analytic role-play method
Stokoe, Elizabeth, Charles Antaki, Emma Richardson, and Sara Willott (2018) “W hen police in-
terview victims of sexual assault: Comparing written guidance to interactional practice”, in The
Discourse of Police Investigation, Marianne Mason, and Frances Rock (eds). Chicago, IL, University
of Chicago Press: 21‒41.
Stokoe, Elizabeth, and Rein Sikveland (2017) “The conversation analytic role-play method: Simu-
lation, endogenous impact and interactional nudges”, in Theoretical Scholarship and Applied Practice,
Sarah Pink, Vaike Fors, and Tom O’Dell (eds). Oxford, Berghahn Books: 73‒96.
Stokoe, Elizabeth, Rein Sikveland, Magnus Hamann, Albert Saul, and William Housley (2020) “Can
humans simulate talking like other humans? Comparing mystery shoppers to real customers in
service inquiries”, Discourse Studies 22 (1): 87‒109.
Turner, Graham, and Andrew Merrison (2016) “Doing ‘Understanding’ in dialogue interpreting”,
Interpreting 18 (2): 137–71.
Van Hasselt, Vincent, Stephen Romano, and Gregory Vecchi (2008) “Role-playing: Applications in
hostage and crisis negotiation skills training”, Behavior Modification 32 (2): 2 48–63.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London/New York, Longman.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (2014) “Perspectives on role-play: Analysis, training and assessments”, The Inter-
preter and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 4 37–51.
Wenger, Etienne (1998) Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning and Identity. New York, Cambridge
University Press.
Widdowson, Henry (2003) Defining Issues in English Language Teaching. Oxford, Oxford University
Press.
Zorzi, Daniela (2008) “La formazione dei mediatori sanitari: fra esperienza e consapevolezza”, in
Immigrazione, mediazione culturale e salute, Claudio Baraldi, Viola Barbieri, and Guido Giarelli (eds).
Milano, Franco Angeli: 191–207.
361
22
TRAINING INTERPRETERS IN
ASYLUM SETTINGS
The REMILAS project
Anna Claudia Ticca, Véronique Traverso and Emilie Jouin
Introduction
This contribution is based on the REMILAS (Refugees, Migrants and their Languages in
healthcare services) research project1 which examines communication and mutual under-
standing in multilingual health, mental and social care consultations in France. We specif-
ically focused on the complexity of resources participants turn to in order to overcome the
communicative problems they encounter. The data we gathered are characterized by diver-
sity at different levels, including data collection methods, types of consultations and partic-
ipation formats, as well as diversity of analytic and conceptual approaches (from linguistic
analyses to medical sociology and the sociology of immigration).
In this chapter, we first discuss the diversity of the collected data and the issues it raises for
the analyses. We then turn to a short overview of recent studies devoted to Public Service
Interpreting (PSI) and asylum contexts. In the last section, we illustrate how we reinvested
our data for training interpreters and caregivers to deal with multilingual encounters with
asylum seekers (henceforth ASs). A particular emphasis is placed on the multimodal character
of such interactions and on the relevance of an interactional approach to training.
of artifacts, such as the computer or the telephone (Piccoli, Ticca and Traverso 2019; Tra-
verso 2019). We also conducted participant observations and informal interviews at the time
of the recordings.
Another set of data consists of more formal interviews with caregivers, interpreters and
migrants (a total of 34 interviews). It also includes self-confrontation interviews in the form
of workshops, to which we invited the caregivers (individually and collectively), as well as
researchers not associated to the project. These data gave us, as researchers, access to the in-
terpreters’ and caregivers’ awareness and understandings of the (general situation of ) consul-
tations with both non-French- and French-speaking migrants and refugees. Moreover, they
offered these professionals the occasion to observe and consider their own practice through
different analytical lenses.
This rich set of data also made it possible to construct a more holistic approach, which con-
jugates the fi ne-grained analytic process of interactions with the s elf-confrontation sessions.
363
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
members to prepare for such formal interviews are not considered, nor are other interviews
and consultations that usually take place beforehand. Yet, the variety of situations that we
have recorded turned out to be invaluable for our research in that it enabled us to observe
the role played by context in the development and understanding of the social encounters
in which participants are engaged (van Dijk 2007). For instance, we realized that ASs were
not always aware of why they were offered a visit to a practitioner, nor the kind of practi-
tioner they were consulting. Not knowing the reasons and goals of the visits can hinder an
AS from understanding conversational practices correctly, such as questions, statements and
recommendations. As for interpreters, they may adapt their practices to the challenges of a
certain type of consultation and be prompted to make interactional choices so as to orient
the conversation in ways considered useful for the outcomes of the encounter (Ticca 2020b;
section In-person training to a mixed public of professionals).
Finally, while some encounters are monolingual (in French or English as lingua franca),
most are interpreter-mediated with either family or community members with knowledge
of English or French or members of a local interpreter’s professional association in which
they have been trained. The receiving health institution generally provides the latter.
364
Training interpreters in asylum settings
professional norms. Such variability depends, in part, on the interpreter’s personal biogra-
phy, professional experience and level of training. This is important because it highlights the
peculiarity of interpreting in the asylum context, at the continuum between the more nor-
malized legal interpreting and PSI (Inghilleri 2005), where the interpreter’s self-initiatives
are not only tolerated, but also encouraged (Baraldi and Gavioli 2012; Ticca and Traverso
2017; Chambon et al. 2021).
Because they are the only ones who understand linguistic and pragmatic meanings
of both sides, the interpreter’s privileged position can promote an alignment with the
dominant, institutional side ( Jacquemet 2011), putting them in the role of institutional
gatekeepers (Davidson 2000; Pöllabauer 2004; Jacquemet 2009). Yet, interpreters’ epis-
temic and cultural knowledge is clearly controversial, even more so in the asylum context.
Indeed, Barsky (1996: 46) suggests considering them as ‘agents of culture’ who should
participate more actively in asylum hearings and openly support the claimants by disam-
biguating seemingly unclear or contradictory testimonies. Nonetheless, Inghilleri (2 005)
cautions against building an idealized vision of the interpreter as a cultural broker. Like
Mason (2 000), she argues that, in reality, interpreters may themselves not have access to
the discursive strategies needed to make such a connection come about. Others underline
the potentially counterproductive use of interpreters’ cultural knowledge vis-à-vis the AS.
This is evident in interpreters’ reluctance to translate tout court for political reasons (Duque
2020), or more occasionally, only segments of talk because of differences in their beliefs
and those of the AS. For instance, Jacquemet (2 016) reported a case in which a Kurdish
Sunni interpreter refused to translate a Kurdish Yazidi AS’s answer about his religion
because she considered it as devilish. Instilling or not elements emanating from the inter-
preter’s own epistemic and cultural knowledge in interaction relates to the more general
issue of providing faithful and precise translation. While the latter is required, important
and desirable in any given context, it is even more so in a sylum-seeking procedures, in
which the future and the very survival of claimants is at stake. In fact, in such multilingual
contexts, the insurgence of ‘c rosstalk problems’ (Gumperz 1979) and/or incoherencies may
determine a negative outcome of a hearing (Barsky 1996; Jacquemet 2011). Interpreters are
therefore considered to play a major role, not only in the formal hearings, but also in the
numerous preparatory interviews and encounter refugees attend throughout the asylum
process.
on-profit associations who are themselves refugees
In the case of France, interpreters of n
or asylees participate in meetings aimed at producing the asylum narratives to be presented
to the OFPRA. Their role is often crucial, in that they reinvest their own experience in
both evaluating beforehand the supposed AS’s legitimacy to ask for protection in the host
country and shaping a refugee’s storytelling according to what is expected by the OFPRA
(Clappe 2019; Niemants 2022). This has inevitable consequences for interpreters’ training
because it makes evident the tensions between the need for neutrality and fidelity of trans-
lation with the importance of mobilizing the interpreter’s knowledge when producing the
AS’s narrative.
365
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
asylum interpreting takes place need to be taken into account in interpreter training,
where different types of knowledge, experience and savoir-faire are to be highlighted and
developed.
Formal, university-based training in interpreting has among its main objectives to en-
hance students’ awareness of the importance of guaranteeing a trustful translation, as well
as their commitment to ensure h igh-quality interpreting (Bergunde and Pöllabauer 2019).
In this regard, the effort of European countries, including France, to train interpreters in a
standardized fashion appears to be insufficient (U NHCR 2010). Moreover, besides the lack of
trained interpreters, especially in the so-called languages of limited diffusion (LLDs), training
in France is clearly distinguished by two main domains: conference interpreting, which is
typically offered at universities, and PSI (interprétariat en milieu social et médical),5 which does
not have a professionalizing university training. Training for court interpreting is offered at
university as well as at a lower, local level by private institutions such as, the Compagnie des
experts de la cour d’appel (Eng. the Companies of Appeal Court Experts) that occasionally ded-
icate its activities to court interpreting.6
Compared to other European countries, such as Austria, Belgium, Norway, Spain,
Sweden, and Switzerland, PSI training in France remains underdeveloped, even though
some efforts to implement new training programs and sensitize governmental institutions
on the necessity of providing professionalization to PSI are nowadays emerging.7 These
initiatives are undertaken both by universities and local organizations involved in migra-
tion and a sylum-related procedures, who, compelled by the growing need for interpret-
ers, ask for institutional support to finance vocational training of interpreters. Training is
also delivered by the associations for whom interpreters work (Chambon, Zeroug-Vial,
and Carbonel 2017).8 Such vocational training courses, limited in hours, do not provide
an official, recognized certification of PS interpreters as professionals. As a consequence,
interpreters are poorly remunerated, making the profession insufficiently attractive to
new entrants.
As for academic interest in training in the asylum context in France, studies focus
mainly on language-related difficulties of ASs navigating different pathways to request
asylum ( see, for instance, the edited volume of S aglio-
Yatzimirsky and G alitzine-
L oumpet 2020). Other work describes the m icro-details of interactions. It highlights
the interpreter’s impact on the coordination of talk in video asylum hearings (L icoppe
and Verdier 2013, 2015), showing how troubles emerging in participants’ turn-taking
transitions may eventually negatively affect the hearing’s outcome (L icoppe and Veyrier
2020). Considerably less research analyzes asylum interpreters’ training itself or ques-
tions how to reinvest observations and analyses of interpreter-mediated interactions in
training (but see Chambon, Z eroug-Vial, and Carbonel 2017; Ticca 2018; Ticca, Lam-
bert and Traverso 2020). An exhaustive report issued by a joint meeting of researchers,
interpreters, lawyers, former OFPRA and CNDA assessors, among others (C ollective
authors 2000), identifies several major difficulties related to interpreting in asylum in-
terviews. One concerns language choice and the fact that when it is impossible to find
an interpreter for the language chosen by the AS, a relay language has to be selected.
Similarly, if an interpreter is available in the first chosen language, there is no guarantee
that his or her skills in the French language are sufficient to conduct the interview, or
that regional differences will not emerge, leading to mistakes that may compromise the
goal of obtaining protection (for example, this is the case for Dari and Pashto, two offi-
cial languages spoken in Afghanistan).
366
Training interpreters in asylum settings
The complexity emerging from cases, such as these, which are more frequent than
we might think, is proportional to the difficulty in proposing holistic training that
embraces a multiplicity of needs (
e.g., linguistic, cultural, interactional, social and
anthropological).
367
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
locally made, and therefore reconsider in a more useful way existing norms and principles.
As is known, prescribing activities or attitudes within a profession implies a simplification
of real-life situations (inevitably circumscribed by certain constitutive elements) which
builds a notable gap between real work and prescribed work (L eplat and Hoc 1983: 18). In
this sense, working on authentic video data has the value of allowing participants to delve
into the complexity of real professional practice and grasp its finer details (Filliettaz and
Zogmal 2017).
In what follows, we present a selection of the topics and phenomena included in our train-
ing courses. We begin with some observations on the multimodal dimension of PSI that are
valuable for any type of interpreter-mediated interactions, focusing on the organization of
participation. Here we draw particularly on C. Goodwin (1981) and C. Goodwin and H. M.
Goodwin (2005), who, even though they did not specifically focus on i nterpreter-mediated in-
teractions, offer invaluable insights on the place of gaze and body orientation in framing partic-
ipation. We will then turn to cases where ‘deviation’ from norms, such as translation fidelity and
neutrality, occurs and gives rise to ethical issues related to interpreters’ professional discretion.
368
Training interpreters in asylum settings
1. ‘Médicaments’11
A close look at the v ideo-recordings shows that, while DOC directs her opening question
to both patients (lines 01–02, 09; Figures 22.2–22.3), when translating, the interpreter only
gazes at PAH (lines 04–05, 10; Figure 22.4) and uses the singular when rendering the doc-
tor’s talk (line 10). This seems to explain why the female patient (PAF) does not speak at
all in this sequence. Indeed, it is PAH who replies (line 14), including PAF in his talk. The
interpreter next renders the patient’s answer, constructed with the indefinite form ‘on’, used
in spoken French as a substitution for nous (‘we’).
As we mentioned above, during the opening sequence of the consultation INT is still
drinking from his bottle (see Figure 22.2). Since he has no access to the doctor’s gaze, he
might be interpreting the use of ‘you’ (vous) in its singular meaning (line 09). In any event,
he might then consider DOC’s addressee to be the male patient, whom he does select as the
recipient of his own turn at talk (line 10).
Even though we prepare our audience to focus on participants’ multimodal behavior
when examining this short extract, it is only a minority of them who actually elaborate a
sequential analysis where ‘g aze’ emerges as a relevant element. Indeed, trainees, mostly in-
terpreters, tend to mobilize cultural reasons when asked to explain why they think it is the
369
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
Figure 22.2 V
iew 2 Line 01, DOC: so we didn’t see#
Figure 22.4 View 3 Line 04, INT: so you didn’t see each other before
male patient who replies to the doctor’s question (‘in Albania men have the right to speak
more than women’ being the most common reply). Although their g ender-oriented analyses
are certainly legitimate, we take this as an opportunity to invite participants to consider in
addition to what they already saw, the impact of the micro details of interaction (that is gaze)
in their analyses and, ultimately, in their professional practice.
370
Training interpreters in asylum settings
2. ‘Truth’
371
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
It is important to keep in mind that in formal hearings, ASs are very often suspected of lying
when telling their asylum story, whose truth needs to be continually proved and assessed
(Fassin and d’Halluin 2005; Ricet and Noûs 2022). What happens in this case, which might
be seen as a reflection of a generalized suspicion of ASs, is particularly interesting because it
highlights the source of the doubt, that is, the (apparent) incoherence of this AS’s talk due to
the alternating use of he and she to refer to the pimp and, thus, the doctor’s confusion about
this person’s gender (line 01).
Also of relevance is the interpreter’s intervention starting in line 08, in which she ac-
complishes several activities during the same turn at talk: she reformulates what the doctor
just said in line 06 (e.g. this becomes very difficult for the doctor, lines 1 1–12) and then adds new
meta-communicative information (e.g. the doctor has to know exactly what happened, to write you
a-you are here for a medical certificate lines 1 2–14), before encouraging PAT to tell the truth (so
just please tell him the truth, line 23).
Trainees’ reactions to the extract are quite interesting. Most of them, regardless of pro-
fession (caregivers or interpreters), disapprove of the interpreter’s self-initiative, considering
it as lying outside the framework of an interpreter’s role. Only a few display a more nuanced
position, finding the interpreter’s talk useful and appropriate. Some also notice the doctor’s
multimodal behavior, illustrated in Images 5 and 6, and also visible in line 06, when he uses
the plural ‘us’. Indeed, such moments show that the doctor himself includes the interpreter
in his m eta-comment, verbally and with his body movements and gaze. Also, when the in-
terpreter speaks, the doctor looks at her several times, showing alignment and affiliation to
her line of action (a s in line 22).
Pointing out that the interpreter’s action is the result of a collaboratively built sequence
of interaction consisting of micro details, such as gaze, body movement and lexical choices,
372
Training interpreters in asylum settings
brings trainees to revise their stern judgments toward the interpreter’s intervention,
which they considered as ‘inappropriate’, ‘unorthodox’, ‘inadequate’ and ‘unprofessional’
(comments heard in class). In fact, PS interpreters quite often consider themselves and are
considered by others, to be ‘translating machines’ whose task is translating while being as
invisible as possible. Interestingly, in PSI, interpreters’ previous contextual knowledge and
(cultural) experience are actually evoked in the formal ‘Competency framework’ for health-
care linguistic interpreting published by Haute Autorité de la Santé in 2017: the interpreter
‘a nalyzes situations and knows how to discern the origins of dysfunctions or blockages in
the exchange that arise from interpretation (lexical void and blind spots), taboo words, and
stereotypes’ (our translation from French).
The case above clearly illustrates the interpreter’s understanding of both the doctor’s
difficulties in carrying on the consultation and the challenges of the visit, in which the fu-
ture of the patient is at stake. This generates a tension between professional discretion and
interpreting practice, visible in the interpreter’s effort to make the interaction progress when
difficulties clearly emerge. Her choices here turn out to be successful, favoring the resolution
of the ongoing communicative troubles.
373
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
3. ‘Dizzy’
There are several elements of the interpreter’s translation (such as, the mention in line 10
of her own question, which is not a translation, or the insertion of the minimizer a little
bit, line 09) that we cannot address here. Instead, we focus on the metalinguistic comment
in lines 14–15. From a normative point of view, this comment is critical. It is not a trans-
lation of the patient’s talk, but the interpreter’s comment on it, which emphasizes a word
choice by pointing it out as important, unusual or surprising (P iccoli and Traverso 2020).
Here the interpreter reveals her own interpretation and subjectivity. Also, it momentarily
stops the translation process of the patient’s turn (the segment when I think of it it makes
me too too nervous, line 7 is not translated). A collective discussion would probably elicit
further remarks about what this (t ype of ) comment does in a negative sense. Conversely,
it might also shed light on its utility in the context of psychotherapeutic consultations by
calling the psychologist’s attention to the patient’s word choice at specific moments of the
interaction. In this respect, Piccoli and Traverso show that these comments appear mainly
during the patient’s evocation of her emotional state. These findings show that meta-
linguistic comments do not reveal weaknesses in the interpreters’ linguistic proficiency.
Rather, they give the psychologist access to nuances of meaning that would otherwise
remain unnoticed (see Piccoli and Traverso 2 020 on the translation and metalinguistic
comments on the Albanese word përjetoj,14 which launches an almost 3 -minute collabo-
rative sequence).
With respect to training for professionals other than interpreters, such considerations can
raise awareness on the usefulness of metalinguistic sequences, thereby encouraging their use
in consultations.
374
Training interpreters in asylum settings
training needs and expectations. If healthcare-oriented content was the priority for caregivers,
interpreters showed interest in receiving training on issues directly linked to the dynamics of
communication, provided that such training was not time-consuming. Taking into account these
criteria of accessibility and time, ODIMEDI15 was designed as a free, concise, simple-to-use and
nomadic self-training tool, accessible via a computer or smartphone without any prior software
installation (Piccoli 2021).
The characteristics we wanted to include posed a number of challenges concerning the
conception of the digital tool. In fact, its design required an important adaptation on our
part, both in terms of content and layout; it had to take into account the constraints re-
lated to our target audiences, our field of research and its specificities, and technical issues
imposed by web distribution. Briefly, we proceeded as follows. We selected the most re-
current and relevant phenomena our analyses foregrounded: convergence, misalignment,
reformulation and the transmission of emotions. Indeed, these issues shed light on com-
plex situations, such as dyadic sequences, telephone interpreting and online administra-
tive procedures conducted during the consultations. In order to make them interesting
for our audience, we formulated these phenomena as assertions or questions responding
to real problems and issues. For example, the module concerning the phenomenon of
‘convergence’ is titled ‘I reproduce the behavior of the one I interpret for’; the unit on the
‘t ransmission of emotions’ is represented by ‘I almost cried while translating her story’; and
the segment on ‘online administrative procedures’ by ‘I try to do the online paperwork
without neglecting the user’.
Each of these phenomena constitutes a chapter, organized as follows: (1) a brief descrip-
tion of the phenomenon; (2) two or three video extracts analyzed from the point of view of
Figure 22.5 O
DIMEDI application
375
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
376
Training interpreters in asylum settings
Figure 22.8 Video 1 chapter ‘Convergence’. Analysis of the interpreter’s point of view
377
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
each participant (caregivers, patients and interpreters) and (3) some final recommendations as
to how to reinvest the results of the proposed analyses into practice.
Some images of the interface are presented above.
The downside of such a tool is that, given the absence of trainers, a simplification of the
contents was necessary. All the material, from the analysis to the recommendations, had to
be phrased in a less specific and less technical way. In order to meet this need, we collabo-
rated with a non-expert external consultant so as to avoid jargon and make the content ac-
cessible to a large audience. To this end, a glossary was added. Similarly, the texts had to be
succinct in order to adapt to the web format, and each chapter quite short, lasting between
15 and 20 minutes, in line with the users’ time constraints.
To sum up, our main goal in such training activities and modalities was to develop an
understanding of how interaction works in practice. By building a critical perspective, we
wished to offer trainees the opportunity to deconstruct a normative approach to their pro-
fessional practice and engage in a more conscious experience of PSI.
Discussion
In this chapter, we have described how we offer our trainees – interpreters and care
providers – ways to understand how and when participants in i nterpreter-mediated asylum
related encounters may be led to behave in ways that are perceived as unorthodox, occa-
sionally breaking the normative and deontological rules of their respective professional
practice. In so doing, we wish to problematize the functioning of communication, bring-
ing to the fore micro details that may be unnoticed in situations, but that play a major
role in meaning-building and interactional organization. Indeed, trainees underlined their
interest in watching real-life data that illustrated situations they come across themselves
in their professional practice. Observing the multimodal details of meaning negotiations,
disagreements and misunderstandings, and how participants get involved in them, pro-
vides them an opportunity to examine their own practice from a different perspective and
reflect on it in new ways.
Several scholars focus their research on exploring, together with the social actors involved,
the social dynamics of professional practice on the basis of v ideo-recordings. From this per-
spective, interaction is conceived as both a research object and a training tool (Filliettaz,
Vinatier, and Laforest 2018). Such an approach enables the identification and collective as-
sessment of the savoir-faire displayed in actual work. Relying on a close collaboration between
researchers and professional actors allows them to share their knowledge and expertise in an
original and constructive way (see among others, Filliettaz 2013; Filliettaz, Vinatier, and
Morrissette 2020; Ticca, Lambert and Traverso 2020).
We have also discussed the use of video data and the ways in which it may challenge the
interactional approach to analyses that we promote in our training sessions by favoring the
unfolding of a cross-cultural perspective. Of course, this does not mean that cultural or
378
Training interpreters in asylum settings
gender-related issues play no role in interpreting practice. As we have seen earlier, these are
among the issues that are immediately perceived by the trainees – perhaps because they were
at the center of prior training or because they are more common questions and are, therefore,
more immediately visible. Nonetheless, our goal is to highlight more fine-grained – and less
evidently culture-related – interactional phenomena, long-neglected despite their crucial role
in meaning making.
Further reading
Antaki, Charles (ed) (2011) Applied Conversation Analysis: Changing Institutional Practices. Basingstoke,
England, Palgrave Macmillan.
The work reported in this collection shows how Conversation Analyses, by exploring naturally occurring
data, can be used to identify, and improve, communicative practices at work.
Goodwin, Charles (1997) “The blackness of black”, in Discourse, Tools and Reasoning. Essays on Situated
Cognition, Lauren B. Resnick, Roger Säljö, Clotilde Pontecorvo, and Barbara Burge (eds), New
York, Springer Verlag: 111–40.
This paper develops the idea of ‘situated activity system’ illustrated by several enlightening examples show-
ing how participants in interaction weave together the different semiotic resources they resort to in order to
accomplish their social actions.
Les Cahiers de Rhizome 75–76, No1 (Mental Health and Precariousness), January 2020. URL: https://
www.c airn- i nt.info/j ournal- r hizome.htm#xd_co_f=MTQ2MGY4YWMtMjVmZi00Yjlm
LTg2Y2ItM2ViODAwMzM3OGI1~
This issue of the Cahiers de Rhizome journal offers an overview of how interpreting is considered by inter-
preters as well as medical practitioners in a various range of medical contexts.
Related chapters
Chapter 3, Agency in and for mediating in public service interpreting by Claudio Baraldi
Chapter 9, Research on interpreter-mediated asylum interviews by Sonja Pöllabauer
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini,
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe
Chapter 16, ‘Interpreter’s mistake’ Why should other professions care about the professionalization of interpreters?
by Hanne Skaaden
Notes
1 The project description and its realizations, funded by the French National Research Agency,
2016–20, can be found here: http://w ww.icar.cnrs.fr/sites/projet-remilas/.
2 The length of consultations varies depending on the setting in which they occurred (psychiatric
and general health care, medical assessment, etc.), that is why providing any quantitative informa-
tion would not contribute to describing our data.
3 Cour National du Droit d’Asile.
4 Office Français de Protection des Réfugiés et Apatrides.
5 It should be noted that PSI in France is provided by private associations that can operate in public
as well as in private contexts.
6 Interestingly, in order to become a sworn interpreter in France one needs to be appointed by the
Courts of appeal, and no specific educational level or curriculum is required. The less the language
requested is spoken, the easier the interpreter will get an assignment.
7 See for instance the training courses financed by the Health Regional Agency (A RS) of the
R hône-A lpes district and organized by the O rspere-Samdarra (http://w ww.ch-le-v inatier.
fr/orspere- s amdarra/ressource/formations/l-i nterpretariat- en- s ante-formations- a- destination-
des-i nterpretes-et-des-soignants/session-de-formation-2020-2651.html), the Inter-University
379
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
Diploma (DIU) organized by INALCO and financed by the French National Research Agency
research project LIMINAL (Hospitalité, mediations, migrations), the new University Diploma im-
plemented by the Orspere-Samdarra, ICAR research lab and the LabEx ASLAN, and supported
by the University Lyon 2 (Mediation, interprétariat, migration). Other University Diplomas (DU)
are also held at Paris-Diderot University (Médiateur-Interprète dans les services publics, jointly orga-
nized with a national interpreting service, Interservice migrant), and at Paris-Descartes University
(P ratiques de médiation et de traduction en situation transculturelle).
8 A number of non- profit interpreting associations has signed in a document ( the “Code of
medical and social professional interpreting in France”, 2012), wishing to improve equal ac-
cess to social and legal rights by guaranteeing the principles of fidelity, confidentiality and im-
partiality (italics is ours). https://w ww.unaf.fr/I MG/pdf/charte- signee- scan19-12-2012.
pdf#xd_co_f=YzBlYmViMmMtY2IyNi00ZDI1LWE4OTYtNzQzODczNDhkZDRk~
9 The images we selected to illustrate the situation are taken from a m ulti-view recording (V1 and
V2), which allows us to grasp more globally the participants’ visual activity.
10 For the purposes of this paper, we opted for a simplified transcription, in which we omitted the
duration of pauses or prosodic features among others.
11 Translation in English of talk originally produced in Albanian and French is provided in italics.
12 An analysis of this extract can also be found, in French, in Ticca 2020b; Ricet and Noûs 2022.
13 This is what the interpreter says just a few moments before the extract we present here: “elle
mélange le he and le she donc ça c’est classique, ça c’est classique”, tr. “she mixes up the he and she
so it’s a classic, it’s a classic”, data not showed.
14 The word përjetoj is composed of the verb jetoj, to live, and the prefixe për, which the interpreter
first rendered by “lived”, and then extensively commented on it as difficult to translate.
15 Project financed by the Technology Transfer Acceleration Company PULSALYS.
References
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( eds) (2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Barsky, Robert F. (1996) “The interpreter as intercultural agent in convention refugee hearings”, The
Translator 1 (2): 45–64.
Bergunde, Annika, and Sonja Pöllabauer (2019) “Curricular design and implementation of a training
course for interpreters in an asylum context”, The International Journal for Translation & Interpreting
Research 11 (1): 1–21.
Bourdieu, Pierre (1997) Méditations pascaliennes. Paris, Seuil.
Chambon, Nicolas, Patricia Lambert, Anna Claudia Ticca, and Véronique Traverso (2021) “L e patient,
le médecin et l’interprète dans les consultations médicales d’expertise pour la demande d’asile”, in
Discours, langues, migrations, Laurence Le Ferrec and Marie Veniard (eds). Limoges, Lambert-Lucas:
139–63.
Chambon, Nicolas, Halima Z eroug-Vial, and Natacha Carbonel (2017) “L’interprétariat en santé
mentale: les enjeux de la formation”, L’Autre 18 (3): 362–68.
Clappe, Maureen (2019) “Interpréter les récits de demande d’asile: une perspective légitimiste et mil-
itante du statut de réfugié”, Lien social et Politiques 83: 167–83.
Collective authors (2020) “L’interprétariat à la Cour nationale du droit d’asile”, Plein droit 124 (1):
23–28.
Davidson, Brad (2000) “The interpreter as institutional gatekeeper: The social-linguistic role of inter-
preters in Spanish-English medical discourse”, Journal of Sociolinguistics 4: 379–405.
De Fina, Anna (2003) Identity in Narrative. A Study of Immigrant Discourse. Oxford, John Benjamins
Publishing Company.
Delahaie, Juliette, and Emmanuelle Canut (2020) “Les entretiens d’évaluation de minorité pour les jeunes
migrants: le rôle de l’évaluateur dans la construction du récit de vie”, Migrations Société 181 (3): 39–52.
Duque, Ada Luz (2020) “Quelles médiations dans les pratiques d’interprétariat?”, Rhizome 75–76 (1):
110–19.
Fassin, Didier, and Estelle d’Halluin (2005) “The truth in the body. Medical certificates as ultimate
evidence for a sylum-seekers”, American Anthropologist 107 (4): 5 97–608.
380
Training interpreters in asylum settings
Fassin, Didier (2012) “How asylum claims are adjudicated. The institution as a moral agent”, Revue
Française de Sociologie (special issue Moral Economies and Social Justice) 53 (4): 4 44–72.
Filliettaz, Laurent (2013) “A ffording learning environments in workplace contexts: An interactional
and multimodal perspective”, International Journal of Lifelong Education 32 (1): 107–22.
Filliettaz, Laurent, and Marianne Zogmal (2017) “Sinon vous allez pas voir »: La mise en place d’une
activité de lecture d’album en institution de la petite enfance”, Forumlecture.ch 1: 1–19.
Filliettaz, Laurent, Isabelle Vinatier, and Marty Laforest (2 018) “L’interaction comme objet de re-
cherche”, in L’analyse des interactions dans le travail. Outil de formation professionnelle et de recherche,
Isabelle Vinatier, Laurent Filliettaz and Marty Laforest (eds). Dijon, Editions Raison et Passion:
2 35–55.
Filliettaz, Laurent, Isabelle Vinatier, and Joëlle Morrissette (2020) “Introduction. De l’observation à
l’intervention: les usages de l’analyse interactionnelle en formation”, Phronesis 9 (2): 1–9.
Good, Anthony (2007) Anthropology and Expertise in the Asylum Courts. New York, Routledge.
Goodwin, Charles (1981) Conversational Organization: Interaction between Speakers and Hearers. New
York, Academic Press.
Goodwin, Charles, and Marjorie Goodwin Harness (2005) “Participation”, in A Companion to Linguis-
tic Anthropology, Alessandro Duranti (ed). Oxford, Basil Blackwell Publishing: 222–44.
Gumperz, John. J. (1979) “Crosstalk”, Communication Documentary Series, Multi-racial Britain,
BBC.
Haute Autorité de la Santé (2017) Référentiel de compétences, formation et bonnes pratiques. www.has-sante.
fr.
Heritage, John (2009) “Questioning in medicine”, in ‘W hy Do You Ask?’: The Function of Questions in
Institutional Discourse, Alice F. Freed and Susan Ehrlich (eds). New York, Oxford University Press:
4 2–68.
Heritage, John, and Jeffrey D. Robinson (2006) “The structure of patients’ presenting concerns: Phy-
sicians’ opening questions”, Health Communication 19 (2): 8 9–102.
Inghilleri, Moira (2005) “Mediating zones of uncertainty: Interpreter agency, the interpreting habitus,
and political asylum adjudication”, The Translator 11 (1): 69–85.
Jacquemet, Marc (2005) “The registration interview: Restricting refugees’ narrative performances”,
in Dislocations/Relocations: Narratives of Displacement, Mike Baynham, and Anna de Fina (eds). Man-
chester, St. Jerome publishing: 197–218.
——— (2009) “Transcribing refugees: The entextualization of asylum seekers’ hearings in a transidi-
omatic environment”, Text & Talk 29 (5): 525–46.
——— (2011) “Crosstalk 2.0: Asylum and communicative breakdowns”, Text & Talk 31 (4): 475–97.
——— (2016) “Sociolinguistic superdiversity and asylum”, Tilburg Papers in Culture Studies 171.
Leplat, Jacques, and Jean-Michel Hoc (1983) “Tâche et activité dans l’analyse psychologique des situ-
ations”, Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 3: 49–63.
Licoppe, Christian, and Maud Verdier (2013) “Interpreting, video communication and the sequential
reshaping of institutional talk in the bilingual and distributed courtroom”, The International Journal
of Speech, Language and the Law 20 (2): 247–76.
——— (2015) “L’interprétariat par visioconférence au sein des chambres de l’instruction en France:
une étude conversationnelle de l’activité d’interprétariat dans un dispositif interactionnel médi-
atisé”, Langage et Société 153: 113–35.
Licoppe, Christian, and Clair-Antoine Veyrier (2020) “The interpreter as a sequential coordinator in
courtroom interaction”, Interpreting 22 (1): 56–86.
Mason, Ian (2000) “Models and methods in dialogue interpreting research”, in Intercultural Faultlines.
Maeve Olohan (ed). Manchester, St. Jerome: 215–31.
Niemants, Natacha (2022), “‘Plus tu racontes, plus on te croit’, ou de l’importance de (bien) répondre
aux questions du juge”, in Raconter, relater, traduire: paroles de la migration, Véronique Traverso and
Nicolas Chambon (eds). Limoges, Lambert Lucas: 27–49
Nossik, Sandra (2018) “Des Dublins aux d ubliné-e-s: Extension de la violence administrative”, in
Penser les mots, dire la migration, Laura Calabrese and Marie Veniard (eds). Louvain, Academia:
81–90.
Peräkylä, Anssi, Charles Antaki, Sanna Vehviläinen and Ivan Leudarin (eds) (2008) “A nalysing psy-
chotherapy in practice”, in Conversation Analysis and Psychotherapy, Anssi Peräkylä, Charles Antaki,
Ivan Leudar, and Sanna Vehviläinen (eds). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 5 –25.
381
Anna Claudia Ticca et al.
Piccoli, Vanessa (2021) “ODIMEDI: un outil numérique pour la formation des interprètes et des
intervenants en santé”, Bulletin de Linguistique et des sciences du langage de l’Université de Lausanne 31:
107–23.
Piccoli, Vanessa, Anna Claudia Ticca, and Véronique Traverso (2019) “’Go on the internet, it’s all
there’: Administrative procedures of people in unstable circumstances and asylum seekers”, Langue
et société 167 (2): 81–110.
Piccoli, Vanessa, and Véronique Traverso (2020) “Quels mots pour dire les mots de l’autre? Les
désignations d’émotions et leur traduction dans les interactions en santé mentale”, Cahiers de Rhi-
zome 71, URL: http://w ww.ch-le-v inatier.fr/documents/Publications/R HIZOME_Orspere-
Samdarra/A rticles_PDF/R HI75_8-Piccoli.pdf.
Pöllabauer, Sonja (2004) “Interpreting in asylum hearings: Issues of role, responsibility and power”,
Interpreting 6 (2): 143–80.
Ricet, Vendredi, and Camille Noûs (2022) “Expertise médicale et réélaboration du récit d’asile: in-
tertextualité et pratiques en interaction”, in Raconter, relater, traduire: paroles de la migration, Nicolas
Chambon and Véronique Traverso (eds). Limoges, Lambert Lucas: 67–93
Saglio-Yatzimirsky, M arie-Caroline, and Alexandra Galitzine-L oumpet (2020) “Traduire l’exil”,
Plein Droit 124. URL: http://w ww.gisti.org/spip.php?article6318.
Ticca, Anna Claudia (2018) “The interpreter’s activity between complexity and simplification in psy-
chotherapy sessions”, in TRAlinea (Special Issue: Translation and Interpreting for Language Learners).
URL: http://w ww.intralinea.org/a rchi ve/a rticle/2312.
——— (2020a) “‘So just please tell him the truth’: Le tiers/pair-interprète dans la résolution des
problèmes de communication”, Projet TIASAMN, Hypotheses.org, 30 Septembre. URL: https://
tiasamn.hypotheses.org/283.
——— (2020b) “R isques et atouts de l’approche interactionniste dans la formation sur l’interprétation
en milieu social”, Bulletin de Linguistique et des sciences du langage de l’Université de Lausanne 31: 61–75.
——— (2022) “L e récit des violences dans les consultations d’expertise médicale pour la demande
d’asile: corps, texte, mémoire”, in Raconter, relater, traduire: paroles de la migration, Nicolas Chambon
and Véronique Traverso (eds). Limoges, Lambert Lucas: 93–123
Ticca, Anna Claudia, Patricia Lambert, and Véronique Traverso (2020) “L e récit de la migration en
santé avec des personnes demandeuses d’asile en France. Réflexions sur la formation des soignants
et des interprètes”, Phronesis 9 (2): 77–92.
Ticca, Anna Claudia, and Véronique Traverso (2017) “Parole, voix et corps: lorsque l’interprète et le
soignant s’alignent dans les consultations avec migrants”, L’autre – clinique, culture et société 18 (3):
304–14.
Traverso, Véronique (2019) “Forms of participation in a mental health care consultation with a non-
present interpreter”, Research on Language and Social Interaction 52 (2): 124–43.
UNHCR (2010) Improving Asylum Procedures. Comparative Analysis and Recommendations for Law and Practice.
URL: http://w ww.unhcr.org/protection/operations/4ba9d99d9/improving-a sylumprocedures-
comparative-analysis-recommendations-law-practice.html (accessed 10 May 2021).
UNHCR Austria, ed. (2017). Handbook for Interpreters in Asylum Procedures. Vienna, UNHCR Austria.
van Dijk, Teun A. (2007) “Comments on context and conversation”, in Discourse and Contemporary
Social Change, Norman Fairclough, Giuseppina Cortese, and Patrizia Ardizzone (eds). Bern, Peter
Lang: 290–95.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
382
23
INTERPROFESSIONAL
EDUCATION … INTERPRETER
EDUCATION, IN OR AND
Taking stock and moving forward
Demi Krystallidou
Introduction
Interprofessional education (IPE) is typically associated with education in health sciences
and social care, but many care pathways (e.g. mental health) involve professionals from other
sectors, such as education or police. Consequently, IPE has become relevant for professions
outside of health sciences and social care. Over the past years, IPE has attracted significant
interest in the field of interpreting, too. This is due to the increasing linguistic diversity:
service providers and beneficiaries do not always share a common language and need to rely
on language support including interpreters. Although interpreters might have been exposed
to the working practices of other professionals at some point in their training, it is only in the
last few years that the concept of IPE has become known in the field of interpreting.
Unlike in interpreting, IPE in healthcare dates back to the 1960s (Thistlethwaite 2012)
and was reinforced in the 70s and 80s through two World Health Organisation (W HO)
reports (W HO 1973, 1988). In 2010, two influential publications highlighted the need for
IPE further: the WHO Framework for action on interprofessional education and collaborative practice
(W HO 2010) and Health professionals for a new century: transforming education to strengthen health
systems in an interdependent world, which was released by the independent Lancet Commission,
a globally constituted group of 20 professional and academic leaders (Frenk et al. 2010). The
commission highlighted ‘a slow-burning crisis’ arising from the ‘m ismatch of professional
competencies to patient and population priorities because of fragmentary, outdated and static
curricula producing ill-equipped graduates’ who are not always able to respond to the new
challenges healthcare systems are faced with (Frenk et al. 2010). In the last two publications,
IPE is portrayed as an optimal way to improve the education of healthcare professionals by
providing them with the attitudes, knowledge and skills needed to work effectively together,
become ‘collaborative practice-ready’ (W HO 2010) and be better prepared to respond to
local health needs. In other words, the need for IPE was born from the mismatch between
the complexity and multifaceted nature of patients’ health and social care needs, and the
outdated and static educational curricula that failed to deliver healthcare professionals who
could address these needs in a coordinated and effective manner.
a teaching and learning process that fosters collaborative work and improves quality of
care between two or more professions. IPE occurs when members or students of two or
more professions learn with, from and about each other to improve collaboration and
the quality of care and services (CAIPE 2016).
In healthcare, the overarching interprofessional goal that students following IPE strive
for is the provision of optimal and personalised care that is tailored to the patient’s needs.
Within that overarching goal, students strive to reach intermediate goals that are specific
to healthcare conditions that are portrayed in the intervention (e.g. medical and nursing
students learning to understand each other’s professional roles by collaborating with each
other while negotiating a care plan for e nd-of-life patients). IPE comes in many shapes and
formats and its specific scope is defined by its developers while taking the local context into
account.
Typically, IPE provides the conditions for students to acquire a hands-on approach with
the facilitators of the sessions maintaining a discrete presence and empowering students to
actively engage in problem solving which reflects professional practice. It should be clarified
that activities that involve learners from different professions merely sharing lectures without
however learning about, from and with each other by interacting with each other, do not fall
within the scope of IPE, as shared listening alone does not lead to interprofessional learning
(see e.g. Hammick et al. 2007). IPE should not be seen as a replacement for uniprofessional
education.
384
Interprofessional education
385
Demi Krystallidou
due to incongruent role expectations with a team approach to the patient’s treatment, health
and care (e.g. students from health sciences might expect interpreting students to work
with them as a team and advise them on sociocultural aspects pertaining to the patient, an
expectation which might be left unmet by interpreting students who might feel that this
would exceed their remit and will therefore limit their contributions to the linguistic ne-
gotiation of meaning as an act of translation). This can be partly explained by the fact that
interpreter education has traditionally been interpreter-centred (K rystallidou 2014) and has
been primarily preoccupied with familiarising student interpreters with genres and speech
types, memory training and note-taking, vocabulary and technical jargon, accuracy, com-
pleteness, naturalness and closeness in meaning between the source and target text, fluency,
presentation skills, ethics and professional conduct. What seems to have been overlooked
in interpreter education is the students’ ability to communicate with other professionals while
working with them as a team to the same end. This is often in stark contrast to the codes
of conduct professional interpreters are requested to abide by which often seem to promote
the idea of the impartial and uninvolved interpreter (see e.g. the code of conduct for public
service interpreters in Flanders, Belgium). The notion of non-involvement can be easily mis-
understood by (especially novice) interpreters who may feel that n on-involvement extends
to their interaction with the other professional(s) across the board.
It is the duality of the negotiation of meaning at the level of translation and interprofes-
sional communication, as well as the clash with uniprofessionally conceived codes of conduct
that might create a sense of uncertainty for the participating students and would therefore
require advance support by trained IPE facilitators who can navigate and model interprofes-
sional interaction and interprofessional communication.
A review of IPE interventions, developed by health sciences scholars and including health-
care professionals and interpreters as the professions at stake, reports on healthcare students
working with trained/professional interpreters (see e.g. Bridges et al. 2011; Bansal, Swann
and Smithson 2014; Strong et al. 2014; McElfish et al. 2018), student interpreters (e.g. Quick
et al. 2019; Woll et al. 2020) or even bilingual students in health sciences who are assigned
the role of interpreter (e.g. Pechak, Summers and Velasco 2018). There have been calls in
the literature for (i) real-life professional practice in interpreter education (Crezee and Grant
2013; Crezee 2015; Krystallidou et al. 2018a), (ii) training interpreters with other profes-
sionals together (Malek 2004; Perez and Wilson 2007; Raval 2007; Cambridge, Singh and
Johnson 2012; Balogh and Salaets 2015), (i ii) involving professionals from other disciplines in
the training of interpreters (Perez and Wilson 2007; Balogh and Salaets 2015), and (iv) famil-
iarising interpreting students with the communicative strategies used by other professionals
(K rystallidou 2014; Tebble 2014). However, when reviewing studies authored by interpreter
educators and scholars in Interpreting Studies that report on training student interpreters
with students from other fields and disciplines, the limited number of studies shows that IPE
in interpreter education at present (in 2021) is still in its infancy. Anecdotal evidence and
personal communication with interpreter educators suggest, however, that interpreting stu-
dents are now more likely than ever before to be exposed to learning activities that involve
students from other fields and disciplines. Yet, such initiatives remain often undocumented
and no reports are available for perusal and systematic investigation by other scholars and
educators in the field. Although undocumented interventions might have direct benefits for
the actors involved in the local context of the intervention (e.g. students, faculty, institution),
the contribution of these interventions to knowledge exchange with interested stakeholders
outside of the local context remains limited, preventing them at the same time from contrib-
uting directly to the advancement of IPE development in public service interpreting, as they
386
Interprofessional education
do not provide us with the opportunity to benefit from the knowledge generated from them.
More importantly, they do not allow us to gain insights into what Freeth and Reeves (2004)
call the presage (influences and constraints on the design and delivery of IPE), process (the
delivery of IPE) or product of the intervention (interprofessional outcomes of the IPE, such
as attitudes, knowledge, skills, collaborative practice and impact on client care). Therefore,
it is of paramount importance to report on and disseminate findings of IPE interventions
and their evaluations, facilitating in this way the exchange of knowledge and evidence and
promoting a collaborative IPE community within the field of Interpreting Studies.
Learning objectives
While most of the studies authored by Interpreting Studies scholars do refer to the explicit
interaction of interpreting students with students from other fields/d isciplines, surprisingly,
only a few of them mention whether the intervention addressed explicit learning objectives
that would help achieve interprofessional learning outcomes (one of the key characteristics
of IPE interventions, as discussed above). Most of the studies refer to overarching learning
outcomes, such as to increase the knowledge, skills, practice and confidence of students in
working effectively with each other, familiarise participants in training with each other’s
professional roles, expertise, institutional constraints and deontological codes of conduct.
Below I will briefly discuss some key aspects as presented in a number of selected studies that
were initiated by Interpreting Studies scholars and which – at the time of w riting – provide
more comprehensive accounts on I PE-related issues than other studies in the field of inter-
preting which refer to IPE aspects only in passing.
In their recent study, Hlavac and Harrison (2021: 576) helpfully clarify that their IPE
sessions were
structured in a way to enable the following learner outcomes: increased familiarity with
the other professional group and how they work; an augmented conceptualisation of
their own professional practice through interaction with and feedback from another
professional group; understanding of the function of a pre-and a post-interactional
exchange between doctor and interpreter; awareness of how to work not only with the
other professional, but also the patient speaking a language other than English (LOTE)
with limited English proficiency.
Crezee and Marianacci (2021: 6) provide insights into the reflections of 22 students on ‘t wo
shared interprofessional role-play scenarios that student interpreters made in their reflective
written assignments’. The authors state that their
aim was for [the interpreting] students to critically reflect on their role in the [speech
language therapy] SLT setting, since interpreters in this setting tend to enter into a
387
Demi Krystallidou
therapeutic alliance with the SLT, for instance by providing metalinguistic commentary
of the client’s speech.
Later on, the authors clarify that the students were asked to ‘critically reflect on their shared
learning experiences in light of the interpreter code of ethics’.
Krystallidou et al. (2018a) report on IPE between 35 interpreting students and 256 third-
and 238 fourth-year medical students. The overarching interprofessional outcome of the
intervention was ‘to familiarize each of the two student groups with the communicative and
interactional practices that the students in the other group are expected to employ in their
professional practice’ (2018: 129). The study states specific learning objectives for interpret-
ing students, clarifying at the same time that the ‘selection of knowledge and skills as mea-
surable objectives was informed by the relevant literature in the fields of Interpreting Studies
and Clinical Communication, as well as by professional practice’ (K rystallidou et al. 2018a:
129). The authors make special reference to the knowledge and skills interpreting students
were envisaged to acquire by working with medical students, namely to become aware of
the different stages of the consultation, of the doctors’ communicative goals, the impact of
structured information, emphasis and empathic communication on the consultation out-
comes. Another learning objective for the interpreting students was to be able to interpret
accurately while maintaining the transparency of the communication and their own impar-
tiality (K rystallidou et al. 2018a: 1 29–30).
388
Interprofessional education
patients (e.g. restore interactional flow), as opposed to skills that were related to self-initiated
actions (e.g. provide complete renditions).
389
Demi Krystallidou
(2021) helpfully situate the students’ reflections in the theory of educational assessment that
distinguishes among assessment for, as and of learning and clarify that the students’ reflections
presented in their study relate to assessment as learning.
Krystallidou et al. (2018a) too situate the development of their IPE intervention within
the six steps to curriculum development for medical education proposed by Kern, Thomas
and Hughes (2009), which is considered a theoretically sound framework for developing,
implementing, evaluating and improving educational experiences in medicine. The six steps
that informed the development of the IPE intervention presented by Krystallidou et al.
(2018a) are the following: (i) problem identification and general needs assessment, (ii) tar-
geted needs assessment, (i ii) goals and objectives, (iv) educational strategies, (v) implementa-
tion and (v i) feedback and evaluation.
Facilitators
In all of the above studies that were discussed in some detail and involve interpreting stu-
dents, there is very little evidence of the role of facilitators. Any information that is provided
is limited to brief descriptions of the tasks performed by the facilitators but no further in-
formation is provided on the facilitators’ own training in relation to the IPE interventions.
In Crezee’s (2015) study, the tasks that were performed by facilitators from the participating
disciplines included listening in, observing, occasionally interjecting with questions and
handing out tests used in professional practice. In the study by Krystallidou et al. (2018a),
facilitators who were trainers in clinical communication skills provided the content of the
consultation, which was further enhanced by facilitators who were interpreter trainers.
Facilitators from both disciplines identified the need for the IPE intervention and partici-
pated in formative assessment following the intervention. In a similar fashion, Crezee and
Marianacci (2021: 2) do state that educators at their institutions ‘have embraced a situated
learning approach to interpreter education, which has involved (…) inter-professional edu-
cation (I PE)’. However, the authors do not provide further information on the facilitators’
IPE skills either.
The following section provides a brief reflection on the available evidence of IPE in
Interpreting Studies as discussed above and suggests a few ways of moving forward. It should
be conceded that the list of recommendations is not exhaustive, and it focuses on aspects
that seem to be common across the board as evidenced in the literature, experienced by
interpreter educators who have developed or participated in IPE interventions (personal
communication) and from my own experience as a c o-developer and facilitator of three IPE
interventions at three universities in Belgium at the time of writing. Other parameters might
merit attention too, depending on the local context.
390
Interprofessional education
could compromise collaborative practice (e.g. they might act in ways that can be to the det-
riment of the goals the primary participants strive for during interaction; see e.g. misalign-
ment between healthcare professionals’ communicative goals and interpreters’ behaviour
in interaction in Hsieh (2 007)). Misalignments of this kind can result in core aspects of
person-centred care, such as empathic communication, being compromised (K rystallidou,
Bylund and Pype 2020). Against this backdrop, it becomes clear that interpreter training
and education would benefit from more collaborative and interprofessional learning pro-
cesses, so that trained interpreters can work collaboratively with other professionals to a
shared goal. It must be conceded that shared goals across disciplines might not be visible
at first glance and it is up to the IPE developers to identify them and design their inter-
ventions accordingly. For example, in a bad news delivery consultation the doctor’s goal
is to inform the patient in an accurate, yet empathic way, and discuss a treatment or care
plan, if applicable, preferably by means of shared decision-making with the patient. The
interpreter’s goal is to ensure that accurate information reaches the patient in an empathic
way while enabling the patient to be involved in the shared decision-making process. The
goals which the two professionals have in common and together should be working to
are accuracy, empathic communication and patient involvement. 3 A uniprofessional take
would be for interpreters to strive for accuracy and completeness while maintaining their
own impartiality and refrain from engaging in interprofessional communication with the
doctor. However, this approach could undermine the doctor’s empathic communicative
practices (e.g. research has shown that interpreters might fail to detect, assess and accu-
rately render patients’ empathic opportunities and doctors’ empathic responses to those,
resulting in empathic communication being compromised; see Krystallidou, Bylund and
Pype (2 020)). It is the interpreter’s responsibility to ensure that they strike a right balance
between the communicative goals they have in common with the other professional (e.g.
healthcare provider), and the patient’s own communicative goals. This requires a specialist
skill set that goes beyond adhering to the principles of accuracy, completeness and im-
partiality that are advocated in many interpreters’ codes of conduct and which, however,
might be misunderstood by (novice) interpreters lacking reflexive understanding in IPE
settings. If we envisage highly skilled interpreters to become an indispensable part of ex-
tended interprofessional healthcare teams in linguistically and culturally diverse healthcare
settings enjoying an equally valuable status as the other members of the healthcare team,
we need to ensure that interpreters receive interprofessional support at all stages of their
professional development. IPE seems to be one of the pathways to ‘collaborative-practice
ready’ (W HO 2010) interpreters, too.
The following subsections provide a brief overview of some key aspects and recommen-
dations that interpreter educators who are considering developing IPE interventions might
find useful.
391
Demi Krystallidou
level?), (iii) availability of resources (e.g. proximity to the venue, availability of learning
spaces and materials), (iv) faculty b uy-in, capacity and development, (v) number and dif-
ficulty of behaviours required by those delivering or receiving the intervention (e.g. are
facilitators required to act in a certain way? Are learners required to have prior knowledge
in order to display certain behaviour during the intervention?). Admittedly, the above
are complex questions that require careful consideration, advance planning and thorough
preparation that can take up considerable amounts of time. Interpreter educators wishing
to develop IPE interventions for their students should address the above questions from
the beginning in partnership with educators from the fields/d isciplines they want their
students to learn about, from and with. In order to do so, IPE developers must have a
good understanding of the curriculum and communicative practices of their own and
other discipline(s), have an open attitude to IPE and be willing to negotiate understand-
ing with the educators from the other discipline(s). This will enable them to identify
shared communicative goals which can inform the learning outcomes and overall process
of the intervention. When it comes to interprofessional outcomes, IPE developers in inter-
preting could draw inspiration from the core (sub)competences for collaborative practice
(Interprofessional Education Collaborative 2016) and tailor (preferably in collaboration
with IPE developers from the involved fields/d isciplines) the envisaged interprofessional
outcomes to the local context.
392
Interprofessional education
Evaluating outcomes
Apart from informing our understanding of the various interactions within the IPE sys-
tem, a theory (or set of theories) also allows us to evaluate the intervention. Once we have
identified the mechanisms that trigger certain interprofessional outcomes within a specific
context under certain circumstances for interpreting students and students from other fields
and disciplines, we will be able to propose solutions that aim to improve the effectiveness of
the IPE intervention. To paraphrase Pawson and Tilley (1997), it is only by understanding
and probing its apparatus of change that one can evaluate an IPE intervention. Ideally, the
evaluation that should be undertaken should consider a realist methodology relying on the
generative theory (i.e., look into (human) action in terms of its location within different
layers of social reality) instead of the successionist theory of causation (i.e., observing only
linear ‘external forces at work’ (Pawson and Tilley 1997: 33)). This would enable evaluators
to determine what (a spects of ) interventions (do not) work for whom, under what circum-
stances and how, and use that knowledge to improve future interventions (on realist evalua-
tion, see Pawson and Tilley’s (1997) seminal work). However, a realist evaluation takes up a
considerable amount of time and requires training in specific realist methodology, resources
and a dedicated (interdisciplinary) team of evaluators. For this reason, IPE developers might
find it useful to measure outcomes at a micro level (e.g. interprofessional communicative
level). In health sciences (education), there are several scales that measure learners’ change in
competences, attitudes and perceptions (e.g. the Interprofessional Collaborative Competency
Attainment Survey (ICCAS), the Readiness for Interprofessional Learning Scale (R IPLS),
the Interdisciplinary Education Perception Scale (IEPS)). However, evaluators wishing to
measure interpreting students’ change in competences, attitudes, perceptions and overall
performance will soon notice that not every item of existing scales can be applied to their
own IPE context and may therefore need to consider alternatives (e.g. draw inspiration from
existing scales, develop and test new ones that are tailored to the local IPE context and their
interpreting students’ needs and report on them for further dissemination). Although many
of the existing tools could be adapted for interpreting students, due care should be taken
when adapting an existing scale or developing a new one and a pilot testing should ideally
be conducted. What is also important to consider is the combination of p re-post evaluations
(a s opposed to only post-evaluations). In this way, evaluators can obtain more clarity on the
outcomes that emerged from the IPE intervention and can make a valid contribution to the
evidence base.
393
Demi Krystallidou
(Wilhelmsson et al. 2009). High levels of commitment and ‘buy-in’ are required from all
participating entities (e.g. schools, departments, faculties) within an institution. On a prac-
tical level, scheduling, timetabling, allocating sufficient time and identifying appropriate
teaching resources (Guraya and Barr 2018) are important. Preparatory work, such as adapt-
ing or translating scenarios into various languages, is required. Ensuring that the sessions
are facilitated by trainers/educators from the concerned disciplines who all are trained in
IPE and together are able to assess interpreting performances in both languages is equally
important. More importantly, ensuring shared understanding with developers and facili-
tators from the concerned disciplines of what IPE is, what the interprofessional goals and
competencies are, where IPE is situated in the respective curricula and what student as-
sessment and IPE evaluation methods will be used is essential. These are only some of the
most common challenges IPE developers working within Interpreting Studies typically are
faced with. Interpreter educators may find useful to identify faculty from the other disci-
pline(s) with an affinity for languages and communication and task them with exploring
opportunities of collaboration at their institution within the framework of an IPE inter-
vention with interpreting students. With regard to teaching resources, the facilitators’ IPE
knowledge and preferably IPE expertise cannot be stressed enough. Facilitators who, for
whatever reasons, do not subscribe to IPE objectives and are resistant to interprofessional
change should ideally not be entrusted with facilitating IPE sessions. After all, as Hammick
and Anderson (2 009: 219) state, introducing IPE means that ‘we need to align language,
learning approaches and curriculum time tables…and arguably the most challenging, we
need to align people’.
Concluding remarks
The development, implementation and evaluation of IPE interventions is a complex and
laborious undertaking, which, however, has the potential to bring about change and deliver
‘collaborative-practice ready’ professionals (W HO 2010). The above discussion has shown
the potential IPE has to revolutionise interpreter education and enhance respect for the
interpreter profession among other professionals, while at the same time acknowledging
the complexity of this learning and teaching process. IPE can be the pathway to delivering
professional interpreters who can function interprofessionally as members of interdisciplin-
ary teams and contribute in this way to more linguistically and culturally inclusive provi-
sion of services. This can be achieved when students immersed in IPE start to develop dual
identities, namely a robust sense of belonging to both own profession and to the interpro-
fessional community in which they see themselves as members of their own profession and
the interprofessional team (K ahlili et al. 2019). However, it should be acknowledged that in
order for the above to occur, consideration in codes of conduct for more collaborative in-
terprofessional practice and, more importantly, shifts in interpreter education towards more
interprofessional attitudes are required.
Further reading
Health Professions Accreditors Collaborative (2019) Guidance on Developing Quality Interprofessional
Education for the Health Professions. Chicago, IL, Health Professions Accreditors Collaborative.
This document provides general consensus-based guidance on the development and implementation of qual-
ity IPE in health sciences.
394
Interprofessional education
Related chapters
Chapter 12, Public service interpreting in health care by Laura Gavioli and Raffaela Merlini
Chapter 13, Challenges and remedies for interpreter-mediated dementia assessments by Charlotta Plejert
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elizabeth Stokoe
Chapter 22, Training interpreters in asylum settings: the REMILAS project by Anna Claudia Ticca, Véro-
nique Traverso and Emilie Jouin
Chapter 24, Training public service providers in how to work with interpreters by Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry
Sagli
Notes
1 The terms that are found in the literature include ‘i nterprofessional training’, ‘workplace training’
(Zhang et al. 2021); ‘skills-based training in working with (students from another discipline)’
(Quick et al. 2019); ‘interactive workshops with students from other professional faculties (…) or
institutions’ (Ozolins 2013); ‘shared pre-professional practice’ (Crezee 2015); ‘joint training ini-
tiative’ (K rystallidou et al. 2018a); ‘ joint training’ (K rystallidou and Salaets 2016).
2 Cognitive interviewing is a psychologically oriented method for empirically studying the ways in
which survey participants mentally process and respond to questionnaires. Cognitive interviewing
is commonly conducted for the purpose of pre-testing questions and determining how they should
be modified to make them more understandable or easier to answer (Lavrakas 2008).
3 For a closer inspection of how student interpreters handled empathic communication in bad news
delivery consultations with medical students within the framework of an IPE intervention at the
University of Antwerp, see Krystallidou et al (2018b). The development of the intervention was
based on a previous IPE intervention that was implemented and evaluated at Ghent University
and described by Krystallidou et al (2018a). However, since the focus of the paper was not on the
IPE intervention as such but on how empathic communication is negotiated and co-constructed
among student interpreters, medical students (a nd simulated patients), the intervention by Krys-
tallidou et al (2018b) is casually described as ‘ joint training’.
References
Balogh, Katalin, and Heidi Salaets (2015) Children and Justice: Overcoming Language Barriers. Cambridge-
A ntwerp-Portland, Intersentia.
Bansal, Aarti, Jennifer Swann, and William Henry Smithson (2014) “Using professional interpreters
in undergraduate medical consultation skills teaching”, Advances in Medical Education and Practice 5:
439–50. https://doi.org/10.2147/A MEP.S71332.
Barr, Hugh, Ivan Koppel, Scott Reeves, Marilyn Hammick, and Della S. Freeth (2008) Effective Inter-
professional Education: Argument, Assumption and Evidence (P romoting Partnership for Health). Oxford,
Blackwell Publishing.
Bridges, Diane, Richard A. Davidson, Peggy Soule Odegard, Ian V. Maki, and John Tomkowiak
(2011) “Interprofessional collaboration: Three best practice models of interprofessional education”,
Medical Education Online 16 (1): 6035. https://doi.org/10.3402/meo.v16i0.6035
395
Demi Krystallidou
396
Interprofessional education
Krystallidou, Demi, Celine Van De Walle, Myriam Deveugele, Evangelia Dougali, Fien Mertens,
Amelie Truwant, Ellen Van Praet, and Peter Pype (2018a) “Training ‘doctor-minded’ interpreters
and ‘interpreter-minded’ doctors: The benefits of collaborative practice in interpreter training”,
Interpreting 20 (1): 126–4 4. https://doi.org/10.1075/i ntp.00005.kry.
Krystallidou, Demi, Aline Remael, Esther de Boe, Kristin Hendrickx, Giannoula Tsakitzidis, Sofie
van de Geuchte, and Peter Pype (2018b) “Investigating empathy in i nterpreter-mediated simulated
consultations: An explorative study”, Patient Education and Counseling 101 (1): 33–42. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.pec.2017.07.022
Krystallidou, Demi and Heidi Salaets ( 2016) “ On interprofessionality in interpreter training: A
few thoughts and two stories”, Dragoman: International Journal of Translation Studies 4 (6). URL:
https://d ragoman-journal.org/w p/w p- content/uploads/0 6-Issue_Volume- 4 -September-2 016.pdf
(accessed 27 December 2021).
Lavrakas, J. Paul (ed) (2008) Encyclopedia of Survey Research Methods. London, Sage Publishing.
Malek, Mhemooda (2004) “Meeting the needs of minority ethnic groups in the UK”, in Mental Health
Services for Minority Ethnic Children and Adolescents, Mhemooda Malek and Carol Joughin (eds). Lon-
don, Jessica Kingsley Publishers: 81–127.
McElfish, Pearl Anna, Ramey Moore, Bill Buron, Jonell Hudson, Christopher R. Long, Rachel S.
Purvis, Thomas K. Schulz, Brett Rowland, and T. Scott Warmack (2018) “Integrating interpro-
fessional education and cultural competency training to address health disparities”, Teaching and
Learning in Medicine 30 (2): 213–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/10401334.2017.1365717.
Medical Research Council (2019) “Developing and evaluating complex interventions”. https://m rc.
ukri.org/documents/pdf/complex-i nterventions-g uidance/ (accessed 27 December 2021).
Ozolins, Uldis (2013) “Role playing ‘Pumpkin’”, in Interpreting in a Changing Landscape: Selected Papers
from Critical Link 6, Christina Schäffner, Krzysztof Kredens and Yvonne Fowler (eds). Amsterdam,
Benjamins: 31– 43.
Pawson, Ray, and Nick Tilley (1997) Realistic Evaluation. London, Sage Publishing.
Pechak, Celia, Connie Summers, and Joanna Velasco (2018) “Improved knowledge following an in-
terprofessional i nterpreter-use training”, Journal of Allied Health 47 (3): 159–68.
Perez, Isabelle and Christine W.L. Wilson (2007) “Interpreter-mediated police interviews: Working
as a professional team”, in The Critical Link 4: Professionalisation of Interpreting in the Community.
Selected Papers from the 4th International Conference on Interpreting in Legal, Health and Social Service
Settings, Cecilia Wadensjö, Birgitta Englund Dimitrova and A nna-Lena Nilson (eds). Amsterdam,
Benjamins Translation Library: 70–79.
Quick, K. Karin, Tehout Selameab, Anne Woll, Cristiano Mazzei, and Jane L. Miller (2019) “Creating
and evaluating skills-based training in working with spoken-language interpreters for oral health
professions students”, Journal of Dental Education 83 (6): 645–53.
Raval, Hitesh (2007) “Therapeutic encounters between young people, bi-lingual co-workers and
practitioners”, in Working with Unaccompanied Asylum Seeking Children: Issues for Policy and Practice,
Ravi K.S. Kohli and Fiona Mitchell (eds). Hampshire/New York, Palgrave Macmillan: 61–76.
Strong, Jenny, Lucinda Chipchase, Shelley Allen, Di Eley, Lindy McAllister, and Bronwyn Davidson
(2014) “Interprofessional learning during an international fieldwork placement”, International Jour-
nal of P ractice-based Learning in Health and Social Care 2 (2): 27–39.
Tebble, Helen (2014) “A genre-based approach to teaching dialogue interpreting: The medical consul-
tation”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 418–36.
Thistlethwaite, Jill (2012) “Interprofessional education: A review of context, learning and the research
agenda”, Medical Education 46 (1): 58–70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2011.04143.x
WHO (1973) Continuing Education for Physicians: Report of a WHO Expert Committee [meeting held in
Geneva from 28 June to 4 July 1973]. Geneva, World Health Organization.
——— (1988) Learning Together to Work Together for Health: Report of a WHO Study Group on Multipro-
fessional Education of Health Personnel: The Team Approach [meeting held in Geneva from 12 to 16 October
1987]. Geneva, World Health Organization.
——— (2010) Framework for Action on Interprofessional Education and Collaborative Practice. Geneva, World
Health Organization.
Wilhelmsson, Margaretha, Staffan Pelling, Johnny Ludvigsson, Mats Hammar, L ars-Owe Dahlgren,
and Tomas Faresjö (2009) “Twenty years experiences of interprofessional education in L inköping–
g round-breaking and sustainable”, Journal of Interprofessional Care 23 (2): 121–33.
397
Demi Krystallidou
Woll, Anne, Karin K. Quick, Cristiano Mazzei, Tehout Selameab, and Jane L. Miller (2020) “Working
with interpreters as a team in health care (W ITH Care) curriculum tool kit for oral health profes-
sions”, MedEdPORTAL 16: 10894.
Zhang, Claire Xiaochi, Emma Crawford, Jeanne Marshall, Anne Bernard, and Katie W alker-Smith
(2021) “Developing interprofessional collaboration between clinicians, interpreters, and translators
in healthcare settings: Outcomes from f ace-to-f ace training”, Journal of Interprofessional Care 35 (4):
521–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/13561820.2020.1786360.
398
24
TRAINING PUBLIC SERVICE
PROVIDERS IN HOW TO
COMMUNICATE VIA
INTERPRETER
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
Introduction
Public service providers around the world, for example, health care personnel, police offi-
cers, teachers, and lawyers, encounter situations in their professional lives in which they need
to communicate with service seekers but are faced with a language barrier. Overcoming
language barriers usually requires engaging an interpreter. Engaging interpreters is an essen-
tial prerequisite for providing equal and fair access to services for all. Inadequate communi-
cation because of poor-quality interpreting can result in a lack of access to services, which
sometimes results in fatal consequences, as abundantly described in the literature ( Jahr 2005;
Kale 2018; see also the Norwegian Official Report on the state-of-the-art public service in-
terpreting profession in Norway published in 2014, henceforth NOU 2014). Therefore, the
quality of interpreting is a key issue.
To ensure the quality of interpreting, there is a general agreement that training inter-
preters is an essential element (Ozolins 2000, 2010; Pöchhacker 2013; Skaaden 2013), and
thus, training interpreters is now a w ell-established field of research and practice (a s Sagli
and Skaaden, this volume; Skaaden, this volume, as well as the other chapters in section 3 of
this volume also show). However, because interpreting is interaction, the training of public
service providers—not only interpreters—is important. Noticeably, the training of service
providers in how to communicate via interpreters has received little attention in the litera-
ture. To address this dearth of knowledge, this chapter will discuss issues of central concern
for the training of public service providers.
When available, service provider training seems to follow a different rationale compared
with interpreter training. It usually consists of a list of recommendations that service provid-
ers are encouraged to follow or a series of impromptu advice about what to do and what not.
Overall, there is little research-based knowledge on how training should be organized and
what topics should be included in training.
Against this background, this chapter discusses the following issues: First, we address the
underlying principles for the training of public service providers. Based on this, we describe
issues that stand out as central to include in the training of public service providers: (a) skills
training for communicating via interpreters, (b) delineating public service providers’ and
interpreters’ areas of responsibility, and (c) the wider socio-political context of the public
service sector and official approach to interpreting. Thereafter, turning to the research liter-
ature, we explore topics that have been discussed in the literature regarding how to prepare
public service providers for service in the multilingual society. Then, in the final section, we
use the example of a Norwegian public service provider training program as a case study to
discuss the organization and content of training (Felberg 2013, 2016). The Norwegian case
is also turned to throughout the chapter to illustrate the issues being discussed.
We believe that the training program in Norway has special relevance for this chapter
for several reasons. One reason is that the training program is different from most available
courses in that it encompasses skills training, discussion about communication participants’
areas of responsibilities, and lectures concerning contextual factors. The training program
also demonstrates how a course can be designed so that it is pedagogically tailored to fit
specific groups of public service providers while still encompassing content that is standard
for all groups.
A characteristic of the Norwegian situation is that the Norwegian government has taken
an active role in the field of public sector interpreting and has developed a policy that in-
cludes university-level interpreter training, a state accreditation exam, testing of bilingual
language skills, a national register of interpreters, and the Interpreting Act, a law specifi-
cally addressing interpreting (NOU 2014: 8; Kunnskapsdepartmentet 2019; Interpreting Act
2021). The government policy also includes training for public service providers on how
to communicate via an interpreter, and this is another reason why we focus on the existing
Norwegian training program.
The Norwegian case may also illustrate the ways in which the policies concerning inter-
preting services reflect and are embedded in wider socio-political contexts. The Norwegian
government’s initiatives concerning interpreting in the public sector are in line with the
engaged role of the state in the Nordic welfare state model. This model implies that the state
is responsible for providing universally available and accessible services for all citizens in
domains such as healthcare, jurisprudence, and education (Christiansen et al. 2006; Pedersen
and Kuhnle 2017). Access to services, therefore, is a given r ight—at least in principle—and
does not depend on charity or aid. This is also valid for interpreting in the public sector,
which the Norwegian government defines as ‘a question relating to the right to due process
of law and equal treatment’ (NOU 2014: title page).
400
Training public service providers
are further discussed later in this chapter, with special reference to the specific Norwegian
training course on how to communicate via an interpreter (Felberg 2013, 2016).
401
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
indication of the situation, increase awareness about the use of interpreters and to obtain an
informed basis on which to implement measures’ (I MDi 2011b: 8).
Public service providers’ work with interpreters also requires familiarity with the inter-
preter’s defined area of responsibility (Felberg 2016; Hlavec and Harrison 2021). In principle,
the trained interpreter’s area of responsibility is defined by the code of ethics they adhere
to because ethical guidelines are essential to the training and education of interpreters ev-
erywhere (Pöchhacker 2016). Ethical guidelines have several core elements that are similar
across different countries (for an overview, see Viezzi, 2020). These elements include an em-
phasis on accuracy, neutrality, impartiality, and confidentiality. We will return to the role of
the interpreters’ ethical guidelines in the training of service providers below.
The range of tasks and duties expected of the interpreter depends on how interpreting
is perceived by those who organize interpreting services, those who educate and certify
interpreters, and those who practice interpreting, with or without being certified and/or ed-
ucated for the task. The competing perceptions range from the interpreter being understood
as a ‘mere conduit’ or tool without agency, via the interpreter being seen as someone assisting
two parties (Skaaden 2013), to the interpreter being understood as an advocate for one of the
participating parties, be it the institutional party or the minority language speaker (Brisset,
Leanza, and Laforest 2013; Pöchhacker 2016).
The problem of interpreters’ ethics and delineation—or their areas of responsibility—is
complex (see Valero Garces, this volume and Skaaden, this volume for a discussion). How-
ever, what is most important here is not necessarily the debate concerning what tasks and
duties interpreters should take on when working with public service providers, but rather
the fact that diverse views on this coexist in praxis (K limidis and Minas 2009: 363). If public
service providers are not aware of the variety of expectations on interpreting that exist, they
may not respond relevantly in practice and, hence, are facing the risk of unnecessary misun-
derstandings in communication (for example, Nilsen 2005; Kale 2018).
In short, clarification regarding the participants’ different ideas of their respective areas of
responsibility stands out as an issue that deserves attention in the training of public service
providers.
402
Training public service providers
the official policy, if any, regarding the provision and organization of interpreting services in
the country or institution targeted, to be a part of service provider training.
As Ozolins (2000: 5) points out, the variation found worldwide in models of how public
services are provided is also reflected in the large differences between interpreting arrange-
ments. Observing how different governments around the world have responded to multi-
lingual challenges, including interpreting needs, Ozolins (2000, 2010) outlines a model of
governmental responses, including those of neglect, ad hoc measures, and the provision of
generic language services based on a comprehensive approach. Here, the comprehensive ap-
proach implies arrangements for interpreter training, accreditation, and registration (Ozolins
2010). The Norwegian case may again serve as an example. The Norwegian government
has adopted a comprehensive approach toward interpreting, which also defines the duties of
public service providers regarding communication over language barriers (NOU 2014). The
policy documents and the new Interpreting Act (Interpreting Act 2021) clearly place the
responsibility for communication with clients, patients, and so forth, on the public service
provider, not on the minority language–speaking individual alone. It is the responsibility of
the public service provider to engage an interpreter when necessary. This also implies that it
is the public institution that must pay for the interpreting services in public service settings.
The range of institutional responsibilities has recently been extended by the Interpreting
Act, which states that public service providers should only hire interpreters who are qualified.
In this context, qualified is defined as being registered in the Norwegian National Register of
Interpreters (Kunnskapsdepartementet 2019; Interpreting Act 2021).
To some extent, the good intentions of the Norwegian system are countered by the
unregulated market. There is no law preventing interpreting service bureaus from deliv-
ering services offered by persons who are unqualified and not registered in the Norwegian
National Register of Interpreters. Statistics from 2019 show that those interpreters listed in
the National Register were used in only 40% of public service assignments (IMDi 2020),
despite the fact that qualified interpreters have reported that they have spare capacity (43% of
their capacity was not utilized) (IMDi 2018b: 3). More than 70% of assignments organized
by private bureaus used unqualified/untrained persons (IMDi 2020). This means that de-
spite the government’s good intentions and policies, public service providers are still likely
to work with untrained and unqualified interpreters, a situation that is useful for public
providers to be aware of.
Today, it is possible for public service providers in Norway to check in the National Reg-
ister whether interpreters are qualified or not. However, countries vary when it comes to
interpreter education as well as national registers, hence possibilities to distinguish between
qualified and unqualified before assigning an interpreter are not always available to public
service providers.
Literature overview
The topic of training public service providers to work successfully with interpreters has so
far received sporadic and fragmented attention, with valid calls to investigate this topic being
expressed for at least the past two decades (Corsellis 2000, 2008; Pöchhacker 2016; Felberg
and Sagli 2019; Pokorn, Viezzi and Felberg 2020). The literature spreads across different
fields, including the healthcare and justice sectors (for example, Avidicus 2008–13; Corsellis
2008; Hsieh, Pitaloka and Johnson 2013; Hsieh 2016; Li et al. 2017; Kale 2018; Krystallidou
et al. 2018; Cox and Li 2019), consequently, it is difficult to obtain a comprehensive over-
view of the subject. Despite this, some recurring research topics can be outlined, such as the
403
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
justification of the importance of training public service providers, the inclusion of train-
ing in professional education, interprofessional cooperation, and the importance of skills
training.
One recurring topic in the literature is that training public service providers is important
and necessary for improving the quality of interpreting (Corsellis 2008; Li et al. 2017; Hsieh
2018; Kale 2018). Several research articles and reports support this idea by including a plea
for training individuals how to communicate via interpreter (for example, Hudelson et al.
2013). A related research topic is the design of the projects and measures undertaken to set
up the training of public service providers in particular contexts (Corsellis 2008; Felberg
2013, 2016).
Yet another research theme is the extent to which communication via interpreter is in-
cluded in the curricula of future professionals, such as dentists, lawyers, doctors, and social
workers. Lessons on this seem to be an exception rather than a rule. In Norway, this is
documented in different reports and surveys (for an overview, see Felberg and Sagli 2019).
A recent survey of nursing students at the largest university that trains future medical profes-
sionals in Norway shows that the topic of communication via interpreters was not fully in-
tegrated into the curricula in either theory or praxis (Felberg and Sagli 2019). This situation
seems to be similar in other countries (H lavec and Harrison 2021: 3).
Interprofessional practice and cooperation have become a more visible topic recently,
for example, in the research about interpreting students and students of other professions
learning together (for example, medical students [Hlavec and Harris 2021; Krystallidou
et al. 2018; Krystallidou, this volume], psychology students [Skaaden 2016], and dentistry
students [Woll et al. 2020]). This type of training prepares students for future interprofes-
sional cooperation (Brisset, Leanza and Laforest 2013; Hlavec and Harris 2021). The degree
of involvement of the interpreter in other professionals’ areas of responsibility is another
question that has been discussed in this context. The tendency seems to be to advocate that
the interpreters assume more tasks than accurate interpreting (Á lvaro Aranda, Lázaro Guti-
érrez and Li 2021; Krystallidou, Langewitz and Muijsenbergh 2021). In the aforementioned
studies, potential valuable effects are envisioned, but possible negative consequences are
seldom addressed.
The importance of skills training has been given due attention in a few recent publica-
tions (Woll et al. 2020; Álvaro Aranda, Lázaro Gutiérrez and Li 2021; Krystallidou, Lange-
witz and Muijsenbergh 2021). For example, Woll et al. (2020) have found a simulation of
real-life situations and interprofessional education to be effective strategies for creating a
hands-on training course for dentistry and interpreting students.
There are also several guidelines on how to communicate via interpreter in different set-
tings that are readily available. Some guidelines are specific to certain sectors and are often
presented for specific areas, such as the end-of-life communication process (K limidis and
Minas 2009: 364) and guidelines for interpreting within healthcare settings (M iletic et al.
2006; Hadziabdic and Hjelm 2013). The guidelines do not usually describe the types of skills
one needs to communicate via an interpreter or how to acquire them.
The research outlines discussed above have inspired our work too. There are some is-
sues, however, including discussions of o n-the-job training programs for service providers,
how these training programs fit in particular public service systems, and the importance
of skill training in training programs for service providers that, to the best of our knowl-
edge, have not been given much attention until recently. In what follows, we draw from
the topics raised in the literature and bring them forward to encompass a discussion of the
latter topics.
404
Training public service providers
Course content
In this section, we revisit the theoretical considerations and questions raised previously in
the section about relevant theoretical considerations, showing how they can be addressed
in training for service providers, here by referring to the case country of Norway. The
t raining—that is, the course referred to here—was financed by the Norwegian Directorate
of Integration and Diversity (IMDi), which is responsible for the strategic development
of the field of interpreting in the public sector in Norway. Thus, training public service
providers is part of a comprehensive governmental approach to interpreting in the public
sector.
The course was developed at Oslo Metropolitan University (OsloMet) (earlier Oslo
and Akershus University College) by interpreting teachers, practicing interpreters, and
m inority-language speakers and in cooperation with public service providers. The inclu-
sion of public service providers, interpreters, and m inority-language speakers in designing
courses, which is in line with the user involvement strategy, can help ensure the relevance
of the training. The course runs for one day and lasts for six hours; and it is offered to public
service providers across Norway. Any individual or group of service providers can apply to
attend the course. The maximum number of participants is between 20 and 25. The courses
are led by course facilitators trained by the course developers. The course is administered
by OsloMet and is an onsite course offered either at OsloMet or at the service providers’
locations.
The learning outcomes of the course are as follows:
Knowledge about
• how the language barriers influence public service employees’ own professionalism
• the interpreter’s area of responsibility and working methods
• prerequisites for successful communication via interpreter
Skills in
• how better to communicate via interpreter in the most typical situations
The course consists of the following topics: (1) general information about the institutions,
actors, and regulations concerning interpreting in Norway; (2) the official view of the areas
of responsibility of public service providers working with interpreters, as well as that of the
interpreters; (3) interpreting ethics and its impact on the work of service providers; (4) informa-
tion about the (un)regulated interpreting market; (5) the interpreters’ required qualifications
to be listed in the National Register; and (6) laws and regulations controlling interpreting in
Norway.
An online version of the course—Communication via interpreter—a virtual dance for three—was
developed in 2021. The online course is based on the same principles as the onsite course, but
it is organized in a different way. It consists of a combination of two digital seminars, lasting 90
minutes each, and a self-study module; an e-learning-course developed in the EdEx platform.
405
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
Pedagogical approach
When it comes to the choice of a suitable pedagogical approach for a heterogeneous group
of adults, such as public service providers, the e xperiential–dialogical pedagogical approach
(Kolb 1984) has produced good results in Norway (Sagli and Skaaden, this volume; Skaaden
2017; Skaaden and Felberg 2020). The experiential–dialogical model starts with the premise
that course participants are the most important resource in the learning process and that
trainers and facilitators do not come with ready-made answers. Their role is to encourage
reflection in a group by asking open-ended questions. It is important to put the course par-
ticipants’ problems at the center of the learning experience. Therefore, all courses start by
eliciting course participants’ expectations of the course outcomes, as well as their former
experiences with interpreting and challenges with interpreter-mediated encounters. Inter-
activity among course participants and between course facilitators and participants through
discussions, group work, and the use of role-play is essential for this type of learning.
Public service providers are usually not a homogenous group; they have a variety of pro-
fessional backgrounds and working-life experiences. In addition, their experiences with
interpreter-mediated encounters can vary. In on-the-job training, it is not unusual to have
different professionals learn together. Therefore, when developing training, it is desirable to
gather information beforehand about the course participants and their work circumstances.
Collecting information is also a crucial element of the course itself—to start with a session de-
voted to course participants’ explication of their previous experiences with language barriers
in their work. The participants are asked questions such as the following: How often do you
communicate via interpreter? What kinds of challenges do you encounter while communi-
cating via interpreter (please describe some situations)? What do you expect to learn from this
course? Typical examples of challenges reported by course participants at the beginning of
training include questions such as the following: Where do I find interpreters? Do interpreters
have to adhere to confidentiality? How do I know when to call an interpreter? How can we
handle intercultural differences in meetings? Can I refuse to engage an interpreter with the
‘w rong’ sex, for example, a male interpreter in an encounter with a sexually abused woman?
All answers are categorized and written on a blackboard, which stays visible throughout
the entire training. In that way, the course facilitators and course participants can make sure
that they have addressed all the challenges during the training. In the online version of the
course, the participants are asked to fill out a questionnaire beforehand so that the course facil-
itators can fine-tune the course for that particular group. The answers are also made available
to all participants in a discussion forum and are revisited during and at the end of training.
As the questions above show, the complexity of the material requires experienced facili-
tators who are trained in the experiential–dialogical model. The course facilitators need to
have in-depth knowledge of about regulations, institutions, services, and actors related to
interpreting in the country in question. Ideally, courses for the course facilitators themselves
should be readily available, as was the case during the development of the o ne-day course
in Norway. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is no account in the literature of
specific training for facilitators of public service providers. This is a topic that we suggest
should be addressed in future research and practice.
406
Training public service providers
Skills training
Being able to communicate via an interpreter is a skill, and it should be practiced like any
other skill. To practice actual communication, the training can make use of simulations
of real-life situations (Woll et al. 2020) or role-playing (Dahnberg 2015 and this volume;
Felberg 2013, 2016; Wadensjö 2014; Hlavec and Harrison 2021), which is similar to the
role-play methods used in the education of future interpreters (see Dahnberg, this volume).
Role-playing and simulations allow service providers to try out the roles of interpreters
and m inority-language speakers, giving them a sense of what others’ positions feel like. To
acquire complex skills obviously requires more time than just a o ne-day course. That being
said, the training in the course gives a fi rst-step experience.
Particular attention is given to developing role-plays that are suitable for the particular
group of course participants. There are at least two strategies for choosing the topics for role-
p lays: one can choose a topic either from (a) the course participants’ area of expertise, for
example, a role-play from a medical setting for medical doctors, or (b) from another field, for
example, a role-play about a visit to an employment office that is given to medical doctors to
role-play. From our experience, introducing role-plays from other fields of expertise makes
407
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
course participants more relaxed and focused on the particularities of interpreted communi-
cation rather than on showing how good they are as professionals.
Role-plays are written in Norwegian and are supposed to be ‘interpreted’ from Norwe-
gian to Norwegian. This is another way to remove unnecessary stress from the course partic-
ipants who might not want to expose their language abilities in English (or other languages).
Interpreting from Norwegian to Norwegian is difficult enough because it challenges course
participants’ memory and t urn-taking abilities. Discussions and reflections become more sub-
stantial when based on actual skills training as an active way of learning rather than being pas-
sive exposure to the subject through lectures or by reading recommendations and guidelines.
Skills training also involves those elements that public service providers should attend to
before, during, and after i nterpreter-mediated encounters. Practicing these skills is interwo-
ven into the role-play. Instead of being just a list of guidelines, these elements become the
issues upon which participants can reflect and connect to their own experiences.
Before the actual interpreter-mediated encounter, public service providers must prepare.
They need to know what qualifications interpreters have, what language will be used, and
whether the interpreter’s gender or ethnicity, or any other elements might be relevant. The
availability of interpreters differs across different countries and regions. In Norway, for ex-
ample, there is the National Register for qualified interpreters, and course participants prac-
tice how to use the register during the course.
Public service providers also need to be aware of which interpreting methods and tech-
niques that can be used in which settings and how they differ. For example, compared to
onsite interpreting, remote interpreting adds challenges both when it comes to the use of
technology and concerning how communication participants can interact. Turn-taking in
remote interpreting requires more coordination and preparation from all participants be-
cause it is more difficult to notice subtle signals, such as sighing or hand movements online.
These subtle signals are often used by interpreters onsite when they want to take their turn
and start interpreting.
The spatial placement of participants in the room also requires adjustments, here depend-
ing on whether all participants are in the same room, two of the participants are in one room
and the third in another place, or each participant is in their own room. The placement
of a participant in a room also depends on the physical setting in a room—whether it is
happening next to a patient’s bed or in a courtroom. The main idea is that the interpreter’s
placement allows service providers and service seekers to establish contact. In the Norwegian
course, we ordinarily suggest service providers to, if possible, inform the interpreter before-
hand about the task, the nature and purpose of the meeting, and the specific terminology and
methods that will be used by the public service provider.
To prepare for what happens during a given encounter, service providers must consider
who is going to introduce the interpreter’s function in the meeting and how language and
speed of utterances might be adjusted to the situation so that the service seekers and service
providers can communicate without unnecessary hindrances. Many misunderstandings can
be avoided if the service provider or interpreter presents the interpreter’s area of responsi-
bility at the beginning of the encounter. Our course participants also get the opportunity
to practice how to word the main points of such a presentation, including short information
about the interpreter’s impartiality, neutrality, and confidentiality. Finally, we advise service
providers to take some time after an encounter with a client, to invite the interpreter to eval-
uate the just finished communication process, that is their own efforts and the communica-
tive efforts of the service provider; if they have ideas concerning how it could be improved,
and so forth.
408
Training public service providers
Summary
In this chapter, we have argued that training public service providers is a key measure for
achieving quality interpreting in public service sectors. Consequently, it requires more at-
tention and the allocation of pedagogical and organizational resources. In our view, the
409
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
training of public service providers should be an obligatory and standardized measure. How-
ever, the details of these elements need to be tailored to fit the specific circumstances of the
group of public service providers. It should be based on service providers’ needs and should
consider their experiences. Being able to communicate via an interpreter is a skill; accord-
ingly, the training should include both theory and skills training. The theoretical part should
address the wider context of the public service sector and official approach—if any—to
interpreting in the public sector of a particular country. The training should be conducted
by professional facilitators, which requires the development of a professional profile of the
trainers of facilitators.
Further reading
Felberg, R. Tatjana, and Gry Sagli (2019) “Training public service employees in how to communicate
via interpreters in Norway: Achievements and challenges”, FITISPos International Journal Public
Service Interpreting and Translation 6 (1): 141–55.
This article provides an overview of the training options for public service providers in how to communicate
via an interpreter in Norway. It approaches this topic not as an isolated phenomenon but rather in the context
of the actors, relations, and systems that constitute interpreting in the public sector.
Ozolins, Uldis (2010) “Factors that determine the provision of public service interpreting: Comparing
perspectives on government motivation and language service implementation”, The Journal of Spe-
cialised Translation 14: 194‒215.
This paper gives an overview of service provisions and language service policy in different countries through
an examination of how different social aspects affect language service policies.
Pokorn, Nike, Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatjana R. Felberg (2020) Teacher Education for Community Inter-
preting and Intercultural Mediation. Selected Chapters. Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press.
The EU policy project Training Newly Arrived Migrants for Community Interpreting and Intercultural
Mediation (T RAMIG) reveals a need for teacher profile descriptions for those who teach interpreters and in-
tercultural mediators. The results from the TRAMIG project, which is built on organizational, interpersonal,
instructional, assessment, and field competences, is found in this publication.
Related chapters
Chapter 16, “Interpreter’s mistake” – Why should other professions care about the professionalization of inter-
preters? by Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 17, Training sign language interpreters for public service interpreting by Christopher A. Stone, Cyn-
thia B. Roy, Jeremy L. Brunson
Chapter 20, Blended learning is here to stay! Combining online and on-site learning in the education of public
service interpreters by Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden
Chapter 23, Interprofessional education …. Interpreter education: In or and? Taking stock and moving forward
by Demi Krystallidou
References
Álvaro Aranda, Cristina, Raquel Lázaro Gutiérrez, and Shuangyu Li (2021) “Towards a collabora-
tive structure of interpreter-mediated medical consultations: Complementing functions between
healthcare interpreters and providers”, Social Science & Medicine 269: 113529.
Avidicus, 2008‒13. “Videoconferencing and interpreting in legal proceedings, a quick guide for
legal practitioners and police officers”. URL: http://w p.v ideoconference-i nterpreting.net/w p-
content/uploads/2 014/01/AVIDICUS2-M ini-g uide-for-L egal-practitioners- a nd-police- officers.
pdf (accessed 30 November 2021).
Berg, Berit, Joakim Caspersen, Marianne Garvik, Veronika Paulsen, and Stina Svendsen (2018) Bruk
av tolk i barnevernsinstitusjoner og omsorgssentre for enslige mindreårige [Use of Interpreters in Child Wel-
fare Services and Family Counciling Services for Unaccompanied Minors]. Trondheim, Report
NTNU Samfunnsforskning.
410
Training public service providers
Brisset, Camille, Yvan Leanza, and Karine Laforest (2013) “Working with interpreters in health care:
A systematic review and m eta-ethnography of qualitative studies”, Patient Education and Counseling
91 (2): 131–40.
Corsellis, Ann (2000) “Turning good intentions into good practice: enabling the public services to
fulfill their responsibilities”, in The Critical Link 2: Interpreters in the Community, Roda Roberts,
Silvana Carr, Diana Abraham, and Aideen Dufour (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing
Company: 89–99.
——— (2008) Public Service Interpreting. The First Steps. Basingstoke, Palgrave Macmillan.
Christiansen, Niels Finn, Klaus Petersen, Nils Edling, and Per Haave (2006) The Nordic Model of Wel-
fare, a Historical Reappraisal. Copenhagen, Museum Tusculanum Press.
Cox, Antoon, and Shuangyu Li (2019) “The medical consultation through the lenses of language and
social interaction theory”, Advances in Health Sciences Education 45: 241–57.
Dahnberg, Magnus (2015) Tolkmedierade samtal som rollspel.[Interpreter-Mediated Conversations as
Role Play] Acta universitatis upsaliensis [Doctoral dissertation, Uppsala University].
——— (2013) Kommunikasjon via tolk for ansatte i offentlig sektor [Communication via Interpreter for the
Employees in Public Sector]. Oslo, HiOA Report 2013/07.
——— (2016) “Towards a unified model for interpreter-user training in communication via an in-
terpreter: The Norwegian experience”, in Challenges and Opportunities in Public Service Interpreting,
Théophile Munyangeyo, Graham Webb, and Maruba R abadán-Gómez (eds). Basingstoke, Pal-
grave Macmillan: 135–56.
Felberg, R. Tatjana, and Gry Sagli (2019) “Training public service employees in how to communicate
via interpreters in Norway: Achievements and challenges”, FITISPos International Journal Public
Service Interpreting and Translation 6 (1): 141–55.
Gray, Ben, James Stanley, Maria Stubbe, and Jo Hilder (2011) “Communication difficulties with lim-
ited english proficiency patients: Clinician perceptions of clinical risk and patterns of use of inter-
preters”, The New Zealand Medical Journal 124 (1342): 23–38.
Hadziabdic, Emina, and Katarina Hjem (2013) “Working with interpreters: Practical advice for use of
an interpreter in healthcare”, International Journal of E vidence-Based Healthcare 11(1): 69–76.
Hlavac, Jim, and Claire Harrison (2021) “Interpreter-mediated doctor-patient interactions: Interpro-
fessional education in the training of future interpreters and doctors”, Perspectives 29(4):1–19.
Hora, Mathew T., Bailey B. Smolarek, Kelly Norris Martin, and Luke Scrivener (2019) “Exploring the
situated and cultural aspects of communication in the professions: Implications for reaching, student
employability, and equity in higher education”, American Educational Research Journal 56 (6): 1–41.
Hsieh, Elaine (2016) Bilingual Health Communication. Working with Interpreters in Cross-cultural Care. New
York, Routledge.
Hsieh, Elaine, Dyah Pitaloka, and Amy J. Johnson (2013) “Bilingual health communication: Distinc-
tive needs of providers from five specialties”, Health Communication 28 (3): 5 57–67.
Hudelson, Patricia, Melissa Dominicé Dao, Noelle Junod Perron, and Alexander Bischoff (2013)
“Interpreter-mediated diabetes consultations: A qualitative analysis of physician communication
practices”, BMS Family Practice 14 (163). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2296-14-163.
IMDi [Directorate of Integration and Diversity] (2007a) Fastleger og tolketjenester [GPs and Interpreting
Services]. Integrerings- og mangfoldsdirektoretet. 6 -2007.
——— (2007b) Tolkeformidling i offentlig sektor, etterspørsel og tilbud [Interpreting Services in Public Sec-
tor, Demand and Offer] Integrerings-og mangfoldsdirektoratet.
——— (2008) Bruk av tolk i barnevernet [Use of Interpreters in Child Welfare Services]. Integrerings-
og mangfoldsdirektoratet. 5 -2008.
——— (2009) Bruk av tolk i straffesakskjede [Use of Interpreters in Criminal Proceedings Chain].
Integrerings- og mangfoldsdirektoratet. 6 -2009.
——— (2011a) ‟Morra mi forstår ikke når lærerne snakkerˮ. Bruk av tolk i grunnskolen i Oslo [“My Mom
Does Not Understand When the Teachers are Talking.” Use of Interpreters in Primary School in
Oslo]. Integrerings- og mangfoldsdirektoratet. 2 -2011.
——— (2011b) Rett til informasjon ved språkbarrierer, Bruk av tolk i A rbeids-og velferdsforvaltningen (NAV)
[Right to Information Despite Language Barriers, Use of Interpreters in Norwegian Labour and
Welfare Service (NAV)]. Integrering-og mangfoldsdirektoratet.
——— (2018a) Behovet for tolk i 2017 [Need for Interpreters in 2017]. Rapport fra Integrerings-og
mangfoldsdirektoratet og NHO Service og Handel. URL: https://w ww.imdi.no/contentassets/
9471e477a7494d11826d63cf9af21ac1/behovet-for-tolk-i-2017 (accessed 10 December 2021).
411
Tatjana R. Felberg and Gry Sagli
——— (2018b) Hvor mye jobber kvalifiserte tolker? [How Much Do Qualified Interpreters Work]. URL:
https://w ww.imdi.no/c ontentassets/9 471e477a7494d11826d63cf 9af 21ac1/hvor- m ye-j obber-
k valifiserte-tolker (accessed 10 December 2021)
Interpreting Act (2021) Act relating to public bodies’ responsibility for the use of interpreters, etc.
(Interpreting Act) URL: https://lovdata.no/dokument/NLE/lov/2021-06-11-79 ( accessed 24
December 2021).
Jahr, Kristian (2005) Rett til tolk. Tolking og oversettelse i norsk straffeprosess [Right to Interpreter. Inter-
preting and Translation in Norwegian Criminal Proceedings]. URL: https://w ww.regjeringen.
no/g lobalassets/upload/k ilde/jd/rap/2005/0 005/ddd/pdfv/240757-rett_til_tolk.pdf (accessed 10
November 2022).
Kale, Emine (2018) Communication Barriers in Healthcare Consultations with Immigrant Patients [Doctoral
dissertation, University of Oslo].
Kolb, David A. (1984) Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ, P rentice-Hall.
Kolb, Y. Alice, and David A. Kolb (2005). “L earning styles and learning spaces: Enhancing experi-
ential learning in higher education”, Academy of Management Learning & Education 4 (2): 193–212.
Krystallidou, Demi, Céline Van De Walle, Myriam Deveugele, Evangelia Dougali, Fien Merterns,
Amélie Truwant, Ellen Van Praet, and Peter Pype (2018) “Training ‘doctor-minded’ interpreters
and ‘interpreter minded’ doctors”, Interpreting 20 (1): 1 26–4 4.
Krystallidou, Demi, Wolf Langewitz, and Maria van den Muijsenbergh (2021) “Multilingual health-
care communication: Stumbling blocks, solutions, recommendations”, Patient Education and Coun-
seling 104 (3): 5 12–16.
Klimidis, Steven, and Harry Minas (2 009) “Working with interpreters and achieving culturally
competent communication”, in Handbook of Communication in Oncology and Palliative Care, David
Kissane, Barry Phyllis, Bultz P. Butow, and Ilora Finlay (eds). Oxford, Oxford University Press:
361–73.
Kunnskapsdepartementet [The Ministry of Education and Research] (2019) Høringsnotat. Forslag til lov
om offentlige organers ansvar for bruk av tolk mv. (tolkeloven) [Hearing Report. Proposal for the Law about
Act relating to public bodies’ responsibility for the use of interpreters, etc. (Interpreting Act) ] URL:
https://w ww.reg jeringen.no/contentassets/d bdc3ab12be743b18a0b1fc0d6fca48a/horingsnotat-
forslag-t il-lov-om-offentlige-organers-a nsvar-for-bruk-av-tolk-mv.-tolkeloven.pdf (accessed 1
December 2021).
Li, Shuangyu, Jeniffer Gerwing, Demi Krystallidou, Angela Rowlands, Antoon Cox, and Peter Pype
(2017) “Interaction – A missing piece of the jigsaw in interpreter-mediated medical consultation
models”, Patient Education and Counseling 100 (9): 1769–71.
Miletic, Tania, Marie Piu, Harry Minas, Malina Stankovska, Yvonne Stolk, and Steven Klimidis
(2006) Guidelines for Working Effectively with Interpreters in Mental Health Settings. Victoria, Victorian
Transcultural Psychiatry Unit.
Molander, Anders, Harald Grimen, and Erik Odvar Eriksen (2012) “Professional discretion and ac-
countability in the welfare state”, Journal of Applied Philosophy 29 (3): 214–30.
Nilsen, Anne Birgitta. (2005) Flerspråklig kommunikasjon i rettssalen. En kasusstudie av en flerspråklig retts-
forhandling [Multilingual Communication in the Courtroom. A Case Study of a Multilingual Court
Proceeding] [Doctoral dissertation, The Faculty of Humanities, University of Oslo].
NOU (Norwegian Official Report) (2014) Tolking i offentlig sektor – et spørsmål om rettssikkerhet og
likeverd [Interpreting in the Public S ector—A Question Relating to the Right to Due Process
of Law and Equal Treatment]. URL: https://w ww.regjeringen.no/no/dokumenter/NOU-2014-
8/id2001246/ (accessed 30 November 2021).
OsloMet (2019) Kommunikasjon via tolk [Communication via Interpreter]. URL: https://w ww.os-
lomet.no/studier/lui/evu-lui/kommunikasjon-v ia-tolk (accessed 30 November 2021).
Ozolins, Uldis (2000) “Communication needs and interpreting in multilingual settings: The inter-
national spectrum of response”, in The Critical Link 2: Interpreters in the Community, Roda Roberts,
Silvana Carr, Diana Abraham, and Aideen Dufour (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing
Company: 21–33.
Ozolins, Uldis ( 2010) “Factors that determine the provision of public service interpreting: Compar-
ing perspectives on government motivation and language service implementation”, The Journal of
Specialised Translation 14: 194–215.
412
Training public service providers
Pedersen, Axel West, and Stein Kuhnle, (2017) “Introduction: The concept of a ‘nordic model’”, in
The Nordic Models in Political Science: Challenged, but Still Viable? Knutsen Oddbjørn (ed). Oslo, Fag-
bokforlaget: 249–72.
Pokorn, Nike, Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatjana R. Felberg (2020) Teacher Education for Community
Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation. Selected Chapters. Ljubljana, Ljubljana University Press.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2016) Introducing Interpreting Studies. London, Routledge.
——— (2013) “Teaching interpreting/t raining interpreters”, in Handbook of Translation Studies: Volume
4, Gambier Yves and Luc van Doorslaer (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company:
174–80.
Rambøl (2010) Evaluering av nasjonalt tolkeregister [Evaluation of the National Register of Inter-
preters]. URL: https://i mdi.no/contentassets/a 35caf84457d473d9f09ec37022703cc/evaluering-
av-nasjonalt-tolkeregister.pdf (accessed 30 November 2021).
Skaaden, Hanne (2013) Den Topartiske Tolken, Lærebok i Tolking [The T wo-Partisan Interpreter. Hand-
book of Interpreting]. Oslo, Universitetsforlaget.
——— (2016) “Training interpreters online. An experiential dialogic approach”, in TraiLLD: Training
in Languages of Lesser Diffusion, Katalin Balogh, Heidi Salaets, and Dominique Van Schoor (eds).
Leuven, Lannoo Campus: 4 6–69.
——— (2018) “‛That we all behave like professionals’ – An experiential-dialogic approach to inter-
preter education and online learning”, in Teaching Dialogue Interpreting, Cirillo Letizia and Niemants
Natacha (eds). Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing Company: 323–40.
Svennevig, Jan (2009) Språklig samhandling: innføring i kommunikasjonsteori og diskursanalyse (2 . utg.) [Lan-
guage Interaction: Introduction into Theory of Communication and Discourse Analysis (2nd ed.)]
Oslo, Landslaget for norskundervisning Cappelen akademisk forlag.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (1998) Interpreting as Interaction. London, Longman.
——— (2014) “Perspectives on role-play: Analysis, training and assessment”, The Interpreter and Trans-
lator Trainer 8 (3): 437–51.
Woll, Anne, Karin K. Quick, Cristiano Mazzei, Tehout Selameab, and Jane L. Miller (2020) “Working
with interpreters as a team in health care (W ITH Care) curriculum tool kit for oral health pro-
fessions”, MedEdPORTAL 16: 10894. URL: https://doi.org/10.15766/mep_2374-8265.10894
(accessed 30 November 2021).
413
25
EDUCATION AND TRAINING OF
PUBLIC SERVICE INTERPRETER
TEACHERS
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
Introduction
Teacher training and education is a c ross-cutting topic across different fields and disciplines.
The topic is naturally present in education sciences, but also taken up in other fields. Besides
academic literature, there is a wide range of handbooks that take a more practice-oriented
perspective, and both general and specialised trainer courses are offered internationally by
approved adult education institutions as well as non-official or in-house providers. However,
in interpreting, teaching often plays the ‘second fiddle’ (Kelly 2008: 100) for practitioners –
teachers are expected to be practitioners, but do not necessarily have much or any teacher
education, when what is actually needed are ‘professional’ teachers, that is educators with
proven teaching skills.
Terms such as ‘teacher’, ‘t rainer’, ‘educator’, ‘instructor’, and ‘facilitator’ are at times used
synonymously, but in other cases, ‘educator’ is used for those providing a broad education,
while ‘trainer’, ‘instructor’, and ‘facilitator’ are used to refer to those who teach practical
skills for a concrete use. In line with Pokorn and Mikolič Južnič (2020: 82–83), we argue
for the use of ‘teacher’ to encompass those teaching at universities as well as those in fur-
ther education programmes and professionalisation workshops: ‘high-quality education of
community interpreters […] should not be narrowly vocational: it should not only enable
students to acquire particular skills which are needed to perform interpreting […] but should
also transform the students’ outlook and their mind’.
In this contribution and with a focus on interpreter education, we use the term dia-
logue interpreting, rather than public service interpreting (PSI), as the fundamental theoretical
and methodological approaches in interpreter education are the same, regardless of spe-
cific interpreted communication settings. PSI can be considered as part of a larger whole of
interpreted encounters in dialogue format in different institutions and n on-governmental
settings. While conference interpreting is roughly defined by two d imensions – conference-
t ype events in a monologue format, and the predominant use of simultaneous interpreting,
with little differentiation by setting or topic of the e vent – PSI is increasingly differentiated
into separate categories according to the settings in which it takes place (healthcare inter-
preting, court interpreting, legal interpreting, with police or asylum interpreting as specific
domains). Although such a differentiation by settings is relevant in describing the discoursal
features of interpreted occurrences, it may not always be needed when the skills and tech-
niques for interpreting are looked at. In some legal systems, moreover, court interpreting is
not considered part of PSI (a lso reflected in the different ISO standards, see ISO 2014, 2019),
which is our reason for using dialogue interpreting as an umbrella term when it comes to
addressing issues of educating interpreters for different fields which are characterised by a
dialogue format. It goes without saying that interpreters working in different fields will
still often use the setting as a classificator to differentiate their work from that of others (for
example, court interpreter, diplomatic interpreter) and will carry out their interpreting/
rendition choices accordingly.
Taking a more general approach to dialogue interpreting, as with conference interpreting
above, the typical setting for dialogue interpreting, with the exception of diplomatic inter-
preting, could be defined as national institutions (a s opposed to international organisations),
that is interpreting in national economic, political and diplomatic institutions (for example,
ministries, expert organisations, and governments, both national and local). Positioning PSI
as a part of this wider field of dialogue interpreting may not only serve to bolster its public
image, but also makes sense in terms of didactics: dialogue interpreting, whether it is done
in a healthcare, legal, political, business, or diplomatic setting, addresses shared theoretical
and methodological questions. Building on a common foundation (for example, interpreting
techniques), teachers as well as interpreters can then add knowledge specific to each setting
(field-specific subject knowledge and skills). Therefore, although we focus predominantly
on literature and programmes for PSI teacher education, we also consider the wider field of
interpreting in our discussion of pedagogy.
415
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
Translation and Interpreting) at the University of Geneva (now Faculty of Translation and
Interpreting), who report on their Master of Advanced Studies for Conference Interpreter
Trainers (for example, Moser-Mercer, Class and Seeber 2006; Class and M oser-Mercer
2013). In PSI, the topic of interpreter teacher education has long been neglected and only
recently more innovative approaches seem to have taken hold (for example, Englund Dim-
itrova and Wadensjö 2013; Pokorn, Viezzi and Radanović Felberg 2020). These may have
been triggered also by work on emancipatory and participatory approaches to teaching and
learning (Baraldi and Gavioli 2012; Cirillo and Niemants 2017), and perhaps fuelled by de-
velopments in sign language (SL) interpreting (Roy 2006).
While teacher education has been a topic in both SL and spoken language interpreting,
American Sign Language (ASL) interpreter educators early on took the lead by establishing
the Conference of Interpreter Trainers (CIT), the first specific trainers’ network, as early
as the late 1970s. Exchange was not confined to ASL, however; the network sought to estab-
lish the involvement of spoken-language interpreter teachers at an early stage (Pöchhacker
2016: 206), as evident in the proceedings of the CIT conferences (see CIT 2020), or contri-
butions in the International Journal of Interpreter Education, published by CIT since 2009. Other
teacher educator networks, that were established later, are the Australian Interpreter Train-
ers’ Network (ITN 2010), an association for SL interpreter teachers founded in 2009, and the
Nordic Network for Interpreter Trainers (N NIT 2015) that was initiated in 2011 and has so
far published three volumes (in English and Scandinavian languages) on educators’ training
(Skaaden and Radanović Felberg 2011; Wadensjö 2013, 2016). More recently, the Russian
Association of Translator and Interpreter Trainers was established in 2017 (Naimushin and
Stepanova 2018).
Publications on translation and/or interpreting (T/I) training and pedagogy deal with an
array of topics associated with training (a ssessment, testing, quality, curriculum design), but
rarely mention teachers’ education, and if so, mostly in passing only. The topic was discussed
in detail in a contribution by Englund Dimitrova as late as 2002, and a popular German
handbook on T/I didactics (K autz 2000) outlines a curriculum for ‘interpreting didactics’
(2000: 520–27) with practical exercises, but no details or references.
When PSI gradually started to gain a foothold in academia in the 1990s, the topic of
teachers’ education remained a niche topic if one takes into consideration the fast expansion
of literature with a specific focus on PSI after the 2000s. Among the range of handbooks
and introductions on interpreting that have been published since the late 2000s, the topic of
interpreter teacher education, for instance, is taken up in a subsection by Pöchhacker (2016:
205–6) and mentioned briefly in the Routledge Handbook of Interpreting (Boéri 2015: 39). That
fact that interpreter teacher education is not included as a separate entry in the Routledge
Encyclopedia of Interpreting Studies (Pöchhacker 2015) also points to the fact that this is a field
that lacks attention in research.
While in the early stages, scholars mention the importance of training the trainers as
one of the steps towards professionalisation of PSI (for example, Schweda Nicholson 1994;
Roberts 1997), practical workbooks or teaching guides that were published in the 2000s
(for example, Lee and Buzo 2009; Rudvin and Tomassini 2011) do not address the topic.
Specific handbooks on PSI, however, devote subchapters to the specialisation of trainers
(Hale 2007: 169–76; Corsellis 2008: 65–66), which they view as an important prerequisite
for h igh-quality interpreter training: ‘Trainers are the vital ingredients in the process of
development and for the future’ (Corsellis 2008: 65). The first more detailed, though mostly
theoretical, discussions of educators’ training in PSI date back to the early 2000s (see below
for references).
416
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
Generally, only few publications on interpreter teacher education are empirical (Englund
Dimitrova and Wadensjö 2013; Lai and Mulayim 2013; Skaaden 2013; Maloney 2018), and
no studies present a longitudinal perspective. Instead, publications are mostly literature-
b ased reflections on the challenges of and recommendations for interpreter teacher educa-
tion (for example, Corsellis 2001; Giambruno-Day Miguélez 2003; Patrie 2003; Lederer
2007; Townsley 2011), or descriptions of specific training courses (for example, Mikkelson
and Neumann Solow 2002; Furmanek 2010; Radanović Felberg 2013), focusing predom-
inantly on PSI teacher education, and to a lesser degree on conference interpreter teacher
education.
What can be inferred from this review is that, while the need for educating the teachers
was voiced early on, it is still overall underrepresented in scholarship. We can only speculate
as to the reasons why. They may be linked to the fact that in the early days of interpreter ed-
ucation and research the field was mostly concerned with establishing training programmes
and consolidating itself as a discipline.
417
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
teachers would have to be able to impart to students would be the same in any interpreting
programme.
Townsley (2011) drafted a sample curriculum and defined prerequisite competencies and
core competencies for the legal field, while Radanović Felberg’s (2013) focus is highly spe-
cific and describes the example of a model for the training of trainers. More than 30 years
after the launch of the course at the Polytechnic of Central London, still only a few uni-
versities offer specific t rain-the-trainer programmes. In addition to those mentioned above,
the (not exhaustive) list includes London Metropolitan University, Monterey Institute of
International Studies, Stockholm University, and University of Massachusetts. Courses
are also offered by non-academic organisations such as the international training agency
Cross-Cultural Communications LLC in the United States, AIIC, or the European Union’s
Directorate-General for Interpretation; in the case of the latter mostly with a focus on train-
ing teachers for conference interpreting.
Outside of h igh-profile conference interpreting courses where the teachers are mostly
full-time professionals (Donovan 2011: 121), interpreter training is mostly delivered by
‘practisearchers’ (Boéri 2015: 39) with backgrounds as both practitioners and scholars, or
‘practitioner trainers’ (Hale 2007: 173). Neither of these usually have formal training in
didactics, though, on a positive note and following Boéri (2015: 39), the academisation of
the discipline seems to have increased ‘autonomy from the organized profession’, that is, the
conference interpreting field, and lead to more reflexivity with respect to teaching/learning
paradigms and curriculum design that are more suited to dialogue interpreter training.
In part of the literature, there appears to be a wide agreement on the matter that teachers
should be qualified interpreters, perhaps a heritage from A IIC-organised conference inter-
preter courses, regardless of whether they are researchers, practitioners, or practisearchers.
As Lederer (2007: 17) writes, ‘to teach translation, one has to be an expert practitioner’.
Townsley (2011: 4) goes even further than that, suggesting ‘a minimum of 200 hours of legal
interpreting and translation, acquired over at least one year’ is needed for being able to teach
(legal) interpreting. Furmanek (2010: 94), in turn, believes that educators
do not need to be expert interpreters in public service interpreting BUT they need
to understand what they are teaching and why they are teaching it; and in order to
be able to do so, they need to have a thorough knowledge of the most recent research
(2 010: 94).
This call for research-based translation-specific knowledge seems particularly relevant for
specialised domains or language combinations for which no teachers with an interpreting
background may be found: for instance, teachers for languages that are not taught in reg-
ular courses, or with self-acquired expertise, such as teachers for specialist areas and new
career fields emerging as a result of societal or technological developments (for example,
remote interpreting, or hybrid modes, such as speech-to-text interpreting, or simultaneous-
consecutive). These individuals will bring a considerable degree of fi rst-hand technical
knowledge to the job, though their lack of insight into practical matters of interpreting
means that they will have to brush up on interpreting-specific knowledge and find a way to
impart their expertise in a way that is suited to the needs of interpreter trainees.
However, there is no such agreement regarding that interpreter educators should be
qualified teachers as well, although voices have long been calling for additional training
in pedagogy, for instance, Corsellis (2001), who advocates for practitioners with teaching
qualifications. Orlando (2019b) considers it the responsibility and task of higher education
418
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
institutions to ensure their staff have teaching skills, and Pokorn and Mikolič Južnič (2020:
83) believe that ‘teachers should not only have practical experience, but also theoretical
knowledge in order to adequately prepare their students for a professional role as reflective
practitioners’.
Theory-based education is one of the hallmarks of academic teaching, which draws from
research and social contexts and considers professional practice. Here we can agree with
those who call for an academisation and integration of theory in courses for interpreter
teachers, such as Lederer (2007) and Furmanek (2010), who considers the research compo-
nent and being aware of the agency of the interpreter as essential, just like Orlando (2019a,
2019b) and Pokorn and Mikolič Južnič (2020). We too believe that a solid academic educa-
tion of interpreter teachers, whether they teach at universities or in vocational programmes,
is the basis for improving the quality of interpreting programmes and the image of the
interpreter.
Teaching qualifications
Standards for teaching, for example, the Australian Professional Standards for Teachers
(A ITSL 2011), or the UK Teachers’ Standards (Department of Education 2011), all include a
number of core competencies teachers need: subject and curriculum knowledge, an under-
standing of teaching methods, of how learning works and how to assess learning outcomes,
and a commitment to professional development and ongoing improvement of their teach-
ing. While these standards focus on schoolteachers – and, indeed, as Kelly (2008) noted,
the teaching competence of university teachers was long neglected by many universities,
although that is beginning to c hange – the competencies are also reflected in the Standards
and Guidelines for Quality Assurance in the European Higher Education Area (ESG) (2015),
which highlights the role of teachers as essential for a h igh-quality education. Teachers’ core
task in higher education is to strengthen the link between research and education and enable
students in the ‘acquisition of knowledge, competences and skills’: ‘Institutions should assure
themselves of the competence of their teachers’ (2015: 62). The Australian Higher Education
Standards Framework (2015) requires teachers at universities to be qualified in their disci-
pline, to keep abreast of current developments through continuing scholarship or research,
and to have ‘skills in contemporary teaching, learning and assessment principles relevant to
the discipline’ (Commonwealth of Australia 2015).
There are a number of fundamental competencies for teachers on which there is con-
sensus in the literature (for example, Corsellis 2001; Townsley 2011). Pokorn and Mikolič
Južnič (2020) summarise the competencies for teachers (of community interpreters and in-
tercultural mediators) as follows: field competence, interpersonal competence, organisational
competence, instructional competence, and assessment competence. These are competencies
that teachers need regardless of whether they teach in university-based programmes or voca-
tional extramural training contexts.
A sustainable solution would include making the education of educators a part of inter-
preting curricula, for example, as a separate module for interpreter education. Given that
the majority of theory, interpreting strategies, and problem-solving approaches are the same
for all (d ialogue) interpreters, regardless of the institutional setting they interpret in, teacher
education might be built on a joint foundation similarly to the model at Stockholm Univer-
sity described by Englund Dimitrova and Wadensjö (2013). Instead of completely separate
education of PSI teachers, business liaison interpreter teachers, court interpreter teachers,
and so on, all teachers would be familiarised with the same fundamental theoretical and
419
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
subject-related content and didactic methods, based on the shared features of interpreted
institutional encounters.
Although it is, of course, important to impart the knowledge, skills, and competencies
that are currently in demand, these alone do not suffice. Especially in professions where you
work with and for people, professional competence requires not only competence in the
topics and methods of the profession, but also socio-communicative and individual compe-
tences (emotional skills, empathy, ethical d ecision-making) as well as the capacity to adopt
adequate solutions for diverse contingencies.
Pedagogic action
Focusing on education also means to consider current and dynamic societal, technical, and
professional developments and challenges. Orlando and Gerber (2020) discuss prevalent
challenges for (translation and) interpreting education in the Routledge Handbook of Trans-
lation and Globalization and argue that contemporary education must take into account the
weighty changes and trends that have taken place in the global higher education landscape,
such as the Bologna Process, the status of English as lingua franca, the increased use of new
technologies, and the integration of theory into practice, while at the same time responding
to industry demands. This underscores the importance of pedagogical competence (Orlando
2019a, 2020; Orlando and Gerber 2020; Pokorn, Viezzi and Radanović Felberg 2020). Some
authors even advocate the establishment of a subdiscipline Translation Pedagogy (Piotrowska
and Tyupa 2014), which would position pedagogy as an important element between inter-
preting research and interpreting practice.
Education embeds interpreting in larger structural, political, and action-related contexts.
With critical discourse strategies, it can contribute significantly to shaping the domain as
well as public awareness and the perception of PSI (a nd dialogue interpreting in general) and
future developments. Interpreting is also a discipline in which both teachers and students
represent a large variety of nationalities, languages, and different educational backgrounds.
These different backgrounds and a number of additional factors influence the choice of
course contents (see Patrie 2003, for instance, on learning progression and sequencing of
tasks) and methods, such as teaching and learning cultures and how interpreter students
with a migration background cope with arrival and settling in a new social environment
and what this means for training (Lai and Mulayim 2013: 2 88–89). Mikkelson and Neu-
mann Solow (2002) discuss challenges of teaching for a multilingual group; Pym (2003)
stresses the challenges of heterogeneous learning groups and different learning styles, which
are also mentioned by Moser-Mercer (2007) with particular reference to non-Western
learning styles.
Research provides findings on cognitive, interactive, and social processes, which didac-
tics research converts into strategies that entail the theoretical and practical reflection on
one’s own actions. The different interconnected and interdependent forms of teaching and
learning described in this section are based on critical-constructive didactics (cf. Klaf ki
2007; Kadrić 2011). They are relevant both as a didactic approach in teacher education and
as systematically taught course content for teachers to learn how they can apply them in
their own teaching. In what follows, we discuss four dimensions of holistic teaching and
learning (K adrić 2011: 27–28, building on Ott 2011: 11–16; our translations): (1) subject-
related teaching and learning (aimed at imparting or acquiring subject competence), (2)
methodological-operative teaching and learning (a imed at imparting or acquiring method-
ological competencies), (3) socio-communicative teaching and learning (a imed at imparting
420
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
or acquiring social competencies), and (4) a ffective-ethical teaching and learning (a imed at
imparting or acquiring individual competencies).
1 S ubject-related teaching and learning as the foundation of education refers mainly to factual
knowledge one acquires. It imparts existing knowledge, initiates discourses, and gen-
erates new knowledge. On the curriculum of interpreter teachers, this might include
didactics, IS, and courses for achieving subject knowledge and specialised language
skills for any of the specialisations they will teach. At the same time, teachers would also
learn how to create teaching material and impart knowledge to their students. In the
literature, we find a number of suggestions for the contents of interpreter education or
joint courses (A ngelelli 2000, 2017; Corsellis 2001; Kadrić 2011, 2019; Townsley 2011;
Englund Dimitrova and Wadensjö 2013) such as, for example, interpreting techniques,
professional ethics, note-taking and memory skills, discourse management skills, or
terminology management. Since Miguélez’ ( 2003) and Patrie’s ( 2003) observation,
that there was a lack of ‘ready-to-use teaching material’ (M iguélez 2003: 46), different
books and manuals have been published, which suggest that there now is a common
ground for teaching and testing (for example, Sawyer 2004; Angelelli and Jacobson
2009; Giambruno 2014 and the Routledge book series Translation Practices Explained).
The development of such material, however, requires not only curricular knowledge
(an understanding of the organisation, principles, and structure of an institution’s or
country’s learning programmes) but also pedagogical competence and subject or field
competence, as Pokorn and Mikolič Južnič (2020) have termed it.
2 Methodological-operative teaching and learning generally refers to competencies and skills
concerning the application of subject-related knowledge. This includes both the meth-
odological approaches and support systems (something that Corsellis 2001 notes is
lacking for teachers after their qualification) and teaching styles, different teaching ap-
proaches and how these are immersed in general adult education pedagogy, as well as
specific didactic principles and approaches. Teachers need to be able to select contents
and graduate them, but also be able to think of adequate methods how to transfer and
impart content to their students. Although an understanding of teaching methods or
instructional competence (Pokorn and Mikolič Južnič 2020) is required for all four di-
mensions of teaching, this specific dimension is perhaps the one where it is most salient.
In interpreter teacher education, skills that might be taught here are how to teach, assess,
and evaluate students. In teaching interpreting students, teachers could focus on the
skills necessary for interpreting and managing interaction. As a form of m ethodological-
operative teaching, theatre-pedagogical approaches in interpreting education or the use
of authentic data were introduced by Bahadır (2009), Kadrić (2011, 2017), Baraldi and
Gavioli (2020), and Pokorn, Viezzi and Radanović Felberg (2020).
Quality assurance and evaluation, which straddle both subject competence (what to
assess and evaluate) and methodological competence (how to assess and evaluate), are also
important elements of teacher education and form the basis for the optimisation of inter-
preter education. The results of quality and evaluation measures will support interpreter
educators in ensuring and improving the quality of their programmes and courses as well
as their teaching. To make best use of quality and evaluation mechanisms, interpreter
teachers require field, organisational, and assessment competence. Quality assurance can
extend to different elements of interpreter education: the quality and design of curric-
ula, recruitment and entrance requirements, forms of assessment, students’ evaluation
and course feedback, inclusion of issues of societal importance (for example, diversity
421
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
or gender sensitive language), integration of new trends and tools (for example, remote
interpreting or interpreting for accessibility, such as speech-to-text interpreting), and
integration of topics that are specifically relevant in different dialogue interpreting set-
tings (for example, vicarious traumatisation, supervision, self-protection).
Several authors offer concrete suggestions on the assessment and establishment of
systems of accreditation for teachers and trainees, and for the evaluation of teaching
experience (for example, Corsellis 2001; Mikkelson and Neumann Solow 2002; Patrie
2003; Kelly 2005, 2008; Angelelli and Jacobson 2009; Giambruno 2014; Orlando 2019a;
Pokorn, Viezzi and Radanović Felberg 2020). The ongoing evaluation of teaching prac-
tice and professional development are central to ensuring a high quality of education.
3 Socio-communicative teaching and learning aims at addressing skills such as teamwork, an
open attitude to different problems and perspectives, feedback methods, active lis-
tening, dealing with conflicts, m eta-communication, or cooperation. Teaching and
learning interpreting as an action-oriented activity is almost intrinsically defined by
process-orientation and a high degree of independent, reflective, and situated inter-
action. It strengthens cognitive and interactional competencies by complementing
problem solving, logical and analytical thinking with emotional intelligence and socio-
communicative awareness.
Perhaps the most important competence to acquire is the ability to cooperate con-
structively with other people. This interpersonal competence is a requirement for re-
sponsible action in one’s professional environment. In terms of methodology, all forms
of holistic communication are suited for this, such as peer group work, teamwork (for
instance, Lai and Mulayim 2013), or various forms of theatre pedagogy (for example,
Wadensjö 2014, 2017; Kadrić 2017). In interpreter teacher education, this subdimension
could focus on cooperation with other teachers, interaction with students, and conflict
resolution (for example, Mikkelson and Neumann Solow 2002; Pokorn, Viezzi and
Radanović Felberg 2020), and also on how to teach and supervise peer cooperation and
teamwork.
4 Affective-ethical teaching and learning, finally, addresses individual competencies, such as
self-reflective skills, independent and responsible action, or developing specific indi-
vidual interests, to name but a few (Ott 2011: 16). Ethical, as it is used in this respect,
does not only refer to deontological ethics and normative views, as is often the case in
IS when ethical d ecision-making is based on deontological codes, but more broadly to
beliefs we consider right or wrong and how we arrive at such a conclusion (see Pölla-
bauer and Topolovec 2020 on ethics in PSI). This dimension approaches topics with a
focus on individual and societal views, ethics, and politics of interpretation in a way
that is suitable for emancipatory interaction (Vandevelde 2010; Kadrić 2011). A ffective-
ethical teaching and learning can be achieved through teaching formats that take a
holistic view of individuals in their socio-political context including the questions of
interpreting politics. It strengthens the subject-related, methodological-operative, and
socio-communicative competences by adding an individual ethical perspective, which
is needed for reflective, responsible action and ethical decision-making.
ffective-ethical questions are based on subject-related knowledge; they discuss and ques-
A
tion them (for example, actions and structures of public authorities, positioning and role
performance, ethics of the field involved, interpreting ethics) but also deal with very con-
crete matters. International legal guarantees (for example, the European Convention on
Human Rights or the Universal Declaration of Human Rights) are subject-related topics,
422
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
but their legal and normative provisions can be discussed in a social or personal/individual
context. The concept of the prohibition of discrimination can be approached in an a ffective-
ethical manner by letting participants share their own experiences with discrimination or
hearing about concrete cases of discrimination others have faced (see, for instance, UNHCR
Handbook for Interpreters in Asylum Procedures). Interpreter teachers who themselves often had
to rely on interpreters in their lives are likely to be particularly sensitised to matters of
discrimination and dignity and how they relate to professional ethics. By the same token,
these questions can be approached through the methodological-operative lens, for example,
through staging a roleplay of an interpreting situation where discrimination plays a role to
explore ways of acting in such a situation, or a socio-communicative one, for example, a
simulation with the professions the interpreters will be working with in the future (a lso see
Phelan et al. 2019).
These complex and important topics can be addressed effectively when they are related to
personal experience or applied in a certain social context. Societal issues, professional ethics,
and individual ethics form the basis for initiating new discourse and trying out new action
spaces. Therefore, these four elements of teaching and learning, which include a subject-
related, a methodological-operative, a socio-communicative, and an a ffective-ethical di-
mension, are not separate from each other.
From the aforementioned dimensions of pedagogic action, we can derive the concept of
key qualifications for (d ialogue) interpreting largely: profession-specific and general strate-
gies, professional ethics, skills, and competencies. In other words, teachers know and un-
derstand the relevant subject topics, techniques and tools, and know how to teach them to
others.
423
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
pedagogical experience. Active exchange creates networks and helps to build a sense of
community and belonging in their respective fields.
The existing platforms, networks, and ideas presented in this text (including Australian
Interpreter Trainers’ Network, Nordic Network for Interpreter Trainers, Russian Associa-
tion of Translator and Interpreter Trainers, CIT, and journals like International Journal of Inter-
preter Education, Interpreter and Translator Trainer and others) could help form a broad forum
of a dialogue of interpreter teachers (DIT) and strengthen the existing foundations, make public
service interpreters more visible and better known to a wider community. A global exchange
between interpreter educators may yield findings and results that may become standard in
their national educational contexts and the public. It would also promote mobility in educa-
tion and could contribute to the equalisation of the interpreting professions.
Further reading
Cirillo, Letizia, and Natacha Niemants (eds) (2017) Teaching Dialogue Interpreting. Amsterdam, John
Benjamins.
This collective volume presents a broad overview of diverse aspects of training, written by experts from differ-
ent fields, and also offers a set of resources that are readily useable for different specific training needs.
Pokorn, K. Nike, Maurizio Viezzi, and Tatjana Radanović Felberg (eds) (2020) Teacher Education
for Community Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation: Selected Chapters. Ljubljana, University Press.
https://doi.org/10.4312/9789610604020.
This volume offers an introduction to ways of implementing courses for prospective teachers of community
interpreters and intercultural mediators, in particular for languages of limited diffusion.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (ed) (2013) Training the Trainers: Nordic Seminar on Interpreter Education/Utbildningen
av utbildare: Nordiskt möte om tolkutbildning. Institute for Interpreting and Translation Studies, Stock-
holm University. URL: http://u rn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:d iva-90853.
This PDF publication presents the results of the second in a series of Nordic meetings with a focus on training
the trainers of public service interpreters, and also is one of the few publications that present empirical data. The
first was published in Oslo and is not available online.
There are also a number of general education journals (for example, Higher Education Journal, Adult
Education Quarterly) that may be consulted on general topics of adult education.
Related chapters
Chapter 1, General issues about PSI: Institutions, codes, norms and professionalisation by Carmen
Valero-Garcés
Chapter 17, Training sign language interpreters for public service interpreting by Christopher A. Stone, Cyn-
thia B. Roy and Jeremy L. Brunson
Chapter 18, Role play as a means of training and testing public service interpreting by Magnus Dahnberg
Chapter 19, Monitoring in dialogue interpreting – cognitive and didactic perspectives by Elisabet Tiselius and
Birgitta Englund Dimitrova
Chapter 20, Blended learning is here to stay! Combining o n-line and on-campus learning in the education of public
service interpreters by Gry Sagli and Hanne Skaaden.
Chapter 21, The conversation analytic role-play method: How authentic data meet simulations for interpreter
training by Natacha Niemants, Jessica Pedersen Belisle Hansen and Elisabeth Stokoe
Chapter 23, Interprofessional education… interpreter education: in or and? Taking stock and moving forward by
Demi Krystallidou
References
A ITSL – Australian Institute for Teaching and School Leadership (2011) Australian professional stan-
dards for teachers. URL: https://w ww.aitsl.edu.au/teach/standards (accessed 22 April 2021).
Angelelli, Claudia V. (2000) “Interpreting pedagogy: A bridge long overdue”, The ATA Chronicle 29 (11):
4 0–47.
424
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
——— (2017) “A nchoring dialogue interpreting in principles of teaching and learning”, in Teaching
Dialogue Interpreting: Research-Based Proposals for Higher Education, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Nie-
mants (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 30–4 4.
Angelelli, Claudia V., and Holly E. Jacobson (eds) (2009) Testing and Assessment in Translation and Inter-
preting Studies. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Bahadır, Şebnem (2009) “Body-a nd-enactment-centred interpreting pedagogy: preliminary thoughts
on a t rain-the-t rainers concept for (medical) interpreting”, in Spürst Du, wie der Bauch rauf-runter?
Is everything topsy-turvy in your tummy? Fachdolmetschen im Gesundheitsbereich, Dörte Andres and Sonja
Pöllabauer (eds). München, Meidenbauer: 29–43.
Baraldi, Claudi, and Laura Gavioli (2020) “Authentic mediated interactions for training healthcare
mediators”, in Teacher Education for Community Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation: Selected Chap-
ters, Nike K. Pokorn, Maurizio Viezzi and Tatjana Felberg Radanović (eds). Ljubljana, Ljubljana
University Press: 1 76–99.
Baraldi, Claudio, and Laura Gavioli ( 2012) Coordinating Participation in Dialogue Interpreting.
eds) (
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
Barnett, Ronald (2016) Understanding the University: Institution, Idea, Possibilities. London, Routledge.
Bergunde, Annika, and Sonja Pöllabauer (2019) “Curricular design and implementation of a training
course for interpreters in an asylum context”, Translation and Interpreting 11 (1): 1–21.
Boéri, Julie (2015) “Key internal players in the development of the interpreting profession”, in Routledge
Handbook of Interpreting, Holly Mikkelson and Renée Jourdenais (eds). London, Routledge: 29–44.
CIT – Conference
of Interpreter Trainers (2020) CIT Conference Proceedings. URL: https://w ww.
cit-a sl.org/new/ (accessed 8 February 2021).
Class, Barbara, and Barbara Moser-Mercer (2013) “Training conference interpreter trainers with
technology – A virtual reality”, in Quality in Interpreting. Widening the Scope, Olalla García Becerra,
E. Macarena Pradas Macías, and Rafael Barranco-Droege (eds). Volume 1. Granada, Editorial
Comares: 293–313.
Commonwealth of Australia (2015) Higher education standards framework (Threshold Standards).
URL: https://w ww.legislation.gov.au/Details/F2015L01639 (accessed 22 April 2021).
Corsellis, Ann (2001) “Training the trainers”, in Aequitas: Access to Justice across Language and Culture in
the EU, Erik Hertog (ed). Antwerp, Lessius Hogeschool: 139–45.
——— (2008) Public Service Interpreting: The First Steps. London, Palgrave Macmillan.
Department of Education ( 2011) Teachers’ standards. URL: https:// w ww.gov.uk/government/
publications/teachers-standards (accessed 22 April 2021).
Donovan, Claire (2011) “Ethics in the teaching of conference interpreting”, The Interpreter and Trans-
lator Trainer 5 (1): 1 09–28.
Duff, Alan (1989) Translation. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Englund Dimitrova, Birgitta ( 2002) “ Training and educating t rainers –
A key issue in trans-
lator’s training”, in Teaching Translation and Interpreting 4. Building Bridges, Eva Hung ( ed).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 73–82.
Englund Dimitrova, Birgitta, and Cecilia Wadensjö (2013) “On common ground? The Swedish experi-
ence with university education of interpreter teachers”, in Training the Trainers: Nordic Seminar on Inter-
preter Education, Cecilia Wadensjö (ed). Stockholm University, Faculty of Humanities, Department
of Swedish Language and Multilingualism, Institute for Interpreting and Translation Studies: 67–81.
Fung, Dilly (2017) A Connected Curriculum for Higher Education. London, UCL.
Furmanek, Olgierda (2010) “Applied interpreting studies at the core of teaching the instructors: A
proposal of solutions”, The Journal of Specialised Translation 14: 80–99.
Giambruno-Day Miguélez, Cynthia (2003) “Fundamentals in course development for the novice in-
terpreter trainer”, The ATA Chronicle 32 (11): 4 6–49.
Hale, Sandra (2007) Community Interpreting. London, Palgrave Macmillan.
Hung, Eva (ed) (2002) Teaching Translation and Interpreting 4. Building Bridges. Amsterdam/Philadelphia,
John Benjamins.
ISO – International Organization for Standardization (2014) ISO 13611: Interpreting – G uidelines for
Community Interpreting. Geneva, ISO. URL: https://w ww.iso.org/standard/54082.html (accessed
8 February 2021).
ISO – International Organization for Standardization (2019) ISO 20228:2019: Interpreting Services –
L egal interpreting – Requirements. Geneva, ISO. URL: https://w ww.iso.org/standard/67327.html
(accessed 8 February 2021).
425
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
ITN – Interpreter Trainers’ Network (2010). URL: https://a slia.com.au/pd-a nd-events/i nterpreter-
t rainers-network/ (accessed 8 February 2021).
Kadrić, Mira (2011) Dialog als Prinzip: Für eine emanzipatorische Praxis und Didaktik des Dolmetschens.
Tübingen, Narr.
——— (2017) “Make it different! Teaching interpreting with theatre techniques”, in Teaching Dialogue
Interpreting, Letizia Cirillo and Natacha Niemants (eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins:
275–92.
——— (2019) G erichts-und Behördendolmetschen. Prozessrechtliche und translatorische Perspektiven. Wien,
Facultas.
Kautz, Ulrich (2000) Handbuch Didaktik des Übersetzens und Dolmetschens. München, Iudicium-Verlag,
Goethe-Institut.
Kelly, Dorothy (2005) A Handbook for Translation Trainers. Manchester, St. Jerome.
——— (2008) “Training the trainers: Towards a description of translator trainer competence and
training needs analysis”, in La formation en traduction: pédagogie, docimologie et technologie I 21 (1):
99–125.
Klaf ki, Wolfgang (2007) Neue Studien Zur Bildungstheorie und Didaktik: Zeitgemäße Allgemeinbildung und
Kritisch-konstruktive Didaktik. Weinheim, Beltz.
Lai, Miranda, and Sedat Mulayim (2013) “Training interpreters in rare and emerging languages: The
problems of adjustment to a tertiary education setting”, in Interpreting in a Changing Landscape.
Selected Papers from Critical Link 6, Christina Schäffner, Krzysztof Kredens, and Yvonne Fowler
(eds). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 287–304.
Lederer, Marianne (2007) “Can theory help translator and interpreter trainers and trainees?”, The
Interpreter and Translator Trainer 1 (1): 15–35.
Lee, Jieun, and Adrian Buzo (2009) Community Language Interpreting. A Workbook. Sydney, The Fed-
eration Press.
Maloney, Sandra L. (2018) Interpreter Educators in the United States: Teaching, Research, and Practice,
Master’s Thesis, Oregon, Western Oregon University. URL: https://d igitalcommons.wou.edu/
theses/45 (accessed 24 Octobre 2020).
Massey, Gary, Don Kiraly, and Maureen E hrensberger-Dow (2019) “Training translator trainers: An
introduction”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 13 (3): 211–15.
Mikkelson, Holly, and Sharon Neumann Solow (2002) “Report from the front lines: Multilingual
training-of-trainers for refugee interpreters”. URL: https://acebo.myshopify.com/pages/report-from-
the-front-lines-multilingual-training-of-trainers-for-refugee-interpreters (accessed 8 February 2021).
Moser-Mercer, Barbara (2007) “Global cognition: Training a new breed of interpreter trainers”, in
Interpreting Studies and Beyond: A Tribute to Miriam Shlesinger, Franz Pöchhacker, Arnt Lykke Jakob-
sen, and Ingert M. Mees (eds). Kopenhagen, Samfundslitteratur: 8 9–101.
Moser-Mercer, Barbara, Barbara Class, and Kilian G. Seeber (2006) “Community building: Training
interpreter trainers”, in Fifth National Conference on Interpreting Practice, Mingjiong Chai and Ailing
Zhang (eds). Shanghai, Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press: 192–210.
Naimushin, Boris, and Maria Stepanova (2018) “The association of translator and interpreter trainers:
Building a network of teaching excellence”, in Society and Languages in the Third Millennium. Com-
munication. Education. Translation, RUDN University (ed). Moscow, RUDN University: 169–78.
NNIT (2015) Nordic network for interpreter trainers. URL: https://w ww.tolk.su.se/english/research/
research-collaborations/nordic-network-for-interpreter-trainers-1.227950 (accessed 8 February 2021).
Orlando, Marc (2019a) Training 21st Century Translators and Interpreters: At the Crossroads of Practice,
Research and Pedagogy. Berlin, Frank & Timme.
——— (2019b) “Training and educating interpreter and translator trainers as p ractitioners-researchers-
teachers”, The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 13 (1): 1–17.
Orlando, Marc, and Leah Gerber (2020) “The impact of globalization on translator and interpreter ed-
ucation”, in The Routledge Handbook of Translation and Globalization, Esperança Bielsa and Dionysios
Kapsaskis (eds). London, Routledge: 202–15.
Ott, Bernd (2011) Grundlagen des beruflichen Lernens und Lehrens. Ganzheitliches Lernen in der beruflichen
Bildung. 4th ed. Berlin, Cornelsen.
Patrie, Carol (2003) “Success strategies for interpreter educators”, The ATA Chronicle 32 (11): 35–40.
Phelan, Mary, Mette Rudvin, Hanne Skaaden, and Patrick Kermit (2019) Ethics in Public Service Inter-
preting. London, Routledge.
426
Education and training of public service interpreter teachers
Piotrowska, Maria, and Sergiy Tyupa (2014) “Translation pedagogy – A new sub-discipline of trans-
lation studies”, inTRAlinea. Special Issue: Challenges in Translation Pedagogy, URL: http://w ww.
intralinea.org/specials/a rticle/2112 (accessed: 8 February 2021).
Pokorn, K. Nike, and Južnič Mikolič (2020) “The community interpreter and/or intercultural me-
diator teacher profile”, in Teacher Education for Community Interpreting and Intercultural Mediation:
Selected Chapters, Nike K. Pokorn, Maurizio Viezzi and Tatjana Radanović Felberg (eds). Ljubljana,
Ljubljana University Press: 80–89.
Pöchhacker, Franz (2007) “Giving access – Or not: A developing-country perspective on healthcare
interpreting”, in Interpreting Studies and Beyond. A Tribute to Miriam Shlesinger, Franz Pöchhacker, A.
Lykke Jakobsen, and Inger M. Mees (eds). Copenhagen, Samfundslitteratur Press: 121–38.
——— (ed) (2015) The Routledge Encyclopedia of Interpreting Studies. London, Routledge.
——— (2016) Introducing Interpreting Studies. London/New York, Routledge.
Pöllabauer, Sonja, and Iris Topolovec (2020) “Ethics in public service interpreting”, in The Routledge
Handbook of Translation and Ethics, Kaisa Koskinen, and Nike K. Pokorn (eds). London/New York,
Routledge: 211–26.
Pym, Anthony (2003) “Trial, error and experimentation in the training of translation teachers”, Inter-
cultural Studies Group, University of Rovira I Virgili, Tarragaona, Spain. URL: https://usuaris.
tinet.cat/apym/on-l ine/t raining/t rialanderror.pdf (accessed 8 February 2021).
Radanović Felberg, Tatjana (2013) “Training course facilitators for an introductory course in commu-
nication via interpreters for public service employees”, in Wadensjö (ed): 55–66.
Roberts, P. Roda (1997) “Community interpreting today and tomorrow”, in The Critical Link: Inter-
preters in the Community. Papers from the First International Conference on Interpreting in Legal, Health,
and Social Service Settings, Silvana E. Carr, Roda P. Roberts, Aideen Dufour, and Dini Steyn (eds).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins: 7–26.
Roy, Cynthia B. (ed) (2006) New Approaches to Interpreter Education. Washington, D.C., Gallaudet
University Press.
Rudvin, Mette, and Elena Tomassini (2011) Interpreting in the Community and Workplace: A Practical
Teaching Guide. London, Palgrave Macmillan.
Sawyer, David ( 2004) Fundamental Aspects of Interpreter Education. Amsterdam/ Philadelphia, John
Benjamins.
Schweda Nicholson, Nancy ( 1994) “ Community interpreter training in the United States and
the United Kingdom: An overview of selected initiatives”, Hermes – Journal of Linguistics 12 (94):
127–39.
Scouller, Alastair M. (1988) “The training of community interpreters”, in ITI Conference – L ondon
2. Translators and Interpreters Mean Business: Proceedings of the Second Annual Conference of the Institute
of Translation and Interpreting (ITI), London, 2 9–30 April 1988, Catriona Picken (ed). London, ITI:
66–70.
Skaaden, Hanne (2013) “No set answers? Facilitating interpreter students’ learning in an experiential
approach”, in Wadensjö (ed): 1 2–27.
Skaaden, Hanne, and Tatjana Radanović Felberg (eds) (2011) Nordic Seminar on Interpreter Training and
Testing. Oslo, Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied Science.
Standards and Guidelines for Quality Assurance in the European Higher Education Area (ESG) (2015).
URL: https://w ww.hrk.de/uploads/media/ESG_German_and_English_2015.pdf (accessed 5
November 2021).
Townsley, Brooke (2011) “The training of trainers for legal interpreting and translation”, in Building
Mutual Trust: A Framework Project for Implementing EU Common Standards in Legal Interpreting and
Translating. Building Mutual Trust. London, Middlesex University: 308–18. URL: https://eprints.
mdx.ac.uk/12235/ (accessed 5 February 2021).
UNHCR Austria (ed) (2018) Handbook for Interpreters in Asylum Procedures. Berlin, Frank & Timme.
Wadensjö, Cecilia (2014) “Perspectives on role-play: Analysis, training and assessments”, The Inter-
preter and Translator Trainer 8 (3): 4 37–51.
——— (ed) (2016) Interpreter Education: Admissions Tests and Digital Platforms. Tolkutbildning –
a ntagningsprov och digitala plattformar. Stockholm, Institute for Interpreting and Translation Studies,
Stockholm University.
——— (2017) “Dialogue interpreting and the distribution of responsibility”, H ERMES – J ournal of
Language and Communication in Business 8 (14): 111–29.
427
Mira Kadrić and Sonja Pöllabauer
Academic institutions:
Faculty of Translation and Interpreting (at the University of Geneva) https://w ww.unige.ch/f ti/en/.
Institute for Interpreting and Translation Studies (at the Stockholm University) https://w ww.tolk.
su.se/.
London Metropolitan University https://w ww.londonmet.ac.uk/.
Macquarie University (in Sydney, Australia) https://w ww.mq.edu.au/.
Middlebury Institute of International Studies at Monterey (formerly known as the Monterey Institute
of International Studies) (Vermont, USA) https://w ww.middlebury.edu/institute/.
Polytechnic of Central London (now University of Westminster) https://w ww.westminster.ac.uk/.
RMIT University (in Melbourne, Australia) https://w ww.rmit.edu.au/.
University of Massachusetts Amherst https://w ww.umass.edu/.
Non-academic institutions:
AIIC (International Association of Conference Interpreters) https://a iic.org/.
Cross-Cultural Communications (CCC) (i nternational training agency in the United States for medi-
cal and community interpreting, and cultural competence) https://w ww.cultureandlanguage.net/.
Directorate-General for Interpretation of the European Union https://ec.europa.eu/i nfo/departments/
i nterpretation_en.
428
INDEX
Note: Bold page numbers refer to tables and italic page numbers refer to figures.
429
Index
430
Index
431
Index
432
Index
433
Index
434
Index
RIPLS see Readiness for Interprofessional turn-taking 246; visual accessibility 246;
Learning Scale (RIPLS) visual orientation 248–249
Risku, H. 311 sign language interpreting (SLI) 64, 140, 277;
RMIT see Royal Melbourne Institute of andragogy 280; crowded curricula 285–286;
Technology (RMIT) deaf interpreters 286; discourse mapping
Robyn, C. 72 282–283; educational preparation 278–279;
Rodriguez, J.A. 101 financial viability 286; identity, power,
role plays 283–284, 292; blended learning social justice 279–280; innovative curricula
334–337; closed 295; continued professional 281–282; post-COVID-19 288; practicum
development 293; fellow students 284–285; professional level 278; program
301–302; individual objectives 303–304; guidelines 280–281; role plays 283–284;
instructions for participants 295; issues 294; sociological issues 285; teaching 288; VMI
open 296–297; participants 299; primary 286–288
participants 299–301; professional 299; Sigrid, B.S. 245
professional knowledge 302–303; role cards situated learning 281, 283, 293
296–298; scripts 295–296; semi-professional situational vulnerability 177–178
299; setups 306; simulation 293; situated Sivunen, N. 150
learning 293; skills 294; spoken and written Skaaden, H. 18, 19, 22, 194, 201, 263
language 298–299; teachers/assessors 301; Skivenes, M. 32, 227
trainers/assessors 304–306 Skutnabb-Kangas, T. 19
Rosenberg, B.A. 92 SLI see sign language interpreting (SLI)
Rosenberg, E. 198 Smith, E. 196
Rousseau, C. 141 Smith, M.B. 244
Roy, B. 246 Sneed, K. 316
Roy, C. 67 social care 225; activity types 226;
Roy, C.B. 2, 11 confidentiality issues 230; double roles
Royal Melbourne Institute of Technology 230–234; establishment of rapport 234–236;
(RMIT) 417 false fluency situations 230; gatekeepers 228,
research method 34 236–238; issues 228–229; language policy
Rueda-Acedo, A. 110 227; literature 226–228; monolingual 232;
Runcieman, A.J. 23 professional interpreters 229–230
Russell, S.R. 282 social encounter 3, 4
social interaction 3, 22
Saenz, T.I. 209 socially-situated interaction 178, 278, 334
Sanders, M. 35, 36 social marketing 117
Sandrelli, A. 328, 329 social pragmatics: identity 64; literature 66–67;
Sasso, A. 24 opposite effect 66; positioning 65; power 65;
Sawrikar, P. 227 re-entextualisation 66; voice 66; web-based
Scheffer, T. 141, 143, 146, 149 sources 72
Schildkamp, K. 303 socialisation 110
Schipper, K. 116 societal function 273
Schön, D.A. 334 Somers, H. 100
scripts 146, 295–296 source-centered interpreting 133, 133
self-training digital tool 367 Spencer-Oatey, H. 69
semi-professional role plays 299 Sperber, D. 70, 72
Shaw, C. 345 spoken language interpreting 176
Shlesinger, M. 77 spontaneous spoken interaction 4
short message service (SMS) communications 94 spotlighting 288
sign-language 242; bilingual support 250–251; Staton, B. 19
definition 243; ethnographic projects 245; Stokoe, E. 342, 344, 345
excerpts and analysis 247; norms 243–244; Stone, C. 283
Norwegian research 253–254; participation summative assessment 293
246; physical learning environment 249–250; Suojanen, T. 114, 116
quality and translation style 245–246; roles super-diverse societies 33
and responsibility 246–247; social learning Sutherlin, G. 95
environment 251–252; talk tempo 247–248; Sutton, J. 311
textbooks and anthologies 244–245; synchronous text-only-chats 330, 332–334
435
Index
Taibi, M. 109, 111, 112, 116 TRP see transition-relevant places (TRP)
Taira, B.R. 97 Tryuk, M. 142
talk tempo 247–248 Tseng, J. 18
target-centered interpreting 133, 133 Tužinská, H. 142
Tate, G 5 Tuominen, T. 114, 116
Taylor, V.L. 2 Turner, A.M. 97, 98, 99, 100
teacher education 415–416; affective-ethical Turner, G. 5
teaching and learning 422–423; language Tyulenev, S. 112
combinations 418; methodological-operative
teaching and learning 421–422; pedagogic UN Refugee Agency (UNHCR) 140
action 420–421; qualifications 417, 419–420; United Nations High Commissioner for
sample curriculum 418; skills 419; socio- Refugees (UNHCR) 95
communicative teaching and learning 422; user-centered translation 109
subject-related teaching and learning 421;
training formats 417; trust 423–424 Van De Mieroop, D. 209
teacher training 414; see also teacher education van der Kleij, S. 141, 145
Tebble, H. 302 Van Hove, L. 209
technology-assisted approaches 89 Verliefde, S. 134
technology-mediated modalities 91–94 Veyrier, C-A. 141, 145, 149
telephone-based interpreting 91 video relay services (VRS) 287
testing interpreting skills 293 video-mediated interpreting (VMI) 92–94,
The Interpreter Project 37 286–288
Thomas, P.A. 390 video/audio-recorded interactions 4–5
Thomassen, G. 246 Viezzi, M. 421
three-way telephone links 92 Virtual Learning Environments (VLE) 329
Thunqvist, D. 344 visual orientation 243, 248–249
Thwala, Z. 197 VLE see Virtual Learning Environments (VLE)
Tilley, N. 393 VMI see video-mediated interpreting (VMI)
Tillman, M. 145, 148 voice 66
Timarová, S. 317 voice of medicine (VoM) 195–196
Tipton, R. 142, 176, 198, 201, 269, 293 voice of the lifeworld (VoL) 195–196
Tirkkonen-Condit, S. 312 VoL see voice of the lifeworld (VoL)
TIS see Translation and Interpreting VoM see voice of medicine (VoM)
Studies (TIS) VRS see video relay services (VRS)
Tiselius, E. 316 vulnerability 175; conceptualising 175;
Tomozeiu, D. 109 corporeal 175; criminal justice system
Toury, G. 310, 312, 316 179–184; discursive (re-) construction
Townsley, B. 418 184–188; epistemic 178–179; linguistic
training interpreters: asylum context 364–365; 183; normative 177–178; perceived 186;
asylum context, France 365–367; diversified sets professional 185, 187; psychological 186;
of data 362–363; fieldwork observations 367; relational approaches 178; situational
in-person training 367–368, 368; interactions 177–178; theoretical debates 176
and participants 363–364; objectives 367; vulnerable subject 188
ODIMEDI platform 367, 374–378, 375–377;
organization of participation 368–370, 370; Wadensjö, C. 3–5, 8, 17, 22, 48, 50, 65, 66,
rule-breaking 371–373; training sessions 78–80, 84, 132, 133, 144, 145, 193, 197, 243,
373–374 262, 265, 267, 269, 270, 273, 309, 314, 318,
transcriptions 82–83, 211–213 400, 417, 419
transidiomacity 143 Wallace, M. 19, 20
transidiomatic 147 Washbourne, K. 118
transition-relevant places (TRP) 156 Watermeyer, J. 51, 197
translanguaging 132 Way, C. 108, 109
translation 84–85 Wenger, E. 64, 66, 73
Translation and Interpreting Studies (TIS) 17–18 Westlake, D. 33, 228, 234
translation machines 1 Wilson, D. 70, 72
translator agency 107–109 window of opportunity 231
translators 64 Winston, E.A. 244, 245, 246, 279, 282
436
Index
437