Competing Memes Analysis
Competing Memes Analysis
Competing Memes Analysis
Dirlam, D. K. (2003). Competing Memes Analysis. Journal of Memetics - Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission, 7.
ht:/fmogjmei/03 o7dra_khm tp/cp.r/o-mt20/ l/ilmd.tl
Abstract
Aunger (2000) and Edmonds (2002) argue that memetics is a theory without a methodology, in imminent danger of dying from lack of novel interpretations and empirical work. Edmonds challenges memeticists to conduct empirical tests. This article presents Competing Memes Analysis, an empirical methodology that can readily be applied to significant social problems. The methodology is implemented in three steps. Step 1 identifies the organization of memes within an activity. Each activity is assumed to exhibit numerous small groups of memes where each meme within a group competes with all other memes in the group and can be combined with any meme from any other group. The succession of memes that occurs with increasing experience can be a powerful clue to identifying competing memes. Step 2 collects records of activities and codes them for the presence or absence of each meme identified in Step 1. Any activity that people acquire from each other by imitation can be readily coded for the presence or absence of competing
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html 1/11
3/31/12
memes. Step 3 analyzes changing frequencies of each coded meme over time or space. Models of these changes can give useful clues to suggest empirical studies that will provide important social and scientific results. Ecologys Lotka-Volterra model of competing species illustrates the usefulness to memetics of population models. Keywords: memetic methodology, meme, drawing, writing, scientific research, Lotka-Volterra, competition
3/31/12
the fundamental questions raised within the specific conception. This occurs especially when studies have a methodological rather than a theoretical focus. Thus, a vast array of ecological, geological and genetic models depends on genetic evolution without addressing it and many mathematical models in these fields preceded evolutionary models by decades. The power of evolutionary thinking was due as much to the remarkable convergence of knowledge that supported it as to its own models. It seems likely that a future principle of the memetics of science will be that converging knowledge is a necessary predecessor to the dominance of any scientific system.
1.3 Edmonds Second Challenge: The falsifiable theory of when to use memetics
In his challenge for a falsifiable theory of when to use memetics, Edmonds rightly argues for a biological advantage of large brains independent to the hosting of memes. The issue of deciding when to apply memetics arguments, however, might be better conceived as methodological than theoretical. Though a theory might be easier to apply to ongoing social change, researchers would benefit more from operational definitions. Once several operational definitions have become established, the common elements would point to the needed theory. The ideal, operational definition would involve direct observation of imitation. Short of that ideal, indirect studies could use situations involving developmental or historical change that (a) minimize the influence of intra-individual processes and (b) allow for a high level of opportunity for imitation. One way of precluding intra-individual processes is to study only a single record per person. A high level of opportunity for imitation occurs by examining situations that contain a high probability of frequent contact between persons being studied.
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
3/11
3/31/12
3/31/12
The number of dimensions in a competing memes analysis is a matter of researcher choice and depends on the state of knowledge in the subject at the time of the study. It should be noted, however, that gathering and examining records is usually much harder than coding them. Therefore, studies with several dimensions require considerably less resources per dimension than single-dimension studies. The memes themselves are like fractals they can apply to content as fine-grained as words, lines and study locations or as general as complete discourses, complete drawings and complete research articles.
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
5/11
3/31/12
Figure 1. Best fitting Lotka-Volterra model for competing data analysis memes. Memetics skeptics would most readily accept studies that operationally define their memetic nature by setting up situations for observing imitation. However, imitation can be inferred rather than directly observed, and can be interpreted broadly as reproduction. This opens up the potential to use archival data (e.g. research reports, crime incident reports, corporate audits and so forth). A dramatic memetics success would occur if memes were discovered that effectively competed with such practices as crime among low income juveniles, self and community destructiveness among religious fundamentalists and fraud among corporate CEOs faced with poor results. Key evidence for successful competition would come from changing frequencies of memes over time or space. Beyond identifying memes, Competing Memes Analysis may involve a search for (1) the path of succession from one meme to the next, (2) the resources required, (3) the growth rate and (4) the competitive strength of such practices. Results of these types of studies may help us to understand such issues as (a) how memes that are seen as "weeds" contribute to general survival and (b) how the harmful effects of overgrowth of such memes can be controlled. An example of (a) would be marketing processes that make it possible to establish products in extremely competitive markets, but permit monopoly-like overgrowth in noncompetitive markets. Examples of(b)would be finding competitors to the emergence of monopolies ranging from antitrust laws to innovations that disrupt them. The problems solved while undertaking a rich variety of such studies could provide the sort of clarity needed to meet Edmonds challenge for a falsifiable theory of when to use memetics. Definitive answers to questions about resources, growth rate and competitive strength may be difficult to obtain without detailed experimental studies. However, once hundreds of records of memes have been systematically
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html 6/11
3/31/12
collected from a population, placed within a meaningful distribution of time or space, coded and counted, it is possible to develop models of the frequency changes. Such models can reveal characteristics of memes that are difficult to observe directly. They also make detailed, testable predictions. A model that influenced the competing memes analysis, proposed here, is the Lotka-Volterra model of species competition (see Appendix B). This model describes the population of species that compete within an ecosystem as depending on four parameters: the initial population, the maximum sustainable population, the growth rate, and the competitive strength. Given these parameter values, at least four life cycles of memes can be identified (see Appendix A). Rapid growth rate is high enough to create chaotic fluctuations in a noncompetitive environment. Data analysis modeled in Figure 1 is an example. High competitive strength reduces the rapid growth of competitors to moderate levels or less. Life c cles Default Niche Pioneering Dominant Parameter values Initial prevalence High Low Low Low Growth rate Near zero Slow Rapid Moderate Competitive strength Near zero High Low High
Table 1. Lotka-Volterra values for four memetic life cycles. Once such vital statistics of a group of memes have been worked out, scientifically significant and socially valuable experiments can be conducted to determine the factors that influence the long-term vitality of particular memes. For example, revisiting the species analogy, we note that pioneering species are often those that adapt to the presence of many predators by growing very rapidly. If such species find an environment where the predators are absent, their populations can grow so rapidly that they consume too much for the environment to sustain them. The formerly adaptive high growth results in a local extinction. It takes little imagination to realize what powerful social consequences would occur when the analogous process operates mimetically. Pioneering growth may be as essential to responding to social threats or establishing new products as it is to establish species in predatory environments. But planners need to be aware of the social or economic danger that these new responses or products pose if their memetic competitors were to suddenly vanish. In conclusion, Competing Memes Analysis provides memetics with a method for conducting precise studies of memetic processes found in any human activity. It can be summarized as a 3 step process. Step 1identifies the organization of memes within an activity. Each activity is assumed to exhibit numerous small groups of memes where each meme within a group competes with all other memes in the group and can be combined with any meme from any other group. The succession of memes that occurs with increasing experience can be a powerful clue to identifying competing groups. Step 2 collects records of activities and codes them for the presence or absence of each meme identified in Step 1. Step 3 analyzes changing frequencies of each coded meme over time or space. Models of these changes can give useful clues to suggest empirical studies that will provide important social and scientific results.
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
7/11
3/31/12
Descriptive Statistics like counts, means or correlations Microlongitudinal Repeated measurements taken weeks apart Dependent Variable Using age as a measure of how long it takes to develop
DESIGNS How often were comparable measurements taken? AGE Was it used to measure time or to assign people to groups? SOCIAL CONTEXT Who was present with the people being studied? LOCATION Where was the study done?
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
Significant Test Alone or With Other Alone or Experimenter With Test School, Home, Other, Lab, or Multiple
Unspecified
8/11
3/31/12
BACKGROUND How many fields used as sources? APPLICATIONS Who benefited from the study?
Interdisciplinary
Researchers Only
Table 2. The life-cycles and dimensions of a Competing Memes Analysis for developmental research methods.
References
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html 9/11
3/31/12
Aunger, R (2000). Conclusions. In R. Aunger (ed.), Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science, Oxford, Oxford University Press, 205-232. Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Dirlam, D. K. (1980). Classifiers and cognitive development. In S. & C. Modgil (Eds.), Toward a Theory of Psychological Development. Windsor, England: NFER Publishing, 465-498. Dirlam, D. K. (1982). Theoretical Framework. In NE NY Board of Cooperative Educational Services, The Second R. Bureau of English Education, New York State Educational Department. Dirlam, D. K. (1996). Macrodevelopmental analysis: From open fields to culture via genres of art and developmental research. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 3, 270-289. Dirlam, D. K., Gamble, K. L., & Lloyd, H. S. (1999). Modeling historical development: Fitting a competing practices system to coded archival data.Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 3, 93-111. Edmonds, B. (2002). Three Challenges for the Survival of Memetics. Journal of Memetics - Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission, 6. http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2002/vol6/edmonds_b_letter.html Kuhn, T. (1969) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press Lowenfeld, V. (1957). Creative and Mental Growth(3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Moffett, J. (1968). Teaching the Universe of Discourse. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1967). The Child's Conception of Space. New York: Norton.
Acknowledgments
Most of the integrating work for this paper was accomplished in 1997-1998 while I was a James McKeen Cattell Fellow at the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition of the University of California San Diego. I would like to thank Michael Cole for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript and for his extensive advice and counsel over the last seven years of this project. Kurt Danziger, James Moffett, and Jerry Balzano also provided important theoretical insights on the road to refining Competing Memes Analysis. The readability and usefulness of this article was greatly improved by the careful editorial commentary of Martin De Jong. Paul Marsden also offered numerous very useful suggestions. Especially, the treatment of pioneering growth was more balanced as a result of his involvement.
JoM-EMIT 2003
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
10/11
3/31/12
cfpm.org/jom-emit/2003/vol7/dirlam_dk.html
11/11