Albert Bandura: Modeling Theory
Albert Bandura: Modeling Theory
Albert Bandura: Modeling Theory
Modeling
Theory
Modeling: The Basis of Observational Learning
• For example, people in a crowd may start a riot, breaking windows and
shouting, exhibiting physical and verbal behaviors they would never
perform when alone.
• In real life, we may be more influenced by someone who appears to be similar to us than by
someone who differs from us in obvious and significant ways.
• In the laboratory, Bandura found that although children imitated the behavior of a child model in the
same room,
a child in a film,
and a filmed cartoon character,
the extent of the modeling decreased as the similarity between the model and the subject
decreased.
• The children showed greater imitation of a live model than an animated character.
• Age and Sex of Models
• Other characteristics of the model that affect imitation are age and sex.
• We are more likely to model our behavior after a person of the same sex than a person of the opposite
sex.
• Peers who appear to have successfully solved the problems we are facing are highly influential models.
• Status of Models
• For example, pedestrians are much more likely to cross a street against a red light if they see a well
dressed person crossing than if they see a poorly dressed person crossing.
• high-prestige models with athletes or celebrities who claim to use a particular product.
• Type of Behavior Displayed by Model
• The type of behavior the model perform affects the extent of imitation.
• Highly complex behaviors are not imitated as quickly and readily as simpler behaviors.
those who attended a school where the older students tended to be overweight, even obese,
gained more weight than students who attended a school where the older students were not
overweigh.
Characteristics of the observers
• Age of Observers
• In infancy,
• Infants have not yet developed the cognitive capacities (the imaginal and verbal representational
systems needed to imitate a model’s behavior some period of time after observing it.
• In infancy, it is necessary for the modeled behavior to be repeated several times after the infant’s
initial attempt to duplicate it.
• Also, the modeled behavior must be within the infant’s range of sensorimotor development.
• People who are low in self-confidence and self-esteem are much more
likely to imitate a model’s behavior than are people high in self-
confidence and self-esteem.
is more susceptible to the influence of models than a child who has not
been so reinforced.
• The reward consequences associated with the behaviors
self-reinforcement
and self-efficacy.
Self-Reinforcement
• We reward ourselves for meeting or exceeding these expectations and standards, and we
punish ourselves for our failures.
• Our past behavior may become a reference point for evaluating present behavior
and an incentive for better performance in the future.
• Failure to achieve may result in lowering the standard to a more realistic level.
• Unrealistic Performance Standards
who observed and learned behavioral expectations from unusually talented and
successful models, for example—
may continue to try to meet those excessively high expectations despite repeated
failures.
• Meeting and maintaining our performance standards enhances self-efficacy; failure to meet and
maintain them reduces it.
• Another way Bandura described self-efficacy was in terms of our perception of the control we have
over our lives.
• People strive to exercise control over events that affect their lives.
• By exerting influence in spheres over which they can command some control, they are better able to
realize desired futures and to forestall undesired ones.
• Low and High Self-Efficacy People
• low in self-efficacy feel helpless, unable to exercise control over life events.
They believe any effort they make is futile.
• When they encounter obstacles, they quickly give up if their initial attempt to deal with a problem did not work.
• People who are extremely low in self-efficacy will not even attempt to cope because they are convinced that
nothing they do will make a difference.
• People high in self-efficacy believe they can deal effectively with events and situations.
• Because they expect to succeed in overcoming obstacles, they persevere at tasks and often perform at a high level.
• High self-efficacy reduces fear of failure, raises aspirations, and improves problem solving and analytical thinking
abilities.
Sources of information about self-efficacy
• Our judgment about our self-efficacy is based on the following four sources
of information:
• • performance attainment
• • vicarious experiences
• • verbal persuasion
• Verbal persuasion, which involves simply reminding people that they have the ability to achieve whatever they want to
achieve, can enhance self-efficacy.
• This may be the most common of the four informational sources and one frequently used by parents, teachers,
spouses, coaches, friends, and therapists who say, in effect, “You can do it.”
• The fourth and final source of information about self-efficacy is physiological and emotional arousal.
• We often use this type of information as a basis for judging our ability to cope.
• We are more likely to believe we will master a problem successfully if we are not agitated, tense, or bothered by
headaches.
• Ways of Increasing Self-Efficacy Bandura concluded that certain
conditions increase self-efficacy: