Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
skip to main content
article

Judging Perceived and Traversed Distance in Virtual Environments

Published: 01 April 1998 Publication History

Abstract

The ability to accurately estimate distance is an essential component of navigating large-scale spaces. Although the factors that influence distance estimation have been a topic of research in real-world environments for decades and are well known, research on distance estimation in virtual environments (VEs) has only just begun. Initial investigations of distance estimation in VEs suggest that observers are less accurate in estimating distance in VEs than in the real world (Lampton et al., 1995). Factors influencing distance estimates may be divided into those affecting perceived distance (visual cues only) and those affecting traversed distance to include visual, cognitive, and proprioceptive cues. To assess the contribution of the various distance cues in VEs, two experiments were conducted. The first required a static observer to estimate the distance to a cylinder placed at various points along a 130-foot hallway. This experiment examined the effects of floor texture, floor pattern, and object size on distance estimates in a VE. The second experiment required a moving observer to estimate route segment distances and total route distances along four routes, each totaling 1210 feet. This experiment assessed the effects of movement method, movement speed, compensatory cues, and wall texture density. Results indicate that observers underestimate distances both in VEs and in the real world, but the underestimates are more extreme in VEs. Texture did not reliably affect the distance estimates, providing no compensation for the gross underestimates of distance in VE. Traversing a distance improves the ability to estimate that distance, but more natural means of moving via a treadmill do not necessarily improve distance estimates over traditional methods of moving in VE (e.g., using a joystick). The addition of compensatory cues (tone every 10 feet traversed on alternate route segments) improves VE distance estimation to almost perfect performance.

References

[1]
Chase, W. G. (1983). Spatial representation in taxi drivers. In D. R. Rogers and J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Acquisition of symbolic skills. New York: Plenum.
[2]
Cutting, J. E. and Vishton, P. M. (1995). Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In W. Epstein and S. J. Rogers, (Eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition: Volume 5, Perception of space and motion (pp. 69-117). New York: Academic Press.
[3]
DeMaio, J., Rinalducci, E. J., Brooks, R., & Brunderman, J. (1983). Visual cueing effectiveness: Comparison of perception and flying performance. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 27th Annual Meeting, 928-932.
[4]
Denton, G. G. (1980). The influence of visual pattern on perceived speed. Perception, 9, 393-402.
[5]
Eggleston, R. G., Janson, W. P., & Aldrich, K. A. (1996). Virtual reality system affects on size-distance judgements. Proceedings of the IEEE Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium (VRAIS '96), 139-146.
[6]
Elliot, D. (1987). The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation, Journal of Motor Behavior, 19(4), 476-485.
[7]
Galantner, E., & Galantner, P. (1970). Range estimates of distant visual stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 14(2), 301- 306.
[8]
Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 473-482.
[9]
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception . Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
[10]
Gilliam (1995). The perception of spatial layout from static optical information. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.), Perception of Space and Motion. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.
[11]
Gillner, S., & Mallot (in press). Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge of a virtual maze. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. An earlier version of the paper is available at http://www.mpik-tueb.mpg.de as Technical Report No. 45.
[12]
Goldin, S. E. and Thorndyke, P. W. (1981) Simulating navigation for spatial knowledge acquisition. Human Factors, 24(4) 457-471.
[13]
Golledge, R. G. (1991). Cognition of physical and built environments. In Garling, T. R. & Evans, W. (Eds.), Environment, cognition, and action: An integrated approach (pp. 35-62). New York: Oxford University Press.
[14]
Haber, R. N. (1985). Toward a theory of perceived spatial layout of scenes. Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing 31, 282-321.
[15]
Hale, J. P., & Dittmar, M. L. (1994). Virtual reality as a human factors design tool for architectural spaces--Control rooms to space stations I: Objective measures. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting, 275-279.
[16]
Holway, A. H., & Boring, E. G. (1941). Determinants of apparent visual size with distance variant. American Journal of Psychology, 54, 21-37.
[17]
James, K. R., & Caird, J. K. (1995). The effects of optic flow, proprioception, and texture on novice locomotion in virtual environments. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 39th Annual Meeting, 1405-1409.
[18]
Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., & Golledge, R. G. (1997). Updating an egocentric spatial representation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Manuscript submitted for publication.
[19]
Kleiss, J. A., & Hubbard, D. C. (1993). Effects of three types of simulator visual scene detail on detection of altitude change. Human Factors, 35(4), 653-672.
[20]
Kolasinski, E. M. (1995). Simulator sickness in virtual environments (ARI Technical Report 1027). Alexandria, VA: US Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences.
[21]
Lampton, D. R., Singer, M. J., McDonald, D., & Bliss, J. P. (1995). Distance estimation in virtual environments. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 39th Annual Meeting, 1268-1272.
[22]
Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5(4), 437-459.
[23]
Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 906-921.
[24]
Montello, D. R. (1991). The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 101-122.
[25]
Okabe, A., Aoki, K., & Hamamoto, W. (1986). Distance and direction judgment in a large-scale natural environment: Effects of a slope and winding trail. Environment and Behavior, 18(6) 755-772.
[26]
Patterson, R., & Martin, W. L. (1992). Human stereopsis. Human Factors, 34, 669-692.
[27]
Pausch, R., Crea, T., & Conway, M. (1993). A literature survey for virtual environments: Military flight simulator visual systems and simulator sickness. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1(3), 344-363.
[28]
Psotka, J., Davison, S. A., & Lewis, S. A. (1993). Exploring immersion in virtual space. Virtual Reality Systems, 1(2), 70-82.
[29]
Regan, C. (1995). An investigation into nausea and other side-effects of head-coupled immersive virtual reality. Virtual Reality: Research, Development and Applications, 1(1), 17-32.
[30]
Rieser, J. J., Ashmead, D. H., Talor, C. R., & Youngquist, G. A. (1990). Visual perception and guidance of locomotion without vision to previously seen targets. Perception 19, 675-689.
[31]
Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 480- 497.
[32]
Rinalducci, E. J., De Maio, J., Patterson, M. J., & Brooks, R. (1983). Psychophysical assessment of simulator visual displays. Proceedings of the Ohio State Aviation Psychology Symposium .
[33]
Rinalducci, E. J. (1994). Characteristics of visual fidelity in the virtual environment. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 5(3), 330-345.
[34]
Robinett, W., & Rolland, J. P. (1992). A computational model for stereoscopic optics of a head-mounted display. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1(1), 45-62.
[35]
Rushton, S., & Wann, J. (1993). Problems in perception and action in virtual worlds. In Virtual Reality: Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference on Virtual Reality (pp. 43-55). Westport, CT: Meckler Ltd.
[36]
Sadalla, E. K., & Magel, S. G. (1980). The perception of traversed distance. Environment and Behavior, 12(1), 65-79.
[37]
Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. J. (1980a). The perception of traversed distance: Intersections. Environment and Behavior, 12(2), 167-182.
[38]
Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. J. (1980b). An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment and Behavior, 12(2), 183-193.
[39]
Siegel, A. W. (1981). The externalization of cognitive maps by children and adults: In search of ways to ask better questions. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, & N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior across the life span: Theory and application (pp. 167-194). New York: Academic Press.
[40]
Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1988). The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: Does accuracy of short-term memory for spatial location exist? Journal of Motor Behavior, 20(4), 399-415.
[41]
Stevens, S. S. (1986). Psychophysics. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. Original edition (1975) Psychophysics: Introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. John Wiley, New York.
[42]
Surdick, T. R., Davis, E. T., King, R. A., Corso, G. M., Shapiro, A., Hodges, L., & Elliot, K. (1994). Relevant cues for the visual perception of depth: Is where you see it where it is? Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting, 1305-1309.
[43]
Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 427- 443.
[44]
Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1980). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation (Report N-1595-ONR). Santa Monica, CA: The Rand Corporation.
[45]
Warren, R. (1990). Preliminary questions for the study of ego-motion. In R. Warren and A. H. Werthhiem (Eds.), Perception and control of self motion (pp. 3-32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[46]
Wickens, C. D. (1990). Three-dimensional stereoscopic display implementation: Guidelines derived from human visual capabilities . SPIE proceedings Vol. 1256, Santa Clara, CA.
[47]
Wickens, C. D., Todd, S., & Seidler, K. (1989). Three-dimensional displays: Perception, implementation, and applications (CSERIAC SOAR 89-001 Technical report, AFAAMRL). Wright-Patterson AFB, Ohio.
[48]
Witmer, B. G., & Singer, M. (1994). Measuring presence in virtual environments (ARI Technical Report 1014). Alexandria, VA: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. (AD A286 183).
[49]
Wolpert, L. (1990). Field-of-view information for self-motion perception. In R. Warren and A. H. Werthhiem (Eds.), Perception and control of self motion (pp. 101-126). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[50]
Wright, R. H. (1995). Virtual reality psychophysics: Forward and lateral distance, height and speed perceptions with a wide-angle helmet display (ARI Technical Report 1025). Alexandria, VA: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences.

Cited By

View all
  • (2024)Exploring the Influence of Avatar Skin Tone in VR Educational GamesCompanion Proceedings of the 2024 Annual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in Play10.1145/3665463.3678799(227-234)Online publication date: 14-Oct-2024
  • (2024)The geometry of the vergence-accommodation conflict in mixed reality systemsVirtual Reality10.1007/s10055-024-00991-428:2Online publication date: 11-Apr-2024
  • (2023)Distance Perception in Virtual Reality: A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Head-Mounted Display CharacteristicsIEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics10.1109/TVCG.2022.319660629:12(4978-4989)Online publication date: 1-Dec-2023
  • Show More Cited By
  1. Judging Perceived and Traversed Distance in Virtual Environments

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Information & Contributors

    Information

    Published In

    cover image Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments
    Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments  Volume 7, Issue 2
    April 1998
    124 pages

    Publisher

    MIT Press

    Cambridge, MA, United States

    Publication History

    Published: 01 April 1998

    Qualifiers

    • Article

    Contributors

    Other Metrics

    Bibliometrics & Citations

    Bibliometrics

    Article Metrics

    • Downloads (Last 12 months)0
    • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)0
    Reflects downloads up to 13 Jan 2025

    Other Metrics

    Citations

    Cited By

    View all
    • (2024)Exploring the Influence of Avatar Skin Tone in VR Educational GamesCompanion Proceedings of the 2024 Annual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in Play10.1145/3665463.3678799(227-234)Online publication date: 14-Oct-2024
    • (2024)The geometry of the vergence-accommodation conflict in mixed reality systemsVirtual Reality10.1007/s10055-024-00991-428:2Online publication date: 11-Apr-2024
    • (2023)Distance Perception in Virtual Reality: A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Head-Mounted Display CharacteristicsIEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics10.1109/TVCG.2022.319660629:12(4978-4989)Online publication date: 1-Dec-2023
    • (2023)Acquisition and retention of spatial knowledge through virtual reality experiencesInternational Journal of Human-Computer Studies10.1016/j.ijhcs.2023.103067177:COnline publication date: 26-Jul-2023
    • (2021)Using Audio Reverberation to Compensate Distance Compression in Virtual RealityACM Symposium on Applied Perception 202110.1145/3474451.3476236(1-10)Online publication date: 16-Sep-2021
    • (2021)Stay on Course in VR: Comparing the Precision of Movement between Gamepad, Armswinger, and TreadmillProceedings of Mensch und Computer 202110.1145/3473856.3473880(354-365)Online publication date: 5-Sep-2021
    • (2021)Measuring Presence in Virtual Environments: A SurveyACM Computing Surveys10.1145/346681754:8(1-37)Online publication date: 4-Oct-2021
    • (2021)The Challenges in Modeling Human Performance in 3D Space with Fitts’ LawExtended Abstracts of the 2021 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems10.1145/3411763.3443442(1-9)Online publication date: 8-May-2021
    • (2021)Monoscopic vs. Stereoscopic Views and Display Types in the Teleoperation of Unmanned Ground Vehicles for Object Avoidance2021 30th IEEE International Conference on Robot & Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN)10.1109/RO-MAN50785.2021.9515455(418-425)Online publication date: 8-Aug-2021
    • (2021)Contributions of pictorial and binocular cues to the perception of distance in virtual realityVirtual Reality10.1007/s10055-021-00500-x25:4(1087-1103)Online publication date: 1-Dec-2021
    • Show More Cited By

    View Options

    View options

    Media

    Figures

    Other

    Tables

    Share

    Share

    Share this Publication link

    Share on social media