Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
skip to main content
article

Early cross-modal plasticity in adults

Published: 01 March 2017 Publication History

Abstract

It is known that, after a prolonged period of visual deprivation, the adult visual cortex can be recruited for nonvisual processing, reflecting cross-modal plasticity. Here, we investigated whether cross-modal plasticity can occur at short timescales in the typical adult brain by comparing the interaction between vision and touch during binocular rivalry before and after a brief period of monocular deprivation, which strongly alters ocular balance favoring the deprived eye. While viewing dichoptically two gratings of orthogonal orientation, participants were asked to actively explore a haptic grating congruent in orientation to one of the two rivalrous stimuli. We repeated this procedure before and after 150 min of monocular deprivation. We first confirmed that haptic stimulation interacted with vision during rivalry promoting dominance of the congruent visuo-haptic stimulus and that monocular deprivation increased the deprived eye and decreased the nondeprived eye dominance. Interestingly, after deprivation, we found that the effect of touch did not change for the nondeprived eye, whereas it disappeared for the deprived eye, which was potentiated after deprivation. The absence of visuo-haptic interaction for the deprived eye lasted for over 1 hr and was not attributable to a masking induced by the stronger response of the deprived eye as confirmed by a control experiment. Taken together, our results demonstrate that the adult human visual cortex retains a high degree of cross-modal plasticity, which can occur even at very short timescales.

References

[1]
Alais, D. (2012). Binocular rivalry: Competition and inhibition in visual perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 87-103.
[2]
Alais, D., & Blake, R. (2005). Binocular rivalry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[3]
Alsius, A., & Munhall, K. G. (2013). Detection of audiovisual speech correspondences without visual awareness. Psychological Science, 24, 423-431.
[4]
Amedi, A., Raz, N., Azulay, H., Malach, R., & Zohary, E. (2010). Cortical activity during tactile exploration of objects in blind and sighted humans. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 143-156.
[5]
Berardi, N., Pizzorusso, T., & Maffei, L. (2000). Critical periods during sensory development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 138-145.
[6]
Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 13-21.
[7]
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436.
[8]
Clavagnier, S., Falchier, A., & Kennedy, H. (2004). Long-distance feedback projections to area V1: Implications for multisensory integration, spatial awareness, and visual consciousness. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 117-126.
[9]
Collignon, O., Vandewalle, G., Voss, P., Albouy, G., Charbonneau, G., Lassonde, M., et al. (2011). Functional specialization for auditory-spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 108, 4435-4440.
[10]
Conrad, V., Bartels, A., Kleiner, M., & Noppeney, U. (2010). Audiovisual interactions in binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 10, 27.
[11]
Conrad, V., Kleiner, M., Bartels, A., Hartcher O'Brien, J., Bulthoff, H. H., & Noppeney, U. (2013). Naturalistic stimulus structure determines the integration of audiovisual looming signals in binocular rivalry. PLoS One, 8, e70710.
[12]
Convento, S., Vallar, G., Galantini, C., & Bolognini, N. (2013). Neuromodulation of early multisensory interactions in the visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 685-696.
[13]
D'Ausilio, A. (2012). Arduino: A low-cost multipurpose lab equipment. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 305-313.
[14]
Deroy, O., Faivre, N., Lunghi, C., Spence, C., Aller, M., & Noppeney, U. (2016). The complex interplay between multisensory integration and perceptual awareness. Multisensory Research, 29, 585-606.
[15]
Falchier, A., Clavagnier, S., Barone, P., & Kennedy, H. (2002). Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 5749-5759.
[16]
Fuchs, E., & Flugge, G. (2014). Adult neuroplasticity: More than 40 years of research. Neural Plasticity, 2014, 541870.
[17]
Gougoux, F., Zatorre, R. J., Lassonde, M., Voss, P., & Lepore, F. (2005). A functional neuroimaging study of sound localization: Visual cortex activity predicts performance in early-blind individuals. PLoS Biology, 3, e27.
[18]
Hamilton, R. H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (1998). Cortical plasticity associated with Braille learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 168-174.
[19]
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology, 206, 419-436.
[20]
Kauffman, T., Theoret, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Braille character discrimination in blindfolded human subjects. NeuroReport, 13, 571-574.
[21]
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., Pelli, D., Ingling, A., Murray, R., & Broussard, C. (2007). What's new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception, 36, 1.
[22]
Levelt, W. J. M. (1965). On binocular rivalry. Soesterberg, The Netherlands: IZF.
[23]
Lunghi, C., & Alais, D. (2013). Touch interacts with vision during binocular rivalry with a tight orientation tuning. PLoS One, 8, e58754.
[24]
Lunghi, C., & Alais, D. (2015). Congruent tactile stimulation reduces the strength of visual suppression during binocular rivalry. Scientific Reports, 5, 9413.
[25]
Lunghi, C., Berchicci, M., Morrone, M. C., & Di Russo, F. (2015). Short-term monocular deprivation alters early components of visual evoked potentials. Journal of Physiology, 593, 4361-4372.
[26]
Lunghi, C., Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2010). Touch disambiguates rivalrous perception at early stages of visual analysis. Current Biology, 20, R143-R144.
[27]
Lunghi, C., Burr, D. C., & Morrone, C. (2011). Brief periods of monocular deprivation disrupt ocular balance in human adult visual cortex. Current Biology, 21, R538-R539.
[28]
Lunghi, C., Burr, D. C., & Morrone, M. C. (2013). Long-term effects of monocular deprivation revealed with binocular rivalry gratings modulated in luminance and in color. Journal of Vision, 13, 1.
[29]
Lunghi, C., Emir, U. E., Morrone, M. C., & Bridge, H. (2015). Short-term monocular deprivation alters GABA in the adult human visual cortex. Current Biology, 25, 1496-1501.
[30]
Lunghi, C., & Morrone, M. C. (2013). Early interaction between vision and touch during binocular rivalry. Multisensory Research, 26, 291-306.
[31]
Lunghi, C., Morrone, M. C., & Alais, D. (2014). Auditory and tactile signals combine to influence vision during binocular rivalry. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 784-792.
[32]
Maffei, A., Nelson, S. B., & Turrigiano, G. G. (2004). Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivation. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1353-1359.
[33]
Maffei, A., & Turrigiano, G. G. (2008). Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4377-4384.
[34]
Merabet, L. B., Hamilton, R., Schlaug, G., Swisher, J. D., Kiriakopoulos, E. T., Pitskel, N. B., et al. (2008). Rapid and reversible recruitment of early visual cortex for touch. PLoS One, 3, e3046.
[35]
Merabet, L. B., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Neural reorganization following sensory loss: The opportunity of change. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 44-52.
[36]
Merabet, L. B., Swisher, J. D., McMains, S. A., Halko, M. A., Amedi, A., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2007). Combined activation and deactivation of visual cortex during tactile sensory processing. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 1633-1641.
[37]
Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F., & Merabet, L. B. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 377-401.
[38]
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442.
[39]
Qin, W., & Yu, C. (2013). Neural pathways conveying novisual information to the visual cortex. Neural Plasticity, 2013, 864920.
[40]
Rockland, K. S., & Ojima, H. (2003). Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 19-26.
[41]
Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibañez, V., Deiber, M. P., Dold, G., et al. (1996). Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature, 380, 526-528.
[42]
Saito, D. N., Okada, T., Honda, M., Yonekura, Y., & Sadato, N. (2006). Practice makes perfect: The neural substrates of tactile discrimination by Mah-Jong experts include the primary visual cortex. BMC Neuroscience, 7, 79.
[43]
Salomon, R., Kaliuzhna, M., Herbelin, B., & Blanke, O. (2015). Balancing awareness: Vestibular signals modulate visual consciousness in the absence of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 289-297.
[44]
Salomon, R., Lim, M., Herbelin, B., Hesselmann, G., & Blanke, O. (2013). Posing for awareness: Proprioception modulates access to visual consciousness in a continuous flash suppression task. Journal of Vision, 13, 2.
[45]
Sasieni, L. (1978). The Frisby stereotest. Optician, 176, 7-10.
[46]
Sathian, K. (2005). Visual cortical activity during tactile perception in the sighted and the visually deprived. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 279-286.
[47]
Snow, J. C., Strother, L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). Haptic shape processing in visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 1154-1167.
[48]
Sterzer, P., Stein, T., Ludwig, K., Rothkirch, M., & Hesselmann, G. (2014). Neural processing of visual information under interocular suppression: A critical review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 453.
[49]
Striem-Amit, E., Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., & Amedi, A. (2012). Reading with sounds: Sensory substitution selectively activates the visual word form area in the blind. Neuron, 76, 640-652.
[50]
Tong, F., Meng, M., & Blake, R. (2006). Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 502-511.
[51]
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1096-1101.
[52]
van Loon, A. M., Knapen, T., Scholte, H. S., St John-Saaltink, E., Donner, T. H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). GABA shapes the dynamics of bistable perception. Current Biology, 23, 823-827.
[53]
Wang, L., Fontanini, A., & Maffei, A. (2012). Experience-dependent switch in sign and mechanisms for plasticity in layer 4 of primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 10562-10573.
[54]
Zhou, W., Jiang, Y., He, S., & Chen, D. (2010). Olfaction modulates visual perception in binocular rivalry. Current Biology, 20, 1356-1358.

Cited By

View all
  • (2024)All rivers run into the sea: Unified Modality Brain-Inspired Emotional Central MechanismProceedings of the 32nd ACM International Conference on Multimedia10.1145/3664647.3681228(632-641)Online publication date: 28-Oct-2024
  1. Early cross-modal plasticity in adults

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Information & Contributors

    Information

    Published In

    cover image Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience  Volume 29, Issue 3
    March 2017
    179 pages
    ISSN:0898-929X
    EISSN:1530-8898
    Issue’s Table of Contents

    Publisher

    MIT Press

    Cambridge, MA, United States

    Publication History

    Published: 01 March 2017

    Qualifiers

    • Article

    Contributors

    Other Metrics

    Bibliometrics & Citations

    Bibliometrics

    Article Metrics

    • Downloads (Last 12 months)0
    • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)0
    Reflects downloads up to 31 Jan 2025

    Other Metrics

    Citations

    Cited By

    View all
    • (2024)All rivers run into the sea: Unified Modality Brain-Inspired Emotional Central MechanismProceedings of the 32nd ACM International Conference on Multimedia10.1145/3664647.3681228(632-641)Online publication date: 28-Oct-2024

    View Options

    View options

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share this Publication link

    Share on social media