Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:08:58.814Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurobiological basis of psychopathy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

R. James R. Blair*
Affiliation:
Unit on Affective Cognitive Neuroscience, Mood and Anxiety Program, National Institute of Mental Health, 15K North Drive, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2003 

To understand a psychiatric disorder we need to know why the pathology causes the behavioural disturbance, the neural structures implicated in the pathology and the cause of the dysfunction in the neural structures. With regard to psychopathy, we have clear indications regarding why the pathology gives rise to the emotional and behavioural disturbance and important insights into the neural systems implicated in this pathology. What remains unclear is why these neural systems are dysfunctional.

WHAT IS PSYCHOPATHY?

Psychopathy is a disorder, defined by Hare's Psychopathy Checklist — Revised (PCL—R) and characterised in part by a diminished capacity for remorse and poor behavioural controls (Reference HareHare, 1991). Classifications of psychopathy are not synonymous with diagnoses of conduct disorder or antisocial personality disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) but represent an extension. These psychiatric diagnoses are poorly specified and concentrate on the antisocial behaviour. Because of this imprecision, the diagnostic rate of conduct disorder is 16% of boys in mainstream education (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and over 80% for antisocial personality disorder in adult forensic institutions (Reference Hart and HareHart & Hare, 1996). In contrast, psychopathy is defined not only by antisocial behaviour but also by emotional impairment such as the lack of guilt. Only onethird of those who are diagnosed with antisocial personality disorder meet criteria for psychopathy (Reference Hart and HareHart & Hare, 1996). Moreover, a diagnosis of psychopathy, unlike antisocial personality disorder, is informative regarding a patient's future risk (Reference HareHare, 1991).

A striking feature of much of the antisocial behaviour shown by individuals with psychopathy is that it is mostly instrumental in nature, i.e. goal-directed towards achieving money, sexual opportunities or increased status (Reference Cornell, Warren and HawkCornell et al, 1996). This suggests that the pathology associated with psychopathy interferes with socialisation. It is well known that empathy-inducing, positive parenting practices give rise to less antisocial behaviour than punishment-based, negative parenting practices. This relationship is shown in healthy developing children as well as in children with conduct disorder who do not present with the emotional dysfunction of psychopathy. However, for children with conduct disorder who do present with emotional dysfunction, there is no relationship between parenting behaviours and level of antisocial behaviour (Reference Wootton, Frick and SheltonWootton et al, 1997). In other words, the emotional impairment found in individuals with psychopathy interferes with socialisation such that the individual does not learn to avoid antisocial behaviour.

Socialisation involves aversive conditioning and instrumental learning. In order to learn that hitting another is bad, this thought must be associated with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (e.g. the distress of the victim). Similarly, learning to avoid committing moral transgressions involves committing a moral transgression and then being ‘punished’ by the aversive response of the victim's distress (Reference BlairBlair, 1995). Individuals with psychopathy present with severe difficulties in both aversive conditioning and instrumental learning (Reference PatrickPatrick, 1994; Reference BlairBlair, 2001). Moreover, they have particular difficulties processing the fearfulness and sadness of others (Reference BlairBlair, 2001).

THE NEURAL BASIS OF PSYCHOPATHY

The amygdala is involved in aversive conditioning and instrumental learning (Reference LeDouxLeDoux, 1998). It is also involved in the response to fearful and sad facial expressions (Reference Blair, Morris and FrithBlair et al, 1999). The amygdala is thus involved in all the processes that, when impaired, give rise to the functional impairments shown by individuals with psychopathy. It is therefore suggested that amygdala dysfunction is one of the core neural systems implicated in the pathology of psychopathy (Reference PatrickPatrick, 1994; Reference Blair, Morris and FrithBlair et al, 1999).

Interestingly, two recent neuroimaging studies have confirmed that amygdala dysfunction is associated with psychopathy (Reference Tiihonen, Hodgins and VaurioTiihonen et al, 2000; Reference Kiehl, Smith and HareKiehl et al, 2001). Thus, Tiihonen et al (Reference Tiihonen, Hodgins and Vaurio2000) used volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to explore the relationship between amygdaloid volume and degree of psychopathy in violent offenders as measured by the PCL-R. They found that high levels of psychopathy were associated with reduced amygdaloid volume. Kiehl et al (Reference Kiehl, Smith and Hare2001) used functional MRI to examine neural responses in individuals with high (>28/40) and low (<23/40) scores on the PCL-R during an emotional memory task where the participant processed words of neutral and negative valence. Kiehl et al found a reduced amygdala response in the high-scoring group, relative to the low-scoring group, during the processing of words of negative valence.

There have been suggestions that other neural systems are dysfunctional in individuals with psychopathy. Thus, on the basis of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings for violent offenders, it has been argued that the frontal cortex could be dysfunctional (Reference Morgan and LilienfieldMorgan & Lilienfield, 2000; Reference Soderstrom, Tullberg and WikkelsoSoderstrom et al, 2000). However, all of these studies, with the exception of one by (Reference Raine, Lencz and BihrleRaine et al, 2000), have been with violent offenders rather than individuals with psychopathy. This is noteworthy as there are crucial differences between the general population of violent offenders and these individuals. Indeed, neuropsychological work with individuals with psychopathy, unlike work with individuals who are violent, has repeatedly found frontal functioning to be intact (Reference Kandel and FreedKandel & Freed, 1989).

The volumetric MRI study conducted by Raine et al (Reference Raine, Lencz and Bihrle2000) did assess individuals scoring highly on the PCL-R. This study reported reduced prefrontal grey, but not white, matter volume in these individuals. However, Raine et al were unable to differentiate between grey matter from different regions of the frontal cortex. This is unfortunate, as although it appears clear that there is no generalised frontal cortical dysfunction in individuals with psychopathy there is one region of the frontal cortex that could be impaired, i.e. the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC).

The OFC, especially the medial OFC, receives extensive projections from, and sends extensive projections to, the amygdala. Moreover, the medial OFC is involved in instrumental learning and response reversal — both of which functions are impaired in individuals with psychopathy (Reference LaPierre, Braun and HodginsLaPierre et al, 1995). In addition, there have been attempts to link the disorder of psychopathy with the neurological condition of ‘acquired sociopathy’ following lesions of the OFC (Reference Anderson, Bechara and DamasioAnderson et al, 1999). Patients with ‘acquired sociopathy’ can present with socially inappropriate behaviour and aggression following their lesion. However, if aggression is seen it is almost exclusively reactive (i.e. frustration/threat-induced) in nature and not the instrumental goal-directed aggression shown by individuals with psychopathy. Indeed, it has been argued that the extensive damage seen in those patients with ‘acquired sociopathy’ who present with reactive aggression includes regions of the lateral OFC that are implicated in modulating basic brain-stem mechanisms that mediate the fight-flight response to threat (Reference BlairBlair, 2001). This modulatory function, and by implication this region, of the OFC is not thought to be dysfunctional in individuals with psychopathy.

Thus, it is suggested that the neural structures implicated in psychopathic pathology include the amygdala and OFC. The basic causes of the pathology remain unclear; however, there are interesting possibilities. One is that pathology in the noradrenergic system can lead to the observed amygdala dysfunction. Certainly, administration of the β-adrenergic blocker propranolol, as well as amygdala damage, blocks the improvement in episodic memory for emotionally arousing events, as well as disrupting the processing of sad facial expressions (Reference Harmer, Perrett and CowenHarmer et al, 2001). A second possibility concerns the potential value of genetic information and mouse models. Many mouse models of aggression concentrate on a heightened reactively aggressive response to an intruder. However, a recent mouse model has been developed without a functional ‘tailless’ protein. The gene tailless is a member of the super-family of genes that encode transcription factors of the ligand-activated nuclear receptor type, and is expressed in the invertebrate and vertebrate brain. Interestingly, mice lacking a functional tailless protein show reduced amygdaloid volume. Moreover, they show heightened aggression even to litter-mates and the females lack normal maternal instincts (Reference Monaghan, Bock and GassMonaghan et al, 1997). Although it is difficult currently to extrapolate from these data to an understanding of psychopathy, the study of such animals should allow a molecular approach to understanding the genetic architecture of the forebrain. These data will undoubtedly be invaluable in furthering our understanding of psychopathy.

It is important to make one final point. The lifestyle of the individual with psychopathy may exacerbate any neurobiological impairments. One feature associated with psychopathy is substance misuse. This could be contributing to apparent impairments. For example chronic amphetamine misuse has been shown to lead to disturbance in functions mediated by the OFC (Reference Rogers, Everitt and BaldacchinoRogers et al, 1999). In this context, it is interesting to note that although performance on measures thought to require amygdala involvement is impaired substantially in both adults with psychopathy and children with psychopathic tendencies, performance on measures thought to require the OFC is impaired substantially only in the adults (Reference Blair, Colledge and MitchellBlair et al, 2001). This, of course, could reflect the developmental course of the disorder. However, alternatively, it could reflect the lifestyle of the individual with psychopathy where substance misuse has interacted with the fundamental pathology to produce additional OFC pathology.

CONCLUSIONS

The suggestion is that the pathology of individuals with psychopathy detrimentally affects two processes that are required for socialisation, i.e. aversive conditioning and instrumental learning. This is thought to result from amygdala dysfunction potentially compounded by OFC dysfunction. However, the reasons for this dysfunction remain unknown. The noradrenergic system could be implicated but why this system should be dysfunctional is, again, unknown. An answer is likely to follow an understanding of the genetic architecture and morphogenesis of the forebrain.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., et al (1999) Impairment of social and moral behaviour related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 10321037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (1995) A cognitive developmental approach to mortality: investigating the psychopathy. Cognition, 57, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2001) Neuro-cognitive models of aggression, the Antisocial Personality Disorders and Psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71, 727731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., et al (1999) Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain, 122, 883893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E. & Mitchell, D. (2001) Somatic markers and response reversal: is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 499511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., et al (1996) Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 783790.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised. Toronto, Ontario: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Harmer, C. J., Perrett, D. I., Cowen, P. J., et al (2001) Administration of the beta-adrenoceptor blocker propranolol impairs the processing of facial expressions of sadness. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 154, 383389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, S. D. & Hare, R. D. (1996) Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 9, 129132.Google Scholar
Kandel, E. & Freed, D. (1989) Frontal lobe dysfunction and antisocial behavior: a review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 404413.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., et al (2001) Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677684.Google Scholar
LaPierre, D., Braun, C. M. J. & Hodgins, S. (1995) Ventral frontal deficits in psychopathy: Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychologia, 33, 139151.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1998) The Emotional Brain. New York: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Monaghan, A. P., Bock, D., Gass, P., et al (1997) Defective limbic system in mice lacking the tailless gene. Nature, 390, 515517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, A. B. & Lilienfield, S. O. (2000) A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113136.Google Scholar
Patrick, C. J. (1994) Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. Psychophysiology, 31, 319330.Google Scholar
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., et al (2000) Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 119127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., Everitt, B. J., Baldacchino, A., et al (1999) Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 322339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soderstrom, H., Tullberg, M., Wikkelso, C., et al (2000) Reduced regional cerebral blood flow in non-psychotic violent offenders. Psychiatry Research, 98, 2941.Google Scholar
Tiihonen, J., Hodgins, S., Vaurio, O., et al (2000) Amygdaloid volume loss in psychopathy. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 2017.Google Scholar
Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., et al (1997) Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 292300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.