Key Points
-
The concept of working memory assumes that a limited capacity system temporarily stores information and thereby supports human thought processes. One prevalent model of working memory comprises three components: a central executive, a verbal storage system called the phonological loop, and a visual storage system called the visuospatial sketchpad.
-
The phonological loop consists of a store that can hold memory traces for a few seconds, and an articulatory rehearsal process. Retrieval and re-articulation are used to refresh memory traces, and the span of working memory is limited by the amount of material that can be articulated before the first item fades from the store. Word length and similarity between items strongly influence performance on tests of verbal working memory.
-
Various models have been proposed to account for how serial order is remembered. In chaining models, each item is a cue for the next item, but these models run into problems with sequences in which an item recurs and with similarity effects. Contextual models assume that successive items are associated with a contextual cue or that recall of order is based on positional associations between items.
-
It has been proposed that the phonological loop evolved to facilitate the acquisition of language. In support of this, phonological loop capacity is a good predictor of second language learning. Future studies seem likely to link the phonological loop more closely to theories of language perception and production.
-
The visuospatial sketchpad is less well understood than the phonological loop. Spatial and visual working memory also have limited capacity and the two can be dissociated. Visuospatial working memory predicts success in fields such as architecture and engineering.
-
One model proposes that the sketchpad is divided into two components, a visual cache and a retrieval and rehearsal process called the 'inner scribe', analogous to the storage and articulatory components of the phonological loop.
-
The central executive is the least understood component of working memory. In one model, control is divided between 'automatic' habits or schemas, and an attentional system called the supervisory activating system that intervenes to overrule habitual control.
-
To move our concept of the supervisory activating system beyond a simple 'homunculus', we need to specify the processes attributed to it, and then to explain them. The processes needed are to focus, divide and switch attention, and to connect working memory with long-term memory. To account for the latter capacity, a fourth component of working memory has been proposed: the episodic buffer, a limited capacity store that binds information to form integrated episodes.
-
Neuroimaging and neuropsychology have provided evidence for localization of the components of working memory. The phonological loop is associated with the left temporoparietal region, and visuospatial working memory with analogous areas in the right hemisphere. The central executive is probably associated with the frontal lobes.
-
Work on the phonological loop, the visuospatial sketchpad and the central executive should be more closely linked to studies on language, visual processing and motor control, and executive control, respectively. In addition, more work is needed on what drives working memory.
Abstract
The concept of working memory proposes that a dedicated system maintains and stores information in the short term, and that this system underlies human thought processes. Current views of working memory involve a central executive and two storage systems: the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad. Although this basic model was first proposed 30 years ago, it has continued to develop and to stimulate research and debate. The model and the most recent results are reviewed in this article.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Andrade, J. Working Memory in Context (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 2001).
Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (eds) Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1999).
Conway, A. R. A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M. J., Miyake, A. & Towse, J. N. Variation in Working Memory (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, in the press). References 1â3 provide excellent accounts of the current states of research on working memory, with reference 2 representing a range of theoretical approaches and highlighting their common features, reference 1 containing the view of a range of young British researchers on the strengths and weaknesses of the multi-component model, and reference 3 having more North American contributors and better reflecting approaches based on individual differences.
Miyake, A. & Shah, P. in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (eds Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 28â61 (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1999).
Cowan, N. in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (eds Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 62â101 (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, 1999). An alternative approach to working memory that focuses on attentional control. This leads to a different emphasis, but is not in any fundamental sense incompatible with a multi-component model.
Nairne, J. S. A feature model of immediate memory. Mem. Cogn. 18, 251â269 (1990).
Neath, I. Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 7, 403â423 (2000).
Nairne, J. S. Remembering over the short-term: the case against the standard model. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 53â81 (2002).
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. Plans and the Structure of Behavior (Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, 1960).
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. in Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation (ed. Bower, G. A.) 47â89 (Academic, New York, 1974).
Hebb, D. O. The Organization of Behavior (Wiley, New York, 1949).
Brown, J. Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 10, 12â21 (1958).
Peterson, L. R. & Peterson, M. J. Short-term retention of individual verbal items. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 193â198 (1959).
Melton, A. W. Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 2, 1â21 (1963).
Baddeley, A. D. Human Memory: Theory and Practice 2nd edn (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 1997).
Atkinson, R. C. & Shiffrin, R. M. in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (ed. Spence, K. W.) 89â195 (Academic, New York, 1968).
Craik, F. I. M. & Lockhart, R. S. Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 11, 671â684 (1972).
Shallice, T. & Warrington, E. K. Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: a neuropsychological study. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 22, 261â273 (1970).
Vallar, G. & Papagno, C. in Handbook of Memory Disorders (eds Baddeley, A. D., Kopelman, M. D. & Wilson, B. A.) 249â270 (Wiley, Chichester, 2002). An excellent summary of the implications of the study of phonological working memory of neuropsychological patient studies.
Conrad, R. Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. B. J. Psychol. 55, 75â84 (1964).
Conrad, R. & Hull, A. J. Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. B. J. Psychol. 55, 429â432 (1964).
Baddeley, A. D. Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 362â365 (1966).
Baddeley, A. D. The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 302â309 (1966).
Baddeley, A. D. Thomson, N. & Buchanan, M. Word length and the structure of short-term memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 14, 575â589 (1975).
Murray, D. J. Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 78, 679â684 (1968).
Vallar, G. & Baddeley, A. D. Fractionation of working memory. Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 23, 151â161 (1984).
Baddeley, A. D. & Wilson, B. Phonological coding and short-term memory in patients without speech. J. Mem. Lang. 24, 490â502 (1985).
Caplan, D., Rochon, E. & Waters, G. S. Articulatory and phonological determinants of word-length effects in span tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 45, 177â192 (1992).
Henson, R. N. A. in Working Memory in Perspective (ed. Andrade, J.) 151â174 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 2001). Offers a clear and useful summary of neuroimaging research viewed from a working memory perspective.
Henson, R. N. A., Norris, D. G., Page, M. P. A. & Baddeley, A. D. Unchained memory: error patterns rule out chaining models of immediate serial recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49, 80â115 (1996).
Brown, G. D. A. Preece, T. & Hulme, C. Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychol. Rev. 107, 127â181 (2000).
Murdock, B. B. TODAM2: A model for the storage and retrieval of item, associative, and serial order information. Psychol. Rev. 100, 183â203 (1993).
Henson, R. N. A. Item repetition in short-term memory: Ranschburg repeated. J. Exp. Psychol. Learning Mem. Cogn. 24, 1162â1181 (1998).
Baddeley, A. D. How does acoustic similarity influence short-term memory? Q. J. Psychol. 20, 249â264 (1968).
Burgess, N. & Hitch, G. J. Towards a network model of the articulatory loop. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 429â460 (1992).
Burgess, N. & Hitch, G. J. Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychol. Rev. 106, 551â581 (1999). A good example of one of a range of applications of computational modelling to the study of working memory.
Henson, R. N. A. Short-term memory for serial order. The startâend model. Cogn. Psychol. 36, 73â137 (1998).
Page, M. A. & Norris, D. The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychol. Rev. 105, 761â781 (1998).
Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Chalmers, P. & Wynn, V. Group aggregates and individual reliability: the case of verbal short-term memory. Mem. Cogn. 24, 305â321 (1996). Probably the only study to attempt to quantify the magnitude and robustness of the phonological similarity and word-length effects.
Larsen, J. D. & Baddeley, A. D. Disruption of verbal STM by irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression and manual tapping: do they have a common source? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (in the press).
Neath, I. Farley, L. A. & Surprenant, A. M. Directly assessing the relationship between speech and articulatory suppression. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (in the press).
Hanley, J. R. & Bakopoulou, E. Irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression and phonological similarity: a test of the phonological loop model and the feature model. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 435â444 (2003). This study emphasizes the role of strategy in working memory, demonstrating powerful effects of strategic instructions to subjects.
Lovatt, P. & Avons, S. E. in Working Memory in Perspective (ed. Andrade, J.) 199â218 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 2001).
Brown, G. D. A. & Hulme, C. Modeling item length effects in memory span: no rehearsal needed? J. Mem. Lang. 34, 594â621 (1995).
Neath, I. & Nairne, J. S. Word-length effects in immediate memory: overwriting trace-decay theory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 429â441 (1995).
Cowan, N., Baddeley, A. D., Elliott, E. M. & Norris, J. List composition and the word length effect in immediate recall: a comparison of localist and globalist assumptions. Psychol. Bull. Rev. 10, 74â79 (2003).
Baddeley, A. D., Chincotta, D., Stafford, L. & Turk, D. Is the word length effect in STM entirely attributable to output delay? Evidence from serial recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55A, 353â369 (2002).
Dosher, B. A. & Ma, J. J. Output loss or rehearsal loop? Output-time versus pronunciation-time limits in immediate recall for forgetting-matched materials. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. 24, 316â335 (1998).
Lovatt, P., Avons, S. E. & Masterson, J. The word length effect and disyllabic words. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 53, 1â22 (2000).
Baddeley, A. D. & Andrade, J. Reversing the word-length effect: a comment on Caplan, Rochon and Waters. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 47A, 1047â1054 (1994).
Caplan, D. & Waters, G. S. Articulatory length and phonological similarity in span tasks: a reply to Baddeley and Andrade. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 47A, 1055â1062 (1994).
Service, E. Effect of word length on immediate serial recall depends on phonological complexity, not articulatory duration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 51A, 283â304 (1998).
Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E. & Meyer, D. E. Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. (in the press). The most careful and sophisticated analysis of the roles of spoken duration and phonological similarity in verbal STM, leading to a clear and simple theoretical conclusion. A good account of potential pitfalls in this area of research is also provided.
Colle, H. A. Auditory encoding in visual short-term recall: effects of noise intensity and spatial location. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 19, 722â735 (1980).
Jones, D. M. in Attention: Selection, Awareness and Control (eds Baddeley, A. D. & Weiskrantz, L.) 87â104 (Clarendon, Oxford, 1993).
Jones, D. M. & Macken, W. J. Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: implications for phonological coding in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 369â381 (1993).
Salame, P. & Baddeley, A. D. Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: implications for the structure of working memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 21, 150â164 (1982).
Colle, H. A. & Welsh, A. Acoustic masking in primary memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 15, 17â32 (1976).
Salame, P. & Baddeley, A. D. Phonological factors in STM: similarity and the unattended speech effect. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 24, 263â265 (1986).
Jones, D. M. & Macken, W. J. Phonological similarity in the irrelevant speech effect. Within- or between-stream similarity? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 103â115 (1995).
Larsen, J. D. Baddeley, A. D. & Andrade, J. Phonological similarity and the irrelevant speech effect: implications for models of short-term verbal memory. Memory 8, 145â157 (2000).
Le Compte, D. C. & Shaibe, D. M. On the irrelevance of phonological similarity to the irrelevant speech effect. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 50A, 100â118 (1997).
Jones, D., Beaman, M. & Macken, W. J. in Models of Short-Term Memory (ed. Gathercole, S.) 209â237 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 1996).
Page, M. A. & Norris, D. The irrelevant sound effect: what needs modeling and a tentative model. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (in the press).
Macken, W. J. & Jones, D. M. Reification of phonological storage. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (in the press).
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E. & Papagno, C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol. Rev. 105, 158â173 (1998). Summarizes the evidence supporting the view that the phonological loop serves as a device for facilitating language acquisition.
Papagno, C., Valentine, T. & Baddeley, A. D. Phonological short-term memory and foreign language vocabulary learning. J. Mem. Lang. 30, 331â347 (1991).
Papagno, C. & Vallar, G. Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: the effect of phonological similarity and item length. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 44A, 47â67 (1992).
Service, E. Phonology, working memory and foreign-language learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 45A, 21â50 (1992).
Atkins, W. B. & Baddeley, A. D. Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. App. Psycholinguistics 19, 537â552 (1998).
Gathercole, S. E. & Adams, A. M. Children's phonological working memory: contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 672â788 (1994).
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. J. Mem. Lang. 28, 200â213 (1989).
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connection? J. Mem. Lang. 29, 336â360 (1990).
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S., Hall, M. & Peaker, S. Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54A, 1â30 (2001).
Houghton, G. Hartley, T. & Glasspool, D. W. in Models of Short-Term Memory (ed. Gathercole, S. E.) 101â128 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 1996).
Gathercole, S. E. Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Mem. Cogn. 23, 83â94 (1995).
Thorn, A. S. C., Gathercole, S. E. & Frankish, C. R. Language familiarity effects in short-term memory: the role of output delay and long-term knowledge. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55A, 1363â1383 (2002).
O'Regan, J. K., Rensink, R. A. & Clark, J. J. Change-blindness as a result of 'mudsplashes'. Nature 398, 34â34 (1999).
Simons, D. J. & Levin, D. T. Change blindness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 261â267 (1997).
O'Regan, J. K. Solving the real mysteries of visual-perception â the world as an outside memory. Can. J. Psychol. 46, 461â488 (1992).
Luck, S. J. & Vogel, E. K. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279â281 (1997).
Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F. & Luck, S. J. Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf. 27, 92â114 (2001).
Wheeler, M. E. & Treisman, A. M. Binding in short-term visual memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 131, 48â64 (2002). A good example of the recent tendency for prominent researchers in visual attention to analyse related processes of visuospatial working memory.
Milner, B. Inter-hemispheric differences in the locatisation of psychological processes in man. B. Med. Bull. 27, 272â277 (1971).
Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A. D., Allamano, N. & Wilson, L. Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia 37, 1189â1199 (1999).
Della Sala, S. & Logie, R. H. in Handbook of Memory Disorders (eds Baddeley, A. D., Kopelman, M. D. & Wilson, B. A.) 271â292 (Wiley, Chichester, 2002)
Smith, E. E. et al. Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 337â358 (1995).
Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G. & Macko, K. O. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414 (1983).
Pickering, S. J. Cognitive approaches to the fractionation of visuo-spatial working memory. Cortex 37, 470â473 (2001).
Smyth, M. M. & Pendleton, L. R. Space and movement in working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 42A, 291â304 (1990).
Logie, R. H. Visuo-spatial processing in working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 38A, 229â247 (1986).
Quinn, G. & McConnell, J. Irrelevant pictures in visual working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49A, 200â215 (1996).
Purcell, A. T. & Gero, J. S. Drawings and the design process. Design Studies 19, 389â430 (1998).
Verstijnen, I. M., van Leeuwen, C., Goldschimdt, G., Haeml, R. & Hennessey, J. M. Creative discovery in imagery and perception: combining is relatively easy, restructuring takes a sketch. Acta Psychol. 99, 177â200 (1998).
Ghiselin, B. The Creative Process (Mentor, New York, 1952).
Finke, R. & Slayton, K. Explorations of creative visual synthesis in mental imagery. Mem. Cogn. 16, 252â257 (1988).
Finke, R., Ward, T. B. & Smith, S. M. Creative Cognition: Theory, Research and Applications (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1992)
Pearson, D. G., Logie, R. H. & Gilhooly, K. Verbal representations and spatial manipulation during mental synthesis. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 11, 295â314 (1999).
Pearson, D. G. in Working Memory in Perspective (ed. Andrade, J.) 33â59 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 2001).
Brandimonte, M. & Gerbino, W. Mental image reversal and verbal recoding: when ducks become rabbits. Mem. Cogn. 21, 23â33 (1993).
Brandimonte, M., Hitch, G. J. & Bishop, D. Verbal recoding of visual stimuli in pairs mental image transformations. Mem. Cogn. 20, 449â455 (1992).
Logie, R. H. Visuo-spatial Working Memory (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hove, Sussex, 1995).
Goldman-Rakic, P. W. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137â156 (1988). A review of studies in which single-unit recording in awake monkeys is used to analyse the nature of visuospatial working memory.
Goldman-Rakic, S. The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 351, 1445â1453 (1996).
Baddeley, A. D. Working Memory (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1986).
Norman, D. A. & Shallice, T. in Consciousness and Self-regulation. Advances in Research and Theory (eds Davidson, R. J., Schwarts, G. E. & Shapiro, D.) 1â18 (Plenum, New York, 1986).
Shallice, T. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988).
Shallice, T. in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function (eds Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T.) 261â277 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2002).
Shallice, T. & Burgess, P. W. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in men. Brain 114, 727â741 (1991).
Bargh, J. A. & Ferguson, M. J. Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychol. Bull. 126, 925â945 (2000). This article makes a strong case for the importance of implicit factors in determining social behaviour.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M. & Burrows, L. Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 71, 230â244 (1996).
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 76, 893â910 (1999).
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A. in Self and Motivation: Emerging Psychological Perspectives (eds Tesser, A., Stapel, D. A. & Wood, J. V.) 13â41 (American Psychological Association, Washington DC, 2002).
Baumeister, R. F. & Exline, J. J. Self control, morality and human strength. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 19, 29â42 (2000).
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. & Tice, D. M. Ego-depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 74, 1252â1265 (1998).
Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. Self regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self control resemble a muscle? Psychol. Bull. 126, 247â259 (2000).
McDougall, W. Outline of Psychology (Scribner, New York, 1923)
Attneave, F. in Sensory Communication (ed. Rosenblith, W.) 777â782 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachussetts, 1960).
Baddeley, A. D. Exploring the central executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49A, 5â28 (1996).
Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. & Spinnler, H. The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal study. Brain 114, 2521â2542 (1991).
Baddeley, A. D., Baddeley, H. A., Bucks, R. S. & Wilcock, G. K. Attentional control in Alzheimer's disease. Brain 124, 1492â1508 (2001).
Baddeley, A. D. & Wilson, B. A. Prose recall and amnesia: implications for the structure of working memory. Neuropsychologia 40, 1737â1743 (2002).
Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., Wynn, V. & Baddeley, A. D. Visual similarity effects in immediate serial recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 53A, 626â646 (2000).
Baddeley, A. D. & Andrade, J. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129, 126â145 (2000).
Baddeley, A. D. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417â423 (2000). This gives a brief account of the argument for assuming a fourth component to the working memory model.
Baars, B. J. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988).
Baars, B. J. The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 47â52 (2002).
Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79, 1â37 (2001). An excellent review of the neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence for a limited capacity work-space system that is associated with conscious awareness.
Ericsson, K. A. & Kintsch, W. Long-term working memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 211â245 (1995).
Warrington, E. J., Logue, V. & Pratt, R. T. C. The anatomical localisation of selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia 9, 377â387 (1971).
Vallar, G., DiBetta, A. M. & Silveri, M. C. The phonological short-term store-rehearsal system: patterns of impairment and neural correlates. Neuropsychologia 35, 795â812 (1997).
Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 362, 342â345 (1993).
Jonides, J. et al. in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (ed. Medin, D.) 43â88 (Academic, London, 1996). A good account of this group's work on applying neuroimaging to the study of both the phonological and visuospatial subsystems of working memory.
Jonides, J. & Smith, E. E. in Cognitive Neuroscience (ed. Rugg, M. D.) 243â276 (Psychology, Hove, Sussex, 1997).
Awh, E. et al. Dissociation of storage and retrieval in verbal working memory: evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychol. Sci. 7, 25â31 (1996).
Smith, E. E. & Jonides, J. Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 5â42 (1997).
Smith, E., Jonides, J. & Koeppe, R. A. Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cereb. Cortex 6, 11â20 (1996).
DeRenzi, E. & Nichelli, P. Verbal and non-verbal short-term memory impairment following hemispheric damage. Cortex 11, 341â353 (1975).
Hanley, J. R., Young, A. W. & Pearson, N. A. Impairment of the visuospatial sketch pad. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 43A, 101â125 (1991).
Kosslyn, S. M. et al. Visual mental imagery activates topographically organised cortex: PET investigations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 263â287 (1993).
Jonides, J. et al. Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363, 623â625 (1993).
Awh, E., Jonides, J. & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. Rehearsal in spatial working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf. 24, 780â790 (1998).
Wilson, F. A. W. Scalaidhe, S. & Goldman-Rakic, S. Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260, 1955â1958 (1993).
Levin, D. N. Warach, J. & Farah M. J. Two visual systems in mental imagery: dissociation of 'what' and 'where' in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions. Neurology 35, 1010â1018 (1985).
Owen, A. M. The functional organisation of working memory processes within the human lateral frontal cortex: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, 1329â1339 (1997).
Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2002).
Braver, T. S. et al. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5, 49â62 (1997).
Cohen, J. D. et al. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386, 604â608 (1997).
Frith, C. D., Friston, K. J., Liddle, P. F. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. Willed action in the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 244, 241â246 (1991).
Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R. & Frith, C. D. The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: a study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 12, 713â725 (2000).
Baddeley, A. D. Emslie, H., Kolodny, J. & Duncan, J. Random generation and the executive control of working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 51A, 818â852 (1998).
Duncan, J. & Owen, A. M. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475â483 (2000). A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of attentional control that argues for the dependence of diverse executive processes on a limited anatomical region of the right frontal lobe.
D'Esposito, M. et al. The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 378, 279â281 (1995).
Klingberg, T. Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. Cereb. Cortex 8, 593â601 (1998).
Fletcher, T. C. et al. Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory: an in vivo study in humans. Brain 118, 401â416 (1995).
Goldberg, T. E. et al. Uncoupling cognitive workload and prefrontal cortical physiology: a PET rCBF study. Neuroimage 7, 296â303 (1998).
Damasio, A. R. Descarte's Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain (Putnam, New York, 1994).
Hume, D. An Enquiry Concerning Human Understanding (Hackett, Indianapolis, 1772/1993).
Lewin, K. Field Theory in Social Science (Harper, New York, 1951).
Kennard, C. & Swash, M. (Eds) Hierarchies in Neurology: reappraisal of Jacksonian Concept (Springer, Berlin, 1989).
Craik, K. J. W. The Nature of Explanation (Cambridge Univ. Press, London, 1943).
Broadbent, D. E. Levels, hierarchies and the locus of control. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 29, 181â201 (1977).
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J. & Wolpert, D. M. Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 355, 1771â1788 (2000). A stimulating attempt to provide an account of the control of action based on a range of experimental phenomena and neuropsychological deficits. It makes a potentially important case for the possible extension of a model of motor control to the more general control of action and behaviour.
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 19, 450â466 (1980).
Daneman, M. & Merikle, M. Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 422â433 (1996).
Turner, M. L. & Engle, R. W. Is working memory capacity task-dependent? J. Mem. Lang. 28, 127â154 (1989).
Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Gunn, D. M. & Baddeley, A. D. The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 71â92 (2003).
Ormrod, J. E. & Cochran, K. F. Relationship of verbal ability and working memory to spelling achievement and learning to spell. Reading Res. Instruction 28, 33â43 (1988).
Kyllonen, C. & Stephens, D. L. Cognitive abilities as the determinants of success in acquiring logic skills. Learn. Individual Diff. 2, 129â160 (1990).
Kiewra, K. A. & Benton, S. L. The relationship between information processing ability and note-taking. Contemp. Edu. Psychol. 13, 3â44 (1988).
Engle, R. W. Carullo, J. J. & Collins, K. W. Individual differences in working memory for comprehension and following directions. J. Edu. Res. 84, 253â262 (1991).
Kyllonen, C. & Christal, R. E. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity. Intelligence 14, 389â433 (1990).
Engle, R. W. Kane, M. J. & Tuholski, S. W. in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (eds Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 102â134 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1999).
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P. & Heggarty, M. How are visuo-spatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related: latent-variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 621â640 (2001).
Miyake, A. et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex 'frontal lobe' tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49â100 (2000). A clear account of the application of the statistical technique of latent variable analysis to the analysis of executive processes in working memory.
Kane, M. J. & Engle, R. W. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention and general fluid intelligence: an individual differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 637â671 (2002).
Spearman, C. The Abilities of Man (Macmillan, London, 1927).
Thurstone, L. L. Primary Mental Abilities (Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, 1938).
Carroll, J. B. Human Cognitive Abilities (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1993).
Herrnstein, R. J. & Murray, C. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life (Free Press, New York, 1994).
Kamin, L. J. Intelligence: The Battle for the Mind (H. J. Eysenck versus Leon Kamin) (Macmillan, London, 1981).
Brooks, L. R. Spatial and verbal components in the act of recall. Can. J. Psychol. 22, 349â368 (1968).
Martin, J. H. Neuroanatomy: Text and Atlas 2nd edn (Appleton & Lange, Stamford, Connecticut, 1996).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Related links
FURTHER INFORMATION
MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences
Glossary
- BRODMANN AREAS
-
(BA). Korbinian Brodmann (1868â1918) was an anatomist who divided the cerebral cortex into numbered subdivisions on the basis of cell arrangements, types and staining properties (for example, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contains subdivisions, including BA 46, BA 9 and others). Modern derivatives of his maps are commonly used as the reference system for discussion of brain-imaging findings.
- ANARTHRIC
-
Unable to speak because of defective articulation.
- DYSPRAXIC
-
Having an impairment of the ability to perform certain voluntary movements, often including speech.
- CONATIVE PSYCHOLOGY
-
McDougall proposed the term 'conative' to denote the activity of mental striving or the will, as opposed to cognitive and affective or emotional processes.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Baddeley, A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 829â839 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
This article is cited by
-
Association of auditory processing abilities and employment in young women
The Egyptian Journal of Otolaryngology (2024)
-
Pan-cortical electrophysiologic changes underlying attention
Scientific Reports (2024)
-
Exploring the impact of intensified multiple session tDCS over the left DLPFC on brain function in MCI: a randomized control trial
Scientific Reports (2024)
-
The role of teaching solfeggio considering memory mechanisms in developing musical memory and hearing of music school students
Current Psychology (2024)
-
Far Transfer Effects of Trainings on Executive Functions in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: A Systematic Review and Metanalysis
Neuropsychology Review (2024)