Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Karen Wynn is an artist and a Canadian and American Yale University Professor Emerita of psychology and cognitive science.[1] She was born in Austin, Texas, and grew up on the Canadian prairies in Regina, Saskatchewan. Her research explores the cognitive capacities of infants and young children. She directed for over 3 decades the Infant Cognition Laboratory, first in the Psychology Department at the University of Arizona, and then in the Psychology Department at Yale University.

Karen Wynn
Wynn in 2012
Born
CitizenshipU.S., Canadian
Alma materMcGill University (BA)
Massachusetts Institute of Technology (Ph.D)
AwardsNational Academy of Sciences Troland Research Award, American Psychological Association Early Career Award. Fellow of the Association for Psychological Science
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology, cognitive science, developmental psychology
InstitutionsYale University, University of Arizona
Doctoral advisorSusan Carey

Education

edit

Wynn received her Bachelor of Arts in psychology from McGill University, and her PhD in cognitive science from the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Her first faculty position was at the University of Arizona. She joined the Yale University Psychology Department in 1999.

Research

edit

Numerical cognition

edit

Karen Wynn is known for her pioneering work on infants' and children's early numerical cognition. The first of her many influential research studies on this topic, published in the scientific journal Nature in 1992, reported that five-month-old human infants are able to compute the outcomes of simple addition and subtraction operations on small sets of physical objects.[2][3]

Methods

edit

Thirty-two five-month-old infants participated in the first experiment reported in the Nature article. Infants were randomly assigned to two-groups ('1+1' and '2-1'). In the 1+1 condition, infants were presented with a single doll. The object was then hidden from view by a small screen. An experimenter brought a second identical doll into the infant's view, and then placed it behind the screen (out of the infant's sight). In the 2-1 condition, a similar procedure occurred. The infant was presented with two dolls, which were then hidden from view by a screen. The experimenter removes one of the objects within the sight of the infant. In both conditions, the set-up was designed so that the infants would witness a mathematical operation being performed (either addition or subtraction), but would not be able to see the final result. For both groups, after this sequence was complete, the screen was removed to reveal either one or two objects. This process was repeated six times for each infant, alternating between one-item and two-item final displays. Looking time (the amount of time that the infant remained visually fixated on an object while remaining attentive to the display) was measured.[4]

Results

edit

The expected results of this experiment follow the theory of violation of expectations, that infants will look for a longer period of time at unexpected events than expected ones. Wynn hypothesized that if infants had the ability to compute numbers, they should look at the incorrect results longer than the correct results. Wynn found that infants in the 1+1 group looked longer when one item was shown as a final result (when the math implied that 1+1=1) than when two items were shown (1+1=2). Infants in the 2-1 group did the reverse, looking longer at the display with two items (2-1=2) than the display with one item (2-1=1). In another experiment within the study, in which infants were presented with 1+1 = 2 or 3, Wynn found that infants looked longer at three objects, the impossible outcome, rather than the two-object display.[4]

Implications

edit

These results indicated that infants are capable of performing simple numerical operations. Wynn has suggested that humans, along with many other animal species, are innately endowed with cognitive machinery for detecting and reasoning about numbers of items. As a result, "psychologists were stunned when Wynn announced her results, and many skeptical researchers around the world devised variants of her procedure to determine whether her conclusions were correct."[5] Wynn's findings were subsequently replicated by independent researchers in the United States and in Europe on human infants[6][7] and later extended to other subject populations, including rhesus monkeys[8] and domesticated dogs[9][10] who, like human babies, distinguished correct from incorrect outcomes of additions and subtractions of objects (eggplants, in the studies with rhesus monkeys; doggie biscuits, in the studies with dogs).

Criticism

edit

Criticism of Wynn's 1992 study has suggested that the difference in looking time should not be attributed to looking time, but rather to the preference given to familiar objects. Leslie B. Cohen and Kathryn S. Marks's 2002 study in Developmental Science suggested the possible explanation that infants may have been displaying a familiarity preference to the quantity of objects, as well as a preference for a greater quantity of objects over fewer objects.[11] Eric P. Charles and Susan M. Rivera also criticized looking time methods and the violation of expectations paradigm, raising the point that because the possible/impossible options are determined by adults, infants are assumed to have the same expectations as adults if they look longer at the impossible outcome.[12]

Follow-up

edit

In response to criticism that Wynn's 1992 results were due to infants' ability to keep track of small quantities of objects rather than mathematics, Wynn and Koleen McCrink's 2004 study published in Psychological Science demonstrates that nine-month-old infants can add and subtract numbers that exceed object-tracking limits.[13]

Social evaluation

edit

Wynn has also investigated humans' early social preferences and judgments. Some of this research, conducted in Wynn's lab with Wynn as senior author and then-graduate student Kiley Hamlin as lead author, found that 6- and 10-month-old infants evaluate individuals based on their behaviors towards others.[14][15]

Methods

edit

The infants were habituated to events in which a "climber" character made attempts to climb a hill. On the third attempt, the climber was either aided by a "helper" who pushed the climber from behind, or was pushed down the hill by a "hinderer." Infants were then asked to reach for their choice of either the helper or hinderer character. In another part of the experiment, infants were habituated the same display and then saw the climber approach either the helper (an unsurprising action) or the hinderer (a surprising action).[14]

Results

edit

When given a choice to reach for one of the characters, infants chose the helper characters, suggesting that the two characters had established impressions based on the actions each character had made towards the climber. Measurements in looking time showed that the 10-month-old infants looked longer at the circumstances when the climber approached the hinderer, following the violation of expectations principle. Six-month-olds looked for an equal amount of time at both situations, despite showing a preference for the helper characters in the choice task. This suggests that while 6-month-olds may be capable of establishing their own social evaluations, they may not have yet developed the ability to infer others' evaluations.[14]

Implications

edit

Notable philosopher of bioethics Peter Singer wrote of these studies that they "have upset the previous wisdom, associated with such stellar figures in psychology as Sigmund Freud, Jean Piaget, and Lawrence Kohlberg, that human moral development is the product of our rearing and our culture."[16] Wynn and her colleagues have suggested that babies' tendency to prefer prosocial individuals may arise from an adaptive capacity to detect good candidates for reciprocal interactions, and to prefer these individuals over those who may be more likely to act in self-interest or to renege on social contracts. The research suggests that the ability to evaluate an individual based on behavior may serve as a foundation for moral thought and action.

Further research

edit

Wynn and her students' further research at Yale continued with this theme, exploring questions such as how infants categorize different individuals into groups and how in-group favoritism develops, as well as investigating adaptive social strategies.[17]

Honors and awards

edit

Wynn was named a Distinguish SAGE Fellow from the SAGE Center for the Study of the Mind in 2009. As part of the fellowship, she presented a series of four lectures at the center, located at the University of California, Santa Barbara, on the social, moral and strategic characters of babies, as well as the baby of the future.[18]

Wynn was named a fellow of The Association for Psychological Science in 2005. Fellowships are awarded for having "made sustained outstanding contributions to the science of psychology in the areas of research, teaching, and/or application."[19]

Wynn received the Troland Research Award from the National Academy of Sciences in 2001, given "for her pioneering research on the foundations of quantitative and mathematical thinking in infants and young children."[20][21]

Wynn received the Distinguished Scientific Award for Early Career Contribution to Psychology in 2000,[22] given by The American Psychological Association "for her outstanding research program on the ontogenetic foundations of mathematical knowledge. Her research has advanced the understanding of the prelinguistic representations of number, and her work on infant representations of events and collections has advanced the understanding of the concepts infants use to establish representations of individual to enumerate."[23]

Wynn received a James McKeen Cattell Foundation Sabbatical Award in 1997.[24]

edit

Karen Wynn edited the 2002 book, Languages, Logic, and Concepts along with Ray Jackendoff and Paul Bloom. The book, published by The MIT Press, focuses on cognitive and linguistic development, as well as the role of mathematics in cognition.[25]

Karen Wynn has been interviewed and her research featured in science documentaries, radio shows, newspapers, and popular science news outlets.

Numerical cognition

edit

Wynn's research in numerical competency has generated much media attention. Articles about her research findings have been featured in The New York Times[26] (1992); LIFE magazine[27] (1993); as a cover story for Science News,[28] (2002); and in Science Daily[29] (2010). In video, Wynn's work has been featured in the documentary television series National Geographic Explorer in Science of Babies[30] (2007); and in the PBS documentary, The Human Spark with Alan Alda[31] (2010).

Social evaluation and morality

edit

Wynn's research in infants' social interactions and moral development has also received media attention. In print, Wynn's work has been featured in Science Daily[32] (2003); The New York Times[33] (2007); in Canada's CBC News[34] (2007); and The New York Times Magazine[35] (2010). Her research can be found in NPR's All Things Considered[36] (2010); in a special report by the National Science Foundation[37] (2011); and on CBS News' 60 Minutes[38] (2012).

Wynn has also appeared internationally in British and Australian television and radio programs.[39] Her research was also named one of the Top 50 Science Stories of 1992 in the January 1993 issue of Discover magazine.[40]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ "Yale Psychology Department Listing of Faculty". Archived from the original on 2012-02-22. Retrieved 2012-03-02.
  2. ^ Keith Devlin (2006), The Math Instinct: Why You're a Mathematical Genius (Along With Lobsters, Birds, Cats, and Dogs), Basic Books, ISBN 978-1-56025-672-4, pages 1-9.
  3. ^ Wynn, K (1992). "Addition and subtraction by human infants". Nature. 358 (6389): 749–750. Bibcode:1992Natur.358..749W. doi:10.1038/358749a0. PMID 1508269. S2CID 4348056.
  4. ^ a b Wynn, Karen (August 1992). "Addition and subtraction by human infants". Nature. 358 (6389): 749–750. Bibcode:1992Natur.358..749W. doi:10.1038/358749a0. PMID 1508269. S2CID 4348056.
  5. ^ Keith Devlin, Ibid., pages 6-7.
  6. ^ Susan Carey (2009), The Origin of Concepts, Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-536763-8, page 64 (fn)
  7. ^ Daniel Tammet (2009), Embracing the Wide Sky: A Tour Across the Horizons of the Mind, Simon and Schuster, ISBN 978-1-4165-6969-5, pages126
  8. ^ Stanislas Dehaene (1999), The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-513240-3, pages 53-55 (pages 41-44 of revised & updated 2011 edition).
  9. ^ Ellen Galinsky (2010), Mind in the Making: The Seven Essential Life Skills Every Child Needs, HarperCollins Publishers, ISBN 978-0-06-173232-4, pages 172-173.
  10. ^ West, R.; Young, R. (2002). "Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count?". Animal Cognition. 5 (3): 183–186. doi:10.1007/s10071-002-0140-0. PMID 12357291. S2CID 28789165.
  11. ^ Cohen, Leslie B.; Kathryn S. Marks (May 2002). "How infants process addition and subtraction events". Developmental Science. 5 (2): 186–212. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00220.
  12. ^ Charles, Eric P.; Susan M. Rivera (May 2009). "Object permanence and method of disappearance: looking measures further contradict reaching measures". Developmental Science. 12 (6): 991–1006. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00844.x. PMID 19840053. S2CID 34545153.
  13. ^ McCrink, Koleen; Karen Wynn (November 2004). "Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-month-old Infants". Psychological Science. 15 (11): 776–781. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x. PMID 15482450. S2CID 11811692.
  14. ^ a b c Hamlin, J. Kiley; Karen Wynn; Paul Bloom (November 2007). "Social evaluation by preverbal infants". Nature. 450 (7169): 557–559. Bibcode:2007Natur.450..557H. doi:10.1038/nature06288. PMID 18033298. S2CID 1255426.
  15. ^ Ellen Galinsky (2010), Mind in the Making: The Seven Essential Life Skills Every Child Needs, HarperCollins Publishers, ISBN 978-0-06-173232-4, page 3.
  16. ^ Peter Singer (2011), The Expanding Circle: Ethics, Evolution, and Moral Progress, Princeton University Press, ISBN 978-0-691-15069-7, page 188.
  17. ^ Wynn, Karen. "Research". Archived from the original on 2 August 2003. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  18. ^ "Distinguished Fellows for 2009–2010". Archived from the original on 11 December 2012. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  19. ^ Association for Psychological Science. "List of APS Fellows". Archived from the original on 25 June 2017. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  20. ^ National Academy of Sciences. "Troland Research Awards Recipients". Archived from the original on 26 January 2013. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  21. ^ Yale Bulletin & Calendar, "Psychologist Karen Wynn cited for pioneering study of infants' ability to recognize numbers," 2001
  22. ^ "APA Distinguished Scientific Awards for an Early Career Contribution to Psychology". Apa.org. Retrieved 2014-02-08.
  23. ^ "American Psychologist, Vol 55(11), Nov 2000, pages 1279–1282". Retrieved 2012-02-28.
  24. ^ "The James McKeen Cattell Fellowship Fund Recipients". Retrieved 10 March 2013.
  25. ^ The MIT Press. "Karen Wynn". Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  26. ^ Goleman, Daniel (27 August 1992). "Study Finds Babies at 5 Months Grasp Simple Mathematics". The New York Times. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  27. ^ "At Five Months, Babies Have the Raw Ability to Add". LIFE Magazine. Archived from the original on April 10, 2013. Retrieved 10 March 2013.
  28. ^ Science News, June 22, 2002 http://www.sciencenews.org/view/issue/id/2846%20 cover story
  29. ^ Science Daily, Dec 05, 2010 https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2010/12/101201142507.htm
  30. ^ National Geographic. "Explorer: Science of Babies". Archived from the original on September 12, 2012. Retrieved 10 March 2013.
  31. ^ PBS, The Human Spark With Alan Alda, January 20, 2010
  32. ^ Science Daily, Sept 30, 2003 https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/09/030930055123.htm
  33. ^ Nagourney, Eric (4 December 2007). "Behavior: Even Babies May be Good Judges of Character". The New York Times. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  34. ^ "Even babies make social judgments, study suggests". CBC News. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  35. ^ Bloom, Paul (5 May 2010). "The Moral Life of Babies". The New York Times. Retrieved 10 March 2013.
  36. ^ National Public Radio All Things Considered, Weekend Edition, May 09, 2010
  37. ^ "Babies and Learning: The good, the bad and the baby". National Science Foundation. Archived from the original on 25 March 2013. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  38. ^ Stahl, Lesley. "The Baby Lab". CBS News. Archived from the original on November 19, 2012. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
  39. ^ Wynn, Karen. "CV" (PDF). Retrieved 10 March 2013.
  40. ^ "January 1993 Archive". Discover Magazine. Retrieved 2 March 2013.
edit