Body memory
Part of a series on |
Alternative medicine |
---|
Body memory (BM) is a hypothesis that the body itself is capable of storing memories, as opposed to only the brain. While experiments have demonstrated the possibility of cellular memory[1] there are currently no known means by which tissues other than the brain would be capable of storing memories.[2][3]
Modern usage of BM tends to frame it exclusively in the context of traumatic memory and ways in which the body responds to recall of a memory. In this regard, it has become relevant in treatment for PTSD.[4]
Overview
[edit]Peter Levine calls BM implicit memory or more specifically procedural memory, things that the body is capable of doing automatically and not in one's consciousness. He clarifies 3 types of BM and frames his work in terms of traumatic memory consequence and resolution:[5]
- Learned motor actions - Action patterns that can be continuously modified over time by higher brain regions.
- Emergency response - Hardwired instinctual behaviors (i.e., fight or flight response, etc...).
- Attraction or repulsion - We are attracted to sources of nourishment and growth and repulsed from sources of injury or toxicity.
Nicola Diamond elaborates on the opinion of philosopher Merleau-Ponty and asserts that BM is formed by doing. Whether practicing a bodily activity or forming a reaction to a traumatic memory.[6]
Edward Casey speaks of BM as, "memory intrinsic to the body, how we remember by and through the body", rather than what is remembered about the body.[7]
Thomas Fuchs defines 6 different types of BM: procedural, situational, intercorporeal, incorporative, pain, and traumatic memory. He notes that they are not strictly separable from one another but "derived from different dimensions of bodily experience.[8]: 12 Michelle Summa further refines this definition as an implicit memory. A pre-thematic, operative consciousness of the past expressed through the body.[8]: 30
Antonio Damasio calls these reactions to memories somatic markers or emotions that are expressed primarily as physical feelings.[9]
These memories are often associated with phantom pain in a part or parts of the body – the body appearing to remember the past trauma. The idea of body memory is a belief frequently associated with the idea of repressed memories, in which memories of incest or sexual abuse can be retained and recovered through physical sensations.[2] It may also be associated with phantom limb sensation but this is less common.[10]
Skepticism
[edit]In 1993, Susan E. Smith, presented a paper relating the idea of "Survivor Psychology" at a false memory syndrome conference, stated about BM that, "body memories are thought to literally be emotional, kinesthetic, or chemical recordings stored at the cellular level and retrievable by returning to or recreating the chemical, emotional, or kinesthetic conditions under which the memory recordings are filed.[2] She went on in the abstract of the paper, "one of the most commonly used theories to support the ideology of repressed memories or incest and sexual abuse amnesia is body memories." and "The belief in these pseudoscientific concepts appears to be related to scientific illiteracy, gullibility, and a lack of critical thinking skills and reasoning abilities in both the mental health community and in society at large"[2]
A 2017 systematic review of cross-disciplinary research in body memory found that the available data neither largely support or refute the claim that memories are stored outside of the brain and more research is needed.[11]
In the Encyclopedia of Phenomenology Embree notes that, "To posit body memory is to open up a Pandora's Box", and links the idea to physical associations of memory rather than as a memory stored in a bodily manner.[12]
Cellular memory
[edit]Cellular memory (CM) is a parallel hypothesis to BM positing that memories can be stored outside the brain in all cells.[13] The idea that non-brain tissues can have memories is believed by some who have received organ transplants, though this is considered impossible. The author said the stories are intriguing though and may lead to some serious scientific investigation in the future.[13] In his book TransplantNation Douglas Vincent suggests that atypical newfound memories, thoughts, emotions and preferences after an organ transplant are more suggestive of immunosuppressant drugs and the stress of surgery on perception than of legitimate memory transference. In other words, "as imaginary as a bad trip on LSD or other psychotropic drug."[14]
Cellular memory refers to the ability of cells to retain information about past states, exposures, or events and adapt their responses accordingly. This concept underpins various physiological and pathological processes, often mediated by hormonal pathways, feedback loops, and epigenetic mechanisms. The following are key examples illustrating the scientific basis of cellular memory.
Stress and emotional memory
[edit]The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, through the release of glucocorticoids like cortisol, plays a pivotal role in stress and emotional memory. Cortisol enhances the consolidation of emotionally charged memories by modulating hippocampal activity, yet it can impair memory retrieval.[15] This dual effect is supported by research showing that glucocorticoids improve consolidation of long-term memory, particularly for emotionally valenced information, while impairing retrieval processes.[16] Dysregulation of this pathway is implicated in stress-related disorders such as PTSD, where the over-consolidation of fear-based memories occurs. Studies have demonstrated that glucocorticoids facilitate memory encoding but may compromise the retrieval of information, creating a dynamic interplay between memory formation and stress responses </ref>.
Recent research has further elucidated how chronic stress shapes neural networks. Prolonged exposure to high cortisol levels can reduce hippocampal volume and inhibit neurogenesis, weakening the brain's capacity to form new memories while reinforcing maladaptive ones. [17] Those same studies have shown that chronic exposure to elevated cortisol levels, whether through stress or medical conditions, can lead to morphological changes in the hippocampus, suppress neuronal proliferation, and reduce hippocampal volume.
The dynamic interplay between memory formation and stress responses is evident in the research demonstrating that glucocorticoids facilitate memory encoding but may compromise the retrieval of information.[18] This relationship is thought to follow an inverted U-shaped curve, with optimal memory performance at moderate levels of cortisol and impairment at both low and high levels.[19] The differential activation of mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) and glucocorticoid receptors (GRs) at varying cortisol concentrations may explain this complex relationship between stress hormones and memory processes.[20]
Furthermore, the impact of glucocorticoids on memory is time-dependent and context-specific. While acute elevations in cortisol can enhance the consolidation of new memories, including extinction memories, chronic exposure to high cortisol levels may lead to detrimental effects on cognitive function.[21] This has important implications for the treatment of fear-related disorders, as glucocorticoid-based interventions may facilitate fear extinction by reducing the retrieval of aversive memories and enhancing the consolidation of extinction memories.[22]
Metabolic memory and nutritional states
[edit]Nutritional and metabolic states are encoded in cellular memory through hormonal and transcriptional mechanisms, including glucose-induced transcriptional hysteresis and thyroid hormone regulation. Prolonged hyperglycemia can induce lasting epigenetic changes in glucose-regulated pathways, contributing to long-term complications of diabetes, such as vascular damage and cognitive decline.[23] This phenomenon, known as "metabolic memory," involves persistent alterations in gene expression and cellular function even after normalization of glucose levels.[24]
Glucose-induced transcriptional hysteresis plays a significant role in this process. Studies have demonstrated that exposure to elevated glucose levels leads to a positive feedback loop, resulting in persistent expression of genes that promote glycolysis and inhibit alternative metabolic pathways.[25]
Similarly, during caloric deficits, the body adapts by lowering the basal metabolic rate and "remembering" prior energy-deprived states through alterations in leptin, ghrelin, and thyroid hormone signaling. These adaptive responses are examples of metabolic memory and highlight how previous nutritional environments shape cellular behavior.[26]
The concept of "memory" in hormonal states is indeed critical for maintaining metabolic homeostasis, but it can also lead to maladaptive outcomes in certain conditions. Chronic high glucose levels have been shown to alter epigenetic markers, leading to persistent vascular inflammation and oxidative stress. Transient hyperglycemia can induce long-lasting activating epigenetic changes in the promoter of the nuclear factor κB (NF-κB) subunit p65 in aortic endothelial cells.[27] These changes persist for at least 6 days of subsequent normal glycemia, resulting in increased expression of pro-inflammatory genes such as monocyte chemoattractant protein 1 (MCP-1) and vascular cell adhesion molecule 1 (VCAM-1).[28]
The establishment of these epigenetic changes may precede cardiovascular complications and help predict vascular lesions in diabetic patients.[29] Importantly, these epigenetic marks may be transmitted across several generations, increasing the individual risk of disease.[30]
The concept of metabolic memory extends beyond glucose regulation. Nutritional and metabolic states are encoded in cellular memory through various hormonal and transcriptional mechanisms.[31] These mechanisms form a complex network that governs metabolic memory and can emerge as novel targets for both detection and intervention of metabolic diseases.[32]
Reproductive and developmental programming
[edit]Hormonal fluctuations during critical developmental periods, such as puberty or pregnancy, create lasting imprints on cellular and systemic physiology. These hormonal effects influence cognitive functions, secondary sexual characteristics, and susceptibility to hormone-sensitive disorders.
Early-life estrogen exposure has been associated with long-term changes in brain plasticity and memory capacity, contributing to gender differences in neuropsychiatric conditions. Estrogen plays a crucial role in brain development, particularly in determining central gender dimorphism.[33] During puberty and other developmental stages, estrogen-induced synaptic plasticity is evident, affecting neurotransmitter synthesis, release, and metabolism.[34]
Estrogen's effects on the central nervous system are multifaceted, involving both genomic and non-genomic mechanisms. These actions protect against a wide range of neurotoxic insults and influence electrical excitability, synaptic function, and morphological features.[35] Clinical evidence shows that estrogen withdrawal during the climacteric period leads to modifications in mood, behavior, and cognition, while estrogen administration can improve cognitive efficiency in post-menopausal women.[36]
Emerging studies indeed reveal that testosterone levels during puberty influence neural development, affecting synaptic pruning and myelination in the prefrontal cortex. These changes have long-term implications for decision-making, risk assessment, and emotional regulation. During adolescence, high testosterone levels are associated with increased anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) involvement in emotion control.[37]
Elevated glucocorticoids during maternal stress have been shown to alter fetal epigenetic markers. Maternal adversity during pregnancy, including stress, anxiety, and depression, is associated with increased maternal and fetal glucocorticoid concentrations, which can lead to long-term physiological and pathophysiological outcomes in offspring.[38] Studies have found a significant correlation between psychosocial maternal stress and offspring methylation at a specific CpG site in the exon 1F of the human glucocorticoid receptor gene NR3C1, which may predispose offspring to mood disorders and metabolic dysregulation.[39]
Flatworms
[edit]Biologists at Tufts University have been able to train flatworms despite the loss of the brain and head. This may show memory stored in other parts of the body in some animals.[40] A worm reduced to 1/279th of the original can be regrown within a few weeks and be trained much quicker to head towards light and open space for food, an unnatural behavior for a flatworm. With each head removed training times appear reduced. This may just be a sign of epigenetics showing the appearance of memory.[41]
However, in the 1950s and 1960s James McConnell flatworm experiments measured how long it took to learn a maze. McConnell trained some to move around a maze and then chopped them up and fed them to untrained worms. The untrained group learned faster compared to a control that had not been fed trained worms. McConnell believed the experiment indicated cellular memory.[42] The training involved stressing the worms with electric shock. This kind of stress releases persistent hormones and shows no evidence for memory transfer. Similar experiments with mice being trained and being fed to untrained mice showed improved learning. It was not a memory that was transferred but hormone enriched tissue.[42]
Current usage and research
[edit]In epigenetics there are various mechanisms for cells to pass on "memories" of stressors to their progeny. Strategies include Msn2 nucleo-cytoplasmic shuttling, changes in chromatin, partitioning of anti-stress factors, and damaged macromolecules between mother and daughter cells.[43]
In adaptive immunity there is a functional CM that enables the immune system to learn to react to pathogens through mechanisms such as cytoxic memory mediation in bone marrow,[44] innate immune memory in stromal cells,[45] fungal mediation of innate and inherited immunological response,[46] and T and B-cell immune training.[47] In this regard CM is essential for vaccine and immunity research.
References
[edit]- ^ Cosier, Susan (May 2015). "Could Memory Traces Exist in Cell Bodies?". scientificamerican.com. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0515-14b. Retrieved 31 May 2018.
- ^ a b c d Smith, Susan E. (1993). "Body Memories: And Other Pseudo-Scientific Notions of "Survivor Psychology"". Issues in Child Abuse Accusations. 5. Retrieved 31 May 2018.
- ^ Scott O Lilienfeld SO; Lynn SJ; Lohr JM, eds. (2002). Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology. The Guilford Press. ISBN 1-57230-828-1.
- ^ Bessel van der Kolk, M.D. (25 September 2014). The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma. Penguin Publishing Group. ISBN 978-1-101-60830-2.
- ^ Peter A. Levine, Ph.D. (27 October 2015). Trauma and Memory: Brain and Body in a Search for the Living Past: A Practical Guide for Understanding and Working with Traumatic Memory. North Atlantic Books. ISBN 978-1-58394-995-5.
- ^ Nicola Diamond (1 April 2013). Between Skins: The Body in Psychoanalysis - Contemporary Developments. Wiley. ISBN 978-0-470-77816-6.
- ^ Edward S. Casey (15 September 2009). Remembering, Second Edition: A Phenomenological Study. Indiana University Press. p. 147. ISBN 978-0-253-11431-0.
- ^ a b Sabine C. Koch; Thomas Fuchs; Michela Summa (1 January 2012). Body Memory, Metaphor and Movement. John Benjamins Publishing. ISBN 978-90-272-1350-1.
- ^ Francois Ansermet (24 April 2018). Biology of Freedom: Neural Plasticity, Experience, and the Unconscious. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-0-429-91143-9.
- ^ Price, Elfed Huw (2006). "A critical review of congenital phantom limb cases and a developmental theory for the basis of body image". Consciousness and Cognition. 15 (2): 310–322. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.07.003. PMID 16182566. S2CID 165404.
- ^ Riva, Guisseppe (2018). "The neuroscience of body memory: From the self through the space to the others". Cortex. 104: 241–260. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.013. PMID 28826604.
- ^ Lester Embree (29 June 2013). Encyclopedia of Phenomenology. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 456. ISBN 978-94-015-8881-2.
- ^ a b Carroll, Robert Todd. "cellular memory". skepdic.com. Retrieved 31 May 2018.
- ^ TransplantNation. Douglas Vincent. pp. 9–. GGKEY:H80NKZZSRW7.
- ^ Shields, G. S.; Hostinar, C. E.; Vilgis, V.; Forbes, E. E.; Hipwell, A. E.; Keenan, K.; Guyer, A. E. (2021). "Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis Activity in Childhood Predicts Emotional Memory Effects and Related Neural Circuitry in Adolescent Girls". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 33 (5): 872–886. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01687. PMC 8764738. PMID 34449842.
- ^ Roozendaal, Benno; Hahn, Emily L.; Nathan, Sheila V.; De Quervain, Dominique J.-F.; McGaugh, James L. (2004). "Glucocorticoid Effects on Memory Retrieval Require Concurrent Noradrenergic Activity in the Hippocampus and Basolateral Amygdala". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (37): 8161–8169. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2574-04.2004. PMC 6729799. PMID 15371517.
- ^ Kim, E. J.; Pellman, B.; Kim, J. J. (2015). "Stress effects on the hippocampus: A critical review". Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.). 22 (9): 411–416. doi:10.1101/lm.037291.114. PMC 4561403. PMID 26286651.
- ^ Rimmele, Ulrike; Besedovsky, Luciana; Lange, Tanja; Born, Jan (2013). "Blocking Mineralocorticoid Receptors Impairs, Blocking Glucocorticoid Receptors Enhances Memory Retrieval in Humans". Neuropsychopharmacology. 38 (5): 884–894. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.254. PMC 3671995. PMID 23303058.
- ^ Rimmele, Ulrike; Besedovsky, Luciana; Lange, Tanja; Born, Jan (2013). "Blocking Mineralocorticoid Receptors Impairs, Blocking Glucocorticoid Receptors Enhances Memory Retrieval in Humans". Neuropsychopharmacology. 38 (5): 884–894. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.254. PMC 3671995. PMID 23303058.
- ^ Rimmele, Ulrike; Besedovsky, Luciana; Lange, Tanja; Born, Jan (2013). "Blocking Mineralocorticoid Receptors Impairs, Blocking Glucocorticoid Receptors Enhances Memory Retrieval in Humans". Neuropsychopharmacology. 38 (5): 884–894. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.254. PMC 3671995. PMID 23303058.
- ^ De Quervain, D.; Wolf, O. T.; Roozendaal, B. (2019). "Glucocorticoid-induced enhancement of extinction—from animal models to clinical trials". Psychopharmacology. 236 (1): 183–199. doi:10.1007/s00213-018-5116-0. PMC 6373196. PMID 30610352.
- ^ De Quervain, D.; Wolf, O. T.; Roozendaal, B. (2019). "Glucocorticoid-induced enhancement of extinction—from animal models to clinical trials". Psychopharmacology. 236 (1): 183–199. doi:10.1007/s00213-018-5116-0. PMC 6373196. PMID 30610352.
- ^ Ceriello, Antonio; Ihnat, Michael A.; Thorpe, Jessica E. (2009). "The "Metabolic Memory": Is More Than Just Tight Glucose Control Necessary to Prevent Diabetic Complications?". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 94 (2): 410–415. doi:10.1210/jc.2008-1824. PMID 19066300.
- ^ Mobbs, C. V. (2018). "Glucose-Induced Transcriptional Hysteresis: Role in Obesity, Metabolic Memory, Diabetes, and Aging". Frontiers in Endocrinology. 9: 232. doi:10.3389/fendo.2018.00232. PMC 5985453. PMID 29892261.
- ^ Mobbs, C. V. (2018). "Glucose-Induced Transcriptional Hysteresis: Role in Obesity, Metabolic Memory, Diabetes, and Aging". Frontiers in Endocrinology. 9: 232. doi:10.3389/fendo.2018.00232. PMC 5985453. PMID 29892261.
- ^ Dong, Hao; Sun, Yuezhang; Nie, Lulingxiao; Cui, Aimin; Zhao, Pengfei; Leung, Wai Keung; Wang, Qi (2024). "Metabolic memory: Mechanisms and diseases". Signal Transduction and Targeted Therapy. 9 (1): 38. doi:10.1038/s41392-024-01755-x. PMC 10899265. PMID 38413567.
- ^ El-Osta, A.; Brasacchio, D.; Yao, D.; Pocai, A.; Jones, P. L.; Roeder, R. G.; Cooper, M. E.; Brownlee, M. (2008). "Transient high glucose causes persistent epigenetic changes and altered gene expression during subsequent normoglycemia". The Journal of Experimental Medicine. 205 (10): 2409–2417. doi:10.1084/jem.20081188. PMC 2556800. PMID 18809715.
- ^ El-Osta, A.; Brasacchio, D.; Yao, D.; Pocai, A.; Jones, P. L.; Roeder, R. G.; Cooper, M. E.; Brownlee, M. (2008). "Transient high glucose causes persistent epigenetic changes and altered gene expression during subsequent normoglycemia". The Journal of Experimental Medicine. 205 (10): 2409–2417. doi:10.1084/jem.20081188. PMC 2556800. PMID 18809715.
- ^ Salvatore, Marco; Schiano, Concetta; Benincasa, Giuditta; Napoli, Claudio (2020). "Differential epigenetic factors in the prediction of cardiovascular risk in diabetic patients". European Heart Journal - Cardiovascular Pharmacotherapy. 6 (4): 239–247. doi:10.1093/ehjcvp/pvz062. PMC 7363021. PMID 31665258.
- ^ Salvatore, Marco; Schiano, Concetta; Benincasa, Giuditta; Napoli, Claudio (2020). "Differential epigenetic factors in the prediction of cardiovascular risk in diabetic patients". European Heart Journal - Cardiovascular Pharmacotherapy. 6 (4): 239–247. doi:10.1093/ehjcvp/pvz062. PMC 7363021. PMID 31665258.
- ^ Dong, Hao; Sun, Yuezhang; Nie, Lulingxiao; Cui, Aimin; Zhao, Pengfei; Leung, Wai Keung; Wang, Qi (2024). "Metabolic memory: Mechanisms and diseases". Signal Transduction and Targeted Therapy. 9 (1): 38. doi:10.1038/s41392-024-01755-x. PMC 10899265. PMID 38413567.
- ^ Dong, Hao; Sun, Yuezhang; Nie, Lulingxiao; Cui, Aimin; Zhao, Pengfei; Leung, Wai Keung; Wang, Qi (2024). "Metabolic memory: Mechanisms and diseases". Signal Transduction and Targeted Therapy. 9 (1): 38. doi:10.1038/s41392-024-01755-x. PMC 10899265. PMID 38413567.
- ^ Genazzani, Andrea Riccardo; Pluchino, Nicola; Luisi, Stefano; Luisi, Michele (2007). "Estrogen, cognition and female ageing". Human Reproduction Update. 13 (2): 175–187. doi:10.1093/humupd/dml042. PMID 17135285.
- ^ Genazzani, Andrea Riccardo; Pluchino, Nicola; Luisi, Stefano; Luisi, Michele (2007). "Estrogen, cognition and female ageing". Human Reproduction Update. 13 (2): 175–187. doi:10.1093/humupd/dml042. PMID 17135285.
- ^ Genazzani, Andrea Riccardo; Pluchino, Nicola; Luisi, Stefano; Luisi, Michele (2007). "Estrogen, cognition and female ageing". Human Reproduction Update. 13 (2): 175–187. doi:10.1093/humupd/dml042. PMID 17135285.
- ^ Genazzani, Andrea Riccardo; Pluchino, Nicola; Luisi, Stefano; Luisi, Michele (2007). "Estrogen, cognition and female ageing". Human Reproduction Update. 13 (2): 175–187. doi:10.1093/humupd/dml042. PMID 17135285.
- ^ Tyborowska, A.; Volman, I.; Smeekens, S.; Toni, I.; Roelofs, K. (2016). "Testosterone during Puberty Shifts Emotional Control from Pulvinar to Anterior Prefrontal Cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 36 (23): 6156–6164. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-15.2016. PMC 6604884. PMID 27277794.
- ^ McGowan, Patrick O.; Matthews, Stephen G. (2018). "Prenatal Stress, Glucocorticoids, and Developmental Programming of the Stress Response". Endocrinology. 159 (1): 69–82. doi:10.1210/en.2017-00896. PMID 29136116.
- ^ Palma-Gudiel, H.; Córdova-Palomera, A.; Eixarch, E.; Deuschle, M.; Fañanás, L. (2015). "Maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy alters the epigenetic signature of the glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter in their offspring: A meta-analysis". Epigenetics. 10 (10): 893–902. doi:10.1080/15592294.2015.1088630. PMC 4844196. PMID 26327302.
- ^ Shomrat, Tal; Levin, Michael (2 July 2013). "An automated training paradigm reveals long-term memory in planaria and its persistence through head regeneration". Journal of Experimental Biology. 216 (20): 3799–3810. doi:10.1242/jeb.087809. PMID 23821717.
- ^ Anthony, Sebastion (11 July 2013). "Decapitated worms can regenerate their brains, and the memories stored inside". Extremetech. Retrieved 31 May 2018.
- ^ a b Bruce Hood (2009). Supersense: From Superstition to Religion - The Brain Science of Belief. Constable. ISBN 978-1-84901-030-6.
- ^ Xue Y, Acar M (2018). "Mechanisms for the epigenetic inheritance of stress response in single cells". Curr Genet. 64 (6): 1221–1228. doi:10.1007/s00294-018-0849-1. PMC 6215725. PMID 29846762.
- ^ Di Rosa F, Pabst R (2005). "The bone marrow: a nest for migratory memory T cells". Trends Immunol. 26 (7): 360–6. doi:10.1016/j.it.2005.04.011. PMID 15978522.
- ^ Crowley T, Buckley CD, Clark AR (2018). "Stroma: the forgotten cells of innate immune memory". Clin Exp Immunol. 193 (1): 24–36. doi:10.1111/cei.13149. PMC 6038004. PMID 29729109.
- ^ Quintin J (2019). "Fungal mediated innate immune memory, what have we learned?". Semin Cell Dev Biol. 89: 71–77. doi:10.1016/j.semcdb.2018.05.023. PMID 29842944.
- ^ Rosenblum, Michael D.; Way, Sing Sing; Abbas, Abul K. (21 December 2015). "Regulatory T cell memory". Nature Reviews Immunology. 16 (2): 90–101. doi:10.1038/nri.2015.1. PMC 5113825. PMID 26688349.
External links
[edit]- Cellular memory hints at the origins of intelligence, Nature, dated 23 January 2008