Abstract
Rule-like behaviour is found throughout human language, provoking a number of apparently conflicting explanations. This paper frames the topic in terms of Tinbergen’s four questions and works within the context of rule-like behaviour seen both in nature and the non-linguistic domain in humans. I argue for a minimal account of linguistic rules which relies on powerful domain-general cognition, has a communicative function allowing for multiple engineering solutions, and evolves mainly culturally, while leaving the door open for some genetic adaptation in the form of learning biases.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
cf. Strunk and White (1979).
As an example, the Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics (Brown 2005) conservatively lists 13 distinct schools of phonology, 12 models of morphology, and 27 syntactic frameworks.
What Huddlestone and Pullum (2002) call the ‘labile ordering of residual pre-head modifiers’.
In this case, something vaguely resembling Optimality Phonology.
However, as an anonymous reviewer points out, there is an extensive literature on potential similarities between action sequencing and language processing which has yet to resolve itself into anything resembling a consensus, so we should remain cautious about making any categorical statements here.
References
Anderson SR (1981) Why phonology isn’t “natural”. Linguist Inq 12(4):493–539
Aslin RN (2012) Questioning the questions that have been asked about the infant brain using near-infrared spectroscopy. Cogn Neuropsychol 29(1–2):7–33
Aslin RN, Shukla M, Emberson LL (2015) Hemodynamic correlates of cognition in human infants. Annu Rev Psychol 66(1):349–379
Berent I (2013) The phonological mind. Trends Cogn Sci 17(7):319–327
Berent I (2016) Commentary: “an evaluation of universal grammar and the phonological mind”—UG is still a viable hypothesis. Front Psychol 7(JUL):1–13
Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2015) Why only us?. MIT Press, Cambridge
Berwick RC, Pietroski P, Yankama B, Chomsky N (2011) Poverty of the stimulus revisited. Cogn Sci 35(7):1207–1242
Blevins J (2004) Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge University Press
Blevins J (2008) Natural and unnatural sound patterns: a pocket field guide. In: Willems K, De Cuypere L (eds) Naturalness and iconicity in language. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 121–148
Boeckx C (2010) Language in cognition. Wiley-Blackwell, Hoboken
Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures, vol 43, No. 02. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Brown K (ed) (2005) Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam
Buckley E (2000) What should phonology explain? Handout from SUNY Buffalo linguistics colloquium.
Bunge SA (2005) Neural circuitry underlying rule use in humans and nonhuman primates. J Neurosci 25(45):10347–10350
Chater N, Clark A, Goldsmith JA, Perfors A (2015) Empiricism and language learnability. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Chater N, Reali F, Christiansen MH (2009) Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(4):1015–1020
Chomsky N (2005) Three factors in language design. Linguist Inq 36(1):1–22.
Chomsky N, Halle M (1968) The sound pattern of English. Harper, New York
Christiansen M, Reali F, Chater N (2011) Biological adaptations for functional features of language in the face of cultural evolution. Hum Biol 83(2):247–259
Christiansen MH, Chater N (2008) Language as shaped by the brain. Behav Brain Sci 31(5):489–508 discussion 509–558
Christiansen MH, Chater N (2016) The Now-or-Never bottleneck: a fundamental constraint on language. Behav Brain Sci 39:1–52
Croft W (2001) Radical construction grammar: syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Croft W (2013) Evolution: language use and the evolution of languages. In: Binder PM, Smith K (eds) The language phenomenon. The frontiers collection. Springer, Berlin, pp 93–120
de Lacy P, Kingston J (2013) Synchronic explanation. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 31(2):287–355
Elman JL (1991) Distributed representations, simple recurrent networks, and grammatical structure. Mach Learn 7(2):195–225
Everett DL (2016) An evaluation of universal grammar and the phonological mind. Front Psychol 7(15):1–10
Fitch WT (2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303(5656):377–380
Gervain J, Berent I, Werker JF (2012) Binding at birth: the newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. J Cogn Neurosci 24(3):564–74
Gibson E, Piantadosi ST, Brink K, Bergen L, Lim E, Saxe R (2013) A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word-order variation. Psychol Sci 24(7):1079–88
Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 205(1161):581–598
Graziano M (2006) The organization of behavioral repertoire in motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 29(1):105–134
Griffiths TL, Chater N, Kemp C, Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB (2010) Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):357–364
Griffiths TL, Kemp C, Tenenbaum JB (2007) Bayesian models of cognition, pp 1–49
Griffiths TL, Lieder F, Goodman ND (2015) Rational use of cognitive resources: levels of analysis between the computational and the algorithmic. Top Cogn Sci 7(2):217–229
Hale M, Reiss C (2008) The phonological enterprise. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Huddlestone R, Pullum GK (2002) The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Johansson S (2013) Biolinguistics or physicolinguistics? Is the third factor helpful or harmful in explaining language? Biolinguistics 2005:249–275
Kaplan RM, Kay M (1994) Regular models of phonological rule systems. Comput Linguist 20:331–378
Kirby S, Tamariz M, Cornish H, Smith K (2015) Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition 141:87–102
Linzen T, Donnell TJO (2015) A model of rapid phonotactic generalization. In: Emnlp, pp 1126–1131
Lisman J (2015) The challenge of understanding the brain: where we stand in 2015. Neuron 86(4):864–882
Mantini D, Corbetta M, Romani GL, Orban GA, Vanduffel W (2013) Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? J Neurosci 33(8):3259–3275
Marcus GF (1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283(5398):77–80
Marr D, Poggio T (1976) From understanding computation to understanding neural circuitry
Ohala JJ (2005) Phonetic explanations for sound patterns: implications for grammars of competence. In: A figure of speech. A festschrift for John Laver, pp 23–38
Oudeyer P-Y (2005) The self-organization of combinatoriality and phonotactics in vocalization systems. Connect Sci 17(3–4):325–341
Pinker S (1991) Rules of language. Science 253(5019):530
Pinker S (1999) Words and rules. Basic Books Inc, New York
Pinker S, Bloom P (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behav Brain Sci 13(13):707–784
Pinker S, Jackendoff R (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition 95(2):201–36
Saffran JR, Pollak SD, Seibel RL, Shkolnik A (2007) Dog is a dog is a dog: infant rule learning is not specific to language. Cognition 105(3):669–680
Samuels B (2009) The third factor in phonology. Biolinguistics 3(2–3):355–382
Sandler W, Aronoff M, Meir I, Padden C (2011) The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 29(2):503–543
Schilthuizen M, Davison A (2005) The convoluted evolution of snail chirality. Naturwissenschaften 92(11):504–515
Strunk W, White EB (1979) The elements of style. Allyn & Bacon, Boston
Tenenbaum JB, Griffiths TL (2001) Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. Behav Brain Sci 24(04):629–640
Thompson B, Kirby S, Smith K (2016) Culture shapes the evolution of cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113(16):4530–4535
Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol 20:410–433
Wallis JD, Anderson KC, Miller EK (2001) Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411(6840):953–956
Walsh M, Möbius B, Wade T, Schütze H (2010) Multilevel exemplar theory. Cogn Sci 34(4):537–582
White J (2014) Evidence for a learning bias against saltatory phonological alternations. Cognition 130(1):96–115
Wonnacott E (2013) Learning: statistical mechanisms in language acquisition. In: Binder PM, Smith K (eds) The language phenomenon. The frontiers collection. Springer, Berlin, pp 65–92
Wulff S (2003) A multifactorial corpus analysis of adjective order in English. Int J Corpus Linguist 8(2):245–282
Zuidema W, de Boer B (2009) The evolution of combinatorial phonology. J Phon 37(2):125–144
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Spike, M. The evolution of linguistic rules. Biol Philos 32, 887–904 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9610-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9610-x