Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content

The evolution of linguistic rules

  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rule-like behaviour is found throughout human language, provoking a number of apparently conflicting explanations. This paper frames the topic in terms of Tinbergen’s four questions and works within the context of rule-like behaviour seen both in nature and the non-linguistic domain in humans. I argue for a minimal account of linguistic rules which relies on powerful domain-general cognition, has a communicative function allowing for multiple engineering solutions, and evolves mainly culturally, while leaving the door open for some genetic adaptation in the form of learning biases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. cf. Strunk and White (1979).

  2. As an example, the Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics (Brown 2005) conservatively lists 13 distinct schools of phonology, 12 models of morphology, and 27 syntactic frameworks.

  3. What Huddlestone and Pullum (2002) call the ‘labile ordering of residual pre-head modifiers’.

  4. In this case, something vaguely resembling Optimality Phonology.

  5. However, as an anonymous reviewer points out, there is an extensive literature on potential similarities between action sequencing and language processing which has yet to resolve itself into anything resembling a consensus, so we should remain cautious about making any categorical statements here.

References

  • Anderson SR (1981) Why phonology isn’t “natural”. Linguist Inq 12(4):493–539

    Google Scholar 

  • Aslin RN (2012) Questioning the questions that have been asked about the infant brain using near-infrared spectroscopy. Cogn Neuropsychol 29(1–2):7–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin RN, Shukla M, Emberson LL (2015) Hemodynamic correlates of cognition in human infants. Annu Rev Psychol 66(1):349–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent I (2013) The phonological mind. Trends Cogn Sci 17(7):319–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent I (2016) Commentary: “an evaluation of universal grammar and the phonological mind”—UG is still a viable hypothesis. Front Psychol 7(JUL):1–13

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2015) Why only us?. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Pietroski P, Yankama B, Chomsky N (2011) Poverty of the stimulus revisited. Cogn Sci 35(7):1207–1242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins J (2004) Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins J (2008) Natural and unnatural sound patterns: a pocket field guide. In: Willems K, De Cuypere L (eds) Naturalness and iconicity in language. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 121–148

  • Boeckx C (2010) Language in cognition. Wiley-Blackwell, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures, vol 43, No. 02. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown K (ed) (2005) Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckley E (2000) What should phonology explain? Handout from SUNY Buffalo linguistics colloquium.

  • Bunge SA (2005) Neural circuitry underlying rule use in humans and nonhuman primates. J Neurosci 25(45):10347–10350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater N, Clark A, Goldsmith JA, Perfors A (2015) Empiricism and language learnability. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chater N, Reali F, Christiansen MH (2009) Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(4):1015–1020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2005) Three factors in language design. Linguist Inq 36(1):1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N, Halle M (1968) The sound pattern of English. Harper, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen M, Reali F, Chater N (2011) Biological adaptations for functional features of language in the face of cultural evolution. Hum Biol 83(2):247–259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen MH, Chater N (2008) Language as shaped by the brain. Behav Brain Sci 31(5):489–508 discussion 509–558

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen MH, Chater N (2016) The Now-or-Never bottleneck: a fundamental constraint on language. Behav Brain Sci 39:1–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croft W (2001) Radical construction grammar: syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Croft W (2013) Evolution: language use and the evolution of languages. In: Binder PM, Smith K (eds) The language phenomenon. The frontiers collection. Springer, Berlin, pp 93–120

  • de Lacy P, Kingston J (2013) Synchronic explanation. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 31(2):287–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman JL (1991) Distributed representations, simple recurrent networks, and grammatical structure. Mach Learn 7(2):195–225

    Google Scholar 

  • Everett DL (2016) An evaluation of universal grammar and the phonological mind. Front Psychol 7(15):1–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303(5656):377–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervain J, Berent I, Werker JF (2012) Binding at birth: the newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. J Cogn Neurosci 24(3):564–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson E, Piantadosi ST, Brink K, Bergen L, Lim E, Saxe R (2013) A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word-order variation. Psychol Sci 24(7):1079–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 205(1161):581–598

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano M (2006) The organization of behavioral repertoire in motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 29(1):105–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths TL, Chater N, Kemp C, Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB (2010) Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):357–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths TL, Kemp C, Tenenbaum JB (2007) Bayesian models of cognition, pp 1–49

  • Griffiths TL, Lieder F, Goodman ND (2015) Rational use of cognitive resources: levels of analysis between the computational and the algorithmic. Top Cogn Sci 7(2):217–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale M, Reiss C (2008) The phonological enterprise. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Huddlestone R, Pullum GK (2002) The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson S (2013) Biolinguistics or physicolinguistics? Is the third factor helpful or harmful in explaining language? Biolinguistics 2005:249–275

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan RM, Kay M (1994) Regular models of phonological rule systems. Comput Linguist 20:331–378

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby S, Tamariz M, Cornish H, Smith K (2015) Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition 141:87–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linzen T, Donnell TJO (2015) A model of rapid phonotactic generalization. In: Emnlp, pp 1126–1131

  • Lisman J (2015) The challenge of understanding the brain: where we stand in 2015. Neuron 86(4):864–882

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mantini D, Corbetta M, Romani GL, Orban GA, Vanduffel W (2013) Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? J Neurosci 33(8):3259–3275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus GF (1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283(5398):77–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr D, Poggio T (1976) From understanding computation to understanding neural circuitry

  • Ohala JJ (2005) Phonetic explanations for sound patterns: implications for grammars of competence. In: A figure of speech. A festschrift for John Laver, pp 23–38

  • Oudeyer P-Y (2005) The self-organization of combinatoriality and phonotactics in vocalization systems. Connect Sci 17(3–4):325–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1991) Rules of language. Science 253(5019):530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1999) Words and rules. Basic Books Inc, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S, Bloom P (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behav Brain Sci 13(13):707–784

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S, Jackendoff R (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition 95(2):201–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran JR, Pollak SD, Seibel RL, Shkolnik A (2007) Dog is a dog is a dog: infant rule learning is not specific to language. Cognition 105(3):669–680

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels B (2009) The third factor in phonology. Biolinguistics 3(2–3):355–382

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandler W, Aronoff M, Meir I, Padden C (2011) The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 29(2):503–543

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilthuizen M, Davison A (2005) The convoluted evolution of snail chirality. Naturwissenschaften 92(11):504–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strunk W, White EB (1979) The elements of style. Allyn & Bacon, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum JB, Griffiths TL (2001) Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. Behav Brain Sci 24(04):629–640

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson B, Kirby S, Smith K (2016) Culture shapes the evolution of cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113(16):4530–4535

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol 20:410–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallis JD, Anderson KC, Miller EK (2001) Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411(6840):953–956

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh M, Möbius B, Wade T, Schütze H (2010) Multilevel exemplar theory. Cogn Sci 34(4):537–582

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White J (2014) Evidence for a learning bias against saltatory phonological alternations. Cognition 130(1):96–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wonnacott E (2013) Learning: statistical mechanisms in language acquisition. In: Binder PM, Smith K (eds) The language phenomenon. The frontiers collection. Springer, Berlin, pp 65–92

  • Wulff S (2003) A multifactorial corpus analysis of adjective order in English. Int J Corpus Linguist 8(2):245–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuidema W, de Boer B (2009) The evolution of combinatorial phonology. J Phon 37(2):125–144

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Spike.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Spike, M. The evolution of linguistic rules. Biol Philos 32, 887–904 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9610-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9610-x

Keywords