The paper introduces PaVeDa (Pavia Verbs Database), a resource that builds on the ValPaL database... more The paper introduces PaVeDa (Pavia Verbs Database), a resource that builds on the ValPaL database of verbs' valency patterns and alternations by adding a number of ancient languages (completely absent from ValPaL) and a number of new features that enable direct comparison, both diachronic and synchronic. For each verb, ValPaL contains the basic frame and ideally all possible valency alternations allowed by the verb (e.g. passive, causative, reflexive etc.). In order to enable comparison among alternations, an additional level has been added, the alternation class, that overcomes the issue of comparing language specific alternations which were added by individual contributors of ValPaL. The ValPaL had as its main aim typological comparison, and data collection was variously carried out using questionnaires, secondary sources and largely drawing on native speaker intuition by contributors. Working with ancient languages entails a methodological change, as the data is extracted from corpora. This has led to rethinking the notion of valency as a usage-based feature of verbs and to planning future addition of corpus data to modern languages in the database. It further shows the impact of ancient languages on theoretical reflection.
Proceedings of the 12th Global Wordnet Conference, 2023
The Sanskrit WordNet is a resource currently under development, whose core was induced from a Ved... more The Sanskrit WordNet is a resource currently under development, whose core was induced from a Vedic text sample semantically annotated by means of an ontology mapped on the Princeton WordNet synsets. Building on a previous case study on Ancient Greek (Zanchi et al. 2021), we show how sentence frames can be extracted from morphosyntactically parsed corpora by linking an existing dependency treebank of Vedic Sanskrit to verbal synsets in the Sanskrit WordNet. Our case study focuses on two verbs of asking, yācand prach-, featuring a high degree of variability in sentence frames. Treebanks enhanced with WordNet-based semantic information revealed to be of crucial help in motivating sentence frame alternations.
Two external possessor constructions occur in ancient Indo-European languages: the dative externa... more Two external possessor constructions occur in ancient Indo-European languages: the dative external possessor construction, and the double case construction. They both indicate adnominal possession by means of syntactically independent NPs, and basically refer to inalienable possession. In this article, I analyze the two constructions, describe their meaning and their syntactic properties, and review the comparative evidence for each of them. Neither construction is uniformly attested throughout the Indo-European language family. In addition, the dative external possessor construction seems to be quite unstable over time. Based on the data presented, I conclude that the former can be reconstructed as an original Proto-Indo-European construction, while the latter must be regarded as a language specific construction, with different properties in the languages in which it occurs.
Zurück zur Wurzel Struktur, Funktion und Semantik der Wurzel im Indogermanischen, 2022
In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE... more In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE voice system. I show that the middle voice played a role in the anticausative alternation, and suggest that this was the original function of voice alternation. Before that, the distribution of active and middle verbs was arguably lexical. The paper is organized as follows. In sec. 2 I discuss basic valency as defined in NICHOLS/PETERSON/BARNES 2004, and its connection with the anticausative alternation. Then I review the data from ancient IE languages, and show how their basic valency can be defined (sec. 3). In sec. 4 I discuss the relation between valency, the anticausative alternation, and lexical aspect, and in sec. 5 I show how the anticausative alternation and lexical aspect relate with the IE middle voice. Sec. 6 contains the conclusion
This paper discusses some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve... more This paper discusses some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve all available features of the controller, targets some normally non-agreeing items, or is triggered by types of word that are normally not controllers. Fuzzy boundaries are shown to exist among word classes (nouns that are treated as adjectives and adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena. Issues discussed include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations (section 3); special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages (section 4), and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers (section 5). In section 2 I outline Corbett’s (2004) canonical approach to typology, especially regarding agreement.
While the semantic role instrument is uniformly encoded through the
instrumental case (or cases t... more While the semantic role instrument is uniformly encoded through the instrumental case (or cases that replaced it, such as the Latin ablative or the Greek dative) in ancient Indo-European (IE) languages, cause is variously encoded through plain cases or adpositional phrases. Comparison allows reconstructing a similar situation for Proto-Indo-European (PIE) as well. In particular, one can reconstruct cause expressions involving the plain instrumental, the plain ablative, and the adverb *pró, which, most likely, already had an adpositional function in the proto-language. It is assumed that cases and adverbs/adpositions originally had a spatial meaning, and that abstract meanings came about through metaphoric extension. In the case of the ablative and of *pró in cause expressions, some common patterns of semantic extension are visible (from source/origin to cause: the cause of a situation is conceived as its origin; from location to cause: a cause is conceived as an entity which constrains a situation by being placed in front of it). The instrumental case is traditionally thought as having originated as a comitative. While extension from comitative to instrument is based on the well-known ‘Companion metaphor’, an earlier local meaning of the PIE instrumental case cannot be reconstructed easily.
Tlie PIE gender system consisted of two genders, most likely animate and neuter; the earliest man... more Tlie PIE gender system consisted of two genders, most likely animate and neuter; the earliest manifestations of feminine gender were derivational and involved the suffix *-h2, which in origin derived abstract nouns. This suffix also gave origin to the neuter nominative-accusative plural, formerly a collective rather than a count plural. The semantic development is accompanied by morphological change: in the case of the neuter nominative-accusative plural, a derivational suffix became an inflectional ending, while, in the case o f feminine gender, a derivational suffix became the marker o f an inflectional class. The two morphological developments are different, and there is no reason for assuming that one of them implied the other. However, when discussing the semantic aspect of the change, it is generally assumed that either collective preceded feminine or the other way around. In my paper I suggest a different solution by considering that the two developments must be kept separated.
The paper discusses the use of cases in Molisean Croatian, a high contact South Slavic variety sp... more The paper discusses the use of cases in Molisean Croatian, a high contact South Slavic variety spoken in Southern Italy by about 1,000 speakers. Based on available texts and on data from informants, we show that cases are still realized by older, fluent speakers, even though the extent to which they actually contribute to distinguishing meanings is limited. As a consequence, case morphology is no longer mastered by younger semi-speakers. Absence of fluent speakers among young generations is thus leading to the decay of the case system. Patterns of reduction of case meanings in Molisean Croatian are similar to those found in other high contact Slavic varieties.
In this chapter, I discuss the relation between language typology and historical linguistics. Aft... more In this chapter, I discuss the relation between language typology and historical linguistics. After briefly illustrating mutual influence in the course of the development of the two disciplines, I discuss some classic topics at the interface between language typology and historical-comparative linguistics, such as language reconstruction. I then focus on language contact as an area of possible interaction between typology and historical linguistics, and show that areal typology has consequences on our understanding of language relatedness, while changes due to contact may result in typologically unlikely developments. Finally, I address the issue of directionality in language change, and show that careful study of attested changes can help gauge the validity of typological generalizations based on large samples of languages. I conclude by suggesting that both typologists and historical linguists could learn a lot from one another, especially if they were willing to accept the idea that their own methodology does not always necessarily provide the most insightful way for tackling specific issues.
In this paper, I discuss some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not inv... more In this paper, I discuss some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve all available features of the controller, targets some normally non-agreeing items, or is triggered by types of word that are normally not controllers. All issues reviewed highlight the impossibility of coming up with clear-cut boundaries between categories. Fuzzy boundaries are shown to exist among word classes (nouns that are treated as adjectives and adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena.
Issues discussed in this paper include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations; special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages, and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers.
The paper introduces PaVeDa (Pavia Verbs Database), a resource that builds on the ValPaL database... more The paper introduces PaVeDa (Pavia Verbs Database), a resource that builds on the ValPaL database of verbs' valency patterns and alternations by adding a number of ancient languages (completely absent from ValPaL) and a number of new features that enable direct comparison, both diachronic and synchronic. For each verb, ValPaL contains the basic frame and ideally all possible valency alternations allowed by the verb (e.g. passive, causative, reflexive etc.). In order to enable comparison among alternations, an additional level has been added, the alternation class, that overcomes the issue of comparing language specific alternations which were added by individual contributors of ValPaL. The ValPaL had as its main aim typological comparison, and data collection was variously carried out using questionnaires, secondary sources and largely drawing on native speaker intuition by contributors. Working with ancient languages entails a methodological change, as the data is extracted from corpora. This has led to rethinking the notion of valency as a usage-based feature of verbs and to planning future addition of corpus data to modern languages in the database. It further shows the impact of ancient languages on theoretical reflection.
Proceedings of the 12th Global Wordnet Conference, 2023
The Sanskrit WordNet is a resource currently under development, whose core was induced from a Ved... more The Sanskrit WordNet is a resource currently under development, whose core was induced from a Vedic text sample semantically annotated by means of an ontology mapped on the Princeton WordNet synsets. Building on a previous case study on Ancient Greek (Zanchi et al. 2021), we show how sentence frames can be extracted from morphosyntactically parsed corpora by linking an existing dependency treebank of Vedic Sanskrit to verbal synsets in the Sanskrit WordNet. Our case study focuses on two verbs of asking, yācand prach-, featuring a high degree of variability in sentence frames. Treebanks enhanced with WordNet-based semantic information revealed to be of crucial help in motivating sentence frame alternations.
Two external possessor constructions occur in ancient Indo-European languages: the dative externa... more Two external possessor constructions occur in ancient Indo-European languages: the dative external possessor construction, and the double case construction. They both indicate adnominal possession by means of syntactically independent NPs, and basically refer to inalienable possession. In this article, I analyze the two constructions, describe their meaning and their syntactic properties, and review the comparative evidence for each of them. Neither construction is uniformly attested throughout the Indo-European language family. In addition, the dative external possessor construction seems to be quite unstable over time. Based on the data presented, I conclude that the former can be reconstructed as an original Proto-Indo-European construction, while the latter must be regarded as a language specific construction, with different properties in the languages in which it occurs.
Zurück zur Wurzel Struktur, Funktion und Semantik der Wurzel im Indogermanischen, 2022
In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE... more In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE voice system. I show that the middle voice played a role in the anticausative alternation, and suggest that this was the original function of voice alternation. Before that, the distribution of active and middle verbs was arguably lexical. The paper is organized as follows. In sec. 2 I discuss basic valency as defined in NICHOLS/PETERSON/BARNES 2004, and its connection with the anticausative alternation. Then I review the data from ancient IE languages, and show how their basic valency can be defined (sec. 3). In sec. 4 I discuss the relation between valency, the anticausative alternation, and lexical aspect, and in sec. 5 I show how the anticausative alternation and lexical aspect relate with the IE middle voice. Sec. 6 contains the conclusion
This paper discusses some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve... more This paper discusses some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve all available features of the controller, targets some normally non-agreeing items, or is triggered by types of word that are normally not controllers. Fuzzy boundaries are shown to exist among word classes (nouns that are treated as adjectives and adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena. Issues discussed include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations (section 3); special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages (section 4), and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers (section 5). In section 2 I outline Corbett’s (2004) canonical approach to typology, especially regarding agreement.
While the semantic role instrument is uniformly encoded through the
instrumental case (or cases t... more While the semantic role instrument is uniformly encoded through the instrumental case (or cases that replaced it, such as the Latin ablative or the Greek dative) in ancient Indo-European (IE) languages, cause is variously encoded through plain cases or adpositional phrases. Comparison allows reconstructing a similar situation for Proto-Indo-European (PIE) as well. In particular, one can reconstruct cause expressions involving the plain instrumental, the plain ablative, and the adverb *pró, which, most likely, already had an adpositional function in the proto-language. It is assumed that cases and adverbs/adpositions originally had a spatial meaning, and that abstract meanings came about through metaphoric extension. In the case of the ablative and of *pró in cause expressions, some common patterns of semantic extension are visible (from source/origin to cause: the cause of a situation is conceived as its origin; from location to cause: a cause is conceived as an entity which constrains a situation by being placed in front of it). The instrumental case is traditionally thought as having originated as a comitative. While extension from comitative to instrument is based on the well-known ‘Companion metaphor’, an earlier local meaning of the PIE instrumental case cannot be reconstructed easily.
Tlie PIE gender system consisted of two genders, most likely animate and neuter; the earliest man... more Tlie PIE gender system consisted of two genders, most likely animate and neuter; the earliest manifestations of feminine gender were derivational and involved the suffix *-h2, which in origin derived abstract nouns. This suffix also gave origin to the neuter nominative-accusative plural, formerly a collective rather than a count plural. The semantic development is accompanied by morphological change: in the case of the neuter nominative-accusative plural, a derivational suffix became an inflectional ending, while, in the case o f feminine gender, a derivational suffix became the marker o f an inflectional class. The two morphological developments are different, and there is no reason for assuming that one of them implied the other. However, when discussing the semantic aspect of the change, it is generally assumed that either collective preceded feminine or the other way around. In my paper I suggest a different solution by considering that the two developments must be kept separated.
The paper discusses the use of cases in Molisean Croatian, a high contact South Slavic variety sp... more The paper discusses the use of cases in Molisean Croatian, a high contact South Slavic variety spoken in Southern Italy by about 1,000 speakers. Based on available texts and on data from informants, we show that cases are still realized by older, fluent speakers, even though the extent to which they actually contribute to distinguishing meanings is limited. As a consequence, case morphology is no longer mastered by younger semi-speakers. Absence of fluent speakers among young generations is thus leading to the decay of the case system. Patterns of reduction of case meanings in Molisean Croatian are similar to those found in other high contact Slavic varieties.
In this chapter, I discuss the relation between language typology and historical linguistics. Aft... more In this chapter, I discuss the relation between language typology and historical linguistics. After briefly illustrating mutual influence in the course of the development of the two disciplines, I discuss some classic topics at the interface between language typology and historical-comparative linguistics, such as language reconstruction. I then focus on language contact as an area of possible interaction between typology and historical linguistics, and show that areal typology has consequences on our understanding of language relatedness, while changes due to contact may result in typologically unlikely developments. Finally, I address the issue of directionality in language change, and show that careful study of attested changes can help gauge the validity of typological generalizations based on large samples of languages. I conclude by suggesting that both typologists and historical linguists could learn a lot from one another, especially if they were willing to accept the idea that their own methodology does not always necessarily provide the most insightful way for tackling specific issues.
In this paper, I discuss some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not inv... more In this paper, I discuss some constructions in which, for various reasons, agreement does not involve all available features of the controller, targets some normally non-agreeing items, or is triggered by types of word that are normally not controllers. All issues reviewed highlight the impossibility of coming up with clear-cut boundaries between categories. Fuzzy boundaries are shown to exist among word classes (nouns that are treated as adjectives and adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena.
Issues discussed in this paper include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations; special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages, and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers.
ABSTRACT: A fully revised version of the author's 1987 Ph. D. dissertation u... more ABSTRACT: A fully revised version of the author's 1987 Ph. D. dissertation under the title: La struttura della frase semplice e dei costituenti di frase nell'ittita arcaico. Includes bibliographical references (p. 178-184) and indexes.
As opposed to the ancient IE varieties in which overt vs. null realization of referential subject... more As opposed to the ancient IE varieties in which overt vs. null realization of referential subjects was largely determined by discourse factors, some modern IE languages of Europe have lost null subjects to different extents. These include all Germanic languages, French and some Northern Italian vernacular varieties (Romance), and Russian. This development raises a number of interesting issues, such as the role of language contact, what started the (partial or complete) loss of referential null subjects in the three languages families in the first place, and whether attested developments are parallel or show significant differences. The talk addresses these and other issues through a description of historical phases of the languages involved.
In Croft (2000: 121) hyperanalysis is defined as a type of form-function reanalysis whereby “… t... more In Croft (2000: 121) hyperanalysis is defined as a type of form-function reanalysis whereby “… the listener reanalyzes an inherent semantic/functional property of a syntactic unit as a contextual property […]. In the reanalysis, this inherent property of a syntactic unit is then attributed to the context […], and so the syntactic unit in question loses some of its meaning or function. Hence, hyperanalysis is a major source of semantic bleaching and/or loss in general”. Among examples of hyperanalysis, Croft mentions the loss of governed oblique cases in Russian and in some Germanic languages, and argues that “The inherent semantic value of … case can be reanalyzed as belonging solely to the verb meaning” (2000: 122). Hyperanalysis leads to substitution of the oblique case with the accusative in Russian, whereas in Icelandic a governed oblique can become the subject of a personal passive, even though it is not substituted by the accusative when it functions as object or by the nominative when it functions as subject. In my paper I would like to discuss a case of hyperanalysis involving governed oblique cases in Ancient Greek. In this language, several verbs can take the genitive or the dative, and also occur in personal passive constructions, in which they regularly have nominative subjects. Conti (1998) has shown that only the partitive genitive could become the subject of a passive construction in Homeric Greek, while verbs taking the dative occur in personal passive constructions only in later prose. Hyperanalysis in this case implies reanalyzing the oblique arguments as direct objects. I argue that this type of reanalysis is easier with the partitive genitive due to the peculiar meaning of the partitive, which does not indicate a semantic role, but rather a way of conceptualizing the a referent (see Luraghi 2003a: 60-62); the dative, on the other hand, could indicate semantic roles, so the reanalysis of dative objects as indicating patient was slower. Note that in origin the difference between accusative objects and oblique objects in Ancient Greek lied in different degrees of affectedness, whereby only the accusative indicated total affectedness and thus a higher degree of transitivity. Hyperanalysis consists in the desemanticization of cases; increasing tendency of oblique objects to be treated as direct objects indicates that they tend to loose their role in indicating partial affectedness. After reanalysis, the degree of transitivity of a sentence is no longer indicated both by the meaning of the verb and by the case, but by the verb alone. As pointed out by Conti (1998), passive constructions only occur with highly transitive verbs, i.e. verbs that indicate controlled situations, while verbs that indicate states or processes and take oblique objects are normally not passivized. Conti further argues that, in much the same way, verbs that take accusative direct objects are more frequently passivized if they are highly transitive. Luraghi (2003b) shows that passive agents can be variously encoded in Ancient Greek, but passive constructions with highly transitive verbs require agent phrases encoded through prepositional phrases consisting of hupó with the genitive. This type of prepositional phrase expresses control over the situation, based on the concrete meaning of hupó ‘over’. Thus, high transitivity is a distinctive feature of the passive construction in Ancient Greek. When governed oblique cases are desemanticized due to hyperanalysis, they start being able to occur in passive constructions, because they no longer contribute to expressing the overall degree of transitivity of a sentence.
As well known, Old Russian was a null subject language. The loss of referential null subjects can... more As well known, Old Russian was a null subject language. The loss of referential null subjects can be observed in texts during the history of Russian. A recent corpus study has shown that the rate at which referential null subjects decrease is much faster in subordinate than in main clauses. In particular, in texts from the 12th to the 17th century the percentage of 3rd person null subjects in subordinate clauses remains stable and close to 100% until the beginning of the 16th century, and drops suddenly to 63% at the end of the century and 25 % in the course of the 17th century, while in the same corpus 3rd person null subjects in main clauses remain above 70% until the end of the period considered. Notably, a higher frequency of overt subjects in subordinate clauses has also been detected in Early Germanic languages in the process of losing null referential subjects. This finding challenges various assumptions concerning the conservative status of subordinate vs. main clauses, and their tendency to preserve more ancient patterns. In our paper, we try to understand the reasons for the observed development, also in the light of ongoing changes in the system of subordination that were taking place in Russian at the same time.
Word order is a central issue in the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European syntax. Categorical ap... more Word order is a central issue in the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European syntax. Categorical approaches have proved to be inadequate because they postulate for the protolanguage a typological consistency which is absent in any of the attested daughter languages. Following recent research, we adopt a gradient approach to word order, which treats word order preferences as a continuous variable. We analyze four word order patterns based on data extracted from treebanks of ancient Indo-European languages. After presenting our results for AdpN/NAdp, GN/NG, AN/NA, and OV/VO, we draw a number of conclusions concerning variation within individual languages, crosslinguistic variation, and variation in diachrony that support the claim that variability should be taken as the normal state across languages, including reconstructed stages. We conclude that a non-discrete approach has the advantage of leading to a reconstruction that better conforms to the situation known from real languages, with variation as a key feature. 2 biagetti et al.
In Hittite, deictic motion verbs pai- ‘go’ and uwa- ‘come’ may co-occur in a monoclausal structur... more In Hittite, deictic motion verbs pai- ‘go’ and uwa- ‘come’ may co-occur in a monoclausal structure with a second verb that carries the lexical meaning. As yet, their exact function remains obscure. I argue that motion verbs involved in such construction underwent transcategorization and function as New Event Markers. I show that this development is best explained as an instance of constructionalization involving both the motion verbs and the second verb in the clause, which is based on a pragmatic inference arising when motion verbs were used without a spatial complement. Either motion verb contributes a different semantics to the construction based on the different perspective regarding the deictic center identified by the ego, whereby pai- ‘go’ (motion originating from the deictic center) marks an event as close in time and controlled, while uwa- ‘come’ (motion originating outside the deictic center) indicates distance in time and possible lack of control.
The IE languages developed different strategies for the encoding of the passive function. In some... more The IE languages developed different strategies for the encoding of the passive function. In some language branches, the middle voice extended to the passive function to varying extents. In addition, dedicated derivational formations arose in a number of languages, such as the Greek-ē-/-thē-aorist and the Indo-Aryan-ya-presents. Periphrastic formations involving a verbal adjective or a participle are also widely attested, and played an important role in the building of the passive paradigm in e.g. Romance and Germanic languages. As the periphrastic passive is also attested in Hittite alongside passive use of the middle, both strategies seem to be equally ancient. Some minor strategies include lexical passives and the extensive lability of verbs. A survey of possible strategies provides evidence for the rise of a disparate number of morphemes and constructions, and for their ongoing incorporation into the inflectional paradigms (paradigmaticization) of given languages, thus adding to our knowledge about cross-linguistic sources of passive morphology and grammaticalization processes involved.
In several ancient and modern Indo-European languages, the partitivegenitive may be used in place... more In several ancient and modern Indo-European languages, the partitivegenitive may be used in place of the accusative to encode the second argument of two-place verbs. In Ancient Greek the two types of object encoding can alternate with change-of-state verbs, alternation being viewed as connected with degrees of patient affectedness: the partitive-genitive encodes partially affected objects. Alternation also extends to experiential verbs, which are typically characterized by a low degree of transitivity and do not imply any change of state of the object-stimulus. Rather than concentrating on the implications of case alternation on the construal of the object, I consider the effects of variation on the whole construction, and argue that genitive vs. accusative marking of the object (NomGen vs. NomAcc constructions) reflects the construal of the subject-experiencer. While the different construal of the experiencer in terms of degrees of control crosslinguistically often results in non-nominative encoding of the experiencer, in Ancient Greek it is object encoding that affects the construal of the experiencer and reflects a scale based on possible control. The distribution of constructions with experiential verbs shows that NomAcc is typical of verbs of sight, thought, intellectual knowledge and emotions connected to sight and awareness, such as wonder and fear. NomGen is connected with touch, smell, taste, memory, forgetfulness, care and desire. In the in-between area, verbs of hearing, learning and verbs of affection may feature both accusative and genitive encoding, thus constituting a fuzzy transition area. The connection between sight and other experiential verbs that feature accusative encoding reflects an embodied conceptualization of experiential situations.
As has been highlighted in the literature (see Luraghi & Huumo 2014a: 3–4)
the word ‘partitive’ h... more As has been highlighted in the literature (see Luraghi & Huumo 2014a: 3–4) the word ‘partitive’ has often been used in reference to different types of linguistic items or constructions without further specification. In recent years, a number of studies have called attention to the different features of items covered by this label. In particular, it has been shown that partitive cases of languages such as Finnish, Estonian and Basque have much in common with partitive genitives known from the Indo-European languages as well as with partitive articles featured by some Romance languages, which in their turn parallel partitive determiners, verbal affixes or other clitics from Oceanic languages. This apparently heterogeneous array of different items did not receive a unified treatment in spite of striking similarities and of extensive language-specific studies, partly because the label ‘partitive’ blurred the difference between these items and partitive constructions (on which see Section 2).
Indo-European linguistics and classical philology, 2022
The encoding of the semantic role of direction may display animacy based differential marking. Cr... more The encoding of the semantic role of direction may display animacy based differential marking. Cross-linguistic data also show that both human and inanimate direction may be encoded in the same way as the semantic role of recipient. After briefly surveying some attested patterns in the encoding of these three semantic roles, the paper concentrates on three Ancient Indo-European languages, Hittite, Latin and Ancient Greek. Among them, only Hittite makes use of the dative case to encode direction, while in the other languages the dative is limited to the role of recipient. Homeric Greek displays a cross-linguistically infrequent pattern, with the illative preposition extending to human direction. This pattern is dropped in Attic-Ionic prose.
Non-configurationality is a property of some languages associated with free word order, discontin... more Non-configurationality is a property of some languages associated with free word order, discontinuous constituents, including NPs, and null anaphora of referential arguments. Quantitative metrics, based both on local networks (syntactic trees and word order within sentences) and on global networks (incorporating the relations within a whole treebank into a shared graph), can reveal correlations among these features. We focus on diachronic varieties of Ancient Greek and Latin, in which non-configurationality tapered off in time, leading to the largely configurational nature of the Romance languages and of Modern Greek. A property of global networks (density of their spectra around zero eigenvalues) measuring the regularity in word order is shown to be strengthened from classical to late varieties. Discontinuous NPs are traced by counting the words creating non-projectivity in dependency trees: these drop dramatically in late varieties. Finally, developments in the use of null referential direct objects are gauged by assessing the percentage of third-person personal pronouns among verb objects. All these three features turn out to change in time due to the decay of non-configurationality. Evaluation of the strength of their pairwise correlation shows that null direct objects and discontinuous NPs are deeply intertwined. La non-configurationalité est une propriété de certaines langues, associée à l'ordre des mots libre, aux constituants discontinus et à l'anaphore nulle des arguments verbaux référentiels. Certaines
The paperaims at giving a unified account of the origins and development of the Anatolian ‘local ... more The paperaims at giving a unified account of the origins and development of the Anatolian ‘local particles’, which, as such, are not attested in any other Indo-European language. The particles are P2 clitics and mostly co-occur with some type of local expression. The function of the Indo-European preverbs is taken by a set of adverbs which, in the earliest stages of Anatolian, display a number of features typical of nominal constituents, as in the expression attas=mas appan, ‘afterm y father’, where the adverb appan takes a modifierin the genitive. The adverbs often co-occur with the particles. Etymologically, both the adverbs and the particles can in part be connected with the Indo-European preverbs. Since postpositional and preverbal syntax for the adverbs appears to develop during the history of Hittite, it has been suggested that Anatolian is particularly archaic, because it mirrors a stage of Indo-European at which there where no adpositions and no preverbs, but only independent adverbs. I argue that the particles go back to adverbial elements, and were formerly the functional equivalent of the Indo-European preverbs/adpositions. Later, these forms underwent semantic bleaching, became clitic and were attracted toward P2. (It must be mentioned that Anatolian is particularly rich in P2 clitics, which also include sentence particles, modal particles, and various types of pronominal forms). After cliticization of the former preverbs, the class of preverbs/adposition was reconstructed through the creation of local adverbs, which, in their turn, partly derive from nominal forms, partly from Indo-European adverbs. My claim has two consequences: a) it shows that the alleged archaism of Anatolian, at least as far as the existence of preverbs/adpositions is concerned, is an illusion; this word class did exist before Anatolian split from the rest of Indo-European already, but it was lost and renewed: what we observe in Old Hittite is the beginning of this renewal; b) it provides an example of a grammaticalization chain shift: former preverbs have undergone semantic bleaching and phonological reduction, becoming particles, and new preverbs have been created out of formerad verbs and nouns to replace the olderones.
I. As a functional moneme,1 the Russian instrumental can indicate a wide range of different funct... more I. As a functional moneme,1 the Russian instrumental can indicate a wide range of different functions. The aim of the present paper is to give an interpretation of the instrumental case in modern standard Russian,2 based on the assumption that functional monemes of the ...
In Ancient Greek two-place verbs can take three different argument structure constructions, with ... more In Ancient Greek two-place verbs can take three different argument structure constructions, with nominative first arguments and either accusative or genitive or dative second arguments. While the accusative is the default case across all verb classes, the genitive indicates low agentivity of the first participant and low affectedness of the second, and the dative is especially frequent with social interaction verbs. The paper focuses on constructional patterns and construction variation with experiential verbs. In particular, fear verbs consistently take the accusative, verbs of desiring the genitive and verbs of affection alternate between the two. Based on data from experimental psychology, I argue that this reflects physical embodiment, with fear verbs showing the same constructional pattern of sight verbs, which indicates narrow attention focus, and verbs of desiring following the constructional pattern of verbs of bodily sensation, smell, taste and touch, which in turn indicates broad attention focus. Verbs that denote positive feelings may take either construction, thus pointing toward a moderately enhanced focus of attention. Emotion verbs that take the dative indicate potentially interactive situations, including those that involve social judgment, thus conforming to the pattern of interaction verbs and reflecting social embodiment. Finally, constructional patterns of verbs of hate and care show that similar situations can be construed differently by profiling either the physical or the social correlates of the emotion.
The paper surveys the development of the voice system with perception verbs in Ancient Greek. Mos... more The paper surveys the development of the voice system with perception verbs in Ancient Greek. Most of such verbs start out as activa or media tantum in Homeric Greek and feature voice opposition in later prose. I argue that with former activa tantum this reflects an increasing tendency to share constructional features of transitive verbs, while former media tantum develop causative counterparts over time. Among the verbs surveyed horáō/eîdon 'see' stands out as the only verb that develops to completely conform to the pattern of changeof-state verbs (transitive prototype).
Word order (WO) has always been a central issue in the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European (PIE... more Word order (WO) has always been a central issue in the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European (PIE) syntax. Categorical approaches to WO have proved to be inadequate for the reconstruction of PIE because they postulate for the proto language a typological consistency which is absent in any of the attested daughter languages. Following recent literature, in this paper, we adopt a gradient approach to WO, which treats word order preferences, both within and across languages, as a continuous variable. To do so, we analyze four word order patterns based on data extracted from treebanks of ancient IE languages. After presenting our results for AdpN/NAdp, GN/NG, AN/NA and OV/VO, we draw a number of conclusions concerning variation within individual languages, cross-linguistic variation, and variation in diachrony. Our conclusions support the claim that variability should be taken as the normal state across languages, including reconstructed stages. Therefore, we argue that a non-discrete approach as the one allowed by our methodology has the advantage of leading to a reconstruction that better conforms to the situation known from real languages, with variation as a key feature.
In Modern Standard Russian, the prefix/preposition pair u-/u is peculiar with respect to other si... more In Modern Standard Russian, the prefix/preposition pair u-/u is peculiar with respect to other similar pairs, due to the meaning mismatch between the two. While the prefix u-has an ablative meaning, as shown when it is prefixed to motion verbs, the prepositional phrase u+gen occurs in locative constructions, and other related constructions, such as predicative possession that is expressed via the cross-linguistically common Locative Schema. Etymological considerations show that the meaning preserved by the prefix is older. The only type of occurrence which, according to the literature, preserves the ablative meaning for the u+gen construction preposition is in connection with verbs of requesting, removing, and buying. Notably, however, in other Slavic languages putative ablative contexts are limited to verbs of requesting. Data from MSR, OCS, Polish and Czech lead to the conclusion that the extension of the u+gen construction to verbs of removing in MSR is based on its use for the encoding of predicative possession. Extension to verbs of buying is better explained through the locative meaning of the construction. As a result of different developments, the u+gen construction has become part of the argument structure of a group of verbs including verbs of asking and requesting, verbs of removing and verbs of buying, which are characterized by the common feature of taking human non-recipient third arguments.
In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE... more In this paper, I discuss basic valency in the ancient IE languages, and its relation with the PIE voice system. I show that the middle voice played a role, though to varying extents, in the anticausative alternation, and suggest that this was the original function fulfilled by voice alternation. Before that, the distribution of active and middle verbs was arguably lexical.
In this chapter, we discuss the syntax and semantics of the Hittite periphrastic perfect. Based o... more In this chapter, we discuss the syntax and semantics of the Hittite periphrastic perfect. Based on the evidence, we argue that the periphrastic perfect is a type of auxiliary verb construction, and show that it must be kept distinct from two other constructions, that is, the stative and the passive. While in the former the finite verbs involved must not be considered auxiliaries, the passive is also a type of auxiliary verb construction. We also argue that possible formal identity of the three constructions, which has cross-linguistic parallels, must not be taken as a reason for not keeping them distinct.
Differential marking of Goal and Source is a relatively underresearched topic. Available cross-li... more Differential marking of Goal and Source is a relatively underresearched topic. Available cross-linguistic evidence points toward two possible triggers of differential marking of spatial relations, that is, nouns that denote spatial regions and animate nouns. In this paper, differential Goal and differential marking are studied on the evidence of a diachronic corpus, consisting of the Homeric poems and Herodotus " Histories. In Homeric Greek, differential marking with nouns that denote spatial regions is available to a marginal extent for Goal, and unavailable for Source, while no differential marking occurs in Herodotus. Animacy conditioned differential marking is well attested both for Goal and for Source in Herodotus, but Homer offers evidence only for differential marking of Source. Apparently, Goal prepositions used for inanimate landmarks are also compatible with animates, while Source prepositions developed at an early stage animacy conditioned meanings that make them unsuitable for indicating simple concrete motion away from a human being.
Old Hittite sentence connectives nu, ta and šu display a significantly different distribution whe... more Old Hittite sentence connectives nu, ta and šu display a significantly different distribution when connecting a preposed subordinate clause to the main clause with respect to their distribution between main clauses. Other distributional restrictions concern ta and šu in all syntactic environments, but do not concern nu. The discourse function of connectives when they occur between main clauses points to partly different roles by which they variously indicate event and/or participant continuity, and contribute to the grounding of information. Ongoing increase in the frequency of P2 clitics also called for the occurrence of connectives as hosts. Lack of distributional restrictions on the occurrence of nu made it multifunctional, and suitable as a host for P2 clitics while preserving at least in part its original discourse function: this brought about the onset of a change by which nu replaced other connectives. The discourse function of connectives is blurred between a subordinate and a main clause, as subordinate clauses explicitly indicate their relation to the main clause. For this reason, nu extended more readily to this syntactic environment. Uneven distribution of connectives in different syntactic environments is a consequence of gradualness in actualization of languages change.
Ancient Greek features a number of syntactically and semantically different double accusative con... more Ancient Greek features a number of syntactically and semantically different double accusative constructions. In part, they occur with ditransitive maleficiary verbs. Most of these verbs allow for construction variation in connection with different types of R-arguments (maleficiary in neutral alignment vs. beneficiaries in indirect alignment). Based on behavioral properties of the constructions discussed, we show that some other verbs share the behavior of maleficiary ditransitives, notably verbs of asking, requesting and ordering. We argue that both addressees of such verbs and part of maleficiaries can be conceptualized as sources, thus providing a link between them. Causative verbs that take double accusative constructions do not share behavioral properties of maleficiary verbs with neutral alignment, due to the connection of causativity with verbal voice.
The verb akoúō 'hear' in Homeric Greek can indicate concrete perception as well as acqusition of ... more The verb akoúō 'hear' in Homeric Greek can indicate concrete perception as well as acqusition of knowledge by hearsay, and mean 'learn'. In addition, it can denote an uncontrolled state, either perceptual or cognitive, the controlled activity of listening, or an inchoative event. In the paper, we discuss its syntax and semantics and compare it with klúō 'listen to', which indicates activities, and punthánomai 'learn', which mostly has an inchoative meaning. We show that construction variation is connected with animacy of the stimulus, and it is not triggered by semantic differences in the verbal meaning, with the partial exception of punthánomai when indicating uncontrolled situations. Different actionalities expressed by the three verbs are often matched by verbal aspect. In conclusion, we argue that the extension of hearing to learning and acquiring knowledge is explained through pragmatic inference. The same can be said to the much more studied extention of seeing to possessing knowledge. Different meanings of perception verbs when referring to the domain of cognition are based on embodiment, in that they depend on our knowledge of the structure of perception events.
• What is basic valency orientation • Problems with determining basic valency • Oriented vs. non-... more • What is basic valency orientation • Problems with determining basic valency • Oriented vs. non-oriented • Basic valency orientation in IE languages • Valency and lexical aspect • Valency orientation and the middle voice
Hittite neuter nouns must take a dedicated -ants/antes form when occurring as the subject of a tr... more Hittite neuter nouns must take a dedicated -ants/antes form when occurring as the subject of a transitive verb. There is still no consensus on the interpretation of this pattern: for some scholars -ant- is a derivational suffix indicating gender motion from neuter to common gender. Others regard -ants as an ergative case marker, assuming that Hittite features a gender-based split-ergativity system. This chapter discusses evidence on the distribution of -ants nouns in chronologically ordered Hittite texts, showing that the suffix started out as derivational in Old Hittite, becoming an ergative case ending in New Hittite. We assess the semantics of the suffix at its derivational stage compared with other -ant- formations, showing how its various functions facilitated its development into an ergative marker. We also discuss the reconstruction of alignment in Proto-Anatolian, compared with areal tendencies in ancient Anatolia, framing the Hittite developments within typological data on the rise of ergativity.
Ancient Greek grammar provides evidence for the close relatedness of the notions of container and... more Ancient Greek grammar provides evidence for the close relatedness of the notions of container and instrument. Cognitively these notions are connected by our experience, as many entities can serve both as containers and as instruments. The paper describes the usage of three ...
Non-configurationality is a linguistic property associated with free word order, discontinuous co... more Non-configurationality is a linguistic property associated with free word order, discontinuous constituents, including NPs, and null anaphora of referential arguments. Quantitative metrics, based both on local networks (syntactic trees and word order within sentences) and on global networks (incorporating the relations within a whole treebank into a shared graph), can reveal correlations among these features. Using treebanks we focus on diachronic varieties of Ancient Greek and Latin, in which non-configurationality tapered off over time, leading to the largely configurational nature of the Romance languages and of Modern Greek. A property of global networks (density of their spectra around zero eigenvalues) measuring the regularity in word order is shown to be strengthened from classical to late varieties. Discontinuous NPs are traced by counting the words creating non-projectivity in dependency trees: these drop dramatically in late varieties. Finally, developments in the use of null referential direct objects are gauged by assessing the percentage of third-person personal pronouns among verb objects. All three features turn out to change over time due to the decay of non-configurationality. Evaluation of the strength of their pairwise correlation shows that null direct objects and discontinuous NPs are deeply intertwined.
Studies in language companion series, Nov 17, 2003
Prepositions and cases constitute a fruitful field of research for semantics. The historical deve... more Prepositions and cases constitute a fruitful field of research for semantics. The historical development of their meaning can shed light on the relations among the semantic roles of participants and on the organization of conceptual space. Ancient Greek allows an in-depth study of such development. The book, based on a wide, diachronically ordered corpus, aims at providing a usage-based analysis of possible patterns of semantic extension, including the mapping of abstract domains onto the concrete domain of space. An analysis of the Greek data further highlights the interplay between specific spatial relations and the internal structure of the entities involved, and shows how case semantics may account for differences on the referential level, rather than merely express clause internal relations. The first chapter contains a typologically based discussion of semantic roles, which sets the language-specific analysis in a wider framework, showing its general relevance and applicability.
Uploads
Papers in edited books by Silvia Luraghi
adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena.
Issues discussed include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations (section 3); special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European
languages (section 4), and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers (section 5). In section 2 I outline Corbett’s (2004) canonical approach to typology, especially regarding agreement.
instrumental case (or cases that replaced it, such as the Latin ablative or the
Greek dative) in ancient Indo-European (IE) languages, cause is variously
encoded through plain cases or adpositional phrases. Comparison allows
reconstructing a similar situation for Proto-Indo-European (PIE) as well. In
particular, one can reconstruct cause expressions involving the plain
instrumental, the plain ablative, and the adverb *pró, which, most likely,
already had an adpositional function in the proto-language. It is assumed
that cases and adverbs/adpositions originally had a spatial meaning, and that
abstract meanings came about through metaphoric extension. In the case of
the ablative and of *pró in cause expressions, some common patterns of
semantic extension are visible (from source/origin to cause: the cause of a
situation is conceived as its origin; from location to cause: a cause is
conceived as an entity which constrains a situation by being placed in front
of it). The instrumental case is traditionally thought as having originated as
a comitative. While extension from comitative to instrument is based on the
well-known ‘Companion metaphor’, an earlier local meaning of the PIE
instrumental case cannot be reconstructed easily.
Issues discussed in this paper include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations; special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages, and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers.
adjectives that syntactically and semantically partly behave as nouns), as well as between morphological processes (derivation serving syntax), thus suggesting that fuzziness is a trademark of linguistic phenomena.
Issues discussed include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations (section 3); special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European
languages (section 4), and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers (section 5). In section 2 I outline Corbett’s (2004) canonical approach to typology, especially regarding agreement.
instrumental case (or cases that replaced it, such as the Latin ablative or the
Greek dative) in ancient Indo-European (IE) languages, cause is variously
encoded through plain cases or adpositional phrases. Comparison allows
reconstructing a similar situation for Proto-Indo-European (PIE) as well. In
particular, one can reconstruct cause expressions involving the plain
instrumental, the plain ablative, and the adverb *pró, which, most likely,
already had an adpositional function in the proto-language. It is assumed
that cases and adverbs/adpositions originally had a spatial meaning, and that
abstract meanings came about through metaphoric extension. In the case of
the ablative and of *pró in cause expressions, some common patterns of
semantic extension are visible (from source/origin to cause: the cause of a
situation is conceived as its origin; from location to cause: a cause is
conceived as an entity which constrains a situation by being placed in front
of it). The instrumental case is traditionally thought as having originated as
a comitative. While extension from comitative to instrument is based on the
well-known ‘Companion metaphor’, an earlier local meaning of the PIE
instrumental case cannot be reconstructed easily.
Issues discussed in this paper include agreement between head nouns and appositions, and the possible transcategorization of nouns that become adjectives starting from specific collocations; special characteristics of possessive adjectives in some Indo-European languages, and case agreement in possessive constructions between head nouns and nominal modifiers.
the word ‘partitive’ has often been used in reference to different types of linguistic items or constructions without further specification. In recent years, a number of studies have called attention to the different features of items covered by this label. In particular, it has been shown that partitive cases of languages such as Finnish, Estonian and Basque have much in common with partitive genitives known from the Indo-European languages as well as with partitive articles featured by some Romance languages, which in their turn parallel partitive determiners, verbal affixes or other clitics from Oceanic languages. This apparently heterogeneous array of different items did not receive a unified treatment in spite of striking similarities and of extensive language-specific studies, partly because the label ‘partitive’ blurred the difference between these items and partitive constructions (on which see Section 2).
particles are P2 clitics and mostly co-occur with some type of local expression. The function of the Indo-European preverbs is taken by a set of adverbs which, in the earliest stages of
Anatolian, display a number of features typical of nominal constituents, as in the expression attas=mas appan, ‘afterm y father’, where the adverb appan takes a modifierin the genitive.
The adverbs often co-occur with the particles. Etymologically, both the adverbs and the particles can in part be connected with the Indo-European preverbs. Since postpositional and
preverbal syntax for the adverbs appears to develop during the history of Hittite, it has been suggested that Anatolian is particularly archaic, because it mirrors a stage of Indo-European at which there where no adpositions and no preverbs, but only independent adverbs. I argue that the particles go back to adverbial elements, and were formerly the functional equivalent of the Indo-European preverbs/adpositions. Later, these forms underwent semantic
bleaching, became clitic and were attracted toward P2. (It must be mentioned that Anatolian is particularly rich in P2 clitics, which also include sentence particles, modal particles, and various types of pronominal forms). After cliticization of the former preverbs, the class of preverbs/adposition was reconstructed through the creation of local adverbs, which, in their turn, partly derive from nominal forms, partly from Indo-European adverbs. My claim has two consequences: a) it shows that the alleged archaism of Anatolian, at least as far as the existence of preverbs/adpositions is concerned, is an illusion; this word class did exist before Anatolian split from the rest of Indo-European already, but it was lost and renewed: what we observe in Old Hittite is the beginning of this renewal; b) it provides an example of a grammaticalization chain shift: former preverbs have undergone semantic bleaching and phonological reduction, becoming particles, and new preverbs have been created out of formerad verbs and nouns to replace the olderones.