Industrial Health 2005, 43, 20–23
Review Article
Shiftwork: Safety, Sleepiness and Sleep
Simon FOLKARD1, 2*, 3, David A. LOMBARDI1 and Philip T. TUCKER3
1
Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety, 71 Frankland Road, Hopkinton, MA 01748, USA
Laboratoire d’Anthropolgie Appliquée, Université René Descartes, 45 rue des Saints Pères, 75006 Paris, France
3
Body Rhythms and Shiftwork Centre University of Wales Swansea, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK
2
Received August 25, 2004 and accepted October 12, 2004
Abstract: This brief paper reviews the available published literature on shiftwork and safety that
allows the relative risk of “accidents” or injuries associated with specific features of shift systems to
be estimated. Three main trends in risk are discussed, namely that (i) risk is higher on the night
shift, and to a lesser extent the afternoon shift, than on the morning shift, (ii) risk increases over a
span of shifts, especially so if they are night shifts, and (iii) risk increases with increasing shift length
over eight hours. We discuss that some of these trends are not entirely consistent with predictions
derived from considerations of the circadian variations in sleep propensity or rated sleepiness, and
consider factors relating to sleep that may underlie the observed trends in risk. Finally, the practical
implications of the trends in risk for the design of safer shift systems are discussed.
Key words: Sleep, Shiftwork, Work Schedules, Safety, Injuries, Accidents, Sleepiness, Fatigue
Introduction
Safety is a primary concern of both workers and their
employers in most shiftworking situations, particularly in
transport operations and the nuclear power or chemical
industries where there may be a high “public” or
“environmental” risk. A number of authors have noted that
many of the “headline hitting” disasters of the last few
decades, such as Three Mile Island, Chernobyl, Bhopal,
Exxon Valdez, and the Estonia ferry, have all occurred in
the early hours of the morning. Further, investigations of
these disasters have concluded that they were, at least
partially, attributable to fatigue and/or human error.
This brief paper reviews the available published
epidemiologic studies on shiftwork and safety that allow
the relative risk of “accidents” or injuries associated with
specific features of shift systems to be estimated. We argue
that these trends are not consistent with predictions derived
from considerations of the circadian variations in sleep
propensity or rated sleepiness and consider factors relating
to sleep that may underlie the observed trends in risk. Finally,
we consider the practical implications of the trends in risk
for the design of safer shift systems.
Trends Associated with Features of Shift
Systems
There are few published studies that allow for an unbiased
calculation of relative risk estimates of “accidents” and/or
injuries associated with specific features of shift systems
due to non-homogeneous a priori risk. In the few studies
of industrial situations where the a priori risk of incidents1
appears to be homogeneous across the 24-h day, the
probability of actually reporting an incident may still vary
by shift. For example, the number of workers or the level
of supervision may vary over the 24-h day, as may the safety
associated with the nature of the job tasks being performed.
1
*To whom correspondence should be addressed.
The term “incidents” is used from hereon to refer to injuries and/or
accidents.
21
SHIFTWORK: SAFETY, SLEEPINESS AND SLEEP
Nevertheless, there appear to be four consistent trends in
incident risk associated with features of shift systems when
confounding factors are taken into account. These trends
could reasonably be assumed to reflect on variations in the
likelihood of errors being made by the individual operators
concerned. We have detailed the studies on which these
trends are based elsewhere1–3).
The first consistent trend relates to the relative risk of
incidents on the morning, afternoon and night shifts on 8-h
shift systems. There are several studies based on relatively
large numbers of incidents that appear to have overcome
the potential confounders and in which the incident
frequencies are reported separately for the morning, afternoon
and night shifts1–3). These frequencies were pooled across
the available studies to estimate the general trend. Based
on these results, risk increased by 18% on the afternoon
shift and by 30% on the night shift, relative to the morning
shift. This finding suggests that when the a priori risk appears
to be homogeneous across the three shifts, there is a consistent
tendency for the relative risk of incidents to be higher on
the afternoon shift than on the morning shift, and to be highest
on the night shift.
There is also a consistent trend in the risk of incidents
over successive night shifts. Seven published studies were
identified that have reported incident frequencies separately
for each night over a span of at least four successive night
shifts 1–3). In order to compare across these studies, the
frequency of incidents on each night was again pooled across
the studies, and was then expressed relative to that on the
first night shift. On average, the risk of an incident was
about 6% higher on the second night, 17% higher on the
third night, and 36% higher on the fourth night.
One important question regarding this substantial increase
in risk over four successive night shifts is whether it is
attributable to the night shift, or whether this increase in
risk represents an accumulation of fatigue over successive
workdays. Of the seven studies, five reported the risk over
successive morning or day shifts1–3). Similar to the previous
analyses, in order to compare across these studies the
frequency of incidents on each shift was expressed relative
to that on the first morning/day shift. On average, the risk
was about 2% higher on the second morning/day, 7% higher
on the third morning/day, and 17% higher on the fourth
morning/day shift than on the first shift. There was evidence
that risk increased over successive morning/day shifts, but
it is important to note however that the increase was
substantially smaller than that over successive night shifts.
The fourth trend compares the impact of different lengths
of shift on incident risk. Four recent studies have reported
the trend in risk over successive hours on shift and have
corrected for exposure in some manner1–3). All four studies
report fairly similar trends to one another and by setting the
mean risk in each study for the first eight hours at one, (i.e.
setting the relative risk for an 8-h shift at one), hourly relative
risk value could be calculated for each study. The hourly
values were then averaged across the four studies to obtain
an average trend. Apart from a slightly heightened risk from
the second to fifth hour (see 4, 5) for a discussion of this),
risk increased in an approximately exponential fashion with
time on shift.
Using this trend the relative risk on shifts of different
lengths was estimated by averaging the hourly values
involved in any given length of shift. Variations in shift
length from about 4 to 9 h had little impact on these estimated
relative risk values because of (i) the exponential nature of
the time on shift trend and (ii) the increased risk from the
second to fifth hours. However, most importantly, we can
now estimate the change in risk associated with shorter or
longer shifts than 8-h. Thus, for example, we can estimate
that relative to 8-h shifts, 10-h shifts are associated with a
13% increased risk and 12-h shifts with a 27% increased
risk.
Finally, it appears that the trend for hours on duty does
not control for the influence of breaks during a duty period,
and one possible explanation for the decrease in risk after
the fifth hour may be that it reflects the influence of rest
breaks. A number of laboratory studies on the effects of
breaks have been conducted, e.g.6), but there appears to be
only a single, recent study that has examined the impact of
rest breaks on the risk of incidents7).
This study examined industrial injuries in an engineering
plant in which breaks of 15, 45 and 10 min, respectively,
were given after each period of two hours of continuous
work. The number of incidents within each of the four 30min periods between breaks was calculated, and the risk in
each 30-min period was expressed relative to that in the
first 30-min period immediately following the break. The
results indicated that risk increased substantially, and
approximately linearly, between successive breaks such that
risk had doubled by the last 30-min period before the next
break. There was no evidence that this trend differed for
the day and night shifts, or for the three successive periods
of two hours of continuous work within a shift.
Theoretical Considerations
Most authors have argued that safety may be compromised
at night since normally people are asleep at this time, and if
22
S FOLKARD et al.
Fig. 2. The trend in Relative Risk over the course of the night shift.
Error bars are 95% Confidence Intervals.
Fig. 1. The trend in sleepiness over the morning ( ), afternoon
( ) and night ( ) shifts and the mean values for each shift (large
open symbols).
awake to work a night shift, their alertness and performance
capabilities typically reach a low ebb in the early hours of
the morning. Thus variations in risk are seen as reflecting
on the circadian rhythms in alertness/sleepiness and
performance capabilities. There are, however, two major
problems with this interpretation.
First, sleepiness is usually higher on the morning shift
than on the afternoon shift, despite the fact that the relative
risk of incidents is lower on the morning shift (see above).
A typical example of the trends in sleepiness over the three
shifts is shown in Fig. 1. This figure is based on the averaged
2-hourly Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) ratings made
by some 500 shiftworkers on a range of rotating 8-h shift
systems, namely, continuous and discontinuous, and forward
and backward rotating systems (see 8) for details). Sleepiness
ratings were almost always higher on the morning shift than
on the afternoon shift, and this is reflected in the mean ratings
for the whole shifts (Fig. 1, large open symbols). Thus,
although the increased risk on the night shift might be
attributable to increased sleepiness, it is clearly not possible
to account for the increased risk on the afternoon shift in
this way.
An alternative explanation of the increased risk on the
afternoon shift relative to the morning shift might be related
to the sleep durations commonly associated with these shifts.
For the 500 shiftworkers referred to above, workers slept
an average of 5.9 (SD ± 1.1) hours between successive
morning shifts and 8.4 (SD ± 1.3) hours between successive
afternoon shifts. There is epidemiological evidence that both
short and long habitual sleep duration are associated with
an increased risk of mortality9) and morbidity conditions
such as diabetes10) and coronary heart disease11), and an
increase in depressive symptoms9). There is also some
evidence that injury risk may show a similar U-shaped
relationship. Thus, for example, a case-control study12)
reported that excess sleep (9–10 h) on the night preceding a
hand injury significantly increased risk (OR=2.7).
Differences in the normal sleep duration between shifts
cannot, however, account for the increased risk on the night
shift relative to morning shift since they are typically rather
similar (e.g. 6.2 versus 5.9 h in the sample described above).
The second problem is the interpretation of the increased
risk on the night shift in terms of the circadian rhythm in
sleepiness. This explanation would predict that risk should
increase over most of the night shift as sleepiness increases
(see Fig. 1), however, studies of “accident” and injury rates
over the course of the night shift have consistently found a
rather different pattern to this. An early study in this area
by Vernon in 192313) reported that the frequency of surgically
treated lacerations occurring in two munitions factories
decreased substantially over the course of the night shift.
Vernon also showed that this decreasing trend, unlike that
over the day shift, could not be accounted for in terms of
variations in productivity.
A number of more recent studies have shown a similar
trend1, 3) and are summarised in Fig. 2. This figure is based
on the summed frequencies across all the studies and the
risk at 22:00 has been set at 1. Risk decreased fairly
substantially after 23:00, with only a slight suggestion of
an increased risk in the early hours of the morning between
03:00 and 05:00. The general trend over the night shift is
Industrial Health 2005, 43, 20–23
23
SHIFTWORK: SAFETY, SLEEPINESS AND SLEEP
essentially the opposite from that which would be predicted
from sleepiness ratings (Fig. 1).
In summary, there are some relatively consistent trends
in risk associated with features of shift systems but these
are not always in line with predictions based on psychological
factors such as sleepiness. In some cases it may be that we
have yet to identify important psychological variables that
underlie “accident” risk, or the nature of the relationship
between sleepiness and risk1). Although the explanations
for the discrepancies between risk and sleepiness are
unknown, it would seem prudent to take the trends in risk
seriously and to design shift systems in a manner that
minimises the risk, especially in “high hazard” situations
where there may be a danger to the general public or to the
environment.
Practical Considerations
From a practical viewpoint, the trends in risk discussed
in this paper could be used to design what should prove to
be safer shift systems. Thus, for example, these trends suggest
that shift length should be restricted, as should the number
of successive shifts before a rest day. They also suggest
that the use of frequent short breaks may reduce risk
substantially. However, it is important to consider these
features in combination rather than in isolation from one
another. We have recently developed a “risk index” based
upon a predictive model of the trends described in this
paper14). The aim was to develop a tool that could be used
to assess different work schedules with respect to their relative
safety. Such a tool might also prove useful in injury and
“accident” investigations to determine whether a particular
injury or “accident” was at least in part, attributable to the
work schedule.
In conclusion, there are consistent trends in incident risk
associated with features of work schedules. In some cases,
these trends are different from what would be predicted based
on our knowledge of the circadian rhythm in sleepiness and
clearly require further research to reveal the underlying
mechanisms. Nevertheless, modelling the trends in risk may
prove useful in designing safer work schedules.
Acknowledgement
This paper was written while the first author was the 2004
Visiting Scholar at the Liberty Mutual Research Institute
for Safety.
References
1) Folkard S, Åkerstedt T (2004) Trends in the risk of
accidents and injuries and their implications for models
of fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med
75 (Suppl 1), A161–7.
2) Folkard S, Lombardi DA (2004) Designing safer shift
systems. In: Aspekte der Arbeitspsychologie aus
Wissenschaft und Praxis. eds. by P Nickel, K Hänecke,
ˇ
M Schütte & H Grzech-Sukalo,
151–66, Pabst Science
Publishers, Lengerich.
3) Folkard S, Tucker P (2003) Shiftwork, safety and
productivity. Occup Med 53, 95–101.
4) Folkard S (1997) Black times: temporal determinants
of transport safety. Accid Anal Prev 29, 417–30.
5) Tucker P, Sytnik N, Macdonald I, Folkard S (2000)
Temporal determinants of accident risk: the “2–4 hour
shift phenomenon”. In: Shiftwork in the 21st Century.
eds. by Hornberger S, Knauth P, Costa G, Folkard S,
99–105, Peter Lang, Frankfurt, Berlin, Bern, Bruxelles,
New York, Oxford and Wien.
6) Dababneh AJ, Swanson N, Shell RL (2001) Impact of
added rest breaks on the productivity and well-being
of workers. Ergonomics 44, 164–74.
7) Tucker P, Folkard S, Macdonald I (2003) Rest breaks
reduce accident risk. Lancet 361, 680.
8) Tucker P, Smith L, Macdonald I, Folkard S (1998) Shift
length as a determinant of retrospective on-shift
alertness. Scand J Work, Environ Health 24 (Suppl 3),
49–54.
9) Kripke DF (2004) Do we sleep too much? Sleep 27,
13–4.
10) Ayas NT, White DP, Al Delaimy WK, Manson JE,
Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE, Patel S, Hu FB (2003) A
prospective study of self-reported sleep duration and
incident diabetes in women. Diabetes Care 26, 380–4.
11) Ayas NT, White DP, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer
FE, Malhotra A, Hu FB (2003) A prospective study of
sleep duration and coronary heart disease in women.
Arch Intern Med 163, 205–9.
12) Hertz RP, Emmett E (1986) Risk factors for occupational
hand injury. J Occup Med 28, 36–41.
13) Vernon HM (1923) The causation of industrial accidents.
J Ind Hyg 5, 14–8.
14) Folkard S, Lombardi DA (2004) Towards a “Risk Index”
to assess work schedules. Chronobiology Int 21, 1063–
72.