In: Alison Henry, Martin Ball and Margaret MacAliskey (eds) 1996. Papers from the
International Conference on Language in Ireland. Belfast Working Papers in Language
and Linguistics, 13. Belfast: University of Ulster, pp. 173-93.
Lenition in Irish English
Raymond Hickey, Essen University
Abstract. Consonant weakening (lenition) is a feature of both Irish and Irish English
which reaches back far in the histories of both languages. Its manifestation and its system
status is quite different in both cases so that any simple transfer theory will fail to
account for lenition in Irish English. The present paper will be primarily concerned with
describing the phonological conditions for consonant weakening in Irish English and with
putting it in the broader perspective of similar phenomena in other varieties of English
and finally with addressing the question of its genesis within Irish English, above all in
the south of the country. Attention with be paid to its distribution in urban and rural
varieties of the Republic and to its extent in colloquial and less- received registers of
Irish English.
Introduction
There is general acceptance that the term ‘lenition’ refers to phonetic weakening, i.e. an
increase in segmental sonority diachronically or occasionally as a morphological device
as in Irish and other Celtic languages. Lenition is a phenomenon which is common in
many languages which usually manifests itself as a shift from stop to fricative or a shift
from voiceless to voiced with obstruents. Lenition normally consists of several steps and
historically a language may exhibit a shift from stop to zero via a number of intermediary
stages. Clean cases of lenition are represented by say the Germanic sound shift (stop to
fricative), West Romance consonantal developments such as lenition in Castilian Spanish
or more dialectal phenomena such as the gorgia toscana (Rohlfs, 1949; Ternes, 1977) or
lenition in Canary Spanish (Oftedal, 1986).
If one now looks at English in this light then one finds that lenition applies to
alveolars in various varieties. These represent a favoured phonetic site for lenition. The
shift of labial stops to velars is almost unknown and velar fricatives, if present, are
retentions of sounds which died out in the standard in the early modern period as in the
case of forms of Scottish and Ulster English.
Alveolars in English can basically undergo three types of alternation summarized
below, the general terms indicate sets of varieties within which these realizations are
frequently found.
(1)
Received Pronunciation:
American English
Cockney
Southern Irish English
Plosive
Tap
Glottal stop
Fricative
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 2 of 10
It is difficult to say whether these represent lenition in the classical sense. Glottalling
involves the removal of the oral gesture from a segment, and in the case of pre- or
post-glottalization can indeed be a case of reeinforcement inasmuch as it adds a phonetic
gesture to a segment already present. Tapping is lenition as it is a temporal reduction in
the articulation of a segment. As an uncontrolled ballistic movement it is shorter than the
alveolar stops which it replaces.
There are further differences between tapping, glottaling and frication. Tapping
can only occur with aveolars (labials and velars are excluded). Furthermore, it is only
found in word-internal position and only in immediately post-stress environments. As
tapping is phonetically an uncontrolled articulation it cannot occur word-finally (sandhi
situations excluded) and cannot initiate a stress syllable.
Glottalling can in principle apply to labials, alveolars and velars but for those
varieties of English best known for it, i.e. popular London, it is most characteristic of
alveolars as in bottle [b>?)], butter [bv?q]. For the matter at hand I will not be
concerned with tapping or pre-/post-glottalization but concentrate on the third alteration
in alveolar stop realization, frication.
1 Lenition and fortition
Now lenition often goes hand in hand with fortition. This can be due to chain effects in a
set of sound changes. Thus in the Germanic sound shift, voiceless stops are fricated but
voiced stops are fortified to voiceless ones seeing as how this slot became empty on the
shift of the original voiceless stops to fricatives.
Independent of chain effects, the weakening of segments in favoured sites for
lenition can be mirrored by fortition in corresponding strengthening environments.
Consider the following phenomenon in this connection.
A peculiarity of Irish English syllable onsets concerns the process known as yod
deletion (general: Wells, 1982:206ff.; Irish English: Hickey, 1984a). Here the yod before
long stressed /u:/ is dropped under certain conditions. Two factors are in the main
responsible for determining yod deletion: place of articulation and stress placement. Yod
is never deleted after labials or velars. When it is deleted after alveolars then only where
these immediately precede the vowel of a stressed syllable.
(2)
a
b
c
d
e
few
cue
venue
news
lute
[fju:]
[kju:]
[/venju:]
[nu:z]
[lu:8]
Assuming that the principle of maximal onset favours minimal sonority before the
stressed vowels then the deletion of the glide /j/ in the initial cluster C + /j/ represents a
decrease in sonority. Note that yod is not deleted in Irish English (in contrast to varieties
of American English) after alveolar stops as it is assibilated to /$/ or /g/ (tune [t$u:n],
duke [dgu:k]) and there is no process of sibilant deletion in Irish English.
Now if yod deletion is motivated by pre-stress fortition then it would be deleted
in unstressed syllables. But this is clearly not the case.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 3 of 10
(3)
a
b
c
d
numerous
numerical
nutrients
nutrition
[/nu:mqrqs]
[nju/merqkql]
[/nu:triqnts]
[nju/tri$qn]
These forms would appear to vindicate the view that unstressed syllables have a global
realisation rule for yod which is independent of position relative to the stressed syllable
of the word.
2 Weakening of aveolars
The fortition of stressed syllable onsets is correlated by the lenition of stressed syllable
codas. This is what I term mirror-image sonority. That this is valid in principle in Irish
English phonology can be shown by looking at a further phenomenon which is peculiar to
this variety of English. Alveolar stops are not always realised as stops. In certain
positions a fricative realisation is found. This involves apico-alveolar fricatives, viz.
segments which agree entirely with /t/ and /d/ except that they are continuant. For this I
use the symbols [8] and [9] respectively (Hickey, 1984b) as the International Phonetic
Association has no symbols for these sounds; note that they are firmly distinguished from
the lamino-alveolar fricatives /s/ and /z/.
(4)
a
b
kit
kiss
[ki8]
[kis]
Note further that fricative articulation where the structural description for its realisation
is met is obligatory, contrast this situation with that of post-stress dental fricatives where
stop or fricative realization is optional for many speakers of more standard varieties of
English in Ireland.
When one examines the distribution of [8] and [9] in Irish English closely one can
establish that it is primarily determined by stress placement.
(5)
a
b
c
d
e
f
top
pot
Italy
Italian
tight
titanic
[t>p]
[p>8]
[/i8qli]
[i/tæliqn]
[/tai8]
[tai/tænik]
While with the forms in (5a) and (5b) one could argue that the distribution of stops and
fricatives is determined by position in a word that in (5c) shows that position relative to
the vowel of the stressed syllable is the determining factor.
Note that fricative realisation of alveolar stops is not restricted to a position in
the coda of a stressed syllable as one might imagine from the forms in (5b) and (5c)
above. Any alveolar stop which is not in the onset of a stressed syllable and which
matches the weakening environment is realised as a fricative.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 4 of 10
(6)
a
b
titillate
assimilate
[/ti8ile:8]
[q/simile:8]
2.1 Conditions for weakening
Now the conditions which govern t-lenition involve two parameters which constitute a
weak environment. Weakness in Irish English is defined by position relative to stressed
nucleus as mentioned above and secondly by the sonority values of flanking segments.
What is necessary is an open environment to the left and right. ‘Open’ must be interpreted
here in a phonological sense so that stops and fricatives are regarded as non-open,
whereas sonorants may classify as open.
(7)
a
b
c
d
e
pact
past
pant
pelt
part
[pækt]
[pa:st]
[pænt]
[pelt]
[p<:rt] ~ [p<:r8]
The weakest environment in Irish English is that of vowels or zero. This accounts for put
[pu8] and putting [pu8in] and would furthermore account for the differential behaviour of
sonorants, i.e. /r/ which does not involve oral closure may have lenition but /n/ and /l/
are excluded from this on the grounds of alveolar contact during their articulation: pert
[pQ8] ~ [pQt] but pent [pent] and pelt [pelt].
Syllable location. For t to be lenited it must be located in a syllable coda. This excludes
it in obvious cases like tin. Furthermore with many speakers for t to be lenited it must be
located in a post-stress position. For there it is not sufficient to specify simply that it
should not immediately precede a stressed syllable (i.e. be part of its onset) as
pronunciations such as litigation [liti/ge:$qn] (*[li8i/ge:$qn]) attest. In this case and in
words like Italy [i8ili] the /t/ forms the coda of one syllable and the onset of the
following syllable. Given the shortness of the present paper I will not discuss the
question of ambisyllabicity in any detail here. Suffice it to say that the /t/ in Italy is
ambisyllabic (given the preceding short [i] which cannot on its own constitute a syllable
rhyme in English) and that ambisyllabicity is not enough to block t-lenition (Italy) but
pre-stress position often is (litigation).
Phonotactics. One should furthermore remark that t-lenition is governed by the
phonotactics of Irish English in general. Here as in other varieties of English then is a
prohibition on sequences of two fricatives. Instances where the restriction on two
fricatives is flouted in present-day English can be seen in the following examples.
(8)
a
b
sphere; sphinx
fifth; fifths; heaves
However in (8a) one is dealing with classical loanwords and in (8b) there is a
morpheme boundary between the fricatives in the syllable-coda of each word.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 5 of 10
The process of yod absorption discussed above leads to phonetic affricates as in
attune [q/t$u:n]. The stop onset of this complex segment does not experience lenition
even if in terms of stress it meets the structural description of t-lenition because of the
superordinate phonotactic prohibition on sequences of two fricatives as can be seen from
a word like saturation which has the realization [sæt$u/re:$qn] and not *[sæ8$u/re:$qn].
Syllable sequences. When remarking on word pairs like kit and kiss it was stated that [8]
and [s] are clearly distinct in Irish English. While this is true, the presence of [s] can
affect the realization of /t/ as [8]. The conditioning is as follows. If the syllable
immediately /t/ in a weak environment begins with /s/ (but not another fricative) then tlenition is blocked for many speakers. Consider the following instances in which there is
a seemingly inexplicable blocking of t-lenition in the first two words but not in the third
despite identical stress placement and weakening environment.
(9)
a
b
c
d
community
solicitor
obesity
pivoted
[kq/mju:ni8i]
[sq/lisitQ], *[sq/lisi8Q]
[o/bi:siti], *[o/bi:si8i]
[pivq8qd]
This is a type of allophonic dissimilation which is widely attested in other languages. In
English there are instances such as Latin purpura which is borrowed as purpel in Old
English. Other phenomena such as Grassmann’s Law in Sanskrit which prohibits two
aspirated consonants in the same lexical root can be seen as parallel cases.
2.2 Lenition as a cline
Finally one should note that lenition is in fact a cline of phonetic weakening. For the
supra- regional variety of Southern Irish English it encompasses only one sound [8].
However in more colloquial urban varieties of the east coast (including Dublin) there are
other attested points on a scale.
(10)
t
button
8
but
h
~
water
?
water
0
what
The removal of the oral gesture as seen in glottal realizations of /t/ can result in either a
fricative or glottal stop. The latter is less frequent and practically confined to
lower-class Dublin speech.
In one or two words, a lenition stage is lexicalised. Thus the colloquial
pronounciation of Saturday, even with speakers who do not lenite beyond [8], is
commonly [sæhQde] (this may well have been influenced by Irish Sahairn ‘Saturday’
which has an internal [-h-]).
The particular manifestation of lenition depends not only on register or
sociolinguistic factors as indicated above. It is also something which varies according to
style of speech. One tends to find it less in more formal situations such as in reading or
word-list styles.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 6 of 10
3 Lenition from a broader perspective
Having dealt with the manifestation of t-lenition in southern Irish English let me look at it
from a broader perspective. Consider the outset of lenition in Irish. Here it chiefly
consists of the frication of stops and is a morphologically conditioned phenomenon and
has been for well over a millenium. The grammatical character is evident in the fact that
in one and the same environment one can have a lenited or non-lenited segment, the
occurrence being determined by such categories as case, number, person, etc.
Now what has happened in Irish English? Lenition has been transferred from Irish
but, of course, the morphological conditioning is lacking. The result is that it appears
erratically. This is the case in the Forth and Bargy material (glosses and some poems
from the end of the 18th / beginning of the 19th century). There are forms to be found like
bhlock for ‘black’ and brough for ‘break’ which show sporadic lenition of labials and
velars.
The question which arise here is: what happens to a language variety which has
defunctionalized lenition as English apparently did in Ireland during the extensive period
of bilingualism before the present century? In the long term one of two situations arises:
either the lenition is dropped or it is regularized. Regularization can take one of two
forms: lenition is either spread across the board or it is fixed for certain words, i.e. it is
lexicalised.
Consider for a moment the position in a variety of English known to have been
affected by Irish English, the local dialect of Liverpool, Scouse. It is characteristic of
working-class speakers to show a degree of frication of /p, t, k/ in weakening
environments such as in word-final position (Knowles, 1978). Scholars such as Wells
(1982) generally ascribe this to an independent development in Scouse. But one could
also postulate that this is a relic of the former situation in Irish English. It is agreed that
the Scouse frication is typical of that section of the community which is directly derived
from Irish immigrants, furthermore the Irish immigration into the Merseyside area took
place chiefly in the first half of the 19th century. This was a period in which Irish in
Ireland was relatively strong (in the pre-Famine period). Furthermore the Irish who were
forced to emigrate were the economically disadvantaged which is tantamount to saying
the Irish speakers or poor bilinguals. The latter group would of course have spoken a
variety of English which was strongly affected by their native Irish and would thus have
been likely to show lenition as a transfer phenomenon.
If this is the case then why is general lenition of all stops not a characteristic of
modern Irish English? Recall that in the supraregional variety of present-day Irish
English lenition only applies to alveolars. The explanation could be as follows. As of
1850 when the population of native Irish speakers in Ireland had been decimated as a
result of the Great Famine in the late 1840s, the position of English strengthened
accordingly. With this the influence of English increased and the least resistant
idiosyncratic features of Irish English can be taken to have been replaced by more
standard pronunciations.
One can now account for t-lenition in Irish English. Recall that t- glottaling and
t-flapping would suggest that the alveolar point of articulation represents the favoured
site for weakening, at least for varieties of English.
Consider the situation with non-coronals: the lenition of /p/ to /f/ would have
lead to considerable homophony as in cup # cuff. T-lenition introduced a new sound but
it did not lead to homophony.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 7 of 10
The weakening of velar stops to fricatives would have introduced a new sound
and but this is a process which is practically unattested for English. Those varieties
which show /x/ such as Ulster Scots and forms of Scottish English have this as a relic
and not an innovation as remarked above.
3.1 Feature retention and relic areas
The mention of relics would suggest classifying the Scouse situation as feature retention
after the severing of ties with the source group of varieties, in this case Irish English.
Now I do not want to labour the Scouse case too much, as stop frication is recessive in
this variety, but it does have parallels. Here are two cases, one from English and one
from German.
The High German consonant shift. It was long thought that the High German consonant
shift started in the upper German area and spread towards the lower plains in the north.
However, recent research by Theo Vennemann has shown that in fact the consonant shifts
(notably affrication and devoicing) were features of the Franks who pressed into the
south of Germany bringing this feature of their speech to the Germans in the more
mountainous south. A later spread of standard German from the north led to the
replacement of Frankish pronunciations in the north but not entirely in the south. Hence
the present-day distribution is due to the superimposition of standard forms, more in the
north than in the south and not to a dissemination of these features from south to north.
Initial fricative voicing in the south of England. The voicing of initial fricatives in the
south of England which can be seen in a few standard pronunciations like vat and vane is
a general feature which going on some misspellings in Old English (uif for five for
instance) could reach back into the 10th century. The present-day distribution in the mid
south and south west may look like an areal feature which still shows its inherited
geographical distribution. However, the non-contiguous areas with initial voicing (some
as far east as Kent) and the known spread of varieties from the London area without
initial voicing point to a former distribution far greater than the present-day one. It is the
superimposition of non-southern varieties which has disguised the original extent of
initial voicing in the south of England.
3.2 Regularization of lenition
Regularization of lenition in present-day Irish English is mostly by phonetic
exceptionaless (/t/ is always lenited in the appropriate evironment). The supraregional
variety of the south does show a degree of lexicalization of lenition as in the
pronunciation /sæhQde/ for Saturday mentioned above or in instances of stop deletion in
immediately post-stress syllables as seen in /rekqnaiz/ for recognize. The latter type of
weakening is of the same kind as produces syncope in words like family, known from
most varieties of English.
The above interpretation of lenition has two advantages: it offers a
sociolinguistically plausible explanation of the Scouse situation and more importantly it
accounts for the situation in present-day Irish English where lenition has been
maximalized to cover all instances of alveolar stops in weakening environments
(basically intervocalically or before a pause) and lexicalized – as /h/ – in a small
number of further cases (Saturday).
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 8 of 10
Of course this situation does not apply to all varieties of Irish English. In urban
varieties of the east coast, above of Dublin, lenition has been extended to embrace both
alveolar lenition (as in the supraregional variety) and to have a manifestation as
glottaliing for alveolars in some instances. There is a cline involved here which is
determined by social affiliation in Dublin.
(11)
a
b
middle class upwards t-frication
lower classes
t-frication and glottaling
(glottaling tends to occur intervocalically
with t-frication word-finally)
There would appear to a folk perception of t-glottaling as a feature of lower-class urban
dialects of Irish English as in such caricaturing phrases as /jq nvo w<? q miqn/ for you
know what I mean where less localized varieties of Irish English the interrogative would
be realized as /w>8/.
4 Conclusion
In conclusion one can say that an acceptable interpretation of lenition involves an
understanding of both its function in Irish and the effect which the transfer to English has
had, especially as the conditioning of Irish is, or course, absent. The treatments of this so
far by scholars in the field, such as Henry and Bliss have been unsatisfactory. Henry in
his 1957 monograph, An Anglo-Irish dialect of North Roscommon is unsystematic in his
description and analysis. Bliss on the other hand is simplistic in his conception of
transfer. He repeatedly claims that transfer occurred on a one to one basis, sound A in
Irish is transferred and substitutes sound B in English. This stance makes no reference to
the status of those segments which are involved in transfer. Indeed closer inspection of
transfer phenomena in Irish English shows that the process would seem to be located on a
more general plane. Consider t-lenition again for a moment. The actual sound which
results from this process, an apico-alveolar fricative does not occur in Irish. How then
can this be interpreted as the result of transfer? The answer lies in the generality of the
phenomenon. Lenition would seem to have been carried over into English from Irish as a
general phonological directive to weaken segments in favoured phonetic sites. This
general directive has led in Irish English to a segment which is not present in Irish but
which illustrates the same principle of lenition just as well as the instances in Irish itself,
namely a weakening in the phonetic nature of segments, chiefly a shift from stop to
fricative.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 9 of 10
5
References
Bliss, Alan J. (1972) “Languages in contact. Some problems of Hiberno-English”,
Proceeding of the Royal Irish Academy, Section C 72, 63-82.
Bliss, Alan J. (1984) “English in the South of Ireland”, in Trudgill (ed.), 135-151.
Harris, John (1990) “Segmental complexity and phonological government”, Phonology
7, 255-300.
Henry, Patrick Leo (1957) An Anglo-Irish dialect of North Roscommon. Phonology,
accidence, syntax. Dublin, Department of English, University College.
Hickey, Raymond (1984a) “Coronal segments in Irish English”, Journal of Linguistics
20, 233-251.
Hickey, Raymond (1984b) “Syllable onsets in Irish English”, Word, 35, 67-74.
Hickey, Raymond (1986) “Possible phonological parallels between Irish and Irish
English”, English World Wide 7, 1-21.
Hickey, Raymond (1993) “The beginnings of Irish English”, Folia Linguistica Historica
14, 213-238.
Hogan, James Jeremiah (1927) The English language in Ireland. Dublin, Educational
Company of Ireland.
Hogan, James Jeremiah (1934) An outline of English philology, chiefly for Irish
students. Dublin, Educational Company of Ireland.
Hughes, Arthur and Peter Trudgill (1987) English accents and dialects. An introduction
to social and regional varieties of British English. London, Edward Arnold.
Knowles, Gerald O. (1978) “The nature of phonological variables in Scouse”, in
Trudgill (ed.), 80-90.
Lass, Roger (1987) The shape of English. London: Dent and Dutton.
Oftedal, Magne (1985) Lenition in Celtic and in Insular Spanish: the secondary
voicing of stops in Gran Canaria. Oslo, University Press.
Rohlfs, Gerhard (1949) Historische Grammatik der italienischen Sprache und ihrer
Mundarten. 3 vols. Bern, Francke.
Swan, Toril et al. (eds.) (1992) Older Germanic languages in a comparative
perspective. Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter.
Raymond Hickey Lenition in Irish English Page 10 of 10
Ternes, Elmar (1977) “Konsonantische Anlautveränderungen in den keltischen und
romanischen Sprachen”, Romanisistisches Jahrbuch 28, 19-53.
Trudgill, Peter (ed.) (1984) Language in the British Isles. Cambridge: University Press.
Vennemann, Theo (1984) “Hochgermanisch und Niedergermanisch: Di.e.
Verzweigungstheori.e. der germanisch-deutschen Lautverschiebungen”, Beiträge
zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 106, 1-45.
Vennemann, Th. (1992) “Dating the division between High and Low Germanic. A
summary of the arguments”, in Swan et al. (eds.).
Wells, John C. (1982) Accents of English. 3 Vols. Cambridge: University Press.