Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Eileen Hebets Publications Papers in the Biological Sciences 10-2014 Multimodal sensory reliance in the nocturnal homing of the amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus (Class Arachnida, Order Amblypygi)? Eileen A. Hebets University of Nebraska-Lincoln, ehebets2@unl.edu Alfonso Aceves-Aparicio Instituto de Ecologia, Xalapa, Veracruz Samuel Aguilar-Argüello Instituto de Ecologia, Xalapa, Veracruz Verner P. Bingman Bowling Green State University, vbingma@bgsu.edu Ignacio Escalante Universidad de Costa Rica, iescalante@berkeley.edu See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets Part of the Animal Sciences Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Biology Commons, Entomology Commons, and the Genetics and Genomics Commons Hebets, Eileen A.; Aceves-Aparicio, Alfonso; Aguilar-Argüello, Samuel; Bingman, Verner P.; Escalante, Ignacio; Gering, Eben J.; Nelson, David R.; Rivera, Jennifer; Sánchez-Ruiz, José Ángel; Segura-Hernández, Laura; Settepani, Virginia; Wiegmann, Daniel D.; and Stafstrom, Jay A., "Multimodal sensory reliance in the nocturnal homing of the amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus (Class Arachnida, Order Amblypygi)?" (2014). Eileen Hebets Publications. 72. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/72 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eileen Hebets Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Authors Eileen A. Hebets, Alfonso Aceves-Aparicio, Samuel Aguilar-Argüello, Verner P. Bingman, Ignacio Escalante, Eben J. Gering, David R. Nelson, Jennifer Rivera, José Ángel Sánchez-Ruiz, Laura Segura-Hernández, Virginia Settepani, Daniel D. Wiegmann, and Jay A. Stafstrom This article is available at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/72 Published in Behavioural Processes 108 (2014), pp 123–130. doi 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.014 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. Used by permission. Submitted April 14, 2014; revised August 30, 2014; accepted September 11, 2014; published online October 17, 2014. digitalcommons.unl.edu Multimodal sensory reliance in the nocturnal homing of the amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus (Class Arachnida, Order Amblypygi)? Eileen A. Hebets,1 Alfonso Aceves-Aparicio,2 Samuel Aguilar-Argüello,2 Verner P. Bingman,3 Ignacio Escalante,4 Eben J. Gering,1,5 David R. Nelsen,6 Jennifer Rivera,4 José Ángel Sánchez-Ruiz,7 Laura Segura-Hernández,4 Virginia Settepani,8 Daniel D. Wiegmann,9 Jay A. Stafstrom1 1 School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, 348 Manter Hall, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA 2 Instituto de Ecologia, Xalapa, Veracruz 91070, México 3 Department of Psychology and J.P. Scott Center for Neuroscience, Mind and Behavior, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403, USA 4 Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, 2060 San José, Costa Rica 5 Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, 3700 E Gull Lake Dr, Hickory Corners, MI 49060, USA 6 Department of Biology and Allied Health, Southern Adventist University, Collegedale, TN 37315, USA 7 Department of Biology, University of Puerto Rico Rio Piedras, P.O. Box 70377, San Juan, PR 00931, USA 8 Department of Bioscience, Aarhus University, Ny Munkegade 116 Building 1540, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark 9 Department of Biological Sciences, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403, USA Corresponding author — E. A. Hebets, tel 420-472-2571, email ehebets2@unl.edu Abstract Like many other nocturnal arthropods, the amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus is capable of homing. The environment through which these predators navigate is a dense and heterogeneous tropical forest understory and the mechanism(s) underlying their putatively complex navigational abilities are presently unknown. This study explores the sensory inputs that might facilitate nocturnal navigation in the amblypygid P. pseudoparvulus. Specifically, we use sensory system manipulations in conjunction with field displacements to examine the potential involvement of multimodal—olfactory and visual—stimuli in P. pseudoparvulus’ homing behavior. In a first experiment, we deprived individuals of their olfactory capacity and displaced them to the opposite side of their home trees (<5 m). We found that olfaction-intact individuals were more likely to be re-sighted in their home refuges than olfaction-deprived individuals. In a second experiment, we independently manipulated both olfactory and visual sensory capacities in conjunction with longer-distance displacements (8 m) from home trees. We found that sensory-intact individuals tended to be re-sighted on their home tree more often than sensory-deprived individuals, with a stronger effect of olfactory deprivation than visual deprivation. Comparing across sensory modality manipulations, olfaction-manipulated individuals took longer to return to their home trees than vision-manipulated individuals. Together, our results indicate that olfaction is important in the nocturnal navigation of P. pseudoparvulus and suggest that vision may also play a more minor role. Keywords: Mushroom body, Navigation, Olfaction, Spatial cognition, Spatial orientation 1. Introduction mechanism in successful arthropod homing behavior (reviewed in Wehner and Srinivasan, 2003; Cheng, 2012). Such a mechanism is likely during “learning walks” in new environments, where animals locate landmarks and produce mental snapshots of their placement (Graham et al., 2010; Muller and Wehner, 2010). Navigation by path integration has been documented in ants (Cheng et al., 2009; Wehner and Srinivasan, 1981, 2003; Wehner and Wehner, 1986, 1990), bees (Von Frisch, 1967), spiders (Moller and Arthropods are known to rely both on idiothetic (internal) and allothetic (external) cues to navigate both short and long distances. For example, path integration, or the assimilation of information that an animal derives from its own movements with its memory of a past position (Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt, 1980, 1982), often abetted by visual cues, is known to be an important internal 123 124 Gorner, 1994), roaches (Rivault and Durier, 2004), and fiddler crabs (Cheng, 2012; Zeil, 1998), and remains a primary focus of many studies of terrestrial arthropod navigation. In combination with idiothetic cues, many arthropod taxa simultaneously rely on allothetic (e.g., visual, chemical) cues. For example, despite the assumed difficulty of vision in low light environments, numerous studies confirm the role of visual input in the navigation of nocturnally active arthropods (Cheng, 2012). In fact, Warrant and Dacke (2010) suggest that visual, landmark-based homing (the ability of an individual to return to its preferred retreat) is an essential mechanism of navigation in nocturnal environments (Warrant and Dacke, 2010). Even species with presumably poor eyesight have been shown to rely on visual cues for nocturnal homing. For example, the wandering spider, Drassodes curpeus, uses polarized moonlight to find its way back to a silk nest (Dacke et al., 1999) and nocturnal Dancing White Lady spiders in the Namibian desert, Leucorchestris arenicola, require only visual cues to return to home burrows (Norgaard, 2005; Norgaard et al., 2003, 2007, 2008). Similarly, African dung beetles, Scarabaeus zambesianus, navigate using celestial polarization and other night sky features, including the Milky Way (Byrne et al., 2003; Dackeet al., 2003a, b, 2004, 2011, 2013). Ultimately, reliance on a diverse set of visual cues is widespread among arthropods, even among those that navigate in low light conditions. While visual cues play a well-characterized role in nocturnal arthropod navigation, the potential role of olfactory cues remains understudied. Past work has largely focused on the role of olfaction in tracking pheromone trails over short distances (Rosengren, 1977; Beugnon and Fourcassie, 1988), but there is evidence that non-pheromonal olfactory cues can also facilitate navigation. For example, the desert ant, Cataglyphis fortis, which was previously thought to rely solely on path integration and visual cues to navigate and find their nests, was recently found to use odor landmarks as well (Steck et al., 2009, 2011). Additionally, German cockroaches, Blattella germanica, use path integration, visual landmarks, and olfactory cues to indicate the end of their path (Rivault and Durier, 2004). Examples of navigation based upon non-visual sensory modalities, such as olfaction, are nonetheless relatively uncommon. The relative paucity of multisensory navigation studies likely relates to the tradition of navigation research focusing predominantly on a small subset of taxa using modality-specific approaches. We suggest that advancing navigation research necessitates the integrated investigation of multimodality, complex navigation, and a diversity of taxa. Such an approach can not only advance our understanding of the mechanisms underlying navigation, but can contribute to our general understanding of the sensory control of complex behavior. Navigation is a tangible manifestation of complex behavior, and its study requires the identification of model species that successfully navigate in structurally complex environments. To that end, this study explores multimodal sensory reliance, olfaction and vision, during nocturnal navigation in the amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus (previously misidentified as P. parvulus; see de Armas and Viquez, 2001). P. pseudoparvulus are capable of nocturnal homing through complex tropical understory habitats (Hebets et al., 2014). These nocturnally active predators can travel more than 36 m in linear distance over the course of several weeks (Hebets, 2002). Prior field displacement studies demonstrate that established individuals can return home without the use of path integration and that they may take indirect return routes, which incorporate stop-overs at non-home trees during homeward journeys (Hebets et al., 2014). These previous studies paint a picture of a central place forager that retains some degree of site-fidelity, but also navigates around a home territory (of unknown dimensions) that may encompass multiple trees. The mechanism(s) of Hebets et al. in Behavioural Processes 108 (2014) their putatively complex navigational capacities are currently unknown, but a prior observation of a single displaced individual whose olfactory perception was compromised and did not return home hints towards a role of olfactory reliance (Beck and Gorke, 1974). Additionally, amblypygids possess unique sensory structures that can enable multisensory (including olfactory) perception as well as enlarged brain processing centers (i.e., mushroom bodies) that may provide neural substrates for complex behavior such as navigation. Amblypygids possess extraordinary sensory appendages that earned them the common name “whip spider.” They walk on only six legs (as opposed to the typical eight), and their thin and elongate first pair of legs (frequently measuring 2.5 times the length of the walking legs or longer) are no longer used for walking (Igelmund, 1987). These “antenniform legs” are highly articulated and covered with thousands of sensory hairs that have mechanosensory and chemosensory functions (Igelmund, 1987; Beck et al., 1977; Foelix, 1975; Santer and Hebets, 2011). The multiporous sensilla, located on the distal ~1 cm of the antenniform legs (reviewed in Santer and Hebets, 2011; Weygoldt, 2000), are confirmed to have an olfactory function (Hebets and Chapman, 2000). The visual capacity of amblypygids, in contrast, is thought to be considerably less impressive, facilitated by eight relatively small, single-lens eyes (reviewed in Santer and Hebets, 2011). The sensory structures of amblypygids cast doubt on the importance of visual cues in nocturnal navigation, but raise the distinct possibility that olfaction may be crucial. Nonetheless, the predominance of vision in the navigation of other arthropods with purportedly poor sight makes this modality worthy of careful examination. Our goals here were to use the amblypygid P. pseudoparvulus to examine the roles of olfactory and visual input in nocturnal homing. We explored this in the field by displacing individuals with manipulated olfactory and visual capacities. 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Animals P. pseudoparvulus were captured between 1800 and 2100 hours from the trunks of trees upon which they were residing. Given that most individuals were collected within 3 h of sunset, it is highly likely that they were collected on the tree possessing their prior night’s refuge — likely their home refuge (for details of site fidelity see Hebets, 2002). Individuals were immediately placed in a Ziplock bag to restrain their movements, and their cephalothorax widths (mm) were measured with digital calipers. Their cephalothoraxes (or sometimes abdomens) were then marked with a unique pattern of two (in one instance, three) colored paint dots using DecoColor paint markers (Uchida of America Corporation, USA; Fig. 1). The variation in the number and placement of paint dots was due to us exhausting potential unique color patterns based upon the paint markers available to us in the field. We determined and recorded each individual’s sex and developmental stage (male, female, or juvenile) and assigned it to a sensory manipulation treatment (sensory-intact vs. sensory-deprived) detailed in specific experiments. Following manipulations, individuals were transferred to a large, opaque plastic snap-cap vial (which precluded visual feed-back) for transport to displacement sites. 2.2. Experiment 1 – Olfaction manipulation (home tree displacement) This experiment was conducted in the Arboretum at La Selva Biological Station in Heredia Province, Costa Rica on January 8–21, 2007. MultiModal sensory reliance in tHe aMblypygid Phrynus PseudoParvulus 125 2.3. Experiment 2 – Multimodal manipulations (olfaction and vision — off-tree displacements) Our second experiment explored the role of both olfaction (using a different methodological approach) and vision in homing of individuals displaced longer distances — 8 m off their home tree. This experiment was also conducted in the Arboretum at La Selva, on January 4–16, 2014. Fig. 1. Phrynus pseudoparvulus individual with markings on its cephalothorax. We conducted manipulations on a total of 28 individuals (n = 28). Half of the individuals (n = 14: 8 females and 6 males) were deprived of olfactory perception (olfaction-deprived) and half underwent a sham manipulation (olfaction-intact) (n = 14: 8 females and 6 males). To deprive individuals of olfactory perception, the distal 1 cm of the antenniform legs (distal tarsi) of olfaction-deprived individuals was clipped with scissors to remove the entirety of their olfactory sensilla. This procedure has been used in similar studies on other arthropods, such as crustaceans (Corotto et al., 1999; Kamio et al., 2005; Maruzzo et al., 2007) and insects (Roth and Barth, 2009; Vacha et al., 2008). Olfactory sensilla, and the capacity for olfaction, are restricted to this distal portion of the antenniform legs (Hebets, 2002). The distal 1 cm of the second pair of walking legs (distal tarsi) was likewise clipped as a procedural control, or sham, on individuals in the olfaction-intact treatment. When possible, we randomized the treatment for the 28 individuals making every attempt to pair sex and age groups across treatments. Individuals for this experiment were collected close to dusk, and thus, close to when they were exiting their home refuge. As such, we were able to identify and mark home refuges. To displace individuals, they were taken to the exact opposite side of the home tree and placed at the same height above ground as their home refuge. In the days following displacements, we measured the linear distance between home refuge and displacement site. We took all distance measurements during the day so as to not disturb the focal individuals. Over the following four nights all home trees were searched at dusk and shortly after to see if displaced individuals had returned to their home refuges. If focal individuals were not re-sighted, the closest trees in each cardinal direction were searched for the presence of the marked amblypygids. Since this experiment was conducted in the Arboretum, which is relatively open compared to the surrounding primary forest, there were few trees to search. 2.3.1. Olfaction manipulations A total of 18 individuals were used in the olfactory manipulation experiment — 10 olfaction-intact (2 juveniles, 4 females, 4 males) and 8 olfaction-deprived (2 juveniles, 3 females, 3 males). In contrast to the clipping procedure used for olfactory deprivation in Experiment 1, for this experiment we used nail polish to fully cover the distal 1 cm of the tarsi of the antenniform legs of olfaction-deprived individuals. Specifically, following the successful measuring and marking of an individual, the distal tips of the antenniform legs were painted with black nail polish (424 Black Crème; Wet ‘n’ Wild, USA). We chose black so that we could clearly see whether the targeted portion of the leg was indeed fully covered, thus presumably making all multiporous sensilla non-functional (Hebets, 2002), and also to minimize the conspicuousness of manipulated individuals. Following painting, individuals were gently held for ~5 min while the nail polish dried, after which they were placed into a labeled and visually isolated snap-cap plastic vial and placed on the forest floor near their home tree. Individuals were held in their transport vial for 30–90 min prior to their displacement so that they could acclimate to their sensory manipulation. The procedure was exactly the same for olfaction-intact individuals, except that the distal portion of the femora of the antenniform legs, as opposed to tarsi, was painted with black nail polish. Although this section of the leg possesses sensory hairs, there are no olfactory (multiporous) sensilla on this leg segment (Hebets and Chapman, 2000; Santer and Hebets, 2011). 2.3.2. Vision manipulations A total of 18 individuals were used in visual manipulations — 10 vision-intact (4 juveniles, 3 females, 3 males) and 8 vision-deprived (4 juveniles, 2 female, 2 males). Visual deprivation was accomplished by covering all three sets of eyes (i.e., all eight eyes; both primary and secondary) with a small amount of dental resin (Heritage 7, USA). Specifically, following the successful measuring and marking of individuals, they were restrained in the Ziplock bag and positioned such that the anterior region of their cephalothorax (the part of the prosoma where their eyes are located) was underneath an open hole in the bag. The dental resin was mixed to its quick-drying solid state and placed atop the three sets of eyes. The dental resin was white in color and thus, these individuals had an additional whitish marking on the cephalothorax. Vision-intact individuals were manipulated in exactly the same manner, except that instead of covering the eyes, a similar amount of dental resin was mixed onto the posterior portion of the cephalothorax (behind the eyes), again causing the same whitish color to be present. As with the olfactory manipulated individuals, vision-manipulated individuals were placed in their transfer vials on the forest floor near their home tree for later displacement, allowing the same acclimation period to adjust to their sensory manipulation. In contrast to Experiment 1, in which individuals were placed on the opposite side of their home tree, individuals in this experiment were displaced to the forest floor 8 m from their home tree, always at a location beyond the catchment zone of the home tree’s buttressing. On any given night, we assigned individuals a displacement direction (a cardinal direction: N, S, E, W) before release to randomize the direction towards home. The first displaced individual on any given night was assigned North, the second – South, the third – East, and so on. At their displacement 126 Hebets et al. in Behavioural Processes 108 (2014) Fig. 2. Number of individuals either re-sighted in their home refuge or never seen again from olfactory-manipulations: home tree displacements (Exp. 1). Olfaction-intact individuals (those with second pair of walking legs clipped) were more likely to be re-sighted in their home refuge than olfaction-deprived individuals (those with the distal 1 cm of their antenniform legs clipped). location, individuals were placed gently onto the forest floor and the displacement site was marked with a stick and labeled flagging. All home and surrounding trees were monitored for a minimum of 3 nights following displacements. On any given night, we collected, marked and displaced anywhere from zero to 15 individuals. Following our initial night (n = 15), we were not only looking for new individuals to mark and displace, but we were also monitoring home trees for the presence/absence of previously marked individuals. At each home tree, we extensively searched the tree as well as the closest trees in every direction. We continued this for the duration of the experiment, with the minimum number of nights that individuals could have been re-sighted being 3 nights total and the maximum number being 12 nights (see Supplementary Table 1; following the References). 3. Results 3.1. Experiment 1 — Olfaction manipulation (home tree displacement) Eleven of the olfaction-intact individuals (four males and seven females, 79%) returned to the refuge from which they were collected, while only three of the olfaction-deprived individuals (two males and one female, 21%) returned (Fig. 2). These two return rates differ significantly (Chi Square Test, X2= 5.40, p = 0.02; effect size: phi (φ) = 0.439). No individuals were re-sighted anywhere other than at their home refuge. The cephalothorax widths of olfaction-intact individuals (mean ± SD = 8.14 ± 1.66 mm) and olfaction-deprived individuals (mean ± SD = 8.31 ± 2.36 mm) were similar (Two-sample t Test, t = −0.21, p = 0.83). Likewise, the displacement distances of olfaction-intact individuals (mean ± SD = 202 ± 70 cm) and olfaction-deprived individuals (mean ± SD = 250 ± 184 cm) did not differ (Two-sample t Test on ln-transformed distances with unequal variances, t = −0.24, p = 0.81). Thus, neither body size nor displacement distance can explain the differences we observed in the return rates of olfaction-intact vs. olfactiondeprived treatment groups. 3.2. Experiment 2: Multimodal manipulations (olfaction and vision — off-tree displacements) 3.2.1. Olfaction manipulation Exactly one-half of the 10 olfaction-intact (n = 5) and one-half of the 8 olfaction-deprived (n = 4) individuals were re-sighted, Fig. 3. Number of re-sighted individuals seen on their home tree vs. a non-home tree across olfactory and visual sensory manipulations following an 8 m displacement. (A) Olfaction-manipulated individuals (Exp. 2: total of 10 olfaction-intact and 8 olfaction-deprived individuals were marked). Although there is no statistical difference between the proportion that made it home between the two groups (p = 0.09), the effect size is large (φ = 0.56). (B) Vision-manipulated (Exp. 2: total of 10 vision-intact and 8 vision-deprived individuals were marked). There is no statistical difference between the proportion that made it home between the two groups (p = 0.46) and the effect size is moderate (φ = 0.26). with no difference in the likelihood of re-sighting individuals from either group (X2= 0.00, p = 1.0; effect size: phi (φ) = 0). Of those that were re-sighted, 2 of the olfaction-intact individuals (both females) were re-sighted on their home tree while zero of the olfaction-deprived individuals accomplished this feat (X2= 2.80, p = 0.09; effect size: φ = 0.56; Fig. 3). There was no difference in the proportion of nights that individuals in the olfactionintact vs. olfaction-deprived treatment were re-sighted (# nights re-sighted/total # of nights they could have been re-sighted; Wilcoxon Test: X2= 0.13, p = 0.72).There was also no difference in the number of nights it took to first re-sight individuals from either treatment group (olfaction-intact ranged from 1 to 6 nights; olfaction-deprived ranged from 2 to 7 nights; Wilcoxon Test: X2= 0.14, p = 0.71). We were notable to statistically compare the minimum movement distances of olfaction-deprived and olfactionintact individuals because logistical complications prevented us from measuring distances for several re-sighted individuals (see Supplemental Table 1). However, the distances appeared similar based on a qualitative comparison only (olfaction-intact: mean ± SD = 831.6 ± 1058.1 cm; olfaction-deprived: mean ± SD = 430.5 ± 1.4 cm). As in Experiment 1, cephalothorax widths were similar between olfaction-intact individuals (mean ± SD = 7.61 ± 2.54 mm) and olfaction-deprived individuals (mean ± SD = 8.64 ± 2.002 mm; two-sample t Test, t = 0.97, p = 0.35). 3.2.2. Vision manipulation One-half of the 10 vision-intact (n = 5) and three of the 10 vision-deprived individuals were re-sighted, with no difference in the likelihood to re-sight individuals from either group (X2= 0.28, p = 0.60; effect size: φ = 0.12). Of the individuals that were resighted, three of the vision-intact individuals (1 juvenile, 2 females)and one of the vision-deprived (juvenile) were re-sighted MultiModal sensory reliance in tHe aMblypygid Phrynus at their home tree (X2= 0.54, p = 0.46; effect size: φ = 0.26; Fig. 3). There was no difference in the proportion of nights that individuals in the vision-intact vs. vision-deprived treatment were resighted (Wilcoxon Test: X2 = 0.92, p = 0.34). There was also no difference in the number of nights it took to first re-sight individuals from either treatment group (vision-intact ranged from 1 to 4 nights; vision-deprived ranged from 1 to 3 nights; Wilcoxon Test: X2 = 0.41, p = 0.52). The minimum distance traveled of vision-intact (mean ± SD = 684.5 ± 231.0 cm) and vision-deprived individuals (mean ± SD = 419 ± 340 cm) did not differ (Mann–Whitney U = 2.50, z = −1.23, p = 0.22), and cephalothorax widths were similar for both vision-intact (mean ± SD = 8.13 ± 1.99 mm) and vision-deprived individuals (mean ± SD = 8.44 ± 1.52 mm; Wilcoxon Test, W = 50, p = 0.65). 3.2.3. Olfaction and vision combined We ran a combined analysis comparing time to home between olfaction and vision manipulated groups in an effort to gain insight into the relative importance of each sensory system. When we combine all olfaction treatments (olfaction-intact plus olfaction-deprived) and all vision treatments (vision-intact plus vision-deprived), we find that displaced olfaction-manipulated individuals took longer to return home than vision-manipulated individuals (X2 = 4.47, p = 0.04). We saw no differences between these two groups in terms of their likelihood to be re-sighted (X2= 0.11, p = 0.74) or in their likelihood of being re-sighted at their home tree, independent of their re-sighting rate (olfaction: 22%, vision: 44%; X2= 1.0, p = 0.32; φ = 0.24). Additionally, the minimum distance traveled did not differ between olfactory manipulated (mean ± SD = 717 ± 887.7 cm) and vision manipulated individuals (mean ± SD = 570.7 ± 292.3 cm; Mann–Whitney U = 20.0, z = −0.57, p = 0.57). These results could indicate a greater reliance on olfactory perception for amblypygid navigation and/or that our olfactory manipulations compromised additional perceptual abilities (see Section 4). 3.2.4. Additional observations Prior to Experiment 2 (in 2012), preliminary displacement studies on sensory-intact individuals were conducted along the CES trail at La Selva Biological Station. Twenty-seven individuals were marked with either DecoColor paint pens (Uchida of America Corporation, USA) or nail polish and were displaced 10 m from their home tree. A total of 8 individuals were re-sighted (30%), 5 of which (63%) were re-sighted on their home tree. We note that this preliminary experiment was conducted in an area of primary forest, as opposed to the more open area of the Arboretum, which was the location of the current experiments. The difference in habitat complexity, along with an increase in number of available trees in the primary forest, is likely responsible for the relatively low re-sighting rates in this preliminary experiment. These additional observations, however, are important for demonstrating the capacity of amblypygids to home even through the complexity of primary forest. Nine days prior to performing Experiment 2, three sensorydeprived individuals (two olfaction-deprived and one visiondeprived) were manipulated and returned to their home refuge. This preliminary assay acted to insure that sensory manipulated individuals did not simply disappear due to an enhanced susceptibility to predation or increased wandering behavior. All three individuals were re-sighted 9 days following their marking. The two olfaction-deprived individuals were re-sighted at their home refuge while the vision-deprived individual was resighted on a nearby tree 7 m linear distance from its home tree. Two of the individuals that were marked on the abdomen in Experiment 2 were re-sighted in the field, deceased at the base of their tree. These individuals were re-sighted no sooner than 4 days PseudoParvulus 127 following painting and we believe that the paint markers (which contain xylene) that we used were able to penetrate the softer cuticle of the abdomen (opisthosoma) and were toxic to these individuals. None of our animals marked on the cephalothorax were found dead and multiple individuals (~25) marked on both their cephalothorax and/or abdomen have remained alive for more than two and half months with no obvious aberrant affects (and are currently still alive). A total of nine additional manipulated individuals were similarly marked on their abdomens during this displacement experiment — none of which were re-sighted. We have included these individuals in the above analyses as, even if they had been intoxicated, they would have had at least 4 nights to successfully return to their home tree, or to be re-sighted on an alternate tree (of the two re-sighted individuals with abdominal markings, one made it home, one was re-sighted on a different tree). We note this observation as a warning to other researchers attempting similar techniques. 4. Discussion Results of two independent experiments, using different experimental methods and different displacement distances, suggest that olfactory input facilitates successful nocturnal homing in the amblypygid P. pseudoparvulus. Our results hint to a more minor role of vision as well. In a first experiment, which employed the removal of olfactory sensilla, olfaction-intact individuals were more likely to be re-sighted at their home refuge as compared to olfaction-deprived individuals. In a second experiment, which involved covering the olfactory sensilla with nail polish to ablate olfaction in conjunction with displacements off of the home tree, results also suggest that olfaction-intact individuals are more likely to successfully home as compared to olfaction-deprived individuals. This second experiment failed to find statistical significance (p = 0.09), but this is likely due to the small sample sizes of resighted individuals in each of our treatments during our available observation period (5 olfaction-intact re-sighted; 4 olfactiondeprived re-sighted; 50% of total animals in each treatment). Our effect test reveals a large φ (0.56), supporting the biological relevance of our findings. In addition to a role of olfactory input in nocturnal homing, we also found preliminary evidence for the importance of visual input, as 60% of our vision-intact individuals that were re-sighted successfully homed compared to only 33% (1 individual) of the vision-deprived individuals. Again, this finding was not significant, but our effect size estimate for the role of vision was moderate (phi of 0.26). Our power to infer treatment effects was limited by small sample sizes for re-sighted individuals (5 vision-intact re-sighted, 50%; 3 vision-deprived re-sighted, 38%). Interestingly, the one vision-deprived individual that successfully homed did so from a distance of 8 m, despite being displaced 2.6 m from another tree. The successful homing of this individual indicates that while navigation may be supported by vision, it is not contingent upon it since a vision-deprived animal was able to successfully home. Together, our experiments provide fairly strong evidence that olfaction plays a role in amblypygid nocturnal navigation and hint that vision might as well, implying multimodal sensory reliance. In addition to ablating olfactory perception, both of our olfactory manipulations (Experiments 1 and 2) likely impacted additional sensory systems. The tips of the antenniform legs possess rod sensilla and bristles (mechanosensory and contact chemosensory capabilities) as well as olfactory club sensilla (reviewed in Santer and Hebets, 2011). Thus, clipping this section of the antenniform legs (Experiment 1) presumably compromised perception through other sensory modalities. These same sensory systems are likely also compromised in our manipulation involving the application of nail polish (Experiment 2). However, although 128 other sensory systems may have been affected during our antenniform leg manipulations, olfaction was the sole sense completely ablated, making it the most likely driver of behavioral differences between amblypygids differing in olfaction treatment. Furthermore, the distal portion of the femur, our control treatment in Experiment 2, possesses tactile sensory structures. Thus, even our olfaction-intact treatment likely influenced sensory perception in other modalities. This sensory compromise may be at least partly responsible for the slower homing rates of olfactorymanipulated vs. vision-manipulated treatments. Regardless, our results are highly suggestive of olfactory and visual-based homing in amblypygids and we suspect that future tracking studies using telemetry on sensory-deprived individuals (olfaction and vision) in combination with tightly controlled laboratory studies focusing on modality-specific cues will confirm the validity of our findings. Despite their ubiquitous presence in other arthropod groups such as insects, olfactory (multiporous) sensilla are not common within arachnids. Amblypygids, however, possess a number of such multiporous sensilla that serve an olfactory function (Santer and Hebets, 2011; Hebets and Chapman, 2000), indicating the capacity for utilizing olfactory stimuli. In addition to these external morphologies that implicate the importance of olfaction (i.e., the presence of multiporous sensilla), amblypygids possess the largest mushroom bodies (relative to their body size) documented in arthropods (Strausfeld et al., 1998). These higher-order processing centers have traditionally been ascribed roles in olfactory discrimination, as well as olfactory learning and memory, but their roles may not be limited to olfaction (Akalal et al., 2006; Heisenberg, 1998, 2003; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Strausfeld et al., 2009). In insects, mushroom bodies are known to be involved in contextual information processing, learning, and memory–including spatial memory (de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994; Heisenberg, 2003; Mizunami et al., 1998; Pascual and Preat, 2001; Zars et al., 2000). In fact, the size of arthropod mushroom bodies has recently been proposed to reflect the complexity of spatial navigation strategies (Jacobs, 2012). This “spatial orientation” hypothesis is consistent with our current knowledge of amblypygid neuroanatomy (i.e., large mushroom bodies) and their seemingly complex navigational capacities (present study). This hypothesis warrants future research, with subsequent studies explicitly exploring the role of multisensory inputs in navigation. Our data are admittedly limited in terms of sample size, a lack of knowledge regarding individual’s past experiences (e.g., navigation patterns and potential knowledge of surroundings), and a lack of knowledge of individual movement patterns. For example, we cannot know for sure if sensory-deprived individuals were (a) less likely to find their way back home due to their sensory deprivation (our presumption here), (b) less motivated to move, (c) displaced to an area outside their previously experienced range, or (d) more likely to succumb to predation or some other source of death. Nonetheless, we find the first explanation most likely. Increased likelihood to succumb to predation seems unlikely since we found no difference in the likelihood to re-sight sensory-intact vs. sensory-deprived individuals and our re-sighting rates are similar to those reported from a larger mark-recapture study of this same species (males: 40% re-sighted; females: 75% re-sighted) where individuals were not displaced, but simply monitored over time (Hebets, 2002). Furthermore, all individuals were randomly assigned a displacement direction, making it highly unlikely, though not impossible, that a larger subset of the sensory-intact individuals happed to be released in areas where they had previously traveled, facilitating their homeward travel. Finally, we believe that our consistent re-sighting rate (olfactory treatment 5:4 and vision 5:3, intact:deprived respectively) combined with the similar minimum distances traveled (olfactory 831.6 ± 1058.1 cm:430.5 ± 1.4 cm and vision 684.5 ± 231.0 cm:419 Hebets et al. in Behavioural Processes 108 (2014) ± 340 cm, intact:deprived respectively) suggests that sensory deprived individuals were not less likely to move compared to sensory intact individuals. Our experimental design was predicated on the assumption that the trees on which the amblypygids were initially located were their home trees. This assumption follows from our protocol in which amblypygids were initially sighted close to dusk, and thus were likely to be emerging from their over-day refuge. Previous work has shown relatively high sight fidelity in this species (Hebets, 2002). However, it is possible that amblypygids seek refuge on alternate trees throughout their home range while still maintaining a dominant home tree. Any comparison of a fieldcollected amblypygid’s ability to navigate back to a home tree after displacement must consider this possibility. In fact, we consistently relocated individuals on a tree not initially deemed to be their “home” tree, and the possibility exists that we collected them on an alternate tree and they did indeed succeed in traveling “home.” Although this scenario is certainly plausible, it is likely pertinent to only a small subset of individuals, yet still may have contributed to our weak statistical values, exacerbated by the small sample size. Though our results provide preliminary evidence of multimodal sensory reliance during amblypygid navigation, we cannot yet say anything about how or what cues are being used. Amblypygids do not follow the same outbound and inbound path (unpublished data, EAH), yet there remains the possibility of chemical marking. Their reliance on olfactory stimuli suggests a capacity for olfactory-learning and prior studies have already established their capacity for tactile-learning (Santer and Hebets, 2009). Our vision-deprived results also indicate a more substantial role of visual input than previously anticipated, making future work exploring their visual capabilities now essential. Finally, our displacements involved already established individuals, many of whom were adults. While these displacement experiments clearly demonstrate that path integration is non-essential for successful homing in established individuals, it may indeed be essential in the initial stages of learning environmental cues. For example, many arthropods, including ants (Nicholson et al., 1999; Wehner et al., 2004) and spiders (Norgaard et al., 2012), engage in “learning walks” to facilitate the learning of visual landmark information and these walks are likely controlled by path integration (Graham et al., 2010; Muller and Wehner, 2010). Learning walks facilitated by path integration and associated with olfactory and visual snapshots remain a possibility in amblypygids. Ultimately, we are at the inaugural stages of navigation research in this unique system and the number of future research directions is plentiful. While field studies of sensory reliance during navigation are a crucial piece of the puzzle that ultimately facilitates our understanding of the mechanisms underlying success navigation through complex environments, they are insufficient on their own and pose unique challenges. The unavoidable challenges of this study, for example, are (1) the necessary use of already established individuals and (2) our low re-sighting rates which unavoidably result in small sample sizes. Given our early stage of understanding of amblypygid navigation, initial studies require us to first establish their capacity for nocturnal homing and second to provide hints towards their sensory reliance. These goals were both achieved in this study, paving the way for more controlled and elaborate field and laboratory studies which hone in on specific sensory system reliance and their potential integration. With respect to our low re-sighting rates, preliminary studies of non-manipulated individuals displaced at 10 m from the home tree revealed even lower re-sighting rates than in our sensory manipulation studies. We suggest that our low re-sighting rates reflect the natural history of these long-lived arthropods (suggested MultiModal sensory reliance in tHe aMblypygid Phrynus lifespans of more than 5 years in the field) that appear to regularly travel across large areas of tropical forest understory, utilizing multiple trees and logs along the way as diurnal refuges. Given their putatively comprehensive knowledge of their local environment, their motivation to return to a specific refuge may not be strong, making field studies such as ours challenging, yet nonetheless essential. In summary, the amblypygid, P. pseudoparvulus, appears to rely on olfactory inputs for successful navigation and likely, though to a lesser degree, visual inputs as well. Currently, the nature of the olfactory or visual stimuli remains unknown and the degree to which successful homing relies on one or both sensory stimuli is inconclusive. Interestingly, work by Steck et al. (2011) have demonstrated that desert ants (C. fortis) also use both olfactory and visual cues to guide their return routes to the nest. Following training with independent or combined cues, they found that ants learned the location of their nest more quickly with multimodal cues (Stecket al., 2011). Similar cross-modal influences on learning have been discussed in non-navigation contexts (e.g., signaling and communication; mate choice; warning coloration) and we suggest that navigation research may benefit from incorporating existing theoretical frameworks for studying multimodality (Candolin, 2003; Hebets and Papaj, 2005; Partan and Marler, 1999, 2005; Rowe, 1999; Rowe and Halpin, 2013). Acknowledgments — We thank La Selva Biological Station and the Organization for Tropical Studies for help with research permits and logistics. Roger Santer was instrumental in early discussions of experimental design and field protocol for Experiment 1. Experiment 2 was conducted as part of an OTS Specialty Course — Ecology and Evolution of Arachnids (2014). A huge thank you to our non-author group members Nicole Garrison, Rebecca Duncan, and José Antonio Cambronero; as well as to Emma Dietrich, Greta Binford, Carlos Vasquez, Gilbert Barrantes, Piper and Jasper Garrett, and Cody and Jessie Storz for help with data collection and preliminary experiments in the field. We would like to additionally thank the remaining participants in the 2014 course (students and faculty alike) for useful feedback throughout the experiment, as well as Andres Santana for helping make this course a reality. Finally, we thank OTS for providing preliminary funding to EAH during her dissertation. V.S. was supported by the European Research Council (ERC StG-2011 282163 to Trine Bilde) and the Augustinus Fonden. References Akalal, D.B.G., Wilson, C.F., Zong, L., Tanaka, N.K., Ito, K., Davis, R.L., 2006. Roles for Drosophila mushroom body neurons in olfactory learning and memory. Learn. Mem. 13, 659–668. Beck, L., Gorke, K., 1974. Tagesperiodik, revierverhalten und beutefang der Gei Belspinne Admetus pumilio C. L. Koch im Freiland. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie 35, 173–186. Beck, L., Foelix, R., Godeke, E., Kaiser, R., 1977. Morphology, larval development, and hair sensilla of antenniform legs of whip spider Heterophrynus longicornis Butler (Arachnida Amblypygi). Zoomorphologie 88, 259–276. Beugnon, G., Fourcassie, V., 1988. How do red wood ants orient during nocturnal foraging in a three dimensional system? II. Field experiments. Insect. Soc. 35, 106–124. Byrne, M., Dacke, M., Nordstrom, P., Scholtz, C.H., Warrant, E., 2003. Visual cues used by ball-rolling dung beetles for orientation. J. Comp. Physiol. A 189, 411–418. Candolin, U., 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol. Rev. 78, 575–595. PseudoParvulus 129 Cheng, K., 2012. Arthropod navigation: Ants, bees, crabs, spiders finding their way. In: Zentall, T.R., Wasserman, E.A. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 347–365. Cheng, K., Narendra, A., Sommera, S., Wehner, R., 2009. Traveling in clutter: Navigation in the Central Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. Behav. Process. 80, 261–268. Corotto, F., Bonenberger, D., Founkeo, J., Dukas, C., 1999. Antennule ablation, sex discrimination and mating behavior in crayfish Procambarus clarkii. J. Crustacean Biol. 19, 298–300. Dacke, M., Nilsson, D.E., Warrant, E., Blest, A.D., Land, M.F., O’Carroll, D.C., 1999. Built-in polarizers form part of a compass organ in spiders. Nature 401, 470–473. Dacke, M., Nilsson, D.E., Scholtz, C.H., Byrne, M., Warrant, E.J., 2003a. Animal behaviour: Insect orientation to polarized moonlight. Nature 424, 33. Dacke, M., Nordstrom, P., Scholtz, C.H., 2003b. Twilight orientation to polarized light in the crepuscular dung beetle Scarabaeus zambesianus. J. Exp. Biol. 206, 1535–1543. Dacke, M., Byrne, M., Scholtz, C.H., Warrant, E.J., 2004. Lunar orientation in a beetle. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, 361–365. Dacke, M., Byrne, M., Baird, E., Scholtz, C.H., Warrant, E.J., 2011. How dim is dim? Precision of the celestial compass in moonlight and sunlight. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 366, 697– 702. Dacke, M., Baird, E., Byrne, M., Scholtz, C.H., Warrant, E.J., 2013. Dung beetles use the Milky Way for orientation. Curr. Biol. 23, 298–300. de Armas, L.F., Viquez, C., 2001. Nueva especie de Phrynus (Amblypygi: Phrynidae) de Costa Rica. Rev. Ibérica Aracnol. 4, 11–15. de Belle, J.S., Heisenberg, M., 1994. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science 263, 692–695. Foelix, R.F., 1975. Occurrence of synapses in peripheral sensory nerves of arachnids. Nature 254, 146–148. Graham, P., Philippides, A., Baddeley, B., 2010. Animal cognition: Multi-modal interactions in ant learning. Curr. Biol. 20, R639–R640. Hebets, E.A., 2002. Relating the unique sensory system of amblypygids to the ecology and behavior of Phrynus parvulus from Costa Rica (Arachnida, Amblypygi). Can. J. Zool. 80, 286–295. Hebets, E.A., Chapman, R.F., 2000. Electrophysiological studies of olfaction in the whip spider Phrynus parvulus (Arachnida, Amblypygi). J. Insect Physiol. 46, 1441–1448. Hebets, E.A., Papaj, D.R., 2005. Complex signal function: Developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57, 197–214. Hebets, E.A., Gering, E.J., Bingman, V.P., Wiegmann, D.D., 2014. Nocturnal homing in the tropical amblypygid Phrynus pseudoparvulus (Class Arachnida, Order Amblypygi). Anim. Cogn. 17 (4), 1013–1018. Heisenberg, M., 1998. What do the mushroom bodies do for the insect brain? An introduction. Learn. Mem. 5, 1–10. Heisenberg, M., 2003. Mushroom body memoir: From maps to models. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 266–275. Igelmund, P., 1987. Morphology, sense-organs, and regeneration of the forelegs (whips) of the whip spider Heterophrynus elaphus (Arachnida, Amblypygi). J. Morphol. 193, 75–89. Jacobs, L.F., 2012. From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 10693– 10700. 130 Kamio, M., Araki, M., Nagayama, T., Matsunaga, S., Fusetani, N., 2005. Behavioral and electrophysiological experiments suggest that the antennular outer flagellum is the site of pheromone reception in the male helmet crab Telmessus cheiragonus. Biol. Bull. 208, 12–19. Maruzzo, D., Minelli, A., Ronco, M., Fusco, G., 2007. Growth and regeneration of the second antennae of Asellus aquaticus (Isopoda) in the context of arthropod antennal segmentation. J. Crustacean Biol. 27, 184–196. Mittelstaedt, M.L., Mittelstaedt, H., 1980. Homing by path integration in a mammal. Naturwissenschaften 67, 566–567. Mittelstaedt, H., Mittelstaedt, M.L., 1982. Homing by path integration. In: Papi, F., Wallraff, H.G. (Eds.), Avian Navigation. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 290–297. Mizunami, M., Weibrecht, J.M., Strausfeld, N.J., 1998. Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: Their participation in place memory. J. Comp. Neurol. 402, 520–537. Moller, P., Gorner, P., 1994. Homing by path integration in the spider Agelenala bryinthica Clerck. J. Comp. Physiol. A 174, 221–229. Muller, M., Wehner, R., 2010. Path integration provides a scaffold for landmark learning in desert ants. Curr. Biol. 20, 1368–1371. Nicholson, D.J., Judd, S.P.D., Cartwright, B.A., Collett, T.S., 1999. Learning walks and landmark guidance in wood ants (Formica rufa). J. Exp. Biol. 202, 1831–1838. Norgaard, T., 2005. Nocturnal navigation in Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae, Sparassidae). J. Arachnol. 33, 533–540. Norgaard, T., Henschel, J.R., Wehner, R., 2003. Long-distance navigation in the wandering desert spider Leucorchestris arenicola: Can the slope of the dune surface provide a compass cue? J. Comp. Physiol. A 189, 801–809. Norgaard, T., Henschel, J.R., Wehner, R., 2007. Use of local cues in the night-time navigation of the wandering desert spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae, Sparassidae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 193, 217–222. Norgaard, T., Nilsson, D.E., Henschel, J.R., Garm, A., Wehner, R., 2008. Vision in the nocturnal wandering spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae: Sparassidae). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 816–823. Norgaard, T., Gagnon, Y.L., Warrant, E.J., 2012. Nocturnal homing: Learning walks in a wandering spider? PLoS One 7, e49263. Partan, S.R., Marler, P., 1999. Behavior — Communication goes multimodal. Science 283, 1272–1273. Partan, S.R., Marler, P., 2005. Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am. Nat. 166, 231–245. Pascual, A., Preat, T., 2001. Localization of long-term memory within the Drosophila mushroom body. Science 294, 1115–1117. Perez-Orive, J., Mazor, O., Turner, G.C., Cassenaer, S., Wilson, R.I., Laurent, G., 2002. Oscillations and sparsening of odor representations in the mushroom body. Science 297, 359–365. Rivault, C., Durier, V., 2004. Homing in German cockroaches, Blattella germanica (l.) (Insecta: Dictypotera): Multi-channelled orientation cues. Ethology 110, 761–777. Rosengren, R., 1977. Foraging strategy of wood ants (Formica rufa group). II: Nocturnal orientation and diel periodicity. Acta Zool. Fenn. 150, 1–30. Hebets et al. in Behavioural Processes 108 (2014) Roth, L., Barth, R., 2009. Dulling the senses: The role of the antennae in mate recognition, copulation and mate guarding in decorated crickets. Anim. Behav. 28, 58–94. Rowe, C., Halpin, C., 2013. Why are warning displays multimodal. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1425–1440. Rowe, C., 1999. Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals. Anim. Behav. 58, 921–931. Santer, R.D., Hebets, E.A., 2009. Tactile learning by a whip spider, Phrynus marginemaculatus CL Koch (Arachnida, Amblypygi). J. Comp. Physiol. A 195, 393–399. Santer, R.D., Hebets, E.A., 2011. The sensory and behavioural biology of whip spiders (Arachnida, Amblypygi). Adv. Insect Physiol. 41, 1–64. Steck, K., Hansson, B.S., Knaden, M., 2009. Smells like home: Desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, use olfactory landmarks to pinpoint the nest. Front. Zool. 6, 108. Steck, K., Hansson, B.S., Knaden, M., 2011. Desert ants benefit from combining visual and olfactory landmarks. J. Exp. Biol. 314, 1307–1312. Strausfeld, N.J., Hansen, L., Li, Y.S., Gómez, R.S., Ito, K., 1998. Evolution, discovery, and interpretations of arthropod mushroom bodies. Learn. Mem. 5, 11–37. Strausfeld, N.J., Sinakevitch, I., Brown, S.M., Farris, S.M., 2009. Ground plan of the insect mushroom body: Functional and evolutionary implications. J. Comp. Neurol. 513, 265–291. Vacha, M., Puzova, T., Drstukova, D., 2008. Ablation of antennae does not disrupt magnetoreceptive behavioral reaction of the American cockroach to periodically rotated geomagnetic field. Neurosci. Lett. 435, 103–107. Von Frisch, K., 1967. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Warrant, E., Dacke, M., 2010. Visual orientation and navigation in nocturnal arthropods. Brain Behav. Evol. 75, 156–173. Wehner, R., Srinivasan, M.V., 1981. Searching behaviour of desert ants, genus Cataglyphis (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). J. Comp. Physiol. A 142, 315–338. Wehner, R., Srinivasan, M.V., 2003. Path integration in insects. In: Jeffrey, K.J. (Ed.), The Neurobiology of Spatial Behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp.9–30. Wehner, R., Wehner, S., 1986. Path integration in desert ants. Approaching a long-standing puzzle in insect navigation. Monitore Zoologico Italiano 20, 309–331. Wehner, R., Wehner, S., 1990. Insect navigation: Use of maps or Ariadnes thread. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2, 27–48. Wehner, T., Meier, C., Zollikofer, C., 2004. The ontogeny of foraging behaviour in desert ants, Cataglyphis bicolor. Ecol. Entomol. 29, 1831–1838. Weygoldt, P., 2000. Whip Spiders (Chelicerata: Amblypygi): Their Biology, Morphology and Systematics. Apollo Books, Stenstrup, Denmark. Zars, T., Fischer, M., Schulz, R., Heisenberg, M., 2000. Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science 288, 672–675. Zeil, J., 1998. Homing in fiddler crabs (Ucalactea annulipes and Uca vomeris: Ocypodidae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 183, 367–377. Supplementary Table 1. Table summarizing all raw data for Experiment 2: Multimodal Manipulations (Olfaction and Vision – OffTree Displacements). Individuals ID: * symbolizes individuals marked on the abdomen, while all other individuals were marked on the cephalothorax; all the dates belong to 2014; Treatments: O+ olfactory-intact individuals, O- olfactory-deprived individuals, V+ vision-intact individuals, V- vision-deprived individuals. CW: cephalothorax width. Individual ID Date Marked Treatment Tree # Sex/Age CW (mm) Displacement Direction Resighted Green, Pink Blue, Green1 Pink, Pink Pink, Green * 04-Jan 04-Jan 04-Jan 07-Jan O+ O+ O+ O+ 2 7 5 15 6.17 5.97 10.02 9.68 East East North South X Green, Pink * Pink, Red * Green, Green * Blue, Blue Blue, Purple2 Pink, Purple * Pink, Green Blue, Red Green, Blue Pink, Pink * Green, Red * 09-Jan 09-Jan 09-Jan 04-Jan 04-Jan 09-Jan 04-Jan 04-Jan 04-Jan 07-Jan 13-Jan O+ O+ O+ O+ O+ O+ OOOOO- 17 10 11 7 8 2 2 7 1 5 13 7.84 7.03 11.12 10.93 7.16 7.95 8.58 5.12 10.79 9.05 9.17 East South West North South West South West East West North X 1.3 X 1.38 X 3.31 Blue, Silver Silver, Red Silver, Purple Green, Red Purple, Green Purple, Purple Green * Blue, Green2 Pink, Pink, Pink Green, Purple 04-Jan 05-Jan 05-Jan 04-Jan 05-Jan 05-Jan 11-Jan 04-Jan 06-Jan OOOV+ V+ V+ V+ V+ V+ 6 12 14 1 9 10 10 8 9 juvenile juvenile female female w/eggs female female male male male male juvenile juvenile female female female w/eggs male male male juvenile juvenile juvenile juvenile female female 11.5 8.44 9.38 6.26 4.32 6.91 4.69 9.63 9.6 North East West West East West South East North X X X X 5.3 NA NA 8 13-Jan V+ 3 female 8.87 South X X X X Homed Minimum distance moved (m) 26.6 X X 4.3 8 X X 8 NA * Green, Purple Pink, Blue * Green, Blue * Green, Green Purple, Pink Silver, Green Pink * Blue, Purple1 04-Jan 07-Jan 13-Jan 04-Jan 05-Jan 05-Jan 11-Jan 04-Jan V+ V+ V+ VVVVV- 1 16 9 4 9 13 10 7 Silver, Silver Blue, Pink Pink, Red * 05-Jan 04-Jan 13-Jan VVV- 11 5 11 w/eggs male male male juvenile juvenile juvenile juvenile female w/eggs female male male 10.4 8.51 10.23 6.66 4.89 5.39 4.88 8.25 North East West West South South North South 9.12 10.3 9.17 North South East X X X X X X 8 3.38 X 8 1.45 3.12