CUTTING TOGETHER-APART
THE MOULD
Physarum polycephalum (slime mould/mold) is an organism used in scientific research that is commonly
found in piles of leaf litter and human composts. Intelligent yet brainless, P. polycephalum forms a fan-like
network of tendrils in its quest for food. Each tendril moves almost visibly by cytoplasmic streaming – a
pulsing movement of the liquid inside its cell walls. It is simultaneously beautiful and repulsive – an oozing,
bright yellow, lace-like blob. This paper proposes to engage in a (s)mattering of Baradian “agential cuts” to
draw out the intra-active performativity of this evocative critter. A dialogue is established between scientific
and artistic ontoepistemologies, focusing on how (and why) the matter of slime mould/mold matters. Visual
images from explorations which include P. polycephalum are woven through the paper, to support Karen
Barad’s assertion that “meaning is not a property of individual words or groups of words but an ongoing
performance of the world in its differential intelligibility” [1, p.821].
Author: Tarsh Bates
Maddi Boyde
Fig.1 Intra-action exhibition detail, 2013. (L-R) Hayden Fowler, New World Order, 2013; Tarsh Bates, in vitero node (Physarum
polycephalum), 2013; Kathy High, Mr Fox, 2012
44
i
n vitero node (Physarum polycephalum) was an
artwork installed as part of the Intra-action: Multispecies becomings in the Anthropocene
exhibition at MOP Gallery, Sydney in 2013.[1] The
exhibition brought together contemporary artists
exploring the relationships between humans and
nonhuman animals to elucidate physicist and
philosopher Karen Barad’s posthuman theory of intraaction[2] and its insistence on ethical engagements
with our world. As this exhibition, including Barad’s
theory, is the focus of this special issue and has been
discussed in detail by others, this article attends
specifically to slime moulds, in particular the species
Physarum polycephalum.
In 2011, I engaged in a seven month long
performance (in vitero) during which I learnt how to
care for and think with Physarum polycephalum.[3] As
Karen Barad thinks intra-action with slime moulds in
her 2012 article Nature’s Queer Performativity, Intraaction exhibition curators Eben Kirksey and Madeleine
Boyd invited me to re-present in vitero in 2013 as a
material instantiation of Barad’s theorizing. Since for
Barad (and for myself as an artist researcher)
“theorizing, like experimenting, is a material
practice” [2, p.55], this exhibition offered a unique
opportunity to entangle Barad’s writing with the
performativity of the living in public space.
Karen
Barad
introduces
her
2012
article, Nature’s Queer Performativity, with an ethicoonto-epistemological discussion of the intra-activity of
thexsocialxslimexmould, Dictyosteliumxdiscoideum.
These slime moulds form a single multi-cellular sluglike entity when genetically identical individuals
swarm together to form a collective to search for
food. Barad’s discussion uses these social
amoebae to reinforce her understanding of the
inseparability of subjects and objects, and shows
that D. discoideum stands-in for a “subterranean”
fear that humans have for this inseparability. She
unearths moral judgements embedded in a 2009
newspaper report of the discovery of a 40 foot slime
mould colony, a “vast and sticky empire,” “oozing”
under Houston, Texas [3], which reprises the
“creeping threat of communism” that lurks in the 1958
movie The Blob [4, p.26].
Slime moulds are usually considered invisible
in discussions of our ethical encounters with
nonhumans and slime mould experimentation is not
subject to institutional ethics approval. However, as
Barad shows in Nature’s Queer Performativity, the
amorphous, mindless bodies of slime moulds
exemplify fear of consumption of the individual by the
Other, epitomised in The Blob [4, p.27]. In this
movie, the threat of Communism is metamorphosed
into a voracious, unstoppable amoeba-alien: an
inhuman blob that uncontrollably accrues mass and
moral agency. It follows that slime moulds are both
ethically non-existent and morally reprehensible. In the
interests of attentiveness and accountability, it is
important to disclose that the species of slime mould
discussed
by
Barad
in Nature’s
Queer
Performativity is Dictyostelium discoideum. The
species exhibited in Intra-action and discussed
here xis xPhysarumx polycephalum. D.xdiscoideum.
These are microscopic, social amoeba that swarm to
forage, whereas P. polycephalum is not social: it is
an individual macroscopic cell with multiple nuclei,
much like a human muscle cell or neuron; pulsations
in its cytoplasm migrate the cell towards nutrients. P.
polycephalum is useful to think intra-action with as it
is has gained popularity since 2000 as a model
organism for understanding the evolutionary
development of intelligence and biocomputational
problem solving.
My interest lies in how various artistic
and scientific explorations of P. polycephalum can
be reread as instantiations of Barad’s intra-action. I
posit that these explorations are Baradian apparatuses
that enact
“agential
cuts”
in
order
to
facilitate understandings of the phenomenon that
is “slime mould.” Rather than arguing for or
against art or science, I show that they are both
useful methods to reveal
aspects
of
the
complex
intra-actions
of humans and slime
moulds.
Cutting together-apart
The “posthumanist” point is not to blur
the boundaries between human and
nonhuman, not to cross out all
distinctions and differences, and not to
simply invert humanism, but rather to
understand the materializing effects of
particular ways of drawing boundaries
between “humans” and “non-humans.”
–Karen Barad, Nature's Queer Performativity
[4, p.31]
Following physicist Neils Bohr and the foundations of
quantum physics, Barad argues that there is no preexisting, independent reality waiting to be discovered.
Rather, the way we understand our world is
determined by the apparatuses we use to examine
it.[4] For Bohr,–and for Barad,–an apparatus includes
both the physical experimental equipment and the
theoretical or conceptual framework (including
unconscious assumptions) upon which an
examination is founded. An apparatus is therefore
“onto-epistemological,” an inseparable entanglement
of material and knowledge [1, p.816]. In a very broad
sense, “science” is one apparatus and “art” is
another. Each has materials, practices, histories and
45
\
Tarsh Bates
Fig.2 in vitero node (Physarum polycephalum), installation detail, 2013, slime mould, agar, glass © Bates
Importantly, phenomena are not just material but as
with
apparatuses
(which are themselves
phenomena), inextricably material and discursive [1,
p.816]. They are also obviously and fundamentally
posthuman, recognising not just the human, but all
bodies, where “bodies” includes all matter, not just
the living. In Barad’s understanding, matter at all
scales has agency in a phenomenon. Bodies
obviously exist, but as Barad argues, “what we
commonly take to be individual entities are not
separate determinately bounded and propertied
objects, but rather are (entangled ‘parts of’)
phenomena…that extend across (what we commonly
take to be separate places and moments in) space
and time” [4, p.32].
Since any apparatus is inherently bounded,
both physically and conceptually, any examination of
our world necessarily includes and excludes. Barad
describes this moment of inclusion/exclusion as an
“agential cut,” which is not a Cartesian break between
subject and object.[8] As has been shown, subjects
and objects do not pre-exist an intra-action. An
agential cut “enacts a ‘local’ resolution within the
phenomenon,” [4, p.32], that is, it focuses on a
particular intra-action within a phenomenon,
“separating” some components of the phenomenon.
Consequently, an agential cut is not an
assumptions, some of which are similar and some
different.
Since the components of any apparatus are
inseparable from each other, the “object” of
examination is inseparable from the “subject” (or
observer). In fact, there can be no object or subject. It
is not just that an observation by a pre-existing
observer influences and changes a pre-existing
observed. The observer and observed are entangled
parts of an apparatus which creates a moment of
observation.[5] Change any part of the observer or
observed, and the moment of observation is
necessarily different.
Barad, following Bohr, describes the
inseparability of subject and object as “intra-action.”
Reality then, is constituted by a series/continuum of
intra-acting “quantum entanglements,” [6] or
“phenomena,” constantly changing, constantly in flux:
“phenomena are entanglements of spacetimematter,
not in the colloquial sense of a connection or
intertwining of individual entities, but rather in the
technical sense of—“quantum entanglements,”—
which are the (ontological) inseparability of agentially
intra-actingx ‘components’” [4,xp.32], i.e.[7]
46
subsequent “materializing effects” produced by these
practices: what it “is-means” to be human and/or
what it “is-means” to be slime mould [4, p.31].[10]
Please note
that I am not interested in
dichotomising “science” and “art” here, but I am
interested in the effects of the differences and
similarities of their methodologies. The following
discussion attends to the “the particular ways” that
specificxexperimentsxwith Physarumxpolycephalum
have been conducted by scientists and artists, and
what experiences and meanings have been
generated from those experiments. I use these
experiments, or apparatuses, not as analogies, but as
intra-actions, as phenomena, which “force us to
confront conditions for the possibility of objectivity, the
nature of measurement, the nature of nature and
meaning making, and the relationship between
discursive practices and the material world” [2, p.24].
observer/subject looking out at an external
Other/object. Rather, self looks at self. In Barad’s
terms, this separation is an “exteriority-within,” a
simultaneous cutting “together-apart” [4, p.32]. An
apparatus therefore, “shows” which and how many
intra-actions get called a phenomenon, i.e.
A time-lapse video taken through a
microscope, commonly used in slime mould
research, is an example of the material-discursive
relationship between apparatus, cut and phenomena.
The operator-microscope-camera is the apparatus –
images are framed by the microscope and captured
by a still camera. A movie camera allows us to view
the film. A series of still images are taken, edited and
then replayed at 24 frames per second. Each still
image is a phenomenon, as is the compiled film.
Each image “cuts out” a part of the world, including
and excluding, “cutting together-apart.” The editor
chooses which parts of the world get cut and which
get shown. The film played by the camera shows us a
particular selection of the world which would be
different if other cuts/images were included. The
speed at which the film is replayed (24 frames per
second) depicts a particular event time: any faster or
slower and we see the world differently. We are
unable to see the film without the camera and the
technology of the camera determines what we see.
Change the technology/apparatus and the
film/phenomenon changes.
Since phenomena clearly do not always
include the human, Barad’s theories offer possibilities
for understanding the “practices of differentiating
engaged in by nonhumans, whereby nonhumans
differentiate themselves from their environments, from
other nonhumans, and from humans, as well as from
other others” [4, p.31].[9] This, for me, is the
posthuman appeal of Barad’s work. Agential cuts are
an obligation to pay attention to what gets included
and what gets excluded (to what counts) and why,
and
to
the
consequences
of
those
inclusions/exclusions. I take up Barad’s exhortation to
become accountable for any cuts made [4, p.31].
Barad insists that “any proposal for a new political
collective [which surely posthumanism is] must take
account of not merely the practices that produce
distinctions between the human and the nonhuman,
but the practices through which their differential
constitution is produced” [2, p.59]. Hence this article
explores “the particular ways” the agential cutting
enacted by the apparatuses of art and science draw
boundaries between “humans” and “non-human”
slime moulds. Further, I am interested in the
The performativity of mould
Images or representations are not
snapshots or depictions of what awaits us
but rather condensations or traces of
multiple practices of engagement.
Karen Barad, Meeting the Universe Halfway [2, p.53]
Barad’s rejection of representationalism and
insistence upon performativity is pivotal to this
discussion
of
the
“particular
ways”
experiences/meanings of slime mould are generated
through these apparatuses. Barad is very clear that
entities are not independent of their representation.
Since there is no subject or object, representationalism,
which presupposes a distinction between “individuals
and their inherent attributes” and knowledge (or
representation) of those individuals, cannot exist in a
Baradian world [1, p.804]. The system which
assumes:
ignores the “practices through which representations
are produced.”[11]
A Baradian world is not waiting to be
represented, it is always already represented, or
more correctly, it is always already a representation:
a performative representation, where representations
are not abstractions of the real, but apparatuses—
contiguous, iterative materializations of phenomena.
Drawings, photographs, diagrams, language all trace
47
Tarsh Bates
Fig.3 in vitero node (Physarum polycephalum) #1, digital print, 2011 © Bates
48
“material engagements with the world” [2, p.49]. It is
instead the following system:
arm leading to the yolk food source. White areas represent choices of the
blank agar arm leading to the oat food source. The x axis shows the
different Y-maze arm lengths. Within each arm length, food was either
placed at the end of the arm at the start of the experiment (0h) or 2h prior
to the start of the experiment. Stars indicate significant preference for 1
arm (binomial test).” Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers
Ltd: [Behavioral Ecology] [Reid et al. Amoeboid organism uses
extracellular secretions to make smart foraging decisions), copyright
(2013).
Thinking of a representation as an apparatus enables
us to recognise its role in framing knowledge, but
more importantly here, forces us to acknowledge its
performative materiality. Photographs of slime mould
are as much slime mould as Figure 4 or even Figure
5. Importantly however, they connote different
understandings of what slime mould is.
Cutting together-apart the mould/mold:
Nodes and mazes
This section discusses the nature of apparatuses
through a number of linguistic, artistic and scientific
apparatuses that think with P. polycephalum . This
section discusses the nature of apparatuses through a
number of linguistic, artistic and scientific apparatuses
that think with P. polycephalum . Firstly, the linguistic
semantics involved in the word “mould,” then a number
of scientific experiments, beginning with 2000 maze
research conducted by Toshiyuki Nakagaki, Hiroyasu
Yamada and Ágota Tóth are examined. Finally, several
artistic explorations, including my own, are considered.
A Baradian apparatus comprises of three
aspects:
•
•
•
Conceptual framework, including methodology;
Material entanglements; and
Material & conceptual outcomes
In the rest of this paper, the artistic and scientific
apparatuses are introduced and each of the three
aspects are thought through in order to understand
the resultant phenomena. This discussion shows
these apparatuses to be agential, intra-active
entanglements of human, slime mould and
environment.
Fig.4 Experimental setups for agar Y-maze choice experiments, where
ES stands for extracellular slime. (a) Self versus conspecific nonself
extracellular slime, (b) response to heterospecific extracellular slime, (c)
choice between fresh and aged extracellular slime, and (d) patch-mark
cues versus food cues behavioral titration.” Reprinted by permission from
Macmillan Publishers Ltd: [Behavioral Ecology] [Reid et al. Amoeboid
organism uses extracellular secretions to make smart foraging decisions),
copyright (2013)].
Apparatus 1: the semantics of mould/mold
Before I do this however, I want to make a brief note
about the word “mould” itself as an apparatus, which
has motivated me to write it here as
“mould/mold.”[12] Firstly,
the
“British”
(and
Commonwealth) spelling is “mould,” and although its
etymology is unclear, it is thought to be derived from
the medieval English word “mouled,”[13] meaning
“mouldy, rotten or decayed.” “Mould” refers to the
“woolly, furry, or staining growth…which forms on
food, textiles, etc., esp. in moist warm air,” and has
since been adopted into biology to refer to “a
fungus, esp. one that produces the abundant visible
mycelium or spore mass”. [14]
Conversely, the “American” spelling, “mold,”
is inherited from the spelling reform that occurred in
Fig.5 Choice experiments between an arm containing extracellular slime
leading to a yolk food source and an arm of blank agar leading to an oat
food source (the experiments in Figure 2d). The proportion of choices
made by plasmodia within each treatment group is presented on the y
axis. Shaded areas represent choices of the extracellular slime-coated
49
“Mould” has also been used since the 1200s
to refer to the action of manipulating matter into “a
required shape by pouring or pressing into a hollow
form or matrix; to press or cast in or into a particular
form.”[16] Although the etymological relationship
between “mould” as a manipulation of matter and
“mould” as decay is unclear (and not within the
scope of this article), slime moulds collapse the
definitions into a material-discursive phenomenon:
the early 19th century. Donald Scragg found that
many
American
English
spellings
follow
Noah Webster's An American Dictionary of the
English Language published in 1828 [7, pp.82–83].
According to Scragg, the change was partly
nationalistic. Following Barad’s insistence on attention
to “the relationship between discursive practices and
the material world,”[15] I am moved to ask about the
political motivations supporting this change, given
Tarsh Bates
Fig.6 in vitero installation detail, 2011 © Bates
that the timing around the revolution and the desire to
claim a separate identity from Britain. Although
these motivations may appear insignificant, or
even irrelevant, to a discussion of slime mould/mold,
they manifest in confusion around the name in
writings about the organism. Allegiances to
British or American spelling produce historical
and political resonances in any discussion of the
organism. As I am writing in a Commonwealth
country for a British journal, I adopt the British
spelling of “mould.”
This one cell is a master shapeshifter. P.
polycephalum takes on
different appearances depending on
where and how it is growing: In the
forest it might fatten itself into giant
yellow globs or remain as unassuming
as a smear of mustard on the underside
of a leaf; in the lab, confined to a petri
dish, it usually spreads itself thin across
the agar, branching like coral [8].
50
Apparatus 2: The science of mould
Apparatus 2.1: Efficient Mapping
Apparatus 2.2: Optimal foraging
In 2000, a group of scientists, led by Toshiyuki
Nakagaki,zannouncedzthat Physarumzpolycephalum
exhibited “primitive intelligence” demonstrated during
a series of maze-solving experiments [9].[17] The
article correlated efficiency with survival, and
intelligence with computation. A further experiment,
published in 2010 by the Nakagaki group, was even
more popular [10]. This experiment, during which
slime mould “re-created” a map of the Tokyo rail
system, consolidated the popularity and usefulness
of “brainless, yet intelligent,” slime moulds as “a
biologically inspired mathematical model” [10,
p.439]. Slime mould mapping of Iberia, Canada,
Australia, Brazil and the UK has since followed,
extoling the amazing ability of slime moulds to
“efficiently
construct
transport
networks” [11,
p.1546].[18] [19] These mapping experiments and the
“problem-solving” capabilities of slime mould have
established P. polycephalum as a model organism
for
biocomputing
and
complex
network
engineering.[20]
Fig.7 “a, Structure of the organism before finding the shortest path.
Blue lines indicate the shortest paths between two agar blocks
containing nutrients: a1 (4151 mm); a2 (3351 mm); b1 (4451 mm);
and b2 (4551 mm). b, Four hours after the setting of the agar blocks
(AG), the dead ends of the plasmodium shrink and the pseudopodia
explore all possible connections. c, Four hours later, the shortest path
has been selected. Plasmodium wet weight, 90510 mg. Yellow,
plasmodium; black, ‘walls’ of the maze; scale bar, 1 cm. d, Path
selection. Numbers indicate the frequency with which each pathway
was selected. ‘None’, no pseudopodia (tubes) were put out.” [9]
Maze-solving by Physarum polycephalum. Reprinted by permission
from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: [Nature] (Nakagaki et al. Intelligence:
Maze-solving by an amoeboid organism), copyright (2000)
(doi:10.1038/35035159)) © Toshiyuki Nakagaki
51
Fig.8 Experimental setup. (A) No-choice experiment, diet 1:2,
with a total concentration of 40 g·L−1. (B) Choice experiment,
diet 6:1 vs. diet 1:2. (C) Multiple-choice experiment. The slime
mold was initially placed at the center of the petri dish. © Audrey
Dussutour
In the wild, slime mould is part of a complex food
chain. It grows on decaying organic matter in damp
dark environments, consuming bacteria and fungi
that in turn feed on the organic matter. It exudes
chemicals that break down bacterial and fungal cell
walls allowing access to the nutrients within the
cells [13, p.121]. In the laboratory, nutrients are
usually provided in the form of either rolled oats or
glucose. A 2010 research project supplied a slime
mould within different ratios of proteins and sugars
and showed that slime moulds prefer a food source
that comprises two thirds protein and one third
carbohydrates [12]. As shown in Fig.8, slime mould
was offered various combinations of proteins and
carbohydrates. The researchers concluded that the
slime mould was able to solve “complex nutritional
challenges without possessing a centralized
processing center or specialized foraging agents.
Plasmodia…were able to alter their growth form and
movement to exploit complementary food resources
and regulate the supply of carbohydrate and protein
to a target ratio that maximized performance” [12,
p.4608]. This study was interested in the implications
of slime mould foraging and nutritional decision
making for soil ecologies. They suggested that
“understanding the nutritional currencies that shape
exploitation of food resources by organisms such as
slime molds and fungi, and the consequences of
such nutritional decisions for growth, form, and
function…has significance for local and global
nutrient cycling and carbon sequestration” [12,
p.4608].
Subsequent research found that foraging
slime mould avoids areas where it encounters its own
slime, increasing its “foraging efficiency” [14,
p.812]. The researchers suggested that the slime is
a type of “patch marking,” “a reliable and widespread
mechanism to ‘memorize’ the location of an
unprofitable patch, allowing individuals to recognize
previously searched areas and relocate their search
to unmarked and unexplored patches” [14,
p.812]. The researchers therefore concluded that the
slime acts as extracellular spatial “memory,” the first
time this has been demonstrated in an organism
without a brain or a nervous system [15].
The efficient foraging characteristic of slime
mould enables it to negotiate such mazes and
transport networks above. However, unconventional
computing researcher Andrew Adamatzky recently
found that the usual attractants of oats and glucose
used in slime mould problem solving experiments
were inadequate for highly complex computational
problems. Consequently, he conducted research into
more complex chemotaxis responses, searching for
“novel ways to control propagation of slime mould, in
order to program it and make it implement
computational operations” [16]. He found that slime
Fig.9 The slime mould (yellow blob) was placed at one
end of a petri dish with the sugary food source (black
smudge) placed at the other end. The U-shaped trap was
setup in between [15]. © Chris Reid
mould preferred herbal sedatives to oats and
glucose, with its strongest preference being for
valerian root. “Using these herbal tablets we can
attract slime moulds and keep them 'concentrated' in
a local domain of space" [Adamatzky cited in 17].
Fig.10 Examples of experiments. (ab) Kalms Sleep (left) vs
Kalms Tablets (right): (a) plasmodium propagates to both targets,
(b) plasmodium propagates to Kalms Sleep (left); (c) Nytol (left)
vs Kalms Tablets (right); (d) Valerian roots (left) vs Nytol (right); (e)
Valerian roots (left) vs. Vervain (right); (f) Hops (left) vs. Passion
flower (right) [16].
52
electrode array (shown in Fig.11) which was
connected via a computer to the robot. Light
detected by sensors on the robot controlled light
shone onto the slime mould. The response of the
mould to the light was registered by the electrodes,
causing a corresponding movement in the legs of the
robot. “The robot then scrabbled away from bright
lights as a mechanical embodiment of the
mould.”[19] The researchers described the process
as a transduction of “microphysical scale”
environmental/chemical signals by the “intracellular
informationxxprocessing”xxsystemxofxxthe Physarum
cell, which is then amplified through the interaction
loop to the “macroscopic environment” of the robot
[18, p.217].
Apparatus 2.3: Emotional cyborgs
Fig.12 Cellular robot controller. Sensors on the robot (RB) detect
lamp input (LI) and transmit a signal to the computer (PC). The
signals from the sensors are recoded by the PC into a spatial light
pattern which is projected as light stimulus with a video projector
(PR) via mirror (MR) onto the surface of the plasmodium (PP).
Oscillations of the plasmodium (PP), which is patterned on an
agar plate (AP) by a plastic sheet (PS), are detected by a CCD
camera (CC) as intensity changes in light transmitted from the
bandpass filtered (BF) light source (LS) [18, p.219]. © Soichiro
Tsuda
Fig.11 Physarum oscillator circuit. The plasmodium is patterned
as six oscillators with star coupling (A)…A diagrammatic view of
one branch is shown on the right (B). The oscillator nodes have a
diameter of 3 mm; channels are 6 mm × 0.5 mm. A dashed box
delineates the area irradiated for light stimulus [18, p.219]. ©
Soichido Tsuda
In 2012, this experiment was extended to control an
expressive, female Jules robot made by Hanson
Robotics [20]. Here, the slime mould was grown on
a 64 electrode array rather than a six node array.
Attraction responses were generated by oat flakes
and repulsion responses were generated by light. The
electrical signals generated by its migration across
the array were converted into sounds. The
researchers assigned each sound an emotion using
the “circumplex model of affect,” which “proposes
that all [human] affective states arise from two
fundamental neurophysiological systems, one related
to valence (a pleasure–displeasure continuum) and
the other to arousal, or alertness. Each emotion can
be understood as a linear combination of these two
dimensions, or as varying degrees of both valence
and arousal” [21]. The slime mould researchers
aligned the attraction-repulsion responses of the
slime mould along the pleasure-displeasure axis of
the psychological model and the volume (or strength)
Slime mould has been used in attraction and
repulsion experiments as it responds rapidly to
chemical changes in its environment, and it is
photophobic (afraid of light). Its migration towards
food sources is an example of an attraction response
and its migration away from light is a repulsion
response. Commonly used to demonstrate the
phenomena of chemotaxis and photophobia to
school students,[22] a number of recent sophisticated
scientific experiments have explored the biorobotic
potential of these phenomena.
The first of these developed a control
interface between slime mould and a six-legged
walking robot in 2006 [18]. Following on from
research into Physarum Boolean logic gates, an
interaction loop amplified the oscillation of
the Physarum in response to light, steering the
robot. The slime mould was grown over a six node
53
Fig.13 A graphical representation of the circumplex model of affect with the horizontal axis representing the valence dimension and the
vertical axis representing the arousal or activation dimension” [21]; The Gale robot [20] © Jonathan Posner; Sandrine Ceurstemont
of the response along the arousal axis. A facial
expression corresponding to each emotion was then
generated in the Jules robot (Fig.13) .
Apparatus 3: Artistic explorations
Apparatus 3.1: in vitero (2011-2013) Tarsh
Bates
Tarsh Bates
Fig.14 in vitero, installation detail, PICA, 2011 © Megan
in vitero was a durational performance that explored
aesthetic experiences of care through prolonged
engagement with eight other species of living
organisms commonly used in reproductive biology,
including P. polycephalum .
The performance
occurred in two locations: a scientific laboratory at
University of Western Australia and The Perth Institute
of Contemporary Art. After two and a half months in
the laboratory, where I learned to take care of the
slime mould, the project moved into the gallery and
was open to the public.[23] The mould was installed in
the gallery in a customised glass vessel and I lived in
the gallery with it for a further three months. During
this time, I engaged in necessary and often mundane
activities required for the care of the organism and
myself. I asked the following questions: How do our
behaviours change when we care for other bodies? Is
caring for slime mould different in a gallery rather than
a laboratory? How do we care for creatures that are
not cute or furry? Is it possible to care about
something as radically other as slime mould?
During the five months of care the slime
mould flourished and died several times. I was
interested in its ongoing performativity, in its
phenomenological
engagement
with
the
environments I provided, rather than capturing a
single moment of beauty as an insight into its
true nature. At times it was dynamic and spectacular,
Schlipalius; Physarum polycephalum, installation detail, 2011 ©
Tarsh Bates
moving towards food within hours and forming bright
yellow lace networks around the glass vessel. It
withdrew to other food, leaving diaphanous traces of
itself. One viewer seeing the lace network declared
that I had captured it at its most beautiful. Ironically it
had run out of food, and was in fact starving rather
than flourishing.
Another instantiation of the slime mould, in
vitero node (Physarum polycephalum ), was included
as part of the Intra-action exhibition that inspired this
Tarsh Bates
Fig.15 in vitero, installation detail, MOP Gallery, 2013
© Tarsh Bates
54
Oliver Kelhammer
Fig.16 The IKEA vexation, research stills, 2013. © Oliver Kelhammer
special issue of Antennae, 18 months after the in
vitero project had finished. A new specimen of P.
polycephalum was sourced from a local scientific
research laboratory, seeded into the same vessel and
placed within the MOP gallery. I considered this
installation to be a node of the previous project; a
continuation of the exploration of the performativity of
slime mould, and an instantiation of Barad’s
posthuman intra-action. However, the slime mould
refused to perform: sporadic plasmodia sent out in
search of food quickly died and no spectacular lace
formed to signify an ideal state.
Apparatus 3.2: The Ikea vexation (2013)
Oliver Kelhammer
In The IKEA vexation, Oliver Kelhammer built a model
of the floor plan of his local IKEA store, placed an oat
in each “department,” and placed the Physarum at
the entrance.[24] The floor plan of the IKEA store is
both the maze of the scientific experiments and an
example of the capitalist architectural phenomenon
now known as the Gruen transfer, which is the spatial
disorientationxexperiencedxinxshoppingxmalls.[25]
Teresa Schubert
Fig.17 Somniferous Observatory, 2010, (L-R: untreated, valerian root, cannabis, tobacco) © Teresa Schubert
55
Apparatus 3.5: Physarum Experiment No:
013: The Spelling Test (2010) Heather
Barnett
This disorientation is designed to encourage
unintended purchasing and is due to the intentionally
confusing layout of shops and other physical barriers
among other environmental cues, such as sound and
lighting.[26] Kelhammer “wondered if the slime mould
could resist such distractions and efficiently find its
way through an IKEA without getting sidetracked?”[27]
Apparatus 3.3: Somniferous Observatory
(2010) Teresa Schubert
Somniferous Observatory is a series of artistic
experiments by Teresa Schubert that explored the
“self-organisation and pattern formation of Physarum
polycephalum under the influence of psychoactive
and
somniferous
substances.”[28] Schubert’s
explorations re-visit a series of experiments by
pharmacologist Peter Witt, which investigated the
influence of various psychoactive drugs on the webs
constructed by garden spiders, including caffeine,
LSD and amphetamines. The drugs used by
Schubert included tobacco, cannabis, St. Johns Wort
(an herbal anti-depressant), sertraline hydrochloride (a
pharmaceutical anti-depressant), and valerian root (an
herbal sedative). The effects of the drugs on the
migration and networks formed by the slime mould
were documented by Schubert in a series of digital
prints.
Heather Barnett
Fig.19 Physarum Experiment No: 013 – The Spelling
Test, 2010. video still © Heather Barnett
The Spelling Test is an animation which documents
one of a series of artistic experiments conducted by
Heather Barnett which explores the “simple yet
complex behaviours of this biological and cultural
phenomenon”xthatxisxP.xpolycephalum.[30]
Directly
inspired by the scientific research described
above, The Spelling Test explores understandings of
intelligence by encouraging the slime mould to form
the words “Physarum polycephalum” through nutrient
placement. Another work by Barnett, Physarum
Experiment No: 019 is an animation which revisits the
maze in Nakagaki et al’. 2000 groundbreaking
experiment.
Apparatus 3.4: Bodymetries (2013)
Teresa Schubert
Apparatus 3.6: The sublime grotesque:
Gail Wight & Phil Ross
Gail Wight produced a series of artworks between
1996 and 2009 which focus on the agency of slime
mould through representations of cytoplasmic
streaming, the mechanism of migration of P.
polycephalum. When foraging, the cytoplasm within
the cell membrane pulses, moving the cell. Wight’s
1996 work, Slime Trace, creates drawings using the
migration
of
the
slime
mould
towards
[31]
oats. Creep (2004) and Hydraphilia (2009) are
time-lapse
videos
of
cytoplasmic
streaming. Creep is a three channel work,
whereas Hydraphilia presents nine large monitors,
reflecting the nine-headed Hydra of Greek mythology
(polycephalum is Latin for “many-headed”). Both
works present the slime mould as simultaneously
beautiful and grotesque. Similarly, Phil Ross’ 2009
video work Leviathans, focuses on the migration of
slime mould.[32]
The work has a highly evocative soundscape
Teresa Shcubert
Fig.18 Bodymetries, 2013. Installation still, Totall
Recall, Ars Electronica Festival © Teresa Schubert
Bodymetries is a second work by Teresa Schubert
that engages with pattern formation by P.
physarum.[29] This interactive work is a computer
simulation which maps “melanin spots on human
bodies to the protoplasmic network of slime mould.”
Images of yellow slime mould protoplasm are
Projected onto human bodies. Bodymetries
presents both slime mould and human skin as
biological sensors.
56
Gale Wight
Fig. 20 Slime Trace (1996), Creep (2004), Hydraphilia, 2009 © Gale Wight
and the beauty of the slime mould is
apparent. However, the threat expressed in The
Blob and described
by
Barad
weighs
heavily in Leviathans, not only because of the
reference to the sea monster, Leviathan, of Jewish
mythology in the title of the work. Cells collide,
merge and engulf, accompanied and enhanced
by the dramatic soundscape. The organism
presented here is a sublime, monstrous alien.
Apparatus 3.7 Being Slime
Mould: observation / simulation /
enactment (2013) Slimoco
Being Slime Mould was a three part artwork during
which audiences could view videos of Heather
Barnett’s Physarum Experiments, participate in
becoming-food for slime mould and become-slime
mould.[33] As viewers moved around the exhibition, a
computerxxxmodelled xxxPhysarum xxxplasmodium
recognised them as food and formed networks
between the human “food sources.” A slime mould
network was also simulated, where viewers were tied
together as a “collective super-organism” and
encouraged to forage: to become-slime mould.
Apparatus 3.8: Morphs (2013) William
Bondin
Morphs was a speculative design project which
proposed a series of architectural forms which are
mobile in response to environmental cues.[34] Inspired
by the scientific research described above, William
Bondin developed the Morphs to reflect the
environmentally stimulated path finding and spatial
memory of slime mould. For Bondin, “the interesting
thing about slime mould, in particular Physarum
polycephalum, is that its cognitive processes occurs
within its environment rather than a centralised brain. It
is an example of an organism which has developed a
clever way of exploiting its surroundings in order to
perform navigational tasks and memory-related
processes.”[35] The
Morphs
are potentially
communicative, autonomous and heuristic structures
that could temporarily form complex structures and
perform collective tasks, learning from experience
and their environment.
Philip Ross
Fig. 21 Leviathans, 2009 © Philip Ross
57
Slimoco
Fig. 22 Being Slime Mould: observation / simulation / enactment, performance stills, 2013 © Heather Barnett
Aspect 1: Conceptual frameworks:
Frivolity and utility, control and agency
In contrast, the artworks tend to be frivolous
explorations of the behaviours of the slime mould.
Even those artworks that revisit or use scientific
experiments do not frame their outcomes in terms of
utility or efficiency, the exceptions being
Bondin’s Morphs and
Schubert’s Bodymetries.
Rather, the inefficiency and performativity of the slime
mould is highlighted in playful ways that gently
subvert the reduction of slime mould to logic gates.
The video works of Gail Wight and Phil Ross
and my work in vitero are framed by notions of
agency and unknowability rather than optimisation
and computation. The slime moulds are allowed to
roam, gazed upon as in science experiments, with
the same technologies, but the focus is on the body
and behaviour of the mould rather than on its
function.
Understandings of intelligence, memory and
The conceptual frameworks for these science and art
explorations are very different. With the exception of
Christopher Reid’s experiments into foraging, the
science experiments adopt an engineering approach
which is goal oriented and interested in the utility of P.
polycephalum for human purposes, in particular, the
solution of complex network problems (transportation)
and biocomputing. Consequently, the experiments
are framed by engineering concepts of efficiency,
optimisation, design and computation. Reid’s
experiments are framed by evolution biology and are
ecologically focused, however the researchers adopt
capitalist concepts of profitability, efficiency and “the
costs of ‘lost opportunity,’” [14, p.813], notions
prevalent in contemporary evolutionary biology.
William Bondin
Fig. 23 Morphs, digital image, 2013 © William Bondin
58
can only interact with a virtual slime mould within a
restricted projection field.
Unlike the scientific experiments however, the
artistic experiments are also interested in chance and
the agency of the organism. In many explorations, the
temporal
and
sensual
aesthetics
of
viewing/experiencing the slime mould are more
important than the visual aesthetics, which opened
the experiments to the agency of the mould. Allowing
for unpredictability and randomness creates
opportunities for complex understandings of the
worlding of mould—not just the world that is the
“wild,” but the naturecultural world. These artworks
position themselves, as Barad says, “not merely to
use non/humans as tools to think with, but in thinking
with them to face our ethical obligations to them, for
they are not merely tools for our use but real living
beings” [4, p.33]. By relinquishing some control and
opening to the performativity of P. polychepalum
these works attempt to bring the human observer into
the world of the slime mould, rather than the slime
mould into the human world.
In attempting to bring the human into the
slime mould world, the aesthetics of care are a
significant aspect of working with living organisms for
both scientists and artists. The aesthetics of care are
the visual and phenomenological experiences that
occur when a living, non-living or semi-living being is
cared for or about. “Caring” in this context constitutes
the physical and emotional actions involved in
maintaining and sustaining the continued existence of
another or attending to the non-existence (or death)
of another being/object (it could also apply to the
self). These experiences are often contingent upon
proximity to the cared for, duration spent caring for,
and the power relations between carer and cared for.
Attentiveness to the aesthetic experiences of care
activities and responses to those activities by the
cared for, can disrupt the long standing
human/animal dualism and inspire a recognition of
our responsibility towards the others with whom we
share our world and are often dependent upon. in
vitero explicitly explored this aesthetic with respect
to P. polychepalum, inherent in the scientific and
other artistic experiments described here.
emotion frame much of the exploration, artistic and
scientific. Mazes have a significant history in our
construction of these understandings, beginning with
rat experiments in the early 20th century. Decisionmaking within a maze in response to a stimuli is a
criteria of intelligence assumed by the science
experiments described above. Kelhammer’s The
IKEA
vexation and
Barnett’s The
Spelling
Test demonstrate the absurdity of using human
structures to attribute intelligence to nonhuman
organisms. These works question assumptions about
intelligence. The slime mould bursts through the walls
of Kelhammer’s IKEA maze and, even encouraged by
food, is unable to “spell” its name. Likewise,
Schubert’s Somniferous Observatory feeds the slime
mould with the same drug (Valerian root) as
Adamatzky’s scientific experiment, but from curiosity
about the effects on the slime mould’s behaviour, not
from a desire to improve its intelligence or efficiency.
Notions of control and agency are crucial to
these explorations. Control is vital to both the
methodology and outcomes of scientific experiments.
Framed by the lens of the scientific method, P.
polychepalum is a pre-existing entity holding
information that needs to be discovered. The slime
mould is constrained within mazes or on electrodes,
and stimulated or repulsed by chemicals or light to
achieve particular outcomes. Data generated by
these experiments is manipulated and filtered to
produce statistical probabilities and predictable,
repeatable, useful results. Any deviation is obscured
by the statistics or the conclusions.
Control is also important in the aesthetic
framing of the artworks. The history of Western
aesthetics influences understandings of the slime
mould as a nonhuman organism and the aesthetic
decisions made to produce the artworks. Constraints
were placed on the slime mould during all these
artworks: physical, such as by mazes or vessels, or
by chemicals, light or food in order to elicit
responses, such as in Schubert’s Sominferous
Observatory and Barnett’s The Spelling Test, and
also in my own in vitero, where I occasionally
withheld food in order to encourage foraging and
migration to coat the glass vessels with yellow lace.
Wight and Ross’ videos are highly framed and
constructed: Wight’s through the addition of coloured
dyes to the substrate and Ross’s through digital
colour enhancement and a highly evocative
soundtrack.
Slimoco’s BeingxxxSlimexxxMould physically
constrains
both
slime
moulds
and
humans. Bodymetries restricts mapping to melanin
spots—the slime mould has no choice in its path or
its stimuli and the human experience of the work is
controlled by technological limitations, as humans
Aspect 2: Material entanglements:
moulding the mould
To maximize its foraging efficiency, and
therefore its chances of survival, the
plasmodium changes its shape in the
maze to form one thick tube covering the
shortest distance between the food
sources. This remarkable process of
cellular computation implies that cellular
59
Tarsh Bates
Fig. 24 in vitero, (Physarum polycephalum), 2011 © Tarsh Bates
materials
can
intelligence.
show
a
This is also the case in a number of the
artworks:
Barnett’s The
Spelling
Test,
Ross’ Leviathans and Wight’s video works. In all
these works, the equipment and the environmental
context of the slime mould are absent. The opposite
is the case in in vitero, Kelhammer’s The IKEA
vexation,xxxSchubert’s xxSomniferousxxObservatory,
Slimoco’s xxBeingxSlimexMould xxandxxxBondin’s
Morphs. In these works, the vessels, food,
participants and technology have almost equal
importance: the organism is embedded in its context,
a Harawayan cyborg.
In in vitero, the materials are a substrate for
the agency of the slime mould and are as much a
part of the aesthetic experience of the human viewer
as the living body of the slime mould. The glass
vessel references scientific experimentation, and the
Victorian-style table evokes that intensive period of
exotic specimen collection, with all its colonising
connotations. The living body of the slime mould is
encountered in all its phases, including death. The
human viewer is forced to bend to see the slime
mould in the vessel, forcing awareness of their own
body in relation to that of the slime mould. They are
able to see their own reflection in the glass of the
vessel, entangling with the body of the slime mould.
Two still images of the original in vitero project were
presented above the living installation in the Intraaction exhibition, both entanglements of slime mould
and human – my gloved fingers or my writing, its
body and food. Both these images show the slime
mould at different stages of migration, in moments of
primitive
–Nakagaki et al. Intelligence: Maze-solving by an
amoeboid organism [9]
One of the primary considerations in working with
living organisms is that these organisms have bodies.
In the case of P. polycephalum, the unique
materiality of its body is fundamental to both the
scientific experiments and the artworks. As Whiting et
al. state, “slime mould is capable of optimising the
shape of its protoplasmic networks in spatial
configurations of attractants and repellents” [23,
p.844]. Consequently,
the
body
of P.
polycephalum is central to all the explorations
described here. However, the body of the slime
mould is not a pre-formed truth to be discovered.
Each exploration, scientific or artistic, creates unique
material conditions in which the body forms and reforms in response to physical, chemical or light
obstacles: a collection of “agential cuts” performing
the phenomena of slime mould.
The materials used in each apparatus are
quite similar: scientific glassware, agar, oats, a table,
lighting, camera, still or video, computer or other
electronic equipment, slime mould plasmodia, the
human experimenter, and the human viewer. The
focus on function within the scientific experiments
relegates all these materials, except for the slime
mould plasmodia, (and possibly the maze), to
irrelevant components of an objective experiment.
60
living slime mould in in vitero node (Physarum
polycephalum): human artefacts overrun by the
sublime, grotesque plasmodia. However, the threat
from the living organism is allayed in both in
vitero andxxSchubert’s xxSomniferousxxObservatory
through containment: the slime mould cannot escape
the vessels. Any threat in the mobile and autonomous
The threat is suppressed in the scientific
escape from human constraints. Despite this
seemingly utopic relationship however, the slime
mould has nowhere to hide. Exposed through glass
and to hot spotlights, the slime mould is inescapably
on display.
The complicated network of “mess mates”
present in experimental set-ups is ignored by all the
explorations described here. Donna Haraway insists
on the consideration of “mess mates” as intrinsic to
natureculture:
architectural forms of Morphs and in the mapping of
human
skin
by
the
projected P.
polycephalumxxx plasmodiaxxin xxBodymetries xis
assuaged by the presence of technology.
The threat is suppressed in the scientific
experiments by the rhetoric of optimization and
rfficiency:
The basic story is simple: ever more
complex life forms are the continual
result of ever more intricate and
multidirectional acts of association of
and with other life forms. Trying to make
a living, critters eat critters but can only
partly digest one another. Quite a lot of
indigestion, not to mention excretion, is
the natural result, some of which is the
vehicle for new sorts of complex
patternings of ones and manys in
entangled association…Organisms are
ecosystems of genomes, consortia,
communities, partly digested dinners,
mortal boundary formations…Eating
one another and developing indigestion
are only one kind of transformative
merger practice; living critters form
consortia in a baroque medley of interand intra-actions…Cum panis, mess
mates [24, pp.31-32].
slime molds first engulfed the entirety of
the edible maps. Within a matter of
days…the protists thinned themselves
away, leaving behind interconnected
branches of slime that linked the pieces
of food…the single-celled brainless
amoebae did not grow living branches
between pieces of food in a random
manner; rather, they behaved like a
team of human engineers, growing the
most efficient networks possible [8].
However, the physical apparatuses that manipulate
the bodies of the P. polycephalum —the mazes,
food, chemicals and light—are the primary
mechanisms of control: “by forcing the slime mould
into a U-shaped obstacle, which we placed between
the creature and a sugary food source it was drawn
to, we found the slime mould used its spatial memory
system to help navigate its way out of the obstacle to
reach the food” [15]. As described by scientist Chris
Reid here, the physical constraints of the maze and
the “U-shaped obstacle” literally mould the bodies of
the P.
polycephalum
into
two-dimensional
configurations. This two-dimensionality is reinforced
by photographs and diagrams of the experiments
which, without exception, present aerial views of the
slime mould. This is also the case with the artworks,
with the exception of in vitero , Somniferous
Observatory , and Morphs .
Adamatzky’s sedative experiments attempt to
find more subtle ways of moulding the mould’s body,
to improve experimental efficiency. In addition,
understandings of slime mould are constrained by
the presentation of experimental data as objective
representations of truth, despite the obvious inclusion
of symbols and overlayed diagrams. In Fig.7
from the Nakagaki maze experiment, for example, the
equivalent size and placement of the statistical path
Although many of the explorations discussed in this
paper allude to supplying oats for the slime mould,
usually as an attractant or stimulant, the slime mould
does not, in fact, eat the oat. Rather, it eats the
bacteria and fungi invisible to the human eye that are
actually eating the oat. All these experiments rely on
the migration of the slime mould towards the
oat/bacteria/fungi mess mates, but the bacteria/fungi
are never mentioned. The slime mould must receive a
chemical signal from the oat/bacteria/fungi in order to
recognise their presence, a type of call-response. Do
the oat/bacteria/fungi exude this chemical in order to
seduce the slime mould? Is it possible that they
“want” to be eaten?
The radical strangeness of the slime mould
and the threat of consumption and moral repugnance
described by Barad manifests in Wight’s
work, Creep and Hydraphilia, andxRoss’xLeviathan.
Even the titles of these works evoke this fear, but it is
most evident in the immense and multiple,
uncontrolled and pulsating bodies, the dramatic use
of colour, and the soundscape of Leviathans. The
threat is also present in the images exhibited with the
61
Tarsh Bates
Fig. 25 in vitero, (Physarum polycephalum), 2011 © Tarsh Bates
selection diagram visually displaces the bodies of the
slime mould into numbers.
Similar constraints are also present in several
of
the
artworks,
including The
IKEA
vexation, SomniferousxxObservatory xandxxBarnett’s
Physarum Experiments (in particular, No: 019 The
Maze). None however, are particularly interested in
efficiency, and allow for the agency of the slime
mould to create unrepeatable and irrepressible
performative encounters. The mould bursts through
the maze walls of The IKEA vexation and migrates
under the walls in Physarum Experiment No: 019.
Interestingly, it does not climb the walls as it does in
the vessels of in vitero. No maze obstructs the slime
mould’s migration within these vessel: it is free to
roam, and moulds itself into three-dimensional forms,
defying gravity.
Kelhammer describes the performative
agency of the slime mould during The IKEA vexation:
Physarum tends to behave in what is
called a ‘ballistic’ manner; once they
have sensed a stimulus they will try to
flow toward it taking the most direct
route possible. Except when they don't.
Because unlike a non-sentient being,
slime mould will often abort its migration
toward an attractant, if it senses a rival
has gotten there first. Sometimes
though, competitors will merge into one
happy super organism and share the
[sic] love and the bounty. Sometimes
even that doesn't happen and a slime
mould gets distracted and decides to
loiter around the perimeter of the petri
dish,
boycotting
the
experiment
altogether, despite the presence of
tasty oatmeal.
Who knows why? Are they following their
bliss? Or are they just confused?[37]
62
Tarsh Bates
Fig. 26 in vitero, (Physarum polycephalum), 2011 © Tarsh Bates
In
fact,
left
to
its
own
devices, P.
polycephalum forages by sending out “step
leaders,” as seen in the image above.[37] The yellow
lace-like plasmodia pulse outward in waves in several
directions, sending out chemical signals to identify
food. The food “responds…with [out]ward signals of
its own” [4, p.35]. This is not an isolated identification
of the shortest path to food, but a multiplex of
communication. Reid et al. showed that these
diaphanous tendrils are in fact the embodied,
extracellular memory of the mould, necessary for
foraging [14]. Is this efficient? Certainly it is a complex
form of intelligence.
Although many of the experiments reduce the
complex behaviour of the mould to Boolean logic
gates, the two robotic experiments complicate the
notions of body, intelligence, emotion, efficiency and
agency. The body of the slime mould is collapsed
onto/into the bodies of the computer and those of the
robots. The insect-like body of the Zauner bot is
described as “scrabbling away from bright lights as a
mechanical embodiment of the mould” [19].
The robot-mould unit is agential, autonomous and
inhuman.
The Gale robot forces us to question how
emotions are expressed and who expresses them.
The behaviour of the slime mould, which is reduced
to Boolean logic gates by others, is transformed
through electricity, sound, a complex psychological
model of human emotion, back to electrical signals,
and
into
robotic
facial expressions. This
transformation passes through at least three stages of
absurdity: firstly, the assignation of human emotion to
slime mould is a surreal notion; secondly, the
synaesthesia manifest in the transference of the
movement of the slime mould’s body into sound; and
finally, the circumplex model of affect was designed
to avoid problems experienced in experimental
psychology when attributing an emotion to particular
facial expression [21]. As Posner et al. describe,
Affective behaviors, however, are neither
sufficient nor necessary to characterize
emotional
states
(Kagan, 2003;
63
of “slime mould,” a multiplicity of cutting togetherapart. Through the phenomena formed by these
apparatuses we perceive some of the performative
intra-activity of Physarum polycephalum. Inherent in
Barad’s agential realism is a disturbing moral
imperative to “face our ethical obligations to
[nonhumans], for they are not merely tools for our use
but real living beings” [4, p.33]. If, as Gale’s slime
mould-robot suggests, slime mould does experience
fear (the Zauner bot is also described as being
“moved by a slime mould's fears,” [19]), we are
forced to rethink our ethical obligations to this radically
alien creature. Our current systems are undone.
This article has attempted to face Barad’s
disturbing imperative, “to stay with the trouble.” It is a
humble gesture of an attention to “the materializing
effects of particular ways of drawing boundaries
between ‘humans’ and [slime moulds]” [4, p.31].
While our ethical obligations to horses, rabbits, mice,
or even rats, may be clear (although this is by no
means given), what are our obligations to slime
mould? Does the matter of slime mould matter?
Panksepp, 1998). An animal, therefore,
could experience feelings without
demonstrating any overt changes in
behavior, and conversely, through
experimental
manipulation,
could
display affective behaviors without any
associated feeling. As Damasio (2003)
notes, paramecium will flee from
predators, but to argue that singlecelled organisms experience fear
makes little sense [21, p.2].
References
[1] Barad, Karen. Posthumanist performativity: Toward an
understanding of how matter comes to
matter. Signs, 2003, 28 (3), pp.801-831.
[2] Barad, Karen. Meeting the universe halfway: Quantum
physics and the entanglement of matter and meaning. Durham
& London: Duke University Press, 2007.
[3] Yoon, Carol Kaesuk. Oozing through texas soil, a team of
amoebas billions strong. The New York Times, 23 March 2009,
[Accessed 24/2/14], Available
from: http://www.nytimes.com/2009/03/24/
science/24amoe.html ?_r=2&.
[4] Barad, Karen. Nature's queer performativity. Kvinder Køn &
Forskning, 2012, 1-2 , pp.25-53.
[5] Hird, Myra J. Indifferent globality. Theory, Culture &
Society, 2010, 27 (2-3), pp.54-72.
Klaus-Peter Zauner
Fig.27 The slime mould - pictured on the screen in the
[6] Haraway, Donna. Simians, cyborgs and women: The
reinvention of nature. New York: Routledge, 1991.
experimental set-up - causes a robot to naturally avoid the
light”, 2006 © Klaus-Peter Zauner
[7] Scragg, Donald. A history of english spelling. Manchester:
Manchester University Press, 1974.
[8] Jabr, Ferris. "How brainless slime molds redefine intelligence."
Scientific American, November 7 2012.
Aspect 3: Material-discursive
understandings: intelligence, complexity
and moral imperatives
[9] Nakagaki, Toshiyuki, Hiroyasu Yamada, and Ágota Tóth.
Intelligence: Maze-solving by an amoeboid
organism. Nature, 2000, 407, p.470.
The apparatuses discussed here facilitate access to
aspects of the queer critter that is slime mould. None
are “correct,” each contributes to our understanding
[10] Tero, Atsushi, Seiji Takagi, Tetsu Saigusa, Kentaro Ito, Dan
P. Bebber, Mark D. Fricker, Kenji Yumiki, Ryo Kobayashi, and
64
Toshiyuki Nakagaki. Rules for biologically inspired adaptive
network design. Science, 2010, 327 (5964), pp.439-442.
Notes
[11] Reid, Chris R., and Madeleine Beekman. Solving the towers of
Hanoi – how an amoeboid organism efficiently constructs
transport networks. The Journal of Experimental
Biology, 2013, 216 (9), pp.1546-1551.
[1] http://intraactionart.com/
[2] “It is through specific agential intra-actions [in contrast to the
usual ‘interaction,’ which presumes the prior existence of
independent entities/relata] that the boundaries and properties of
the ‘components’ of phenomena become determinate and that
particular embodied concepts become meaningful. A specific
intra-action (involving a specific material configuration of the
‘apparatus of observation’) enacts an agential cut (in contrast to
the Cartesian cut—an inherent distinction—between subject and
object) effecting a separation between ‘subject’ and
‘object’…relata do not preexist relations; rather, relatawithinphenomena emerge through specific intra-actions” [1, p.815].
[12] Dussutour, Audrey, T. Dussutour, M. Latty, and S. J.
Beekman. Amoeboid organism solves complex nutritional
challenges. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America, 2010, 107 (10), pp.4607-4611.
[13] Lloyd, Sarah. Slime moulds: An exquisite obsession. PAN:
Philosophy Activism Nature, 2013, 10, pp.119-129.
[14] Reid, Chris R., Madeleine Beekman, Tanya Latty, and Audrey
Dussutour. Amoeboid organism uses extracellular secretions to
make smart foraging decisions. Behavioral
Ecology, 2013, 24 (4), pp.812-818.
[3] Along with seven other organisms. in vitero was developed
at SymbioticA, an art-science research laboratory in the School of
Anatomy, Physiology and Human Biology at the University of
Western Australia and Perth Institute of Contemporary Art. This
project investigated the aesthetics of care, which are the aesthetic
experiences which develop through long term encounters with
other organisms.
[15] Reid, Chris. "The brainless slime mould that remembers where
it’s been." The Conversation, 6 October 2012.
[16] Adamatzky, Andrew. On attraction of slime mould Physarum
polycephalum to plants with sedative properties. Nature
Preceedings, 2011.
[4] By examination, I mean any process by which we attempt to
understand or make our way through the natureculture of our
world (consciously or not).
[17] Palmer, Jason. Slime mould prefers sedatives, say
researchers. BBC News, 10 June 2011, [Available
from: http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/scienceenvironment-13683377.
[5] By observation, I don’t just mean the visual outcome of a
conscious experiment, but any material or conceptual experience.
[6] Quantum is defined as “the minimum amount of a physical
quantity which can exist” (“quantum, n. 5,” OED online,
http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/155941)
[18] Tsuda, Soichiro, Klaus-Peter Zauner, and Yukio-Pegio Gunji.
Robot control with biological cells. Biosystems, 2007, 87 (2–3),
pp.215-223.
[7] The following equations are my attempt to understand the
relationships between phenomena, intra-actions, apparatuses
and agential cuts.
[19] Knight, Will. "Robot moved by a slime mould's fears." New
Scientist, 13 February 2006.
[8] In fact, for Barad, every intra-action is a moment of
inclusion/exclusion (refer to Meeting the Universe Halfway for an
in-depth discussion [2]).
[20] Biever, Celeste. "Robot face lets slime mould show its
emotional side." New Scientist, 8 August 2013.
[21] Posner, Jonathan, James A. Russell, and Bradley S. Peterson.
The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to
affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and
psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005, 17 (3),
pp.715-734.
[9] Myra Hird argues that, in fact, most phenomena do not
include the human [5]. It should be noted that Hird does not use
the term phenomena. She persists in the notion of “actants,”
which Barad categorically rejects as maintaining a subject/object
distinction. As this distinction cannot exist within Barad’s universe.
Hird does suggest that “actants” exist in “biophysical and
biosocial entanglements,” which is definitely a Baradian
notion [5, p.1]. Barad however, refers to the entanglement of
matter-discourse at a quantum level, which Hird does not. Rather,
Hird hinges entanglements on bacteria.
[22] Whybrow, Nicolas. Encountering the city: On 'not taking
yourself with you'. In Godela Weiss-Sussex and Franco Bianchini,
eds. Urban mindscapes of Europe, 97-109. Amsterdam & New
York: Rodopi, 2006, pp.97-109.
[23] Whiting, James G. H., Ben P. J. de Lacy Costello, and Andrew
Adamatzky. Towards slime mould chemical sensor: Mapping
chemical inputs onto electrical potential dynamics of physarum
polycephalum. Sensors and Actuators B:
Chemical, 2014, 191, pp.844-853.
[24] Haraway, Donna. When species meet.
University of Minneapolis Press, 2008.
[10] I use the neologism “is-means” here since material and
knowledge are inseparably entangled (see Barad’s “materialdiscursive” [2] and Donna Haraway’s “material-semiotic” [6]).
[11] These diagrams are my re-interpretations.
[12] OED (http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/122811)
Minneapolis:
[13] OED (http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/122808)
[14] OED (http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/122808)
65
[15] I am well aware of the post-structuralist and feminist legacy
here, but others, including Barad herself, eloquently discuss this
elsewhere.
[35] http://we-make-money-notart.com/archives/2013/10/morphs-mobile-reconfigurablep.php#.U0vHz1eEf7J
[16] OED (http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/122813)
[36] http://www.oliverk.org/art-projects/research/slime-moldoracle
[17] Research had occurred before this, but this article in
particular caught popular attention, possibly due to recent interest
in biological computing.
[37] For a discussion of the role of “step leaders” in the formation
of lightning see Barad [4], who takes up Vicky Kirby’s discussion.
[18] This subsequent research has been conducted by Andrew
Adamatzky’s group in the Department of Unconventional
Computing at the University of West England, Bristol.
[19] http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=uifP0FFmDyo]
[20] Refer to research conducted at the Department of
Unconventional Computing at the University of West England,
Bristol, the Research Institute for Electronic Science, Hokkaido
University, and the Behaviour and Genetics of Social Insects
Laboratory, University of Sydney
[21] See Supplementary Information [9] for an animated version
of a–c .
[22] http://www.southernbiological.com/specimens/livingspecimens-and-supplies/plants-and-physarum/l2-30-physarumslime-mouldlive/; http://file.southernbiological.com/Assets/Products/Specime
ns/Living_Specimens_and_Supplies/Plants_and_Fungi/L2_30Physarum_slime_mould_culture/CarolinaTipsPhysarum.pdf
[23] I also learned how to care for the other organisms, but
discussion of the care of these others is undertaken elsewhere.
[24] http://www.oliverk.org/art-projects/research/slime-moldoracle
[25] Although according to Nicolas Whybrow, this was not the
intention of Victor Gruen, the architect to whom this phenomenon
is attributed [22, p.97].
[26] http://www.abc.net.au/tv/gruentransfer/faq.htm
[27] http://www.oliverk.org/art-projects/research/slime-moldoracle
[28] http://theresaschubert.org/3D-SlimeMold.html
Tarsh Bates is an artist/researcher interested in how
meanings and experience are created and transferred
between materials, bodies, environment and culture. She
completed a Master of Science (Biological Arts) in 2012 which
explored care behaviours for scientific model organisms. She
has worked variously as a pizza delivery driver, a fruit and
vegetable stacker, a toilet paper packer, a researcher in
compost science and waste management, a honeybee
ejaculator, an art gallery invigilator, a raspberry picker, a
lecturer/tutor in art/science, art history and gender &
technology, an editor, a bookkeeper, a car detailer, and a life
drawing model. Tarsh is a candidate for a PhD at
SymbioticA, The University of Western Australia, where her
research is concerned with the aesthetics of interspecies
relationships and the human as a multispecies ecology. She
is particularly enamoured withCandida albicans.
[29] http://theresaschubert.org/bodymetries.html
[30] http://theresaschubert.org/bodymetries.html
[31] http://www.stanford.edu/~gailw/artdocs/slimetrace.html
[32] http://philross.org/#projects/leviathans/
[33] http://slimoco.ning.com/profiles/blogs/being-slime-mouldobservation-simulation-enactment
[34] http://beyondperformance.tumblr.com/
66