364
Review
14 Chen, F.C. and Li, W.H. (2001) Genomic
divergences between humans and other
hominoids and the effective population size of the
common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees.
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 68, 444–456
15 Ruvolo, M. (1997) Molecular phylogeny of the
hominoids: inferences from multiple independent
DNA sequence data sets. Mol. Biol. Evol. 14,
248–265
16 Bailey, W.J. et al. (1991) Molecular evolution of the
psi-eta-globin gene locus: gibbon phylogeny and
the hominoid slowdown. Mol. Biol. Evol. 8,
155–184
17 Horai, S. et al. (1992) Man’s place in Hominoidea
revealed by mitochondrial DNA genealogy. J. Mol.
Evol. 35, 32–43
18 Li, Y.J. et al. (1999) Paleo-demography of the
Drosophila melanogaster subgroup: application of
the maximum likelihood method. Genes Genet.
Syst. 74, 117–127
19 Hey, J. and Kliman, R.M. (1993) Population genetics
and phylogenetics of DNA-sequence variation at
multiple loci within the Drosophila melanogaster
species complex. Mol. Biol. Evol. 10, 804–822
20 Takahata, N. and Satta, Y. (1997) Evolution of the
primate lineage leading to modern humans:
phylogenetic and demographic inferences from
DNA sequences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 94,
4811–4815
21 Wright, S. (1969) Evolution and the Genetics of
Populations: the Theory of Gene Frequencies
(Vol. 2), University of Chicago Press
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
22 Wakeley, J. (1999) Nonequilibrium migration in
human history. Genetics 153, 1863–1871
23 Takahata, N. (1995) A genetic perspective on the
origin and history of humans. Annu. Rev. Ecol.
Syst. 26, 343–372
24 Slatkin, M. (1977) Gene flow and genetic drift in a
species subject to frequent local extinction. Theor.
Popul. Biol. 12, 253–262
25 Pannell, J.R. and Charlesworth, B. (1999) Neutral
genetic diversity in a metapopulation with
recurrent local extinction and recolonization.
Evolution 53, 664–676
26 Whitlock, M.C. and Barton, N.H. (1997) The
effective size of a subdivided population. Genetics
146, 427–441
27 Slatkin, M. (1991) Inbreeding coefficients and
coalescence time. Genet. Res. 58, 167–175
28 Barton, N.H. and Wilson, I. (1995) Genealogies
and geography. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London
Ser. B 349, 49–59
29 Berry, A.J. et al. (1991) Lack of polymorphism on
the Drosophila 4th chromosome resulting from
selection. Genetics 29, 1111–1117
30 Kreitman, M.E. and Aguade, M. (1986) Excess
polymorphism at the ADH locus in Drosophila
melanogaster. Genetics 114, 93–110
31 Hewitt, G.M. (1999) Post-glacial re-colonization of
European biota. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 68, 87–112
32 Ibrahim, K.M. et al. (1996) Spatial patterns of
genetic variation generated by different forms of
dispersal during range expansion. Heredity 77,
282–291
33 Taberlet, P. et al. (1998) Comparative
phylogeography and postglacial colonization
routes in Europe. Mol. Ecol. 7, 453–464
34 Hewitt, G.M. (2000) The genetic legacy of the
quaternary ice ages. Nature 405, 907–913
35 Rieseberg, L.H. and Noyes, R.D. (1998) Genetic
map-based studies of reticulate evolution in
plants. Trends Plant Sci. 3, 254–259
36 Song, K. et al. (1995) Rapid genome change in
synthetic polyploids of Brassica and its
implications for polyploid evolution. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 92, 7719–7723
37 Liu, B. et al. (1998) Rapid genomic changes in
newly synthesized amphiploids of Triticum and
Aegilops. I. Changes in low-copy noncoding DNA.
Genome 41, 272–277
38 Bromham, L. et al. (2000) The power of relative
rates tests depends on the data. J. Mol. Evol. 50,
296–301
39 Rand, D.M. (1994) Thermal habit, metabolic rate
and the evolution of mtDNA. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9,
125–131
40 Chao, L. and Carr, D.E. (1993) The
molecular clock and the relationship between
population-size and generation time. Evolution
47, 688–690
41 Gillespie, J.H. (1989) Lineage effects and the
index of dispersion of molecular evolution. Mol.
Biol. Evol. 6, 636–647
42 Li, W.H. et al. (1987) An evaluation of the
molecular clock hypothesis using mammalian
DNA-sequences. J. Mol. Evol. 25, 330–342
Sexual selection and speciation
Tami M . Panhuis, Roger Butlin, M arlene Zuk and Tom Tregenza
The power of sexual selection to drive changes in mate recognition traits gives
it the potential to be a potent force in speciation. M uch of the evidence to
support this possibility comes from comparative studies that examine
differences in the number of species between clades that apparently differ in
the intensity of sexual selection. We argue that more detailed studies are
needed, examining extinction rates and other sources of variation in species
richness. Typically, investigations of extant natural populations have been too
indirect to convincingly conclude speciation by sexual selection. Recent
empirical work, however, is beginning to take a more direct approach and rule
out confounding variables.
SEXUAL SELECTION (see Glossary) results from
Tami M . Panhuis*
M arlene Zuk
Dept of Biology,
University of California,
Riverside, CA 92521, USA.
* e-m ail:
panhuist@citrus.ucr.edu
Roger Butlin
Tom Tregenza
Centre for Biodiversity
and Conservation, School
of Biology, University of
Leeds, Leeds, UK LS2 9JT.
differential mating success among individuals within
a population. Competition for fertilization occurs
through direct competition between members of the
same sex (e.g. male–male competition and sperm
competition) or through the attraction of one sex to
the other (e.g. female choice). Although long
recognized as important in intrapopulation evolution,
sexual selection has more recently been invoked as a
driving force behind SPECIATION. Speciation, the
splitting of one SPECIES into two or more, occurs by
sexual selection when a parallel change in mate
preference and SECONDARY SEXUAL TRAITS within a
population leads to PREZYGOTIC ISOLATION between
populations, and when this is the primary cause of
REPRODUCTIVE ISOLATION.
Classic models of speciation1,2 recognized that
reproductive isolation, and subsequent speciation,
could be generated by differences in sexual traits
(including behaviours). Divergence in sexual traits
between allopatric populations was considered to
result either from drift, PLEIOTROPY or adaptation to
environmental conditions, or following SECONDARY
CONTACT, because individuals benefited by avoiding
heterospecific matings (i.e. by REINFORCEMENT). It
became clear, however, that changes between
populations in sexual traits could also result from
sexual selection and that this might represent a
distinct process of speciation3,4. Sexual selection has
the potential to lead to rapid divergence between
populations, it can be independent of environmental
differences, and it is predisposed to generate
reproductive isolation because of its direct effect on
traits involved in mate recognition. It is important to
point out that the rapid change between populations
http://tree.trends.com 0169–5347/01/$ – see front m atter © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02160-7
Review
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
365
Box 1. Sexual conflict and speciation
In species w here fem ales have m ore than
one m ate, each m ale is selected to
m axim ize the proportion of the
reproductive effort of a female going into
his offspring. This is in his interest even if it
decreases the lifetime reproductive output
of the female. A conflict between the sexes
is created as males attempt to both
manipulate females and to out-compete
other males (even if this is costly to the
female) and females evolve to avoid these
costs and controls. Such antagonistic
coevolution has the potential to drive rapid
evolutionary change, particularly in aspects
of the reproductive system, giving it the
potential to create divergence between
allopatric populationsa.
A mathematical model for the evolution
of reproductive barriers via sexual conflict
has recently been proposed b in which
conflicts of interest between the sexes over
mating rate are predicted to cause
divergence, because it pays females to
reduce the proportion of the male
population with whom they are
reproductively compatible. The model
demonstrates that, under some scenarios,
conflict can lead to runaway coevolution
driving divergence. Also, in contrast to
models where change occurs because of
GENETIC DRIFT (see Glossary), sexual conflict
generates direct selection on reproductive
traits: divergence is expected to be more
rapid when populations are larger.
The generality of this model has been
questioned on the basis that females will
only benefit from incompatibility with a
proportion of males if such
incompatibilities actually prevent matings.
If this is the case, males might be able to
as a result of sexual selection can also play an indirect
role in speciation by increasing the overall rate of
change within isolated populations5. Some have
argued that this indirect role might be more
important than a direct role for sexual selection in
speciation6.
Theoretical studies have convincingly
demonstrated the potential for speciation to result
from sexual selection (see Turelli et al.7, this issue).
Here, we focus on recent empirical evidence for
SPECIATION BY SEXUAL SELECTION. Much of this support
comes from comparative studies that examine
differences in the number of species between CLADES.
Patterns of variation in secondary sexual traits and
mating preferences between populations or species
also suggest that sexual selection plays a role in
speciation, but these ‘signatures’ have the drawback
that evolutionary processes cannot be tracked through
time and thus exclude alternative explanations.
We discuss the strengths and weaknesses in using
the comparative method for understanding the role of
sexual selection in speciation. We then consider why
the common ‘signatures’ that lend support for
speciation by sexual selection are not conclusive, and
discuss criteria for demonstrating the role of sexual
selection in speciation.
Comparative evidence
Important evidence showing how sexual selection
affects speciation comes from comparisons across
taxa. Darwin8 noted that elaborate secondary sexual
characters tended to occur in groups that also had high
species richness, suggesting that sexually selected
ornamentation might cause taxa to diversify, but
rigorous tests of this association have appeared only
recently. The first such test used 20 sister pairs of
http://tree.trends.com
redirect their mating effort, changing the
dynamics of the system c. An alternative
approach to conflicts of interest has been
suggested d wherein species in which males
invest very little in matings might be less
prone to speciation, as there will be less
selection on males to avoid outbreeding.
References
a Rice, W.R. (1998) Intergenomic conflict,
interlocus antagonistic co-evolution, and the
evolution of reproductive isolation. In Endless
Forms: Species and Speciation (Howard, D.J. and
Berlocher, S.H., eds), pp. 261–270, Oxford
University Press
b Gavrilets, S. (2000) Rapid evolution of
reproductive barriers driven by sexual conflict.
Nature 403, 886–889
c Tregenza, T. et al. (2000) Sexual conflict and
speciation. Nature 407, 149–150
d Parker, G.A. and Partridge, L. (1998) Sexual
conflict and speciation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
London Ser. B 353, 261–274
passerine bird tribes that differ in their proportions of
sexually dichromatic and monochromatic species9.
Sister taxa have, by definition, accumulated diversity
over the same period of time. Therefore, the tribe with
the greater number of extant species necessarily has
the higher net speciation rate (rate of speciation minus
the rate of extinction). If sexual selection promotes
speciation, diversity is expected to be higher in clades
with more dichromatic species, assuming that sexual
differences in plumage result from sexual selection. Of
15 comparisons with marked differences in the
frequency of dichromatism, 12 showed differences in
species richness in the expected direction9.
Similar comparative studies have since supported
speciation by sexual selection: in birds, by comparing
the number of species in taxa with different mating
systems10, and different degrees of feather
ornamentation11; and in plants, by comparing lengths
of nectar spurs12. More ornamented species have
more subspecies11, suggesting ongoing
differentiation.
These studies concentrate on the contribution of
sexual selection to speciation through divergence in
mating signals and preferences. Comparative
evidence also suggests, however, that postmating
effects promote speciation and that sexual selection
influences the evolution of POSTZYGOTIC ISOLATION. If a
female mates more than once, the opportunity for
sexually antagonistic selection arises (Box 1). If such
selection has a role in speciation, MONANDROUS clades
should show lower speciation rates than would related
POLYANDROUS clades. A strong pattern of this type was
recently documented using data from 25 pairwise
comparisons in five orders of insects13. Another
comparative analysis14 demonstrated that genital
morphology evolves more rapidly in polyandrous
366
Review
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
Box 2. Haldane’s rule and speciation
In pairs of anim al species w here prem ating
barriers can be overcom e and som e hybrid
offspring generated, it is com m on to find
that one sex is inviable or infertile. In 1922,
J.B.S. Haldane noted a striking pattern in
these crosses: the sex that is inviable or
sterile is alm ost invariably the
heterogam etic (XY) sex. This is one of the
strongest ‘rules’ in evolutionary biology. It
is obeyed in 151 out of 157 crosses in
Drosophila and m am m als (m ale
heterogam etic) and 91 out of 98 crosses in
Lepidoptera and birds (fem ale
heterogam etic)a. Exam ining the tim e
course of speciation in Drosophila
suggests that Haldane’s rule is a nearly
obligatory ‘first step in the evolution of
postzygotic isolation’ a .
How does Haldane’s rule im plicate
sexual selection in speciation? There are
several com peting explanations for the
pattern a, one of w hich, the ‘faster-m ale’
theory b , invokes sexual selection. The
argum ent is that m ale sterility m ight be
m ore com m on than fem ale sterility in
hybrids either because sperm atogenesis is
inherently m ore sensitive than oogenesis,
or because sexual selection and sexual
conflict cause m ore rapid divergence of
genes expressed in m ales. Divergent loci
interact poorly in hybrids, leading to
sterility. If sperm atogenesis is a sensitive
process, this is presum ably because sexual
selection or sexual conflict favours a high
rate of sperm production.
Clearly, this cannot be the only
mechanism underlying Haldane’s rule
because it is unlikely to explain
asymmetrical inviability and because it
works against the rule in taxa with
heterogametic females. However, evidence
that it contributes to sterility in taxa with XY
males would demonstrate a role for sexual
selection in the origin of postzygotic
isolation. The faster-male theory is now
supported by two lines of evidence. First,
chromosome regions introgressed from
one species of Drosophila into another are
more likely to contain male-sterile than
female-sterile genesc,d. This fits the
expectation of more rapid divergence at
loci expressed only in males. Second, the
faster-male theory is not dependent on the
Y chromosome being inert and thus
insect clades, implicating postmating sexual selection
in the rapid evolutionary divergence of genitalia. If
polyandry generates greater opportunity for sexual
selection as well as for sexual conflict, the causal link
to speciation remains unclear. Sex-related genes
(i.e. those involved in sex determination or any aspect
of mating and fertilization) show higher
nonsynonymous:synonymous nucleotide substitution
ratios between closely related species than do other
classes of genes15. This suggests divergence resulting
from sexual selection around the time of speciation.
Finally, the evidence explaining HALDANE’S RULE by
faster male evolution (Box 2) strongly suggests that
sexual selection can promote genetic divergence
underlying hybrid male sterility.
Counting species is not enough
Comparisons based on the numbers of species in
different groups are not without problems. If
taxonomists use secondary sexual traits in species
definitions, the numbers of named species could be
upwardly biased in dimorphic taxa. Comparative
studies also tend to assume that dimorphic traits
have diverged by sexual selection, often with limited
direct evidence. At best, such studies demonstrate a
correlation between speciation and sexual selection
(across taxa or in time) rather than a causative link.
Comparative analyses must deal with the problem of
confounding variables: sexual dichromatism, for
http://tree.trends.com
recessive loci on the X chromosome being
expressed in hybrids, which is the basis of
the other major explanation for the rule.
Taxa with non-degenerate Y chromosomes
are, therefore, expected to follow Haldane’s
rule for sterility but not for inviability. This is
exactly the pattern observed in a
comparison between two mosquito
genera: Aedes, where the Y chromosome is
homologous to the X, and Anopheles,
where the Y is inert e.
References
a Orr, H.A. (1997) Haldane’s rule. Annu. Rev. Ecol.
Syst. 28, 195–218
b Wu, C-I. and Davis, A.W. (1993) Evolution of
postmating reproductive isolation: the composite
nature of Haldane’s rule and its genetic bases.
Am. Nat. 142, 187–212
c True, J.R. et al. (1996) A genome-wide survey of
hybrid incompatibility factors by introgression of
marked segments of Drosophila mauritiana
chromosomes into Drosophila simulans.
Genetics 142, 819–837
d Hollocher, H. and Wu, C-I. (1996) The genetics of
reproductive isolation in the Drosophila
simulans clade: X vs. autosomal effects and male
vs. female effects. Genetics 143, 1243–1255
e Presgraves, D.C. and Orr, H.A. (1998) Haldane’s
rule in taxa lacking a hemizygous X. Science 282,
952–954
example, could be correlated with some other variable
that actually causes elevated speciation rates. The
only safeguard against this possibility is to control for
as many such variables as possible. A particularly
thorough analysis16 attempted to minimize the
problem by simultaneous analysis of six different
explanations for variation in species richness among
bird families: chance (i.e. by fitting models of random
CLADOGENESIS), body size, life history, sexual selection,
intrinsic ecological factors and extrinsic abiotic
factors. Using comparisons of species numbers in 28
unequivocal sister taxa, strong support was found for
an association with sexual dichromatism but not with
mating system. Generalist feeding habits, high
dispersal and large, fragmented geographical ranges
are also associated with high diversity.
Despite the limitations of comparisons based on
species counts, comparative studies do suggest that
sexual selection can accelerate the net rate of
speciation. Divergence in secondary sexual characters
might, however, generate reproductively isolated, but
ecologically equivalent species unable to coexist in
SYMPATRY. Some evidence suggests that the processes of
extinction and speciation in birds are determined by
different attributes16 and might, therefore, be
uncorrelated. If this is true, it becomes difficult to
interpret what net speciation rates tell us about the
speciation process. In the insect study mentioned
previously13, elevation in net speciation rate is much
Review
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
367
mating signals and preferences; these differences will
be the major barrier to gene exchange and the species
will differ little in other traits. If sexual selection is a
common cause of speciation, these patterns should be
encountered frequently.
Signatures from population differences
Fig. 1. Exam ples of
sexual selection. (a) A
m ale from the centre of a
hybrid zone betw een
subspecies of the
m eadow grasshopper
Chorthippus parallelus. A
steep cline in fem ale
preference across the
zone indicates that
preference is under
selection 24. (b) A m ating
pair from a laboratory
m ate choice test in the
m eadow grasshopper.
There is sufficient
divergence betw een
allopatric populations of
this species in Europe to
generate significant
assortative m ating. In
som e cases, this
divergence m ust have
been accum ulated since
the end of the last
glaciation 23. Sexual
selection m ight explain
such rapid evolution.
greater in comparisons within than between families
(relative rate 3.3 and 1.1, respectively), suggesting only
a short-term change in the balance between speciation
and extinction. This implies that comparative analyses
should focus on recently diverged sister taxa where the
signal of elevated speciation rate will be strongest.
Although sexual selection might promote speciation,
ecological processes are more important in
determining the numbers of species that can coexist
and, over longer time scales, might obscure the effect of
increased speciation rate.
Even if the comparative evidence does point to a
role for sexual selection in speciation, it does not
directly distinguish between acceleration of
divergence in ALLOPATRY, initiation of divergence in
sympatry, or involvement in reinforcement. Some
attempts have been made to address this problem.
Further analysis17 of earlier data9, focusing on the
number of species of each taxon living together rather
than the total species richness, could not show that
more diverse, sexually dichromatic taxa have greater
numbers of species in sympatry than do the less
diverse, monochromatic taxa. This suggests that
sexual selection promotes allopatric divergence
rather than promoting SYMPATRIC SPECIATION and that
the allopatric species are ecological equivalents,
prevented from expanding their ranges into sympatry
by competition. In flocks of cichlid fish species,
however, frequent sympatric occurrence of different
colour morphs suggests that sexual selection can
promote sympatric speciation despite a lack of
ecological separation18.
Are there conclusive signatures of speciation by sexual
selection?
Via19 (this issue) refers to patterns that provide
indirect evidence of sympatric speciation as
‘signatures’. Two signatures might be recognized for
speciation by sexual selection: (1) within species,
populations will vary in sexually selected traits and
associated preferences and this might generate
partial premating isolation between populations; and
(2) closely related species will differ markedly in
http://tree.trends.com
There are many examples of variation among
populations in potentially sexually selected traits20,21.
In some cases, the variable traits can be shown to be
under sexual selection within populations
(e.g. guppies; Table 1), but, even in these cases, the
cause of divergence among populations is usually
unclear. In other examples22,23, partial premating
isolation has been demonstrated among populations;
however, the signal traits involved are unknown.
Variation in female preference has only been studied
rarely, using either mating tests (e.g. grasshoppers24 :
Fig. 1) or playback experiments (e.g. lacewings25).
Table 1 illustrates the extent to which some recent
studies of geographical variation in sexually selected
traits have covered these aspects. Clearly, the
signature is strongest if all components are included,
but this has yet to be achieved.
It is possible that divergence between populations in
sexually selected traits and/or preferences could result
in speciation. Speciation, however, is just one of several
evolutionary outcomes of the sexually selected
differences. Upon secondary contact, the partial barrier
to gene flow between populations might diminish if
postmating isolation is absent and if the differences in
behaviour are insufficient to prevent hybridization. For
example, isolation could be asymmetrical (i.e. only one
population exhibits female preference for males of their
own population) as in the case of Drosophila
melanogaster populations from Zimbabwe (Table 1).
These populations exhibit behavioural premating
isolation from other non-African populations, but this is
the result of asymmetrical female preference, and
postmating isolation has not been detected26. This
suggests that populations would interbreed in
sympatry and differentiation would be lost. This type of
breakdown of signal differences and ASSORTATIVE MATING
is seen in some hybrid zones (e.g. fire-bellied toads,
Bombina27 ), although natural and sexual selection may
restrict hybridization to a narrow region.
The evolutionary outcome of population
differences in sexually selected traits and preferences
could also depend on whether there is a mismatch
between the male trait and the female preference. A
mismatch could result if a population changes its
male display trait (possibly as a result of
environmental pressures), but not in female
preference. A study28 on female preferences in the
house finch Carpodacus mexicanus illustrates this
phenomenon. Geographical variation in male
plumage colouration and the amount of ventral
pigmentation (patch size) is driven by environmental
differences in regional and local access to carotenoid
plumage pigments. Females, independently of their
Review
368
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
Table 1. Examples of detecting signatures of speciation by sexual selectiona–c
Taxa
Exampled
Insects Drosophila m elanogaster
D. silvestris
Chrysoperla spp.
Chorthippus parallelus
Ephippiger ephippiger
Gryllus texensis and
G. rubens
Agrotis segetum
Type of trait
Trait under Preference
sexual
variation
selectione
Prezygotic
reproductive
isolatione,f
Character
Refs
displacement
Zim babw e fem ales prefer
Zim babw e m ales over
non-Zim babw e m ales
Asym m etrical fem ale preference
am ong different Israel populations
Sex com b row num ber
Substrate-transm itted acoustic
signals
Unknow n
Yes
Asym m etrical
Unlikely
Unknow n
Yes
Asym m etrical
Unlikely
Yes
Unknow n
Asym m etrical
Yes
Unlikely
Unlikely
54
25
Unknow n
Yes
Yes, betw een
species and
‘song m orphs’
Yes
No
23
Unknow n
Yes
Yes, in som e
population pairs
Probably
Unlikely
55
Yes
Yes, betw een
species
Probably w eak
No
39
Unknow n
Yes, betw een
species
Yes
Unlikely
56
M ale song, pherom ones and
assortative m ating
Fem ale preference functions and
m ale calling song (num ber of
syllables/chirp)
Pulse rate of m ale song and
associated fem ale preference
Pherom one blend and m ale response
26,52,53
Frogs
Acris crepitans
M ale advertisem ent calls
(independent of body size) and
fem ale preference for m ale calls
Unknow n
Yes
Probably
No
57–59
Fish
Poecilia reticulata
M ale colour pattern and fem ale
preference for m ales from their
ow n locality
Sexually dim orphic colouration
and associated preferences
Yes
Yes
Little
Unlikely
60
Yes
Yes, betw een
Yes, betw een
species/m orphs species/m orphs
Possible
18
M ale strut displays, body size and
feather m orphology differing in
the Gunnison Basin, CO (USA)
population (recently given species
status) com pared w ith Jackson
County, CO and M ono County, CA,
(USA) populations
M ale display traits, such as bow er
structures, and fem ale preferences
in at least one population
M ale song betw een tw o sym patric
subspecies that do not interbreed,
yet are connected by a ring of
populations that do interbreed
Yes
Unknow n
Unknow n
Unlikely
61,62
Yes
Probably
Yes
Unknow n
Probably, but
Unlikely
m ight be
asym m etric
Only dem onstrated No
betw een
subspecies
Haplochrom ine cichlids
of Lake Victoria
Birds
Centrocercus
urophasianus
Am blyornis inornatus
Phylloscopus
trochiloides
45
49, 50
aThis
is not a com plete list of exam ples, but is intended only as an illustration of the phenom enon. Also see Refs 5,6,35.
exam ples included have low genetic divergence as judged by allozym e, DNA or m itochondrial DNA (m tDNA) sequence divergence, or taxom onic status.
Phylloscopus50 ssp. and Centrocercus populations63 show m tDNA divergence betw een subspecies/species. A Zim babw ean D. m elanogaster population show s greater
population structure com pared w ith North Am erican populations on the basis of DNA sequence divergence64.
cSom e degree of postzygotic isolation has been found betw een Chrysoperla25 spp. and Chorthippus23 populations. Studies have not dem onstrated postzygotic isolation
betw een Zim babw e and ‘cosm opolitan’ populations of D. m elanogaster 26 or betw een Gryllus texensis and G. rubens39. Postzygotic isolation in the cichlids is inferred to be
low. To the best of our know ledge, postzygotic isolation data are not available for the other exam ples in the table.
d Com m on nam es: Acris creptians, Cricket frogs; Agrotis segetum , Turnip m oths; Am blyornis inornatus, Vogelkop Bow erbirds; Centrocercus urophasianus, Sage grouse;
Chorthippus parallelus, Grasshoppers; Chrysoperla spp., Lacew ings; Drosophila m elanogaster and D. silvestris, Fruit flies; Ephippiger ephippiger , Bush crickets; Gryllus
texensis and G. rubens, Crickets; Poecilia reticulata, Guppies; Phylloscopus trochiloides, Greenish Warblers.
eUnknow n – to the best of our know ledge, data are not available.
f Not necessarily com plete reproductive isolation.
b M ost
population, prefer the ancestral male type with the
brightest and largest patches. Upon subsequent
contact, this mismatch between the male trait and
the female preference would probably lead to
interbreeding between populations, rather than
prezygotic isolation.
Mismatches are also associated with the sensory
bias and sensory exploitation model of sexual
selection29–31. This model proposes that females respond
http://tree.trends.com
to certain signals because of pre-existing properties of
their sensory system30,32 and hence male traits evolve
after female preferences33. This phenomenon has been
demonstrated in several taxa (e.g. fish31, frogs33 and
spiders34)35 by revealing that females of closely related
species prefer males with the heterospecific derived
male trait rather than conspecific males without the
trait. Population divergence in male traits might or
might not occur, depending on the environmental or
Review
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
developmental constraints on the rapidity with which
the male trait could evolve to exploit the female
preference. There is also evidence that female sensory
biases are rather generalized and can be satisfied by a
variety of specific male traits32,36. Therefore, different
male traits might evolve in response to the same
sensory bias leading to divergence in signal traits
without increasing isolation. This model of sexual
selection seems unlikely to lead directly to speciation
given the fact that females, all else being equal, will
tend to interbreed with males that exploit their sensory
bias, independent of their population. However, it could
exaggerate already established species differences32,35.
Alternatively, it could be that local environmental
conditions alter the female sensory bias or make some
male traits better at exploiting biases, just as specific
traits might be better indicators of male condition in
some environments than others37. In this case, different
coevolutionary paths of signal and preference might
generate speciation.
A different perspective on mismatches between
traits and preferences suggests that they might
provide good evidence for the role of sexual selection
in speciation. If female preferences evolve for ‘good
genes’ or Fisherian reasons, they are expected to be
open-ended, or at least to have mean preferences for
trait values above the male population mean. This is
because the sexual advantage of the trait is balanced
by its cost to the male. Therefore, if two populations
differ in the mean value of the male trait, they are
likely to have even greater differences in female
preference. This can be contrasted with the
expectation for signals and preferences that have
evolved for species recognition, where a match
between male trait mean and female preference mean
would be expected38. The mismatch between signal
and preference can generate asymmetric isolation
between populations with intermediate levels of
divergence: this pattern has been observed in tests of
mate choice between populations of Desmognathus
salamanders22. However, this model does not explain
the pattern of assortative mating observed among
Chorthippus grasshopper populations23.
Signatures from species differences
Because we cannot see into the future, observations
on divergent populations can only ever show that
steps towards speciation might be promoted by sexual
selection: how often the process is completed, and by
what route, remains uncertain. Studying species
differences suffers from the opposite problem: the
difficulty of inferring the past. Many morphologically
and genetically similar species differ markedly in
mating signals and preferences. It might be inferred
from this that sexual selection has driven speciation,
but to do so requires that other modes of divergence be
excluded.
The possibility that species recognition has been
an important force in shaping the changes seen
between male traits used to attract mates or female
http://tree.trends.com
369
preferences can be difficult to exclude. Species
recognition is important when interpopulation or
heterospecific mating is costly, producing relatively
inviable or infertile offspring. Traits facilitating
species recognition might then diverge under natural
selection when populations or closely related species
have overlapping geographical ranges2. Thus,
reinforcement or REPRODUCTIVE CHARACTER
DISPLACEMENT (RCD) could drive the change in male
traits and female preferences, eliminating a direct
role of sexual selection39. However, it has been
proposed33,40 that sexual selection and species
recognition are part of a continuum and that sexual
selection might facilitate reinforcement41. Several
studies have shown (e.g. butterflies42; fish35 and
frogs30) that male display traits can be both sexually
selected and used in species recognition. Contrary
results have been found in two sibling species of
Hawaiian Drosophila, where the sexually selected
trait is not used for species recognition40.
Evidence favouring speciation by sexual selection
rather than reinforcement or RCD could demonstrate
that: (1) the trait in question does not differ
significantly in areas of sympatry versus allopatry (the
opposite trend is a distinguishing feature of character
displacement39); (2) there is trivial or no postzygotic
isolation (i.e. the fitness reduction required to drive
reinforcement or RCD is absent); and (3) the trait used
in mate acquisition is used both in sexual selection and
species recognition. However, it is possible that the
initial divergence in traits or preferences could have
been the result of natural selection to decrease
hybridization and then been subject to sexual selection,
or vice versa33, making a clean separation difficult.
An analysis of data on 41 species of haplochromine
cichlids from Lake Victoria showed that species pairs
with a substantial range overlap were more likely to
differ in sexually selected colouration than were pairs
with little range overlap18. This is the pattern
expected from reinforcement or RCD, but, in this case,
there is little evidence for hybrid fitness reduction and
the extremely short time scale for speciation in the
lake makes the two stage process of allopatric
divergence followed by range expansion and
divergence in secondary sympatry improbable. The
authors favoured a model of speciation as a result of
disruptive sexual selection in sympatry (see Turelli
et al.7, this issue).
What should new studies aim to demonstrate?
It is improbable that any single test will separate
speciation by sexual selection from other modes
of speciation. This is a problem common to all
speciation research and stems from our inability to
observe the whole process, forcing us either to infer
the most probable future course of events or to
separate different possible histories. Therefore,
recent studies have aimed to overcome these
difficulties by accumulating observations consistent
with speciation by sexual selection and by trying to
370
Acknow ledgements
We thank A. Uy for
com m ents on this article
and access to unpublished
m aterial.
Review
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
rule out alternative scenarios. These studies have
suggested the following major components:
• Substantial differences occur among populations or
species in male sexually selected traits with
correlated female preferences43–45.
• Changes are sufficient to result in prezygotic
isolation should populations come into contact39.
• There is little genetic differentiation between
populations or species, suggesting rapid divergence43–45.
• There is little or no reduction in hybrid viability or
fertility, although hybrid fitness might be reduced
by inappropriate signals or preferences.
• If applicable, there is no character displacement39.
Without the first element, there is no case for
speciation by sexual selection. As noted earlier, sexual
selection should be demonstrated directly from the
effect of variation in the trait on mating success rather
than simply being inferred from elaboration or sexual
dimorphism. The second point is required because
divergence under sexual selection does not necessarily
result in a substantial barrier to gene exchange. The
catch here is to demonstrate that prezygotic isolation is
the direct result of the changes in sexually selected
traits and not a result of other evolutionary forces
(e.g. drift or natural selection) or evolutionary history.
This might be difficult, but could be achieved through
comparative studies using a phylogenetic approach to
help separate independent evolutionary origins from
historical effects resulting from common ancestry35 (see
Barraclough and Nee46, this issue). Genetic ‘dissection’
of the mating traits important in prezygotic isolation
has also been proposed47. This consists of hybridization
studies between closely related species to analyse the
genetic and phenotypic difference in male traits and
female preferences. The third component might be
necessary to help rule out pleiotropic effects of genetic
divergence45. INCIPIENT SPECIES or sibling species could be
useful systems to study speciation by sexual selection
because they will, by definition, have experienced
relatively little genetic divergence. Lack of postzygotic
isolation is important for the same reason, as well as
helping to exclude the possibility that prezygotic
isolation has arisen secondarily rather than being the
initial cause of speciation. The final component is
important for studies of closely related species that
overlap in their geographical range. By ruling out
character displacement, a stronger case is made for the
role of sexual selection in the speciation event39.
These criteria are not applicable to all systems, but
the objective should be to cover as many as possible.
Some of the best examples from nature that suggest
that sexual selection could be involved in the
speciation process are listed in Table 1, but it is clear
that each one of them falls short of being conclusive.
The studies on cichlids suggest that sexual selection
could be important in generating sympatric
speciation18,48. Recent studies49,50 on greenish
warblers (Phylloscopus trochiloides) imply that
changes in the intensity of sexual selection on song
are important in generating prezygotic isolation
http://tree.trends.com
between two sympatric subspecies forming the ends
of a ring species. Character displacement is
improbable because the degree of song variation
between allopatric and sympatric populations is
small within both subspecies. Work on two cryptic
species of field crickets (Gryllus texensis and
G. rubens) demonstrates that: (1) a change between
species in male calling pulse rate is genetically
correlated with a change in female preference; (2) the
difference in male traits and female preferences leads
to prezygotic isolation; (3) males are morphologically
almost identical (only ovipositor length differs
between females); (4) signs of postzygotic isolation are
lacking; and (5) there is no evidence for character
displacement39. However, even this last study was
only able to conclude that the data are ‘substantially
more consistent with speciation by sexual selection
than they are with the alternatives’ of speciation by
reinforcement or by ecological differentiation.
Even when incipient species are described,
speciation might not be completed by sexual selection.
For example, in the Vogelkop bowerbirds (Amblyornis
inornatus; Table 1) males from allopatric populations
are morphologically and genetically very similar and
only differ in their display traits; however, it is not
known whether the female preference is symmetrical
and whether it will result in prezygotic isolation45.
Some have also argued that rapid radiation in the
Hawaiian Drosophila can be explained by other
evolutionary forces, such as drift followed by natural
selection51, or that the sexually selected traits are not
involved in species recognition40. Thus, sexual selection
might not be as important in the origin of some of the
Hawaiian Drosophila species pairs as it first seemed.
Prospects
There is no doubt that sexual selection has the potential
to play a major role in speciation. Models of both
allopatric and sympatric populations indicate that
sexual selection has the power to drive rapid divergence
and generate reproductive isolation (see Turelli et al.7,
this issue). The evidence suggesting that theory
translates to reality comes from comparative studies
that repeatedly indicate higher species diversity in more
sexually selected clades and from observations of
patterns of divergence between populations and species
that are consistent with the expectations of speciation by
sexual selection. Further comparative studies are
needed, examining extinction rates as well as numbers
of extant species, confirming the role of sexual selection
in the evolution of the traits used as indicators (such as
plumage dimorphism in birds) and quantifying
ecological and other sources of variation in species
richness. Empirical work has taken a more direct
approach to demonstrating speciation via sexual
selection, but more examples are needed in which all
elements of the sexual selection model have been
demonstrated and that eliminate, so far as is possible,
alternative factors such as character displacement and
pleiotropic effects of divergence.
Review
References
1 Mayr, E. (1942) Systematics and the Origin of
Species, Belknap Press
2 Dobzhansky, T. (1937) Genetics and the Origin of
Species, Columbia University Press
3 West-Eberhard, M. (1983) Sexual selection, social
competition, and speciation. Q. Rev. Biol. 58,
155–183
4 Coyne, J.A. (1992) Genetics and speciation. Nature
355, 511–515
5 Ligon, J.D. (1999) The Evolution of Avian Breeding
Systems, Oxford University Press
6 Questiau, S. (1999) How can sexual selection
promote population divergence? Ethol. Ecol. Evol.
11, 313–324
7 Turelli, M. et al. (2001) Theory and speciation.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 330–343
8 Darwin, C. (1871) The Descent of Man and
Selection in Relation to Sex, John Murray
9 Barraclough, T.G. et al. (1995) Sexual selection
and taxonomic diversity in passerine birds. Proc.
R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci. 259, 211–215
10 Mitra, S. et al. (1996) Species richness
covaries with mating system in birds. Auk 113,
544–551
11 Møller, A.P. and Cuervo, J.J. (1998) Speciation and
feather ornamentation in birds. Evolution 52,
859–869
12 Hodges, S.A. and Arnold, M.L. (1995) Spurring
plant diversification: are floral nectar spurs a key
innovation? Proc. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci. 262,
343–348
13 Arnqvist, G. et al. (2000) Sexual conflict promotes
speciation in insects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
97, 10460–10464
14 Arnqvist, G. (1998) Comparative evidence for the
evolution of genitalia by sexual selection. Nature
393, 784–786
15 Civetta, A. and Singh, R.S. (1998) Sex-related
genes, directional sexual selection, and speciation.
Mol. Biol. Evol. 15, 901–909
16 Owens, I.P.F. et al. (1999) Species richness among
birds: body size, life history, sexual selection or
ecology? Proc. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci. 266,
933–939
17 Price, T. (1998) Sexual selection and natural
selection in bird speciation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
London Ser. B 353, 251–260
18 Seehausen, O. and van Alphen, J.M. (1999)
Can sympatric speciation by disruptive
sexual selection explain rapid evolution of
cichlid diversity in Lake Victoria? Ecol. Lett. 2,
262–271
19 Via, S. (2001) Sympatric speciation in animals: the
ugly duckling grows up. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16,
381–390
20 Butlin, R.K. (1995) Genetic variation in mating
signals and responses. In Speciation and the
Recognition Concept: Theory and Applications
(Lambert, D.M. and Spencer, H.G., eds),
pp. 327–366, Johns Hopkins University Press
21 Bakker, T.C.M. and Pomiankowski, A. (1995) The
genetic basis of female mate preferences. J. Evol.
Biol. 8, 129–171
22 Arnold, S.J. et al. (1996) The evolution of
asymmetry in sexual isolation: a model and a test
case. Evolution 50, 1024–1033
23 Tregenza, T. et al. (2000) The origins of premating
reproductive isolation: testing hypotheses in the
grasshopper Chorthippus parallelus. Evolution
54, 1687–1698
24 Butlin, R.K. and Ritchie, M.G. (1991) Variation in
http://tree.trends.com
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.16 No.7 July 2001
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
female mate preference across a grasshopper
hybrid zone. J. Evol. Biol. 4, 227–240
Wells, M.M. and Henry, C.S. (1998) Songs,
reproductive isolation, and speciation in cryptic
species of insects. In Endless Forms: Species and
Speciation (Howard, D.J. and Berlocher, S.H.,
eds), pp. 217–233, Oxford University Press
Wu, C-I. et al. (1995) Sexual isolation in
Drosophila melanogaster: a possible case of
incipient speciation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
92, 2519–2523
Sanderson, N. et al. (1992) Variation in mating
call across the hybrid zone between the firebellied toads Bombina bombina and Bombina
variegata. Evolution 46, 595–607
Hill, G.E. (1994) Geographic variation in male
ornamentation and female mate preference in the
house finch: a comparative test of models of sexual
selection. Behav. Ecol. 5, 63–73
Endler, J.A. and McLellan, T. (1988) The process
of evolution: towards a newer synthesis. Annu.
Rev. Ecol. Syst. 19, 385–421
Ryan, M.J. (1990) Signals, species, and sexual
selection. Am. Sci. 78, 46–52
Basolo, A.L. (1990) Female preference predates
the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish.
Science 250, 808–810
Kime, N.M. et al. (1998) Consistency of female
choice in the tungara frog: a permissive
preference for complex characters. Anim. Behav.
55, 641–649
Ryan, M.J. and Rand, A.S. (1993) Species
recognition and sexual selection as a unitary
problem in animal communication. Evolution 47,
647–657
McClintock, W.J. and Uetz, G.W. (1996) Female
choice and pre-existing bias: visual cues during
courtship in two Schizocosa wolf spiders
(Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim. Behav. 52, 167–181
Ptacek, M.B. (2000) The role of mating
preferences in shaping interspecific divergence in
mating signals in vertebrates. Behav. Processes
51, 111–134
Ryan, M.J. and Rand, A.S. (1993) Sexual selection
and signal evolution – the ghost of biases past.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 340, 187–195
Schluter, D. and Price, T. (1993) Honesty,
perception and population divergence in sexually
selected traits. Proc. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci.
253, 117–122
Butlin, R.K. (1996) Co-ordination of the sexual
signaling system and the genetic basis of
differentiation between populations in the brown
planthopper, Nilaparvata lugens. Heredity 77,
369–377
Gray, D.A. and Cade, W.H. (2000) Sexual selection
and speciation in field crickets. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. U. S. A. 97, 14449–14454
Boake, C.R.B. et al. (1997) Is sexual selection and
species recognition a continuum? Mating
behavior of the stalk-eyed fly Drosophila
heteroneura. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 94,
12442–12445
Liou, L.W. and Price, T.D. (1994) Speciation by
reinforcement of premating isolation. Evolution
48, 1451–1459
Wiernasz, D.C. and Kingsolver, J.G. (1992) Wing
melanin pattern mediates species recognition in
Pieris occidentalis. Anim. Behav. 43, 89–94
Kaneshiro, K.Y. (1988) Speciation in the
Hawaiian Drosophila. BioScience 38, 258–263
Meyer, A. (1993) Phylogenetic relationships
371
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
and evolutionary processes in East African
cichlid fishes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 8,
279–284
Uy, J.A.C. and Borgia, G. (2000) Sexual selection
drives rapid divergence in bowerbird display
traits. Evolution 54, 273–278
Barraclough, T.G. and Nee, S. (2001)
Phylogenetics and speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol.
16, 391–399
Shaw, K.L. (2000) Specific genetics of mate
recognition: inheritance of female acoustic
preference in Hawaiian crickets. Evolution 54,
1303–1312
Galis, F. and Metz, A.J. (1998) Why are there so
many cichlid species? Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 1–2
Irwin, D.E. (2000) Song variation in an avian ring
species. Evolution 54, 998–1010
Irwin, D.E. et al. (2001) Speciation in a ring.
Nature 409, 333–337
Templeton, A.R. (1979) Once again, why 300
species of Hawaiian Drosophila? Evolution 33,
513–517
Hollocher, H. et al. (1997) Incipient speciation by
sexual isolation in Drosophila melanogaster:
variation in mating preference and correlation
between sexes. Evolution 51, 1175–1181
Korol, A. et al. (2000) Nonrandom mating in
Drosophila melanogaster laboratory populations
derived from closely adjacent ecologically
contrasting slopes at ‘Evolution Canyon’. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 12637–12642
Carson, H.L. (1985) Genetic variation in a
courtship related male character in Drosophila
silvestris from a single Hawaiian locality.
Evolution 36, 678–686
Ritchie, M.G. (1996) The shape of female mating
preferences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 93,
14628–14631
LaForest, S. et al. (1997) A genetic analysis of
population differences in pheromone production
and response between two populations of the
turnip moth, Agrotis segetum. J. Chem. Ecol. 23,
1487–1503
Ryan, M.J. and Wilczynski, W. (1988) Coevolution
of sender and receiver: effects on local mate
preference in cricket frogs. Science 240, 1786–1788
Ryan, M.J. and Wilczynski, W. (1991) Evolution of
intraspecific variation in the advertisement call of
a cricket frog (Acris crepitans, Hylidae). Biol. J.
Linn. Soc. 44, 249–271
Ryan, M.J. et al. (1992) Auditory tuning and call
frequency predict population-based mating
preferences in the cricket frog, Acris creptians. Am.
Nat. 139, 1370–1383
Endler, J.A. and Houde, A.E. (1995) Geographic
variation in female preferences for male traits in
Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 49, 456–468
Young, J.R. et al. (1994) Phenotypic divergence of
secondary sexual traits among sage grouse,
Centrocercus urophasianus, populations. Anim.
Behav. 47, 1353–1362
Young, J.R. et al. (2000) A new species of Sage
Grouse (Phasianidae: Centrocercus) from
Southwestern Colorado. Wilson Bull. 112, 445
Kahn, N.W. et al. (1999) Molecular analysis of
genetic variation among large- and small-bodied
Sage Grouse using mitochondrial control-region
sequences. Auk 116, 819–824
Begun, D.J. and Aquadro, C.F. (1993) African and
North American populations of Drosophila
melanogaster are very different at the DNA level.
Nature 365, 548–550