Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Journal of Urban Design ISSN: 1357-4809 (Print) 1469-9664 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cjud20 Shaping pedestrian movement through playful interventions in security planning: what do field surveys suggest? Marialena Nikolopoulou, Karen Martin & Ben Dalton To cite this article: Marialena Nikolopoulou, Karen Martin & Ben Dalton (2015): Shaping pedestrian movement through playful interventions in security planning: what do field surveys suggest?, Journal of Urban Design, DOI: 10.1080/13574809.2015.1106913 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13574809.2015.1106913 © 2016 The Author(s). Published by Taylor & Francis Published online: 30 Nov 2015. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 38 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=cjud20 Download by: [University of Kent] Date: 13 January 2016, At: 14:53 JOURNAL OF URBAN DESIGN, 2016 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13574809.2015.1106913 Shaping pedestrian movement through playful interventions in security planning: what do ield surveys suggest? Marialena Nikolopouloua, Karen Martina and Ben Daltonb a Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Centre for Architecture and Sustainable Environment, Kent School of Architecture, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK; bFaculty of Art, Environment & Technology, Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds, UK ABSTRACT The control and shaping of pedestrian movement recurs as an aspect of security planning for crowded spaces. Using the concepts of triangulation, performance and low, this paper presents a series of experiments designed to shape pedestrian movement patterns in public spaces in diferent spatial and operation contexts, by eliciting noticeable behaviours and disrupting routine use of space. The hypothesis investigated is that playful, non-obstructive interventions foster a positive social experience yet can be used to shape pedestrian movement. The interventions examined were around the themes of loor marking and mirrors. Analysis demonstrated that the interventions were able to create zones of attraction and exclusion, engage people’s curiosity and elicit playful actions. Habituation, goaldirected behaviour and the inluence of increased cognitive load at personal level were all important factors responsible for reducing the level of engagement with an intervention. The results suggest that increased understanding between environmental and interpersonal stimuli and behavioural responses can provide guidance in using socially acceptable design interventions to inluence use of space in diferent operational contexts, contributing to sustainable security. Introduction Design can play a strategic role in altering the low and shaping pedestrian movement in a space across diferent scales. Such strategies are considered desirable for a range of purposes from commerce and entertainment to security. The ields of art, architecture and human-computer interaction can provide a broad palette of potential interventions for this aim. This paper presents a series of experiments designed to shape people low and pedestrian movement in various types of public spaces, in a positive social context for security planning in counter-terrorism. It investigated the hypothesis that playful, non-obstructive interventions foster a positive social experience yet can be used to alter pedestrian movement. CONTACT Marialena Nikolopoulou M.Nikolopoulou@kent.ac.uk This article was originally published with errors. This version has been corrected. Please see Erratum (http://dx.doi.org/10. 1080/13574809.2015.1129132) © 2016 The Author(s). Published by Taylor & Francis This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/ Licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 2 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Security design for public space includes consideration of a wide range of potential elements from landscaping, gateways and buildings, to barriers, crowd control, sculpture, furniture and materials, to signage, smart architecture, games and advertising, to police tactics, performance and social stakeholder norms. These can be further divided into either permanent or temporary interventions. There is a growing emphasis on the role of design in counter-terrorism policy and research for public spaces. The UK planning process now requires evidence of counter-terrorism security design for new projects (RIBA 2010). At the same time, there has been an increasing recognition that reduction of fear, and in particular community acceptance and comfort, must play a primary role in counter-terrorism policy, and design practice in particular, if it is to be efective and sustainable (Braithwaite 2013). permanent interventions have progressed signiicantly in this respect, with a shift from often alarmist and ad hoc ‘strong-arm tactics’ to architecturally sympathetic and socially acceptable design interventions. Unfortunately, too frequently counter-terrorism design interventions have resulted in hastily retroitted target-hardening or ad hoc solutions focusing on extensive attempts to disrupt covert terrorist activities, with little consideration for the resulting public realm, openness of the space and social interaction. Target hardening is visible in the construction of the ‘ring of steel’ fortiications, around the City of london following the Bishopsgate bombing in 1993 (Coafee, o’Hare, and Hawkesworth 2009) and in the sunken paths encircling the Washington Monument, Washington DC (Benton-Short 2007). Such authority-focused responses can be disorientating and threatening for the general public, diminishing the quality of experience of a space (Benton-Short 2007). Furthermore, the proliferation of security bollards and barriers has caused detrimental impact to the quality of the space in many urban environments (FeMA 430 2007), leading to what the National Capital Urban Design and Security plan for Washington referred to in 2002 as the “present intolerable environment” (NCpC 2002, 5). petroski’s exasperation with the above situation is relected in a paradigm shift with evidence that: … architects and engineers have been rising to the occasion and recapturing public spaces from ugly Jersey barriers, unattractive metal detectors, and other oppressive obstructions of virtually no design value. (petroski 2004, 166) Indeed, recent strategies for securing public space have shifted from such overt security measures to less conspicuous responses such as the manipulation of form, materials and coniguration to create spaces, spatial features and street furniture that intend to inluence people’s movement and emotional state (Adey 2008; Coafee, o’Hare, and Hawkesworth 2009). In comparison to permanent architecture and built environment policy, temporary interventions have lagged behind. Analysis of existing counter-terrorism strategies for temporary interventions in public space highlights an overemphasis on a small palette of authority-focused approaches. However, a wider palette of socially acceptable interventions is required for long-term sustainable efectiveness (Martin, Dalton, and Nikolopoulou 2013). Here, this paper focuses speciically on testing temporary interventions for shaping pedestrian low. The emphasis on design is highlighted in a recent Home oice report (2012) which provides advice on how to integrate such measures at diferent stages, from conception to development so that “… vulnerability of crowded places to terrorist attack can be tackled Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 3 in an imaginative and considered way” (Home oice 2012, 3). This last point is critical if we are to develop and manage public spaces in way that will not have a detrimental efect on the quality of the public realm, but will be socially responsive, enhancing a sense of vitality and well-being. This paper focuses on the above dilemma, investigating whether it is possible to shape pedestrian movement in public spaces, in what the Home oice aspire to be an imaginative and considered way, applying efective uses of disruption while being socially responsive, fostering corresponding routine and comfort. This is done employing the concepts of ‘triangulation’, ‘performance’ and ‘low’, as suggested by Martin, Dalton, and Nikolopoulou (2013), to describe the desired efects of disruption in the current publicly visible counter-terrorism measures. The irst section of this paper provides an overview of the literature on aspects of pedestrian movement low that are currently employed in counter-terrorism strategies. This informed the approach here. The interventions are described in the second section, with the analysis and indings described in the third section. The inal section highlights the conclusions drawn from this work. Efects of disruption in publicly visible counter terrorism The manipulation of the built environment for social change has been extensively explored at diferent scales extending the concepts of ‘natural surveillance’ from Jacobs (1961) and Newman (1972) to the context of counter-terrorism (Secured by Design 2011). Such ‘natural policing’ (Hillier 2004) is at the core of crime prevention through environmental design (CpTeD), which has been employed globally (Cozens, Saville, and Hillier 2005) with the goal of reducing both fear and incidence of crime. Natural surveillance can be linked with social resilience, as increasing social cohesion and connections is identiied as a means of augmenting social resilience (Durodié 2004). Indeed, researchers have highlighted the synergies between CpTeD and urban sustainability (Du plessis 1999; Cozens 2002), however, understanding how to achieve this social resilience is challenging. Triangulation (Whyte 1988), the process that provides a linkage between people and the stimulus for strangers to talk to each other, can be understood as a temporary disruption of a state of ‘civil inattention’. In counter-terrorism strategy, reducing civil-inattention acts to heighten opportunity for identifying and reporting of unusual activity of a potential hostile actor, or an unexpected item in a public space. This concept provides a framework to understand existing counter terrorism signage, announcements and security, and begins to suggest the ways in which alternative design approaches could support heightened awareness and reporting. Numerous examples from art and advertising illustrate how temporary, playful interventions can increase social connections, encourage social interaction and raise engagement with public space (lozano-Hemmer 2001; thefuntheory.com, 2009; Martin, Dalton, and Nikolopoulou 2013). This provided the informed background for a series of interventions to be employed that can inluence movement patterns. publicly visible counter-terrorism interventions are often intended to evoke performances from people in a space (edwards 2010) in an attempt to raise levels of discomfort. The goal of this type of intervention is to elicit behavioural cues that help security staf identify people who have something to hide. A visible security regime to ‘deter, detect and delay’ can play an important role in deterring suspicious terrorist activity, including hostile reconnaissance Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 4 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. (Home oice 2012). Initiatives such as BASS (Behaviour Analysis Screening System) in the UK and SpoT (Securing passengers by observation Techniques) in the US train security staf to try to identify suspicious behaviour and body language in response to temporary, high-visibility disruption of spaces such as temporary cordons and high visibility patrols (Coafee et al. 2008). The queue and search routines of the archway metal detector are some of the various security interventions that require pedestrians to perform in public view. For someone conducting hostile reconnaissance, the deterrence seems to lie in the performance of normality that these interventions require of them. In the context of deterring hostile reconnaissance, design and art can create physical examples of unexpected playful disruption or a heightened sense of being on view. Such design concepts are not only useful for stimulating triangulation but can also be efective at encouraging performance from audiences. Shaping the low of pedestrian movement is an integral aspect of security planning for crowded spaces. examples vary from physical barriers to shape crowd low, particularly in airports, stations and sports events, to overt displays of security as a means of ‘heating up’ key locations (Stratfor 2010) to move potential hostile reconnaissance away from one area and into other areas where covert surveillance may be. physical and social characteristics of a space are inter-dependent, and changes to one element will elicit change in the other (Hillier and Sahbaz 2009). As an example, airport designers manipulate form, materials and coniguration to elicit speciic emotions and to direct passengers’ movement around airport departure areas (Adey 2008). literature from criminology discusses how people carrying out crime develop a ‘crime template’ or idealized site for their criminal act and then try to match this location with places they already know or those that they come into contact with (Brantingham and Brantingham 1993). As a result, the ideal crime location is one where criminals are comfortable and feel that they it in. By intervening at the point where situational aspects of covert activity converge, that is the times and places where the actors, location and opportunity for criminal endeavours overlap, the intended action can be interrupted (Cornish 1994). In the context of security planning in public places, this suggests a particularly strong efect on low for disruptions that heighten crowd pleasure for many, while reducing feelings of control over ‘crime templates’ for a few. playfulness can be particularly efective in encouraging pedestrians to engage, whilst reducing feelings of personal control through disruption of routine for those with malicious intent. In this framework using the concepts of triangulation, performance and low, the aim of the study here was to design, install and evaluate a series of temporary interventions in public spaces of diferent types that can shape pedestrian movement pattern by eliciting noticeable behaviours and disrupting routine. This can be employed as a strategy to augment existing policing techniques for identifying people engaged in suspicious activities. In all cases, the interventions had to be able to win public support, which can be considered as vital for counter-terrorism in order to reduce public anxiety and increase social resilience and cohesion. Design of interventions The design of the interventions has built on existing work in the area of deception studies and counter-terrorism, borrowing vocabulary from the arts. Martin, Dalton, and Nikolopoulou Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 5 (2013) argued how a palette of art, advertising, architecture and entertainment projects ofered examples of unexpected interventions that can heighten attention, disrupt planned routine and elicit noticeable behaviours. The Safer Spaces Design guidelines (Triggs et al. 2012) advocate taking a design-led approach, drawing on designers’ perspectives, processes and methods to develop site-speciic, participatory processes that build on current research in counter-terrorism to create responses that are efective yet publically acceptable. In choosing playful interventions to test, emphasis was placed on techniques that might dissuade or reveal signs of malicious activity in addition to shaping general pedestrian low. Findings and methods from deception studies were adapted for the experiments. The potential for unanticipated questions to elicit noticeable responses in interviews has been discussed in literature (Vrij et al. 2009). This method was appropriated and the studies were designed to initiate unanticipated interactions and moments of public performance. The literature suggests that asking temporal questions when suspecting a scripted answer can help reveal deceptive responses. The analogy in physical space is shaping of crowd low to expose temporary areas of high or low occupancy that in turn may highlight individual movement routines. The strategic use of evidence gathered from observations of typical movement patterns was used to decide the location of the interventions with the goal of zones of attraction. These zones aimed to encourage people to linger in areas where movement is normally lowing without pause. Types of intervention The hypothesis here is that playful, non-obstructive interventions foster a positive social experience yet can be used to shape pedestrian movement. With this hypothesis in mind, the aims of the interventions were to (a) alter the low of pedestrian movement through a space, (b) create zones of attraction and exclusion within the space, and (c) engage people’s curiosity and elicit playful actions. The irst two of these aims intend to address the ability of interventions to shape pedestrian movement. The third aim addresses the ability of interventions to foster heightened awareness within a positive social experience. The design interventions were intended to be lightweight, low-cost and temporary, which could be installed for short periods of time as required. Two categories of non-obstructive interventions were investigated, around the themes of loor markings and mirrors, in a range of spatial and situational contexts. Floor markings intervene in a space by changing the appearance of the ground. They were chosen as a suitable form of intervention as they can guide movement through a space or establish points of interest, yet do not physically impede pedestrian low. Floor marks are regularly used in public spaces to assist direction to services (e.g. train platforms, lifts, taxis, etc.) or advertising. As it is not necessary for pedestrians to alter their movement path in order to pass through the space investigated, changes in behaviour in response to the intervention can be attributed to engagement with the intervention. Mirrors were considered as a form of intervention that suggests the potential to catch and hold attention, and may also foster heightened self-awareness. Merleau-ponty (1958) suggests that seeing relections of oneself in the mirror is engaging yet alienating. Becoming a spectator of oneself is an illusory perception, which while often playful can be confusing at the level of emotions, universally unsettling and even a source of anxiety and fear in the context of heightened self-awareness (Rochat and Zahavi 2011). This unsettling experience has been explored in various experimental studies investigating deceit. Mirrors have also Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 6 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. been demonstrated to be a potential means to distinguish liars from truth-tellers in contained environments (lawson et al. 2011, 2013). Building on these indings, it was speculated that people trying to hide or disguise their activities would feel uncomfortable seeing their relection in addition to the mirror surface acting to increase the likelihood of an intervention attracting the attention of passers-by, visually exaggerating change and movement in the space. Finally, mirrors ofer the potential for altering movement low through using the relective surface to create playful and unexpected efects, evoking the spectacle of a hall-of-mirrors. By strategically placing mirrors in unanticipated locations people may slow down, or stop, to investigate. It was speculated that the sense of movement and change produced by this type of intervention would contribute to eliciting curiosity from passers-by. Two loor-marking and one mirror experiments were integrated with deception studies to explore the efects of disruption, self-awareness and increased cognitive load on the behaviour elicited by the interventions. In the experimental studies, the general public were considered to be non-primed participants representing the behaviour of individuals with benign intentions. This revealed how the interventions it into patterns of everyday behaviour and the types of response they elicit. The participants in the deception studies acted as primed participants and they were not aware of the link between the deception study and the interventions. The hypothesis was that primed participants are involved with their task to such an extent that they are less aware of their environment or less willing to deviate from prepared scripts of activity, and therefore exhibit less playful behaviour than non-primed participants. This could then elicit noticeable diferences in behaviour between those engaged in benign and suspicious activities. Methodology A number of studies were developed for each type of intervention between May 2011 and May 2012. pilot studies were run for both types of intervention to assess their viability before installing them in more challenging public environments. It was essential for all the selected spaces to have a high footfall of transitory routes passing through the space while also containing a cross-section of pedestrian uses with longer dwell times. In addition, all spaces were publicly accessible, whether privately or publicly owned. Action research techniques enabled designing, implementing and analyzing interventions in an iterative cycle. lessons learned and indings uncovered in one study informed subsequent experiments. grounded theory analysis was used to reveal and construct hypotheses of how people respond to interventions in public space (Strauss and Corbin 1998). Diferent methods of data analysis were employed. The researchers observing the spaces were taking structured notes, while they also used maps to record movement patterns and novel behaviours. Further analysis included video transcribing, along with automated video recording in order to sample over longer time periods. For the selection of the public spaces for the experiments, an essential criterion was that cameras could be positioned to capture top-down views of pedestrian movement. Researchers watched the video output and noted pre-selected behaviours, for example, direction of movement or pausing by the intervention. Spatial analysis of the movement and behaviour of passers-by enabled patterns of JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 7 movement to be visualized and indicated how passers-by chose to position themselves in relation to the intervention, which was supplemented by MATlAB image analysis. Heatmaps of space usage were derived from hand-mapped observations. This technique was extended through the use of computer vision tracking of pedestrian movement using the openCV library implementation of blobtracking. Averaging collections of movement trails and normalizing for the busiest section of the map generated heat-maps. Visualizing patterns in this way allows for analysis of space usage. Questionnaires and surveys ofered an additional method of data collection for three of the interventions. This provided further insights into the attitudes of passers-by to the space and respective interventions. The responses enabled highlighting the relationship between the properties of the space, the intervention and behaviour. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Experimental studies Seven diferent experiments are presented with diferent types of loor markings and mirrors presenting the main interventions. Floor-marking studies Four diferent loor-marking interventions were designed, installed and studied in diferent operational contexts of increasing complexity. Tapelines A pilot study was carried out with a pattern created using tape on the loor on the campus at the University of Kent. The pattern was based on one of paul Klee’s drawings and covered an area approximately 8 m long. The design was composed of a tapeline in two contrasting colours, luorescent pink and luorescent yellow. This was to ensure visibility while minimizing the possibility for any confusion with tape used to indicate safety issues. The tapeline was positioned to relect the direction of pedestrian low through the space. A path joins the area about halfway along the length of the line. The path is perpendicular to the line. The hypothesis was that people might change their existing path of movement and begin following the line. This response would be considered playful behaviour. Tape maze The next stage of loor intervention consisted of a maze design covering half the width of the corridor in Warwick Business School (WBS).1 It was created out of black tape with pink tape feet at each entrance to the maze to indicate that people should walk through it, covering an area of 5 m x 2.5 m. The intervention was presented to students as an artwork. In addition, WBS ran a deception study, where participants had to lie about the contents of a bag they were given to carry along the corridor. They were not told of any connection between the deception study and the maze intervention. The video rerecording was for four days. Floor text: Portsmouth The intervention consisted of a paragraph of text printed onto vinyl appropriate for attaching to outdoor loor surfaces. It was a simple design of black text on a white background, 8 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 4 m x 3 m, placed in the centre of guildhall Square, portsmouth, for eight days. The space consisted of low-density population at any one time, with a number of well-used pedestrian commuter routes through the square along with a cafe, lunch spots and proximity to public amenities, including a library. The text was a paragraph from Nicholas Nickleby by Charles Dickens (1839), from a section of the novel set in portsmouth. No accreditation for the text was given on the vinyl, as the authors speculated this anonymity would provoke curiosity and increase the chance of the text being read in its entirety. portsmouth strongly associates with Charles Dickens who was born in the town. During the time the loor vinyl was in place, the city celebrated the bicentenary of his birth. Floor text: Glasgow Queen Street station A similar intervention to the loor vinyl used in portsmouth was applied to the Queen Street railway station in glasgow. The space consisted of high-density population, particularly at rush hour, with a mix of focused train activities, travelling directly to a platform or waiting for information, along with shopping and cafes. The researchers speculated that people on the station concourse would look around the environment while they waited. The proposal was that the large loor vinyl would arouse their curiosity and draw them over to investigate further. The 3 m x 3 m loor vinyl was installed on the station concourse. It contained text from the 1865 edition of Bradshaw’s Tourist Guide to the United Kingdom describing glasgow city and the railway journeys it was possible to take from glasgow at this time. It was oriented towards the passenger information boards at the far end of the concourse. Researchers experimented with colouring the background and text, however, using colour created the impression that the loor vinyl was a piece of advertising. The simple design with text in black on a white background minimized this efect. Beyond the video recording and analysis, surveys were also used, aiming to understand if the loor vinyl contributed to a positive environment for occupants of the space. A small number of the individuals on the station concourse, who had shown an interest in the intervention, were invited to take part. The questions focused on people’s perception of the intervention (see Appendix A). Mirror studies Three studies with mirrors were designed and carried out in diferent settings, employing both physical and digital mirrors ( Figure 1). Library mirrors A series of three mirrors were set up outside the entrance to the library at the University of Kent, a busy area with students entering the library or waiting to meet friends. The mirrors were set up to create a feedback efect as one passed between them. Digital mirror: BBC Big Screen This intervention was installed in guildhall Square, portsmouth. The BBC Big Screen is a permanent ixture in the square. This study used the application Hand from Above by Chris o’Shea (2009) to create a digital mirror (Figure 1). Hand from Above was designed for a BBC JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 9 Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Figure 1. Mirrors (left) on the station concourse, Glasgow; (right) digital mirror with Hand from Above on the BBC Big Screen, Portsmouth. Big Screen and shows a live image of the space on the BBC Big Screen, efectively acting as a digital mirror. At irregular intervals a large hand entered the screen to tickle or pick up one of the people seen on the video image. The authors believed that the Hand from Above artwork would create a fun experience for visitors to the square. This assumption was based on documentation of previous showings of Hand from Above (o’Shea 2009). The study ran for a period of a month, where for 12 days a parallel study on deception with primed participants was also taking place, run by the University of portsmouth.2 The deception study required primed participants to visit the square for a photography task. Half of the participants were instructed to take photographs for a tourist brochure and the others for use in hostile reconnaissance. The participants who took photographs for use in hostile reconnaissance were asked to lie about the purpose of their photographs during a subsequent interview with researchers. participants were not told of any connection between the deception study and the Hand from Above artwork. There were 83 participants in the deception study, 64 of whom were captured on video. A short questionnaire to elicit participants’ feelings about their experience in guildhall Square was also completed at the end of the subsequent interview (see Appendix B). Analysis focused on the participants in the deception study and their movement paths were mapped onto a plan of the square. The time of their entry and exit into the space was documented as well as times at which they exhibited notable behaviours. Mirrors: Glasgow Queen Street station Sheets of silver mirror HIpS were installed into three freestanding poster frames at Queen Street station (Figure 1). organizational safety concerns prevented the mirrors being placed in a location that the researchers felt had the potential to inluence pedestrian low and behaviour, so the frames were placed adjacent to each other between two sets of gates leading to the station platforms (Figure 1). In addition to the video data collection, researchers approached people standing on the station concourse in the vicinity of the intervention to enquire if they had noticed the mirrors, and if so what they thought the purpose of the mirrors might be. Due to the restrictions on the location little awareness of the intervention was demonstrated by passers-by. This was 10 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Figure 2. Playful behaviour engaging with the intervention (left) tapeline at Kent; (right) maze at Warwick. Table 1. Movement path of passers-by in relation to the maze intervention. Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Across (disregard intervention) 664 1079 1183 1034 Around (indirect engagement) 810 773 609 742 Follow (direct engagement) 61 40 25 28 Around & Follow (percentage of direct & indirect engagement) 57% 43% 35% 43% highlighted by the survey indings (n = 35) that found very few people noticed anything unusual about the station or noticed the mirrors in particular. As a result, this intervention was discounted from further analysis. Findings This section is organized around the three aims: (a) alter the low of pedestrian movement through a space, (b) create zones of attraction and exclusion within the space, and (c) engage people’s curiosity and elicit playful actions. In addition, the efect of cognitive load and task-oriented activity became increasingly noticeable during the implementation of the interventions and this is considered as an additional point. Alter low The studies showed that pedestrians altered their movement paths in response to the interventions. This was observed for both loor marking (Figure 2) and mirror interventions. There were two aspects to this. First, people deviated from their current movement path to avoid and go around the intervention. Second, interventions were seen to elicit a temporal deviation where people would stop for longer in the space than their current movement suggested they were planning to. It became apparent that the tapeline intervention acted as an anchor point for people engaged in other activities, e.g. talking on the phone; stopping on the line without paying close attention to the intervention. This effect was also seen with the library mirror study. JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 11 Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Figure 3. Visual analysis indicating movement paths: (left) without floor vinyl, 9–11 am Tuesday 3 April; (right) with floor vinyl in place, 9–11am Friday 20 April. Table 2. Level of engagement with the floor vinyl in Portsmouth. Saturday Sunday Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday TOTAL Walk across 25 30 28 22 18 37 48 208 Stop to read 82 65 109 40 53 38 57 444 Analysis demonstrated diferent levels of interaction with the interventions. Deviating from the current path to follow the WBS maze or the tapeline was considered to be direct engagement with the intervention. people alone and in groups engaged directly with the maze in this way. This behaviour was only observed in a small number who passed-by. Altering the movement path to walk around the loor markings was considered to indicate an indirect acknowledgement of the intervention. This behaviour only occurs if there is an awareness of its presence. Taking into account the number of people who acknowledge the presence of the maze by walking around it shows a signiicantly higher level of engagement (Table 1). on average, the movement of 45% of the passers-by was afected by the intervention. The text intervention in portsmouth supported the indings that interventions can elicit deviation from routine movement paths. The analysis of a two-hour period in the morning rush-hour on a weekday shows the extensive use of the square. However, with the intervention in place, there is a more limited use of the section of ground covered by the intervention, creating an exclusion zone, with movement paths deviating at the edges of the loor vinyl with very few people walking across (Figure 3). As with the WBS maze, there were diferent levels of engagement with the loor text intervention. Stopping to read the text was considered to be direct engagement with the intervention. Those who showed a peripheral acknowledgement of the intervention deviated from their current movement path so as to avoid walking on the text. Table 2 demonstrates that the number of people stopping to read the text was consistently higher than the number of people walking across the intervention. 12 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Figure 4. Examples of noticeable behaviour, people stopping to read the text (left) in Portsmouth; (right) at the concourse of Glasgow Queen Street train station. Another important point when considering the ability of interventions to alter movement low is the efect of habituation, i.e. the decrease in response to a stimulus after repeated exposure to it. In the WBS maze study, the number of people engaging with the maze consistently decreased across the four days the intervention was installed (Table 1). The efect of habituation was also noted at the portsmouth text intervention. The number of pedestrians walking across the loor markings was always greater than number stopping to read, but a decreased engagement with the intervention over the time it was installed can be seen (Table 2). Zones of attraction and exclusion The tape maze intervention illustrated how a loor-marking intervention can act as an attractor for people. The library mirrors intervention indicated that mirrors can elicit a similar response. The text interventions in portsmouth and glasgow also attracted passers-by. people who engaged with the intervention by reading the text were observed to stand still as they did so (Figure 4). They usually stood at the base of the text in order to read it easily. The text interventions also seemed to provide a zone of exclusion as people rarely stood on the loor vinyl unless preoccupied by another activity. even at busy times the loor vinyl would remain clear (Figure 5). For the high density, task focused context of the station, it was a diferent situation, and analysis of the video time-sections indicated no easily discernible efect. As the vinyl was located in front of the gates leading to the train platforms, people crossed the loor vinyl regularly, especially going to or from the platform gates. Elicit playful behaviour The analysis revealed that the digital mirror intervention was successful in eliciting playful and noticeable, performative behaviour from the general public (Figure 6), e.g. waving to the camera. This type of response was also observed in response to the library mirror intervention. Following the loor markings of the tapelines and maze was considered playful and noticeable behaviour. Both interventions were successful at eliciting this behaviour, although the number of people who engaged was low. The maze intervention also revealed a diferent Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 13 Figure 5. Example of the mapping of movement around the intervention. Dots denote people standing, whereas arrows denote people walking towards the direction of the arrow. Vinyl provided zone of exclusion for stationary people. Figure 6. Mirrors eliciting noticeable behaviour (left) in Portsmouth, (right) in Kent. form of noticeable behaviour as people deviated from their movement path to walk around the maze. Analysis of the text interventions at portsmouth and glasgow clearly demonstrated their ability to attract attention and elicit curiosity as people were observed stopping to read the loor vinyl (Figure 4). Efect of cognitive load and goal-directed activity The hypothesis was that participants in the deception study connected to the WBS maze intervention would be less likely to engage in playful activity due to their heightened cognitive load caused by participating in the study. In total, participants passed the maze 236 times during the deception study. participants followed the maze on only two occasions, corresponding to 0.8%. For other users of the corridor direct engagement was signiicantly higher (over 4% on the irst day, dropping to 1.4% on day four). Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 14 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Figure 7. Visual analysis indicating movement paths of (left) truth tellers, taking pictures for a tourist brochure and (right) liars, taking photos for reconnaissance. The analysis also highlighted the efect of goal-directed behaviour on non-primed passers-by, as people engaged with other tasks, such as delivery of goods, more often walked across the intervention than around it. For the text intervention in glasgow, as with the tape maze, there is inluence of goal-directed behaviour, reducing the level of engagement with the intervention. people crossing, or standing on, the loor vinyl were often preoccupied with another activity looking for tickets or friends, running for trains, managing luggage, watching the departure boards, etc. It is speculated that this may help to explain the diferences in the indings of the text intervention in portsmouth where a clear zone of exclusion was created, and the text intervention in glasgow where passers-by were more inclined to cross the intervention. The deception study associated with the digital mirror intervention in portsmouth ofered the opportunity to examine potential diferences in behaviours arising from the task-oriented activity of truth tellers and liars. The assumption was that all participants in the deception study would be involved with their task to such an extent that they would exhibit less playful behaviour in front of the digital mirror than members of the general public. In addition, the authors speculated that liars engaged in taking photographs for hostile reconnaissance purposes might not wish to draw attention to themselves by having their image appear on the screen and so would stand on the edge of the area detected by the camera. None of the participants were observed interacting with or acknowledging the intervention irrespective of whether they were asked to carry out benign or suspicious activities. This efect of increased cognitive load and goal-directed behaviour is consistent with the results from the previous studies. Analysis of movement using image processing of the participants’ individual paths on MATlAB revealed diferences in patterns of spatial movement between those engaged in benign and suspicious activities, or else truth-tellers and liars. This is best understood when evaluated against the geometry of the space. The square is contained between buildings on three sides, with steps on the east and west leading to the guildhall and the council oices. overlaying the loor plan with heat maps of pedestrian movement through the space, it is noticeable that truth-tellers appear to use more of the available loor area, including the stairs (Figure 7). Conversely, the liars have a tendency to concentrate their movement nearer the steps by the guildhall. This may be seen as ofering better protection and avoiding unnecessary exposure. JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 15 This feeling of exposure was conirmed by the post-survey interviews. A total of 23% of the participants mentioned the space coniguration inluencing feelings of exposure, with a focus on the openness of the space and their location in the space. There was a moderate correlation between their feelings of exposure and being watched (r = 0.579, p = 0.000), which then inluenced their conidence to get the required photos (r = -0.176, p = 0.003). overall, there were no statistically signiicant diferences between liars and truth-tellers, or males and females in the survey responses. Another diferentiation between truth-tellers and liars was dwell time, as the latter on average spent less time in the space. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Relections Using the concepts of triangulation, performance and low, the work described in this paper presented the indings from seven experimental studies designed to disrupt routine use of space stimulating triangulation, while eliciting noticeable behaviours encouraging performances from audiences, as well as inluencing pedestrian movement and altering low in public spaces. This was done through the design and installation of interventions in diferent spatial and operational contexts ‒ university campuses, a civic square and a railway station. These temporary and low-cost interventions were around the themes of loor markings and mirrors. The four loor-marking installations and the library mirrors were all successful in disrupting routine behaviour in the space by altering low. This occurred as people deviated from their existing movement path as a result of being attracted to the intervention. In that respect loor markings had a greater efect on movement low than mirror interventions. The text interventions also elicited a temporal deviation as people were encouraged to linger in the space for longer, increasing their dwell time. The disruption of routine movement paths illustrated how interventions can draw people to a speciic area or establish a zone of exclusion. Zones of attraction and exclusion are not mutually exclusive efects. Interventions were observed to provide anchor points for passers-by that simultaneously attracted people towards the area of space where the intervention was located, while also encouraging them to keep certain areas of the space clear. The orientation of the text-based interventions ofers further opportunities to inluence people’s position while they engage with the intervention. All the interventions, with the exception of the mirrors in Queen Street station, were successful in stimulating triangulation and performances from the public, eliciting noticeable behaviours, mainly in terms of playfulness and curiosity. playfulness can be deined as behaviour that appears out of place, unnecessary for the norms of the space and is intended to entertain or amuse. Behaviour such as following the maze (WBS) or waving (Big Screen; library mirrors) was considered to be playful behaviour. Curiosity can be deined as behaviour that indicates a desire to know more about the intervention. Stopping to read the text (portsmouth; glasgow) or walking over to look at the relections in the mirror (library mirrors) were considered to be examples of curiosity. Such behaviours have been explored extensively in arts projects to engage with audiences in public spaces (Martin, Dalton, and Nikolopoulou 2013). luke Jerram’s (2008) ‘play Me, I’m Yours’ placed upright pianos into a number of empty locations with the invitation to play as people chose, while the ‘piano Stairs’ transformed the entrance to a Stockholm subway station into a keyboard where each step produced a diferent note, next to the escalator Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 16 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. (DDB Stockholm 2009). Both were familiar objects, placed in atypical locations drawing people in, and inviting unprompted, unexpected behaviour, encouraging triangulation and performances from members of the public. Furthermore, the ‘piano Stairs’ demonstrates how alterations to a physical space can lead to dramatic changes in low behaviour, as the following day passengers altered their routine path to exit the station, with 66% more people than usual choosing to take the stairs instead of the escalator. In analyzing the interventions two levels of noticeable behaviour were identiied ‒ direct engagement and acknowledgement. The former can be deined as a deliberate response to an intervention, while the latter as behaviour indicating the person is aware of it. Direct engagement refers to, for example, following the maze, waving into the mirror or reading the text, while an example of acknowledging an intervention is altering their path to avoid the loor marking, without appearing to pay any attention to the intervention. Direct engagement was a noticeable trend in all studies that elicited noticeable behaviour, but only from a small percentage of passers-by. Acknowledgement, on the other hand, was elicited from a high percentage of users of the space. When the two are considered together then approximately half of the passers-by elicited a form of noticeable behaviour. In all cases, the greatest efect occurs when the interventions are unexpected, which can be regarded as a pre-requisite to attract curiosity and playfulness. They may have a form that contrasts with its surrounding environment (maze; loor vinyls; library mirrors), or present a diferent experience of the space from usual (digital mirror Big Screen). Interventions that blend in with their surroundings in terms of form and experience (glasgow mirrors) are less likely to elicit noticeable behaviours from passers-by. The ability of interventions to alter low and elicit noticeable behaviours is also afected by personal factors, such as increased cognitive load and goal-directed behaviour. As the maze intervention at WBS and the text intervention in glasgow demonstrated with increasing goal-directed behaviour (whether it was carrying goods, running for the train or checking departure boards), people were less likely to respond to the interventions by disrupting routine behaviour or displaying signs of curiosity or playful engagement. This was particularly noticeable with the increased cognitive load imposed on the participants in the deception studies (maze; digital mirror), where they were least likely to alter their path or engage with the interventions directly or indirectly. The interventions had a more noticeable efect in spaces that were not very crowded. The text interventions in portsmouth and glasgow provided an opportunity to compare near-identical interventions in locations with diferent spatial and social characteristics. The analysis showed that the text intervention in portsmouth produced more noticeable disruption to movement paths and routine behaviour. outside peak periods of use in glasgow there was a tendency for bystanders to engage with the text; however, this diminished rapidly as the station became densely populated and the intervention less visible. Another very important factor is the efect of habituation, which became noticeable in all studies. The studies indicated that the potential for interventions to disrupt routine behaviour, create zones of attraction and exclusion decreases with repeated encounter. Findings from the loor-marking studies suggest that the number of people passing by who are inluenced by an intervention drops after the irst two or three days. The same efect was noted for the ability of interventions to elicit noticeable behaviour. JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 17 Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 Towards design guidelines Analysis of the data can also support the provision of basic guidelines for designing interventions to disrupt pedestrian low and elicit noticeable behaviours to be used in security planning alongside more authority-focused interventions. The form and purpose of an intervention should be led by the characteristics of the space in which it will be located. Understanding the ongoing activities is essential to suggest appropriate types of interventions that may disrupt existing activities without causing unnecessary inconvenience. Spatial and morphological characteristics of the space afecting existing movement patterns, along with the temporal rhythm of activities, should be considered. The level of exposure experienced in the space, the arrangement of the buildings and the access points inluence movement in and through a space. The crowdedness of the space should also be considered, as a successful intervention may become less efective in crowded conditions. Interventions in crowded situations may need to be bolder in order to attract attention. To have the greatest chance of altering movement low and eliciting noticeable behaviour, it is vital that the position of the intervention in the space is considered carefully and sited in a location with the potential to disrupt existing low. If the intervention is away from the area of routine movement or activity, the opportunity for meaningful engagement is minimized. Heat mapping typical movement patterns can assist in understanding low norms. To maximize the potential for disrupting movement and eliciting noticeable behaviours the interventions should allow for multiple levels of engagement and interaction ‒ direct and indirect. Direct engagement with the interventions was the least frequent; however, combined with acknowledgement and indirect interaction (such as altering one’s movement path) the number of interactions provided a noticeable efect in the space through the creation of attraction and exclusion zones. For best results the intervention should be unexpected and avoid the vocabulary of advertising, which is ubiquitous in public spaces, hence reducing the opportunities for engagement minimizing its efect. Finally, even with the most successful design intervention, habituation will occur. As a result, within a few days the level of engagement will begin to decrease and eventually they will be part of the wallpaper, as was also highlighted at the ‘Safer Spaces’ project emphasizing the need for periodic change (Triggs et al. 2012). Such indings are consistent with more traditional strategies, such as CCTV where initial deterrence fades with time (Scottish government 2009). This suggests that interventions should be quick and easy to install and remove, allowing for a rapid turnaround to ensure continuing engagement. As designing and producing an intervention can be time-consuming, it may be useful to create interventions that can easily be implemented in a number of locations in diferent contexts. Conclusions Using the concepts of triangulation, performance and low, the paper has extensively discussed diferent ways to disrupt routine use of space, by inluencing pedestrian movement and altering low in public spaces, as well as eliciting noticeable behaviours. The control and shaping of pedestrian movement recurs as an aspect of security planning for crowded spaces. Triangulation can be viewed as an efective way to disrupt a state of civil inattention. It is argued that using Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 18 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. art-inspired interventions as an alternative to an overt security approach invites people to perform in a public space, disrupting routine use of space to act as a form of deterrence, and shaping the low of pedestrian movement which is an integral aspect of security planning. In the context of deterring hostile reconnaissance, design and art can create physical examples of ‘unexpected questioning’ through playful disruption or a heightened sense of being on view (Dalton et al. 2015). Such playfulness can be regarded as a key strength in amplifying desired disruption efects in public spaces. This is particularly beneicial, considering that a constant presence of traditional performances of authority in public space risks trivializing the impact of necessary future displays of security strength. Furthermore, in a wider cultural context over-policing has the potential to foment radicalization, particularly among communities who feel particularly targeted because of a recurring, heightened authority presence in the spaces they use (Fussey 2007). All the above highlight the growing awareness of the necessity for security measures to be socially acceptable (Németh 2010) and this design-led approach has a positive inluence on one’s experience of space, “dwelling through mobility” (McFarlane 2011, 663), increasing social sustainability. The beneicial impact of art in drawing people in public spaces, as identiied by Whyte (1980) over 30 years ago, has been highlighted as an important component in security design. The Arts Council argues that “good art encourages greater use of public places and increases individuals’ sense of security” (Arts Council 1991, 17), while the Mayor’s oice in New York advocates its use for a similar purpose (Russell et al. 2002). More recently, the UK Home oice (2012) recommends attracting people to spaces using positive features, even incorporating public art, as a key strategy for enhancing security. In a position paper discussing the logic of fear in terrorism and counter-terrorism, Braithwaite argued that counter-terrorism policy would be well served by “more proactively attempting to improve average participant enjoyment of public spaces” (Braithwaite 2013, 100). Could then investing in the potential of a space or the opportunities for creating a sense of place through a design-led approach for counter-terrorism provide a new era in security planning? An era of ‘sustainable security’ (Crampton 2013), in which sustainability does not refer to the inancial viability of the scheme, but the basis for human well-being. Notes 1. Warwick Business school (WBS) were partners in the project. 2. The University of portsmouth were partners in the project. Acknowledgements The authors wish to express their gratitude to their partners, the Centre for the protection of National Infrastructure, User perspectives, the BBC Big Screen Manager in portsmouth and the following teams for allowing them to install the various interventions on their property: University of Kent, glasgow Queen Street railway station and portsmouth City Council. The authors would also like to thank the following research students at the University of Kent: Carolina Vassilikou for her assistance with the statistical analysis and Christina Chatzipoulka for her assistance with the MATlAB analysis. JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 19 Disclosure statement No potential conlict of interest was reported by the authors. Funding This research was conducted as part of the engineering and physical Sciences Research Council (epSRC) project ‘Shades of grey ‒Towards a Science of Interventions for eliciting and Detecting Notable Behaviours’ [grant number: ep/H02302X/1]. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 References Adey, p. 2008. “Airports, Mobility and the Calculative Architecture of Afective Control.” Geoforum 39: 438–451. Arts Council. 1991. Percent for Art: A Review. london: Arts Council of great Britain and AN publications. Benton-Short, l. 2007. “Bollards, Bunkers, and Barriers: Securing the National Mall in Washington, DC.” Environment and Planning D: Society and Space 25 (3): 424–446. Braithwaite, A. 2013. “The logic of Fear in Terrorism and Counterterrorism.” Journal of Police & Criminal Psychology, 27(1), Journal of Police & Criminal Psychology 28 (2): 95–101. Brantingham, p. l., and p. J. Brantingham. 1993. “Nodes, paths and edges: Considerations on the Complexity of Crime and the physical environment.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 13 (1): 3–28. Bradshaw. 1865. Bradshaw’s Tourist Guide to the United Kingdom Coafee, J., C. Moore, D. Fletcher, and l. Bosher. 2008. “Resilient Design for Community Safety and Terror-Resistant Cities.” Proceedings of the ICE - Municipal Engineer 161 (2): 103–110. Coafee, J., p. o’Hare, and M. Hawkesworth. 2009. “The Visibility of (in) Security: The Aesthetics of planning Urban Defences against Terrorism.” Security Dialogue 40 (4-5): 489–511. Cornish, D. 1994. “The procedural Analysis of ofending and Its Relevance for Situational prevention.” Crime Prevention Studies 3: 151–196. Cozens, p. M. 2002. “Sustainable Urban Development and Crime prevention through environmental Design for the British City. towards an efective Urban environmentalism for the 21st Century.” Cities 19 (2): 129–137. Cozens, p. M., g. Saville, and D. Hillier. 2005. “Crime prevention through environmental Design (CpTeD): a Review and Modern Bibliography.” Property Management 23 (5): 328–356. Crampton, J. W. 2013. “Is Security Sustainable?” Environment and Planning D: Society and Space 31 (4): 571–577. Dalton, B., K. Martin, C. McAndrew, M. Nikolopoulou, and T. Triggs. 2015. “Design Strategies for Visible Counter-Terrorism in public Spaces.” In Counter-Terrorism & Hostile Intent: Human Factors Theory and Application, 261–276. Farnham: Ashgate. DDB Stockholm. 2009. Piano Stairs. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2lXh2n0apyw Dickens, C. 1839. The Life and Adventures of Nicholas Nickleby. london: Chapman & Hall. Du plessis, C. 1999. “The links between Crime prevention and Sustainable Development.” Open House International 24 (1): 33–40. Durodié, B. 2004. “Sociological Aspects of Risk and Resilience in Response to Acts of Terrorism.” World Defence Systems 7: 214–216. edwards R. 2010, January 15. “Heathrow Staf given Body language Training to Spot Suspected Terrorists.” Telegraph.Co.Uk. http://www.telegraph.co.uk/travel/travelnews/6990006/Heathrow-insecurity-alert-as-two-men-arrested-on-light.html FeMA 430. 2007. Site and Urban Design for Security: Guidance against Potential Terrorist Attacks, Providing Protection to People and Buildings. Washington: Federal emergency Management Agency. Fussey, p. 2007. “observing potentiality in the global City: Surveillance and Counterterrorism in london.” International Criminal Justice Review 17 (3): 171–192. Hillier, B. 2004. “Can Streets Be Made Safe?” Urban Design International 9 (1): 31–45. Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 20 M. NIKolopoUloU eT Al. Hillier, B., and o. Sahbaz. 2009. “Crime and Urban Design: An evidence-Based Approach.” In Designing Sustainable Cities, edited by R. Cooper, g. evans and C. Boyko, 163–186. Chichester, UK: WileyBlackwell. Jacobs, J. 1961. The Death and Life of Great American Cities. NY: Random House. Home oice. 2012. Protecting Crowded Places: Design and Technical Issues. http://www.homeoice.gov. uk/publications/counter-terrorism/crowded-places/design-tech-issues?view=Binary Jerram l. 2008. Play Me, I’m Yours. http://www.lukejerram.com/projects/play_me_im_yours lawson, g., A. W. Stedmon, C. Zhang, D. l. eubanks, and l. A. Frumkin. 2013. “The efects of SelfAwareness on Body Movement Indicators of the Intention to Deceive.” Applied Ergonomics 44: 687–693. lawson, g., A. W. Stedmon, C. Zhang, D. l. eubanks, and l. A. Frumkin. 2011. “Deception and SelfAwareness.” Lecture Notes in Computer Science 6781: 414–423. lozano-Hemmer R. 2001. Body Movies. http://www.lozano-hemmer.com/body_movies.php Martin, K., B. Dalton, and M. Nikolopoulou. 2013. “Art as a Means to Disrupt Civil Inattention and Routine Use of Space.” Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology 28: 139–149. McFarlane, C. 2011. “The City as Assemblage: Dwelling and Urban Space.” Environment and Planning D: Society and Space 29: 649–671. Merleau-ponty, M. 1958. Phenomenology of Perception. london: Routledge. NCpC. 2002. The National Capital Urban Design and Security Plan. Washington: National Capital planning Commission. Németh, J. 2010. “Security in public Space: An empirical Assessment of Three US Cities.” Environment and Planning a 42: 2487–2507. Newman, o. 1972. Defensible Space: Crime Prevention through Urban Design. New York, NY: MacMillan. o’Shea C. 2009. Hand from above. http://www.chrisoshea.org/hand-from-above petroski, H. 2004. “Technology and Architecture in an Age of Terrorism.” Technology in Society 26: 161–167. RIBA. 2010. Guidance on Designing for Counter-Terrorism. london: Royal Institute of British Architects. Rochat, p., and D. Zahavi. 2011. “The Uncanny Mirror: A Re-Framing of Mirror Self-experience.” Consciousness and Cognition 20 (2): 204–213. Russell J. S., D. Bershad, e. Felicella, M. Kelly and e. Kennedy. 2002. Designing for Security: Using Art and Design to Improve Security. New York: Design Trust for public Space Art Commission of the City of New York. Secured by Design. 2011. Secured by Design. http://www.securedbydesign.com/ Stratfor. 2010. How to Look for Trouble: A Stratfor Guide to Protective Intelligence, CreateSpace Thefuntheory. Com, 2009, Piano Stairs. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2lXh2n0apyw&feature=player_ embedded Triggs, T., C. McAndrew, M. Brooke Rogers, and A. B. Wootton. 2012. Safer Spaces: Communication Design for Counter Terror - Design Guidelines. london: Information environments Research Network with Centre for protection of National Infrastructure. Scottish government. 2009. The Efectiveness of Public Space CCTV: A Review of Recent Published Evidence regarding the Impact of CCTV on Crime. edinburgh: police and Community Safety Directorate, Scottish government, Justice Analytical Services. Strauss, A., and J. Corbin. 1998. Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory. 2nd ed. Thousand oaks: Sage. Vrij, A., S. leal, p. A. granhag, S. Mann, R. p. Fisher, J. Hillman, and K. Sperry. 2009. “outsmarting the liars: The Beneit of Asking Unanticipated Questions.” Law and Human Behavior 33 (2): 159–166. Whyte, W. H. 1980. The Social Life of Small Urban Spaces. New York, NY: project for public Spaces. Whyte W. H. 1988. City: Rediscovering the Center. New York: Doubleday. Appendix A: Questionnaire for the glasgow Queen Street station. Where 1‒7 scale required: [Strongly disagree, Disagree, Somewhat disagree, Neither agree or disagree, Agree, Strongly agree] How many times a month do you come through glasgow Queen Street station? [I’ve never been here before ‒ once a month or less ‒ once a week ‒ every day] Downloaded by [University of Kent] at 14:53 13 January 2016 JoURNAl oF URBAN DeSIgN 21 I feel at ease in Queen Street station [1‒7] I feel vulnerable in Queen Street station [1‒7] I believe I am aware of what is happening around me at the station [1‒7] Do you notice anything unusual about the station today / this week? (depending on how often they come through the station ‒ interventions will be in place for one week) Have you noticed this at the station today? [Show photograph of current intervention] Y/N I felt curious when I was looking at the text/mirrors [1‒7] I felt awkward when I was looking at the text/mirrors [1‒7] What prevented you from going over and looking at the text / mirrors? What do you think about the text / mirrors being at the station? gender [M/F] Age group Appendix B: Questions used during the guildhall Square surveys (as part of an extensive interview with portsmouth University). gender [M/F] Age group When you irst entered the square, how conident were you that you could get the photographs you needed? [Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 very conident] Did you feel that people were watching you while you were taking photographs? [Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 very much so] How exposed did you feel in the space? [Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 very exposed]