The Reality of Repressed Memories
Elizabeth F. Loftus
Repression is one of the most haunting concepts in psychology. Something shocking happens, and the mind
pushes it into some inaccessible comer of the unconscious.
Later, the memory may emerge into consciousness.
Repression is one of the foundation stones on which the
structure ofpsychoanalysis rests. Recently there has been
a rise in reported memories of childhood sexual abuse
that were allegedly repressedfor many years. With recent
changes in legislation, people with recently unearthed
memories are suing alleged perpetrators for events that
happened 20, 30, even 40 or more years earlier. These
new developments give rise to a number of questions: (a)
How common is it for memories of child abuse to be repressed? (b) How are jurors and judges likely to react to
these repressed memory claims? (c) When the memories
surface, what are they like? and (d) How authentic are the
memories?
I
n 1990, a landmark case went to trial in Redwood
City, California. The defendant, George Franklin, Sr.,
51 years old, stood trial for a murder that had occurred
more than 20 years earlier. The victim, 8-year-old Susan
Kay Nason, was murdered on September 22, 1969.
Franklin's daughter, Eileen, only 8 years old herself at
the time of the murder, provided the major evidence
against her father. What was unusual about the case is
that Eileen's memory of witnessing the murder had been
repressed for more than 20 years.
Eileen's memory did not come back all at once. She
claimed that her first flashback came one afternoon in
January 1989 when she was playing with her two-yearold son, Aaron, and her five-year-old daughter, Jessica.
At one moment, Jessica looked up and asked her mother
a question like "Isn't that right, Mommy?" A memory
of Susan Nason suddenly came back. Eileen recalled the
look of betrayal in Susie's eyes just before the murder.
Later, more fragments would return, until Eileen had a
rich and detailed memory. She remembered her father
sexually assaulting Susie in the back of a van. She remembered that Susie was struggling as she said "No
don't" and "Stop." She remembered her father saying
"Now Susie," and she even mimicked his precise intonation. Next, her memory took the three of them outside
the van, where she saw her father with his hands raised
above his head with a rock in them. She remembered
screaming. She remembered walking back to where Susie
lay, covered with blood, the silver ring on her finger
smashed.
Eileen's memory report was believed by her therapist, by several members of her family, and by the San
518
Mateo County district attorney's office, which chose to
prosecute her father. It was also believed by the jury, which
convicted George Franklin, Sr., of murder. The jury began
its deliberations on November 29, 1990, and returned a
verdict the next day. Impressed by Eileen's detailed and
confident memory, they found her father guilty of murder
in the first degree.
Eileen's detailed and confident memory impressed
a number of people. But is her memory authentic? Did
she really witness the murder of her best friend 20 years
earlier? The idea of repression of early traumatic memories is a concept that many psychotherapists readily accept (Bruhn, 1990). In fact, it has been said that repression
is the foundation on which psychoanalysis rests (Bower,
1990). According to the theory, something happens that
is so shocking that the mind grabs hold of the memory
and pushes it underground, into some inaccessible corner
of the unconscious. There it sleeps for years, or even decades, or even forever—isolated from the rest of mental
life. Then, one day, it may rise up and emerge into consciousness. Numerous clinical examples fitting this model
can be readily found. Many of these examples involve
not memory of murder but rather memory of other sorts
of childhood trauma, such as sexual abuse, that allegedly
has been repressed for decades until recovered in therapy.
Rieker and Carmen (1986) described a woman who entered psychotherapy for sexual dysfunction and recovered
memories of incest committed by her father. Schuker
(1979) described a woman who entered psychotherapy
for chronic insomnia, low self-esteem, and other problems
and recovered memories of her father sexually assaulting
her. M. Williams (1987) described a man who entered
therapy for depression and sleep disturbances and recovered memories of a servant molesting him. These anecdotal reports constitute the clinical evidence that clients
do indeed manage later to remember some earlier inacStanley L. Brodsky served as action editor for this article.
This article is an expanded version of an invited address, the Psi
Chi/Frederick Howell Lewis Distinguished Lecture, presented at the
100th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,
Washington DC, August 1992.1 thank Geoffrey Loftus, Ilene Bernstein,
Lucy Berliner, Robert Koscielny, and Richard Ofshe for very helpful
comments on earlier drafts. I thank many others, especially Ellen Bass,
Mark Demos, Judie Alpert, Marsha Linehan, and Denise Park for illuminating discussion of the issues. My gratitude for the vast efforts of
the members of the Repressed Memory Research Group at the University
of Washington is beyond measure. The National Institute of Mental
Health and the National Science Foundation have generously supported
the underlying research on memory.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Elizabeth F. Loftus, Department of Psychology, University of Washington,
Seattle, WA 98195.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. OO03-066X/93/S2.00
Vol.48. No. 5. 518-537
cessible painful experience (Erdelyi, 1985). The reports
constitute evidence for the core ideas inherent in the theory of repression. Several respected scholars once made
the point that, from a clinical standpoint, "the evidence
for repression is overwhelming and obvious" (Erdelyi &
Goldberg, 1979, p. 384).
On the other hand, the clinical anecdotes and the
loose theory used to explain them remain unconvincing
to some psychotherapists and to many laboratory researchers. One psychiatrist who has seen more than 200
severely dissociative patients explicitly referred to such
anecdotes as "empirical observations lacking in scientific
underpinnings" (Ganaway, 1992, p. 203). One researcher
described them as "impressionistic case studies" and
claimed that they could not be counted as "anything more
than unconfirmed clinical speculations" (Holmes, 1990,
p. 97). After reviewing 60 years of research and finding
no controlled laboratory support for the concept of
repression, Holmes suggested, only halfjokingly, that any
use of the concept be preceded by a warning: "Warning.
The concept of repression has not been validated with
experimental research and its use may be hazardous to
the accurate interpretation of clinical behavior" (p. 97).
Even if Holmes (1990) was right that there is virtually
no scientific evidence to demonstrate the authenticity of
repressed memories that return, Eileen's memory could
still be authentic. Even if Holmes is proved wrong and
there does develop solid scientific evidence to support the
authenticity of some repressed memories that return, that
would not prove that Eileen's memory is authentic. If
Eileen's memory is not authentic, where else might all
those details come from? Media reports from 20 years
before—December 1969, when the body was found—
were filled with some of these same details. The facts that
the murdered girl's skull was fractured on the right side
and that a silver Indian ring was found on the body were
reported prominently on the front page of the San Francisco Chronicle ("Susan Nason Body Found," 1969). The
fact that she apparently held her hand up to protect herself, inferred from the crushed ring, was also well-known
(e.g., San Jose Mercury, "Nason Girl Fought," 1969).
Most of the details that fill the rich network of her memory, however, are unfalsifiable or uncheckable—such as
her memory for the door of the van that her father got
out of after he raped Susie. One additional feature of
Eileen's memory, worth noting, is that it changed across
various tellings. For example, when she gave a statement
to the police in November 1989, she told the police that
her father was driving her and her sister Janice to school
when they first saw Susie and that he made Janice get
out of the van when Susie got in. However, months later
at the preliminary hearing, she did not report Janice being
in the van. In the statement to the police, the trip happened on the way to school in the morning or on the way
back from lunch. During the preliminary hearing, after
she presumably was reminded that Susie had not been
missing until after school was out, she said it was in the
late afternoon because the sun was low. Eileen's memory
changed over the tellings, and there were alternative pos-
May 1993 • American Psychologist
sible sources for details that made the memory seem so
rich. This proves that at least some portions of these distant memories are wrong, although other parts could, in
theory, still be authentic.
When George Franklin, Jr., was convicted on the
basis of little more that the return of a repressed memory,
Newsweek magazine called it an "incredible" story
("Forgetting to Remember," 1991). It was apparently the
first time that an American citizen had been tried and
convicted of murder on the basis of a freshly unearthed
repressed memory.
More Repressed Memories
Soon after the Franklin case, a string of others involving
newly emerged distant memories appeared in the media.
People accused by the holders of repressed memories
wrote letters asking for help. Lawyers found themselves
being asked to represent parties in legal cases involving
repressed memories.
Popular Articles
Long-repressed memories that return after decades, often
while a person is in therapy, have become highly publicized through popular articles. In 1991, actress Roseanne
Barr Arnold's story was on the cover of People magazine.
Memories of her mother abusing her from the time she
was an infant until she was 6 or 7 years old had returned
in therapy ("A Star Cries Incest," 1991; Darnton, 1991).
Barr Arnold's was not the first such case to capture the
cover of People magazine that year. Just three months
earlier, People had also reported a story about former
Miss America Marilyn Van Derbur, who had repressed
any knowledge of sexual violation by her father until she
was 24 years old and told the world about it after her
father died ("The Darkest Secret," 1991; Darnton, 1991).
Highly publicized cases involving memories that recently
sprang into consciousness were told repeatedly in numerous popular articles in such publications as the
Washington Post (Oldenberg, 1991), the Los Angeles
Times (Ritter, 1991), Seventeen (Dormen, 1991), Glamour (Edmiston, 1990), Newsweek (Kantrowitz, 1991),
and Time (Toufexis, 1991).
L&tters
Scores of spontaneously written letters from strangers also
describe the emergence of memories. I have received letters written by people who had been accused of abuse by
their children. A 75-year-old physician from Florida
wrote, desperate to understand why his 49-year-old
daughter was suddenly claiming that he had abused her
during her early childhood and teen years. A woman from
Canada wrote about the nightmare of being "falsely accused of sexual abuse by our 30-year-old daughter." A
woman from Michigan wrote about her 38-year-old
daughter who, "after a year of counseling now accuses us
of abuse . . . very much like Roseanne Barr and the
former Miss America, Marilyn Van Derbur." A couple
from Texas wrote to tell about their youngest son, who
519
had accused them of abusing him long ago. One letter
from a mother in California well expresses the pain:
One week before my husband died after an 8-month battle
against lung cancer, our youngest daughter (age 38) confronted
me with the accusation that he had molested her and I had not
protected her. We know who her "therapist" was: a strange young
woman . . . In the weeks, months that followed, the nature of
the charges altered, eventually involving the accusation that my
husband and I had molested our grandson, for whom we had
sometimes cared while our daughter worked at her painting.
This has broken my heart; it is so utterly untrue. This daughter
has broken off all relationship with her four siblings. She came
greatly under the influence of a book, The Courage to Heal [by
Bass & Davis, 1988].
The letters articulately convey the living nightmares
and broken hearts experienced by those accused by their
adult children who suddenly remembered past abuse (see
also Doe, 1991). The parents vehemently deny the abuse.
Who is right and who is wrong? Is the adult child misremembering, or perhaps lying? Are the parents misremembering when they deny abuse, or are they deliberately
lying?
Legal Cases
Another development after the Franklin conviction was
that lawyers started calling psychologists to obtain assistance with a puzzling new type of legal case. For example,
one case involved a 27-year-old San Diego woman (KL)
who began to have recollections of molestation by her
father (DL), that were repressed but then were later
brought out through "counseling and therapeutic intervention" (Loffi v. Lofft, 1989). The daughter claimed that
her father had routinely and continuously molested and
sexually abused her, performing "lewd and lascivious acts,
including but not limited to touching and fondling the
genital areas, fornication and oral copulation." Her earliest memories were of her father fondling her in the master bedroom when she was three years old. Most of her
memories appeared to date back to between the ages of
three and eight. She sued her father for damages for emotional and physical distress, medical expenses, and lost
earnings. She claimed that because of the trauma of the
experience, she had no recollection or knowledge of the
sexual abuse until her repression was lifted, shortly before
she filed suit.
A few years ago, plaintiffs like KL who claimed to
be survivors of childhood sexual abuse would have been
barred from suing by statutes of limitations. Statutes of
limitations, which force plaintiffs to initiate claims
promptly, exist for good reason: They protect people from
having to defend themselves against stale claims. They
exist in recognition that with the passage of time, memories fade and evidence becomes more difficult to obtain.
Succinctly and articulately put, the primary purpose of
statutes of limitation is to prevent "surprises through the
revival of claims that have been allowed to slumber until
evidence has been lost, memories have faded, and witnesses have disappeared" (Telegraphers v. Railway Express Agency, 1944, pp. 348-349). When much time has
520
passed, defendants find it hard to mount an effective defense. Although a statute of limitations on child sexual
abuse might be suspended until a victim reaches the age
of majority or a few years beyond, it previously would
not typically have been extended to the age of 27, for
example, when KL first recalled her abuse.
In 1989, things changed for plaintiffs in the state of
Washington. Legislation went into effect that permitted
people to sue for recovery of damages for injury suffered
as a result of childhood sexual abuse at any time within
three years of the time they remembered the abuse
(Washington, 1989; see also Petersen v. Bruen, 1990). The
legislature invoked a novel application of the delayed discovery doctrine, which essentially says that the statute of
limitations does not begin to run until the plaintiff has
discovered the facts that are essential to the cause of action. Traditionally, the delayed discovery doctrine has
been used in the area of medical malpractice. For example, a patient who discovered during a physical examination that his abdominal discomfort was caused by
a surgical instrument left after an appendectomy performed 20 years earlier could sue because he could not
have discovered the facts essential to his harm until he
had the examination. Analogizing to the surgical instrument that was hidden from the patient until an exam
made its presence known, so the memory for abuse was
hidden away until it too is discovered and the plaintiff
possesses the facts that are essential to the cause of action.
Within three years of enactment of the Washington
statute, 18 other states enacted similar legislation allowing
for the tolling of the statue of limitations.1 Many other
states introduced bills in the 1991-1992 legislative sessions that would achieve the same result, or they have
begun studying similar legislation. As a consequence, repressed memories now form the basis for a growing number of civil law suits. As one writer put it, "Such wholesale
forgetting—or more precisely, the eventual remembering—is forcing society to grapple in unaccustomed ways
with the old problem of child molestation" (Davis, 1991,
p. 81). Increasing numbers of women, and also some men,
are coming out of therapy with freshly retrieved memories
of abuse. They sue for damages rather than file criminal
complaints, because criminal charges are often too difficult to prove (Davis, 1991). In a few states (e.g., Wyoming), they can also bring criminal charges, and moves
are afoot to change laws in more states to permit criminal
prosecutions to go forward. As a consequence, juries are
now hearing cases in which plaintiffs are suing their parents, relatives, neighbors, teachers, church members, and
others for acts of childhood sexual abuse that allegedly
occurred 10, 20, 30, even 40 years earlier. Juries and
judges are learning about repression of memory and about
newly emerged memories of molestation not only in the
1
Alaska, California, Colorado, Connecticut, Florida, Idaho, Illinois,
Iowa, Maine, Minnesota, Missouri, Montana, Nevada, New Hampshire
(revising an earlier law), Oregon, South Dakota, Vermont, and Virginia
(see, for example, Napier, 1990).
May 1993 • American Psychologist
United States but also in Canada, Great Britain, and other
parts of the world.2
Many interesting questions leap to mind about repressed memories. Chief among them are, How common
are claims of repressed memory? How do people in general and jurors in particular react to claims of recently
unburied repressed memories? What are the memories
like? How authentic are they?
How Common Are Claims of Repressed
Memory?
There is little doubt that actual childhood sexual abuse
is tragically common. Even those who claim that the statistics are exaggerated still agree that child abuse constitutes a serious social problem (Kutchinsky, 1992). I do
not question the commonness of childhood sexual abuse
itself but ask here about how the abuse is recalled in the
minds of adults. Specifically, how common is it to repress
memories of childhood sexual abuse? Claims about the
commonness of repressed memories are freely made: It
is typical to read estimates such as "most incest survivors
have limited recall about their abuse" or "half of all incest
survivors do not remember that the abuse occurred"
(Blume, 1990, p. 81). One psychotherapist with 18 years
of experience has claimed that "millions of people have
blocked out frightening episodes of abuse, years of their
life, or their entire childhood" (Fredrickson, 1992, p. 15).
Later, she reported that "sexual abuse is particularly susceptible to memory repression" (p. 23).
Beliefs about the commonness of repressed memories are expressed not only by those in the therapeutic
community but also by legal scholars who have used these
beliefs to argue for changes in legislation. For example,
Lamm (1991) argued in favor of legislation that would
ease access to the courts for victims of childhood sexual
abuse. She applauded legislation, such as that enacted in
California in 1991, that allows victims, no matter how
old they are, to sue within three years after discovering
their injuries or eight years after reaching majority,
whichever date occurs later. As part of her argument that
victims should have more time to file claims against their
abusers, she expressed a view that "total repression of
memories of abuse is common" (p. 2198).
Despite the confidence with which these assertions
are made, there are few studies that provide evidence of
the extent to which repression occurs. One study (Briere
& Conte, in press) sampled 450 adult clinical clients who
had reported sexual abuse histories. Therapists approached their individual clients or group clients with
this question: "During the period of time between when
the first forced sexual experience happened and your 18th
birthday was there ever a time when you could not remember the forced sexual experience?" The main result
obtained in this largely female (93%) largely White (90%)
sample was that 59% said yes. A yes response was more
likely in cases involving violent abuse (physical injury,
multiple perpetrators, and fears of death if abuse was disclosed) than nonviolent abuse. Reported amnesia was
more likely with early molestation onset, longer abuse,
May 1993 • American Psychologist
and greater current symptomatology. The authors concluded that amnesia for abuse was a common phenomenon (see also Briere, 1992).
Briere and Conte's (in press) result has been taken
by others as evidence for the widespread extent of repression. For example, Summit (1992) interpreted the 59%
yes rate as evidence that this proportion of people "went
through periods of amnesia when they were not aware of
their prior abuse" (p. 22). He used the finding to support
the commonness of childhood dissociation.
One problem with Briere and Conte's (in press) estimate is that it obviously depends on how the respondent
interprets the eliciting question. A yes response to the
question could be interpreted in a variety of ways other
than "I repressed my memory for abuse." For example,
it could mean "Sometimes I found it too unpleasant to
remember, so I tried not to"; or "There were times when
I could not remember without feeling terrible"; or "There
were times I could not bring myself to remember the
abuse because I would rather not think about it." Although no question is free of the possibility of multiple
interpretations, the great potential for idiosyncratic interpretation by respondents to the particular wording used
by Briere and Conte warrants a further examination of
the issue with a different eliciting question.
A further problem with Briere and Conte's (in press)
study is that the respondents were all in therapy. If some
of their clinicians were under the belief that repression
of memory is common, they may have communicated
this belief to their clients. Clients could readily infer that,
if repression of memory is so common, it is likely to have
happened to them, thus the answer to the question is
probably yes. This would, of course, inflate the estimates
of the prevalence of repression.
Other studies have given much lower estimates for
the existence of repression. Herman and Schatzow (1987)
gathered data from 53 women in therapy groups for incest
survivors in the Boston area. Of the 53 cases, 15 (28%)
reported severe memory deficits (including women who
could recall very little from childhood and women who
showed a recently unearthed repressed memory). Severe
memory problems were most likely in cases of abuse that
began early in childhood and ended before adolescence.
Cases of violent or sadistic abuse were most likely to be
associated with "massive repression as a defense" (p. 5).3
An even lower estimate was obtained in a study of
100 women in outpatient treatment for substance abuse
in a New York City hospital (Loftus, Polonsky, & Fullilove,
2
In Stubbings v. Webb and Another, 1991, in which a British plaintiff
claimed she was raped by her adoptive father and brother while she was
a child. She sued 12 years after entering adulthood. The court permitted
her suit, even though she knew she had been raped, arguing that she
might not have associated the mental impairment that she was experiencing with the past rapes until she gained that knowledge as an adult.
3
One curiosity about this report is that the chief investigator published a landmark book on incest six years before this study (Herman,
1981). In the earlier book, the word repression did not even appear in
the index. The 40,incest victims interviewed in depth appeared to have
persisting, intrusive memories.
521
1993). More than one half of the women in this sample
reported memories of childhood sexual abuse. The vast
majority of them remembered the abuse their whole lives.
Only 18% claimed that they forgot the abuse for a period
of time and later regained the memory. Whether the
women remembered the abuse their whole lives or forgot
it for a period was completely unrelated to the violence
of the abuse.
Of course, the data obtained from the New York
sample may include an underestimation factor because
there could have been many more women in the sample
who were sexually abused, repressed the memory, and
had not yet regained it. In support of this hypothesis, one
could point to the research of L. M. Williams (1992),
who interviewed 100 women, mostly African American,
known to have been abused 17 years earlier in their lives.
Of these, 38% were amnestic for the abuse or chose not
to report it. Perhaps there were women in the New York
sample who denied sexual abuse but who were still repressing it. Possibly there are women who were actually
abused but do not remember it; however, it is misleading
to assume that simple failure to remember means that
repression has occurred. If an event happened so early
in life, before the offset of childhood amnesia, then a
woman would not be expected to remember it as an adult,
whether it was abuse or something else. This would not
imply the mechanism of repression. Moreover, ordinary
forgetting of all sorts of events is a fact of life but is not
thought to involve some special repression mechanism.
For example, studies have shown that people routinely
fail to remember significant life events even a year after
they have occurred. One study consisted of interviews
with 590 persons known to have been in injury-producing
motor vehicle accidents during the previous year. Approximately 14% did not remember the accident a year
later. Another study consisted of interviews with 1,500
people who had been discharged from a hospital within
the previous year. More than one fourth did not remember
the hospitalization a year later (U.S. government studies,
cited in Loftus, 1982).
How common are repressed memories of childhood
abuse? There is no absolute answer available. There are
few satisfying ways to discover the answer, because we are
in the odd position of asking people about a memory for
forgetting a memory. For the moment, figures range from
18% to 59%. The range is disturbingly great, suggesting
that serious scholarly exploration is warranted to learn
how to interpret claims about the commonness of repression and what abuse characteristics the repression might
be related to.
speaking, laypeople's implicit or intuitive theories about
repressed memories guide society's thinking on this topic.4
Such implicit theories can also illuminate how therapists'
theories of repression are formed; in part they derive from
a therapist's own implicit theories.
On a more practical level, understanding implicit
theories of repression is important. Plaintiffs' lawyers who
are deciding whether to file repressed memory cases are
eager to know their likelihood of a successful outcome.
Defense lawyers also care, because such subjective probabilities affect their decisions about whether to proceed
to trial or to settle a case early. Perhaps most importantly,
the plaintiffs should care. Plaintiffs bring lawsuits for
myriad reasons. Some therapists encourage their clients
to sue as "hope for emotional justice" (Forward & Buck,
1988). One therapist who had treated more than 1,500
incest victims argued that the lawsuit, although grueling,
is "a very important step towards devictimization," "a
further source of validation," and that "the personal satisfaction can be significant" (Forward & Buck, p. 159).
If the lawsuit is good for a plaintiff's mental health, what
happens to mental health if a jury does not find the notion
of repressed memories tenable and the plaintiff, consequently, does not prevail?
Actual Cases
I start by examining actual cases that have gone to trial
in recent years, with a wide range of outcomes. Some
trials ended in defense verdicts (e.g., Lqfft v. Lofft, 1989,
in San Diego; Collier v. Collier, 1991, in Santa Clara
County). Others ended in plaintiff verdicts. For example,
a 39-year-old woman sued her father in Los Angeles, and
the jury awarded $500,000 (McMillan, 1992). A 33-yearold woman sued her uncle in Akron, Ohio, and the jury
awarded $5.15 million ($ 150,000 in compensatory damages and $5 million in punitive damages; Fields, 1992).
Because the laws are new and most cases have settled,
there are too few actual trials from which to gather data
about reactions to repressed memory claims. Until more
cases are tried to verdict, it may be necessary to rely on
simulated jury research to gather information on this issue.
Simulations
Jurors' Reactions to Repressed
Memory Cases
Several juror simulation studies have explored how people
are likely to react to repressed memory cases (Loftus,
Weingardt, & Hoffman, 1992). In these studies, mock
jurors learned about a legal case that arose out of allegations of sexual assault. Subjects considered the case of
a daughter (Roberta) and her father (Jim), a case modeled
loosely after an actual case tried in the state of Washington
in 1991. Roberta, they learned, accused her father of raping her on several occasions when she was approximately
How do people in general and jurors in particular react
to repressed memory cases? Are memories that were once
previously repressed as credible as memories that were
never repressed? Understanding laypeople's reactions and
credibility judgments is important not only for theoretical
reasons but for practical ones as well. Theoretically
4
Many cognitive psychologists have argued that implicit theories
about any topic (e.g., giftedness) guide a given society's thinking on that
topic (e.g., Sternberg, 1992). More generally, intuitive theories are considered constructing working models of the world that people use in the
service of understanding their world (Medin & Ross, 1992).
522
May 1993 • American Psychologist
10 years old. She claimed she repressed all memory for
these incidents. At about age 20, Roberta's memory returned while she was in therapy. She filed charges against
her father a year after her memory came back. Roberta
and her therapist blamed her current problems of depression, anxiety, and sexual dysfunction on the sexual abuse
that happened when she was 10. Jim denied the allegations, claiming that Roberta was influenced by her therapist's suggestive questioning and that she was looking
for someone or something to blame for her troubles.
How did people react to Roberta's claim? Did their
reactions differ from reactions to a case that was identical
except for the repression of memory? A different set of
subjects reacted to a modified scenario involving a different woman (Nancy) whose memory was not repressed.
Nancy's factual situation was identical to Roberta's except, subjects were told, Nancy never told anyone until
age 20, when she went into therapy and told her therapist.
Who is believed more, Roberta or Nancy? Several consistent findings emerged from these studies. First, people
tended to be slightly more skeptical about Roberta's case
(the repressed memory) than they were about Nancy's
case (the nonrepressed memory). Both male and female
subjects reacted this way, with males overall being more
skeptical.
When subjects disbelieved the claims, they were
more likely to think that the false claims were due to an
honest mistake than a deliberate lie. One small difference
emerged—repressed and nonrepressed memory cases
appear to bring slightly different thoughts to mind. When
subjects considered Nancy's case, thoughts of lying were
slightly more likely to be evoked than when they considered Roberta's case. One of the clearest results was that,
in general, the majority of subjects believed that the claims
of both Roberta and Nancy were true and accurate.
What Are the Memories Like?
The quality of the memories that filter back vary tremendously. They are sometimes detailed and vivid and
sometimes very vague. Sometimes they pertain to events
that allegedly happened in early childhood and sometimes
in adolescence. Sometimes they pertain to events that
allegedly happened 5 years ago and sometimes 40 years
ago. Sometimes they include fondling, sometimes rape,
and sometimes ritualism of an unimaginable sort.
Highly detailed memories have been reported even
for events that allegedly happened more than 25 years
earlier and during the first year of life. One father-daughter case recently tried in Santa Clara County, California,
illustrates this pattern {Collier v. Collier, 1991). The
daughter, DC, a college graduate who worked as a technical writer, claimed that her father sexually abused her
from the time she was six months old until she was 18.
She repressed the memories until the age of approximately
26, when she was in individual and group therapy.
Other cases involve richly detailed allegations of a
more bizarre, ritualistic type, as in a case reported by
Rogers (1992a). The plaintiff, Bonnie, in her late 40s at
the time of trial, accused her parents of physically, sexMay 1993 • American Psychologist
ually, and emotionally abusing her from birth to approximately age 25. A sister, Patti, in her mid-30s at the time
of trial, said she was abused from infancy to age 15. The
allegations involved torture by drugs, electric shock, rape,
sodomy, forced oral sex, and ritualistic killing of babies
born to or aborted by the daughters. The events were first
recalled when the plaintiffs went into therapy in the late
1980s.
In short, reports of memories after years of repression are as varied as they can be. One important way that
they differ is in terms of the age at which the events being
remembered allegedly happened. In many instances, repressed memory claims refer to events that occurred when
the child was one year old or less. This observation invites
an examination of the literature on childhood amnesia.
It is well known that humans experience a poverty of
recollections of their first several years in life. Freud (1905/
1953) identified the phenomenon is some of his earliest
writings: "What I have in mind is the peculiar amnesia
which . . . hides the earliest beginnings of the childhood
up to their sixth or eighth year" (p. 174). Contemporary
cognitive psychologists place the offset of childhood amnesia at a somewhat earlier age: "past the age of ten, or
thereabouts, most of us find it impossible to recall anything that happened before the age of four or five" (Morton, 1990, p. 3). Most empirical studies of childhood amnesia suggest that people's earliest recollection does not
date back before the age of about three or four (Kihlstrom
& Harackiewicz, 1982; Howe & Courage, 1993; Pillemer
& White, 1989). One study showed that few subjects who
were younger than three recalled any information about
where they were when they heard about the assassination
of President Kennedy, although most subjects who were
more than eight at the time had some recall (Winograd
& Killinger, 1983). Although one recent study suggests
that some people might have a memory for a hospitalization or the birth of a sibling that occurred at age two
(Usher & Neisser, in press), these data do not completely
rule out the possibility that the memories are not true
memories but remembrances of things told by others
(Loftus, in press). Still, the literature on childhood amnesia ought to figure in some way into our thinking about
recollections of child molestation that supposedly occurred in infancy.
Are the Memories Authentic?
Therapists Beliefs About Authenticity
Many therapists believe in the authenticity of the recovered memories that they hear from their clients. Two empirical studies reveal this high degree of faith. Bottoms,
Shaver, and Goodman (1991) conducted a large-scale
survey of clinicians who had come across, in their practice, ritualistic and religion-related abuse cases. Satanic
ritualistic abuse (SRA) cases involve allegations of highly
bizarre and heinous criminal ritual abuse in the context
of an alleged vast, covert network of highly organized,
transgenerational satanic cults (Braun & Sachs, 1988;
Ganaway, 1989, 1991). Clients with SRA memories have
523
reported vividly detailed memories of cannibalistic revels
and such experiences as being used by cults during adolescence as serial baby breeders to provide untraceable
infants for ritual sacrifices (Ganaway, 1989; Rogers,
1992b). If therapists believe these types of claims, it seems
likely that they would be even more likely to believe the
less aggravated claims involving ordinary childhood sexual abuse. Bottoms et al.'s (1991) analysis revealed that
30% of responding clinicians had seen at least one case
of child sexual abuse. A detailed analysis of 200 clinicians'
experiences revealed that a substantial number of cases
involved amnesic periods (44% of adult survivor cases).
Overall, 93% of clinicians believed the alleged harm was
actually done and that the ritualistic aspects were actually
experienced by the clients. The conclusion was, in the
investigators' own words, "The clinical psychologists in
our sample believe their clients' claims" (p. 10).
A different approach to the issue of therapist belief
was taken by Loftus and Herzog (1991). This study involved in-depth interviews with 16 clinicians who had
seen at least one repressed memory case. In this small,
nonrandom sample, 13 (81%) said they invariably believed their clients. One therapist said, "if a woman said
it happened, it happened." Another said, "I have no reason not to believe them." The most common basis for
belief was symptomatology (low self-esteem, sexual dysfunction, self-destructive behavior), or body memories
(voice frozen at young age, rash on body matching inflicted injury). More than two thirds of the clinicians reacted emotionally to any use of the term authentic, feeling
that determining what is authentic and what is not authentic is not the job of a therapist. The conclusion from
this small study was that therapists believe their clients
and often use symptomatology as evidence.
These and other data suggest that therapists believe
in their clients' memories. They point to symptomatology
as their evidence. They are impressed with the emotional
pain that accompanies the expression of the memories.
Dawes (1992) has argued that this "epidemic" of belief
is based in large part on authority and social consensus
(p. 214).
Are the Memories Accurate?
There are those with extreme positions who would like
to deny the authenticity of all repressed memories and
those who would accept them all as true. As Van Benschoten (1990) has pointed out, these extreme positions
will exacerbate our problems: "Denial fosters overdetermination, and overdetermination invites denial" (p. 25).
If we assume, then, that some of the memories might
be authentic and some might not be, we can then raise
this question: If a memory is recovered that is not authentic, where would it come from? Ganaway (1989) proposed several hypotheses to explain SRA memories, and
these same ideas are relevant to memories of a repressed
past. If not authentic, the memories could be due to fantasy, illusion, or hallucination-mediated screen memories,
internally derived as a defense mechanism. Further paraphrasing Ganaway, the SRA memories combine a mix524
ture of borrowed ideas, characters, myths, and accounts
from exogenous sources with idiosyncratic internal beliefs. Once activated, the manufactured memories are indistinguishable from factual memories. Inauthentic
memories could also be externally derived as a result of
unintentional implantation of suggestion by a therapist
or other perceived authority figure with whom the client
desires a special relationship, interest, or approval.
The Memories Are Authentic
There is no doubt that childhood sexual abuse is tragically
common (Daro, 1988). Surveys reveal a large range in
the estimated rates (10%-50%), but as Freyd (1991) has
argued, even the most conservative of them are high
enough to support the enormity of child abuse. A sizeable
number of people who enter therapy were abused as children and have always remembered their abuse. Even when
they have severe emotional problems, they can provide
rich recollections of abuse, often with many unique, peripheral details (Rogers, 1992a). Occasionally the abuse
is corroborated, sometimes with very cogent corroboration, such as pornographic photographs. If confirmed
abuse is prevalent, many instances of repressed memory
abuse cases also could be authentic. Unfortunately, in the
repressed memory cases, particularly when memories do
not return for 20 or 30 years, there is little in the way of
documented corroboration. This, of course, does not
mean that they are false.
Claims of corroborated repressed memories occasionally appear in the published literature. For example,
Mack (1980) reported on a 1955 case involving a 27year-old borderline man who, during therapy, recovered
memories of witnessing his mother attempting to kill
herself by hanging. The man's father later confirmed that
the mother had attempted suicide several times and that
the son had witnessed one attempt when he was 3 years
old. The father's confirmation apparently led to a relief
of symptoms in the son. It is hard to know what to make
of examples such as these. Did the son really remember
back to age 3, or did he hear discussions of his mother's
suicide attempts later in life? The memories could be
real, that is, genuine instances of repressed memories that
accurately returned much later. If true, this would only
prove that some memory reports are authentic but obviously not that all reports are authentic. Analogously,
examples of repressed memories that were later retracted,
later proved to be false, or later proved to be the result
of suggestion would only prove that some memory reports
are not authentic but obviously not that all such reports
are illusory.
Some who question the authenticity of the memories
of abuse do so in part because of the intensity and sincerity
of the accused persons who deny the abuse. Many of the
thousands of people who have been accused flatly deny
the allegations, and the cry of "witch hunt" is often heard
(Baker, 1992, p. 48; Gardner, 1991). Witch hunt is, of
course, a term that has been loosely used by virtually
anyone faced by a pack of accusers (Watson, 1992). Analogies have been drawn between the current allegations
May 1993 • American Psychologist
and the witch craze of the 16th and 17th centuries, when
an estimated half million people were convicted of witchcraft and burned to death in Europe alone (Harris, 1974;
Trott, 199 lb). Although the denials during the witch craze
are now seen as authentic in the light of hindsight, the
current denials of those accused of sexual abuse are not
proof that the allegations are false. Research with known
rapists, pedophiles, and incest offenders has illustrated
that they often exhibit a cognitive distortion—a tendency
to justify, minimize, or rationalize their behavior (Gudjonsson, 1992). Because accused persons are motivated
to verbally and even mentally deny an abusive past, simple
denials cannot constitute cogent evidence that the victim's
memories are not authentic.
The Memories Are Not Authentic
To say that memory might be false does not mean that
the person is deliberately lying. Although lying is always
possible, even psychotherapists who question the authenticity of reports have been impressed with the honesty
and intensity of the terror, rage, guilt, depression, and
overall behavioral dysfunction accompanying the awareness of abuse (Ganaway, 1989, p. 211).
There are at least two ways that false memories could
come about. Honestly believed, but false, memories could
come about, according to Ganaway (1989), because of
internal or external sources. The internal drive to manufacture an abuse memory may come about as a way to
provide a screen for perhaps more prosaic but, ironically,
less tolerable, painful experiences of childhood. Creating
a fantasy of abuse with its relatively clear-cut distinction
between good and evil may provide the needed logical
explanation for confusing experiences and feelings. The
core material for the false memories can be borrowed
from the accounts of others who are either known personally or encountered in literature, movies, and television.5
Sources of Details That Could Affect
Memory
There are at least two important sources that could potentially feed into the construction of false memories.
These include popular writings and therapists' suggestions.
Popular Writings
All roads on the search for popular writings inevitably
lead to one, The Courage to Heal (Bass & Davis, 1988),
often referred to as the "bible" of the incest book industry.
The Courage to Heal advertises itself as a guide for women
survivors of child sexual abuse. Although the book is undoubtedly a great comfort to the sexual abuse survivors
who have been living with their private and painful memories, one cannot help but wonder about its effects on
those who have no such memories. Readers who are
wondering whether they might be victims of child sexual
abuse are provided with a list of possible activities ranging
from the relatively benign (e.g., being held in a way that
made them uncomfortable) to the unequivocally abusive
May 1993 • American Psychologist
(e.g., being raped or otherwise penetrated). Readers are
then told "If you are unable to remember any specific
instances like the ones mentioned above but still have a
feeling that something abusive happened to you, it probably did" (p. 21). On the next page, the reader is told
You may think you don't have memories, but often as you begin
to talk about what you do remember, there emerges a constellation of feelings, reactions and recollections that add up to
substantial information. To say, "I was abused," you don't need
the kind of recall that would stand up in a court of law. Often
the knowledge that you were abused starts with a tiny feeling,
an intuition.. . . Assume your feelings are valid. So far, no one
we've talked to thought she might have been abused, and then
later discovered that she hadn't been. The progression always
goes the other way, from suspicion to confirmation. If you think
you were abused and your life shows the symptoms, then you
were. (p. 22)
What symptoms? The authors list low self-esteem,
suicidal or self-destructive thoughts, depression, and sexual dysfunction, among others.6
Others have worried about the role played by The
Courage to Heal. A recent survey of several hundred
families accused by derepressed memories revealed that
the book was implicated "in almost all cases" (Wakefield
& Underwager, 1992, p. 486). Complaints about the book
range from its repeated suggestion that abuse probably
happened even if one has no memories of it and that
demands for corroboration are not reasonable, to its overt
encouragement of "revenge, anger, fantasies of murder or
castration, and deathbed confrontations" (Wakefield &
Underwager, 1992, p. 485). In all fairness, however, it
should be mentioned that the book is long (495 pages),
and sentences taken out of context may distort their intended meaning. Nonetheless, readers without any abuse
memories of their own cannot escape the message that
there is a strong likelihood that abuse occurred even in
the absence of such memories.
The recent incest book industry has published not
only stories of abuse but also suggestions to readers that
they were likely abused even if there are no memories,
that repressed memories of abuse undoubtedly underlie
one's troubles, or that benefits derive from uncovering
repressed memories and believing them. 7 One popular
5
For those who think it is unlikely that one would ever borrow
episodes from movies and popular literature and misremember them as
actual events, one only has to examine Lou Cannon's (1991) biography
of former President Reagan. A curious journalist who tried to verify
Reagan's most famous mismemory of heroism found two that were
suspiciously similar—one in the movie A Wing and a Prayer, and the
other in a Reader's Digest story.
6
Since the publication of Courage to Heal, a number of cases have
emerged in which women were led to believe they were abused, and later
realized their memories were false (Watters, 1993). Lynn Gondolf is a
case in point. During more than a year of therapy she discovered repressed
memories of her father raping her. After she stopped therapy, she realized
that her therapist had "coerced her and the other members of her group
into imagining memories of abuse" (Watters, 1993, p. 26).
'Consider a brief sampling: From Poston and Lison (1990),
"Women usually do not make an immediate incest connection. They
may not recall for years that the incest occurred: memories have an
uncanny way of coming only when the survivor can deal with them"
525
book about incest is the paperback by E. Sue Blume
(1990), the book jacket of which itemizes one of the author's chief credentials as the "Creator of the Incest Survivors' Aftereffects Checklist."8 Blume, a private practice
therapist, tells readers that she has "found that most incest
survivors have limited recall about their abuse" (p. 81).
She goes on to say that "Indeed, so few incest survivors
in my experience have identified themselves as abused in
the beginning of therapy that I have concluded that perhaps half of all incest survivors do not remember that
the abuse occurred" (p. 81).
Some of the volumes provide exercises to help readers lift the repression. Farmer (1989), for example, tells
readers to try one particular exercise "whether or not you
have any conscious recollection of the abuse you suffered"
(p. 91). The reader is to sit down, relax, and mentally
return to childhood. The next step is to choose a particular
memory, whether fuzzy or clear, and "bring that memory
to your full attention" (p. 91). Details about what to do
with the memory are provided, along with an example
from the life of "Danielle," who thought about how verbally abusive her father had been, and "Hazel," who remembered anger at her mother's treating her like a rag
doll. This exercise allegedly helped to "lift the lid of
repression" and unbury the "Hurting Child."
Do these examples lift the lid of repression? Perhaps.
But another equally viable hypothesis is that the examples
influence the creation of memories or, at the very least,
direct the search through memory that the reader will
ultimately take.9
Therapists' Suggestions
Blume's (1990) observation that so many individuals enter therapy without memories of abuse but acquire memories during therapy naturally makes one wonder about
what might be happening in therapy. According to Ganaway (1989), honestly believed but false memories could
come about in another way, through unintentional suggestion from therapists. Ganaway noted a growing trend
toward the facile acceptance and expressed validation of
uncorroborated trauma memories, perhaps in part due
to sensitization from years of accusations that the memories are purely fantasy. Herman (1992, p. 180) made a
similar point: Whereas an earlier generation of therapists
might have been discounting or minimizing their patients'
traumatic experiences, the recent rediscovery of psychological trauma has let to errors of the opposite kind. Some
contemporary therapists have been known to tell patients,
merely on the basis of a suggestive history or symptom
profile, that they definitely had a traumatic experience.
Even if there is no memory, but merely some vague
symptoms, certain therapists will inform a patient after
(p. 193); and "Many women do not remember the incest; how are they
then to connect adult problems with childhood pain?" (p. 196). From
Farmer (1989): "You may have even repressed the memories of the abuse.
The more severe the abuse, the more likely you were to repress any
conscious recollection of it" (p. 52).
526
a single session that he or she was very likely the victim
of a satanic cult. Once the "diagnosis" is made, the therapist urges the patient to pursue the recalcitrant memories. Although some therapists recommend against persistent, intrusive probing to uncover early traumatic
memories (e.g., Bruhn, 1990), others enthusiastically engage in these therapeutic strategies. Evidence for this claim
comes in a variety of forms: (a) therapist accounts of
what is appropriate to do with clients, (b) client accounts
of what happened during therapy, (c) sworn statements
of clients and therapists during litigation, and (d) taped
interviews of therapy sessions.
Therapist accounts.
One therapist, who has
treated more than 1,500 incest victims, openly discussed
her method of approaching clients (Forward & Buck,
1988). "You know, in my experience, a lot of people
who are struggling with many of the same problems you
are, have often had some kind of really painful things
happen to them as kids—maybe they were beaten or molested. And I wonder if anything like that ever happened
to you?" (p. 161). Other clinicians claim to know of therapists who say "Your symptoms sound like you've been
abused when you were a child. What can you tell me
about that?" (Trott, 1991a, p. 18); or worse, "You sound
to me like the sort of person who must have been sexually
abused. Tell me what that bastard did to you" (Davis,
1991, p. 82).
At least one clinician advocated "It is crucial . . .
that clinicians ask about sexual abuse during every intake" (Frawley, 1990). The rationale for this prescription
is that a clinician who asks conveys to the client that the
client will be believed and that the clinician will join with
the client in working through the memories and emotions
linked with childhood sexual abuse. Asking about sexual
abuse along with a list of other past life events makes
sense given the high instance of actual abuse, but the
concern is how the issue is raised and what therapists do
when clients initially deny an abusive past.
Evidence exists that some therapists do not take no
for an answer. One therapist (who otherwise seemed sensitive to problems of memory tampering) still recommended "When the client does not remember what happened to her, the therapist's encouragement to 'guess' or
'tell a story' will help the survivor regain access to the
lost material" (Olio, 1989, p. 6). She went on to provide
the example of a client who suspected sexual abuse but
8
This book proudly displays an endorsement by Gloria Steinem:
"This book, like the truth it helps uncover, can set millions free."
9
Popular writings might also be the source of some questionable
lay beliefs about early memories. Bradshaw (1990, 1992),a leading figure
in the field of recovery and dysfunctional families, invited readers to
consult his "index of suspicion": Do you have trouble knowing what
you want? Are you afraid to try new experiences? If someone gives you
a suggestion, do you feel you ought to follow it? According to Bradshaw,
if you answered even one of these questions "yes," then you "can count
on some damage having been done to you . . . between the 9th and
18th months of your life" (1992, p. 49). How many Bradshaw aficionados
have struggled through their memories trying to find that childhood
trauma?
May 1993 • American Psychologist
had no memories. The client had become extremely anxious at a social gathering in the presence of a three-yearold girl. She had no idea why she was upset except that
she wanted the little girl to keep her dress down. When
encouraged in therapy to tell a story about what was going
to happen to the little girl, the client ultimately related
with tears and trembling one of the first memories of her
own abuse. She used the story to "bypass her cognitive
inhibitions and express the content of the memory" (p.
6). Later she "integrated the awareness that she was indeed
the little girl in the story" (p. 6). One cannot help but
wonder about these mental fantasy exercises in light of
known research showing that the simple act of imagination makes an event subjectively more likely (e.g.,
Sherman, Cialdini, Schwartzman, & Reynolds, 1985).
Even if the therapist does not encourage the client
to guess or tell a story, stories sometimes get told in the
form of client dreams. If discussions of incest go on during
the day, and day residue gets into the dreams at night, it
would not be surprising to see that dreams of incest might
result. Poston and Lison (1990) described a woman with
"repressed memories" of incest who reported a dream
about watching a little girl ice skate on a frozen river. In
her dream, the woman tried desperately to warn the child
that monsters and snakes were making their way through
the ice to devour her. Although frightened, the woman
was powerless and could not warn the innocent child. A
few days later, the client began remembering incest from
her childhood. Knowing she had "a trusted relationship
with a therapist and a survivor's group that would understand and accept her" (p. 197), the memories began
to flow.
Examples of therapists interpreting dreams as signs
of memory of abuse can be found throughout the literature. One clinician described with pride how she communicated to her male patient the basis for her suspicions
that he had been abused: "On many occasions, I explained
that these dreams had preserved experiences and impressions of an indelible nature" (M. Williams, 1987, p. 152).
Frederickson (1992), who has worked with many
incest survivors, has also described in detail her methods
of getting patients to remember. She recommended that
the therapist guide the patient "to expand on or explore
images that have broken through to the conscious mind,
allowing related images of the abuse to surface. The process lets the survivor complete the picture of what happened, using a current image or flash as a jumping-off
point" (p. 97). She also suggested that the therapist help
the patient expand on the images and sensations evoked
by dreams "to shed light on or recover our repressed
memories" (p. 98). She extolled the virtues of hypnosis
to "retrieve buried memories" (p. 98) and recommended
that patients "jot down suspected memories of abuse you
would like to explore. Include your own felt sense of how
you think you were abused" (p. 102).
Even if clinicians are not the first to bring up sexual
abuse, they will often reinforce what begins as a mere
suspicion. One client developed the idea that she might
have been sexually abused, tried hypnosis to help her
May 1993 • American Psychologist
recover memories, and obsessed for years. Only after her
therapist stated that she believed sexual assault was "indeed possible" and cited nightmares, phobia of men, and
other symptoms as evidence did the client come up with
some specific memories (Schuker, 1979, p. 569).
Before leaving the examples of therapist accounts of
what goes on in therapy, it is important to add a word of
caution. Sherrill Mulhern, a psychiatric anthropologist,
has documented the alarming discrepancies that often
exist between therapists' accounts of what they have done
in therapy and what is revealed in video- or audiotapes
of those same sessions (Mulhern, 1991).
If memories are uncovered—whether after repeated
probing, after telling stories, after dreams, or seemingly
spontaneously—or even if the memories remain buried,
therapists often send their clients to support groups. In
one study of clients who had, in the course of therapy,
verbalized their victimization through ritualistic abuse,
the majority reported that they had participated in these
types of groups (Shaffer & Cozolino, 1992). One group,
Survivors of Incest Anonymous (SI A), publishes extensive
reading materials intended to aid the recovery of incest
survivors. (SIA merged with Sexual Abuse Anonymous
in 1987.) The criteria for admission make it clear that
entry is fine for those with no memories of sexual abuse:
"Do you have blocks of your childhood you can't remember? Do you have a sense that 'something happened'? " (SIA, 1985). These and other questions (e.g.,
Do you have problems with self-confidence and self esteem? Do you feel easily intimidated by authority figures?)
are among the set of 20 questions that help a potential
survivor decide whether SIA can be of assistance. SIA
emphasizes that it is OK not to remember at first, because
"Many survivors have 'repressed' actual abuse memories
in order to survive." However, the goal is to remember:
"Participating in SIA helps us to remember what happened to us so we can stop being controlled by incest"
(SIA, 1990, p. 1).
Although support groups are undoubtedly invaluable
for genuine survivors of sexual abuse, as they are for other
survivors of extreme situations, such as combat and political persecution (Herman, 1992, p. 215), concerns about
the incest survivor groups have been expressed. Do these
groups foster the development of constructed memories?
An investigative journalist attending a four-day workshop
watched the construction of memory at work (Nathan,
1992). With members recounting graphic details of SRA
abuse, how long will they listen to the person who can
only say "I think I was abused, but I don't have any memories." Others have worried in the literature that such
groups may induce proto-extension—that is, they actually
encourage a troubled person to remember details from
other survivor stories as having happened to them as well
(Ellis, 1992).
Client accounts. Another source for suggestions
in therapy can be found in client accounts of what happened to them. Recently, clients have been reporting that
a therapist has suggested that childhood abuse was the
cause of their current distress. However, these clients have
527
no memories of such abuse. One woman from Oregon
entered therapy to deal with depression and anxiety, and
within a few months her therapist suggested that the cause
could be childhood sexual abuse. She wrote asking for
help in remembering:
Since that time, he has become more and more certain of his
diagnosis . . . I have no direct memories of this abuse. . . .
The question I can't get past is how something so terrible could
have happened to me without me remembering anything. For
the past two years I have done little else but try to remember.
I've tried self-hypnosis and light trance work with my therapist.
And I even travelled to childhood homes . . . in an attempt to
trigger memories.
One client revealed the suggestive nature of his therapist's questioning on ABC's Primetime Live (ABC News,
1992). Attorney Greg Zimmerman went to a psychotherapist in Boulder, Colorado, to deal with his father's
suicide. He told ABC, "I would try to talk to her about
the things that were very painful in my life and she kept
saying that there was something else" (p. 1). Zimmerman
grew more and more depressed as the mystery of that
"something else" would not unravel, and then, during a
therapy session, his therapist stunned him with her diagnosis: "I don't know how to tell you this, but you display
the same kinds of characteristics as some of my patients
who are victims of Satanic ritualistic abuse" (p. 1). Zimmerman had said nothing whatsoever to her to provoke
this diagnosis, apparently her standard.
It is easy to find published accounts that describe
the emergence of memories in therapy and the techniques
that therapists have used to uncover those memories (e.g.,
Bass & Thornton, 1991). One account, written under the
pseudonym of Jill Morgan, told of a series of positively
horrifying memories of abuse by her father. He raped her
when she was 4 years old, again at age 9, once again at
age 13, for seven straight days and nights at age 15, and
for the final time at age 18. For the next several years, all
misery was withheld from conscious memory, and then,
at age 29, she was helped to remember in therapy:
"Through hypnosis and age regression, a skilled therapist
gave me back my memory" (p. I l l ) . The involvement
of hypnosis and age regression prompts the natural inquiry into whether these techniques produce authentic
memories. Unfortunately, the evidence is discouraging:
There is an extensive literature seriously questioning the
reliability of hypnotically enhanced memory in general
(Smith, 1983), and hypnotic age regression in particular
(Nash, 1987). Hypnotic attempts to improve memory increase the confidence in what is recalled more than the
accuracy (Bowers, 1992). Even more worrisome is the
impossibility of reversing the process; the hypnotically
induced memory becomes the person's reality (Orne,
1979). With hypnotic regression, men and women have
been known to recall being abducted by aliens aboard
exotic spacecraft and other forgotten events (Gordon,
1991).
A more detailed client account is that of Betsy Petersen (1991), as described in an autobiographical ac528
count, Dancing With Daddy. Petersen, a Harvard graduate and accomplished writer, revealed in her first book
that she repressed memory of sexual abuse by her father
until she was 45 years old. She now remembers sexual
abuse from the time she was 3lh until she was 18. Betsy
entered therapy (with "Kris") for problems relating to
her children, and almost a year after starting therapy she
started worrying, "I'm afraid my father did something to
me." She tried hard to recall, putting "together a scenario
of what might have happened" (p. 65). When she told
her therapist about this, she said "I don't know if I made
it up or if it's real." Kris replied, "It feels like a story to
you, because when something like that happens, everybody acts like it didn't." Betsy: "You mean it might really
have happened!" Kris told her there was a good chance
it had happened. Kris told her, in Betsy's words, "It was
consistent with what I remembered about my father and
my relationship with him, and with the dreams I had
been having, and with the difficulties I had being close to
my children, and also, she said, with the feelings I had
during and after sex with my husband" (p. 65). Betsy
worked hard to retrieve incest memories: "I had no
memory of what my father had done to me, so I tried to
reconstruct it. I put all my skill—as a reporter, novelist,
scholar—to work making that reconstruction as accurate
and vivid as possible. I used the memories I had to get
to the memories I didn't have" (p. 66).10 If accurate, this
account tells us something about one therapist's approach.
The therapist convinces the patient with no memories
that abuse is likely, and the patient obligingly uses reconstructive strategies to generate memories that would support that conviction. These techniques can be found in
numerous autobiographical accounts (see also Smith &
Pazder, 1980).
In addition to the first-person accounts, more formal
studies of incest survivors provide clues to what might
be happening in therapy. One study (Shaffer & Cozolino,
1992) of 20 adults who uncovered ritualistic abuse memories stemming from childhood revealed that the majority
sought psychotherapy because of symptoms (e.g., depression and anxiety). The primary focus of their therapy was
"the uncovering of memories" (p. 189). The majority
participated in 12-step programs (e.g., Incest Survivors
Anonymous) as "necessary adjuncts to their psychotherapy" (p. 190). These groups provided substitute families
for the clients who had severed ties with their families of
origin. Other similar studies of ritualistic abuse rememberers have revealed that most of the victims have no
memory of the abuse before therapy (e.g., Driscoll &
Wright, 1991) but that techniques such as hypnosis
(Driscoll & Wright, 1991) or dreams and artwork (e.g.,
Young, Sachs, Braun, & Watkins, 1991) were used by
therapists to unlock those recalcitrant memories.
10
Dancing with Daddy was reviewed in the New York Times by
Culhane (1991). The reviewer called the book "as much a story about
our desperate search for one dimensional solutions to multidimensional
problems as it is a story about incest and its consequences" (p. 18).
May 1993 • American Psychologist
After he stuck his tongue in my mouth—Well, it seemed to last
for hours and hours even though I know it didn't. But it was
awful to me and an event that seemed to last for hours. I started
crying, and I crawled over to the wall. And I started banging
my head on the wall. And my mother came into the room, and
she picked me up. And I tried to tell her in baby talk what had
happened. I said "Ma, ma, ma, ma," and I said, "Da Da, Da,
Da" and I said, "Me-e-e-." And that's all that I can remember.
(Deposition of PB, May 1991, Barton v. Peters, 1990, p. 193)
began to appear. Then the psychologist, with the help of
a detective, "interviewed" Ingram's son. In that interview,
the son reported on his dreams, and the therapist and
detective convinced him that the dreams were real.13
In another case, a father (Mr. K) hired a private
investigator after his 26-year-old daughter reported a recently uncovered repressed memory and accused him of
incest. The investigator, acting under cover, went to see
the daughter's therapist complaining that she had nightmares and had trouble sleeping. On the third visit, the
therapist told undercover agent that she was an incest
survivor. According to the investigator's report (Monesi,
1992), the therapist said this to her pseudopatient: "She
then told me that she was certain I was experiencing body
memory from a trauma, earlier in life, that I could not
remember. I could not remember because my brain had
blocked the memory that was too painful to deal with."
When the patient said she didn't remember any trauma,
the therapist told her "that is the case and many people
at far later times in their lives go through this when the
memory starts to surface." The therapist told her that
many people go through this experience, such as "Viet
Nam Vets, Earthquake Survivors and Incest Survivors."
When the patient said that she had never been in Vietnam
or in an earthquake, the therapist nodded her head and
said "Yes, I know." The therapist then said she should
read Courage to Heal, a book she recommends to all abuse
survivors. After that there was the Courage to Heal Workbook, which tells survivors how to cope with the fears
Later, Patti would remember that her father touched
her in her crotch and put his penis in her mouth when
she was three years old, and that she stroked his penis
over and over at age four. Rape would come later. Patti's
father eventually agreed to give his daughter the deed to
a piece of land he owned, but he continued to deny the
charges. Her brother, a Baptist minister in Alaska, claimed
that Satan's wicked spirits planted untruths in Patti's head
(Laker, 1992). Did it take 30-some sessions for the therapist to uncover actual memories of abuse, or 30-some
sessions for false memories of abuse to begin to be visualized and constructed?
Taped interviews.
Often, confidentiality considerations prevent access to interactions between therapists
and clients. However, when cases get into litigation, special
interviewing is frequently done, and occasionally it is recorded. Recordings were done in a case implicating a
man named Paul Ingram from Olympia, Washington
(Watters, 1991). Ingram was arrested for child abuse in
1988, amid expressions of shock from his community.
At the time he was chair of the county Republican committee and was chief civil deputy in the sheriff's office.
He had worked in law enforcement for more than a decade.
The Ingram case began at a time when waves of
rumor and media hype over satanic ritualistic abuse were
rampant. At first Ingram denied everything, and detectives
told him he was in denial. With the help of a psychologist
who exerted enormous pressure over endless hours of interrogation, Ingram's memories of abusing his daughter
" The laws in Washington and other states were changed in part
due to the efforts of Kelly Barton and Patti Barton of Seattle. Patti's
allegations were described in Seattle newspapers, in Newsweek magazine
(Darnton, 1991), and on the Sally Jesse Raphael show. According to
the Newsweek account, Patti remembered the alleged abuse when she
was in her 30s—too late to sue under the old Washington law. So, with
the help of a lawyer, she lobbied to extend the statute of limitations to
allow victims to bring suit for up to three years after their memory
returns. Patti's father, who denied all charges, moved to Alaska, whereupon Patti turned her efforts on the Alaska legislature and introduced
the same legislation there.
12
In the dream, there was "a fellow who was out to hurt and perhaps
rape her. In the dream she could not get rid of him. In her primal, she
fought him with words until I nudged her on the shoulder with a batacka
(a padded bat), and what I did was, I nudged her on the shoulder and
she was there. . . . She came unglued at that. She took the batacka,
grabbed it, and began striking out. She worked and worked until she got
rid of him at the door. She opened the door of the room and kicked him
out and locked the door. The man looked like her manic-depressive
brother but had mannerisms like Kelly (her husband)" (p. 43).
13
Here is one segment:
Son: "I would have dreams of uh little people. . . short people coming
and walking on me . . . walking on my bed."
Psychologist: "What you saw was real."
Son: "Well, this is a different dream . . . everytime a train came by,
a whistle would blow and . . . witch would come in my window . . . I
would wake up, but I couldn't move. It was like the blankets were tucked
under and . . . I couldn't move my arms."
Psychologist: "You were being restrained?"
Son: "Right and there was somebody on top of me."
Psychologist: "[Son's name] these things happened to you. . . . It's
real. It's not an hallucination."
Before long, the dreams became the reality: The son soon remembered witches holding him down and joining his father in abuse.
Litigation accounts. Information gathered during
litigation is another source of knowledge about the emergence of memories in therapy. Take the case of Patti Barton against her father, John Peters, a successful businessman." Depositions taken in the case of Barton v. Peters
(1990) reveal that Patti Barton began therapy with a Dr.
CD, a doctor of divinity, in July 1986. Dr. CD's notes
indicate that, during the 32nd session of therapy, Patti
expressed "fear her father has sexually tampered with
her" (Deposition of CD, April 21, 1991, Barton v. Peters,
1990, p. 39). This was the first time that anything like
that had come up in any of the sessions. Shortly thereafter,
Patti related a dream that a man was after her.12 Dr. CD
apparently then used the technique of visualization
wherein Patti would try to visualize her past. He got her
to remember eye surgery at the age of 7 months. As for
the abuse, one of the earliest acts of abuse he managed
to dredge up with this method occurred when Patti was
15 months old. "I visualized that my father stuck his
tongue in my mouth."
May 1993 • American Psychologist
529
and memories. She pulled Secret Survivors by E. S. Blume
(1990) from the shelf, opened the cover, and read the list
of symptoms of incest survivors. With two thirds of the
symptoms, she would look at the pseudopatient and shake
her head yes as if this was confirmation of her diagnosis.
She recommended incest survivor groups. In the fourth
session, the diagnosis of probable incest victim was confirmed on the basis of the "classic symptoms" of body
memory and sleep disorders. When the patient insisted
that she had no memory of such events, the therapist
assured her this was often the case.
Why Would Therapists Suggest Things to Their
Patients?
The core of treatment, it is widely believed, is to help
clients reclaim their "traumatic past" (Rieker & Carmen,
1986, p. 369). Therapists routinely dig deliberately into
the ugly underbelly of mental life. They dig for memories
purposefully because they believe that in order to get well,
to become survivors rather than victims, their clients must
overcome the protective denial that was used to tolerate
the abuse during childhood (Sgroi, 1989, p. 112). Memory
blocks can be protective in many ways, but they come at
a cost; they cut off the survivors from a significant part
of their past histories and leave them without good explanations for their negative self-image, low self-esteem,
and other mental problems. These memories must be
brought into consciousness, not as an end in itself but
only insofar as it helps the survivors acknowledge reality
and overcome denial processes that are now dysfunctional
(p. 115).
Another reason therapists may be unwittingly suggesting ideas to their clients is that they have fallen prey
to a bias that affects all of us, known as the "confirmatory
bias" (Baron, Beattie, & Hershey, 1988). People in general,
therapists included, have a tendency to search for evidence
that confirms their hunches rather than search for evidence that disconfirms. It is not easy to discard long-held
or cherished beliefs, in part because we are eager to verify
those beliefs and are not inclined to seek evidence that
might disprove them.
The notion that the beliefs that individuals hold can
create their own social reality is the essence of the selffulfilling prophecy (Snyder, 1984). How does "reality"
get constructed? One way this can happen is through interview strategies. Interviewers are known to choose
questions that inquire about behaviors and experiences
thought to be characteristic, rather than those thought to
be uncharacteristic, of some particular classification. If
therapists ask questions that tend to elicit behaviors and
experiences thought to be characteristic of someone who
had been a victim of childhood trauma, might they too
be creating this social reality?
Whatever the good intentions of therapists, the documented examples of rampant suggestion should force
us to at least ponder whether some therapists might be
suggesting illusory memories to their clients rather than
unlocking authentic distant memories. Or, paraphrasing
Gardner (1992), what is considered to be present in the
530
client's unconscious mind might actually be present solely
in the therapist's conscious mind (p. 689). Ganaway
(1989) worried that, once seeded by the therapist, false
memories could develop that replace previously unsatisfactory internal explanations for intolerable but more
prosaic childhood trauma.
Creation of False Memories
The hypothesis that false memories could be created invites an inquiry into the important question of what is
known about false memories. Since the mid-1970s at
least, investigations have been done into the creation of
false memories through exposure to misinformation.
Now, nearly two decades later, there are hundreds of
studies to support a high degree of memory distortion.
People have recalled nonexistent broken glass and tape
recorders, a cleanshaven man as having a mustache,
straight hair as curly, and even something as large and
conspicuous as a barn in a bucolic scene that contained
no buildings at all (Loftus & Ketcham, 1991). This growing body of research shows that new, postevent information often becomes incorporated into memory, supplementing and altering a person's recollection. The new
information invades us, like a Trojan horse, precisely because we do not detect its influence. Understanding how
we can become tricked by revised data about our past is
central to understanding the hypothesis that suggestions
from popular writings and therapy sessions can affect autobiographical recall.
One frequently heard comment about the research
on memory distortion is that all changes induced by misinformation are about trivial details (Darnton, 1991;
Franklin & Wright, 1991). There is no evidence, the critics
allege, that one can tinker with memories of real traumatic
events or that one can inject into the human mind whole
events that never happened.
Can Real Traumatic Memories Be
Changed?
There are some who argue that traumatic events leave
some sort of indelible fixation in the mind (e.g., "traumatic events create lasting visual images . . . burned-in
visual impressions," Terr, 1988, p. 103; "memory imprints are indelible, they do not erase—a therapy that
tries to alter them will be uneconomical," Kantor, 1980,
p. 163). These assertions fail to recognize known examples
and evidence that memory is malleable even for life's
most traumatic experiences. If Eileen Franklin's memory
of witnessing her father murder her eight-year-old best
friend is a real memory, then it too is a memory replete
with changes over different tellings. However, there are
clearer examples—anecdotal reports in which definite
evidence exists that the traumatic event itself was actually
experienced and yet the memory radically changed.
In the category of documented anecdotes there is
the example of one of the worst public and personal tragedies in the history of baseball (Anderson, 1990; described in Loftus & Kaufman, 1992). Baseball aficionados
May 1993 • American Psychologist
may recall that Jack Hamilton, then a pitcher with the
California Angels, crushed the outfielder, Tony Conigliaro,
in the face with a first-pitch fastball. Although Hamilton
thought he remembered this horrible event perfectly, he
misremembered it as occurring during a day game, when
it was actually at night, and misremembered it in other
critical ways. Another example will be appreciated by
history buffs, particularly those with an interest in the
second world war. American Brigadier General Elliot
Thorpe recalled the day after the bombing of Pearl Harbor
one way in a memoir and completely differently in an
oral history taken on his retirement. Both accounts, in
fact, were riddled with errors (Weintraub, 1991).
Evidence of a less anecdotal, more experimental nature supports the imperfections of personally experienced
traumatic memories. For example, one study examined
people's recollections of how they heard the news of the
1986 explosion of the space shuttle Challenger (Harsch
& Neisser, 1989; Neisser & Harsch, 1992). Subjects were
questioned on the morning after the explosion and again
nearly three years later. Most described their memories
as vivid, but none of them were entirely correct, and more
than one third were wildly inaccurate. One subject, for
example, was on the telephone having a business discussion when her best friend interrupted the call with the
news. Later she would remember that she heard the news
in class and at first thought it was a joke, and that she
later walked into a TV lounge and saw the news, and then
reacted to the disaster.
Another study (Abhold, 1992) demonstrated the
malleability of memory for a serious life-and-death situation. The subjects had attended an important high
school football game at which a player on the field went
into cardiac arrest. Paramedics tried to resuscitate the
player and apparently failed. The audience reactions
ranged from complete silence, to sobbing, to screaming.
(Ultimately, fortunately, the player was revived at the
hospital.) Six years later, many of these people were interviewed. Errors of recollection were common. Moreover,
when exposed to misleading information about this lifeand-death event, many individuals absorbed the misinformation into their recollections. For example, more
than one fourth of the subjects were persuaded that they
had seen blood on the player's jersey after receiving a
false suggestion to this effect.
These anecdotes and experimental examples suggest
that even details of genuinely experienced traumatic
events are, as Christianson (1992) put it, "by no means,
completely accurate" (p. 207).
Can One Inject a Complete Memory for
Something That Never Happened?
It is one thing to discover that memory for an actual
traumatic event is changed over time but quite another
to show that one can inject a whole event into someone's
mind for something that never happened. There are numerous anecdotes and experimental studies that show it
is indeed possible to lead people to construct entire events.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
Piaget's memory.
Whole memories can be implanted into a person's real-life autobiography, as is best
shown by Piaget's classic childhood memory of an attempted kidnapping (Piaget, 1962; described in Loftus
& Ketcham, 1991, p. 19). The false memories were with
him for at least a decade. The memory was of an attempted kidnapping that occurred when he was an infant.
He found out it was false when his nanny confessed years
later that she had made up the entire story and felt guilty
about keeping the watch she had received as a reward. In
explaining this false memory, Piaget assumed, "I, therefore, must have heard, as a child, the account of this story,
which my parents believed, and projected into the past
in the form of a visual memory."
Loud noises at night. Although widely disseminated and impressive at first glance, Piaget's false memory is still but a single anecdote and subject to other interpretations. Was this really a memory, or an interesting
story? Could it be that the assault actually happened and
the nurse, for some inexplicable reason, lied later? For
these reasons it would be nice to find stronger evidence
that a false memory for a complete event was genuinely
implanted.
An apparently genuine 19th-century memory implantation was reported by Laurence and Perry (1983):
Bernheim, during hypnosis, suggested to a female subject
that she had awakened four times during the previous
night to go to the toilet and had fallen on her nose on the
fourth occasion. After hypnosis, the woman insisted that
the suggested events had actually occurred, despite the
hypnotist's insistence that she had dreamed them. Impressed by Bernheim's success, and by explorations by
Orne (1979), Laurence and Perry asked 27 highly hypnotizable individuals during hypnosis to choose a night
from the previous week and to describe their activities
during the half hour before going to sleep. The subjects
were then instructed to relive that night, and a suggestion
was implanted that they had heard some loud noises and
had awakened. Almost one half (13) of the 27 subjects
accepted the suggestion and stated after hypnosis that the
suggested event had actually taken place. Of the 13, 6
were unequivocal in their certainty. The remainder came
to the conclusion on basis of reconstruction. Even when
told that the hypnotist had actually suggested the noises,
these subjects still maintained that the noises had occurred. One said "I'm pretty certain I heard them. As a
matter of fact, I'm pretty damned certain. I'm positive I
heard these noises" (Laurence & Perry, 1983, p. 524).
The paradigm of inducing pseudomemories of being
awakened by loud noises has now been used extensively
by other researchers who readily replicate the basic findings. Moreover, the pseudomemories are not limited to
hypnotic conditions. Simply inducing subjects to imagine
and describe the loud noises resulted in later "memories"
for noises that had never occurred (Weekes, Lynn, Green,
& Brentar, 1992).
Other false memories.
Other evidence shows
that people can be tricked into believing that they experienced an event even in the absence of specific hypnotic
531
suggestions. For example, numerous studies have shown
that people misremember that they voted in a particular
election when they actually had not (Abelson, Loftus, &
Greenwald, 1992). One interpretation of these findings
is that people fill in the gaps in their memory with socially
desirable constructions, thus creating for themselves a
false memory of voting.
In other studies, people have been led to believe that
they witnessed assaultive behavior when in fact they did
not (e.g., Haugaard, Reppucci, Laurd, & Nauful, 1991).
In this study, children aged four to seven years were led
to believe that they saw a man hit a girl, when he had
not, after hearing the girl lie about the assault. Not only
did they misrecall the nonexistent hitting, but they added
their own details: Of 41 false claims, 39 children said it
happened near a pond, 1 said it was at the girl's house,
and 1 could not specify exactly where the girl was when
the man hit her.
Violent false memories
People can hold com-
pletely false memories for something far more traumatic
than awakening at night, voting in a particular election,
or a simulation involving a man and a girl. Pynoos and
Nader (1989) studied children's recollections of a sniper
attack at an elementary school playground. Some of the
children who were interviewed were not at the school
during the shooting, including some who were already
on the way home or were on vacation. Yet, even the nonwitnesses had memories:
One girl initially said that she was at the school gate nearest the
sniper when the shooting began. In truth she was not only out
of the line of fire, she was half a block away. A boy who had
been away on vacation said that he had been on his way to the
school, had seen someone lying on the ground, had heard the
shots, and then turned back. In actuality, a police barricade
prevented anyone from approaching the block around the
school, (p. 238)
The memories apparently were created by exposure
to the stories of those who truly experienced the trauma.
Memories of being lost. A question arises as to
whether one could experimentally implant memories for
nonexistent events that, if they had occurred, would have
been traumatic. Given the need to protect human subjects, devising a means of accomplishing this was not an
easy task. Loftus and Coan (in press), however, developed
a paradigm for instilling a specific childhood memory for
being lost on a particular occasion at the age of five. They
chose getting lost because it is clearly a great fear of both
parents and children. Their initial observations show how
subjects can be readily induced to believe this kind of
false memory. The technique involved a subject and a
trusted family member who played a variation of "Remember the time that. . . .?" To appreciate the methodology, consider the implanted memory of 14-year-old
Chris. Chris was convinced by his older brother, Jim, that
he had been lost in a shopping mall when he was 5 years
old. Jim told Chris this story as if it were the truth: "It
was 1981 or 1982.1 remember that Chris was 5. We had
gone shopping at the University City shopping mall in
532
Spokane. After some panic, we found Chris being led
down the mall by a tall, oldish man (I think he was wearing a flannel shirt). Chris was crying and holding the man's
hand. The man explained that he had found Chris walking
around crying his eyes out just a few moments before
and was trying to help him find his parents."
Just two days later, Chris recalled his feelings about
being lost: "That day I was so scared that I would never
see my family again. I knew that I was in trouble." On
the third day, he recalled a conversation with his mother:
"I remember mom telling me never to do that again."
On the fourth day: "I also remember that old man's flannel shirt." On the fifth day, he started remembering the
mall itself: "I sort of remember the stores." In his last
recollection, he could even remember a conversation with
the man who found him: "I remember the man asking
me if I was lost."
It would be natural to wonder whether perhaps Chris
had really gotten lost that day. Maybe it happened, but
his brother forgot. But Chris's mother was subjected to
the same procedure and was never able to remember the
false event. After five days of trying, she said "I feel very
badly about it, but I just cannot remember anything like
this ever happening."
A couple of weeks later, Chris described his false
memory and he greatly expanded on it.
I was with you guys for a second and I think I went over to look
at the toy store, the Kay-bee toy and uh, we got lost and I was
looking around and I thought, "Uh-oh. I'm in trouble now."
You know. And then I . . . I thought I was never going to see
my family again. I was really scared you know. And then this
old man, I think he was wearing a blueflannel,came up to me
. . . he was kind of old. He was kind of bald on top . . . he
had like a ring of gray hair . . . and he had glasses.
Thus, in two short weeks, Chris now could even remember the balding head and the glasses worn by the
man who rescued him. He characterized his memory as
reasonably clear and vivid.
Finally, Chris was debriefed. He was told that one
of the memories presented to him earlier had been false.
When asked to guess, he guessed one of the genuine
memories. When told that it was the getting-lost memory,
he said, "Really? I thought I remembered being lost. . .
and looking around for you guys. I do remember that.
And then crying. And mom coming up and saying 'Where
were you. Don't you . . . Don't you ever do that again.' "
A false memory of abuse.
The lost-in-a-shop-
ping-mall example shows that memory of an entire mildly
traumatic event can be created. It is still natural to wonder
whether one could go even further and implant a memory
of abuse. Ethically, of course, it would not be possible,
but anecdotally, as it happens, it was done. It is one of
the most dramatic cases of false memory of abuse ever
to be documented—the case of Paul Ingram from Olympia, Washington (Ofshe, 1992; Watters, 1991). As described above, Ingram, was arrested for child abuse in
1988 at the time he was chair of the county Republican
committee. At first Ingram denied everything, and deMay 1993 • American Psychologist
tectives told him he was in denial. After five months of
interrogation, suggestions from a psychologist, and continuing pressure from detectives and advisors, Ingram
began to confess to rapes, assaults, child sexual abuse,
and participation in a Satan-worshiping cult alleged to
have murdered 25 babies (Ofshe, 1992). To elicit specific
memories, the psychologist or detectives would suggest
some act of abuse (e.g., that on one occasion, Ingram and
several other men raped his daughter). Ingram would at
first not remember these fragments, but after a concerted
effort on his part, he would later come up with a detailed
memory.
Richard Ofshe, a social psychologist hired by the
prosecution to interview Ingram and his family members,
decided to test Ingram's credibility. Ofshe had made up
a completely fabricated scenario. He told Ingram that
two of his children (a daughter and a son) had reported
that Ingram had forced them to have sex in front of him.
As with the earlier suggestions, Ingram at first could not
remember this. But Ofshe urged Ingram to try to think
about the scene and try to see it happening, just as the
interrogators had done to him earlier. Ingram began to
get some visual images. Ingram then followed Ofshe's instructions to "pray on" the scene and try to remember
more over the next few hours. Several hours later, Ingram
had developed detailed memories and wrote a three-page
statement confessing in graphic detail to the scene that
Ofshe had invented (Ofshe, 1992; Watters, 1991). Ofshe
(1989, 1992) noted that this was not the first time that a
vulnerable individual had been made to believe that he
had committed a crime for which he originally had no
memory and which evidence proved he could not have
committed. What is crucial about the Ingram case is that
some of the same methods that are used in repressed
memory cases were used with Ingram. These include the
use of protracted imagining of events and authority figures
establishing the authenticity of these events.
These examples provide further insights into the
malleable nature of memory. They suggest that memories
for personally experience traumatic events can be altered
by new experiences. Moreover, they reveal that entire
events that never happened can be injected into memory.
The false memories range from the relatively trivial (e.g.,
remembering voting) to the bizarre (e.g., remembering
forcing one's daughter and son to have sex). These false
memories, with more or less detail, of course do not prove
that repressed memories of abuse that return are false.
They do demonstrate a mechanism by which false memories can be created by a small suggestion from a trusted
family member, by hearing someone lie, by suggestion
from a psychologist, or by incorporation of the experiences of others into one's own autobiography. Of course,
the fact that false memories can be planted tells nothing
about whether a given memory of child sexual abuse is
false or not; nor does it tell how one might distinguish
the real cases from the false ones. These findings on the
malleability of memory do, however, raise questions about
the wisdom of certain recommendations being promoted
in self-help workbooks, in handbooks for therapists, and
May 1993 • American Psychologist
by some therapists themselves. The false memories created in the examples above were accomplished with techniques that are not all that different from what some therapists regularly do—suggesting that the client was probably abused because of some vague symptoms, labeling
a client's ambiguous recollections as evidence of abuse,
and encouraging mental exercises that involve fantasy
merging with reality.
Final Remarks
The 1990s brought a blossoming of reports of awakenings
of previously repressed memories of childhood abuse.
One reason for the increase may be the widespread statistics on sex abuse percentages that are published almost
daily: "By 1980. . . the government tallied almost 43,000
cases of child sex abuse annually" (Nathan, 1991, p. 154);
"One in five women are 'incest victims,' " (p. 155); "6.8
million women nationwide would say they had been raped
once, 4.7 million more than once" (Johnston, 1992, p.
A9); "In 1972, 610,000 [child abuse cases] were reported
nationally, and by 1985 the number had exceeded 1.7
million" (Baker, 1992, p. 37). "If it happens so often, did
it happen to me?" is a question many women and some
men are asking themselves now more than ever before.
The appearance of abuse statistics is one battle in the war
waged against an earlier tendency on the part of society
to disbelieve the abuse reports of women and children—
a tendency that we should all deplore. The repressed
memory cases are another outlet for women's rage over
sexual violence. Although women's anger is certainly justified in many cases, and may be justified in some repressed memory cases too, it is time to stop and ask
whether the net of rage has been cast too widely, creating
a new collective nightmare.
Repressed memories of abuse often return in therapy, sometimes after suggestive probing. Today, popular
writings have been so fully absorbed by the culture that
these too can serve as a source of suggestion that can
greatly influence what happens in therapy and outside of
it (Guze, 1992). The result is memories that are often
detailed and confidently held. Despite lack of corroboration, some of these recollections could be authentic.
Others might not be.
Several implications of these observations follow.
First, we need a renewed effort at research on the problem
of repressed memories. This should encompass, in part,
a reexamination of some of the widely cherished beliefs
of psychotherapists. Is it true that repression of extremely
traumatic experiences is common? Do these experiences
invade us despite the fact that "all the good juice of consciousness has drained out" (Dennett, 1991, p. 325). It
is common to see analogies drawn between Vietnam War
veterans and the incest survivors (e.g., Herman, 1992;
Rieker & Carmen, 1986). Do they share in common the
use of "massive repression" (Wolf & Alpert, 1991, p. 314)
as a mechanism for coping? If so, how do we explain
findings obtained with children who witness parental
murder and other atrocities? In one study (Malmquist,
1986), not a single child aged 5 to 10 years who had
533
witnessed the murder of a parent repressed the memory.
Rather, they were continually flooded with pangs of emotion about the murder and preoccupation with it.
Is it true that repressed material, like radioactive
waste, "lies there in leaky canisters, never losing potency,
eternally dangerous" (Hornstein, 1992, p. 260) and constantly threatens to erupt into consciousness? Psychotherapists have assumed for years that repressed memories
are powerful influences because they are not accessible
to consciousness (Bowers, 1992). Is there evidence for
this assumption? Is it necessarily true that all people who
display symptoms of severe mental distress have had some
early childhood trauma (probably abuse) that is responsible for the distress? With cutting-edge research now
showing that mental distress involves neuronal and hormonal systems of a much wider scope than previously
realized (Chrousos & Gold, 1992; Gershon & Rieder,
1992), should not other potential causes be at least considered?
Questions must also be examined about the wellintentioned treatment strategies of some clinicians. Is it
possible that the therapist's interpretation is the cause of
the patient's disorder rather than the effect of the disorder,
to paraphrase Guze (1992, p. 78)? Is it necessarily true
that people who cannot remember an abusive childhood
are repressing the memory? Is it necessarily true that
people who dream about or visualize abuse are actually
getting in touch with true memories? Good scientific research needs to be done to support these assumptions,
or they should be challenged. Challenging these core assumptions will not be an easy thing to do, anymore than
it was for psychologists of the 1930s to challenge the radical subjectivity of psychoanalysis (Hornstein, 1992), or
for psychologists of the 1980s to challenge the reliability
of the clinical judgments made by psychologists and psychiatrists (Faust & Ziskin, 1988; Fowler & Matarazzo,
1988).'4 Nonetheless, when we move from the privacy of
the therapy session, in which the client's reality may be
the only reality that is important, into the courtroom, in
which there can be but a single reality, then we as citizens
in a democratic society are entitled to more solid evidence.
Until we have better empirical answers, therapists
might consider whether it is wise to "suggest" that childhood trauma happened, to probe relentlessly for recalcitrant memories, and then to uncritically accept them
as fact. Uncritical acceptance of uncorroborated trauma
memories by therapists, social agencies, and law enforcement personal has been used to promote public accusations by alleged abuse survivors. If the memories are fabricated, this will of course lead to irreparable damage to
the reputations of potentially innocent people, according
to Ganaway (1989), who discussed the problem in the
context of SRA memories.
Uncritical acceptance of uncorroborated trauma
memories poses other potentially dangerous problems for
society. According to Ganaway (1991), reinforcing the
validity of unverifiable memories in the therapeutic setting
may lead to diversionary paths in the patient's therapy
away from actual childhood trauma. This could lead to
534
interminable therapy and a total draining of the patient's
financial resources as the therapist and patient collaborate
in a mutual deception to pursue a bottomless pit of memories. Worse, the patients initial wonderings supported
by therapist affirmations could then become fixed beliefs,
precipitating suicidal thoughts and behaviors based on
the new belief system, because the patient would no longer
challenge the veracity of the new memories. Like Betsy
Ross sewing the first American flag, the abuse becomes
a myth that was never true but always will be (E. Frishholz, personal communication, May 1992). Patients who
are reinforced into a new belief system could develop
newer, larger problems. If actual childhood sexual abuse
is associated with numerous negative long-term effects
(e.g., severe sexual dysfunction; Ambrosoe-Bienkowski,
Stahly, & Wideman, 1991), what might be the consequence of implanted childhood sexual abuse? If the
memories are ultimately shown to be false, therapists may
then become the targets of future ethics violations and
lawsuits. They will be charged with a grave form of mind
abuse—charges that have already been initiated in several
states.
What should therapists do instead? As a first step,
it is worth recognizing that we do not yet have the tools
for reliably distinguishing the signal of true repressed
memories from the noise of false ones. Until we gain
these tools, it seems prudent to consider some combination of Herman's (1992) advice about probing for traumatic memories and Ganaway's (1991) advice about SRA
memories. Zealous conviction is a dangerous substitute
for an open mind. Psychotherapists, counselors, social
service agencies, and law enforcement personnel would
be wise to be careful how they probe for horrors on the
other side of some presumed amnesic barrier. They need
to be circumspect regarding uncorroborated repressed
memories that return. Techniques that are less potentially
dangerous would involve clarification, compassion, and
gentle confrontation along with a demonstration of empathy for the painful struggles these patients must endure
as they come to terms with their personal truths.
There is one last tragic risk of suggestive probing
and uncritical acceptance of all allegations made by
clients, no matter how dubious. These activities are bound
to lead to an increased likelihood that society in general
will disbelieve the genuine cases of childhood sexual abuse
that truly deserve our sustained attention.
14
British historian, Hugh Trevor-Roper (1967) pointed out that the
skeptics during the 16th century witchcraft craze did not make much
dent in the frequency of bonfires and burnings until they figured out
that they had to challenge the core belief—that is, the belief in Satan.
REFERENCES
ABC News. (1992, April 2). Primetime Live [Transcript]. Washington,
DC: American Broadcasting Corporation.
Abelson, R. P., Loftus, E. F , & Greenwald, A. G. (1992). Attempts to
improve the accuracy of self-reports of voting. In J. M. Tanur (Ed.),
Questions about survey questions: Meaning, memory, expression, and
social interactions in surveys (pp. 138-153). New York: Russell Sage
Foundation.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
Abhold, J. (1992). [Unpublished doctoral dissertation data.] University
of Arkansas.
Ambroso-Bienkowski, M., Stahly, G. B., & Wideman, K. (1991, August).
Relationship of sexual fantasy and sexual dysfunction to childhood
molestation. Paper presented at the 99th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, San Francisco.
Anderson, D. (1990, February 27). Handcuffed in history to Tony C.
New York Times, p. B9.
Baker, R. A. (1992). Hidden memories. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
Baron, J., Beattie, J., & Hershey, J. D. (1988). Heuristics and biases in
diagnostic reasoning: Congruence, information, and certainty. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 42, 88-110.
Barton v. Peters. (1990). Case No 4FA-90-0157, Superior Court for the
State of Alaska, 4th Judicial District.
Bass, E., & Davis, L. (1988). The courage to heal. New York: Harper &
Row.
Bass, E., & Thornton, L. (1991). / never told anyone: Writings by women
survivors of child sexual abuse. New York: Harper Perennial.
Blume, E. S. (1990). Secret survivors: Uncovering incest and its aftereffects
in women. New York: Ballantine.
Bottoms, B. L., Shaver, P. R., & Goodman, G. S. (1991, August). Profile
of ritualistic and religion-related abuse allegation reported to clinical
psychologists in the United States. Paper presented at the 99th Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco.
Bower, G. H. (1990). Awareness, the unconscious, and repression: An
experimental psychologist's perspective. In J. Singer (Ed.), Repression
and dissociation: Implications for personality, theory, psychopathology,
and health (pp. 209-231). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bowers, K. S. (1992, November 2). Preconscious processes: How do we
distinguish mental representations that correspond to perceived events
from those that reflect imaginal processes? Paper presented at National
Institute of Mental Health workshop "Basic behavioral and psychological research: Building a bridge," Rockville, MD.
Bradshaw, J. (1990). Homecoming. New \brk: Bantam Books.
Bradshaw, J. (1992). Discovering what we want. Lear's, 5, 49.
Braun, B. G., & Sachs, R. G. (1988, October). Recognition of possible
cult involvement in MPD patients. Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Multiple Personality/Dissociative States,
Chicago.
Briere, J. (1992). Studying delayed memories of childhood sexual abuse.
The Advisor (Publication of the American Professional Society on the
Abuse of Children), 5, 17-18.
Briere, J., & Conte, J. (in press). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in
adults molested as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress.
Bruhn, A. R. (1990). Earliest childhood memories: Vol. 1. Theory and
application to clinical practice. New York: Praeger.
Cannon, L. (1991). President Reagan: The role of a lifetime. New York:
Simon & Shuster.
Christianson, S.-A. (1992). Do flashbulb memories differ from other
types of emotional memories? In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.),
Affect and accuracy in recall: studies of "flashbulb" memories (pp.
191-211). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1992). The concept of stress and stress
system disorders: Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis.
JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 1244-1252.
Collier, D. v. Collier, J. (1991, December). Deposition of plaintiff, Case
No. 711752, Superior Court, County of Santa Clara, California.
Culhane, D. (1991, August 4). Sins of the father [Peterson, B. Review
of Dancing with Daddy] New York Times Book Review, p. 18.
The darkest secret (1991, June 10) People, pp. 88-94.
Daro, D. (1988). Confronting child abuse. New York: Free Press.
Darnton, N. (1991, October 7). The pain of the last taboo. Newsweek,
pp. 70-72.
Davis, L. (1991). Murdered memory. In Health, 5, 79-84.
Dawes, R. M. (1992). Why believe that for which there is no good evidence? Issues in Child Abuse Accusations, 4, 214-218.
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
Doe, J. (1991). How could this happen? Coping with a false accusation
of incest and rape. Issues in Child Abuse Accusations, 3, 154-165.
Dormen, L. (1991, April). A secret life. Seventeen, pp. 164-167.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
Driscoll, L. N., & Wright, C. (1991). Survivors of childhood ritual abuse:
Multi-generational satanic cult involvement. Treating Abuse Tbday,
1, 5-13.
Edmiston, S. (1990, November). Daddy's girl. Glamour, pp. 228-231,
280-285.
Ellis, B. (1992). Satanic ritual abuse and legend ostension. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 20, 274-277.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1985). Psychoanalysis: Freud's cognitive psychology. New
York: Freeman.
Erdelyi, M. H., & Goldberg, B. (1979). Let's not sweep repression under
the rug: Toward a cognitive psychology of repression. In J. F. Kihlstrom
& F. J. Evans (Eds.), Functional disorders of memory (pp. 355-402).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Farmer, S. (1989). Adult children ofabusive parents. New York: Ballantine.
Faust, D., & Ziskin, J. (1988). The expert witness in psychology and
psychiatry. Science, 241, 31-35.
Fields, R. (1992, August 4). Hood must pay in sex-abuse case. Beacon
Journal [Akron, OH], p. C5.
Forgetting to remember. (1991, February 11). Newsweek, p. 58.
Forward, S., & Buck, C. (1988). Betrayal of innocence: Incest and its
devastation. New York: Penguin Books.
Fowler, R. D., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1988). Psychologists and psychiatrists
as expert witnesses. Science, 241, 1143-1144.
Franklin, E., & Wright, W. (1991). Sins of the father. New York: Crown.
Frawley, M. G. (1990). From secrecy to self-disclosure: Healing the scars
of incest. In G. Strieker & M. Fisher (Eds.), Self-disclosure in the
therapeutic relationship (pp. 247-259). New York: Plenum Press.
Fredrickson, R. (1992). Repressed memories: A journey to recoveryfrom
sexual abuse. New York: Simon & Shuster.
Freud, S. (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In J. Strachey
(Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of
Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7, pp. 135-243). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1905)
Freyd, J. J. (1991, August 21). Memory repression, dissociative stales,
and other cognitive control processes involved in adult sequelae of
childhood trauma. Paper presented at the Second Annual Conference
on Psychodynamics-Cognitive Science Interface, University of California, San Francisco.
Ganaway, G. K. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying the
role of exogenous trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants.
Dissociation, 2, 205-220.
Ganaway, G. K. (1991, August). Alternative hypotheses regarding satanic
ritual abuse memories. Paper presented at the 99th Annual Convention
of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco.
Ganaway, G. K. (1992). Some additional questions. Journal ofPsychology
and Theology, 20, 201-205.
Gardner, R. A. (1991). Sex abuse hysteria. Cresskill, NJ: Creative Therapeutics.
Gardner, R. A. (1992). True and false accusations of child sex abuse.
Cresskill, NJ: Creative Therapeutics.
Gershon, E. S., & Rieder, R. O. (1992). Major disorders of mind and
brain. Scientific American, 267(3), 127-133.
Gordon, J. S. (1991). The UFO experience. The Atlantic Monthly, 268,
82-92.
Gudjonsson, G. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions
and testimony. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Guze, S. B. (1992). Why psychiatry is a branch of medicine? New York:
Oxford University Press.
Harris, M. (1974). Cows, pigs, wars, and witches: The riddles of culture.
NY: Vintage Books.
Harsch, N., & Neisser, U. (1989, November). Substantial and irreversible
errors in flashbulb memories of the Challenger explosion. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Atlanta,
GA.
Haugaard, J. J., Reppucci, N. D., Laurd, J., & Nauful, T. (1991). Children's definitions of the truth and their competency as witnesses in
legal proceedings. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 253-272.
Herman, J. L. (1981). Father-daughter incest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
535
Herman, J. L., & Schatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of
memories of childhood sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4,
1-14.
Holmes, D. (1990). The evidence for repression: An examination of
sixty years of research. In J. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation:
Implications for personality, theory, psychopaihology, and health (pp.
85-102). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hornstein, G. A. (1992). The return of the repressed. American Psychologist, 47, 254-263.
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of
infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305-326.
Johnston, D. (1992, April 24). Survey shows number of rapes far higher
than official figures. New York Times, p. A9.
Kantor, D. (1980). Critical identity image. In J. K. Pearce & L. J. Friedman (Eds.), Family therapy: Combining psychodynamic and family
systems approaches (pp. 137-167) New York: Grune & Stratton.
Kantrowitz, B. (1991, February 11). Forgetting to remember. Newsweek,
p. 58.
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Harackiewicz, J. (1982). The earliest recollection: A
new survey. Journal of Personality, 50, 134-148.
Kutchinsky, B. (1992). The child sexual abuse panic. Nordisk Sexoligi,
10. 30-42.
Laker, B. (1992, April 14). A nightmare of memories. Seattle Post-Intelligencer, p. Cl.
Lamm, J. B. (1991). Easing access to the courts for incest victims: Toward
an equitable application of the delayed discovery rule. The Yale Law
Journal, 100, 2189-2208.
Laurence, J. R., & Perry, C. (1983). Hypnotically created memory among
highly hypnotizable subjects. Science, 222, 523-524.
LorTt, K. v. Lofft, D. (1989). Complaint for Damages. Case No. 617151,
Superior Court of the State of California for County of San Diego.
Loftus, E. F. (1982). Memory and its distortions. In A. G. Kraut (Ed.),
G. Stanley Hall lectures (pp. 119-154). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Loftus, E. F. (in press). Desperately seeking memories of the first few
years of childhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
Loftus, E. F., & Coan, D. (in press). The construction of childhood
memories. In D. Peters (Ed.), The child witness in context: Cognitive,
social and legal perspectives. New York: Kluwer.
Loftus, E. F, & Herzog, C. (1991). Unpublished data. University of
Washington.
Loftus, E. F., & Kaufman, L. (1992). Why do traumatic experiences
sometimes produce good memory (flashbulbs) and sometimes no
memory (repression)? In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and
accuracy in recall: Studies of "flashbulb" memories (pp. 212-223).
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1991). Witness for the defense. New York:
St. Martin's Press.
Loftus, E. F , Polonsky, S., & Fullilove, M. T. (1993). Memories of childhood sexual abuse: Remembering and repressing. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Washington and Columbia University School
of Public Health.
Loftus, E. F , Weingardt, K., & Hoffman, H. (1992). Sleeping memories
on trial: Reactions to memories that were previously repressed. Unpublished manuscript, University of Washington.
Mack, J. E. (1980). Psychoanalysis and biography: Aspects of a developing
affinity. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 28, 543562.
Malmquist, C. P. (1986). Children who witness parental murder: Posttraumatic aspects. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 320-325.
McMillan, P. (1992, April 28). Woman, 39, wins $500,000 in incest
case. Los Angeles Times, p. B3.
Medin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. (1992). Cognitive psychology. Ft. Worth,
TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Monesi, L. (1992). [Reports of a private investigator, Falcon International
Inc., Columbus, Ohio.]
Morton, J. (1990). The development of event memory. The Psychologist,
J, 3-10.
Mulhern, S. (1991). Satanism and psychotherapy: A rumor in search of
an inquisition. In J. T. Richardson, J. Best, & D. Bromley (Eds.), The
salanism scare (pp. 145-172). New Vbrk: Aldine de Gruyter.
536
Napier, C. W. (1990). Civil incest suits: Getting beyond the statute of
limitations. Washington University Law Quarterly, 68, 995-1020.
Nash, M. (1987). What, if anything, is regressed about hypnotic age
regression? A review of the empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin,
102, 42-52.
Nason girl fought. (1969, December 6). San Jose Mercury.
Nathan, D. (1991). Women and other aliens. El Paso, TX: Cinco Puntos
Press.
Nathan, D. (1992, October). Cry incest. Playboy, pp. 84-88, 162-164.
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections
of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser
(Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of "flashblub" memories
(pp. 9-31). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ofshe, R. (1989). Coerced confessions: The logic of seemingly irrational
action. Cultic Studies Journal, 6, 1-15.
Ofshe, R. J. (1992). Inadvertent hypnosis during interrogation: False
confession due to dissociative state, misidentified multiple personality
and the satanic cult hypothesis. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 125-156.
Oldenberg, D. (1991, June 20). Dark memories: Adults confront their
childhood abuse. Washington Post, p. Dl.
Olio, K. A. (1989). Memory retrieval in the treatment of adult survivors
of sexual abuse. Transactional Analysis Journal, 19, 93-94.
Orne, M. T. (1979). The use and misuse of hypnosis in court. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 27, 311-341.
Petersen, B. (1991). Dancing with daddy: A childhood lost and a life
regained. New York: Bantam.
Petersen v. Bruen (1990). 792 P.2d 18 (Nev. 1990).
Piaget, J. (1962). Plays, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York:
Norton.
Pillemer, D. B., & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by
children and adults. In Advances in child development and behavior
(Vol. 21). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Poston, C , & Lison, K. (1990). Reclaiming our lives: Hope for adult
survivors of incest. New York: Bantam.
Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1989). Children's memory and proximity
to violence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry 28, 236-241.
Rieker, P. P., & Carmen, E. H. (1986). The victim-to-patient process:
The disconfirmation and transformation of abuse. American Journal
of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 360-370.
Ritter, M. (1991, June 30). Sudden recall offorgottencrimesisa puzzler
for juries, experts say. Los Angeles Times, p. A10.
Rogers, M. L. (1992a, March). A case of alleged satanic ritualistic abuse.
Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, San Diego.
Rogers, M. L. (1992b). A call for discernment—natural and spiritual:
Introductory editorial to a special issue on SRA. Journal of Psychology
and Theology, 20. 175-186.
Schuker, E. (1979). Psychodynamics and treatment of sexual assault
victims. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 7, 553573.
Sgroi, S. M. (1989). Stages of recovery for adult survivors of child sex
abuse. In S. M. Sgroi (Ed.), Vulnerable populations: Sexual abuse
treatment for children, adult survivors, offenders and persons with
mental retardation (Vol. 2, p. 112). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Shaffer, R. E., & Cozolino, L. J. (1992). Adults who report childhood
ritualistic abuse. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 20, 188-193.
Sherman, S. J., Cialdini, R. B., Schwartzman, D. F., & Reynolds, K. D.
(1985). Imagining can heighten or lower the perceived likelihood of
contracting a disease. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11,
118-127.
Smith, M. (1983). Hypnotic memory enhancement of witnesses: Does
it work? Psychological Bulletin, 94, 387-407.
Smith, M., & Pazder, L. (1980). Michelle remembers. New York: Congdon
& Lattes.
Snyder, M. (1984). When belief creates reality? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.).
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 247-305).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Snyder, M., & Swann, W. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in social
interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 12021212.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
"A star cries incest." (1991, October 7). People, pp. 84-88.
Sternberg, R. (1992). Five-part theory of giftedness. Paper presented at
the Conference on Developmental Approaches to Identifying Exceptional Ability, Lawrence, KS.
Stubbings v. Webb and Another (1991). 3 Weekly Law Reports 383.
[Cited in Reports and Developments (1992). Expert Evidence: The
International Digest of Human Behaviour, Science, and Law, 1, 26.]
Summit, R. (1992). Misplaced attention to delayed memory. The Advisor
(published by the American Professional Society on the Abuse of
Children), 5, 21-25.
Survivors of Incest Anonymous. (1985). Is Survivors of Incest Anonymous
for you? Baltimore, MD: Author.
Survivors of Incest Anonymous. (1990). Questions and answers. (Distributed at Thursday night San Francisco SIA meeting, October 17).
Susan Nason body found in a dump. (1969, December 3). San Francisco
Chronicle, p. 1.
Telegraphers v. Railway Express Agency. (1944). 321 U.S. 342, 348349.
Terr, L. (1988). What happens to early memories of trauma? A study
of 20 children under age five at the time of documented traumatic
events. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 27, 96-104.
Trevor-Roper, H. R. (1967). Religion, the Reformation, and social change.
London: Macmillan.
Trott, J. (1991a). The grade five syndrome. Cornerstone, 20, 16-18.
Trott, J. (1991b). Satanic panic. Cornerstone, 20, 9-12.
Toufexis, A. (1991, October 28). When can memories be trusted? Time,
pp. 86-88.
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (in press). Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory for four early life events. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General.
May 1993 • American Psychologist
Van Benschoten, S. C. (1990). Multiple personality disorder and satanic
ritual abuse: The issue of credibility. Dissociation, 3, 22-30.
Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R. (1992). Recovered memories of alleged
sexual abuse: Lawsuits against parents. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 10, 483-507.
Washington. (1989). Rev. Code Ann. Sec. 4.16.340 (1989 Supp).
Watson, B. (1992). Salem's dark hour: Did the devil make them do it?
Smithsonian, 23, 117-131.
Watters, E. (1991). The devil in Mr. Ingram. Mother Jones, July-August,
pp. 30-33, 65-68.
Watters, E. (1993). Doors of memory. Mother Jones, January-February,
pp. 24-29, 76-77.
Weekes, J. R., Lynn, S. J., Green, J. P., & Brentar, J. T. (1992). Pseudomemory in hypnotized and task-motivated subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 356-360.
Weintraub, S. (1991, December 4). Three myths about Pearl Harbor.
New York Times, p. A27.
Williams, L. M. (1992). Adult memories of childhood abuse: Preliminary
findings from a longitudinal study. The Advisor, 5, 19-20.
Williams, M. (1987). Reconstruction of an early seduction and its aftereffects. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 15,
145-163.
Winograd, E., & Killinger, W. A., Jr. (1983). Relating age at encoding
in early childhood to adult recall: Development of flashbulb memories.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 413-422.
Wolf, E. K., & Alpert, J. L. (1991). Psychoanalysis and child sexual
abuse: A review of the post-freudian literature. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 8, 305-327.
Young, W. C , Sachs, R. G., Braun, B. G., & Watkins, R. T. (1991).
Patients reporting ritual abuse in childhood: A clinical syndrome report of 37 cases. International Journal of Child Abuse and Neglect,
15, 181-189.
537