Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Pedagogy, Culture & Society ISSN: 1468-1366 (Print) 1747-5104 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpcs20 Reading two rhizomatic pedagogies diffractively through one another: a Reggio inspired philosophy with children for the postdevelopmental child Karin Murris To cite this article: Karin Murris (2017): Reading two rhizomatic pedagogies diffractively through one another: a Reggio inspired philosophy with children for the postdevelopmental child, Pedagogy, Culture & Society To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14681366.2017.1286681 Published online: 20 Mar 2017. Submit your article to this journal View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpcs20 Download by: [169.1.144.18] Date: 20 March 2017, At: 04:07 PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY, 2017 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14681366.2017.1286681 Reading two rhizomatic pedagogies difractively through one another: a Reggio inspired philosophy with children for the postdevelopmental child Karin Murris School of Education, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch, South Africa ABSTRACT KEYWORDS After situating the iguration of the postdevelopmental child in the context of hegemonic colonising developmental discourses about child rearing and education, I engage with posthumanist perspectives that rupture the binaries, power relations and age discrimination these discourses assume. Developmentalism raises concerns about how child as knower is positioned. Drawing on the ground-breaking work of feminist philosopher and quantum physicist Karen Barad, an airmative difractive methodology is used with two approaches to teaching and learning: Reggio Emilia and Philosophy with Children (P4C). Through the experimental difractive reading of the rhizomatic concept development that is core to both educational philosophies, new ideas are proposed for postdevelopmental curriculum construction. A new pedagogical practice of working with concepts is created, thereby deterritorialising what conceptual knowledge is. By putting philosophy at the heart of deterritorialising knowledge, room is made for children as knowledge producers, rather than knowledge consumers. Philosophy for children; Reggio Emilia; critical posthumanism; posthuman child; postdevelopmental curriculum; intra-active pedagogies; diffractive methodology Postdevelopmental child Child subjectivity is constructed within a web of knowledge claims, drawn mostly from Western versions of histories, institutions, economies, politics and practices which are riddled with distinctions, neo-liberal norms and yardsticks by which children’s progress and development is measured and found wanting (Dahlberg and Moss 2005; Dahlberg, Moss, and Pence 2013; Viruru 2005). In the ields of childhood studies and early childhood education, entangled connections have been made between colonialism, imperialism, the institutionalisation of childhood, capitalist discourses of progression, and so-called ‘natural’ development (Burman 2008; Cannella and Viruru 2004; Taylor and Pacini-Ketchabaw 2015). Notions of progress and reason have colonised education through its curriculum construction that positions children in need of recapitulating the development of the species (Matthews 1994): children are regarded as simple, concrete, immature thinkers who need age-appropriate CONTACT Karin Murris Karin.murris@uct.ac.za © 2017 Pedagogy, Culture & Society 2 K. MURRIS interventions in order to mature into autonomous fully-human beings. Since modernity, the mature, white, able-bodied, heterosexual man of humanism (Braidotti 2013) is the yardstick by which ‘normal’ and ‘natural’ child development is measured with children at the lowest level of the patriarchal hierarchy (Cannella and Viruru 2004, 109). Throughout the history of Western thought, education has been regarded as the formation of childhood (Kohan 2015; Stables 2008). Dominant educational discourses and material practices position child1 as a lesser human being, marginalised and excluded, despite sustained academic critique from many disciplinary quarters in higher education. For decades, scholars in childhood studies and early childhood education have argued that the normative knowing subject is assumed to be of a particular age (adult) (Burman 1994; Cannella and Viruru 2004; Dahlberg, Moss, and Pence 2013; Fendler 1998; Jenks 1996; Walkerdine 1984). Although there has been a shift in childhood studies from psychology of childhood, to sociology of childhood, to philosophy of childhood, psychology is still the dominant discipline in early childhood education policies, practices and curriculum design globally (File, Basler Wisneski, and Mueller 2012). This orientation informs curricula that move from the concrete to the abstract, the simple to the complex and from the familiar to the unfamiliar (Egan 1992, 1993, 1997). It assumes that knowledge is acquired through representation of a world, not immersion, thereby presupposing a language/reality dualism: sign systems created by humans represent a world inhabited by independently existing, passive objects without agency. Pedagogies that focus on the psychological, the social or the discursive only, and do not acknowledge the material as having agency in knowledge production, assume this human exceptionalism (relexive self-consciousness), and with it the humanist ‘two-world view’: childhood vs. adulthood. Through critical engagement with postdevelopmental perspectives we can begin to address existing power relations and age discrimination. The iguration of postdevelopmental or posthuman child ruptures humanist binaries and is a lever to rethink subjectivity more generally (Murris 2016). My overall argument is driven by a desire to show how matters of ontology and epistemology have implications for ethical relationships in educational institutions, and that they cannot, and should not, be reduced to apolitical governmental concerns about eicacy and standardisation. Critical posthumanism Critical posthumanism as navigational tool ofers a new imaginary for postdevelopmental pedagogies as it moves beyond predeined boundaries, assumed categories of diference and language. For posthumanists, the latter has been ‘substantializing’, allowing semiotic systems to determine our understanding of the world (Barad 2007, 133). Language has become an epistemic tool to have power over less-fully humans and more-than-humans. For example, for Deleuze and Guattari (1987), everything in our thinking lows from the habit of saying ‘I’. Through language, the self is named and cut apart from other selves and things, also when we talk about diversity and diference. Feminist philosopher and quantum physicist Barad (2007, 134) describes how this ‘I’ includes or excludes the other ‘I’s – a distancing or cutting apart from the ‘other’ human that is not ‘fully’ formed or developed (as yet) – the ‘I’ who is ‘disabled’, ‘female’, ‘child’, ‘black’, ‘poor’. PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 3 A postdevelopmental move away from the exclusive anthropocentric focus on language and the discursive in education is particularly urgent to enable more just educational encounters with people who are not only young, but who might also live in poverty and not have English as their ‘home’ language. Critical posthumanism does so by removing language as the main hub of knowledge production and with it the ‘fully-human’ sophisticated language speaker of age as the sole producer of knowledge. Deleuze and Guattari (1987, 89) and Adkins (2015, 67) give the example of bees and how they can communicate messages about food sources they have seen to other bees. But bees who do not have that experience cannot communicate that information. They can transmit signs, but their language is limited to irst-hand accounts. In contrast, human animals have a language about language, which makes it possible to transmit messages about the experiences without ever having experienced it, but on the basis of ‘hearsay’. This second-order language has become equated with real knowledge that in turn has to be somehow ‘transmitted’ (or ‘facilitated’ or ‘mediated’) to children, who are themselves less competent language users. Deleuze and Guattari (1987, 161) propose bold experimentation, not interpretation or signiication: ‘Find your own places, territorialities, deterritorializations, regime, lines of light! Semiotize yourself instead of rooting around in your prefab childhood and Western semiology’. Posthumanism disrupts the anthropocentric nature of binary thinking, and ofers a new ontology that enables a re-evaluation of child. As Barad herself puts it (2007, 378), posthumanism welcomes ‘females, slaves, children, animals, and other dispossessed Others (exiled from the land of knowers by Aristotle more than two millennia ago) into the fold of knowers’. Matter is also among the ‘dispossessed Others’ and ontologically entangled in all knowledge construction. The removal of the dominance of language (reading and writing) in communicating and creating new knowledge complexiies what knowledge is, thereby also doing justice to the material world of which humans are a part. Barad has introduced the helpful neologism intra-action (Barad 2007, 2013, 815). Intraaction does not presuppose individualised existence – not only of subjects, but also of objects ‘in’ the world – and like other critical posthumanists, Barad emphasises mutual relationality: things ‘are’ because they are in relation to and inluencing each other. Intra-action is diferent from ‘interaction’ in that ‘nature’ and ‘culture’ are never ‘pure’, are never unafected by each other, but are always in relation (Barad 2007, 152). Bodies (including human bodies) are unbounded quantum entanglements constituted by concepts and material forces, where the social, the political, the biological, and its observing, measuring and controlling machines are interwoven and entwined. Importantly, these mutual entanglements are not unities, as each bit of matter, each position in space, at each moment of time, is not ‘a blending of separate parts or a blurring of boundaries, but in the thick web of its speciicities, what is at issue is its unique material historialities and how they come to matter’ (Barad 2014, 176). As Barad (2014, 178) explains: ‘Quantum entanglements are not the intertwining of two (or more) states/entities/events, but a calling into question of the very nature of twoness, and ultimately of one-ness as well’. Entangled elements have agentic force, which opens up possibilities of including a much wider selection of causal factors for understanding what is going on (as a matter of fact) in knowledge production. Barad’s scientiic study of small organisms has called into question the individual/group binary and problematises the nature of identity and individual existence. These tiny creatures 4 K. MURRIS show behaviours and ways of thinking that traditionally would have been located in the brain but in organisms that have none, for example the brittle star. Barad (2007, 369–384) argues that the brainless and eyeless brittle star is a ‘living testimony’ of the idea that knowing, being and doing are inseparable (Barad 2007, 369). Relationships are necessary for uniqueness to be articulated, for a subject to become singular in an ethical sense, and child subjectivity includes relationships with other earth dwellers such as brittle stars, other human animals, nonhuman animals and the material world (e.g., sticks, furniture, glue or paint, but also questions, thoughts and concepts). The key pedagogical questions this raises is who, where and how are child’s material-discursive beginnings taken up and materialised in formal and informal education? What does ‘taking up’ mean and what is the role of the teacher/learners in this process of ‘taking up’? Could/are teachers also learners, and vice versa, learners also teachers. And what are the response-abilities of educators in these complex situations? A difractive reading I suggest that these questions can be answered through a difractive reading of two rhizomatic pedagogies that do justice to the ontology of postdevelopmental child. Although difractive readings are gaining traction, the manner by which the method is used cannot be prescribed for the researcher, because the person doing the research is always already part of the apparatus that measures. I have used Barad’s article Difraction Difractions: Cutting Together-Apart (2014) as my main guidance. The method is airmative, creative, intuitive and non-representational. Posthumanists favour non-representational methodologies that move beyond the personal and avoid psychological, psycho-analytical or sociological interpretations that involve relection on the past (Coleman and Ringrose 2013; Koro-Ljungberg 2016; Lather 2016; Patel 2016; St Pierre, Jackson, and Mazzei 2016; Taylor and Hughes 2016; Vannini 2015). Difraction is a postqualitative methodology developed, irst by Haraway (1988), and built on by Karen Barad through her interpretation of quantum physics (2003, 2007, 2014), but importantly should not be understood as a metaphor, which would imply representationalism. The implications of using a difractive methodology for pedagogy and educational research have been explored only recently by Lenz Taguchi (2010), Palmer (2011), Hultman and Lenz Taguchi (2010), Jackson and Mazzei (2012), and Murris (2016). In this article, I read difractively through one another, human and more-than-human bodies, in particular Barad’s notion of intra-action, the Deleuzian notion of concept, philosophical concepts as theorised and practised in Philosophy with Children (P4C), and Reggio Emilia’s concept formation through the use of the ‘hundred languages’ at the heart of progettazione (explained below). Reading texts, theories about concepts, and the neologism ‘intra-action’ difractively through one another, ofers a non-representational way of ‘considering the entanglement of bodies, texts, relationships, data, language, and theory that we are just beginning to understand and that presents the possibility of much productive potential’ (Mazzei 2014, 745). Like Barad’s scientiic study of small organisms, the difractive method calls into question the individual/group binary and problematises the nature of identity and individual existence. I, myself as researcher, am also part of the entanglement that is not a unity. In other words, the posthuman philosophy brings forth its own posthuman method of analysis: difraction queers binaries such as object/subject, cognition/emotion, world/researcher. This is why I do not use rational argumentation as PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 5 my method in this article, for example, by putting forward arguments for a pedagogical practice that combines certain aspects of Reggio inspired practices and P4C on the basis of evidence of the similarities and diferences between the two. Representational methods of research position the researcher (the subject) at a distance from the world, with so-called ‘objective’ access to this world with the mind through cognition. Instead, my difractive analysis moves rhizomatically in ceaseless variations leading in indeterminate directions, including unpredictable causal factors, and opening up a space for human and more-thanhuman bodies to also produce afect and intensities, thereby keeping analysis and knowledge production on the move. Knowledge production is both material and discursive, and includes the more-than-human. The experiment creates a new interference pattern, a practice of knowledge construction that queers (an undoing of identity) the powerproducing binaries of humanism: mind/body, nature/culture, child/adult, language/reality, knowing/being and individual/group. Difraction as a methodology is diferent from relection. Difraction means ‘to break apart in diferent directions’ (Barad 2014, 168). Difraction patterns hold for water waves, as well as sound waves, or light waves (Barad 2007, 74). It is where they interfere or overlap that the ‘waves change in themselves in intra-action’ (Lenz Taguchi 2010, 44) and create a ‘superposition’ (Barad 2007, 76). So how does the difractive methodology work in this case? The making of the new interference pattern does not happen by rejecting or critiquing one text or theory, or by looking for the known or what is similar (as in relection), but through airmation of each educational philosophy by paying attention to the diferences that matter – the power-producing binaries that include or exclude. The difractive activity of reading these philosophies through one another is methodologically a ‘cutting togetherapart’ as one move (Barad 2014). The ideas remain entwined, nothing is ‘left behind’, because the diference does not exist within ‘itself’, but is exterior. Barad’s approach is to place diferent transdisciplinary practices in conversation with one another whilst paying attention to ine details and the exclusions this action produces by investigating how ‘objects’ and ‘subjects’ and other diferences matter, and for whom (Barad 2007, 90–94, 2014). In my difractive reading, I am not looking for similarities or diferences; I make no comparisons or try and identify themes, but guided by the above questions and further questions that e/merge, new pedagogical ideas of working with concepts is created, thereby deterritorialising what conceptual knowledge is, which also includes child as knowledge co-creator. What e/merges are new difraction patterns that include and that do not reduce one of these rhizomatic pedagogies to the other. My reading is not guided by a ‘lack’ either. The superposition created by the difraction is not ‘critical’, but adds force to ‘both’, without assuming that either is a unity, nor the interference pattern that has been created. The challenge has been not to theorise the difraction pattern, but to put it into practice in this article. Unlike a literature review, I have focused on carefully chosen citations that do the work of making the reader feel and think diferently about pedagogical practice. I am reading theory with practice difractively. As part of the difractive analysis, I briely introduce the two educational philosophies with a deliberate focus on the material-discursive force of the embedded and embodied life experiences of the originators of these approaches to teaching and learning and the salient implications for a particular conception of child. The e/mergence of both Reggio and P4C can itself be understood as a difraction with certain geo-political events, in this case war. This is followed by a difractive reading focusing on conceptual knowledge construction. 6 K. MURRIS Reggio Emilia Reggio Emilia is a city in the Emilia Romagna region of Northern Italy. It is famous for its development of an early education system, often referred to simply as ‘Reggio’, or ‘Reggioinspired practice’. It is not a method or prescribed curriculum to follow and to copy, but a socially and culturally embedded educational approach and philosophy (Stremmel 2012, 134). According to its founder, Loris Malaguzzi, its history started six days after the end of the Second World War, in the Spring of 1945 (Malaguzzi 1998, 49). He had heard that a group of mothers in nearby Villa Cella wanted to build and run a school for young children, so, a teacher himself, he literally jumped on his bike and joined them. In an interview, Malaguzzi (1998, 57) beautifully describes the force of the material in that process of starting a school: ‘…you have to agree that seeing an army tank, six horses, and three trucks generating a school for young children is extraordinary’. They had been left by the Germans, and the irst preschool was inanced by selling them and then built by their own hands (Malaguzzi 1998, 49–51). Run by parents in the poorest parts of this village, they struggled at irst to communicate well with the children, who were often hungry, in poor health and spoke a local dialect. But these socioeconomic, cultural and material factors became a positive opportunity. In Loris Malaguzzi’s own words: ‘A simple, liberating thought came to our aid, namely that things about children and for children are only learned from children’ (Malaguzzi 1998, 51). It was the ‘irst spark’ (Malaguzzi 1998, 50) of a now globally well-known and admired (secular2) approach to early childhood education. The irst municipally funded preschool opened in 1963. Concerned about Italy’s political role during the Second World and its fascism, a philosophical practice developed (and is still developing) that regards schools as places for democratic conversation, critical and creative thinking and caring relationships (Stremmel 2012, 134). Reggio is a living organism – it shapes itself through the various psychological, sociological and philosophical theories that theorists bring to the practice. The kind of education it inspires cannot simply be replicated or imitated in other settings. In efect, to avoid domination by hegemonic theories of child and childhood, Reggio practitioners continually engage with educational theories critically and overtly in collaborative dialogue with significant others: parents and colleagues. In that way, a school becomes an enquiring, inexhaustible and dynamic ‘living organism’ (Malaguzzi 1998, 62, 63). The literature suggests that Reggio does not exclude developmental psychologists, social constructionists, social constructivists, pragmatists, phenomenologists, poststructuralists or postmodernists (Cagliari et al. 2016, 377), although the relational pedagogy certainly resonates more with some than with others. Theories and practices are constantly evolving, shifting and changing like a rhizome. Unlike ‘arborescent’ systems of thought and binary logic that have dominated western epistemological concepts (e.g., ‘root’, ‘foundation’, ‘ground’), rhizomes (e.g., ginger, couchgrass, rats) are multiple and move dynamically and unpredictably ceaselessly establishing connections (Deleuze and Guattari 1987, 5–7). In fact, the rhizomatic nature of the Reggio school curriculum is echoed in how their schools are run and managed. Core to Reggio is the view of child as rich, resilient and resourceful. In an interview, Malaguzzi describes how he sees child as part of the world: ‘an active co-habitant’. Disrupting the innate (nature) and acquired (culture) binary, he argues for ‘an ecological child…an PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 7 organism disposed to interaction and active self-construction’ (Malaguzzi 1990 in Cagliari et al. 2016, 375). Malaguzzi (1990, in Cagliari et al. 2016, 374; emphasis in the original) explains: …as new-born babies they weave dynamic processes of interactive co-existence and growth with life, and from birth they continue with their speciic ways of relating. Their interactions with adults, cultures, environment, things, shadows, colours, spaces, times, sounds, smells and tastes, immediately situate them in a world of communication and exchange. The iguration of ecological child disrupts humanism’s power producing-binaries (Malaguzzi 1990 in Cagliari et al. 2016, 375) and the pedagogical possibilities this enables for looking afresh at knowledge production will e/merge in my difractive reading of Reggio and P4C. Philosophy with children Philosophy with children (P4C) was pioneered in the late 1960s by philosopher Lipman (1988, 1991), whose passion to efect real change in the world was also ignited by the Second World War and its after efects; he had served as a soldier on foreign soil in Europe. Moreover, lack of critical thinking about the Vietnam War by his fellow Americans (including his own undergraduate philosophy students) gave rise to his inspirational idea to start with teaching philosophy in childhood. Essential for a well-functioning truly democratic society, he speculated that early intervention through a specially written curriculum (see: Lipman, Sharp, and Oscanyan 1977) would tap into children’s original curiosity, sense of wonder and enthusiasm for intellectual enquiry, and strengthen their philosophical thinking. In his autobiography, Lipman (2008, 53) explains the salient inluence on his philosophy of education by Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky and the philosophy of, and personal relationship, he had with John Dewey. Lipman was particularly attracted to study philosophy as an academic discipline by its concerns with thinking about thinking (Lipman 2008, 58). Kennedy (2011, 96) describes the revolution Lipman and his colleagues at The Institute for the Advancement of Philosophy for Children (IAPC)3 brought about as ‘the radical reconstruction of philosophy as dialogue, and dialogue, moreover, among children’. This revolution – the emergence of philosophy of childhood – was also supported by philosopher, Gareth Matthews, who questioned Piagetian developmentalism, and argued for child as ‘natural philosopher’. His highly accessible books (1980, 1984, 1994) are littered with arguments and dialogues with children that exemplify that children’s thinking is similar to that of well-known adult philosophers. Advocates of philosophy with children disagree about their views of child (Haynes and Murris 2012, 2013; Murris 1997, 2000, 2015). For example, Kohan (1998, 2002, 2011, 2015), Kennedy (1996, 2000), Haynes (2008, 2009, 2014), Stanley and Lyle (2017) have been a strong voice in arguing against eforts to include children in the rational world of adults. They see it as problematic to use adult philosophy as the norm. The encounter between philosophy and childhood opens up the challenge for adults of a diferent form of reason and knowledge, resulting in diferent philosophies that children may bring to academic philosophy itself. Despite these diferences between P4C proponents, childhood became the site for a radical democratisation of academic philosophy, and, educationally, Lipman combined the ideas of Paulo Freire (democratic dialogue) and John Dewey (education as enquiry) into the community of enquiry pedagogy. Resisting the representationalist demand for a deinition, I put the P4C pedagogy at the centre of practice in my difractive reading and foreground 8 K. MURRIS how concepts are constructed and reconstructed and who and what is included and excluded. Rhizomatic concept formation – a difractive reading Like ‘an earthworm’ making compost, I start by turning ‘the soil over and over – ingesting and excreting it, tunnelling through it, burrowing, all means of aerating the soil, allowing oxygen in, opening it up and breathing new life into it’ (Barad 2014, 168). For the remainder of this article, carefully selected quotes, sometimes followed by comments or questions, interspersed with practical information informed by theory, are an ‘agential cut’ (Barad 2007). Agential cuts are speciic intra-actions: they are about matter and meaning at the very same time. Agential cuts are part of what Barad (2007, 183) calls ‘an apparatus’ – a doing, not a thing. Apparatuses are boundary-making practices and include and exclude (Barad 2007, 148), because boundaries become determinate through the agential cuts and the intraactions; and it is here that the ethical responsibility of the educator becomes clear as part of knowledge production and curriculum choices. It is not the case that there are no separations or diferentiations, but they are always within relationships (Barad 2012, 77). The posthuman methodology I have used here aims to breathe new life into existing pedagogical practices and create an interference pattern that disrupts humanist binaries, in particular the child/ adult, learner/teacher binary. The assemblage is a response to the key questions I raised earlier. How can people respond to children’s initial curiosity, sense of wonder and enthusiasm for learning in formal and informal educational encounters? How can critical posthumanism help create postdevelopmental pedagogical practices that position children as knowledge producers and co-creators, not as knowledge consumers? In order to answer these questions, I focus on the practice of concept formation in communities of enquiry and have let myself be afected by the writings of Barad, Malaguzzi, Lipman, Deleuze and Guattari and a few other philosophers. The idea is to look afresh at pedagogy and although I start at the beginning of what could constitute ‘a lesson’, it is not linear in terms of chronology – it is always possible to start in the ‘middle’ as Deleuze and Guattari (1987, 25) explain: a rhizome has no beginning or end; it is always in the middle, between things, interbeing, intermezzo … The tree imposes the verb ‘to be’, but the fabric of the rhizome is the conjunction, ‘and … and … and’. Instead of starting from ‘the beginning’, they ‘proceed from the middle, through the middle, coming and going rather than starting and inishing’. The idea is to provide an imaginary of what a philosophical education could look like that moves beyond lessons as units, prescribed lesson plans and curricula and individualised age-appropriate practice, through the deterritorialising practice of a transdisciplinary philosophical exploration of concepts that ‘meets the universe halfway’ (Barad 2007). The energy and dynamism of such playful activities produce strategies, not an implementation of a programme4. The quotations and the surrounding text difract with each other, sometimes in terms of highlighting the newly created, other times, in a question-answer format. The assemblage works as a Baradian apparatus to afect the reader and produce an ethical and political commitment to particular pedagogical strategies, such as provocation, dwelling, questioning, rhizomatic concept building and communal thought creating. These material-discursive practices ‘render each other capable’ (Haraway 2016, 18) and bring into PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 9 existence a iguration of child as rich, resilient and resourceful. Deliberately, no attempt has been made to capture the essence of the quotes that are part of this assemblage through, for example, paraphrasing, or introducing the citations. Instead, care-fully placed in a particular sequence and through the use of diferent fonts and font size, the vitality, intensity, and aesthetic force of the philosopher’s own words have been left to do their carefully staged, unpredictable work. This choice of methodology is a material-discursive expression and acknowledgment of the author’s entanglement with the research apparatus. In posthuman researchpractice the researcherpractitioner cannot stand back at an epistemological distance, taking up a metacognitive position as in relection. Knowledge creation is part of the world’s ininite becoming: ontology and epistemology e/merge (Barad 2007). Such an ontoepistemology is at the heart of protagezzione, the term used in Reggio to describe as Loris Malaguzzi puts it a lexible approach in which initial hypotheses are made about classroom work…, but are subject to modiications and changes of direction as the actual work progresses’, in contrast ‘to programma or programmazione, which implies predeined curricula, programmes, stages and so on (Malaguzzi 1990 in Cagliari et al. 2016, 357fn8). The strategies do not belong to any particular stage of a method, but can be used in endless variation. Strategy 1: Provocation of a project (‘protagezzione’) The educator reads a picturebook. It could be with people of any age. The books are ‘post-age’ and carefully selected to open up questions and puzzlement about philosophical concepts that interest them (Haynes and Murris 2012, 2016). They feature unusual characters (e.g., mermaids, humans covered in body hair, aliens, cyborgs), extreme concepts (e.g., immortality, the size of the universe), or obscure thought experiments. Picturebooks feature creatures that mediate between binary opposites: e.g., a gorilla who has a cat as a pet (Browne’s 2008 Little Beauty), a monster with human feet (Sendak’s 1963 Where the Wild Things Are), a tree that can talk (Silverstein’s 1964 The Giving Tree) or elephants that use their trunks as guns (McKee’s 1978 Tusk Tusk). They are often ambiguous, mysterious, macabre, gruesome or fantastical – they play with binaries, therefore perfect provocations for philosophical wonder and questioning (Murris 2016), involving the imagination, emotions, lust and desire (Lenz Taguchi 2010, 59); the material and the discursive are intra-connected and inseparable (Lenz Taguchi 2010, 30). 10 K. MURRIS Strategy 2: Giving time to dwell in silence Children are given opportunities to dwell on the ideas provoked by the picturebook. It requires an openness to indeterminacy (Barad 2012) and what is thought-provoking. Time spent in silence during this period can be used to write words down, to draw them, or to express them through other materials that are made available (e.g., clay). It is a good idea as educator to reassure children that spelling does not matter and to give instructions that move away from representationalism (e.g., copying the pictures in the picturebook). Giving material-discursive opportunities to shape their own responses is prioritised using PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 11 children’s ‘a hundred, a thousand creative and communicative potentials’ (Rinaldi 2006, 175) without prioritising reading and writing for knowledge construction or to emphasise a product.5 12 K. MURRIS Typically, extended projects start with ‘verbal outpourings’ that allow easy access to memories and a ‘free reign to thought’ (Edwards 1995, 42), aided also by removal of the obstacle of having to read or write. Regular philosophical enquiries have other afordances, for example, formulating new theories and hypotheses out of these oral exchanges, real or imagined, which in turn can be put to the ‘test’ through drawing what they know. Then when drawing, they might discover ‘gaps’ in their knowledge, and have to readjust the theories (Forman 1994), thereby continuously building on and reining earlier thinking. The diferent languages difract with one another and create new conceptual understandings in the process. The imagination is part of the cognitive process here by ex-pressing what is not there, sometimes in 2D, other times in 3D, simultaneously creating a kind of eco-system, and, in the process, creating deep empathetic understanding for the relational dimension of one’s being in the world. For example, by drawing a pet by focusing attention on the detail of the network of relationships the animal is a part of. As Malaguzzi puts it: we begin to see a world not made of coexisting islands, separate parts, distant unique languages, but a world held in a web. Migrations, changes of tradition, changes of language are all in this web. Migrations, changes of tradition, changes of language are all in this web … A world of networks is a world that communicates even when it does not wish to. (Malaguzzi 1990 in Cagliari et al. 2016, 390) Strategy 3: Developing questions in small groups In small groups, children are invited to develop their questions, which are written down for all to see and subsequently one question is selected democratically to move the project forward. During this process, the educator listens to the children by walking around, supporting the conceptual work (e.g., ‘pet’ in one of the picturebooks mentioned above), making notes, taking photos or videos. PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 13 This pedagogical documentation demands that teachers be ‘response-able’ (Barad 2007) for their observations, descriptions, interpretations and explanations, and dare to see the ambiguities (Dahlberg, Moss, and Pence 2013, 155) – always selective, partial, contextual and situated. A willingness to be open to surprises and the unexpected is key. The documentation brings into the world a material-discursive expression of children’s learning and their intra-action with more-than-human things, thought, afect, concepts and environment (Edwards 1995). As a living record of the pedagogical practice (Dahlberg, Moss, and Pence 2013, 156, 162), the documentation brings ‘forces and energies’ to the project work (Dahlberg 2003). The philosophical questions are not products of individuals, but they e/merge through the relationality between the human and more-than-human. The documentation is shared with the children, colleagues, parents and other members of the community through philosophical dialogue, including the process and materials used to create it (see e.g., the philosophical play work by Stanley 2012). The documentation is an ‘apparatus’; it is ‘a doing, not a thing’ without clear identity-producing boundaries (Barad 2007, 183) between teacher and learner, objectivity and subjectivity. Crucially the documentation is not about the children’s abilities or capacities as individuals, but the learning that is made visible through the documentation shows the relationality that has made the learning possible and how the human and more-than-human render each other able. This completely changes the landscape of what counts as academic performance and ability, or what is meant by quality education. As a result, these material-discursive documentations ofer transformative opportunities to reconigure notions of child and childhood. Strategy 4: Building a community of philosophical enquiry The philosophical question children chose to enquire into is itself divergent. For example, if they had elected ‘What is a pet?’ many other questions could be generated by the community of enquirers, such as ‘Can animals have pets?’, ‘Can a wild animal be a pet?’, or ‘Does a pet have to have a name in order to be a pet? Why is this philosophical work educationally important, and how is conceptual knowledge constructed in a community of enquiry? 14 K. MURRIS The community of enquiry pedagogy is an elusive concept that cannot be deined. Why use a community of enquiry in education? Will the concept development through philosophical questioning not develop automatically? Why not just use a dictionary or Google the answer to these questions? Concepts are complex, not discrete, or ixed (like Plato’s Forms as heavenly bodies), but intensive coordinates, in that they are composed of many parts (Deleuze and Guattari 1987, 2014). They are continuously becoming, with tendencies towards chaos and change. Concepts also have tendencies towards stasis and opinion when concepts are ‘(re)absorbed in a dominant or traditional way of thinking’ (Adkins 2015, 19), thereby reproducing existing hierarchical, nesting classiications, as e.g., in text books where ‘pet’, ‘animal’, ‘human’ are not aforded equal status in the hierarchy of being. For Deleuze6, concepts are a ‘toolbox’, not bricks to build a wall with, but a ‘pragmatics’ that does not reduce the meaning of a concept by an apriori determined set of propositions that you can either enter into or not – for example, by answering a question by tracing the meaning of a concept back to its ‘roots’, or through ixed deinitions. Kennedy (2012) argues that the community of enquiry in its communal problematisation of the meaning of concepts ofers unique possibilities for rhizomatic curriculum development that puts philosophy in a transdisciplinary position to ‘meet the universe halfway’ (Barad 2007) – for example, by examining the concept ‘pet’ through diferent disciplinary lenses using the hundred languages: biological, anthropological (treatment of pets in various cultures), historical (mapping its domestication), ethico-aesthetic (docking tails), political (what goes into dog food, puppy farms), legal (animal rights), media studies7 or literary (pets as characters in literature). The community of enquiry deterritorialises concepts from ‘their imprisonment within ideologically locked-down networks of concepts’ (Kennedy 2012, 2) and reterritorialises transdisciplinary. It includes children as players in this political project. PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 15 Concepts make up the system of beliefs that constitute a school curriculum, but every classiication is already geo-political (culturally and historically constructed). We teach children the distinction between pet animals, wild animals, farm animals and human animals as if these categorisations are unproblematic. They are not just common and central concepts, but they are also contestable. Philosophical enquiry into their meaning shows up real diiculties in establishing criteria for the unique diferences between them (reason?, communication? etc.). Curricula are so designed that concepts have been assigned to speciic disciplines (e.g., number is a mathematics concept), but when allowed to be explored rhizomatically and through connecting with our experiences, de and re-territorialising happens by making connections with other, related concepts, they suddenly come to life (Kennedy 2012, 2). Understanding the concept ‘pet’ through binary, arborescent logic would regard the concept as not abstract enough. Massumi argues that abstraction is embodied thought, a ‘lived abstraction’, thereby ‘actuality swells with possibility’ (Massumi 2014, 7–9). Through my actions, I ‘comment’ on what I am doing as I am doing it, that is, I treat my dog as a pet not a wild animal – the diference is in the performance. What a pet means, cannot be ‘captured’ through deinition, because it is the embodied performance of acts that constitutes the diference between a pet animal and other animals. Importantly, a deinition would not do justice to the relationality of the concept itself. So in the very same move as the doing, an abstraction is performed on its action: a lick from my dog, looking in her eyes, walking her, the cuddles, making a space in our home for her, being part of our family, giving her a name. In other words, ‘pet’ as a concept is too complex to capture through binary logic. It needs to be understood through a ‘pragmatics’, that is, directly embodied in action, creatively and imaginatively. Interrogating the curriculum itself as a knowledge system A Reggio inspired philosophy with children concept creation moves away from prioritising reading and writing, emphasises oral language and iterative intra-actions with materialdiscursive bodies, the material environment (e.g., sitting in a circle), texts, emotions, 16 K. MURRIS questions, ideas, each other, the teacher, smells, sounds and even silences. The practice includes questioning contestable, but signiicant concepts, and ‘responsive’ and ‘responseable’ listening, which involves listening out for the new and unfamiliar, enabling a reconstruction of adult/child relations in education, that is, a disruption of practices of power (Kennedy 2006, 9). This kind of listening is not with a particular part of the body, for example, the organ ‘ear’, but manifests itself in the relationship through action (e.g., asking a question in response to what a learner says or does) and making the learning visible through documentation, mind maps, recording questions on paper – continuously intra-acting and difracting. Barad uses quantum physics as empirical evidence that discursive practices have performative agency in that thinking, observing, and theorising are practices of engagement with, and as part of, the world in which we have our being (Barad 2007, 133). A Reggio inspired philosophy with children enacts Barad’s agential realism8 and satisies Deleuze and Guattari’s philosophy of immanence by disrupting current educational practices that use arborescent, hierarchical knowledge systems that position children as knowledge consumers: all classiications are seen as ahistorical, apolitical and meanings irmly established and transmittable. The superposition or interference pattern created by reading P4C and Reggio through one another, understands knowledge production and expression as e/mergent, as part of being, by drawing on all of child’s material-discursive languages. The new practice powerfully disrupts binaries between adult/child, nature/culture, and language/reality. In Reggio inspired philosophy with children, concepts are not ‘given’ or ‘transmitted’, because that would mean using the One language that interprets, represents, and deines what a concept is – assigning it an essence, like pinning a dead butterly to a display board. To deine, MacLure (2013, 661) points out, is ‘to return to the logic of representation, where words “refer” to entities as if they were separate and distinct from one another.’ The question ‘what does a concept mean?’ is not crucial but ‘how does it work?’ in lived experience, thereby positioning children as part of the world they share with other human and more-than-human others and explore intra-actively through materialdiscursive research and experimentation. Notes 1. Throughout my work I have adopted the use of the term ‘child’ without including an article ‘the’, to try and distance myself from the practice of talking about ‘the child’ as a bounded entity in space and time with a set of essential and universal characteristics (often resulting in the marginalisation of children), but still to allow discussion of the concept: ‘child’. 2. It was a break from the then dominant Catholic preschools carried out by nuns. 3. Founded at what is now called Montclair State University (New Jersey US) in 1974. 4. For the distinction between the two see Malaguzzi’s explanation in Cagliari et al. (2016, 335– 338). 5. Downloaded from http://www.innovativeteacherproject.org/reggio/poem.php on 29/09/2016. 6. See Brian Massumi in his introduction to Deleuze and Guattari’s A Thousand Plateaus (1987/2014, pp. xiii–iv). 7. For example, Laurie Andersen’s ilm The Heart of a Dog (2015). 8. Agential realism is Barad’s relational materialist philosophy of immanence. Things or objects are real, but they emerge through particular intra-actions (relationships), boundary-making practices that include or exclude. In agential realism the epistemological, the ontological and the ethical e/merge. PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 17 Disclosure statement No potential conlict of interest was reported by the author. Funding This work was supported by National Research Foundation [grant number 98992]. References Adkins, B. 2015. Deleuze and Guattari’s a Thousand Plateaus: A Critical Introduction and Guide. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Anderson, L. 2015. The Heart of a Dog (Film). Barad, K. 2003. “Posthumanist Performativity: Toward an Understanding of How Matter comes to Matter.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 28 (31): 801–831. Barad, K. 2007. Meeting the Universe Halfway: Quantum Physics and the Entanglement of Matter and Meaning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Barad, K. 2012. Intra-actions: An interview with Karen Barad by Adam Kleinman. Mousse #34 June, 76–81. Barad, K. 2014. “Difracting Difraction: Cutting Together-apart.” Parallax 20 (3): 168–187. Braidotti, R. 2013. The Posthuman. Cambridge: Polity Press. Browne, A. 2008. Little Beauty. London: Walker Books. Burman, E. 1994. Deconstructing Developmental Psychology. New York: Routledge. Burman, E. 2008. Developments: Child, Image, Nation. London: Routledge. Cagliari, P., M. Castagnetti, C. Giudici, C. Rinaldi, V. Vecchi, and P. Moss. 2016. Loris Malaguzzi and the Schools of Reggio Emilia: A Selection of Writings and Speeches 1945–1993. Translated by J. McCall. London: Routledge Contesting Early Childhood Series. Cannella, G. S., and R. Viruru. 2004. Childhood and Postcolonization: Power, Education, and Contemporary Practice. New York: RoutledgeFalmer. Cherkasova, E. 2004. “Philosophy as Sideshadowing: The Philosophical, the Literary and the Fantastical.” In What Philosophy Is, edited by H. Carel and D. Gamez, 200–211. London: Continuum. Coleman, R., and J. Ringrose. 2013. Deleuze and Research Methodologies. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Dahlberg, G. 2003. “Pedagogy as a Loci of an Ethics of an Encounter.” In Governing Children, Families and Education: Restructuring the Welfare State, edited by M. Bloch, K. Holmlund, I. Moqvist, and T. Popkewitz, 261–286. New York: Palgrave McMillan. Dahlberg, G., and P. Moss. 2005. Ethics and Politics in Early Childhood Education. London: Routledge. Dahlberg, G., P. Moss, and P. Pence. 2013. Beyond Quality in Early Childhood Education and Care: Postmodern Perspectives. 3rd ed. London: Falmer Press. Deleuze, G., and F. Guattari. 1987. A Thousand Plateaus. 2014 ed. Translated and a foreword by B. Massumi. London: Bloomsbury. Edwards, C. 1995. “Democratic Participation in a Community of Learners: Loris Malaguzzi’s Philosophy of Education as Relationship.” Lecture prepared for an international seminar, University of Milano, October 16–17. Accessed June 11, 2015. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent. cgi?article=1014&context=famconfacpub Egan, K. 1992. Imagination in Teaching and Learning; Ages 8–15. London: Routledge. Egan, K. 1993. “The Other Half of the Child.” In Thinking, Children and Education, edited by M. Lipman, 281–286. Montclair: Kendall/Hunt. Egan, K. 1997. The Educated Mind: How Cognitive Tools Shape Our Understanding. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Fendler, L. 1998. “What is It Impossible to Think? A Genealogy of the Educated Subject.” In Foucault’s Challenge: Discourse, Knowledge, and Power in Education, edited by T. Popkewitz and M. Brennan, 39–63. New York: Teachers College Press. 18 K. MURRIS File, N., D. Basler Wisneski, and J. Mueller. 2012. “Strengthening Curriculum in Early Childhood.” In Curriculum in Early Childhood Education: Re-examined, Rediscovered, Renewed, edited by N. File, J. Mueler, and D. Basler Wisneski, 200–205. New York: Routledge. Forman, G. 1994. “Diferent Media, Diferent Languages.” In Relections on the Reggio Emilia Approach, Perspectives from ERIC/EECE: A monograph series, no.6, edited by L. G. Katz and B. Cesarone, 37–46. Urbana: University of Illinois. Haraway, D. 1988. “Situated Knowledges: The Science Question in Feminism and the Privilege of Partial Perspective.” Feminist Studies 14: 575–599. Haraway, D. 2016. Staying with the Trouble: Making Kin in the Chthulucene. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Haynes, J. 2008. Children as Philosophers: Learning through Enquiry and Dialogue in the Primary School. 2nd ed. London: RoutledgeFalmer. Haynes, J. 2009. “Listening to the Voice of Child in Education.” In Perspectives on Participation and Inclusion: Engaging Education, edited by S. Gibson and J. Haynes, 27–41. London: Continuum. Haynes, J. 2014. “Already Equal and Able to Speak: Practising Philosophical Enquiry with Young Children.” In Routledge International Handbook on Young Children’s Thinking and Understanding, edited by S. Robson and S. Quinn, 463–476. London: Routledge. Haynes, J., and K. Murris. 2012. Picturebooks, Pedagogy and Philosophy. New York: Routledge Research in Education. Haynes, J., and K. Murris. 2013. “The Realm of Meaning: Imagination, Narrative and Playfulness in Philosophical Exploration with Young Children.” In Special Issue: Developing Children’s Thinking in Early Childhood Education. Early Child Development and Care, edited by P. Costello 183 (8): 1084–1100. Heidegger, M. 1971. “Building Dwelling Thinking.” In Poetry, Language, Thought, translated by Albert Hofstadter, 344–363. New York: Harper Colophon Books. Hultman, K., and H. Lenz Taguchi. 2010. “Challenging Anthropocentric Analysis of Visual Data: A Relational Materialist Methodological Approach to Educational Research.” International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education 23 (5): 525–542. Irigaray, L. 1996. I Love to You: Sketch of a Possible Felicity in History. Translated from the French by Alison Martin. New York: Routledge. Jackson, A. Y., and L. A. Mazzei. 2012. Thinking with Theory in Qualitative Research: Viewing Data across Multiple Perspectives. New York: Routledge. Jenks, C. 1996. Childhood. New York: Routledge. Kennedy, D. 1996. “After Reggio Emilia: May the Conversation Begin!” Young Children 51: 24–27. Kennedy, D. 2000. “The Roots of Child Study: Philosophy, History, and Religion.” Teachers College Record 102 (3): 514–538. Kennedy, D. 2006. Changing Conceptions of the Child from the Renaissance to Post-modernity: A Philosophy of Childhood. New York: Edwin Mellen Press. Kennedy, D. 2011. “From Outer Space and across the Street: Matthew Lipman’s Double Vision.” Childhood & Philosophy 7 (13): 49–74. Kennedy, D. 2012. “Rhizomatic Curriculum Development in Community of Philosophical Inquiry.” In Educating for Complex Thinking through Philosophical Inquiry, edited by M. Santi and S. Oliverio, 231–243. Napoli: Liguori Editore. Kohan, W. O. 1998. “What can Philosophy and Children Ofer Each Other?” Thinking, American Journal of Philosophy for Children 14 (4): 2–8. Kohan, W. O. 2002. “Education, Philosophy and Childhood: The Need to Think an Encounter.” Thinking, American Journal of Philosophy for Children 16 (1): 4–11. Kohan, W. O. 2011. “Childhood, Education and Philosophy: Notes on Deterritorisation.” In Special issue Philosophy for Children in Transition: Problems and Prospects, edited by N. Vansieleghem and D. Kennedy. Journal of Philosophy of Education 45 (2): 339–359. Kohan, W. 2015. Childhood, Education and Philosophy: New Ideas for an Old Relationship. New York: Routledge. Koro-Ljungberg, M. 2016. Reconceptualizing Qualitative Research: Methodologies without Methodology. New York: Sage. PEDAGOGY, CULTURE & SOCIETY 19 Lather, P. 2016. “Top Ten + List: (Re)Thinking Ontology in (Post)Qualitative Research.” Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies 16 (2): 125–131. Lenz Taguchi, H. 2010. Going beyond the Theory/Practice Divide in Early Childhood Education. London: Routledge Contesting Early Childhood Series. Lipman, M. 1988. Philosophy Goes to School. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. Lipman, M. 1991. Thinking in Education. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Lipman, M. 2008. A Life Teaching Thinking. Mont Clair: Institute for the Advancement of Philosophy for Children. Lipman, M., and A. M. Sharp. 1978. Growing up with Philosophy. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. Lipman, M., A. M. Sharp, and F. S. Oscanyan. 1977. Philosophy in the Classroom. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. MacLure, M. 2013. “Researching without Representation? Language and Materiality in Post-qualitative Methodology.” Special Issue: Post-qualitative Research International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education 26 (6): 658–667. Malaguzzi, L. 1998. History, Ideas, and Basic Philosophy: An Interview with Lella Gandini. In The Hundred Languages of Children: The Reggio Emilia Approach – Advanced Relections, edited by C. Edwards, L. Gandini, and G. Forman, 2nd ed. 49–99. Westport Connecticut: Ablex Publishing. Massumi, B. 2014. What Animals Teach Us about Ethics. Durham: Durham University Press. Matthews, G. 1980. Philosophy and the Young Child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Matthews, G. 1984. Dialogues with Children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Matthews, G. 1994. The Philosophy of Childhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mazzei, L. A. 2014. “Beyond an Easy Sense: A Difractive Analysis.” Qualitative Inquiry 20 (6): 742–746. McKee, D. 1978. Tusk Tusk. London: Andersen Press. Murris, K. 1997. “Metaphors of the Child’s Mind: Teaching Philosophy to Young Children.” Phd thesis, University of Hull. Murris, K. 2000. “Can Children Do Philosophy?” Journal of Philosophy of Education 34 (2): 261–279. Murris, K. 2015. “The Philosophy for Children Curriculum: Resisting ‘Teacher Proof’ Texts and the Formation of the Ideal Philosopher Child.” Studies in Philosophy and Education 4 (2): 1–17. doi:10.1007/ s11217-015-9466-3. Murris, K. 2016. The Posthuman Child: Educational Transformation through Philosophy with Picturebooks. London: Routledge. Palmer, A. 2011. “‘How many Sums can I Do’? Performative Strategies and Difractive Thinking as Methodological Tools for Rethinking Mathematical Subjectivity.” Reconceptualizing Educational Research Methodology 1 (1): 3–18. Patel, L. 2016. Decolonising Educational Research: From Ownership to Answerability. New York: Routledge. Rinaldi, C. 2006. In Dialogue with Reggio Emilia: Listening, Researching and Learning. London: Routledge Contesting Early Childhood Series. Sendak, M. 1963. Where the Wild Things Are. London: Bodley Head. Silverstein, S. 1964. The Giving Tree. 1992 ed. New York: Harper Collins. Splitter, L. J., and A.-M. Sharp. 1995. Teaching for Better Thinking; the Classroom Community of Enquiry. Melbourne: ACER. St Pierre, E. A., A. Y. Jackson, and L. A. Mazzei. 2016. “New Empiricisms and New Materialisms: Conditions for New Enquiry.” Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies 16 (2): 99–110. Stables, A. 2008. Childhood and the Philosophy of Education: An Anti-Aristotelian Perspective. London: Continuum Studies in Educational Research. Stanley, S. 2012. Why Think? Philosophical Play from 3–11. London: Continuum Books. Stanley, S., and S. Lyle. 2017. “Philosophical Play in the Early Years Classroom.” In The Routledge International Handbook of Philosophy for Children, edited by M. Rollins Gregory, J. Haynes, and K. Murris, 53–63. London: Routledge. Stremmel, A. J. 2012. “A Situated Framework: The Reggio Experience.” In Curriculum in Early Childhood Education: Re-examined, Rediscovered, Renewed, edited by N. File, J. Mueler, and D. Basler Wisneski, 133–146. New York: Routledge. Taylor, C., and C. Hughes. 2016. Posthuman Research Practices in Education. London: Palgrave. 20 K. MURRIS Taylor, A., and V. Pacini-Ketchabaw. 2015. “Unsettling Pedagogies through Common World Encounters: Grappling with (Post) Colonial Legacies in Canadian Forests and Australian Bushlands.” In Unsettling the Colonialist Places and Spaces of Early Childhood Education, edited by V. Pacini-Ketchabaw and A. Taylor, 43–62. New York: Routledge. Vannini, P. 2015. Non-representational Methodologies: Re-envisioning Research. London: Routledge. Viruru, R. 2005. “The Impact of Postcolonial Theory on Early Childhood Education.” Accessed June 1, 2015. http://joe.ukzn.ac.za/Libraries/No_35_2005/The_impact_of_postcolonial_theory_on_early_ childhood_education.slb.ashx Walkerdine, V. 1984. “Developmental Psychology and the Child-centred Pedagogy.” In Changing the Subject: Psychology, Social Regulation and Subjectivity, edited by J. Henriques, W. Holloway, C. Unwin, C. Venn, and V. Walkerdine, 148–198. London: Routledge. Whitehead, A. N. 1953. On Mathematical Concepts of the Material World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.