Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci. 2009 August 12; 29(32): 10153–10159. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Evidence of Mirror Neurons in Human Inferior Frontal Gyrus James M. Kilner, Alice Neal, Nikolaus Weiskopf, Karl J. Friston, and Chris D. Frith The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, IoN, UCL, 12 Queen Square, London UK, WC1N 3BG Abstract There is much current debate about the existence of mirror neurons in humans. To identify mirror neurons in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) of humans we employed a repetition suppression paradigm while measuring neural activity with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Subjects either executed or observed a series of actions. Here we show that in the IFG, responses were suppressed both when an executed action was followed by the same rather than a different observed action and when an observed action was followed by the same rather than a different executed action. This pattern of responses is consistent with that predicted by mirror neurons and is evidence of mirror neurons in the human IFG. Keywords Mirror Neurons; IFG; F5; Action observation; Repetition Supression; fMRI Introduction Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Mirror neurons were first discovered in the premotor area F5 of macaque monkeys (Di Pellegrino et al. 1992; Gallese et al. 1996; Umilta et al. 2001; Rizzolatti et al. 2001) and subsequently in inferior parietal lobule, area PF (Gallese et al. 2002; Fogassi et al. 2005) Mirror-neurons discharge not only during action execution but also during action observation. Since the discovery of mirror neurons a number of neuroimaging studies have claimed that a mirror neuron system (MNS) exists in humans and that homologous areas in the human brain are activated when observing and executing movements (Buccino et al. 2001; Decety et al. 1997; Rizzolatti et al. 1996; Grezes and Decety 2001; Gazzola and Keysers 2009). However, over a decade after their discovery there is still debate as to whether any of the human neuroimaging studies constitute conclusive evidence for mirror neurons in the human homologue of area F5, the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Dinstein et al. 2008). To date, the best evidence for mirror neurons in the human IFG is the demonstration that there is a significant spatial overlap between activity in this region during both action observation and execution (Buccino et la. 2001; Decety et al. 1997; Rizzolatti et al. 1996; Grezes and Decety 2001). The problem with this evidence is that the majority of neurons active during either action execution and observation are not mirror neurons (Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004). Therefore, the fact that a volume of cortex in IFG has an increased BOLD signal during observation and execution of an action does not necessarily mean that the same neurons are active in both conditions (Dinstein et al. 2007; 2008). One approach to attribute the fMRI response to a single neuronal population is adaptation, or repetition suppression. The logic of this approach is that as stimuli that evoke activity in a specific neuronal population are repeated, the magnitude of the response decreases or adapts Corresponding Author, James M. Kilner, The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, UCL, 12 Queen Square, UK WC1N 3BG, Tel (44) 020 7833 7472, Fax (44) 020 7813 1445, j.kilner@fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk. Kilner et al. Page 2 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts (Dinstein et al. 2007; 2008; Dinstein 2008; Grill-Spector et al. 2006). fMRI adaptation effects have been reported previously both when the observed actions are repeated (Dinstein et al. 2007; Hamilton and Grafton 2006; 2008) and when the executed actions are repeated (Dinstein et al. 2007; unimodal repetition suppression (umRS)). Areas of the cortex that contain mirror neurons should show adaptation both when an action is executed and subsequently observed and when an action is observed and subsequently executed (crossmodal repetition suppression (xmRS)). Three previous fMRI studies have failed to demonstrate significant xmRS in the human IFG (Dinstein et al. 2007; Chong et al. 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009; see also Grafton 2009). Here we employed an fMRI adaptation paradigm that was designed to be maximally sensitive to xmRS in the IFG. Using this paradigm we were able to show significant xmRS effects in human IFG, at the single subject level, at the group level using a ROI analysis and also at the group level using a typical whole brain analysis. These results are consistent with the existence of mirror neurons in human IFG. Materials and Methods Data were recorded from 10 healthy right-handed subjects (7 females, 25-45 yrs). All subjects gave written informed consent prior to testing and the recordings had local ethical committee approval. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts The task and scanning paradigms employed in this study were designed to increase the probability of activity in human IFG. Subjects were asked to either observe or execute one of two actions performed by the right hand - a precision grip or an index finger pull. These actions were chosen for two reasons. First, the three previous studies that failed to show cross modal repetition suppression effects in the IFG used meaningless or pantomime actions (Chong et al. 2008; Dinstein et al 2007; Lingnau et al. 2009). Studies of mirror neurons in area F5 of the macaque have focussed on goal directed actions. Indeed, mirror neurons in macaque monkeys do not even respond to mimed actions (Umilta et al. 2001). In light of this we built an fMRI compatible manipulandum that allowed the subject to make two different grip actions. The actions were made in a way that required only movements around the wrist and the hand, thus minimising movement artefacts in the fMRI data. The second reason these actions were chosen was that previous research has shown that the discharge of neurons in area F5 of the macaque monkey are modulated by the nature of the grip used during action execution (Rizzolatti et al. 1988; Gentilucci et al. 1988; Jeannerod et al. 1995). We reasoned that tasks that are known to selectively activate different populations of neurons within the IFG would increase our chances of seeing repetition suppression. In the scanner on each trial subjects were presented with pairs of stimuli sequentially. These pairs could consist of two executions, two observations, or mixed execution and observation conditions. During the observation conditions subjects observed a video of one of the actions - index finger ring pull and precision grip. In total there were 28 different exemplar videos of each movement made by two different actors, one female and one male. Each video lasted ~750 ms. Subjects were required to execute an action when a red arrow appeared on the screen that pointed towards the object that they were required to move. The arrow either pointed up the screen - indicating the index finger ring pull - or left - indicating the precision grip (Fig. 1). Subjects were trained prior to scanning to execute the correct action as rapidly as possible following the imperative cues. These imperative cues were presented for 500 ms. Trials were categorised into 8 types: execute-execute same, executeexecute different, observe-observe same, observe-observe different, execute-observe same, execute-observe different, observe-execute same and observe-execute different. Within each trial there was a 500 ms gap between presentations of the two stimuli and there was a J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 3 between-trial jittered ‘wait’ with a mean of 5000 ms (std 1000 ms). A rapid inter-stimulus interval was chosen as the time constant of the neuronal repetition effects in IFG are not known. Subjects performed 4 sessions, where each session consisted of 96 trials with each of the 8 trial types presented 12 times. Data acquisition and analysis Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts We acquired T2*-weighted echo-planar images (EPI) with blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) contrast on a 3T whole-body MRI scanner (Magneton TIM TRIO, Siemens Medical, Erlangen, Germany) operated with the standard 12-channel head receive and body transmit coil. A total of 600 volumes were collected for each of the 4 sessions, these included 6 dummy volumes at the start of each session to allow for T1 equilibration. Each volume consisted of 16 slices that were positioned prior to scanning to cover left and right IFG (slices/volume, 16; repetition time 1072 ms). Imaging parameters: in-plane resolution 3×3 mm2, slice thickness 2 mm with 1 mm interslice gap, echo time (TE) 30 ms, 64×64 matrix. High-resolution T1-weighted structural scans (MDEFT ;Diechmann et al. 2004) were collected for each subject and were coregistered to their mean EPI images. Data preprocessing of the EPI functional scans, including spatial realignment, normalisation to a standard EPI template, and smoothing with a 4 mm (full-width at half-maximum) Gaussian Kernel, using SPM5 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The event-related fMRI data were then analysed using a linear convolution model in the usual way: Stimulus functions comprised a set of delta functions corresponding to the onset times of the different conditions. The first stimulus of each within-trial pair was modelled by two regressors, one for a pair beginning with an execution condition and one for a pair beginning with the observation condition. For the second within-trial stimulus there were 8 different stimulus functions depending upon the trial category (see above). In addition, the ‘wait’ period that occurred between every pair of stimuli was explicitly modelled. These functions were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function for explanatory variables or regressors. Subject-specific movement parameters and drift terms (high pass filter cut-off, 128 s) were also modelled as covariates of no interest. Condition-specific estimates of neural activity (betas), corresponding to the amplitude of the modelled response were computed at each voxel for each subject. The contrasts of interest in this study are execution > wait (where execution is the regressor depicting the onset of the execution conditions when execution was the first of the pair) observation > wait (where observation is the regressor depicting the onset of the observation conditions when observation was the first of the pair) and xmRS same < different (where the repeated action is less than the different action in mixed or cross modal trials). All voxels reported conform to MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) co-ordinate space. For display, the right side of the image corresponds to the right side of the brain. All analyses were restricted to left and right IFG using a small volume search. This volume was created from the maximal extent of peak activations reported in the meta-analysis in Table 1 of Dinstein et al. (2007). This is depicted graphically in Fig. 2D (red boxes). For display purposes activations in the whole of the IFG are shown this area is shown graphically in Fig. 2D (yellow box). In this study it was not appropriate to use a region of interest based on published cytoarchitectonic maps. Some previous studies have used a ROI analysis approach based in the probabilistic maps of BA44. However, the human homologue of area F5 is known not to be restricted to BA44 but also includes ventral BA6 (Morin and Grezes). Results An initial analysis focussed on the simple effects of increases in BOLD during action execution and observation. In agreement with previous literature, all subjects showed significantly greater activity in both left and right IFG when subjects either executed or observed an action compared to rest (p<0.05 corrected for FWE see Fig. 2 for an example J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 4 from a single subject). Furthermore, the conjunction of these contrasts showed that there was significant overlap between voxels active for action execution and observation in each subject in both left and right IFG (Fig. 2 for single subject & Table 1 for all subjects). Group level analysis: ROI approach Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Due to the failure of three previous studies that employed group level analysis being able to show significant xmRS effects in the IFG (Dinstein et al. 2007; Chong et al. 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009) in a first analysis of xmRS the critical group level comparisons were performed on data extracted from the single subject data. To determine whether there was significant xmRS we performed a 2×2 repeated measures ANOVA at the between-subject level on the estimated responses to the second stimulus of each trial pair. These responses were during execute-observe same, execute-observe different, observe-execute same and observeexecute different trials, in left and right IFG. These regions were identified with an orthogonal contrast; the conjunction between action execution and action observation. We used the estimates from the peak voxel of each subjects SPM, which is equivalent to taking a local weighted average as the data were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel. It should be noted that this is an unbiased method as the region of interest (ROI) was defined from a fully balanced orthogonal contrast (as prescribed in Kriegeskorte et al. 2009). The within-subject factors were therefore congruence (same or different) and order (executed then observed or observed then executed). The analysis of responses in left IFG showed a significant effect of congruence (F(1,9)=20.888 p<0.001) and order (F(1,9)=93.657 p<0.001), but no significant interaction between the two (F(1,9)=0.068, p=0.801) (Fig. 3). Importantly, a post hoc t-test revealed that this effect was driven by repetition suppression effects; both when the action was first executed and then observed and when the action was observed and then executed (p<0.05). The equivalent analysis in right IFG showed only a significant main effect of order (F(1,9)=29.71 p<0.001). The main effect of congruence (F(1,9)=0.481 p=0.505) and the interaction (F(1,9)=0.720 p=0.418) were not significant. The absence of a significant congruence effect in the right hemisphere does not mean that there are no repetition suppression effects in the right hemisphere, just that these effects were not consistent across subjects at the peak conjunction of action execution and action observation. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Group level and single subject analyses: non-ROI approach The previous analysis assumes that xmRS occurs at the peak of the conjunction of observing an action and executing an action. This is sensible; given that the minimum requirement for mirror neurons is that the area activated should be activated by both action observation and execution. However, the converse is not true. In other words, maximal xmRS does not necessarily have to be at the peak of responses to observation and execution. To address this, we characterised the xmRS at both the group level and in each individual subject (Fig. 3, Fig. 4 and Table 2). At the group level only two clusters survived a liberal statistical threshold (p<0.05 uncorrected). This liberal threshold was chosen to demonstrate the spatial specificity of the effect (Fig. 4). However, although a liberal threshold was chosen to show spatial specificity, the peak voxel within the left IFG was significant at the required conservative threshold (p<0.05 corrected for small volume correction for left IFG; t = 6.35 at -50, -2, 12). In other words, at the group level, there was a significant xmRS in the left IFG that had high spatial specificity to the left IFG. In addition, at the single subject level 8 out of 10 subjects showed an xmRS effect (p<0.001 uncorrected) in either left or right IFG. Importantly, the peak voxel in left IFG for the xmRS contrast was also significant during action execution and action observation (t(9) = 3.23, p<0.05; t(9) = 2.98, p<0.05). This demonstrates that the area of cortex that was significantly modulated by xmRS also shows the basic properties of mirror neurons, namely it is significantly activated during both action execution and observation. In summary, here we have shown significant xmRS effects in the J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 5 IFG at the group level using a region of interest analysis, at the group level using a whole brain SPM and in addition at the single subject level. Spatial differences in peak responses Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts In addition, we noticed that the location of the peak for the three contrasts of interest, execution, observation and xmRS were not at the same co-ordinates within IFG (Tables 1 & 2, Fig. 5). Furthermore, the different contrasts peaked in systematically different locations across subjects. These small yet significant differences were observed in both the left and right IFG (Fig. 5). On average the peak execution effect was 6.7 mm more lateral than the corresponding peak for action observation (left IFG, 4.2 mm; right IFG, 9.2 mm). A 2×2 repeated measures ANOVA on the locations, where the factors were condition (execution or observation) and hemisphere (left or right) revealed only a significant effect of condition (F(1, 9) = 10.048, p=0.011). Similarly, the location of the peak for the xmRS effect was on average 4.4 mm more ventral than the corresponding peaks for the observation conditions (left IFG, 5 mm; right IFG, 3.8 mm). A 2×2 repeated measures ANOVA on the locations, where the factors were condition (xmRS or observation) and hemisphere (left or right) revealed only a significant effect of condition (F(1, 9) = 9.991, p=0.012). These results suggest that the locations of the peak effects are in different areas of IFG; with the peak elicited during execution being more lateral than that when observing actions and the peak neural activity of mirror neurons (xmRS) being more ventral than that when observing actions. Discussion Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Although mirror-neurons were discovered over a decade ago in the macaque monkey (Di Pellegrino et al. 1992) there is still a lively debate as to whether any of the human neuroimaging studies constitute conclusive evidence for mirror neurons in the human homologue of area F5, the IFG (Dinstein et al. 2007; Dinstein 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009; Grafton 2009). Here we adopted the fMRI technique of fMRI adaptation or repetition suppression to provide evidence of mirror neurons in the human IFG. Areas of the cortex that contain mirror neurons should show adaptation both when an action is first executed and subsequently observed and when an action is first observed and subsequently executed. Here we able to demonstrate these effects in human IFG, at the single subject level, at the group level using a ROI analysis and also at the group level using a typical whole brain analyses. Importantly we were able to demonstrate significant xmRS effects both when an observed action was followed by an executed action and visa versa. These results are consistent with the existence of mirror neurons in human IFG. This is evidence in favour of the existence of mirror neurons in what is considered the human homologue of area F5, IFG. We can assert this because the context established by the first within-trial stimulus (same or different) affects the respond to the second stimulus in a different modality (observation or execution). This influence can only be mediated by neuronal populations that respond to both observation and execution. Relationship to previous studies on xmRS Here we have demonstrated significant xmRS effects in the human IFG where three previous fMRI studies have failed (Dinstein et al 2007; Chong et al 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009;). There are a number of differences between the studies that might explain why the current study has yielded significant effects. First, the task adopted here was designed to maximise the sensitivity to mirror-neuron discharge based on what is known of the properties of mirror neurons in area F5 of the macaque monkey. Here, the subjects executed and observed actions that involved grasping J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 6 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts an object as opposed to the pantomime actions that were employed in previous studies (Dinstein et al 2007; Chong et al 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009). Neurons in F5 of the macaque monkey have been shown to discharge predominantly during observation of actions with a goal (Umilta et al. 2001; Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004). Indeed, mirror neurons in the macaque have not been shown to discharge to mimed or pantomimed actions (Umilta et al. 2001). Therefore, a priori there is no reason to expect mirror neurons to behave differently in humans. Therefore in the current study subjects observed and executed actions to actually physically grasp objects. However, not only did subjects perform and observe actual grasping actions but the objects grasped were carefully selected based on what is known of the firing properties of neurons in area F5 in the macaque monkey. The logic of the repetition suppression paradigm assumes that when the same stimulus is presented twice the same neuronal population is activated and when different stimuli are presented sequentially that different neuronal populations are activated. In other words, the success of the repetition suppression paradigm is contingent upon the different stimuli activating neuronal populations that are predominantly non-overlapping. The two objects grasped in the current study were chosen because they have been shown in previous studies to activate different neuronal populations in area F5 of the macaque monkey (Rizzolatti et al. 1988; Gentilucci et al. 1988; Jeannerod et al. 1995). Given that the success of a repetition suppression paradigm is contingent upon the different stimuli activating neuronal populations that are predominantly non-overlapping, it is of perhaps of no surprise that the effects at the individual subject level are focal, i.e have high spatial specificity, even though the peak of the distribution within these clusters was often highly significant. This is similar to the fMRI results of Nelissen et al. (2005) that also demonstrated high spatial specificity of the BOLD response in area F5 of the macaque monkey. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Although, the difference in the transitive nature of the action both executed and observed between the study described here and the three previous studies that failed to show xmRS could explain the difference in the results of the studies (Dinstein et al 2007; Chong et al 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009) the requirement for actual object grasping actions presents a potential interpretational issue (see Lingnau et al. 2009) - separating adaptation effects due to mirror neurons from those due to canonical neurons that respond to the object alone. Here we designed the manipulandum so that both objects were present in all observation trials. In this way subjects had no way of knowing which object was to be grasped and the only difference between the repetitions was in the observed action and not in the objects presented. In this way we can assert that any xmRS effects observed here were not simply due to the discharge of object related neurons in IFG. The second difference between the study described here and the three previous studies that failed to show xmRS in the IFG (Dinstein et al 2007; Chong et al 2008; Lingnau et al. 2009), was that in the current study the scanning parameters were optimised to increase signal to noise in the human IFG. Specifically, here we did not scan the whole brain but only scanned 16 slices that covered the IFG. This has the advantage that we collected approximately three times as much data from the IFG than we would have done had we collected whole brain scans. However, this approach has the disadvantage that data was not collected from other regions of the cortex where previous studies have shown evidence for mirror neurons, such as the inferior parietal lobule (Fogassi et al 2005; Hamilton and Grafton 2006; 2008). The final difference between the study described here and the three previous studies is that in the current study one of the three analyses that demonstrated a significant xmRS effect in the IFG was a group level ROI analysis where the ROI was defined functionally and not anatomically. This is, we believe, an important factor. The human homologue of area F5 is not known. There is no probabilistic map of the human homologue of area F5 that one could J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 7 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts define a priori. The human homologue of area F5 is known to contain areas of both BA44 and BA6 (Rizzolatti et al. 2001; Morin and Grezes 2008) and possibly even BA45. In other words, it is currently impossible to accurately define a ROI that corresponds to the areas of F5 in which mirror neurons have been found a priori. In light of this we defined our ROI, a local weighted average centred on a single voxel, from the results of an orthogonal functional dataset (Note that defining a ROI based on functional data can be biased (Kriegeskorte et al. 2009). Here, the ROI defining contrast was orthogonal from the xmRS contrast and was even based on different columns of the same design matrix). The use of a subject specific functionally defined ROI allowed for spatial variance in the xmRS response location within the IFG. All other analyses were restricted to a broad search region that included BA45, BA44 and BA6 In addition to the uncertainty over the human homologue of area F5, area F5 has been shown to be subdivided into three cytoarchitecturally different regions - F5a, F5p and F5c (Belmalih et al. 2009; Nelissen et al. 2005). Neurons in each of these sub divisions are activated under observation and execution of actions but mirror neurons have been demonstrated only in area F5c (Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004; Nelissen et al. 2005) It is therefore likely that the human homologue of area F5 is organised with similar subdivisions. Indeed here we show some evidence that this might be the case. We showed that the peaks of activity with IFG are in systematically difference locations depending on whether the contrast was observing, executing, or xmRS (Fig. 5). These effects are small, but significant. The different locations of the peak sensitivity to the different contrasts in area IFG reported here may correspond to such homologous subdivisions of human IFG. To date human neuroimaging of mirror neuron activity has focussed upon reporting activations with IFG at the group level where these effects would not necessarily be observed and as far as we are aware no study has demonstrated subdivisions of IFG. Summary Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts This study was designed to provide evidence that mirror neurons exist within the human homologue of monkey area F5, IFG. Previous studies, demonstrating that fMRI activations have overlapping spatial representations within IFG, are not sufficient evidence for human mirror neurons. Here we have shown that there is significant repetition suppression within IFG both when actions are first observed and then executed and vice-versa. This is consistent with the behaviour of mirror neurons in the monkey area F5. Furthermore we provide evidence that human IFG may contain subdivisions in a homologous manner to area F5 of the macaque monkey. Supplementary Material Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material. Acknowledgments JMK, AN, NW, KJF and CDF were funded by the Wellcome trust, UK. References Belmalih A, Borra E, Contini M, Gerbella M, Rozzi S, Luppino G. Cortical connections of the visuomotor parietooccipital area V6Ad of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2009; 512:183. [PubMed: 19003790] Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001; 13:400. 2001. [PubMed: 11168545] J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 8 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Chong TT, Cunnington R, Williams MA, Kanwisher N, Mattingley JB. fMRI adaptation reveals mirror neurons in human inferior parietal cortex. Curr Biol. 2008; 18:1576. [PubMed: 18948009] Decety J, Grèzes J, Costes N, Perani D, Jeannerod M, Procyk E, Grassi F, Fazio F. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain. 1997; 120:1763. [PubMed: 9365369] Deichmann R, Schwarzbauer C, Turner R. Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T. Neuroimage. 2004; 21(2):757–67. [PubMed: 14980579] Di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp. Brain Res. 1992; 91:176. [PubMed: 1301372] Dinstein I, Thomas C, Behrmann M, Heeger DJ. A mirror up to nature. Curr Biol. 2008; 18:R13. [PubMed: 18177704] Dinstein I, Hasson U, Rubin N, Heeger DJ. Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 98:1415. [PubMed: 17596409] Dinstein I. Human cortex: reflections of mirror neurons. Curr Biol. 2008; 18:R956. [PubMed: 18957251] Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G. Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science. 2005; 308:662. [PubMed: 15860620] Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 1996; 119:593. [PubMed: 8800951] Gallese, V.; Fogassi, L.; Fadiga, L.; Rizzolatti, G. Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In: Prinz, W.; Hommel, B., editors. Attention & Performance XIX. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action. 2002. p. 247 Gazzola V, Keysers C. The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data. Cereb Cortex. 2009; 19(6):1239–55. [PubMed: 19020203] Gentilucci M, Fogassi L, Luppino G, Matelli M, Camarda R, Rizzolatti G. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. I. Somatotopy and the control of proximal movements. Exp Brain Res. 1988; 71:475. [PubMed: 3416964] Grafton ST. Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009; 1156:97–117. [PubMed: 19338505] Grèzes J, Decety J. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001; 12:1. [PubMed: 11198101] Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006; 10:14. [PubMed: 16321563] Hamilton AF, Grafton ST. Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci. 2006; 26:1133. [PubMed: 16436599] Hamilton AF, Grafton ST. Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008; 18:1160. [PubMed: 17728264] Jeannerod M, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G, Sakata H. Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci. 1995; 18:314. [PubMed: 7571012] Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci. 2009; 12(5):535–40. [PubMed: 19396166] Lingnau A, Gesierich B, Caramazza A. Asymmetric fMRI adaptation reveals no evidence for mirror neurons in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009; 106(24):9925–30. [PubMed: 19497880] Morin O, Grèzes J. What is “mirror” in the premotor cortex? A review. Neurophysiol Clin. 2008; 38(3):189–95. [PubMed: 18539253] Nelissen K, Luppino G, Vanduffel W, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA. Observing others: multiple action representation in the frontal lobe. Science. 2005; 310:332. [PubMed: 16224029] Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004; 27:169. [PubMed: 15217330] J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 9 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Matelli M, Bettinardi V, Paulesu E, et al. Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain Res. 1996; 111:246. [PubMed: 8891654] Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001; 2:661. [PubMed: 11533734] Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Fogassi L, Gentilucci M, Luppino G, Matelli M. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp Brain Res. 1988; 71:491. [PubMed: 3416965] Umilta MA, Kohler E, Gallesse V, Fogassi L, Fadiga L, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G. I know what you are doing. A neurophsyiological study. Neuron. 2001; 31:155. [PubMed: 11498058] Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 10 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Figure 1. Experimental task. Each trial consisted of a pair of stimuli presented one after the other. The pairs either contained two execution conditions, two observation conditions or one execution and one observation condition. For cross-modal repetition suppression, the key trials are those that contain one execution and one observation stimulus. The 4 trials shown in Fig. 1 are cross-modal trials. In the execution conditions, the red arrow pointed to the object that was to be moved - either the ring pull or the precision grip. The four trials show exemplars of the following trial types - execute-observe same, execute-observe different, executeobserve same and observe-execute different. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 11 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Figure 2. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Single subject data. (A-C,E,F) show SPMs of the t-values from a single subject, Subject 7, for the different contrasts of interest. This subject was the median subject based upon the peak t-value for the xmRS same<diff contrast. All the SPM search volumes (shown in (D) by a yellow box) were restricted to the IFG but are shown on the whole brain. All further analysis was restricted to posterior IFG corresponding to a complex of BA44 and ventral BA6. This is shown in (D) by a red box. (A) SPM showing where the BOLD activity is greater during action execution than during the wait period and (B) shows where the BOLD activity is greater during action observation than during the wait period. (C) Voxels that were conjointly active during both action observation and action execution. The SPMs in (A-C) are shown thresholded at p<0.001 (uncorrected). (e) Regions of IFG with a cross modal repetition suppression (xmRS) effect and (F) shows regions with a unimodal repetition suppression (umRS) effect. These SPMs are thresholded at p<0.05 (uncorrected). In (A-C,E & F) the SPM is shown at for the axial slice at maximal activity in the left and right hemispheres separately. J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 12 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Figure 3. xmRS effects (A) The parameter estimates for each of the 10 subjects (black bars) for the xmRS when subjects first observed and then executed an action. In 8/10 subjects the parameter estimate was less when subjects executed the same action compared with a different action. The mean parameter estimate across all subjects is shown in white, error bars show the standard error. (B) shows the same data as (A) but for pairs in which the subjects first executed and then observed an action. Here 9/10 subjects show a repetition suppression effect. In both cases the parameter estimate was taken at the voxel that corresponded to the peak t-value for the conjunction of unimodal execution>wait and observation>wait in the left IFG (this voxel selection was orthogonal to any differences between cross model trials). * indicates significance at p<0.05. J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 13 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Figure 4. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Group level SPM. A) This show the SPM showing where the xmRS effect was significant at the second level. The SPM has been thresholded liberally (p<0.05 uncorrected at peak level and p<0.1 uncorrected at cluster level). This liberal threshold was chosen to show the spatial specificity of the effect. Peak level inference revealed significant peaks in two clusters. One was significant p<0.05 corrected for small volume correction for left IFG; t = 6.35 at -50, -2, 12. The second was significant at p<0.001 uncorrected t = 5.11 at -32, -48, 30. This cluster could reflect activity in the anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS) and has a similar location to BOLD activations reported in previous action observation studies (see Dinstein et al. 2007 for comparison). However, the scanning parameters of the current study did not allow for a total coverage of the aIPS and therefore further studies would be required to confirm whether this did in fact correspond to activity aIPS. For display the clusters are projected onto a rendered image of a template brain. B) shows the position of the top and bottom slice showing the extent of the brain included in the 16 slices for a single subject J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Kilner et al. Page 14 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Figure 5. Spatial organisation of IFG Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts (A-D) shows the locations of the mean of the co-ordinates of the peak t-value for the 3 contrasts of interest: exe>wait (blue) obs>wait (green) and xmRS same<different (red). In (A-B) the locations are depicted upon an axial slices and (C-D) on a coronal slice. (B & D) show a magnified view of the data shown in (A & C) respectively. In each plot the ellipsoid is centred at the mean co-ordinates across subjects and the size of the ellipsoid is the standard error in the x, y and z co-ordinates. J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Table 1 Left IFG Subject Right IFG Peak MNI coordinates P value t value J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. x y z 1 -38 2 36 <0.001 9.62 2 -56 8 24 <0.001 5.31 3 -62 8 16 0.012 4 -56 4 12 <0.001 5 -56 8 32 6 -60 8 16 7 -58 10 28 8 -58 0 18 9 -60 6 10 -60 8 Subject Peak MNI coordinates P value t value x y z 1 44 12 32 <0.001 9.1 2 48 12 22 <0.001 5.6 2.25 3 40 6 24 <0.001 9.96 3.86 4 42 12 24 <0.001 3.87 <0.001 11.66 5 46 8 30 <0.001 11.71 <0.001 20.82 6 54 4 26 <0.001 20.09 <0.001 8.01 7 40 12 28 <0.001 9.71 <0.001 12.86 8 60 6 32 <0.001 11.72 30 <0.001 4.06 9 42 10 30 <0.001 11.47 26 <0.001 6.15 10 48 4 32 <0.001 4.99 Kilner et al. Conjunction: exe > wait & obs > wait Page 15 Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts View publication stats Table 2 Left IFG Subject Right IFG Peak MNI coordinates P value t value J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 12. x y z 1 -36 12 28 0.084 1.36 2 -50 8 18 <0.001 5.5 3 -40 8 12 0.069 1.47 4 -56 -4 16 0.007 2.48 5 -46 12 20 0.009 2.38 6 -58 8 24 <0.001 3.48 7 -62 -4 26 0.003 8 -62 -2 20 <0.001 9 -58 6 26 <0.001 10 -60 12 22 0.002 Subject Peak MNI coordinates P value t value x y z 1 62 10 30 0.019 2.06 2 58 12 22 <0.001 3.21 3 50 -4 10 0.026 1.95 4 46 8 18 <0.001 3.49 5 62 10 18 <0.001 .3.12 6 62 2 24 <0.001 3.75 2.77 7 64 -2 22 <0.001 3.37 4.01 8 64 -4 36 <0.001 3.19 3.49 9 44 12 28 -0.003 2.72 2.83 10 58 12 36 -0.007 2.44 Kilner et al. xmRS: same < different Page 16