Avar Grammar Sketch
Diana Forker
Submitted to Polinsky, Maria (ed.) Handbook of Caucasian languages. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
1. Language and its speakers
Avar (ISO 639-3 ava) is by far the largest indigenous language spoken in Daghestan. The
total number of ethnic Avars is around one million, of who live, according to the 2010
census, 912,090 people in the Russian Federation and around 49,800 in Azerbaijan (census
data from 2009). Traditionally, Avars have lived in the central and western parts of
Daghestan, and there are also Avar communities in northern Azerbaijan and very few in
Georgia. Since the last 150 years many Avars migrated to Turkey and to various parts of
Russia.
Avar can be roughly divided into a northern and a southern dialectal group. Standard
(literary) Avar developed from an Avar koiné called bolmac’ (‘МШЦЦШЧ ХКЧРЮКРО’, ПrШЦ bo
‘ЩОШЩХО, sШМТОЭв, МШЦЦЮЧТЭв’ + -l genitive case and mac’ ‘ХКЧРЮКРО’) ЮsОН Лв sЩОКФОrs ШП
northern varieties for interdialectal communication, in particular the Hunzakh variety. Avar
has also served as lingua franca for other Daghestanian ethnic groups such as Andic and
Tsezic people and has left many traces in their languages.
A part of the territory where Avar was spoken was under the cultural influence of
Georgia, and some inscriptions in Avar with Georgian letters have been found (Alekseev et al
2014: 71). The first dated document of Avar, written with the Arabic script, originates from
the 15th century (Aleskeev & Ataev 1997: 20), but a major production of texts started only in
the 18th century when a stable orthography for Avar was developed. This orthography made
also use of the Arabic script and was in use until 1928 when Avar, as many other minority
languages of the Soviet Union, received a Latin orthography. Finally in 1938 a Cyrillic
orthography was introduced, which is still in use today.
Avar is one of the 14 official languages of Daghestan. It is taught in schools; there are
a number of Avar newspapers and journals, radio and TV programs and nowadays many
Internet sites (Wikipedia in Avar, two Avar corpora1, Radio Free Europe in Avar2, etc.).
2. History of research and documentation
Important publications on the Avar language are, among others, von Uslar (1889), Bokarev
(1949), Saidov (1967), Charachidzé (1981), Alekseev & Ataev (1998), Madieva (1967, 1981,
2000), Mallaeva (2007), Alekseev et al. (2014) and Gimbatov (Ms). Alekseev (1988)
contains a reconstruction of proto-Avar. Most of these works treat Standard Avar as spoken
in Daghestan. A notable exception is Charachidzц’s (1981) РrКЦЦКr ШП HЮЧгКФС Avar (that
is close to Standard Avar) as spoken by migrants in Turkey in the 1960s and 1970s.
Grammatical sketches in English are Ebeling (1966) and Rudnev (2015; especially Chapter
2). There exists far more literature on Avar, but the publications are almost exclusively in
Russian, Avar or Georgian, often somewhat outdated and hardly available to scholars from
outside the Caucasus.
1
2
http://baltoslav.eu/avar/ and http://web-corpora.net/AvarCorpus/.
http://www.radioerkenli.com/
1
3. Phonology
3.1. The phoneme inventory and the alphabet
Avar has five vowels given in Table 1. According to Ebeling (1966: 60), “ЭСОrО Тs СКrНХв КЧв
Avar dialect where i and e, or u and o КrО НТsЭТЧМЭ ТЧ КХХ ЩШsТЭТШЧs.” There are also nasalized
variants of the vowels, e.g. ũ, ã, but they seem to lack phonemic status and are mostly
confined to onomatopoetic words and a few interjections. The diphthongs uj, aj, ej, oj, aw,
ow, etc. can be analyzed as consisting of two phonemes, a vowel and a semivowel j or w.
Table 1: Vowel inventory
Front Central Back
High
i
u
Mid
e
o
Low
a
The consonant inventory of Avar is given in Table 2. The Table is largely based on
Magomedov (Ms). Avar has voiced, voiceless, ejective (glottalized) and tense (strong)
obstruents, but only for the dental and palatal fricatives consonants of all four series are
attested. Ejectives and tense consonants are not positionally restricted. The uvular stops q and
qʼ are phonetically affricates q͡χ and q͡ʼχ. Ejectivization is restricted to voiceless stops and
affricates. The distinction tense vs. lax is found with voiceless fricatives and affricates, but
not all show it due to the lack of the (mostly) lax counterpart, e.g. lax š is (almost) absent.
Examples of minimal pairs are given in (1) (partially from Magomedov, In Prep.).
(1)
М Яs. Мː
Мʼ Яs. Мːʼ
s vs. sː
ɬ Яs. ɬː
č Яs. čː
čʼ Яs. čːʼ
χ vs. χː
χʷ Яs. χːʷ
ic ‘moth’ vs. icː ‘spring, source’
МʼО ‘fill.IMP’ vs. cːʼe ‘she-goat’
si ‘tower’ vs. sːi ‘dignity’
ɬel ‘cover, case’ vs. ɬːel ‘water.GEN’,
maɬ ‘fingernail, claw, hoof’ vs. maɬː ‘ЭОКМСТЧР, education’
bičize ‘sell’ vs. bičːize ‘get wet’
bičʼize ‘stab, pierce, unlock’ Яs. bičːʼize ‘understand’
χam ‘raw skin’ vs. χːam ‘cloth, canvas’
χʷeze ‘die’ vs. χːʷeze ‘sХЮrЩ’
All velar and uvular consonants and many other obstruents occur in plain and labialized form.
Labialization of bilabial and dental stops, as well as most dental, lateral/alveolar and palatal
fricatives is (almost) inexistent. For instance, zʷ, žʷ, tʷ, tʼʷ, dʷ occur only in onomatopoeia, the
occurrence of ɬʷ is also very rare, and šʷ is absent. In the Cyrillic orthography, labialization is
ТЧНТМКЭОН Лв . Labialization is restricted to syllable-initial positions and mostly found wordinitially. There are also a number of examples with word-internal labialization, especially
within verbs. Syllable-final (and therefore word-final) labialization is not attested. Examples
of labialized consonants are (2). Labialized consonants are delabialized before labialized
vowels o and u in inflected words.
(2)
čʼʷantʼ ‘long, thin stick’
sʷer ‘turn’
bakʼʷali ‘stomach, tripe’
tʼiqʼʷa ‘horseshoe, sole’
cʼʷakizabize ‘make shine’
ƛʼʷaha- anqize ‘shoot’
ʁʷetʼ ‘tree’
kʼalχʷeze ‘not fast, skip fasting’ (e.g. Ramadan)
There are very few minimal pairs that prove the phonemic status of at least some of the
labialized consonants (3).
2
(3)
b-aqʼara-b3 ‘hungry’ vs. b-aqʼʷara-b ‘dry, dried’
kar ‘hair, mane’ vs. kʷar ‘rope’
The current Avar alphabet, which is based on Cyrillic, is presented in Table 3. The Table also
shows the Latin transliteration used in this article and the IPA transcription. The alphabet
МШЧЭКТЧs ЭаШ ЦШrО ХОЭЭОrs. TСО ХОЭЭОr Ё ё ОбТsЭs ШЧХв ТЧ RЮssТКЧ ХШКЧs ЭСКЭ КrО sЩОХХОН ТЧ
Russian with thО sКЦО ХОЭЭОr; ЭСО ХОЭЭОr
, Тs КХsШ ШЧХв ПШЮЧН ТЧ ХШКЧs.
Table 3: The Avar alphabet and its transliteration
Avar transl. IPA Avar transl. IPA
Avar
a
a
q
q
͡Эɬːʼ
b
b
ƛ’
w
w
Ӏ Ӏ Фʼ
Фʼ
g
ɡ
l
l
Ӏ Ӏ
͡Эɬː, ɬ, ɬː
ʁ
ʁ
ɬ, ƛ
h
h
m
m
Ӏ Ӏ
Ӏ Ӏ
n
n
d
d
o
o
Ӏ Ӏ
je, e
je, e
p
p
ž
ʒ
r
r
z
z
s
s, sː
i
i
t
t
j
j
Ӏ Ӏ Эʼ
Эʼ
k
k
u
u
transl.
χ
q
x
IPA
χ, χː
Ъː
бː
c
Мʼ
č
čʼ
š
šː
͡Эs, ͡Эsː
͡Эsʼ, ͡Эsːʼ
͡ Эʃ
͡Эʃʼ, ͡Эʃːʼ
ʃ
ʃː
e
ju
ja
e
ju
ja
Although the distinction tense vs. lax has phonemic status, it is not always indicated in the
orthography. The phonemes čː, kː and kːʼ КrО КХаКвs sЩОХХОН , , КЧН Ӏ Ӏ rОsЩОМЭТЯОХв.
The phonemes cː, cːʼ, sː, χː, and čːʼ by contrast, are only spelled with double consonants ( ,
Ӏ Ӏ, , , Ӏ Ӏ) in minimal pairs. This means, in turn, that the occurrences of many tense
phonemes are not indicated in the orthography.
Furthermore, tense phonemes that lack a lax counterpart (e.g. xː, qː) are never spelled with
double graphemes. TСО ЦШsЭ МШЧПЮsТЧР РrКЩСОЦОs КrО
(=ДЭ͡ɬːЖ, [ɬ], [ɬː]) КЧН
(= ДɬːЖ,
͡ДЭɬːЖ). TСОy МКЧ ЛШЭС rОЩrОsОЧЭ ЭОЧsО КЧН ХКб ПrТМКЭТЯОs КЧН КППrТМКЭОs, НЮО ЭШ ЭСО КЛsОЧМО ШП К
РrКЩСОЦО ПШr ЭСО КППrТМКЭОs ДЭ͡ɬЖ / ДЭ͡ɬːЖ.
The glottal stop is written when it occurs in syllable-final position, but never when it occurs
in syllable initial position.
The data presented in this grammar sketch is transliterated from the Avar Cyrillic
orthography. Therefore, the phonemic contrasts that are not marked in the orthography are
also not indicated in the transliteration.
3.2. Phonotactics and accent
The syllable structure is comparatively simple. The minimal syllable consists of a single
vowel. Initial vowels are always preceded by a non-phonemic glottal stop not indicated in the
orthography. The syllables in monomorphemic native words are V, VC, VRC, CV, CVC and
CVRC, with CV and CVC being the most frequent syllable types. In other words, syllables
3
Gender agreement in cited words is always displayed as neuter singular, i.e., via the marker b, unless otherwise
indicated.
3
never have complex onsets, but can have complex codas, and the general syllable structure
can be schematized as (C)V(R)(C). The only types of superheavy syllables are VRC and
CVRC with only sonorants (/r/, /l/, /n/, /m/, /j/) permitted in the position of the first consonant
in the coda. The minimal word (i.e. free root) has the shape V (4).
(4)
V
VC
VRC
CV
CVC
CVRC
a ‘РШ!’ (РШ.IMP)
aχ ‘garden’, ic ‘moth’, al ‘they’
anƛʼ ‘аООФ’
bi ‘blood’, si ‘tower’, či ‘man’, co ‘one’, ču ‘horse’, cʼa ‘fire’
dun ‘I’, mun ‘you’ (sg.), čed ‘bread’, tʼutʼ ‘fly’
qʼʷerqʼ ‘frog’, pinqʼ ‘blister’, čerχ ‘body’, ħerčː ‘wooden plate’
Avar has dynamic accent. Word stress is variable, but always occurs on the first or second
syllable. In morphologically complex words it can be distinctive (Alekseev et al. 2014: 2729). Some suffixes such as the iterative suffix -dar attract stress.
3.3. Morphophonology
The most important morphophonological processes are vowel syncope, assimilation and
mutation, consonant alternation and metathesis.
Vowel syncope occurs when stress shifts from the second to either the first or the third
syllable in inflected words (5). In all examples, stressed vowels are underlined.
(5)
ereqʼ ‘lump’ > erqʼ-i-l lump-OBL-GEN; erqʼ-al lump-PL
kaʁat ‘letter’ > kaʁt-i-l letter-OBL-GEN; kaʁt-al letter-PL
Vowel assimilation and vowel mutation often occur with inflected nouns. In some cases the
stem vowels completely assimilate to vowels of oblique stem markers and plural markers,
e.g. i, e, a > o; i, e > a, and e, a > u in the following examples (6).
(6)
ƛːʼili ‘saddle’; genitive singular ƛːʼol-o-l; plural ƛːʼa-lal
betʼer ‘head’; genitive singular botʼr-o-l; plural butʼr-ul
lačen ‘falcon’; genitive singular ločn-o-l; plural lučn-ul
Consonant alternation n > m and m > n (7) as well as metathesis (8) usually occur wordinternally and affect stem consonants.
(7)
n > m keren ‘breast’ > plural kurm-ul; daran ‘trade, commerce’ > plural darm-al
emen ‘father’ > umum-ul ‘fathers, parents’
m > n tʼamaχ ‘leaf’ > plural tʼanχ-al; gomog ‘gutter’ > plural gong-al
(8)
ebel ‘mother’ > plural ulb-ul
qumur ‘wolf’ > plural qurm-al
4. Morphology
4.1. Nouns
From a morphological point of view, Avar is agglutinative with some elements of fusion. The
categories grammatically expressed by nominals are gender, number, and case. Avar has
three genders, namely human masculine, human feminine and neuter comprising all other
4
nouns. Gender is mainly marked through affixes on verbs, adjectives, certain pronouns and
some other agreement targets.
The most common suffix that forms nouns in the absolutive plural is -bi. Other plural suffixes
are, e.g., -ul, -l, -al, and -jal. In some cases the plural formation is accompanied by vowel
mutation or vowel deletion in the nominal stem.
The core cases are absolutive (Ø), ergative (-s/-ɬ/-ca), dative (-e / -je) and genitive (-l / -ul / r). Furthermore, there are 20 spatial cases. Nouns are divided into three declension classes.
For nominals of the first and second class, the ergative form is also used as oblique stem
marker to which all other case suffixes are added. Thus, we can treat nouns of these two
declension classes as having a zero suffix for the ergative that follows the overt oblique stem
marker.
The first declension class, to which most human masculine singular nouns belong, has the
oblique stem marker -s (allomorphs -as and -jas); the second declension class comprises all
human feminine singular nouns and many neuter nouns and has -ɬ/-aɬ/-jaɬ for the oblique
stem. The ergative suffix of the third declension class is -ca (-aca), which is not used as
oblique stem marker. Instead, many nouns of the third declension class make use of oblique
stem markers before case suffixes. The oblique stem markers are -i, -o, -u, -a, -du, -da, -zu, ro (only one word), and -lu (only one word).
For nominals in the plural the ergative suffix is mostly -z, and for a few nouns it is -ca. When
case suffixes are added to nominals in the plural a number of morphophonological changes
take place, i.e. the final -l of plural suffixes such as -ul or -al is deleted. Table 4 displays
partial paradigms of nouns from all three declension classes.
Table 4: Partial paradigms of inflected nouns
1. Declension class
‘sШЧ’
absolutive
ergative
genitive
dative
SPR-essive
APUD-essive
APUD-lative
APUD-ablative
was
was-as
was-as-ul
was-as-e
was-as-da
was-as-uq
was-as-uq-e
was-as-uq-a
absolutive
ergative
dative
apud-essive
was-al
was-az
was-az-e
was-az-uq
2. Declension class
‘НКЮРСЭОr’
Singular
jas
jas-aɬ
jas-aɬ-ul
jas-aɬ-e
jas-al-da
jas-aɬ-uq
jas-aɬ-uq-e
jas-aɬ-uq-a
Plural
jas-al
jas-az
jas-az-e
jas-az-uq
3. Declension class
‘ПШб’
cer
car-aca
car-al
car-aje
car-ada
car-aq
car-aq-e
car-aq-a
cur-du-l
cur-du-z
cur-du-z-e
cur-du-z-uq
The absolutive marks:
- the sole argument of intransitive and extended intransitive verbs (including detransitivized
verbs)
- the patient and theme argument of transitive and ditransitive verbs
- the stimulus argument of affective verbs
- the subject-like arguments and nominal predicates in copula clauses
The ergative marks:
5
- the agent of transitive and ditransitive predicates
- the instrument
- temporal adjunctions and reason/cause adjuncts
The genitive marks various types of relations, e.g. adnominal attributes denoting possession,
and complements of most postpositions. The dative, which is in form identical to the lative,
marks the experiencer with a restricted set of affective predicates, and it expresses goals and
goal-like functions, recipients, and beneficiaries.
The suffixes for the spatial cases are shown in Table 5. The spatial cases are formally and
functionally rather transparent and organized along the two dimensions location and direction
(movement). There are five suffixes that express different ways of locating an item with
respect to a reference point:
- SPR-series -da: on or in a reference point
- APUD-series -q: at, by, close to a reference point
- INTER-series -ɬ: location in a substance or amorphous mass
- SUB-series -ƛ’: under a reference point
- IN-series -GM / -s: in a reference point (often container-like)
There is a four-way distinction in terms of direction:
- essive (zero marked): location at a reference point, absence of movement
- lative (-e): direction to a goal
- ablative (-a/-sa): movement away from a reference point
- translative (-an/-san): movement through or along a reference point
Table 5: The spatial cases in Avar
SPR ‘ШЧ, in’
APUD ‘at’
essive
-da
-q
lative
-d-e
-q-e
ablative
-da-s-a
-q-a
translative
-da-s-an
-q-an
INTER
-ɬ
-ɬ-e
-ɬ-a
-ɬ-an
‘in’
SUB
‘ЮЧНОr’
-ƛ’
-ƛ’-e
-ƛ’-a
-ƛ’-an
IN
‘ТЧ’
-GM
-GM-e
-s-a
-s-an
In addition to their spatial meaning a number of the spatial cases also fulfill grammatical
functions. The most important case in this respect is the SPR-essive, which marks the
experiencer with many affective predicates (33), the addressee (31), (38), temporal adjuncts,
and the causee (21a, b).
Some extended intransitive verbs such as b-alah-ize ‘ХШШФ КЭ’, enekː-ize ‘ХТsЭОЧ ЭШ’ (16) and
ħinq’-ize ‘ЛО КПrКТН ШП’ ЭКФО КrРЮЦОЧЭs ЦКrФОН аТЭС sЩКЭТКХ МКsОs sЮМС Кs ЭСО APUD-essive,
SPR-essive (the first two verbs) and the SUB-ablative. The standard of comparison in
comparative constructions takes the SPR-ablative.
4.2. Pronouns
Avar has the following types of pronouns: personal, demonstrative, reflexive, reciprocal,
interrogative, and indefinite pronouns.
Personal pronouns only encode the first and second person. First person plural pronouns
distinguish an inclusive from an exclusive form. Table 6 displays a partial paradigm of the
personal pronouns.
Table 6: Partial paradigms of personal pronouns
1SG ‘I’ 2SG ‘вШЮ’ 1PL ТЧМХЮsТЯО ‘аО’ 1PL ОбМХЮsТЯО ‘аО’ 2PL ‘вШЮ’
absolutive dun
mun
niɬ
ЧТž
ЧЮž
ergative
di-ca
du-ca
niɬ-eca
ЧТž-eca
ЧЮž-eca
6
genitive
di-r
dative
di-je
SPR-essive di-da
du-r
du-je
du-da
ЧТž-er
ЧТž-eje
ЧТž-eda
niɬ-er
niɬ-eje
niɬ-eda
ЧЮž-er
ЧЮž-eje
ЧЮž-eda
Third person is expressed by demonstratives which distinguish gender, number, proximity to
speech act participants, and height.
- (h)a-w4 / (h)a-j / (h)a-b / (h)a-l: close to the speaker; same height
- (h)e-w / (h)e-j / (h)e-b / (h)e-l: away from the speaker; close to the addressee
- do-w / do-j / do-b / do-l // hada-w / hada-j / hada-b / hada-l: away from the speaker
- ʁo-w / ʁo-j / ʁo-b / ʁo-l // haʁa-w / haʁa-j / haʁa-b / haʁa-l: away from the speaker and
lower than the speaker
- ɬo-w / ɬo-j / ɬo-b / ɬo-l: away from the speaker and higher than the speaker
Partial paradigms showing their inflectional patterns can be found in Table 7. In addition to
encoding third person (especially hew, hej, heb and hel) (29b), (40b), Avar demonstratives
can be used in the typical demonstrative function (9).
(9)
ha-b surat-aɬ-uq
b-alah-e,
do-z-uq
guro!
this-n picture-obl-apud hpl-look-imp that-obl.pl-apud cop.neg
‘LШШФ КЭ ЭСТs ЩТМЭЮrО, ЧШЭ КЭ ЭСШsО!’ (ЩХЮrКХ КННrОssОО) (elicited)
There are three different types of reflexive pronouns that all express gender, number and
case:
- the simple reflexives ži-w / ži-j / ži-b / ža-l
- the reduplicated reflexives, e.g. ži-w=go žinca=go
- the complex reflexives ži-w=go / ži-j=go / ži-b=go / ža-l=go
A partial paradigm of the simple reflexives in the singular and plural is shown in Table 7.
In reflexive constructions, the reflexive pronouns refer only to third person. For first and
second person reflexivization ordinary personal pronouns are used.
The simple reflexive pronouns are strictly non-local and include logophoric contexts. The
reduplicated and the complex reflexives contain the emphatic/contrastive enclitic =go. The
reduplicated reflexive pronouns can only be bound within the clause, whereas the complex
reflexives can be used in local and long-distance reflexivization. In the function of reciprocal
ЩrШЧШЮЧ ЭСО rОНЮЩХТМКЭОН ЧЮЦОrКХ ‘ШЧО’ Тs ЮsОН, Т.О. co.co (co.ca-z in the ergative). The
reduplicated reflexive pronouns as well as the reciprocal pronoun are composed of two parts.
One part takes the case marking according to the function of the pronoun in the clause. The
other part bears the same case suffix as the antecedent of the pronoun (42), (43), (45a, b). For
a detailed account of reflexive constructions see Rudnev (2017). Reciprocal constructions are
treated in Yamada (2013).
The interrogative pronouns are:
- šːi-w / šːi-j / šːa-l ‘аСШ’
- šːi-b ‘аСКЭ’
- čan ‘СШа ЦКЧв’ (countable)
- ki an ‘СШа ЦЮМС’ (ЮЧМШЮЧЭКЛХО ЦКss)
- kina-w / kina-j / kina-b / kina-l ‘аСТМС’
- kin ‘СШа’
4
The initial glottal fricative in these pronouns and the pronouns given in the next line is typical of the standard
variety of Avar. In non-standard varieties including dialects it is often omitted.
7
- šːaj ‘аСв’
- ki-w / ki-j / ki-b / ki-r ‘аСОrО’ (location)
- ki-w-e / ki-j-e / ki-b-e / ki-r-e ‘ЭШ аСОrО’ (НТrОМЭТШЧ)
- kisa ‘ПrШЦ аСОrО’
- kida ‘when’
As ЭСО ХТsЭ sСШаs, ЭСО ЩrШЧШЮЧs ‘аСШ’ КЧН ‘аСКЭ’ СКЯО ЭСО sКЦО КЛsШХЮЭТЯО sЭОЦ, НТППОrТЧР
only in the final gender suffix. However, they have differing suppletive stems when they are
inflected (Table 7). Furthermore, many of the interrogative pronouns and adverbs derive from
the same root ki-, to which inflectional and derivational suffixes are added.
Table 7: Partial paradigms of demonstratives, reflexives and interrogatives
demonstratives
reflexives
interrogative pronouns
‘СО’
‘sСО’ / ‘ТЭ’ ‘ЭСОв’ singular plural
‘аСШ’
‘аСКЭ’
absolutive
ha-w
ha-j/a-b
ha-l
žТ-w
žКХ
šːТ-w / šːТ-j / šːТ-b
šːК-l
ergative
has
haɬ
haz
žТЧ-ca
žТНО-ca ɬi-ca
sun(du)-ca
genitive
has-ul haɬ-ul
haz-ul žТЧ-di-r žТНО-r
ɬi-l
sundu-l
dative
has-ije haɬ-ije
haz-ije žТЧ-di-je žТНО-je ɬi-je
sundu-je
SPR-essive
has-da hal-da
haz-da žТЧ-da
žТНО-da ɬi-da
sun-da
APUD-essive has-uq haɬ-uq
haz-uq žТЧ-di-q žТНО-q ɬi-q
sundu-q
Indefinite pronouns are formed by adding various suffixes and enclitics to the (case-marked)
interrogative pronouns or to the numeral co ‘ШЧО’. Major indefinite pronouns (illustrated on
the basis of šːi-w ‘аСШ’, ki-b ‘аСОrО’ КЧН kida ‘аСОЧ’ and other quantifiers are:
- specific indefinite
- suffix -ali + =go: šːi-w-ali=go ‘sШЦОЛШНв’ (masc. sg.), ki-b-ali=go ‘sШЦОаСОrО’
- free-choice indefinite
- enclitic =ni + enclitic =gi: šːi-w=ni=gi ‘КЧвЛШНв’ (masc. sg.), ki-b=ni=gi
‘КЧваСОrО’
- universal indefinite:
- enclitic =go (in clauses with positive polarity): šːi-w=go ‘ОЯОrвЛШНв’, ki-b=go
‘ОЯОrваСОrО’, kida=go ‘КХаКвs’
- distributive indefinite:
- reduplication + enclitic =ni + enclitic =gi: šːi-w-šːi-w=ni=gi ‘КЧв, ЯКrТШЮs ЩОШЩХО’
(masc. sg.), ki-b-ki-b=ni=gi ‘ТЧ ЯКrТШЮs ЩХКМОs’
- negative indefinite (only used in clauses with negative polarity)
- enclitic =ni + enclitic =gi: šːi-w=ni=gi ‘ЧШЛШНв’, co=ni=gi ‘ЧШ ШЧО’, ki-b=ni=gi
‘ЧШаСОrО’
- enclitic =go (in clauses with positive polarity these pronouns have universal
indefinite readings): šːi-w=go ‘ЧШЛШНв’, ki-b=go ‘ЧШаСОrО’, kida=go ‘ЧОЯОr’
- ‘ОЯОrв’: šːiba-w / šːiba-j / šːiba-b / šːiba-l
- ‘КХХ, entire, complete’: t’ol=go; t’ola-b=go; t’ol=go-jal
4.3. Verbal morphology
The morphosyntactic categories of verbs in Avar are polarity, tense, mood, modality, aspect
and evidentiality. For a detailed description of the verb in Avar see Mallaeva (2007).
Avar does not have person agreement, only gender and number agreement. Most
vowel-initial verbs, including the affirmative copula-auxiliary =ugo, show gender/number
agreement by means of prefixes. All participles take gender/number agreement suffixes. The
8
agreement affixes, which can appear as prefixes, suffixes, or rarely infixes are displayed in
Table 8. The plural marker -l occurs word-finally, except for adverbials; the marker r occurs
in all other positions.
Table 8: Gender agreement affixes
masculine (I) feminine (II) neuter (III)
singular
w
j
b
plural
-l / r
4.3.1. Synthetic and analytic tenses
Avar has a rich inventory of inflected verb forms. Verbs can be divided into three inflectional
classes depending on the suffixes for the simple present and the future. The finite synthetic
verb forms are (Table 9):
(i) Simple present (-ula / -ola / -la / -una)
The main function of the simple present (Russ. nastojaščee obščee ‘РОЧОrКХ ЩrОsОЧЭ’) is the
expression of situations and events that lack a concrete temporal reference, e.g. because of
their habitual character (10a).
(ii) Future (-ila / -ela / -la / -ina)
The future refers to future situations and actions including intentions, plans, assumptions and
predictions that will be realized in the future (10b).
(iii) Aorist (-ana / -una / -na; negation -č’o / -ič’o / -inč’o)
The aorist is a perfective past that is used as the default past tense in Avar (10c).
(10a) Simple present
čЮ=РТ
ɬa-la
di-da,
çi=gi
ɬa-la
di-da
horse=emph know-prs 1sg-spr
man=emph know-prs 1sg-spr
‘I know horses and I know people.’ (i.e. I am an expert) (Axlakov 1976: 11)
(10b) Future
[For mentioning the king in the prayer he will give me 20 goats.]
hez-ul
КЧМ’go-ja-b di-ca
du-je
ƛ’-ela
dem.pl-gen ten-adj-n
1sg-erg
2sg-dat
give-fut
‘Of them, I will give ten to you.’ (Axlakov 1976: 23)
(10c) Aorist
qartaj
j-ТФ’-un
j-igo,
di-da=go
ɬК-č’Ш
witch
f-be-cvb
f-cop
1sg-spr=emph
know-aor.neg
‘SСО аКs К аТЭМС, ЛЮЭ I НТН ЧШЭ ФЧШа ЭСТs ЦвsОХП.’ (CСКrКМСТНгц 1981: 188)
In the simple present and the future negation is expressed by means of adding a suffix -ro to
the tense suffix. This suffix as well as the suffix for the negative form of the aorist are also
part of guro and heč’o, the two variants of the negative present tense copula-auxiliary.
Table 9: The major inflectional forms (negated forms given in parenthesis)
‘rОКН, study’
‘МШЦО’
‘ФЧШа’
Infinitive
М’КХ-ize
w-Кč’-ine
ɬa-ze
Masdar
М’КХ-i
w-Кč’-in
ɬa-j
Finite synthetic verb forms
Simple present
М’КХ-ula (М’КХ-ula-ro) w-Кč’-una (w-Кč’-una-ro) ɬa-la (ɬa-la-ro)
Future
М’КХ-ila (М’КХ-ila-ro) w-Кč’-ina (w-Кč’-ina-ro) ɬa-la (ɬa-la-ro)
9
М’КХ-ana (М’КХ-Тč’Ш)
w-Кč’-ana (w-Кč’-ТЧč’Ш)
Finite analytic tense forms
Perfect
М’КХ-un b-ugo
w-Кč’-un w-ugo
Pluperfect
М’КХ-un b-ЮФ’КЧК
w-Кč’-un w-ЮФ’КЧК
Compound present М’КХ-ule-b b-ugo
w-Кč’-une-w w-ugo
Compound past
М’КХ-ule-b b-ЮФ’КЧК w-Кč’-une-w w-ЮФ’КЧК
Compound future М’КХ-ule-b b-ЮФ’ina
w-Кč’-une-w w-uk’ТЧК
Prospective future М’КХ-ize b-ugo
w-Кč’-ine w-ugo
Aorist
ɬa-na (ɬa-č’Ш)
ɬa-n b-ugo
ɬa-n b-ЮФ’КЧК
ɬa-le-b b-ugo
ɬa-le-b b-ЮФ’КЧК
ɬa-le-b b-ЮФ’ina
ɬa-ze b-ugo
Many tense/aspect/modality oppositions are expressed by means of analytic verb forms that
make use of the present participle, the perfective converb, and the infinitive. The major
analytic tenses are:
(iv) Perfect (perfective converb + present tense copula-auxiliary) (11a)
Most commonly, the Avar perfect has only a resultative/perfect meaning. It is used when
referring to events accomplished in the past that have relevant consequences for the present.
The perfect is also used when the speaker has only indirect knowledge of the event s/he
narrates, e.g. used when talking about historical events.
(v) Pluperfect (perfective converb + aorist copula-auxiliary) (11b)
The pluperfect is employed when talking about events that happened before a certain moment
in the past or when the resulting state of an event obtained in the past. It can also express
indirect evidentiality.
(vi) Compound present (present participle + present tense copula-auxiliary) (11c)
This tense refers to ongoing situations and activities, but can also easily be used with stative
ЯОrЛs sЮМС Кs ‘ФЧШа’ КЧН ‘ХШЯО’ ЭСКЭ НШ ЧШЭ КХХШа ПШr ЭСО ЩrОsОЧЭ ЩrШРrОssТЯО ТЧ EЧРХТsС.
(vii) Compound past (present participle + aorist copula-auxiliary) (11d)
The compound past corresponds to the compound present in the sense that it is used for
ongoing situations as well as for states in combination with past time reference.
(viii) Compound future (present participle + future copula-auxiliary) (11e)
The compound future expresses ongoing situations and states in combination with temporal
reference to the future.
(ix) Prospective future (infinitive + present tense copula-auxiliary) (11f)
The prospective refers to situations in the immediate future, i.e. to actions and events that are
about to happen or that are intended by an agent, sometimes with a slight flavor of deontic
modality (Mallaeva 2007: 227).
(11a) Perfect
hani-r
ЮМ’-un
r-ugo
kina-l=go
šКrЭ’-al
here-pl provide-cvb pl-cop
all-pl=emph condition-pl
‘Here aХХ МШЧНТЭТШЧs КrО ЩrШЯТНОН.’
(AЯКr ЧОаsЩКЩОr КЪ’ТЪ’КЭ 18, MКв 09, 2013, Щ. 23, sОО СЭЭЩ://ааа.СКФТФКЭ.ТЧПШ/)
(11b) Pluperfect
erkena-b gugari-УКɬ-ul
Ъ’ОМ-az-Юɬ
КбКХɬТ-ze
free-n
wrestling-obl-gen competition-pl.obl-inter take.part-inf
b-Кč’-un
n-come-cvb
b-ЮФ’-ana Ъ’ШРШУКХ-da-sa
М’ТФː’ЮЧ komanda
n-be-aor
twenty-spr-abl
more
team
‘More than twenty teams had come to take part in the competition of free wrestling.’
(AЯКr ЧОаsЩКЩОr КЪ’ТЪ’КЭ 18, MКв 09, 2013, Щ. 7, see http://www.hakikat.info/)
10
(11c) Compound present
jas-Кɬ
čОН
ha-b-ule-b
b-ugo
girl-erg bread do-n-ptpc.prs-n n-cop
‘TСО РТrХ Тs ЦКФТЧР ЛrОКН.’ (elicited)
(11d) Compound past
КРКrɬ-ule-l
r-ЮФ’-ana Ъ’Кžar-al co ros-da-l
rКƛ’-al-d-e
approach-ptcp.prs-pl pl-be-aor Iranian-pl one village-obl-gen earth-obl-spr-lat
‘TСО IrКЧТКЧs аОrО КЩЩrШКМСТЧР ШЧО ЯТХХКРО.’
(Text from an Avar primer, grade 4; http://dugrichilov.livejournal.com/1719.html)
(11e) Compound future
ha-b
Э’Об
М’КХ-ule-w
w-ЮФ’-ina dun
anƛ’-i-ca
dem-n book read-ptcp.prs-m m-be-fut
1sg
week-obl-erg
‘I (masc.) аТХХ rОКН ЭСО ЛШШФ аТЭСТЧ К аООФ.’ (Mallaeva 2007: 227)
(11f)
Prospective future
dun
ОУ-al-d-e
ine
b-ugo
1sg
Hajj-obl-spr-lat go.inf n-cop
‘I аТХХ РШ ШЧ ЭСО HКУУ.’ (Axlakov 1976: 24)
There are more analytic verb forms such as the evidential pluperfect, the evidential imperfect,
and the evidential intentional, which all make use of the copula-auxiliary in the perfect
buk’un bugo. These verb forms have both indirect evidential and non-evidential meanings.
The evidential pluperfect can additionally express relative past time reference, whereas the
evidential imperfect combines past time reference with imperfective aspect, referring to
events in the past described as ongoing and the evidential intentional conveys past time
reference together with indirect evidentiality and intentionality. The indirect evidential
meanings expressed by these forms are hearsay and inferential. They occur in traditional
narratives, in accounts of historical events and in other contexts that allow for indirect
evidentials. (12a) is the first sentence of a traditional tale that was orally transmitted, hence
we have hearsay evidentiality, and then the story goes on with the perfect. Example (12b) is
from the Avar Wikipedia entry about the village of Malib and illustrates the evidential
imperfect For more information see Forker (In Press).
(12a) co čТ
un
w-ЮФ’-un
w-ugo
bazar-al-de
čЮ
one man go.CVB M-be-CVB M-COP
market-OBL-LAT horse
‘OЧО ЦКЧ аОЧЭ ЭШ ЭСО ЦКrФОЭ ЭШ ЛЮв К СШrsО.’ (AбХКФШЯ 1976: 24)
(12b) СКЧžОsО-l
contemporary-PL
ЦК ХКХ
Malib.people
r-Кč’-ine- КЧ
PL-come-INF-UNTIL
b-os-ize
N-take-INF
ЦК ХК-de-r-il
Malib.people-LAT-PL-GEN
rКƛ-al-da
ЮЦrЮ
ha-b-ule-b
b-ЮФ’-un
b-ugo
РЮržТУК-z
earth-OBL-LOC life
do-N-PTCP.PRS-N N-be-CVB
N-COP
Georgians-ERG
‘BОПШrО ЭСО ЩrОsОЧЭ-day Malib people came, Georgians were living on the earth of the
MКХТЛ.’ (СЭЭЩ://КЯ.аТФТЩОНТК.ШrР/аТФТ/
I
)
4.3.2. Non-indicative verb forms
Non-indicative verb forms are imperative, prohibitive, and three different optatives.
(i) Imperative (-a / -e / -j)
11
The suffix of the imperative partially depends on transitivity for verbs ending in a consonant:
intransitive verbs take -a, whereas transitive verbs take -e (13a). Verbs whose stems end in a
vowel take -j.
(ii) Prohibitive (-uge / -unge / -ge) (13a)
(iii) Optatives
The first type of optative is formed by adding -gi to the imperative, the second by attaching ja-GM to the imperative, and the third makes use of the suffix -ad. The gender marker of the
second suffix agrees with the addressee, which is not necessarily the argument in the
absolutive case. The Optative expresses wishes, hopes and also curses (13b). The meaning of
the second and third type are very close to each other (Mallaeva 2007: 71).
(iv) Hortative (infinitive or future + emphatic enclitic =in)
The hortative expresses encouragement or discouragement on part of the speaker with respect
to the addressee performing an action or the wish or intention of the speaker to bring about an
action (13c) (Mallaeva 2007: 66-68; Alekseev & Ataev 1997: 61).
(13a) Imperative & prohibitive
a
dur=go,
amma du-ca
do-s-da
kʷОr
Ъʷa-ge!
go.imp 2.gen=emph but
2sg-erg dem-obl-spr hand scratch-proh
‘GШ, ЛЮЭ НШЧ’Э МШЧЯТЧМО СТЦ аТЭС вШЮr СКЧН!’ (Axlakov 1976: 13)
(13b) Optative
kija-b=go
ber b-aq-a-ja-w!
two-n=emph eye n-fall-imp-opt-m
‘MКв ЛШЭС вШЮr ОвОs ПКХХ ШЮЭ!’ (said to a man) (Rudnev 2015: 39)
(13c) Hortative
di-ca
b-ic-in=in,
du-ca
ТЧ Э’КЦ-e,
Bolo!
1sg-erg n-tell-fut=emph 2sg-erg
ear spread-imp Bolo
‘Let me tell you (something), Bolo, you listen!’ (Axlakov 1976: 15)
4.3.3. Non-finite verb forms: conditionals, deverbal nouns, participles and converbs
The most important non-finite verb forms are:
(i) Infinitive (-ize / -ze / -ine)
The infinitive occurs in analytic tenses such as the prospective future and in many types of
complement constructions (35a). It is also the basis for the formation of certain types of
adverbial clauses (12b).
(ii) Masdar (-i / -j / -in)
The masdar is a deverbal noun that can be inflected for case and is mainly used in
complement clauses (40c).
(iii) Participles
Participles are used in a wide range of constructions such as relative clauses (Section 5.5.4
below), complement clauses (Forker 2016), interrogative clauses (cf. Alekseev & Ataev
1998: 85-91), and term focus constructions (Sumbatova 2009: 316), etc. One of their uses as
heads of independent clauses has been called deklarativnost’ (RЮss. ‘КssОrЭТЯТЭв’) Лв BШФКrОЯ
(1949: 72). I аТХХ ЮsО ЭСО ЭОrЦ ‘КssОrЭТЯО ЦШНКХТЭв’. AssОrЭТЯО ЦШНКХТЭв ШММЮrs ТЧ ЩrШЯОrЛs КЧН
sayings and when the speaker explicitly and emphatically asserts that an action will take
place (Forker In Press). Only in relative clauses and complement clauses the participles head
12
non-finite clauses. In the other three constructions (interrogative clauses, focus construction,
assertive modality) they function as the head of finite clauses. All participles end in gender
agreement suffixes.
(iiia) Present participle (-ula-b / -ola-b / -la-b / -una-b) & future participle (-ila-b / -ela-b / la-b / -ina-b)
These two participles are formed from the respective synthetic tenses by adding a gender
suffix. In addition to the above functions, the present participle is found in analytic tenses
such as the Compound present (11c), the Compound past (11d), and the Compound Future
(11e).
(iiic) Past participle (-ara-b / -ra-b)
The past participle is also used to express indirect evidentiality, although this usage is rather
rare (Forker In Press).
(iv) Converbs
Converbal clauses are the main way of expressing adverbial relations between a dependent
МХКЮsО КЧН К ЦКТЧ МХКЮsО ТЧ AЯКr. FШХХШаТЧР NОНУКХФШЯ’s (1995: 106) НОПТЧТЭТШЧ, МШЧЯОrЛs
МКЧ ЛО МХКssТПТОН ТЧЭШ ‘sЩОМТКХТгОН’ КЧН ‘МШЧЭОбЭЮКХ’ МШЧЯОrЛs. SКЧгСТ СКs ЭаШ МШЧЭОбЭЮКХ
converbs, the Perfective converb and the Imperfective converb. They are semantically rather
vague and allow for great variety in interpreting the relation between the converbal clause
and the matrix clause. In addition, they are used in a number of analytic tenses and serve a
narrative function. The specialized temporal converbs express the major temporal meanings
of posteriority, simultaneity, and anteriority various other meanings (e.g. location, causation,
manner of action).
(iva) Perfective converb (-un / -n)
The perfective converb is used for the formation of the perfect and the pluperfect as well as
for temporal adverbial clauses expressing anteriority. The negated perfective converb is
formed by encliticizing =go to the negated aorist.
(ivb) Imperfective converb (Simple present + =go)
The imperfective converb occurs in temporal adverbial clauses expressing simultaneity or
manner of action and in certain types of complement clauses.
(ivc) Specialized converbs
In addition to the two general converbs (imperfective and perfective), Avar has a wide range
of specialized converbs that express temporal, spatial and other meanings. The specialized
converbs are often formed by combining a non-finite verb form with a spatial case suffix. The
most important specialized converbs are given below, illustrated by means of the verb b-osize ‘ЭКФО’:
- ТЦЦОНТКЭО КЧЭОrТШr МШЧЯОrЛ ‘ТЦЦОНТКЭОХв аСОЧ’: b-os-ara-b=go (past participle + =go)
- ЩШsЭОrТШr МШЧЯОrЛ ‘ЛОПШrО / ЮЧЭТХ’: b-os-ila-l-de (future + -l-de)
- ЭОrЦТЧКЭТЯО МШЧЯОrЛ ‘ЮЧЭТХ, till’ b-os-ize- an (infinitive + - an)
- sТЦЮХЭКЧШЮs МШЧЯОrЛ ‘аСОЧ, аСТХО’ b-os-i-dal (masdar + -dal)
- КЧЭОrТШr МШЧЯОrЛ ‘КПЭОr, sТЧМО’ b-os-ara-l-da-sːa (past participle without gender marker + -lda-sːa)
- sТЦЮХЭКЧШЮs МШЧЯОrЛ ‘аСТХО, КЭ ЭСО sКЦО ЭТЦО’: b-os-i-gun (masdar + -gun)
- ХШМКЭТШЧКХ МШЧЯОrЛ / ЦКЧЧОr ШП КМЭТШЧ ‘аСОrО / СШа’ b-os-uqe (-uqe / -qe / -owqe)
- causative converbs ‘because’ b-os-ara-ɬuq, b-os-ule-ɬul (participles + -ɬu-q, -ɬu-l, -ɬidal)
13
- spatial converbs / participles that are inflected for the spatial cases: essive b-os-ara-ɬɬu-b,
lative b-os-ara-ɬɬu-b-e, ablative b-os-ara-ɬɬu-sːa, translative b-os-ara-ɬɬu-san
The last types of non-finite clauses are conditionals and concessives that are formally and
functionally close to other types of adverbial clauses.
(i) Realis conditional (-ani / -uni / -ni; negation -č’o-ni / -ič’o-ni / -inč’o-ni):
The suffix of the realis conditional is formed on the basis of the aorist, by replacing -na with ni, e.g. b-os-ani ‘ТП s/СО ЭКФОs ТЭ’ / b-os-ič’o-ni ‘ТП s/СО НШОs ЧШЭ ЭКФО ТЭ’.
(ii) Irrealis conditional (participle + -ani)
The irrealis conditional is formed by adding -ani to a participle form of the verb, e.g. present
participle b-os-ule-b-ani ‘ТП s/СО аШЮХН ЭКФО ТЭ’, Шr ЩКsЭ ЩКrЭТМТЩХО b-os-ara-b-ani ‘ТП s/СО
аШЮХН СКЯО ЭКФОЧ ТЭ’. Irrealis conditional sentences express low probability, typically
counterfactual events and situations whose temporal location depends on the participle that is
used as basis.
(iii) Concessives (-ani=gi / -uni=gi / -ni=gi; negation -č’o-ni=gi / -ič’o-ni=gi / -inč’o-ni=gi)
CШЧМОssТЯО МХКЮsОs ЭСКЭ ОбЩrОss ЩrШЩОr МШЧМОssТЯО ЦОКЧТЧР ‘КХЭСШЮРС Щ, Ъ’ and concessive
МШЧНТЭТШЧКХs ‘ОЯОЧ ТП’ КrО ПШrЦОН Лв КННТЧР ЭСО КННТЭТЯО =gi to the realis conditional form.
Examples of specialized converbs and conditional clauses can be found in Section 5.5.2.
4.3.4. Valency classes and valency alternations
Valency patterns of predicates can be divided into one-place (monovalent), two-place
(bivalent) and three-place patterns (trivalent). I distinguish for bivalent predicates between an
A argument (the argument with the most agentive properties) and a P argument (the argument
with the least agentive or most patientive properties). Three-place verbs have, in addition to
the A argument, a G argument and a T argument. The G (goal-like) argument is more
stationary than T (theme-like) and might be receiving an experience or being exposed to an
experience, in contrast to T. The three basic classes can be further subdivided into valency
subclasses according to the case marking patterns and the semantic roles fulfilled by the
arguments of these predicates.
Monovalent predicates generally take one single argument in the absolutive (14a). This
argument can be agentive or patientive, depending on the semantics of the verb. In addition,
there are very few experiencer predicates that take one single argument in the SPR-essive (da) (14b), (39b).
(14a)
ОЦОrК-b
ЦОχ b-a-č’ШРШ,
ЩКНТšКС
χʷ-ana
much-n
time n-go-cvb.neg king
die-aor
‘Not much time went by and the king died.’ (Charachidzé 1981: 192)
(14b)
ТЧЪ’-ara-w
či-jas-da
ОЭ’
b-a-n
b-ugo
fear-ptcp.pst-m man-obl-spr sweat n-come-cvb n-cop
‘The man became afraid and sweated.’ (Axlakov 1976: 29)
Bivalent verbs have an A and a P argument. The semantic functions and accordingly the case
markings of A and P vary considerably depending on the semantics of the verb and on other
factors. The largest group of bivalent verbs are canonical transitive predicates with an
agentive A marked by the ergative and a patientive P marked by the absolutive (11c), (20a).
14
Another clearly identifiable predicate class contains almost exclusively experiential verbs that
assign to the experiencer argument the dative or the SPR-essive and to the stimulus argument
the absolutive. To the first type belong b-oƛʼ-ize ‘Хove, like, want’ and riχ-ine ‘dislike, hate,
be bored’ (15a). The second group is larger and contains, among others b-ix-ize ‘see’, b-ičːʼize ‘understand’, kʼoče-ne ‘forget’, ɬa-ze ‘know’, ra -ize ‘hear’, rakʼalde šʷe-ze ‘remember’,
and b-at-ize ‘find’ (15b). The last verb is also used as auxiliary in conditional constructions
(39a) and epistemic modality constructions.
(15a) di-je
ha-b
ЩТrТЧčʼ b-Шƛʼ-ula-ro
1sg-dat dem-n rice
n-like-prs-neg
‘I dislike the rice.’ (elicited)
(15b) di-da
he-s-ul
МʼКr
1sg-spr dem-obl-gen name
‘I forgot his name.’ (elicited)
ФʼШč-ana
forget-aor
TСОrО Тs К ЩrОНТМКЭО МХКss ЭСКЭ I МКХХ ‘ОбЭОЧНОН ТЧЭrКЧsТЭТЯО ЩrОНТМКЭОs.’ TСОв ЭКФО КЧ A
argument in the absolutive that has usually rather an agentive semantics, and a further P
argument marked by a spatial case (e.g. APUD-essive, SPR-essive, SUB-ablative). Verbs of
movement fall into this class, КМЭТЯО ЩОrМОЩЭТШЧ ЩrОНТМКЭОs sЮМС Кs ‘ХТsЭОЧ ЭШ’ (16) КЧН ‘ХШШФ
КЭ’ Кs аОХХ Кs other predicates.
(16)
dun
ha-b ФШčʼ-oq
ОЧОФː-un
1sg
dem-n song-apud
listen-cvb
‘I listened to the song.’ (elicited)
w-ugo
m-cop
Trivalent predicates include verbs like ƛʼe-ze ‘РТЯО’, b-ixizab-ize ‘sСШа’ (МКЮsКЭТЯО ПШrЦ ШП
‘sОО’) (17), verbs of speech such as har-ize ‘КsФ’, КЧН ЯОrЛs ШП СТЭЭТЧР, О.Р. ƛʼab-ize ‘СТЭ’,
which mark the instrument with the absolutive case.
(17)
“w-ix-iza-w-ila
m-see-caus-m-fut
di-ca
du-da
1sg-erg 2sg-spr
[kina-w
which-m
čТ
man
jas-az-e
girl-pl.obl-dat
w-Шƛ’-ule-wЖ”=ali
jan
ab-un=ila
wex-as
m-love-ptcp.prs-m=quot quot
say-cvb=rep shepherd-erg
‘“I will show you which man girls love most,” said the shepherd.’ (Axlakov 1976: 13)
There are a small number of labile verbs that can be used both intransitively and transitively
without the verb undergoing any morphological process. The majority of them are patientpreserving labile verbs (S=P-labile) that can be used with or without a P argument, e.g. beɬine ‘ЛШТХ’ (ТЧЭr.), ‘МШШФ’ (Эr.), b-i -ine ‘ЦОХЭ’ (ТЧЭr., Эr.) (18a, b). There is also one S=A labile
verb, namely c’al-ize ‘rОКН, sЭЮНв’ (19a, b).
(18a) Alekseev et al. 2014: 204
Ъ’ШЭ’ЧШ-b
КгЮ b-Т -ana
street.in-n
snow n-melt-aor
‘TСО sЧШа ЦОХЭОН ШЧ ЭСО sЭrООЭ.’
(18b) ebel-Кɬ
mother-erg
naχ
butter
b-Т -ana
n-melt-aor
15
‘MШЭСОr ЦОХЭОН ЛЮЭЭОr.’
(19a) jas
М’КХ-ana
šКСКr-al-da
girl study-aor
town-obl-spr
‘TСО РТrХ sЭЮНТОН ТЧ ЭСО ЭШаЧ.’ (elicited)
(19b) jas-Кɬ
Э’Об
М’КХ-ana
girl-erg
book read-aor
‘TСО РТrХ rОКН ЭСО ЛШШФ.’ (ОХТМТЭОН)
Avar has one detransitivizing construction that makes use of derivational suffixes and one
transitivizing construction, the causative. Detransitivization in Avar shows similarities with
antipassive constructions because it leads to a deletion of the P argument and the A argument
changes its case marking from the ergative to the absolutive (20a). Furthermore, it has
iterative, durative or habitual meaning as it is often observed for antipassives (20b).
(20a) do-s
ɬТЦ
СОЪʼ-ana
dem-erg
water
drink-aor
‘He drank water.’ (Mallaeva 2007: 343)
(20b) СОЪ’-old-ana
ʁШ-w,
χʷ-ana
drink-iter-aor
dem-m
die-aor
‘HО НrКЧФ rОРЮХКrХв КЧН НТОН.’ (СЭЭЩ://ЛКХЭШsХКЯ.ОЮ/КЯКr/?ЦШЯК=ОЧ)
However, detransitivization in Avar should rather be regarded as belonging to a general
operation of verbal derivation that forms intransitive verbs with an iterative/durative
meaning. It can be applied to verbs of various valency patterns including intransitive verbs
and also to parts of speech other than verbs such that detransitivization cannot be taken as the
core function (Section 4.7).
The causative form of verbs transparently derives from a periphrastic construction with the
verb ha-b-ize ‘НШ, ЦКФО’, аСТМС loses its initial glottal fricative and is added to the infinitive
of the lexical verb. The synthetic as well as the analytic variants are both in use, e.g. qʷa-zab-ize < qʷaze habize ‘ЦКФО аrТЭО’, heq’e-za-b-ize < heq’eze habize ‘ЦКФО НrТЧФ’. The
causative normally adds an argument to the predicate, i.e. an intransitive predicate becomes
transitive and a transitive predicate becomes ditransitive. However, in some cases the
causative changes the semantic roles without adding a new argument (21b).
(21a) di-ca
do-s-da
СОЪ’О-za-b-una
ɬТЦ
1sg-erg
dem-obl-spr drink-caus-n-aor water
‘I ЦКНО СТЦ НrТЧФ аКЭОr.’ (MКХХКОЯК 2007: 343)
(21b) di-ca
do-w
СОЪ’О-za-w-una
1sg-erg
dem-m drink-caus-m-aor
‘I ЦКНО СТЦ НrЮЧФОЧ.’ (MКХХКОЯК 2007: 343)
Detransitivizing and causative suffixes can be combined, e.g. heq’e-ze ‘НrТЧФ’ > heq’-old-ize
‘НrТЧФ rОРЮХКrХв, ЛО К НrТЧФОr’ (КЧЭТЩКssТЯО) > heq’-old-iza-b-ize ‘ЦКФО НrТЧФ rОРЮХКrХв’.
Furthermore, the biabsolutive construction, which can also be counted as a means of
detransitivization, is available with transitive verbs inflected for periphrastic tenses (Forker
16
2012). It is characterized by absolutive marking of both A and P. The lexical verb shows
gender agreement with P and the copula-auxiliary agrees with A (11e).
4.4. Adjectives
Adjectives have a short and a long form. The latter is formed on the basis of the former by
adding the derivational suffix -a-GM / -ija-GM, which is also used to derive adjectives from
other parts of speech, to the short stem. The short stem is rather restricted in its occurrence
and, in a few cases, can also be used as an adverb. In those cases, in which both long and
short form are possible, there can be differences in meaning (see, e.g., Ebeling 1966: 73-74).
All adjectives in their long form have gender agreement suffixes; a few adjectives also have a
gender prefix, e.g. b-icat-a-b ‘thick, fat’, b-acːʼad-a-b ‘clean’ (22a-c) (Alekseev et al. 2014:
182). The agreement suffix is controlled by the head of the noun phrase in which the
adjectives occur, or by the gender of the referent in case of nominal usages. The gender
agreement of the prefix is controlled by the complement of the adjective if there is any (22c)
as it is the case with bivalent verbs in relative clauses (41b). Otherwise it agrees with the head
noun (22b). There is no case concord within the noun phrase, so adjectives do not change for
case when used attributively.
(22a) w-icata-w či ‘ПКЭ ЦКЧ’
j-icata-j č’užu ‘ПКЭ аШЦКЧ’
b-icata-b t’ex ‘ЭСТМФ ЛШШФ’
r-icata-l5 ča i ‘ПКЭ ЦОЧ’
r-icata-l ručːabi ‘ПКЭ woЦОЧ’
r-icata-l t’axal ‘ЭСТМФ ЛШШФs’
(22b) j-КМː’КН-a-j
jas
f-clean-adj-f girl
‘К МХОКЧ РТrХ’ (Ebeling 1966: 73)
(22c) rЮЪ’
b-acː’КН-a-j
jas
house n-clean-adj-f girl
‘ЭСО РТrХ аСШsО СШЮsО Тs МХОКЧ’ (EЛОХТЧР 1966: 73)
Nominalized adjectives are inflected in the same manner as demonstrative pronouns. For all
cases except for the absolutive, the gender suffix is omitted and the ergative/oblique suffix is
added to which, if required, further case suffixes attach (Table 10, from Alekseev & Ataev
1997: 53).
Table 10: Inflection of adjectives exemplified with χera-b ‘ШХН’
masc. singular feminine / neuter singular plural
absolutive χОra-w
χОrК-У / χОrК-b
χОra-l
ergative
χОrК-s
χОra-ɬ
χОra-z
genitive
χОrК-s-ul
χОrК-ɬ-ul
χОra-z-ul
dative
χОrК-s-e
χОrК-ɬ-e
χОrК-z-e
SPR-essive χОrК-s-da
χОrК-l-da
χОrК-z-da
In comparative constructions, neither the target of comparison nor the standard of comparison
bears any special marking; the standard of comparison is marked with the SPR-ablative case
(23).
(23)
5
di-da-sa
ɬТФ’-a-w
qazaq
du-je=gi
The agreement prefix in the plural is r-, whereas the suffix is -l.
17
šːʷ-ela-r=in
1sg-spr-abl good-adj-m worker 2sg-dat=add
get-fut -neg=emph
‘You will also not get a better farm worker than me.’ (Bokarev 1949: 165)
Superlative constructions basically have the same structure as comparative constructions. The
only differences are the marking of the standard, which is now done by means of the
postposition horƛʼo-b ‘КЦШЧР’ + SPR-lative, and the additional degree adverb bišːun(go)
‘ЦШsЭ’ modifying the parameter.
4.5. Numerals
Avar has
- cardinal numerals
- ordinal numerals
- distributive numerals
- group numerals
- multiplicative numerals
- collective numerals
Table 11 displays the cardinal numerals. As many Nakh-Daghestanian languages, Avar has a
vigesimal numeral system for the numerals 40 (= 2 20), 60 (= 3 20) and 80 (= 4 20). All
numerals except for co ‘ШЧО’ are morphologically complex. The last numeral always takes the
multifunctional enclitic =go. TСО ЧЮЦОrКХ ‘ЭОЧ’ аСОЧ ЮsОН ТЧ МШЦЩШЮЧНs (Т.О. -tenth) has the
form anc’-ila. All other numerals make use of the suffix -alda (-jalda / -ijalda) that replaces
=go. Higher numerals have been borrowed from Russian (e.g. millijon ‘ЦТХХТШЧ’).
Table 11: Cardinal numerals
1 co
11 КЧМ’-ila co
2 Ф’Т=РШ
12 КЧМ’-ТХК Ф’Т=РШ
3 ɬab=go
13 КЧМ’-ila ɬab=go
4 ЮЧЪ’=РШ 14 КЧМ’-ТХК ЮЧЪ’=РШ
(ЮЧЪ’ )
5 šːЮ=РШ
15 КЧМ’-ila šːЮ=РШ
21
22
30
40
Ъ’Ш-lo co
Ъ’Ш-ХШ Ф’Т=РШ
ɬeber=go
Ф’Т-Ъ’Ш=РШ
66
76
200
ɬab-Ъ’Ш-jalda anɬ=go
ɬab-Ъ’Ш-УКХНК КЧМ’-ila
anɬ=go
Ф’Т-nus=go
50
Ф’Т-Ъ’o-jalda
КЧМ’=РШ
ɬab-Ъ’Ш=РШ
ɬab-Ъ’Ш-jalda
КЧМ’=РШ
ЮЧЪ’Ш=РШ (<
ЮЧЪ’-Ъ’Ш=РШ)
ЮЧЪ’Ш-jalda
КЧМ’=РШ
nus=go
300
ɬab-nus=go
6
7
anɬ=go
16 КЧМ’-ila anɬ=go
КЧЪ’=РШ 17 КЧМ’-ТХК КЧЪ’=РШ
60
70
8
miƛ=go
18 КЧМ’-ila miƛ=go
80
9
Тč’=РШ
19 КЧМ’-ТХК Тč’=РШ
90
20 Ъ’Ш=РШ
100
10 КЧМ’=РШ
ЮЧЪ’-nus=go
šːЮ-nus=ijalda Ф’Т-Ъ’ШУКХНК КЧМ’-ila šːЮ=РШ
1000 azar=go
400
555
1001 azar-alda co
2000 Ф’Т-КгКr=Р
The other types of numerals are formed in the following manner:
- ordinal numerals: suffix -abile-b (derived from the participle of the verb ab-ize ‘sКв, ЭОХХ’):
k’i-abili-b ‘sОМШЧН’, ɬab-abile-b ‘ЭСТrН’
- distributive numerals: co-co ‘ШЧО ОКМС’, k’i-k’i ‘two ОКМС’, ɬab-ɬab ‘ЭСrОО ОКМС’
- multiplicative numerals (‘ПШЮr ЭТЦОs’)
- collective / group numerals:
(i) expressing units -(j)a-b=go: k’i-ja-w=go ‘ЛШЭС, КХХ ЭаШ (masc.)’, e.g. k’i-ja-b=go ber ‘ЛШЭС
ОвОs’; ɬab-a-b=go ‘КХХ ЭСrОО’, unq’-a-b=go ‘КХХ ПШЮr’ (28)
18
(ii) referring to parts of a totality =go-ja-w / =go-ja-j / =go-ja-b: k’i=go-ja-b ‘ЭаШ’ (ШЮЭ ШП К
group of more), ɬab=go-ja-b ‘ЭСrОО’ (ШЮЭ ШП ЦШrО) (10b)
Numerals are used as nominal modifiers with a following noun in the singular. Most of the
numerals have the morphosyntactic properties of adjectives. Numerals can be nominalized
and take case suffixes. Cardinal numerals are nominalized by means of the adjectivizing
suffix -ja-GM, and then inflected in the same way as demonstrative pronouns or adjectives.
When case suffixes are added, the final gender suffix is dropped (24).
(24)
Ф’ТРШ-ja-s-ul
two-adj-obl-gen
ФʷОЧ
food
ɬКЛРШ-ja-s-e
three-adj-obl-dat
ШХК;
be.enough
ɬКЛРШ-ja-s-ul
three-adj-obl-gen
ФʷОЧ
ЮЧЪ’Ш-ja-s-e
ШХК
food
four-adj-obl-dat
be.enough
‘The food of two is enough for three; the food of three is enough for four.’
(http://baltoslav.eu/avar/?mova=en)
4.6. Postpositions, adverbs, enclitics, and particles
The majority of the postpositions can also be used as adverbs, and it is not always possible to
determine whether a linguistic item functions as the one or the other. This is even more the
case for spatial postpositions. Relevant examples that most commonly govern spatial cases of
the SPR-series are t’ad ‘ШЧ, КЛШЯО, ЮЩ’, ʁortƛʼ ‘НШаЧ, ЮЧНОr’, ask’o-b ‘ЧОКr, МХШsО’, žani-b
‘ТЧsТНО’ (also inter-series), ce-b-e ‘ЛОПШrО, ТЧ ПrШЧЭ’, naqa ‘ЛОСТЧН, КПЭОr’, xadu-b ‘ЛОСТЧН,
КПЭОr, ЛКМФ’, req’on ‘КММШrНТЧР ЭШ’, and cadaq ‘ЭШРОЭСОr аТЭС’. The postposition olo ‘ПШr ЭСО
sКФО ШП’, ТЧ МШЧЭrКsЭ ЭШ КХХ ШЭСОr ЩШsЭЩШsТЭТШЧs РШЯОrЧs ЭСО НКЭТЯО.
The spatial postpositions/adverbs themselves can be inflected for the directional cases (lative,
ablative, translative). Those postpositions/adverbs that have gender agreement affixes agree
with the nominal in the absolutive case of the clause in which they occur. This nominal is not
identical withe the complement of the postposition.
Apart from the postpositions that can also be used as adverbs, Avar has many other adverbs,
which form a rather heterogeneous category. Most adverbs are derived by means of various
suffixes. For instance, there are spatial adverbs formed from demonstrative pronouns, e.g. dob-a ‘ЭСОrО’, and manner adverbs also derived from demonstrative pronouns do-din ‘ХТФО ЭСКЭ’
< do-b ‘ЭСКЭ’ (КаКв ПrШЦ ЭСО sЩОКФОr). Many adverbs bear the multifunctional enclitic =go,
but its use is optional and it cannot be analyzed as a derivational suffix for the formation of
adverbs.
Few underived adverbs show gender agreement that is controlled by the absolutive argument
of the clause, e.g. ask’o-b ‘МХШsОХв’.
The most common enclitics and particles are =go, the additive=gi ‘КХsШ, ЭШШ, КЧН, ОЯОЧ’, =cin
‘ОЯОЧ’, =ni, =in, =ya, =χa, the evidential enclitic =ila(n), the quotative enclitics (Section
5.5.3) and the interrogative enclitics (Sections 5.4). The central function of =go is the
expression of emphasis and/or contrast. Other uses are the expression of identity, scalar
additivity, reflexivity (including local and long-distance reflexives, emphatic reflexive uses),
the marking of indefinite pronouns and adverbs, and the derivation of cardinal numerals and
converbs (Forker 2015). The enclitic =ni is highly multifunctional covering in addition to its
general emphatic meaning the formation of conditional clauses, a verb doubling construction
used for topicalizing predicates and, in combination with =gi, concessive clauses and certain
indefinite pronouns. The enclitic =χa is sometimes referred to as focus particle (Kazenin
1997: 38-40).
19
4.7. Word formation
For the formation of new words Avar makes use of compounding, derivation, conversion and
reduplication. A detailed account can be found in Khalilov & Khalilova (2016).
Compounding is prevalent among nouns and verbs, but there are also compound adjectives
and other parts of speech (25). Most compounds have a modifier-head structure, but there are
also some dvandva compounds and other morphosyntactic types.
(25)
ebel-emen ‘ЩКrОЧЭs’ (ЦШЭСОr-father)
q’asikʷen ‘НТЧЧОr’ (< q’asi ‘ТЧ ЭСО ОЯОЧТЧР’ + kʷen ‘ПШШН’)
ɬabboχila-b ‘ЭСrОО-ХОРРОН’ (< ɬabgo ‘ЭСrОО’ + boχ ‘ХОР’ + adjectivizer -ila-GM)
k’i-b-ekize ‘ЭШ ЛrОКФ ТЧ ЭаШ (ЩТОМОs)’ (< k’igo ‘ЭаШ’ + bekize ‘ЛrОКФ’)
Derivation is most productive for nouns and the language has a number of borrowed suffixes
that derive nouns, but none to derive other parts of speech. Examples are:
-či derives nouns denoting people from nouns, adjectives, e.g. heresi-či ‘ХТКr’ < heresi
‘ХТО’
-ɬi derives abstract nouns from various parts of speech, e.g. co-ɬi ‘unity’ < co ‘ШЧО’
-ro derives agent, place, instrument and other nouns from verbal nouns, e.g. hešːtʼero
‘аСТsЭХО’ < hešːtʼeze ‘ЭШ аСТsЭХО’
There are also derived adjectives, adverbs (Section 4.6) and indefinite pronouns (Section 4.2).
Verbs are derived from other verbs by means of causativization and detransitivization
(Section 4.3.4). But it is also possible to derive verbs from nouns, short adjectives, adverbs
and numerals. This mostly done by means of the iterative/durative suffixes and by means of
the inchoative suffix -ɬi that can be added to short adjectives, numerals, interjections, etc.
(26).The most productive suffix for the formation of intransitive verbs with iterative/durative
meaning is -ar; other suffixes are -dar, -d, -e, -aqd, -de, -old, -ard and -anq. With some verbs
ablaut is employed for the formation of (detransitivized) iterative/durative verbs. Avar has a
few more suffixes for the derivation of verbs such as -χːin, which is added to nouns (26).
(26)
berzuq-aqd-ize ‘ПКХХ / ШММЮr ШЧ ЭСО ОвО’ < ber-zu-q ‘ШЧ ЭСО ОвО’ (ОвО-OBL-APUD)
co-ɬi-ze ‘ЭШ УШТЧ, ЭШ ЛОМШЦО ШЧО’ < co ‘ШЧО’
maƛʼiχːine ‘ЭШ ЛО sХООЩв’ < maƛʼi ‘sХООЩ.OBL’
Conversion is most common for the nominal use of adjectives which does not require any
special marking. Reduplication, which can be fully and partial, is found with nouns,
adjectives, adverbs, numerals (for the formation of distributive numerals, see Section 4.5) and
verbs. Depending on the part of speech and on the individual lexical item reduplication can
convey plurality/distributivity, resemblance, emphasis, attenuation or diminishment (27).
(27)
kʷalkʷal ‘СТЧНrКЧМО, ШЛsЭКМХО’ (ЧШ аШrН kʷal),
ašːt’i-mašt’i ‘КЧ КбО КЧН ЭСТЧРs ХТФО ТЭ’ < ašːt’i ‘КбО’
kunčʼ-kunčʼize ‘glitter, sparkle’ < kunčʼ-ize ‘glow, shine’
ɬik’a-ɬik’a-b ‘rОКХХв РШШН’ < ɬik’a-b ‘РШШН’
q’asi-q’asi ‘many evenings, at various evenings’ < q’asi ‘ТЧ ЭСО ОЯОЧТЧР’
20
5. Syntax
5.1. Noun phrases and other types of phrases
Noun phrases are normally head-final, and all types of modifiers precede their head:
demonstrative pronouns (11e), (15a), numerals and other quantifiers (13b), (28), (31), (32),
adjectives (32), genitive NPs (28), (36a) and relative clauses (Section 5.5.4), (44a). The order
of modifiers with respect to each other is relatively free and at least partially depends on the
semantics of the NPs and possible ambiguous interpretations. Thus, demonstratives and
genitives often occur in the initial position preceding adjectives or relative clauses (28), but
the reverse order is also attested (41a).
(28)
azdaho-УКɬ-ul
Тčʼ-a-b=go
ЛОЭʼОr
dragon-obl-gen
nine-adj-n=emph head
‘all nine heads of the dragon’ (Alekseev & Ataeev 1998: 72)
Adjectives in NPs can occur in their short (33) or their long form (29a, b). The gender prefix
of the adjective in (29b) agrees with the complement of the adjective, whereas the suffix
agrees with the head noun.
(29a) [Bolo=jin
ab-ule-w]
co МʼКЪʼ miskin-a-w čТ
Bolo=quot
say-ptcp.prs-m one very poor-adj-m man
‘one very poor man called Bolo’ (Alekseev & Ataeev 1998: 72)
(29b) do-j
ЦЮˤКХТЦ=gi
namus
b-acː’КН-a-j
dem-f educated=add
consciousness n-clean-adj-f
‘She is an educated РТrХ аТЭС К МХОКЧ МШЧsМТШЮsЧОss.’
(http://baltoslav.eu/avar/?mova=en)
jas
girl
j-igo
f-cop
Only adjectives, relative clauses or genitive modifiers can follow the head at all (30).
Testelets (1998a, b) found that in the Andalal dialect of Avar only modifiers that are
contrasted or emphasized follow the head.
(30)
КЧƛ’=go
qat
rКƛ’-Кɬ-ul
b-ЮЪ’-ana ʁajaba-ca
seven=emph hand
earth-obl-gen
n-dig-aor
shoulder.pl.obl-erg
‘His shoulders dug seven depths of earth.’ (Charachidzé 1981: 174)
Definite NPs can but need not to contain a demonstrative pronoun. In example (31) both NPs
are definite, but only the second one contains a demonstrative pronoun. In the function of the
indefinite article the numeral one is used (32).
(31)
СТЭ’ТЧ-a-b
little-adj-n
КЧč’-i-ca
bird-obl-erg
he-s-da
this-obl-spr
ha-b
dem-n
ɬab-a-b=go
three-adj-n=emph
КЪ’ХЮ
advice
ЦКɬː-an=ila
teach-aor=rep
‘The little bird taught these three advices to him.’ (Axlakov 1976: 28)
Appositions consist of nouns having the same referent that immediately follow each other
and behave like a single noun phrase. They most frequently consist of a proper name and a
kinship term, of which the name often precedes the kinship term, but the reverse order is also
possible and common, e.g. wacʼ ali ‘brother Ali.’
21
NPs are coordinated by means of the additive enclitic =gi that is added to every member of
the coordination (45a,b).
In adjective phrases, adverbials precede adjectives (29a). In postpositional phrases, the
postposition strictly follows the NP (34), (44b), but since many postpositions can also be used
as adverbials, one can find examples of clauses that seem to contain postpositions in positions
other than their canonical position.
5.2. Simple clause
Simple clauses can be divided into copula clauses and simple verbal clauses. Copula clauses
can express identity, group membership, attribution, possession, location, and existence. The
copula subject is always in the absolutive case (32). The copula complement, which bears the
absolutive case or any other case, may be a noun phrase, but it may also be an adjective, an
adverbial, a postpositional phrase or a nominalized clause
(32)
co
one
ЛОčОНК-w
rich-m
či
man
w-uk’-an=ila
m-be-aor=rep
ʁШ-s-ul
dem-obl-gen
kuli=gi
hamlet=add
kolo-da
Ф’ЮНТУК-b čː’КЪОЧ=gi
b-ЮФ’-an=ila
hamlet.obl-spr large-n
wealth=add n-be-aor=rep
‘There was one rich man, he had a hamlet; and on the farm there was a lot of wealth.’
(Axlakov 1976: 14)
Simple verbal clauses contain monovalent predicates, bivalent predicates, and trivalent
predicates. Cases used to encode the arguments are absolutive, ergative, dative (15a), SPRessive (15b), (33), and to a lesser extent other spatial cases (16). Gender/number agreement
of verbs, adverbs and postpositions is always determined by the absolutive argument of the
clause. The number of arguments, the case-marking of the arguments and the agreement
exponents on verbs controlled by the arguments depend on the valency classes of the verbs
and on whether further valency-changing operations such as detransitivization or
causativization have been applied.
(33)
dibir-as-da
miskin
mulla-obl-spr poor
či
man
w-ix-an=ila
[ КЧχri-da
m-see-aor=rep hamster.obl-spr
РКЦč’-al
stone-pl
r-Оč’-ule-w]
pl-throw-ptcp.prs-m
‘The mullah saw the poor man throwing stones at the hamster.’ (Axlakov 1976: 22)
While agreement and case marking follow the ergative alignment, no ergative patterns are
found outside the realm of morphology.
The most frequent constituent order in simple clauses is A-P-V, but all other logically
possible orders are also allowed and can be found in texts to various extents (see Testelets
1998b for examples). The second most common order is A-V-P. However, it is not always
straightforward to determine the constituent order because arguments that can be retrieved
from the context are frequently omitted.
In focus constructions with special focus particles (=χa) and also in interrogative clauses,
which can be analyzed as a subtype of focus constructions, the constituent order displays
some restrictions, e.g. verb-initial orders are prohibited (Testelets 1998a; Rudnev 2015).
Furthermore, the constructions are cleft-like with a focused constituent to which the focus
22
marker or the interrogative particle is encliticized and a topical part in the form of a headless
relative clause containing a verb in the form of a participle (Section 5.4).
5.3. Agreement
Avar has combined gender/number agreement expressed by agreement prefixes, suffixes, and
very occasionally infixes. The exponents are w (masculine singular), j (feminine singular), b
(neuter singular) and l/r (plural) (see also Section 4.3).
Within the noun phrase all modifiers that show agreement agree with the head noun (34).
Within the clausal domain the agreement of verbs, adverbs and postpositions is controlled by
the absolutive argument of the clause if there is any (18b), (34). Otherwise the default
agreement marker b for neuter singular is used (40c). In biabsolutive constructions the agent
controls the agreement of the copula-auxiliary and the suffix on the lexical verb, whereas the
agreement prefix of the lexical verb is controlled by the patient (11e).
(34)
heni-w
there-m
Э’ШМОЛe
first.n
b-at-ara-b
n-find-ptcp.pst-n
rЮЪ’-ow-e
house-in.m-lat
žКЧТ-w-e
inside-m-lat
ɬЮС-an=ila
enter-aor=rep
‘TСОrО СО ОЧЭОrОН ЭСО ПТrsЭ СШЮsО ЭСКЭ СО ПШЮЧН.’ (AбХКФШЯ 1976: 19)
Note that the agreement suffixes of participles occurring in relative clauses are determined by
the head noun, which these participial clauses modify (34), (41a, b). The prefixes, however,
follow the rules of clausal agreement, i.e., they are controlled by the absolutive argument
inside the participial clause (33).
In complement clauses of Standard Avar, we have local agreement of the matrix predicate
with the complement clause (35a, c), which means that the complement-taking predicate
bears the neuter singular agreement prefix b- (35a, d). It seems that long-distance agreement
is only found in Avar dialects, but not in the standard language. Examples (35b, c) represent
Anzuq Avar, which considerably differs from Standard Avar, and illustrate long-distance
agreement. For a complement-taking verb to show long distance agreement means that the
predicate agrees with the absolutive argument of the embedded clause, which can be the
single argument of an intransitive verb (35c), the patient (35b) or stimulus of an bivalent
verb, etc. For instance, in (35b) the matrix verb b-aƛ’- ‘аКЧЭ, ХШЯО’ rОЪЮТrОs КЧ ОбЩОrТОЧМОr
argument in the dative and a stimulus argument in the absolutive. The absolutive argument in
(35b) belongs to the embedded clause since it represents the single argument of the verb
ƛ’ap- ‘ЛОКЭ, СТЭ’, ЛЮЭ ТЭ МШЧЭrШХs the agreement on the matrix predicate. Similarly, (35c) does
not contain an argument in the absolutive because the dative argument controls the absolutive
argument of the infinitive. Nevertheless the gender agreement prefixes of the matrix predicate
are w- and thus controlled by the omitted absolutive argument waša ‘ЛШв’. Sentence (35d)
shows the Standard Avar version of (35c), and instantiated local agreement.
(35a) Standard Avar
[ʁo-j-e
Э’КН-e j-aχ-ine]
b-oƛʼ-ila=an
di-je
dem-f-lat
up-lat f-move-inf
n-want-fut=quot
1sg-dat
‘I (fem.) want to go up there.’ (modified from Charachidzé 1981: 171)
(35b) Anzuq Avar (Kibrik 2003: 458)
УКšК-ɬu-je
[аКšК ƛ’КЩ-žТЖ
girl-obl-dat boy beat-inf
w-Кƛ’-un
m-want-cvb
23
w-ona
m-cop
‘TСО РТrХ аКЧЭs ЭШ ЛОКЭ ЭСО ЛШв.’
(35c) Anzuq Avar (Kibrik 2003: 459)
аКšК-s-e
[ƛ’ТЭ’-žТЖ
w-Кƛ’-un
boy-obl-dat sleep-inf
m-want-cvb
‘TСО ЛШв аКЧЭs ЭШ sХООЩ.’
(35d) Standard Avar
was-as-e
[ƛ’Тž-ize]
b-Шƛʼ-un
boy-obl-dat sleep-inf
n-want-cvb
‘TСО ЛШв аКЧЭs ЭШ sХООЩ.’ (elicited)
w-ona
m-cop
b-ugo
n-cop
5.4. Interrogative clauses
Polar interrogatives are obligatorily marked with the enclitics =(j)išː and =daj. The enclitics
are added to the verb or to the focus of the clause. The verb can be finite (36a, b) or occur in
the form of participles (37a, b). When the interrogative particle is encliticized to a finite verb
form the final vowel of the TAM suffix is dropped (36a, b). In analytic verb forms the
particle can be added to the lexical verb, which occurs in the form of a converb or participle
(37b), or to the copula-auxiliary (37a).
(36a) beh-ila-r=Тšː
be.possible-fut-neg=q
dun
1sg
ЧТž-er=go
čК Кг-d-e
1pl.excl-gen=emph man.pl.obl-spr-lat
КРКr-d-e
close-spr-lat
w-ТЭ’-ize?
m-send-inf
‘Is ТЭ ЧШЭ ЩШssТЛХО ЭШ sОЧН ЦО (ЦКsМ.) МХШsОr ЭШ Цв ЩОШЩХО?’ (AбХКФШЯ 1976: 29)
(36b) PКЭ’ТЦКЭ, mun žОРТ
ƛ’Тž-un
j-ТР=Тšː?
Patimat, 2sg still
sleep-cvb
f-cop=q
‘Patimat, are you still sleeping?’ (AХОФsООЯ ОЭ КХ. 2014: 278)
The use of participles in polar questions is very widespread. Formally, this means that a nonfinite verb form (i.e. a participle) functions as head of the clause (37a, b). The use of
participles instead of finite verb forms is not only common in Avar interrogative clauses but
also in focus construction that make use of the enclitics =in and =χa. Thus, it is natural to
analyze them analyze them along similar lines as cleft-like focus constructions with a focal
part marked by the interrogative particle and a topical part expressed like a relative clause by
means of participles.
(37a) ku-n
r-aq-ara-l=daj,
naqe r-uge-l=daj?
eat-cvb pl-finish-ptcp.pst-pl=q back pl-cop.ptcp-pl=q
‘DТН (аО) ПТЧТsС ОКЭТЧР Шr КrО ЭСОrО ХОПЭ ШЯОrs?’ (AбХКФШЯ 1976: 9)
(37b) mun
žОРТ ƛ’Тž-ЮЧ=Тšː
j-ige-j?
2sg
still sleep-cvb=q f-cop.ptcp-f
‘ArО вШЮ sЭТХХ sХООЩТЧР?’ (AХОФsООЯ ОЭ КХ. 2014: 278)
Content interrogatives can have the interrogative pronoun in-situ and ex-situ in clause-initial
or clause-final position, the use of participles is obligatory, and no interrogative particle is
24
employed. The verb can never occur in the initial position of a content interrogative. Rudnev
(2015: Chapter 4) argues for major syntactic differences between in-situ and ex-situ
interrogatives.
(38)
“šːКУ=ТЧ
why=emph
žТЧ-da
refl.sg-spr
ɬК-za-b-Тč’О-b?”=КЧ
know-caus-n-ptcp.pst.neg-n=quot
Д КгЮ-jal-da
snow-obl-spr
ЛКНТЛč’аКУ
ha-b-ize]
ɬЮС-un
w-ugo
reproach
do-n-inf
begin-cvb
m-cop
‘(The man) began to blame the snow, “Аhy did you not inform me?”’ (Axlakov
1976: 28)
5.5. Complex clauses
Sentences in Avar can be fairly complex containing a number of subordinate clauses are
combined with one main clause.
5.5.1. Coordination
The major way of expressing clause coordination is constructions with general converbs such
as the perfective converb (39c). Another possibility is asyndetic coordination, i.e.,
juxtaposition of main clauses. The third option is the use of conjunctions. Avar has the
conjunctive coordinators wa ‘КЧН’, amma ‘ЛЮЭ’ (13a) and the disjunctions ja, jagi and jaɬuni
‘Шr, ОТЭСОr, ЧОТЭСОr’ of which wa, amma and ja are loans. The use of wa is rather rare and
confined to official, written texts.
5.5.2. Adverbial clauses
Adverbial clauses are formed by means of general or specialized converbs, including
conditionals (39a) and concessives. Adverbial clauses are mostly but not exclusively verbfinal and precede the main clause, but other constituent orders are allowed. For instance, they
may appear within the main clause. They can but need not to share the subject with the main
clause. In case of shared subject the subject referent will normally appear only once per
complex clause (39c). Adverbial clauses cannot express absolute tense or evidentiality, but
take over these values from the main clause. Similarly, illocutionary marking is banned from
adverbial clauses, but the illocutionary value of the main clause can optionally be extended to
include the adverbial clause. Focus-sensitive particles and interrogative pronouns are allowed
to occur.
(39a) b-Тč-Тč’ШРШ
b-at-ani,
ʁШ-b
ЦО Оr
ЧШžШ-je ƛ’-ela
n-sell-cvb.neg n-find-cond
dem-n mountain 2pl-dat give-fut
‘If (he) did not sell it, he will give you the pasture (i.e. mountain).’ (Charachidzé
1981: 196)
(39b) ЦОχ
moment
šːʷ-ela-ldego
return-ptcp.fut-before
χʷ-ara-w
die-ptcp.pst-m
χКХЪ’-al-da
people-obl-spr
rТχ-ara-w
w-ЮФ’-una-ro
dislike-ptcp.pst-m m-be-prs-neg
‘People do not dislike the one who dies early (i.e. before the time came).’ (because he
did not have enough time to behave badly and commit sins) (adopted from Axlakov
1976: 10)
25
(39c) he-l=gi
co
para-jal-da
ɬО-ɬ
Э’Оrб-un, ʁКЧЪ’-an=ila
this-pl=add one
moment-obl-spr water-inter sink-cvb drown-aor=rep
‘In one second they also sank into the water and drowned.’ (Axlakov 1976: 36)
Semantically, adverbial clauses headed by the perfective converb, which belongs to the
general converbs, often resemble coordination (39c). The syntactic characteristics of
constructions with general converbs in Nakh-Daghestanian languages have repeatedly been
discussed because they exhibit a mixed behavior showing features of subordination as well as
of coordination (see, e.g., Kazenin & Testelets 2004, Creissels 2010, Forker 2013), although
for Avar this issue still deserves further investigation.
5.5.3. Complement clauses
Complement constructions is a rather heterogeneous type of complex sentences. Non-finite
complement clauses prevail. They make use of the infinitive (36a), participles (40a), the
masdar (40c) or occasionally converbs. The masdar can take cases suffixes in certain types of
complement clauses. Furthermore, complement clauses of the fact type often contain the
nominalizing suffix -ɬi (40b). Which complementation strategy is employed depends on the
complement-taking predicate and on the semantics of the complement clause (e.g.
propositional type vs. fact type). For instance, complement clauses of ɬaze ‘ФЧШа’ КrО СОКНОН
by a participle if they contain an indirect question (40a), or otherwise they are headed by a
finite verb or a participle plus -ɬi (40b).
(40a) di-da
ɬК-la-ro
[kin
du-ca
Ъ’КЛЮХ
ha-b-ile-b
dun]
1SG-SPR know-PRS-NEG how
2SG-ERG accepting do-N-PTCP.PRS-N 1SG
‘I НШ ЧШЭ ФЧШа СШа вШЮ аТХХ rОМОТЯО ЦО.’ (Alekseev & Ataev 1998: 111)
(40b) di-da
ɬК-na
[soncicin
he-j
zawod-al-da
j-ТФ’-ТЧč’Ш-ɬТЖ
1SG-LOC know-AOR yesterday DEM-F factory-OBL-SPR F-be.AOR.NEG-NMLZ
‘I ФЧОа ЭСКЭ sСО аКs ЧШЭ ТЧ ЭСО factory вОsЭОrНКв.’ (Alekseev & Ataev 1998: 110-111)
(40c) he-l-da-ɬЮЧ
this-OBL-SPR-LIKE
b-Тčː’-ana
N-understand-AOR
di-da
1SG-SPR
[dur
aslu
2SG.GEN origin
harduqa-baz-ul
b-ЮФ’-in]
beggar-PL.OBL-GEN N-be-MSD
‘FrШЦ ЭСТs I ЮЧНОrsЭШШН ЭСКЭ вШЮ аОrО ШП К ЛОРРКr’s ШrТРТЧ.’ (AбХКФШЯ 1976: 12)
CШЦЩХОЦОЧЭs ШП ЭСО ЩОrМОЩЭТШЧ ЯОrЛ ‘sОО’ ШП ЭСО ПКМЭ ЭвЩО МШЧЭКТЧ ЩКrЭТМТЩХОs (33).
Infinitival complements can take their own subject, but not in case of identity between the
subject of the main clause and the subject of the complement clause (38).
As other types of complex clauses, non-finite complement clauses show a strong preference
for verb-final constituent order, but as (40a) shows there are exceptions.
The gender affix of the matrix predicate shows either local agreement with the complement
clause as a whole (neuter singular) (40c) or it agrees with the absolutive argument of the
embedded clause, which is called ‘long-distance КРrООЦОЧЭ’ (35b, c). Long-distance
agreement is not attested in Standard Avar, but see Kibrik (2003: 458-459) for more data
from Anzuq Avar.
Reported speech constructions allow for finite complements that are marked with the
quotative enclitics =(j)an (44b), =ila(n) (also used as hearsay evidential marker), =jilan and,
more rarely, =(j)in. Embedded questions additionally take the marker =ali (17).
26
5.5.4. Relative clauses
The major strategy for forming relative clauses in Avar is participle constructions. The gap in
the particle clause is identical to the head of the relative clause. The suffix of the participle
exhibits gender agreement with the head of the relative clause. For instance, in (41a) it is
controlled by the noun in the absolutive and not the immediately preceding noun. Therefore,
the head noun of the relative clause must be jas ‘РТrХ’. Relative clauses, just like other
nominal modifiers, precede the head noun. Constituent order in the relative clause is
predominantly verb-final, and verb-initial relative clauses are strictly prohibited. Almost all
positions can be relativized with a gap, including S, A (41a), P (44a), experiencer (41b) and
stimulus (41c). See Rudnev (2015: Chapter 3) for more details on the syntactic properties of
relative clauses in Avar.
(41a) [dur
wac-al
č’ʷК-ra-j]
ЩКčК -as-ul
jas=gi
2sg.gen brother-pl kill-ptcp.pst-f
king-obl-gen girl=add
‘and the ФТЧР’s НКЮРСЭОr who has killed your brothers’ (Bokarev 1949: 71)
(41b) [he-b
ЩТšК
b-ix-ara-j]
xera-j
dem-n business n-see-ptcp.pst-f
old-f
‘ЭСО ШХН (ХКНв) аСШ sКа ЭСО ЛЮsТЧОss’ (http://baltoslav.eu/avar/?mova=en)
(41c) [b-Шƛ’-ara-b]
žШ
har-e!
n-want-ptcp.pst-n
thing ask-imp
‘Ask the thing that you want!’ (Axlakov 1976: 32)
Polinsky et al. (2012) present the results of a processing experiment on relativization in Avar,
so far the only experimental study of a Daghestanian language. They show that an S gap in a
relative clause is processed much faster than A and P gaps, and the two latter gaps are
processed at about the same rate.
5.6. Reflexivization and reciprocalization
In local reflexive constructions, pronouns are bound by an antecedent within the same clause.
With first and second person the normal personal pronouns occur; for third person the
reduplicated and the complex reflexive pronouns are used (Section 4.2). As Rudnev (2017:
158) shows, the reflexive pronoun can be semantically interpreted as a bound variable.
(42)
КХТ-ca
(žТЧ-ca=go)
žТ-w=go
ɬЮЪʼ-ana
Ali-erg
refl-erg=emph
refl-m=emph wound-aor
‘Ali wounded himself.’ (Testelets & Toldova 1998: 45)
With verbs of certain valency classes (e.g. affective verbs that take a dative experiencer) it is
possible to reverse the case marking of experiencer and stimulus (43), a phenomenon
widespread in Nakh-Daghestanian languages (Forker 2014).
(43)
Madina žТ-j=go
žТЧ-di-je=go
Madina refl-f=emph
refl-obl-dat=emph
‘Madina loves herself.’ (ОХТМТЭОН)
j-Шƛʼ-ula
f-love-prs
In non-local reflexive constructions, the complex reflexive ži-b=go occurs (44a). The simple
reflexive ži-b, by contrast, functions only as a logophoric pronoun in reported speech and
27
other constructions in which the pronouns indicates that the speaker or attitude holder is
identical to a referent in the quote (44b).
(44a) Ф’ЮНТУa-w
old-m
čТ
man
[žin-ca=go
refl-erg=emph
ab-ura-l]
say-ptcp.pst-pl
rК КЛКг-da-sa
word.pl.obl-spr-abl
ЩКšЦКЧɬ-an=ila
regret-aor=rep
‘The old man regretted for the words said by him.’ (Axlakov 1976: 10)
(44b) “žТ-w=gi
cadaq
w-Кč-e!=УКЧ”
har-an=ila
Malla.Nasrudin-i-ca
refl-m=add
together m-bring-imp=quot ask-aor=rep Mulla.Nasredin-obl-erg
‘“Take me also together with you!” begged Mullah Nasrudin.’ (Axlakov 1976: 33)
In reciprocal constructions, the reversal of case marking with transitive and affective
predicates is almost obligatory (Yamada 2013). The pronoun co ca- is usually not controlled
by an antecedent in the ergative, but the antecedent must be in the absolutive (45a). However,
some speakers allow both the canonical roles and the reversed roles (45b). Yamada (2013)
comes to the conclusion that the construction in (45a) must be intransitive. This means that
the antecedent that is the agent in the canonical reciprocal construction becomes an S
argument marked with the absolutive case when the roles are reversed. The reflexive is
demoted from P to oblique marked with the ergative case.
(45a)
КХТ=РТ
Musa=gi
co.caca
r-ОМː-ana
Ali=ADD
Musa=ADD
REC.ERG
PL-praise-AOR
‘AХТ КЧН MЮsК ЩrКТsОН ОКМС ШЭСОr.’ (elicited)
(45b)
КХТ-ca=gi
Musa-ca=gi
co.cal
r-ОМː-ana
Ali-ERG=ADD
Musa-ERG=ADD
REC.ABS PL-praise-AOR
‘AХТ КЧН MЮsК ЩrКТsОН ОКМС ШЭСОr.’ (elicited)
6. Potential areas of research
- phonology: tense vs. geminate consonants,
- Avar dialects that strongly differ from Standard Avar (e.g. Zakatal Avar)
- syntax is generally not very well-researched (except for reflexivization, reciprocalization
and relative clauses)
- sociolinguistic topics: language contact within the Nakh-Daghestanian family and Avar
influence on other Daghestanian languages
- any experimental approaches
Acknowledgements
Zabikhat Ocomieva-Tagirova, Zulajkhat Mallaeva and Diana Shabanova for help with the
Daghestanian data.
Abbreviations
abl ablative
abs absolutive
28
add additive
adj adjectivizer
aor aorist
КЩЮН ХШМКХТгКЭТШЧ ‘ЧОКr, Лв, КЭ’
caus causative
cond conditional
cop copula
cvb converb
dat dative
excl exclusive
f feminine
fut future
gen genitive
gm gender marker
emph emphatic
erg ergative
imp imperative
inf infinitive
ТЧЭОr ХШМКХТгКЭТШЧ ‘ТЧЭОr’
lat lative
m masculine
n neuter
neg negation
obl oblique stem
pl plural
proh prohibitive
prs present
pst past
ptcp participle
q interrogative
quot quotative
rec reciprocal
refl reflexive
rep reportative
sg singular
sЩr ХШМКЭТШЧ ‘ШЧ, ТЧ’
References
Axlakov, A. A. 1976. Avarskie teksty. In Axlakov, A. A. & X M. Xalilov (eds.) Satira i
jumor narodov Dagestana, 7–42. Makhachkala: Daginogoizdat.
Alekseev, Mixail E. & Boris M. Ataev. 1997. Avarskij jazyk. Moscow: Academia.
Alekseev, Mixail E., S. Z. Alixanov, B. M. Ataev, M. A. Magomedov, M. I. Magomedov, G.
I. Madieva, P. A. Saidova & Dzh. S. Samedov. 2014. Sovremennyj avarskij jazyk.
Makhachkala: Aleph.
Bokarev, Evgenij A. 1949. Sintaksis avarskogo jazyka. Moscow: Izd. AN SSSR.
Charachidzé, Georges. 1981. Grammaire de la langue avar (langue du Caucase Nord-Est).
Paris: Farvard.
Ebeling, Carl L. 1966. The grammar of literary Avar. Studia Caucasica 2. 58-100.
29
Forker, Diana. 2013. Microtypology and the Tsezic languages: A case study of syntactic
properties of converbal clauses. SKY Journal of Linguistics 26. 21–40.
Forker, Diana. 2014. Are there subject anaphors? Linguistic Typology 18. 51–81.
Forker, Diana. 2015. Towards a semantic map for intensifying particles: Evidence from Avar.
Language Typology and Universals 68. 485–513.
Forker, Diana. 2016. Complementizers in Hinuq. In Kasper Boye & Petar Kehayov (eds.)
Semantic functions of complementizers in European languages. Berlin: De Gruyter, 745–
792.
Forker, Diana. In Press. Evidentiality and related categories in Avar. In Diana Forker &
Timur Maisak (eds.), The semantics of verbal categories in Nakh-Daghestanian
languages: Tense, aspect, evidentiality, mood/modality. Submitted to the Series Brill's
Studies in Language, Cognition and Culture. Leiden: Brill.
Gimbatov, M. M. (eds.) Ms. Avarsko-russkij slovar’ (2 volumes). Makhachkala: Institut JaLI
DNC RAN.
Kazenin, Konstantin & Yakov G. Testelets. 2004. Where coordination meets subordination:
Converb constructions in Tsakhur (Daghestanian). In Martin Haspelmath (ed.),
Coordinating Constructions, 227–240. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Kibrik, Aleksandr E. 2003. Konstanty i peremennye jazyka. St. Petersburg: Aletheia.
KСКХТХШЯ, MКНžТН Š. & ГКТrК KСКХТХШЯК. 2016. AЯКr. IЧ PОЭОr O. MüХХОr, IЧРОЛШrР OСЧСОТsОr,
Susan Olsen & Franz Rainer (eds.), Word-Formation: An International Handbook of the
Languages of Europe. Volume 5. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Madieva, G. I. 1967. Avarskij jazyk. In Evgenij A. Boakrev & K. V. Lomtatidze (eds.) Jazyki
narodov SSSR, Vol. IV, Iberijsko-kavkazskie jazyki, 255–271. Moscow: Nauka.
Madieva, G. I. 1981. Morfologija avarskogo literaturnogo jazyka. Makhachkala.
Madieva, G. I. 2000. Avarskij jazyk. In G.G. Gamzatov (ed.) Jazyki Dagestana, 45–62.
MКбКčФКХК, MШsМШа: RШssТУsФКУК AФКНОЦТУК NКЮФ, ШЭНОХОЧТО ХТЭОrКЭЮrК Т УКгвФК.
Magomedov, Magomed I. & Magomed A. Magomedov. 2009. Škol’nyj russko-avarskij
slovar’. Makhachkala: Lotos.
Magomedov, Magomed A. In Prep. Phonology. In Comrie, Bernard, Magomed I.
Magomedov & Yakov G. Testelets (eds.), A Grammar of Avar.
Mallaeva, Zulajkhat M. 2007. Glagol avarskogo jazyka. Makhachkala: Institut JaLI DNC
RAN.
Nedjalkov, Vladimir P. 1995. Some typological parameters of converbs. In Haspelmath,
Martin & Ekkehard König (eds.) Converbs in Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter, 97-136.
Polinsky, Maria, Carlos Gómez Gallo, Peter Graff & Ekaterina Kravtchenko. 2012. Subject
preference and ergativity. Lingua 122. 267–277
Rudnev, Pavel 2015. Dependency and discourse-configurationality: A study of Avar.
Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 129.
Rudnev, Pavel. 2017. Minimal pronouns, logophoricity and long‐distance reflexivisation in
Avar. Studia Linguistica 71.154-177.
Saidov, Magomedsajid. 1967. Avarsko-russkij slovar’. MШsМШа: SШЯОЭsФКУК ̀nciklopedija.
Saidova, P. A. 2007. Zakatal’skij dialect avarskogo jazyka. Makhachkala: Institut JaLI DNC
RAN.
Sumbatova, Nina R. 2009. Constituent questions and argument-focus constructions: Some
data from the North-Caucasian languages. In Johannes Helmbrecht, Yoko Nisima, Yong‐
Min Shin, Stavros Skopeteas & Elisabeth Verhoeven (eds.), Form and function in
language research: Papers in honour of Christian Lehmann, 313–328. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
30
Testelets, Yakov G. 1998a. Word order variation in some SOV languages of Europe. In Anna
Siewierska (ed.), Constituent order in the languages of Europe, 649–679. Berlin: Mouton
de Gruyter.
Testelets, Yakov G. 1998b. Word order in Daghestanian languages. In Anna Siewierska (ed.),
Constituent order in the languages of Europe, 257–280. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Testelets, Yakov & Svetlana Y. Toldova. 1998. Refleksivnye mestoimenija v dagestanskix
jazykax i tipologija refleksiva. Voprosy Jazykoznanija 4.35–57.
von Uslar, Petr K. 1889. Ава і яз
. Tiflis: The Caucasian Academic District Office
Printing House.
Yamada, Hisanari. 2013. Reciprocal constructions in Avar. Lingua 126. 150–171.
31