Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Academia.eduAcademia.edu

UNDERSTANDING THE HUMAN MIND

2020, Understanding the Human Mind

Drawing on current research in anthropology, cognitive psychology, neuro-science, and the humanities, Understanding the Human Mind explores how and why we, as humans, find it so easy to believe we are right-even when we are outright wrong. Humans live out their own lives effectively trapped in their own mind and, despite being exceptional survivors and a highly social species, our inner mental world is often misaligned with reality. In order to understand why, John Ed-ward Terrell and Gabriel Stowe Terrell suggest current dual-process models of the mind overlook our mind's most decisive and unpredictable mode: creativity. Using a three-dimensional model of the mind, the authors examine the human struggle to stay in touch with reality-how we succeed, how we fail, and how winning this struggle is key to our survival in an age of mounting social problems of our own making. Using news stories of logic-defying behavior, analogies to famous fictitious characters, and analysis of evolutionary and cognitive psychology theory, this fascinating account of how the mind works is a must-read for all interested in anthropology and cognitive psychology. John Edward Terrell is internationally known for his pioneering research and publications on human biological and cultural diversity, social network analysis , human biogeography, and the peopling and prehistory of the Pacific Islands. Gabriel Stowe Terrell is studying industrial relations with an emphasis on conflict resolution techniques, organizational behavior, and labor history.

UNDERSTANDING THE HUMAN MIND Drawing on current research in anthropology, cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and the humanities, Understanding the Human Mind explores how and why we, as humans, find it so easy to believe we are right—even when we are outright wrong. Humans live out their own lives effectively trapped in their own mind and, despite being exceptional survivors and a highly social species, our inner mental world is often misaligned with reality. In order to understand why, John Edward Terrell and Gabriel Stowe Terrell suggest current dual-process models of the mind overlook our mind’s most decisive and unpredictable mode: creativity. Using a three-dimensional model of the mind, the authors examine the human struggle to stay in touch with reality—how we succeed, how we fail, and how winning this struggle is key to our survival in an age of mounting social problems of our own making. Using news stories of logic-defying behavior, analogies to famous fictitious characters, and analysis of evolutionary and cognitive psychology theory, this fascinating account of how the mind works is a must-read for all interested in anthropology and cognitive psychology. John Edward Terrell is internationally known for his pioneering research and publications on human biological and cultural diversity, social network analysis, human biogeography, and the peopling and prehistory of the Pacific Islands. Gabriel Stowe Terrell is studying industrial relations with an emphasis on conflict resolution techniques, organizational behavior, and labor history. “Why are humans so good at self-deception? What does that remarkable ability mean for our future on this planet? Terrell and Terrell provide a brilliantly provocative and honest assessment of the human condition and mind. Weaving insights from various scientific disciplines, from anthropology to neuroscience, they compellingly argue that evolution has bestowed humans with a handful of advantages, advantages that imperil humanity as a whole. This book is a remarkable achievement given both the breadth and complexity of ideas contained within, and their fidelity to those ideas in making them digestible and resonant with non-experts. It is a must read.” — Lane Beckes, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Neuroscience and Psychology, Bradley University “Explorers Terrell and Terrell take us on a guided tour of our own minds and the marvelous advantages and hidden uncertainties of our human commitment to social life. Informed by contemporary psychology and neuroscience, the Terrells’ collaboration offers original insights and perspectives on human nature and the future of our species illustrated using familiar, everyday experiences and stories. As a practicing psychologist, I think readers will benefit personally from the wellspring of meaning that flows from “knowing thyself ” in this illuminating way. The compelling conclusion asks us to consider “Do I need to do something? Should I look again?”—to which I would add “Should I read this book?” Yes, yes, and again yes.” — Thomas L. Clark, Ph.D., psychologist in private practice, Tallahassee FL “Terrell and Terrell draw upon deep time, trans-oceanic cultural research, and inter-generational cooperation in this bold, vivid work on self-persuasion and delusion. Brain function, social process, and ideology come together here in evolutionary perspective as the same topic in fresh, smooth prose, recruiting familiar characters from fiction. This engaging but humbling study contends that human creative thinking—including selective perception, logical reasoning, and dreaming—is also dangerous thinking. It urges us to recheck our own accruing presumptions, showing why it’s vital we do.” — Parker Shipton, Ph.D., Professor, Department of Anthropology, Research Fellow, African Studies Center, Boston University UNDERSTANDING THE HUMAN MIND Why You Shouldn’t Trust What Your Brain is Telling You John Edward Terrell and Gabriel Stowe Terrell First published 2020 by Routledge 52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017 and by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2020 Taylor & Francis The right of John Edward Terrell and Gabriel Stowe Terrell to be identified as authors of this work has been asserted by them in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers. Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A catalog record for this title has been requested ISBN: 978-0-367-85580-2 (hbk) ISBN: 978-0-367-85578-9 (pbk) ISBN: 978-1-003-01376-1 (ebk) Typeset in Bembo by codeMantra QUESTION Why are all of us so good at believing what we want to believe (even when we don’t know we want to believe it)? CONTENTS List of Figures Acknowledgements: The Powers of More than Two Getting Started 1 How Your Mind Works: Travels in Wonderland ix xi 1 4 2 Models of the Human Mind: How Do We Think About Thinking? 13 3 Human Failings in Reasoning: Why Do You Trust Yourself? 24 4 The Great Human High-Five Advantage: What Makes Us Human? 40 5 The Brain As a Pattern Recognition Device: How Do You Know That? 54 6 The Brain As a Pattern Learning Device: Why Do We Have Habits? 66 7 The Brain As a Pattern Making Device: What Makes Us Creative? 77 viii Contents 8 The Impact of Creativity: How Did You Learn That? 9 Lies, Deceit, and Self-Deception: How Gullible Are You? 91 102 10 Human Isolation and Loneliness: Private Lives and Public Duties 115 11 Pros and Cons of Being Human: The War of the Worlds 125 12 Making Sense of Our Future Prospects: Are We an Endangered Species? 138 Index 151 FIGURES 2.1 Mr Sherlock Holmes (Credit: Strand Magazine, 1892) 2.2 George Edward Challenger (Credit: Chronicle/Alamy Stock Photo) 2.3 Alice and her cat Dinah (Credit: John Tennial, 1871) 4.1 The high-five advantage 11.1 SWOT matrix (Credit: adapted from SWOT analysis diagram in English Language by Xhienne) 12.1 Extinct “Irish elks” (Megaloceros giganteus) on display at the Natural History Museum (Músaem Stair an Dúlra) in downtown Dublin, Ireland 17 18 21 43 128 139 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The Powers of More than Two We all know that two heads can be better than one. We ourselves are also quite sure that two heads are often not enough to get anything worth doing done well. We thank the staff and cuisine at Willalby’s Cafe, 1351 Williamson St, Madison, WI for facilitating our weekly writers’ breakfast on Saturday mornings while the two of us were crafting this book. Others have given us sustenance of another sort, namely their time, attention, and helpfully critical thoughts. We thank in particular Lane Beckes, Emma Cieslik, Tom Clark, Jane Connolly, Helen Dawson, Mark Golitko, Jim Koeppl, Scott Lidgard, Jonathan Maynord, Francie Muraski-Stotz, Neal Matherne, Esther Schechter, Pat Shipman, Parker Shipton, Uliana Solovieva, and John Stowe. It’s true what they say. It takes a community to raise a child. It also takes friends to write a book. GETTING STARTED The authors of this book are father and son. We suspect such a writing partnership is unusual enough to call for explanation. Let us begin, therefore, at the beginning. John and Jane, his twin, were born five minutes apart and a month premature in Washington, D.C., in 1942. John nearly died shortly after birth, but he managed to pull through more or less unscathed. He has never let Jane forget she is older than he is. Gabriel (Gabe) was born to a single mother in Lambaré, Paraguay, in 1990. The following year, John went down to Asunción, the capital city, to adopt Gabriel when he was 13 months old. That is a story in itself, but not one for here and now. John is seen by more than a few nowadays as a feisty senior citizen. Gabe qualifies as a millennial, but unlike the view of his generation as lazy, selfabsorbed, and without aspirations, he is definitely not afraid to challenge conventional wisdom. Regardless of how others see us, we both like to see ourselves as two thoughtful but kindly guys. Now it happens that beyond being father and son, we actually like talking to one another. By profession, John is an anthropologist who has been working in the South Pacific ever since he went out to New Zealand as a Fulbright Fellow in 1965. He has been trying to figure out what it means to be human ever since he took his first college anthropology course four years earlier. As a South American adoptee growing up in Wisconsin where the old dictum blood is thicker than water is still believed by many to make sense, Gabe has long known that for those of us living north of the Rio Grande, adoption is often seen as dangerous and a second-rate way of becoming a family (Terrell and Model 1994). Given these biographical facts, is it any wonder that the two of us might want to write a book together? But why this book? Chiefly for two reasons. 2 Getting Started First, Gabe’s mother in Paraguay tried to raise him successfully for the first five and a half months of his infancy. Thereafter, however, she had placed him in foster care in Asunción. He spent the next seven and a half months living mostly in a crib in a room filled with other babies in neighboring cribs. We have no firm idea what life was like for him all those months in a crib beyond feeding and diaper changes. We do know that he was evidently out of his crib so rarely that when John and he were introduced to one another for the first time in a hotel in Asunción, his feet were puffy and showed scant evidence of his ever trying to walk. We do know he only learned that vital set of motor skills after reaching his new home in Wisconsin on November 1st. Now without trying to go into clinical detail, the first year of life for any infant is a critically formative time. This is when babies begin to master the emotional and cognitive fundamentals of human social life. To be confined to a crib in a room filled with cribs may not be intentionally cruel, but social isolation of this sort can put any infant at risk. Hence one of the reasons we decided to write this book as a partnership is that by doing so we hoped we could find out more about what anthropology, psychology, and the cognitive sciences have discovered about why someone like Gabe had to struggle at times—as a child in grade school, and later, too, as a teenager—to know how much to trust the words, deeds, and reactions of other people. One reason for our writing partnership, therefore, is that we were able to take a journey together of self-discovery about what it means to be human. Our second reason is less personal. In his provocative book Powers of Two: Finding the Essence of Innovation in Creative Pairs (2014), Joshua Wolf Shenk has sought to document how, contrary perhaps to popular wisdom, the intellectual chemistry of two minds working together can spark a level of creativity and innovation far above and beyond the ordinary. He may well be right about the remarkable creativity of such dynamic pairs as John Lennon and Paul McCartney, or Marie and Pierre Curie, but as we explore in the chapters to follow, how Shenk accounts for the apparently elevated chemistry between such famous pairs of high achievers strikes us as simplistic. After all, Shenk himself acknowledges that 2 is not a magic number (2014, xxii–xxiii). Therefore, here is a hint about what follows in this book. All of us who are human struggle to get our thoughts, feelings, and hopes outside the confines of our own bony skulls. Elaborate explanations aside, it is a no-brainer to think that having at least one other person to talk to who is actually ready, willing, and able to listen to us and respond both positively and productively is a human connection that is literally priceless. What this ancient truth meant for the two of us is easily said. Although John took on the job Getting Started 3 of writing down most of what there is to read in this book, almost all of the thinking behind these words was done as a shared enterprise between the two of us, father and son. Works Cited Shenk, Joseph Wolf (2014). Powers of Two: Finding the Essence of Innovation in Creative Pairs. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Terrell, John Edward and Judith Modell (1994). Anthropology and adoption. American Anthropologist 96: 155–161. doi:10.1525/aa.1994.96.1.02a00130 Birhane, Abeba (2017). Descartes was wrong: “A person is a person through other persons.” AEON (7 April 2017). Retrieved from: https://aeon.co/ideas/ descartes-was-wrong-a-person-is-a-person-through-other-persons Carruthers, Peter (2017). The illusion of conscious thought. Journal of Consciousness Studies 24: 228–252. Chambliss, Bryan (2018). The mind–body problem. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 9, no. 4: e1463. doi:10.1002/wcs.1463 Fuentes, Agustín (2017). Creative collaboration is what humans do best. New York, March 22, 2017. Retrieved from: www.thecut.com/2017/03/how-imaginationmakes-us-human.html Gilbert, Scott F. (2017). Biological individuality: A relational reading. In Biological Individuality. Integrating Scientific, Philosophical, and Historical Perspectives, Scott Lidgard and Lynn K. Nyhart (eds.), pp. 297‒317. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Godwin, Christine A. Godwin, Michael A. Hunter, Matthew A. Bezdek, Gregory Lieberman, Seth Elkin-Frankston, Victoria L. Romero, Katie Witkiewitz, Vincent P. Clark, and Eric H. Schumacher (2017). Functional connectivity within and between intrinsic brain networks correlates with trait mind wandering. Neuropsychologia 103: 140–153. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.006 Goodenough, Ward H. (1967). Componential analysis. Science 156: 1203–1209. doi:10.1126/science.156.3779.1203 Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2012). The remarkable, yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its associated cost. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, Suppl. 1: 10661–10668. doi:10.1073/pnas.1201895109 Junger, Sebastian (2016). Tribe. On Homecoming and Belonging. London: 4th Estate. Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Kandel, Eric R. (1999). Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: A new intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. American Journal of Psychiatry 156: 505–524. doi:10.1176/ajp.156.4.505 Kandel, Eric R. (2019). The Disordered Mind: What Unusual Brains Tell Us about Ourselves. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Lupyan, Gary and Andy Clark (2015). Words and the world: Predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24: 279–284. doi:10.1177/0963721415570732 Melnikoff, David E. and John A. Bargh (2018). The mythical number two. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 280–293, 668–669. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.001 Pennycook, Gordon, Wim De Neys, Jonathan St. B.T. Evans, Keith E. Stanovich, and Valerie A. Thompson (2018). The mythical dual-process typology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 667–668. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.008 Reber, Thomas P., Jennifer Faber, Johannes Niediek, Jan Boström, Christian E. Elger, and Florian Mormann (2017). Single-neuron correlates of conscious perception in the human medial temporal lobe. Current Biology 27: 2991–2998. doi:10.1016/ j.cub.2017.08.025 Satel, Sally and Scott O. Lilienfeld (2013). Brainwashed: The Seductive Appeal of Mindless Neuroscience. New York: Basic Books. Simon, Herbert A. (1969). The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Simon, Herbert A. (1986). Rationality in psychology and economics. Journal of Business 59: S209–S224. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2352757 Sowden, Paul T., Andrew Pringle, and Liane Gabora (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning 21: 40–60. doi:10.1080/13546783.2014.885464 Terrell, John Edward (2000). Anthropological knowledge and scientific fact. American Anthropologist 102: 808–817. doi:10.1525/aa.2000.102.4.808 Bressler, Steven L. and Vinod Menon (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 277–290. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004 Buller, David J. (2005). Evolutionary psychology: The emperor’s new paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Psychology 9: 277–283. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0601_1 Crichton, Michael (2003). Introduction to The Lost World by Arthur Conan Doyle. Modern Library Classics edition. Random House. Retrieved from: http://www. michaelcrichton.com/introduction-arthur-conan-doyle-s-lost-world/ De Neys, Wim, and Gordon Pennycook (2019). Logic, fast and slow: Advances in dual-process theorizing. Current Directions in Psychological Science 28: 503–509. doi:10.1177/0963721419855658 Eagleman, David (2011). Incognito: The Secret Lives of the Brain. New York: Vintage Books. Hackel, Leor M., Grace M. Larson, Jeffrey D. Bowen, Gaven A. Ehrlich, Thomas C. Mann, Brianna Middlewood, Ian D. Roberts, Julie Eyink, Janell C. Fetterolf, Fausto Gonzalez, Carlos O. Garrido, Jinhyung Kim, Thomas C. O’Brien, Ellen E. O’Malley, Batja Mesquita, and Lisa Feldman Barrett (2016). On the neural implausibility of the modular mind: Evidence for distributed construction dissolves boundaries between perception, cognition, and emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39, E246. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15002770 Hansen, Bradley A. (2002). The fable of the allegory: The Wizard of Oz in economics. Journal of Economic Education 33: 254–264. doi:10.1080/00220480209595190 Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Levins, Richard (1966). The strategy of model building in population biology. American Scientist 54: 421–431. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/27836590 Littlefield, Henry M. (1964). The Wizard of Oz: Parable on populism. American Quarterly 16: 47–58. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2710826 Mekern, Vera, Bernhard Hommel, and Zsuzsika Sjoerds (2019). Computational models of creativity: A review of single-process and multi-process recent approaches to demystify creative cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 27: 47–54. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.008 Parker, David B. (1994). The rise and fall of The Wonderful Wizard of Oz as a “parable on populism.” Journal of the Georgia Association of Historians 15: 49–63. Pinker, Steven (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton. Raichle, Marcus E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38: 433–447. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030 Rockoff, Hugh (1990). The “Wizard of Oz” as a monetary allegory. Journal of Political Economy 98: 739–760. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2937766 Thomson, June (2012). Holmes and Watson. London: Allison & Busby. Van Dam, Nicholas T., Marieke K. van Vugt, David R. Vago, Laura Schmalzl, Clifford D. Saron, Andrew Olendzki, Ted Meissner, Sara W. Lazar, Catherine E. Kerr, Jolie Gorchov, Kieran C. R. Fox, Brent A. Field, Willoughby B. Britton, Julie A. Brefczynski-Lewis, and David E. Meyer (2018). Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science 13: 36–61. doi:10.1177/1745691617709589 Waddington, Conrad H. (1977). Tools for Thought. St. Albans: Paladin. Wittmann, Marc (2017). Felt Time: The Science of How We Experience Time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 3-year-old accidentally shoots both parents at Albuquerque motel. CBS News, February 1, 2015. Retrieved from: www.cbsnews.com/news/3-year-oldaccidentally-shoots-both-parents-at-albuquerque-motel/ Adams, Michael Anthony and Vic Ryckaert (2016). Child, 2, kills self with gun from mom’s purse. IndyStar, April 20, 2016. Retrieved from: www.indystar.com/ story/news/crime/2016/04/20/2-year-old-dies-self-inf licted-gunshot-wouldnorthwest-side-police-confirm/83317528/ Barnard, Ann (2016). ISIS militant kills mother on group’s orders, activists say. New York Times, January 8, 2016. Retrieved from: www.nytimes.com/2016/01/09/ world/middleeast/isis-militant-mother-raqqa.html Chodosh, Sarah (2017). Sorry, but New Zealand really needs to kill these adorable rabbits. Popular Science, July 28, 2017. Retrieved from: www.popsci.com/ new-zealand-needs-to-kill-those-adorable-rabbits-on-its-sheep/ Cleary, Tom (2015). Devon Staples: 5 fast facts you need to know. Heavy, July 8, 2015. Retrieved from: https://heavy.com/news/2015/07/devon-staples-calais-mainef ireworks-accident-man-put-shoot-f ireworks-off-head-killed-dead-photosfacebook-4th-of-july-fourth/ Craig, Scott (n.d.). Mast Brothers: What lies behind the beards (Part 1, Taste/ Texture). DallasFood. Retrieved from: http:// dallasfood.org/2015/12/mast-brotherswhat-lies-behind-the-beards-part-1-tastetexture/ Darwin awards (n.d.). Retrieved from: https://darwinawards.com/ Fiske, Alan Page and Tage Shakti Rai (2015). Virtuous Violence: Hurting and Killing to Create, Sustain, End, and Honor Social Relationships. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Freedman, Samuel G. (2012). If the Sikh Temple had been a mosque. New York Times, August 10, 2012. Retrieved from: www.nytimes.com/2012/08/11/us/if-the-sikhtemple-had-been-a-muslim-mosque-on-religion.html?ref=topics Fresno (2015). On-going hate crime investigation. Fresno Police Department, December 27, 2015. Retrieved from: www.facebook.com/FresnoPoliceDepartment/ posts/10156341080330006 Goode, Erica and Serge F. Kovaleski (2012). Wisconsin killer fed and was fueled by hate-driven music. Retrieved from: New York Times, August 6, 2012. https://www. nytimes.com/2012/08/07/us/army-veteran-identified-as-suspect-in-wisconsinshooting.html?_r=1&pagewanted=all Greenberg, Will (2015). Maine man didn’t mean to launch fireworks from his head that killed him, say friends. Washington Post, July 7, 2015. Retrieved from: https:// www.washingtonpost.com/news/morning-mix/wp/2015/07/07/friends-familydefend-maine-man-who-killed-himself-with-fireworks/ Hall, John (2016). Isis militant Ali Saqr al-Qasem publicly executes own mother in Raqqa after accusing her of ‘apostasy’. Independent, January 8, 2016. Retrieved from: www.independent.co.uk/news/world/middle-east/isis-militant-ali-saqr-al-qasempublicly-executes-his-own-mother-in-raqqa-after-accusing-her-of-a6801811.html Holley, Peter (2015). Americans attack Sikhs because they think they’re Muslims. The Sydney Morning Herald, December 30, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www. smh.com.au/world/americans-attack-sikhs-because-they-think-theyre-muslims20151228-glw1oz.html Holley, Peter (2016). Obama White House sends highest-ever official to Sikh house of worship following attacks. Washington Post, January 11, 2016. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/news/acts-of-faith/wp/2015/12/28/americans-arestill-attacking-sikhs-because-they-think-theyre-muslims/?hpid=hp_no-name_hpin-the-news%3Apage%2Fin-the-news Islamic State militant “executes own mother” in Raqqa. BBC News, January 8, 2016. Retrieved from: http://www.bbc.com/news/world-middle-east-35260475 Landau, Joel (2015a). Maine man dies after launching firework off his head. New York Daily News, July 6, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.nydailynews.com/news/ crime/maine-man-dies-launching-firework-top-head-article-1.2282157 Landau, Joel (2015b). Maine man who died after putting firework on his head thought it was a dud, almost eloped days before tragic accident. New York Daily News, July 7, 2015. Retrieved from: www.nydailynews.com/news/national/ maine-man-died-firework-thought-dud-article-1.2283915 Larimer, Sarah (2016). Two-year-old fatally shoots himself with gun found in mother’s purse, police say. Washington Post, April 21, 2016. Retrieved from: https://www. washingtonpost.com/news/post-nation/wp/2016/04/21/two-year-old-fatallyshoots-himself-with-gun-found-in-mothers-purse-police-say/ Libardi, Manuella (2015). Alligator in deadly attack shot and killed. Chron, July 7, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.chron.com/news/article/Alligator-suspectedof-deadly-attack-shot-and-6369345.php Man killed by giant 11 ft alligator after ignoring warning signs and jumping into Texas bayou taunted the beast before leaping into the water. Daily Mail, July 5, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-3149839/Man-killedalligator-Texas-mocked-beast-jumped-marina.html Marks two years since Sikh Temple shooting. NBC News, August 5, 2014. Retrieved from: www.nbcnews.com/news/asian-america/oak-creek-communitymarks-two-years-sikh-temple-shooting-n171981 Mast. Retrieved from: mastbrothers.com/ Merton, Robert K. (1936). The unanticipated consequences of purposive social action. American Sociological Review 1: 894–904. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2084615 Naylor, Hugh and Justin Wm. Moyer (2016). Islamic State fighter publicly executes own mother, Syrian activists say. Washington Post, January 8, 2016. Retrieved from: https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/morning-mix/wp/2016/01/08/islamicstate-fighter-publicly-executes-his-mother-report-says/ Nelson, Sara C. (2015). Tommie Woodward killed by alligator seconds after leaping into water screaming “f**k that alligator.” Huffington Post, July 7, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/2015/07/06/tommie-woodward-killedalligator-leaping-marina-screaming-f k-alligator_n_7733946.html Newport, Frank (2015). Mississippi, Alabama and Louisiana most conservative states. Gallup, February 6, 2015. Retrieved from: www.gallup.com/poll/181505/ mississippi-alabama-louisiana-conservative-states.aspx Nir, Sarah Maslin (2015). Unwrapping the mythos of Mast Brothers chocolate in Brooklyn. New York Times, December 20, 2015. Retrieved from: www.nytimes. com/2015/12/21/nyregion/unwrapping-mast-brothers-chocolatier-mythos.html Pashman, Dan (2015). Are you a sucker if you like Mast Brothers chocolate? The Salt, December 23, 2015. Retrieved from: www.npr.org/sections/thesalt/2015/12/23/ 460819387/are-you-a-sucker-if-you-like-mast-brothers-chocolate Preuss, Andreas (2015). Man mocks alligators, jumps in water and is killed in Texas. CNN, July 4, 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.cnn.com/2015/07/04/us/texasalligator-attack/index.html Rabbits – Rāpeti. Introduction into New Zealand. Christchurch City Libraries. Retrieved from: https://my.christchurchcitylibraries.com/rabbits/ Robinson, Carol (2015a). “I hurt my Dad”: Family distraught after 2-year-old boy accidentally shoots, kills father. AL.com Birmingham Real-Times News, August 20, 2015 (revised January 13, 2019). Retrieved from: https://www.al.com/news/ birmingham/index.ssf/2015/08/i_hurt_my_dad_family_distraugh.html Robinson, Carol (2015b). Man fatally shot at Hoover apartment; child possibly pulled trigger. AL.com Birmingham Real-Times News, August 18, 2015 (revised January 13, 2019). Retrieved from: https://www.al.com/news/birmingham/index.ssf/2015/ 08/man_fatally_shot_at_hoover_apa.html Shanker, Deena (2015). How the Mast Brothers fooled the world into paying $10 a bar for crappy hipster chocolate. Quartz, December 17, 2015. Retrieved from: https:// qz.com/571151/the-mast-brothers-fooled-the-world-into-buying-crappy-hipsterchocolate-for-10-a-bar/ Stack, Liam (2015). Hate crime inquiry opened into vandalism of Sikh Temple in California. New York Times, December 9, 2015. Retrieved from: http://www.nytimes. com/2015/12/10/us/hate-crime-inquiry-opened-into-vandalism-of-sikh-templein-california.html Thaler, Richard H. (2015). Misbehaving: The Making of Behavioral Economics. New York: Norton. Thaler, Richard H. and Cass R. Sunstein (2011). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tierney, John (2014). Which states are givers and which are takers? The Atlantic, May 5, 2014 (revised March 8, 2017). Retrieved from: https://www.theatlantic.com/ business/archive/2014/05/which-states-are-givers-and-which-are-takers/361668/ Yaccino, Steven, Michael Schwirtz, and Marc Santora (2012). Gunman kills 6 at a Sikh Temple near Milwaukee. New York Times, August 5, 2012. Retrieved from: www. nytimes.com/2012/08/06/us/shooting-reported-at-temple-in-wisconsin.html Adolphs, Ralph (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology 60: 693–716. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514 Agamben, Giorgio (2001). The Open: Man and Animal. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Barrett, Louise and Peter Henzi (2005). The social nature of primate cognition. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 272: 1865–1875. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3200 Bowden, Margaret (2004). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms, 2nd ed. London: Routledge. Boyd, Robert, Peter J. Richerson, and Joseph Henrich (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, Suppl. 2: 10918–10925. doi:10.1073/pnas.1100290108 Cacioppo, John T., Gary G. Berntson, and Jean Decety (2010). Social neuroscience and its relationship to social psychology. Social Cognition 28: 675–685. doi:10.1521/ soco.2010.28.6.675 Christakis, Nicholas A. (2019). Blueprint: The Evolutionary Origins of a Good Society. New York: Little, Brown Spark. Costandi, Moheb (2016). Neuroplasticity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DeCasien, Alex R., Scott A. Williams, and James P. Higham (2017). Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature Ecology and Evolution 1: article no. 0112. doi:10.1038/s41559-017-0112 Dunbar, Robin I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6: 178–190. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998) Dunbar, Robin I. M. and Susanne Shultz (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317: 1344–1347. doi:10.1126/science.1145463 Eagleman, David (2015). The Brain. The Story of You. New York: Pantheon Books. Fuentes, Agustín (2017a). Creative collaboration is what humans do best. New York, March 22, 2017. Retrieved from: https://www.thecut.com/2017/03/how-imaginationmakes-us-human.html Fuentes, Agustín (2017b). The Creative Spark: How Imagination Made Humans Exceptional. New York: Penguin. Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2012). The remarkable, yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its associated cost. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, Supplement 1: 10661–10668. doi:10.1073/pnas.1201895109 Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2016). The Human Advantage. A New Understanding of How Our Brain Became Remarkable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Ingold, Tim (2006). Against human nature. In Evolutionary Epistemology, Language and Culture: A Non-Adaptationist, Systems Theoretical Approach, edited by Nathalie Gontier, Jean Paul Van Bendegem, and Diederik Aerts, vol. 39, pp. 259–281. Dordrecht: Springer Science & Business Media. James, William (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co. Junger, Sebastian (2016). Tribe. On Homecoming and Belonging. London: 4th Estate. Kinreich, Sivan, Amir Djalovski, Lior Kraus, Yoram Louzoun, and Ruth Feldman (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Scientific Reports 7: 17060. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5 Koestler, Arthur (1964). The Act of Creation. London: Hutchinson & Co. Lende, Daniel H. and Greg Downey (2012). The Encultured Brain: An Introduction to Neuroanthropology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Lieberman, Matthew D. (2013). Social. Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown Publishers. Malmberg, Kenneth J., Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers, and Richard M. Shiffrin (2019). 50 years of research sparked by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). Memory & Cognition 47: 561–574. doi:10.3758/s13421-019-00896-7 Marks, Jonathan (2017). Is Science Racist? Malden, MA: Polity Press. Martin, Robert (2013). How We Do It: The Evolution and Future of Human Reproduction. New York: Basic Books. Parkinson, Carolyn, Adam M. Kleinbaum, and Thalia Wheatley (2018). Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nature Communications 9: 332–345. doi:10.1038/ s41467-017-02722-7 Pontzer, Herman, Mary H. Brown, David A. Raichlen, Holly Dunsworth, Brian Hare, Kara Walker, Amy Luke, Lara R. Dugas, Ramon Durazo-Arvizu, Dale Schoeller, Jacob Plange-Rhule, Pascal Bovet, Terrence E. Forrester, Estelle V. Lambert, Melissa Emery Thompson, Robert W. Shumaker, and Stephen R. Ross (2016). Metabolic acceleration and the evolution of human brain size and life history. Nature 533: 390–392. doi:10.1038/nature17654 Shenk, Joshua Wolf (2014). Powers of Two: Finding the Essence of Innovation in Creative Pairs. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Sigman, Mariano (2017). The Secret Life of the Mind: How Your Brain Thinks, Feels, and Decides. New York: Little, Brown and Company. Simon, Herbert A. (1996). The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Sowden, Paul T., Andrew Pringle, and Liane Gabora (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning 21: 40–60. doi:10.1080/13546783.2014.885464 Stagoll, Brian (2017). Systemic therapy and the unbearable lightness of psychiatry. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy 38: 357–375. doi:10.1002/anzf.1234 Sussman, Robert (2010). Human nature and human culture. In: Agustín Fuentes, Jonathan Marks, Tim Ingold, Robert Sussman, Patrick V. Kirch, Elizabeth M. Brumfiel, Rayna Rapp, Faye Ginsburg, Laura Nader, and Conrad P. Kottak, On nature and the human. American Anthropologist 112: 512–521. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01271.x Terrell, John Edward (2015). A Talent for Friendship. New York: Oxford University Press. Tomasello, Michael and Ivan Gonzalez-Cabrera (2017). The role of ontogeny in the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature 1–15. doi:10.1007/s12110-017-9291-1 Trevathan, Wenda R. and Karen R. Rosenberg, eds. (2016). Costly and Cute: Helpless Infants and Human Evolution. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. Watts, Amanda (2017). An 8-month-old baby is recovering after she was stuffed in a plastic bag for three days. CNN, August 11, 2017. Retrieved from: https://www. cnn.com/2017/08/10/us/baby-plastic-bag---trnd/index.html Wilson, Edward O. (1978). On Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wilson, Edward O. (2012). The Social Conquest of the Earth. New York: Liveright (a division of W. W. Norton). Wrangham, Richard (2009). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human. New York: Basic Books. Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Beckes, Lane, Hans IJzerman, and Mattie Tops (2015). Toward a radically embodied neuroscience of attachment and relationships. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9: 266. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00266 Bjork, Daniel W. (1993). B. F. Skinner: A Life. New York: Basic Books. Buonomano, Dean (2017). Your Brain is a Time Machine: The Neuroscience and Physics of Time. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. Chemero, Anthony (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology 15: 181–195. doi:10.1207/S15326969ECO1502_5 Chemero, Anthony and Michael J. Silberstein (2008). After the philosophy of mind: Replacing scholasticism with science. Philosophy of Science 75: 1–27. doi:10.1086/587820 Chomsky, Noam (1971). The case against B. F. Skinner. The New York Review of Books 17(11): 18–24. de Lange, Floris P., Micha Heilbron, and Peter Kok (2018). How do expectations shape perception? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 764–779. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.002 Dehaene, Stanislas (2014). Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts. New York: Penguin Books. Ding, Stephanie, Christopher J. Cueva, Misha Tsodyks, and Ning Qian (2017). Visual perception as retrospective Bayesian decoding from high- to low-level features. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: E9115–E9124. doi:10.1073/pnas.1706906114 Eagleman, David (2015). The Brain. The Story of You. New York: Pantheon Books. Esenkaya, Tayfun and Michael J. Proulx (2016). Crossmodal processing and sensory substitution: Is “seeing” with sound and touch a form of perception or cognition? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: e241. doi:10.1017/S0140525X1500268X Felin, Teppo, Jan Koenderink, and Joachim I. Krueger (2017). Rationality, perception, and the all-seeing eye. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1040–1059. doi:10.3758/ s13423-016-1198-z Firestone, Chaz and Brian J. Scholl (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for” top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: e229. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15000965 Frankland, Paul W., Sheena A. Josselyn, and Stefan Köhler (2019). The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience 22: 1576–1585. doi:10.1038/ s41593-019-0493-1 Gantman, Ana P. and Jay J. Van Bavel (2016). Behavior is multiply determined, and perception has multiple components: The case of moral perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: e242. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15002800 Garfield, Jay L. (2016). Illusionism and givenness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 23, no. 11–12: 73–82. Gigerenzer, Gerd (2016). Towards a rational theory of heuristics. In Minds, Model, and Milieu. Commemorating the Centenary of Herbert Simon’s Birth, Roger Frantz and Leslie Marsh (eds.), pp. 34–59. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Graziano, Michael S. A. (2019). Rethinking Consciousness: A Scientific Theory of Subjective Experience. New York: W. W. Norton. Gross, James J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation 2nd ed., James J. Gross (ed.), pp. 3–20. New York, Guilford Press. Harris, Julia J., Renaud Jolivet, and David Attwell (2012). Synaptic energy use and supply. Neuron 75: 762–777. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.019 Hawkins, Jeff, Subutai Ahmad, and Yuwei Cui. (2017). A theory of how columns in the neocortex enable learning the structure of the world. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 11: 81. doi:10.3389/fncir.2017.00081 Henderson, John M. and Taylor R. Hayes (2017). Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps. Nature Human Behaviour 1, no. 10: 743–747. doi:10.1038/s41562 Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2016). The Human Advantage: A New Understanding of How Our Brain Became Remarkable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hoffman, Donald D. (2016). The interface theory of perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science 25: 157–161. doi:10.3758/s1342 Hoffman, Donald D. (2019). The Case Against Reality. Why Evolution Hid the Truth from Our Eyes. New York: W. W. Norton. Hohwy, Jakob (2018). The predictive processing hypothesis. In The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, Albert Newen, Leon De Bruin, and Shaun Gallagher (eds.), pp. 129–146. Oxford University Press. Holland, Peter C. (2008). Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Learning & Behavior 36: 227–241. doi:10.3758%2Flb.36.3.227 Lupyan, Gary (2015). Cognitive penetrability of perception in the age of prediction: Predictive systems are penetrable systems. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6: 547– 569. doi:10.1007/s1316 Lupyan, Gary and Andy Clark (2015). Words and the world: Predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24: 279–284. doi:10.1177/0963721415570732 Mergenthaler, Philipp, Ute Lindauer, Gerald A. Dienel, and Andreas Meisel (2013). Sugar for the brain: The role of glucose in physiological and pathological brain function. Trends in Neurosciences 36: 587–597. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.07.001 Ponce, Carlos R., Will Xiao, Peter F. Schade, Till S. Hartmann, Gabriel Kreiman, and Margaret S. Livingstone (2019). Evolving images for visual neurons using a deep generative network reveals coding principles and neuronal preferences. Cell 177: 999–1009. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2019.04.005 Raichle, Marcus E. (2010). Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 180–190. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008 Samaha, Jason, Bastien Boutonnet, Bradley R. Postle, and Gary Lupyan (2018). Effects of meaningfulness on perception: Alpha-band oscillations carry perceptual expectations and influence early visual responses. Scientific Reports 8, no. 1: 6606. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-25093 Simon, Herbert (1978). Rationality as process and as product of thought. The American Economic Review 68, no. 2: 1–16. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/1816653 Simon, Herbert A. (1996). Sciences of the Artificial, 3rd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Skinner, Burrhus F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Simon and Schuster. Skinner, Burrhus F. (1971). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Knopf. Skinner, Burrhus F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5: 1–10. Skinner, Burrhus F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science 213: 501–504. doi:10.1126/science.7244649 Skinner, Burrhus F. (1985). Cognitive science and behaviourism. British Journal of Psychology 76: 291–301. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01953.x Skinner, Burrhus F. (1990). Can psychology be a science of mind? American Psychologist 45: 1206–1210. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.11.1206 Thaler, Richard H. (2015). Misbehaving: The Making of Behavioral Economics. New York: W. W. Norton. von Helmholtz, Hermann (1924). Treatise on Physiological Optics, vol. 3 (English translation of Handbuch der physiologischen Optik originally published by the Optical Society of America in 1924 and edited by James Powell Cocke Southall), Dover edition, 1962 and reprinted 2005. Wilson, Margaret (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9: 625–636. doi:10.3758/BF03196322 Ananthaswamy, Anil (2017). The brain’s 7D sandcastles could be the key to consciousness. New Scientist 3145: 28–32. Retrieved from: https://www.newscientist.com/article/ mg23531450-200-the-brains-7d-sandcastles-could-be-the-key-to-consciousness/ Ayala, Francisco J. (2008). Science, evolution, and creationism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 3–4. doi:10.1073/pnas.0711608105 Beebe, Beatrice, Daniel Messinger, Lorraine E. Bahrick, Amy Margolis, Karen A. Buck, and Henian Chen (2016). A systems view of mother–infant face-to-face communication. Developmental Psychology 52: 556–571. doi:10.1037%2Fa0040085 Bornstein, Marc H. (2014). Human infancy . . . and the rest of the lifespan. Annual Review of Psychology 65: 121–158. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100359 Clark, Andy (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36: 181–253. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X12000477 Colombo, Matteo (2013). Moving forward (and beyond) the modularity debate: A network perspective. Philosophy of Science 80: 356–377. doi:10.1086/670331 Dehaene, Stanislas (2014). Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts. New York: Penguin Books. Developmental milestones (n.d.). Birth to 12 months. Office of Child Development, University of Pittsburgh. Retrieved from: www.ocd.pitt.edu/Files/PDF/ Foster/27758_ocd_DM_b-12.pdf Fleissner, Peter and Wolfgang Hof kirchner (1996). Emergent information. Towards a unified information theory. BioSystems 38: 243–248. doi:10.1016/0303-2647(95) 01597-3 Fodor, Jerry A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Francis, Mark (2007). Herbert Spencer and the Invention of Modern Life. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Frankland, Paul W., Sheena A. Josselyn, and Stefan Köhler (2019). The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience 22: 1576–1585. doi:10.1038/ s41593-019-0493-1 Friston, Karl (2008). Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology 4(11): e1000211. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000211 Graziano, Michael S. A. (2019). Rethinking Consciousness: A Scientific Theory of Subjective Experience. New York: W. W. Norton. Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (2016). The Human Advantage: A New Understanding of How Our Brain Became Remarkable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kumar, Krishan (1997). Spencer, Herbert (1820–1903). In The Dictionary of Anthropology, Thomas Barfield (ed.), pp. 443–444. Oxford: Blackwell. Kuo, Zing-Yang (1921). Giving up instincts in psychology. The Journal of Philosophy 18: 645–664. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2939656 Lupyan, Gary (2015). Cognitive penetrability of perception in the age of prediction: Predictive systems are penetrable systems. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6: 547–569. doi:10.1007/s1316 Palecek, Martin (2017). Modularity of mind: Is it time to abandon this ship? Philosophy of the Social Sciences 47: 132–144. doi:10.1177/0048393116672833 Panksepp, Jaak and Mark Solms (2012). What is neuropsychoanalysis? Clinically relevant studies of the minded brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 6–8. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2011.11.005 Pepperell, Robert (2018). Consciousness as a physical process caused by the organization of energy in the brain. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 2091. doi:10.3389%2Ffpsyg. 2018.02091 Pinker, Steven (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Penguin Books. Sanborn, Adam N. and Nick Chater (2016). Bayesian brains without probabilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 883–893. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.003 Sapolsky, Robert M. (2017). Behave. The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. New York: Penguin Press. Thomason, Moriah E. (2018). Structured spontaneity: Building circuits in the human prenatal brain. Trends in Neurosciences 41: 1–3. doi:10.1016%2Fj.tins.2017.11.004 Thorpe, Ian J. N. (2003). Anthropology, archaeology, and the origin of warfare. World Archaeology 35: 145–165. doi:10.1080/0043824032000079198 Wei, Xue-Xin and Alan A. Stocker (2015). A Bayesian observer model constrained by efficient coding can explain “anti-Bayesian” percepts. Nature Neuroscience 18: 1509– 1517. doi:10.1038/nn.4105 Wenger, Elisabeth, Claudio Brozzoli, Ulman Lindenberger, and Martin Lövdén (2017). Expansion and renormalization of human brain structure during skill acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 930–939. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.008 Wilson, Edward O. (1978). On Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wilson, Edward O. (2012). The Social Conquest of the Earth. New York: Liveright (a division of W. W. Norton). Wood, Wendy and Dennis Rünger (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology 67: 289–314. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417 Abraham, Anna (2016). The imaginative mind. Human Brain Mapping 37: 4197–4211. doi:10.1002/hbm.23300 Adolphs, Ralph and David J. Anderson (2018). The Neuroscience of Emotion: A New Synthesis. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Andrews-Hanna, Jessica R., Jonathan Smallwood, and R. Nathan Spreng (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1316: 29–52, 32. doi:10.1111/nyas.12360 Baird, Benjamin, Jonathan Smallwood, Michael D. Mrazek, Julia W. Y. Kam, Michael S. Franklin, and Jonathan W. Schooler (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science 23: 1117–1122. doi:10.1177/0956797612446024 Barrett, Deirdre (2001). Comment on Baylor: A note about dreams of scientific problem solving. Dreaming 11: 93–95. doi:10.1023/A:1009436621758 Barrett, Deirdre (2011). Answers in your dreams. Scientific American Mind 22, no. 5: 27–33. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind1111-26 Barrett, Deirdre (2015). Dreams: Thinking in a different biochemical state. In Dream Research, Milton Kramer and Myron Glucksman (eds.), pp. 94–108. New York and Hove, East Sussex: Routledge. Barrett, Deirdre (2017). Dreams and creative problemsolving. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1406: 64–67. doi:10.1111/nyas.13412 Buckner, Randy L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. Neuroimage 62: 1137–1145. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.035 Buckner, Randy L., Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna, and Daniel L. Schacter (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124: 1–38. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011 Buckner, Randy L. and Lauren M. DiNicola (2019). The brain’s default network: Updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 20: 593–608. doi:10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7 Bulkeley, Kelly (2018). The meaningful continuities between dreaming and waking: Results of a blind analysis of a woman’s 30-year dream journal. Dreaming 28: 337– 350. doi:10.1037/drm0000083 Bulkeley, Kelly (2019). Dreaming is imaginative play in sleep: A theory of the function of dreams. Dreaming 29: 1–21. doi:10.1037/drm0000099 Carruthers, Peter (2017). The illusion of conscious thought. Journal of Consciousness Studies 24: 228–252. doi:10.1080/13869795.2013.723035 Christoff, Kalina, Alan M. Gordon, Jonathan Smallwood, Rachelle Smith, and Jonathan W. Schooler (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 106: 8719–8724. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900234106 Christoff, Kalina, Zachary C. Irving, Kieran C. R. Fox, R. Nathan Spreng, and Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17: 718–731. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.113 Corballis, Michael C. (2016). The Wandering Mind: What the Brain Does When You’re Not Looking. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Crittenden, Ben M., Daniel J. Mitchell, and John Duncan (2015). Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. Elife 4: e06481. doi:10.7554/eLife.06481 Dehaene, Stanislas (2014). Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts. New York: Penguin Books. Doyen, Stéphane, Olivier Klein, Daniel J. Simons, and Axel Cleeremans (2015). The other side of the mirror: Priming in cognitive and social psychology. Social Cognition 32, special issue: 12–32. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.12 Dyson, Freeman (2011). How to dispel your illusions. Review of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Daniel Kahneman. The New York Review of Books 58 (December 22): 40–43. Retrieved from: www.nybooks.com/articles/2011/12/22/how-dispel-your-illusions/ Gilhooly, Ken (2016). Incubation in creative thinking. In Cognitive Unconscious and Human Rationality, Laura Macchi, Maria Bagassi, and Riccardo Viale (eds.), pp. 301– 312. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Globus, Gordon (2019). Lucid Existenz during dreaming. International Journal of Dream Research 12: 70–74. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2019.1.52858 Kahneman, Daniel. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist 58: 697–720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Kandel, Eric R. (1999). Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: A new intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. American Journal of Psychiatry 156: 505–524. doi:10.1176/ajp.156.4.505 Pearce, Richard M. (1912), Chance and the prepared mind. Science 35: 941–956. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/1638153 Poerio, Giulia L., Mladen Sormaz, Hao-Ting Wang, Daniel Margulies, Elizabeth Jefferies, and Jonathan Smallwood (2007). The role of the default mode network in component processes underlying the wandering mind. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 12: 1047–1062. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx041 Poincaré, Henri (1910). Mathematical creation. The Monist 20: 321–335. doi:10.1093/ monist/20.3.321 Raichle, Marcus E. (2015a). The brain’s default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38: 433–447. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030 Raichle, Marcus E. (2015b). The restless brain: how intrinsic activity organizes brain function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 370: 20140172. doi:10.1098/ rstb.2014.0172 Shine, James M. and Michael Breakspear (2018). Understanding the brain, by default. Trends in Neurosciences 41: 244–247. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2018.03.004 Smallwood, Jonathan and Jonathan W. Schooler (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin 132: 946–958. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 Smallwood, Jonathan and Jonathan W. Schooler (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology 66: 487–518. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331 Strunz, Franz (1993). Preconscious mental activity and scientific problem-solving: A critique of the Kekulé dream controversy. Dreaming 3: 281–294. doi:10.1037/ h0094386 Vatansever, Deniz, David K. Menon, Anne E. Manktelow, Barbara J. Sahakian, and Emmanuel A. Stamatakis (2015). Default mode network connectivity during task execution. Neuroimage 122: 96–104. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.053 Vatansever, Deniz, David K. Menon, and Emmanuel A. Stamatakis (2017). Default mode contributions to automated information processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 114: 12821-12826. doi:10.1073/pnas.1710521114 Ananthaswamy, Anil (2018). What does quantum theory actually tell us about reality? Scientific American, September 3, 2018. Retrieved from: https://blogs.scientificamerican. com/observations/what-does-quantum-theory-actually-tell-us-about-reality/ Bardone, Emanuele and Lorenzo Magnani (2007). Sharing representations through cognitive niche construction. Data Science Journal 6, Suppl.: S87–S91. doi:10.2481/ dsj.6.S87 Barrett, Deidre (2015). Dreams: Thinking in a different biochemical state. In Dream Research: Contributions to Clinical Practice, Milton Kramer and Myron Glucksman (eds.), pp. 94–108. New York and Hove, East Sussex: Routledge. Biro, Dora, Michael Haslam, and Christian Rutz (2013). Tool use as adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120408. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0408 Bulkeley, Kelly (2017). The future of dream science. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1406: 68–70. doi:10.1111/nyas.13415 Cameron, Ross P. (2008). Turtles all the way down: Regress, priority and fundamentality. The Philosophical Quarterly 58: 1–14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9213.2007.509.x Domhoff, G. William and Kieran C. R. Fox (2015). Dreaming and the default network: A review, synthesis, and counterintuitive research proposal. Consciousness and Cognition 33: 342–353. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.019 Freeman, Daniel (2016). Persecutory delusions: A cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry 3: 685–692. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00066-3 Gleick, James (2018). What does quantum physics actually tell us about the world? The New York Times, May 8, 2018. Retrieved from: https://www.nytimes.com/2018/ 05/08/books/review/adam-becker-what-is-real.html Hoshino, Takatoshi and Yoshihiko Tanno (2017). Trait anxiety and impaired control of reflective attention in working memory. Cognition and Emotion 30: 369–377. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.993597 Koban, Leonie, Ethan Kross, Choong-Wan Woo, Luka Ruzic, and Tor D. Wager (2017). Frontal-brainstem pathways mediating placebo effects on social rejection. Journal of Neuroscience 37: 3621–3631. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2658-16.2017 Lakens, Daniel (2014). Grounding social embodiment. Social Cognition 32, suppl.: 168–183. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.168 Luria, A. R. (1987). The Mind of a Mnemonist: A Little Book about a Vast Memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. McGrew, W. C. (2013). Is primate tool use special? Chimpanzee and New Caledonian crow compared. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120422. doi:10.1098%2Frstb.2012.0422 Odling-Smee, John F., Kevin N. Laland, and Marcus W. Feldman (2003). Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Paulson, Steve, Deirdre Barrett, Kelly Bulkeley, and Rubin Naiman (2017). Dreaming: a gateway to the unconscious? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1406: 28–45. doi:10.1111/nyas.13389 Putnam, Adam L. (2015). Mnemonics in education: Current research and applications. Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1: 130–139. doi:10.1037/tps0000023 Quantum. (n.d.). Retrieved from: en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/quantum Sari, Berna A., Ernst H. W. Kostera, and Nazanin Derakshan (2016). The effects of active worrying on working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion 31: 995–1003. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1170668 Siclari, Francesca, Benjamin Baird, Lampros Perogamvros, Giulio Bernardi, Joshua J. LaRocque, Brady Riedner, Melanie Boly, Bradley R Postle, and Giulio Tononi (2017). The neural correlates of dreaming. Nature Neuroscience 20: 872–878. doi:10.1038/nn.4545 Siegel, Robert Anthony (2017). Why I take fake pills. Smithsonian Magazine, May 2107. Retrieved from: www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/why-i-takefake-pills-180962765/ Startup, Helen, Katherine Pugh, Graham Dunn, Jacinta Cordwell, Helen Mander, Emma Cernis, Gail Wingham, Katherine Shirvell, David Kingdon, and Daniel Freeman (2016). Worry processes in patients with persecutory delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 55: 387–400. doi:10.1111/bjc.12109 Sternberg, Eliezer J. (2015). NeuroLogic: The Brain’s Hidden Rationale behind Our Irrational Behavior. New York: Vintage Books. Terrell, John Edward (2015). A Talent for Friendship. New York: Oxford University Press. Vatansever, Deniz, David K. Menon, and Emmanuel A. Stamatakis (2017). Default mode contributions to automated information processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 114: 12821–12826. doi:10.1073/pnas.1710521114 Windt, Jennifer M., Tore Nielsen, and Evan Thompson (2016). Does consciousness disappear in dreamless sleep? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 871–882. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2016.09.006 Adolphs, Ralph (2017). How should neuroscience study emotions? By distinguishing emotion states, concepts, and experiences. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 12: 24–31. doi:10.1093%2Fscan%2Fnsw153 Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2012). Emotions are real. Emotion 12: 413–429. doi:10.1037/ a0027555 Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Beckes, Lane and James A. Coan (2011). Social baseline theory: The role of social proximity in emotion and economy of action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 5: 976–988. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00400.x Beckes, Lane, James A. Coan, and Karen Hasselmo (2012). Familiarity promotes the blurring of self and other in the neural representation of threat. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8: 670–677. doi:10.1093/scan/nss046 Beckes, Lane, Hans IJzerman, and Mattie Tops (2015). Toward a radically embodied neuroscience of attachment and relationships. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9: 266. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00266. Capozzola, Christopher (2002). The only badge needed is your patriotic fervor: Vigilance, coercion, and the law in World War I America. Journal of American History 88: 1354–1382. Coan, James A. and Erin L. Maresch (2014). Social baseline theory and the social regulation of emotion. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd ed., James J. Gross (ed.), pp. 221–236: 228. New York: Guilford. Coan, James A., Hillary S. Schaefer, and Richard J. Davidson (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological Science 17: 1032–1039. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x Cohen, Gabriel (2005). For you, half price. The New York Times, Sunday, November 27, 2005. Retrieved from: www.nytimes.com/2005/11/27/nyregion/thecity/for-youhalf-price.html Dezecache, Guillaume, Pierre Jacob, and Julie Grèzes (2015). Emotional contagion: Its scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19: 297–299. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2015.03.011 Dixon, Thomas (2017). Labels, rationality, and the chemistry of the mind: Moors in historical context. Psychological Inquiry 28: 27–30. doi:10.1080%2F10478 40X.2017.1256605 Felin, Teppo, Jan Koenderink, and Joachim I. Krueger (2017). Rationality, perception, and the all-seeing eye. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1040–1059. doi:10.3758/ s13423-016-1198-z Fleissner, Robert F. (1977). Stein’s Four Roses. Journal of Modern Literature 6: 325–328. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/3831176 Gross, James J. (2013). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd ed., James J. Gross (ed.), pp. 3–20. New York: Guilford Press. Hickey, Donald R. (1969). The Prager affair: A study in wartime hysteria. Journal of the Illinois State Historical Society 62: 117–134. Retrieved from: https://www.jstor.org/ stable/40191045 Kant, Immanuel (1784). An answer to the question: What is Enlightenment? Retrieve from: archive.org/stream/AnswerTheQuestionWhatIsEnlightenment/Kant EnlightmentDanielFidelFerrer2013_djvu.txt or en.wikisource.org/wiki/What_is_ Enlightenment%3F Krueger, Joel and Thomas Szanto (2016). Extended emotions. Philosophy Compass 11: 863–878. doi:10.1111/phc3.12390 Laland, Kevin N. (2017). Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. LeDoux, Joseph (2012). A neuroscientist’s perspective on debates about the nature of emotion. Emotion Review 4: 375–379. doi:10.1177/1754073912445822 Moors, Agnes (2017). Integration of two skeptical emotion theories: Dimensional appraisal theory and Russell’s psychological construction theory. Psychological Inquiry 28: 1–19. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2017.1235900 Panksepp, Jaak (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS ONE 6, no. 9: e21236. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021236 Pessoa, Luiz (2017). A network model of the emotional brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 357–371. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.002 Richards, Robert J. (2003). Darwin on mind, morals, and emotions. In The Cambridge Companion to Darwin, Jonathan Hodge and Gregory Radick (eds.), pp. 92–115. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CCOL0521771978.005 Schwartz, Earl Albert (2002/2003). The lynching of Robert Prager, the United Mine Workers, and the problems of patriotism in 1918. Journal of the Illinois State Historical Society 95: 414–437. https://www.jstor.org/stable/40193598 Shweder, Richard A. (2014). The tower of appraisals: Trying to make sense of the one big thing. Emotion Review 6: 322–324. doi:10.1177%2F1754073914534500 Swenson, Kyle (2017). Stomped to death and burned, a Muslim immigrant’s fate offers tragic lesson in U.K. The Washington Post, July 7, 2017. Retrieved from: www. washingtonpost.com/news/morning-mix/wp/2017/07/07/stomped-to-death-andburned-a-muslim-immigrants-fate-offers-tragic-lesson-in-uk/. The Brooklyn Bridge. (n.d.) A world wonder. Retrieved from: http://www.brooklyn bridgeaworldwonder.com/ Truth. (n.d.). Oxford Living Dictionaries. Retrieved from: en.oxforddictionaries.com/ definition/truth Wittmann, Marc (2017). Felt Time: The Science of How We Experience Time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Barker, Jessica L., Judith L. Bronstein, Maren L. Friesen, Emily I. Jones, H. Kern Reeve, Andrew G. Zink, and Megan E. Frederickson (2017). Synthesizing perspectives on the evolution of cooperation within and between species. Evolution 71: 814–825. doi:10.1111/evo.13174 Barth, Fredrik (1969). Introduction. In Ethnic Groups and Boundaries: The Social Organization of Culture Difference, Frederik Barth (ed.), pp. 9–38. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company. Bernhardt, Chris (2016). Turing’s Vision: The Birth of Computer Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Brubaker, Rogers (2004). Ethnicity without Groups. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Del Vicario, Michela, Fabiana Zollo, Guido Caldarelli, Antonio Scala, and Walter Quattrociocchi (2017). Mapping social dynamics on Facebook: The Brexit debate. Social Networks 50: 6–16. doi:10.1016/j.socnet.2017.02.002 Granovetter, Mark S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology 78: 1360–1380. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2776392 Granovetter, Mark S. (1983). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Sociological Theory 1: 201–233. doi:10.1007/978-3-658-21742-6_55 Lieberman, Matthew D. (2013). Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown. Onnela, Jukka-Pekka, Samuel Arbesman, Marta C. González, Albert-László Barabási, and Nicholas A. Christakis (2011). Geographic constraints on social network groups. PLoS ONE 6, no. 4 (2011): e16939. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016939 Raybeck, Douglas (2009). Introduction: Diversity not uniformity. Ethos 37: 155–160. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/20486607 Social media. Cambridge English Dictionary. Retrieved from https://dictionary.cambridge. org/us/dictionary/english/social-media Spiro, Emma S., Zack W. Almquist, and Carter T. Butts (2016). The persistence of division: Geography, institutions, and online friendship ties. Socius 2. doi:10.1177/2378023116634340 Terrell, John Edward. (2018) “Plug and Play” genetics, racial migrations and human history. Scientific American, May 29, 2018. Retrieved from: blogs.scientificamerican. com/observations/plug-and-play-genetics-racial-migrations-and-human-history/ Tomasello, Michael (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology 44: 187–194. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2015 Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–735. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000129 Wallace, Anthony F. C. (1961). The psychic unity of human groups. In Studying Personality Cross-Culturally, Bert Kaplan (ed.), pages 129–164. New York: Harper & Row. Wallace, Anthony F. C. (1970). Culture and Personality, 2nd ed. New York: Random House. Wilson, Edward O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wilson, Edward O. (2012). The Social Conquest of the Earth. New York: Liveright (a division of W. W. Norton). 14 Free SWOT analysis templates. Retrieved from: www.smartsheet.com/14-freeswot-analysis-templates A SWOT analysis, with its four elements in a 2×2 matrix. Retrieved from: https:// en.wikipedia.org/wiki/SWOT_analysis#/media/File:SWOT_en.svg Borgatti, Stephen P., Martin G. Everett, and Jeffrey C. Johnson (2013). Analyzing Social Networks. Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Crowe, Jonathan (2016). Human fallibility and the separation of powers. Policy: A Journal of Public Policy and Ideas 32: 42–46. Retrieved from: https://auspublaw.org/2015/07/ human-all-too-human/ Eisenstein, Alex (1976). “The Time Machine” and the end of man. Science Fiction Studies 4: 161–165. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/4239020 Forgas, Joseph P., Lee Jussim, and Paul A. M. Van Lange (2016). In search for Homo moralis: The social psychology of morality. In Social Psychology and Morality, Joseph P. Forgas, Lee Jussim, and Paul A. M. Van Lange (eds.), pp. 1–18. New York: Psychology Press. Fuller, Jack (2010). What is happening to news? Daedalus 139, no. 2: 110–118. Retrieved from: /www.jstor.org/stable/20749829 Haight, Gordon S. (1958). H. G. Wells’s “The Man of the Year Million.” NineteenthCentury Fiction 12: 323–326. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/3044429 How artificial intelligence and robots will radically transform the economy. Retrieved from: www.newsweek.com/2016/12/09/robot-economy-artificial-intelligence-jobshappy-ending-526467.html Kahan, Alan (2012). Arnold, Nietzsche and the aristocratic vision. History of Political Thought 33: 125–143. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/26225689 Lieberman, Matthew D. (2013). Social. Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown Publishers. Loader, Brian D., Ariadne Vromen, and Michael A. Xenos (2016). Performing for the young networked citizen? Celebrity politics, social networking and the political engagement of young people. Media, Culture & Society 38: 400–419. doi:10.1177%2F0163443715608261 Nomination database. Retrieved from: www.nobelprize.org/nomination/archive/ show_people.php?id=10075 Pagliarini, Luigi and Henrik Hautop Lund (2017). The future of robotics technology. Journal of Robotics Networking and Artificial Life 3: 270–273. doi:10.2991/ jrnal.2017.3.4.12 Partington, John S. (2000). The death of the static: H. G. Wells and the kinetic utopia. Utopian Studies 11: 96–111. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/20718177 Phillips, Kristine (2017). The world as we know it is about to end—again—if you believe this biblical doomsday claim. Washington Post, September 17, 2017. Retrieved from: https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/acts-of-faith/wp/2017/09/17/the-worldas-we-know-it-is-about-to-end-again-if-you-believe-this-biblical-doomsdayclaim/?hpid=hp_no-name_hp-in-the-news%3Apage%2Fin-the-news&utm_ term=.2e7438bbee20 Scott, John (2000). Social Network Analysis: A Handbook, 2nd ed. Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Stiles, Anne (2009). Literature in “Mind:” H. G, Wells and the evolution of the mad scientist. Journal of the History of Ideas 70: 317–339. Retrieved from: https://www. jstor.org/stable/40208106 SWOT analysis (strengths, weaknesses, opportunities and threats analysis). Retrieved from: https://searchcio.techtarget.com/definition/SWOT-analysis-strengths-weaknessesopportunities-and-threats-analysis Terrell, John Edward (2015). A Talent for Friendship. New York: Oxford University Press. Thaler, Richard H. and Cass R. Sunstein (2008). Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Von Drehle, David (2017). Steve Jobs gave us President Trump. Washington Post, September 5, 2017. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/steve-jobs-gaveus-president-trump/2017/09/05/f4f487e4-9260-11e7-aace-04b862b2b3f3_story. html?hpid=hp_no-name_opinion-card-d%3Ahomepage%2Fstory&utm_term=. c63669ceb63b Wilson, Edward O. (2014). The Meaning of Human Existence. New York: Liveright. Wolfe Alan (2015). Libertarianism’s iron cage: From Ayn Rand to Rand Paul. Commonweal 142 (15): 14–18. Retrieved from: https://www.commonwealmagazine.org/ libertarianisms-iron-cage 11 injured as museum staircase collapses. Retrieved from: https://www.rte.ie/news/ 2007/0705/90868-museum/ Ceballos, Gerardo, Paul R. Ehrlich, and Rodolfo Dirzo (2017). Biological annihilation via the ongoing sixth mass extinction signaled by vertebrate population losses and declines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: E6089‒E6096. doi:10.1073/pnas.1704949114 Deegan, Gordon (2015). “Taxpayers’ €900k bill over Dead Zoo staircase collapse.” Herald.ie October 1, 2015. Retrieved from www.herald.ie/news/taxpayers-900kbill-over-dead-zoo-staircase-collapse-31572396.html Gould, Stephen Jay (1974). The origin and function of “bizarre” structures: Antler size and skull size in the “Irish elk,” Megaloceros giganteus. Evolution 28: 191–220. doi:10.2307/2407322 Graziano, Michael S. A. (2019). Rethinking Consciousness: A Scientific Theory of Subjective Experience. New York: W. W. Norton. History & Architecture of Natural History. Retrieved from: www.museum.ie/ Corporate-Media/History-of-the-Museum/History-Architecture-(3) Jacob, François (1977). Evolution and tinkering. Science 196: 1161–1166. doi:10.1126/ science.860134 Matheny, Jason G. (2007). Reducing the risk of human extinction. Risk Analysis: An International Journal 27: 1335–1344. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2007.00960.x Neylon, Laoise (2016). “It’s lonely at the Dead Zoo.” Dublin InQuirer June 8, 2016. Re- trieved from: www.dublininquirer.com/2016/06/08/it-s-lonely-at-the-dead-zoo Nuzzo, Regina (2015). Fooling ourselves. Nature 526: 182–185. doi:10.1038/526182a Pinker, Steven (2018). Enlightenment Now: The Case for Reason, Science, Humanism, and Progress. New York: Viking (Penguin). Ripple, William J., Christopher Wolf, and Thomas M. Newsome (2019). World scientists’ warning of a climate emergency. BioScience, biz088. doi:10.1093/biosci/biz088 Rovelli, Carlo (2017). Reality is Not What It Seems. The Journey to Quantum Gravity. New York: Riverhead Books. Ruse, Michael (1993). Evolution and progress. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 8: 55–59. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(93)90159-M Sandberg, Anders (2018). Human extinction from natural hazard events. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Natural Hazard Science. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199389407.013.293 Shanahan, Timothy (2012). Evolutionary progress: Conceptual issues. In eLS. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd: Chichester. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0003459.pub2 Shapiro, Lucy and Richard Losick (2013). François Jacob (1920–2013). Science 340: 939. doi:10.1126/science.1239975 Simon, Herbert (1986). Rationality in psychology and economics. Journal of Business 59: S209–S224. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/2352757 van der Plicht, Johannes, V. I. Molodin, Yaroslav V. Kuzmin, S. K. Vasiliev, A. V. Postnov, and V. S. Slavinsky (2015). New Holocene refugia of giant deer (Megaloceros giganteus Blum.) in Siberia: Updated extinction patterns" Quaternary Science Reviews 114: 182–188. doi:10.1016/j.quascirev.2015.02.013 Varela, Sara, Matheus Souza LimaRibeiro, José Alexandre Felizola DinizFilho, and David Storch (2015). Differential effects of temperature change and human impact on European Late Quaternary mammalian extinctions. Global Change Biology 21: 1475–1481. doi:10.1111/gcb.12763 Yarnold, David (2019). “Birds Are Telling Us It’s Time to Take Action on Climate.” Audubon Magazine, Fall 2019 climate issue. Retrieved from: https://www.audubon. org/magazine/fall-2019/birds-are-telling-us-its-time-take-action-climate Notes 1 As Richard Passingham has explained in Cognitive Neuroscience: A Very Short Introduction (Oxford, 2016): “Now that we know so much about where there is activity in the brain while people perform cognitive tasks, the next stage is to find out how that activity makes cognition happen. In other words we need to understand the mechanisms” (106–107). See also: Satel and Lilienfeld (2013). 2 Folk models of the mind are not always two-dimensional. Although what philosophers call “mind-body dualism” (Chambliss 2018) has long been popular, wisdom has also long maintained that the brain has three different ways of dealing with the world conventionally labeled as “thoughts,” “habits,” and “emotions.” 1 As Richard Passingham has explained in Cognitive Neuroscience: A Very Short Introduction (Oxford, 2016): “Now that we know so much about where there is activity in the brain while people perform cognitive tasks, the next stage is to find out how that activity makes cognition happen. In other words we need to understand the mechanisms” (106–107). See also: Satel and Lilienfeld (2013). 2 Folk models of the mind are not always two-dimensional. Although what philosophers call “mind-body dualism” (Chambliss 2018) has long been popular, wisdom has also long maintained that the brain has three different ways of dealing with the world conventionally labeled as “thoughts,” “habits,” and “emotions.” 1 A less charitable view of the trio of characters in our three-dimensional portrayal of the ways in which the human brain deals with the world: Sherlock wants to learn the truth about what is outside their yellow submarine; George’s orientation is toward personal convenience; and Alice, as young and charming as she undoubtedly is, aims at controlling the outer world not only in ways that are creative, but as we shall be arguing more fully later, also more predictable. It is not Sherlock or George who is the chief architect of our species ability to dumb down the world to fit our needs, wants, and desires. It is Alice. 1 Sherlock Holmes to John H. Watson in A Study in Scarlet by A. Conan Doyle, 1887, Chapter 2, “The science of deduction”: “You see,” he explained, “I consider that a man’s brain originally is like a little empty attic, and you have to stock it with such furniture as you choose. A fool takes in all the lumber of every sort that he comes across, so that the knowledge which might be useful to him gets crowded out, or at best is jumbled up with a lot of other things so that he has a difficulty in laying his hands upon it. Now the skillful workman is very careful indeed as to what he takes into his brain-attic. He will have nothing but the tools which may help him in doing his work, but of these he has a large assortment, and all in the most perfect order. It is a mistake to think that that little room has elastic walls and can distend to any extent. Depend upon it there comes a time when for every addition of knowledge you forget something that you knew before. It is of the highest importance, therefore, not to have useless facts elbowing out the useful ones.” 1 For an alternative way of saying this simple mantra, see: Gross (2014, fig. 1.2). We prefer our way of saying it because Gross’s “modal model of emotion” assumes that the brain is constantly assessing the situations it is dealing with in light of “relevant goals,” a core assumption that we feel begs the issue of how rational is habitual behavior. It would not be an exaggeration to say much of psychology today still accepts the Enlightenment fallacy that we are goal-oriented, rational animals. 2 Current thinking in neuroscience and the cognitive sciences evidently favors using the word prediction rather than recognition (e.g., Hohwy 2018). Our own understanding of how the brain interacts with the world and responds is closer to that of Anthony Chemero (2003), Chemero and Silberstein (2008), Lane Beckes and others 3 4 1 2 1 2 3 1 2 1 (2015), Gantman and Van Bavel (2016), Hawkins et al. (2017), and Henderson and Hayes (2017). For an example of what we mean here by “recognition,” see Barrett (2017, 25–26, 308). Barrett herself favors the idea that the brain is constantly and rapidly making predictions (59–60). For one possible model of how working memory plays a key role in perception, see Ding et al. (2017). We think it is important to add here that how someone reacts to what their senses are telling them need not be obvious to anyone watching them. For example, a recent study of how your eyes recognize what they are seeing supports the proposition that visualization is basically a neural reaction to what your eyes are showing you. Thus our guiding mantra could be restated as SENSE — RECOGNIZE — VISUALIZE. In short, what we “see” is something the brain reassembles for us out of our memories of prior experiences (perhaps from only a few milliseconds ago) as a response to a surprisingly small number of current visual cues. For further discussion, see Ponce et al. (2019); also Frankland et al. (2019). Currently, scientists are using computers to simulate how the brain accomplishes such work (Ananthaswamy 2017; Sanborn and Chater 2016). While we will not explore the likelihood here, instead of thinking the human brain does elaborate computer-like data analysis and probability calculation, a better model for what the brain is actually doing might be information science (Fleissner and Hof kirchner 1996; Frankland et al. 2019). It is hard not to conclude, somewhat tongue-in-cheek, that Dehaene would have us replace what has long been called the “homunculus fallacy” (Adolphs and Anderson 2018, 11–13)—the notion that each of us as a small “mini-me” in residence inside our skull—with a veritable corporate think tank of specialized computer programmers. Notably, Herbert Simon, awarded in 1978; Daniel Kahneman 2002; and Richard Thaler 2017. It is also evident that the habitual learned responses associated with George (System 1) can be prompted not only by input from Sherlock (System 2), but also from Alice without any obvious external provocation (Buckner and DiNicola 2019). For a broader perspective on mob violence versus the rule of law in the early years of twentieth century America, see Capozzola (2002). For a more recent example of mob violence, see Swenson (2017). Although we will not try to explore the issue here, an analogous problem in mathematics is captured by the fact that for a number to be computable, it is first necessary to define what is to be the finite number of symbols completing the calculation (Bernhardt 2016, 141–142). 1 It would not be farfetched to say also that like evolution, Alice also works like a tinkerer to come up with new ideas, ways of doing things, and the like.