Received: 10 July 2019
|
Accepted: 21 May 2020
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2694
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Subjective status and perceived legitimacy across countries
Mark J. Brandt1
| Toon Kuppens2 | Russell Spears2 | Luca Andrighetto3 |
Frederique Autin4 | Peter Babincak5 | Constantina Badea6 | Jaechang Bae7 |
Anatolia Batruch8 | Julia C. Becker9 | Konrad Bocian10 | Bojana Bodroža11 |
David Bourguignon12 | Marcin Bukowski13 | Fabrizio Butera8 | Sarah E. Butler14 |
Xenia Chryssochoou15 | Paul Conway16 | Jarret T. Crawford17 | Jean-Claude Croizet18 |
Soledad de Lemus19 | Juliane Degner20 | Piotr Dragon13 | Federica Durante21 |
Matthew J. Easterbrook22 | Iniobong Essien23 | Joseph P. Forgas24 |
Roberto González25 | Sylvie Graf26 | Peter Halama27 | Gyuseog Han28 | Ryan
Y Hong29 | Petr Houdek30 | Eric R. Igou31 | Yoel Inbar32 | Jolanda Jetten33 |
William Jimenez Leal34 | Gloria Jiménez-Moya25 | Jaya Kumar Karunagharan35 |
Anna Kende36 | Maria Korzh37 | Simon M. Laham38 | Joris Lammers39 | Li Lim33 |
Antony S. R. Manstead40 | Janko Međedović41 | Zachary J. Melton42 | Matt Motyl43 |
Spyridoula Ntani15 | Chuma Kevin Owuamalam35 | Müjde Peker44 | Michael J. Platow45 |
J. P. Prims42 | Christine Reyna46 | Mark Rubin47 | Rim Saab48 | Sindhuja Sankaran49 |
Lee Shepherd50 | Chris G. Sibley51 | Agata Sobkow52 | Bram Spruyt53 |
Pernille Stroebaek54 | Nebi Sümer55 | Joseph Sweetman56 | Catia P. Teixeira2 |
Claudia Toma57 | Adrienn Ujhelyi36 | Jojanneke van der Toorn58 | Alain van Hiel59,60 |
Alejandro Vásquez-Echeverría61 | Alexandra Vazquez62 | Michelangelo Vianello63 |
Marek Vranka64 | Vincent Yzerbyt65 | Jennifer L. Zimmerman46
Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
1
2
University of Groningen
3
University of Genova
4
CeRCA, Université de Poitiers CNRS
5
University of Presov
6
Université Paris Nanterre
Gwangju Welfare Foundation
7
8
University of Lausanne
9
University of Osnabrueck
Sopot Faculty of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities
10
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia
11
12
University of Lorraine
13
Jagiellonian University
14
College of DuPage
15
Panteion University of Social and Political Sciences
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2020 The Authors. European Journal of Social Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Soc Psychol. 2020;50:921–942.
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ejsp
|
921
922
|
Florida State University
16
17
The College of New Jersey
18
Université Clermont-Auvergne et CNRS
19
University of Granada
Hamburg University
20
University of Milano-Bicocca
21
22
University of Sussex
23
FernUniversität in Hagen
University of New South Wales
24
25
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
26
Czech Academy of Sciences
27
Slovak Academy of Sciences
28
Chonnam National University
29
National University of Singapore
University of Economics in Prague
30
University of Limerick
31
University of Toronto
32
University of Queensland
33
Universidad de los Andes
34
35
The University of Nottingham Malaysia Campus
36
ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
Ural State Law University
37
University of Melbourne
38
University of Cologne
39
Cardiff University
40
Institute of Criminological and Sociological Research, Belgrade, Serbia
41
University of Illinois, Chicago, IL, USA
42
43
Civil Politics
44
MEF University
The Australian National University
45
DePaul University
46
The University of Newcastle, Australia
47
American University of Beirut
48
University of Warsaw
49
Northumbria University
50
University of Auckland
51
52
Wroclaw Faculty of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities
Vrije Universiteit Brussel
53
University of Copenhagen
54
55
Sabanci University
56
University of Exeter
57
Universite libre de Bruxelles
58
Utrecht University
59
Leiden University
60
University of Gent
Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay
61
62
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia
63
University of Padova
64
Charles University
65
Université catholique de Louvain
BRANDT eT Al.
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
Correspondence
Mark J. Brandt, Tilburg University, Tilburg,
The Netherlands.
Email: M.J.Brandt@tilburguniversity.edu
Funding information
Center for Intercultural and Indigenous
Research, Grant/Award Number: 15110006;
H2020 European Research Council, Grant/
Award Number: 759320; Center for Social
Conflict and Cohesion Studies, Grant/
Award Number: 15130009; Fondo Nacional
de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico,
Grant/Award Number: 1161371; Spanish
Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness,
Grant/Award Number: PSI2016-79971-P;
Czech Science Foundation, Grant/
Award Number: 20-01214S; Institute of
Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences,
Grant/Award Number: RVO: 68081740
923
Abstract
The relationships between subjective status and perceived legitimacy are important
for understanding the extent to which people with low status are complicit in their
oppression. We use novel data from 66 samples and 30 countries (N = 12,788) and
find that people with higher status see the social system as more legitimate than
those with lower status, but there is variation across people and countries. The association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy was never negative at any
levels of eight moderator variables, although the positive association was sometimes
reduced. Although not always consistent with hypotheses, group identification, selfesteem, and beliefs in social mobility were all associated with perceived legitimacy
among people who have low subjective status. These findings enrich our understanding of the relationship between social status and legitimacy.
KEYWORDS
legitimacy, social identity, status, system justification
1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N
consistent with one another; however, for people with low social
status who are disadvantaged by the social system, seeing the self
Scholars across the social sciences have debated whether or not peo-
and one's group as positive conflicts with the motivation to see the
ple with low social status are complicit in their oppression. On the one
social system as positive. To resolve this psychological conflict peo-
hand, unequal social systems tend to be relatively stable across time,
ple with low social status may, under some conditions, legitimize the
suggesting some degree of passivity, if not complicity among the pop-
social system more than people with high social status.
ulace; however, on the other hand, social movements aimed at dis-
Scholars have debated the status-legitimacy hypothesis, testing
rupting or altering social systems are often spearheaded by the very
the hypothesis several times with mixed results (find support: e.g.,
people disaffected by existing status arrangements. The status-legiti-
Henry & Saul, 2006; Li, Yang, Wu, & Kou, 2020; Sengupta, Osborne, &
macy hypothesis (as coined by Brandt, 2013; Jost, Banaji, &
Sibley, 2015; see Jost, 2017 for a recent summary of relevant work; do
Nosek, 2004; Jost & Hunyady, 2003; Jost, Pelham, Sheldon, & Ni
not find support: e.g., Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi-
Sullivan, 2003) predicts that people with lower status will, at least in
Cioldi, 2012; Vargas-Salfate, Paez, Liu, Pratto, & Gil de Zúñiga, 2018;
some conditions, be more likely to see the social system as legitimate
Zimmerman & Reyna, 2013). This work suggests that there is still
than people with higher status.1 In this article, we examine the condi-
broad scholarly interest in how status and legitimacy are related. We
tions under which this hypothesis finds support. We used a novel,
aim to add two things to this literature. With Approach 1, we test po-
30-country study on the association between subjective status and
tential individual- and societal-level moderators of the associations
perceived legitimacy. We had two aims. First, we aimed to understand
between different levels of subjective status and perceived legitimacy
whether and when people with lower subjective status perceive
(i.e., a relative focus). Under what conditions and among people with
higher levels of legitimacy than people with higher subjective status.
what kinds of perceptions and psychological characteristics is there
Second, we aimed to understand what types of factors are associated
evidence for the status-legitimacy hypothesis (cf. McGuire, 2013)?
with perceived legitimacy for people with low subjective status.
With Approach 2, we focus on people with low levels of social status and tested potential predictors of perceived legitimacy among
1.1 | Status-legitimacy hypothesis
The logic behind the status-legitimacy hypothesis is that people have
motivations to see themselves, their groups, and their larger social
systems in a positive light (for full details see Jost et al., 2003). For
people with high social status, all of these motivations are generally
1
A related hypothesis predicts that lower power will be associated with higher levels of
perceived legitimacy (van der Toorn et al., 2015). Our data also test this hypothesis;
however, to sharpen the focus of the manuscript at the request of reviewers we have
moved the power related analyses and discussion to supplemental materials.
this group. Unlike the first approach, Approach 2 does not focus on
whether the predictors differ from people with high levels of status.
1.2 | Approach 1: Testing
moderators of the association between subjective
status and perceived legitimacy
The status-legitimacy hypothesis follows from the idea that people are addressing the threatening feelings of aversive anxiety and
arousal resulting from psychological conflict. People alter their
924
|
BRANDT eT Al.
behaviors, beliefs, and perceptions in order to address this threat,
cognitions that could increase dissonance. The dissonance that
and when the feelings of threat are assuaged they are less likely to
people experience from being in a low status position might
alter their behaviors, beliefs, and perceptions (for reviews see Jonas
emerge “from the contradictory cognitions that (a) the system
et al., 2014; Proulx, Inzlicht, & Harmon-Jones, 2012). Factors that
is putting me (and my group) at a disadvantage, and (b) through
make social hierarchy appear less consequential in material or psy-
our acquiescence, my group and I are contributing to the stability
chological terms (we discuss several factors below) should reduce
of the system” (Jost et al., 2003, p. 16). Therefore, people who
the effects of low subjective status on the anxious arousal that
recognize that they are not doing enough to mitigate inequality
results from status-based dissonance. Therefore, this reasoning
should experience more dissonance. This recognition should in-
suggests that when people's feelings of threat are addressed, the
crease perceived legitimacy for people who are low status and is
status-legitimacy link will be positive (i.e., low status people will be
part of the contradictory cognitions originally predicted to cause
less inclined to legitimize the system as a way to assuage feelings of
lower status people to see the system as more legitimate than
threat). Conversely, when people's anxious arousal is exacerbated,
higher status people do.
the status-legitimacy link will be negative (i.e., low status people will
see the system as more legitimate).
The amount of civil liberties, meritocratic culture, and inequality within a country could also affect feelings of dissonance. Countries and contexts with more civil liberties and more
1.2.1 | Moderators that reduce threat
meritocratic cultures increase the amount of dissonance people low in status experience by implying they have choice and
control over their outcomes (Jost et al., 2003, p. 17). Inequality
There are potentially many factors that can mitigate anxious
may increase the conflict between self/group motivations and
arousal. We focus on factors that have appeared in the system
acceptance of the system for low status group members (e.g.,
justification and social identity literatures because these litera-
Henry & Saul, p. 376). It may also create the impression that
tures have been the focus of debates on this issue (for a recent
people have less control over their place within society. Both
example see Jost, 2019; Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2019). The
factors should increase the amount of anxious uncertainty
most obvious factor is the identification with and salience of a val-
and increase the necessity of rationalizing the system. No
ued group, which is one way social identity theorists suggest peo-
support was found for these predictions in some prior studies
ple address anxious arousal (Hogg, 2014) or cope with the threat
(Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi-Cioldi, 2012;
of having a low status (McMahon & Watts, 2002; Tajfel &
Trump & White, 2018; for one exception on one measure see
Turner, 1979). Consistent with this, the original paper proposing
Vargas-Salfate et al., 2018).
the status-legitimacy hypothesis argued that if group interests are
salient and accessible (something that is likely to be correlated
with identification), then support for the status-legitimacy hy-
1.2.3 | Structural moderators that affect threat
pothesis is unlikely to emerge (Jost et al., 2003, 2004). Similarly,
people with high self-esteem and positive self-views make plans,
Structural factors, including levels of inequality or the stability
have high levels of personal agency, and high levels of self-cer-
of the hierarchy, influence what types of options people per-
tainty (e.g., Campbell, 1990; Harter, 1978), suggesting that they
ceive that they have and their place within the society. These
are less likely to be psychologically affected by uncontrollability
perceptions may affect people's perceptions of the whole sys-
and similar types of threats associated with low status (see Laurin,
tem. For example, to the extent that inequality exacerbates
Kay, & Landau, 2018 or Schoel, Bluemke, Mueller, & Stahlberg,
feelings of hierarchy and the threatening feelings of low status,
2011 for a similar argument in different domains). This is consist-
as well as making status differences more salient, this should
ent with the idea that self-esteem should push against system jus-
further motivate challenges to the inequality by the low status
tification motivations for low status people (cf. Jost, Gaucher, &
group (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). This is the opposite of the inequal-
Stern, 2015, p. 330). 2
ity prediction above. When looking at the stability of the hierarchy, one of the traditional predictions of social identity theory
1.2.2 | Structural moderators that increase threat
through dissonance
is that low status group members are less likely to seek social
change when the status hierarchy is perceived as stable, compared to when the hierarchy is perceived as unstable (Ellemers,
van Knippenberg, & Wilke, 1990; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). That
Other factors can exacerbate feelings of anxious arousal by in-
is, when the status hierarchy is stable, there is little scope and
creasing the amount of dissonance people experience. The
hope for social change (i.e., no cognitive alternative to the sta-
most direct prediction comes from assessing the contradictory
tus quo), and people are more likely to accept the system as
2
Social identity theory and system justification theory have also both used self-esteem
as an outcome variable. Although this is interesting, it is not the focus of our
investigation.
legitimate. This hypothesis is also consistent with work finding
that system stability increases a system justification motivation
(Laurin, Gaucher, & Kay, 2013).
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
TA B L E 1 Summary of the moderator
hypotheses tested in this investigation
(Approach 1)
Variable
925
Moderator hypothesis
Reduce threat
Group Identification
Identification-Moderation Hypothesis: The link between status and
legitimacy will be negative for people low in group identification
and positive for people high in group identification.
Self-Esteem
Self-Esteem-Moderation Hypothesis: The link between status and
legitimacy will be negative for people low in self-esteem and
positive for people high in self-esteem.
Increase threat through dissonance
Inequality contribution
Inequality Contribution-Moderation Hypothesis: The link between
status and legitimacy will be negative for people who see
themselves as contributing to inequality and positive for people
who do not see themselves as contributing to inequality.
Civil Liberties
Civil Liberties Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy
will be negative in countries with high levels of civil liberties and
positive for people in countries with low levels of civil liberties.
Meritocracy
Meritocracy Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will
be negative in countries with meritocratic cultures and positive for
people in countries with less meritocratic cultures.
Inequality
System Justification Theory (SJT) Inequality Hypothesis: The link
between status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with
high levels of inequality and positive for people in countries with
low levels of inequality.
Structural Factors That Affect Threat
TA B L E 2 Summary of the predictor
hypotheses tested in this investigation
(Approach 2)
Inequality
Social Identity Theory (SIT) Inequality Hypothesis: The link between
status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with low levels
of inequality and positive for people in countries with high levels of
inequality.
Stability
Stability-Moderator Hypothesis: The link between status and
legitimacy will be negative when people see the status hierarchy
as stable and positive when people see the status hierarchy as
unstable.
Variable
Predictor hypothesis
Social mobility
Social Mobility-Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with
low status who see the system as having high social
mobility will be more likely to see the system as
legitimate.
Stability
Stability-Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low status
who see the status hierarchy as stable will be more
likely to see the system as legitimate.
Identification
Identification-Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low
status who have high group identification will be less
likely to see the system as legitimate.
Self-esteem
Self-Esteem Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low
status who have high self-esteem will be less likely to
see the system as legitimate.
1.2.4 | Summary
We have identified several potential moderators that might help us
1.3 | Approach 2: Finding the predictors of
perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of
subjective status
predict when we are more or less likely to find support for the status-legitimacy hypothesis. These moderators are expressed as indi-
In addition to searching for moderators, we also ask what pre-
vidual hypotheses in Table 1. In the cross-national study that follows,
dicts perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of social
we test these eight hypotheses.
status. Put another way, assuming that people with low levels of
926
|
BRANDT eT Al.
subjective status vary in the extent to which they see the system
allows them to eventually achieve social change as a group (e.g., due
as legitimate, what predicts this variation? In contrast to the prior
to longer-term status instability; Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2016,
section, this line of questioning does not necessarily imply mod-
2019; Owuamalam, Rubin, Spears, & Weerabangsa, 2017; for a
eration effects, as the predictors of perceived legitimacy for lower
countervailing view see Jost, 2019). In short, using the system rather
status groups could be the same as the predictors of perceived
than rejecting it, can be seen as a viable vehicle for group interests,
legitimacy for higher status groups (moderation is possible, but
especially for high identifiers. This idea can also be tested with our
not necessary). This approach moves away from the explicit status
data and essentially predicts the opposite of the predictions in the
comparisons of the status-legitimacy hypothesis and is consistent
prior paragraph.
with the broader questions that inspired this hypothesis: Do people see systems that oppress them as just and legitimate, and if so
under what conditions? We focus on four different variables that
1.3.1 | Summary
might predict perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of
social status (see Table 2).
We identified several potential predictors of perceived legitimacy
Some work suggests that people desire to live in social systems
for people with lower social status that might help us understand
in which there is upward mobility and the system is relatively stable
the reasons some people with low social status perceive the social
(Laurin et al., 2013; Martorana, Galinsky, & Rao, 2005). Consistent
system as legitimate. Some predictors indicate that the system is ful-
with this, people who perceived no possibilities to move up to a higher
filling the person's goals and other predictors indicate that personal
status group are less likely to justify the system and more likely to
and group goals are prioritized. These hypotheses are specified indi-
engage in collective action (Day & Fiske, 2017; Ellemers et al., 1990;
vidually in Table 2. In the cross-national study that follows, we test
Mandisodza, Jost, & Unzueta, 2006; Tajfel, 1981; Wright, Taylor, &
these four hypotheses.
Moghaddam, 1990). Similarly, systems with more stable social hierarchies are seen as more legitimate (Laurin et al., 2013) and are less
likely to trigger efforts to change the system (Bettencourt, Charlton,
1.4 | An international, multi-lab approach
Dorr, & Hume, 2001; Ellemers et al., 1990; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). This
suggests that when people see the system as providing social mobil-
We conducted a cross-country study on the association between
ity and stability, they are more likely to see the system as legitimate.
subjective status and perceived legitimacy that allowed us to test
When people's group- and self-interests are prioritized, it
for moderators of the status-legitimacy relationships, as well as to
may be less likely that people perceive the system as legitimate
understand what predicts perceived legitimacy for people with
if they or their group are not benefitting from that system. That
lower levels of subjective status. 3 We consider three types of
is, higher levels of group identification and self-esteem may both
effects:
be associated with lower levels of perceived system legitimacy
To assess moderators (Approach 1):
among people with low social status. This follows from the idea
that group- and self-interest motivations are negatively related
1. We test the interactions between status and the
to system-related motivations among low status groups (Jost
proposed moderator variables. If the interaction is
et al., 2004). It is also consistent with the idea that the effects of
significant, we test whether the effect of subjective
group- and self-interests—when sufficiently strong—may be more
status is negative (consistent with the status-legiti-
prominent than the effects of system-interests (Jost et al., 2003,
macy hypothesis) at the predicted levels of the mod-
2004, 2011) and with the finding that people with low levels of
erator variables (e.g., when identification is low).
group identification may be more likely to accept the current situation (Rubin & Hewstone, 2004; Spears, Jetten, & Doosje, 2001).
According to these ideas, people with lower subjective status who
The assessment of predictors of perceived legitimacy among people with low status will take two steps (Approach 2):
have high group identification or who have high self-esteem will
be less likely to see the system as legitimate.
From our reading of the literature, the predictions in the prior
2a. We examine the main effects of the predictors on
perceived legitimacy. Because the hypotheses about
paragraph seem most consistent with a straightforward extension
of social identity theory's work on group identification to research
on perceived legitimacy. However, it is important to note that recent
work by some scholars (and co-authors of this article) has predicted
the opposite, at least as a function of an additional qualifier. A series
of papers by Owuamalam and colleagues have argued that high group
identifiers and people under conditions of high group salience may
be likely to see the system as legitimate to the extent that it is seen as
one that affords the group collective social mobility in the long term and
3
Recently, system justification theorists have proposed that a low sense of power, rather
than status, is associated with greater perceived legitimacy. Whereas status indicates the
amount of prestige and respect a person or group is accorded in the system, power
indicates the amount of control a person has over valued resources (Magee &
Galinsky, 2008). Trusting and legitimizing outside sources of control (e.g., governments)
can help restore people's sense of control, something that is lacking with low feelings of
power (van der Toorn et al., 2015; cf. Friesen et al., 2014; Kay et al., 2008). By perceiving
the system as legitimate, people with low feelings of power can regain some feelings of
control. We therefore simultaneously tested all hypotheses for interpersonal sense of
power. Complete results are in the supplemental materials.
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
927
predictors of perceived legitimacy are not specific to
a list of all measures and manipulations. Analyses include partici-
people with lower levels of subjective status (e.g., social
pants who are over 18 and have completed at least the subjective
mobility could predict perceived legitimacy for people
status and the system justification, trust in government, confidence
with both high and low status), this main effect analysis
in societal institutions, and legitimacy of the status hierarchy mea-
tells us if there is an average effect across the sample.
sures. The final sample included 12,788 participants (4,252 men,
8,478 women, 58 with missing responses, Mage = 25.3, SDage = 10.7).
2b. When there is a significant interaction between
the predictor and status, we examine whether the
predictor still has the predicted significant effect
2.2 | Key predictor variable: subjective status
(e.g., a positive effect of social mobility) for people
with lower levels of status. This will tell us whether
To measure subjective social status, we used the MacArthur Scale of
the effect of the predictor is specific to people with
Subjective Social Status (Adler, Epel, Castellazzo, & Ickovics, 2000),
low subjective status.
modified to capture people's sense of status within their country.
We chose this measure to allow easier comparisons across countries.
Participants were asked to rate themselves on a ladder that ranged
2 | M E TH O D
from 1 to 10, where 10 was high status and 1 was low status. The
instructions for the measure read as follows:
2.1 | Participants and procedure
Think of this ladder as representing where people
Sixty-six distinct samples were collected by researchers from 30 coun-
who are the best off—those who have the most money,
tries. We aimed for 150 participants per sample, so that we had at
the most education, and the most respected jobs. At
least 150 participants per country to give us approximately 80%
the bottom are the people who are the worst off—who
stand in [country]. At the top of the ladder are people
power to detect a small to medium effect (i.e., r = .22) within each
have the least money, least education, and the least re-
sample. Data collection was not continued after analysis. To ensure
spected jobs or no job. The higher up you are on this
that respondents were part of the social and political system, we only
ladder, the closer you are to the people at the very
included participants who indicated they were either born in the coun-
top; the lower you are, the closer you are to the people
try or had lived in the country for six or more years and so not every
at the very bottom. Please choose the number of the
sample resulted in 150 participants.4 All exclusions are reported.
rung of the ladder where you think you stand at this
Countries, samples, type of sample, proportion of men, mean age, and
time in your life, relatively to other people in [country].
sample size are presented in Table 3. Samples included a mix of student samples, community samples, and samples with both students
This one-item measure had adequate test-retest reliability in prior
and members of the community (the latter two were both considered
samples (e.g., Operario, Adler, & Williams, 2004) and is correlated
non-student samples for the sake of analyses). Samples are primarily
with objective measures of status (e.g., income; Goodman et al., 2003;
samples of university students, although some include community
Sakurai, Kawakami, Yamaoka, Ishikawa, & Hashimoto, 2010). Responses
samples from Mechanical Turk (USA2), community email lists (e.g.,
in our sample were above the midpoint (M = 6.03, SD = 1.53), but spanned
USA6), or a representative sample (NLD3). Multiple samples per coun-
the entire range of the measure. See supplemental materials for means
try helps guard against the possibility that results for any particular
across countries on this and all other individual-level variables.
country are dependent on one particular sample. Research was conducted in accordance with APA and national guidelines. Data, code,
and materials are available at the following link: https://osf.io/5uxc7/.
2.3 | Key outcome variable: perceived legitimacy
Participating labs used materials designed by the first three authors. These labs translated the materials and adjusted them when
We included four measures of perceived legitimacy and system justifi-
necessary for their language and cultural context (e.g., replacing
cation. Multiple research groups studying the status-legitimacy hypoth-
“United States” in the system justification measure). Participants
esis have used all these measures (e.g., Brandt, 2013; Henry &
first completed measures of demographics, including the measure
Saul, 2006; Jost et al., 2003; Li et al., 2020). The 8-item general system
of subjective social status. They then completed measures about
justification scale (Kay & Jost, 2003) includes items like “In general, I find
either their perceptions of themselves and their group's position in
society to be fair” and “Society is set up so that people usually get what
society (the moderators and predictors) or their perceptions of sys-
they deserve” (M α = .78, SD α = .06, α range [.66, .85]; Scale M = −0.64;
tem legitimacy (the outcomes). The supplemental materials include
Scale SD = 1.14; −3 = Disagree strongly to +3 = Agree strongly).5
4
Six or more years was chosen to ensure that participants who were international or
exchange students were not included in the sample.
5
For each scale, alpha or the correlation coefficient (for 2-item scales) was calculated in
each country separately. Mean, standard deviation, and range of alphas and correlation
coefficients across countries are reported in the text.
928
|
TA B L E 3
Sample
BRANDT eT Al.
Sample demographic information. Sorted in alphabetical order by country
Country
Type
Proportion
Male
M age
N
AUS1
Australia
Student
0.27
21.4
209
AUS2
Australia
Student
0.34
19.5
114
AUS3
Australia
Student
0.34
19.2
80
AUS4
Australia
Student
0.29
20.1
163
AUS5
Australia
Student
0.32
23.2
222
BEL1
Belgium
Student
0.35
20.0
623
BEL2
Belgium
Student
0.54
21.7
137
BEL3
Belgium
Student
0.28
19.3
194
BEL4
Belgium
Student
0.24
21.9
91
CAN1
Canada
Student
0.33
18.7
180
CHL1
Chile
Student
0.28
20.9
156
COL1
Colombia
Student
0.27
19.9
139
CZE1
Czech Republic
Non-Student
0.23
27.5
154
CZE2
Czech Republic
Non-Student
0.29
23.7
241
CZE3
Czech Republic
Non-Student
0.30
24.8
311
DNK1
Denmark
Student
0.16
24.0
162
FRA1
France
Student
0.31
21.2
303
FRA2
France
Student
0.11
20.8
163
FRA3
France
Student
0.46
18.6
180
DEU1
Germany
Student
0.36
25.3
50
DEU2
Germany
Student
0.18
22.6
133
DEU3
Germany
Student
0.22
22.7
151
DEU4
Germany
Non-Student
0.81
37.2
89
GBR1
Great Britain
Student
0.14
19.8
213
GBR2
Great Britain
Student
0.07
19.1
138
GBR3
Great Britain
Student
0.18
19.6
169
GBR4
Great Britain
Student
0.08
19.1
118
GRC1
Greece
Non-Student
0.49
35.9
444
HUN1
Hungary
Student
0.22
20.2
144
IND1
India
Non-Student
0.74
31.2
449
IRL1
Ireland
Student
0.46
24.6
145
ITA1
Italy
Student
0.47
43.0
103
ITA2
Italy
Student
0.47
44.0
103
ITA3
Italy
Student
0.05
26.9
109
LBN1
Lebanon
Student
0.51
18.9
204
MYS1
Malaysia
Student
0.43
20.8
146
MYS2
Malaysia
Non-Student
0.44
24.6
63
NLD1
Netherlands
Student
0.26
19.8
184
NLD2
Netherlands
Student
0.19
20.0
232
NLD3
Netherlands
Non-Student
0.49
40.4
766
NLD4
Netherlands
Student
0.20
21.2
176
NZL1
New Zealand
Student
0.18
21.0
180
POL1
Poland
Student
0.16
28.0
214
POL2
Poland
Non-Student
0.20
23.2
160
(Continues)
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
TA B L E 3
929
(Continued)
Sample
Country
Type
Proportion
Male
M age
N
POL3
Poland
Non-Student
0.65
26.4
166
RUS1
Russia
Student
0.34
19.4
117
SRB1
Serbia
Student
0.24
20.3
159
SRB2
Serbia
Non-Student
0.39
30.7
173
SGP1
Singapore
Student
0.27
19.9
196
SVK1
Slovakia
Student
0.19
22.6
268
SVK2
Slovakia
Non-Student
0.49
46.7
166
KOR1
South Korea
Student
0.30
20.6
119
ESP1
Spain
Student
0.24
23.1
148
ESP2
Spain
Student
0.30
32.7
252
CHE1
Switzerland
Student
0.51
20.3
131
TUR1
Turkey
Student
0.30
20.5
122
TUR2
Turkey
Student
0.23
20.0
99
USA1
United States
Student
0.20
20.6
224
USA2
United States
Non-Student
0.57
35.1
214
USA3
United States
Student
0.42
19.8
316
USA4
United States
Student
0.27
19.9
195
USA5
United States
Student
0.17
19.4
181
USA6
United States
Non-Student
0.23
36.1
368
USA7
United States
Non-Student
0.48
41.5
116
URY1
Uruguay
Student
0.28
22.2
169
URY2
Uruguay
Student
0.21
20.6
184
The 4-item trust in government scale often included in the
Finally, a 2-item measure of the perceived legitimacy of the status
American National Election Studies (2015; cf. Brandt, 2013) in-
hierarchy was used to capture perceived legitimacy in this specific
cludes items like “How much of the time do you think you can trust
domain (based on Mummendey, Kessler, Klink, & Mielke, 1999). The
the government to do what is right?” (1 = None of the time, 2 = Some
items were prefaced with “Differences in power and status between
of the time, 3 = Most of the time, 4 = Just about always) and “Would
groups in [country] are...” and then participants rated the stems “…
you say the government is pretty much run by a few big interests
illegitimate” and “…unfair” on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree
looking out for themselves or that it is run for the benefit of all the
strongly to 3 = Agree strongly (M r = .52, SD r = .14, r range [.33, .80];
people?” (1 = Few big interests, 2 = Benefit of all). Items were first
Scale M = −0.83; Scale SD = 1.30). These items were measured in
standardized using z-scores and then averaged to form a scale (M
the same block of questions as the stability of the status hierarchy
α = .61, SD α = .17, α range [−.10, .78,]; Scale M = −0.02; Scale
measure (see below).
SD = 0.75).
6
The 7-item measure assessing confidence in societal institutions adopted from the General Social Survey (2017), and used
in Brandt (2013), was used to tap into perceptions of both governmental and economic systems. For each of seven institutions,
participants are asked how much confidence they have in them
2.4 | Individual level measures of
moderators and predictors
2.4.1 | Group identification
(0 = None at all, 3 = A great deal). Institutions include the armed
forces, the police, the courts, the governments of the country,
We measured group identification with a three-item scale about
congress, major companies, and banks and financial institutions
participants’ identification with their social class. The scale was
(M α = .77, SD α = .04, α range [.63, .83,]; Scale M = 1.35; Scale
prefaced with, “The following questions are about people with a
SD = 0.55).
similar background and social class as yourself. Social class refers
to people with similar opportunities in terms of income, education,
6
The negative α (−.06) for the trust in government scale is found in Russia. The next
lowest α is .20 in Lebanon.
and social standing, as in the ladder measure that you completed
earlier.” The items were “I identify with people from my social
930
|
BRANDT eT Al.
class”, “I feel solidarity with my social class”, and “My social class
groups in [country] are...” and then participants rated the stems “…
is an important part of how I see myself”. Participants responded
difficult to change” and “…will remain stable over time” on a scale
to these items on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to
ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly (M r = .31,
3 = Agree strongly (M α = .67, SD α = .10, α range [.38, .78]; Scale
SD r = .14, r range [−.06, .76]; Scale M = 0.82; Scale SD = 1.14).7
M = 0.51; Scale SD = 1.17).
2.4.2 | Self-esteem
2.5 | Societal level measures of moderators
We included three measures to assess societal level conditions.
We measured self-esteem with the validated single-item measure
To capture the amount of civil liberties, we used the 2015 Civil
(Robins, Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). This measure reads “I have
Liberties subscale of the Democracy index created by the Economist
high self-esteem” with answers that ranged from 0 = Not at all to
Intelligence Unit (2016). This index uses a combination of survey
6 = Very true of me (Item M = 3.50; Item SD = 1.57).
data and expert ratings to estimate how democratically free individual countries are. It has been used in prior work testing similar
2.4.3 | Social mobility
questions (Brandt, 2013).
To assess the extent to which the culture holds meritocratic values, we combined four items from international surveys. The first two
We measured perceptions of social mobility with six items. They
are the importance of ambition and hard work for getting ahead in
included, “In general, people can easily get ahead in society”, “In
life in the ISSP (2009). The third and fourth come from the sixth wave
general, people can climb the social ladder and be successful”,
of the World Values Survey (2016). The third asks participants to re-
“People with a similar background and social class to my own can
spond to an item ranging from 1 = “Competition is good. It stimulates
easily get ahead in society”, “It is easy for people with a similar
people to work hard and develop new ideas” to 10 = “Competition
background and social class to my own to climb the social ladder
is harmful. It brings out the worst in people”. The fourth asks partic-
and be successful”, “I am motivated to climb up the social ladder”,
ipants to respond to an item ranging from 1 = “In the long run, hard
and “I am able to climb up the social ladder”. All items were meas-
work usually brings better life” to 10 = “Hard work doesn't generally
ure on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree
bring success—it's more a matter of luck and connections”. All four
strongly and had good reliability (M α = .72, SD α = .06, α range [.52,
items were standardized, the third and fourth items were reverse
.81]; Scale M = 0.82; Scale SD = 0.93).
scored, and all four items were combined to form a scale (α = .82).
To measure the objective inequality, we used the Gini index
2.4.4 | Contribute to inequality
from 2013 and obtained from the World Bank (World Bank, 2016).
For some countries, the 2013 data were not available, and in
these cases the most recent data were included instead. The Gini
We created five items to measure whether people felt that they
index assesses the amount of income inequality within a region,
contributed to inequality and the stability of the system. One item,
with higher scores indicating greater inequality. It is a common
“I could do more to change differences in power and status between
measure of inequality within a society (e.g., Oishi, Kesebir, &
groups in society”, did not correlate as expected with the other items
Diener, 2011).
and substantially reduced the reliability of the scale (α = .58), so we
omitted it. The remaining four items read “I contribute to keeping
society the way it is”, “I contribute to maintaining the current social
2.6 | Covariates
hierarchy”, “I don't do anything to change the current differences
in power and status in society”, and “I am not trying to change the
We included three covariates for analyses that focused on the
current differences in power and status in society”. All items were
individual level: self-reported age, gender (0 = women, 1 = men),
measured on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree
and type of sample (0 = student, 1 = non-student) to adjust for
strongly and the four remaining items created a reliable scale (M α =
potential background influences. For the models that include pre-
.71, SD α = .09, α range [.46, .84]; Scale M = −0.14; Scale SD = 1.12).
dictors at the societal level of analysis, we included two countrylevel covariates. We included the GDP per capita for each of the
2.4.5 | Stability of the status hierarchy
countries to control for overall wealth (World Bank, 2016). To
control for broader regional trends (Kuppens & Pollet, 2014), we
also included contrast codes for each of the continents repre-
We used a 2-item measure of the perceived stability of the status
sented in our data.
hierarchy. This was based on measures used in studies from a social identity perspective (Mummendey et al., 1999). The items
7
were prefaced with “Differences in power and status between
next lowest correlation is .18 in Canada.
The negative correlation (−.06) between the stability items is found in Malaysia. The
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
2.7 | A note on coding
931
highest and people with the lowest levels of subjective status. This
is in contrast to the prediction of the status-legitimacy hypothesis.
We coded all of the variables to range from 0 to 1. Multilevel regression
To test whether these results are impacted by covariates, we in-
coefficients are then the proportion difference in the outcome variable as
cluded age (country-mean-centered), gender (country-mean-centered),
one goes from 0 (minimum) to 1 (maximum) on the predictor variable. For
type of sample (grand mean-centered), and the type of perceived legiti-
example, a coefficient of .05 is a 5% difference in the outcome between
macy measure (contrast coded) as covariates, including the interactions
people scoring the lowest and the highest on the predictor variable.
between these contrast codes and status and sense of power.9 This gives
us the average effect of the primary predictors across the four measures
of perceived legitimacy and controls for mean differences between the
3 | R E S U LT S
measures. The estimate for subjective status is nearly identical to the
3.1 | Preliminary analyses: the effects of subjective
status on perceived legitimacy
estimate without covariates (b = .151, SE = 0.005, 95% CI [0.141, 0.161]).10
The effect of subjective status is not the same across all people
in all situations. We re-estimated the model with covariates and included random slopes at the country-level for subjective status and
We use multilevel models to account for participants’ nesting within coun-
sense of power. The estimated slope of subjective status on perceived
tries and within samples/labs. Models were estimated using lme4 (Bates,
legitimacy for each of the countries is in Figure 2. Although the effect
Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, &
of subjective status is always estimated to be positive when predict-
Christensen, 2016) packages in R (R Core Team, 2016). Because we are
ing perceived legitimacy, it varies in size. In the next section of the
interested in the individual-level association between subjective status
article, we consider moderators of the effects of subjective status.
and perceived legitimacy, we centered subjective status at the countrymean (Enders & Tofighi, 2007). The correlations between the predictor
variables and the measures of perceived legitimacy are in Figure 1 (created
with GGally, Schloerke et al., 2016). The measures of system justification,
3.2 | Approach 1: Moderators of subjective status
on perceived legitimacy
trust, confidence, and legitimacy of the status hierarchy were moderately
inter-correlated (M α = .70, SD α = .06, α range [.56, .82]). To combine items,
We tested individual-level moderators and country-level modera-
reduce the overall number of models, and facilitate generalization across
tors. Individual level moderators were country-mean-centered and
measures, we nested these four measures within participants, resulting in
included in separate models as fixed effects in the four-level multi-
a four-level multilevel model: legitimacy measures nested in persons,
level model used above (see Figure 3 for correlations between
nested in samples/labs, nested in countries. In short, this estimates the
these measures).11 We built the country-level models using the
average effect across multiple measures of system legitimacy that have
same four-level multilevel models, with the addition of the coun-
been used in the literature, while increasing measurement precision (see
try-level moderators and country-level covariates.12 All country-
for similar suggestions, Gelman, 2018; McShane, Tackett, Böckenholt, &
level variables were grand-mean-centered, with the exception of
Gelman, 2019). It is analogous to conducting four studies and estimating
the contrast codes for continents.13 In the country-level models,
the meta-analytic estimate across the four studies.
8
First, we tested the main effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy. This test conceptually replicates many prior
tests of these hypotheses (e.g., Brandt, 2013; Henry & Saul, 2006;
Jost et al., 2003). We regressed perceived legitimacy on country-mean-centered subjective status. We also included country-mean-centered sense of power in this analysis and all other
analyses because it was part of our original analysis plan (see
Appendix S1). People with higher subjective status are more likely to
see the social system as legitimate compared to people with lower
subjective status (b = .152, SE = 0.005, 95% CI [0.142, 0.162]). There
is a 15% difference in perceived legitimacy between people with the
We also conducted the analyses with only the system justification scale because this
scale is perhaps the most well-developed of the four scales and it was reliable in all of the
countries. Conclusions in the main text are essentially unchanged. There are two
8
exceptions. The negative interaction between stability and status was negative and
non-significant and the positive interaction between civil liberties and status was
positive and non-significant when only looking at the system justification scale.
Importantly, the sizes of these coefficients were very similar to the coefficients in the
models including all legitimacy measures, suggesting that the reduction in error variance
when using additional data is the reason for the different conclusions when using all
legitimacy measures or only the system justification scale.
9
We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our
model and sample size had to detect the effects of subjective status when b = .01, .02,
.05, and .10. These analyses showed that we had adequate power to detect effects of at
least .02 (i.e. a 2% difference in the outcome variable between the minimum and
maximum of the predictor variable; power ≈ 49%, 97%, 100%, 100% respectively).
10
The variance for the intercept at each level of the multilevel models that included covariates
was also calculated (Participants σ = .049, Labs σ = .006, Countries σ = .098, Residual σ = .18).
Descriptively, there is more variation between countries than there is between labs.
11
We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our model
and sample size had to detect individual-level interaction effects when b = .02, .05, .10, and
.20. We used the model with the identification moderator as our base model for these
analyses. These analyses showed that we had adequate power to detect moderation effects of
at least .10 (i.e., a .10 difference in the unstandardized slope of status/power at the minimum
and maximum of the moderator variable; power ≈12%, 46%, 98%, 100% respectively).
12
We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our
model and sample size had to detect country-level interaction effects for both subjective
status and sense of power when b = .01, .02, .05, .10, and .20 We used the model with
the meritocracy moderator as our base model for these analyses. These analyses showed
that we had adequate power to detect country-level moderation effects of at least .20
(power ≈6%, 10%, 45%, 87% respectively).
13
To check the robustness of our results to outliers at the country-level (cf. Ullrich, &
Schlüter, 2012), we visually inspected histograms of the country-level predictors. There
were clear outliers for both the Gini index and the measure of civil liberties. These
models were run both with and without outliers. All models are in the figures
summarizing results and the primary models discussed in the text include all data.
932
|
BRANDT eT Al.
F I G U R E 1 Correlations (above the diagonal) and density plots (below the diagonal) between subjective status, sense of power, and the
outcome variables. All variables were country-mean-centered before calculating the correlations. Density plots for each individual variable
are on the diagonal. Density plots are a variant of a histogram. The area below the diagonal uses density plots between two variables.
SJ = System Justification, Trust = Trust in Government, Confid = Confidence in Societal Institutions, Legit = Legitimacy of the status
hierarchy [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
we included random slopes for subjective status and sense of
power.
the effect across the range of the moderator variables, rather than
only focusing on points ±1 SD of the mean of the moderator variable
To interpret the interactions, we used marginal effects plots cre-
(cf. Aiken & West, 1991). We also included a histogram of the mod-
ated with interplot (Solt & Hu, 2015). This plots the marginal effect
erator variable in each plot to illustrate how much of the sample is
(sometimes called the simple effect) of the predictor variable for the
located at different levels of the moderator variable (cf. Hainmueller,
entire range of the moderator variable. This helps us to understand
Mummolo, & Xu, 2019).
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
933
F I G U R E 2 Estimated slopes of subjective status for each country from the multilevel model. Dots are randomly jittered horizontally to
help show the data.
3.2.1 | Moderators that reduce threat
emerged between identification and subjective status (Figure 4a).
We tested the hypotheses that are based on the assumption that
levels of identification and non-significant at the lowest levels of
group identification and self-esteem reduce the experience of
identification (Figure 4b). It is stronger and significant at high levels.
threat. If the hypotheses are supported, we should find positive in-
This interaction is in the direction predicted by the hypothesis; how-
teractions between the moderators and status, such that at high lev-
ever, the effect of subjective status is never significantly negative.
els of the moderator variables the link between status and perceived
Because this effect is not negative, it is only partially consistent with
legitimacy is positive and at low levels of the moderator variables the
the full prediction that perceived legitimacy will be higher for low
link is negative.
status than for high status groups among lower identifiers.
The positive marginal effect of subjective status is weaker at low
The results for group identification and self-esteem are in
Self-esteem, however, did not significantly interact with subjec-
Figures 4 and 5, respectively. One significant positive interaction
tive status (Figure 5). In short, although group identification showed
934
|
BRANDT eT Al.
F I G U R E 3 Correlations (above the diagonal) and density plots (below the diagonal) between primary predictor variables and the
individual level variables. All variables were country-mean centered before calculating the correlations. Density plots for each individual
variable are on the diagonal. ID = Group identification; SelfEst = Self-esteem; SocMob = Social mobility; ConInqu = Contribution to
inequality; Stable = Stability of the status hierarchy [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
partial support for the hypotheses in the status domain, there was
the hypotheses are supported, we should find negative interactions be-
no support for the hypothesis for self-esteem.
tween the moderators and status, such that at high levels of the moderator variables the link between status and perceived legitimacy is
3.2.2 | Moderators that increase threat
through dissonance
negative and at low levels of the moderator variables the link is positive.
There were no significant negative interactions between any
of the four moderator variables expected to increase the experience of threat (Figures 6–9). In the case of civil liberties, there
Inequality contribution, civil liberties, meritocracy, and inequality were
was a positive interaction between civil liberties and subjective
all expected to increase the experience of threat through dissonance. If
status. When probing the interaction, we found that the effect
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
935
F I G U R E 4 Panel A: Effects of subjective status and group identification on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model.
Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy (y-axis) across the range of
group identification (x-axis). Panel C: Marginal effect of group identification on perceived legitimacy (y-axis) across the range of subjective
status (x-axis). For both Panels B and C, the grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is
highlighted with the dashed line
hypothesis. In short, in no cases did we find support for the hypotheses about moderators that could increase the experience of
threat.
3.2.3 | Structural factors as moderators
The hypotheses suggested that inequality and perceived stability of
the social system were structural factors that might moderate the
association between status and perceived legitimacy. In contrast to
the hypothesis tested in the prior paragraph, one hypothesis is that
F I G U R E 5 Effects of subjective status and self-esteem on
perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error
bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with
the dashed line
countries with high levels of inequality will have stronger, positive
associations between status and perceived legitimacy because it
makes status differences more salient. We did not find evidence for
this (Figure 9).
For stability, the hypothesis predicted that the link between
status and perceived legitimacy would be negative when perceived stability was high compared to low. This hypothesis would
be supported with a negative interaction effect. This effect did
not emerge (Figure 10a). Instead, we found the opposite interaction effect (Figure 10b). Contrary to the hypothesis, the link between status and perceived legitimacy is strong and positive when
stability is high and it is weaker or non-significant when stability
is low.
In short, we do not find evidence that inequality may exacerbate status-based conflict. We also find that stability is a significant
moderator, but in the opposite direction from that expected. It indi-
F I G U R E 6 Effects of subjective status and contributing to
inequality on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the
model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is
highlighted with the dashed line
cates that stability is interpreted differently in the context of real-life
social inequality (see also Verkuyten & Reijerse, 2008), compared
to stability that is manipulated in groups created in the lab (e.g.,
Ellemers et al., 1990). A possible interpretation of the pattern found
in this study could be that the perception that social inequality will
of subjective status (Figure 7b) was positive and stronger at high
not change might be an additional reason for judging that inequality
levels of civil liberties. At lower levels of civil liberties, the effect
is unacceptable (assuming people perceive there to be inequality). At
of subjective status was still positive, but weaker and non-signifi-
the same time, we note that the reliability of our stability measure
cant. This interaction is in the opposite direction predicted by the
could be improved.
936
|
BRANDT eT Al.
F I G U R E 7 Panel A: Effects of
subjective status and civil liberties on
perceived legitimacy. Covariates are
included in the model. Error bars are 95%
confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal
effect of subjective status on perceived
legitimacy (y-axis) across the range of civil
liberties (x-axis). The grey band around the
slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all
panels a null effect is highlighted with the
dashed line. See footnote 13 for outlier
explanation [Colour figure can be viewed
at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
3.3 | Approach 2: Predictors of perceived legitimacy
for people with low levels of subjective status
We tested the hypotheses about the predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of subjective status using the same
models described and presented above. To assess whether there
was support for the hypotheses, we first examined the main effects
of the predictors on perceived legitimacy. This main effect analysis
tells us whether there is an average effect across the sample. When
there is a significant interaction between the predictor and status,
we examine whether the predictor still has the expected effect (e.g.,
a positive effect of social mobility) for people with lower levels of
status (regardless of the pattern for those higher in status). This will
F I G U R E 8 Effects of subjective status and meritocracy on
perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error
bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with
the dashed line
tell us whether the effect of the predictor is specific to people with
low social status or sense of power.
The hypotheses predicted that people would see the system as
more legitimate when it fulfilled a social mobility or stability related
goal. For social mobility, there was clear support for the hypothesis.
There was a large positive effect of social mobility, such that higher
levels of perceived social mobility were associated with higher levels
of perceived legitimacy (Figure 11a). This positive effect was moderated by subjective status (Figure 11a), suggesting that the positive
effect is variable across levels of subjective status. At low levels of
subjective status (Figure 11b), perceived social mobility remained a
significant positive predictor of perceived legitimacy. This is consistent with the hypothesis.
For stability, results were not consistent with the hypothesis.
There was a negative effect of stability on perceived legitimacy
(Figure 10a). This negative effect was moderated by subjective
status (Figure 10a); however, at low levels of subjective status (Figure 10c) the effect of stability remained negative and
significant.
In short, when people see social mobility as a possibility, people
with low subjective status see the system as more legitimate; however,
F I G U R E 9 Effects of subjective status and inequality on
perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error
bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with
the dashed line. See footnote 13 for outlier explanation [Colour
figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
perceiving the system as more stable is associated with less legitimacy
at lower levels of status, which is inconsistent with the hypothesis.
We also made the prediction that when personal or group interests are fulfilled, perceived legitimacy of the system is less necessary. The hypotheses predicted that identification and self-esteem
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
937
F I G U R E 1 0 Panel A: Effects of subjective status and stability of the subjective status hierarchy on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are
included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy (y-axis)
across the range of stability (x-axis). Panel C: Marginal effect of stability on perceived legitimacy (y-axis) across the range of subjective status
(x-axis). For Panels B and C the grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is highlighted with the
dashed line
would be negatively associated with levels of perceived legitimacy
this debate beyond straightforward main effects by taking two
for people with low levels of subjective status.
approaches.
Inconsistent with hypotheses, there are significant positive effects of identification (Figure 4a) and self-esteem (Figure 5). To see
whether this was consistent across people with low subjective status,
we examined the interaction effects. The positive effect of self-es-
4.1 | Approach 1: Moderator of the association
between subjective status and perceived legitimacy
teem was not moderated by subjective status (Figure 5). The positive
effect of group identification was moderated by subjective status
Approach 1 examined how constructs that reduce threat, constructs
(Figure 4a). Here, we find that the effect of identification is not differ-
that increase threat, and perceived structural factors may be moder-
ent from zero for people who are very low status (Figure 4c). Although
ators of the association between status and perceived legitimacy. We
this does not confirm the hypothesis, it is also not contrary to it.
tested eight specific moderator hypotheses and found partial support for one: the identification-moderation hypothesis (see Table 4).
4 | GENER AL DISCUSSION
That is, the most common result was no clear support for the hypotheses. Sometimes the lack of support was due to non-significant
interactions. The sensitivity analyses reported in footnotes 9, 11, and
Debates around status and perceptions of legitimacy in psychology
12 indicate that we have substantial power to detect main effects
are characterized by mixed findings and different theoretical foci
and interactions at the individual level. For country-level interaction
from different research groups. Consistent with some past work
effects, we only had sufficient power to detect large effects. In the
(e.g., Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi-Cioldi, 2012;
cases of null results, studies with even greater statistical power may
Kraus & Callaghan, 2014; Vargas-Salfate et al., 2018; Zimmerman
find evidence in support of these hypotheses, in opposition to these
& Reyna, 2013), but inconsistent with the status-legitimacy hy-
hypotheses, or for a null effect (time will tell). In many other cases,
pothesis (e.g., Henry & Saul, 2006; Jost et al., 2003), we found that
the lack of support was due to a significant interaction that was the
subjective status is positively associated with perceived legitimacy.
opposite of the prediction (e.g., stability-moderator hypothesis). In
However, this is not our primary contribution. We sought to advance
these cases (highlighted with ✗op in Table 4), it seems less plausible
F I G U R E 1 1 Panel A: Effects of
subjective status and social mobility
on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are
included in the model. Error bars are 95%
confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal
effect of social mobility on perceived
legitimacy (y-axis) across the range of
subjective status (x-axis). For Panel B, the
grey band around the slope is the 95%
confidence interval. In all panels a null
effect is highlighted with the dashed line
938
|
BRANDT eT Al.
TA B L E 4 Summary of moderation hypotheses (Approach 1) and
whether they were supported
used as a vehicle for improvements to group status in the long term
(Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2018). Our data lendsome credence
to this hypothesis.
Moderator hypothesis
Although almost never in a direction supportive of the hypothe-
Reduce threat
Identification-moderation hypothesis
✓
Self-esteem-moderation hypothesis
✗
Increase threat through dissonance
ses (see Table 5), we find that group identification, self-esteem, and
beliefs in social mobility are all associated with greater perceived legitimacy among people with low subjective status. In this way, we
helped fulfill Jost’s (2017) call to better understand what leads peo-
Inequality contribution-moderation hypothesis
✗
Civil liberties hypothesis
✗op
Meritocracy hypothesis
✗
SJT inequality hypothesis
✗
4.3 | Strengths, limitations, and future directions
SIT inequality hypothesis
✗
We used a design that leverages larger samples from multiple countries
Stability-moderator hypothesis
✗
to understand how subjective status is associated with perceived legiti-
ple who are oppressed to uphold the system.
Structural factors that affect threat
op
Note: ✓ = indicates support and partial support for the hypothesis,
✗ = indicates no support for the hypothesis. ✗op = indicates a significant
interaction effect in the opposite direction.
Abbreviations: SIT, Social identity theory; SJT, System justification
theory.
macy and related constructs. This helps us avoid low statistical power
(Sedlmeier & Gigerenzer, 1989) and uses less-WEIRD sampling (Henrich,
Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). That said, hardly any of the samples were
representative, and we were not able to recruit participants from all the
regions of the globe. Most notably, we were unable to secure data from
the continent of Africa, and Asia is underrepresented. However, given
TA B L E 5 Summary of predictor hypotheses (Approach 2) and
whether they were supported
the consistency of our results with those of researchers who have used
data from even more diverse and representative samples (Brandt, 2013;
Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi-Cioldi, 2012; Vargas-Salfate
Predictor hypothesis
Social Mobility-Legitimacy Hypothesis
✓
Stability-Legitimacy Hypothesis
✗op
Identification-legitimacy hypothesis
✗
Self-esteem legitimacy hypothesis
✗op
Note: ✓ = indicates support and partial support for the hypothesis,
✗ = indicates no support for the hypothesis. ✗op = indications a
significant interaction effect in the opposite direction.
et al., 2018), we believe that our data provide a reasonable approximation
at this time for the regions that we do cover. A related limitation is that
although we report the range of Cronbach's α and r for all scales across
countries, we have not formally tested measurement invariance (He &
van de Vijver, 2012). It is encouraging that some of the scales we use have
been shown to be invariant across at least some countries (e.g., Davidov &
Coromina, 2013). Clearly, more work is needed on the social psychology
of perceived legitimacy and its development in understudied regions; this
future work will surely have both practical and theoretical benefits.
that the issue was a lack of statistical power. The predictive power of
the hypotheses for moderators was essentially nil.
Many of our samples are student samples. This allowed us to
use our resources efficiently, but necessarily implies that the samples are younger and better educated than the general population.
4.2 | Approach 2: Predictors of perceived legitimacy
for people with lower levels of subjective status
Although studies on similar topics (and social psychology more
broadly) have relied on student and non-representative samples, it
is always possible that samples with more objectively and subjectively low status people, or that deliberately recruit people from
Approach 2 examined the predictors of perceived legitimacy for
disadvantaged groups (e.g., homeless shelters, soup kitchens) would
people with low subjective status. We tested four specific hypoth-
reveal different results. That being said, 34% of our sample self-re-
eses about these predictors and found support for one: the social
ported a subjective social status below the midpoint of the scale,
mobility-legitimacy hypothesis (see Table 5). Most of the predictions
indicating that our sample cannot easily be dismissed as a sample
were not confirmed and, in most cases, results were opposite to the
consisting only of people who seem themselves as high status.
predicted direction. As one example, the identification-legitimacy
Moreover, our samples included representative (sample NLD3) and
hypothesis predicted a negative association for members of low
community samples similar to those which have been used to study
status/power groups. Yet, we found the opposite. Some members
low social status in other work (e.g., MTurk in sample USA1 was
of our team have started to think through this type of effect (and
used in Plantinga, Krijnen, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2018; Shah,
have published some findings consistent with the data in this manu-
Shafir, & Mullainathan, 2015). Additionally, our student samples are
script). As explained earlier, they have proposed that among people
from a diverse array of university types, including public and private
with low subjective status, identification will be positively associ-
universities and community colleges serving a diverse range of stu-
ated with system legitimacy, especially when the system can be
dents. Lastly, the main effects of status are similar to studies using
|
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
representative samples (e.g., Brandt, 2013), suggesting that our find-
939
4.5 | Conclusion
ings are consistent with data using other sampling techniques.
We started this project because we noted that there is evidence
both consistent and inconsistent with the status-legitimacy hy-
4.4 | Where to now?
pothesis. Our study found very little support for moderator hypotheses across 30 countries and several moderator variables. To
Our project provides scholars with additional evidence that they can
the extent that the phenomenon predicted by the status-legitimacy
14
use to inform the direction of research on status and legitimacy.
hypothesis exists, this appears to be quite rare. That does not mean
This additional evidence comes in two forms. First, these hypothe-
that people with low subjective status never see the system as rel-
ses were inspired by both social identity and system justification
atively legitimate. Our data also shed light on when this is most
theories and primarily made inaccurate predictions in our study.
likely to be the case. We find that group identification, self-esteem,
This may be because the prediction was wrong, or because some
and beliefs in social mobility are all associated with perceived le-
auxiliary assumptions did not hold in our particular samples and
gitimacy among people who are low status (as well as those with
measures. For example, nearly every moderator predicted to reduce
high status). We hope that our findings inspire scholars interested
feelings of threat or increase feelings of threat through dissonance
in this domain to pursue questions related to status and legitimacy
were not supported. This may mean that these moderators are not
to enrich our understanding of these constructs around the world.
associated with threat as expected (auxiliary assumption did not
hold), or that threat is not a key mechanism linking status to legiti-
AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S
macy (theoretical prediction was wrong). Learning from theoretical
All authors provided feedback on the drafts of the manuscript and
failures and inaccurate predictions like these can often be more in-
helped collect data. Mark J. Brandt, Toon Kuppens, and Russell
formative than learning from theoretical successes (Ferguson &
Spears organized the project, designed the study, and wrote
Heene, 2012; Popper, 1959).
the initial drafts of the manuscript. Julia C. Becker, Matthew J.
It is also plausible that some of the inaccurate predictions (and
Easterbrook, Simon M. Laham, Jolanda Jetten, Chuma Kevin
perhaps most of them) can be addressed by increasing the specific-
Owuamalam, Mark Rubin, Joseph Sweetman, and Jojanneke van
ity of the theories and how theoretical concepts are translated into
der Toorn provided additional feedback in the drafting of the
concrete operationalizations. For example, social identity theory has
manuscript. This research was supported by Fondecyt program
traditionally treated legitimacy as a moderator and used outcome
(1161371), the Center for Social Conflict and Cohesion Studies
measures such as group evaluations, allocation, and social change
(15130009), the Center for Intercultural and Indigenous Research
strategies. Here, we tried to extend the theory's predictions to le-
(15110006), the European Research Council (ERC) under the
gitimacy, but did not find consistent support. This may suggest that
European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation pro-
social identity theory's predictions about social change strategies do
gramme (grant agreement No 759320), and the Spanish Ministry
not extend to legitimacy in a straightforward way, which may also
of Economy and Competitiveness (PSI2016-79971-P). The con-
help to explain the limited support for predictions derived from social
tribution of Sylvie Graf was supported by the grant 20-01214S
identity theory. Recent work has therefore investigated how a social
from the Czech Science Foundation and by RVO: 68081740 of the
identity perspective can be better applied to predict system legiti-
Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences.
macy (Owuamalam et al., 2019).
Second, our data provide descriptive information about the precisely estimated associations between a large number of socially and
CONFLIC TS OF INTEREST
There are no conflicts of interest to report.
politically relevant variables. That is, not only did our study fail to confirm a number of hypotheses about status and legitimacy, but it also
T R A N S PA R E N C Y S TAT E M E N T
provides the relevant associations and non-associations that relevant
Data, code, and materials are available at the following link: https://
theories need to explain. For example, despite expectations of both
osf.io/5uxc7/.
perspectives, a negative association between status and legitimacy
was not found at low levels of identification. Theories need to account
ORCID
for this pattern of results. Similarly, perceived stability was associated
Mark J. Brandt
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7185-7031
with lower levels of legitimacy, despite expectations. Updates will
need to account for this pattern of results. Although post-hoc expla-
REFERENCES
nations are possible, we hope that theorists can build clearly specified
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000).
Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in
healthy, White women. Health Psychology, 19, 586–592. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
models that can be used to explain the current data and to make new,
falsifiable predictions (e.g., Muthukrishna & Henrich, 2019).
14
See Appendix S1 for data that can inform scholars interested in people's sense of
power.
940
|
American National Election Studies (2015). Codebook for the time series
cumulative data file 1948–2012. Retrieved from http://www.elect
ionst udies.org/study pages /anes_times eries_cdf/anes_times eries_
cdf_codebook_int.pdf
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.
Bettencourt, B., Charlton, K., Dorr, N., & Hume, D. L. (2001). Status
differences and in-group bias: A meta-analytic examination of
the effects of status stability, status legitimacy, and group permeability. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 520–542. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.520
Brandt, M. J. (2013). Do the disadvantaged legitimize the social system? A large-scale test of the status–legitimacy hypothesis. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 765–785. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0031751
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538–549. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.538
Caricati, L. (2017). Testing the status-legitimacy hypothesis: A multilevel
modeling approach to the perception of legitimacy in income distribution in 36 nations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 157, 532–540.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2016.1242472
Caricati, L., & Lorenzi-Cioldi, F. (2012). Does status matter? Testing
hypotheses from strong form of system justification theory. Revue
Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 25, 67–95.
Davidov, E., & Coromina, L. (2013). Evaluating measurement invariance
for social and political trust in Western Europe over four measurement time points (2002–2008). Ask: Research & Methods, 22, 37–54.
Day, M. V., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). Movin’ on up? How perceptions of social mobility affect our willingness to defend the system. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 267–274. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1948550616678454
Economist Intelligence Unit (2016). Democracy Index 2015. Democracy
in an age of anxiety. London, UK: The Economist. Retrieved from
https://www.eiu.com/democracy2015
Ellemers, N., Knippenberg, A., & Wilke, H. (1990). The influence of permeability of group boundaries and stability of group status on strategies of
individual mobility and social change. British Journal of Social Psychology,
29, 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00902.x
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an
old issue. Psychological Methods, 12, 121–138. https://doi.
org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121
Ferguson, C. J., & Heene, M. (2012). A vast graveyard of undead theories: Publication bias and psychological science’s aversion to the
null. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 555–561. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1745691612459059
Friesen, J. P., Kay, A. C., Eibach, R. P., & Galinsky, A. D. (2014). Seeking
structure in social organization: Compensatory control and the psychological advantages of hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 106, 590–609. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035620
Gelman, A. (2018). The failure of null hypothesis significance testing when studying incremental changes, and what to do about it.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 16–23. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0146167217729162
General Social Survey (2017). General social surveys, 1972–2016:
Cumulative Codebook. Retrieved from http://gss.norc.org/docum
ents/codebook/GSS_Codebook.pdf
Goodman, E., Adler, N. E., Daniels, S. R., Morrison, J. A., Slap, G. B., &
Dolan, L. M. (2003). Impact of objective and subjective social status
on obesity in a biracial cohort of adolescents. Obesity Research, 11,
1018–1026. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2003.140
Green, P., & MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: An R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in
Ecology and Evolution, 7, 493–498.
BRANDT eT Al.
Hainmueller, J., Mummolo, J., & Xu, Y. (2019). How much should we trust
estimates from multiplicative interaction models? Simple tools to improve empirical practice. Political Analysis, 27, 163–192. https://doi.
org/10.1017/pan.2018.46
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34–64.
He, J., & van de Vijver, F. (2012). Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural
research. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2, 2307-919.
https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1111
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people
in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. https://doi.
org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Henry, P. J., & Saul, A. (2006). The development of system justification
in the developing world. Social Justice Research, 19, 365–378. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s11211-006-0012-x
Hogg, M. A. (2014). From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity processes. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 23, 338–342. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414
540168
International Social Survey Programme (2009). ISSP 2009 Social Inequality
IV. Retrieved from https://www.gesis.org/issp/modules/issp-modul
es-by-topic/social-inequality/2009/
Jonas, E., McGregor, I., Klackl, J., Agroskin, D., Fritsche, I., Holbrook, C.,
… Quirin, M. (2014). Threat and defense: From anxiety to approach.
In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (vol. 49, pp. 219–286). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.
Jost, J. T. (2017). Working class conservatism: A system justification
perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 73–78. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.020
Jost, J. (2019). A quarter century of system justification theory: Questions,
answers, criticisms, and societal applications. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 58, 263–314. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12297
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x
Jost, J. T., Gaucher, D., & Stern, C. (2015). “The world isn’t fair”: A system
justification perspective on social stratification and inequality. In J. F.
Dovidio & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social
psychology (vol. 2, pp. 317–340). Washington, DC: APA.
Jost, J., & Hunyady, O. (2003). The psychology of system justification and
the palliative function of ideology. European Review of Social Psychology,
13, 111–153. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280240000046
Jost, J. T., Liviatan, I., van der Toorn, J., Ledgerwood, A., Mandisodza,
A., & Nosek, B. A. (2011). System justification: How do we know it’s
motivated. In D. Ramona Bobocel, A. C. Kay, M. P. Zanna & J. M.
Olson (Eds.), The psychology of justice and legitimacy (pp. 173–204).
New York, NY: Taylor and Francis.
Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Sheldon, O., & Ni Sullivan, B. (2003). Social inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the
system: Evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvantaged. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 13–36. https://
doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.127
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). God
and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for
the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 95, 18–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.18
Kay, A. C., & Jost, J. T. (2003). Complementary justice: Effects of “poor
but happy” and “poor but honest” stereotype exemplars on system
justification and implicit activation of the justice motive. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 823–837. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823
Kraus, M. W., & Callaghan, B. (2014). Noblesse oblige? Social status and
economic inequality maintenance among politicians. PLoS One, 9,
e85293. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0085293
SUBJECTIVE STATUS AND PERCEIVED LEGITIMACY
Kuppens, T., & Pollet, T. V. (2014). Mind the level: Problems with two
recent nation-level analyses in psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5,
1110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01110
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2016). lmerTest:
Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-33. Retrieved
from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest
Laurin, K., Gaucher, D., & Kay, A. (2013). Stability and the justification of
social inequality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 246–254.
Laurin, K., Kay, A. C., & Landau, M. J. (2018). Structure and Goal Pursuit:
Individual and cultural differences. Advances in Methods and Practices
in Psychological Science, 1, 491–494. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152
45918797130
Li, W., Yang, Y., Wu, J., & Kou, Y. (2020). Testing the status-legitimacy
hypothesis in China: Objective and subjective socioeconomic status divergently predict system justification. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 46, 1044–1058.
Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. Academy of Management Annals,
2, 351–398.
Mandisodza, A. N., Jost, J. T., & Unzueta, M. M. (2006). “Tall poppies”
and “American dreams” reactions to rich and poor in Australia and
the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 659–668.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022106292076
Martorana, P. V., Galinsky, A. D., & Rao, H. (2005). From system justification to system condemnation: Antecedents of attempts to change
power hierarchies. In M. C. Thomas-Hunt (Ed.), Status and groups (research on managing groups and teams, Vol. 7) (pp. 283–313). Bingley,
UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
McGuire, W. J. (2013). An additional future for psychological science.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 414–423.
McMahon, S. D., & Watts, R. J. (2002). Ethnic identity in urban African
American youth: Exploring links with self-worth, aggression, and
other psychosocial variables. Journal of Community Psychology, 30,
411–431. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.10013
McShane, B. B., Tackett, J. L., Böckenholt, U., & Gelman, A. (2019). Largescale replication projects in contemporary psychological research.
The American Statistician, 73, 99–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/00031
305.2018.1505655
Mummendey, A., Kessler, T., Klink, A., & Mielke, R. (1999). Strategies to cope
with negative social identity: Predictions by social identity theory and
relative deprivation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
76, 229–245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.229
Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nature
Human Behaviour, 3(3), 221–229. https://doi.org/10.1038/s4156
2-018-0522-1
Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., & Diener, E. (2011). Income inequality and happiness.
Psychological Science, 22, 1095–1100. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567
97611417262
Operario, D., Adler, N. E., & Williams, D. R. (2004). Subjective social status: Reliability and predictive utility for global health. Psychology &
Health, 19, 237–246. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870 4403100016
38098
Owuamalam, C. K., Rubin, M., & Spears, R. (2016). The system justification conundrum: Re-examining the cognitive dissonance basis
for system justification. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1889. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01889
Owuamalam, C. K., Rubin, M., & Spears, R. (2018). Addressing evidential and theoretical inconsistencies in system-justification theory
with a social identity model of system attitudes. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 27, 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637
21417737136
Owuamalam, C. K., Rubin, M., & Spears, R. (2019). Revisiting 25 years of
system motivation explanation for system justification from the perspective of social identity model of system attitudes. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 58, 362–381.
|
941
Owuamalam, C. K., Rubin, M., Spears, R., & Weerabangsa, M. M. A.
(2017). Why do people from low-status groups support class systems
that disadvantage them? A test of two mainstream explanations in
Malaysia and Australia. Journal of Social Issues, 73, 80–98.
Plantinga, A., Krijnen, J. M., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M.
(2018). Evidence for opportunity cost neglect in the poor. Journal
of Behavioral Decision Making, 31, 65–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/
bdm.2041
Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Proulx, T., Inzlicht, M., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). Understanding all
inconsistency compensation as a palliative response to violated
expectations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 285–291. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.002
R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Available from https://www.R-project.org/
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring
global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and
the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 27, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201272002
Rubin, M., & Hewstone, M. (2004). Social identity system justification and social dominance: Commentary on Reicher Jost et al.,
and Sidanius et al. Political Psychology, 25, 823–844. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00400.x
Sakurai, K., Kawakami, N., Yamaoka, K., Ishikawa, H., & Hashimoto,
H. (2010). The impact of subjective and objective social status on
psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Social
Science & Medicine, 70, 1832–1839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socsc
imed.2010.01.019
Schloerke, B., Crowley, J., Cook, D., Briatte, F., Marbach, M., Thoen,
E., … Larmarange, J. (2016). GGally: Extension to ‘ggplot2’. R package
version 1.3.0. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-proje ct.org/packa
ge=GGally
Schoel, C., Bluemke, M., Mueller, P., & Stahlberg, D. (2011). When autocratic leaders become an option: Uncertainty and self-esteem predict implicit leadership preferences. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 101, 521–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023393
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1989). Do studies of statistical power
have an effect on the power of studies? Psychological Bulletin, 105,
309–316. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.309
Sengupta, N. K., Osborne, D., & Sibley, C. G. (2015). The status-legitimacy hypothesis revisited: Ethnic-group differences in general and
dimension-specific legitimacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54,
324–340. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12080
Shah, A. K., Shafir, E., & Mullainathan, S. (2015). Scarcity frames value.
Psychological Science, 26, 402–412. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567
97614563958
Solt, F., & Hu, Y. (2015). Interplot: Plot the effects of variables in interactions
terms. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=inter
plot
Spears, R., Jetten, J., & Doosje, B. (2001). The (il)legitimacy of ingroup
bias: From social reality to social resistance. In J. Jost & B. Major
(Eds.), The psychology of legitimacy: Emerging perspectives on ideology, justice, and intergroup relations (pp. 332–362). New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. CUP Archive.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Trump, K. S., & White, A. (2018). Does inequality beget inequality?
Experimental tests of the prediction that inequality increases system
justification motivation. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 5,
206–216. https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2018.2
942
|
Ullrich, J., & Schlüter, E. (2012). Detecting nasty data with simple plots of
complex models: Comment on Brandt (2011). Psychological Science,
23(7), 824–825. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434541
Van der Toorn, J., Feinberg, M., Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C., Tyler, T. R., Willer,
R., & Wilmuth, C. (2015). A sense of powerlessness fosters system
justification: Implications for the legitimation of authority, hierarchy, and government. Political Psychology, 36, 93–110. https://doi.
org/10.1111/pops.12183
Vargas-Salfate, S., Paez, D., Liu, J. H., Pratto, F., & Gil de Zúñiga, H.
(2018). A comparison of social dominance theory and system justification: The role of social status in 19 nations. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 44, 1060–1076. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461
67218757455
Verkuyten, M., & Reijerse, A. (2008). Intergroup structure and identity
management among ethnic minority and majority groups: The interactive effects of perceived stability, legitimacy, and permeability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 106–127. https://doi.
org/10.1002/ejsp.395
World Bank (2016). World Development Indicators. Retrieved from https://
data.worldbank.org/data-catalog/world-development-indic ators
World Values Survey Association (2016). World Values Survey Wave 6
2010-2014. Retrieved from http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/
WVSDocumentationWV6.jsp
BRANDT eT Al.
Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M., & Moghaddam, F. M. (1990). Responding to
membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective
protest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 994–1003.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.994
Zimmerman, J. L., & Reyna, C. (2013). The meaning and role of ideology
in system justification and resistance for high-and low-status people.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 1–23. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0032967
S U P P O R T I N G I N FO R M AT I O N
Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Supporting Information section.
How to cite this article: BrandtMJ, Kuppens T, Spears R, et al.
Subjective status and perceived legitimacy across countries.
Eur J Soc Psychol. 2020;50:921–942. https://doi.org/10.1002/
ejsp.2694