Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
DOI 10.1007/s12671-016-0594-9
ORIGINAL PAPER
Phenomenological Fingerprints of Four Meditations: Differential
State Changes in Affect, Mind-Wandering, Meta-Cognition,
and Interoception Before and After Daily
Practice Across 9 Months of Training
Bethany E. Kok 1 & Tania Singer 1
Published online: 19 August 2016
# The Author(s) 2016. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com
Abstract Despite increasing interest in the effects of mental
training practices such as meditation, there is much ambiguity
regarding whether and to what extent the various types of
mental practice have differential effects on psychological
change. To address this gap, we compare the effects of four
common meditation practices on measures of state change in
affect, mind-wandering, meta-cognition, and interoception. In
the context of a 9-month mental training program called the
ReSource Project, 229 mid-life adults (mean age 41) provided
daily reports before and after meditation practice. Participants
received training in the following three successive modules:
the first module (presence) included breathing meditation and
body scan, the second (affect) included loving-kindness meditation, and the third (perspective) included observing-thought
meditation. Using multilevel modeling, we found that body
scan led to the greatest state increase in interoceptive awareness and the greatest decrease in thought content, lovingkindness meditation led to the greatest increase in feelings of
warmth and positive thoughts about others, and observingthought meditation led to the greatest increase in metacognitive awareness. All practices, including breathing meditation, increased positivity of affect, energy, and present focus
and decreased thought distraction. Complementary network
analysis of intervariate relationships revealed distinct
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(doi:10.1007/s12671-016-0594-9) contains supplementary material,
which is available to authorized users.
* Bethany E. Kok
bethkok@cbs.mpg.de
1
Department of Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute for Human
Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstraße 1A,
04103 Leipzig, Germany
phenomenological clusters of psychological change congruent with the content of each practice. These findings together
suggest that although different meditation practices may have
common beneficial effects, each practice can also be characterized by a distinct short-term psychological fingerprint, the
latter having important implications for the use of meditative
practices in different intervention contexts and with different
populations.
Keywords Meditation . Meta-cognitive awareness .
Decentering . Interoception . Affect . Multilevel modeling
Introduction
In recent years, the potential effects of mental training on
well-being, brain, health, and behavior have become a focus of both popular and scientific interest. A wide variety
of training programs now exist that offer secularized meditation training, comprised of standardized protocols with
instruction in a variety of contemplative practices (KabatZinn 1990), often in combination with other forms of mental training such as cognitive-behavioral therapy (Fjorback
et al. 2011). Mental training has promise, both as a potential treatment for mental disorders from schizophrenia
(Johnson et al. 2011) to PTSD (Lang et al. 2012) to depression and anxiety (Strauss et al. 2014), among others, and as
a method for improving quality of life in individuals not
diagnosed with a disorder (Chiesa and Serretti 2009).
Initial research on the benefits of secularized meditation
programs rested on comparisons to passive control groups
or on comparing pre-meditation and post-meditation effects in the same participants with no control (Ospina
et al. 2008). Such studies found significant effects on
physical and mental health, attention, stress reduction,
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
and even brain activity (Chiesa and Serretti 2010). Other
studies focused on defining the subjective experience, or
phenomenology, of meditation, sometimes linking phenomenological measures to objective indices of neural or
physiological activity (Lutz and Thompson 2003).
Meditation, like sports, refers to many different types of
activities that can vary greatly in content, focus, effort, complexity, and duration. Widely studied secular mental training
programs like Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR)
or Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) combine
many different mental practices, making it impossible to isolate the effects of any one specific practice (Kabat-Zinn 1990;
Williams et al. 2014). Furthermore, meditation is commonly
performed in a specific context which can have effects independent of practice content. When meditative practices are
compared to active control groups rather than waitlist controls
or pre-post comparisons without a control group, findings of
specific effects for meditation often become smaller and more
bounded (Feldman et al. 2010; Kuyken et al. 2015; MacCoon
et al. 2012; Obasi et al. 2013). Similarly, due to the intensive
measurement requirements of phenomenological research, the
vast majority of phenomenological studies focus only on the
phenomenological space of one meditative practice, without
direct comparison to others (for an exception, see
Louchakova-Schwartz (2013)).
Four commonly studied meditative practices are breathing
meditation, body scan, loving-kindness meditation, and
observing-thought meditation. These practices are drawn
mostly from the Buddhist contemplative traditions, although
many other traditions employ similar exercises. Focusing on
the breath is a meditation aimed at stabilizing attention and the
mind; practitioners learn to direct attention to the breath in an
intentional way, to monitor the direction of attention and detect when the mind wanders, and to return attention to the
breath when mind-wandering is detected. As the practice
teaches stable attention, an ability required for meditation, it
is often taught to beginners yet remains central to the practices
of many experienced meditators (Hart 1987; Sakyong 2003).
In their review of the literature, Lutz et al. (2008) categorized
focus on breath as a type of focused attention meditation and
linked it to changes in attentional processing. For example,
after practicing attentional focus meditation, experienced
Tibetan Buddhist monks were able to perceive, as one stable
percept, two dissimilar images presented to separate eyes; this
effect did not occur after the same monks practiced compassion meditation (Carter et al. 2005).
The practice of body scan involves extending awareness to
each individual part of the body in turn, typically starting at
the head (Kabat-Zinn 1990). Practitioners focus on directing
their attention exclusively toward the targeted body part and
observing the sensations in that part. Body scan is an element
of MBSR and is another kind of focused attention meditation,
this time using different parts of the body as attentional
219
objects. Body scan practice, in combination with focusing
on the breath, improves interoceptive sensitivity and accuracy
and also trains attentional control, as the meditator constantly
monitors the object of attention and returns attention to the
targeted body part if the mind wanders (Mirams et al. 2013).
Loving-kindness meditation is a practice that focuses on
the cultivation of benevolence, love, and care toward others
and the self (Salzberg 2005). Participants strengthen feelings
of warmth and care through the visualization of a close loved
person. Participants sequentially extend these feelings toward
themselves, a close person, a neutral person, a person whom
they dislike or have difficulties with, and finally toward
strangers and human beings in general. Longitudinal studies
comparing 8 weeks of loving-kindness meditation to a waitlist
control have found that meditators increase in trait positive
emotions (Fredrickson et al. 2008; Kok et al. 2013) and feelings of closeness to others (Kok et al. 2013) but do not change
in negative emotions. There is also evidence that lovingkindness meditation induces neurological and physiological
changes; 1 week of training in combined loving-kindness
meditation and compassion increased neural activity in networks associated with positive affect and affiliation, relative to
an active memory control group (Klimecki et al. 2013, 2014;
Singer and Klimecki 2014).
Observing-thought meditation teaches Bdecentering,^ a
meta-cognitive process allowing thoughts to arise and fall
without identifying with or becoming absorbed in their content or emotions. Learning to observe thoughts is an element
of mindfulness meditation and is taught in courses including
MBSR and MBCT (Fjorback and Walach 2012). Observingthought meditation training cultivates meta-cognitive awareness of thoughts via two different meta-cognitive skills, taught
sequentially. First, participants learned to categorize upcoming thoughts with labels such as Bpast,^ Bfuture,^ Bpositive^
or Bnegative,^ and Bself^ or Bother.^ Then, participants
learned to observe thoughts coming and going without
reacting or engaging with the thoughts. Results of a recent
empirical study suggest that training in observing-thought
meditation has cognitive effects; compared to both body scan
and mindful yoga, the observing-thought meditation practice
of Bsitting meditation^ was associated with the greatest improvement in non-judging of thoughts (Sauer-Zavala et al.
2013). The opposite of decentering, persistent identification
and enmeshment with thoughts, is known as rumination and is
associated with a variety of negative mental health outcomes
(Olatunji et al. 2013). Mindfulness meditation including an
observing-thought component effectively reduced rumination
relative to both relaxation training and measurement control
groups (Jain et al. 2007).
These four meditative practices appear to have psychological and physiological effects when compared to waitlist
control groups or to active non-meditative controls. There
are also hints of practice-specific effects for breathing
220
meditation and observing thoughts, compared to other meditative practices. Overall, however, while these four meditative practices have distinct conceptual bases and appear in
theoretical work as distinct practices, there is no empirical
evidence to directly differentiate them.
This study systematically investigates the common and differential state effects of these four meditation practices across
dimensions of subjective experience commonly targeted by
meditation, affect, mind-wandering, content of thoughts, and
meta-cognitive and body awareness. Body scan and breathing
meditation should most effectively increase feelings of presence
and body awareness and decrease distraction by thoughts.
Loving-kindness meditation should most effectively increase
positivity of affect and feelings of warmth, as well as positive
and other-focused thoughts. Finally, observing-thought meditation should not change the content of thought or decrease the
amount of thoughts but should most effectively increase metacognitive awareness of thought contents and processes and decrease distraction by thoughts. In addition, we hypothesize that
loving kindness will be more effective after 3 months of body
scan and breathing practice. We also conducted exploratory
network analyses of the interrelationships among the various
outcome measures (Borsboom and Cramer 2013).
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited through flyers, radio and newspaper advertisements, and local news coverage in two major
German cities. Potential participants attended one of multiple
evening information sessions offered by the principal investigator (Singer), then indicated their interest in participating via
a website. Potential participants were then sent a battery of
screening questionnaires designed to identify individuals from
vulnerable populations (underage, pregnant or nursing, suffering from mental or physical illness), individuals who would be
unable to complete the behavioral or neurological measurement portions of the study, and individuals with previous meditation experience, all of whom were excluded from the present study. More details concerning participant recruitment,
screening, and demographics in the ReSource Project are
available in the online Supplementary material.
The sample at start of data collection included 80 participants
in training cohort 1 (TC1), 81 participants in training cohort 2
(TC2), and 81 participants in training cohort 3 (TC3). The three
training cohorts did not differ significantly in gender, age, or
personality/mental health as assessed by a wide range of trait
measures (listed in Singer et al. (2016), Appendix C2). Of the
initial sample, 13 did not complete any meditation sessions using
the online platform, thus providing no meditation data. Analyses
are based on the following groups: TC1 78 participants, 58 %
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
female, mean age 41.4 (minimum age = 20, maximum age = 55);
TC2 78 participants, 59 % female, mean age 41.3 (minimum
age = 21, maximum age = 55); and TC3 73 participants, 60 %
female, mean age 40.7 (minimum age = 21, maximum age = 55).
Additionally, by the start of the second module (affect for TC1,
perspective for TC2), one participant provided no meditation
data in TC1 and three provided no meditation data in of TC2.
By the start of the third module (perspective for TC1, affect for
TC2), four additional participants ceased to provide meditation
data in TC1 and two participants ceased to provide meditation
data in TC2. The total participant-level meditation data loss rate
by the last module completed was thus 8.75 % for TC1, 10 % for
TC2, and 10 % for TC3.
Procedure
Four different meditation practices were taught as part of the
ReSource Project, a module-based 9-month secularized mental
training program (Singer et al. 2016). The practices, different in
content and goals, are matched in training context (e.g., length of
retreats, amount of practice each week, length and structure of
weekly group sessions, and a shared pool of teacher practitioners). As with most recent meditation research performed in
the West, the training has been secularized. The first module,
called presence, teaches two core practices; Bbreathing
meditation^ and Bbody scan^ cultivate attention and interoceptive
awareness. The second module, called affect, teaches Blovingkindness^ meditation, with the purpose of creating positive,
other-focused mental states and prosocial motivation. The third
module, called perspective, teaches Bobserving-thought^ meditation, in order to learn to identify and detach from the contents of
thought and decrease reactivity to mental events. The affect and
perspective modules also include near-daily dyadic meditations,
the effects of which are discussed elsewhere (Kok and Singer,
Contemplating the Other: Introducing the Contemplative Dyad
and its effects on social closeness, motivation and personal disclosure over six months of mental training via a randomized
clinical trial, under review). Participants underwent the same
number of hours in retreats and teacher-led meditation training
for all three modules. Teaching was conducted in teams to avoid
confounding content with a particular teacher, although it was not
possible to fully counterbalance teachers across practices to
completely eliminate the potential effects of particularly effective
teachers (Ospina et al. 2007). Each of the three modules of the
ReSource Project serves as an active control for the other modules, allowing the assessment of practice-specific effects.
To explore sequence effects on training and create active
control groups, participants were divided into three training
cohorts, each of which experienced the modules in a different
order. Training cohorts 1 and 2 began the study with a 3-day
retreat led by experienced meditation teachers, where they
started training breathing meditation and body scan (details
about the retreats and content of the training program have
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
been published in Singer et al. 2016). At the retreat, participants were also introduced to the ReSource data collection
website, including guided meditation recordings and a series
of online questions to be answered before and after meditating. For the next 13 weeks, participants were asked to practice
breathing meditation for a minimum of 5 days a week and to
conduct a body scan for a minimum of 5 days a week, using
the guided meditation recordings and completing the premeditation and post-meditation questions before and after
each practice. Guided meditations were available in 20-min
(body scan) and 10-min (breathing meditation) lengths. In
addition, participants attended weekly 2-h sessions with meditation teachers from the retreat.
After 13 weeks of presence training (see also Fig. 1), participants in TC1 began 13 weeks of affect training in lovingkindness meditation and an emotion-focused dyadic exercise,
starting with a 3-day retreat. Participants in TC2 began
13 weeks of perspective training in observing-thought meditation and a perspective-taking dyadic exercise, also starting
with a 3-day retreat. For reasons of different format and content, the effects of the dyadic exercises will not be addressed
here. As before, participants were asked to practice their core
meditation (loving-kindness meditation for TC1 and
observing-thought meditation for TC2) at least 5 days a week
(in addition to a 2-h teacher-led group session each week),
using guided meditation recordings available on the study
website, and to complete the online questionnaire before and
after practice. Participants also continued to attend their 2-h
long weekly training sessions with meditation teachers, focusing on the new practice.
After the second 13-week training module was completed,
participants in TC1 were assigned to the perspective module
and participants in TC2 were assigned to the affect module,
following the same pattern of retreat, meditation and dyadic
activities, daily training and assessment, and weekly meetings
described previously (see Fig. 1). Aside from the
counterbalanced order of the affect and perspective modules
and inevitable variations in season due to the staggered start
times of the two training cohorts, participants’ experiences
were intended to be identical across TC1 and TC2; all participants in both cohorts experienced the exact same combination of the same meditation practices taught by the same group
of teachers, listened to the same text recorded on the meditation platform, and answered the same questions before and
after daily practice.
Training cohort 3 attended a 3-day retreat structured
similarly to the affect retreat for TC1 and TC2, where
they were introduced to all core exercises of the affect
module including loving-kindness meditation, and to the
ReSource data collection website. Their practice expectations and assessment schedule were identical to TC1 and
TC2, with the exception that they only practiced for one
13-week period.
221
At the end of the final 13-week period, training and assessment ended for all participants in all cohorts, although participants were still able to use the online meditation recordings
and complete the questionnaires if they wished. Follow-up
assessments were completed 4 and 10 months after the end
of training. The results of those assessments will not be
discussed here.
Measures
The ReSource Project involved a wide range of assessments,
with a full list available in Singer et al. (2016). Here, we
analyze changes measured by a battery of quantitative premeditation and post-meditation questions.
In order to reduce demands on the participants’ time, questions were divided into four blocks, with two question blocks
administered each day (see Fig. 1; note that the Bopen
response^ free-writing block is not shown in the figure).
One block of questions (Bfeeling states^) was administered
every day, while the others were presented in counterbalanced
order across the weeks. The same question was asked before
the meditation began and after it ended. Unless otherwise
noted, answers were given using a continuous slider ranging
from 0 (Bnot at all^) to 19 (Bvery much^).
The first question group was comprised of eight items, the
six questions of the cube of thought, which assess the contents
of thought, and two questions concerning attitude toward
thoughts (Ruby et al. 2013). Participants indicated the extent
that their thoughts were about the future, the past, the self, and
others and how positive and negative these thoughts were.
Participants then reported how much they had judged the
thoughts (considering some thoughts good and some bad,
for example) and how sure they were that they had accurately reported the content of their thoughts. These final two
questions (Bjudging thoughts^ and Bsureness^) were not analyzed due to participant reports that those questions were
difficult to understand. The pre-meditation questions concerned thoughts from the 30 min previous to answering the
questions. The post-meditation questions concerned thoughts
during the meditation.
The second question block assessed meta-cognition.
Participants indicated the extent that in the 30 min before
meditating (pre) and during the meditation (post), they felt
Bdistracted by thoughts,^ Boccupied by thoughts,^ and that
their mind was Bbusy^ with thoughts. Participants also indicated how aware they were of having thoughts and how aware
they felt of the contents of their thoughts. The three questions
concerning distraction (distracted, occupied, busy) were averaged to create a Bthought distraction^ score (mean daily α =
0.87, SD = 0.03). The two questions concerning thought
awareness and thought content were averaged to create a
Bthought awareness^ score (mean daily α = 0.77, SD = 0.06).
222
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Fig. 1 Study design, timeline, and day-to-day meditation measures. A
fourth question block requesting that participants free-write about their
mental state was also included in the design but is not discussed here.
Section A adapted with permission from Singer et al. (2016), p. 36,
Figure 4.1, with the timelines for control groups and long-term followups removed
The third question block was comprised of 2 min of freewriting before and after the meditation, where participants
were asked to record their thoughts and feelings as they occurred during the 2-min period. Analyses of these data are not
yet complete.
The questions that were asked every day assessed affect,
present-focused awareness, and interoception. They included
an affect grid where participants reported valence and arousal
just before and just after meditating, using a scale from 0 to 8
for each dimension (Russell et al. 1989). Participants also
reported how warm they felt, how present they felt, and how
aware of their body they felt.
In total, participants who followed the instructions to practice the core meditations 5 days a week would have completed
each of the three question groups 20 times per 3-month period
and completed the fourth daily group 60 times per 3-month
period. Ultimately, we recorded approximately 66,390 measurement points representing over 11,000 h of meditation.
Data Analyses
To test for within-person training effects, a three-level hierarchical linear model was fitted using the nlme package in R for
each variable. Data was structured into measurement days
(level 1), nested within practices (level 2), and nested within
persons (level 3); comparisons between practices occur at level 2 and are within-person. Daily measurements were provided in groups of two, one before the meditation and one afterward. Tests of random effects (available in the online
Supplementary material) revealed that, for all variables in
TC1 and TC2, a three-level model was a better fit to the data
than either a simple linear model or a two-level hierarchical
linear model.
Each model included predictors representing type of meditation practice (practice, a categorical variable with four
levels), whether the measurement took place before or after
practice (post), and their interaction. State effects of practice
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
223
were not moderated by a linear effect for the passage of time,
and thus, the interactions of time with post and practice are not
included in the final models described here; tests of the effect
of time on the magnitude of state change are available in the
Supplemental material. Details concerning the selection and
coding of person-level and day-level covariates (including
time), selection of random effects, contrast coding, the final
model equations, and other model-specific information are
available in the Supplementary material.
To test for between-person sequence effects, we fitted a
two-level hierarchical linear model with time nested within participant for each variable. Each model included predictors representing training cohort (training cohort, a
categorical variable with three levels), whether the measurement took place after practice (post), and their interaction. Details concerning the selection and coding of
person-level and day-level covariates (including time), selection of random effects, contrast coding, the final model
equations, and other model-specific information are available in the Supplementary material.
To explore potential changes in intervariate relationships,
we utilized network analysis, a technique for graphically exploring relationships between multiple variables (Borsboom
and Cramer 2013). Additional details are provided in the
Supplementary material.
Results
Compliance
Participants were asked to practice the core meditation(s) of
the current module at least five times per week, in addition to
the weekly training session. Mean weekly meditation frequencies are shown in Table 1. For a detailed multilevel analysis of
compliance rates by group and practice type over time, see
section 10.2 of Singer et al. (2016). In general, compliance
rates were higher for the presence module than for the other
two modules.
Table 1
Number of meditation sessions per person per week
Meditation
Breathing
Body scan
Loving kindness
Observing thoughts
TC1
TC2
TC3
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
4.89
4.38
3.89
3.57
1.19
1.20
1.14
1.22
4.52
4.34
3.38
3.69
1.25
1.24
1.30
1.24
4.05
SD represents the between-person variance in mean compliance
1.57
Within-Person Analyses
Table 2 provides the p values for the omnibus F tests,
change estimates for each practice, and the contrasts
among the practices as appropriate. Individual practice
changes are also shown in Fig. 2. Information on the
degrees of freedom, F values, and significance tests for
all predictors are included in the Supplemental material.
Models were also run without covariates (age, gender,
media, time, weekend, Christmas), and the results showed
a highly similar pattern of significance. These analyses are
also included in the Supplemental material.
The presence of two cohorts undergoing the same training
program at different times means that TC2 can be interpreted
as a replication of TC1. In light of this, correction for multiple
testing was not used, but only statistically significant changes
and practice differences that are shared across both cohorts
will be interpreted.
While practicing breathing meditation did significantly increase present focus, this change was not greater than for the
other three meditation types. Body awareness also increased
significantly after breathing meditation practice but no more
so than after loving-kindness meditation and observingthought meditation and less than after body scan. Finally, there
was a statistically significant decrease in the amount of
thoughts encompassing three of the six descriptors of thought
content (future, others, negative), and no significant increases
in the amount of thought types for the other three thought
content descriptors, indicating an overall decrease in thought
quantity during breath-focused meditation.
As shown in Fig. 3, the phenomenological space for
breathing meditation was defined by two clusters, one
comprised of meta-cognition, interoceptive awareness,
and present-moment focus and the other linking thoughts
of others to future-oriented, non-self-focused thoughts.
Body scan showed the highest increase in body awareness
of all practices studied; present focus also increased but no
more than for other practices. Body scan was also associated
with a statistically significant decrease in the amount of
thoughts encompassing four of the six descriptors of thought
content (future, past, others, negative), and no significant increases in the amount of thoughts for the other two descriptors
of thought content, indicating an overall decrease in thought
quantity during body scan meditation.
Consistent with similarities in training and goals for the
two practices, the phenomenological space for body scan
is very similar to that of breathing meditation, two clusters, one characterized by links between present focus,
body awareness, thought awareness, and distraction by
thoughts and one representing the structure of non-taskrelated thought (mind-wandering).
As anticipated, during loving-kindness meditation, participants reported the greatest increase in positively valenced
224
Table 2
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Hierarchical linear model-derived state change estimates by training cohort
Characteristic
Future
Training cohort 1
Training cohort 2
Breathing
Body
scan
−1.20a
−2.32
Past
−0.43
Self
Others
−0.09a
−1.76
Positive
Negative
Affect
Energy
Warmth
Present
Body aware
Distraction
Thought
aware
−0.44a
−0.76
0.44a
0.36a
0.56ab
1.74a
1.49a
−1.08a
0.79a
Loving
kindness
a
−0.90
0.16a
−2.90
−0.56a
−1.16a
0.51a
0.77
0.34a
1.79a
2.34
−2.00
−0.17
−1.44a
a
−0.85
0.05a
0.29
0.58
−1.28a
0.39a
0.42a
0.65b
1.57a
1.37a
−1.46b
0.44a
Observing
thoughts
0.18
0.66
0.16a
−0.34
−0.20a
−0.34
0.30
0.45a
0.48ab
1.62a
1.68a
−1.29ab
1.47
p value
Breathing
Body
scan
0
−1.49a
−0.20
0.00a
−1.96
−0.23a
−0.64
0.50a
0.53a
0.51a
1.71a
1.42a
−1.42a
0.56a
0
0.66
0
0
0
0
0
0
0.4
0
0
0
Loving
kindness
Observing
thoughts
p value
−2.46b
−2.00ab
−0.28
0
a
−0.76a
0.42a
0.65
1.03
−1.57
0.51a
0.56ac
0.59a
1.74a
1.47a
−1.64ab
0.71a
0.45
0.17a
−0.45
−0.06a
−0.25
0.52a
0.67bc
0.42ab
1.85a
1.70a
−1.57ab
1.25
0
0.48
0
0
0
0
0.01
0
0.43
0
0.06
0
−0.81
0.13a
−2.64
−0.10a
−1.12
0.65
0.74b
0.29b
1.94a
2.34
−1.88b
0.01
Change estimates significantly different from zero (p < 0.05) are indicated in ital. For each row within each training cohort, values sharing a superscript
are not significantly different from one another (p ≥ 0.05)
thoughts and in fact, were the only group to show statistically
significant positive change in that variable. Regarding otherfocused thoughts, loving-kindness meditation again had the
greatest increase in other-focused thoughts relative to the other
groups, but this positive change was only significant for TC2.
In TC1, this difference was mainly due to large decreases in
other-focused thought in breathing meditation and body scan.
Contrary to our hypothesis, while loving-kindness meditation
was related to increases in subjective warmth, these changes
were no greater than the warmth changes that occurred during
the breathing and observing-thought meditations, although
Fig. 2 Estimates and 95 %
confidence intervals for modelderived state changes in the four
mental training practices of
training cohorts 1 and 2
they were higher than the change that occurred in body scan.
Finally, all four practices demonstrated statistically significant
increases in positivity of affect, but the increase associated
with loving-kindness meditation was not reliably larger than
in the other practices as had been hypothesized.
In loving-kindness meditation, changes in the phenomenological space are divided into three clusters. Awareness of
thoughts and thought distraction are now distinct from awareness of the body and the present moment, the latter now
linking to feelings of warmth. Finally, the thought content
cluster is consistent with the goal of loving-kindness
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
225
Fig. 3 Partial correlation
structures for each mental training
practice combined across TC1
and TC2. Amount of change is
represented by relative circle size
and corresponds to the estimates
in Table 2. Paths between circles
represent the significant partial
correlations, with solid lines
representing positive and
dashed lines representing
negative correlations. Variables
without significant correlations
are not shown
meditation linking positive thoughts to self- and otheroriented thoughts and negatively to a cluster of negatively
valenced, past-related thoughts.
As anticipated, observing-thought meditation led to the
largest statistically significant increase in thought awareness of all four practices. Observing thoughts also significantly decreased distraction by thoughts; however, this
change was not statistically different from the change in
thought distraction caused by breathing meditation or
loving-kindness meditation. In TC1, body scan practice
caused participants to be even less distracted by thoughts
than observing-thought meditation.
Observing thoughts led to four small, narrowly defined
clusters. One cluster now relates affect and energy; the second
past-negative thoughts; the third future-other related thoughts;
and the last, similar to loving-kindness meditation, links body
awareness, present focus, and warmth.
All four practices showed significant increases in positivity of affect, present focus, ability to avoid being distracted by thoughts, energy, and body awareness. BSelflessness^
is an additional pursuit often associated with meditation.
However, in both TC1 and TC2, there was no significant
change in the amount of self-related thoughts for any of the
four practices.
Between-Person Analyses
Figure 4 shows the differences between the training cohorts in
the effects of loving-kindness meditation, reflecting a significant effect for interaction of group and practice. Information
on the significance tests for all coefficients is included in the
Supplemental material. Models were also run without covariates (age, gender, media, time, weekend), and the results
showed a highly similar pattern of significance. These analyses are also included in the Supplemental material.
For the majority of variables, there was no difference between groups in the amount of change occurring due to
loving-kindness meditation. Critically, even in the absence
of 3 months of attention training, loving-kindness meditation
still increased present focus, body awareness, thought awareness, and ability to disengage from distracting thoughts just as
much as in other training cohorts, despite the fact that these
changes are more closely associated with presence training
than with loving-kindness meditation.
226
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Fig. 4 Model-derived state
change estimates for lovingkindness meditation for training
cohorts 1, 2, and 3. The error bars
represent the person-level
standard errors. The stars
represent change significantly
different from zero
Some unanticipated differences between cohorts did appear. Participants who did not receive preparatory presence
training, but did the affect module immediately, showed a
significantly smaller decrease in negative thoughts during
meditation compared to the other groups. These participants
also showed the highest increase in positivity of affect and the
greatest increase in subjective warmth.
The pattern of intervariate relationships for lovingkindness meditation without the presence module (shown in
Fig. 5) differed somewhat from the pattern for lovingkindness meditation with presence training. While thoughts
of others were still associated with positively valenced
thoughts and decreased negatively valenced thoughts, the relationship between thought awareness and present-focus was
different. In the original two cohorts, there was no link
between thought awareness and present focus in lovingkindness meditation, although they were positively linked
in body scan and breathing meditations. Here, they are
negatively linked through changes in energy, suggesting that
in the absence of presence training, participants found it difficult to sustain awareness of their thoughts and the present
moment simultaneously.
Discussion
The purpose of the present work was to compare the differential psychological effects of daily practice of four different
types of meditation included in the Resource Project, a 9month long longitudinal study of mental training (Singer
et al. 2016). We specifically focused on identifying patterns
of training-related change in experienced affect, thought content, meta-cognition, and body awareness. Training consisted
of three 13-week modules (presence, affect, perspective), each
comprised of two core practices that targeted attentional,
socio-affective, and socio-cognitive functions, respectively.
Two of the core practices were dyadic in nature, and their
effects will be discussed elsewhere. The other four were
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Fig. 5 Partial correlation structures for each TC3. Amount of change is
represented by relative circle size. Paths between circles represent the
significant partial correlations, with solid lines representing positive and
dashed lines representing negative correlations. Variables without
significant correlations are not shown
single-person meditations, breathing meditation and body
scan (presence), loving-kindness meditation (affect), and
observing-thought meditation (perspective). Training modules
were matched in the time required, the learning environment,
the structure and length of retreats, the frequency of practice,
the availability of support through the online platform and a
smartphone app, and teacher investment. We expected differential effects of these types of mental practices on subjective
outcome measures assessed before and after daily practice of a
given meditation.
As hypothesized, each of the four mental practices showed
unique Bmental fingerprints^ of change. These fingerprints
were expressed as differential training-related effects on selfreported measures of affect, cognition, and body and metacognitive awareness, as well as differences in the structure
of the relationships among variables as illustrated by the network analyses.
Specifically, breathing meditation, trained during the presence module, decreased the overall number of thoughts and
the tendency to be distracted by thoughts and increased present focus, interoceptive awareness, positivity of affect,
warmth, and energy. The fingerprint of body scan, also trained
during the presence module, was similar to that of breathing
meditation. Participants decreased in overall number of
thoughts and the tendency to be distracted by thoughts and
increased present focus, positivity of affect, and energy. Body
scan led to the highest increase in body awareness of all four
practices. Network analyses of breathing meditation and body
227
scan revealed two main clusters, the first linking awareness of
thought and distraction by thoughts to body awareness and
present focus and the second linking increased thoughts about
others and thoughts about the future to decreased thoughts
about the self. This suggests that participants may have used
body focus and present-moment focus as a way of coping with
distraction by thoughts. Such a strategy is congruent with the
training goal to increase awareness of all present-moment experience, including bodily sensations and thoughts. The second cluster could be interpreted as Bmind-wandering,^ as attention is diverted from the self (and the breathing or bodily
sensations that are the target of the meditations) toward others
and potential future or past events.
Loving-kindness meditation led to expected increases
in positively valenced thoughts, a change not observed for
the other three practices. Other-focused thoughts also increased relative to the other practices, but only training
cohort 1 showed a statistically significant absolute increase in other-focused thought. Feelings of subjective
warmth and positivity of affect also increased but no more
than for the other practices. As with all other practices,
participants also increased present focus, interoceptive
awareness, and energy. This mental pattern maintained
both for loving-kindness meditation learned in sequence
with other modules and loving-kindness meditation taught
without other modules. Loving-kindness meditation without
other modules, however, was more effective at enhancing
warmth and positivity of affect.
Follow-up network analyses further differentiated lovingkindness meditation from other practices. Changes in metacognition were uncoupled from changes in interoceptive
awareness and present focus, suggesting a shift from cultivating general attention to domain-specific attention.
Furthermore, body awareness and present focus were not only
positively related to each other but also to warmth. Finally,
positively valenced thoughts were related to thinking about
self and others, and this thought pattern was negatively associated with negatively valenced, past-focused thoughts. This
last Bthought cluster^ is consistent with the instructions of
loving-kindness meditation to visualize loved others and extend good wishes of loving kindness and happiness to them
(Salzberg 1995). It further suggests that the more participants
think positive thoughts of self and others, the more ruminative
thoughts (negative and past-focused) are suppressed.
Interestingly, the uncoupling of meta-cognition and
interoception in loving-kindness meditation occurred only
when loving-kindness meditation was taught after the presence or perspective modules. When the affect module was
taught in isolation, meta-cognition and interoception were
positively associated, suggesting that this dissociation is not
inherent to loving-kindness meditation but may result from
approaching loving-kindness meditation with already
established meditation skills.
228
Finally, observing-thought meditation, trained during the
perspective module, led as expected to the highest increase
in meta-cognitive awareness of thoughts. Participants were
also less distracted by thoughts but no more so than for other
practices. The goal of observing-thought meditation is to refine the ability to categorize and observe thoughts without
reacting, as well as to cultivate awareness of the thought process. This is manifest in the network analysis of many small,
separate clusters of conceptually linked variables, indicating a
nuanced approach to thoughts similar to what is trained in
MBCT (Fjorback et al. 2011; Strauss et al. 2014; Williams
et al. 2014). The cluster of affect and energy represent the
participant’s subjective state, while the past/negative cluster
may reflect a common ruminative pattern and the future/
other cluster has been linked to planning (Ruby et al. 2013).
The body awareness and present-focus cluster is also conceptually consistent, although the inclusion of warmth is unexpected. Finally, thought awareness and thought distraction are
uncoupled in observing-thought meditation, suggesting that
participants become more proficient in distinguishing between
meta-cognitive awareness of thoughts and being distracted by
thoughts. As for the other practices, participants also increased
in present focus, interoceptive awareness, positivity of affect,
warmth, and energy. Interestingly, no practice significantly
changed the amount of self-related thought experienced by
participants.
In summary, the following effects uniquely defined each of
the four core meditative practices: body scan led to the greatest
increase in interoceptive awareness, loving-kindness meditation was best in increasing positively valenced and otherfocused thought and was the only practice to positively link
thoughts of self and others, and observing-thought meditation
was most effective in increasing meta-cognitive awareness of
thoughts and resulted in the highest number of distinguishable
thought-content clusters. There were no unique effects for
breathing meditation, which is used as a basic practice in
many contemplative traditions.
The fingerprints observed for the meditative practices, both
in individual variable changes and in distinct networks of related intervariate change, provide validation for the idea that the
type of meditation matters. The choice of what type of meditation to engage in, or what to focus on within a particular type of
meditation, has experiential consequences. Other studies have
shown that repeated subjective experiences such as anxiety
(Kubzansky et al. 2006) or positive affect (Fredrickson et al.
2008; Kok et al. 2013) can have significant and far-reaching
consequences for life, health, and longevity. In such a context,
meditation may prove a critical tool in helping to Bshift^ the
tone of subjective experiences away from those known to be
noxious and toward more salubrious states.
Our findings have a number of every day and clinical implications. By demonstrating distinct subjective Bfingerprints^
for four types of meditation taught in a shared context and
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
assessed using a shared question pool, we provide clear evidence that some forms of meditation may be better suited to
certain professions or patient populations than others. The
fingerprints revealed here may help practitioners to choose a
meditative practice that is the best Bmatch^ for the current
needs of their client. In addition, the overlapping effects
shared between practices also suggest that in some cases, the
choice of meditation can be guided by individual preference
without compromising the efficacy of the chosen practice.
Our findings also have implications for researchers who
may wish to study the effects of repeated subjective experiences. Challenges in such research include attempting to
reliably and regularly induce the desired subjective experience and finding an effective matched control. Additional
analyses, reported in the Supplemental material, suggest
that the magnitude of change is relatively consistent over
time, indicating a lack of habituation. Furthermore, since
the state effects of breathing meditation appear to be a subset of the effects found in the other three practices, pairing
breathing meditation with body scan, loving-kindness meditation, or observing-thought meditation would allow the
researcher to observe the long-term effects of repeatedly
experiencing interoceptive awareness, positive other focus,
or meta-cognitive awareness, respectively.
While practice-specific hypotheses were mostly confirmed
for breathing meditation, body scan, loving-kindness meditation, and observing-thought meditation, there was also a substantial amount of un-hypothesized overlap between the different meditations. All practices left participants feeling happier, more energized, more present in the moment, more aware
of their bodies, and better able to disengage from distracting
thoughts.
Some of the shared or overlapping effects may be attributed
to continuing practice in directing, re-focusing, and sustaining
attention to a given object in the present moment, be it the
breath, parts of the body, a mental image, or a mental event
like a thought. Learning to focus attention and stay in the
present moment, detect when attention has wandered, and
return attention to the original target are fundamental to all
contemplative practices (Kabat-Zinn 1990), and a muscle is
exercised by all four mental training exercises studied here.
The analogy may be to a runner learning the geography of a
city; depending on where the runner goes, she will learn about
a different neighborhood, but no matter where she runs, her
muscular and cardiovascular system will show a similar pattern of development over time. In the same way, different
meditative practices can simultaneously offer unique benefits
while sharing a common underlying pattern of gains.
Other effects, such as the improvements in mood and energy, could be due to the pleasant effects of learning a new
self-care skill. It could be also argued, since participants in
training cohorts 1 and 2 received breathing meditation and
body scan training before learning the other two practices, that
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
these apparent Bcommonalities^ might in actuality represent
carryover effects from the previous module. We were, however, able to investigate such sequence effects; TC1 and TC2,
where loving-kindness meditation was learned after the presence module, were compared to TC3, where loving-kindness
meditation was taught without any prior training. As described
above, we found few differences and many commonalities. In
particular, the increases in present focus, interoceptive awareness, and energy, and decreases in distraction by thoughts, did
not differ significantly across the affect modules in TC1, TC2,
and TC3. This suggests that cultivating bodily awareness and
present focus is a core common feature of our four meditative
practices (or their context), as is feeling more positive and
energized after contemplative practice of any kind.
We have repeatedly referred to the richness of this data due
to the intensive measurement strategy of the project. While the
consistent and extensive measurement of participants’ subjective states is a strength of this work, it retains the weaknesses
common to subjective self-report measures. The project was
designed to account for these weaknesses; asking participants
to report their mental state in the moment eliminates retrospective biasing of responses, and recording answers using sliding
scales rather than discrete values reduces the ability to repeat a
particular response from memory. However, self-report remains
only as valid as the participant’s own ability to introspect.
Collecting many repeated measurements from each participant,
and using multilevel modeling to identify overall trends, helps
to separate the Bsignal^ of real effects from the Bnoise^ of error.
Similarly, the within-person nature of much of the ReSource
Project design is a strength that allows inferences about intrapersonal change due to meditation. However, within-person
designs are vulnerable to sequence effects. In the case of the
two main training cohorts (TC1 and TC2) of the ReSource
Project, the presence module was always taught first because
it laid the foundation for the other two modules, affect and
perspective. As practice compliance decreased over time, differences between the presence practices and the other two core
practices (loving-kindness meditation in affect and observingthought meditation in perspective) could in part be attributed to
higher compliance rates in the presence module. Differences
between breathing meditation and body scan, however, would
not be affected by changes in compliance. Furthermore, the
counterbalanced training sequence for the affect and perspective modules across TC1 and TC2 controls for the potential
effects of compliance differences for those two modules.
Finally, participants in TC1 and TC2 underwent 9 months
of meditation training, a much longer period than is typical in
meditation research. A longer training period is useful for
observing whether the effectiveness of the various practices
changes over time. Training for three quarters of a year, however, means that the three modules were inevitably taught in
noticeably different seasons, with accompanying differences
in temperature, amount of sunlight, day length, etc. Seasonal
229
effects could be potentially confounded with differences in
training content. However, training in TC2 began 2 months
after TC1. In combination with the counterbalanced order of
the perspective and affect modules, the offset group design
ensures that none of the training modules were taught twice
in the same season. In addition, as shown in the Supplemental
material, there were no differences in the magnitude of state
changes for the two presence practices between TC1 and TC2,
despite being taught in different seasons. Thus, we conclude
that the effects of the training were not season-dependent.
Seasonal and compliance differences are confounding factors
in the between-person design used to investigate sequence effects
in the affect module across the three training cohorts. The affect
modules taught in TC1, TC2, and TC3 differ in sequence, season, and compliance rates. Thus, any differences between the
modules could potentially be due, in whole or in part, to nontraining-related differences. It is all the more surprising, therefore,
that almost no group differences were found; the affect module
training thus appears to be robust against variation in seasons and
compliance rates as observed in this study.
In conclusion, the unique fingerprints associated with different types of contemplative mental training reveal that meditation is not about engaging in indiscriminate mental effort;
each practice appears to create a distinct mental environment,
the long-term consequences of which are only beginning to be
explored.
Acknowledgments Open access funding provided by Max Planck
Society. We are thankful to all the members of the Department of Social
Neuroscience involved in the ReSource study over the years, in particular
to all ReSource teachers that taught the intervention program, to Astrid
Ackermann, Christina Bochow, Matthias Bolz, and Sandra Zurborg for
managing the large-scale longitudinal study; to Hannes Niederhausen,
Henrik Grunert, and Torsten Kästner for their technical support; and to
Sylvia Tydeks, Elisabeth Murzick, Manuela Hofmann, Sylvie Neubert,
and Nicole Pampus for their help with recruitment and data collection.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Funding Tania Singer, as principal investigator, received funding for
the ReSource Project from (a) the European Research Council under the
European Community’s Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007–2013/
ERC Grant Agreement Number 205557 to T.S.) and (b) from the Max
Planck Society.
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict
of interest.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link
to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
230
References
Borsboom, D., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2013). Network analysis: An integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of
Clinical Psychology, 9(9), 91–121. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy050212-185608.
Carter, O. L., Presti, D. E., Callistemon, C., Ungerer, Y., Liu, G. B., &
Pettigrew, J. D. (2005). Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in
Tibetan Buddhist monks. Current Biology, 15(11), R412–R413.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.043.
Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for
stress management in healthy people: A review and meta-analysis.
Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 15(5), 593–
600. doi:10.1089/acm.2008.0495.
Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological
and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological
Medicine, 40(8), 1239–1252. doi:10.1017/S0033291709991747.
Feldman, G., Greeson, J., & Senville, J. (2010). Differential effects of
mindful breathing, progressive muscle relaxation, and lovingkindness meditation on decentering and negative reactions to
repetitive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10),
1002–1011. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006.
Fjorback, L. O., & Walach, H. (2012). Meditation based therapies—a
systematic review and some critical observations. Religions, 3(1),
1–18. doi:10.3390/Rel3010001.
Fjorback, L. O., Arendt, M., Ornbol, E., Fink, P., & Walach, H. (2011).
Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy—a systematic review of randomized controlled trials.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 124(2), 102–119. doi:10.1111
/j.1600-0447.2011.01704.x.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M.
(2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through
loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1045–1062.
doi:10.1037/a0013262.
Hart, W. (1987). The art of living: Vipassana-meditation as taught by S.N.
Goenka. San Francisco: Harper and Row.
Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, P. J., Bell, I., &
Schwartz, G. E. R. (2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: Effects on distress,
positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of
Behavioral Medicine, 33(1), 11–21. doi:10.1207/s15324796
abm3301_2.
Johnson, D. P., Penn, D. L., Fredrickson, B. L., Kring, A. M., Meyer, P.
S., Catalino, L. I., & Brantley, M. (2011). A pilot study of lovingkindness meditation for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Research, 129(2–3), 137–140. doi:10.1016/j.
schres.2011.02.015.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your
body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delacorte
Press.
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2013). Functional
neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cerebral Cortex, 23(7), 1552–1561. doi:10.1093
/cercor/bhs142.
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2014). Differential
pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
9(6), 873–879. doi:10.1093/Scan/Nst060.
Kok, B.E., Coffey, K.A., Cohn, M.A., Catalino, L.I., Vacharkulksemsuk,
T., Algoe, S., …Fredrickson, B.L. (2013). How positive emotions
build physical health: perceived positive social connections account
for the upward spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone.
Psychological Science, 24(7), 1123–32. doi: 10.1177
/0956797612470827
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Kubzansky, L.D., Cole, S.R., Kawachi, I., Vokonas, P., Sparrow,
D. (2006). Shared and unique contributions of anger, anxiety,
and depression to coronary heart disease: a prospective study
in the normative aging study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,
31(1), 21–29.
Kuyken, W., Hayes, R., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Dalgleish, T., Kessler, D.,
… Byford, S. (2015). Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy compared with maintenance
antidepressant treatment in the prevention of depressive relapse or
recurrence (PREVENT): A randomised controlled trial. Lancet,
386(9988), 63–73. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(14)62222-4.
Lang, A. J., Strauss, J. L., Bomyea, J., Bormann, J. E., Hickman, S. D.,
Good, R. C., & Essex, M. (2012). The theoretical and empirical
basis for meditation as an intervention for PTSD. Behavior
Modification, 36(6), 759–786. doi:10.1177/0145445512441200.
Louchakova-Schwartz, O. (2013). Cognitive phenomenology in the
study of Tibetan meditation: phenomenological descriptions versus
meditation styles. Neurophenomenology and Its Applications to
Psychology, 61–87. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-7239-1_3
Lutz, A., & Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology—integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies,
10(9–10), 31–52.
Lutz, A., Slagter, H.A., Dunne, J.D., Davidson, R.J. (2008). Attention
regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.
MacCoon, D.G., Imel, Z.E., Rosenkranz, M.A., Sheftel, J.G., Weng,
H.Y., Sullivan, J.C., … Lutz, A. (2012). The validation of an active
control intervention for Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction
(MBSR). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(1), 3–12.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.011
Mirams, L., Poliakoff, E., Brown, R. J., & Lloyd, D. M. (2013).
Brief body-scan meditation practice improves somatosensory
perceptual decision making. Consciousness and Cognition,
22, 348–359.
Obasi, C.N., Brown, R., Ewers, T., Barlow, S., Gassman, M., Zgierska,
A., … Barrett, B. (2013). Advantage of meditation over exercise in
reducing cold and flu illness is related to improved function and
quality of life. Influenza and Other Respiratory Viruses, 7(6), 938–
944. doi: 10.1111/Irv.12053
Olatunji, B. O., Naragon-Gainey, K., & Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2013).
Specificity of rumination in anxiety and depression: A multimodal
meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice, 20(3),
225–257. doi:10.1111/Cpsp.12037.
Ospina, M.B., Bond, T.K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L., Vandermeer, B.,
Liang, Y., … Klassen, T.P. (2007). Meditation practices for health:
State of the research. Evidence report/technology assessment No.
155. Rockville, MD
Ospina, M.B., Bond, K., Karkhaneh, M., Buscemi, N., Dryden, D.M.,
Barnes, V., … Shannahoff-Khalsa, D. (2008). Clinical trials of meditation practices in health care: Characteristics and quality. Journal
of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14(10), 1199–1213.
doi:10.1089/acm.2008.0307
Ruby, F.J.M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., Singer, T. (2013). How selfgenerated thought shapes mood—the relation between mindwandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of
thoughts. PLoS One, 8(10). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077554
Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid—a
single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 57(3), 493–502. doi:10.1037/00223514.57.3.493.
Sakyong, M. (2003). Turning the mind into an ally. New York: Riverhead
Books.
Salzberg, S. (1995). Loving kindness: The revolutionary art of happiness.
Boston: Shambhala Publications.
Mindfulness (2017) 8:218–231
Salzberg, S. (2005). The force of kindness: Change your life with love and
compassion. Louisville: Sounds True, Inc.
Sauer-Zavala, S. E., Walsh, E. C., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., & Lykins,
E. L. B. (2013). Comparing mindfulness-based intervention
strategies: Differential effects of sitting meditation, body scan,
and mindful yoga. Mindfulness, 4(4), 383–388. doi:10.1007
/s12671-012-0139-9.
Singer, T., & Klimecki, O.M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Current
Biology, 24(18), R875-R878. Retrieved from <Go to ISI>://WOS:
000342396900018
Singer, T., Kok, B.E., Bornemann, B., Zuborg, S., Bolz, M., Bochow,
C.A. (2016). The ReSource project: Background, design, samples,
231
and measurements (2nd ed.). Leipzig, Germany: Max Planck
Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences.
Strauss, C., Cavanagh, K., Oliver, A., Pettman, D. (2014). Mindfulnessbased interventions for people diagnosed with a current episode of
an anxiety or depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of randomised
controlled trials. PLoS One, 9(4). doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0096110
Williams, J.M.G., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Brennan, K., Duggan, D.S.,
Fennell, M.J.V., … Russell, I.T. (2014). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for preventing relapse in recurrent depression: A randomized dismantling trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 82(2), 275–286. doi: 10.1037/A0035036