HHS Public Access
Author manuscript
Author Manuscript
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Published in final edited form as:
Child Dev. 2015 May ; 86(3): 800–811. doi:10.1111/cdev.12340.
Infants Learn Baby Signs From Video
Shoshana Dayanim and Laura L. Namy
Emory University
Abstract
Author Manuscript
There is little evidence that infants learn from infant-oriented educational videos and television
programming. This four week longitudinal experiment investigated 15-month-olds’ (N=92) ability
to learn ASL signs (e.g., patting head for hat) from at-home viewing of instructional video, either
with or without parent support, compared to traditional parent instruction and a no-exposure
control condition. Forced choice, elicited production, and parent report measures indicate learning
across all three exposure conditions, with a trend towards more robust learning in the parent
support conditions, regardless of medium. There were no differences between experimental and
control conditions in the acquisition of corresponding verbal labels. This constitutes the first
experimental evidence of infants’ ability to learn expressive communication from commercially
available educational videos.
Keywords
Author Manuscript
Infant Media; Infant Learning; Baby Signs
Author Manuscript
High quality educational television programming can be an effective source of learning in
preschool- and school-aged children (e.g., Anderson, Huston, Schmitt, Linebarger & Wright,
2001; Naigles & Mayeux, 2001; Rice, Huston, Truglio, & Wright, 1990). However,
evidence supporting infants’ learning from purportedly educational videos is more
equivocal. Several studies suggest a negative correlation between overall media exposure
and measures of communicative development in infants under the age of 2 (Chonchaiya &
Pruksananonda, 2008; Linebarger & Walker, 2005; Zimmerman, Christakis, & Meltzoff,
2007a), leading the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP; 2011) to renew their original
recommendation (AAP, 1999) to avoid exposing children under 2 to television. Despite this
recommendation, an estimated 90% of parents show television and videos to their infants
(Zimmerman, Christakis, & Meltzoff, 2007b) including many “educational” videos targeting
infants. Videos purporting to promote infants’ vocabulary and communicative development,
in particular, have saturated the market (Vaala, et al. 2010).
Few studies support the notion that educational videos can facilitate language acquisition in
infancy. There is limited evidence that repeated exposure via video augments infants’
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shoshana Dayanim, Department of Graduate Psychology, Keiser
University Graduate School, c/o 1380 Jody Lane, Atlanta, GA 30329. Contact: sdayanim@keiseruniversity.edu.
Shoshana Dayanim, Department of Psychology, Emory University.
Laura Namy, Department of Psychology, Emory University.
Shoshana Dayanim is now at Department of Psychology, Keiser University.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 2
Author Manuscript
acquisition of words also heard in routine input, relative to infants who encounter the words
in routine input alone (Lemish & Rice, 1986; Vandewater, 2011). Vandewater, Park, Lee,
and Barr (2010) have also found that repeatedly pairing words and shapes over a period of
15 days enables toddlers as young as 18 months to identify which shapes correspond to
particular words.
There is, however, strong evidence that even when infants learn from video, there is a
“video deficit” (Anderson & Pompek, 2005; Schmitt & Anderson, 2002) –attenuated
learning relative to learning from live, interactive instruction (e.g., Barr & Hayne, 1999;
Krcmar, Grela, & Lin, 2007). Furthermore, numerous studies report a failure to exhibit
heightened learning of words introduced through a video medium in children younger than
24 months (see DeLoache et al, 2007; Krcmar, 2011; Robb, Richert, & Wartella, 2009).
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
This debate also extends to whether parental involvement in the viewing experience
enhances learning. There is compelling evidence that parent co-viewing enhances learning
from television in preschoolers (e.g. Reiser, Williamson, & Suzuki, 1988; Roseberry, HirshPasek, Parish-Morris & Golinkoff., 2009; Singer & Singer, 1998) and increases attention
and verbal interactions during viewing in infants (Barr, Zack, Muentener & Garcia, 2008;
Fender, Richert, Robb & Wartella, 2010; Fidler, Zack & Barr, 2010; Lemish, 1987). These
consequences of parent co-viewing may enhance the depth of infants’ cognitive processing
of the video stimuli (Strouse, O’Doherty & Troseth, 2013; Strouse & Troseth, 2014).
However, recent longitudinal experimental studies (DeLoache et al., 2010; Robb et al.,
2009) reported no evidence of heightened word-learning in an infant co-viewing condition,
relative to viewing-alone or controls. Despite this lack of evidence that parent co-viewing
augments learning from television in infants, the AAP (2011) revised their guidelines to
suggest that if infant viewing is inevitable, co-viewing is better than allowing an infant to
view alone.
Although previous longitudinal studies of infants’ learning from videos have reported null
effects, those studies focused exclusively on the acquisition of high frequency words to
which infants are exposed regularly outside the video viewing environment. Because
exposure to the target stimuli was not fully controlled, there was evidence of word learning
across conditions, including control conditions (e.g. DeLoache et al., 2010; Robb et al.,
2009).
Author Manuscript
In the current longitudinal experiment, we asked whether better-controlled exposure
delivered exclusively through video would lead to any evidence of learning relative to a no
exposure control condition, and if so whether there was evidence of a video deficit, a benefit
for parental co-viewing or both. To ensure experimental control of exposure, we
investigated infants’ ability to learn symbolic gestures, or baby signs instead of focusing on
word learning. Baby signs also offer better experimental control of exposure because they
are not routinely employed in everyday interactions with infants. Thus, by using signs in
place of spoken words, we are able to assess the independent impact of video exposure on
learning more directly. Baby signs are a strong test case because infants begin using words
and signs at around the same time, shortly after their first birthdays (see, e.g., Acredolo &
Goodwyn, 1988) and appear to use them for the same communicative purposes (Namy,
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 3
Author Manuscript
2001; Namy & Waxman, 1998). The availability of numerous educational videos marketed
towards infants that offer sign instruction enables us to systematically and ecologically
investigate the impact of medium (video versus traditional instruction) and parental
involvement (co-viewing versus alone) on infant learning.
We investigated 15-month-olds’ learning of baby signs from at-home viewing of
commercially available videos over the course of three weeks of exposure and also tested
retention following one week without exposure. Acquisition of baby signs when viewing the
videos alone or co-viewing with parents was compared to traditional parent instruction and
to a no-exposure control condition.
Method
Author Manuscript
Participants
Ninety-Two 15-month-olds (M =15.17 months at study onset, range = 13.95 to 16.81,
SD=7.04; 51 males) were recruited from the greater [location] area. The sample included
approximately 74% Caucasian, 18% African-American, 1% Asian, 1% Native Hawaiian or
Other Pacific Islander, and 6% Mixed Ethnicities with 8% identifying as being of Hispanic
or Latino descent.
Inclusion criteria included exposure to videos or screen media prior to recruitment contact,
and lack of prior exposure to “baby signs”. Previous exposure to screen media was required
to avoid inducing parents who were not already doing so to violate the AAP’s
recommendation. No parents contacted were excluded for this criterion. Parents’ informed
consent included acknowledgement of the AAP’s recommendation to avoid television
exposure for infants under the age of two.
Author Manuscript
Stimuli
We identified 18 target signs to use in the experiment. We selected only target signs that
were object names to accommodate the use of still photos of referents in learning
assessments, and to mirror the types of labels (both verbal and gestural) most frequently
acquired at this age. Target signs were selected based on familiarity of their referents as
indexed by age of comprehension of their verbal labels (comprehended at a threshold of
50% of infants by M= 12.3 months; Fenson, et al., 1994). These items included: airplane,
apple, baby, ball, banana, bear, bird, book, car, cat, cookie, cracker, dog, fish, flower, hat,
juice, and shoe. All referents were depicted in at least three different commercially available
video productions intended to teach baby sign to infants.
Author Manuscript
Participants assigned to video-viewing conditions received a DVD compilation derived from
six commercially available videos intended to teach infants baby signs. Each compilation
included three chapters approximately 20 minutes in length with footage sampled from 3 to
5 videos in each chapter. Each chapter included clips depicting the signs (and accompanying
verbal labels) for each of the 18 objects. Parents were directed to rotate through the chapters
across viewings to vary the order in which exposure to the signs occurred. We have opted
not to identify the titles of the videos, as we did not seek to test the efficacy of individual
video productions. We were interested in assessing the impact of exposure to varying range
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 4
Author Manuscript
of commercially available videos. By declining to identify the video productions employed,
we avoid any potential opportunities for the products’ marketers to make claims that may or
may not be warranted based on this composite evidence.
There was variability in the formal features employed across the videos. These included 1)
how each sign was introduced (e.g. introduction of sign occurs while an image of the
referent was present versus sequential presentation of the sign and the referent, 2) whether
foreground or background music was employed, 3) whether still or moving images of the
referents were presented, 4) how many scene changes were involved for each sign (ranging
from 6 to approximately 21), and 5) the number of times the sign was repeated (ranging
from approximate 3 to 15).
Author Manuscript
Although these formal features varied across productions, there were also several common
factors across all sampled videos. Each video introduced signs in the first or second scene by
showing an engaging person (either adult or child) producing a sign while simultaneously
speaking the English verbal label for the referent. This introduction of the sign was
consistently followed by several images of the referent, followed by a repetition of the sign
and verbal label. For example, the video might depict an adult producing a sign for “dog”
while saying “dog.” This would be followed by images of various types of dogs (real and
toy dogs, still and moving pictures). Each clip concluded with the adult or child once again
saying and signing “dog.”
Author Manuscript
Parents in the parent instruction condition introduced their infants to the 18 signs using a
lab-designed picture book in lieu of a video. Each page in the book was dedicated to one
target sign (for a total of 18 pages) and included three different still photographs for each
target. The stills were taken directly from screen shots included on the videos. A thumbnail
picture of an adult producing the target sign with arrows signaling directions of motion was
included in the top corner of each page to remind parents how to produce the sign. Appendix
A depicts sample pages from the book.
Each parent in the parental instruction condition also received a set of printed instructional
materials on sign production including still frames from the sign videos demonstrating the
sign being performed and pictures of the referents being labeled. These still photos were
augmented by separate printed verbal instructions describing how to perform each sign. The
experimenter also demonstrated the signs to the parents in person during their baseline visit
to the lab.
Procedure
Author Manuscript
Infants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: video viewing alone (n = 20),
video co-viewing with a parent (n = 27), a parent instruction condition that involved
teaching signs using a picture book (n=21), and a no sign exposure control condition (n=24).
Seven additional infants who dropped out after one or two weeks were excluded from the
study analysis (2 each in video alone, parent instruction and control groups; 1 in the video
co-viewing group). In all three experimental groups, parents were instructed to expose
infants to 15–20 minutes of sign instruction at home 4 days a week for 3 weeks, with no
exposure to signs between instructional sessions. Given that the children tend to acquire the
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 5
Author Manuscript
verbal labels for the 18 objects early in development, we expected children in all conditions
to exhibit learning of the verbal labels for these objects due to routine, incidental exposure.
As a result, we did not necessarily expect condition effects in word learning, despite the
enhanced exposure to both words and signs for these referents resulting from participation in
the study.
Video viewing groups—Parents in the video alone condition were instructed not to
interact with their child during viewing sessions. Those in the co-viewing condition were
instructed to watch with their child as they typically would at home and were told they could
engage in any of the following behaviors: directing their child’s attention to the screen,
imitating signs, and eliciting sign production from the child during viewing. Although such
mediated (i.e., interactive and responsive) co-viewing was encouraged, it was not mandated.
Author Manuscript
Parent instruction group—Parents in the parent instruction condition were instructed to
introduce their infants to the 18 signs using the picture book. Parents were encouraged to
teach their infants signs as they might teach new words from picture books at home and to
point to the photos and use verbal labels as well as signs. They were asked to limit
instruction time to a maximum of 20 minutes per day, four days a week, to match exposure
in the video viewing conditions.
Author Manuscript
Parents in all conditions were instructed to avoid using or imitating signs outside of the
viewing or instructional sessions. If infants signed between instructional sessions, parents
were asked to acknowledge the sign verbally and not to imitate it themselves. No
instructions were specified regarding the use of verbal labels for the target objects outside
the viewing environment. Parents were asked to complete a diary at home documenting the
date of each instructional session and noting if they saw their child producing a sign during a
session or using a sign appropriately in-between sessions. After completing three weeks of
sign instruction, parents were directed not to expose their infants to any signs for one week
prior to returning to the lab at the end of the fourth week.
Learning Assessments
Children and parents in all conditions visited the lab weekly for four weeks for a total of five
visits including baseline intake, to complete sign learning assessments. Learning
assessments included a weekly parent report checklist, a weekly forced choice
comprehension task, and a single elicited production task administered at the fifth visit after
a week without at-home exposure.
Author Manuscript
Parent report—Parents indicated weekly on a vocabulary checklist whether their infants
comprehended or produced appropriately each of the 18 target signs. They also indicated
whether their infants comprehended or produced the verbal labels corresponding to the
signs. (See Appendix B).
Forced choice—The lab-based forced choice task was designed to measure sign
comprehension. The experimenter, who sat across a table from the infant, placed photos of
two objects from the stimulus set (e.g., airplane and dog) on the table and produced the sign
for one of them (e.g., the airplane sign), asking the child, “Can you get it?” To minimize
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 6
Author Manuscript
fatigue, the experimenter administered 6 trials at each lab visit with the target items
randomly selected. Across sessions, all items were tested at least once. Objects in each
picture pair were matched for salience based on pilot testing with 15-month-olds (n = 19)
who did not participate in the experiment proper. During piloting, we selected pairings we
believed were well matched for salience and presented them to the infants, asking them to
“get one”. Any pairings that elicited a bias towards selecting one object were altered by
pairing more salient objects from one pair with more salient objects from another pair and
re-administering the choice elicitation with additional infants to ensure that infants did not
exhibit default systematic preferences within any pairing.
Author Manuscript
Experimenters were generally blind to condition assignment, although conversations with
parents occasionally inadvertently revealed to which condition the infant was assigned.
Experimenters were instructed to ensure that the two picture cards and sign production were
equidistant from the child, and to ensure that their eye gaze while eliciting a choice
remained fixed on the infant’s face. Coders were also blind to condition.
Elicited production—On their final visit, in addition to completing the checklist and
forced-choice comprehension task, infants in the experimental conditions also completed an
elicited sign production task as a conservative test of learning and retention. We also
administered the elicited production task to 5 infants in the no exposure control group but
discontinued this with subsequent control participants due to the distress and confusion
displayed by these infants. None of the five who participated produced any signs.
Author Manuscript
This measure was somewhat exploratory as infants of this age often fail to produce
communicative signals in lab-based elicited production tasks. As a result, we expected that
production would be low, but nonetheless had the potential to provide a compelling index of
depth of learning. To elicit production, the experimenter presented photographs of the target
objects one at a time and elicited the sign by asking the infant, “Can you say [points to
photograph] this with your hands?” or “Can you show me [verbal label] with your hands?”
The number of trials administered varied across infants based on their attention and
fussiness (M = 7.61 trials, SD = 3.92, range = 2 – 18). Eleven infants from the experimental
groups did not complete this task due to fussiness (4 = video alone, 3 = supported video, 4 =
parent instruction). This resulted in a total sample size for this measure of 57 across the three
experimental conditions.
Author Manuscript
Coding—A coder blind to condition classified the infants’ choices during the forced-choice
task and the infants’ responses to the sign elicitation from video. Choice response was based
on which card the infant first touched. Elicitation was coded by crediting infants with correct
sign production if they produced an intentional hand movement that included at least two of
the three central elements of the sign: hand shape, motion trajectory, and sign-space location
which were each coded independently. A second coder evaluated a randomly selected 10%
of the sessions. Intercoder agreement was 97% on forced choice trials and 87% on elicited
production.
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 7
Author Manuscript
Results
Below, we report the parent report data for both sign learning and word learning followed by
the more conservative lab-based forced-choice sign comprehension and elicited sign
production measures. Data collected during the first three weeks of the study (4 lab visits
including baseline) were used to evaluate learning whereas data collected after the noexposure delay were analyzed separately as a measure of retention.
Parent Report
Author Manuscript
We investigated infants’ baby sign learning as well as parents’ report of children’s use of
verbal labels for each of the 18 target objects. We tracked verbal label acquisition both as a
replication of previous longitudinal research and as a manipulation check to ensure that there
were no systematic reporting biases exhibited by parents in particular conditions. For both
sign and word production, we calculated the proportion of target items (out of 18) that
parents reported their children produced each week. We investigated whether parents
reported growth in target sign and verbal label acquisition over time and whether this varied
as a function of exposure condition using 2-way Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) with
exposure condition (video alone, co-viewing, parent instruction, and no exposure control) as
a between-subjects variable and lab visit (baseline, 1, 2, 3) as a within-subject variable.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Sign learning—We analyzed the proportion of signs parents reported infants produced
using a 2-way (Condition x Lab Visit) ANOVA. This analysis yielded a main effect of
condition, F(3, 88) = 7.01, p < .0005, partial η2 = .19, and a main effect of lab visit, Λ = .49,
F(3, 86) = 29.34, p < .0005, partial η2 = .51, mediated by a condition by lab visit interaction,
Λ = .66, F(9, 88) = 4.33, p < .0005, partial η2 = .13 (see Figure 1). This interaction was
driven by the control group which deviated from the three experimental conditions. Whereas
parents of infants in all three experimental groups reported growth in their children’s sign
production across lab visits, those in the control condition did not. A follow-up analysis that
excluded the control condition revealed only a main effect of lab visit, Λ= .43, F(3, 63) =
28.14, p < .0005, partial η2 = .57 suggesting that the exposure conditions did not differ
reliably from each other. Follow-up one way ANOVAs comparing the four conditions for
each lab visit independently revealed no condition effects at baseline or after 1 week of
exposure, but a reliable condition effect after 2 weeks, F(3,88) = 6.04, p= .001, and 3 weeks
of exposure, F(3,88) = 11.05, p<.0005 suggesting that parents in the experimental conditions
began to observe evidence of learning after two weeks of exposure. Post-hoc analysis using
both Tamhane (to adjust for violation of homogeneity given that parents never reported any
sign production in the control group) and Tukey’s HSD indicated that all three experimental
conditions differed from control after 2 weeks’ exposure. None of the experimental
conditions differed reliably from each other at any lab visit, although the difference between
co-viewing and video alone approached significance at lab visit 3 (Tukey’s p = .064).
Word Learning—We predicted that acquisition of verbal labels for the included objects
would increase over time at this age in all four conditions, due to incidental daily exposure.
However, exposure to the baby signs also enhanced exposure to the accompanying verbal
labels. To assess whether heightened exposure to the verbal labels in the sign exposure
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 8
Author Manuscript
conditions accelerated word learning, we conducted an ANOVA on parent report of
children’s word production for these 18 items with condition as a between-subject variable
and lab visit (baseline, 1, 2, 3) as a within-subjects variable. As expected, there was a main
effect of lab visit indicating vocabulary growth over time, Λ = .47, F(3, 86) = 31.67, p <.
0005, partial η2 = .52. However, there was no effect of condition and no interaction (see
Figure 2).
Author Manuscript
Sign Retention—We conducted a one-way ANOVA to assess condition differences in
sign retention as indicated by parental report at lab visit 4 (following the one-week retention
interval). This analysis yielded a significant condition effect, F(3,87) = 14.68, p<.0005.
Post-hoc analysis using Tamhane revealed significant differences between the experimental
groups and the control group (m=.0116), p< .01, but no differences among experimental
groups. Tukey’s HSD, a less conservative measure, indicated that parents of infants in the
co-viewing condition (m = .442) group reported that their infants produced significantly
more of the signs than those in the video alone condition (m = .242), p = .025. Parents
reported an intermediate level of sign production in the parent instruction condition (m=.
310) that did not differ reliably from either of the other sign exposure conditions.
Forced Choice Assessment
Author Manuscript
Proportion of forced choice trials (out of 6) on which infants selected the target picture was
calculated for each lab visit. Given the longitudinal nature of the study, there were
occasional missing data points due either to a missed lab visit or infant fussiness. Overall,
there were a total of 21 missing data points out of 460 planned lab visits (<5%). These
missing lab visits were distributed across 19 participants (1 in the video alone, 12 in the coviewing, 4 in the parent instruction and 4 in the control condition). Missing data points were
replaced by the mean of the remaining children in the same condition for the same lab visit
(see Parent, 2013; Rubin, Witkiewitz, St. Andre, & Reily, 2007 for support for this
approach).
Sign Learning—We conducted two sets of analyses on the lab-based forced-choice sign
learning task. We compared performance in the experimental groups both to performance in
the control condition and to chance (random, 50%) responding. We compared performance
of each condition (including the control condition) to chance using single-sample t-tests.
Comparisons to the control condition (and among experimental conditions) involved 2-way
condition-by-lab visit ANOVAs.
Author Manuscript
Comparisons to chance indicated, as expected, that control performance did not differ from
chance at any lab visit. Children in the video alone condition responded at chance at the
baseline visit and after 1 and 2 weeks of exposure but performed at above chance rates after
3 weeks of viewing, t(19) = 4.15, p=.001. In the co-viewing condition, the same pattern was
observed with performance above chance only at lab visit 3, t(26) = 2.51, p=.018. The parent
instruction group exhibited chance performance at baseline and lab visits 1 through 3,
performing marginally above chance in lab visit 3, t(20) = 2.05, p=.053.
An ANOVA with condition as a between subject factor and lab visit (baseline, 1, 2, 3) as a
within-subject factor, yielded a main effect of lab visit Λ = .90, F(3, 86) = 3.01, p=.035,
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 9
Author Manuscript
partial η2 = .09 indicating overall improvement with exposure. There was also a marginal
effect of condition, F(3, 88) = 2.17, p=.097, partial η2= .07 (see Figure 3). The interaction
was not significant, Λ = .89, F(9, 88) = 1.13, p=.342, partial η2 = .04. Post-hoc analysis
using Tukey’s HSD indicated that the marginal main effect of condition was driven by a
reliable overall difference between the video alone scores (M = .55, SD = .16) and the
control condition scores, M = .49, SD = .18, p = .049. No other pairwise condition
differences were significant.
Comparing Parent Report and Forced-Choice Measures—Parent report of infants
signing and performance in the Forced Choice task at lab visit 3 were marginally
significantly correlated collapsed across all condition, r(84) = .178, p = .10. However, this
correlation was non-significant when infants in the control condition were removed from the
analysis, r (61) = .049, p = .70.
Author Manuscript
Sign Retention—We conducted a separate analysis of performance on the forced-choice
task at visit 4 after a one-week delay during which children were not exposed to the baby
signs. Comparisons to chance indicated that only those in the parent instruction group
performed significantly above chance after a one-week retention interval, t(19) = 2.85, p = .
010. A one-way ANOVA revealed no reliable differences across conditions at this lab visit
(see Figure 4A).
At the sign retention session, there was a significant correlation between parent report and
performance on the forced-choice task collapsed across all conditions, r(84) = .247, p = .
022. The correlation remained marginally significant when infants in the control condition
were removed from the analysis, r(62) = .202, p = .11.
Author Manuscript
Elicited production as a measure of sign retention
Author Manuscript
Performance on the elicited production task administered at the final lab visit (after a oneweek delay with no exposure) was measured based on the proportion of signs elicited that
infants produced. Children’s mean production across conditions is reported in Figure 4B.
Because only five infants completed the task in the no exposure control condition and none
of those infants produced any signs, we did not analyze the data from this condition further.
Single-sample t-tests comparing each experimental condition to the expected population
mean of zero (representing no knowledge of the target signs) indicated that infants in all
three experimental groups showed significant evidence of learning (t’s = 5.47, 5.94, and
6.93 for the video alone, co-viewing, and parent instruction conditions, respectively, all p’s
<.0005). A one-way ANOVA with condition (video alone, co-viewing, and parent
instrution) as a between subjects variable revealed no significant differences in rates of
elicited sign production across the sign exposure conditions, F(2, 52) = .18, p = .832.
Among those infants who participated in the elicited production task, 82% of video alone
infants (n=16) and 100% of those in the co-viewing (n = 23) and parental instruction groups
(n = 17) produced at least one sign successfully. However, performance in the elicited
production task was not correlated with either parent report, r(53) = −.039, p = .778, or
forced-choice performance, r(52) = .004, p=.978, at lab visit 4 (collapsed across
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 10
Author Manuscript
experimental conditions) suggesting that the production task may better serve as an
existence proof for learning than an accurate index of how many signs were retained.
General Discussion
Author Manuscript
These data indicate that infants under the age of two can learn baby signs from video, even
without the support of parents during viewing. Parent report and lab based assessments
revealed striking evidence of learning after 3 weeks of exposure (12 viewings) in all three
experimental conditions. The evidence for sign retention was mixed. The forced-choice
measure suggested that only those in the parent instruction condition retained the signs after
a week without exposure. However, infants in all three experimental conditions reliably
produced signs in the lab after a one-week delay. Because production is typically considered
the more conservative measure, and evidence of production clearly implies comprehension,
it appears that infants in all three exposure conditions retained at least some sign knowledge
over a delay. However given that the production measure relies on performative factors such
as fatigue and shyness, it is not likely to be the most sensitive index of variability in
retention across conditions. The absence of correlations between elicited production and
either the parent report or forced choice assessment underscores that the production measure
was a less sensitive index.
Author Manuscript
Interestingly, the more sensitive forced-choice data suggest that children in the parental
instruction condition exhibited a ‘sleeper’ effect, exhibiting marginally above chance
performance in the forced-choice task after 3 weeks’ exposure but robust evidence of
learning after an additional week without exposure. The superior performance of the parent
instruction condition relative to the video viewing conditions after a delay may reflect a
video deficit in retention after as little as one week. Taken together, the findings from this
study suggest the potential for video-based learning but hint that the most robust retention
occurs following traditional parent instruction.
Author Manuscript
The parental report data echoed the lab-based evidence suggesting learning across all
exposure conditions. According to parent report, all three sign exposure groups exhibited
learning after 3 weeks’ exposure. The two parent-supported groups (i.e., co-viewing and
parental instruction) reported numerically, but not statistically, higher rates of learning than
those who viewed videos alone, according to parental report. This evidence of more robust
learning in parentally-supported learning environments was predicted and may imply that
the medium through which information is presented is less critical to infant learning than the
involvement of a parent in the learning endeavor. However, the fact that these differences
emerged only in the parental report measure raises the possibility that this outcome may be
due, at least in part, to either greater sensitivity to sign production or over-reporting among
parents who had participated in the learning sessions.
Although we anticipated that the infants in the parent instruction condition would
demonstrate evidence of learning, it is worth noting that parents in this condition reported
that they found the instructional sessions challenging. The task may not have been especially
naturalistic, given that parents needed to simultaneously manipulate the book, sustain their
infants’ attention, and track which signs to use. That the parents were teaching material with
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 11
Author Manuscript
which they were not especially familiar may also have limited their consistency or comfort
with producing the signs during interactions with their infants. In contrast, the adults on the
commercial videos were adept signers. This, in conjunction with the more dynamic
information included in the videos, may actually have limited learning potential in the parent
instruction condition (Simcock, Garrity & Barr, 2011).
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
It is notable that infants whose parents supported learning were successful at acquiring signs
from both picture books and videos. However, our most surprising finding is that those in
the video alone condition learned the signs as well. There are several factors that may have
contributed to learning in this condition. The first is that infants were exposed to the videos
repeatedly over the course of the study and repetition seems to support learning from video
(Barr, Muentener & Garcia, 2007; Barr, Muentener, Garcia, Fukimmoto & Chavez, 2007;
Strouse & Troseth, 2008). In addition, although signs serve the same communicative
functions as words, they are based on manual movement; Numerous imitation studies
demonstrate infants’ ability to learn a sequence of movement from a screen (Barr & Hayne,
1999; Barr, Muentener, & Garcia, 2007; Barr, Muentener, Garcia, Fujimoto, & Chavez,
2007; Barr, Shuck, Slaerno, Atkinson, & Linebarger, 2010; Barr & Wyss, 2008; Hayne,
Herbert, & Simcock, 2003; Meltzoff, 1988; Strouse & Troseth, 2008). Furthermore, verbal
labels, included in the elicited production task, may have served as reminders for the
movements (Barr & Weiss, 2008; Hayne & Herbert, 2004; Khu, Graham & Ganea, 2014).
Although signs did serve as labels in this context, it may be that this manual form of labeling
is easier to learn from the screen than is verbal labeling. Furthermore, the familiarity with
the words and objects may have scaffolded learning by drawing infants’ attention to the
movement associated with the familiar word and referent (Strouse & Troseth, 2014). The
fact that viewing alone did not require infants to divide their visual attention between the
screen and the parent might also have aided learning in this context (Strouse & Troseth,
2014).
Author Manuscript
Although not a direct goal of our study, we also tracked word learning across lab visits via
parent report. As we anticipated, word learning increased over time for the stimulus items
involved in this study, but the rate was consistent across all conditions, including the no
exposure control condition. This outcome replicates previous longitudinal studies suggesting
no enhanced word learning following 4 weeks of exposure to a commercial video as
measured by parent report (see Robb et al., 2009) and forced choice assessments (see
DeLoache et al., 2010). That the parent instruction condition did not show accelerated
learning relative to the video viewing conditions differs from DeLoache and colleagues’
findings. This may have been due to the lack of emphasis on verbal labels in this study, or to
the more limited duration and frequency of exposure that we employed relative to DeLoache
et al. Because these words are frequent in input to children, it may be that the degree of
enhanced exposure employed in our study was either insufficient or unnecessary to impact
the rate of word learning. However, the dissociation between word learning and gesture
learning raises important questions for future research regarding how video learning varies
for different learning materials. Of particular interest is whether the visual versus auditory
modality impacts the relative efficacy of video versus live instruction (see Brito, Barr,
McIntyre, & Simcock, 2012; Simcock, et al., 2011, for additional discussion of this issue).
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 12
Author Manuscript
Conclusion
Author Manuscript
This experimentally controlled, longitudinal investigation reveals that, at least for some
stimuli and some video formats, infants exhibit a surprising ability to acquire information
obtained via video viewing. These findings bolster Vandewater’s (2011) findings that
infants’ communicative repertoire can be expanded through video exposure, and add to the
literature by generalizing to sign learning and to production measures. Although the effects
were nominally more robust in the parent-supported learning conditions, even infants who
viewed videos alone exhibited clear evidence of learning over the course of three weeks’
exposure. Because infants viewed multiple presentation formats, we cannot determine how
format or formal features (e.g., use of foreground music v. background music v. no music)
impact learning. Likewise, we cannot determine the optimal duration or frequency of
exposure to facilitate learning. This study also does not address the potential risks associated
with exposure to media. Nonetheless, we find clear and compelling evidence that, at least for
baby signs, videos constitute one possible instructional medium for infants.
Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.
Acknowledgments
We thank Diana Neeves, Saryn Levy, Cassie Hendrix, Kathleen Ghio, Trisha Patel and Jhonelle Bailey for their
assistance with this study. We also thank the parents and participants for their extraordinary dedication of time and
effort to this research endeavor.
This research was supported by NICHD Grant Number 5-R03-HD058777.
Author Manuscript
References
Author Manuscript
Acredolo LP, Goodwyn S. Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development. 1988; 59:450–
466.10.2307/1130324 [PubMed: 2452052]
American Academy of Pediatrics. Media Education. Pediatrics. 1999; 104:341–343.10.1542/peds.
2010-1636 [PubMed: 10429023]
American Academy of Pediatrics. Media Use by Children Younger Than 2 Years Council on
Communications and Media. Pediatrics. 2011; 128:1040–5.10.1542/peds.2011-1753 [PubMed:
22007002]
Anderson DR, Huston AC, Schmitt KL, Linebarger DL, Wright JC. Early childhood television
viewing and adolescent behavior: The recontact study. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development. 2001; 66:vii–147.10.1111/1540-5834.00120
Anderson DR, Pempek TA. Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist. 2005;
48:505–522.10.1177/0002764204271506
Barr R, Hayne H. Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child
Development. 1999; 70:1067–1081.10.1111/1467-8624.00079 [PubMed: 10546335]
Barr R, Muentener P, Garcia A. Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18month-olds. Developmental Science. 2007; 10:910–921.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00641.x
[PubMed: 17973804]
Barr R, Muentener P, Garcia A, Fujimoto M, Chávez V. The effect of repetition on imitation from
television during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology. 2007; 49:196–207.10.1002/dev.20208
[PubMed: 17299795]
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 13
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Barr R, Shuck L, Salerno K, Atkinson E, Linebarger DL. Music interferes with learning from
television during infancy. Infant And Child Development. 2010; 19:313–331.10.1002/icd.666
Barr R, Wyss N. Reenactment of televised content by 2-year olds: Toddlers use language learned from
television to solve a difficult imitation problem. Infant Behavior & Development. 2008; 31:696–
703.10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.04.006 [PubMed: 18514319]
Barr R, Zack E, Muentener P, Garcia A. Infants’ attention and responsiveness to television increases
with prior exposure and parental interaction. Infancy. 2008; 13:3–56.10.1080/15250000701779378
Brito N, Barr R, McIntyre P, Simcock G. Long-term transfer of learning from books and video during
toddlerhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 111:108–119.10.1016/j.jecp.
2011.08.004 [PubMed: 21911223]
Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development.
Acta Paediatrica. 2008; 97:977–982.10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00831.x [PubMed: 18460044]
DeLoache JS, Chiong C, Sherman K, Islam N, Vanderborght M, Troseth GL, O’Doherty K. Do babies
learn from baby media? Psychological Science. 2010; 21:1570–1574.10.1177/0956797610384145
[PubMed: 20855901]
Fender JG, Richert RA, Robb MB, Wartella E. Parent teaching focus and toddlers’ learning from an
infant DVD. Infant and Child Development. 2010; 19:613–627.10.1002/icd
Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick J, Bates E, Thal DJ, Pethick SJ, Tomasello M, Mervis CB, Stiles J.
Variability in early communicative development. Monographs Of The Society For Research In
Child Development. 1994; 59:v–173.10.2307/1166093
Fidler AE, Zack E, Barr R. Television viewing patterns in 6- to 18- month olds : the role of caregiver –
infant interactional quality. Infancy. 2010; 15:176–196.10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00013.x
Hayne H, Herbert J. Verbal cues facilitate memory retrieval during infancy. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology. 2004; 89:127–139.10.1016/j.jecp.2004.06.002 [PubMed: 15388302]
Hayne H, Herbert J, Simcock G. Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. Developmental
Science. 2003; 6:254–261.10.1111/1467-7687.00281
Krcmar M, Grela B, Lin K. Can toddlers learn vocabulary from television? An experimental approach.
Media Psychology. 2007; 10:41–63.10.1080/15213260701300931
Krcmar M. Word learning in very young children from infant-directed DVDs. Journal Of
Communication. 2011; 61:780–794.10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01561.x
Lemish, D. Viewers in diapers: The early development of television viewing. In: Lindlof, TR., editor.
Natural audiences: Qualitative research of media uses and effects. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1987. p.
33-57.
Lemish D, Rice ML. Television as a talking picture book: A prop for language acquisition. Journal of
Child Language. 1986; 13:251–274.10.1017/S0305000900008047 [PubMed: 3745331]
Linebarger DL, Walker D. Infants’ and toddlers’ television viewing and language outcomes. American
Behavioral Scientist. 2005; 48:624–645.10.1177/0002764204271505
Meltzoff AN. Imitation of televised models by infants. Child Development. 1988a; 59:1221–
1229.10.2307/1130485 [PubMed: 3168638]
Meltzoff AN. Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple
stimuli. Developmental Psychology. 1988b; 24:470–476.10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.470 [PubMed:
25147404]
Naigles, LR.; Mayeux, L. Television as incidental language teacher. In: Singer, DG.; Singer, JI.,
editors. Handbook of children and the media. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. p. 135-152.
Namy LL. What’s in a name when it isn’t a word? 17-month-olds’ mapping of nonverbal symbols to
object categories. Infancy. 2001; 2:73–86.10.1207/S15327078IN0201_5
Namy LL, Waxman SR. Words and symbolic gestures: Infants’ interpretations of different forms of
symbolic reference. Child Development. 1998; 69:295–308.10.2307/1132165 [PubMed: 9586206]
Parent MC. Handling item-level missing data: simpler is just as good. The Counseling Psychologist.
2013; 41:568–600.10.1177/0011000012445176
Reiser RA, Williamson N, Suzuki K. Using “Sesame Street” to facilitate children’s recognition of
letters and numbers. Educational Communication and Technology Journal. 1988; 36:15–21.
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 14
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Rice ML, Huston AC, Truglio R, Wright J. Words from Sesame Street: Learning vocabulary while
viewing. Developmental Psychology. 1990; 26:421–428.10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.421
Robb M, Richert R, Wartella E. Just a talking book? Word learning from watching baby videos.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2009; 27:27–45.10.1348/026151008X320156
[PubMed: 19972661]
Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Parish-Morris J, Golinkoff RM. Live action: Can young children learn
verbs from video? Child Development. 2009; 80:1360–1375.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01338.x
[PubMed: 19765005]
Rubin LH, Witkiewitz K, St Andre J, Riley S. Methods for Handling Missing Data in the Behavioral
Neurosciences: Don’t Throw the Baby Rat out with the Bath Water. Journal of Undergraduate
Neuroscience Education. 2007; 5:A71–A77. [PubMed: 23493038]
Schmitt K, Anderson D. Television and reality: Toddler’s use of visual information from video to
guide behavior. Media Psychology. 2002; 4:51–76.10.1207/S1532785XMEP0401_03
Simcock G, Garrity K, Barr R. The effect of narrative cues on infants’ imitation from television and
picture books. Child Development. 2011; 82:1607–1619.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01636.x
[PubMed: 21883157]
Singer, JL.; Singer, DG. Barney & Friends as entertainment and education: Evaluating the quality and
effectiveness of a television series for preschool children. In: Asamen, J.; Berry, GL., editors.
Research paradigms, television, and social behavior. Thousand Oaks, CA US: Sage Publications,
Inc; 1998. p. 305-367.
Strouse GA, O’Doherty K, Troseth GL. Effective coviewing: Preschoolers’ learning from video after a
dialogic questioning intervention. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49:2368–2382.10.1037/
a0032463 [PubMed: 23544859]
Strouse GA, Troseth GL. ‘Don’t try this at home’: Toddlers’ imitation of new skills from people on
video. Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology. 2008; 101:262–280.10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.010
[PubMed: 18675431]
Strouse GA, Troseth GL. Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video. Cognitive
Development. 2014; 30:47–64.10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.002
Troseth GL, Saylor MM, Archer AH. Young Children’s Use of Video as a Source of Socially Relevant
Information. Child Development. 2006; 77:786–799.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00903.x [PubMed:
16686801]
Vaala S, Linebarger DL, Fenstermacher S, Tedone A, Brey E, Barr R, Moses A, Shwery C, Calvert
SL. Content analysis of language-promoting teaching strategies used in infant directed media?
Infant and Child Development. 2010; 19:628–648.10.1002/icd.715
Vandewater EA. Infant Word Learning From Commercially Available Video In The US. Journal Of
Children & Media. 2011; 5:248–266.10.1080/17482798.2011.584375
Vandewater E, Barr R, Park SE, Lee SJ. A US study of transfer of learning from video to books in
toddlers. Journal of Children and Media. 2010; 4:451–467.10.1080/17482798.2010.510013
Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger
than two years. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2007b; 161:473–479.
Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between Media Viewing and Language
Development in Children Under Age 2 Years. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2007a; 151:364–
368.10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071 [PubMed: 17889070]
Author Manuscript
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 15
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Mean proportion of signs accumulated across sessions in each condition, based on parental
report. Error bars indicate confidence intervals (95%).
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 16
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
Mean proportion of words accumulated across sessions in each condition, based on parental
report
Error bars indicate confidence interval (95%)
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 17
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 3.
Mean Proportion of Target Selected Across Sessions in Each Condition, Based on Forced
Choice Task
Error Bars Indicate Confidence Interval (95%)
Chance = .50
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.
Dayanim and Namy
Page 18
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 4.
Author Manuscript
Figure 4a. Mean proportion of target selected after a one week delay, based on forced choice
task.
Error bars indicate confidence interval (95%).
Chance = .50.
Figure 4b. Mean proportion of elicited signs produced after a one week delay
Error bars indicate standard error. ‘*’ indicates reliable difference from zero, indicating
learning.
* p < .001
Child Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 May 01.