Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

DST 05 0283

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Volume 5, Issue 2, March 2011 Diabetes Technology Society

Journal of Diabetes Science and Technology

SYMPOSIUM

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders
Giuseppe Riva, Ph.D.

Abstract
Obesity and eating disorders are usually considered unrelated problems with different causes. However, various studies identify unhealthful weight-control behaviors (fasting, vomiting, or laxative abuse), induced by a negative experience of the body, as the common antecedents of both obesity and eating disorders. But how might negative body imagecommon to most adolescents, not only to medical patientsbe behind the development of obesity and eating disorders? In this paper, I review the allocentric lock theory of negative body image as the possible antecedent of both obesity and eating disorders. Evidence from psychology and neuroscience indicates that our bodily experience involves the integration of different sensory inputs within two different reference frames: egocentric (first-person experience) and allocentric (third-person experience). Even though functional relations between these two frames are usually limited, they influence each other during the interaction between long- and short-term memory processes in spatial cognition. If this process is impaired either through exogenous (e.g., stress) or endogenous causes, the egocentric sensory inputs are unable to update the contents of the stored allocentric representation of the body. In other words, these patients are locked in an allocentric (observer view) negative image of their body, which their sensory inputs are no longer able to update even after a demanding diet and a significant weight loss. This article discusses the possible role of virtual reality in addressing this problem within an integrated treatment approach based on the allocentric lock theory.
J Diabetes Sci Technol 2011;5(2):283-292

Introduction
besity and eating disorders are usually considered unrelated problems with different causes. However, research with adolescents is questioning this belief; apparently, unhealthful weight-control behaviorssuch as fasting (going without eating for 24 hours for weight control), vomiting, or laxative abuseare the common

antecedents of both obesity and eating disorders.16 For example, Neumark-Sztainer and colleagues2 discussed the results of the Project EAT II (Eating Among Teens), a longitudinal study involving 2516 ethnically and socioeconomically diverse adolescents. They report that, 5 years later, the use of unhealthful weight-control behaviors

Author Affiliation: Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Laboratory, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy Abbreviations: (CBT) cognitive behavioral therapy, (VR) virtual reality Keywords: body-image disturbances, eating disorders, experiential cognitive therapy, obesity, virtual reality Corresponding Author: Giuseppe Riva, Ph.D., Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Laboratory, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Via Ariosto 13, 20145 Milan, Italy; email address auxo.psylab@auxologico.it 283

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

increased six times the risk for binge eating with loss of control, three times the risk for being overweight, and two to five times the risk for extreme weight-control behaviors such as the use of diet pills and self-induced vomiting. A similar result was found by Stice and associates;6 in a longitudinal studywhere fasting was the best predictor for the onset of binge eating and bulimia nervosa 5 years later. Epidemiological studies indicate that childhood obesity has different ethnic, socioeconomic (compared with affluent white children, poor Hispanic, white, and black children have 2.7, 1.9, and 3.2 times higher odds of obesity, respectively), and behavioral risk factors.7 Behavioral variables, such as higher television viewing and higher physical inactivity levels, were all independently associated with higher obesity prevalence. In a 4-year longitudinal study on 496 adolescent girls, Stice and coworkers5 studied the psychological and behavioral risk factors for the onset of obesity in adolescent girls. Their data show that participants who were on a weight-loss diet or who used maladaptive compensatory behaviors for weight control were at increased risk for obesity 4 years later. The present results have several clinical implications. First, they suggest that it is particularly important to educate adolescents about effective weight-control strategies. This conclusion has been highlighted by Raynor and colleagues8 who stated that, randomized trials testing family-based, behavioral modification interventions for pediatric obesity, which provide low-calorie dietary prescriptions emphasizing nutrient-dense food choices, greatly improve weight status and show a decrease or no change in eating pathology in children. Second, the evidence that youths practicing unhealthful weightcontrol behaviors are at higher risk for obesity and eating disorders implies that prevention and treatment interventions should also focus on the causes of these behaviors. In a message on the Yahoo Answers UK site, an adolescent girl writes, Im 16 - so the classic insecure girl - although on the outside I cover up with a massive personality and I seem really confident about myself but just recently and defiantly in the past few days I have been looking at myself and yes I can see bone but I also see fat - I know Im skinny but Im stupidly skinny yet I have a huge bum and boobs and Ive stopped eating properly and it hurts. (http://uk.answers.yahoo.com/question/ index?qid=20100718191717AAvCXjN).
J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

The words of the girl clearly explain her behavior: she stopped eating properly because she did not like her body.9 A study by Kostanski and Gullone10 with a sample of 431 Australian preadolescent children (7 to 10 years) confirms this interpretation: preadolescents as young as 7 years of age are unsatisfied with their body appearance and deliberately engage in restrictive eating behaviors. But how does a negative body image influence the development of obesity and eating disorders? In their provocative editorial, Schwartz and Henderson admitted that there is no simple answer to this question.11 In this article, I review the allocentric lock theory proposed as an antecedent to both obesity and eating disorders.12,13 Specifically, I consider the possibility of an allocentric negative body image that is not altered by contrasting egocentric representations driven by perception. In other words, these patients are locked to an allocentric (observer view) negative image of their body that their sensory inputs are unable to update even after dramatic body changes. I discuss the possible role of virtual reality (VR) in addressing this problem as part of an integrated treatment approach. Specifically, I recommend adding a 10-session body-image rescripting protocol based on VR to the treatments of obesity and eating disorders to improve long-term outcome. The results of two controlled trials with 211 obese patients14 and 36 binge eating patients15 are used to support this treatment recommendation.

Allocentric Lock Hypothesis


As noted by Klatzky,16 A reference frame is a means of representing the locations of entities in space. Evidence from psychology and neuroscience indicates that our spatial experience, including the bodily one, involves the integration of different sensory inputs within two different reference frames: egocentric and allocentric (see Figure 1).17,18 The egocentric frame (first-person view) refers to the body of the observer and allows him/her to locate objects relative to the body center (as in the video game Crysis; Figure 1, left-hand side); within this frame, the position of an object changes if the subject moves.19 In contrast to this frame, the allocentric frame (thirdperson view) refers to space external to the perceiver. Within this frame, the position of an object does not change if the subject moves (as in the video game Pac-Man; Figure 1, right-hand side). More, the object thus exists even if there is no relation with the self or another person.19
www.journalofdst.org

Our Experience of the Body

284

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

These two frames have different sources of reference.18 The egocentric frame has its primary source in somatoperceptions: representations of the present state of the body and tactile stimuli from sensory inputs. The allocentric frame has its primary source in somatorepresentations: abstract knowledge, beliefs, and attitudes related to the body. The ability to represent and recall an object, including our own body, changes according to the frame of reference: when we adopt an egocentric stance, we represent the object relative to ourselves; when we adopt an allocentric stance, the object is represented independently of our own current relation with it.1921 For instance, imagine a subject who is trying to find the train station in an unknown city using a map. Using an egocentric perspective, the subject will rotate the map to align it with his actual physical heading. Using an allocentric perspective, the subject will mentally rotate himself to align his heading with the north/south orientation of the map. These frames of reference also influence the way memories are stored and retrieved: using the egocentric frame, the remembering subject sees the event from his or her perspective as in normal perception (field mode; see Figure 2); using the allocentric frame, the remembering subject sees the self engaged in the event as an observer would (observer mode; see Figure 2).2123 As highlighted by many authors, the body has a critical role in the Western culture;2427 the body is not only the first and the most natural tool of man, but also a culturally negotiated symbol that shapes the self and the experience of the subject. In this context, the body becomes a symbol of personal order or disorder:28 slenderness is associated with happiness, success, and social acceptability; being overweight is linked to laziness, lack of willpower, and being out of control. In addition, to control our body, we are taught to disembody ourselves (self-objectification) and consider the body as an object that others look at.29 In other words, our culture asks us to develop an allocentric view of ourselves by forcing us to view our body just as a series of measurements and value it more on the basis of appearance than performance. This is also true for children and adolescents; they are aware of the actual size of their own bodies and are likely to have judged the acceptability of their own body. Many are dissatisfied, and some feel anxious despite
J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

Figure 1. Egocentric and allocentric representation of space. The egocentric representation (on the left-hand side; image taken from the first-person shooter video game Crysis) integrates perceptual impressions gathered from a first-person perspective relative to the position and heading (frontback, rightleft, and updown) of the subject. In this representation, the world constantly changes while the navigator remains spatially fixed in the center of the reference system. The allocentric representation (on the right-hand side; image taken from the Pac-Man video game) instead establishes a map with an origin and a reference direction external to the subject. In this representation the world remains stationary while the subject moves inside it.

Developing an Allocentric Negative Body Image

Figure 2. Field and observer modes in episodic memory. In field mode, the subject remembers the view of the Shenandoah Valley from his hiking experience through the subjects own eyes, as if he were looking outward (on the left-hand side). In the observer mode, the subject remembers the view of the Shenandoah Valley including him as an actor in the memory image (on the right-hand side).

having a healthy body size.1,30 To reduce the impact of this situation, different governments and institutions are introducing laws or codes of conduct banning so-called cult of the body advertisingincluding fashion magazines, television, and Web sitesand making it illegal to publicly promote extreme thinness. Anxiety disorders, which are common in patients with obesity and eating disorders, may also have a critical role in developing a negative allocentric body image. Anxious subjects tend to evaluate specific situations as being excessively dangerous. When this happens, they shift attention away from the situation and become highly self-focused.31 Clark and Wells 32 propose that this self-processing occurs as an impression of appearance from an observer perspective, seeing oneself as if from an external point of view (Figure 2).

Link with Anxiety Disorders

285

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

As demonstrated by Eich and associates,33 taking an observer perspective during autobiographical memory retrieval appears to significantly decrease activity in regions of the cerebral cortex associated with interoceptive awareness. Episodic memory theories34 also emphasize that the arousal associated with an object (e.g., a commercial using thin models) elicits focused attention (e.g., my body in the mirror is not so thin as the one of the model) that enhances binding of its constituent features (e.g., my body in the mirror is fat), facilitating the development of an allocentric representation of the body (e.g., Im that disgusting body in the mirror). The most important factor in this process seems to be how arousing the experiences are rather than their valence.34

play a significant role in the functioning of the medial temporal lobe and may induce a locked negative body image. In fact, research underlines the role of anxiety and stress in influencing the brain areas involved in the egocentric/allocentric transformation. The endogenous cholinergic tone in the dorsal hippocampus decreases with increases in anxiety, and it is associated with an increase in the serotonergic tone.41 Moreover, a growing body of evidence suggests that stressand, in particular, chronic stresscan cause hippocampal damage42,43 through the excessive activity exerted by the amygdala on the hippocampus.44 Interestingly, this datum is in accordance with many studies suggesting the influence of interpersonal problems45 and stress in the onset of eating disorders4649 and obesity.5052

As suggested by Byrne and Becker, the transformation from egocentric to allocentric representations of space is done by neurons in different medial temporal lobe structures. The hippocampus has a critical role in spatial memory by generating allocentric representations for long-term memory.36 Furthermore, Parron and Save37 suggested that the parietal cortex, the retrosplenial cortex, the entorhinal cortex, and the hippocampus are part of a functional network devoted to the processing of allocentric spatial information. Specifically, the lateral entorhinal cortex carries nonspatial information from the perirhinal cortex into the dorsal hippocampus, where it is combined with spatial information from the medial entorhinal cortex to create conjunctive objectplace (or eventplace) representations in the hippocampus.38
35

Locked to an Allocentric Negative Body Image: How Neurosciences May Explain It

As stated earlier, in the Western culture, the body is considered a symbol of personal order or disorder: slenderness is associated with happiness, success, and social acceptability; being overweight is linked to laziness, lack of willpower, and being out of control.28 Interestingly, our culture also provides a clear path for dealing with it: eating control. As reported by Rosen,53 the most common reason for attempting to lose weight in women is the desire to improve physical appearance. This datum was confirmed by Lu and Hou:54 in a sample of female college students, body dissatisfaction positively predicted their intention to lose weight ( = .51, p < .001) From this perspective, the passage from a locked allocentric negative body image to obesity or eating disorders may be explained by social influence: media and culture promote diet and controlled eating as the best ways to improve body-image satisfaction. However, the impossibility of using sensory inputs for updating the allocentric representation of the bodypatients hate their body even after a demanding diet and a significant weight loss locks the patients in an unsatisfying body and into more and more radical attempts to improve it.

From an Allocentric Negative Body Image to Obesity and Eating Disorders

If this transformation is impaired, the subjects can no longer use the sensory inputs to update the contents of the allocentric representation of their body. This is what may be behind the etiology of obesity and eating disorders: a negative somatorepresentationthat is not updated by contrasting egocentric parietal representations driven by perception39priming the processing of any further body-related experience.40 In simpler words, the egocentric perception-driven experience of the real body is no longer able to modify the allocentric memory-driven experience of a negative body; these patients are locked in an allocentric (observer view) negative representation of their body.13 How does this happen? Apparently, psychological stress mechanismsexogenous and/or endogenous

Use of Virtual Reality for Unlocking the Allocentric Negative Body Image
Virtual Reality in Behavioral Health Care
The convergence between technology and medicine55,56 is providing new tools and methods for behavioral healthcare.57 Between them, an emerging trend is the use of VR to improve the existing cognitive behavioral protocols for different psychological disorders, especially

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

286

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

anxiety disorders5863 and addiction.6467 Using VR, clinicians can offer in-office, systematic, controlled exposure therapy (VR exposure) to their patients, without the complications of in vivo exposure.68 Moreover, the specific characteristics of the VR experience provide patients with a safe setting where they can exist and experience life,69 thereby allowing them to be emotionally present inside the virtual environment.7072 Virtual reality can also be described as an advanced imaginal system: an experiential form of imagery that is as effective as reality in inducing emotional responses.7376

experiential cognitive therapy uses a combination of nutritional, cognitive, and behavioral procedures to help the patient identify and change the maintaining mechanisms in both obesity and eating disorders (see Table 2). However, experiential cognitive therapy differs from the typical CBT approach in the VR bodyimage rescripting protocol, in its focus on empowerment, and in its focus on the negative emotions related to both body and eating. In VR sessions, the therapist uses a 20/20/20 rule. During the first 20 minutes, the therapist focuses on getting a clear understanding of the patients current concerns, level of general functioning, and experiences related to food. This part of the session tends to be characterized by patients doing most of the talking, although the therapist guides with questions and reflection to get a sense of the patients current status. The second 20 minutes is devoted to the VR experience. During this part of the session, the patient enters the virtual environment and faces a specific critical situation (see Table 1). Here the patient is helped in developing specific strategies for avoiding and/or coping with it. In the final 20 minutes, the therapist explores the patients understanding of what happened in VR and the specific reactionsemotional and behavioralto the different situations experienced. If needed, some new strategies for coping with the VR situations are presented and discussed. Experiential cognitive therapy has been tested in different case studies and two controlled trials with 211 obese patients14 and 36 binge-eating patients.15 Both controlled trials show that experiential cognitive therapy provides better results in the follow-up than competing approaches, including both nutritional therapy and CBT. Possible consequences of interacting in a virtual environment are side effects such as nausea and dizziness.92 However, in the studies aforementioned, thanks to the use of a commercial head-mounted display developed for use with video games (the Z800 Emagin 3D Visor), a VR exposure time no longer than 20 minutes, and the exclusion of subjects with vestibular problems (e.g., vertigo, labyrinthitis), no side effects were reported.

Virtual Reality and the Experience of the Body

Virtual reality has also been used to induce controlled changes to the experience of the body.7779 On one hand, different authors showed that is possible to use VR both to induce an illusory perception of a fake limb80 or a fake hand81 as part of our own body and to produce an out-of-body experience79 by altering the normal association between touch and its visual correlate. It is even possible to generate a body transfer illusion: Slater and coworkers80 substituted the experience of male subjects own bodies with a life-sized virtual human female body. On the other hand, it is also possible to use VR to improve body image,82,83 even in patients with eating disorders84,85 or obesity.14,86 In a VR experiment, Burgess and colleagues87 examined the neural systems involved in the retrieval of the spatial context of an event. The measured activation showed the buffering of the location of scene elements in successively translated frames of reference (allocentric, body centered, head centered) between the parahippocampus and the precuneus.

All these data suggest that it may be possible to use VR to induce a controlled sensory rearrangement that facilitates an update of the locked allocentric representation of the body. A possible strategy toward this goal is the adaptation to VR the imagery rescripting method developed for the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorders.88 Specifically, Riva77,83 developed a specific body-image rescripting protocol based on VR (see Table 1) that was included as part of the experiential cognitive therapy. Developed by Riva and associates,89,90 experiential cognitive therapy is a relatively short-term (812 weeks), patientoriented approach that focuses on individual discovery.14,91 As in the case of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT),

Conclusions
Other investigators have reported that a distorted body image that drives behavior and influences information processing may be associated with eating-related diseases.9395 Stice and Agras96,97 introduced the dualpathway model of bulimic pathology that has some

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

287

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

Table 1. Virtual Reality Body-Image Rescripting Protocol


Phase 1: interview Phase 2: development of the VR scene During a clinical interview, the patient is asked to relive the contents of the allocentric negative body image and the situation(s) in which it was created and/or reinforced (e.g., being teased by my boyfriend at home) in as much detail as possible. The meaning of the experience for the patient was also elicited. The clinician reproduces the setting of the identified situation (e.g., the corridor of the classroom where my boyfriend teased me) using one of the different scenes available in the free NeuroVR software (http://www.neurovr.org) The available scenes are as follows: Phase 3: allocentric experience of the VR scene apartment kitchen office school restaurant pub supermarket beach swimming pool city square garden the body-image room (in which the patient can select her/his ideal and perceived body from a list of predefined body shapes and compare them with her/his own real body)

The patient is asked to re-experience the event in VR from a first-person perspective (the patient does not see his/ her body in the scene), expressing and discussing his/her feelings. The patient is then asked what needed to happen to change their feelings in a positive direction. The questions asked follow the Socratic approach, for example, What would need to happen for you to feel better? How does it look through the eyes of a third person? Is there anything you as a third person like to do? How do the other people respond? The main cognitive techniques used in this phase, if needed, are as follows: Countering: Once a list of distorted perceptions and cognitions is developed, the process of countering these thoughts and beliefs begins. In countering, the patient is taught to recognize the error in thinking and substitute more appropriate perceptions and interpretations. Label shifting: The patient first tries to identify the kinds of negative words she uses to interpret situations in her life, such as bad, terrible, obese, inferior, and hateful. The situations in which these labels are used are then listed. The patient and therapist replace each emotional label with two or more descriptive words.

Phase 4: egocentric experience of the VR scene

The patient is asked to re-experience the event in VR from a third-person perspective (the patient sees his/her body in the scene), intervening to both calm and reassure his/her virtual avatar and to counter any negative evaluation. The main cognitive techniques used in this phase, if needed, are as follows: Alternative interpretation: The patient learns to stop and consider other interpretations of a situation before proceeding to the decision-making stage. The patient develops a list of problem situations, evoked emotions, and interpretative beliefs. The therapist and patient discuss each interpretation and, if possible, identify the kind of objective data that would confirm one of them as correct. Deactivating the illness belief: The therapist first helps the client list her beliefs concerning weight and eating. The extent to which the illness model influences each belief is identified. The therapist then teaches the client a cognitive/behavioral approach to interpreting maladaptive eating behaviors and shows they can be understood from this framework.

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

288

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

Table 2. Structure of the Experiential Cognitive Therapy


Week 1 Psychometric test (test) Preliminary group (motivation to treatment and definition of rehabilitative protocol) Nutritional assessment

Psychodiagnostic interview

similarities with the proposed approach. In their view, internalization of the thin ideal contributes to body dissatisfaction because this ideal is virtually unattainable. This model hypothesizes that elevated pressure to be thin fosters body dissatisfaction because it promotes discontent with ones body. This increased body dissatisfaction, in turn, fosters dieting and negative affect, which consequently increases the risk for eating disorders. However, our proposed theory has some advantage. It identifies the source of the distorted body image in a specific cognitive process, the egocentric allocentric transformation in spatial cognition. This cognitive bias is not the only cause of the problem (necessary but not sufficient condition), but it is associated with two more factors: The individuals inability to counter the bias using normal sensory inputs (allocentric lock).

Session 1 VR Assessment + body image (virtual balance + sitting room)

Weeks 2 and 3 Session 2 VR Eating control + interpersonal reframe (kitchen + bathroom + bedroom) Session 3 VR Body image (body image VR scale) Session 4 VR Eating control (supermarket) Nutritional group (2/3 sessions)

Psychological group (1 session)

Physical activity Weeks 4 and 5

Session 5 VR Body image + interpersonal reframe (gymnasium) Session 6 VR Eating control + interpersonal reframe (pub) Session 7 VR Body image + interpersonal reframe (clothes shop)

Nutritional group (2/3 sessions)

The cultural suggestion promoted by media that the best way to address negative emotions related to the bias and to improve body image is through eating restraint and dietary control. Virtual reality is a possible tool for modifying the locked negative body image. An integrated treatmentthe experiential cognitive therapythat included a specific body-image rescripting protocol based on VR was able to achieve good results in the long term with obese and binge-eating patients, and may be as good or better than other approaches involving nutritional and cognitive behavioral therapies. Two final suggestions are made for improving programs used for weight control in adolescents. First, the inclusion of a body-image protocol, targeting body dissatisfaction, may reduce the rate of unhealthful weight-control behaviors. Second, the inclusion of a social support program, helping the adolescent to cope with interpersonal stress, may reduce the risk of an allocentric lock. In conclusion, as for any new perspective, much more research is needed. However, treatment is best accomplished when we know the causes of a disorder. For this reason, any step, even if partial, toward a better understanding of obesity and eating disorders may be useful for advancing treatment of these complex and multifaceted diseases.

Psychological group (1 session)

Physical activity

Weeks 6 and 7 Session 8 VR Eating control + interpersonal reframe (restaurant) Session 9 VR Body image + interpersonal reframe (swimming pool + beach) Session 10 VR Eating control + body image (kitchen + body image VR scale + 9 doors room) Week 8 Psychological support (2/3 sessions) Physical activity Final group (motivation to out-patient phase) Psychometric tests (retest) Nutritional group (2/3 sessions)

Psychological group (1 session)

Physical activity

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

289

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

Funding: This work was partially funded by the VEPSY Updated (IST-200025323; http://www.cybertherapy.info) and INTERSTRESS (FP7-247685; http://www.interstress.eu) European research funded projects and by the Italian MIUR FIRB IVT 2010 (RBIN04BC5C) research project. References: 1. Haines J, Neumark-Sztainer D. Prevention of obesity and eating disorders: a consideration of shared risk factors. Health Educ Res. 2006;21(6):77082. 2. Neumark-Sztainer D, Wall M, Guo J, Story M, Haines J, Eisenberg M. Obesity, disordered eating, and eating disorders in a longitudinal study of adolescents: how do dieters fare 5 years later? J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(4):55968. 3. Neumark-Sztainer D. Preventing obesity and eating disorders in adolescents: what can health care providers do? J Adolesc Health. 2009;44(3):20613. 4. Johnston JM. Eating disorders and childhood obesity: who are the real gluttons? CMAJ. 2004;171(12):145960. 5. Stice E, Presnell K, Shaw H, Rohde P. Psychological and behavioral risk factors for obesity onset in adolescent girls: a prospective study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(2):195202. 6. Stice E, Davis K, Miller NP, Marti CN. Fasting increases risk for onset of binge eating and bulimic pathology: a 5-year prospective study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117(4):9416. 7. Singh GK, Kogan MD, Van Dyck PC, Siahpush M. Racial/ethnic, socioeconomic, and behavioral determinants of childhood and adolescent obesity in the United States: analyzing independent and joint associations. Ann Epidemiol. 2008;18(9):68295. 8. Raynor H, Maier D, Dietz K, Kieras MA. What is the evidence of a causal relationship between dieting, obesity, and eating disorders in youth? J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(9):135961. 9. Riva G, Marchi S, Molinari E. Body image and eating restraint: a structural modeling analysis. Eat Weight Disord. 2000;5(1):3842. 10. Kostanski M, Gullone E. Dieting and body image in the childs world: conceptualization and behavior. J Genet Psychol. 1999;160(4):48899. 11. Schwartz MB, Henderson KE. Does obesity prevention cause eating disorders? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(8):7846. 12. Riva G. Virtual body, real pain: the allocentric lock hypothesis. www.cimec.unitn.it/events/brw/Poster/107_abs_GIUSEPPE_RIVA.pdf. 13. Riva G. Neuroscience and eating disorders: the role of the medial temporal lobe. Nature Precedings. 2010. http://precedings.nature.com/ documents/4235/. Accessed October 6, 2010. 14. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Cesa G, Conti S, Castelnuovo G, Mantovani F, Molinari E. Is severe obesity a form of addiction? Rationale, clinical approach, and controlled clinical trial. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2006;9(4):45779. 15. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Cesa G, Conti S, Molinari E. Six-month follow-up of in-patient experiential-cognitive therapy for binge eating disorders. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2003;6(3):2518. 16. Klatzky RL. Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In: Freska C, Habel C, Wender KF, eds. Spatial Cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing of spatial knowledge. Berlin: Springer; 1998,117. 17. Galati G, Lobel E, Vallar G, Berthoz A, Pizzamiglio L, Le Bihan D. The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133(2):15664. 18. Longo MR, Azan L, Haggard P. More than skin deep: body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(3):65568.
J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

19. Frith U, de Vignemont F. Egocentrism, allocentrism, and Asperger syndrome. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):71938. 20. Juurmaa J, Lehtinen-Railo S. Visual experience and access to spatial knowledge. J Vis Impair Blind. 1994;88(2):15770. 21. Amorim MA. What is my avatar seeing?: the coordination of out-of-body and embodied perspectives for scene recognition across views. Visual Cognition. 2005;10(2):15799. 22. Robinson JA, Swanson KL. Field and observer modes of remembering. Memory. 1993;1(3):16984. 23. Mace JH, Atkinson E, Moeckel CH, Torres V. Accuracy and perspective in involuntary autobiographical memory. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2011;25(1):208. 24. Bordo S. Unbearable weight: feminism, Western culture, and the body. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1993. 25. Featherstone M, Hepworth M, Turner BS, eds.. The body: social process and cultural theory. London: Sage; 1991. 26. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Exacting beauty: theory, assessment and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. 27. Jackson LA. Physical appearance and gender: sociobiological and sociocultural perspectives. Albany: State University of New York; 1992. 28. Grogan S. Body image: understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women, and children. London: Routledge; 1999. 29. Fredrickson BL, Roberts TA, Noll SM, Quinn DM, Twenge JM. That swimsuit becomes you: sex differences in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75(1):26984. 30. Cash TF, Pruzinsky T, eds. Body image: a handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. 31. DArgembeau A, Van der Linden M, dAcremont M, Mayers I. Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for social and non-social events in social phobia. Memory. 2006;14(5):63747. 32. Clark DM, Wells A. A cognitive model of social phobia. In: Heimberg RG, Liebowitz MR, Hope DA, Schneier FR, eds. Social phobia: diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 1995, 6993. 33. Eich E, Nelson AL, Leghari MA, Handy TC. Neural systems mediating field and observer memories. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(11):223951. 34. Mather M. Emotional arousal and memory binding: an objectbased framework. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(1):3352. 35. Byrne P, Becker S. A principle for learning egocentric-allocentric transformation. Neural Comput. 2008;20(3):70937. 36. Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007;114(2):34075. 37. Parron C, Save E. Comparison of the effects of entorhinal and retrosplenial cortical lesions on habituation, reaction to spatial and non-spatial changes during object exploration in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82(1):111. 38. Hargreaves EL, Rao G, Lee I, Knierim JJ. Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science. 2005;308(5729):17924. 39. Smeets MA, Ingleby JD, Hoek HW, Panhuysen GE. Body size perception in anorexia nervosa: a signal detection approach. J Psychosom Res. 1999;46(5):46577. 40. Sachdev P, Mondraty N, Wen W, Gulliford K. Brains of anorexia nervosa patients process self-images differently from non-selfimages: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(8):21618. 41. File SE, Kenny PJ, Cheeta S. The role of the dorsal hippocampal serotonergic and cholinergic systems in the modulation of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;66(1):6572.

290

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

42. Vyas A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Chattarji S. Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci. 2002;22(15):68108. 43. McLaughlin KJ, Gomez JL, Baran SE, Conrad CD. The effects of chronic stress on hippocampal morphology and function: an evaluation of chronic restraint paradigms. Brain Res. 2007;1161:5764. 44. Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(6):45362. 45. Hartmann A, Zeeck A, Barrett MS. Interpersonal problems in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2010;43(7):61927. 46. Rojo L, Conesa L, Bermudez O, Livianos L. Influence of stress in the onset of eating disorders: data from a two-stage epidemiologic controlled study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(4):62835. 47. Sassaroli S, Ruggiero GM. The role of stress in the association between low self-esteem, perfectionism, and worry, and eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2005;37(2):13541. 48. Ruggiero GM, Bertelli S, Boccalari L, Centorame F, Ditucci A, La Mela C, Scarinci A, Vinai P, Scarone S, Sassaroli S. The influence of stress on the relationship between cognitive variables and measures of eating disorders (in healthy female university students): a quasi-experimental study. Eat Weight Disord. 2008;13(3):1428. 49. Lo Sauro C, Ravaldi C, Cabras PL, Faravelli C, Ricca V. Stress, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and eating disorders. Neuropsychobiology. 2008;57(3):95115. 50. De Vriendt T, Moreno LA, De Henauw S. Chronic stress and obesity in adolescents: scientific evidence and methodological issues for epidemiological research. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis. 2009;19(7):5119. 51. Coccurello R, DAmato FR, Moles A. Chronic social stress, hedonism and vulnerability to obesity: lessons from rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33(4):53750. 52. Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, La Fleur SE, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Bell ME, Bhatnagar S, Laugero KD, Manalo S. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of comfort food. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(20):11696701. 53. Rosen JC. Improving body image in obesity. In: Thompson JK, ed. Body image, eating disorders and obesity. Washington DC: APA; 1996, 42540. 54. Lu HY, Hou HY. Testing a model of the predictors and consequences of body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2009;6(1):1923. 55. Castelnuovo G, Gaggioli A, Mantovani F, Riva G. From psychotherapy to e-therapy: the integration of traditional techniques and new communication tools in clinical settings. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2003;6(4):37582. 56. Riva G. From telehealth to e-health: internet and distributed virtual reality in health care. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2000;3(6):98998. 57. Myers TC, Swan-Kremeier L, Wonderlich S, Lancaster K, Mitchell JE. The use of alternative delivery systems and new technologies in the treatment of patients with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2004;36(2):12343. 58. Gorini A, Gaggioli A, Vigna C, Riva G. A second life for eHealth: prospects for the use of 3-D virtual worlds in clinical psychology. J Med Internet Res. 2008;10(3):e21. 59. Manzoni GM, Pagnini F, Gorini A, Preziosa A, Castelnuovo G, Molinari E, Riva G. Can relaxation training reduce emotional eating in women with obesity? An exploratory study with 3 months of follow-up. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109(8):142732. 60. Riva G. Virtual reality: an experiential tool for clinical psychology. Br J Guidance Counsel. 2009;37(3):33745. 61. Rothbaum BO, Anderson P, Zimand E, Hodges L, Lang D, Wilson J. Virtual reality exposure therapy and standard (in vivo) exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying. Behav Ther. 2006;37(1):8090.

62. Botella C, Villa H, Garca-Palacios A, Baos RM, Quero S, Alcaiz M, Riva G. Virtual reality exposure in the treatment of panic disorder and agoraphobia: a controlled study. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2007;14(3):16475. 63. Garcia-Palacios A, Hoffman H, Carlin A, Furness TA 3rd, Botella C. Virtual reality in the treatment of spider phobia: a controlled study. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(9):98393. 64. Nemire K, Beil J, Swan RW. Preventing teen smoking with virtual reality. Cyberpsychol Behav. 1999;2(1):3547. 65. Gatti E, Massari R, Sacchelli C, Lops T, Gatti R, Riva G. Why do you drink? Virtual reality as an experiential medium for the assessment of alcohol-dependent individuals. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2008;132:1327. 66. Bordnick PS, Graap KM, Copp HL, Brooks J, Ferrer M. Virtual reality cue reactivity assessment in cigarette smokers. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2005;8(5):48792. 67. Bordnick PS, Traylor A, Copp HL, Graap KM, Carter B, Ferrer M, Walton AP. Assessing reactivity to virtual reality alcohol based cues. Addict Behav. 2008;33(6):74356. 68. Wiederhold BK, Wiederhold MD. Virtual reality therapy for anxiety disorders: advances in evaluation and treatment. Washington DC: APA; 2005. 69. Botella C, Quero S, Baos RM, Perpi C, Garca Palacios A, Riva G. Virtual reality and psychotherapy. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2004;99:3754. 70. Baos RM, Botella C, Alcaiz M, Liao V, Guerrero B, Rey B. Immersion and emotion: their impact on the sense of presence. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2004;7(6):73441. 71. Riva G, Mantovani F, Capideville CS, Preziosa A, Morganti F, Villani D, Gaggioli A, Botella C, Alcaiz M. Affective interactions using virtual reality: the link between presence and emotions. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2007;10(1):4556. 72. Riva G. Virtual reality and telepresence. Science. 2007;318(5854):12402. 73. Vincelli F. From imagination to virtual reality: the future of clinical psychology. Cyberpsychol Behav. 1999;2(3):2418. 74. Riva G, Molinari E, Vincelli F. Interaction and presence in the clinical relationship: virtual reality (VR) as communicative medium between patient and therapist. IEEE Trans Inf Technol Biomed. 2002;6(3):198205. 75. Gorini A, Capideville CS, De Leo G, Mantovani F, Riva G. The role of immersion and narrative in mediated presence: the virtual hospital experience. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2010. Epub ahead of print. 76. Gorini A, Griez E, Petrova A, Riva G. Assessment of the emotional responses produced by exposure to real food, virtual food and photographs of food in patients affected by eating disorders. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2010;9:30. 77. Riva G. Modifications of body-image induced by virtual reality. Percept Mot Skills. 1998;86(1):16370. 78. Alcaiz M, Perpia C, Baos R, Lozano JA, Montesa J, Botella C, Garcia Palacios A, Villa H, Villa J. A new realistic 3D body representation in virtual environments for the treatment of disturbed body image in eating disorders. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2000;3(3):4339. 79. Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O. Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science. 2007;317(5841):10969. 80. Slater M, Perez-Marcos D, Ehrsson HH, Sanchez-Vives MV. Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Front Neurosci. 2009;3(2):21420. 81. Perez-Marcos D, Slater M, Sanchez-Vives MV. Inducing a virtual hand ownership illusion through a brain-computer interface. Neuroreport. 2009;20(6):58994.

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

291

www.journalofdst.org

The Key to Unlocking the Virtual Body: Virtual Reality in the Treatment of Obesity and Eating Disorders

Riva

82. Riva G, Melis L. Virtual reality for the treatment of body image disturbances. In: Riva G, ed. Virtual reality in neuro-psychophysiology: Cognitive, clinical and methodological issues in assessment and rehabilitation. Amsterdam: IOS Press; 1997, 95111. 83. Riva G. Virtual environment for body-image modification: Virtual reality system for the treatment of body image disturbances. Comput Human Behav. 1998;14(3):47790. 84. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Baruffi M, Molinari E. Virtual-realitybased multidimensional therapy for the treatment of body image disturbances in binge eating disorders: a preliminary controlled study. IEEE Trans Inf Technol Biomed. 2002;6(3):22434. 85. Perpia C, Botella C, Baos R, Marco J, Alcaiz M, Quero S. Body image and virtual reality in eating disorders: is exposure by virtual reality more effective than the classical body image treatment? Cyberpsychol Behav. 1999;2(2):14955. 86. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Baruffi M, Molinari E. Virtual reality-based multidimensional therapy for the treatment of body image disturbances in obesity: a controlled study. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2001;4(4):51126. 87. Burgess N, Becker S, King JA, OKeefe J. Memory for events and their spatial context: models and experiments. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356(1413):1493503. 88. Smucker MR, Dancu C, Foa EB, Niederee JL. Imagery rescripting: A new treatment for survivors of childhood sexual abuse suffering from posttraumatic stress. J Cognitive Psychother. 1995;9(1):317. 89. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Baruffi M, Rinaldi S, Molinari E. Experiential cognitive therapy in anorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disord. 1998;3(3):14150. 90. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Baruffi M, Rinaldi S, Vincelli F, Molinari E. Virtual reality based experiential cognitive treatment of obesity and binge-eating disorders. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2000;7(3):20919. 91. Riva G, Botella C, Lgeron P, Optale G, eds. Cybertherapy: internet and virtual reality as assessment and rehabilitation tools for clinical psychology and neuroscience. Amsterdam: IOS Press; 2004. http://www.cybertherapy.info/pages/book3.htm. 92. Stanney KM, Kennedy RS. The psychometrics of cybersickness. Commun ACM. 1997;40(8):678. 93. Williamson DA. Body image disturbance in eating disorders: a form of cognitive bias. Eating Disord. 1996;4(1):4758. 94. Altabe M, Thompson JK. Body image: a cognitive self-schema construct? Cognitive Ther Res. 1996;20(2):17193. 95. Williamson DA, Stewart TM, White MA, York-Crowe E. An information-processing perspective on body image. In: Cash TF, Pruzinsky T, eds. Body image: a handbook of theory, reasearch, and clinical practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2002, 4754. 96. Stice E. A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001;110(1):12435. 97. Stice E, Agras WS. Predicting onset and cessation of bulimic behaviors during adolescence: a longitudinal grouping analyses. Behav Ther. 1998;29:25776.

J Diabetes Sci Technol Vol 5, Issue 2, March 2011

292

www.journalofdst.org

You might also like