Applying Social Psychology
Applying Social Psychology
Applying Social Psychology
research and learning that transforms society and our understanding of individuals, groups
and cultures. SAGE is the independent, innovative, natural home for authors, editors and
societies who share our commitment and passion for the social sciences.
Find out more at: www.sagepublications.com
ISBN 978-1-4462-4907-9
ISBN 978-1-4462-4908-6
Contents
About the Authors
Preface
How to Use this Text
INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND
1 APPLYING SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Example of the Application of Social Psychological Theories
Other Relevant Decisions
Applying Social Psychology: The PATH from Problem to Intervention
Problems with Applying Theories
Conclusion
2 THE PROBLEM PHASE: FROM A PROBLEM TO A PROBLEM DEFINITION
Introduction
Key Questions for a Problem Definition
Research for a Problem Definition
Completing the Problem Definition
3 THE ANALYSIS PHASE: FINDING THEORY-BASED EXPLANATIONS FOR PROBLEMS
Introduction
Specifying the Outcome Variable
Requirements for the Outcome Variable
The Divergent Phase: Generating Explanations
Theoretical Approaches under Investigation: The Case of Safe Sex Promotion
The Convergent Phase: Reducing the Number of Explanations
4 THE TEST PHASE: DEVELOPING AND TESTING THE PROCESS MODEL
Introduction
Formulating a Process Model
Heuristics for Developing a Process Model
Testing the Process Model
Abraham (Bram) P. Buunk has been an Academy Professor in Evolutionary Social Psychology at
the University of Groningen, on behalf of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, since
2005. His main current interest is the application of evolutionary theorizing to human social
behaviour. He has published over 500 articles and chapters on applied topics, including professional
burnout, jealousy, absenteeism, AIDS-prevention, loneliness, depression, marital satisfaction, wellbeing among the elderly, psychological effects of oral contraception, and coping with cancer. He is
co-editor of Health, coping and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory
(Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1997), and Applied Social Psychology (Cambridge University Press,
2008). He has served, among others, on scientific boards for the Dutch Cancer Foundation (NKBKWF) and the Dutch AIDS Foundation. He was a member of the Programme Committee on Evolution
and Behaviour of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). Currently Bram
Buunk isconducting research particularly in Latin America on various applied psychology themes
from an evolutionary perspective. He received in 2012 the distinction of Honorary Professor at the
Universidad Catolica de Santa Maria (Arequipa, Peru).
Mark van Vugt is Professor of Social and Organizational Psychology at VU University, the
Netherlands, and he holds honorary positions at the University of Oxford and the University of Kent,
United Kingdom. He started his academic studies in Psychology in the Netherlands and he worked at
various universities in the UK before returning to his birth town Amsterdam to take on a
professorship in Psychology. He has published over 100 articles in outlets such as Nature,
Psychological Science, and the American Psychologist on various topics in social, organizational
and evolutionary psychology, including leadership and management, altruism and cooperation, social
identity and intergroup conflict, sex differences, transport, and environmental sustainability. He is a
former Associate Editor of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology and co-author of
several books, including a trade book on leadership, titled Selected (Profile/Harper, 2010). Mark van
Vugt is a Fellow of the Royal Academy for Arts and Commerce in London and blogs onPsychology
Today.
Preface
One of the wonderful experiences in life is that of having a problem and calling an expert, who walks
in, takes a look, makes reassuring noises, goes to work and, hey presto, your central heating system
starts spreading comfort and happiness again. When I returned to academia after a stint as
management consultant, I realized, looking back, that I might have fallen somewhat short in providing
clients with these wonderful experiences.
Reflecting on what I had actually been using of the knowledge and tools acquired during my
training as a social psychologist, I realized that the tools had come in handy but that the application of
knowledge/theories hardly figured prominently. I was well equipped to interview people, construct
questionnaires and surveys and arrive at an adequate analysis of problems. Yet when it came to
providing solutions it seemed I had been mainly relying on common sense combined with the usual
role of process consultant. This is a bit like your central heating engineer presenting you with,
admittedly, a fine diagnosis of the problem and then offering to hold your hand while you wrestle with
finding a way of getting the system to deliver some heat again.
People, groups and organizations are obviously much more complex than the simple systems that
keep the house operating: all the more reason to train future practitioners in using the theories and
accumulated bodies of knowledge available. Extensive screening of the literature at the time did not
throw up the desired textbook/training manual. So I started out developing my own course, which
after the usual evolutionary developments has now taken shape as the PATH (ProblemAnalysis
TestHelp) model presented in this book.
At first sight this model looks the same as every other problem-solving course. The essential
differences the PATH model introduces are twofold:
1. From the very beginning it stresses using theories (plural) that might help to define and
delineate the problem and, in the problem-solving phase, suggests solutions that consequently
have a solid foundation in theory and research.
2. In finding solutions, it examines factors that have a realistic chance of being changed.
In addition to making better use of the available knowledge, the PATH model has the happy sideeffect that practising social psychologists are better protected against confusing themselves or their
clients.
When in later life I was in charge of a large organization, using consultants from time to time, I was
often struck by the difference between the business school alumni and social/organizational
psychologists. The first category was strong on analysis and practical solutions they claimed had
worked for others. The psychologists were strong on analysis and nearly always flavoured their
solutions with a whiff of how things ought to be. Both groups succeeded in keeping any reference to
research and theory well out of sight. This is actually good practice in an applied setting: the average
The book contains several text boxes in which well-known social psychologists around the
globe discuss why they got interested in applied social psychology and give examples of
their applied research programmes.
7. Tests, measurements and instruments
The text contains various examples of standard tests and measurement scales that are
frequently used in applied social psychology. Examples are the self-esteem scale and the
SYMLOG group observation instrument.
8. Glossary of key social psychological theories and concepts
For best use of the text, we have identified a list of key social psychological theories and
concepts and provide brief summaries of these in text boxes. It is advisable to use a core
introductory text in social psychology for further details about theories and relevant
research.
9. Case studies
Each core chapter contains an example of research into a particular applied social
psychology topic. This example serves as an illustration of how to conduct applied social
psychology research.
10. Key novel features
11. Appendix with examples of schematic PATH models
and includes examples of applied social psychology research programmes from around the world.
We hope that our book will inspire many new generations of students across the world in doing
social psychology and give them the necessary tools for applying social psychology to pressing social
issues. There is much work to be done!
Abraham P. Buunk, Amsterdam, 2013
Mark van Vugt, Amsterdam, 2013
1
Applying Social Psychology
Contents
Example of the Application of Social Psychological Theories
Step 1 Problem: Formulating a Problem Definition
Step 2 Analysis: Finding Explanations for the Problem
Step 3 Test: Developing and Testing the Process Model
Step 4 Help: Towards an Intervention Programme
Other Relevant Decisions
Applying Social Psychology: The PATH from Problem to Intervention
Step 1 Problem: From a Problem to a Problem Definition
Step 2 Analysis: From a Problem Definition to Analysis and Explanation
Step 3 Test: From Explanations to a Process Model
Step 4 Help: From a Process Model to Intervention
Problems with Applying Theories
Oversimplification
External Validity
Contradictory Evidence
Conclusion
Can social psychology help in solving societal problems? And if this is the case, how can social
psychology do so? Social psychology is a basic science which tries to build knowledge primarily
through experiments and surveys (see, for examples, Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2010; Baumeister
& Bushman, 2010; Hewstone, Stroebe, & Jonas, 2012; Hogg & Cooper, 2007; Kenrick, Neuberg, &
Cialdini, 2010; Myers, 2012; Van Lange, Kruglanski, & Higgins, 2011).
Sometimes the theories and findings from social psychology may seem a bit remote from the
problems in society. However, many, if not most, societal problems have social psychological
aspects (for example, crime, racism, environmental pollution), and therefore social psychology may
not only help in clarifying such problems, but also contribute to finding solutions. In this chapter we
give an example of one such problem to illustrate this point, the debilitating problem of HIV/AIDS in
Africa and the lack of support for HIV/AIDS victims. We also show how social psychological
knowledge could lead to the development of a theoretical model on which an intervention might be
based. Finally, we briefly outline the approach presented in this book, the PATH methodology,
through which such models may be developed. This chapter thus summarizes the entire approach.
Although the possibilities for treatment have improved considerably, HIV/AIDS still is an
incurable disease that deeply affects the lives of those involved. In addition, more than any other
disease, HIV/AIDS is surrounded by taboos and often leads to the stigmatization and isolation of
patients (Dijker, Koomen, & Kok, 1997). Patients are often abandoned by their families and friends.
For adequate forms of medical and psychosocial help and support of people with HIV/AIDS in poor
countries, considerably more money is required than is currently available. Yet while the treatment of
people with HIV/AIDS has steadily improved, the willingness to donate money to help and support
people with HIV/AIDS has actually decreased (Van Vugt, Snyder, Tyler, & Biel, 2000).
Raising Money to Fight AIDS A team of volunteers from a national HIV/AIDS charity foundation
wishes to set up a campaign to raise funds for the purpose of providing medical and psychosocial
care for people with HIV/AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa. Some team members argue that the campaign
should not be too dramatic as it is now generally known how serious it is to be infected with HIV.
They are concerned that showing too many depressing stories and pictures of people with HIV/AIDS
will adversely affect the willingness to donate money. Others argue that just because there has been
less media interest in HIV/AIDS recently, the campaign should highlight the severe and incurable
nature of the disease. In doing so, there is a need to emphasize that the victims are not to blame, and
that everybody is potentially at risk of contracting HIV. Accordingly, one part of the team wants to
actively approach the media, whereas the others are concerned about the lack of media interest in this
topic. A related point of debate concerns the campaign slogan. Should it be something positive, like
Standing Up Against AIDS, or something more dramatic like Fighting the Horrors of AIDS?
One volunteer suggests it would be better as part of the campaign to develop a product which
people can buy, like a music CD of African artists, because in that case giving money would look less
like charity. Another issue that comes up in the discussion is whether to use television and newspaper
advertisements to raise money for the campaign, or to take a more personal, door-to-door, approach.
Regarding the latter, should potential donors see a list of contributors and how much they have each
contributed? One of the volunteers suggests showing just one large gift to encourage potential donors
to match this donation. Other volunteers worry that this might put people off, because it will be
difficult to match such an amount.
The Relevance of Social Psychology The volunteer team decides to consult a social psychologist to
help them develop their campaign. What suggestions should the social psychologist make? This
psychologist might have little experience with campaigns to raise money for the fight against
HIV/AIDS. Yet she will know the social psychological literature on how to influence people and
might know how to apply this to cases such as the HIV/AIDS campaign.
The social psychologist might of course conclude that more research is needed on why people
donate money to charities. Given the urgency of the issue, however, this might take too long. Instead,
there is an abundant amount of social psychological literature on peoples willingness to donate
money for charity that the psychologist can consult. Based on this, she might come up with specific
suggestions on how to set up the campaign. Yet a better approach would be to first analyse the issue
in greater detail and address the relevant causes and conditions for charity giving. Therefore, what
she must do first is develop an adequate problem definition. This is the P-phase of the PATH
methodology.
After a series of discussions with the team, the social psychologist defines the problem as follows:
Many people in Africa suffer from HIV/AIDS, and there is insufficient funding to provide adequate forms of medical and
psychosocial help and support for these people. Which factors determine potential donors willingness to donate money for this
cause? How can we set up a campaign that would raise money to help people with HIV/AIDS in Africa?
literature first, and formulates the problem in terms of two general questions:
1. When are people most inclined to help others?
2. What attributes of victims elicit the most helping responses?
She states these questions quite broadly because it is better at this stage to explore the literature more
globally in order not to miss any relevant knowledge. Next, she conducts a search on the internet for
books on helping with key words such as helping, altruism, cooperation, and prosocial
behaviour, and finds a number of recent titles, including The psychology of prosocial behavior by
the German and American social psychologists Stefan Strmer and Mark Snyder (2009),The
altruism question by the American psychologist Dan Batson (1991), The psychology of helping and
altruism by the American social scientists Jack Dovidio, Jane Piliavin, David Schroeder, and Louis
Penner (2006), the German social psychologist Hans Werner Bierhoffs Prosocial behaviour (2002),
a n d Cooperation in modern society: Promoting the welfare of communities, states, and
organizations by the Dutch, American, and Swedish social psychologists Mark Van Vugt, Mark
Snyder, Tom Tyler, and Anders Biel (2000). These books are usually all available in the local
university library. After consulting the literature, the social psychologist concludes that there are, in
fact, three different types of helping:
1. Emergency intervention, for example, helping someone who is the victim of a robbery or
accident.
2. Organizational helping, for example, volunteering to take on an administrative job at the
request of a manager.
3. Sharing and donating resources, for example, donating money to a charity.
It is quite obvious that the present problem, raising money for people with HIV/AIDS, concerns the
third prosocial behaviour. Yet, after reading the relevant literature, the social psychologist concludes
that most of the prosocial literature deals with emergency helping and organizational helping. There is
much less known about raising money for good causes. She explores the literature further, now by
consulting PsycINFO the electronic database that comprises all scientific articles and books in the
field of psychology between 1872 and the present day. There she finds a theoretical model belonging
to the Israeli social psychologist Shalom Schwartz, published in Advances in experimental social
psychology in 1977, which can be applied to all kinds of helping. The social psychologist decides to
use Schwartzs model as a basis for understanding the problem that underlies the campaign, that is,
how to increase peoples willingness to donate money for people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. She
presents this model to the team of volunteers and outlines the implications of the model for their
campaign.
The Schwartz Model In Schwartzs (1977) model there are various steps that affect peoples
prosocial behaviour. We present the most important here:
1. Awareness: There must be an awareness that others need help. The perceived need has to be
prominent, clear, and serious. We therefore need to draw attention to the fact that people with
HIV/AIDS in many African countries face severe physical and mental distress, and need more
medical, financial, and psychological support than is currently provided.
2. Opportunities to help: People must be aware that there are genuine opportunities for relieving
the needs of people with HIV/AIDS. Therefore, the campaign must convey that there are
various concrete actions that can improve the situation of victims.
3. Ability to help: People have to recognize their own ability to provide relief. If people feel
helpless, their awareness of the problem is reduced, and they will not feel very motivated to
offer help. Therefore, it should be emphasized, for instance, that even small donations make a
difference (for example, a 1 contribution means a family of five can eat for two days).
4. Personal norms: A major factor affecting helping behaviour is personal norms. These are
feelings of moral obligation that one should help specific needy others. Emphasizing the needs
of people with HIV/AIDS in Africa is an effective way to activate personal norms.
5. Responsibility: Finally, people also need to accept some responsibility for the problem in
order to become involved and offer aid. As we will discuss later on, this is an obstacle in the
case of the African HIV/AIDS problem.
Further, the literature suggests that people are more inclined to help when the recipients are
considered blameless. In general, people with an illness evoke more sympathy if they are not held
responsible for their fate (Graham, Weiner, Giuliano, & Williams, 1993; Steins & Weiner, 1999).
Also, the more sympathy individuals evoke the more help they receive (Rudolph, Roesch,
Greitemeyer, & Weiner, 2004). Knowing this, the social psychologist concludes that one of the
primary aims of the campaign should be to eradicate the (erroneous) belief that people with
HIV/AIDS in Africa are always themselves to blame for their illness.
Finally, helping is more likely when people are able to identify with the victims, for example,
because they are similar in age, profession and values. Similarity leads to empathy seeing oneself
in someone elses place which in turn leads to helping (Batson, 1991; Levy, Freitas, & Salovey,
2002; Niezink, Siero, Dijkstra, Buunk, & Barelds, 2012; Strmer, Snyder, Kropp, & Siem, 2002).
Although this may not be easy to achieve when the victims are in a remote place, this can nevertheless
be accomplished by providing potential donors with personal reports from HIV/AIDS victims in
Africa. This reduces the distance between helper and recipient and encourages people to empathize
with victims.
Belief in a Just World In the team meeting to discuss the campaign, someone suggests that people
may respond differently to victims of disasters abroad rather than at home. The social psychologist
tries to find out more about this possibility. She explores the literature further, and comes across a
chapter in a German book that explicitly deals with this theme. This chapter Solidaritt mit der
Dritten Welt [Solidarity with the Third World] is written by the German psychologist Leon
Montada (Montada, 2001). In this chapter Montada discusses the determinants of helping people in
Third World countries, including giving to charity and political activities. From Montadas study it
appears that helping is not related to empathy but to personal norms and one feeling a responsibility
to do something. This sense of responsibility is caused by guilt about ones privileged situation, anger
about the injustice/unfairness of the situation of people in poor countries, and the perception that
people in poor countries are not responsible for their fate.
When the social psychologist reports this information to the team, the team decides to focus on the
injustice that HIV/AIDS victims in the Third World receive and that due to the poverty and
insufficient health care, help for people with HIV/AIDS is desperately needed. The issues of justice
and fairness lead the psychologist to consider a theory about the belief in a just world
formulated by the Canadian social psychologist Melvin Lerner (1980), which assumes that people
have a natural tendency to believe they live in a just world in which everyone gets what they deserve.
This belief is a common worldview but while this belief is a universal phenomenon, there are
presumably considerable differences between people as to the degree to which they share it. For
someone who strongly adheres to the just world belief, events that shake this belief are threatening.
People are especially upset by the unexplained suffering of others, for example, someone who has
been working hard getting fired, young women getting raped in a war, or parents losing their child in
an accident. For someone who strongly believes in a just world, such events are so upsetting that they
will try to reduce this threat, sometimes by helping the victim to relieve their own suffering.
The social psychologist therefore concludes that the campaign would have to emphasize the
unfairness of the plight of people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. Yet she also discovers that helping a
victim is not the only way to deal with a threat to the just world belief. Lerner (1980) suggests that
people also sometimes cognitively reinterpret an unjust event by holding victims responsible for their
fate (He could have used a condom) or derogating them (He is morally irresponsible). In addition,
the social psychologist finds out about several studies which show that as individuals believe more
strongly in a just world they are less likely to donate to charity goals in Third World countries
(Campbell, Carr, & MacLachlan, 2001). Contrary to her initial thoughts, she therefore concludes that
the team should be careful to stress the injustice of the fate of people with HIV/AIDS in Africa.
Further scrutiny of the social psychological literature suggests a number of other factors that may
influence attitudes towards people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. In general, people have more sympathy
for victims the greater their belief that a similar event might happen to them (Montada, 1992; Silver,
Wortman, & Crofton, 1990). More specifically, as individuals have more HIV/AIDS-related
experiences (such as knowing people who have HIV/AIDS) they are more willing to help people with
HIV/AIDS (Cassel, 1995). Also, the greater the sympathy, the more social pressure there is to help
the victims (Batson & Powell, 2003). Finally, people with HIV/AIDS may be regarded as outgroup
members and this further reduces empathy and helping (Strmer & Snyder, 2009). On the basis of
these and other findings, obtained in the social psychological literature, the social psychologist then
builds a process model, an example of which is presented in Figure 1.1. We have added some more
examples of process models in the Appendix to give an idea of the diversity and opportunities of such
models in trying to analyse social psychological problems. These examples were adapted from the
coursework essays of students from Maastricht University taking a course in applied social
psychology, using this text (see Appendix).
In the model, the key outcome variable is the willingness to donate money to help people with
HIV/AIDS in Africa. There are a number of processes that influence this willingness, according to the
model. One factor is the attitude towards people with HIV/AIDS. Based on the just world hypothesis,
the more people believe that being infected with HIV is preventable, and the more they believe in a
just world, the more they will hold people with HIV/AIDS responsible for their own fate and donate
less. Thus, a potential problem for the campaign is that some people will feel that HIV/AIDS could
be prevented by having safe sex, and that, as a result, many feel that people with HIV/AIDS somehow
brought it upon themselves (for example, by living promiscuously).
Furthermore, research has shown that bad events happening to others evoke anger rather than
compassion if they could have been prevented. More specifically, research shows that illnesses and
diseases that are seen as controllable and preventable, such as AIDS and obesity, lead to a more
negative attitude towards the patient and less helping than uncontrollable diseases, such as
Alzheimers (Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson, 1988). This is especially the case among people who
strongly believe in a just world (Mantler, 2001). As a consequence, people with HIV/AIDS are often
negatively stereotyped, for instance, as having low moral worth (Walker, Taylor, & Green, 1990). A
social psychologist who has done significant work in the area of prejudice and stereotyping is
Professor Susan Fiske of Princeton University (see Box 1.1).
Box 1.1 Interview with Professor Susan Fiske of Princeton University (USA)
I always wanted to make the world a better place. My grandmother and great grandmother were suffragists (never
suffragettes!). And my mother worked full-time as a civic volunteer for citizens participation, urban neighbourhood
organizations, cleaner air, and better parks. My father was a psychologist, so I put the two lines together, wanting to use
psychology to improve things, especially for underdogs. But I realized, early on, that if you do not have the methodological
tools to make a convincing scientific argument, no one will listen. My father was a methodologist, so that probably helped
to drive that point home. Social psychology was the logical choice.
What I love about social psychology is that it argues for the importance of the social situation, the impact of people on
other people. If you think the important variance is in the situation (as opposed to, say, genes, or the first year of life), then
to improve peoples lives, you change the situation. This is an inherently progressive perspective.
My most exciting professional impact was being cited by the Supreme Court. Ann Hopkins had been the star of her
cohort at Price Waterhouse, billing more hours and respected by clients and colleagues alike. She was tough and exacting
and effective. Unfortunately, she was also the only female partner candidate out of about 90 that year, and in a business
utterly dominated by men at that point. People who didnt know her well, but who nevertheless voted, disliked this
aggressive female manager. She was turned down for partner on the basis of allegedly deficient social skills, being advised
that she could improve her chances by walking, talking, and dressing more femininely. Instead of going to charm school,
she sued.
Social psychology had a lot to offer Ann Hopkins, so I agreed to be an expert witness. I explained how perfectly wellintentioned P W partners could end up prescribing make-up and hair styling to a top-earning manager. Gender roles are
intrinsically prescriptive, and this makes sexism ambivalent. Let me explain. People love the stereotypic homemaker but
would not want her to run a company. At the same time, people respect the stereotypic businesswoman, but they tend to
dislike her. Peter Glick and I captured this Catch-22 in our Ambivalent Sexism Inventory, which picks up on the
benevolence toward traditional women and the hostility toward nontraditional women. Hostile sexism is not a new idea, but
subjectively benevolent sexism is. And it goes a long way toward explaining certain kinds of barriers to women in the
workplace.
A negative attitude towards people with HIV/AIDS is bolstered if they are perceived as not coping
well with the situation. Research has shown that victims who do not complain, and try to make the
best of their situation, receive more help and sympathy (Dovidio, Piliavin, Schroeder, & Penner,
2006). This implies that the campaign should not present people with HIV/AIDS in Africa as passive
victims who do not try to improve their situation. Although the attitude towards people with
HIV/AIDS in Africa will also be influenced by a perception of emotional closeness, this might be
difficult to evoke, and therefore the social psychologist decides to leave this factor out of the model.
As we have seen, any willingness to help people with HIV/AIDS in Africa is also affected by
feelings of moral obligation (Schwartz, 1977). Two factors, in particular, activate feelings of moral
obligation. First, the perceived needs of those people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. A second factor is
the perceived injustice of the poverty in Africa, but such feelings are weaker when people believe in
a just world (Lerner, 1980). A willingness to donate money will, in addition to feelings of moral
obligation and the attitude towards people with HIV/AIDS in Africa, also be affected by social
pressure from relevant others and by the perceived effectiveness of helping (Batson, 1991; Van Vugt
et al., 2000). The latter implies that the campaign must convince the public that the donated money is
3. Feelings of moral obligation will be induced by both showing that people with HIV/AIDS in
Africa are in desperate need of help and also by making a subtle appeal to feelings of injustice
with respect to the poverty in Africa.
4. The messages will be predominantly positive to prevent a negative attitude towards people
with HIV/AIDS in Africa (With a little gift, this person may have a long and productive life
ahead of them).
5. It will be made clear that every gift, no matter how small, will help (for example, feeding a
family for two days for as little as 1), and it will be clearly stated for what purpose the
donations will be used.
6. To lower the threshold to donate money, people will be able to donate money over the internet.
Using the PATH methodology as a helpful tool, we have introduced you to the main steps in moving
from a problem (how to raise money for people with HIV/AIDS in Africa) to the development of an
intervention programme to tackle this same problem. We have formulated the details of a campaign to
raise money for this worthy cause. Although the general approach of the campaign has now been
formulated by the team with the help of an applied social psychologist, many more decisions still
need to be made.
First, a decision must be made regarding the communication channel (McGuire, 1985). For
example, the team will have to decide whether to run a media campaign (television, radio, internet), a
door-to-door campaign, or a combination of the two. Each has its own logistical problems. The
media will not easily provide broadcast time for free, especially if they consider the topic to be of
insufficient interest to the public at large. For a door-to-door campaign one needs to recruit, organize,
and coordinate a large group of reliable volunteers throughout the country, which might be
cumbersome.
Another issue is whether donors receive something in exchange for their gift, for example, a music
CD by African artists for every donation over 50. The helping literature suggests that this may be a
good thing to do, and the popularity of crowdsourcing websites like Kickstarter show that such
practices can be highly effective. The norm of reciprocity states that individuals feel best when they
receive something in return for what they give (Buunk & Schaufeli, 1999; Cialdini & Trost, 1998).
Reciprocity is a powerful mechanism that has evolved to facilitate social exchange, and when this
norm is being violated, for instance when someone does not return a favour, people get upset and
angry (Delton, Krasnow, Cosmides, & Tooby, 2011; Van Vugt & Van Lange, 2006). As a
consequence, and considering the tendency to blame the victims, people might be more willing to give
if they know they will receive something in return. Gifts are more likely to be associated with an
acute disaster such as a drought or tsunami. With the HIV/AIDS problem a situation of prolonged
suffering people might be more willing to donate if they are to receive something in return which
will have intrinsic value to them, while at the same time they are doing something good. People can
engage in such a transaction without having to take a position about the causes of the problem. They
may think they are just getting a good deal.
Many other details will have to be decided, for example which product to offer, which media
channels to use, and a slogan for the campaign. For many of these questions, there is relevant social
psychological literature that can be consulted, for example, on persuasion (Cialdini, 2006; OKeefe,
2002), communication (Fassett & Warren, 2010), and social influence (Kenrick, Goldstein, &
Braver, 2012; Schultz & Oskamp, 2000). In addition, there is an applied literature on how to set up
fundraising campaigns (Clarke, Botting, & Norton, 2001).
Arriving at an adequate problem definition requires much consideration and deliberation. Usually, the
problem definition is more extensive than the one we formulated earlier in this chapter, where the
team knew already that they wanted to set up a fundraising campaign to help people with HIV/AIDS
in Africa. Often there is just a general feeling within a team, community, or organization that there is a
problem and something must be done without much further thought being involved. In the example of
an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, the team of volunteers may have simply been frustrated about a
lack of attention towards the plight of people with HIV/AIDS in Africa within their country. Getting
this attention would require quite a different approach from that required in setting up a fundraising
campaign. Further, an internal controversy on policy priorities within a charity organization is often
better dealt with by organizational psychologists and consultants.
As will be addressed in Chapter 2, it is very important to describe precisely what the problem is
(for example, How can we raise money to help people with AIDS in Africa?). But even when the
problem is presented clearly, other questions also need to be asked. We must assess if the problem is
sufficiently concrete rather than it being a general scientific question like: How can we make people
more altruistic? Also, why is it a problem at all (for example, People with HIV/AIDS in Africa
suffer greatly and have few opportunities for treatment) and for whom is it a problem (for example,
People with HIV/AIDS in Africa, their families, and their countries)? In addition, we must specify
the main causes of the problem: in this case, why we think people might be reluctant to give money to
this particular charitable cause, for example because they find it difficult to empathize with people in
Africa or there is competition coming from other charity organizations. Further, we should specify the
population we aim to target with our intervention (target group). Who do we need to convince that
this problem has to be solved? Who must help solve this problem? In the example of an HIV/AIDS
fundraising campaign, the volunteer team should determine who they want to encourage to donate
money the general public or specific subgroups (such as families with high incomes), private
persons or organizations and companies? Because they want to convince as many people as possible
to donate money, the team in the above example chose to target the general public.
Finally, the key aspects of the problem need to be considered. That is, a good problem definition
makes clear that the problem has an applied rather than a basic nature, and is formulated in concrete
terms. In the example of an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, this would give answers to the question
of why people may be reluctant to give money to HIV/AIDS charities. Last but not least, there must be
a feeling that the problem has social psychological aspects and that it is potentially solvable or
relievable.
In the first discussions with the AIDS team of volunteers, the focus might be on the irresponsible
attitude of some political leaders in Africa with respect to HIV/AIDS, or the attitude of the Roman
Catholic Church towards condom use. It is obvious that these issues are not problems that social
psychologists can easily solve (or should even want to solve). Changing the attitudes of political and
religious leaders may be done by using social psychological knowledge, but it probably requires a
sustained political and diplomatic effort.
In contrast, changing the attitudes of the general public towards people with HIV/AIDS in Africa is
a good example of the type of issue to which social psychologists may contribute. Such attitudes are
social psychological constructs, and there is a wealth of theorizing and research on how such attitudes
may be changed. In general, social psychological factors concern behaviours (for example, giving
money), attitudes (say, a negative evaluation of people with HIV/AIDS), cognitions (for example,
negative perceptions of people with HIV/AIDS), and affective/emotional responses (say, a fear of
AIDS). When the problem cannot be defined along one or more of these terms behaviours, attitudes,
cognitions, affective responses it is probably not suitable for a PATH analysis.
one first has to decide what the outcome variable is, that is, which variable eventually needs
changing. In the example of an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, it is a willingness to donate money
for people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. As will be described inChapter 3, after having defined this
variable, in the divergent stage, one starts looking for explanations through techniques such as free
association and through applying relevant social psychological theories. In the development of a
process model to explain a willingness to donate money for people with HIV/AIDS in Africa, the
psychologist in the example knew right away that he had to look in the literature on helping behaviour
and prosocial behaviour. Through a search in the helping literature, he found the model by Schwartz
(1977) that seemed quite relevant.
In retrospect, it might seem evident to look into this literature, but someone without a background in
social psychology might not have known where to look. Moreover, even when confining oneself to the
social psychological literature on helping, one might have found many different models and theories.
There are, for example, social exchange and reciprocity theories, emphasizing the role of egotistic
concerns in helping, which stem from evolutionary theories of altruism (Buunk & Schaufeli, 1999;
Hardy & Van Vugt, 2006; Van Vugt & Van Lange, 2006). By performing acts of kindness, individuals
may receive many benefits. They may, for instance, feel happier (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade,
2005), experience positive self-evaluations and a boost in self-esteem (for example, I did something
good today!, I am a caring person), receive praise, or experience the joy of seeing the needy person
experience relief. In addition, helpers may avoid negative feelings, such as shame or guilt (Batson &
Powell, 2003). At a group level, helping can also be used as a strategic attempt to present ones own
group as warm and generous in relation to a lower-status outgroup (Nadler & Halabi, 2006; Van
Leeuwen & Tuber, 2012). There are also theories that emphasize truly altruistic motivations, for
example, empathy-altruism theory (Batson, 1991; Bierhoff & Rohmann, 2004). The basic idea of
this theory is that empathic concern motivates altruistic behaviour aimed at relieving a victims
suffering. This theory suggests, for example, that people will support HIV/AIDS victims in Africa if
they can easily see themselves in their shoes (namely, high empathy). After generating many different
explanations, one must then reduce the explanations based on their relevance, validity, and
plausibility. In the example of an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, the social psychologist disregards
the empathy factor as people may not feel very similar to people with HIV/AIDS in Africa.
To determine the validity of the social psychological theories, it is important to assess the extent to
which the typical experiments on which the theory is based represent the real world. Many theories in
their abstract form may seem readily applicable in a given situation, but what people often tend to
forget is that most theories in social psychology are usually based upon a specific research paradigm
that may only be generalized to a limited number of situations in real life. This concern refers to the
external validity of an experiment. It is possible that research findings, because of the specific
research paradigm or limitations in samples or settings, can only be applied to a limited number of
real-life situations. In that case, the external validity of an experiment is low.
For example, in a typical example of the experiments that form the basis of Batsons (1991)
empathy-altruism theory, people observe another person (the worker) who they think is suffering
from a series of uncomfortable electric shocks that have been administered to them by the
experimenter for failing to give correct answers. They are given a chance to help the worker by taking
the shocks themselves. There are at least two major differences between this situation and the
situation of donating money to people with HIV/AIDS in Africa. First, it concerns others who are
close in proximity, and, second, one is asked to take on the suffering of the victim oneself. Thus,
Batsons theory may have limited relevance for this particular problem.
On the basis of a limited set of variables resulting from the previous stage, a process model can be
formulated like the one presented in Figure 1.1 and like those in the Appendix. (How to build such a
model is described in much more detail in Chapter 4.) The model contains the outcome variable that
must be influenced, in this case a willingness to donate money for people with HIV/AIDS. In
addition, the model should primarily contain variables that can be influenced, at least to some extent,
and should describe the relationship between the variables in the form of a process model. This
process model is at the core of PATH methodology. Although the model in Figure 1.1 seems
plausible, this is by no means the only model that could have been formulated on the basis of the
selected variables. Why, for example, does the belief in a just world not directly affect a negative
attitude towards people with HIV/AIDS in Africa? Why does the way in which people with
HIV/AIDS in Africa cope with the situation not lead to feelings of moral obligation? Why is a
willingness to donate money not directly affected by the perceived injustice of poverty in Africa?
In general, the process model specifies just a few possible relationships between its variables.
Any given variable should not affect more than two or three other variables. This forces practitioners
to be selective and specific about the causal relationships in the model. By including too many
relationships, it may become a model in which everything is explained by everything, and it would
be difficult to formulate specific interventions based on it.
In the example of an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, the social psychologist formulated his model
on the basis of existing empirical research. However, often one is forced to formulate a model in
which it is not yet clear to what extent the various paths between the variables are empirically
supported. Ultimately, a model is only complete if there is sufficient evidence from research for the
relationships between the variables. (In Chapter 4 we discuss how to assess the empirical support for
the model.) Of course, because we aim to develop explanations and interventions based on social
psychological knowledge, in the present approach we need to use as much existing knowledge as
possible. This knowledge can be derived from basic social psychological research as well as from
other research more or less directly applied to the problem (Fliszar & Clopton, 1995; Montada,
2001).
Frequently, however, one can only find empirical evidence that validates parts of the process
model, and not the entire model. In the example of an HIV/AIDS fundraising campaign, there is, for
instance, little research on willingness to donate money for people with HIV/AIDS in Africa, or on
charity donation in general. If one cannot find research on the specific problem (for example, charity
donation) to support (parts of) the model, one may look for evidence in research on the generic
behaviour (for example, altruism). The social psychologist who advised the volunteer team, for
instance, found support for (parts of) his model in the general literature on helping.
Oversimplification
The situation examined in experiments is virtually, by definition, a reduction and simplification of
reality. A single laboratory experiment can never examine the complex interplay of variables that
affect human social behaviour in the real world, and can examine at most two or three factors. For
example, the social psychologist assisting the AIDS team concluded from laboratory research on
emergency helping (Latan & Darley, 1970) that often bystanders do not intervene when they see
another person is in need. One could come up with numerous factors that may affect a willingness to
help in such situations, including the bystanders personality, family background, mood,
preoccupation with other issues, fear, embarrassment, lack of control, and the age and sex of the
victim. Yet in the classic bystander experiment, Latan and Darley (1970) only examined one
factor, that is, the number of other people present. They showed that a willingness to help someone
allegedly experiencing a seizure was reduced the more other people were present. Although Latan
and Darleys experiment is a very interesting one, it did not show how important this factor was in
comparison with other factors that may influence willingness to help, such as the victims age or sex,
or how it interacted with other factors. One way to enable more accurate comparisons between the
enormous range of factors that psychologists test in their laboratory experiments is to look at effect
sizes. Effect sizes are statistical measurements of the magnitude of the relationships between
variables that can help researchers to assess the real world significance of laboratory findings. For
example, a laboratory experiment may show that after receiving persuasive messages about the costs
of unsafe sex (for example, an unwanted pregnancy) 1 per cent more participants intend to engage in
safer sex practices compared to a control condition. Although this difference may be statistically
significant, the effect size appears to be quite small in terms of influencing real-world behaviour.
Another limitation of laboratory experiments is exemplified by a research programme by the
American social psychologists Tanya Chartrand and John Bargh (1999) on the effects of mimicry. In a
typical mimicry experiment, participants interact with a researcher who either mimics or does not
mimic their behaviour. They are then asked to give their impressions of the interaction, as well as
their feelings towards the researcher. In general, it appears that we tend to like other people who
mimic our own behaviour more than those who do not, and this is a quite strong and robust effect.
However, in real life, such as when a dating agency wants to match potential partners, other factors,
such as physical attractiveness, status, or educational level, may turn out to be more important than
mimicry.
It is, of course, possible to examine such factors as these in experiments. For example, further
experiments have shown that attitude mimicry and liking are also influenced by factors such as
interpersonal similarity, relative status, shared group membership, and shared goal orientation.
Although researchers can include a second, third, or even fourth variable in their experiments, it is
impossible to include all potentially relevant factors in a laboratory experiment. The social
psychologist must therefore assess what the most important variables are, for example, through a
survey among the target populations.
External Validity
A second limitation is that all kinds of factors in real life may obscure the impact of the variables that
are so clearly manipulated in experiments. For instance, in the experiments by Chartrand and Bargh
(1999), participants were asked to give their impressions of strangers who were deliberately
mimicking or not mimicking them. In real life, mimicry is frequently an automatic, unconscious
process which neither party is acutely aware of when it happens. In fact, the same researchers have
since shown that too much deliberate mimicry may give people the chills because it is seen as
inappropriate (Leander, Chartrand, & Bargh, 2012). Thus, if one had been asked by an organization
how to make cohesive teams, and one had proposed to advise team members to try to increase their
behavioural mimicry, the results would have been quite disappointing.
Another example of this limitation comes from research on unconscious priming. There is
considerable evidence that priming individuals with stimuli that are offered subliminally, that is,
without being consciously perceived, may affect behaviour. In a fascinating line of recent
experiments, a number of researchers have demonstrated that priming participants with clean smells
increases their trust and willingness to donate to charity (Liljenquist, Zhong, & Galinsky, 2010), and
even causes them to spend more time keeping their desks tidy (Holland, Hendriks, & Aarts, 2005).
The effects of cleanliness primes are so pervasive that allowing participants to wash their hands
after reading a morally repulsive story causes them to judge the offender less harshly (Schnall,
Benton, & Harvey, 2008). Despite the striking results of such experiments, in real life the success of
these interventions may be weak, or there may be ethical concerns.
Contradictory Evidence
Another limitation of social psychological research is that studies often produce contradictory
findings. For instance, Griffith (1970) found that participants who waited in a room with
uncomfortable environmental conditions (high temperature, high humidity level) liked the person with
whom they were waiting less than participants who waited in a room with comfortable environmental
conditions (normal temperature and low humidity level). Yet Bell and Baron (1974) failed to
replicate this effect. Other social psychological research shows that people tend to like others more
when they meet them in fear-arousing, uncomfortable situations (Dutton & Aron, 1974).
Findings like these may be confusing and difficult to interpret. Fortunately, researchers are
sometimes able to reconcile contrasting findings. Often, contradictory results stem from the fact that,
on numerous occasions, studies have subtly different methods. Kenrick and Johnson (1979) found, for
instance, that negative feelings which are due to uncomfortable circumstances will induce aversion
for another person, a stranger, even when those being studied do not interact with this person. In
contrast, when individuals actually interact with someone, uncomfortable circumstances can often
increase liking. This illustrates that one should not take the conclusions from experiments as general
truths, but that one should carefully examine the experimental paradigm on which a particular finding
is based before applying it to the real world.
From a broader perspective, seemingly contradictory conclusions from experiments support the
idea that humans are complex social beings with many different behavioural tendencies. For example,
they will seek out factual confirmation of who they are as well as flattering information on how good
they are; they are egoistical as well as altruistic; they are rational as well as emotional. There are
numerous theories in social psychology, and each theory tends to emphasize a distinct human
tendency. For instance, Batsons (1991) empathy-altruism theory emphasizes that people have a basic
tendency to respond with altruistic empathy to others, whereas social exchange theory emphasizes that
people first and foremost pursue their self-interest in helping relations (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959).
Swanns self-confirmation theory (see, for example, Swann, Stein-Seroussi, & Giesler, 1992)
suggests that people tend to seek out information that confirms their self-image, be it positive or
negative, whereas self-esteem theory (Baumeister & Tice, 1990; Sedikides & Gregg, 2003) would
suggest that people simply prefer all information to make them feel good about themselves.
Conclusion
This book introduces the PATH model, a step-by-step approach for addressing and resolving societal
problems through the application of social psychological theory and knowledge, from the formulation
of the problem to the shaping of interventions. Although every practitioner can potentially benefit
from PATH methodology, some background in social psychological theory is desirable.
The PATH model should not be used in a rigid way. Going from a problem to intervention is
usually an iterative process, and one frequently moves back and forth between the different steps in
the model. For instance, one may start with defining the problem, but when exploring the literature,
one can discover that there are certain aspects of the problem that one has overlooked. In that case,
one first has to redefine the problem. Or one may see explanations and solutions before having
formulated a clear problem definition. There is nothing wrong with adapting the problem definition
after having explored the research literature. It is even advisable to do so. What counts is not strictly
following the steps of the PATH model, but developing a clear problem definition, a process model
that fits the empirical findings as closely as possible, and an effective intervention.
Suggested Further Reading
Dovidio, J.F., Piliavin, J.A., Schroeder, D.A., & Penner, L.A. (2006).The social psychology of
pro-social behaviour. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schultz, P.W. & Oskamp, S. (2000). Social psychology: An applied perspective. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Strmer, S., Snyder, M., Kropp, A., & Siem, B. (2002). Empathy-motivated helping: The
moderating role of group membership. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(7),
943956.
Van Vugt, M., Snyder, M., Tyler, T., & Biel, A. (2000). Cooperation in modern society:
Promoting the welfare of communities, states, and organisations. London: Routledge (p. 245).
worse off? To answer this question the American psychologists Joanne Wood, Shelley Taylor, and
Rosemary Lichtman* interviewed 78 breast cancer patients about their illness and the ways they
coped, including the type of social comparisons they made. These researchers found that over 60
per cent of respondents said that another patient was coping less well than she was; 80 per cent
said that they adjusted at least somewhat better than other women. In other words, the researchers
found a preponderance of downward comparison, indicating that, among breast cancer patients,
self-enhancement is the most dominant motive for social comparison.
Findings like these are important for interventions that aim to help patients adjust (see Brakel,
Dijkstra, & Buunk, 2012; Buunk et al., 2012; Buunk, Gibbons, & Visser, 2002). Consistent with
patients preference for downward comparisons, they may, for instance, point out what patients
are still able to do (rather than what they cannot do any more).
* Wood, J.V., Taylor, S.E., & Lichtman, R.R. (1985). Social comparison in adjustment to breast
cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 11691183.
Assignment 1
Read Box 1.2. Imagine you are asked to develop an intervention programme to enhance the wellbeing of cancer patients on the basis of the study described in this box (Wood, Taylor, & Lichtman,
1985).
a. Describe in this context:
what exactly the problem is that you aim to solve with the intervention programme;
why the problem is a problem (in, among others, emotional, financial and societal terms)
and since when;
for whom it is a problem (for patients or also, for instance, for their relatives and/or for
taxpayers);
what the possible causes of the problem are (for instance, relevant behaviours, emotions or
cognitions, lack of information);
whom you aim to target with your intervention (target group);
the key aspects of the problem (applied, concrete, social psychological, whether the
problem is solvable or relievable).
b. Discuss to what extent the belief in a just world, as described in this chapter, may influence
cancer patients well-being when they socially compare themselves with other patients. To
what extent do you think that the belief in a just world is relevant to the intervention
programme?
c. Design an intervention on the basis of the results of Wood, Taylor, and Lichtmans study to
relieve the problem you have described in (a) above. Describe specifically the social
psychological variables you aim to manipulate by intervention and by what means you aim to
do so and why.
You may read the following articles to come up with ideas about interventions for cancer patients:
Bennenbroek, F.T.C., Buunk, B.P., Van der Zee, K.I., & Grol, B. (2002). Social comparison and
patient information: What do cancer patients want? Patient Education and Counselling, 47(1), 512.
Brakel, T.M., Dijkstra, A., & Buunk, A.P. (2012). Effects of the source of social comparison
information on former cancer patients quality of life. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17,
667681.
Wood, J.V., Taylor, S.E., & Lichtman, R.R. (1985). Social comparison in adjustment to breast
cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 11691183.
2
The Problem Phase: From a Problem to a Problem
Definition
Contents
Introduction
Towards a Problem Definition
Reformulating the Problem
The Path from a Problem to a Problem Definition
Examples of Interviews
Key Questions for a Problem Definition
What is the Problem?
Why is it a Problem?
For Whom is it a Problem?
What are the Possible Causes of the Problem?
What is the Target Group?
What are the Key Aspects of the Problem?
Research for a Problem Definition
Background Materials
Scientific Literature
Interviews
Observation
Completing the Problem Definition
Introduction
Social problems are everywhere around us. Open up a newspaper, watch television or listen to the
radio, and suddenly you are confronted with a rich variety of social issues, many of which have a
social psychological dimension. Regardless of whether it concerns the problem of teenage
pregnancies, smoking and health, divorce, anti-social behaviour in residential communities, school
truancies, prejudice towards ethnic minority members, or even global warming, social psychological
factors play a role in all of them. Indeed, as societies grow larger and individuals live and work more
closely together, social and environmental problems are bound to rise, affecting an ever larger
proportion of the population (Gardner & Stern, 2002; Van Vugt et al., 2000). Hence, there is an urgent
need for the involvement of social psychologists to study these problems, and to offer solutions while
working together with fellow scientists and policy makers (see, for examples, Aronson et al., 2010;
Baumeister & Bushman, 2010; Hewstone et al., 2005; Kenrick et al., 2010; Myers, 2012). This is not
to say, of course, that all or most societal problems can be directly attributed to social psychological
factors.
For example, the primary cause of lung cancer is smoking, and pollution is primarily caused by the
use of cars. Hardly social psychological matters, so it seems. Yet social psychologists will try to
understand why, despite the widespread knowledge about the health risks, so many individuals still
continue to smoke (Gerrard, Gibbons, Lane, & Stock, 2005; Suls, Davidson, & Kaplan, 2010). Or
they will investigate why most people continue to drive cars, although there are sometimes much
better travel options available (Joireman, Van Lange, & Van Vugt, 2004; Van Vugt, Meertens, & Van
Lange, 1995).
Perhaps the clearest illustration of the important role for social psychology can be seen from a
sample of headlines that appeared in British newspapers in the week that this chapter was being
written. This list reads as follows:
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
This summary shows not just that our society (as well as many societies around the world) is facing a
diversity of problems today; it also indicates that there are vast differences in the way these problems
are stated. Some of them are based merely on an observation (for example, one in seven students
drops out of university) and it is not always that clear what the problem is exactly (the what
question). Others are based on a piece of systematic research (for example, workplace bullying is on
the increase), and while it is immediately clear what the problem is, it does not state anything about
why it is perceived as a problem or when it first appeared (the why question). Other statements do
give a potential cause for the problem (for example, a charity saying fewer people get involved
because of commitments in modern life) the causes question but it is unclear for whom it is a
problem, how widespread this problem is (the for whom question), and whose cooperation is
essential to solve the problem (the question about the target group). Finally, there are problems
which are merely expressed as an intention to do something about an unsatisfying situation (for
example, the police force looking for more ethnic officers), but it is unclear how these can be solved
(the aspects question). Thus, although from each of these above statements inferences can be made
about what the problem is, important details are lacking, and many more questions need to be asked in
order to establish a formal problem definition.
Snyder (see Box 2.1), Allen Omoto and their colleagues (Snyder & Omoto, 2008; Omoto, Snyder, &
Hackett, 2010; Strmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005). In their research, they first recognized, through
surveys and interviews, that the act of volunteering to assist people with HIV/AIDS served
different psychological functions for different volunteers. Some volunteers were primarily driven by
a pan-altruistic motivation to help others, while others were motivated more by a specific attachment
to the community of people with HIV/AIDS or by the effects of volunteering for their personal growth
and development. On the basis of these results, they argued that it would be wise to develop a
volunteer recruitment campaign that would target individuals on the basis of their main motive for
volunteering. They were indeed more successful in their recruitment if they tailored their message to a
specific target audience of potential volunteers (Kiviniemi, Snyder, & Omoto, 2002). Below is an
interview with Professor Mark Snyder, from the University of Minnesota, about his applied social
psychological research.
Box 2.1 Interview with Professor Mark Snyder of the University of Minnesota (USA)
As a psychologist, I wear many hats. I am a basic scientist, and I am an applied researcher. I work in the laboratory, and
I work in the field. I address problems of theoretical significance, and ones of practical concern. Rather than keeping
these various facets of my professional identity separate from each other, I have worked to integrate them. Thus, I have a
particular fondness for research that, at one and the same time, advances the state of theoretical understanding and also
speaks to the challenges that confront society.
Accompanying my belief that social science should contribute to solving the problems of society has been a longstanding
fascination with people who themselves take action for the benefit of society. In my research, I have sought to understand
how and why people become actively involved in doing good for others and for society. Such involvement can take the
form of participation in volunteerism and philanthropy, community and neighbourhood organizations, social activism and
political movements. In this research on social action, my collaborators and I are discovering why individuals become
involved in various forms of social action, what sustains their involvement over time, and the consequences of such action
for individuals and for society.
A defining feature of my work is its focus on real people involved in real social action in real settings. For instance, in our
studies of volunteerism, we have followed volunteers over the entire course of their service with community-based
organizations, thereby allowing us to chart their life histories as volunteers and to study the unfolding processes of
volunteerism. Such work can be time-consuming and effortful but has been well worth it. For such research is contributing
meaningfully, I believe, to an emerging understanding of the nature of volunteerism and other forms of social action.
Moreover, it is speaking directly, I believe, to critical concerns with the role of individual and collective involvement in
society. And, for me, it is extremely rewarding to be engaged in scientific activity in ways that can deliver benefits to
science and to society a win-win situation for all concerned, I believe.
Snyder, M. & Dwyer, P.C. (2012). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In I.B. Weiner, H.A. Tennen,
& J.M. Suls (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology, Vol. 5: Personality and Social Psychology (2nd
edn) (pp. 467486). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Strmer, S., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A.M. (2005). Prosocial emotions and helping: The moderating
role of group membership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 532546.
they receive in return), a social comparison perspective (if it appears that people feel frustrated and
deprived in comparison with their colleagues), or from a commitment perspective (if it appears that a
lack of organizational commitment plays a role; see Ellemers, De Gilder, & Haslam, 2004; Geurts,
Buunk, & Schaufeli, 1994; Geurts, Schaufeli, & Buunk, 1993).
in health centres near company premises will be a waste. The importance of problem definitions is
illustrated below by various examples, both good and bad, of a hypothetical conversation between a
social psychologist and a potential client, the chief constable of a large police force. The problem
revolves around the recruitment of officers from different ethnic backgrounds.
Examples of Interviews
Example of a Bad Interview
Chief:
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Well, when we hold officer recruitment days for the public, hardly
anyone from these communities turns up. And if they do, they dont
submit the application form that they receive at the end of the day.
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Thats correct, but I also want to make sure that we retain our best
ethnic officers. Quite a few of them have left recently, and we dont
know why.
Social psychologist:
Chief (hesitantly):
Social psychologist:
Evaluation In this example, the social psychologist and their client may decide to focus on
understanding the lack of enthusiasm among ethnic officers for doing police work. Yet it is clear that
they have developed, at best, an incomplete problem definition and, at worst, a problem definition
that is plainly wrong. There are key questions that remain unanswered here. Is this a recruitment
problem, a retention problem, or is it both? For whom is it a problem for the police, the community,
the government? Is this really about a lack of interest in doing police work? Can the problem be
attributed entirely to the ethnic community, or is the police force itself (also) responsible for causing
these problems? Furthermore, when have these issues first been noticed, and if these problems have
emerged only recently, how does that inform the search for possible causes? The next conversation
provides a better example of the development of an adequate problem definition.
Example of a Better Interview
Chief:
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Well, when we hold officer recruitment days for the public, hardly
anyone from these communities turns up. And if they do, they dont
submit the application form that they receive at the end of the day.
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Well, there have been targets set by the government for the intake of
ethnic officers, and so far, we have failed to reach any of these
targets.
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Some believe that we pay less attention to crimes when the victims
are of Asian or African background. People also accuse us of being
prejudiced in terms of who we stop and search on the streets.
Social psychologist:
So what is your main problem? That you are not recruiting enough
ethnic police officers, that many crimes are unreported, or that some
members of the community think the police are biased?
Chief:
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Yes, indeed.
Evaluation In this example, the problem definition is being shaped more clearly through the specific
questions the social psychologist asks regarding the various attributes of the problem: what is the
problem, why is it a problem and for whom? As a consequence, the problem has switched from
specific recruitment to a more general issue regarding the relationship between the police force and
ethnic community members.
It may seem that this is complicating matters unnecessarily. Yet think about the implications in
terms of intervention. Had the social psychologist concentrated exclusively on the recruitment
problem, then he or she might have suggested organizing a recruitment campaign directly targeted at
members of the ethnic population, for example, by advertising in mosques, temples, or churches.
Knowing that the recruitment failure might possibly result from a more widely held negative
perception of the police force suggests that such an intervention is doomed to fail, because it does not
directly alter the image of the police force. However, there are yet more issues to be raised by the
social psychologist to establish a more complete problem definition.
Interview Continued
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Over the past few years, Ive also noticed an increase in turnover
among ethnic officers; some of them were really the pick of the crop
when they started here.
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Well, we havent done any systematic research, but some at least felt
isolated in their working units. They didnt get enough support from
colleagues in their units. They also felt under threat from members of
their own community who didnt approve of them working for the
police force.
Social psychologist:
Have you interviewed the chief officers in the units about the
problems that the ethnic officers experienced?
Chief:
Not yet I havent had the time to go around the units and gather
information.
Social psychologist:
But you think its an important problem and that it may be related to
what we previously mentioned: a lack of trust in the police force
among the ethnic population.
Chief:
Keeping the best ethnic officers is paramount for our force. I think
these problems are related, but, as I said before, where do we start?
Social psychologist:
What are your ideas about who best to focus our efforts on? Whose
cooperation is necessary for the problem to be solved?
Chief:
officers. They talk on a daily basis with their officers and are
responsible for the atmosphere in the unit. It is the task of the chief
unit officer to stop bullying and discrimination among his officers.
Social psychologist:
Chief:
Evaluation This is a good start towards a workable, albeit preliminary, problem definition for a
social psychologist. The problem definition is sufficiently concrete in terms of what the problems are
(the recruitment and retention of ethnic police officers) and it suggests a possible cause for these
problems (lack of trust in the police force). Furthermore, it addresses a number of questions relevant
to establishing an adequate problem definition, such as:
1. What is the problem? The summary focuses on two possibly related problems in the force:
the recruitment and retention of ethnic police officers.
2. Why is it a problem? In the summary, it becomes clear that these problems affect the quality of
the police work in the community as well as the wider public image of the police force. Also,
there is the issue about not meeting the governments ethnicity targets, which may result in the
withdrawal of funds. It is not yet clear when these problems first appeared though.
3. For whom is it a problem? The social psychologist identifies at least four possible parties
with an interest in solving these problems. First, the police force that wants to recruit more
ethnic officers. Second, the community that wants a reliable and effective police force. Third,
the ethnic members of the community who want to join/remain in the police force. And, finally,
the government that wants police forces to meet their targets.
4. What are the possible causes of the problem? The social psychologist identifies one major
cause of the problem in his summary, the lack of trust in the police force by members of the
ethnic population. Although this may be a good working assumption for further examination, it
is worthwhile realizing that there may be other causes as well. For example, the higher
turnover among ethnic officers may be due to an inadequate support system within the police
force for dealing with ethnicity. Or, it may turn out that ethnic officers are turned down more
quickly for promotions. Finally, it may be that recruitment and retention failures are themselves
the cause rather than the consequence of a lack of trust in the police. It is good to keep
alternative causes in mind when building an explanatory model (see Chapter 3).
5. What is the target group? The social psychologist identifies those groups who, according to
the chief, are essential for the success of a possible intervention, that is, members of the ethnic
population and the chief unit officers. Although this may be a good working assumption for
further examination, it is worthwhile realizing that there may be other groups which need to be
targeted as well. For instance, it seems wise to address all police officers, not just the chief
unit officers. It is important to keep alternative target groups in mind when generating strategies
and developing an intervention (see Chapter 5).
6. What are the key aspects of the problem?
Is it an applied problem? It is clear from the summary that the client the police force
will not be satisfied if the social psychologist merely concentrates on finding the causes of
the problems in recruitment and retention. They have asked for expert advice because they
need to find solutions to these problems.
Is it a concrete problem? It seems rather obvious that the problem is concrete rather than
abstract as it deals with specific groups and individuals.
Is it a social psychological problem? From the problem analysis it is clear that there are
important social psychological dimensions to the problem. Underlying these problems are
potential issues of trust, prejudice, social support, power, and legitimacy all social
psychological phenomena. Also, there will be important social psychological aspects to the
proposed intervention programme, such as trust-building, reducing prejudice, and
increasing the legitimacy of the police force.
Can the problem be solved or relieved? From the above, it appears that there are enough
clues to reassure the social psychologist that the problems are solvable or relievable, at
least in part, for example, through improving contact and trust among ethnic and non-ethnic
officers (Crisp & Turner, 2009). Moreover, there is clearly a willingness on the part of the
client, the Chief Constable, to work towards reducing the problem.
It is good to note that the above list is by no means an exhaustive list of questions that could be asked
in order to develop an adequate problem definition. There are many other issues that could be raised
by the social psychologist in order to get a clearer picture of the problem. For example, it is not
entirely clear from the interview when these problems first emerged or exactly when they were first
noted by the Chief Constable (part of the why question). It may be that recruitment failure is a longstanding issue, but that it has only been noticed recently by the Chief in response to feedback about the
government targets. Or it may be a relatively recent phenomenon, perhaps caused by particular
incidents where ethnic community members have been treated badly and unjustly by some police
officers, thus raising a negative image of the police. Such an historic analysis is important because it
sheds further light on the potential causes of the problem. Also, the social psychologist did not
explore further the remark made by the Chief that the problem had been observed in other police
forces as well. It is important to establish whether this is true or not, as this may help to build a more
complete picture of the problem, and may provide an opportunity to study interventions that may have
been tried elsewhere.
A good preliminary problem definition is one in which all the relevant questions have been
addressed, but in no obvious order. We will focus below on the specific questions that need to be
addressed to develop a problem definition. It is possible that there will be some overlap in the
answers to each of these questions. That is not a problem. At this stage, omissions are greater sins
than overlaps. Further, as we noted earlier, it is important not to get fixated on one particular cause or
explanation (for example, in this case, trust in the police force). A good social psychologist should
always leave open the possibility of adjusting the problem definition once he or she gets under way in
building an explanatory model, collecting literature, and conducting research.
Why is it a Problem?
It is important for social psychologists to ask why a particular issue is perceived as a problem in the
first place. How does the problem express itself? What are the consequences of the problem? What
makes it problematic? When did it first emerge? If the vice-chancellor of a university approaches a
social psychologist with a question on what to do about university drop-out rates, they must first
establish why it is a problem that some students do not finish their studies. Is it largely a financial
matter for the university, or is there also a concern about the self-esteem and well-being of such
students? Furthermore, if the vice-chancellor believes it has something to do with the motivation of
particular students, the social psychologist should ask them to specify what they mean by this rather
abstract term. Is it a question of them not attending lectures (in which case compulsory attendance
may be considered) or did they have the wrong expectations about the study or university life in
general (leading to an intervention to alter prospective students course expectations)? Thus,
answering the why question not only helps to specify the problem, but also suggests directions for the
proposed intervention programme.
It may take some effort to find out which problems really bother a client. A first impression about
the Chief Constable is that he is concerned with meeting the targets set by the government about the
intake of ethnic officers. This may indeed be an immediate concern, but it is only after further enquiry
that the social psychologist finds out that it is really the quality of police functioning and a lack of
cooperation from the community that concern him. If the problem is solely defined in terms of meeting
targets, then the success of interventions will be based on this criterion. This may not necessarily be
desirable in the long run. In the police example, the force may use substantial incentives to recruit
ethnic officers in order to meet the intake targets, but new recruits may not have the right attitude and
motivation to continue in the job, eventually dropping out. Hence, it is vital not to be persuaded by the
quick-fix solutions that clients themselves may offer by simply looking at their perspective on the
problem. Equally, however, one should be careful to define the problem entirely within the subjects
interests. For example, ethnic police-officer recruits should be subject to the same stringent criteria
for admission into the force and should therefore not be seen as getting an easy ride (Heilman, Simon,
& Repper, 1987; Maio & Esses, 1998).
To answer the why question, it may help to ask since when it was a problem. This might reveal
important clues about the potential causes and solutions of the problem. By asking these questions a
social psychologist attempts to build an historical picture of the problem, which can be quite
informative. First, it can reveal that what appears to be a problem may not necessarily be a problem
after all. For example, sales of the morning-after pill at chemists around the country may appear to
be rising, but a brief investigation may reveal that this is really due to the government being able to
give a more precise sales figure for the first time. Second, an historical analysis might point to a
particular time when the problem first started to emerge or be noticed. For example, citizens and
nations first started to become aware of issues regarding environmental conservation after the oil
crisis in 1972, when oil-producing countries cut their supplies (Gardner & Stern, 2002). Similarly,
the drought in England in the summer of 1995 made English people suddenly aware of the finiteness
of important water supplies. Knowing this might help a social psychologist to propose an intervention
programme using the salience of this crisis (Van Vugt, 2009; Van Vugt & Samuelson, 1999). Third, a
brief historical analysis may indicate reasons why a problem has suddenly increased in severity. For
example, the sudden rise in sales of the morning-after pill at chemists may be due to the difficulty
some underage teenagers have in getting the contraceptive pill from their doctor without parental
consent.
Of course, it is also possible that the issue has been ongoing for a much longer time, but suddenly
has been perceived as a problem by the client because of some ulterior reason. It is important to be
aware of this. The Chief Constable in the police example may have known about the recruitment
failure of ethnic officers for a long time, but only been willing to consider it a genuine problem
because of the intake targets recently set by the government. If a social psychologist is unaware of
this, he or she may be tempted to propose a quick-fix solution to the problem rather than one that
would prove successful in the long term.
problem, and if so, can use this to create a preliminary causal model (see Chapter 3). Note that the
purpose of establishing a problem definition is not to be definite about the exact causes underlying the
problem. At this stage, it is more important to get a first impression of the causal model and the
possible social psychological processes involved.
Building a preliminary causal model is facilitated by asking two interrelated questions:
1. What seems to cause this problem?
2. How may these causes affect the problem?
In the volunteering example, the Nottingham charity experienced difficulties in recruiting volunteers
and they attributed this problem to a potential cause, which is that people nowadays have many other
commitments. Suppose this is true; how would this cause then affect the problem? A straightforward
causal explanation would be that because people nowadays work at the weekend more, they will
have less time to engage in other activities, such as volunteering.
It is also important at this point to distinguish between immediate causes and more distal causes.
By making this distinction, one can develop a model of the causal process leading to the problem. For
example, the Chief Constable conveyed that the force was failing to recruit ethnic officers and noticed
an immediate cause of the problem: members of ethnic groups simply not attending officer recruitment
days in large numbers. The social psychologist then has to delve deeper into this problem by asking
what causes this. Suppose the chief suggested that ethnic community members have little interest in
joining the police and suffer from a lack of motivation. The social psychologist should then ask why
this might be the case and the Chief Constable might answer that there is a lack of trust in the police
force among the ethnic community. What causes this? Perhaps they feel that the police are prejudiced
against their community. Why may this be the case? The Chief might then reveal a number of highprofile incidents whereby crimes against the ethnic population, including some racial crimes, were
never solved. He may also point to the failure of current ethnic officers to get promotion, which may
have caused grievances in the population. A process-like causal model now emerges, leading all the
way from unresolved racial crimes (a distant cause), to the failure to recruit ethnic officers, with a
number of more immediate causes in between (accusations of prejudice, a lack of trust in the police, a
lack of motivation to do police work).
Establishing the process of events reveals a number of different clues about the causal model
underlying the problem as well as the proposed intervention programme to tackle it. Nevertheless, at
this stage it may not be clear what exactly the causal chain of events leading to the problem is or
which factors play a major or minor role in causing the problem. This is not a worry. In the next steps
of the model it will become clear what the causal model looks like through further research and
theorizing. And yet, the factors that have been identified in the problem definition could certainly play
a role in choosing the theories to develop the causal model.
bring it to the attention of police officers and try to influence their attitudes. He may approach
members of the ethnic community, especially those aged between 20 and 30 years, and encourage
them to apply for a place in the police force, or he may do both. He may even focus his efforts on
Members of Parliament, and try to persuade them to lower the strict targets the government has set for
the intake of ethnic officers.
Selecting a target group narrows down the broad field of actors that may play a role in the
problem. It clarifies the problem and makes it more specific. This in turn makes it easier to come up
with strategies to help solve the problem (see Chapter 5). Should the social psychologist later
discover that it is too difficult or costly to aim his intervention at the target group, he can always
backtrack and redefine the problem. Remember, the PATH model is not a rigid process and going
from problem to intervention is usually an iterative process that includes moving back and forth
between the different steps in the model.
community in joining this police force can a coherent programme of intervention be put together.
Is it a Concrete Problem? It is also important that the problem is formulated in sufficiently concrete
terms. All its important aspects must be operationalized appropriately if they are to be useful. For
example, a problem definition aimed at doing something about bullying in the workplace should try to
define bullying in a manner which makes it sufficiently clear to everyone what it means, such as the
act of intimidating a less powerful person into doing something against their wish (see the Wordnet
definition at http://wordnet.princeton.edu/). Once the behaviour has been operationalized, it is much
easier to recognize it and measure the frequency of bullying behaviour in the workplace.
Furthermore, it is sensible to specify the properties of the particular sample of people who
experience or cause the problem in as much detail as possible. Rather than stating the problem as the
failure to recruit ethnic officers, one could specify the backgrounds of the potential recruits the force
is looking for (for example, officers of Pakistani and Indian descent). This makes it easier to find out
if an intervention has been successful or not.
Finally, one should be clear in the problem definition about the kind of specific behaviours that one
wants to tackle with an intervention. A problem concerning the anti-social behaviour of teenagers in
urban neighbourhoods can only be dealt with when a social psychologist has not only defined the
behaviour, but also when he or she has a clear indication of the specific behaviours that fit within the
general domain of anti-social behaviour. This could include such diverse acts as littering, graffiti,
vandalism, joy-riding or verbal and/or physical aggression against other children or adults. Only by
being clear about the set of behaviours is a social psychologist able to develop and test the
effectiveness of an intervention programme.
To encourage specifying the problem, a social psychologist is advised to step into the shoes of a
researcher. Suppose you are asked to develop a research programme measuring bullying in the
workplace how would you measure this behaviour? What questions would you ask the employees in
the company that you are investigating, and what behaviours would you be interested in observing?
You may decide that you are interested primarily in finding out whether male employees make any
derogatory comments about female colleagues or about females in general in the presence of other
staff. In order to do this, you may decide to interview staff members and get permission to record
conversations and email exchanges between staff members. Once you have established the prevalence
of the problem, you may suggest a possible intervention to the client (for example, informing staff that
email traffic will be monitored). Because you have specified the bullying problem that you are
interested in, you can then go on to measure the effectiveness of this intervention.
Is it a Social Psychological Problem? This question will have been addressed, at least in part, by
answers to the previous questions. There are two related concerns here. Are there any causes other
than social psychological causes of the problem, and, if there are, are these perhaps more important
than the social psychological determinants? For example, there is little point in asking a social
psychologist to solve a shortage of hospital beds because this is largely a financial and administrative
matter. Similarly, if we know that the major factor influencing residential water use is the presence of
a water meter, a social psychologist can do little in terms of social psychological interventions to
influence people to conserve more water (Van Vugt, 2009).
A second, related, question is what contribution a social psychological perspective on the problem
Box 2.2 A Classic Case Study Revisited: The Broken Windows Theory and the Spreading
of Disorder
For police officers in many urban neighbourhoods, the tolerance of petty crimes such as vandalism
and littering was once seen as an unavoidable but relatively harmless aspect of city living. Before
the 1980s, most metropolitan police interventions were directed towards drug enforcement and
other means of directly preventing violent crimes, while officers assigned to the old-fashioned
style of foot patrol were becoming increasingly rare. This began to change, however, after
criminologists James Q. Wilson and George L. Kelling of Harvard University published in March
1982 an article in The Atlantic Magazine that popularized a social psychological idea now called
Broken Windows Theory.
Based on their extensive experience evaluating police enforcement measures, and inspired
partly by a 1969 study conducted by Stanford psychologist Philip Zimbardo, they argued that small
and subtle cues of disorder such as graffiti, broken windows, and littering tended to escalate
and contribute to even higher rates of both petty and serious crimes. While their suggestions were
adopted enthusiastically by some officials, the theory was initially viewed as controversial, due to
its correlational nature and lack of empirical support.
Recently, a group of social psychologists from the University of Groningen in the Netherlands
set out to test the predictions of Broken Windows Theory, coming up with a series of cleverly
designed field experiments which were eventually published in the prestigious journal Science.*
In the first of these experiments the researchers set up a kind of public laboratory by posting a
large sign prohibiting graffiti in an alley near a shopping area where people commonly parked
their bicycles. In the order condition, they ensured that the walls behind this sign were blank,
while in the disorder condition they were covered in graffiti. Then, while the unsuspecting
participants were out shopping, the researchers would attach a flyer to their handlebars which had
to be removed before riding away. Their study showed that in the order (no-graffiti) condition, the
majority of participants (67%) took the flyer off and rode away with it in their hands, while in the
disorder (graffiti) condition, the majority (69%) of the participants simply threw the flyer on the
ground. By varying the visibility of similar petty violations, such as unreturned shopping carts,
illegal fireworks displays, and graffiti-covered mailboxes, the authors were able to provide strong
support for the Broken Windows Theorys prediction when people are confronted with evidence
of disorder or neglect, they will often decide to neglect the law themselves.
* Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder.Science, 322, 1681
1685.
Is the Problem Solvable? A final issue that should be mentioned here is the judgement by a social
psychologist about whether a problem, which has been analysed carefully in terms of its social
psychological aspects, can be solved or, at least, substantially relieved. A careful analysis of the
viability of several possible solutions is important because it could avoid a lot of frustration on the
part of both client and social psychologist if they find out that the intervention they have chosen is
simply impractical or socially undesirable.
For example, based on the suggestion from the social psychological expert, a company may
develop an excellent carrot-and-stick plan to promote cycling by building safe bike racks, showers,
and reducing the number of car parking spaces. But if it then turns out that most employees live more
than 10 miles from work it becomes a very impractical solution indeed. Employees may feel deprived
of something which they think they are entitled to and may experience anger and resentment as a
consequence (a phenomenon called relative deprivation; see Buunk, Zurriaga, Gonzalez-Roma, &
of a social psychologist so they may be reluctant to give permission to them to gather data. Or the
problem or intervention may still be confidential, and therefore it may be too sensitive to collect data,
for example, by interviewing interested parties. Nevertheless, a social psychologist should try to
conduct a preliminary investigation into the problem in order to establish a sound problem definition
and ensure that relevant information is not ignored. There are various sources available to collect
preliminary information about a problem.
Background Materials
For some social problems there may be a variety of materials already available to the social
psychologist. Bigger social issues (for example, poverty, crime, anti-terror policies) usually appear
in newspapers or on television and it would be worthwhile to inspect these media for information
about a particular problem. In addition, it is always sensible to do a search on the internet the
largest database of all for information. For us, as authors of this book, the internet will often be the
first port of call if we need to read about a particular issue.
The facts and figures surrounding a specific problem may be readily available. For example, if one
wants to inspect crime figures in a particular area, one should consult the statistics that are available
from the local police or government. In general, national and local governments are a great source of
information on all sorts of matters and it would be advisable to contact them with specific requests.
Finally, it is advisable not to rely exclusively on information that has come directly from a client as
this may often be incomplete. In the police example, the Chief Constable may have had information
regarding the percentage of ethnic officers currently in the force, but he may not necessarily have
known how many had been turned down after they had applied. Similarly, relying on a client as the
sole source of information may give a somewhat distorted picture of the problem. The Chief
Constable may have ascertained that in the past recruitment efforts had been specifically designed to
recruit ethnic officers, but present-day interviews with potential and current ethnic applicants may
now reveal that they believe that they have not received any special attention from the force. To gain
more insight into a problem, it is therefore important to rely on a number of different sources to obtain
background knowledge of a particular issue.
Scientific Literature
It may also be instructive to conduct a brief review of the available scientific literature at this point.
Although a more systematic literature review will be done at a later stage, it is good to know what
information is out there in order to facilitate the search for potential problem causes and solutions.
Going back to the example of threatening and abusive teenagers, a brief summary of the social
psychological literature may provide a wealth of data that can give a valuable insight into the
problem of aggression among children. A closer scrutiny of the literature could be made by consulting
PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES or Google Scholar (electronic databases that comprise all scientific
articles and books in the field of psychology between 1872 and today). An investigation of these
sources may, for instance, reveal that:
1. Boys are more aggressive than girls.
2. Aggression is more common in so-called cultures of honour.
3. The literature distinguishes between instrumental aggression, that is, aggression to achieve a
goal, and emotional aggression, that is, aggression that stems from anger and frustration.
4. Violence is often associated with alcohol intake.
5. The hotter it is on any given day, the more common violence is.
6. The presence of an aggressive object, such as a knife or baseball bat, increases peoples
aggression if they are provoked.
7. Children can learn aggression by watching violence on television or in video games.
8. Punishment decreases aggression if the punishment is prompt and certain.
9. Aggression can be reduced by improving communication skills.
A closer scrutiny of the literature may also yield a number of additional perspectives and
explanations that could shed further light on the prevalence of anti-social behaviour and aggression
among youngsters. These may be used to develop hypotheses about the causal model and, ultimately,
to set up a plan for intervention. In the problem phase, the main purpose is to generate as many ideas
as possible about the possible antecedents of the problem, which would facilitate the establishment of
a causal model to develop later on.
Interviews
It is good to talk. Even though there may be plenty of background and scientific materials available on
a particular issue, it is always good practice to organize interviews with those individuals who are
party to a particular problem (the for whom question). This helps to get an intuitive understanding of
the problem, which is not as easy to acquire from studying the literature alone. Interviews also enable
social psychologists to get a better picture about how the parties experience their problems and,
importantly, whether they view matters in a similar or in a different way. Only through interviews can
a problem definition be developed that all parties recognize and are willing to sign up to.
In general, it is advisable to interview the members of all relevant parties, that is, those who
experience the problem, those who may cause the problem, and those who are responsible for solving
the problem. In the latter category, one should interview the key figures in an organization who are
responsible for finding solutions to the problem. In the police example, the social psychologist had
already interviewed the Chief Constable, but it would also be advisable in this case to interview the
key staff members who are responsible for recruitment and training. They may be part of the problem,
but even if they are, they may have valuable observations and insights to offer. In addition, the social
psychologist here may want to approach the Chief Constables of other forces to find out if they are
experiencing a similar problem.
It is, of course, equally important to interview people who are affected by the problem and have
experienced the negative consequences themselves. In the above example, these would be those
ethnic officers who have left the police force (or are thinking about leaving) and the ethnic members
of the community who have an interest in doing police work or, in other examples, those teenagers
who have been the victims of anti-social behaviour. The perpetrators of bullying must be interviewed
by the social psychologist concerned.
These initial interviews ought to have a number of important features in order to be useful. First,
they must be relatively unstructured as they should enable the interviewer plenty of room to interact
freely with the interviewees based upon what they raise. Unstructured interviews are the best way to
gather information at this stage of the investigation. One would only need a checklist of different
topics and questions to use as a guideline for the interview. The rigorous scientific standards of
objectivity and reliability do not apply as much to this stage of data collection as they do to the more
advanced stages of this methodology. This is not to say that just about anything can be raised in these
interviews, but it is important not to be too prejudiced about certain explanations or interpretations
that interviewees might bring up. Even a rather unlikely explanation should not be dismissed a priori.
If the interviewee believes it, then he or she will judge any problem definition or intervention in
terms of dealing with the explanation they offer. For example, the Chief Constable may prove
genuinely convinced that members of ethnic populations are not very interested in conducting police
work, full stop. Although this may be proven to be untrue at a later stage, he will most likely not
accept a problem definition that excludes this as a possibility.
Another feature of these interviews is to investigate if there are differences between interested
parties in their perspectives on a problem. If all parties perceive the problem in the same way, it will
be much easier to solve the problem. However, the mere fact that they have hired a social
psychologist to intervene suggests that the different parties may have a different version of what has
caused the problem. Interviewers should be focused on detecting these differences as they provide
useful information. For instance, the social psychologist who investigates the lack of enthusiasm for
doing volunteer work at a charity would interview not only the board of the charity, but also current
and former volunteers. After an initial impression from the charity board that people are simply too
busy to want to get involved in charity work these days, the social psychologist might become a bit
more sceptical about this version of events when they had interviewed current and ex-volunteers who
indicated that:
1. The charity needs to raise more money to be successful (which partly answers the what is the
problem question).
2. The charity has just replaced its director (which partly answers the issue for whom it is a
problem).
3. There are conflicts between the paid staff and volunteer staff members (which partly answers
the question regarding the causes of the problem).
4. The charity has not made any major recruitment efforts (which partly answers whether the
problem is solvable).
5. The charity has approached the wrong people (target group) to volunteer, that is, mainly
students and working mothers. Although they might want to, both students and working mothers
are generally just too busy to do volunteer work.
Observation
Sometimes it can be difficult to work towards a problem definition by collecting material from
interviews alone. Interviewees may have such different views on a problem that it will be extremely
difficult to generate a problem definition that is universally agreed upon. Equally, it is possible that
there may appear to be a sense of unanimity among interviewees about a problem, but the social
psychologist involved is a bit suspicious about whether everyone is telling the entire truth. Some
issues, such as institutional racism or sexual harassment in the workplace, are so sensitive to deal
with that it may be in the interests of all parties to hide important information in the interviews. In
such cases, a social psychologist may want to rely on an indirect method, such as observation, to
gather more reliable data about a specific issue.
There are various methods of observation that may be informative in establishing a problem
definition. First, one could rely on a more unstructured observation method where no formal
observation and coding scheme is necessary. As with the interviews, one could prepare a checklist of
different topics that one would like to pay attention to in a particular setting. For example, in studying
anti-social behaviour among youngsters it may be helpful to go into the neighbourhoods in which
these youngsters live and actually observe their social interactions. In studying household recycling,
one could observe and analyse the contents of recyclable and non-recyclable bags to see what people
put in them which is what we did in a recent project (Lyas, Shaw, & Van Vugt, 2002). In companies
with high levels of absenteeism, it may be relevant to have a look around the workplace to investigate
under what conditions employees do their work and to what extent absenteeism is accepted in work
teams (Buunk, 1990; Geurts et al., 1994).
Sometimes it is better to remain unidentified as a social psychologist. The classic Hawthorne effect
tells us that people behave differently when they realize that they are being watched (for example, Big
Brother; see also Gillespie, 1991). This is particularly important in the study of more sensitive social
issues, such as racial prejudice and sexual harassment. For example, in a classic study on prejudice
(LaPiere, 1934), a team of researchers in the USA contacted various motels and hotels across the
country by pretending to be interested in renting a room. The white researcher had a much higher
success rate in getting the room than the Chinese researcher, perhaps an indication of prejudice. It is
quite clear that had the research team relied on interview data it would have been rather unlikely that
the home owners would have expressed any open signs of prejudice. Naturally, the success of this
participant observation technique stands or falls with the quality of fit between the researchers
profile and the profile of the sample he or she is studying.
applied problem that can potentially be solved after an intervention (problem aspects). The
problem is specific in so far as it needs to be tackled in Birmingham (the target group question),
but the results may have implications for anti-littering strategies elsewhere (problem aspects).
There is likely to be a social psychological dimension to the problem (problem aspects). The
literature suggests that people litter more if they see others doing it (Griskevicius, Cantu, & Van
Vugt, 2012; Sundie et al., 2012) and when they think they can get away with it, for instance at night
(the cause question). They may also think that it is not their responsibility to clean it up (the cause
question). It is possible that the problem is aggravated by a lack of street bins in the centre (the
cause question). The problem might be tackled through an intervention (psychological and/or
infrastructural), although it is questionable whether the problem can be completely solved.
Example 2: Obesity among school children in the UK
According to government statistics in Britain, almost one in ten children in the UK is now
seriously overweight, and if nothing is done this figure is likely to rise (see
www.nationalobesityforum.org.uk/). Obesity is a problem (the what question) as it is linked to a
number of health problems, such as high blood pressure, heart and kidney problems and diabetes,
which lower the life expectancy of these individuals (the who question) and lead to a rise in the
costs of health care for the nation as a whole (the why and who questions). Obese children are
also more likely to be stigmatized at school (the who question), and as a consequence may suffer
from underachievement and, more generally, a low self-esteem (the why question; see Crandall,
1988). As preventing obesity seems to be easier than tackling it after it has emerged in children,
the main question is how obesity can be prevented in children (the what question). Although there
are basic questions that still need to be answered regarding the causes of obesity, the problem is
applied. Obesity is caused by many different factors, but it most certainly involves unhealthy
eating habits and a lack of exercise (the cause question). Unhealthy eating and a lack of exercise
are two behaviours that have a clear social psychological dimension, because they are influenced
by, among other factors, prevailing social norms, peer pressure, and modelling by parents
(problem aspects). The target group constitutes at a minimum obese children and their parents.
As obesity is such a complex problem, it is likely best tackled through a mix of interventions,
including incentives, infrastructural arrangements and social psychological intervention.
problem, what are the main causes, which is the target group and what are the relevant problem
aspects. Box 2.3 contains two examples of good problem definitions.
We must stress that for educational purposes we would recommend that social psychologists
initially develop the problem definition by answering systematically all the key questions on the list.
Later on, once the social psychologist has gained experience working with our methodology, they can
prepare a longer version for themselves and an abbreviated version for a client. This shorter version
would usually describe what the problem is, why it is a problem, for whom it is a problem, and the
background and potential causes of the problem.
Box 2.4 The Problem Phase: Steps from a Problem to a Problem Definition
In reaching a solid problem definition you need to address the following questions (not
necessarily in the order below). In so doing, you will have to gather data about the problem by
collecting background information, by exploring the scientific literature, by conducting interviews
with relevant parties, and by relying on observations. The following six key questions must be
answered in order to establish a problem definition.
1. What is the problem?
Describe the problem in as much detail as possible by asking various specific questions about the
nature of the problem.
2. Why is it a problem?
Describe the consequences of the problem in detail and make clear to what extent each of those is
perceived to be a problem. Since when is it a problem? Describe the historical background of the
problem. When did it first emerge and when was it first noticed? Has the severity of the problem
increased or decreased over time?
3. For whom is it a problem?
Describe all the parties that are involved in the problem, both in terms of who causes the problem,
who suffers from the problem, and who is responsible for tackling the problem. Describe the
different perspectives that each of these parties has on the problem and whether these are
compatible or not. Also, describe whether the problem definition needs to be adjusted in order to
incorporate the different takes on the problem.
4. What are the possible causes of the problem?
Describe the possible causes and background of the problem. What might cause it and what could
explain the emergence of the problem? Use the double question: What may cause the problem and
how may these causes affect the problem?
5. What are the target groups?
Assignment 2
The manager of a university hospital asks you, as a social psychologist, for advice. According to the
manager, the burn-out rate among the nurses is unacceptably high and, worse, it is still rising. Last
year about 10 per cent of all nurses called in sick with symptoms of burn-out. Two years ago this was
only 5 per cent. Like a virus, the burn-out seems to be spreading throughout the hospital: departments
that used to have very low absenteeism now report high numbers of burned-out nurses. In addition,
patients have started complaining about the hastiness and bad temper of the nurses, about receiving
too little attention and being physically cared for insufficiently. According to the manager, the nurses
are simply not very motivated and should be dealt with firmly. The nurses, however, complain about
the workload that has increased ever since the reorganization of the hospital that took place two years
previously.
a. What is the problem exactly and why? Conduct imaginary interviews with the manager of the
hospital plus some nurses (both healthy and those on sick leave) and patients to clarify the
problem.
b. What are the possible causes of the problem?
How could the problem have started? Give preliminary explanations for the problem.
What may be the causes of the problem and by what processes might these causes generate
the problem?
To generate ideas about possible causes and explanations, read:
Aiken, L.H., Clarke, S.P., Sloane, D.M., Sochalski, J., & Silber, J.H. (2002). Hospital nurse staffing
and patient mortality, nurse burnout, and job dissatisfaction. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 288, 19871993.
Bakker, A.B. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2000). Burnout contagion processes among teachers. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 30(11), 22892308.
Cook, A., Gaynor, M., Stephens, M., & Taylor, L. (2012). The effect of a hospital nurse staffing
mandate on patient health outcomes: Evidence from Californias minimum staffing regulation. Journal
of Health Economics, 21, 340348.
Schaufeli, W.B. & Buunk, B.P. (2003). Burnout: An overview of 25 years of research and theorizing.
In M.J. Schabracq, J.A.M. Winnubst, & C.L. Cooper (Eds.),The handbook of work and health
psychology (pp. 383425). Chichester: Wiley.
c. To what extent is the problem an applied, concrete, and social psychological problem and why?
What attitudes, affective responses, or behaviour play a role in the problem? To what extent are
other types of explanation and intervention relevant?
d. Can the problem be solved or relieved? What should be the target group(s) of the social
psychological intervention? To what extent are those involved motivated to solve the problem?
e. Use the information you have gathered and the questions in Box 2.4 to formulate an adequate
problem definition.
3
The Analysis Phase: Finding Theory-Based
Explanations for Problems
Contents
Introduction
Specifying the Outcome Variable
Requirements for the Outcome Variable
Relevance
Specificity
Continuity
The Divergent Phase: Generating Explanations
Free Association
Interviews and Observations
Social Psychological Theories
Theoretical Approaches under Investigation: The Case of Safe Sex Promotion
A Topical Strategy
A Conceptual Strategy
A General Theory Strategy
The Convergent Phase: Reducing the Number of Explanations
Getting Rid of Redundant and Irrelevant Explanations
Getting Rid of Invalid Explanations
Getting Rid of Implausible Explanations
Introduction
In the Problem phase (Chapter 2) we have already explored some possible explanations for the
problem. In the Analysis phase we continue the search for explanations. First, we define the outcome
variable, that is, the variable that we want to change. Ideally, the outcome variable should be phrased
in terms of the desired end situation (for example, tolerance towards ethnic police officers).
Subsequently, in the divergent stage, we try to generate as many explanations as possible and try to
link these explanations to relevant social psychological theories. Finally, in the convergent stage, we
evaluate each of the theory-based explanations in terms of their relevance, validity, and plausibility
for the problem under investigation.
absenteeism.
With any problem, it is vital for applied social psychologists to determine as early as possible
what their primary outcome variable will be. Frequently the problem is defined only at a macrolevel, for example, environmental pollution, gun crime, or the incidence of breast cancer. In many
cases, such societal problems are the starting point for research and intervention. For example, policy
makers generally will be concerned about an increase in gun or knife killings, the air pollution in
main industrial areas, or the number of women dying of breast cancer. However, social psychologists
can affect these problems only indirectly, through inducing changes in a specific set of behaviours,
attitudes, and feelings in a specific set of individuals. For example, pollution can be reduced if more
people ride bikes instead of driving cars (Van Vugt, 2009). Gun crime can be tackled by making it
more difficult for people to buy guns (Podell & Archer, 1994). The incidence of breast cancer can be
reduced if women regularly engage in breast self-examination. Biking to work, a decreased interest in
guns, and engaging in breast self-examination are the sort of outcome variables which are of interest
to applied scientists using the PATH model.
It is preferable in the first instance to focus on a single outcome variable rather than a set of
variables. First, the variables might be so closely related that a change in one factor will
automatically produce a change in the other. In the context of healthy eating, for example, attitudes and
behaviours are often related (Brug, Oenema, & Campbell, 2003). Thus, it is assumed that a change in
preference towards healthy food increases the sales of healthy foods like fruit and vegetables.
Therefore, it is not necessary to include both variables as outcome variables: focusing on a
preference for healthy food as the outcome variable will suffice.
Second, when outcome variables are not directly related, it is often because they have a different
ontogenetic history, and therefore require quite different explanations and interventions. For example,
there are generally weak correlations between a range of environmentally relevant behaviours like
recycling, energy use, water conservation, and transportation (Gardner & Stern, 2002; Schultz &
Oskamp, 2000). Hence, explanations for garbage recycling may have little to do with accounts of why
people use buses or conserve energy or water. Accordingly, it would be unwise to incorporate them
into a single PATH model. It is therefore clear that social psychologists must be selective in their
decisions on what to focus on, and choose between several outcome variables.
Box 3.1 Interview with Professor Dieter Frey of the University of Munich (Germany)
I first got interested in applied social psychology more than 25 years ago. I began my applied research with a series of
studies on the recuperation process after severe accidents and surgeries. I applied the basic social psychology of control
theory and helplessness theory to this area. Our research showed that the recuperation process after severe accidents in
large part depends on the answers to the following questions:
Are victims asking the Why me? question (Why did this happen to me?)?
Do they think the accident could have been avoided?
Do they think they were responsible for the accident?
Can they foresee the process of recuperation?
We found that the people who recover best are those who do not ask the Why me? question, who see the accident as
unavoidable, who do not hold themselves responsible for the accident, who can foresee the process of recuperation and
who think that they can influence the recuperation process. Hopefully, this research contributed to a better understanding
of the aftermath of severe accidents and to a better way of coping with such life crises for victims, their families, and the
social workers working with them.
Later in my career, I did applied research on the environment and in the organizational field. In particular, I got more
interested in how our social psychological theories can be applied in the natural setting of processes of motivation,
leadership, optimizing teamwork, and innovation. For the future, I am very optimistic about applied social psychological
research. I think that we will see an increase in research and the application of our knowledge, especially with regard to
problems such as an ageing population and the needs of elderly people. I am glad about this development. In my opinion,
basic research alone is too boring (at least for me). Apart from this we have fascinating ideas in basic research that can
be excellently applied in natural settings. It would be a waste not to do so.
Relevance
The outcome variable must be relevant to the problem. First, the outcome variable must follow on
logically from the problem definition. If the problem analysis suggests, for example, that there is a
high turnover among ethnic officers in the police force, then it would make sense to choose as the
outcome variable a reduction in the turnover of ethnic officers rather than recruiting more ethnic
minority staff. Or, in efforts to promote volunteering activities to help the aged, the outcome variable
should be the willingness of people to do volunteer work rather than improving the welfare of the
people that are being helped, which is, of course, the ultimate goal. Thus, the outcome variable must
closely follow the problem definition and should ideally reflect the desired state of the intervention
(for example, less turnover, more energy conservation).
Specificity
The variable must be described in specific, concrete terms. In the PATH model, the outcome variable
ought to be as concrete as possible. Rather than talking about a need to discourage anti-social
behaviours in general, one should target a particular activity. For instance, one should focus instead
on concrete behaviours like littering, vandalism, graffiti, and so on. Even something like household
recycling may not be specific enough for an intervention, and one might need to focus instead on
recycling garden waste in particular. Specifying the outcome variable is important because outcome
variables that are formulated too broadly make it hard to develop an intervention programme that
effectively deals with the problem. An intervention programme based on an outcome variable that is
defined too broadly runs the risk of influencing aspects of the outcome variable that are not
problematic at all, while they may leave intact the aspect that is. For instance, when the government
wants to encourage citizens to recycle paper, an information campaign developed to affect citizens
household recycling (outcome variable) may affect the recycling of glass (which is not the problem)
but not the recycling of paper. Thus, social psychologists must be very specific about which variables
they wish to focus on.
Continuity
The variable must be continuous so that it can be described in quantitative terms (less or more). It
is useful to describe the outcome variable in quantitative terms, for example, in terms of frequency
(How often do you go to work by car?) or intensity (How much do you enjoy smoking?). Factors
such as recruitment policy or choice of travel mode are inadequate as outcome variables because
they cannot be described in terms of more or less. There are two reasons why it is important to
choose a continuous outcome variable. First, it makes it easier to generate explanations for the
problem and describe the causal model. For example, one can think of specific explanations for why
some people use their car more frequently than others, or why people enjoy exercise more or less. In
contrast, finding a satisfactory explanation for an insufficiently quantified outcome variable such as
choice of travel mode is almost impossible, because it is unclear what aspect of the outcome
variable one aims to influence and how. However, the frequency of travelling by car, or the frequency
of travelling by train, might be adequate outcome variables.
Second, a quantifiable variable helps in evaluating the success of an intervention programme. If
interventions to promote fitness and exercise are successful, then people should report that they
exercise more frequently after the intervention. If an intervention to decrease littering in a
neighbourhood is effective, this means there should be less litter on the streets after the intervention.
In contrast, if an outcome variable cannot be described in quantitative terms, the social psychologist
or policy maker will not be able to measure and evaluate the effectiveness of the intervention so
easily. The result is that no one will know for sure whether the intervention has helped or not. For
example, it is impossible to evaluate an intervention aimed at influencing the outcome variable
ethnic recruitment policy, simply because it is not possible to measure the variable recruitment
policy in a quantitative way.
We appreciate that it is not always possible to come up with a quantification of the outcome
variable. For obvious reasons, health professionals might be more interested in whether or not
teenagers smoke rather than how much they smoke a day. In that case, their outcome variable is binary
(namely, smoker vs non-smoker) and the success of an intervention is measured in terms of the
number or percentage of teenagers who give up smoking.
Free Association
To use an association technique for generating explanations, it is important not to be overly critical
and selective in the first instance. Much like brainstorming techniques (see Brodbeck & Greitemeyer,
2000; Paulus & Dzindolet, 1993), it is best to first generate many explanations. This is followed by a
more systematic analysis, which looks into the validity of each explanation and selects the more
promising ones for further enquiry. Furthermore, free association can lead from one explanation to
another, possibly better, explanation. In explaining why young male drivers are more likely to be
involved in traffic accidents, a social psychologist might initially conclude that these drivers simply
do not have the money to buy a new and safer car, until one realizes that this then should also apply to
young female drivers. Yet female drivers are much less accident prone (Elander, West, & French,
1993). This leads to a new explanation that young males take more risks when they are driving, and
are therefore more likely to be involved in traffic accidents (which is true just ask the car insurance
industry). Thus, building on other ideas via association can be fruitful.
1. Problem Association
The most straightforward form of association is to start with the problem itself, for example, traffic
accidents caused by young male drivers. The social psychologist could begin by generating five or
more explanations for the problem by asking herself why the problem is a problem. Again, it does not
matter at this stage whether the explanations are valid or not. For example, the social psychologist
could come up with the following explanations:
2. Concept Association
Another way to generate explanations is to move beyond the problem and look for phenomena that
might be conceptually similar to the problem under investigation. For example, car accidents among
young male drivers can be viewed in terms of risk-taking, which poses the question of whether young
men are generally more likely to take risks. Research in evolutionary psychology suggests that this is
indeed the case. Young men take greater physical and monetary risks, especially in the presence of
females (Daly & Wilson, 2001; Iredale, Van Vugt, & Dunbar, 2008; Wilson & Daly, 2004).
Similarly, male driving behaviour could be looked at by invoking explanations based on status
(driving riskily gives more status), optimism (young men are too optimistic about the risks of fast
driving), responsibility (young men have a lack of responsibility), and social norms (norms in their
peer group encourage risky driving). By introducing these concepts, the social psychologist has
translated the problem into a more abstract, scientific problem, which facilitates further analysis.
As another example, if one wants to explain why smiling waiters receive more tips (Van Baaren,
Holland, Steenaert, & Van Knippenberg, 2003), one could, among others, focus on concepts such as
sympathy (people give more to others they like), positive mood (seeing another person smile
enhances ones mood), and exchange (people feel more obliged to give a tip to someone who has just
given them a smile). Each of these concepts can then be used to formulate a preliminary explanatory
model which can be tested in subsequent research.
3. Perspective Taking
Perspective taking might also be useful as an association technique. Here one looks at the problem
Perspective taking might also be useful as an association technique. Here one looks at the problem
through the eyes of different actors. First, one defines which individuals are possibly involved in the
problem. Next, one puts oneself in the shoes of each of them. For example, do young male drivers
actually perceive that they take more risks than others? How would I feel about myself as a young
man when I drove very carefully? How would women view risky or careful drivers? How would I
react to an unfriendly waiter? What type of feeling does it give me when someone smiles at me? How
would I feel as an ethnic minority member in an overwhelmingly white police force? How would I
feel if ethnic minority people were my colleagues? Various concepts might be invoked through
perspective-taking techniques, which could be useful in generating explanations. For instance,
imagine yourself as a young man with a car you may come up with such concepts as adventure,
excitement, adrenaline, or girls, which offer potentially useful avenues for further enquiry.
1. Interviews
A specific interview tool to help generate explanations is the why interview. This could be a
genuine interview with relevant parties, but it could also be an imaginary exercise to force the social
psychologist to think about potential causes for the problem. Such interviews are very suitable to look
at the processes underlying the problem, and are therefore more detailed than the exploratory
interviews that we discussed in the Problem phase (Chapter 2). It is important in these interviews to
consider which outcome variables must ultimately be influenced through intervention.
With social psychological problems, the most likely questions concern why people behave the way
they behave, and why they think or feel the way they do. It is important in such interviews to vary the
questions. Constantly repeating the why question might annoy the interviewees, and such questions
might put them on the defensive. Instead, try asking questions like What makes you think that?, What
is it about that that ?, Why do you think that?, which might be more fruitful. Here is an example
of a social psychologist interviewing a female employee who has refused to accept a management
Social psychologist:
Why not?
Female employee:
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Maybe because most of them are men and they dont take women
managers very seriously.
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
There havent been any female managers and the one who was briefly
here left the job after less than a year.
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Her staff thought that the only reason she got the job was because of
an affirmative action programme.
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
No, but I dont think the top management in the company did enough
to support her.
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Hm maybe they thought that helping her would give out the wrong
signal.
Social psychologist:
Female employee:
Perhaps they were afraid that it would undermine her authority if they
offered help.
Admittedly, the questions posed by the social psychologist are a bit unimaginative. But the example
shows that by systematically asking a series of why-type questions, the social psychologist gets a
feeling for the underlying processes that might explain why women are not so keen to take on
management jobs in this particular company. Through the interview, a picture emerges in which the
lack of interest in management positions among women might have something to do with the (lack of)
support they get from subordinates and superiors in the company. This might be reinforced by the
affirmative action programme within the company that sends out the wrong message about the quality
of female managers.
We can present this explanatory model in a figure (see Figure 3.1), whereby we move back in the
causal chain, bottom-up, from the outcome variable (the lack of interest among women in management
positions) to a potential obstacle for achieving this (the performance expectations of female managers
as influenced by affirmative action plans).
This model is by no means complete and it raises many new why questions. For example, what is
the relationship between affirmative action policies and the performance expectations of female
managers? Do different departments respond differently to female managers, for example, depending
upon whether they are male- or female-dominated? Are there other reasons why women are not
interested in taking up management positions in the company? These questions might lead to a whole
set of new explanations, which could be captured in a process model like the one in Figure 3.1. At
this stage it is important to be exhaustive, so one should not yet focus too much on one set of
explanations, for example, the low performance expectations of female managers.
Cutting down the number of explanations and concentrating on the most relevant ones is something
that will not happen until the convergent phase.
2. Observations
Observational data are also useful for generating explanations. In the Problem phase (see Chapter 2),
observations were unsystematic. They were used to get a better understanding of the problem.
Observational research in the Analysis phase is more structured, and social psychologists may use
standard observation instruments in order to illuminate the causes and consequences of a particular
social problem. We can distinguish between the observations of others and self-observation
(introspection).
In the case of observation, the social psychologists and/or their assistants observe a process in a
group or organization. Take, for example, a social psychologist who has been asked to aid a large
hospital in solving decision-making problems in a management team which is concerned with patient
waiting lists. Some team members have complained about the poor decision-making processes and
the constant conflicts between representatives of the hospital management and medical staff. After
several interviews with key members of the staff, the social psychologist defines the problem in terms
of trying to improve the consensus and decision-making quality in this team with regard to the waiting
lists for patients. He now wants to find out why there are problems in the decision-making processes
and decides to systematically observe the team meetings.
He uses SYMLOG, a group observation instrument (Bales & Cohen, 1979), which he is trained in.
The SYMLOG instrument consists of 26 ratings that are given to each group member. (The full list of
items is displayed in Box 3.2.) Examples are items such as Active, dominant, talks a lot,
Unfriendly, negativistic, and Analytical, task oriented, problem solving that the social
psychologist has to score on a three-point scale (1 = rarely, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often). These 26
categories are then combined to yield scores for each team member on three main dimensions: (a)
dominantsubmissive, (b) friendlyunfriendly, (c) instrumentally controlledemotionally expressive.
With these data, observers can create a graphic representation of a group.
For example, SYMLOG could reveal that one or two members are clearly dominating the team
discussion. Furthermore, the analysis might reveal that there is a conflict between more analytical and
more emotional team members. This might help the social psychologist to understand the poor quality
of team decision making.
In the case of introspective methods, people are enabled to examine their own behaviour within a
certain time interval. Rather than through external, expert observation, the social psychologist might
ask the actors to rate themselves as they interact with others. One of the authors of this book used this
technique in a study among police officers (Buunk & Verhoeven, 1991). This research examined the
causes of stress among police officers in the Netherlands. Each working day within a five-day period,
officers were asked to write down every stressful experience they had using a diary method. The
researchers were able to distinguish between five stress categories based on these self-observations:
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
5. work underload, for example, a night shift without much work to do.
Because each stress incident was described in detail, it was possible to create a rich database of
different stressors. The researchers then focused on the greatest stressors among police officers.
Interestingly, the most stressful category was collaboration with colleagues. Hence, the process
model and the subsequent intervention plan focused on this particular stressor. This example shows
how a carefully conducted diary study can help explain a particular problem and set up an
intervention plan.
family are an important cause of job absenteeism, especially among women (Boyar, Maertz, &
Pearson, 2005). As another example, if smoking cessation is the outcome variable, then the social
psychologist will discover that there are numerous studies that examine factors involved in stopping
smoking, and that social support from peers plays an important role (DiClemente, Prochaska,
Fairhurst, & Velicer, 1991).
2. Conceptual Strategy
This approach reformulates the problem on a conceptually higher level to find links with relevant
social psychological phenomena and theories (see the Glossary). For example, absenteeism can be
seen as a stress response, which might encourage the social psychologist to look into stress theories
(Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 2000). Absenteeism may also be regarded
as a form of free-riding in which people do not pull their weight for the organization they work for
(Cooper, Dyck, & Frohlich, 1992; Koslowsky & Krausz, 2002). This leads the social psychologist to
look into the literature on social loafing (Karau & Williams, 1993), social dilemmas (Kerr &
Tindale, 2004; Van Lange, Balliet, Parks, & Van Vugt, 2013; Van Vugt & Samuelson, 1999), and
diffusion of responsibility (Latan & Darley, 1970). Absenteeism might be caused by feelings of
unfair treatment by management, which suggests a link with the literature about distributive and
procedural justice (Tyler, 2006; Van Prooijen, 2009), or absenteeism might be explained as a lack
of identification with the organization, suggesting the relevance of applying concepts from social
identity theory (Abrams & Hogg, 2004; Haslam, Reicher, & Platow, 2011) or self-categorization
theory (Hornsey, 2008).
global idea of the literature, but is by no means exhaustive. For further information about social
psychological theories, we would refer to introductory textbooks on social psychology (for example,
Aronson et al., 2010; Baumeister & Bushman, 2010; Hewstone et al., 2005; Kenrick et al., 2010;
Myers, 2012). We particularly recommend The Blackwell encyclopedia of social psychology, edited
by Manstead and Hewstone (1995), which provides a concise summary of all major theories and
concepts in social psychology, or, for more lengthy treatments, see the recent Handbook of theories
in social psychology, edited by Van Lange, Kruglanski, and Higgins (Sage, 2011).
Thus, the social psychologists initially focus on outcome variables that are cognitive (knowledge
about STDs) and behavioural (using condoms).
A false identification may have enormous consequences. In the case of wrongful imprisonment, the
innocent person is punished for a crime he or she did not commit, while the real criminal is still
on the loose and may strike again. In addition, the government and taxpayer suffer financially: one
year of imprisonment costs about $33,000 per prisoner. In general, judges and/or jurors value a
witness testimony more, as the eyewitness is more certain of his or her identification. There is,
however, only a very modest relation between eyewitness confidence and eyewitness accuracy.
Psychologists Wells and Bradfield,* from the Iowa State University, examined a potentially
important factor that may affect eyewitness certainty, namely feedback from a police officer
following identification in a line-up. In their experiment, participants viewed a security video in
which a gunman walked in front of the camera. Participants were subsequently asked to identify
the gunman from a photo-spread. The actual gunman was, however, not in the photo-spread and all
of the eyewitnesses made false identifications. Following the identification, witnesses were given
confirming feedback (Good, you identified the actual suspect in the case), disconfirming
feedback (Oh. You identified X. Actually, the suspect is Y), or no feedback. Participants were
then asked questions about the video and to give a written description of the gunman.
Compared to participants who had received no feedback, participants who had received
disconfirming feedback reported less certainty about the identification, having had a lower clarity
of memory and a worse view, and estimated they needed more time to arrive at an identification.
On the contrary, despite the fact that they had made false identifications as well, participants who
had received confirming feedback reported more certainty about the identification, having had a
better view and a higher clarity of memory, and estimated they needed less time to arrive at an
identification. On the basis of their study, Wells and Bradfield strongly recommend that the police
officer who administers the line-up or photo-spread should be someone who does not know which
person is the real suspect and that he or she should secure a confidence statement from the
eyewitness at the time of the identification.
* Wells, G.L. & Bradfield, A.L. (1998). Good, you identified the suspect: Feedback to
eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology,
83(3), 360376.
A Topical Strategy
In generating theory-based explanations for the problem of STDs and condom use failure, the most
straightforward approach is to find examples in the literature of research programmes on STDs and
condom use. There might not be much on condom use in relation to STDs, but there is presumably a
lot of research on determinants of condom use, for example, in relation to HIV/AIDS and pregnancy.
Searches on PsycINFO, the electronic database for the psychological literature, reveal no fewer than
2,075 hits with condom use as key words and 1,512 with STD as a key word (at the time of
writing). The combination of condom use and STD reveals a more manageable number of 312 hits,
and their abstracts can be inspected in terms of their relevance for this problem. Articles that review
a large part of the literature so-called reviews and meta-analyses are particularly useful. This
literature might reveal a number of interesting conclusions which could be used to generate
explanations for why teenagers fail to use condoms in relation to STDs. For example:
It is generally recommended to start with a topical approach, first, because it allows social
psychologists to use the knowledge of previous research to distinguish between likely and unlikely
explanations for problems. For example, research shows that most teenagers perceive condom use as
a sensible strategy. This suggests that knowledge about the benefits of condom use is perhaps not the
main obstacle, which could help social psychologists in developing a process model. The second
reason why it is recommended to begin with this strategy is that one immediately obtains a valid
insight into a problem. Indeed, if several studies show that knowledge about the benefits of condom
use is widespread among teenagers, then social psychologists may safely assume that this is the case.
A third advantage is that often there will be examples of intervention programmes reported in the
literature. This enables social psychologists to make a judgement at an early stage about the
intervention potential of particular explanations.
There are also disadvantages with this approach. First, the generalization of the research might be
a problem. Suppose most of the research on condom use has been done in Western Europe. One
cannot simply assume that the same findings will also be found among teenage populations in North
America or Africa. Second, there is always a risk of changing the problem into one that is already in
the literature. By doing this, social psychologists could lose sight of the specific problem that they
were asked to investigate. For example, research might suggest that teenagers are more likely to use
condoms to avoid pregnancy. Yet the social psychologists were being asked to examine strategies to
foster condom use in relation to the threat of STDs. A third problem is that by looking in detail at
other programmes, social psychologists could become a bit complacent and not think actively and
creatively about a problem. There is, for example, the risk of uncritically adopting programmes that
are not properly evaluated or do not incorporate recent scientific insights.
A Conceptual Strategy
The conceptual problem analysis enables social psychologists to look for theories that could be
fruitfully applied to the problem. Through association techniques, the problem is translated into
another set of more abstract and generic problems which may have been reported in the social
psychological literature (see the Glossary). These problems can be used as key words in an
electronic database search, like PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, or Web of Science. Also, one could
look at relevant social psychology textbooks for information about these topics. In the case of socalled emergency helping, through association with key terms such as altruism and prosocial
behaviour, we find bystander intervention theory and social dilemmas. In the STD example, one
could look for associated words such as health, risk, vulnerability, optimism, and peer
pressure.
The difference between a topical and a conceptual strategy is sometimes minor. A social
psychologist taking a topical perspective to explore how victims of accidents cope in the aftermath of
the event will quickly discover that one of the more commonly used social psychological models to
explain coping with accidents is attribution theory (Weiner, 1990), a theory that she might also
come across with the conceptual strategy. Attribution research into coping with accidents shows that
victims cope better with the consequences if they perceive themselves at least in part to blame for
their misfortune. As another example of this overlap, in trying to explain the lack of enthusiasm for
sustainable transport use, researchers will quickly find in the literature a reference to theories about
social dilemmas (Van Vugt et al., 1995, 2000).
In the STD example, a conceptual strategy might result in a list of social psychological terms such
as risk, risk perception, cognition, optimism, health promoting behaviours, habit,
negotiation and power, and self-efficacy. Once such a list has been prepared, one could then
examine the social psychological literature for further information about these concepts. We will give
just two examples of how a conceptual approach might inform the search for explanations of
inconsistent condom use.
First, research on cognitive biases shows that people generally underestimate the chance that
something bad will happen to them, like an illness, while overestimating the chance that something
good will happen to them like winning the lottery. This is called unrealistic optimism (Weinstein &
Klein, 1996) and it may apply to the way teenagers think about contracting STDs. Although this
phenomenon might not have been studied in relation to STDs, it has been studied for a range of other
health-related behaviours and so it is plausible that the same mechanism might be at work in our
example. Further reading on unrealistic optimism suggests that: (a) young people are more likely to
hold such beliefs than old people, (b) optimism is generally higher with regard to bad things than
good things, and (c) optimism is higher with behaviours that are controllable than uncontrollable
(Rutter, Quine, & Albery, 1998; Sparks, Shepherd, Wieringa, & Zimmermans, 1995). These results
suggest that this theory may be meaningfully applied to the safe sex example.
Second, the literature on negotiation and bargaining (Bazerman, Curhan, Moore, & Valley, 2000;
Thompson, 2006) suggests that individuals with less power in a relationship have more difficulties in
negotiating a good deal. Power is related to how much people depend upon a relationship, both
materially and psychologically. Having many opportunities to fulfil needs outside a relationship
increases peoples power position. Based on this research, social psychologists would expect that
condom use might be affected by the power position that teenagers have in their relationship. People
who feel less powerful might not want to discuss condom use with their partner although they might
be aware of the benefits of it.
There are many other examples of how the conceptual approach might lead to explanations of
inconsistent condom use. The risk perception literature suggests, for example, that people
underestimate risks that are statistically small, and which involve a one-time activity, such as sexual
intercourse (Linville, Fischer, & Fischhoff, 1993). This implies that, although the chances of
contracting STDs are small without a condom, people tend to believe they are invulnerable.
Alternatively, research suggests that habits are difficult to change, in part, because people do not
attend to information criticizing the habit (Verplanken & Aarts, 1999). This suggests that once people
have established a habit of not using condoms, changing this habit through intervention might be very
difficult indeed.
These examples show that the essence of the conceptual approach is to use the problem definition
to find concepts that are related to the problem. These concepts can then be used to find relevant
theories that make predictions about the social psychological processes underlying a particular
problem. The main advantage is that it can lead to a rich pattern of explanations, each of which can be
elaborated further using appropriate theories and research. Furthermore, from the relevant theories, it
is much easier to think of a set of interventions to tackle the problem. The main disadvantage is, of
course, that it is easy to get overwhelmed by a multitude of theories. A social psychologist must
therefore make an important decision about what she will focus on in the subsequent steps of the
PATH model.
explanations is reduced by getting rid of irrelevant and redundant explanations. Second, the
theoretical validity of each of the remaining explanations is tested. Third, the remaining explanations
are checked for their plausibility to account for the problem. This results in a smaller set of
explanations that can be used in the next two phases of the PATH model developing and testing a
process model (Chapter 4) and setting up a help programme (Chapter 5). It is important to end up
with a set of explanations that describe the social psychological processes leading to a problem in
sufficient detail. One should avoid ending up with a set of dead-end explanations like teenagers
who fail to use contraceptives are less intelligent.
conditions of the problem. For example, to apply social dilemma theory (Van Lange et al., 2013; Van
Vugt, 2009) to a particular social problem, it must be shown that the characteristics of the problem
are, in fact, a social dilemma. A social dilemma refers to a situation in which the rational pursuit of
self-interest can lead to collective disaster (Komorita & Parks, 1994). According to social dilemma
theory, a problem is only a social dilemma when two conditions are met, namely, with regard to the
particular situation that, first, selfishness has to be more attractive than cooperation, and second, that
selfishness by all has to make everyone worse off in the long run (Dawes & Messick, 2000). Framed
in this way, it seems inappropriate to use a social dilemma framework to explain why some drug
addicts continue to commit street robberies even though they know that they will be caught and put in
prison. The benefits of the selfish act (committing a robbery) do not outweigh the cost (that
selfishness is more attractive than cooperation) and so a social dilemma explanation cannot easily be
invoked.
It should be kept in mind that many social psychological theories are described in very generic
terms, but really only apply to specific situations. A review of the scientific literature tells you under
what conditions the theory has been tested and proven to work. Cognitive dissonance theory
(Festinger, 1957; see the Glossary), for example, assumes that when people experience dissonance
between their beliefs (for example, I want to be a reliable worker) and their behaviour (for
example, I am often late for work) they will try to reduce this dissonance by changing their beliefs.
The theory, however, states quite clearly that this will only happen if people think that they are
personally responsible for their action (Cooper & Fazio, 1984). If they dont believe they are
responsible, then they should not experience dissonance (for instance, you arrive late for work
because of traffic congestion).
Another classic example is social comparison theory (for a review, see Buunk & Gibbons, 2007;
see the Glossary). This generic social psychological theory states that people, when comparing
themselves to their peers in terms of their abilities, frequently make upward social comparisons, that
is, they compare themselves to people slightly better than them. Research has generally supported this
prediction, but many results were obtained in one classic experimental setting only (Wheeler & Suls,
2005). In this setting, a number of participants took an ability test and then received feedback about
their test score. False feedback ensures that they always occupy the middle position in their group.
The scores of the other group members were not given. The participants then got a chance to look at
the score of one other member of their group. Participants usually chose the group member with the
next-best score, lending support to the upward comparison tendency, as predicted by social
comparison theory.
Given the restrictions of this research paradigm, to what situations could this theory be
meaningfully applied? Suppose a company asks a social psychologist about the implications of
making public what managers in the company earn. The social psychologist turns to social
comparison theory and predicts that employees will be primarily concerned about the earnings of
staff members that are one level higher up the echelon and that they are relatively less concerned
about the salaries of the board of directors. This may be what the theory predicts, but given that in
experiments on this theory people have only been allowed to look at the salary of one other person
(or group), this conclusion might be slightly premature. Moreover, many recent studies have
suggested that social comparison may occur with a wide variety of others (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007).
It is therefore very well possible that, when given the opportunity, employees will also compare their
salary with that of the directors. Translating the results of internally valid experiments into applicable
knowledge about real-world problems is a recurrent concern of any applied social psychologist (see
for example, Aronson et al., 2010; Baron & Branscombe, 2012; Kenrick et al., 2010).
Finding the conditions under which a particular theory is applicable is an important task because it
helps social psychologists decide on whether the theory can be fruitfully applied to a particular
problem. In general, it is not enough to simply read the theory. Recent review articles can be helpful
because they may give an updated summary of the state of a particular theory. However, usually one
must look into how the experiments were conducted to find out about the theorys boundaries.
Applying social psychological theories requires a basic knowledge about the research literature on a
particular theory.
them tip more, for example, by playing relaxing music in a restaurant or offering pleasant food? If so,
there is another reason to support the mood hypothesis.
A second explanation is that customers tip friendly waiters more because they think the waiter likes
them, perhaps more so than other customers. Many people are prepared to pay a little extra for royal
treatment. If this explanation is correct, then customers should tip waiters less if they appear to be
friendly to everyone. Now we can conduct a thought experiment in which we compare the two
situations: a waiter who is friendly to all customers versus a waiter who is friendly to some
customers only. In the latter case, we might be flattered but we might also doubt the professional
integrity of the waiter and therefore tip less. Hence, there is a question mark about the validity of the
royal treatment explanation.
A third explanation is that customers perceive a smile or a friendly face as a gift, and they therefore
feel obliged to reciprocate this favour. This reciprocity (Delton et al., 2011; Van Vugt & Van Lange,
2006) hypothesis is plausible if we can find other situations in which people feel they must return a
favour by giving money when someone is friendly to them. There might be such situations, for
example, when we donate to charity collectors who approach us with a friendly face. However, often
we simply return a smile with a smile and there is no obligation to give money for a smile. It could
be, of course, that friendliness is reciprocated with a tip only in restaurant settings. To test this
hypothesis, a thought experiment can be carried out in which a customer returns a smile with a smile
or in which the customer is the first to smile. In those cases, there should be less need to give a tip
according to the obligation hypothesis. As this appears to be rather implausible, this explanation can
be effectively ignored.
Of course, thought experiments such as these do not produce any hard evidence. Therefore it is
unwise to rely solely upon them. However, thought experiments do make it easier to select the most
relevant causes for a problem. They may also serve as a basis for conducting further interviews or
observations that reveal the most likely causes for the problem. In addition, it must be noted that most
social psychological phenomena are based on a rather complex interaction between various different
factors. Thus, it is important to concentrate on several plausible explanations in developing and
testing a process model (Chapter 4) and setting up an intervention programme (Chapter 5).
conduct observations.
4. Try to come up with theory-based explanations for the problem. Use not only a topical
approach but also a conceptual and a general theory approach by looking at relevant social
psychological theories.
5. Reduce the number of explanations through:
i. combining redundant explanations and eliminating irrelevant explanations;
ii. inspecting the validity of theory-based explanations;
iii. checking the plausibility of the explanations (through real or thought experiments).
Assignment 3
A manager at a large video game company (Warmonger Studios) asks you, as a social psychologist,
for advice. His team is facing internal communication problems which are affecting their team
cohesiveness and creativity. Many of the weekly team meetings degenerate into unproductive
arguments, and they are falling behind on their deadlines. The manager believes this may be due to a
wave of new hires that occurred when the company underwent a recent merger, purchasing a smaller
company called EyeCandy to help them release a sequel to their wildly popular game CandyQuest.
The new employees are mainly female and much younger than the average Warmonger employees,
who are primarily men in their forties, and the manager believes these differences may be the cause of
the recent problems.
Prior to the merger, the atmosphere at Warmonger had been very relaxed, and not exactly a
professional environment. Employees kept loose working hours, dressed very casually, and even
brought pets to the office. The EyeCandy employees are used to a much more professional
atmosphere, and some are allergic to pets. Management changed the corporate policy to reflect no
animals in the office, regular business hours, and an appropriate business casual dress code, making
all employees sign the new policy document. So the old and the new employees seem to hold some
resentment towards each other as they transition into one working business.
This type of organizational dilemma is referred to as managing team diversity. The manager is
determined to encourage a positive atmosphere within his team, and to get the best out of every
employee, but traditional measures, such as extra vacation time and Casual Fridays, just dont seem
to be working.
He now asks you as a social psychologist to develop a strategy to help him improve team cohesion
and performance:
a. Formulate a problem definition according to Chapter 2s criteria (see Box 2.4, p. 51).
b. Specify the outcome variable in terms of behaviour, attitude, and/or emotions. The outcome
variable must be relevant to the problem as well as specific, concrete, and continuous.
c. Try to generate as many explanations as possible (at least five!) for the problem by means of
problem and concept association and perspective taking. Dont be too critical and selective in
the first instance.
d. Conduct two imaginary why interviews to find out what process may underlie the problem:
one with the manager and one with an employee. Vary your questions (for example, What
makes you think that?, What is it about that that ?, Why do you think that?).
e. Generate as many theoretical explanations as you can on the basis of:
social identity theory;
leadership theory;
cross-categorization;
intergroup bias;
the common ingroup identity model.
f. Reduce the number of explanations by:
getting rid of irrelevant and redundant explanations;
testing the theoretical validity of each of the remaining explanations;
checking the remaining explanations for their plausibility in accounting for the problem by
using thought experiments.
To generate ideas about possible causes and explanations, read:
Crisp, R.J. & Hewstone, M. (2001). Multiple categorization and implicit intergroup bias: Differential
category dominance and the positivenegative asymmetry effect. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 31, 4562. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.31.
Dovidio, J.F., Gaertner, S.L., & Saguy, T. (2009). Commonality and the complexity of we: Social
attitudes and social change. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 320. doi:
10.1177/1088868308326751.
Homan, A.C., Hollenbeck, J.R., Humphrey, S.E., van Knippenberg, D., Ilgen, D.R., & van Kleef
G.A. (2008). Facing differences with an open mind: Openness to experience, salience of intra-group
differences, and performance of diverse groups. Academy of Management Journal, 51, 12041222.
4
The Test Phase: Developing and Testing the Process
Model
Contents
Introduction
Formulating a Process Model
Case Study: Improving Cooperation in a Pharmaceutical Company
Case Study: Reducing the Risk of STD Infection
Heuristics for Developing a Process Model
Testing the Process Model
Doing Your Own Research
Introduction
Once a set of explanations has been identified and selected by a social psychologist, he or she then
develops a process model. This model serves as a template for developing interventions. In this
chapter, we discuss the third step of the PATH model, the Test phase. In this stage we make
suggestions on how to develop the process model and how to test the empirical validity of the model.
The Analysis Based on interviews with the director and the scientists at the firm, the applied
psychologists realize that the success of the R&D division is dependent upon scientists working
closely together on developing a narrow range of new products and ideas. They are also aware that
this is not necessarily in each scientists interest. Each of them would presumably rather work on
developing their own products than on helping to develop someone elses, even though the latter
might be better from the companys point of view. From this analysis, the psychologists conclude that
they have to find ways to resolve this conflict of interests in order to facilitate cooperation. They turn
to the social psychological literature for answers and find a paradigm that might be useful: the social
dilemma paradigm. A social dilemma is a situation in which individuals with competing interests
damage the collective interest (see Chapter 3). If many individuals pursue their narrow, selfish
interests, the collective suffers and in the end everyone will be worse off. The social dilemma
literature identifies many different factors that facilitate group cooperation (Kerr & Tindale, 2004;
Van Lange et al., 2013; Van Vugt, 2009).
The psychologists believe that the problem in R&D is essentially a social dilemma and examine
the literature for clues. They find that cooperation in social dilemmas is promoted through offering
selective (financial) incentives for cooperation (Van Vugt, 2009). They also find that better
communication facilitates teamwork, and people work better together if they identify more strongly
with their team (Dawes, Van de Kragt, & Orbell, 1988). Together, these factors promote the
commitment that people feel towards their group, which in turn increases team cooperation and
productivity (Dovidio et al., 2006; Van Lange et al., 2013).
Developing a Process Model How to develop a process model on the basis of these explanations?
First, we must be clear about the outcome variable. What is it that we wish to influence or improve?
Ideally, this has already been done in the previous steps of the PATH model, but perhaps there is a
concern that the outcome variable is still not concrete enough. For example, the generic term
cooperation could be turned into something more specific like willingness to help your
colleagues.
Next, we must draw up a diagram of the preliminary process model (see Figure 4.1). We can put
the outcome variable in a box to the right of the figure. Subsequently, we will select one of the
possible explanations for the problem. The social dilemma literature suggests that the primary cause
of competition among the scientists lies in the nature of the task competition for new products and
ideas and that this inhibits cooperation (Van Lange et al., 2013). Thus, a first sketch of the process
model might look something like Figure 4.1.
Figure 4.1 A preliminary process model: What determines cooperation in this company?
This looks like a plausible first model, but there are several problems with it. First, the
explanatory factor (nature of the task) is not described as a continuous and quantitative variable
(see Chapter 3). It is unclear how this variable could vary from less to more, and therefore how
Figure 4.2 An improved process model: What determines cooperation in this company?
This second model is a substantial improvement yet it is still lacking in details about the process
This second model is a substantial improvement yet it is still lacking in details about the process
leading to the manifestation of the problem. We have now identified three task-related characteristics
that might cause the problem, but we do not know whether they have a direct or indirect influence on
the problem. In other words, does A cause B directly, or does A influence B via its effect upon C?
For example, poor coordination of task activities presumably leads to poor communication between
scientists, and this in turn affects the absence of a helping culture in R&D. The social dilemma
research suggests that communication promotes cooperation (Van Lange et al., 2013), so this is
clearly an important intermediate factor. Similarly, the absence of a shared group product may lead
the scientists to not feel very responsible for the activities of the research team as a whole. Thus, a
shared group product might influence willingness to help via an increased responsibility for the
group product that scientists feel. In turn, all these factors may undermine team cohesion within the
division, which could be the proximate cause of an unwillingness to help each other.
In addition, the variables introduced in Figure 4.2 might in turn be caused by other factors. For
example, salaries tend to depend upon each scientists personal achievements (how many product
ideas they have developed in their labs), which probably creates a climate of excessive competition
between individuals. In turn, this might affect the cohesion of the team and their willingness to help
each other. Furthermore, the fact that each scientist has his or her own laboratory may make it difficult
for them to coordinate their activities and appreciate that there is ultimately a shared group product.
This leads to a much improved model, depicted in Figure 4.3.
Figure 4.3 Improved process model 2: What factors determine cooperation within this company?
If we work our way backwards then, this model suggests that the willingness to help each other in
the R&D division is undermined by poor team cohesion. This, in turn, is affected by the absence of
sufficient communication between scientists and the absence of feelings of personal responsibility for
the group product. These factors in return are caused, ultimately, by an excessive competition
between the scientists (perhaps due to the salary system) and by the absence of task coordination and
of a shared group product (perhaps due to each scientist only working in his or her own lab).
This is one possible way to construct a process model, but it is certainly not the only way. In
general, it takes a lot of time and adjustment to come up with a process model that describes the
relationships between the variables as precisely and completely as possible. It is worth it because a
good process model should give a clear recommendation regarding the nature of interventions
necessary to tackle the problem. For example, as a result of their process analysis, the psychologists
might recommend to the director that laboratories should be shared, where possible, so that scientists
communicate more with each other, thereby learning to work together on each others ideas.
Box 4.1 Interview with Professor Michael West of the Lancaster University Management
School (UK)
When I finished my PhD in psychology, I worked underground as a labourer for a year in a coal mine, in order to earn
money to pay off student debts. Once I got over my fear of working in such a dangerous environment, I became
fascinated by the teamworking and camaraderie that are necessary for safety and effectiveness in such a hostile
environment. That experience shaped much of my subsequent interest in applied social and organizational psychology.
Research too is about teamworking and vigorous collaboration across boundaries.
Since my work in the coal mine, I have worked at colleges all over the world: among others, at the University of Kent
(UK), the Bermuda College (Bermuda), the University of Queensland (Australia) and the Eindhoven University of
Technology (The Netherlands). Since joining Aston Business School in 1999 and more recently Lancaster University
Management School, I have bridged many more boundaries (disciplinary and international) and feel privileged to work in
the domains and with the wide variety of colleagues that academic life offers.
In general, my work in social/organizational psychology has been applied with a particular focus on influencing
organizational and public policy. I am currently running an annual survey for the English National Health Service on staff
attitudes and experiences and linking the findings to outcomes such as patient mortality, patient satisfaction, errors and
staff behaviours. It is very rewarding to see applied social and organizational psychological research being translated into
government policies and thereby positively affecting patient care.
For the future I foresee an important role for social psychology in the field of ageing. The ageing of the population is a
major change in society, and social psychologists must urgently engage with this currently neglected topic. In addition,
globalization, international travel and migration make diversity a fundamentally important topic. And who better than social
psychologists to explore and explain how we can make a strength and virtue of these changes? We must discover how we
can successfully overcome the inherent pathology of intergroup prejudice which threatens our survival as a species. This
requires understanding positive social processes associated with successful diversity and promulgating those far and wide.
Blackwell Publishing.
The Problem Definition According to an estimate by UNAIDS, in 2005 more than 26 million people
worldwide are suffering from HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS/WHO, 2012; see also
Chapter 1). In Britain
alone, around 80,000 people are believed to be infected with the HIV virus. There is currently no
cure or remedy available for HIV/AIDS. The only feasiblestrategy is to engage in behaviours that
reduce ones risk of contracting HIV/AIDS. The main preventative behaviour is condom use. Condom
use also protects against the transmission of other, less deadly, types of sexually transmitted diseases
(STDs), such as chlamydia and gonorrhoea.
One particular health authority in a coastal area of Britain (for whom is it a problem?) has
documented a rise in STDs (what is the problem?) among the young adult population. This is
attributed to an increase in late night entertainment facilities in the city that attract a lot of youngsters
to the area, particularly during the summer months (causes of the problem). It has been decided that
there will be a campaign to promote safe sex practices among youngsters in the area, and the
authorities decide to focus on condom use in particular. A team of health professionals and social
psychologists is hired to set up a campaign to foster condom use and they begin by reviewing the
relevant behavioural and psychological literature (see, for example, Adih & Alexander, 1999; Buunk
et al., 1998; Harvey et al., 2006; Prat, Planes, Gras, & Sullman, 2012; Sheeran, Abraham, & Orbell,
1999; Sheeran & Taylor, 1999). From this literature, several potential causes appear for the failure to
use condoms:
A lack of awareness about STDs, and the relation between STDs and unprotected sex.
People dont believe they are at risk of contracting an STD, possibly because they use other
contraceptives, such as the pill.
People think that sex without condoms somehow feels better.
People believe their partner is not bothered about using a condom.
People do not bring up condom use, possibly because they are too embarrassed or they think
their partner will reject them.
People are too drunk to consider using a condom.
People do not bring condoms when they go out.
People lack practice in putting on a condom.
There is no positive inclination to use a condom.
In working towards an intervention programme to foster condom use, the psychologists decide to
concentrate on two well-known attitude-behaviour models: the theory of reasoned action (TRA;
Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) and the theory of planned behaviour (TPB; Ajzen, 1987, 2012). They
specify condom use as the outcome variable in their process model. Next, they select a potential
cause. The main determinant of condom use, according to these theories, is a positive intention to use
a condom. Hence, the preliminary causal model that they draw up (see Figure 4.4) contains two
factors.
example, educational level) to the arrow between two existing variables (see Figure 4.8). In addition,
it could be argued that control beliefs exercise both an indirect effect on condom use, via shaping the
intention, as well as a direct effect. For example, a positive intention towards using a condom might
fail to materialize in the desired behaviour because a person might simply have forgotten to bring a
condom (Gage, 1998).
Indirect effects: one variable affects another variable via a third variable. For example, the
belief that condom use may prevent STDs affects the behaviour by adding to the intention to use
a condom.
Reinforcing effects: one variable strengthens the impact of another variable on the behaviour.
For example, educational level might interact with subjective norms to produce a stronger
effect on the intention to use condoms.
Undermining effects: one variable mitigates the relationship between two other variables. For
example, being in a long-term relationship might weaken the influence of positive attitudes on
the intention to use a condom.
studies on a particular topic (Balliet, Li, Macfarlan, & Van Vugt, 2011). In the condom use example,
the health psychologist finds an article that contains a very helpful and recent meta-analytic review of
the psychological and behavioural correlates of condom use, entitled: Theories of reasoned action
and planned behaviour as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. The article is published in the
scientific journal Psychological Bulletin and is written by psychologist Dolores Albarracn and her
colleagues. This review suggests the correlations between the different variables shown in the model
(see Figure 4.9).
It can be seen from this model that the intention to use a condom has a moderately strong positive
influence on condom use (r = .45). Attitudes, subjective norms, and control beliefs all have a
moderately strong positive influence on the intention to use a condom (r = .58, r = .39, and r = .45
respectively). In a similar vein, attitudes are positively influenced by beliefs about condom use (r =
.56) and subjective norms by normative beliefs (r = .46). Control beliefs also show a direct positive
influence on condom use (r = .25). However, the article also shows that, after controlling for
intentions, the direct influence of control beliefs on condom use becomes very small. That is, the
influence of control beliefs on condom use can be explained almost entirely through their effect on
intentions.
level of the participants. If these background variables are comparable with the characteristics of the
situation and population the social psychologist aims to influence, she might decide to keep the arrow
in the model, despite the negative conclusion of the review article. In a similar vein, the social
psychologist should try to find empirical support in the literature for all of the relationships in her
process model.
Thus, it is important to examine the relationships between the model variables by consulting the
empirical literature. This means that for every relationship in the model, one must specify the strength
and direction of the relationship. Obviously, the variables with the strongest influence are the most
useful ones for an intervention, because a change in that variable is likely to lead to a significant
change in the variables it influences.
How does one find relevant research? In seeking to find literature one could use one of the three
approaches that we outlined in Chapter 3 to finding explanations: the topical, the conceptual, and the
general theoretical approach. It is generally advisable to search for research that is directly relevant
to the problem definition, thus the issue-related approach. The reason is that the relevance of the
findings diminishes with greater difference between the research and the problem the social
psychologist is interested in. Although there are various literature search programs available (like
PsycINFO or Google Scholar), it may not be easy to find all the available evidence. First, not all
research is published, and this is more likely for null effects, that is, research that supports the null
hypothesis of no difference. It is therefore often useful to send a message to an email list of
researchers in a particular field (like the SPSP or EAESP mailing lists for social psychologists).
Second, not all the published research is of the same relevance and quality so it is important to
carefully read each article and judge its quality and relevance. Third, research articles may contradict
each other in their results or conclusions. Ones best bet is often a meta-analytic review of a research
domain, which summarizes the available evidence and estimates the strengths of a particular effect
across a broad domain of studies (as in the condom use example). Such meta-analytic reviews appear
in journals like Psychological Bulletin, Personality and Social Psychology Review, Health
Psychology, and Health Education Quarterly, and it is worth looking out for them.
Even if there is no research available on the topic of interest, it may still be useful to examine the
literature. For example, a social psychologist may be interested in organizational citizenship
behaviour, but because there is not yet a lot of research available on it, he may look instead at the
broader helping literature for clues (Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin, & Schroeder, 2005). But even if a
conceptual approach does not work, a general theory approach might be helpful. For example,
research on the theory of reasoned action (TRA) or the theory of planned behaviour (TPB) might be
helpful even when the behaviours which are of interest are entirely different. For instance, the
knowledge that subjective norms are an important influence on smoking decisions might help a social
psychologist in thinking about what factors might influence recycling. Similarly, a general
evolutionary psychological theory like reciprocal altruism might give an applied psychologist the
suggestion that stress and burn-out in the workplace may be caused by feelings of inequity and
unfairness (Buunk & Schaufeli, 1999).
Unfortunately, not all the relationships in the model will have been documented in the research
literature. For example, there simply may not be any existing research on the relationship between
alcohol intake and condom use. Thus, it is important that, if necessary, social psychologists are able
to do their own research to find answers. This book is not the place to discuss in any great detail the
research methods needed for doing applied social psychological research. Each person studying for a
degree in social psychology will have a solid training in research methods in psychology. And there
are many good texts available on conducting social psychological research (see, for example,
Breakwell, 2004; Crisp & Turner, 2010; Schneider, Gruman, & Coutts, 2012).
The importance of doing research in answering an applied social psychological problem cannot be
overemphasized. Often, there is empirical literature on a particular topic but the research may not
have been adequately done. Or the research has tested a particular relationship in a slightly different
domain from the one the social psychologist is interested in. For example, a psychologist interested in
the determinants of using contraceptives cannot simply rely on the research on condom use.
Nevertheless, there may be important clues to be found in this literature. In addition, many field
studies contain cross-sectional data (all gathered at the same time), and the resultant correlation
between two variables may not indicate a causal relationship whereby one variable causes another.
One must therefore be cautious in simply applying the results of other studies to the problem under
investigation.
Furthermore, many studies in social psychology are laboratory-based, primarily with students as
research participants. These studies are high in internal validity (that is, the results are reliable), but
they are sometimes lacking in external validity (that is, the results may not be applicable to other
situations and/or populations). Hence, one should consider the possibility that there are limitations in
generalizing the results to apply to the real world. For example, some people might take task risks in
the laboratory, gambling with small sums of money, but does it mean that they also take risks in realworld tasks like drug use, not wearing seatbelts or having unprotected sex? Even if results have been
obtained in field research, there is often the question of whether data can be generalized from one
population to another. This is particularly important in cross-cultural research. Research suggests that
American people differ from Asian people in the way they respond to social psychology experiments.
American students, for example, see themselves as more independent whereas Japanese students see
themselves as more interdependent (Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997). This
difference might be relevant in finding strategies to promote, for instance, cooperation in the
workplace or at school.
In conducting research to establish the validity of a process model, it can be useful to conduct a
quick survey. In survey research, the main variables are operationalized and measured and then,
through statistical procedures, the researcher determines the relationship between the variables.
Whenever possible, researchers use existing scales of variables that have been tested in previous
research and reported in the psychological literature. An example of a commonly used scale is
Rosenbergs self-esteem scale (containing ten items) which is depicted in Box 4.3. This can be used
in a school, sports, or work environment.
agree, circle SA. If you agree with the statement, circle A. If you disagree, circle D. If you
strongly disagree, circle SD.
Scoring: SA = 3, A = 2, D = 1, SD = 0. Items with an asterisk (*) are reverse scored, that is, SA =
0, A = 1, D = 2, SD = 3. Sum the scores for the ten items. The higher the score, the higher the selfesteem.
Another example of a commonly used scale is the PANAS (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), which
contains 20 items and measures ones emotional state. Normally, researchers first determine the
reliability of a scale using Cronbachs alpha index, which may run from 0 to 1. In social research, a
Cronbachs alpha of .70 or higher is considered acceptable. It means that the items on the scale all
measure the same underlying construct, and that therefore the scale is reliable. The social
psychologist may then use a median split to categorize the sample into those with a low level and
those with a high level of the trait under investigation in order to compare the two groups on a
dependent variable. For example, when a social psychologist wants to know whether individuals
with low self-esteem experience different emotions from individuals with high self-esteem, she could
ask individuals to fill in a questionnaire that assesses their self-esteem (Box 4.3) and their emotions
on a certain day (Box 4.4). After having established that the Cronbach alphas for the two scales are
satisfactory, she could then divide the group of participants in half: individuals with low self-esteem
and individuals with high self-esteem. With a t-test or analysis of variance she could calculate to
what extent individuals with low self-esteem experience different emotions from individuals with
high self-esteem.
Box 4.4 The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen,
1988)
This scale consists of 20 words that describe different feelings and emotions. Read each item and
then mark the appropriate number in the space next to that word. Indicate to what extent you have
felt this way today.*
* The PANAS can also be used with the following time instructions:
Indicate to what extent:
you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment;
you have felt this way during the past few days;
you have felt this way during the past week;
you have felt this way during the past few weeks;
you have felt this way during the past year;
you generally feel this way, that is, how you feel on average.
It is possible that there are no scales available that directly measure what the social psychologist is
interested in, for example, a scale on attitudes towards terrorism. In this case, psychologists must
develop their own scale based on a careful problem analysis. Constructing a reliable scale is not as
easy as it seems. However, this book is not the place to discuss the way to construct a reliable scale.
To learn more about scale construction, you may have to take an (advanced) course in methodology
and/or read a good book about scale construction (see, for example, Introduction to test
construction in the social and behavioural sciences: A practical guide, by Fishman & Galguera,
2003).
Box 4.5 The Test Phase: Developing and Testing a Process Model
Developing an empirically based process model includes the following steps:
1. Develop a process model and work your way from the outcome variable on the right to the
causal variables on the left; picture the process model in a diagram.
2. Determine whether a particular variable has a direct, indirect, reinforcing, or undermining
effect on the outcome variable.
3. Limit the number of boxes (variables) in the model to about ten.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
Assignment 4
a. Make a list of the variables you selected in Assignment 3 (Chapter 3) and develop a process
model. Remember to:
Work your way from the outcome variable on the right to the causal variables on the left.
Ask yourself whether each variable has a direct, indirect, reinforcing, or undermining effect
on the outcome variable, and determine the direction of the relationship between two
variables by using + or signs.
Ensure that you limit the number of variables to about ten and dont take more than four
steps back in the model.
Ensure that the model does not consist of two or more separate processes.
b. Read the following articles in detail and examine the empirical validity of the relationships in
the process model. If necessary, examine what causes or explanations have to be added to the
model.
Gaertner, S.L., Dovidio, J.F., Anastasio, P.A., Bachman, B.A., & Rust, M.C. (1993). The
common ingroup identity model: Recategorization and the reduction of intergroup bias.
European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 126.
Van Knippenberg, D., De Dreu, C.K.W., & Homan, A.C. (2004). Work group diversity and
5
The Help Phase: Developing the Intervention
(With Arie Dijkstra)
Contents
Introduction
Preparing Intervention Development
Modifiability
Effect Size
The Balance Table
Developing the Intervention
The Channel
The Method
The Strategy
Building the Intervention Programme
Pre-testing the Intervention
Implementation of the Intervention
The Implementation Process
Mapping the Implementation Route
The Implementation Plan
The Actual Implementation
The Evaluation
Case Study: The Millennium Campaign I Can Do That Too
Introduction
Once the factors causing the outcome variable have been identified and mapped in the process model,
the intervention can be developed. An intervention is a means to change the causal factors and thus the
outcome variables in the desired direction. An adequate intervention targets one or more causal
factors in the process model. Yet often it is not feasible or even necessary to target all variables in
this model. Therefore, the first step in the Help stage of the PATH model is to determine which causal
factors will be targeted in the intervention. The modifiability of the factors and the expected effect
sizes of interventions will direct this choice. Once these factors have been identified, an intervention
that targets these factors can be developed. Decisions must be made about how the target group will
be reached and what the content of the intervention will be. The content depends largely on empirical
evidence. The last step in the Help phase concerns the implementation process. Here care is taken
that the intervention is used as intended. We want to emphasize that the present chapter only gives an
introduction to the art of intervention development, and that more detailed approaches are available
elsewhere, for instance, in the context of health education (see for example, Bartholomew, Parcel,
Kok, Gottlieb, & Fernandez, 2011).
falling ill as a consequence of feeling discriminated against may lead to job absenteeism, a loss of
productivity, and a rise in health-care costs. Moreover, the study suggests that, in addition to antidiscrimination policies aiming to prevent discrimination, the government might also develop a
campaign to encourage black people not to accept discrimination as a fact of life but to become
assertive and to claim their right to fair treatment.
* Krieger, N. & Sidney, S. (1996). Racial discrimination and blood pressure: The CARDIA study
of young black and white adults. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 13701378.
Modifiability
Although presumably many variables in the selected process model can be influenced, there may be
considerable differences in the degree to which this is possible. Three questions can help to exclude
factors that are difficult to change:
1. Does the factor concern a stable personality trait? For example, when a social psychologist
wants to develop an intervention to tackle shyness, one may include introversion as a
personality variable with a high degree of explanatory power in the model, but this variable
has little potential for change outside intensive psychotherapy. Or when one includes
neuroticism as a variable in a model predicting burn-out, one needs to realize that this is a
stable personality trait that will be difficult to change.
2. Is the factor related to deeply held political or religious values? For example, it will be
virtually impossible to attract attention for a programme on condom use from people who,
based on their religious convictions, are strongly against premarital sex. Or it may be difficult
to convince selection officers with deeply rooted racist beliefs to endorse a policy to employ
people from minority groups.
3. Is the factor related to stable environmental conditions? For example, a problem might be that
students do not park their bikes in the appropriate parking spaces at university. Perhaps they
see many other bikes parked in inappropriate spaces and follow that example. However, this
might be due to insufficient bike storage facilities on campus, which is a stable environmental
condition.
Effect Size
Not all factors in the process model have an equally strong impact on the outcome variable, and
applied psychologists should focus on the ones that have the strongest effect. This selection is
facilitated if there is empirical evidence for the strength of the causal relationships in the model.
Often, however, this is not available and psychologists guestimate the effect sizes. Various sources
of information can be helpful to estimate the effects.
Past Experience with Similar Situations Suppose a school aims to tackle cultural segregation among
their students. In the process model, the recruited psychologist identifies knowledge about people
from other cultures and personal contact with people from other cultures as factors predicting
segregation. Yet last year the school board decided to provide youngsters with positive knowledge on
other cultures, and, although students were more positive, this seemed to have no effect on social
interactions in the school. Armed with this knowledge, it does not seem very sensible to try this
strategy again.
Empirical Evidence For a number of factors in the model, there may be empirical evidence that they
are resilient to change. For example, the literature shows that most people are unrealistically
optimistic about their lives (Weinstein & Klein, 1996). Most children, for example, believe they have
less risk than the average child of becoming overweight and developing health problems as a result of
being overweight. As this optimism is a statistical impossibility it simply cannot be that most
people are better off than the average person a psychologist may believe it could help to educate
people about this illusion. However, research suggests that such biased perceptions are hard to
correct and that their influence on behaviour is limited (Weinstein, 2003). Thus, one would not select
this factor to be targeted in an intervention. In general, it makes sense to look for evidence showing
the degree to which the model variables are changeable.
outcomes of regulating ones childs weight, the perceived response-efficacy of a childs behaviour
to lose weight are beliefs based on factual knowledge and on interpretations of past events or
experiences. In general, beliefs can be influenced quite well. Furthermore, the perceived positive
outcomes of weight regulating parental behaviours can only be brought about under conditions of
sufficient response-efficacy regarding the childs behaviour, that is, when the parental behaviours
produce the desired change in the childs behaviour. The knowledge about weight regulating parental
behaviour, and how to perform it, can also be modified as it only requires adequate basic information
processing and storage. Structured parenting refers to a parenting style in which children are actively
guided and given clear directions for choices and behaviours, for example, concerning food intake
and physical exercise. The experience with this type of parenting is also modifiable and can be
changed by practising it. Structured parenting is less likely the more parents tend to engage in a
democratic parenting style, that is, the more they have a parenting style in which children are
stimulated (or often just left) to make their own choices. A positive attitude with regard to a
democratic parental style is perhaps difficult to change as it may be based on political beliefs, on
parental modelling and on a history of perceived reinforcement of that style. The conclusions with
regard to the factors modifiability are depicted in Table 5.1.
Figure 5.1 Process model: What factors influence weight regulating parental behaviours?
Table 5.1 Balance table
Note: With regard to modifiability: ++ = high modifiability; + = medium modifiability; 0 = low modifiability, = not modifiability, +/0 =
depends on another variable.
With regard to the effect size: ++ = large effect; + = moderate effect; 0 = small effect; = no effect; +/0 = depends on another variable.
Next, the social psychologist will evaluate the effect size of the causal factors in the process
model. How strong is the effect on the outcome variable? Changes in the first three variables in the
balance table are probably possible but their effects on the outcome variable largely depend on
parents perceived control over regulating their childrens weight. Just changing these variables may
have a small effect as only some parents will have sufficient control beliefs. Changing the knowledge
on weight regulating parental behaviour can have a large effect, as it is a primary condition to engage
in such behaviour. Changing the positive attitude with regard to a democratic parental style has
uncertain effects, as it does not guarantee that an adequate alternative style will be adopted. Changing
the perceived control over the childs behaviour can have large effects as it is a basis for developing
perceived positive outcomes of regulating the weight of ones children and perceived control over
this behaviour. Finally, experience with structured parenting can have various effects but it does not
guarantee that parents are skilled in the specific behaviours regulating the weight of their children.
The psychologist will summarize her findings in the balance table (see Table 5.1).
From the balance table it appears that an intervention that targets the knowledge on weight
regulating parental behaviour and the perceived control over a childs behaviour will probably be
most successful. In addition, the psychologist may also target the perceived negative outcomes of a
child being overweight, the perceived positive outcomes of the parental behaviour, or the perceived
response-efficacy of a childs behaviours.
The Channel
The channel is the means through which people are reached and the intended changes will only take
place if people are exposed to the channel. In Table 5.2 an overview is given of various channels and
their characteristics. Channels have several features, and may vary from simple (for example, sticker
or label) to complex (for example, community intervention), each communicating a distinct type of
information (for example, text, picture) and a different volume of information (for example, one
simple message versus a complex set of arguments). Some channels communicate with high intensity
(say, group therapy) and others with low intensity (say, information signs).
Channels also differ in the potential reach of the target group (see Table 5.2). For example, a label
on a product has the potential to reach all the users of a product, while a radio message reaches only
part of the population of users. In addition, some channels will only have small effects on the
individual level (say, a sticker), while others can have large effects (the example of group therapy).
Lastly, channels may bring about different types of effects. For example, a label is appropriate to
increase peoples knowledge, while counselling is more appropriate to change complex and deeply
rooted behaviours.
The channel is chosen on the basis of information about the target group and the relevant variables,
methods and strategies. The following issues should be considered when choosing the channel:
1. Is the channel an effective way to reach the target group? (See Table 5.3, potential reach.)
When people want to know how to use a product, say a wrist-exercise tool, using the label on
the tool works better than a television ad. The label ensures that people who buy the tool have
access to the information.
2. Is exposure through this channel intensive enough to change the variable? (See Table 5.2,
effect on individual level.) A billboard depicting a young woman during a physical work-out
may remind people that physical exercise is desired, but may not lead to a change in attitudes
towards working-out. Daily feedback through the internet, however, may shape peoples
positive experiences with fitness, leading to the desired changes.
3. Is the channel appropriate for the method and strategy? A sticker is less appropriate for
modelling complex skills, such as learning to lead a healthier lifestyle, while an interactive
DVD gives several possibilities for modelling and practising healthy skills. (See Table 5.2,
effect type.)
4. What is the impact on the population level of an intervention using this channel? The impact of
an intervention is determined by the participation rate, that is, the percentage of people who
eventually participate in the intervention, and its effectiveness, that is, the proportion of people
who change after being targeted. Consider the development of an intervention to lower alcohol
abuse in students. We might choose group counselling as the channel because it has been shown
to be highly effective (Galanter, Hayden, Castaeda, & Franco, 2005). For example, a social
psychologist estimates that 20 per cent of students who are exposed to this intervention will
show a reduction in alcohol intake. However, few students will participate in such an
intervention. Less intensive interventions (for example, computer-individualized persuasive
information sent through the internet) may only lead to a 10 per cent reduction, and this may be
less effective, but many more students may participate in the programme. The impact of an
intervention can now be calculated: it is equal to the interventions effectiveness times the
interventions participation rate. For example, if the participation rate of group counselling is 1
per cent while the participation rate for the computer-individualized persuasive information is
5 per cent, the impact on the population level would be 0.002 (20 per cent 1 per cent) for the
group counselling and 0.005 (10 per cent 5 per cent) for the computer-individualized
intervention, making the impact of the latter greater than that of the first.
The Method
Intervention methods also require consideration. Methods are often derived from theoretical
frameworks. For example, the foot-in-the-door technique (Cialdini & Trost, 1998), according to
which people more easily accept a major request after first complying to a minor request, is
embedded in self-perception theory which argues that people adjust their attitudes to their
behaviours (Bem, 1972). Such theories are important because they specify the conditions under which
the method is most or least likely to be successful. For example, according to social learning theory,
modelling is most effective when the similarity between model and target individual is high (Bandura,
1986). From the various theories, phenomena and concepts in the Glossary one can often deduce
ideas about methods.
Selection of a method depends, first, on consideration of the balance table (see Table 5.1, p. 112).
For each variable, an intervention method must be chosen. Suppose attitudes of police officers
towards foreigners and communication with foreigners are the selected variables to improve the
treatment of tourists in a city in Spain. Modelling might be used to demonstrate communication skills,
whereas the method of argumentation might target attitude change. Second, selection of a method
depends on the extent to which the method fits the variable one aims to change. Whereas some
channels can motivate people to show the desired behaviour, they cannot teach them how to change it.
For example, it is easy to arouse fear in smokers through a 30-second television advertisement, but it
is difficult to help them quit smoking using this channel. In contrast, it is often sufficient to remind
people of the benefits of a certain behaviour by using a prompt. For example, a sign on an elevator
door can prompt people to use the stairs for exercise.
The following methods are frequently used in psychological interventions.
Goal Setting Setting concrete and specific goals is important. Goals direct peoples attention and
effort, provide them with expectations, and give the opportunity for feedback on goal
accomplishment, thereby regulating motivation. Goal setting changes behaviour by defining goals that
people must reach in a given period of time (Locke & Latham, 2002). For instance, as regards being
overweight, a goal can be set in terms of weight loss in kilos over a particular time period. Sub-goals
can help people work on small but important steps in reaching the superordinate goal. For example, in
patients who have had a stroke, a goal in the rehabilitation could be: After three months I can walk
200 metres by myself. Many studies support the effectiveness of goal setting. Evans and Hardy
(2002) examined the effects of a five-week goal-setting intervention for the rehabilitation of injured
athletes. The results showed that a goal-setting intervention fostered adherence and self-efficacy.
McCalley and Midden (2002) provided participants with feedback to increase household energy
conservation behaviour. They showed that participants who had set goals for themselves eventually
saved more energy.
Fear Communication Fear communication can be effective to motivate certain behaviours. For
example, health behaviours, such as using condoms, can be encouraged by graphic information about
sexually transmitted diseases (Sutton & Eiser, 1984). An interesting study by Smith and Stutts (2003)
compared the effects of a fear appeal showing the cosmetic effects of smoking (unhealthy looking
faces) with the effects on ones health (cancer). They found that both fear-appeal conditions
effectively reduced smoking. It must be noted that fear communication is only effective (and ethically
justified) when it is accompanied by explicit guidelines on how to avert the health threat (Peters,
Ruiter, & Kok, 2012).
Modelling Modelling refers to learning through the observation of others. Watching others behave
and showing the consequences can teach people to perform a new behaviour (Bandura, 1986).
Modelling is useful for all kinds of skills, for example, coping with criticism, cooking meals, and
using condoms. In a meta-analysis, Taylor, Russ-Eft, and Chan (2005) examined the effects of
different types of modelling. Do people learn more when a skill is modelled positively (showing
what one should do), negatively (showing what one should not do), or in combination? These
psychologists concluded that skill development was greatest when role models were mixed, that is,
when they showed what one should, as well as what one should not, do.
Enactive Learning The most effective way of learning a skill is to try to accomplish it yourself. This
is called enactive learning. In interventions, people can be stimulated to practise a certain skill and
evaluate it. For instance, to foster students interest in science subjects like mathematics, Luzzo and
his colleagues (1999) exposed students to either a video presentation of two university graduates
discussing how their confidence in maths had increased (so-called vicarious learning) or a maths task
providing these students with feedback on their maths skills (enactive learning). The enactive learning
condition proved more effective.
Social Comparison Social comparison information on how others are doing may affect ones
mood and well-being (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007; Festinger, 1954). For example, in the context of
coping with cancer, Bennenbroek et al. (2002) provided cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy
with social comparison information to increase the quality of their life. The intervention consisted of
a tape-recording of fellow patients telling their personal stories about either the treatment procedure,
emotions experienced during the treatment, or a tape about the way they tried to cope with the
situation. The latter tape especially reduced anxiety over the treatment and improved patients quality
of life.
Implementation Intentions Implementation intentions are intentions to perform a particular action in
a specified situation (Sheeran, Webb, & Gollwitzer, 2005). Sometimes people are asked to formulate
their implementation intentions. For a person who wants a low-fat diet, an implementation intention
could be: When I am at the supermarket I will put the low-fat butter in the shopping trolley, or
When I am at the party on Friday night and somebody offers me cake, I will decline. Asking people
about their implementation intentions may increase the occurrence of desirable behaviours. Steadman
and Quine (2004), for instance, showed that asking participants to write down two lines about
performing testicular self-examination led to the desired action. Likewise, Sheeran and Silverman
(2003) compared three interventions to promote workplace health and safety and found that asking
people to write down their implementation intentions was the most effective.
Reward and Punishment In general, people repeat behaviours that are followed by a positive
experience (reward), while avoiding a negative experience (punishment). In the smoking example, a
reward may take the form of a refund for the costs of a smoking cessation course from the health
insurance company, if people quit smoking for at least three months. In contrast, the government may
punish people for smoking by increasing the price of cigarettes. Furthermore, people can learn to
reward or punish themselves. For example, people who succeed in refraining from smoking for a
week could treat themselves to a cinema visit. In general, there is much evidence for the effects of
punishment and rewards. Punishment of undesirable behaviours (for example, high fines for drunk
driving) works best when it is accompanied by rewards for desirable behaviours (for example,
praise for staying sober before driving) (Martin & Pear, 2003).
Feedback Feedback on accomplishments is essential in behavioural change. People losing weight
want to know how much weight they have lost. Without feedback people become uncertain and their
motivation deteriorates because they do not know whether they have made progress (Kluger &
DeNisi, 1998). Brug and his colleagues (1998) provided people with tailored computer feedback on
their diet (vegetables, fruit, and fat intake) and on dietary changes. Both feedback types appeared to
improve dietary habits. Likewise, Dijkstra (2005) showed that a so-called fear appeal to smokers a
single sentence of individual feedback (It appears you are not aware of the changing societal norms
with regard to smoking) was twice as effective in reducing smoking as no feedback.
Box 5.2 Interview with Professor Gerjo Kok of the University of Maastricht (The
Netherlands)
One of the oldest and most prominent application areas of social psychological theory is health.
Professor Gerjo Kok is one of the leading scientists in this field.
Social psychological processes are very helpful in understanding real life. It helps us understand why, for example, people
find it so hard to quit smoking or why they practise unsafe sex, despite the risks involved. Insight into the causes of
unhealthy behaviours makes it possible to develop interventions that may help people adopt a healthier lifestyle. In one of
our studies we found, for instance, that self-evaluations are of central importance in smoking cessation. Self-evaluations
refer to the standards that people have set for themselves. When people meet their standards, they will experience positive
self-evaluations. They may feel proud when they succeed in not smoking for a day. In contrast, when people fail to meet
their standards, they will experience negative self-evaluations. They may regret the fact that they have started smoking
again, or feel fed up with themselves. These self-evaluations are of essential importance in smoking cessation, and
intervention programmes that help people to stop smoking should pay attention to them. An intervention programme may,
for instance, explicitly point out to people how bad they would feel when they would start smoking again.
In addition to research on the causes of unhealthy behaviour, we have developed a process protocol, called Intervention
Mapping, that provides guidelines and tools for the development of health promotion programmes. In certain ways the
protocol is like this book. It helps social psychologists, health organizations and/or the government to translate social
psychological theory and research in actual health programme materials and activities, and develop health intervention
programmes that are maximally effective. In general I see a great future for applied social psychology. I believe that
social psychology will play a growing role in the solution of societal and health problems. Especially with regard to the
study of unconscious processes (such as habits), the field of group dynamics, and the study of environmental determinants
of behaviour (such as social norms and social control), I expect social psychologists to become (even) more active.
The Strategy
Methods have to be translated into a specific strategy. The strategy is the actual intervention people
will be exposed to. For example, using television as the channel and modelling as the method, the
strategy would specify in the first place the age and gender of the role model. In the case of flu
vaccinations for elderly people, the strategy might be a television spot ad in which viewers watch an
older woman, with good health, being interviewed in her doctors waiting room, before having her
vaccination.
To come up with strategies, a global intervention plan could be made, specifying the methods,
channels, target groups, and variables to be changed, as identified on the basis of the balance table.
Here are some examples:
Modelling (method) on television (channel) to motivate women with overweight children
(target group) to monitor their childrens body weight (variable to be changed).
Giving feedback (method) through the internet (channel) about the length of time youngsters
(target group) engaged in exercise during the past week to support an increase in their
physical stamina (variable to be changed).
Offering arguments (method) to motivate quitting smoking (variable to be changed) in a self-
Offering arguments (method) to motivate quitting smoking (variable to be changed) in a selfhelp book (channel) for smokers of all ages (target group).
Repetition (method) of the word action in the text of a model (method) presented in a leaflet
(channel), designed to motivate obese people (target group) to formulate implementation
intentions with regard to reserving a seat with extra space (variable to be changed) on
international flights.
Next, based on these global intervention descriptions, the social psychologist could select a strategy
for intervention. This usually takes place in two phases, a divergent and a convergent phase. In the
divergent phase, the psychologist lists as many strategies as possible. In the convergent phase, he or
she critically evaluates these strategies.
The Divergent Phase: Inventing Strategies There are various techniques to generate interventions:
Direct intervention association: Ideas for strategies can be based on all kinds of sources, such
as what one has seen on television, what makes intuitive sense, what has proven to be effective
in the literature, or what people have experienced themselves.
Direct method approach: This approach consists of looking at strategies that have been used in
similar situations. For example, suppose that the global intervention description is: Provide
information on the appropriate use of a new type of toothbrush on a label to prevent mouth
injury in patients with bad teeth. A psychologist might then inspect existing labels on
toothbrushes. Also labels with regard to other devices that could injure people could be used
to generate ideas.
Debilitating strategies: This approach is to come up with strategies that have undesired
effects on the problem. In the case of the global intervention description Modelling on
television to motivate women to monitor their childrens weight, the model should probably
not be a retired millionaire on his or her ranch. By generating ideas of what would probably
have no effect or reverse effects, we can learn about what would have an effect, about the
relevant dimensions of an effective intervention, and about ways to operationalize the
strategies.
Interviews: Interviewing people from the target group could generate additional ideas for
strategies. With the global intervention description Modelling on television to motivate
women to monitor their childrens body weight, a woman with young children might be
interviewed about her preference for role models that might inspire her. Here is an example of
such an interview:
Social psychologist:
Interviewee:
Social psychologist:
Do you have any other ideas about the person and what the person
Social psychologist:
Interviewee:
When I got, one way or another, the impression that they are
indirectly trying to sell a commercial product I would immediately
stop watching.
This kind of interview asking for the desired but also the undesired characteristics can
generate new perspectives and ideas about strategies. From the above, we learn that people
might feel they are being manipulated, which should of course be avoided.
Insights from theory: This approach consists of looking at relevant social psychological
theories. With regard to the method of goal setting, for instance, the difficulty of the goal is
crucial in determining the success of an intervention (Locke & Latham, 2002). In general, goals
stimulate performance when they are difficult and offer a challenge, but at the same time are
within someones reach. Losing two pounds in two months might not motivate a person much
because the outcome is not very attractive, but losing 20 pounds in two months might be
unrealistic. Thus, in developing strategies, the psychologist should look carefully at what the
theory predicts.
Insights from research: This approach consists of looking at relevant social psychological
research. For example, research shows that people are less defensive with regard to
processing threatening information (for example, on cancer risks) when their self-esteem is
boosted (Sherman, Nelson, & Steele, 2000). Therefore, in developing a fear-appeal we might
want to include a self-esteem boosting method, for example, asking people to write an essay
on the good things they have done recently (Reed & Aspinwall, 1998). Such manipulations
are described in the method sections of empirical articles and can provide the social
psychologist with creative ideas for strategies.
comparison to the average population. This in turn means that the external validity of the study
the translatability of ones findings on to the population as a whole could suffer.
One way to gain the benefits of online studies, while avoiding the pitfalls of convenience
sampling, is through crowd-sourcing sites such as Amazons Mechanical Turk. These are webbased services typically used by companies looking to distribute routine tasks to an enormous and
relatively cheap workforce (Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis, 2010). After designing a study,
researchers can make use of such services to recruit and pay participants, quickly gaining access
to an even broader section of the population than can be found in a typical university setting
(Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). As these platforms become increasingly widespread,
there can be little doubt that the pace of psychological research will increase experiments will
become cheaper, faster, and more externally valid. Of course, there will never be a true
replacement for a properly conducted laboratory experiment, since only in a controlled setting can
we rule out a wide range of external factors that could be affecting participants as they conduct an
online study.
The Convergent Phase: Choosing the Strategy The divergent phase often results in a laundry list of
strategies. Therefore, a limited number of strategies need to be selected. The choice for a particular
strategy or set of strategies must have both a theoretical and empirical basis. First, the strategy should
take into account the conditions underlying the theory. For example, in the case of modelling, the
theory specifies that the actual model must be similar or at least relevant to people in the target group
(Bandura, 1986). Second, it is preferable that the choice of strategy is based on empirical evidence
from either laboratory experiments or field studies. Ideally, evidence ought to be available for the
combination of the channel, the method, the strategy, the variable to be changed, and the target group.
For example, for the global intervention description Modelling on television to motivate women to
monitor their childrens body weight, the strongest evidence would come from a field experiment in
which such an intervention was tested in a specified target group against a control condition.
Somewhat weaker evidence would come from testing the intervention video in the laboratory. The
stronger the empirical evidence for the intervention, the higher the chances that the intervention will
indeed be effective.
Sometimes evidence for the effectiveness of a certain strategy is simply not there. In that case,
especially when the costs of an intervention programme are high, we recommend that the
effectiveness of a new strategy should first be tested through research.
introduction, link the arguments, and choose the font size and type. In the case of a video, with
the method of modelling, write the script and include what should be said and done and what
should happen in the video.
In the case of an intervention with several channels (for example, billboards and television
spots) or sequential elements (for example, group counselling sessions), all parts must be finetuned and a protocol must be written, as well as planning the intervention.
If professional artists are involved, it should be clear how much influence they can have over
the end-product. The communication with professional artists should be highly interactive and
several versions may have to be designed by the artist in order to come up with a product.
The intervention often includes more than one strategy. In principle, all aspects of the
intervention that can be read, seen, or heard should be part of a strategy. Thus, the colours, the
sizes, the sounds, the timing, the wording, the movement, the background, the aspects of the
background, and the specific shapes should all refer to an identifiable strategy. One way to test
this is to point to a single aspect of the intervention and ask: What strategy is this part of and
what is the method of operationalization?
Recall: The psychologist may also use a recall task, which assesses which aspects of the
intervention people from the target group remember after having being exposed to the
intervention. This might give an insight into which strategies have the highest salience. Imagine
a billboard depicting a celebrity promoting safe sex, but when individuals from the target group
are asked to recall the characteristics of the billboard, half of them only remember the name of
the celebrity and not what he or she was promoting. In this case, the salience of the messenger
has apparently distorted the message of the intervention.
Observation: People from the target group may also be observed while being exposed to the
intervention. For example, in the case of testing a billboard, eye movements may be monitored
to track the aspects of the billboard to which they pay most attention. Likewise, when testing an
internet website, the link-choices and the time spent on each page might be monitored.
Expert opinions: For pre-testing the intervention, one may also ask the experts involved in
bringing about the effects of the intervention. For example, in the case of a leaflet to increase
treatment adherence, a doctor might be asked to give her opinion. Or a shop-keeper who is
supposed to hand out a leaflet to everyone who buys product X may be asked whether he or she
thinks people will indeed look at the leaflet.
In general, participants seldom agree completely about an intervention in a pre-test. Therefore, an
applied social psychologist must not only look at the pre-test data, but must also consider theoretical
aspects as well as empirical evidence that may be relevant. After revisions have been made, the
improved intervention can be pre-tested a second time. The final version of the intervention can now
be developed and distributed.
Figure 5.3 Proportion of people who report needing the intervention programme, who are aware of
its existence, who have started using it, and who have completed the programme
Thus, although psychologists may develop an excellent intervention programme, if only a few
people are actually exposed to the intervention because professionals who are essential for its
implementation, such as teachers or doctors, are not aware of it or do not use it properly, the impact
of the intervention on the problem may be small or non-existent.
women who may be victims of domestic violence) and to hand out the leaflets to these women,
supporting the practitioners in the actual execution of the behaviours, and providing feedback and
reinforcement to maintain the behaviours (for example, by calling practitioners on the phone and
giving them support and advice).
insurers that the detection of battered women should be reimbursed. Thus the implementation
route contains for all actors a diagnosis of the potential problems to engaging in the
implementations task and the specific barriers to performing it.
3. Identifying relevant policies: Besides identifying the actors involved, the relevant policies
also need to be known. For example, it is possible that there is a policy for general
practitioners that states that the general practitioner will not engage in detection tasks with
regard to family matters. Although some general practitioners might still be motivated, this
would not be an ideal situation for the implementation of the intervention. Or it may be that
there is a law which says that the police can only offer protection to a battered woman when
there is objective evidence of domestic violence. This might inhibit women from seeking
professional help. This law would counter the desired effects of the intervention structurally.
The Evaluation
To assess whether the problem that was targeted has indeed changed for the good, the last step in this
interventiondevelopment cycle is to evaluate the effects of the intervention. At least three types of
evaluation are important: the effect evaluation, the process evaluation, and the cost-effectiveness
evaluation.
In the effect evaluation, the extent to which variables that are directly related to the problem have
changed over time is assessed. At the very least, the effect of the intervention on the specified
outcome variable in the process model should be assessed. There are, however, more variables that
may be evaluated to determine the effectiveness of the intervention. Imagine the case of the problem
of obesity in which the level of exercise is the outcome variable that the social psychologist aims to
influence. The primary outcome variable in the process model is thus the level of exercise. However,
the number of people who engage in sufficient exercise can also be a meaningful variable, as would
be the percentage of obese people 6 or 12 months after exposure to the intervention.
To assess to what extent the effects are temporary or permanent, an appropriate follow-up period
must be specified. Long-term behavioural effects can be assessed 12 months after the exposure,
although the 12-month period is based on consensus rather than on rationale. The best follow-up
periods are based on specific arguments for the behaviour that is targeted. For example, because in
smoking cessation most smokers who relapse do so within the first six months after the initiation, a
six-month follow-up should be sufficient; after this period very few ex-smokers relapse.
In the process evaluation, the processes through which the intervention has its effects are
examined. This type of evaluation refers to an assessment of the extent to which effective elements of
the intervention have been executed. For example, for individual counselling it may be essential that
the counsellor and the client develop a therapeutic relationship because the therapeutic relationship
serves as one of the methods of intervention. In a secondary outcome assessment, the extent to which
the therapeutic relationship has been formed is assessed.
In a cost-effectiveness evaluation the costs of interventions are assessed and compared with the
benefits. For example, obesity has huge societal costs specifically in terms of health-care provision.
If an intervention leads to a yearly decline of 500 people suffering from obesity, the health-care
savings can be calculated. A second aspect of the cost-effectiveness concerns the costs of
intervention. The intervention development and implementation are costly as they involve
professional labour and material costs. For a television advertisement to be broadcast, the costs for
broadcasting must be paid. In the cost-effectiveness evaluation the savings caused by the intervention
are compared to the costs of the intervention.
It is important that there are usually data sources available for the effect evaluation as well as the
cost-effectiveness evaluation. Many commercial research agencies gather information on societal
phenomena, such as the percentage of obese people and the number of unemployed. Thus, it may not
always be necessary to gather additional data. On the other hand, it is important that outcome
variables are carefully assessed. Therefore, a self-developed outcome assessment may be necessary.
Especially with regard to the process evaluation, reliable measures must often be developed. (For
further reading on the evaluation of intervention we would refer to other sources, such as Action
evaluation of health programmes and changes, by John Ovretveit, 2001.) In the last paragraphs of
this chapter we present a case study of a large-scale intervention that was successfully implemented
to help smokers quit in the Netherlands.
In the Netherlands, the Dutch Expertise Centre of Tobacco Control developed the Millennium
campaign I can do that too to reduce the percentage of smokers. The campaign consisted of a series
of interventions through several channels to stimulate smokers to quit and support their attempt. The
campaign started in October 1999 and ended in February 2000.
The intervention programme is depicted in Figure 5.5. In addition, the population was exposed to
free publicity about the campaign. In the written media, no less than 519 articles were published on
the Millennium campaign and 79 radio and TV items gave information on it.
Figure 5.5 Number of smokers (x 1000) as a function of type of channel that were reached by the
Millennium campaign I can do that too
The effectiveness of the campaign was assessed using a so-called panel design with measurement
control groups (see Box 5.3). That is, before the campaign started, in October 1999 (Time 1), an
initial measurement among smokers was conducted. This group constituted the panel group. It is
common that such measurements may influence measurements done later with the same group. If one
finds a change in the panel group, this could be an artefact of the first measurement (for instance,
because it made them aware of the risks of their smoking habits). Therefore, when the second
measurement was applied to the panel group (thus at Time 2, in February 2000) there was also a
control group of smokers with no Time 1 measurement. The same was done for the Time 3
measurement (January 2001).
Box 5.4 Evaluation Model for the Millennium Campaign I Can Do That Too
In evaluating the effectiveness of the Millennium campaign, three outcome measures were used:
1. seven-days abstinence (not smoking for at least the past seven days);
2. having engaged in an attempt to quit;
3. having positive intentions to stop smoking.
It was found that, at Time 2, those smokers who had watched the TV programme or the TV talk
show at Time 1 had made significantly more attempts to quit. The long-term follow-up (Time 3)
showed that smokers who knew the Millennium campaign also had more positive intentions to
stop smoking. The researchers concluded that the Millennium campaign led more smokers to quit
smoking and, for those who had not yet made an actual attempt, it had made smoking cessation a
higher priority.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
Assignment 5
A company that produces computer software asks you, as a social psychologist, for advice. The
company consists of ten departments, each of 50 employees, with every department managed by an
executive. These ten executives are in turn subordinate to a team of five directors that leads the
company. Although 50 per cent of the employees are female, of the directors and executives only one
person the executive that runs the household department is female. The team of directors asks you
how they can improve the upward mobility of women in the companys hierarchy so the company will
have more female leaders in the near future.
a. Read the following three articles:
Eagly, A.H. & Karau, S.J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders.
Eagly, A.H. & Karau, S.J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders.
Psychological Review, 109, 573598.
Ryan, M.K. & Haslam, S.A. (2005). The glass cliff: Evidence that women are overrepresented
in precarious leadership positions. British Journal of Management, 16, 8190.
Van Vugt, M. & Spisak, B.R. (2008). Sex differences in leadership emergence during
competitions within and between groups. Psychological Science, 19, 854858.
Select from these articles causal factors and develop a process model. Make sure that you limit
the number of variables to about ten and dont take more than four steps back in the model.
b. Estimate for each causal factor in the process model its modifiability and effect size. Make a
balance table and select the causal factors at which the intervention should be targeted.
c. For each of the selected factors, come up with possible strategies to influence this factor. Use
direct intervention association and the direct method approach, explore debilitating strategies,
conduct interviews, and look at relevant theories and research.
d. Reduce the potential list of strategies by examining their theoretical and empirical basis.
e. Outline a global intervention plan, a number of ways to present the intervention, and make a
plan for the implementation of the intervention.
f. Develop an evaluation procedure to determine the effectiveness of the intervention programme.
This implies, among other things, that a psychologist should not constantly bother a client with all
kinds of doubts and concerns about the chosen approach and methodology. There is no need to let
clients share in all the deliberations in choosing a given theory or intervention. When you cannot
convince yourself, it is unlikely that you can convince your client. Of course, a psychologist may
explicitly want to present various alternative interventions and show the client the advantages and
disadvantages of each approach. But the goal then is to facilitate choice by the client, not to
recapitulate all the considerations that led to the selection of possible interventions.
To conclude, this book presents an elaborate procedure for developing theory-based interventions,
the PATH methodology, based on current social psychological science and theory. This PATH
method is a tool rather than a ritual that one should go through at all costs. It is our belief that social
psychological concepts, theories, and methods may help in understanding and trying to solve social
issues that societies all over the world are facing. The goal of this book is to facilitate this and
provide practitioners and policy makers with the knowledge, skills, and tools to help them
accomplish the important task of making this world a better place for everyone.
Appendix
Figure 1 Example of process model outlining social psychological variables that could increase or
decrease the motivation to engage in football hooliganism. In this example it is not clear what
exactly the difference is between group pressure and normative pressure, and these could be folded
into one variable. Also some variables are not continuous (e.g., deindividuation) and are therefore
harder to influence
Figure 2 Example of a process model outlining social psychological variables (in the solid boxes)
that could increase or decrease the likelihood of people littering garbage. In the dashed boxes are
factors that could moderate the impact of a particular variable (e.g., awareness may influence the
impact of anti-litter signs). The model is simple but adequate and can be used to target a potential
intervention such as putting up clearly visible anti-litter signs
Figure 3 Example of process model accounting for excessive alcohol drinking. For some variables
it is not entirely clear whether they have a positive or negative influence on excessive drinking (e.g.,
mood) which could be further investigated. Also, some variables moderate the relationships
between others (e.g., availability of alcohol moderates the impact of positive attitudes on excessive
drinking) which may require more empirical study
Figure 4 Example of process model explaining the motivation to participate in a social movement.
Although many of the important social psychological variables have been identified and the model is
useful to target potential interventions (e.g., providing information about how many others are
expected to join), it is a bit complex and some variables are not as concrete as others (e.g., problem
cannot be escaped)
Glossary
Social Psychological Theories, Phenomena and Concepts for Explaining
Social Psychological Problems
Theories
affect-infusion model: a model that holds that mood affects the individuals judgement depending on
the type of reasoning being used.
arousal: cost-reward model : a model that holds that another persons distress causes physiological
arousal in an observer which, in turn, initiates the process of deciding whether to help.
attachment theory: a theory about the emotional bond between infants and their caregivers that
assumes that individuals may develop on the basis of the bonds with their caregivers either a secure
and avoidant or an anxious-ambivalent attachment style. This attachment style affects ones capacity
for relationships in ones adult life, as well as other aspects in ones life, such as ones commitment
to work.
attribution theory: a theory that explains the causes of events that happen to oneself or others. The
attribution of these causes is known to influence performance affecting reactions, etc.
balance theory: a theory that holds that people have an innate preference for a harmonious and
consistent relationship among their cognitions.
cognitive dissonance theory: according to this theory people will experience unpleasant
psychological tension if they perceive that their cognitions are psychologically inconsistent with their
behaviour (dissonance). Dissonance will motivate individuals to find ways to reduce it.
complementarity hypothesis: an hypothesis that states that persons with dissimilar but compatible
traits will be attracted to each other.
contact hypothesis: according to this hypothesis, bringing members of different groups into contact
with one another will reduce any pre-existing prejudice between them and result in more positive
intergroup attitudes and stereotypes.
cultural theories: theories that state that, as individuals engage in particular cultural contexts, their
psychologies are shaped accordingly.
elaboration-likelihood model (ELM): a model of persuasion that states that people may follow two
routes of information processing: the central route (i.e., elaborate issue-relevant arguments) and the
peripheral route (i.e., paying attention to peripheral cues such as the attractiveness of the source).
empathy-altruism hypothesis: hypothesis that states that empathy is associated with the selfless
desire to benefit another and that empathically motivated altruistic behaviour is not due to the desire
for external rewards, the goal of avoiding guilt, or other selfish desires.
equity theory: a theory that states that people are happiest in relationships where the give-and-take
are about equal.
evolutionary leadership theory: a theory which argues that leadership is a biological adaptation that
evolved to facilitate group coordination and cooperation.
evolutionary psychology: a theory that aims to explain the origins and maintenance of social
behaviour from how that behaviour may have contributed to survival and reproductive success in our
evolutionary past.
frustrationaggression model: according to this model, the interruption of goal-directed behaviour,
arising from either the arbitrary interference of other persons or personal inability, evokes frustration
which in turn evokes a negative effect and aggressive behavioural tendencies.
general adaptation syndrome model (GASM): a model that presents a three-stage reaction to stress
(alarm reaction, resistance, exhaustion).
heuristic-systematic model (HSM): a model of attitude change that specifies two routes to
persuasion: systematic processing (i.e., an analytic orientation to information processing) and
heuristic processing (i.e., a more restricted mode of information processing that makes fewer
demands on cognitive resources).
interdependence theory: a theory that focuses on how individuals weigh the costs and benefits of a
particular relationship and how their behaviours are affected by these evaluations. It holds that
individuals will be most committed to a relationship when their satisfaction is high, the alternatives
for their current relationship are unattractive, and the emotional and practical barriers against leaving
the relationship are high.
male warrior hypothesis: the tendency of men to form coalitions to initiate, plan, and execute acts of
intergroup aggression.
mismatch theory: the idea that behaviour that was adaptive in ancestral environments may no longer
be adaptive (e.g., the innate preference for sweet and salty foods).
negative state relief model: according to this model, a negative mood is accompanied by a
corresponding drive to reduce whatever bad feelings are present. For instance, according to this
model, a bad mood increases helpfulness because helping another person reduces ones own bad
feelings.
norm theory: a theory that postulates that every experience brings its own frame of reference or norm
into being, either by guiding memory retrieval or by constraining mental simulation.
prospect theory: a theory that describes decisions under uncertainty in which the value of an outcome
and its alternatives is calculated as the summed products (n) over specified outcomes (x).
prototype theory: a theory that a categorys mental representation is based on a prototypical
exemplar or prototype.
rational choice theory: according to this theory, in choosing lines of behaviour, individuals make
rational calculations with respect to the utility of alternative lines of conduct, the costs of each
alternative in terms of utilities foregone, and the best way to maximize utility.
Phenomena
actor-observer effect: the phenomenon that actors tend to attribute their actions to situational factors
whereas observers tend to attribute the same actions to stable personal dispositions.
buffering effect: the phenomenon that having a particular resource or positive quality protects a
person against the adverse impact of a stressful event.
bystander effect: the phenomenon where persons are less likely to intervene in an emergency
situation when others are present than when they are alone.
cognitive consistency: the tendency for people to prefer congruence or consistency among their
various cognitions, especially their beliefs, values, and attitudes.
competitive altruism: the tendency of people to help when they think their actions are being observed
by others (so as to gain a good reputation).
deindividuation: a state of reduced self-awareness associated with immersion and anonymity within
a group.
diffusion of responsibility: the tendency for people to feel that responsibility for acting is shared, or
diffused, among those present. That is, the greater the number of people who are present, the lower
the individuals sense of responsibility.
distributive justice: the phenomenon that people evaluate the outcomes they receive from others not
by their absolute favourability but by their consistency with principles of outcome fairness.
emotional contagion: the tendency to express and feel emotions that are similar to and influenced by
those of others.
excitation transfer: the phenomenon that arousal elicited by one stimulus may mistakenly be
attributed to another.
false consensus effect: the phenomenon that people tend to perceive their own preferences, attributes,
or behaviour as more common and situationally appropriate than those of individuals who have
Concepts
affect: a general term describing mental processes that involve feeling, such as an emotional
experience or mood.
altruism: a type of helping behaviour which is costly for the individual helper.
arousal: an organisms level of physiological activation or excitation.
associative network: a model of human memory as the connections among isolated items of stored
knowledge.
attention: whatever occupies consciousness or a persons mental focus at a particular time.
attitude: a psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity with some
degree of favour or disfavour.
authoritarianism: an orientation which is overly deferential to those in authority while
simultaneously adopting an overbearing and hostile attitude towards those perceived as inferior.
automaticity: information processing that occurs without conscious control.
bogus pipeline: a procedure intended to reduce distortions in self-report measures by convincing
participants that the researcher has a valid and reliable means of knowing what their true responses
are.
categorization: the process of classifying things or people as members of a group or category,
similar to other members of that group or category and different from members of other groups or
categories.
cognition: mental functions, such as the ability to think, reason and remember.
cognitive appraisal: a mental process by which people assess whether a demand threatens their
well-being and whether they have the resources to meet this demand.
collectivism (vs individualism): circumstances in which the meaning of a person and its realization
are expressed predominantly in relationships.
commitment: the binding of an individual to a specific line of activity or relationship.
comparison level (CL): an internal standard representing the quality of outcomes an individual
expects to obtain in a relationship.
comparison level of alternatives (CLalt): an internal standard representing the quality of outcomes
an individual perceives to be available outside the current relationship.
construct validity: the extent to which the measured variables accurately capture the constructs of
theoretical interest.
counterfactual thinking: the thoughts people have about the alternative ways in which an event could
have occurred.
descriptive norms: perceptions of how other people are actually behaving, whether or not this is
approved of.
diversity: individual and group differences in terms of demographics such as race, religion, ethnicity,
gender, sexual orientation, age, or educational background.
door-in-the-face: technique that obtains compliance to the target request by first obtaining noncompliance to a larger request.
drive: a persons internal state that energizes and maintains behaviour.
effect size: a statistical measurement of the magnitude of the relationship between two variables.
ego-involvement: the extent to which a task or issue is personally significant or motivating to an
individual, and hence carries implications for that individuals self-concept or self-esteem.
empathy: seeing oneself in someone elses place, and imagining how one would feel.
experiment: a type of research in which a researcher randomly assigns people to two or more
conditions, varies the treatments that people in each condition are given, and then measures the effect
on some response.
external validity: the extent to which one can generalize from one particular setting to another.
foot-in-the-door: a technique that predisposes people to comply to a critical request by first
obtaining compliance to a minor request.
gender difference: differences between females and males (also called sex-difference).
gender identity: individuals subjective feeling of themselves as males or females.
gender roles: the socially assigned roles traditionally associated with each sex.
gender stereotypes: beliefs about the behaviours and characteristics of each sex.
helping behaviour: voluntary acts performed with the intent of providing benefit to another person.
heuristic: a cognitive structure or process that serves the creative function of knowledge enrichment
and productive thinking.
implicit personality theories: tacit assumptions regarding peoples personality traits and the
relationships among them.
impression formation: the process of forming evaluative and descriptive judgements about a target
person.
impression management: the goal-directed activity of controlling or regulating information in order
to influence the impression formed by an audience.
individualism (vs collectivism): circumstances in which the worth of a person is predominantly
defined as independent of the membership of groups.
injunctive norms: norms that state the ideal behaviour and that reflect basic values what one ought
to do.
internal validity: the extent to which the research permits causal inferences about the effects of one
variable upon another.
intrinsic motivation: a form of motivation produced by the experience of free choice and autonomy.
locus of control: an individuals generalized expectancies regarding the forces, internal or external,
that determine rewards and punishments.
loneliness: the unpleasant experience that occurs when a persons network of social relationships is
deficient in some important way, either quantitatively or qualitatively.
memory: mechanisms by which people store and retrieve the knowledge they have encoded.
meta-analysis: the statistical integration of the results of independent studies in a specific area of
research.
minimal group paradigm: research in which anonymous participants are experimentally classified as
members of ad hoc, arbitrary, or minimally meaningful categories (e.g. X vs Y), and respond to nonidentifiable members of their own and other categories.
minority social influence: a form of social influence in which a deviant subgroup rejects the
established norm of the majority of group members and induces the majority to move to the position
of the minority.
modelling: a process in which human thought, affect, and action are altered by observing the
behaviour of others and the outcomes they experience.
mood: generalized positive or negative feeling states.
nonverbal communication: the transmission of information and influence by an individuals physical
and behavioural cues.
path analysis: the analysis of data to estimate the coefficients of a class of hypothesized causal
models.
personality: intrinsic human qualities that lead to differences among individuals in their
characteristic patterns of behaviour.
personal space: the distances and angles of orientation that people maintain with one another as they
interact.
person perception: the detection of peoples internal psychological qualities, such as abilities,
emotions, beliefs, and goals.
power: the ability to control the outcomes of other individuals. The main sources of power are
reward, referent, coercive, legitimate, and expert power.
prejudice: holding derogatory attitudes or beliefs, the expression of negative affect, or the display of
hostile or discriminatory behaviour towards members of a group on account of their membership
within that group.
priming: the activation of particular connections or associations in memory just before carrying out
an action or task.
prisoners dilemma: a mixed-motive reward structure in which each of two or more parties must
choose between cooperation and non-cooperation.
prosocial behaviour: a broad category of interpersonal actions that benefit others, which are
positively evaluated with reference to cultural or societal standards.
prototype: the most typical member of a category.
reactance: a motivational state in response to influence-attempts from others, directed towards the
re-establishment of ones behavioural freedoms.
reciprocity: responding to the positive or negative actions of others in a similar way.
reference group: any group that individuals use as a basis for social comparison.
reinforcer: any stimulus that, when contingent on a response, serves to increase the rate of
responding.
social support: the existence of positive social relationships that may help maintain or advance ones
health and well-being.
social value orientation: individual differences in the utility derived from outcomes for others. Three
types are distinguished: a prosocial orientation, an individualistic orientation, and a competitive
orientation.
status: the rank position of an individual in a social hierarchy. In many societies, status is acquired at
birth and varies depending on factors such as age, gender, and socioeconomic background.
stereotypes: societally shared beliefs about the characteristics that are perceived to be true of social
groups and their members.
stereotyping: the use of stereotypes when judging others.
stress: peoples physiological or psychological response to demands from the environment that either
approach or exceed their capacities to respond.
subjective expected utility (SEU): the probability of an outcome times the utility of that outcome.
survey: systematic data collection about a sample drawn from a larger population.
trait: a stable, internal property that distinguishes among individuals.
transactive memory: a system shared among group members for encoding, storing, and retrieving
information such that detailed memories are available to group members without actual physical
possession.
type A behaviour: behavioural attributes that increase the risk of coronary heart disease, such as
striving for achievement, competitiveness, impatience, and hostility.
unconscious processes: mental processes that occur without awareness.
unobtrusive measure: a non-reactive form of data collection (i.e., it does not require the cooperation
of participants).
values: trans-situational goals that serve as guiding principles in the life of a person or group.
Bibliography
Abrams, D. & Hogg, M.A. (2004). Meta-theory: Lessons from social identity research.Personality
and Social Psychology Review, 8, 98106.
Adih, W.K. & Alexander, C.S. (1999). Determinants of condom use to prevent HIV infection among
youth in Ghana. Journal of Adolescent Health, 24, 6372.
Aiken, L.H., Clarke, S.P., Sloane, D.M., Sochalski, J., & Silber, J.H. (2002). Hospital nurse staffing
and patient mortality, nurse burnout, and job dissatisfaction. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 288, 19871993.
Ajzen, I. (1987). Attitudes, traits and actions: Dispositional predictions of behavior in personality
and social psychology. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
20, pp. 163). New York: Academic Press.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 50, 179211.
Ajzen, I. (2012). The theory of planned behavior. In P.A.M. Van Lange, A.W. Kruglanski, & E.T.
Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 438459).Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage Publications.
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (2000). The prediction of behaviour from attitudinal and normative
variables. In T.E. Higgins & A.W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Motivational science: Social and
personality perspectives (pp. 177190). New York: Psychology Press.
Albarracn, D., Johnson, B.T., & Fishbein, M. (2001). Theories of reasoned action and planned
behaviour as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 142161.
Aral, S. & Walker, D. (2012). Identifying influential and susceptible members of social networks.
Science, 337, 337341.
Aronson, E., Wilson, T.D., & Akert, R.M. (2010). Social psychology (7th edn). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Aydin, N., Graupmann, V., Fischer, J., Frey, D., & Fischer, P. (2011). My role is my castle: The
appeal of family roles after experiencing social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 47, 981986.
Bakker, A.B. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2000). Burnout contagion processes among teachers. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 30, 22892308.
Bales, R.F. & Cohen, S.P. (1979). SYMLOG: A system for the multiple level observation of small
groups. New York: Free Press.
Balliet, D., Li, N., Macfarlan, S., & Van Vugt, M. (2011). Sex differences in cooperation: A metaanalytic review of social dilemmas.Psychological Bulletin, 137, 881909.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Baron, R.A. & Branscombe, N.R. (2012). Social psychology (13th edn). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Bartholomew, L.K., Parcel, G.S., Kok, G., Gottlieb, N.H., & Fernandez, M.E. (2011).Planning
health promotion programs: An intervention mapping approach (3rd edn). San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Batson, D.C. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Batson, D.C. & Powell, A.A. (2003). Altruism and prosocial behaviour. In T. Millon & M.J. Lerner
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 463484).
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Baumeister, R.F. (1999). The self in social psychology. New York: Psychology Press.
Baumeister, R.F. & Bushman, B.J. (2010). Social psychology and human nature (2nd edn). Belmont,
CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.
Baumeister, R.F. & Tice, D.T. (1990). Anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 9, 165195.
Baumeister, R.F. & Vohs, K.D. (2007). Encyclopedia of social psychology. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Bazerman, M.H., Curhan, J.R., Moore, D.A., & Valley, K. (2000). Negotiation.Annual Review of
Psychology, 51, 279314.
Becker, R. & Mehlkop, G. (2006). Social class and delinquency: An empirical utilization of rational
choice theory with cross-sectional data of 1990 and 2000 German General Population Surveys
(Allbus). Rationality and Society, 18, 193235.
Bell, P.A. & Baron, R.A. (1974). Environmental influences on attraction: Effects of heat, attitude
similarity and personal evaluation. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 479481.
Bem, D.J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 162). New York: Academic Press.
Bennenbroek, F.T.C., Buunk, B.P., Van der Zee, K.I., & Grol, B. (2002). Social comparison and
patient information: What do cancer patients want? Patient Education and Counselling, 47, 512.
Bierhoff, H.W. (2002). Prosocial behaviour. London: Psychology Press.
Bierhoff, H.W. & Rohmann, E. (2004). Altruistic personality in the context of the empathy-altruism
hypothesis. European Journal of Personality, 18, 351365.
Boyar, S.L., Maertz, C.P., & Pearson, A.W. (2005). The effects of workfamily conflict and family
work conflict on non-attendance behaviors. Journal of Business Research, 58, 919925.
Brakel, T.M., Dijkstra, A., & Buunk, A.P. (2012). Effects of the source of social comparison
information on former cancer patients quality of life. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17,
667681.
Breakwell, G.M. (2004). Doing social psychology research. Leicester: British Psychological
Society.
Brennan, A. & Kline, T. (2000). A justice explanation of employee reactions to office relocation: A
case study. Psychologist-Manager Journal, 4, 105115.
Brodbeck, F.C. & Greitemeyer, T. (2000). Effects of individual versus mixed individual and group
experience in rule induction on group member learning and group performance. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 621648.
Brug, J., Glanz, K., Van Assema, P., Kok, G., & Van Breukelen, G. (1998). The impact of computertailored feedback on fat, fruit and vegetable intake. Health Education & Behavior, 25, 357371.
Brug, J., Oenema, A., & Campbell, M. (2003). Past, present and future of computer-tailored nutrition
education. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 77, 10281034.
Brug, J., Van Vugt, M., Van den Borne, B., Brouwers, A., & Van Hooff, H. (2000). Predictors of
willingness to register as an organ donor among Dutch adolescents. Psychology & Health, 15,
357368.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, S., & Gosling, S.D. (2011). Amazons Mechanical Turk: A new source of
inexpensive, yet high quality data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 35.
Buunk, A.P. (1990). Affiliation and helping interactions within organizations: A critical analysis of
the role of social support with regard to occupational stress. European Review of Social
Psychology,1, 293322.
Buunk, A.P., Bakker, A.B., Siero, F.W., van den Eijnden, R.J.J.M., & Yzer, M.C. (1998). Predictors
of AIDS-preventive behavioral intentions among adult heterosexuals at risk for HIV-infection:
Extending current models and measures. AIDS Education and Prevention, 10, 149172.
Buunk, A.P., Bennenbroek, F.T.C., Stiegelis, H.E., Van den Bergh, A.C.M., Sanderman, R., &
Hagedoorn, M. (2011). Follow-up effects of social comparison information on the quality of life of
cancer patients: The moderating role of social comparison orientation. Psychology & Health, 1
14.
Buunk, A.P. & Dijkstra, P. (2012). The social animal within organizations. In S.C. Roberts (Ed.),
Applied evolutionary psychology (pp. 3651). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Buunk, A.P. & Gibbons, F.X. (2007). Social comparison: The end of a theory and the emergence of a
field. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 102, 321.
Buunk, A.P., Gibbons, F.X., & Visser, A. (2002). The relevance of social comparison processes for
prevention and health care. Patient Education and Counseling, 47, 13.
Buunk, A.P., Massar, K., & Dijkstra, P. (2007). A social cognitive evolutionary approach to
jealousy: The automatic evaluation of ones romantic rivals. In J. Forgas, M. Haselton, & W. Von
Hippel (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition
(pp. 213228). New York: Psychology Press.
Buunk, A.P. & Schaufeli, W.B. (1999). Reciprocity in interpersonal relationships: An evolutionary
perspective on its importance for health and well-being. European Review of Social Psychology,
10, 259291.
Buunk, A.P. & Verhoeven, K. (1991). Companionship and support at work: A microanalysis of the
stress-reducing features of social interaction. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 243258.
Buunk, A.P., Zurriaga, R., Gonzalez-Roma, V., & Subirats, M. (2003). Engaging in upward and
downward comparisons as a determinant of relative deprivation at work: A longitudinal study.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 370388.
Campbell, D., Carr, S.C., & MacLachlan, M. (2001). Attributing third world poverty in Australia
and Malawi: A case of donor bias? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 409430.
Cassel, B.J. (1995). Altruism is only part of the story: A longitudinal study of AIDS volunteers.
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 56, 2937.
Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perceptionbehavior link and social
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R.S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R.J. (2000). The dynamics
of a stressful encounter. In T.E. Higgins & A.W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Motivational science: Social
and personality perspectives (pp. 111127). New York: Psychology Press.
Gaertner, S.L., Dovidio, J.F., Anastasio, P.A., Bachman, B.A., & Rust, M.C. (1993). The common
ingroup identity model: Recategorization and the reduction of intergroup bias. European Review of
Social Psychology, 4, 126.
Gage, A.J. (1998). Sexual activity and contraceptive use: The components of the decision-making
process. Studies in Family Planning, 29, 154166.
Galanter, M., Hayden, F., Castaeda, R. & Franco, H. (2005). Group therapy, self-help groups, and
network therapy. In R.J. Frances & S.I. Miller (Eds.),Clinical textbook of addictive disorders
(3rd edn, pp. 502527). New York: Guilford Press.
Gardner, G.T. & Stern, P.C. (2002). Environmental problems and human behaviour (2nd edn).
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F.X., Lane, D.J., & Stock, M.L. (2005). Smoking cessation: Social comparison
level predicts success for adult smokers. Health Psychology, 24, 623629.
Geurts, S.A., Buunk, B.P., & Schaufeli, W.B. (1994). Health complaints, social comparisons, and
absenteeism. Work & Stress, 8, 220234.
Geurts, S.A., Schaufeli, W.B., & Buunk, B.P. (1993). Social comparison, inequity, and absenteeism
among bus drivers. European Work and Organizational Psychologist, 3, 191203.
Gillespie, R. (1991). Manufacturing knowledge: A history of the Hawthorne experiments.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Goldstein, N.J., Cialdini, R.B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472
482.
Graham, S., Weiner, B., Giuliano, T., & Williams, E. (1993). An attributional analysis of reactions to
Magic Johnson. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 9961010.
Griffitt, W. (1970). Environmental effects on interpersonal affective behavior: Ambient effective
temperature and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 240244.
Griskevicius, V., Cantu, S., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Evolutionary basis for sustainable behaviours:
Implications for policy, marketing and social entrepreneurship. Journal of Public Policy and
Marketing, 31, 115128.
Guagnano, G.A., Stern, P.C., & Dietz, T. (1995). Influences of attitudebehavior relationships: A
natural experiment with curbside recycling. Environment and Behavior, 27, 699718.
Hardy, C. & Van Vugt, M. (2006). Nice guys finish first: The competitive altruism hypothesis.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 14021413.
Harris, L.T. & Fiske, S.T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuro-imaging responses to
extreme outgroups. Psychological Science, 17, 847853.
Harvey , S., Beckman, L.J., Gerend, M.A., Bird, S.T., Posner, S., Huszti, H.C., & Galavotti, C
(2006). A conceptual model of womens condom use intentions: Integrating intrapersonal and
relationship factors. AIDS Care, 18 , 698709.
Haslam, S.A., Reicher, S.D., & Platow, M.J. (2011).The new psychology of leadership: Identity,
influence and power. New York: Psychology Press.
Heilman, M.E., Simon, M.C., & Repper, D.P. (1987). Intentionally favored, unintentionally harmed?
Impact of sex-based preferential selection on self-perceptions and self-evaluations. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 72, 6268.
Henrich, J., Heine, S.J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010) The weirdest people in the world.Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 33, 6183.
Hewstone, M., Stroebe, W., & Jonas, K. (Eds.) (2012).Introduction to Social Psychology. Oxford:
Blackwell Publishing.
Hogg, M. & Cooper, J.M. (2007). The SAGE handbook of social psychology. London: Sage.
Holland, R.W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit: Nonconscious effects of
scent on cognition and behavior. Psychological Science, 16, 689693.
Homan, A.C., Hollenbeck, J.R., Humphrey, S.E., van Knippenberg, D., Ilgen, D.R., & van Kleef
G.A. (2008). Facing differences with an open mind: Openness to experience, salience of intragroup differences, and performance of diverse groups. Academy of Management Journal, 51,
12041222.
Hornsey, M.J. (2008). Social identity theory and self-categorization theory: A historical review.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 204222.
Huhmann, B.A. & Brotherton, T.P. (1997). A content analysis of guilt appeals in popular magazine
advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 26, 3545.
Iredale, W., Van Vugt, M., & Dunbar, R.I.M. (2008). Showing off in humans: Male generosity as
mating signal. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 386392.
Iyengar, R., Van den Bulte, C., & Valente, T.W. (2010). Opinion leadership and social contagion in
new product diffusion. Marketing Science, 30, 195212.
Jackson, D., Mannix, J., & Faga, P. (2005). Overweight and obese children: Mothers strategies.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 52, 613.
Joireman, J.A., Van Lange, P.A.M., & Van Vugt, M. (2004). Who cares about the environmental
impact of cars? Those with an eye toward the future. Environment and Behavior, 36, 187206.
Karau, S.J. & Williams, K.D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical
integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681706.
Kashima, Y., Gallois, C., & McCamish, M. (1993). The theory of reasoned action and cooperative
behaviour: It takes two to use a condom. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 227239.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder. Science, 322, 16811685.
Kenrick, D.T., Goldstein, N., & Braver, S.L. (Eds.) (2012). Six degrees of social influence: Science,
application and the psychology of Robert Cialdini. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kenrick, D.T. & Johnson, G.A. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in aversive environments: A problem
for the classical conditioning paradigm? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 572
579.
Kenrick, D.T., Neuberg, S.L., & Cialdini, R.B. (2010).Social psychology: Goals in interaction (5th
edn). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kerr, N.L. (1989). Illusions of efficacy: The effects of group size on perceived efficacy in social
dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 287313.
Kerr, N.L. & Tindale, S.R. (2004). Group performance and decision making.Annual Review of
Psychology, 55, 623655.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective
processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in
Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 12451267.
Kiviniemi, M.T., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A.M. (2002). Too many of a good thing? The effects of
multiple motivations on stress, cost, fulfillment, and satisfaction. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 732743.
Kluger, A.N. & DeNisi, A. (1998). Feedback interventions: Toward the understanding of a doubleedged sword. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 6772.
Kok, G.J., Schaalma, H., De Vries, H., Parcel, G., & Paulussen, T. (1996). Social psychology and
health education. European Review of Social Psychology, 7, 241282.
Kok, G.J., van Essen, G.A., Wicker, S., Llupi, A., Mena, G., Correia, R., & Ruiter, R.A.C. (2011)
Planning for influenza vaccination in health care workers: An Intervention Mapping approach.
Vaccine, 29, 85128519.
Komorita, S.S. & Parks, C.D. (1994). Social dilemmas. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
Koslowsky, M. & Krausz, M. (2002). Voluntary employee withdrawal and inattendance: A current
perspective. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Kramer, G.P. & Kerr, N.L. (1989). Laboratory simulation and bias in the study of juror behavior: A
methodological note. Law and Human Behaviour, 13, 8999.
Krieger, N. & Sidney, S. (1996). Racial discrimination and blood pressure: The CARDIA study of
young black and white adults. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 13701378.
LaPiere, R.T. (1934). Attitudes versus actions. Social Forces, 13, 230237.
Latan, B. & Darley, J. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesnt he help? New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Leander, N.P., Chartrand, T.L., & Bargh, J.A. (2012). You give me the chills: Embodied reactions to
inappropriate amounts of behavioral mimicry. Psychological Science, 23, 772779.
Lerner, M.J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press.
Levy, S.R., Freitas, A.L., & Salovey, P. (2002). Construing action abstractly and blurring social
distinctions: Implications for perceiving homogeneity among, but also empathizing with and
helping, others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 12241238.
Liljenquist, K., Zhong, C.B., & Galinsky, A.D. (2010). The smell of virtue: Clean scents promote
reciprocity and charity. Psychological Science, 21, 381383.
Linville, P., Fischer, G.W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk perceptions and decision biases. In
J.B. Pryor & G.D. Reeder (Eds.),The social psychology of HIV infection (pp. 538). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Locke, E.A. & Latham, G.P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task
motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705717.
Luzzo, D.A., Hasper, P., Albert, K.A., Bibby, M.A., & Martinelli, E.A. (1999). Effects of selfefficacy-enhancing interventions on the math/science self-efficacy and career interests, goals, and
actions of career-undecided college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 233243.
Lyas, J.K., Shaw, P.J., & Van Vugt, M. (2002). Recycling in a London borough: Perceptions,
participation and barriers to the use of survival bags. Paper presented at Waste 2002. Stratfordupon-Avon, 2426 September.
Lyness, K.S. & Thompson, D.E. (2000). Climbing the corporate ladder: Do female and male
executives follow the same route? Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 86101.
Lynn, M. & Mynier, K. (1993). Effect of server posture on restaurant tipping. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 23, 678685.
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K.M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of
sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111131.
Maio, G.R. & Esses, V.M. (1998). The social consequences of affirmative action: Deleterious effects
on perceptions of groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 6574.
Manstead, A.S.R. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.) (1995),The Blackwell encyclopedia of social psychology.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Mantler, J.L. (2001). Judgements of responsibility for HIV-infection: A test of Weiners social
motivation theory in the context of the AIDS epidemic (immune efficiency). Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 61, 5057.
Markus, H.R. & Kitayama, S. (2003). Culture, self, and the reality of the social.Psychological
Inquiry, 14, 277283.
Martin, C.L. & Brennett, N. (1996). The role of justice judgments in explaining the relationship
between job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Group & Organization Management, 21,
84104.
Martin, G. & Pear, J. (2003). Behavior modification: What is it and how to do it? Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W.B., & Leiter, M.P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,
397422.
McCalley, L.T. & Midden, C.J.H. (2002). Energy conservation through product-integrated feedback:
The roles of goal-setting and social orientation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 23, 589603.
McGuire, W.J. (1985). Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Arouson (Eds.),Handbook
of social psychology (pp. 233346). New York: Random House.
Montada, L. (1992). Attribution of responsibility for losses and perceived injustice. In L. Montada,
S.H. Fillip, & M. Lerner (Eds.),Life crises and experiences of loss in adulthood (pp. 133162).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Montada, L. (2001). Solidaritt mit der Dritten Welt/Solidarity with the Third World. In H.W.
Bierhoff & D. Fetchenhauer (Eds.), Solidaritt: Konflikt, Umwelt, Dritte Welt/Solidarity:
Conflict, Environment, Third World (pp. 6592). Opladen: Leske & Budrich.
Myers, D.G. (2012). Social psychology (11th edn). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Nadler, A. & Halabi, S. (2006). Intergroup helping as status relations: Effects of status stability,
identification, and type of help on receptivity to high status groups help. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 91, 97110.
Niezink, L.W., Siero, F.W., Dijkstra, P., Buunk, A.P., & Barelds, D.P.H. (2012). Empathic concern:
Distinguishing between tenderness and sympathy. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 544549.
OKeefe, D.J. (2002). Persuasion: Theory and research (2nd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Omoto, A.M., Snyder, M., & Hackett, J.D. (2010). Personality and motivational antecedents of
activism and civic engagement. Journal of Personality, 78, 17031734.
Oskamp, S., Burkhardt, R.L., Schultz, W.P., Hurin, S., & Zelezny, L. (1998). Predicting three
Rutter, D.R., Quine, L., & Albery, I.P. (1998). Perceptions of risk in motorcyclists: Unrealistic
optimism, relative realism and predictions of behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 681
696.
Ryan, M.K. & Haslam, S.A. (2005). The glass cliff: Evidence that women are overrepresented in
precarious leadership positions. British Journal of Management, 16, 8190.
Saad, G. (Ed.) (2011). Evolutionary psychology in the business sciences. Heidelberg, Germany:
Springer.
Schaalma, H.P., Kok, G., Bosker, R.J., & Parcel, G.S. (1996). Planned development and evaluation
of AIDS/STD education for secondary school students in the Netherlands: Short-term effects.
Health Education Quarterly, 23, 469487.
Schaller, M., Simpson, J.A., & Kenrick, D.T. (2006).Evolution and social psychology. New York:
Psychology Press.
Schaufeli, W.B. & Buunk, B.P. (2003). Burnout: An overview of 25 years of research and theorizing.
In M.J. Schabracq, J.A.M. Winnubst, & C.L. Cooper (Eds.),The handbook of work and health
psychology (pp. 383425). Chichester: Wiley.
Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the
severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 12191222.
Schneider, F., Gruman, J., & Coutts, L. (2012). Applied social psychology (2nd edn). Los Angeles,
CA: Sage.
Schultz, P.W. & Oskamp, S. (2000). Social psychology: An applied perspective. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Schwartz, S.H. (1977). Normative influences of altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221279). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Sedikides, C. & Gregg, A.P. (2003). Portraits of the self. In M.A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.),Sage
handbook of social psychology (pp.110138). London: Sage.
Senior, C., Martin, R., Thomas, G., Topakas, A., West, M., & Yeats, R. (2012). Developmental
stability and leadership effectiveness. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 281291.
Sheeran, P., Abraham, C., & Orbell, S. (1999). Psychosocial correlates of heterosexual condom use:
A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 90132.
Sheeran, P. & Silverman, M. (2003). Evaluation of three interventions to promote workplace health
and safety: Evidence for the utility of implementation intentions. Social Science & Medicine, 56,
21532163.
Sheeran, P. & Taylor, S. (1999). Predicting intentions to use condoms: A meta-analysis and
comparison of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 29, 16241675.
Sheeran, P., Webb, T.L., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and
implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 8798.
Sherman, D.A.K., Nelson, L.D., & Steele, C.M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the
self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 10461058.
Silver, R.C., Wortman, C.B., & Crofton, C. (1990). The role of coping in support provision: The selfpresentational dilemma of victims of life crises. In B. Sarason, I. Sarason, & G. Pierce (Eds.),
Index
absenteeism 28, 38, 52, 567, 64, 689, 109
accidents
coping behaviour 58, 73
driving 612
emergency situation 4, 67
just world 6, 150
action plans 131
actor-observer effect 149
adoption phase 129, 131
affect 57, 104, 145, 152, 155
affect-infusion model 145
affective responses 14, 53
affirmative action 44, 645
ageing 91, 158
Albarracn, D. 99
alcohol use 46, 102, 118, 1412
altruism 4, 157, 20, 25, 27, 68, 73, 80, 101, 146, 149, 152
ambivalent sexism inventory 9
analysis phase 48, 13, 156, 5683, 87
anti-social behaviour 24, 412, 468, 56, 59, 75, 142
applied
problem 22, 25, 34, 40, 49, 51
social psychological research 26, 58, 74, 102
argumentation 118
arousal 145, 150, 151, 152
association techniques 73, 81
direct intervention association 123, 136
free association 15, 61, 68
problem association 612, 82
concept association 612, 82
perspective taking 612, 82
associative network 152
attachment 26, 145
attention 5, 119, 127, 145, 146, 151, 156
attitude 14, 53, 56, 74, 152
attitude-behaviour models 69, 74, 92, 94, 96, 149
attribution theory 73, 145
authoritarianism 152
automaticity 142
awareness 5, 92, 128, 129, 131, 142, 158
background materials 4549
balance table 109113, 122, 1356
balance theory 145
Bargh, J. 189
Baron, R. A. 19
barriers, implementation route 130, 131, 135
Batson, D. C. 16, 20, 25
behaviours/behavioural intentions 14, 53, 56, 74, 149
belief in a just world 611, 22, 150
Bell, P. A. 19
Bem, D. J. 118
Bennenbroek, F. T. C. 120
Bias(ed perceptions) 73, 77, 82, 83, 106, 110, 125, 151
Blame, victims 3, 5, 112, 73
blood pressure
racial discrimination 1089
obesity 50
bogus pipeline 152
Bradfield, A. L. 71
Broken windows theory 43
Brug, J. 121
buffering effect 149
bullying, workplace 24, 25, 28, 32, 401, 47, 50, 56, 68
bystander
experiment 18
unresponsiveness 58
bystander cont.
effect 140, 149
intervention theory 68, 73
cancer 212, 24, 57, 119, 120
car use 245, 28, 57, 602
categorization 82, 83, 148, 153, 157
causal
factors/variables 105, 108, 1112, 1356
model 3840, 44, 46, 60, 68, 93, 155
causal model 3940, 41, 47
causes 76, 106, 121
analysis phase 60, 63, 767 801, 83
attribution of 145, 151
immediate/distal 38, 39, 99
problem definition 14, 22, 25, 30, 33, 35, 389, 401, 44, 512
Johnson, G. A. 19
justice 611
distributive 149
procedural 69, 77, 151
Kelling, G. L. 43
Kenrick, D. T. 19
key
aspects 14, 22, 25, 34, 404, 51
questions 30, 3544, 49, 50, 51
words 4, 713
Kok, G. 1212
Krieger, N. 1089
Latan, B. 18
Lerner, M. 6
Lichtman, R. R. 21
literature,
reviewing the 46, 72, 92, 105
scientific 5, 46, 51, 61, 99
litter 36, 43, 49, 60, 142
locus of control 155
loneliness 155
Luzzo, D. A. 120
McCalley, L. T. 119
mapping
actors 130
implementation route 1301
media 3, 12, 45, 77, 133
campaign 12, 17
social 1245
memory 71, 147, 152, 155, 158
transactive 158
mere exposure effect 151
meta-analysis/meta-analytic review 72, 99, 101, 119, 155
methods, help phase 1134, 11822, 135
Midden, C. J. H. 119
Millennium Campaign 1335
minimal group paradigm 155
minority social influence 155
mimicry 189
misattribution of arousal 151
modelling 50, 1123, 1189, 1223, 125, 155
modifiable 10812, 1356
Montada, L. 6
mood 18, 62, 80, 120, 142, 145, 147, 152, 155
moral obligation 5, 10, 11, 16
motivation, implementation route 1301, 135
negative state relief model 147
negativity effects 151
negotiation 724
nonverbal communication 155
normative
beliefs 75, 936, 99, 100
influence 151
pressure 36, 141
norm theory 147
obesity 4950, 1103, 132
observation
analysis phase 623, 667, 81
problem phase 489, 51
help phase 127
modeling 119
unstructured 48
outcome variables
analysis phase 15, 56, 57, 5960, 79, 812
evaluation 132
test phase 8, 16, 867, 989, 105
relevance of 59
outgroup homogeneity 151
panel design 134
PATH method/model 2, 3, 12, 13, 16, 20, 39, 57, 59, 68, 74, 76, 86, 87, 108, 140
path analysis 155
patients 2, 21, 22, 52, 66, 91, 114, 120, 129
Paulussen, T. 128
perceived control 94, 1113, 148
perceived outcomes 1113
personality 17, 18, 42, 109, 154, 155
personal
norms 5
responsibility 10, 68, 78, 90
space 155
person perception 155
perspective taking 62, 68, 82
plausibility, of explanations 15, 76, 7981, 83
police force 245, 27, 2940, 42, 445, 47, 59, 623
policies
affirmative action 44, 645
anti-discrimination 109
governmental 91, 143
relevant 130, 135
policy makers 24, 57, 60, 98, 140
political values/beliefs 109, 112, 125
pollution 24, 40, 57, 75
positive illusion 151
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) 1034
power 28, 34, 40, 50, 73, 74, 156
prejudice 8, 17, 24, 31, 34, 39, 42, 489, 68, 75, 91, 146, 155
preliminary
causal model 38, 93
process model 878, 92
presentation 131, 140
pre-testing, intervention 1267, 135
primacy effect 151
priming 19, 155
prisoners dilemma 156
problem
definition 2, 13, 2551, 56, 59, 70, 82, 86
phase 2551
reformulation 278
process evaluation 1323
process model 7, 8, 167, 136, 1413
analysis phase 65, 72, 77
help phase 108112, 132, 135
test phase 86106
prosocial behaviour 45, 15, 73, 156
prospect theory 147
prototype 147, 156
theory 147
proximate
analysis 75
causes/explanations 77, 89
PsycARTICLES 46, 73
PsycINFO 5, 46
punishment 46, 75, 120, 155
quantitative
assessment 127, 135
terms 59, 60
Quine, L. 120
racism 40, 48
rational choice theory 756, 147
reactance 156
recall task 127, 135
recency-effect 151
reciprocity 12, 15, 80, 156
recycling 25, 48, 56, 57, 59, 79, 101
reference group 156
reinforcement 112, 130
theory 147
reinforcer 156
reinforcing effects 979, 105
relative deprivation 44, 77, 156
reliability 103
religious values/beliefs 109, 125
repetition 122
responsibility 5, 6, 49, 62, 69, 89, 90, 149
reward 75, 120, 155, 156
risk perception 734
risky shift 151
Rogers, E. M. 129
Rosenbergs self-esteem scale 1023
Russ-Eft, D. F. 119
salience 37, 127, 156
scale
construction 104
existing 102
self-esteem 1023
seven point 127
three point 66
schema 156
Schwartz, S. 5, 15
script 126, 156
self-affirmation theory 147
self-appraisal 156
self-awareness 156
self-categorization theory 69, 147
self-concept 156
self-disclosure 156
self-discrepancy 147
self-efficacy 73, 94, 119, 140, 156
self-enhancement 21, 157
self-esteem 36, 42, 50, 68, 76, 157
boosting 124
scale 1023
theory 20
self-evaluation 21, 121, 148
maintenance model (SEM) 148
self-fulfilling prophecy 151
self-handicapping 157
self-monitoring 157
self-perception theory 118, 148
self-presentation 157
self-serving bias 151
self-verification theory 148
sexism 9, 56
sexually transmitted diseases (STD) 705, 86, 917
shared group products 8890
Sidney, S. 1089
Silverman, M. 120
similarity 6, 18, 118, 143
hypothesis 148
Smith, K. H. 119
smoking 24, 57, 68, 978, 101, 119122, 132, 1345
Snyder, M. 267
social categories 157
social cognition 42, 157
social comparison 21, 28, 78, 120, 156, 157
social comparison orientation 157
social dilemmas 69, 73, 778, 87, 89, 152
social network analysis 98
social exchange theory 15, 20, 28, 69, 77, 148
social facilitation 152
social identity theory 69, 82,148
social impact theory 148
social influence 4, 155
social interaction 42, 48, 110, 157
socialization 157
social learning theory 118, 148
social loafing 69, 152
social norms 17, 42, 50, 62, 745, 93, 94, 967, 122, 149
social role theory 148
social support 34, 68, 157
social value orientation 157
specificity, outcome variable 59
status-expectation states theory 148
Steadman, L. 120
stereotypes 146, 152, 154, 158
strategy