Sex and Sexuality - Book
Sex and Sexuality - Book
Sex and Sexuality - Book
Volumes 1-3
Edited by
Richard D. McAnulty
M. Michele Burnette
PRAEGER
SEX AND SEXUALITY
SEX AND SEXUALITY
Volume 1
SEXUALITY TODAY: TRENDS
AND CONTROVERSIES
1
PRAEGER PERSPECTIVES
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Contents
Preface vii
Introduction ix
1. Sex Research 1
Michael Wiederman
2. Theories of Human Sexuality 17
Roy F. Baumeister, Jon K. Maner, and
C. Nathan DeWall
3. Sexuality in Childhood 35
Ryan P. Kilmer and Ariana Shahinfar
4. Adolescent Sexuality 61
Charlene Rapsey and Tamar Murachver
5. Sexual Behavior in the United States 103
Tom W. Smith
6. Later Life Sexuality 133
Thomasina H. Sharpe
7. Sexual Orientation and Identity 153
Michael R. Kauth
vi Contents
We have had many opportunities to teach and interact with both college
students and professional audiences about some very important topics and
issues in human sexuality in our roles as authors and college professors. When
we were approached to write this three-volume set on sex and sexuality, we
were intrigued with the idea of having a forum in which to reach a broader
audience. That is our goal for this work. With that in mind, we encouraged
our contributors to ‘‘talk to’’ a general audience when writing about the topics
that were most important to them. The authors we selected to write these
chapters represent both established authorities and budding scholars on the
various topics in human sexuality. We are confident that they have all helped
us accomplish our goal.
To us, few, if any, other topics in the realm of human behavior are more
interesting, exciting, or controversial than sex. And we hope that you will
agree after reading the chapters from this set. Each chapter stands alone, and
you can choose to read as many or as few as you would like—pick the ones
that interest you. We hope that you will find this work to be of significant
value to you, whether you are in pursuit of a better general understanding of
sexuality or are looking for answers to specific questions.
One theme you will find throughout these texts is that human sexual
function is affected by a whole host of factors. These factors are biological,
sociocultural, and psychological in nature. The scientific study of sexuality is
for all practical purposes a ‘‘young’’ field, and we have only touched the
viii Preface
surface in an attempt to fully understand how these factors interact and impact
sexuality.
Another theme or concern you will find throughout this work is the
question whether ‘‘scientific’’ views of sex are biased by social judgments about
normal versus abnormal and/or functional versus dysfunctional sexual be-
havior. U.S. culture, in particular, holds many strong values and prohibitions
about sex. In this context, studying and interpreting research on sexuality in an
unbiased manner can be a challenge. Many of our authors caution the reader
about this concern.
We wish to thank all the researchers and clinicians, past and present, who
have contributed to the science of sex. Many of them have contributed
chapters to this set, and for that we are grateful. We also thank our colleagues,
families, and friends who supported us during the writing and editing process.
Finally, we thank ‘‘the team’’ at Praeger Publishers.
Introduction
In the past few years, we have witnessed major developments in our under-
standing of sex and sexuality. A landmark survey of sexual practices in the
United States offered new insights. The theory of evolutionary psychology
inspired countless studies on sexual differences between women and men.
Discoveries relating to sexual orientation, including possible brain and genetic
factors, have emerged. A medication for the treatment of erection problems
became a household term. The scandal of sexual molestation by the clergy
captivated the media. Pornography in all of its forms became readily available
via the Internet. And sex continued to sell.
The topic of sex is fascinating, intriguing, and even disturbing. We are
seemingly surrounded by sexual themes. The media bombard us with sex.
Sexuality is a fundamental need and a part of our identity. Yet, this basic human
need is often misunderstood, and is often controversial, and sometimes prob-
lematic.
Volume 1 in Sex and Sexuality offers an overview of recent trends and
developments in the field. It provides a summary of the sociocultural deter-
minants of sexual practices and of sexual development through the lifespan.
Additionally, some of the more sensitive and controversial topics, including
pornography and the sex trade, are reviewed. Each chapter entails an analysis
of a topic and related issues, a review of relevant and recent findings, a de-
scription of explanations for the issues and trends, and a summary that usually
includes suggestions for further study and research.
x Introduction
In Chapter 1, Wiederman explores not only methods but also motives for
conducting sex research. Challenges include the stigma attached to sex re-
search, possible volunteer bias, and the potential inaccuracies of sex surveys. In
Chapter 2, Baumeister and colleagues offer a concise and informative over-
view of the leading theories of sexuality. They argue that the two leading
theoretical camps—social constructionism and evolutionary psychology—
approach the status of cults with their respective dogmatic doctrines and
dedicated followers. The historical significance of Freudian theory and, more
recently, of social exchange theory is addressed. Kilmer and Shahinfar, in
Chapter 3, lament the lack of research on sexuality in childhood, which is truly
one of the last frontiers in sex research because of enduring taboos. The limited
research has been mostly concerned with abnormal sexual development. The
authors favor an ecological systems approach, which considers the contribution
of such influences as peer culture, family factors, and community structure to
sexual development. Adolescent sexuality has been the subject of much dis-
cussion and debate. As Rapsey and Murachver note in Chapter 4, adolescence
has invariably been viewed as a problematic phase in human development,
including sexual development. This flawed depiction of adolescence has
hindered research on normal sexual expression during this phase. Although
unwanted pregnancies and sexually transmitted diseases first become evident in
this age-group, these problems do not affect most teens, nor should they define
adolescent sexuality. Drawing from the General Social Surveys (GSS), Smith,
in Chapter 5, offers a succinct summary of important social trends in sexual
behavior in the United States. The summary traces changes in sexual activity
over the past few decades, including changing rates of cohabitation, extra-
marital relations, gender of sexual partners, frequency of sexual intercourse and
of sexual inactivity, and the impact of HIV. Few topics make people more
uncomfortable than sexuality in later life. Sharpe criticizes these prejudiced
attitudes and their detrimental impact on mature adults in Chapter 6. The
limited research confirms that older adults can, and generally do, enjoy sexual
intimacy into late life, even as they redefine the meanings of sex and intimacy.
Sexual orientation remains a fascinating if controversial aspect of human
sexuality. In Chapter 7, Kauth reviews the growing amount of research on
sexual orientation and identity. His comprehensive and objective review fo-
cuses on the relevant findings in America and in other cultures, the current
sexuality theories, and recent events involving sexual orientation. In Chapter 8,
Burnette examines the relevant research on sex and gender, concluding that
men and women are more similar than different. Although popular stereotypes
about gender are resistant to change, the findings on transgenderism challenge
these simplistic notions. Our views about sexuality are highly influenced by
such major sociocultural institutions as the family, medicine, religion, and the
media. Bay-Cheng’s critical examination in Chapter 9 of these influences
dissects or ‘‘deconstructs’’ the various messages, thereby highlighting the
comparative and arbitrary nature of these ‘‘truths’’ relating to sex and sexuality.
Introduction xi
Her analysis challenges many dominant views of sex in the United States, such
as the idea that sexual intercourse alone qualifies as ‘‘real’’ sex. In the same
vein, Lewis demonstrates in Chapter 10 that our views of race and ethnicity
are determined by culture rather than nature. In other words, categories of
race are socially created rather than absolute and biologically determined.
Here, too, we find racial stereotypes are oversimplifications. For example,
contrary to the stereotype of higher levels of sexual activity among African
Americans, Lewis finds this group to be more sexually conservative than
others.
The sex trade, in all of its forms, remains a highly visible and controversial
aspect of sexuality. Chapter 11 by Brown reviews the extensive and conflicting
research on pornography, concluding that it is not as innocuous as some have
argued. Brown proposes that even nonviolent pornography promotes callous
attitudes toward women. Bullough and McAnulty offer an overview of the
research on the ‘‘world’s oldest profession’’ in Chapter 12. Despite the stigma,
the sex trade thrives in most parts of the world. The authors also discuss a
group that has largely been ignored in sex research: exotic dancers. Gil-Rivas
and Kooyman discuss sexual risk-taking in Chapter 13. Efforts to understand
and prevent sexual risk-taking require an examination of the social context,
which in turn is influenced by a variety of individual and contextual factors
such as characteristics of the individual, aspects of close interpersonal rela-
tionships, attitudes, beliefs, cultural norms, and social and economic condi-
tions. Finally, in a thought-provoking thesis, Baumeister and Stillman offer
what will be one of the most controversial chapters in the set. Their discussion
of erotic plasticity in Chapter 14 proposes that women’s sexual responses and
feelings are more affected by social, cultural, and situational factors, whereas
male sexuality is relatively more shaped by genetic, hormonal, and other bi-
ological factors. For example, the authors point out that women are more
likely than men to alter the frequency of sexual activity based on situational
factors, and they are also more likely to explore sexual variations, such as same-
sex experimentation, than men. Although this chapter is unlikely to resolve
this debate, it should inspire lively discussion and productive research.
1
Sex Research
Michael Wiederman 1
Sex research. Wow! What could be more exciting? People are frequently
titillated by the notion of someone conducting research on human sexuality.
Because the topic is taboo, and sexual activity is exciting, people often assume
that sex research must involve many interesting tasks. Sex research may hardly
seem like ‘‘work.’’
The truth is that, although research on human sexuality can be intellectually
stimulating, it is rarely arousing sexually or in other ways. Why? Sexuality is
deeply private, so most research involves asking people about their sexuality.
Researchers typically try to provide a setting in which the respondents are most
comfortable—usually by gathering information through anonymous surveys. In
the end, sex research often involves handing out and gathering printed survey
forms. After analyzing and tabulating the responses, the result is a set of numbers
and graphs, but hardly anything that resembles sexually stimulating material
(except, perhaps, to some mathematicians).
Although the process of conducting most sex research may not be partic-
ularly exciting, people are generally interested in the results of the research. It is
implied in Western culture that each individual is supposed to experience a
robust sex life, consisting of definite interest in sex (but not too much) and
varied sexual experience (but not to include certain behaviors or certain types of
partners). So how do we know how we, as individuals, stack up against the rest
of society? Even though Western culture often seems saturated with sexual
images and references to sex, there is surprisingly little serious discussion or
2 Sexuality Today
presentation of facts. This is where sexuality research comes in, and this is
probably why most people are interested in its results. Sex research holds the
promise of providing objective information about what other people do and
think and feel sexually.
As members of Western culture, we most likely encounter with interest
reports of sexuality research. So, better understanding the process of sex re-
search, and the various pitfalls along the way, will help us become more savvy
consumers of sex research results. We will know the questions to ask, even if
only to ourselves, so that we remain appropriately critical in our evaluation of
what we hear and read about ‘‘the latest research.’’ The process of sex research
begins with the people who conduct the research.
drawn to the use of pornography. There are no data on the subject, so it remains
speculation as to why researchers choose the research topics they do. Indeed,
sometimes it does seem to result from personal experiences, but many times,
research topics are simply those that the professional was exposed to in graduate
school, or through colleagues, or those that were being funded by grants.
Unfortunately, assumptions about the personal motives of sex researchers
often result in sexuality research being stigmatized compared to most other
research topics in the social and behavioral sciences. Sex researchers have been
known to study nonsexual topics early in their careers, until they have achieved
a degree of respectability (and tenure), so that embarking on the study of sex-
uality does not jeopardize their livelihood. For example, the sex research pio-
neer William Masters became well respected for his research and clinical work
on infertility before deciding he could afford to study sexual behavior. Some sex
researchers choose to study nonsexual topics in addition to sexual ones, perhaps
as a way to keep from being pigeonholed as ‘‘just a sex researcher.’’ Even within
sex research, however, some topics (such as childhood sexuality and adult-child
sexual contact) are more stigmatized than others. The less socially desirable the
topic, the more the research on that topic seems to be stigmatizing for the
researcher who seeks to better understand it.
If sex researchers are frequently stigmatized for their choice of research
topic, what about people who choose to participate in sex research? Because
sexuality is a private topic, who is most likely to volunteer to participate in sex
research? What is in it for them?
when we learn that a study based on college students revealed certain trends, we
should ask whether it is likely that those same results would have occurred
among research participants from the general public.
Even though college students represent a fairly unique sample compared to
the general public, only certain types of college students are usually studied:
those who are taking psychology or sociology courses. How might these stu-
dents differ from those who choose not to take such courses? Students interested
in social and behavioral sciences may be more open-minded and introspective
compared to students uninterested in those courses. What about the social
science students who then decide to participate in sex research? In what ways
might they be different from social science students who choose not to par-
ticipate, or who choose to fulfill their course requirements through participating
in research on nonsexual topics? Research comparing students who participate
in sex research to those who choose not to has revealed some consistent dif-
ferences: sex research participants are more likely to be male, open-minded, and
adventurous, and to have more liberal sexual attitudes and greater sexual ex-
perience (Dunne, 2002).
Even when sex researchers target the general population, not everyone
chosen agrees to participate. Even in the most conscientiously conducted
studies, perhaps only 70 percent of those contacted end up participating. How
might those 30 percent who do not participate differ from the 70 percent who
do? Compared to participants, nonparticipants tend to be older, more con-
servative in their attitudes and values, and more likely to be female. So, when
we hear that a certain percentage of people believe such and such or have had
some particular sexual experience, chances are this percentage does not ac-
curately reflect what would be found among the population as a whole.
What we have been discussing in this section is the extent to which any
particular sample is representative of the population the researcher is trying to
understand. The ideal is for the sample to be perfectly representative, meaning,
the sample perfectly matches the characteristics of the population. In reality this
never occurs, mainly because people cannot be forced to participate in research.
So, those who choose to do so will probably always differ in some ways from
people who choose not to—and this is especially the case when the topic is
sexuality. Some people are simply more open to sharing with researchers details
of their sexual attitudes and experiences than are others. These people tend to
have more open and liberal attitudes about sex.
Perhaps the least representative of samples occurs when participants
themselves have to initiate participation. For example, if a magazine publishes a
questionnaire, asking readers to complete it and mail it to the magazine, or asks
them to go to a Web site and complete a questionnaire, who is most likely to do
so? Participation here requires some effort on the part of respondents, and there
is no obvious incentive to participate. So the readers most likely to respond are
those who find the topic most interesting or most relevant. If the topic is
extramarital sex, we can imagine that those readers who have had some expe-
Sex Research 5
rience with it will be the ones most likely to respond to the survey. After all, if a
reader does not have any experience with extramarital sex, the reader is liable to
assume the survey does not even apply. It is not very interesting to check ‘‘no’’
or ‘‘does not apply’’ for most survey items.
In the end, savvy consumers of sexuality research need to ask how research
participants were recruited. The ultimate question is the extent to which the
sample is representative of the population. The less representative the sample,
the less accurate the results, and the less we should let the results influence us in
our own decision making or opinions. These issues are separate from another
important set of issues having to do with how variables are measured.
anal sexual intercourse. After the term is defined, respondents may be asked to
choose one of five responses given: (a) never, (b) once, (c) twice, (d) 3–10 times,
(e) more than 10 times. These choices imply that the researcher does not expect
respondents to have engaged in anal sexual intercourse more than a few times
(if ever). Notice that the middle response choice is ‘‘twice.’’ Many respondents
might assume that the middle choice represents the average or typical respon-
dent. The person who has engaged in anal sex numerous times may feel
somewhat embarrassed because the response choices imply that his or her ex-
perience is extreme and unusual.
Now let us consider another researcher interested in the same variable. This
researcher, however, uses the following response choices: (a) never, (b) 1–10
times, (c) 11–25 times, (d) 26–100 times, (e) more than 100 times. What do
these response choices imply about what the researcher, the ‘‘expert,’’ expects
and considers ‘‘normal’’? Indeed, research indicates that people will report
greater levels of sexual experience with response choices like the second set,
compared to the first.
Social desirability response bias might be greatest when data are gathered
through face-to-face interviews. Respondents might feel most self-conscious
here because they have to report their sexual attitudes and experiences to
another person directly. Respondents are no longer completely anonymous.
So, why would researchers use interviews rather than paper surveys? One
advantage is that the interviewer can make sure all questions are answered
(none are skipped or left blank) and can clarify any questions the participant
might have. With paper surveys there is no way to clarify the questions or the
responses, and there will always be some respondents who interpret the words
differently than the researcher intended. One compromise involves using
portable computers for ‘‘interviews.’’ The respondents wear headphones so
that the questions can be read to them privately by the computer. If the
respondent has questions about the meanings of particular words, he or she can
click on those words to pull up a help window. Of course, this format for
gathering data probably works best with respondents who are comfortable
using computers (such as college students).
So far we have considered self-report in a general way, or as it applies to
reporting one’s sexual experience. When researchers are interested in some
abstract concept, such as ‘‘sexual self-esteem,’’ for example, they typically use
scales to measure it. These scales simply comprise several questions, all per-
taining to the same concept. Respondents might rate how much they agree or
disagree with each statement in the scale, and their total across the items makes
up their score on the measure. Constructing valid scales involves many issues,
most of which are beyond the scope of our presentation here (see Wiederman,
2002). However, it is important to consider the labels and underlying meanings
of such scales.
Suppose a researcher constructed a scale consisting of the following five
items. For each item, respondents indicate how much they agree or disagree.
Sex Research 9
It is debatable whether these five items all measure the same concept. For
the sake of argument, let us suppose they do. What might you say this scale
measures? What if the researcher called it a measure of ‘‘sexual depression’’?
Do you agree? Now suppose that you encountered a description of the re-
searcher’s results and learned that ‘‘older adults have greater sexual depression
than do younger adults.’’ Apparently, older respondents scored higher on this
measure than did younger respondents, but does the conclusion seem accurate
given the items constituting the scale? Unfortunately, in mass-media descrip-
tions of research results, we never get to examine the measures used in the
research. Despite being left in the dark about this important aspect of research,
we need to try to determine what the results of the research really mean.
attitudes toward women prior to exposure to the films, and so any difference
between the groups after exposure to the film must be caused by having viewed
the sexually explicit films.
Note that even in this example of a very simple experiment, there are
several important assumptions: that the two groups were comparable in their
attitudes toward women prior to the experiment; that the sexually explicit
films shown during the experiment are similar to pornographic films viewed
by people in the real world; and that the measure of attitudes toward women
indeed measures accurately such attitudes. If any of these assumptions is false,
the conclusion that exposure to sexually explicit film affects people’s attitudes
toward women in certain ways is flawed. Then there are also the issues of
who the research participants were, and whether what the researcher found
with those participants is what would be found with people in general.
Even with all of these potential concerns, it is only the results of an ex-
periment that can be used to conclude that one thing caused another. In all other
kinds of studies, the researcher can only conclude that one variable is related to
another. This may seem to be a small difference, but it is an important one.
There are many topics that the researcher cannot study with an experiment, so
concluding that one thing caused another in those areas is simply wrong. Im-
portant things researchers cannot manipulate include gender differences, sexual
orientation, upbringing or past experiences, sexual activity and experiences,
whether people are involved in a sexual relationship, and prior sexual attitudes.
Since it is impossible to manipulate these things in an experiment, researchers
cannot determine directly what causes them. The best they can do is investigate
whether the variable is related (correlated) to other variables, and then speculate
about what might cause what.
As an example, suppose a researcher interested in the possible effects of
exposure to pornography on attitudes toward women asks respondents to re-
port how much pornography the respondent views and to complete a self-
report measure of attitudes toward women. It is then possible to correlate the
amount of porn reported with scores on the scale measuring attitudes toward
women. If the researcher finds a correlation between the two, can the researcher
conclude that exposure to pornography affects attitudes toward women? If
not, why?
The simplest explanation for why the researcher cannot legitimately con-
clude that one thing causes or affects another is that the research was not an
experiment. When researchers examine correlations among variables, as was the
case in this example, it is impossible to determine which variable causes or affects
another. So perhaps people with certain attitudes toward women are more likely
to seek out and view pornography. This would be an instance of the attitudes
affecting the behavior, rather than the other way around. It is also very possible
that both viewing pornography and attitudes toward women are influenced by
some other variable or set of variables. Perhaps viewing pornography and
holding certain attitudes toward women are more likely among people of lower
Sex Research 11
large sample drawn from that population. So, even small results are statistically
significant. These are difficult concepts to understand, especially by just reading
about them. The ultimate message is that all research results reported to the
public are statistically significant, but the term is misleading because whether a
research result is statistically significant depends on both the size of the result and
the size of the sample upon which it is based.
Without being told how large a group difference exists or how large a
correlation is between two variables, it is impossible to judge the strength of
the relationship between two things. Taking our earlier example of the po-
tential effects of watching pornographic films on subsequent attitudes toward
women, let us say that the researcher found that the experimental group
(exposed to the porn) had statistically significant higher scores on the measure
of negative attitudes toward women than did the control group (exposed to
nonsexual films). The conclusion is that exposure to porn influenced the self-
reported attitudes toward women. But to what degree? The difference be-
tween the experimental and control groups was statistically significant, so we
know that the difference the researcher found is unlikely if indeed there is no
difference between the two groups as they exist in the larger population. Still,
there is no guarantee that the difference between the groups accurately reflects
what exists in the population. Also, we are not told how large the difference is
so that we can judge for ourselves whether to be impressed or dismiss the effect
as trivial.
1. Who were the researchers? Were they independent faculty members, or were
they employees of a group that has a vested interest in certain findings or
conclusions?
Sex Research 13
2. Who was studied? How were the participants recruited, and who would be
most likely to agree to participate? Are these participants likely to be different
from the general population?
3. How large were the differences or relationships found? You probably will not
have access to this information, but if you know there was a large sample, then
it is very possible that the statistically significant finding is actually small.
4. Is the report implying that one thing causes another, when in fact the research
was not an experiment? This is very common, probably because people nat-
urally tend to think in terms of causal relationships. However, just because two
things are statistically related does not mean that one caused the other—even if
such a causal relationship obviously makes sense.
CONCLUSION
All research on sexuality is imperfect. Rather than despair, we should be
appropriately critical of any sweeping conclusions we encounter in the media
regarding ‘‘the latest research.’’ Because of the sensitive nature of the topic, the
results of sex research are generally of interest to people. At the same time, the
sensitive nature of sexuality leads to special problems when researchers decide
to study it. Who will agree to participate? How do you gather information or
data? How do you measure sexuality? What do the results mean? All attempts
to answer these questions raise just as many questions and potential problems as
answers. Still, some research conclusions, even imperfect ones, are better than
none at all. When many researchers each contribute their own pieces to the
overall puzzle, eventually the picture starts to come into view.
REFERENCES
Brannigan, G. R., Allgeier, E. R., & Allgeier, R. (Eds.). (2000). The sex scientists.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bullough, B., Bullough, V. L., Fithian, M. A., Hartman, W. E., & Klein, R. S.
(Eds.). (1997). Personal stories of ‘‘How I got into sex.’’ Buffalo, NY: Pro-
metheus Books.
Dunne, M. P. (2002). Sampling considerations. In M. W. Wiederman & B. E.
Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research on human sexuality
(pp. 85–112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Janssen, E. (2002). Psychophysiological measurement of sexual arousal. In M. W.
Wiederman & B. E. Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research
on human sexuality (pp. 139–171). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Sex Research 15
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual
behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. New York:
Little, Brown.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. New York:
Little, Brown.
Moore, M. M. (2002). Behavioral observation. In M. W. Wiederman & B. E.
Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research on human sexuality
(pp. 113–137). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Okami, P. (2002). Dear diary: A useful but imperfect method. In M. W. Wie-
derman & B. E. Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research on
human sexuality (pp. 195–207). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rubin, Z., & Mitchell, C. (1976). Couples research as couples counseling.
American Psychologist, 31, 17–25.
Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwartz, N. (1996). Thinking about answers: The
application of cognitive processes to survey methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., Larsen, S. F., & Betz, A. L. (1996). Auto-
biographical memory: Remembering what and remembering when. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey re-
sponse. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Veroff, J., Hatchett, S., & Douvan, E. (1992). Consequences of participating in a
longitudinal study of marriage. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 315–327.
Whitley, B. E. (2002). Group comparison research. In M. W. Wiederman & B. E.
Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research on human sexuality
(pp. 223–254). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wiederman, M. W. (2002). Reliability and validity of measurement. In M. W.
Wiederman & B. E. Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), The handbook for conducting research
on human sexuality (pp. 25–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
2
fill up with articles that simply report patterns of data while giving little or no
theoretical elaboration (Weis, 1998).
In the long run, however, we see more grounds for optimism regarding
theory development in the study of sexuality. Sex remains one of the most
broadly interesting spheres of human activity, and the diversity of interest in it is
reflected in a wide range of outlets for new ideas, so that no established elite can
dominate all outlets or suppress contrary views. After the taboos against dis-
cussing or researching sex have been set aside, the field has begun to discover
that a wide range of theoretical approaches to human behavior have something
to offer to the study of sex. The coming decades promise to be fertile ones for
the development of sex theory.
Feminist Sexology
It is possible to be a social constructionist without being a feminist, and vice
versa, but in practice, and particularly in the practice of theorizing about sex, the
two overlap heavily and few thinkers embrace one without the other. The two
theoretical approaches are quite compatible. Moreover, in our view, the high-
water mark of both approaches coincided: the 1970s saw the flowering of social
constructionist approaches, in part swept along in the wake of the hugely
popular and influential book by Berger and Luckmann (1967). That same de-
cade also witnessed the triumph of feminism in many spheres, not the least
of which was in sexual theory, driven in part by the so-called sexual revolution
of the late 1960s and the early 1970s, which by all accounts took the form of
sweeping and extensive changes in female sexuality. (Male sexuality changed far
less than female sexuality; see Ehrenreich, Hess, & Jacobs, 1986; also Arafat &
Yorburg, 1973; Bauman & Wilson, 1974; Birenbaum, 1970; Robinson, Ziss,
Ganza, Katz, & Robinson, 1991.) The undeniable fact that sexual attitudes and
behaviors, and female sexuality in particular, had changed so radically in such a
short period of time created the sense that almost anything was possible, and
thereby seemed to prove the constructionist point that sex depended on his-
torical and cultural context. In our view, large parts of sexual theory today are
still deeply rooted in the feminist thinking from the 1970s, including such
landmark theoretical works as Brownmiller (1975).
Summarizing feminist theory is hampered by disagreements among femi-
nists, some stemming from core contradictions. For example, some feminists
seek to assert gender equality in all things, whereas others seek to establish the
superiority of women in as many spheres as possible. (Admittedly, both views
share a rejection of theories that men are properly or naturally superior to
20 Sexuality Today
women.) Some feminists embrace the scientific method and seek to use em-
pirical findings to establish gender equality or female superiority, whereas others
regard objective science as a sham and assert that the conclusions of research are
inevitably biased by the political (and other) views of the researchers, especially
in such socially fraught issues as sex. The controversial book Who Stole Femi-
nism? (Sommers, 1995) asserted that feminism had changed over time in fun-
damental ways but was reluctant to admit having changed, particularly because
the newer form seeks to benefit from the moral legitimacy of the earlier. In
Sommers’s account, feminism of the early 1970s promoted gender equality and
fairness, advocated questioning of established ideas, and welcomed the support
of men. Feminism since the 1980s, in contrast, has promoted female superiority
and female gains at the expense of men, opposed questioning of its own en-
trenched dogmas, and regarded men (with a few exceptions) as irredeemable
enemies. Regardless of whether one accepts that particular analysis, the diversity
of feminist views is undeniable. We shall summarize several themes as best we
can, but it would not be surprising if here or there a self-proclaimed feminist
thinker could be found to deviate from each of them.
A major theme of feminism is that sexual attitudes and practices are rooted
in the gender roles that each particular culture and historical period have
constructed. Feminism is primarily concerned with male and female differences,
and, to feminists, sex is just one manifestation of gender. (We use the term ‘‘sex’’
to refer to sexuality and the term ‘‘gender’’ to refer to both the social and
biological distinctions between men and women.)
Feminists have then elaborated a variety of implications of that basic as-
sumption. Tiefer (1995) proposed that sexuality is not solely a product of bi-
ology, but is socially constructed and frequently negotiated. From the social
constructionist perspective, sexuality is not a universal human phenomenon but
is instead only a part of social life and identity that can be sexualized or de-
sexualized through its cultural meaning and regulation (Weeks, 1991). Social
constructionists believe that sexual authorities create and maintain expectations
regarding sexuality only when such expectations will benefit those in power.
Indeed, power is central to feminist thought. Many have asserted that power
is a key ingredient to all feminist analyses (e.g., Riger, 1992; Yoder & Kahn,
1992). Feminist thought emphasizes the concept of patriarchy in explaining
gender differences and, in particular, women’s problems. Patriarchy is the political
domination of males over females. Thus, gender relations and women’s problems
are explained by examining the oppressive and exploitative social structures al-
legedly set up by men to favor themselves at women’s expense.
In the sexual sphere, power and patriarchy remain centrally useful concepts
to feminist theory. Rich (1980) asserted that heterosexuality is not a natural state
of affairs, but instead is due to the existence of a social structure in which men
occupy many of the high-power positions. Brownmiller (1975) helped popu-
larize the view that rape results from a conspiracy by all men to intimidate all
women so as to keep men in power. The view that power rather than sex is the
Theories of Human Sexuality 21
motivating force behind rape has become a central principle of feminist sexology
(see later section on rape theory). Dworkin (1981) and others have emphasized
that human civilization is built on men’s ability to rape and abuse women.
In the 1970s, there was an attempt to deny or minimize gender differences
in sexuality and to claim that any observed differences were likely due to pa-
triarchal influences such as the so-called double standard, which prohibits
women from enjoying sexual activities that are permitted to men. More recent
empirical work by feminist scholars has, however, confirmed the existence of
large gender differences in sexuality. Across more than 170 studies testing
128,363 participants, Oliver and Hyde (1993) found large gender differences in
the incidence of masturbation and in attitudes about casual sex. Men also ex-
pressed more positive attitudes toward sexual intercourse in a marriage or com-
mitted relationship than women, and women felt more anxious and guilty about
sex than men. Thus, there appear to be substantial differences in how men and
women express their sexuality privately and with relationship partners.
Feminists argue that these differences in sexuality perpetuate differences be-
tween the genders and create male-female conflicts in close relationships. Regarding
the large gender difference in masturbation, women having difficulty reaching
orgasm during intercourse—also known as anorgasmia (Andersen, 1981; Hyde,
1994)—is sometimes attributed to a lack of experience in masturbation, though
other factors such as inept male sexual technique and guilt induced by patriarchal
socialization may also contribute. Hyde (1996) recommended sexual education that
contains specific instructions for female masturbation as part of the curriculum.
Feminists also argue that the large gender difference in attitudes about casual
sex sets the stage for male-female conflict, including sexual harassment and date
rape. Gender role socialization, including the sexual double standard (i.e., casual
sex is fine for men but not for women), defines social norms and expectations for
male and female sexual behavior. These differences are rooted in men having
more power than women, both physically and institutionally. According to this
line of thought, if the power difference could be eliminated, then the difference in
attitudes toward casual sex would vanish also. Clearly, this view is fundamentally
opposed to evolutionary and biological approaches such as that of Buss and
Schmitt (1993), who propose that the different attitudes toward casual sex are
innately different and inextricably linked to the different reproductive strategies
and the biological constraints on the sex organs. (That is, having sex with more
partners increases the likely number of offspring for men but not for women.) In
the field of sex, evolutionary theory arose in part as a reaction against feminist and
constructionist thinking. The next section turns to evolutionary theory.
Evolutionary Theory
Some of the earliest thinkers in modern psychology (e.g., James, 1890/
1950) thought that an evolutionary perspective was essential for a full theory of
human social behavior. This sentiment was largely ignored, however, for the
22 Sexuality Today
better part of the twentieth century. Theorists did not start actively importing
ideas from evolutionary theory until about the late 1960s. When this theory did
start to become integrated into theories of human behavior, it did so to the
greatest extent in the domain of sexuality. This should come as no great sur-
prise—classic Darwinian theory (e.g., 1859/1964, 1871) implies that differential
reproductive success is the key to biological evolution. Sexuality was therefore a
natural focus for evolutionarily minded scientists, because mating is central to
reproductive success. Nevertheless, many evolutionary researchers found them-
selves accused of being obsessed with sex.
When an evolutionary approach to sexuality started becoming popular, many
people were uncomfortable with its implications. To some, such theories took
people’s sexual identities out of their own hands and put them into the hands of
their genes. Many were distressed by the idea that their sex lives—including their
most intimate feelings and desires—were determined not by their own hearts and
minds, but by human ancestors who had been dead for hundreds of thousands of
years. In suggesting that much of human behavior is rooted in biology, evolu-
tionary theory was viewed as a cynical and even oppressive standpoint, as this
implied that many harmful behaviors could not be changed or avoided. Some
even viewed the evolutionary perspective as a strategic tool designed by the
patriarchy to maintain the sociopolitical status quo.
Evolutionary approaches to sexuality draw upon unifying principles of
biological adaptation and evolution by natural selection. The fundamental
premise is this: organisms possessing adaptive physical and psychological design
features tend to reproduce at a greater rate than organisms with less adaptive
features. As a result, these features—referred to as ‘‘adaptations’’—can become
characteristic of the species over evolutionary time.
Applied to sex, evolutionary theory seeks to understand the desires and be-
haviors of modern individuals as the result of ancestral (both human and prehu-
man) patterns that produced more and better offspring. Evolutionary theorists
contend that virtually all aspects of human mating and sexuality—from the
excitement of initial romantic attraction, to the day-to-day maintenance of a long-
term relationship, to the anger and distress experienced at a relationship’s break-
up—have been shaped, at least in part, by evolutionary processes (Cosmides &
Tooby, 1992; Maner et al., 2003). That is, they have been shaped by the mating-
related constraints under which ancestral men and women evolved.
Many studies show, for example, that men and women differ in the traits they
look for in their romantic partners, as well as in their willingness to engage in
casual sex. Whereas men tend to place a premium on the physical attractiveness
and youth of their partners, women favor partners with maturity and high social
status (Kenrick & Keefe, 1992; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002). Men
are generally quite willing to engage in casual sex, without any prospect of a long-
term relationship, whereas women are relatively more inclined to require some
level of commitment before agreeing to intercourse (Clarke & Hatfield, 1989;
Simpson & Gangestad, 1991).
Theories of Human Sexuality 23
Psychoanalytic Theory
Sexuality theory was heavily influenced by psychoanalytic theorizing during
the first part of the twentieth century, though this influence has progressively
diminished. Probably few researchers currently emphasize psychoanalytic theory
in their work, though occasional findings may seem relevant. Psychoanalytic
theory has its roots in the seminal thinking of Sigmund Freud (1905/1975).
The sex drive was regarded by Freud as one of the two main motivations
that underlie all human striving (the other being aggression). Freud interpreted
the sex drive very broadly, so as to encompass desires for love, affiliation, and
24 Sexuality Today
respect, marriage or other commitments, or, in some cases, cash. In the sexual
marketplace, women operate as sellers, and men as buyers.
The greater cultural value attached to female sexuality (than male sexuality)
is seen as creating a variety of patterns. Female virginity is more precious and
important than male virginity, and women may regard their virginity as a valued
gift they give to someone, whereas men do not regard their own virginity with
the same positive value. (If anything, some males regard it as a stigmatizing sign
of sociosexual incompetence; Sprecher & Regan, 1996). Female virgins are
regarded in many cultures as more desirable sex partners than nonvirgins,
whereas no such distinction exists for males. Female infidelity is prohibited and
punished more severely than male infidelity (e.g., Tannahill, 1980). Social
exchange theory sees this difference as stemming from the view that the un-
faithful wife is giving away something precious that belongs to the couple. In
contrast, the unfaithful male is not giving away something of value (unless the
sex is accompanied by other resources, such as if he spends the household’s
money on a mistress). Marriage is regarded as a contract in which the woman
contributes sex and the man contributes money and other resources. Hence,
when divorce criteria differ by gender, the woman’s but not the man’s refusal to
have sex is grounds for divorce, whereas the man’s but not the woman’s refusal
to provide money is grounds (Betzig, 1989). Laws such as those regarding
statutory rape are seen as necessary to protect female sexuality from men, but
protecting male sexuality from women is not regarded as worthy of legislation.
Social exchange theory emphasizes the development of a local sexual
marketplace with a more or less standard price for sex, as indicated in norms
that dictate how much money, time, commitment, or other resources a man
should invest in a relationship before the woman owes him sex. One principle
of economic theory is that sellers compete more than buyers (e.g., Becker,
1976), and this would be reflected in women not only seeking to ‘‘advertise’’
their wares with makeup and sexually attractive clothing, but also possibly
competing to offer sex at a slightly lower price than other women so as to
attract more or better quality male attention.
Seller competition may become especially acute when supply exceeds
demand, which in sexual terms entails that there are more eligible women than
men in a community. Studies of sex ratio have confirmed that when there are
more women than men, the price of sex goes down (so to speak), so that pre-
marital and extramarital sex become more common and the need for men to
invest extensive resources is reduced (see Guttentag & Secord, 1983). Such
things may happen after a major war, for example, in which many eligible young
men are killed. In contrast, a surplus of males relative to females corresponds to
demand exceeding supply, and such communities typically have restrictive or
prudish sexual norms that permit sex only when the man has invested and
committed a great deal, corresponding to a high price for sex.
Economic theory holds that sellers not only compete but also collude more
than buyers. In sex, this would entail women working together to manipulate
Theories of Human Sexuality 27
the price of sex. According to social exchange theory, this underlies traditional
patterns in many cultures by which girls and women are socialized to restrain
their sexual impulses and hold back from sexual activity. Although some fem-
inists and some evolutionary theorists have assumed that cultural constraints on
women’s sexuality stem from men’s attempts to control women, empirical
evidence overwhelmingly indicates that women rather than men are the prin-
cipal sources of pressure on women to restrain their sexuality (for review, see
Baumeister & Twenge, 2002). The social exchange theory proposes that a
rational strategy for women would be to work together to restrict the supply of
sex available to men, in order to drive up the price. ‘‘Cheap’’ women who offer
sex without demanding commitment or other resources in return undermine
the bargaining position of other women and are therefore punished by the
female community with ostracism, bad reputations, and other disincentives.
Sexual Desire
Feminist theory saw itself as liberating women from accumulated false
stereotypes. Some rebelled against the view that men desire sex more than
women do, proposing instead that women’s desire for sex is equal to and
perhaps greater than men’s (e.g., Sherfey, 1966; see also Hyde & DeLamater,
1997). However, a different tradition of feminist thought has emphasized the
view of sexual intercourse as inherently coercive. This would seemingly assume
that men want sex more than women do (which is why men would use
coercion), though it is possible for feminists to propose that men coerce women
for political reasons, so equal desire could still result in unequal coercion.
In contrast, evolutionary theory depicts male desire for short-term sex as
stronger than that of female (and empirical evidence overwhelmingly supports
this; for review, see Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Oliver & Hyde, 1993). In evo-
lutionary perspective, this is because males (unlike females) can reproduce
more if they have sex with a greater number of women. Moreover, males must
work hard and take risks to get sex (given widespread female reluctance), and
so a low sex drive might leave them disinclined to do so. Human females can
28 Sexuality Today
normally have only one baby per year, and not much sex is required to
accomplish that, so there is no biological payoff for high sex drive.
The social exchange theory is based partly on the assumption of greater
male sex drive in general, which research findings also support (Baumeister,
Catanese, & Vohs, 2001). According to the theory, this is why men will offer
women other resources to induce them to have sex. If women wanted sex as
much as or more than men did, the basis for the economy might be undercut.
That gender differences in desire for short-term sex will produce different
patterns of sexual decision making has recently been proposed by Haselton and
Buss (2000; see also Maner et al., 2005). Their ‘‘error management theory’’ is a
midlevel theory though nominally linked to evolutionary theory. It proposes
that men and women seek to minimize the more costly type of error. Men seek
to minimize the chances of missing out on sexual opportunities, so they pursue
all chances and interpret ambiguous behavior by women (such as smiling) as
indications of sexual interest. Women, in contrast, seek to minimize the chances
of having sex with an unreliable or genetically substandard mate, and so they
avoid or refuse sexual opportunities and require suitors to furnish ample signs of
suitability before engaging in sex.
Theories about prostitution and pornography are shaped by views of sexual
desire. Clearly, prostitution and pornography appeal more to men than to
women. To the social exchange theorist, these are low-cost substitutes that cater
to the excess male desire that women themselves refuse to satisfy. In contrast,
feminist theory assumes equality of desire, and so the greater male interest in
prostitution and pornography must be attributed to other, nonsexual motives,
such as the wish to degrade and exploit women. (For example, Dworkin, 1981,
concluded that ‘‘[p]ornography is a celebration of rape and injury to women.’’)
Interpreting male interest in pornography and prostitution as an exploitative
political strategy has been a contentious feminist stance that has fueled hostile
confrontations between men and women.
Rape
Although recent evidence indicates that women occasionally coerce men
into unwanted sexual activity (e.g., Anderson & Struckman-Johnson, 1998) and
that homosexual coercion also occurs, the bulk of theorizing has focused on
explaining why men rape women. In our view, the major grand perspectives
have mostly offered sometimes contentious and mostly unhelpful theorizing, and
so the development of midlevel theories to explain rape specifically is needed.
Evolutionary theory has proposed that men rape women because it is one
biological strategy for passing one’s genes into the next generation (Thornhill &
Palmer, 2000). In nature, and presumably in human prehistory, most females
mated with a few high-status males, and so the other males were left out.
Forcing sex on women was the only way that these lower-status males could
reproduce, and so males who were sexually aggressive would have been more
Theories of Human Sexuality 29
likely to pass on their genes than nonaggressive males (which is why, presum-
ably, some genetic impulse to rape remains today). Though plausible, this theory
runs far beyond the available evidence (as Thornhill and Palmer acknowledge)
and leaves unanswered many specific questions in predicting rape.
Feminist theory depicts rape as reflecting the general pattern of male violence
toward and oppression of women. A landmark feminist analysis by Brownmiller
(1975) asserted, first, that rape reflects a conspiracy by all men (implying that even
nonrapist men support rape) to intimidate and subjugate all women. She even
claimed that men are socialized to rape. Second, the analysis insisted that the
driving force behind rape is power rather than sex. Although a large amount of
empirical evidence has discredited this view point by point (for reviews, see
Felson, 2002; Palmer, 1988; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994), it remains popular with
many feminists, especially those who regard quantitative data collection as merely
another male tool to undercut subjective experience and oppress women. In
fairness, the feminist view that rape is about power rather than sex may be an
accurate depiction of the female victim’s experience. Moreover, the feminist
methodological insistence that people cannot fully understand the subjectivity of
others in different roles entails that female theorists could not possibly understand
the motives or actions of male rapists anyway.
Various other theories have begun to be put forward to explain rape. One
early view emphasized low social skills, proposing that men who could not
obtain sex by charm would resort to rape, but it has been contradicted by actual
studies of rapists, which show that they do have skills and in fact often manage to
have a higher amount of consensual sex than other men (e.g., Kanin, 1985). A
newer theory by Malamuth (1996) is derived from observations of sexually
coercive men, and proposes that a combination of hostile masculinity (mascu-
line personality traits plus negative attitudes toward women), grievance (sense of
having been victimized by women), and a view of heterosexual relations as
inherently antagonistic, if not downright exploitative, is what predisposes some
men toward rape. Malamuth’s approach is admirably cautious in its scope and
carefully grounded in systematic observations.
Using a similar, empirically based approach, Baumeister, Catanese, and
Wallace (2002) proposed a narcissistic reactance theory of rape. Reactance theory
(Brehm, 1966) proposes that people respond to loss of options by trying to reassert
those options, and in regard to sex, some men may respond to a female’s refusal of
sex (especially when the man regards the refusal as unexpected or illegitimate) by
using force to obtain sex. Narcissistic men, who overestimate their entitlements
and are comfortable exploiting others to satisfy their own needs, may be most
prone to make that kind of cognitive distortion.
Homosexuality
Explaining homosexuality is a difficult challenge for most theories. Evo-
lutionary theory explains modern sexual desire as based on what patterns of
30 Sexuality Today
CONCLUSION
The vagaries of why people enjoy sex, why they enjoy variety, why they
make good and bad decisions about sex, and further questions continue to
preoccupy the general public as well as a broad realm of thinkers, but most of this
thinking remains at the amateur level. The extensive discussion of sexuality
Theories of Human Sexuality 31
indicates that sex remains an interesting topic, and in the long run, one can
expect theorizing about sex to continue and even become better.
Offering an exhaustive account of sexual theories would require a tome
written by a team of experts, and even they might not have gotten them all. Our
project has been to summarize what we see as the major grand perspectives and
provide a smattering of more focused, midlevel theories about sexual behavior.
Recent decades have seen progress in both, though perhaps not as much as
might have been. Our opinion is that the grand theories should be retained but
de-emphasized, and especially the hostility between their adherents should not
be permitted to restrict the development of midlevel theorizing. That is, the
field of sex research would benefit if young and new researchers were en-
couraged to develop midlevel theories without having to declare allegiance to a
grand perspective or justify themselves to devotees of those great camps.
Freudian theory has been criticized for being notoriously elastic and hence
resistant to empirical disconfirmation. We think social constructionist, evolu-
tionary, and probably sexual exchange theories are likewise flexible, and so they
may be more useful as explanatory frameworks than as sources of competing,
testable, and falsifiable hypotheses. The next decade should concentrate on the
cultivation of empirically informed midlevel theories, and once several of those
have been refined and honed against the sharp edge of data, only then will it
be fruitful to revisit the clash of grand perspectives.
REFERENCES
Adams, H. E., Wright, L. W., & Lohr, B. A. (1996). Is homophobia associated
with homosexual arousal? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 440–445.
Andersen, B. L. (1981). A comparison of systematic desensitization and directed
masturbation in the treatment of primary orgasmic dysfunction in females.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 568–570.
Anderson, P. B., & Struckman-Johnson, C. (Eds.). (1998). Sexually aggressive
women: Current perspectives and controversies. New York: Guilford.
Arafat, I. S., & Yorburg, B. (1973). On living together without marriage. Journal
of Sex Research, 13, 97–106.
Bauman, K. E., & Wilson, R. R. (1974). Sexual behavior of unmarried university
students in 1968 and 1972. Journal of Sex Research, 10, 327–333.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a gender
difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual dis-
tinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 5, 242–273.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Wallace, H. M. (2002). Conquest by force: A
narcissistic reactance theory of rape and sexual coercion. Review of General
Psychology, 6, 92–135.
Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2002). Cultural suppression of female
sexuality. Review of General Psychology, 6, 166–203.
32 Sexuality Today
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Sexual economics: Sex as female re-
source for social exchange in heterosexual interactions. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 8, 339–363.
Becker, G. S. (1976). The economic approach to human behavior. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Bem, D. J. (1996). Exotic becomes erotic: A developmental theory of sexual
orientation. Psychological Review, 103, 320–335.
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality: A treatise in
the sociology of knowledge. Garden City, NJ: Anchor.
Betzig, L. (1989). Causes of conjugal dissolution: A cross-cultural study. Current
Anthropology, 30, 654–676.
Birenbaum, A. (1970). Revolution without the revolution: Sex in contemporary
America. Journal of Sex Research, 6, 257–267.
Blau, P. N. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.
Bobrow, D., & Bailey, M. (2001). Is homosexuality maintained via kin selection?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 361–368.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.
Brown, N. O. (1966). Love’s body. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: Men, women, and rape. New York: Simon
and Schuster.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.
Clarke, R. D., & Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual
offers. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 2, 39–55.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In
J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 163–
228). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cott, N. (1978). Passionlessness: An interpretation of Victorian sexual ideology.
Signs, 4, 219–236.
Darwin, C. (1859/1964). On the origin of species. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
Dworkin, A. (1981). Pornography: Men possessing women. London: The Women’s Press.
Ehrenreich, B., Hess, E., & Jacobs, G. (1986). Re-making love: The feminization of
sex. Garden City, NJ: Doubleday Anchor.
Felson, R. B. (2002). Violence and gender reexamined. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Freud, S. (1905/1975). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-
offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.
Guttentag, M., & Secord, P. F. (1983). Too many women? The sex ratio question.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Haselton, M., & Buss, D. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on
biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
81–91.
Theories of Human Sexuality 33
Homans, G. C. (1950). The human group. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World.
Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: Harcourt,
Brace, & World.
Hyde, J. S. (1994). Understanding human sexuality (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Hyde, J. S. (1996). Where are the gender differences? Where are the gender
similarities? In D. M. Buss & N. M. Malamuth (Eds.), Sex, power, conflict:
Evolutionary and feminist perspectives (pp. 107–118). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Hyde, J. S., & DeLamater, J. D. (1997). Understanding human sexuality (6th ed.).
New York: McGraw-Hill.
James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.
Kanin, E. J. (1985). Date rapists: Differential sexual socialization and relative
deprivation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 219–231.
Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences
in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 75–133.
Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., Butner, J., Li, N. P., Becker, D. V., & Schaller, M.
(2002). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Mapping the domains of the new
interactionist paradigm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 347–356.
Krebs, D. L. (2003). Fictions and facts about evolutionary approaches to human
behavior: Comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological
Bulletin, 129, 842–847.
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The
necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the trade-offs. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947–955.
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a bi-
ologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
819–835.
Malamuth, N. M. (1996). The confluence model of sexual aggression: Feminist
and evolutionary perspectives. In D. Buss & N. Malamuth (Eds.), Sex,
power, conflict (pp. 269–295). New York: Oxford University Press.
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C.,
et al. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: Beauty captures the mind of the
beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1107–1120.
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T., Hofer, B., Neuberg, S.,
et al. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social motives can
bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,
63–78.
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: William Morrow.
Mead, M. (1961). Cultural determinants of sexual behavior. In W. C. Young (Ed.),
Sex and internal secretions (pp. 1433–1479). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.
Palmer, C. T. (1988). Twelve reasons why rape is not sexually motivated: A
skeptical examination. Journal of Sex Research, 25, 512–530.
34 Sexuality Today
Sexuality in Childhood
not until there was a heightened awareness of sexual abuse (and its impact),
brought on largely by the institution of mandatory reporting laws over thirty
years ago, that the need for data on the normal bounds of childhood sexual
behavior was highlighted to the research community (Heiman, Leiblum, Es-
quilin, & Pallitto, 1998). Thus, it appears that researchers, professionals, and
others made decisions about what was ‘‘abnormal’’ without first identifying and
understanding what was ‘‘normal.’’ Nonetheless, the need for information on
normal sexual development has been recognized, and some investigations have
begun to shed light on child sexuality.
One problem with the limited research in this field is that much of the work
draws on findings from anecdotes or case studies. Because they may not apply to
the majority of children, these methodologies are considered weak for the
purpose of providing a representative picture of the normal course of sexual
development. Furthermore, some researchers relying on vignettes or individual
experiences have made claims that are difficult to validate, couching ‘‘results’’ as
normative without evidence that the descriptions are indeed typical. Admit-
tedly, there are numerous impediments to conducting well-controlled research
in this area. First, parents are often considered the most qualified reporters
regarding children’s daily behaviors, yet sexuality involves private behaviors,
many of which parents may not have observed. Even when children are old
enough to offer self-reports, the private nature of sexuality may make them
hesitant to report on such behavior. There are also ethical constraints regarding
the nature of research that can be done with children, particularly when con-
sidering the societal taboo surrounding sexuality in many cultures.
This cultural bias may also contribute to difficulty in gathering sound data in
that survey respondents may balk at the use of the term ‘‘sexual’’ to describe
behaviors engaged in by infants and children, particularly within societies that
are less open regarding sexuality (e.g., the United States). While some re-
searchers and theorists (e.g., Friedrich et al., 1998; Martinson, 1997) have noted
that it is possible to describe aspects of children’s sexual behavior using categories
with a corresponding adult behavior or presentation (including self-stimulation,
sexual interest, exhibitionism, sexual knowledge, and voyeuristic behaviors),
there are some key points that differentiate the presentation of such behaviors in
children versus adults. It is possible that emphasizing these differences may make
the larger discussion of normative childhood sexuality more palatable and, thus,
more possible within a greater variety of cultures. Toward this end, Heiman et al.
(1998) have suggested that discussion and investigation of child sexuality might
be freed if researchers modify the description of the behaviors under study from
‘‘sexual’’ to ‘‘self-exploratory.’’
We would further suggest that there is a meaningful difference between
behaviors motivated by curiosity or ‘‘scientific’’ interest and those driven by
sexual interest and, importantly, between behaviors that are pleasurable and those
that are sexual. Put another way, although some of the behaviors in which young
children engage involve their genitalia, they are not necessarily sexual in nature.
Sexuality in Childhood 37
A second useful distinction between adult and child sexuality relates to the fact
that an individual’s intentions and cognitions serve an important function in
determining whether a behavior is sexual per se (Gordon & Schroeder, 1995).
For instance, while numerous parental and professional reports describe infants,
toddlers, and preschoolers touching their own genitals, this self-stimulation is
qualitatively different in meaning, intention, and experience than similar be-
haviors would be later in the developmental span. For this reason, and as
mentioned at the opening of this chapter, we believe that childhood sexuality is
more usefully described as the foundation for, rather than an early sign of, adult
sexuality.
With appreciation for the still evolving state of the field’s current knowl-
edge base regarding child sexuality, this chapter seeks to (a) provide an
overview of theoretical frameworks that may prove useful in considering the
processes and experiences that underlie the development of child sexuality, (b)
summarize current knowledge regarding childhood sexuality from birth to 12
years of age, (c) consider briefly cross-cultural findings to clarify cultural dif-
ferences in youngsters’ sexual behavior, (d) describe selected nonnormative
child sexual experiences and outcomes, and (e) discuss recommendations for
future research in this area.
THEORIES RELATING TO
CHILDHOOD SEXUALITY
In considering developmental theories of childhood sexuality, the ideas of
Sigmund Freud present a natural starting point. Freud’s psychosexual theory is,
in fact, the only developmental theory that explicitly names sexuality as a central
force in driving human growth and behavior. In brief, Freud argued that
children pass through five distinct stages of development (oral, anal, phallic,
latency, and genital), each of which centers around a specific area of the body
that is most sensitive to excitation and sensual pleasure and thus serves as the
primary source of satisfaction and gratification. Successful navigation of each
stage involves the child being allowed to experience and explore freely the
sources of pleasure attached to each phase while carefully balancing desires/
drives (‘‘id’’), what the environment will provide or allow, that is, the constraints
of reality (‘‘ego’’), and the judgments and limitations enforced by social and
societal boundaries and mores (‘‘superego’’). Failure to navigate one of the
phases of psychosexual development (e.g., by obtaining too much or too lit-
tle gratification) would, according to Freud, result in fixation at a particular
stage. In Freud’s view, this fixation could very much influence personality traits
and even take the form of various psychological problems, but the key element
in Freud’s thinking is that a child’s ability to master sensual pleasure serves as the
foundation for growth and development (see Freud & Gay, 1995, for more on
Freud’s theory and writings).
38 Sexuality Today
Distal Influences
National Customs (e.g.,
marital customs; rites of passage)
School Systems Religious Settings
(e.g., sex education
curricula)
Proximal Influences (e.g., teachings and norms
regarding sexuality)
Parents (e.g., family
Peers (e.g., norms; attitudes & openness
pressures; opportunities regarding sexuality)
for experimentation)
Physiological Development
Social Development
Individual Child
Emotional Development
Cognitive Development
Siblings (e.g., norms; Teachers (e.g.,
exposure to different attitudes & tolerance
age and/or gender in classroom; direct
Mass Media (e.g., experiences) teaching) Popular Culture
media portrayals of (e.g., image and clothing
sexuality and sexual styles; acceptance of sexual
norms/roles) content in music and
dance trends)
Cultural Values (e.g., definitions of and
limits to acceptable sexual behavior; laws
regarding age of consent)
found a correlation between openness within the home regarding sexuality and
reported child sexual behavior. Other researchers have replicated this finding,
identifying an association between an index of family sexuality (including items
such as seeing nude adults in the home, bathing with adults, having witnessed
intercourse, having nude pictures available in the home, and seeing nudity on
television) and sexual behavior scores for youngsters in both U.S. and Swedish
samples (Larsson et al., 2000). Although this finding may simply reflect the fact
that parents who promote more sexual openness are also more open in reporting
their child’s sexual behavior, it is also true that a child’s behavior is influenced
by, and indicative of, the environment in which he or she is raised (Friedrich
et al., 1998). Through modeling and the reactions and responses of caregivers
and other adults to their acts, statements, and questions, children learn to shape
their behaviors, understand which behaviors are viewed as acceptable or not
(and which are more appropriate for private settings), and identify the proper
language for describing their thoughts, impulses, and behavior (Friedrich &
Trane, 2002).
Similarly, the child’s peers may play a role in the development and ex-
pression of sexual behavior in childhood. Specifically, peers have been found to
exert a strong influence on defining norms for each other regarding sexual
behavior during preadolescence (Kinsman, Romer, Furstenburg, & Schwarz,
1998), and these influences appear to be stronger than parental social influence
in this age-group (Beal, Ausiello, & Perrin, 2001; see also Kinsman, Nyanzi, &
Pool, 2000, for an interesting exploration of this idea with an African sample).
Additionally, Haugaard and Tilly (1988) found that those undergraduates who
made retrospective reports of having experienced a childhood sexual encounter
also reported having had more friends as a child, and were more likely to have
had a friend in whom they could confide. These findings suggest that the
presence of close peers may facilitate childhood sexual exploration by presenting
a safe and trusted partner with whom to experiment (Haugaard & Tilly). Peer
socialization has also been hypothesized to affect children as young as preschool-
age; Friedrich et al. (1998) found that the number of hours spent in day care
positively correlated with children’s reported sexual behavior. The suggestion is
that greater exposure to children from families with other values and experi-
ences may lead to a greater variety of learned sexual behaviors among even very
young children.
More distal factors can also have important influences on a child’s devel-
oping sexuality. For example, ecological systems theory would specifically con-
sider the impact of school programming in the form of sex education on the
child’s developing sexuality. One theory to explain the cross-cultural differences
noted particularly between youngsters growing up in Scandinavian countries
and in the United States is that the widespread acceptance and encourage-
ment of sex education in Scandinavia and other Western European countries
has a long-standing history, whereas federal standards regarding such educa-
tion have a more mixed record in the United States (Goldman & Goldman,
42 Sexuality Today
1988). For example, the United States has gone from having no federally
mandated sex education prior to the 1980s, to the surgeon general calling for sex
education as early as the third grade in 1986, at the height of the AIDS crisis
(Donovan, 1998). At the time of the writing of this chapter, the debate within
the United States regarding sex education centered around whether the current
‘‘abstinence-only’’ approach to sex education favored by the federal govern-
ment should be changed to an ‘‘abstinence plus information on contraception’’
approach, despite the fact that the United States has a teenage pregnancy rate
that is among the highest of any industrialized nation (Singh & Darroch, 2000).
As an example of the effect of the educational ecology on children’s sexual
behavior, the European approach to sexuality education and contraception
availability for teenagers has been credited as a central factor in contributing to
more rapidly decreasing rates of teenage pregnancy in Western European coun-
tries as compared to the United States (Furstenberg, 1998).
Another example of a community influence on the development of chil-
dren’s sexuality that has been noted in the research is the impact of religiosity on
sexual behavior and practices. In a retrospective study of childhood sexuality,
Haugaard and Tilly (1988) found that in the case of respondents who reported
having had a sexual encounter in childhood, the strength of religion in their
childhood family was slightly lower than in the case of those respondents who
did not report having had a childhood sexual encounter. Of course, this finding
could possibly be explained by the emphasis on religion in the childhood family
leading to an adult respondent being less likely to report on childhood expe-
riences that involved sexuality, due to shame or guilt. In either case, the point is
that religiosity or other cultural frameworks that dictate family attitudes toward
sexuality are likely to have an impact on the behaviors a child may engage in or
be willing to share through reporting.
Larger sociocultural influences also considered by ecological systems theory
include the cultural environment in which the communities and families of
developing children reside and function. As described by Frayser (1994, p. 180),
‘‘[C]ultural ideas and beliefs can shape the meaning of sexual behavior and
define the participants in and limits to that behavior.’’ These cultural messages
may exist in the form of laws such as the age at which an individual is able to give
consent for participation in intercourse, cultural taboos against incest, and
cultural beliefs regarding the age at which children enter adulthood and are
ready for sexual initiation and marriage. Cultural messages may also take less
organized but equally influential forms, such as how media portrayals treat
sexuality and depict gender roles (Ryan, 2000). Ecological theory proposes that
these cultural messages are created by communities and influence what messages
those communities send to families and children regarding sexuality. Not only
are cultural messages regarding sexual behavior available to children through
what communities and families teach directly, but they have also become in-
creasingly available through the television, video, and Internet technologies that
are ubiquitous in most industrialized nations. It should also be noted that cultural
Sexuality in Childhood 43
attitudes are not always directly reflected in media expressions of sexuality. For
example, the disconnect between the open display of eroticism in the American
media and that same society’s reluctance to speak openly about private sexual
practices has been noted (e.g., Heiman et al., 1998). The effect this mismatch
between purported values and media expression has on children’s developing
sexuality is unclear; however, there is no doubt that cultural messages regard-
ing sexuality are important in shaping children’s attitudes toward their devel-
oping sexual selves (Frayser, 1994; Goldman & Goldman, 1988; Gordon &
Schroeder, 1995; Martinson, 1997).
The transactional influences specified by the ecological systems model
described above were presented as a means of organizing the various fac-
tors that contribute to the normal development of sexuality in childhood. This
model should be kept in mind as we move forward to outline what is currently
known.
more abnormal than did males (Heiman et al.). Similarly, Lamb and Coakley
(1993) found in a retrospective study of female undergraduates that those who
came from a ‘‘restrictive’’ versus an ‘‘open’’ home were more likely to rate
their own childhood sexual encounters as ‘‘not normal.’’ These interpretations
likely impact the reporting of childhood sexual behavior and thus limit the
currently available data.
Recognizing the difficulty in defining normal sexual behavior and the
various issues associated with reporting bias, it is safe to say that research on
normative child sexual development is in its infancy. What follows is a de-
scription of some of the prominent behaviors and processes that are currently
thought to constitute normative aspects of childhood sexual development across
major age-groups. It is important to note that the findings described below are
based on two main sources of information: (1) reports by parents or caretakers
based on observed sexual behavior among children, and (2) retrospective reports
by adults (usually undergraduate students) regarding their own childhood sexual
experiences. Although not directly represented in the current section, data from
studies of children who have been brought in for treatment due to concerns
about age-inappropriate, sexualized behavior have also yielded information on
normal childhood sexuality, if only by offering exclusionary criteria and com-
parisons. These data are touched upon in a separate section of this chapter and
more fully considered in a later volume in this set. Rather than providing
definitive answers, the information below is meant to provide a starting point
for understanding the normative development of sexuality among children
across different age-groups—a knowledge base that is not yet well developed.
exploration takes the form of fingering, pleasurable handling, and random ex-
ploration and has been noted to disappear in female infants within a few weeks
of onset. Male infants, on the other hand, have been noted to continue this
behavior as casual play (Martinson, 1997). It should be noted, however, that this
self-stimulation is considered to be exploratory and pleasurable rather than
masturbatory or sexual in nature (McAnulty & Burnette, 2004). In support of
this idea, Martinson (1997, p. 44) notes that ‘‘the average infant is not innately
motivated and lacks the muscular capacity for the degree of self-stimulation
necessary to produce orgasm.’’
From an anthropologic perspective, Fisher (1989) notes that an important
goal of the bodily exploration in which infants engage is to create a map of the
body, to which the child will later add significance through transaction with the
ecological systems surrounding him or her. This body mapping begins by ex-
ploring and then labeling the parts of the body, including genitals. Parents or
caretakers have an important role in the child’s developing sexuality at this
point, as they can either help the child to correctly label or choose to ignore the
child’s bid for information regarding genitalia. It has been argued that as the
infant makes connections between his or her awareness of the body and how
adults respond to that awareness, the foundation is being laid for the child’s
attitude toward his or her body, gender, and sexuality (Frayser, 1994).
As the infant develops further into toddlerhood, genital stimulation takes on
a slightly more purposeful note and appears to involve, in addition to manual
manipulation of the genitals, the use of objects with which to rub genitals
(Levine, 1957). There is also some suggestion that toddlerhood marks the
psychological awareness of the genitals and that the sexual behaviors displayed
during this period are accompanied by signs of pleasure (Roiphe & Galenson,
1981). In one of the only recent studies of normative child sexual behavior to
include toddler-age children, Friedrich et al. (1998) found that 2-year-old
children were reported by parents to be relatively sexual (as compared to 10- to
12-year-olds), but that the sexual behaviors displayed by the young children fell
within the category of self-stimulation, exhibitionism (i.e., displaying one’s
genitals), and a lack of personal boundaries rather than behaviors directed to-
ward another individual with sexual intent. Taken together, the available evi-
dence suggests that sexuality in infancy and toddlerhood can best be charac-
terized as prompted by curiosity about one’s own and others’ bodies.
that certain behaviors may result from sexual abuse or victimization (e.g., see
Lindblad, Gustafsson, Larsson, & Lundin, 1995). Such work underscores the
clear need to better understand the range of behaviors that may be charac-
terized as ‘‘normal.’’
Although retrospective self-reports have been utilized, more recent studies of
preschoolers’ sexual behavior have generally involved reporters representing two
major domains of a child’s functioning—parents (home) and teachers (preschool
or day care)—with parental reports being the most widely used method (Frie-
drich et al., 1998). Overall, early studies suggested that high frequency behaviors
included self-stimulating behaviors, attempting to look at people when nude or
undressing, and observing the genitals of other children; other more intrusive
behaviors, such as masturbating with an object, inserting an object into one’s
vagina or rectum, or touching an adult’s genitals, were considerably more un-
common (see, e.g., Friedrich, Grambsch, Broughton, Kuiper, & Bielke, 1991;
Larsson et al., 2000; Lindblad et al., 1995).
In a study that included 574 2- to 5-year-old American children, parents
reported that over 60 percent of boys and nearly 45 percent of girls touched
their genitals at home, the most commonly reported behavior in this sample
(Friedrich et al., 1998). Other common behaviors for both sexes (reported in
over 20 percent of children) included standing too close to people, touching or
trying to touch their mother’s or other women’s breasts, and trying to look at
people when they are nude or undressing; touching sex parts in public was also
a relatively common behavior among boys, reported for 26.5 percent. Be-
haviors far more rare in this age-group included putting one’s mouth on sex
parts, trying to have intercourse, asking others to engage in sexual acts, and
pretending toys are having sex, all of which were reported in less than 2
percent of both boys and girls.
A comparative study of sexual behavior in preschoolers in the United
States found similar results among the 467 3- to 6-year-olds in the sample
(Larsson et al., 2000). Parents reported several common behaviors for both
boys and girls, including trying to look at people undressing or nude, being shy
about undressing, walking around the house without clothes, touching or
trying to touch their mother’s or other women’s breasts, and touching genitals
at home. Over one-fifth of girls pretended to be the opposite sex, and boys
tended to more commonly touch their genitals in public, show their genitals to
adults, and masturbate with their hands. Rare behaviors for both sexes (re-
ported in 2 percent or less) included asking to watch sexually explicit televi-
sion, asking others to engage in sexual acts, and masturbating with an object;
boys also rarely imitated sexual behavior with dolls and put their tongues in
others’ mouths when kissing.
As might be expected, preschool-age youngsters appear more inclined to
engage in sexual behaviors and/or explore sexuality at home than in more
structured settings, such as day care or preschool, which generally involve
higher levels of monitoring and more specific behavioral rules (see, e.g., Larsson
Sexuality in Childhood 47
& Svedin, 2002b). These studies, conducted in Sweden, have suggested that
children of both sexes more often walked around indoors without clothes,
talked about sex, showed genitals to adults within the family, masturbated or
touched their own genitals, and tried to touch other children’s genitalia when at
home than when at the center. Girls were also significantly more likely to use
sexual vocabulary at home, as well as show their genitals to other children, and
pretend to be the opposite sex when playing. Larsson and Svedin note that some
data suggest that boys do not change their behavior as much between settings,
perhaps reflecting socialization pressures on girls. These same researchers denote
multiple behaviors common to both home and day care in their sample, in-
cluding trying to look at other children’s genitals, exposure of body and genitals
to peers, trying to look at people undressing, and playing ‘‘doctor’’ and other
games (e.g., playing ‘‘house,’’ including giving birth). More explicit sexual
games were very much unusual in both environments studied (Larsson &
Svedin, 2002b). This latter finding is supported by the research of Lindblad et al.
(1995), who reported that behaviors such as attempting to make an adult touch
the child’s genitals, using objects against one’s own/other children’s genitals/
anus, and compulsive masturbation, seemingly without pleasure or to the point
that it appeared to cause pain, were very uncommon (reported in 1 percent
of youngsters or fewer) in a sample of 251 Swedish preschoolers in day care
centers.
Thus, it appears that, as with infants and toddlers, many of the more
common behaviors exhibited by preschool-age children fall into two broad
categories: (1) self-stimulation and (2) sexual curiosity or exploratory play and
behavior. Although the former behavior appears to become less random or
accidental as children get older, it is important to keep in mind that it is not
necessarily sexual in nature and meaning for preschool-age youngsters. For
instance, some authors have reported observations or findings that various
means of self-stimulation may serve to release tension or may be associated with
other feelings, such as security (see, e.g., Martinson, 1994). Researchers and
theorists have also described examples of behavior falling in the second category
(i.e., sexual curiosity or exploratory play and behavior), labeling some of them as
sexual ‘‘rehearsal’’ play. Among preschoolers, this sexual rehearsal play typically
involves exposing themselves to each other and touching one another’s genitals,
including children of the same and opposite sex (Friedrich et al., 1998; Mar-
tinson, 1994), and there is some suggestion that there is more interest in boys’
genitals (McAnulty & Burnette, 2004). These often spontaneous behaviors are
generally driven by attempts to satisfy curiosity, as opposed to being fueled by
sexual interest (e.g., Eisenberg, Murkoff, & Hathaway, 1996), and will manifest
in mutual exploration activities such as ‘‘playing doctor/nurse’’ or ‘‘I’ll show you
mine, if you show me yours.’’
It is difficult to estimate the rates and frequencies of these behaviors, for
obvious reasons. However, despite their methodological limitations, some
retrospective studies have yielded data regarding sexual rehearsal play and related
48 Sexuality Today
also noted that secrecy and giggling were regularly observed in restrooms and on
the playground.
Another study used parental reports of the sexual behaviors of 362 6- to 9-
year-old children and identified two behaviors common among both boys and
girls (Friedrich et al., 1998). First, as with preschoolers, touching genitals at
home was the most frequent behavior reported in this age-group, endorsed by
parents for 40 percent of boys and 21 percent of girls. Trying to look at people
when they are undressing or nude was the next most common behavior,
reported in 20 percent of boys and 21 percent of girls. Several behaviors were
rarely endorsed for either sex in this sample, including putting objects in the
vagina/rectum, putting mouth on genitals, trying to have intercourse, asking
others to do sex acts, undressing other children, and kissing other children.
The researchers noted that items relating to sexually intrusive acts (e.g.,
touching their mother’s or other women’s breasts) or self-stimulating behaviors
dropped off in observed frequency relative to the younger, preschool-age
children they also studied (Friedrich et al., 1998). In fact, they found that the
frequency of parent-reported sexual behavior seemed to peak at age 5 and
decrease in the years that followed. Importantly, however, they also noted the
likelihood that parents are not as aware of their child’s behavior at these ages as
they may have been at earlier stages when, for example, the child spent less time
with peers (Friedrich et al., 1998). Some children in this age band may also have
learned that some behaviors were viewed as private in their households and
others were perhaps not accepted. Given such socialization factors, they may
have been less overt with their behaviors.
In addition, data from retrospective reports of child sexual behavior indicate
fairly common engagement in self-stimulatory behaviors as well as sex rehearsal
play among school-age children (e.g., Martinson, 1994). In a study involving
those from the United States, Canada, England, Australia, and Sweden, 40 per-
cent recalled sex rehearsal play experiences between ages 6 and 9 (McAnulty &
Burnette, 2004). Among their sample of senior high school students, Larsson and
Svedin (2002a) found that, of the age ranges they studied, several mutual sexual
experiences occurred most frequently between 6 and 10 years, including showing
one’s genitals (28 percent of boys, 23 percent of girls), touching and exploring
genitals of the other child (17 percent of boys, 19 percent of girls), and the other
child touching the respondent’s genitals (17 percent of boys, 19 percent of girls).
Other commonly recalled behaviors included simulating intercourse and teasing
that involved using sex words, lifting skirts, or peeking in toilet stalls (Larsson &
Svedin, 2002a). In another retrospective study, 85 percent of the 128 American
female undergraduates surveyed recalled engaging in a childhood sexual game and
that the average age of occurrence of the sexual play was 7.5 years (Lamb &
Coakley, 1993). The most common experiences reported (29.6 percent) fell
within the category of sex rehearsal play (i.e., imitation of adult sex, love scenes,
commercialized sexuality, and coercive scenes), followed by playing ‘‘doctor,’’
another sexual rehearsal ‘‘game’’ (16.3 percent), exposure (15.3 percent), and
50 Sexuality Today
Parental reports of their preadolescent’s sexual behavior suggest that the most
frequent activity observed in both preadolescent boys and girls was being ‘‘very
interested in the opposite sex,’’ with 24.1 percent of boys and 28.7 percent of
girls reportedly displaying this interest (Friedrich et al., 1998). This observation
by parents matches other information regarding the development of crushes
on, and attachments to, individuals outside of the family during preadolescence
(Martinson, 1997).
With respect to more private sexual behaviors in preadolescence, how-
ever, the information that is available is often dependent on statistics gathered
decades ago. For example, Ramsey (as cited in Martinson, 1997) found that
Sexuality in Childhood 51
CROSS-CULTURAL DIFFERENCES:
A BRIEF CONSIDERATION
Several authors (see, e.g., Goldman & Goldman, 1982; Martinson, 1994)
have discussed differences in sexual knowledge and understanding (e.g., be-
ing able to describe intercourse) across cultures. For instance, Goldman and
Goldman (1982) interviewed 5- to 15-year-olds from Australia, Sweden,
Sexuality in Childhood 53
Canada, England, and the United States, and, in line with other findings in this
area, children and youth from the United States were the least well informed.
Although some theorists have raised the possibility that such differential
knowledge may influence behavior rates as well as what is viewed as normal in a
given context, cross-cultural comparisons of sexual behavior have been the
focus of little empirical research.
As one key exception, Larsson et al. (2000) used parental reports to examine
and compare the sexual behaviors of two samples of 3- to 6-year-old children, one
from Sweden and one from the United States (Minnesota). The researchers found
that exhibitionistic (e.g., walking around house without clothes), voyeuristic
(e.g., trying to look at people undressing), and touching behaviors were most
common in both samples. Overall, the preschool-age children from Sweden
evidenced higher rates of sexual behavior than those from the United States.
Among boys, significant differences were identified for fourteen of the twenty-
five behaviors assessed, with Swedish boys exhibiting higher levels for thirteen of
the measured behaviors. The differences were somewhat less pronounced among
girls; that is, reliable differences were detected on ten behaviors, with Swedish
girls displaying higher rates in nine cases. For boys, the behaviors with the largest
reported differences (with frequencies varying by 20 percentage points or more)
included walking around the house without clothes, talking about sexual acts,
using sexual words, touching or trying to touch their mother’s or other women’s
breasts, and touching private parts in public places. Swedish boys exceeded
American boys on all but the last behavior. For girls, similar large differences
occurred on three items, that is, touching or trying to touch their mother’s or
other women’s breasts, trying to look at others when they are nude or undressing,
and talking about sexual acts, all favoring the Swedish youngsters.
In offering interpretations of their findings, Larsson et al. (2000, p. 256)
noted that ‘‘American children are brought up in a more strict or cautious
atmosphere concerning sexual matters, and . . . Swedish children are brought up
in a more liberal atmosphere.’’ This possible mechanism of influence, reflecting
differences at the familial (e.g., attitudes and values regarding sexuality) as well as
sociocultural level (e.g., cultural differences in sexual attitudes, disparate ap-
proaches to sexual education), accords well with the tenets of the ecological
approach. The researchers also noted that a similar unpublished study comparing
Dutch and American preschoolers yielded findings consistent with their own,
with higher frequencies of sexual behavior reported in the Dutch sample
(Larsson et al.), although cross-cultural findings involving these two groups have
been mixed (e.g., Schoentjes, Deboutte, & Friedrich, 1999). Given that Dutch
society is among the most open and liberal about sexuality, reported differences
in sexual behavior between Dutch and American children may be viewed as
providing further support for the notion that larger cultural attitudes about
sexuality may influence the behaviors observed. Nevertheless, the literature base
is still quite limited in this area, both in number of studies and cultures involved,
with the available research largely focusing on Western cultures.
54 Sexuality Today
alized behaviors, and this symptom does not occur only in sexually abused
children (Deblinger, McLeer, Atkins, Ralphe, & Foa, 1989; Kendall-Tackett
et al., 1993). Friedrich et al. (Brilleslijper-Kater et al., 2004; Friedrich et al.,
2001) have noted that age-inappropriate sexual behaviors are also related to a
range of factors, including family sexuality, externalizing (i.e., acting out)
behavior problems, domestic violence, physical abuse, and life stress. Such
findings, particularly when considered in conjunction with the research in-
dicating that a wide range of behaviors occur in normative samples of children
(Friedrich et al., 1991; Friedrich et al., 1998), complicate efforts to identify
factors that discriminate between sexually abused children and other youngsters.
Thus it is critical, as Larsson and Svedin (2002b) emphasize, for professionals
working with children and responding to questions about, and reports of, child
sexual behavior(s) to be thorough in their information gathering and thoughtful
in their interpretation, examining the context in which the sexual behavior
occurs, identifying antecedents of the behavior, and noting distress that may be
present.
REFERENCES
Beal, A. C., Ausiello, J., & Perrin, J. M. (2001). Social influences on health-risk
behaviors among minority middle school students. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 28, 474–480.
Brilleslijper-Kater, S. N., Friedrich, W. N., & Corwin, D. L. (2004). Sexual
knowledge and emotional reaction as indicators of sexual abuse in young
children: Theory and research challenges. Child Abuse and Neglect, 28,
1007–1017.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human devel-
opment. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature
and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental
processes. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief) & R. M. Lerner (Ed.),
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development
(5th ed., pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley.
Browne, A., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). The impact of child sexual abuse: A review
of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66–77.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004). Youth risk behavior surveillance—
United States, 2003. Washington, DC: Author.
Damon, W. (Editor-in-Chief), & Lerner, R. M. (Ed.). (1998). Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed.). New
York: Wiley.
Deblinger, E., McLeer, S. V., Atkins, M. S., Ralphe, D., & Foa, E. (1989). Post-
traumatic stress in sexually abused, physically abused, and nonabused
children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 403–408.
Donovan, P. (1998). School-based sexuality education: The issues and challenges.
Family Planning Perspectives, 30, 188–193.
Eisenberg, A., Murkoff, H. E., & Hathaway, S. E. (1996). What to expect: The
toddler years. New York: Workman.
Finkelhor, D. (1990). Early and long-term effects of child sexual abuse: An update.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21, 325–330.
Fisher, H. E. (1989). Sexual images of the self: The psychology of erotic sensations and
illusions. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
58 Sexuality Today
Kinsman, J., Nyanzi, S., & Pool, R. (2000). Socializing influences and the value of
sex: The experience of adolescent school girls in rural Masaka, Uganda.
Culture, Health and Sexuality, 2, 151–166.
Kinsman, S. B., Romer, D., Furstenburg, F. F., & Schwarz, D. F. (1998). Early
sexual initiation: The role of peer norms. Pediatrics, 102, 1185–1192.
Lamb, S., & Coakley, M. (1993). ‘‘Normal’’ childhood sexual play and games:
Differentiating play from abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 515–526.
Larsson, I., & Svedin, C. G. (2002a). Sexual experiences in childhood: Young
adults’ recollections. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 263–274.
Larsson, I., & Svedin, C. G. (2002b). Teachers’ and parents’ reports on 3- to 6-
year-old children’s sexual behavior—A comparison. Child Abuse and Ne-
glect, 26, 247–266.
Larsson, I., Svedin, C. G., & Friedrich, W. N. (2000). Differences and similarities
in sexual behaviour among pre-schoolers in Sweden and USA. Nordic
Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 251–257.
Levine, M. I. (1957). Pediatric observations on masturbation in children. Psy-
choanalytic Study of the Child, 6, 117–124.
Lindblad, F., Gustafsson, P. A., Larsson, I., & Lundin, B. (1995). Preschoolers’
sexual behavior at daycare centers: An epidemiological study. Child Abuse
and Neglect, 19, 569–577.
Martinson, F. M. (1994). The sexual life of children. Westport, CT: Bergin &
Garvey.
Martinson, F. M. (1997). Sexual development in infancy and childhood. In
G. Ryan & S. Lane (Eds.), Juvenile sex offending: Causes, consequences, and
correction (pp. 36–58). Boston: Lexington Books.
McAnulty, R. M., & Burnette, M. M. (2004). Exploring human sexuality: Making
healthy decisions (2nd ed.). Boston: Pearson Education.
National Adolescent Perpetrator Network. (1988). Preliminary report from the
National Task Force on Juvenile Sexual Offending. Juvenile and Family
Court Journal, 39, 1–67.
National Adolescent Perpetrator Network. (1993). Revised report from the
National Task Force on Juvenile Sexual Offending. Juvenile and Family
Court Journal, 44, 1–120.
Putnam, F. W. (2003). Ten-year research update review: Child sexual abuse.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 269–
278.
Roiphe, H., & Galenson, E. (1981). Infantile origins of sexual identity. New York:
International Universities Press.
Ryan, G. (2000). Childhood sexuality: A decade of study. Part I—Research and
curriculum development. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 33–48.
Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum
of caretaking casualty. In F. D. Horowitz, M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-
Salapatek, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review of child development research (pp. 187–
244). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
60 Sexuality Today
Schoentjes, E., Deboutte, D., & Friedrich, W. N. (1999). Child sexual behav-
ior inventory: A Dutch-speaking normative sample. Pediatrics, 104, 885–
893.
Singh, S., & Darroch, J. E. (2000). Adolescent pregnancy and childbearing:
Levels and trends in developed countries. Family Planning Perspectives, 32,
14–23.
4
Adolescent Sexuality
CONCEPTUALIZATIONS OF ADOLESCENCE
Adolescence refers to the period of time that marks the transition from
childhood to adulthood. This span of a decade or more encompasses a period
of rapid physical development, the onset of puberty and consequent matura-
tion to full reproductive capacity, substantial social and cognitive develop-
ments, and the attainment of the rights and responsibilities of adulthood.
Few periods of development have been reified as adolescence has. Al-
though adolescence marks the transition from childhood to adulthood, with
62 Sexuality Today
characteristics of both, it is often viewed as a separate entity with its own peculiar
characteristics (Lesko, 1996; Rosenblum & Lewis, 2003). A person during this
period becomes defined by it—an adolescent—and her or his behavior is in-
terpreted or explained as being caused by this period. Thus, when a 13-year-old
daughter storms out of the room, it is ‘‘because she is an adolescent.’’ How-
ever, if a colleague demonstrates similar behavior, it is not because ‘‘she is an
adult.’’
Contemporary ideas about adolescence highlight some key dimensions
that supposedly distinguish adolescence from childhood on the one hand and
adulthood on the other. During adolescence, sexual awareness, interest, and
maturity are established. But rarely today is this coupled with the expectation
of making commitments to a lifelong partner or to a family. Thus, part of
societal concerns is of controlling adolescent sexuality until careers, income,
and housing are adequate to support the responsibilities of a family. In this
conceptualization, adolescence is like a holding tank where the emerging adult
awaits release into adulthood.
Most societies recognize the adolescent’s increased abilities compared to
those of childhood. And most cultures begin to insist that individuals during
adolescence take greater responsibility for their behavior. This is reflected in
the laws and punishments within contemporary societies. Nonetheless, many
of the more prevalent ideas about adolescence focus less on the abilities and
responsibilities of adolescents and more on adolescence as a problem.
There are a number of variations on the ‘‘adolescence-as-problem’’ con-
ceptualization. Adolescence has often been characterized as a time of substantial
turmoil and rebellion. Sometimes, this is explained as the effects of ‘‘uncon-
trolled hormones’’ when adolescents are at the whim of the new levels of
hormones invading their bloodstream. Adolescents are also depicted as extreme
and unthinking conformists. According to this view, unlike adults, they are
more concerned with fitting in with their peers than with making good choices.
Other popular depictions of adolescence focus on heightened conflict with
parents and extreme risk taking.
These depictions of adolescence have serious flaws. The ‘‘storm and stress’’
perspective on adolescence was promoted in the twentieth century by G. Stanley
Hall (1904). Hall placed human development within an evolutionary frame-
work, and viewed adolescence as the link between more primitive beings and
civilized ones. Research over the past few decades has not supported this view of
adolescence. Although adolescents might experience more frequent minor
conflicts with parents as adolescents come to expect greater influence on choices
about their day-to-day lives, most families with adolescent children do not suffer
from regular major conflicts (Arnett, 1999; Smetana & Gaines, 1999). Estimates
are that only 20–25 percent of adolescents are involved in major conflicts with
their parents. The frequency of conflicts decreases from early to late adolescence
as children and parents negotiate new boundaries and expectations. It is im-
portant to note that changes in the parent-child relationship are driven not only
Adolescent Sexuality 63
by adolescents, but also by parents, who need to balance protectiveness with the
increased granting of autonomy.
Adolescents are often depicted as being unduly influenced by peers—of
being conformists. Whereas choices about clothing, hairstyle, and music might
be more in line with the choices of peers rather than of parents, most major
decisions reflect parental values. Adolescents are not choosing careers, uni-
versity programs, religious and political values, or ideas about the role of family
and society by mainly considering their peers’ behavior. In fact, according to
Harris (1995), they are likely to choose peers who come from families like
their own, and who share similar values to those of their own parents. One of
the shortfalls of developmental science is that childhood is extensively studied
and then compared to assumed behavior in adulthood. Adolescent conformity
to peers is compared to conformity in earlier childhood, with the verdict that
adolescents are great conformists. Yet, comparisons with adult populations are
rarely provided. It is hard to argue that individuals with only minor respon-
sibilities and very little to lose would conform more to expectations than an
older group who have major expectations and responsibilities placed on them
by colleagues, employers, and family members. Perhaps adolescents have been
viewed as high in conformity, in part, because they have not always conformed
to the expectations for adult behavior, which is rarely seen as conformist. In
other words, putting on a suit is no less conforming than putting on the right
brand of jeans.
Compared to children, adolescents are more likely to take risks. This is
partly due to increased opportunity. Adolescents are given permission to drive,
to spend time away from adults, and to make some decisions independent from
the adults in their lives. They are also relatively free of responsibility for others’
well-being. Adolescents also have less experience than older adults. Their
decisions might not be guided by the same knowledge or concerns, and they
might have fewer social and cognitive skills to deal with awkward situations.
Adolescents are more influenced by the perceived benefits of risk-taking be-
havior than by the perceived risks (Leigh & Stacy, 1993; Parsons, Halkitis,
Bimbi, & Borkowski, 2000). This is partly because the benefits are more direct
and immediate than are the negative consequences. It means, however, that
the adult focus on negative outcomes might not be the best way to motivate
and influence adolescents. More importantly, high risk-taking behavior is not
necessarily characteristic of the majority of adolescents, even though it is more
likely to occur in adolescence than in childhood.
Adolescent sexual behavior is generally perceived of and treated as a
problem. Adolescents in Western societies are confronted with a number of
conflicting messages. On the one hand, they are immersed in a highly sexu-
alized culture. Sexual images and themes are prevalent in television, movies,
music videos and lyrics, and magazines. Sexualized appearances, even for youn-
ger children, are promoted through the available clothing choices. On the
other hand, especially in North America, adolescents are often told that they
64 Sexuality Today
are not ready to, or should not, engage in many sexual behaviors. They are
like drivers faced with traffic lights where both the green and the red lights
are on. And, of course, their bodies are becoming more and more ready for
green.
Adolescent sexual behavior need not be a problem, but sometimes it is.
Many studies actually define delinquent behavior as including some sexual
behaviors (e.g., those involving intercourse). Responsible sexual behavior that
cannot result in pregnancy or sexually transmitted disease, is consensual, and
is respectful of one’s partner might not be a problem, depending on one’s
viewpoint. As we will show, much of the research on adolescent sexual behavior
not only treats it as a problem, but also focuses on the most problematic aspects
of sexuality: unwanted pregnancy, sexually transmitted diseases (STDs), and
promiscuity. Because of this focus, we know far more about the antecedents and
consequences of unprotected adolescent sexual intercourse, and far less about
how adolescents develop into healthy adults capable of enjoying their sexuality.
Psychosocial Explanations
One of the most influential theories of adolescent development describes
adolescence as a period of resolving one’s identity. Erik Erikson (1959, 1968)
theorized that during healthy adolescent development, individuals must ques-
tion and resolve who they are in terms of their occupation, their political and
religious values, and their sexual identity. According to this view, adolescents
must ‘‘find themselves,’’ and thereafter, they will be ready to launch their lives in
the right direction.
Erikson’s ideas about identity development during adolescence were de-
veloped further by Marcia (1980). Marcia described four stages of identity
development that an adolescent could be placed within. The two least mature
stages were foreclosure and diffusion, whereas the more mature stages were
moratorium and achievement. In foreclosure, the adolescent unquestioningly
follows the identity chosen for him or her by influential adults, such as the
parents. Adolescents categorized as identity diffused have avoided making a
choice or commitment and have given little thought to who they are and the
direction they wish to follow. In moratorium, adolescents are in the process of
actively searching for and exploring their identity. Finally, adolescents in
identity achievement have worked through the process of establishing their
identity and have made a commitment toward that end.
The importance of resolving one’s identity during adolescence is not taken
for granted in more current understandings of adolescent development. Al-
though the concept is not dismissed, a number of findings have questioned
whether finding one’s identity is a key achievement during adolescence. One
problem is that identity crises do not seem to happen all at once. Rather,
Adolescent Sexuality 65
Cognitive Explanations
A number of significant cognitive developments occur during adoles-
cence. Relative to their younger counterparts, adolescents are more able to
reason abstractly; consider future, potential outcomes; and reflect on them-
selves and their own behavior. These abilities in turn impact on adolescents’
learning from their environment, their social relations, and their understanding
of moral behavior. Reasons for these cognitive advances during adolescence
generally focus on one or more of the following: changes in thought as a child
enters formal operations (promoted by Piaget, 1952, 1972), changes in in-
formation processing (see Siegler, 1998), and advances in metacognition (see
Kuhn, 1999).
Compared to children, adolescents are able to think more abstractly. They
begin to be able to consider ‘‘what would happen if . . . ’’ and to imagine
outcomes they have never experienced. They start to understand more subtle
forms of humor and behavior, and can reflect on their thoughts and behaviors
to a much greater degree. These changes are reflected in the widening interests
adolescents show. These might include politics, questions about morality, and
balancing one’s obligation to others versus taking care of one’s own needs.
Whereas adolescents are developing the ability to reason and think in the
abstract, most of them lack the general knowledge and experience to apply these
abilities faultlessly (Byrnes, 2003). Hence, they might show superb reason-
ing within a familiar domain, but fail when the area is beyond their knowl-
edge base.
Because adolescents more readily engage in reflective thought, they are
able to use metacognitive strategies to a greater extent than younger children
can. Reflecting on one’s own thoughts and behaviors allows for insights into
critical thinking and effective planning. This also allows greater self-regulation.
Adolescents show greater ability to monitor their performance, to think about
their goals and progress toward them, and to make necessary adaptations.
During adolescence, there is an increased awareness of how oneself and
others view the world. Adolescents demonstrate the ability to reflect on their
own thoughts and how those thoughts came to be. Similarly, they begin to
accept more readily that truths might be relative rather than absolute. This
extends into domains such as reasoning about moral issues. Whereas a younger
66 Sexuality Today
child might believe that a particular behavior is wrong because society accepts
it as wrong, an adolescent might consider and appreciate that there could be
instances where this might not apply.
Neurological Explanations
There have been a number of exciting advances in the field of neuro-
logical development recently. For example, contrary to what was believed
only a decade ago, brain development does not stop during adolescence, but
continues through to at least the mid- or late-twenties. Most dramatic de-
velopments take place in the frontal regions of the cerebral cortex (Giedd et al.,
1999). These areas are believed to be critical to abstract thought, the initiation
and inhibition of behavior, and other more complex cognitive skills such as
advanced planning. The frontal areas are also involved in the regulation of
emotion.
Two important neurological developments during adolescence are the
decrease in synaptic connections within the prefrontal cortex and the greater
connectivity among different brain regions due to increased myelination of
nerve fibers (Keating, 2004). Although it might seem counterintuitive, a de-
crease in synaptic connections between neurons is associated with more efficient
neural processing. Brain development after birth is characterized first by a
proliferation of synaptic connections, followed by substantial pruning of syn-
apses. The pruning process is guided by experience such that the most used
pathways remain and the lesser used ones are removed. During adolescence, the
number of synapses in the prefrontal regions in particular declines dramatically,
thus increasing the efficiency within this region. In addition, pathways linking
different brain regions are made more efficient by the myelination of nerve
fibers. Myelin acts as an insulator of neurons and allows faster transmission of
nerve impulses.
Recent developments in neuroimaging techniques and their availability are
opening up exciting avenues for research. Parallels have been drawn between
changes in cognitive abilities during adolescence and neural development. As
yet, little research has demonstrated a conclusive link between specific brain
changes and consequent cognitive changes or risk-taking behaviors. None-
theless, interesting patterns of change in both neural processing and behavior
across time have been noted. For example, when adults identify the emotional
expression in faces, they engage the frontal cortical regions predominantly, as
well as a part of the limbic system called the amygdala. Adolescents, on the other
hand, respond predominantly within the limbic system—a more primitive re-
gion of the brain—and to a lesser degree within the frontal cortex (Baird,
Gruber, Cohen, Renshaw, & Yureglun-Todd, 1999). One interpretation of
this finding is that adults respond to emotions in a more controlled and rational
way compared to adolescents.
Adolescent Sexuality 67
PUBERTY
Puberty begins with increased growth and development of secondary
characteristics and ends with a fully functional reproductive system. These
developments are orchestrated by an elegant cascade of various chemicals
produced within the developing child. Even by 8 or 9 years of age, children’s
bodies are preparing for puberty (Susman & Rogol, 2004). By the end of
primary school, many girls will be noticeably in the grips of pubertal change,
with the boys soon to follow. Puberty lasts somewhere between two and six
years, with a one- to two-year gap in progression on average between girls and
boys. Thus, puberty often begins before what is conventionally referred to as
adolescence and ends well before adolescence is over.
One of the noticeable features of puberty is the growth spurt. This growth
spurt occurs earlier in girls than in boys, and the rate of increase for girls is less
than that for boys. For example, the greatest rate of growth for girls is at
around 12 years, where they gain approximately 9 cm during the year. Height
ceases to increase by around 15 years in girls. Boys, however, reach their
highest growth rate at around 14 years, when they gain 10 cm. They begin
later, and thus grow for an additional two years on average, and they grow at a
greater rate. These two factors account for much of the final height difference
between women and men.
Secondary sex features show noticeable changes soon after the increased
growth begins. Pubic hair forms, breast buds develop in girls, testicles enlarge
in boys, and boys also experience changes to the larynx, which lead to the
initial cracking and then lowering of the male voice. Approximately two and a
half years after initial breast development, menstruation begins in girls. Sper-
marche, or first ejaculation, occurs approximately two years after initial tes-
ticular enlargement begins in boys. Other changes include increased muscle
growth in boys, redistribution of fat in girls and boys, and increases in bone
mineral content in both (see Rogol, Roemmich, & Clark, 2002).
A number of psychological factors are associated with puberty. Many of the
mechanisms controlling their relationships are not well understood. Pubertal
timing is believed to be related to the onset of depressive symptoms in girls
(Angold & Worthman, 1993). Antisocial behavior has been linked with puberty,
but this might be due to the increased association with older, deviant peers that is
likely with early-maturing adolescents (see Susman & Rogol, 2004). Greater
moodiness is associated with puberty, but causal links between adolescent
moodiness and hormone levels are not particularly strong (Buchanan, Eccles, &
Becker, 1992). Greater moodiness might also be influenced by the greater oc-
currence of negative life events during adolescence (Larson & Ham, 1993).
Timing of puberty is believed to be important, and the effects of timing
are different for girls than for boys. Girls who reach puberty earlier than their
peers might feel uncomfortable with their changed bodies, particularly the
68 Sexuality Today
increased body fat in the hips and thighs. They are also susceptible to the
influence of older peers. Early-maturing girls might receive attention from
older boys and may be encouraged to engage in more deviant behaviors
(Caspi, Lynam, Moffitt, & Silva, 1993). Early-maturing boys, on the other
hand, tend to feel self-confident and gain status from their more adult-looking
bodies.
Secular trends in pubertal timing suggest that nutrition and weight play a
role in the onset of puberty (Parent et al., 2003). There has been a general
trend over the past century for an earlier age of menarche over time, with
similar estimates of earlier puberty for boys. This trend is believed to be due to
better nutrition, health care, and living conditions for children. Although it
appeared that this trend toward earlier puberty was leveling off, in countries
with significant rises in youth obesity rates, menarche continues to occur at
even earlier ages.
Researchers have also noted an apparent link between father absence and
the onset of daughter puberty. Girls without fathers in the home show earlier
onset of menarche compared to those living with fathers in the home. Belsky,
Steinberg, and Draper (1991) suggest that father absence increases stress for
daughters, which in turn leads to depression, weight gain, and thus, earlier
menarche. An alternative explanation is offered by Comings, Muhleman, John-
son, and MacMurray (2002) who argue that the relationship between father
absence and daughter’s early menarche is genetic. They investigated a particular
allele of the AR gene in 121 males and 164 females. In males, this allele was
associated with impulsiveness and aggression, and increased number of sexual
partners. In females, the allele was associated with father absence, father divorce,
and early onset of menarche. These findings do not yet resolve this issue, but they
offer another way of understanding the association between father absence and the
timing of daughter menarche.
more likely to overreport sexual experience (Siegel, Aten, & Roghmann, 1998).
In addition, Hollander (1997) found that, in comparison with their medical
records, only 46 percent of participants were able to accurately identify how
many STDs they had contracted, and only 76 percent of women accurately
reported their pregnancies.
Inconsistencies do not occur randomly, but they occur to different degrees
according to gender, age, family variables, and ethnicity. This indicates that
participants’ answers are affected by more-than-random errors and mem-
ory lapses (Lauritsen & Swicegood, 1997; Upchurch et al., 2002). Adolescents
may give inconsistent reports regarding coitus, STDs, and pregnancy because
they have not understood their own medical treatment, they may wish to give
socially desirable answers, they may have misunderstood the question, or they
may be reluctant to disclose personal information. Illustrating this reluctance to
disclose, another study comparing adult retrospective report with prior ado-
lescent report found no inconsistency for age at first reported coitus, but a
significant inconsistency for age at first masturbation (Halpern, Udry, Su-
chindran, & Campbell, 2000). The authors stated that masturbation, even more
so than other behaviors, is a sensitive topic. Adolescents may be reluctant to
disclose sensitive information even if they are assured that their reporting is
confidential. Furthermore, research suggests that individuals least comfortable
with questions about sexuality or with the least amount of sexual experience
may decline to participate completely, potentially biasing accounts to a greater
degree (Strassberg & Lowe, 1995).
Another factor related to inconsistent reporting includes the language used
to ask about sexual behavior. Reluctance by researchers to use explicit or
colloquial language may result in varying interpretations by participants. For
example, ‘‘having sex’’ may be interpreted in various ways, from coitus,
oral sex, the presence of orgasm, through to a variety of individual inter-
pretations (Sanders & Reinisch, 1999; Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2004).
Capturing homosexuality may be particularly difficult because attraction,
behavior, and labeling can be distinct categories that are often discordant
(Diamond, 2000; Friedman et al., 2004). Frequently, researchers conceptualize
sex as entailing vaginal/penile penetration. Even if this is clearly communi-
cated to participants, failure to ask about a broader range of behaviors leaves
large omissions in our understanding of adolescent sexual experience and the
meaning attributed to experiences. It is worth noting that some research-
ers have begun asking about a wider variety of behaviors and defining what
they mean by ‘‘to have sex.’’ It is wise to remain aware of potential differences
in perception between researchers and participants when conducting and in-
terpreting research. Crucially, researchers must extend their investigations
beyond behavior, to attend to the meanings attributed to those behaviors
and the social and emotional facets that are an integral part of sexual ex-
perience.
70 Sexuality Today
Sexual Knowledge
Based on the results of a national survey, Terry-Humen and Manlove
(2003) reported that only 26 percent of 13-year-olds were able to state the
most effective contraceptive from a choice of withdrawal, condoms, and the
contraceptive pill. One-third was unable to identify the condom as the most
effective STD prevention method. Very few (8 percent) were able to correctly
state the most fertile point in the female fertility cycle. Similarly, adolescents’
knowledge about normative behavior is not very accurate, with a tendency to
overestimate the sexual behavior of their peers. Results from a sample of 958
12- to 14-year-olds showed that 39 percent of boys and 51 percent of girls
endorsed the statement ‘‘Most teens your age are having sex,’’ whereas, in
reality, only 15 percent of boys and 8 percent of girls in that sample reported
that they had had sex (Gomez, Coyle, Gregorich, Marin, & Kirby, 2003).
Sexual Behavior
Masturbation
Despite being a safe and common form of sexual expression, masturbation
is still a taboo topic. Research conducted from the 1970s through to the 1990s
showed, disconcertingly, that self-stimulation is associated with high degrees of
guilt for many people (Davidson & Moore, 1994; Lopresto, Sherman, &
Sherman, 1985), and more than any other topic, questions about it made re-
spondents feel very uneasy (Bradburn, Sudman, Blair, & Stocking, 1978; Da-
vidson & Moore; Lopresto et al.). Reflecting this uneasiness, and maintaining it,
is the absence of masturbation from media representations of sexuality (Ward,
2003). As a result, masturbation, perhaps more than any other sexual behavior,
may be underreported (Halpern, Udry et al., 2000). Furthermore, there has
been little research attention on it. With this in mind, the research conducted
among U.S. college students showed that 81–86 percent of males and 45–51
percent of females reported ever having masturbated (Leitenberg, Detzer, &
Srebnik, 1993; Weinberg, Lottes, & Shaver, 1995). In these studies, males were
not only more likely to report having masturbated, but they also reported
beginning it at a younger age (an average of 13.5 years versus 14.2 years) and
doing so around three times as often as females. In addition, only 15 percent of
males but 63 percent of females reported that they had never masturbated by the
first time they had sex (Schwartz, 1999). While it may be common sense to
contend that self-awareness and self-exploration are healthy aspects of sexual
development, research has shown that masturbation is not related to sexual
satisfaction, sexual difficulties, or intercourse experience. This suggests that
masturbation is neither harmful to, nor necessary for, sexual enjoyment in
young adulthood (Leitenberg et al., 1993).
Adolescent Sexuality 71
Precoital Behaviors
The majority of studies devoted to adolescent sexuality present sex as a
dichotomous variable (Whitaker, Miller, & Clark, 2000). Sexual experience is
defined as having engaged in vaginal/penile sex, or not. Yet, a much broader
range of behaviors and feelings constitute sexual experience. Unfortunately,
few studies explore these broader behaviors and feelings or the meanings
adolescents attribute to them. As a result, it is possible to give descriptive
accounts of some precoital behaviors but difficult to provide any substantive
analysis of the relationship between them, their developmental course, how
they are interpreted, or how behaviors and cognitions relate to later coitus.
Precoital sexual expression, as with coitus, becomes more common with
increasing age. A nationally representative study revealed that 12 percent of
virgins and 18 percent of all participants aged 12–14 years had been in a
relationship in the last eighteen months that had included ‘‘touching under
clothes,’’ while 6 percent of virgins and 13 percent of all students reported
genital touching within at least one romantic relationship (Bruckner & Bear-
man, 2003). For older students, another nationally representative study of
males found that approximately 40 percent of 15-year-olds and 60 percent of
16-year-olds had precoital sexual experience, such as masturbating or engaging
in oral sex (Gates & Sonenstein, 2000). In another study, 35 percent of stu-
dents in ninth through twelfth grade had engaged in noncoital heterosexual
activity in the prior year; specifically, masturbation of a partner (29 percent)
and by a partner (31 percent), fellatio with ejaculation (9 percent), and cun-
nilingus (10 percent). Homosexual masturbation and oral sex were less com-
monly reported (around 1–2 percent) for different behaviors (Schuster, Bell, &
Kanouse, 1996). Retrospective reports by college students of their experiences
prior to coitus revealed that most had engaged in kissing and fondling of
breasts and genitals, 70 percent of males had performed cunnilingus, and 57
percent of females had performed fellatio at least once; moderate to high
72 Sexuality Today
Coitus
In 2003, according to the results of a nation-wide survey (Grunbaum et al.,
2004), 46.7 percent of students in grades nine to twelve had had sex. This
Adolescent Sexuality 73
Sexual Attitudes
Love, curiosity, and desire were the most popular motives given by young
(12- to 14-year-old) adolescents for having sex, whereas wanting to avoid
AIDS or other diseases was the most common reason for not having sex,
followed by not wanting a baby, parent’s anger, and feeling they were too
young (Gomez, Coyle, Gregorich, Marin, & Kirby, 2003). Teenagers seem to
believe that society should encourage them to delay having sex. For example,
90 percent of adolescent boys stated that teenagers should be given a ‘‘strong’’
abstinence message from society (unpublished data cited by the National
Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy, 2003a).
endorsing the statement ‘‘I was in love,’’ more parental supervision, and a
higher level of maternal education. A public opinion poll conducted in 2002
by the National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy found similar results,
with 81 percent of 12- to 14-year-olds and 55 percent of 15- to 19-year-olds
stating that they wished they had waited until they were older to have sex
(Albert, Brown, & Flanigan, 2003).
Mutual Consent/Coercion
A girl’s propensity to regret is likely to be related to the degree she desired
intercourse in the first place. Although most girls define the first time they
have intercourse as being consensual in that the experience was not forced on
them, there is variation in how much they desired sex. Flanigan (2003) asked
girls about the ‘‘wantedness of sex’’ in consensual intercourse. She reported
that among girls younger than 15, 27 percent rated the wantedness as low, 48
percent as medium, and 26 percent as high. Girls older than 15 were less likely
to give a low rating (15 percent) and more likely to give a high rating (42
percent); a similar number (43 percent) reported medium wantedness. This
substantial number of girls who were ambivalent, if not reluctant, about par-
ticipating in first coitus suggests that girls are not being sufficiently equipped to
have their desires met. Issues of communication, self-esteem, self-efficacy,
subjugation, or pressure of perceived social norms may impact a girl’s ability to
say and get what she wants in a relationship.
Contraceptive Use
A summary of the trends in contraceptive use by Terry and Manlove
(2000) showed that from 1988 to 1995 there was an increase in contracep-
tive use at first intercourse for all teens who had ever had sex, but among those
who were currently sexually active, there was a decline in contraceptive use at
last sex. The one exception to this trend was that the contraceptive use of
sexually active black females remained stable across the periods. To prevent
pregnancy, it is important that adolescents use contraception consistently;
unfortunately, the data indicated that 30 to 38 percent of females do not do
this, with younger adolescents being the least consistent users.
The factors that determine whether adolescents use contraceptives con-
sistently differ from the factors affecting early sexual debut. Unlike early sexual
involvement, contraceptive nonuse does not appear to be associated with other
risk factors. In a literature review, Manlove, Ryan, and Franzetta (2004) re-
ported factors specific to the couple that reduce the likelihood of condom use.
These included an age gap between partners, physical violence, younger age at
first intercourse, and a greater number of partners. They also noted that although
having sex early in the relationship increased the likelihood of initial condom
use, it reduced likelihood at last intercourse. The impact of relationship type,
Adolescent Sexuality 75
CONTEXTS OF SEXUALITY:
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS
Romantic Relationships
Romantic interests are a defining aspect of adolescence, whether adoles-
cents actually engage in a relationship or participate in fantasy and talk about a
person they ‘‘like.’’ In one study, 55 percent of all adolescents had been in a
romantic relationship in the past eighteen months. The proportion increased
with age so that by age 18, 76 percent of adolescents reported having experi-
enced a romantic relationship, and 8 percent reported a ‘‘liked’’ relationship
(Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003). In another study, with adolescents 14 and
younger, 42 percent reported ever having dated. Of those, the majority (40
percent) had only dated 1–3 times (Terry-Humen & Manlove, 2003).
In the Carver et al. (2003) study, approximately half the reported romantic
relationships were sexual; again, the proportion increased with the age of the
adolescents involved. More relationships involved ‘‘touching each other under
clothing’’ (57 percent) than ‘‘touching each other’s genitals’’ (52 percent), and
41 percent involved intercourse.
Nonromantic Relationships
Not all sexual involvement occurs within a romantic relationship. A na-
tionally representative sample showed that 14.9 percent of teenagers aged 15
years and older had sex with someone they were not romantically involved with
at the time (Manning, Longmore, & Giordano, 2005). The relational context
76 Sexuality Today
Stability of Relationships
Relationships within which first coitus occurs tend to be short. For ad-
olescents younger than 15, 44 percent of their relationships ended within three
months. For those 15 years and older, 39 percent of relationships ended within
three months (Flanigan, 2003). On the other hand, relationships that include
intercourse tend to be more enduring, lasting almost twice as long as platonic
relationships. Analyzed at what stage 25 percent, 50 percent, and 75 percent of
relationships ended, sexual relationships endured for 5, 11, and 27 months,
respectively, whereas relationships that did not include intercourse endured for
only 2, 5, and 13 months, respectively (Bruckner & Bearman, 2003).
Age Disparity
The greater the age disparity between a young girl and her partner, the
greater the likelihood that they will be sexually involved, that they will not use
contraception, and that she will become pregnant (Darroch, Landry, & Oslak,
1999). In adolescents under 14, 8–13 percent of same-age relationships in-
cluded sex. This doubled when the partner was two years older, increased to
33 percent when the partner was three years older, and was 47 percent when
the partner was four or more years older (Albert et al., 2003). Another study
replicated this pattern and found that it applied for boys as well. Thirty percent
of girls and 73 percent of boys aged 14 years reported sexual involvement
when their partner was at least two years older, compared to 13 percent of girls
and 29 percent of boys when their partner was within one year of their own
age (Marin, Kirby, Hudes, Gomez, & Coyle, 2003).
Sexual Orientation
Sexual identity development is difficult to study because sexual orientation
labels may not accurately represent the experiences of many adolescents
(Friedman et al., 2004). Hence, although attention has been given to adoles-
cents who label themselves as gay, lesbian, or bisexual, a large number of factors
may affect teenagers’ decision to label themselves, including uncertainty about
the meaning of their feelings and behaviors, diversity within their experiences
of attraction, and fear of negative evaluation. Furthermore, heterosexual, ho-
mosexual, and bisexual appear to present as dimensional continuums rather than
as distinct categories to which one either does or does not belong. Therefore,
the term ‘‘sexual minority’’ includes those youth who experience same-sex
attraction, but recognize that they may also experience other-sex attraction, and
that their patterns of experience may change over time (Diamond & Savin-
Williams, 2003).
Sexual identity development involves interpreting and integrating attrac-
tions, behaviors, intimate relationships, fantasy, and labels into a representation
of the self. It should not be assumed that the different aspects of sexual identity
are stable and/or concordant with each other (Diamond, 2000; Friedman et al.,
2004). For example, adolescents who report same-sex attractions do not nec-
essarily participate in same-sex behavior, and adults who identify as gay do not
always recall same-sex attractions during adolescence. Experiencing same-sex
attraction does not automatically mean that other-sex attraction either does
not or has not occurred, or is unimportant, unsatisfying, or unappealing. Most
sexual minority youth have been involved with other-sex peers in a variety of
ways. As Diamond and Savin-Williams explained, ‘‘[I]ndividuals typically ex-
perience a diverse array of attractions and behaviors during their adolescent
years some of which reflect curiosity and experimentation, some of which
78 Sexuality Today
reflect social pressure, and some of which reflect an underlying sexual orien-
tation’’ (2003, p. 395).
Fluidity of attractions and behaviors is particularly true of the experiences
of women. This is illustrated by a study of eighty sexual minority women aged
between 16 and 23 years, where one-third changed their identity label and
half changed their identity more than once (Diamond, 2000). Nevertheless, it
appears that men’s experiences are not static either, according to the findings
of a longitudinal study that comprised men born in New Zealand in 1972/
1973 and questioned at age 21. The majority (93.2 percent) reported that they
had only ever experienced attraction to women, a few (6.5 percent) reported
ever being sexually attracted to a man, and a smaller number (4.2 percent)
reported current same-sex attraction. Overall, 4.2 percent reported ever having
had sex with a man. Of that group, a minority (9.5 percent) reported being
only solely attracted to men, and almost half reported that they were now
solely attracted to women (Paul, Fitzjohn, Eberhart-Phillips, Herbison, &
Dickson, 2000).
A large number of adolescents question aspects of their sexual identity.
Much of this uncertainty resolves with time, although a degree of fluidity
continues to exist. Unfortunately, wrestling with sexual identity issues is not
easy, and sexual minority youth experience a greater number of health risks,
including depression, suicide, and substance abuse (Garofalo & Katz, 2001;
Savin-Williams, 1994). It is important to remember the ‘‘powerful needs for
physical affection, emotional security, and simple companionship that underlie
all adolescents’ close relationships,’’ and ensure that adolescents are able to have
these needs met safely (Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2003, p. 406).
Pregnancy
Birthrates
Since the 1970s, in industrialized countries worldwide, there has been an
overall decline in the childbearing of teenagers aged 15–19 years (Singh &
Darroch, 2000). In the United States this decline, particularly in the last de-
cade, has created a degree of optimism—optimism tempered by the fact that
the current rate is still one of the highest in the Western world. Decreased
adolescent childbearing has occurred in a context of declining fertility for
women of all ages, but this trend has generally been more marked among
adolescents (Darroch, Singh, & Frost, 2001).
In the United States and several other countries, the decline in birthrates is
accompanied by a declining abortion rate. Although abortion trends are less
consistent across countries, generally, from 1980 to 2000, there has been a
stable or decreasing rate among 15- to 19-year-olds. In the United States, there
Adolescent Sexuality 79
has been a 31 percent decrease in abortions since the mid-1980s (Darroch &
Singh, 1999). Younger adolescents are more likely to seek an abortion. For
example, in the United Kingdom in 1997, approximately 50 percent of births
were aborted in those under 16, but this figure fell to around 30 percent for
those aged between 16 and 19 (Macleod & Weaver, 2003).
Overall, 25 percent of reported STDs are for adolescents (Weinstock, Berman, &
Cates, 2004). This number is particularly alarming when considering that it only
applies to the proportion of adolescents who are sexually active and those who
seek medical care.
Upchurch et al. (2004) reported that age at first intercourse was the pri-
mary predictor of adolescent sexual health. Adolescents who postpone sex tend
to have fewer sexual partners, have longer-lasting relationships, and use con-
doms more consistently (Albert et al., 2003; Flisher & Chalton, 2001). In
addition to being at increased risk of disease due to risky behaviors, young
women are more physically susceptible to infection. The cervix does not
mature fully until late adolescence and until then is less able to resist infection
( Joffe, 1997). Furthermore, postponing sexual involvement until late adoles-
cence is particularly important for reducing transmission of those STDs that are
not affected by condom use, specifically, those caused by herpes simplex virus,
pubic lice, and human papillomavirus (HPV), which leads to cervical cancer
(Cothran & White, 2002; Warner et al., 2004; Winer et al., 2003). Although
consistently wearing condoms leads to reductions in transmission of most
STDs, the aforementioned diseases are contracted through any skin-to-skin
contact. Therefore, interventions that lower disease rates need to focus on
methods (other than condom use) of reducing risk behaviors, such as delayed
sexual debut, STD screening and treatment, and fewer sexual partners.
In addition to interventions that reduce STD transmission, it is crucial to
provide, and educate about the necessity of, screening and treatment of STDs.
Research to date suggests that adolescents are not being adequately assessed
and that professionals and adolescents need to be aware of the importance of
regular screening (Fiscus, Ford, & Miller, 2004). It is also important that health
practitioners are aware of the possible need to screen for STDs contracted
through behaviors other than coitus, such as oral sex (Remez, 2000). Gen-
erally, education and media campaigns have focused on vaginal/penile inter-
course as being synonymous with sexual behavior (Halpern-Felsher et al.,
2005). However, a variety of sexual behaviors outside of this definition still
place the young person at risk.
that women be sexually naı̈ve result in her holding limited knowledge about the
importance of protected sex. In addition, even if she did know about the
importance of using a condom, she may have limited power to assert that one be
used (UNAIDS/WHO, 2004). As well as the increased incidents of HIV/AIDS,
STDs, and unintended pregnancy, adolescents in developing countries also have
reduced access to health care. Consequently, there is less opportunity for
treatment and there are increased incidents of pregnancy complications, which
lead to increased mortality for mother and child.
Culture
Cultural atmosphere is the broadest and the most inescapable influence on
sexual behavior. The media, schools, families, extracurricular clubs, and com-
munities of faith all give messages about appropriate sexual behavior. Out of
this melting pot of sources, youth are given contradictory messages. On the
one hand, sexuality is glamorized, and on the other, it is prohibited. The
media, from movies to magazines, generally depict sexuality in a way that is
discordant with reality (Ward, 2003). Equally discordant with reality is the
information that depicts only the biophysical aspects and risks of sexual in-
volvement. Adolescents do not merely hear these messages, but they interpret
them and choose to accept or disregard them. These choices and interpreta-
tions affect adolescent beliefs about what is acceptable and what is normative.
These beliefs may be one of the most powerful underlying influences on their
sexual behavior (Kirby, 2001b).
Media
Adolescents with unsupervised access to television were more likely to
engage in sexual activities (Gruber, Wang, Christensen, Grube, & Fisher,
Adolescent Sexuality 83
2005), and adolescents who watched television with more sexual content were
more likely to initiate intercourse within the following year (Collins et al.,
2004). However, as with most predictors of sexual behavior, the link between
media and behavior is not necessarily unilateral. Chapin’s (2000) develop-
mental approach to mass media suggests that teenagers actively use the media
to find information relevant to their interests and developmental stage. Their
interpretation of the media will then also differ accordingly. This theory is
consistent with the findings of Brown, Halpern, and L’Engle (2005). They
found that pubertal timing influenced girls’ likelihood of engaging with media
containing sexual content. Early-maturing girls showed more interest in music,
magazines, and movies with sexual content and were also more likely to
interpret sexual messages as being permissive. Interestingly, late-maturing girls
were also more likely to report that they viewed STD- and birth control–
related messages. As the authors noted, the rarity of this information in the
media suggests that these girls may have been particularly attuned to such
information.
Family
Despite the powerful influence of wider community and societal influ-
ence, research unequivocally supports the impact of the family (Miller, 1998;
Miller, Benson, & Galbraith, 2001; Upchurch, Aneshensel, Sucoff, & Levy-
Storms, 1999). This large body of research supports the finding that the most
effective way to protect adolescents from disadvantage, including early sexual
debut and pregnancy, is a functional and supportive family. Such families have
been found to include qualities such as parent-child connectedness, good
communication, strong values with expectations for abstinence, parental in-
volvement, awareness and supervision of adolescent activities, high level of
maternal education, hereditary influences, and presence of both biological
parents in the home (Borawski, Ievers-Landis, Lovegreen, & Trapl, 2003; Di-
Lorio, Dudley, Soet, & McCarty, 2004; Ellis et al., 2003; Hutchinson, Jemmott,
Jemmott, Braverman, & Fong, 2003; Miller, 1998; Miller et al., 2001; Rose
et al., 2005; Sieverding, Adler, Witt, & Ellen, 2005).
Peers
An adolescent’s peer group also impacts his or her behavior (Whitaker &
Miller, 2000). Adolescents who perceive that their peers are having sex, who
have older friends, or who have friends who participate in delinquent activities
are more likely to have sex (Bearman & Bruckner, 1999). The importance of
social context was exemplified in a study conducted by Bearman and Bruckner
(2001).They found that the effectiveness of virginity pledging relied on there
being at least some other pledgers within the pledger’s school, and differed
according to whether the school environment was relatively socially closed or
84 Sexuality Today
open. Schools whose students primarily formed social groups with peers inside
their school were more likely to be affected by the pledge, whereas schools
where students formed social groups from diverse contexts were less likely to
be affected.
The Individual
There are many studies investigating individual attributes associated with
sexual risk behaviors. These include physical maturation; depression and low
self-esteem (Longmore, Manning, Giordano, & Rudolph, 2004); low academic
aspirations; low community attachment; alcohol and other drug use (Rashad &
Kaestner, 2004; Stueve & O’Donnell, 2005); history of abuse; previous preg-
nancy; and having an older romantic partner, which is associated with greater
likelihood of sex, reduced contraceptive use, and higher reports of coercion
(Young & d’Arcy, 2005). Individual factors that have been identified as pro-
tective include being involved in sports or youth groups, having strong religious
beliefs (Whitehead, Wilcox, & Rostosky, 2001), self-efficacy for abstinence
(DiLorio et al., 2004), sexual attitudes and knowledge (O’Donnell, Myint,
O’Donnell, & Stueve, 2003; Rosengard et al., 2001), and good cognitive ability.
The whole picture is complex, however, because these factors interact and are
more or less appropriate according to the age, gender, and ethnicity of the
individual.
Looking Further
The literature addressing adolescent sexuality has almost exclusively fo-
cused on pregnancy and disease. Very little attention has been given to the
social and emotional aspects that are integral to sexuality. In one study
(Widdice, Cornell, Liang, & Halpern-Felsher, 2005), teenagers were asked
their opinions about the risks and benefits of having sex. Along with noting
pregnancy and STDs, teenagers noted several social consequences, including
negative effect on the relationship, negative emotions such as regret and loss of
self-esteem, and parental or peer disapproval. Positive consequences of sexual
involvement were predominately social, such as improving the relationship,
having fun, and lifting social status. Although pregnancy and STDs are clearly
high costs and have therefore been the focus of most interventions, the social
and emotional aspects of sexuality should nevertheless be considered in future
research.
SEX EDUCATION/PREVENTION
Virtually every North American student in public school will receive
some form of sexuality education. Darroch, Landry, and Singh (2000) found
that while the content and timing differs, the focus of the curriculum is likely
Adolescent Sexuality 85
Abstinence-Only Programs
Abstinence-only programs have not been sufficiently studied to justify
drawing conclusions about their effectiveness at this stage (Besharov & Gardi-
ner, 1997; Kirby, 2002). In his review of pregnancy prevention programs, Kirby
(2001a) stated that only three studies (Kirby, Korpi, Barth, & Cagampang, 1997;
Olsen, Weed, Nielsen, & Jensen, 1992; St. Pierre, Mark, Kaltreider, & Aikin,
1995) have evaluated abstinence-only programs in a sufficiently rigorous
manner. The findings from those studies suggested that the programs did not
impact the sexual behavior or contraceptive use of participants. Yet, as Kirby
Adolescent Sexuality 87
discussed, there are huge variations in the type of programs that come under
abstinence-only education. The above three studies do not reflect the wide
range of abstinence programs, some of which may be more effective than others.
A program adjunctive to that offered by schools, which is abstinence based, is
the virginity pledge. This, perhaps surprisingly, has been found in specific cir-
cumstances to effectively delay sexual debut. Bearman and Bruckner (2001)
reported that sexually inexperienced young adolescents, in a context where
there are enough other pledgers but not too many, are likely to delay intercourse
for a substantial period of time. This is consistent with an intention not to have
intercourse. However, when they do have intercourse, they are less likely to use
contraception but pledge-breakers report no negative effects on self-esteem for
breaking the pledge.
Noncurriculum-based Interventions
In addition to curriculum-based sex education, other initiatives have been
developed to improve adolescent outcomes. Kirby (2001a) divided interventions
into three broad categories based upon whether they target primarily sexual an-
tecedents, nonsexual antecedents, or both. Within each of these broad categories,
additional groupings can be made according to the structure of the programs.
Programs that target sexual antecedents include curriculum-based programs,
community-wide initiatives, sex education for parents and families, and clinic-
based programs. Programs that target nonsexual antecedents are broad adolescent
development programs that typically include structured preparation time leading
to voluntary community service followed by reflection time. The third category of
programs includes components of comprehensive health education and adolescent
development.
Clinic-based services that provide contraception have been set up in
the community and sometimes in schools. Kirby (2001a) explained that clinic-
based interventions have not been studied in a comprehensive manner; how-
ever, common sense would suggest that contraceptive availability is an important
factor dictating contraceptive usage. It does appear that clinic protocols are
important. Specifically, providing information about reproductive health, the
merits of abstinence, and opportunities for one-to-one discussions were com-
ponents that facilitated effective service provision. He stated that school-based
clinics do not appear to increase contraceptive use overall. Rather, teenagers
substituted use of the school-based clinics over the community based clinics. As
already stated, studies show that condom availability does not increase sexual
activity (e.g. Kirby, 1991; Kisker & Brown, 1996).
Community-wide Interventions
Recognition of the multiple influences on teenage behavior has led to the
development of community-wide interventions, including media campaigns
88 Sexuality Today
Children’s Aid Society-Carrera Program. It has been used with at-risk youth
and includes the following components:
Significant effects were found for girls and included postponement of sexual
debut, increased condom use, and reduced pregnancy and birthrates.
Complicating the evaluation of program effectiveness is the fact that
different programs may have different success with different groups. For ex-
ample, different interventions may be more or less appropriate to individuals
depending on their gender, ethnicity, sexual experience, at-risk status (e.g.,
those affected by sexual abuse or substance abusers), and so on (Kirby et al.,
2004). The ability to target interventions to specific groups may improve the
success of the program.
A consideration that has been missing from intervention research is that
sex involves more than the decisions and desires of one individual. The vast
majority of intervention research has considered individuals rather than cou-
ples. Little attention has been given to the interpersonal aspects of sexuality.
Likewise, the physical consequences of sexuality have been emphasized, with
the emotional consequences less emphasized. Researchers have only recently
attempted to address this imbalance.
attitudes, but were less likely to impact on behavior. For each of the various
approaches, at least one study found an impact on a behavior. Interventions
that aimed to alter multiple behaviors generally did not achieve this goal
although they may have succeeded with one behavior or one group of par-
ticipants, for example, women.
Uganda is one country that, through a consistent countrywide effort, has
seen a reduction in the AIDS epidemic (Blum, 2004). The effort, referred to as
ABC, includes several tactics: Abstinence, Be faithful, and Condoms; vol-
untary counseling and testing; and a focus on reducing mother-to-child
transmission. Dramatic reductions in AIDS are accompanied by equally dra-
matic increases in condom use and abstinence.
school, but at home and in the media. However, contrary to what has some-
times been assumed, there are strong social expectations regarding acceptable
behavior.
Lastly, the different approaches taken by these countries in determining
the content of sex education may also contribute to the portrayal of mixed
messages and less effective messages. An adversarial approach is used in Britain,
but in the Netherlands, there is an attempt to find points of similarity within
opposing viewpoints and to reach consensus.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
In this chapter, we have tried to provide a snapshot of the key issues in and
contemporary research on adolescent sexuality. The field of adolescent de-
velopment is itself going through a period of rapid change. Whereas past
research was heavily focused on children under 5 years of age, this focus has
shifted, and a greater proportion of research now addresses older children and
adolescents. Other changes include new theoretical approaches and methods
that view development as integrated systems, combining neurological, bio-
logical, cognitive, and social factors, explanations, and applications (see Lerner
& Steinberg, 2004).
The study of adolescent sexuality presents some striking contradictions
and dilemmas within our society. On the one hand, the period between sexual
maturity and the establishment of a family is extending, with the average age of
first childbirth reaching into the late twenties and early thirties in many
Western countries. In addition, the external, public world we live in has
become highly sexualized. And yet, the attitudes reflected within many so-
cieties remain ambivalent toward sexual expression, especially by adolescents.
Although the lyrics to a popular song might say, ‘‘Let’s talk about sex,’’ real talk
is less forthcoming.
Perhaps it is not sexuality in the media per se that is problematic, but
rather it is the way in which sexuality is portrayed and conceptualized in the
media. Sex is often presented as something that girls give and boys take,
something naughty, something based on physical acts leading to orgasm rather
than the shared physical and psychological intimacy created. This might be
part of sexual expression, but it is a very limited picture presented. Limited also
are the types of people presented as sexual. And this might have serious im-
plications for how adolescents feel about their own bodies and their own
sexuality.
We know more about the frequency, contexts, and consequences of ad-
olescent intercourse than we know about how adolescents feel about their
sexuality (see Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2004). What factors, for example,
predict healthy attitudes toward sexuality during adolescence and into adult-
hood? What do adolescents think, feel, and know about sexuality, and how
does this influence the decisions they make? More importantly, how can
92 Sexuality Today
REFERENCES
Albert, B., Brown, S., & Flanigan, C. (Eds.). (2003). 14 and younger: The sexual
behavior of young adolescents. Washington, DC: National Campaign to
Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Angold, A., & Worthman, C. W. (1993). Puberty onset of gender differences in
rates of depression: A developmental, epidemiological and neuroendocrine
perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29, 145–158.
Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress reconsidered. American Psychologist,
54, 317–326.
Baird, A. A., Gruber, S. A., Cohen, B. M., Renshaw, R. J., & Yureglun-Todd,
D. A. (1999). FMRI of the amygdala in children and adolescents. American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 195–199.
Bearman, P. S., & Bruckner, H. (1999). Power in numbers: Peer effects on adolescent
girls’ sexual debut and pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Campaign to
Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Bearman, P. S., & Bruckner, H. (2001). Promising the future: Virginity pledges
and first intercourse. American Journal of Sociology, 106(4), 859.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interper-
sonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of
socialization. Child Development, 62, 647–670.
Besharov, D. J., & Gardiner, K. N. (1997). Sex education and abstinence: Pro-
grams and evaluation. Children and Youth Services Review, 19(7), 501–506.
Bingham, C. R., & Crockett, L. J. (1996). Longitudinal adjustment patterns of
boys and girls experiencing early, middle, and late sexual intercourse.
Developmental Psychology, 32, 647–658.
Adolescent Sexuality 93
Cotton, S., Mills, L., Succop, P. A., Biro, F. M., & Rosenthal, S. L. (2004).
Adolescent girls’ perceptions of the timing of their sexual initiation: ‘‘Too
young’’ or ‘‘just right’’? Journal of Adolescent Health, 34(5), 453–458.
Darroch, J. E., Landry, D. J., & Oslak, S. (1999). Age differences between sexual
partners in the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 31(4), 160.
Darroch, J. E., Landry, D. J., & Singh, S. (2000). Changing emphases in sexuality
education in U.S. public secondary schools, 1988–1999. Family Planning
Perspectives, 32(5), 204–211.
Darroch, J. E., & Singh, S. (1999). Why is teenage pregnancy declining? The roles of
abstinence, sexual activity and contraceptive use. Occasional report. New York:
The Alan Guttmacher Institute.
Darroch, J. E., Singh, S., & Frost, J. J. (2001). Differences in teenage preg-
nancy rates among five developed countries: The roles of sexual activ-
ity and contraceptive use. Family Planning Perspectives, 33(6), 244–250,
281.
Davidson, J. K., & Moore, N. B. (1994). Masturbation and premarital sexual
intercourse among college-women—Making choices for sexual fulfill-
ment. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 20(3), 178–199.
Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among young
sexual-minority women over a 2-year period. Developmental Psychology,
36(2), 241–250.
Diamond, L. M., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2003). The intimate relationships of
sexual-minority youths. In G. Adams & M. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell
handbook of adolescence (pp. 393–411). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
DiLorio, C., Dudley, W. N., Soet, J. E., & McCarty, F. (2004). Sexual possibility
situations and sexual behaviors among young adolescents: The moderating
role of protective factors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35(6), 528.e11–528.e20.
Ellis, B., Bates, J., Dodge, K., Fergusson, D., Horwood, L., Pettit, G., et al.
(2003). Does father absence place daughters at special risk for early sexual
activity and teenage pregnancy? Child Development, 74, 801–821.
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues, 1, 18–164.
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: W. W. Norton.
Fagan, J., Barnett, M., Bernd, E., & Whiteman, V. (2003). Prenatal involvement
of adolescent fathers. Fathering, 1, 283–302.
Felice, M. E., Feinstein, R. A., Fisher, M. M., Kaplan, D. W., Olmedo, L. F.,
Rome, E. S., et al. (1999). Adolescent pregnancy—current trends and
issues: 1998 American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Adolescence,
1998–1999. Pediatrics, 103(2), 516–520.
Fiscus, L. C., Ford, C. A., & Miller, W. C. (2004). Infrequency of sexually
transmitted disease screening among sexually experienced US female ad-
olescents. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 36(6), 233–238.
Flanigan, C. (2003). Sexual activity among girls under age 15: Findings from the
National Survey of Family Growth. In B. Albert, S. Brown, & C. Flanigan
(Eds.), 14 and younger: The sexual behavior of young adolescents (pp. 57–64).
Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Adolescent Sexuality 95
Hobcraft, J., & Kiernan, K. (2001). Childhood poverty, early motherhood and
adult social exclusion. British Journal of Sociology, 52(3), 495–517.
Hoffman, S. (1998). Teenage childbearing is not so bad after all . . . or is it? A
review of the new literature. Family Planning Perspectives, 30, 236–244.
Hollander, D. (1997). Take teenagers with a grain of salt. Family Planning Per-
spectives, 29(5), 198.
Hutchinson, M. K., Jemmott, J. B., III, Jemmott, L. S., Braverman, P., & Fong,
G. T. (2003). The role of mother-daughter sexual risk communication in
reducing sexual risk behaviors among urban adolescent females: A pro-
spective study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(2), 98–107.
Joffe, A. (1997). Sexually transmitted diseases. In R. Hoekelman (Ed.), Primary
Pediatric Care (3rd ed., pp. 1577–1593). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Kaestle, C. E., & Halpern, C. T. (2005). Sexual activity among adolescents in
romantic relationships with friends, acquaintances, or strangers. Archives of
Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 159(9), 849–853.
Kalil, A., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Coley, R. L. (2005). Perceptions of father in-
volvement patterns in teenage-mother families: Predictors and links to
mothers’ psychological adjustment. Family Relations, 54(2), 197–211.
Keating, D. P. (2004). Cognitive and brain development. In R. M. Lerner &
L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 45–84).
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Kirby, D. (1991). School-based clinics—research results and their implications
for future-research methods. Evaluation and Program Planning, 14(1/2),
35–47.
Kirby, D. (2001a). Emerging answers: Research findings on programs to reduce teen
pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Preg-
nancy.
Kirby, D. (2001b). Understanding what works and what doesn’t in reducing
adolescent sexual risk-taking. Family Planning Perspectives, 33(6), 276.
Kirby, D. (2002). Do abstinence only programs delay the initiation of sex among young
people and reduce teen pregnancy? Washington, DC: National Campaign to
Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Kirby, D., Baumler, E., Coyle, K. K., Basen-Engquist, K., Parcel, G. S., Harrist,
R., et al. (2004). The ‘‘safer choices’’ intervention: Its impact on the sexual
behaviors of different subgroups of high school students. Journal of Ado-
lescent Health, 35(6), 442–452.
Kirby, D., Korpi, M., Barth, R. P., & Cagampang, H. H. (1997). The impact of
the postponing sexual involvement curriculum among youths in Cali-
fornia. Family Planning Perspectives, 29(3), 100–108.
Kisker, E. E., & Brown, R. S. (1996). Do school-based health centers improve
adolescents’ access to health care, health status, and risk-taking behavior?
Journal of Adolescent Health, 18(5), 335–343.
Kroger, J. (2003). Identity development during adolescence. In G. R. Adams &
M. D. Berzonsky (eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 205–226).
Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Adolescent Sexuality 97
Marin, B. V., Kirby, D., Hudes, E., Gomez, C., & Coyle, K. K. (2003). Youth with
older boyfriends and girlfriends: Associations with sexual risk. In B. Albert, S.
Brown, & C. Flanigan (Eds.), 14 and younger: The sexual behavior of young
adolescents. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. D., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Scholarship on
fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
62(4), 1173.
Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Ventura, S., Menacker, F., & Park, M. M. (2002).
Births: Final data for 2000. Retrieved September 19, 2005, from
www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr50/nvsr50_05.pdf
Milan, S., Ickovics, J. R., Kershaw, T., Lewis, J., Meade, C., Ethier, K., et al.
(2004). Prevalence, course, and predictors of emotional distress in pregnant
and parenting adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
72(2), 328–340.
Miller, B. C. (1998). Families matter: A research synthesis of family influences on ado-
lescent pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen
Pregnancy.
Miller, B. C., Benson, B., & Galbraith, K. A. (2001). Family relationships and
adolescent pregnancy risk: A research synthesis. Developmental Review,
21(1), 1–38.
Moran, J. (2000). Teaching sex: The shaping of adolescence in the 20th century.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy. (2002). Not just another single issue:
Pregnancy prevention’s link to other critical social issues. Washington, DC: Author.
National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy. (2003a). The sexual attitudes and
behavior of male teens. Washington, DC: Author.
National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy. (2003b). Where and when teens first
have sex. Washington, DC: Author.
Nelson, T. J. (2004). Low-income fathers. Annual Review of Sociology, 30, 427–451.
O’Donnell, L., Myint, U. A., O’Donnell, C. R., & Stueve, A. (2003). Long-term
influence of sexual norms and attitudes on timing of sexual initiation
among urban minority youth. Journal of School Health, 73(2), 68–75.
O’Donnell, L., Stueve, A., O’Donnell, C., Duran, R., San Doval, A., Wilson,
R. F., et al. (2002). Long-term reductions in sexual initiation and sexual
activity among urban middle schoolers in the Reach for Health service
learning program. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31(1), 93–100.
Olsen, J., Weed, S., Nielsen, A., & Jensen, L. (1992). Student evaluation of sex-
education programs advocating abstinence. Adolescence, 27(106), 369–380.
Osofsky, J., Hann, D., & Peebles, C. (1993). Adolescent parenthood: Risks and
opportunities for mothers and infants. In C. Zeahah (Ed.), Handbook of
infant mental health (pp. 106–119). New York: Guilford Press.
Parent, A., Teilmann, G., Juul, A., Skakkeback, N. E., Toppari, J., & Bourgui-
gnon, J. (2003). The timing of normal puberty and the age limits of sexual
precocity: Variations around the world, secular trends, and changes after
migration. Endocrine Reviews, 24, 668–693.
Adolescent Sexuality 99
Parsons, J. T., Halkitis, P. N., Bimbi, D., & Borkowski, T. (2000). Perceptions of
the benefits and costs associated with condom use and unprotected sex
among late adolescent college students. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 377–391.
Paul, C., Fitzjohn, J., Eberhart-Phillips, J., Herbison, P., & Dickson, N. (2000).
Sexual abstinence at age 21 in New Zealand: The importance of religion.
Social Science and Medicine, 51(1), 1–10.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International
Universities Press.
Piaget, J. (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human
Development, 15, 1–12.
Rashad, I., & Kaestner, R. (2004). Teenage sex, drugs and alcohol use: Problems
identifying the cause of risky behaviors. Journal of Health Economics, 23(3),
493–503.
Reid, M. (2000). Kids in trouble. Auckland: Maxim Institute.
Remez, L. (2000). Oral sex among adolescents: Is it sex or is it abstinence? [Cover
story]. Family Planning Perspectives, 32(6), 298.
Rogol, A. D., Roemmich, J. N., & Clark, P. A. (2002). Growth at puberty.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 192–200.
Rose, A., Koo, H. P., Bhaskar, B., Anderson, K., White, G., & Jenkins, R. R.
(2005). The influence of primary caregivers on the sexual behavior of early
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 37(2), 135–144.
Rosenblum, G. D., & Lewis, M. (2003). Emotional development in adolescence.
In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence
(pp. 26–89). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Rosengard, C., Adler, N. E., Gurvey, J. E., Dunlop, M. B., Tschann, J. M., Millstein,
S. G., et al. (2001). Protective role of health values in adolescents’ future
intentions to use condoms. Journal of Adolescent Health, 29(3), 200–207.
Sanders, S. A., & Reinisch, J. M. (1999). Would you say you ‘‘had sex’’ if . . . ?
Journal of the American Medical Association, 281(3), 275–277.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (1994). Verbal and physical abuse as stressors in the lives of
lesbian, gay male, and bisexual youths: Associations with school problems,
running away, substance abuse, prostitution, and suicide. Journal of Con-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 261–269.
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Diamond, L. (2004). Sex. In R. M. Lerner &
L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 189–
231). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Schuster, M. A., Bell, R. M., & Kanouse, D. E. (1996). The sexual practices of
adolescent virgins: Genital sexual activities of high school students who
have never had vaginal intercourse. American Journal of Public Health,
86(11), 1570.
Schwartz, I. M. (1999). Sexual activity prior to coital initation: A comparison
between males and females. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28(1), 63.
Siegel, D. M., Aten, M. J., & Roghmann, K. J. (1998). Self-reported honesty
among middle and high school students responding to a sexual behavior
questionnaire. Journal of Adolescent Health, 23(1), 20–28.
100 Sexuality Today
Siegler, R. S. (1998). Children’s thinking (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Sieverding, J. A., Adler, N., Witt, S., & Ellen, J. (2005). The influence of parental
monitoring on adolescent sexual initiation. Archives of Pediatrics and Ado-
lescent Medicine, 159(8), 724–729.
Singh, S., & Darroch, J. E. (2000). Adolescent pregnancy and childbearing: Levels
and trends in developed countries. Family Planning Perspectives, 32(1),
14–23.
Smetana, J., & Gaines, C. (1999). Adolescent-parent conflict in middle-class African-
American families. Child Development, 66, 229–316.
Smith, E. A., & Udry, J. R. (1985). Coital and non-coital sexual behaviors of
white and black adolescents. American Journal of Public Health, 75(10),
1200.
Speizer, I. S., Magnani, R. J., & Colvin, C. E. (2003). The effectiveness of
adolescent reproductive health interventions in developing countries: A
review of the evidence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(5), 324–348.
Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Wei, E. H. (1998). The precursors of young father-
hood and its effect on delinquency of teenage males. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 22(1), 56–65.
St. Pierre, T. L., Mark, M. M., Kaltreider, D. L., & Aikin, K. J. (1995). A 27-
month evaluation of a sexual-activity prevention program in boys and girls
clubs across the nation. Family Relations, 44(1), 69–77.
Strassberg, D. S., & Lowe, K. (1995). Volunteer bias in sexuality research. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 24(4), 369.
Stueve, A., & O’Donnell, L. N. (2005). Early alcohol initiation and subsequent
sexual and alcohol risk behaviors among urban youths. American Journal of
Public Health, 95(5), 887–893.
Susman, E. J., & Rogol, A. (2004). Puberty and psychological development. In
R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd
ed., pp. 45–84). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Terry, E., & Manlove, J. (2000). Trends in sexual activity and contraceptive use among
teens. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Terry-Humen, E., & Manlove, B. L. (2003). Dating and sexual experiences
among middle school youth: Analyses of the NLSY97. In B. Albert,
S. Brown, & C. Flanigan (Eds.), 14 and younger: The sexual behavior of young
adolescents (pp. 17–30). Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent
Teen Pregnancy.
Terry-Humen, E., Manlove, J., & Moore, K. (2005). Playing catch-up: How children
born to teen mothers fare. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent
Teen Pregnancy.
Thornberry, T. P., Smith, C. A., & Howard, G. J. (1997). Risk factors for teenage
fatherhood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59(3), 505–522.
UNAIDS/WHO. (2004). AIDS epidemic update, December 2004. Geneva: Joint
United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS (UN-AIDS) and World
Health Organisation (WHO).
Adolescent Sexuality 101
Tom W. Smith 1
INTRODUCTION
Sexual behavior is not only of basic biological importance but also of central
social importance. Not only does it perpetuate the human species, but it is also
the central behavior around which families are formed and defined, a vital
aspect of the psychological well-being of individuals, and a component of a
variety of social problems. Among current concerns tied in part to sexual
behavior are (1) the familial problems of marital harmony and divorce;
(2) criminal problems of rape, incest, child molestation, and prostitution; (3)
reproductive problems of infertility, sterility, unwanted and mistimed preg-
nancies, and abortion; and (4) health problems related to sexually transmitted
diseases (STDs).
Moreover, with the coming of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV),
the problem of STDs has taken on increasing urgency (Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention [CDC], 2004a; Yankauer, 1994). Deaths from ac-
quired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) rose at a rapid pace in the 1980s
and early 1990s (CDC, 1995a). By 1992, AIDS had become the number one
cause of death among men aged 25 to 44. Cases diagnosed as AIDS peaked in
1993, and rates of AIDS-related deaths peaked in 1995. Both have since
declined substantially; cases of AIDS have fallen by more than two-thirds and
AIDS-related deaths by over 80 percent since 2001 (CDC, 1998a, 2001;
Ventura, Anderson, Martin, & Smith, 1998). Most HIV infections have
104 Sexuality Today
Cohabitation
The rise in premarital and adolescent sexual activity, coupled with delays
in marriage, has led to more unmarried people living together. Since 1970, the
rate of living together outside of marriage has increased more than sixfold,
from 1.1 percent to 7.4 percent of couples (see Bramlett & Mosher, 2002;
Smith, 1998, table 2). Similarly, the proportion of single mothers who were
cohabiting grew from 2 percent in 1970 to 12 percent in 1995 (London,
1998). While the proportion of cohabiting couples at any one point in time
106 Sexuality Today
remains relatively small, a large and growing percentage of couples live to-
gether at some point in their relationships. Currently, over one-third of adults
in their midtwenties to midthirties cohabited before their first marriage, and
half of this age-group has cohabited at some point in their lives (see Smith,
1998, table 3A). Cohabitation after a failed first marriage and between sub-
sequent marriages is even more common. According to the General Social
Survey (GSS), among those 25 to 44 years old who are in a second marriage,
61 percent cohabited with their new spouse before marriage (Davis, Smith, &
Marsden, 2003).
Rates of cohabitation are fairly consistent for both sexes and for most
ethnic and racial groups. Higher rates occur among younger adults, the di-
vorced, separated, never married, those living in urban areas, and those who
attend church less frequently (see Smith, 1998, table 3B). Cohabitation is
usually a short-term arrangement, leading to either marriage or a breakup after
about a year (median duration is 1.3 years) (Bumpass & Sweet, 1989; Thomson
& Colella, 1992; Thornton, 1988).
Cohabitation has often been characterized as a trial marriage, which is
fairly accurate: in 40 percent of cases it leads to marriage within two years, and
60 percent of cohabiting couples eventually marry (Bumpass & Sweet, 1989).
However, marriages formed after cohabitation are usually less stable and more
likely to end in divorce than marriages not preceded by living together (Axinn
& Thornton, 1992; Brown & Booth, 1996; Clarkberg, Stolzenberg, & Waite,
1995; DeMaris & MacDonald, 1993; DeMaris & Rao, 1992; Lillard, Brien, &
Waite, n.d.; Popenoe, 1993; Thomson & Colella, 1992). Cohabitation thus
does not seem to serve very well the function of a trial marriage (Popenoe).
Unmarried persons who are cohabiting have fewer sexual partners than
unmarried individuals who are not, but more sexual partners than married
couples (Waite & Joyner, 1996). For example, according to the GSS (Davis et
al., 2003), married persons averaged 1.01 partners over the preceding year, the
never married who were cohabiting had 1.39 partners, and the noncohabiting
never married had 1.67 partners. That fact, along with the temporary nature of
most cohabiting relationships, makes living together riskier than marriage
when it comes to STDs (Kost & Forrest, 1992; Turner et al., 1989).
Nonmarital Births
With the growing acceptance of sexual activity prior to marriage, the
connection between marriage and procreation has also lessened. In the 1960s
(and presumably before), when premarital sexual intercourse resulted in
conception, it usually resulted in marriage before the child was born (see
Driscoll et al., 1999; Smith, 1998, table 4; South, 1999). Since that time, the
likelihood of unmarried parents getting married before the birth of their child
has steadily fallen. By the 1990s, fewer than 25 percent of women who
conceived before marriage got married before the child’s birth.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 107
As a result of the higher level of premarital sexual activity and the decline
in marriages after a conception but prior to birth, there has been a large
increase in out-of-marriage births (Miller & Heaton, 1991; see Smith, 2003,
table 5). In 1960, only 5 percent of all births were to unmarried women. This
climbed to 14 percent by 1975 and 33 percent by 1994. Then, after over thirty
years of increase, the rate leveled off between 1994 and 2000 at approximately
33 percent of all births being to unmarried women.
The trend in the United States has been parallel to that in similar Western
and industrialized cultures. While the percent of births to unmarried mothers
climbed from 5 percent in 1960 to 33 percent in 1998 in the United States, it
rose from 5 percent to 38 percent in Great Britain, from 4 percent to 28
percent in Canada, and from 6 percent to 40 percent in France (Teitler, 2002;
U.S. Census Bureau, 2001).
The rate of increase in births to unmarried women has been much greater
for whites than for African Americans. For whites, the percent of unmarried
births has increased over elevenfold from 2.3 percent of all births in 1960 to
27.1 percent in 2000, while the number for African Americans grew from 21.6
percent in 1960 to 70.4 percent in 1994 (and then down to 68.5 percent by
2000). While the black-to-white ratio has fallen from a little over 9:1 in 1960
to under to 3:1 in 1990s, the gap between African Americans and whites rose
from 19 percentage points in 1960 to 44 to 46 percentage points from 1980 to
1996 (with a peak in 1993). The cumulative difference between whites and
African Americans is further shown by the fact that by ages 30 to 34, only 23
percent of never-married white women have given birth, while 69 percent of
never-married African American women have had a child (Bachu, 1991, 1995;
Loomis & Landale, 1994).
While both whites and African Americans have a greater number of
births occurring outside of marriage, these increases reflect distinct patterns for
these groups (see Smith, 1998, table 5). For whites, the unmarried birth rate
(number of births to unmarried women per 1,000 unmarried women aged 15–
44) rose throughout the period. It increased more than four times, from 9 in
1960 to 37–39 in 1994–2000. For African Americans, the rate was quite
variable over time. It fell from 98 in 1960 to 79 in 1985 before climbing again
to 91–93 in 1989–1990. In the early 1990s, this rate declined again, falling to
71.5 in 1999.
In addition, there are many unplanned births in the United States (Abma,
Chandra, Mosher, Peterson, & Piccinino, 1997; Williams, 1991). Of women
aged 15 to 44 who had a child in 1995, 28 percent reported that they had an
unintended birth, and this was 36 percent for women aged 40 to 44. Of those
with an unintended birth, 80 percent described the birth as mistimed and 20
percent as unwanted.
In brief, over the last century, premarital sexual activity became more
widespread, sexual initiation started at younger ages, the period of premarital
sexual activity lengthened, and the number of premarital sexual partners
108 Sexuality Today
Extramarital Relations
There are probably more scientifically worthless ‘‘facts’’ on extramarital
relations than on any other facet of human behavior. Popular magazines (e.g.,
Redbook, Psychology Today, Cosmopolitan), advice columnists (Dear Abby and
Joyce Brothers), pop sexologists (e.g., Morton Hunt and Shere Hite) have all
conducted or reported on ‘‘studies’’ of extramarital relations. These studies
typically report extremely high levels of extramarital activity (Gibbs, Hamil, &
Magruder-Habib, 1991; Reinisch, Sanders, & Ziemba-Davis, 1988; Smith,
1989, 1991b). Hite, for example, reported that 70 percent of women who
have been married five or more years ‘‘are having sex outside of their mar-
riage’’ (Smith, 1988). These questionable sources also often claim that extra-
marital relations have become much more common over time. Joyce Brothers
(1990), for example, claimed that 50 percent of married women were having
sex outside of marriage, twice the number of the previous generation.
However, representative scientific surveys (Choi, Catania, & Dolcini,
1994; Forste & Tanfer, 1996; Greeley, 1994; Greeley, Michael, & Smith, 1990;
Laumann et al., 1994; Leigh, Temple, & Trocki, 1993; Tanfer, 1994; Treas &
Giesen, 1996, 2000) indicate that extramarital relations are in fact much less
prevalent than claimed (see Smith, 2003, table 6). The best estimates suggest that
in a given year, approximately 3 to 4 percent of currently married people have a
Sexual Behavior in the United States 109
sexual partner other than their spouse. Over 90 percent of women and over 75
percent of men report being faithful to their spouses throughout their marriage
(Laumann et al.).
There is little reliable information on the prevalence of extramarital re-
lations before 1988. Some indirect evidence suggests that extramarital relations
may have increased across recent generations. The reported rates in 2002 were
13 percent among those 18 to 29 years old, and 20 percent among those 40 to
49 (see Smith, 2003, table 7). It then falls to 9.5 percent among those 70 and
older. Since these are lifetime rates, one would normally expect them either to
increase across age-groups or to increase until a plateau is reached (this would
be the case if few first-time extramarital relations were started among older
adults). The leveling off and then the drop among those 50 and older suggests
that members of birth cohorts before about 1940 were less likely to engage in
extramarital relations than are spouses from more recent generations (Greeley,
1994; Laumann et al., 1994).
Extramarital relations are apparently more common among younger
adults. This is probably a function of younger adults having been married a
shorter period of time and the difficulty shifting from a premarital pattern of
multiple sexual partners to an exclusive monogamous partnership; related to
that trend, recent marriages are more likely to end in divorce than be long-
term relationships. The rates of extramarital relations are about twice as high
among husbands as among wives (see Smith, 2003, table 7). Extramarital
relations are also more common among African Americans, those with lower
incomes, those who attend church less frequently, those who have been
separated or divorced (including those who have remarried), and those who
are unhappy with their marriage. It also may be more frequent among residents
of large cities, but the overall relationship with community type is fairly small
and somewhat inconsistent. Finally, extramarital relations do not vary signif-
icantly by education level.
indirect measurements (Aguilar & Hardy, 1991), and statistics from other
comparable countries such as Great Britain, France, Norway, and Denmark
(Biggar & Melbye, 1992; Diamond, 1993; Johnson, Wadsworth, Wellings,
Bradshaw, & Field, 1992; Melbye & Biggar, 1992; Sandfort, Hubert, Bajos, &
Bos, 1998; Spira, Bajos, Bejin, & Beltzer, 1992; Sundet, Magnus, Kvalem,
Groennesby, & Bakketeig, 1989; see also Smith, 2003, table 8B).
Rates of same-sex sexual activity increase as the reference period is ex-
tended. Recent figures (see Smith, 2003, table 9B) indicate that 3.4 percent of
sexually active males have had a male sexual partner in the preceding twelve
months, 4.1 percent during the previous five years, and 4.9 percent since age
18 (see also Michael, Laumann, & Gagnon, 1993; Smith, 1991a).1 As the time
frame is lengthened, the percentage of men with exclusively male partners
declines. Over the preceding twelve months, 2.8 percent of men identify as
gay and 0.6 percent as bisexual; over the last five years, 2.7 percent are gay and
1.4 percent are bisexual; and since age 18, fewer than 1 percent of men identify
as gay and 4 percent as bisexual. Most of those who report having both male
and female sexual partners since age 18 report only opposite sex partners
during the preceding year (Smith, 1991a). Lesbians follow these same patterns.
There is little reliable evidence on whether sexual orientation has changed
before the late 1980s. In terms of attitudes, levels of approval of homosexuality
declined slightly from 1973 to 1991, but then rose notably during 1992–2000
(Davis et al., 2003; Laumann et al., 1994; Smith, 1994). Studies of male and
female homosexuality both in the United States and in Europe regularly find a
higher proportion of gay men than lesbians (see Hubert, Bajos, & Sandfort,
1998; Johnson, Wadsworth, Wellings, & Field, 1994; Sandfort et al., 1998;
Smith, 1998, tables 8A and 8B; Spira, Bajos, & Ducot, 1994; Wells & Sell,
1990).2
Sexual orientation does not vary much across sociodemographic groups
(see Smith, 2003, table 9). The most distinctive pattern for both gays and
lesbians is that they are less likely to have been married. About 60 percent of
those with same-sex partners during the previous twelve months have never
been married, compared to the 16 percent of female heterosexuals and 21
percent of male heterosexuals. Second, gays, but not lesbians, are distinctive in
congregating in the largest central cities. About 8.8 percent of men in large
central cities have had a same-sex partner in the last year, as have 9.6 percent
over the last five years and 11.7 percent since age 18. Rates are lowest outside
of metropolitan areas. The relative concentration of gay men in large urban
centers also occurs in Europe ( Johnson et al., 1992; Spira et al., 1992). Les-
bians, like gays, are underrepresented in rural areas. Third, more gays and
lesbians are found in the lower-income categories, but the relationship is
stronger for men than for women. Fourth, race is only weakly related to sexual
orientation. Fifth, being gay is unrelated to education, but lifetime lesbian
activity appears higher among those with graduate degrees. Sixth, lesbians are
more common among younger age-groups. This could indicate an increase in
Sexual Behavior in the United States 111
homosexual activity among women across cohorts (see Rogers & Turner,
1991) or it could be a life-cycle effect. Gays show a similar but less pronounced
pattern. Finally, lesbians, but not gays, attend church less frequently than
heterosexuals. About 4.2 percent of women who rarely attend church have
had a female sexual partner in the last year compared to only 1.7 percent of
those who attend regularly.
Marsden, 2003) and those who say they are still ‘‘in love’’ (Greeley, 1991),
frequency of intercourse declines with age. Third, some research indicates that
the quality of sexual activity declines with marital duration which might re-
duce the frequency (Liu, 2003).
Marital status also influences sexual activity (see Smith, 2003, table 10B;
Wade & DeLamater, 2002). Frequency of sexual intercourse is greatest among
married couples (with those remarried slightly exceeding those in their first
marriage probably because of the honeymoon effect). The never married and
divorced have lower rates, probably because of less continuous and convenient
availability of a partner. The widowed have by far the lowest rates, a function
of their age as well as lack of a partner. The higher rates of intercourse among
married persons compared to unmarried persons are even more apparent when
age is taken into consideration. Sexual activity is 25 to 30 percent higher
among the married compared to the nonmarried at various ages. Among the
married, intercourse is more frequent among those who have happier marriages
(Smith, 1991a; Waite & Joyner, 1996). Interestingly, one national survey re-
ported that the highest frequency of intercourse occurred among cohabiting
couples (Laumann et al., 1994), perhaps due to the fact that most of those
relationships are relatively new.
Husbands and wives generally closely agree on the frequency of inter-
course, whether reporting jointly or separately (Bachrach, Evans, Ellison, &
Stolley, 1992; Smith, 1992a, 1992b). However, unmarried men and women
differ considerably, with men reporting more sexual activity than women do
(Bachrach et al.); this statistical anomaly holds up even after accounting for the
greater number of widowed women in the population.
A multivariate analysis indicates that higher rates of sexual intercourse are
separately and independently related to (a) being younger, (b) having been
married less than three years, and (c) rating one’s marriage as happy. It is
unrelated to gender when controlling for these other factors (Davis et al.,
2003).
Frequency of sexual activity also decreases as church attendance increases.
While this is somewhat related to the fact that church attendance increases
with age, there is still a decline controlling for age. There are few differences in
intercourse frequency across racial/ethnic groups, community type, education
level, or income. When these factors do seem to affect frequency of inter-
course, it can usually be explained by age and/or marital status. Likewise,
frequency does not vary if one or both partners are employed (Hyde, DeLa-
mater, & Durik, 2001).
Sexual Inactivity
Sexual inactivity can take three distinct forms: (1) the period prior to first
sexual intercourse, (2) periods of extended inactivity after first intercourse and
prior to last intercourse, and (3) a period of inactivity after last intercourse. The
Sexual Behavior in the United States 113
first has been dealt with above in the discussion of premarital sexual relations.
The latter two are discussed here, although it is difficult to distinguish between
them.
Sexual inactivity appears to have modestly declined since the early 1980s
(see Smith, 2003, table 11). For women of childbearing age and all adults, the
proportion not engaging in sex over extended periods (three to twelve months)
has decreased in the late 1980s and early 1990s. However, sexual inactivity has
increased since 1996.
For adults, there is a U-shaped curve with sexual inactivity most frequent
among the youngest and the oldest. Sexual inactivity among the elderly is fairly
common and is a function of aging, poor health, and unavailability of a partner.
As we saw in the section on frequency of sexual intercourse, sexual activity
decreases markedly with age even when a partner is available, perhaps due to
habituation or health problems. Higher rates of sexual inactivity are due to a
decline in frequency of sexual intercourse among those remaining sexually
active and also an increase in the percentage of sexually inactive persons.
Among those over 70 years old, 61 percent are not currently sexually active. In
this age-group, sexual abstinence occurs in 33 percent of married persons; for
unmarried adults, it is closer to 90 percent (see Smith, 1998, table 11).
Sexual inactivity is much less common among younger adults. Among
married young adults aged 18 to 49, only 1.0 to 2.4 percent are completely
sexually inactive. Virtually every case of sexual inactivity in this age-group is
associated with health problems and relationship dissatisfaction (Smith, 1992a;
see also Donnelly, 1993; Edwards & Booth, 1976). While 6 percent of married
couples of all ages were sexually inactive over the preceding year (Davis et al.,
2003), as many as 16 percent of married couples have not engaged in sexual
intercourse in the previous four weeks (Donnelly; see also Dolcini et al., 1993).
Sexual abstinence is much higher among the nonmarried, ranging between 15
percent and 28 percent for those under 50.
Most other sociodemographic differences are small and merely reflect
underlying differences in age and/or marital status, but sexual inactivity is
lower in households with higher incomes. While there have been significant
increases in all aspects of premarital and adolescent sexual activity, there is little
evidence of similar trends in adult sexual behavior. Moreover, adult sexual
behavior appears to be more restrained and traditional than it has commonly
been portrayed.
stricting the spread of the virus is to adopt safer sexual practices and injection
drug use behaviors.
On the one hand, the long latency period of HIV greatly complicates
matters since infected people are often not aware of the fact and can trans-
mit the virus to others. On the other hand, since the mid-1980s over 90
percent of the public have known that HIV is spread by sexual intercourse, and
knowledge about HIV in general has grown over time (CDC, 1998b, 2000;
Herek, Capitanio, & Widaman, 2002; Rogers, Singer, & Imperio, 1993;
Singer, Rogers, & Corcoran, 1987).3 Given the existence of widespread, if
imperfect, knowledge about the role of sexual intercourse in spreading HIV,
the question arises whether sexual behavior has been modified in light of the
known risk.
young males, the number of partners was probably rising for most of the
century until the early 1990s. However, the evidence is somewhat mixed for
the 1980s. The mean number of lifetime partners among sexually active males
aged 17–19 in metropolitan areas fell from 7.3 to 6.0 between 1979 and 1988,
while among sexually active males aged 17.5 to 19, the mean number of sexual
partners in the last twelve months rose from 2.0 in 1988 to 2.8 in 1991 (Ku et
al., 1993). During the 1990s, there appeared to be a decline in number of
sexual partners. The percentage of male high school students with a lifetime
total of four or more partners declined from 31 percent in 1989 to 14 percent
in 2001 (see Smith, 1998, table 1C). For young females, there was less clear
evidence of a decline in number of partners from the 1990s. The YRBS data
(CDC, 2004b) indicate year-to-year fluctuation rather than any definite trend.
The GSS shows a decline from the late 1980s to early 1990s to the mid-1990s
in the number of sexual partners among those aged 18 to 24, but no further
decrease and even possibly a partial increase in the late 1990s and into the
twenty-first century.
Despite reductions in number of partners among teenagers and young
adults, many youths are still at risk of HIV and other STDs because of having
multiple partners and other risky sexual behaviors (Anderson & Dahlberg, 1992;
Beckman, Harvey, & Tiersky, 1996; Ku, Sonenstein, & Pleck, 1994; Leigh et al.,
1993; Luster & Small, 1994; Smith, 1991a; Trocki, 1992; Tubman, Windle, &
Windle, 1996).
Whether the reported decline in number of partners among teenagers and
young adults will translate into a lower number of lifetime sexual partners is
unknown. If it does, it will reverse a trend that began several generations ago.
We can see evidence of that increase in the number of sexual partners since age
18 (see Smith, 2003, table 14). The increase in the number of sexual partners
from ages 18 to 29 to ages 40 to 59 mostly represents the accumulation of
partners over a person’s lifetime. The sharp drop in cumulative partners for
those 60 and older occurs because this age-group represents a generation that
came to age before the peak in premarital sexual activity. That is, they had
fewer premarital partners, married relatively early in life, and, as a result,
accumulated fewer lifetime sexual partners than subsequent generations.
Among adults, having multiple sexual partners during the previous year
and during the last five years is most strongly associated with being young,
unmarried, and male. It is also higher among African Americans (Bakken &
Winter, 2002), residents of large central cities, those with low incomes and less
education, and infrequent church attenders. The adult lifetime figures show a
similar pattern except that there is no relationship between income or race and
number of sexual partners, and the less educated have fewer partners than the
better educated. The reversal of the education relationship results from earlier
cohorts with less education having fewer partners than more recent and better
educated cohorts.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 117
Multiple partners are thus found in two main social niches: young un-
married adults and adolescents who have not yet ‘‘settled down,’’ and among
disadvantaged segments of society in general, including inner-city minorities,
who also tend to lead less stable and less conventional lifestyles (Ford & Norris,
1995; Wagstaff et al., 1995).
see that those sociodemographic groups with a high number of lifetime partners
also tend to have casual sexual relationships.
SUMMARY
Over the past century, the bonds between marriage and sexual activity
have been unraveling. A majority of young men and women have engaged in
premarital sexual intercourse, they have become sexually active at earlier ages,
and they have accumulated more lifetime sexual partners. While premarital
and adolescent sexual activity has increased for both men and women, the
most significant changes have been in the sexual behavior of women. The
higher rates of sexual behavior parallel a rise in cohabitation and a surge in
nonmarital births, and they have contributed to a variety of public health and
social welfare problems (Besharov & Gardiner, 1997).
Rather than being an isolated phenomenon, these changes in sexual be-
havior, living together, and childbearing have been part of broader social
changes toward an individualistic rather than a family-centered society (Glenn,
1987; Popenoe, 1993; Smith, 1999). These sexual and relationship trends also
mirror the changing roles in society for women (Firebaugh, 1990; Simon &
Landis, 1989). Moreover, there are signs of similar shifts in other postindustrial
societies. As such, the changes in American premarital and adolescent sexual
behavior may result from the development of advanced economies, welfare
states, and liberal governments in general rather than from any special situation
peculiar to North America.
These trends have recently slowed and, in a few aspects, have shown signs
of reversing. First, the increase in premarital and adolescent sexual activity has
slowed and waned to some extent. Second, the number of nonmarital births has
leveled off (albeit at near record levels). Third, condom use has more than
doubled over the last twenty years and apparently continues to increase. Al-
though there have not been decreases in every form of risky sexual behavior in
all segments of the population, these changing trends are notable and may
reflect an underlying shift in social values.
While marriage is no longer the entry point into sexual activity for most
Americans, it remains an important regulator of sexual behavior, and thus may
serve as a barrier to STDs. Since most married people tend to be monogamous,
marriage limits one’s total number of sexual partners and reduces the spread of
HIV and other STDs. However, marriage may be less of a barrier than it used
to be. Extramarital relations seem to be more prevalent among younger
married adults relative to older generations. Yet, there has been no change in
disapproval of extramarital relations over the years (Davis et al., 2003; Smith,
1990, 1994). Overall, though, extramarital relations have not increased since
1988, and marital infidelity is less common than suggested by popular culture.
Of course, marriages themselves are also not as enduring as they were in
the past. The two-and-a-half-fold increase in divorce rates from the 1960s to
Sexual Behavior in the United States 119
the early 1980s and its continuation at historically high levels to this day
suggest that approximately half of all recent marriages will end in divorce
(Smith, 1999). For most divorced people this means accumulating new sexual
partners, especially for those under 50 (Stack & Gundlach, 1992).
Sexual behavior is strongly influenced by age. In general, sexual activity
diminishes with age as evidenced by a declining number of sexual partners, less
extramarital sex, a reduced frequency of sexual intercourse, and higher rates
of sexual abstinence. Cohabitation rates and nonmarital births also decline
with age.
There are also significant differences in sexual behavior between whites
and African Americans (Bowser, 1992; Brewster, 1994; Brunswick et al., 1993;
Kilmarx et al., 1997; Peterson, Catania, Dolcini, & Faigeles, 1993; Quadagno,
Sly, Harrison, Eberstein, & Soler, 1998; Reitman et al., 1996; Smith, 1999;
Sterk-Elifson, 1992; but see Wyatt, 1989). On average, African Americans
become sexually active at an earlier age, accumulate more lifetime sexual
partners, have more casual partners, are less likely to marry, have shorter-term
marriages, and have many more children born outside of marriage.
Sexual behavior also varies by community type. Residents of large central
cities have more sexual partners, casual partners (including prostitutes), and
extramarital relations than those living in rural areas. In addition, probably due
to selective migration, gay men congregate in large cities. Since sexual and
injection-drug risk behaviors are more common in large cities as is the
prevalence of HIV, the chances of becoming infected are especially high in
these areas (Catania et al., 1992).
Finally, religion exercises a restraint on sexual behavior for some persons
(Brewster, Cooksey, Guilkey, & Rindfuss, 1998; Goldscheider & Mosher,
1991; Hogan, Sun, & Cornwell, 1998; Seidman, Mosher, & Aral, 1992; Stack
& Gundlach, 1992; Tanfer & Schoorl, 1992; Thornton & Camburn, 1989).
Those who attend church regularly are less likely to become sexually active, to
have multiple and casual partners, and to have extramarital relations. Church
attendance, like rural residence, imposes a restrictive influence on sexual be-
havior.
Despite the potentially deadly nature of HIV and the widespread knowl-
edge of risk factors, its impact on sexual behavior has been relatively limited.
The largest changes occurred among men who have sex with men in large
metropolitan centers who adopted considerably safer sexual practices. But the
resurgence in new cases of HIV infection suggests some complacency and
underestimation of risk in recent years (CDC, 2005a).
Among the heterosexual population, the largest change has been the in-
creased use of condoms. However, condom use is still inconsistent and hap-
hazard. The small reductions in the number of partners among adolescents and
youths may represent improved safer sex practices in response to the HIV
epidemic. However, those changes have not been universal: most people still
have numerous sexual partners, many of which involve casual relationships. It
120 Sexuality Today
NOTES
1. It is generally believed that including adolescent behavior would further
increase these rates, but firm numerical estimates are not available. For some
indication of this, see Billy et al., 1993, and Faulkner and Cranston, 1998.
However, other surveys of young adult and teenage sexual orientation do not
confirm this trend (Ku, Sonenstein, & Pleck, 1993). Spanning the lower and
higher estimates, Turner et al. (1998) found that among males aged 15–19 in 1995,
1.5 percent reported homosexual relations on a paper self-completion question-
naire, but 5.5 percent did so on an audio-computer-assisted self-completion
questionnaire.
2. A notable exception is a 1991 U.S. sample of men aged 20–39 and women
aged 20–37, which found that 2.3 percent of men and 4.1 percent of women had a
same-sex partner in the last ten years (Tanfer, 1994). This anomalous finding may
have resulted from the question format, which used a five-point scale ranging
from exclusively heterosexual to exclusively homosexual.
3. On knowledge among adolescents, see Kann et al., 1998.
4. On the relation of HIV-related risk behaviors and perceptions of risks, see
Holtzman, Bland, Lansky, and Mack, 2001, and on the positive relationship be-
tween risky behavior and testing for HIV, see Anderson, Carey, and Taveras,
2000.
5. One sexual risk factor not discussed is type of sexual activity (e.g., vaginal,
anal, and oral intercourse). On the comparative risk of these behaviors, see Susser,
Desvarieux, and Wittkowski, 1998.
REFERENCES
Abma, J. C., Chandra, A., Mosher, W., Peterson, L., & Piccinino, L. (1997).
Fertility, family planning, and women’s health: New data from the 1995 National
Survey of Family Growth. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 23. Hyattsville,
MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 121
Abma, J. C., & Sonenstein, F. L. (2000). Sexual activity and contraceptive practices
among teenagers in the United States, 1988 and 1995. Vital and Health Sta-
tistics, Series 23. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Aguilar, S., & Hardy, A. M. (1993). AIDS knowledge and attitudes for 1991: Data
from the National Health Survey. Advance Data, 225, 1–20.
Anderson, J. E., Carey, J. W., & Taveras, S. (2000). HIV testing among the
general U.S. population and persons at increased risk: Information from
national surveys, 1987–1996. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 1089–
1095.
Anderson, J. E., & Dahlberg, L. L. (1992). High-risk sexual behavior in the
general population: Results from a national survey, 1988–1990. Sexually
Transmitted Diseases, 19, 320–325.
Anderson, J. E., & Stall, R. (2002). Increased reporting of male-to-male sexual
activity in a national survey. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 29, 643–646.
Aral, S. O. (1994). Sexual behavior in sexually transmitted disease research. Sex-
ually Transmitted Diseases, 21, S59–S64.
Axinn, W. G., & Thornton, A. (1992). The relationship between cohabitation
and divorce: Selectivity or causal influence? Demography, 29, 357–374.
Bachrach, C. A. (1998, May). Assessing new survey findings. Paper presented to the
American Enterprise Institute Panel on Teenage Sexual Activity and
Contraceptive Use: An Update. Washington, DC.
Bachrach, C. A., Evans, V. J., Ellison, S. A., & Stolley, K. S. (1992, May). What
price do we pay for single sex fertility surveys? Paper presented to the Popu-
lation Association of America, Denver, CO.
Bachrach, C. A., & Horn, M. (1987, July). Married and unmarried couples: United
States, 1982. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 23, No. 15. Hyattsville,
MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Bachu, A. (1991). Fertility of American women: June 1990. Current Population
Reports, Series P-20, No. 454. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Bachu, A. (1995). Fertility of American women: June 1994. Current Population
Reports, Series P-20, No. 482. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Bakken, R. J., & Winter, M. (2002). Family characteristics and sexual behaviors
among black men in the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 34,
252–258.
Beckman, L. J., Harvey, S. M., & Tiersky, L. A. (1996). Attitudes about condoms
and condom use among college students. Journal of American College Health,
44, 243–249.
Besharov, D., & Gardiner, K. N. (1997). Trends in teen sexual behavior. Children
and Youth Services Review, 19, 341–368.
Biggar, R. J., & Melbye, M. (1992). Responses to anonymous questionnaires
concerning sexual behavior: A method to examine potential biases.
American Journal of Public Health, 82, 1506–1512.
122 Sexuality Today
Billy, J. O. G., Tanfer, K., Grady, W. R., & Klepinger, D. H. (1993). The sexual
behavior of men in the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 25, 52–60.
Black, D., Gates, G., Sanders, S., & Taylor, L. (2000). Demographics of the gay
and lesbian population in the United States: Evidence from available
systematic data sources. Demography, 37, 139–154.
Blair, J. (1999). A probability sample of gay urban males: The use of two-phase
adaptive sampling. Journal of Sex Research, 36, 39–44.
Bowser, B. P. (1992, May). African American male sexuality through the early life
course. Paper presented to the MacArthur Foundation Research Network
on Successful Mid-Life Development, New York.
Bramlett, M. D., & Mosher, W. D. (2002). Cohabitation, marriage, divorce, and
remarriage in the United States. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 23, No.
22, 1–93. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Brewster, K. L. (1994). Race differences in sexual activity among adolescent
women: The role of neighborhood characteristics. American Sociological
Review, 59, 408–424.
Brewster, K. L., Cooksey, E. C., Guilkey, D. K., & Rindfuss, R. R. (1998). The
changing impact of religion on the sexual and contraceptive behavior of
adolescent women in the United States. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
60, 493–504.
Brothers, J. (1990, February 18). Why Wives Have Affairs. Parade, 4–6.
Brown, S. L., & Booth, A. (1996). Cohabitation versus marriage: A comparison of
relationship quality. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 668–678.
Brunswick, A. F., Aidala, A., Dobkin, J., Howard, J., Titus, S. P., & Banaszak-
Holl, J. (1993). HIV-1 seroprevalence and risk behaviors in an urban
African-American community cohort. American Journal of Public Health,
83, 1390–1394.
Bumpass, L. L., & Sweet, J. (1989). National estimates of cohabitation. Demog-
raphy, 26, 615–626.
Butler, A. C. (2000). Trends in same-gender sexual partnering, 1988–1998.
Journal of Sex Research, 37, 333–343.
Call, V., Sprecher, S., & Schwartz, P. (1996). The incidence and frequency of marital
sex in a national sample. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 609–634.
Carballo-Dieguez, A., & Dolezal, C. (1996). HIV risk behaviors and obstacles to
condom use among Puerto Rican men in New York City who have sex
with men. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 1619–1622.
Catania, J. A., Coates, T. J., Stall, R., Turner, H. A., Peterson, J., Hearst, N., et al.
(1992). Prevalence of AIDS-related risk factors and condom use in
United States. Science, 258, 1101–1106.
Catania, J. A., Stone, V., Binson, D., & Dolcini, M. M. (1995). Changes in
condom use among heterosexuals in wave 3 of the AMEN survey. Journal
of Sex Research, 32, 193–200.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1992a). Selected behaviors that
increase risk for HIV infection among high school students—United
States, 1990. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 47, 231, 237–240.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 123
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1992b). Sexual behavior among
high school students—United States, 1990. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly
Report, 40, 885–888.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1994). Heterosexually acquired
AIDS—United States, 1993. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 43,
155–160.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1995a). First 500,000 AIDS cases—
United States. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 44, 850–853.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1995b). Trends in sexual risk taking
among high school students—United States, 1990, 1991, and 1993.
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 447, 124–125, 131–132.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1998a). Trends in the HIV and AIDS
epidemic, 1998. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1998b). Youth risk behavior
surveillance—United States, 1997. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report,
47, 1–89.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2000). HIV-related knowledge and
stigma—United States, 2000. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 50,
1062–1064.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001). Year-end edition HIV/AIDS
surveillance report. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004a). HIV/AIDS Surveillance
Report, 2003 (Vol. 15). Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004b). Youth risk behavior surveillance—
United States, 2003. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005a). HIV/AIDS among men who
have sex with men. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005b). HIV/AIDS Surveillance
Report, 2004 (Vol. 16). Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services.
Choi, K. H., Catania, J. A., & Dolcini, N. M. (1994). Extramarital sex and HIV
risk behavior among US adults: Results from the National AIDS Behav-
ioral Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 2003–2007.
Clarkberg, M., Stolzenberg, R. M., & Waite, L. J. (1995). Attitudes, values, and
entrance into cohabitational versus marital unions. Social Forces, 74, 609–634.
Davis, J. A., Smith, T. W., & Marsden, P. V. (2003). General Social Surveys, 1972–
2002: Cumulative codebook. Chicago: National Opinion Research Council.
DeMaris, A., & MacDonald, W. (1993). Premarital cohabitation and marital in-
stability: A test of the unconventionality hypothesis. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 55, 399–407.
124 Sexuality Today
DeMaris, A., & Rao, K. V. (1992). Premarital cohabitation and subsequent marital
stability in the United States: A reassessment. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 54, 178–190.
Diamond, M. (1993). Homosexuality and bisexuality in different populations.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22, 291–310.
di Mauro, D. (1995). Sexual research in the United States: An assessment of the social
and behavioral sciences. New York: Social Science Research Council.
Dolcini, M. M., Catania, J. A., Coates, T. J., Stall, R., Hudes, E. S., Gagnon, J. H.,
et al. (1993). Demographic characteristics of heterosexuals with multiple
partners: The National AIDS Behavior Surveys. Family Planning Perspec-
tives, 25, 208–214.
Donnelly, D. A. (1993). Sexually inactive marriage. Journal of Sex Research, 30,
171–179.
Driscoll, A. K., Hearn, G. K., Evans, V. J., Moore, K. A., Sugland, B. W., & Call,
V. (1999). Nonmarital childbearing among adult women. Journal of Mar-
riage and the Family, 61, 178–187.
Edwards, J. N., & Booth, A. (1976). The cessation of marital intercourse. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 1333–1336.
Ehrhardt, A. A., & Wasserheit, J. N. (1991). Age, gender, and sexual risk be-
haviors for sexually transmitted diseases in the United States. In J. N.
Wasserheit, S. O. Aral, K. K. Holmes, & P. J. Hitchcock (Eds.), Research
issues in human behavior and sexually transmitted diseases in the AIDS era (pp.
97–121). Washington, DC: American Society for Microbiology.
Ehrhardt, A. A., Yingling, S., & Warne, P. A. (1991). Sexual behavior in the era
of AIDS: What has changed in the United States? Annual Review of Sex
Research, 2, 25–47.
Faulkner, A. H., & Cranston, K. (1998). Correlates of same-sex sexual behavior in
a random sample of Massachusetts high school students. American Journal of
Public Health, 88, 262–266.
Feinleib, J. A., & Michael, R. T. (1998). Reported changes in sexual behavior
in response to AIDS in the United States. Preventive Medicine, 27, 400–
411.
Feldman, H. A., Goldstein, I., Hatzichristou, D. G., Krane, R. J., & McKinlay, J.
B. (1994). Impotence and its medical and psychosocial correlates in men
aged 40–70: Results of the Massachusetts Male Aging Study. Journal of
Urology, 151, 54–61.
Firebaugh, G. (1990, May). Components of change in gender role attitudes, 1972–
1988. Paper presented to the Population Association of America, Toronto,
Canada.
Ford, K., & Norris, A. E. (1995). Factors related to condom use with casual
partners among urban African-American and Hispanic males. AIDS Ed-
ucation and Prevention, 7, 494–503.
Forste, R., & Tanfer, K. (1996). Sexual exclusivity among dating, cohabiting, and
married women. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 594–600.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 125
Horowitz, S. M., Weis, D. L., & Laflin, M. T. (2001). Differences between sexual
orientation behavior groups and social background, quality of life, and
health behaviors. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 205–218.
Hubert, M., Bajos, N., & Sandfort, T. (Eds.). (1998). Sexual behaviour and HIV/
AIDS in Europe. London: University College London Press.
Hughes, M., & Gove, W. R. (1992, May). Sexual behavior and psychological well-
being over the life course. Paper presented to the MacArthur Foundation
Research Network on Successful Mid-Life Development, New York.
Hyde, J. S., DeLamater, J. D., & Durik, A. M. (2001). Sexuality and the dual-
earner couple, part II: Beyond the baby years. Journal of Sex Research, 38,
10–23.
James, W. H. (1981). The honeymoon effect on marital coitus. Journal of Sex
Research, 17, 114–123.
James, W. H. (1998). Coital frequency among married and cohabiting couples in
the United States. Journal of Biosocial Science, 30, 131–132.
Jasso, G. (1985). Marital coital frequency and the passage of time: Estimating
the separation effects of spouses’ ages and marital duration, birth, and
marriage cohorts, and period influences. American Sociological Review, 50,
224–241.
Jasso, G. (1986). Is it outlier deletion or is it sample truncation? Notes of science
and sexuality. American Sociological Review, 51, 738–742.
Johnson, A. M., Wadsworth, J., Wellings, K., Bradshaw, S., & Field, J. (1992).
Sexual lifestyles and HIV risk. Nature, 360, 410–412.
Johnson, A. M., Wadsworth, J., Wellings, K., & Field, J. (1994). Sexual attitudes
and lifestyles. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.
Joyner, K., & Laumann, E. O. (2001). Teenage sex and the sexual revolution. In
E. O. Laumann & R. T. Michael (Eds.), Sex, love, and health in America:
Private choices and public policies (pp. 41–71). Chicago: University of Chi-
cago Press.
Kahn, J. R., Kalsbeek, W. D., & Hofferth, S. L. (1988). National estimates of
teenage sexual activity: Evaluating the comparability of three national
surveys. Demography, 25, 189–204.
Kahn, J. R., & Udry, J. R. (1986). Marital coital frequency: Unnoticed outliers
and unspecified interactions lead to erroneous conclusions. American So-
ciological Review, 51, 734–737.
Kalichman, S. C. (1996). Continued sexual risk behavior among HIV-seropositive,
drug-using men—Atlanta, Washington, DC, and San Juan, Puerto Rico.
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 45, 151–152.
Kilmarx, P. H., Zaidi, A. A., Thomas, J. C., Nakashima, A. K., St. Louis, M. E.,
Flock, M. L., et al. (1997). Sociodemographic factors and the variation in
syphilis rates among U.S. counties, 1984 through 1993: An ecological
analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 1937–1943.
Kost, K., & Forrest, J. D. (1992). American women’s sexual behavior and ex-
posure to risk of sexually transmitted diseases. Family Planning Perspectives,
24, 244–254.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 127
Ku, L., Sonenstein, F. L., & Pleck, J. H. (1993). Young men’s risk behaviors for
HIV infection and sexually transmitted diseases, 1988 through 1991.
American Journal of Public Health, 83, 1609–1615.
Ku, L., Sonenstein, F. L., & Pleck, J. H. (1994). The dynamics of young men’s
condom use during and across relationships. Family Planning Perspectives,
26, 246–251.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Leiblum, S. R. (1990). Sexuality and the midlife woman. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 14, 495–508.
Leigh, B. C., Temple, M. T., & Trocki, K. F. (1993). The sexual behavior of U.S.
adults: Results from a national survey. American Journal of Public Health, 83,
1400–1408.
Levy, J. A. (1992, May). Sex and sexuality in later life stages. Paper presented to the
MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Mid-Life De-
velopment, New York.
Lillard, L. A., Brien, M. J., & Waite, L. J. (n.d.). Premarital cohabitation and sub-
sequent marital dissolution: A matter of self-selection? Unpublished paper.
Liu, C. (2003). Does quality of marital sex decline with duration? Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 32, 55–60.
London, R. A. (1998). Trends in single mothers’ living arrangements from 1970
to 1995: Correcting the current population survey. Demography, 35, 125–
131.
Loomis, L. S., & Landale, N. S. (1994). Nonmarital cohabitation and childbearing
among black and white American women. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 56, 949–962.
Luster, T., & Small, S. A. (1994). Factors associated with sexual risk-taking be-
haviors among adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 622–632.
McKinlay, J. B., & Feldman, H. A. (1992, May). Changes in sexual activity and
interest in the normally aging male: Results from the Massachusetts Male Aging
Study. Paper presented to the MacArthur Foundation Research Network
on Successful Mid-Life Development, New York.
McQuillan, G. M., Ezzati-Rice, T. M., Siller, A. B., Visscher, W., & Hurley, P.
(1994). Risk behavior and correlates of risk for HIV infection in the Dallas
County Household HIV Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 84,
747–753.
Melbye, M., & Biggar, R. J. (1992). Interactions between persons at risk for AIDS
and the general population in Denmark. American Journal of Epidemiology,
135, 593–602.
Michael, R. T., Laumann, E. O., & Gagnon, J. H. (1993, February). The number of
sexual partners in the U.S. Unpublished paper.
Michaels, S. K. (1998). Queer counts: The sociological construction of homosexuality
via survey research. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chi-
cago.
128 Sexuality Today
Miller, B. C., & Heaton, T. B. (1991). Age at first sexual intercourse and the timing
of marriage and childbirth. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 719–732.
Morokoff, P. J. (1988). Sexuality in perimenopausal and post-menopausal women.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 489–511.
National Council on the Aging (1998). Healthy sexuality and vital aging: A study by
the National Council on the Aging. Washington, DC: Author.
Osmond, D. H., Page, K., Wiley, J., Garret, K., Sheppard, H. W., Moss, A. R.,
et al. (1994). HIV infection in homosexual and bisexual men 18 to 29 years
of age: The San Francisco Young Men’s Health Study. American Journal of
Public Health, 84, 1933–1937.
Ostrow, D. G., Beltran, E., & Joseph, J. (1994). Sexual behavior research on a
cohort of gay men, 1984–1990: Can we predict how men will respond to
interventions? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 531–552.
Peipert, J. F., Domagalski, L., Boardman, L., Daamen, M., McCormack, W. M.,
& Zinner, S. H. (1997). Sexual behavior and contraceptive use: Changes
from 1975 to 1995 in college women. Journal of Reproductive Medicine, 42,
651–657.
Peterson, J. L., Catania, J. A., Dolcini, M. M., & Faigeles, B. (1993). Multiple
sexual partners among blacks in high-risk cities. Family Planning Perspec-
tives, 25, 263–267.
Popenoe, D. (1993). American family decline, 1960–1990: A review and appraisal.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 527–555.
Quadagno, D., Sly, D. F., Harrison, D. F., Eberstein, I. W., & Soler, H. R.
(1998). Ethnic differences in sexual decisions and sexual behavior. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 27, 57–75.
Rao, K. V., & VandenHeuvel, A. (1995). Coital frequency among married and
cohabiting couples in the United States. Journal of Biosocial Science, 27,
135–150.
Reinisch, J. M., Sanders, S. A., & Ziemba-Davis, M. (1988). The study of sexual
behavior in relation to the transmission of human immunodeficiency virus:
Caveats and recommendations. American Psychologist, 43, 921–927.
Reitman, D., St. Lawrence, J. S., Jefferson, K. W., Alleyne, E., Brasfield, T. L., &
Shirley, A. (1996). Predictors of African American adolescents’ condom
use and HIV risk behavior. AIDS Education and Prevention, 8, 499–515.
Rogers, S. M., & Turner, C. F. (1991). Male-male sexual contact in the U.S.A.:
Findings from five sample surveys, 1970–1990. Journal of Sex Research, 28,
491–519.
Rogers, T. F., Singer, E., & Imperio, J. (1993). AIDS—An update. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 57, 92–114.
Sandfort, T. G. M., Hubert, M. C., Bajos, N., & Bos, H. (1998). Sexual be-
haviour and HIV risk: Common patterns and differences between Euro-
pean countries. In M. C. Hubert, N. Bajos, & T. G. M. Sandfort (Eds.),
Sexual behaviour and HIV/AIDS in Europe (pp. 403–426). London: Uni-
versity College London Press.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 129
Schiavi, R. C. (1990). Sexuality and aging in men. Annals of Sex Research, 1, 227–
249.
Schiavi, R. C. (1992, May). Impact of chronic disease and medication on sexual func-
tioning. Paper presented to the MacArthur Foundation Research Network
on Successful Mid-Life Development, New York.
Seidman, S. N., Mosher, W. D., & Aral, S. O. (1992). Women with multiple
sexual partners: United States, 1988. American Journal of Public Health, 82,
1388–1394.
Seidman, S. N., & Rieder, R. O. (1994). A review of sexual behavior in the
United States. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 330–341.
Sell, R., & Becker, J. B. (2001). Sexual orientation data collection and progress
toward Healthy People 2010. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 876–882.
Simon, R. J., & Landis, J. M. (1989). Women’s and men’s attitudes about a
woman’s place and role. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53, 265–276.
Singer, E., Rogers, T. F., & Corcoran, M. (1987). AIDS. Public Opinion Quarterly,
51, 580–595.
Singh, S., & Darroch, J. E. (1999). Trends in sexual activity among adoles-
cent American women, 1982–1995. Family Planning Perspectives, 31, 212–
219.
Smith, T. W. (1988). Hite vs. Abby in methodological messes. AAPOR News, 15,
3–4.
Smith, T. W. (1989). Sex counts: A methodological critique of Hite’s Women in
Love. In C. F. Turner, H. G. Miller, & L. E. Moses (Eds.), AIDS: Sexual
behavior and intravenous drug use (pp. 537–457). Washington, DC: National
Academy of Sciences Press.
Smith, T. W. (1990). The Sexual Revolution? Public Opinion Quarterly, 54, 415–
435.
Smith, T. W. (1991a). Adult sexual behavior in 1989: Number of partners, fre-
quency of intercourse, and risk of AIDS. Family Planning Perspectives, 23,
102–107.
Smith, T. W. (1991b). A critique of the Kinsey Institute/Roper Organization
National Sex Knowledge Survey. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 449–457.
Smith, T. W. (1992a). Discrepancies between men and women in reporting
number of sexual partners: A summary from four countries. Social Biology,
39, 203–211.
Smith, T. W. (1992b). A methodological analysis of the sexual behavior questions
on the General Social Surveys. Journal of Official Statistics, 8, 309–326.
Smith, T. W. (1994). Attitudes towards sexual permissiveness: Trends, correlates,
and behavioral connections. In A. Rossi (Ed.), Sexuality across the lifecourse
(pp. 63–97). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Smith, T. W. (1998). American sexual behavior: Trends, socio-demographic differences,
and risk behavior. GSS Topical Report. Chicago: National Opinion Re-
search Council. Retrieved September 9, 2005, from www.norc.uchicago
.edu/library/sexual.pdf
130 Sexuality Today
Smith, T. W. (1999). The emerging 21st century American family (GSS Social Change
Report No. 42). Chicago: National Opinion Research Council.
Smith, T. W. (2003). American sexual behavior: Trends, socio-demographic differences,
and risk behavior. GSS Topical Report. Chicago: National Opinion Re-
search Council.
Sonenstein, F. L., Pleck, J. H., & Ku, L. C. (1991). Levels of sexual activity among
adolescent males in the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 23, 162–
167.
South, S. J. (1999). Historical changes and life course variation in the determinants
of premarital childbearing. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 753–763.
Spira, A., Bajos, N., Bejin, A., Beltzer, N., Bozon, M., Ducot, B., et al. (1992).
AIDS and sexual behaviour in France. Nature, 360, 407–409.
Spira, A., Bajos, N., & Ducot, B. (1994). Trends in sexual behavior. Sexually
Transmitted Diseases, 21, S14–S18.
Stack, S., & Gundlach, J. H. (1992). Divorce and sex. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
21, 359–367.
Sterk-Elifson, C. (1992, May). Sexuality among black American women. Paper pre-
sented to the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful
Mid-Life Development, New York.
Stokes, J. P., & McKiran, D. J. (1993). Estimating the prevalence of homosexual
behavior. Family Planning Perspectives, 25, 184–185.
Stossel, S. (1997). The Sexual Counterrevolution. American Prospect, 33, 74–82.
Sundet, J. M., Magnus, P., Kvalem, I. L., Groennesby, J. K., & Bakketeig, L. S.
(1989). Number of sexual partners and the use of condoms in the het-
erosexual population of Norway—relevance to HIV-infection. Health
Policy, 13, 159–167.
Susser, E., Desvarieux, M., & Wittkowski, K. M. (1998). Reporting sexual risk
behavior for HIV: A practical risk index and a method for improving risk
indices. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 671–674.
Swann, K. (1993, March/April). Percentage gay. Bad Subjects, 5, 1–3.
Tanfer, K. (1994). Sex and disease. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 21, S65–S72.
Tanfer, K., & Cubbins, L. A. (1992). Coital frequency among single women:
Normative constraints and situational opportunities. Journal of Sex Re-
search, 29, 221–250.
Tanfer, K., & Schoorl, J. J. (1992). Premarital sexual careers and partner change.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 45–68.
Teitler, J. O. (2002). Trends in youth sexual initiation and fertility in developed
countries, 1960–1995. Annals of AAPSS, 580, 134–152.
Thomson, E., & Colella, U. (1992). Cohabitation and marital stability: Quality or
commitment? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 259–267.
Thornton, A. (1988). Cohabitation and marriage in the 1980s. Demography, 25,
497–508.
Thornton, A., & Camburn, D. (1989). Religious participation and adolescent
sexual behavior and attitudes. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 641–
653.
Sexual Behavior in the United States 131
Treas, J., & Giesen, D. (1996, August). Sex, gender, and rational choice: Determinant
of infidelity among married and cohabiting couples. Paper presented to the
American Sociological Association, New York.
Treas, J., & Giesen, D. (2000). Sexual infidelity among married and cohabitating
Americans. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 48–60.
Trocki, K. (1992). Patterns of sexuality and risky sexuality in the general popu-
lation of a California county. Journal of Sex Research, 29, 85–94.
Trussell, J., & Westoff, C. F. (1980). Contraceptive practice and trends in coital
frequency. Family Planning Perspectives, 12, 246–249.
Tubman, J. G., Windle, M., & Windle, R. C. (1996). Cumulative sexual inter-
course patterns among middle adolescents: Problem behavior precursors and
concurrent health risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, 182–191.
Turner, C. F., Ku, L., Rogers, S. M., Lindberg, L. D., Pleck, J. H., & Sonenstein,
F. L. (1998). Adolescent sexual behavior, drug use, and violence: Increased re-
porting with computer survey technology. Science, 280, 867–873.
Turner, C. F., Miller, H. G., & Moses, L. E. (Eds.). (1989). AIDS: Sexual behaviors
and intravenous drug use. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Udry, J. R. (1980). Changes in the frequency of marital intercourse from panel
data. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 9, 319–325.
Udry, J. R., Bauman, K. E., & Morris, N. (1975). Changes in premarital coital
experience of recent decade-of-birth cohorts of urban American women.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 37, 783–787.
Udry, J. R., Deven, F. R., & Coleman, S. J. (1982). A cross-national comparison
of the relative influence of male and female age on the frequency of marital
intercourse. Journal of Biosocial Science, 14, 1–6.
Udry, J. R., & Morris, N. M. (1978). Relative contribution of male and female
age to the frequency of marital intercourse. Social Biology, 25, 128–134.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2001). Statistical abstract of the United States, 2001. Wash-
ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Ventura, S. J., Anderson, R. N., Martin, J. A., & Smith, B. L. (1998). Births and
deaths: Preliminary data for 1997. National vital statistics reports, 47 (4).
Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.
Wade, L. D., & DeLamater, J. D. (2002). Relationship dissolution as a life stage
transition: Effects on sexual attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 64, 898–914.
Wagstaff, D. A., Kelly, J. A., Perry, M. J., Sikkema, K. J., Solomon, L. J.,
Heckman, T. G., et al. (1995). Multiple partners, risky partners, and HIV
risk among low-income urban women. Family Planning Perspectives, 27,
241–245.
Waite, L. J., & Joyner, K. (1996, December). Men’s and women’s general happiness and
sexual satisfaction in marriage, cohabitation, and single living. Paper presented to
the Population Research Center’s Demography Workshop, Chicago, IL.
Wells, J. A., & Sell, R. L. (1990). Project HOPE’s international survey of AIDS
educational messages and behavior change: France, the United Kingdom and the
United States. Chevy Chase, MD: Project HOPE.
132 Sexuality Today
Whitbeck, L. B., Simons, R. L., & Goldberg, E. (1996). Adolescent sexual in-
tercourse. In R. L. Simons (Ed.), Understanding differences between divorced
and intact families: Stress, interaction, and child outcome (pp. 144–156).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Williams, L. B. (1991). Determinants of unintended childbearing among never-
married women in the United States: 1973–1988. Family Planning Per-
spectives, 23, 212–215, 221.
Wyatt, G. E. (1989). Reexamining factors predicting Afro-American and white
American women’s age at first coitus. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 271–
298.
Wyatt, G. E., Peters, S. D., & Guthrie, D. (1988). ‘‘Kinsey revisited, part I:
Comparisons of sexual socialization and sexual behavior in white women
over 33 years.’’ Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17, 201–239.
Yankauer, A. (1994). Sexually transmitted diseases: A neglected public health
priority. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 1894–1897.
6
Thomasina H. Sharpe 1
SEXUALITY IN LATER LIFE
The subject of sexuality triggers awkwardness and discomfort in many people.
The topic of older adults’ sexuality evokes even more discomfort, if not dis-
gust. Although sexuality is a fundamental need across the life span, society
typically ignores the sexual needs and concerns of older adults. The older
population of the United States is growing at an unprecedented rate. In 2003,
persons 65 and older accounted for 12.4 percent of the population. By 2030,
that number will rise to 20 percent or higher (U.S. Census Bureau, 2001). The
so-called baby boomer generation (the cohort of people born between 1946
and 1964) will increasingly present many challenges to views of sexuality in
late life.
There are signs that the growing population of older persons will force
society to reconsider its views of aging. Being able to live in one’s own home
is important to most adults, and older adults are no exception (Novelli, 2002).
‘‘Aging in place,’’ as it is called, is especially important to baby boomers, who
are challenging many existing views of aging. Not only do they intend to stay
in their own homes, they also intend to maintain their current lifestyles into
advancing age. They expect to remain healthy well into their seventies and
eighties (Lenahan, 2004). This generation, which came of age during the
sexual revolution of the 1960s, seems determined to retain its sexual life as
well.
134 Sexuality Today
Today, adults are living longer, healthier, and wealthier lives than their
parents. And they expect to have healthy sexual lives too (American Associ-
ation of Retired Persons [AARP], 1999, 2005). Most older adults view sex as
essential to a successful relationship, and for many it is important to overall
quality of life. Marketing executives certainly have realized that sex sells not
only to young adults: aging baby boomers are interested in sex (see Katz &
Marshall, 2003). Magazines put steamy pictures of graying models on their
covers and announce, ‘‘Sixty Is the New Thirty!’’ Beer ads feature elders
drinking and necking on the sofa only to be caught by their shocked and much
older parents. A Round-Heeled Woman, a memoir, follows the sexual exploits of
a 66-year-old woman on a quest to have ‘‘a lot of sex with a man I like,’’
becoming a best seller ( Juska, 2003). Television news shows and newspapers
lead with headlines that tout the results of the latest surveys such as those by
the Association of Reproductive Health Professionals, the National Council
on the Aging, and the Kaiser Family Foundation, which show that people are
maintaining satisfying sex lives well into their eighties and even nineties
(Sexuality Education and Information Council of the United States [SIECUS],
2002). Yet, despite this budding awareness that older adults are sexual beings,
there is little information on sexuality in late life.
Late life is a stage of development like any other across the life span. Aging
brings with it many challenges similar to those faced in childhood or adoles-
cence. Sexuality, sexual behavior, intimacy, and relationships are fundamental
human needs, whether we are 14 or 84. This chapter will address some of the
unique tasks that we face as we age and how they affect our sexuality.
First, it is important to define some of the terms related to sexuality and
aging. Sexual behavior is any form of physical intimacy that may be motivated
by the desire to reproduce or to enjoy sexual gratification. Sexual desire is the
need for sexual intimacy. Sexuality is often used as the general term for the
feelings and behaviors of a human being concerning sex (Carroll & Wolpe,
1996). Sexuality encompasses both sexual behaviors and sexual desire. Sexual
development occurs through distinct stages, each with its own tasks, chal-
lenges, and outcomes. To understand what this means in later life, it is helpful
to examine sexual development at the earlier stages, beginning with child-
hood.
Sexuality and sexual behavior in very young children are mainly based on
curiosity. Preadolescents have little physical or mental investment in sexuality.
Most of their energy is devoted to forming a sense of identity as part of a
community, collecting information and myths about sexuality from friends,
school, and family, as well as forming a sense of morality they will use later in
their sexual lives. Along with the striking changes of puberty, adolescents
begin to revise their individual identities to include sexuality and sexual de-
velopment. In Western cultures, adolescence provides opportunities to ex-
periment with intimacy, the freedom to explore one’s own maturing body,
and the chance to master skills useful in the transition to adulthood. During
Later Life Sexuality 135
findings from these small samples to the general older population (Rossi,
1994).
Similarly, the cultural and social changes that have occurred over the last
thirty to sixty years since these studies were conducted are too significant to
ignore. Improvements in health and advanced longevity each challenge the
relevance of these studies to today’s older adults. For example, the average
woman now lives to be 82 and, thus, she can be expected to live one-third of
her life post-menopausal (Kingsberg, 2002). Simply stated, one-fourth of a
woman’s sexual life will be experienced in her later years. Given that fact, it is
remarkable that there has been so little research on sexual development in
older adults.
Finally, the elderly are a more varied group than most people believe. In
fact, the definition of ‘‘aged’’ continues to change as medical advances improve
the quality of life and longevity. The definition of ‘‘late life’’ tends to be
reserved for persons over the age of 65, historically, the age of retirement. Yet,
the growing numbers of 70-, 80-, and even 90-year-olds in our population
means that late life is a period that can span thirty years or more. We must
remember that the differences between people in their sixties and those in
their eighties are often greater than those between 20- and 60-year-olds. The
differences between a healthy 65-year-old and his 90-year-old parent who
suffers from dementia can be more extreme than the differences between a
teenager and her 43-year-old mother. In fact, our definition of late life means
that two living generations of the same family could be included in the same
category, and hence they could be facing some of the very same challenges,
including those relating to sexual development.
W H A T I S ‘‘ O L D ’’ ?
When discussing late life, it is important to determine what is ‘‘old.’’
Obviously, like beauty, ‘‘old’’ is in the eyes of the beholder. In other words,
‘‘old’’ is highly subjective. People in their eighties may not consider themselves
old. In fact, many adults in later life do not consider themselves old because
they feel healthy and maintain active lives. They have fallen prey to stereotypes
of the elderly as decrepit or infirm and, therefore, believe that as long as they
‘‘feel’’ young and remain active they are not old.
We must consider that how we define old has very real ramifications for
policy formation, public perceptions, and for individuals’ self-concept (Cala-
santi & Slevin, 2001). For our purposes, we will use chronological age as our
criterion and define later life as the period after age 65 until death. But this too
can be a biased and flawed definition because age identification is made up of
more than chronological age or the number of years since a person’s birth. It
also is made up of how old one feels as compared to one’s peers, known as
subjective age, and one’s assessment of one’s own status in relation to the ideal
for one’s age-group, or functional age (Calasanti & Slevin). It is important to
Later Life Sexuality 137
note that as life expectancy is extended and our knowledge of what is ‘‘normal
development’’ for this stage grows, it may become necessary to divide later life
into further stages.
When discussing aging and sexual physiology in late life, most articles and
texts adhere closely to the observations of Masters and Johnson (1966). They
concentrated on senescence or the weakening and decline in the body, as well
as the sexual response cycle and the changes in individuals over the age of 40.
The sexual response cycle consists of four phases: drive or desire, arousal, release
or orgasm, and resolution, which includes the refractory period in men. In the
presence of desire, a person experiences the drive to engage in sexual behavior.
Effective sexual stimulation, whether psychological or physical, triggers erotic
arousal or excitement. The plateau phase, a period of sustained arousal, follows.
Continued effective sexual stimulation leads to orgasm, followed by resolution,
during which organs and tissues return to their resting state. The pattern of
changes throughout the sexual response cycle applies to both sexes with the
exception of the resolution phase. Following orgasm, men experience a re-
fractory period during which they are unable to respond to further stimulation
until an obligatory period of rest has occurred (for a review, see Trudel, Tur-
geon, & Piché, 2000).
Drive or desire remains fairly stable in both men and women throughout
life. Multiple studies, including those by Avis (2000), Laumann et al. (1999), as
well as surveys by the National Council on the Aging, AARP (1999, 2005), and
ARHP (2002), have shown that frequency of sexual activity may decrease but
satisfaction and desire do not (Kingsberg, 2002; Schuster & Ashburn, 1992).
Arousal is the phase most affected by aging. In women, the decline and
eventual cessation of estrogen production during menopause may lead to at-
rophy of urogenital tissues and an overall decrease in genital vasocongestion and
lubrication during arousal. Both sexes may experience a prolonged arousal
phase, which may require more direct genital stimulation (Demeter, 1998;
Masters & Johnson, 1966; Miller, Versi, & Resnik, 1999).
Orgasm tends to be the phase least affected by aging, especially in women.
Men may need sustained direct stimulation and additional time to reach orgasm.
140 Sexuality Today
They also tend to have decreased volume of ejaculate and less forceful ejac-
ulation. Women retain their ability to achieve orgasm. Some women may
experience pain during orgasm that is associated with the orgasmic contrac-
tions of the uterus and vagina becoming less rhythmic and coordinated (De-
meter, 1998; Masters & Johnson, 1966; Miller et al., 1999). Yet, despite this
change, older persons report that their orgasms are as satisfying as ever. For
those who have problems achieving orgasm, it is likely to be related to
medications, illness, or previous problems with orgasm. In other words, aging
alone does not interfere with the ability to have an orgasm.
The refractory period is the phase most affected in older men. The res-
olution phase right after orgasm becomes shorter with advanced age: in other
words, men return to the non-aroused state more quickly with advanced age.
But the time that it takes a man to regroup, the refractory period, is prolonged
as it can take anywhere from twelve to twenty-four hours or longer before he
can achieve another orgasm (Milsten & Slowinski, 1999). Women tend not to
have a refractory period and, therefore, are less affected by aging during this
phase. A woman who is multiorgasmic will remain so in late life (Beers &
Berkow, 2000; Demeter, 1998; Masters & Johnson, 1966; Miller et al., 1999).
In women, other changes that occur with aging include shortening and
narrowing of the vagina, as well as changes in the chemistry of the vagina;
decreased acidic secretions increase the risk of vaginal infections (Trudel et al.,
2000). Cystitis, or bladder infection, is more common in older women. De-
creased estrogen levels may also lead to a decrease in clitoral size, incontinence,
and a graying and thinning of pubic hair. Estrogen replacement therapy pre-
vents or reduces many of these problems. However, estrogen replacement may
also increase the risk of some cancers and heart attacks in women with heart
disease and therefore must be used with caution. Women who remain sexually
active have fewer problems maintaining their sexual activity and genital
health. Some of the declines in sexual functioning that are more common in
older persons are related to illness, medication side-effects, or even sexual
inactivity rather than to aging per se (Beers & Berkow, 2000; Demeter, 1998;
Miller et al., 1999).
Normal physical changes in older men include decreased production of
testosterone, which levels off around age 60. Likewise, the testicles decrease in
size and firmness, sperm production is reduced, and the prostate increases in
size. In addition, men may notice that preejaculatory fluid is reduced. Erec-
tions may be less durable and less firm. Men do not experience an equivalent
of menopause, and they often remain fertile throughout life. Although sexual
dysfunction is not a part of aging, erectile dysfunction is a common concern
for many older men. The incidence of erectile dysfunction does increase with
age, but aging per se is not the cause. Medical conditions and medications are
usually responsible for the increased rate of erectile dysfunction in older men
(Laumann et al., 1999). Stress and emotional problems also can affect erectile
Later Life Sexuality 141
relations, and those who were able to maintain active sex lives were believed
to have exceptional qualities that helped them gain social status and even
increase their life span. In contrast, older women were thought to be able to
have sex in later years only if they were able to trick a man into going to bed
with them, a feat so abhorrent that it required the aid of witchcraft. Bullough
(1976) revealed that medieval religious prohibitions mirrored popular beliefs
about sexuality in the elderly. At the core is the belief that sexual intercourse
was intended for procreation only. This doctrine promoted the belief that
older adults who had sex were engaged in a ‘‘sin against nature.’’ Thus, we can
trace clichés of ‘‘wicked witches’’ and ‘‘dirty old men’’ to distant history
(Covey, 1989; Hillman, 2000).
In contemporary Western cultures, we equate aging with dying, and we
view older adults as defective or decrepit. This view is fortunately not uni-
versal. Many cultures prize and admire the characteristics of old age. In a
groundbreaking study of more than 106 cultures, Winn and Newton (1982)
described many of the beliefs about sexuality in late life as stereotypes. In fact,
most cultures they studied had favorable views of sexuality and aging: fewer
than 3 percent have prohibitions against sex for older adults. The vast majority
of older adults, 70 percent of men and 84 percent of women, enjoyed active
sexual lives. In many Eastern and Middle Eastern cultures, men who were as
old as 100 continued to engage in sexual relations. Many cultures did not view
a loss of sexual functioning as an inevitable part of aging. For example, some
African cultures attribute erectile problems to such unnatural phenomena as
illness or witchcraft (Winn & Newton).
In the majority of these traditional cultures, menopause is not associated
with changes in older women’s level of sexual activity: it is simply a phase in a
woman’s life. In certain African and Asian cultures, an older woman’s physical
attractiveness is unrelated to her sexual status: older women are considered as
sexually desirable as younger women. In addition, although a double standard
operates with regard to elderly sexuality, it is in the opposite direction: older
women are more likely to engage in sexual relations and are often described as
becoming less sexually inhibited and more sexually adventuresome with age.
In certain South American and Eastern cultures, older women even serve as
sex educators for sexually inexperienced young men (Winn & Newton, 1982).
An ancient Turkish proverb illustrates the positive sexual attitudes espoused by
many traditional and preindustrial cultures: ‘‘Young love is from earth, while
late love is from heaven’’ (Hillman, 2000).
Health care providers, unfortunately, are not exempt from ageist attitudes.
Even physicians often assume that sexuality is unimportant in late life (Butler,
1975). The American health care system also perpetuates ageism by focusing
on acute care and cure rather than on chronic care, which some older adults
need. It is also done covertly by denying or limiting services, by not including
aging issues in training materials or educational offerings for providers, and by
not requiring training in geriatrics in medical schools even though older adults
144 Sexuality Today
constitute a significant proportion of their future patients. But ageism is not the
only obstacle to a healthy expression of sexuality for older adults. Some of the
other barriers include lack of a partner, sexual dysfunction, attitudes of adult
children, altered body image, previous attitudes toward sex, attitudes of peers,
religious prohibitions against sex outside of marriage, depression, lack of au-
tonomy of choice, lack of privacy, marital conflict, and libido mismatch. As
Judith Levy (1994) noted in Sexuality Across the Life Course:
well-being rather than separating it out. She points out the importance of
understanding that all meaningful sexual relationships are not heterosexual;
accepting masturbation as an expression of sexuality; providing touch along
with ‘‘feeling’’ objects to handle, fondle, and hold; using live pets to provide
sensory stimulation; encouraging the use of music (romantic, sentimental,
sensuous, and erotic) in nursing homes; and encouraging the opportunity for
sexes to meet, mingle, and spend time together without structuring the trysting
time or place too rigidly (Stiffl; Burnside, 1988).
Staff attitudes and institutional barriers against sexual expression are not
the only factors affecting sexual expression in nursing-home residents. Illness,
dementia, lack of privacy, and medications continue to be negative factors.
Elderly people in nursing homes are also limited in their sexual opportunities
by their own attitudes. Although sexual activity outside of marriage is widely
accepted now, this was not the case during 1920–1940, when these elderly
were developing a sense of morals. Also, the inequality in the ratio of men to
women in these institutions effectively deprives lone elderly women of an
opportunity to maintain an active sex life.
CONCLUSIONS
Sexuality is a fundamental need for adults of all ages. Older adults usually
retain an interest in sexual intimacy, and they view it as an important part of
rewarding relationships (AARP, 2005). Unfortunately, negative views of aging
and common stereotypes are detrimental to sexual enjoyment later in life. A
growing body of research reveals that older adults are able to enjoy sexual
fulfillment if they are physically and psychologically healthy and if they have a
partner (Trudel et al., 2000). The sexual problems that are more prevalent in
148 Sexuality Today
older populations are not products of aging per se: they are due to factors such as
medications or illnesses. There are predictable changes in sexual functioning
with advancing age, but none of these preclude sexual activity although they
may require some adjustments.
The sexual needs of the growing segment of the population have been largely
neglected. Similarly, the needs of the oldest adults, those living in institutions, and
those suffering from health problems, including dementia, are only beginning to
be understood. More research is needed to address these needs and to combat the
negative stereotypes and myths about sexuality and aging.
REFERENCES
Abbink, C. (1983). Adult development and the impact of disruption. In S. Lewis
& I. Collier (Eds.), Medical-surgical nursing: Assessment and management of
clinical problems (pp. 14–31). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Adams, M. S., Oye, J., & Parker, T. S. (2003). Sexuality of older adults and the
Internet: From sex education to cybersex. Sexual and Relationship Therapy,
18, 405–415.
American Association of Retired Persons. (1999). Modern maturity sexuality study.
Washington, DC: Author.
American Association of Retired Persons. (2005). Sexuality at midlife and beyond:
2004 update of attitudes and behaviors. Washington, DC: Author.
Association of Reproductive Health Professionals. (2002). Sexuality in middle and
later life. Retrieved October 10, 2003, from www.siecus.org/pubs/fact/
fact0018.html
Atchley, R. C. (1989). A continuity theory of normal aging. Gerontologist, 29(2),
183–190.
Avis, N. E. (2000). Is there an association between menopause status and sexual
functioning? Menopause, 7(5), 286–288.
Beers, M. H., & Berkow, R. (2000). Sexuality. Retrieved October 10, 2003, from
www.merck.com/mrkshared/mm_geriatrics/sec14/ch114.jsp
Berger, K. S., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). The developing person through the lifespan
(4th ed.). New York: Worth.
Berger, R. M. (1995). Gay and gray: The older homosexual man (2nd ed.). New
York: Haworth Press.
Booth, B. (1990). Does it really matter at that age? Nursing Times, 86, 51–52.
Brogan, M. (1996). The sexual needs of elderly people: Addressing the issue.
Nursing Standard, 10, 42–45.
Bullough, V. L. (1976). Sex, society, and history. New York: Science History.
Burnside, I. M. (1988). Nursing and the aged: A self care approach (3rd ed.). New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Butler, R. N. (1969). Age-ism: Another form of bigotry. Gerontologist, 9, 243–
246.
Butler, R. N. (1975). Psychiatry and the elderly: An overview [Abstract]. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 893–900.
Later Life Sexuality 149
Calasanti, T. M., & Slevin, K. F. (2001). Gender, social inequalities, and aging.
Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.
Carroll, J. L., & Wolpe, P. R. (1996). Sexuality and gender in society. New York:
HarperCollins.
Cole, T. (1992). The journey of life: A cultural history of aging in America. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Covey, H. C. (1989). Perceptions and attitudes toward sexuality of the elderly
during the Middle Ages [Abstract]. Gerontologist, 29, 93–100.
Cox, H. (2005). Annual editions: Aging 05/06. Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill/
Dushkin.
Cummings, E., & Henry, W. (1961). Growing old: The process of disengagement.
New York: Basic Books.
Demeter, D. (1998). The human sexuality web: Sex and the elderly. Retrieved Oc-
tober 10, 2003, from www.umkc.edu/sites/hsw/age/index.html
Dunkle, R., Roberts, B., & Haug, M. (2001). The oldest of the old in everyday life:
Self-perception, coping with change and stress. New York: Springer.
Ehrenfeld, M., Bronner, G., Tabak, N., Alpert, R., & Bergman, R. (1999).
Sexuality among institutionalized elderly patients with dementia. Nursing
Ethics, 6, 144–149.
Ekerdt, D. J. (1986). The busy ethic: Moral continuity between work and re-
tirement. Gerontologist, 26(3), 239–244.
Erikson, E. H. (1984). Reflections on the last stage—and the first. Harvard Edu-
cational Review, 51, 249–269.
Fischer, D. H. (1977). Growing old in America. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Fogel, C. I., & Lauver, D. (Eds.) (1990). Sexual health promotion. Philadephia:
W. B. Saunders.
Francher, J. S., & Henkin, J. (1973). The menopausal queen: Adjustment to aging and
the male homosexual. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 43(4), 670–674.
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis (1st ed.). New York: W. W. Norton.
Friend, R. A. (1990). Older lesbian and gay people: A theory of successful aging.
Journal of Homosexuality, 20(3/4), 99–118.
Galindo, D., & Kaiser, F. E. (1995). Sexual health after 60. Patient Care, 29, 25–38.
Haber, C. (1983). Beyond sixty-five: The dilemma of old age in America’s past. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Hillman, J. (2000). Clinical perspectives on elderly sexuality. New York: Kluwer
Academic.
Hordern, A. J. (2003). A patient-centered approach to sexuality in the face of life-
threatening illness. Medical Journal of Australia, 179, S8–S11.
Human Sexuality. (2005). In Wikipedia (Vol. 1). Retrieved March 8, 2005, from
www.en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sexual_behavior
Juska, J. (2003). A round-heeled woman: My late-life adventures in sex and romance.
New York: Villard Books.
Kart, C. S. (1989). The realities of aging—An introduction to gerontology. Boston: Allyn
& Bacon.
150 Sexuality Today
Michael R. Kauth 1
For two centuries, Western culture has devoted enormous attention to sexual
orientation, particularly, homosexuality (Bullough, 1994). Shifting social be-
liefs about homosexuality have influenced psychiatric concepts and practices,
law and civil rights such as marriage, as well as military service. This chapter
describes the concept of sexual orientation, sexual identities in America and in
other cultures, current sexuality theories, and recent events involving sexual
orientation.
and passive sexual partners (i.e., penetrated by partner) (Carrier, 1980; Espin,
1993). Cross-gender behavior in these cultures violates gender role norms and
is stigmatized. Men who are sexually penetrated are considered effeminate, and
vice versa, while men who penetrate a male partner bear no stigma and are
considered masculine. In some Latin and Mediterranean cultures, anal inter-
course with a receptive male may demonstrate hypermasculinity and may be
witnessed by one’s peers (Carrier). In fact, male-dominant societies with rigid
gender roles report a high incidence of male-male sexual behavior (Reiss,
1986). For women, however, same-sex encounters in gender-role-rigid so-
cieties are often seen as antifamily and are taboo (Espin).
Some cultures have classified individuals who behave or dress as the other
sex as a third gender, both male and female. Same-sex activity is often associated
with third-gendered individuals (Greenberg, 1988). The waria of Indonesia
(including Java, Sumatra, most of Borneo, West Irian, and other small islands),
hijra of India, mahu of Tahiti, xanith of Middle Eastern Oman (Mihalik, 1988),
and washoga of Muslim Mombasa in Kenya represent third genders (Carrier,
1980). Several Native American Indian societies have reported third and even
fourth genders, including the Iroquois, California, Eskimo, Comanche,
Cherokee, Illinois, Nadowessi, Chippewas, Koniag, Oglala, Quinault, Crow,
Cheyenne, Creek, Yokot, Sioux, Fox, Sac, Zuni, Pima, Mohave, Navajo,
Cree, Dakota, Siksika, Arikara, Mandan, Florida, and Yucatan, although the
practice has largely ended with increased Westernization (Greenberg; Wil-
liams, 1986). Third- or fourth-gendered individuals are sometimes referred to
as berdache (those with male physiology) or amazons (those with female phys-
iology), but the more neutral collective term, two-spirited people, is preferred
today (Roscoe, 1998). Social roles and statuses for two-spirited people varied
across cultures. The Navajo nádleehi were farmers, sheepherders, and weavers
who often achieved wealth, while Lakota winkte were typically powerful
spiritual leaders and warriors.
Exclusive same-sex erotic relationships are rare in human history and
more typical of contemporary Western culture. While male-female relation-
ships are typical in most societies, same-sex relationships, at least for males, are
not uncommon and may have been the norm in some cultures (Bleys, 1995;
Cantarella, 1992; Greenberg, 1988). The frequency of same-sex behavior
among diverse societies suggests that many people have the capacity for same-
sex eroticism or a bisexual orientation (Kauth, 2000).
attractions are orthogonal dimensions, forming a grid. This model allows for
strong attraction to both sexes, and no attraction to either sex. Klein, Sepekoff,
and Wolf (1985) have proposed seven dimensions of sexual orientation, each
assessed for three time periods, producing twenty-one scores. Coleman (1987)
developed a nine-dimensional model in which current and ideal self-identities
are scored, along with physical, gender, sex-role, and sexual orientation. Al-
though these models are conceptually more sophisticated, they are also more
complicated and rarely employed in research (Chung & Katayama, 1996).
The models of sexual orientation just discussed tie attraction to sex-
gender. However, Ross (1987) has argued that social characteristics—age,
physical build, race, personality traits, mentoring, and dominance—and pleasure
play a greater role in partner choice than sex or gender. Anthropologists claim
that social context and cultural gender roles largely determine partner choice
(Blackwood, 1999; Herdt, 1997). Gagnon and Simon (1973) have purported
that sexuality exists only in a social context and that social scripts determined
which relationships are sexual. Diamond (2003) proposed that sexual desire and
affectional bonding are functionally independent. Especially for women, af-
fectional bonding may be less oriented toward one or the other sex. Rather,
people can be characterized by their capacity to form affectional bonds, re-
gardless of sex of partner.
The nature of sexual orientation refers to whether the concept is viewed as
a natural kind—universal and invariant—or a social construction in which sexual
identities and social roles are specific to a sociohistorical period (Stein, 1999). If
a natural kind, then sexual attraction is presumed to be experienced today as it
was in the past, and similar forms of attraction should be evident across cul-
tures. If so, particular attractions represent kinds of people. Researchers who
hold this view use homosexual and heterosexual as nouns, rather than adjectives,
and believe that these sexual kinds of people are represented in non-Western
cultures that hold very different ideas about sexuality. However, if sexual
orientation is a social construction, then gay and straight represent specific ideas
about personhood, politics, gender, psychology, and sexuality, whose meaning
has evolved with changing social beliefs (Foucault, 1978/1990; Katz, 1995).
Researchers who hold this view recognize that sexual identities are specific to
a particular culture and time and do not describe a universal kind of person.
Other writers have claimed that while sexual attractions are universal, their
meaning is influenced by individual, social, and cultural factors (Baumeister,
2000; Kauth, 2000).
Finally, the concept of sexual orientation is influenced by presumed bi-
ological and environmental effects on attraction (Stein, 1999). Researchers
who allege direct environmental effects on sexual orientation tend to view
attraction as flexible, while investigators who propose direct biological effects
view attraction as relatively stable but perhaps not fixed. The conventional
hormonal theory that atypical exposure to fetal androgens results in same-sex
attraction is often presented as a direct biological effect (Hershberger, 2001).
Sexual Orientation and Identity 159
Evolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary theories attempt to explain ultimate causation—why a trait
developed among ancestral humans to promote reproductive success. Traits
that over many generations advantaged reproductive success are called adapta-
tions and were dispersed throughout the population (Buss, 1998). Evolutionists
have long puzzled over the persistence of same-sex attraction, a trait that pre-
sumably leads to few offspring and less reproductive success. Traits can
persist without reproductive benefit, as by-products of an adaptation (e.g.,
poetry) or random effects of a genetic variation, but these are conclusions of last
resort.
Wilson (1978) suggested that same-sex-oriented individuals may benefit
their siblings’ reproductive success if they directed their energies and resources
to raising their siblings’ children (who share some of their genes). Trivers
(1974) has also proposed that altruistic same-sex-oriented individuals advan-
tage their parents by maximizing reproductive success for some offspring and
minimizing competition for mates and scarce resources. Both theories rest on
shaky assumptions that same-sex-oriented individuals are altruistic and parents
can manipulate their children’s sexuality. Little evidence supports either as-
sumption (Bobrow & Bailey, 2001; Kirkpatrick, 2000).
Separately, Kauth (2000), Kirkpatrick (2000), and Muscarella (2000) have
proposed that same-sex attraction developed as a survival strategy to manage
conflict and competition within same-sex groups among ancestral hunters and
gatherers. That is, same-sex eroticism facilitated long-term intimate alliances
with equal- or higher-status peers and served to manage within-group hos-
tilities and ensure mutual cooperation, loyalty, access to high-quality resources,
160 Sexuality Today
and social status. An alliance with a high-ranking male may have helped young
males achieve social status and ultimately gain access to high-status females, and
females may have preferred mates with loyal friends and high social status
(Muscarella, 2000). For young females, an alliance with a high-ranking, older
female may have been critical for gaining within-group acceptance, quality
nutritional and material resources, and assistance during pregnancy and child
rearing (Kauth, 2000). In many preagricultural societies, and perhaps in early
human societies, very young females leave their natal group to live with their
husband’s female kin (Campbell, 1985). Sexual behavior may have strength-
ened same-sex alliances by reducing conflict and providing pleasure. Thus,
reproductive success is hypothesized to have been enhanced by erotic rela-
tionships with both sexes. Several cultures have favored same-sex alliances
(Kirkpatrick, 2000; Muscarella, 2000) and, in the ancient world, sexual rela-
tionships with both sexes were not uncommon (Cantarella, 1992; Greenberg,
1988).
Muscarella, Cevallos, Siler-Knogl, and Peterson (2005) found support for
perceived advantages of contemporary same-sex erotic alliances. Despite stigma
associated with homosexuality, university students viewed same-sex relation-
ships as providing increased social and reproductive opportunities if the indi-
vidual benefited by greater social status, economic advantages, or career
opportunities.
Ultimate causation is contrasted with proximate causation or how a trait
occurs. Proximate causal theories describe immediate events that produce a
trait. The following are proximate causal theories.
Psychoanalytic Theory
Freud produced four different theories of male homosexuality and one
weak theory of female homosexuality. The theory most repeated by Freud to
explain male homosexuality involved a young boy who overvalued his penis
and avoided castration anxiety and disgust that mother lacked a penis by
choosing sexual partners who resembled himself (Freud, 1905/1953; Lewes,
1988). Later, he speculated that unconscious same-sex erotic identifications
represented the most common form of homosexuality (Freud, 1918/1953). In
this case, males struggle with their desire to be loved by a father figure (in the
way that father loves mother) and yet maintain their (heterosexual) masculin-
ity. Freud suggested that men who admired other men struggled with un-
conscious homosexual feelings. He explained female homosexuality as a girl’s
disappointment that her father would not give her a child, leading to her
rejection of him and all other men (Freud, 1920/1953).
Although ambivalent about whether homosexuality was a mental illness,
Freud saw it as a disruption of normal psychosexual development. Freud is one
of the few theorists who proposed theories of heterosexual development. These
are described in a later section in this chapter, ‘‘Sexual Identity Formation.’’
Sexual Orientation and Identity 161
Conversion/Reparative Therapy
Psychoanalytic ideas resurfaced in sexual-orientation conversion therapies,
frequently blended with Christian fundamentalism (Besen, 2003). Conversion
therapies purport to change homosexual orientation to heterosexual. These
therapies explain same-sex attraction as sin, inferiority and social inadequacy,
confusion of gender roles, poor masculine identity, weak attachment to the
same-sex parent, depression, fetal trauma, and/or poor heterosocial skills.
Treatment involves a combination of self-labeling as heterosexual, prayer, Bible
study, rejection of gay friends and the gay ‘‘lifestyle,’’ suppression of same-sex
feelings, sports, heterosexual activity, and, ultimately, marriage. Recipients of
conversion therapies are often called ‘‘ex-gays.’’ Nicolosi (1991), a leading
proponent of conversion, coined the term reparative therapy to emphasize that the
treatment corrects a problem that prevents full psychological maturity. Nicolosi
leads the National Association for Research and Therapy of Homosexuality
(NARTH), an organization of conversion therapists whose goals are to make
homosexuality a mental illness again and promote a cure for it (Besen). Love in
Action, Exodus, Homosexuals Anonymous, Evergreen International, Desert
Stream, and Living Waters represent religious-based conversion organizations.
Nicolosi (1991) has claimed to cure one-third of his gay patients and
improve heterosexual functioning in another one-third, but has refused to
provide verifiable data for examination. However, Beckstead (2001) has de-
scribed twenty individuals who claimed benefit from conversion therapies.
162 Sexuality Today
Personality Theories
In a classic study, Green (1987) followed a group of extremely feminine
boys who had been referred for treatment and an age-matched group of
masculine boys for fifteen years. As young adults, thirty-two (73 percent)
formerly feminine boys identified as gay or bisexual, while only one (4 per-
cent) masculine boy was bisexual. Green attributed same-sex attraction among
some boys to parenting that permitted feminine behavior and failed to en-
courage traditional masculine behavior, but cautioned against concluding
that childhood femininity leads to male homosexuality. However, in a set of
identical twins in the study, the 23-year-old feminine twin was married
but acknowledged a strong attraction to males and identified as gay. His
masculine twin had a male lover and a pregnant girlfriend and reported a
strong attraction to females. Another study of identical male triplets where one
brother was gay (HM) and two were heterosexual (HT1 and HT2) further
weakens the relationship between male femininity and adult sexual orienta-
tion (Hershberger & Segal, 2004). While both HT1 and HM scored similarly
on measures of femininity, HT1 endorsed the ‘‘desirability’’ of having male
sexual partners, although he had never had sex with men and was currently
married.
Nevertheless, Byne and Parsons (1993) have hypothesized that cross-
gender traits promote same-sex attraction, citing the prevalence of childhood
gender nonconformity among gay men and lesbians. They described male-
typical traits as novelty seeking, low harm-avoiding, and reward independent
and female-typical traits as low novelty-seeking, harm avoiding, and reward
dependent. Presumably, boys who lack one or more male traits feel different
from their male peers and are more open to female-typical experiences. Byne
and Parsons did not explain how feeling different and spending time with girls
leads to male homosexuality.
Bem (1996) has hypothesized that children with cross-gender traits so-
cialize with other-sex children because they fear and feel anxious around same-
sex peers. During puberty, this fear and anxiety gets interpreted as sexual
arousal, a process Bem called exotic-to-erotic. Although the idea of eroticized
differences is consistent with heterosexual development, the theory has strange
implications for nonheterosexuals. For example, feminine (pregay) boys should
be sexualized to masculine (heterosexual) boys, not men and not other gay
males. The theory does not account for masculine gay men or feminine
heterosexual men. Bem appears to confuse sexual orientation with comple-
mentary gender roles and has dismissed bisexuality as irrelevant, when in
actuality his model is likely to produce pansexuals (Kauth, 2000). If children
eroticized traits or classes of people who differ from their peer group, then
sexual attraction may not be limited to one sex, and sex of partner may not be
the sole characteristic that influences attraction.
164 Sexuality Today
Biomedical Theories
Biomedical theorists generally presume that sexual orientation is a product
of genes, exposure to hormones, or some other internal event. Theorists rarely
discuss other-sex attraction and often imply a gender inversion model of
homosexuality. Typical development is assumed to produce heterosexual,
masculine males who play a sexually active role (i.e., the inserter) and het-
erosexual, feminine females who are sexually receptive. Being natural, het-
erosexual orientation is given no further explanation. Gay men and lesbians are
thought to experience atypical development and possess cross-gender traits.
Bisexuals are virtually ignored by biomedical theorists.
Genetic Studies
Linking genes to homosexuality suggests a biological effect. Indeed,
Hamer, Hu, Magnuson, Hu, and Pattatucci (1993) found an 82 percent cor-
relation among five consecutive markers on the X chromosome (Xp28) for
forty pairs of gay brothers. In addition, the men’s maternal uncles were more
likely to be gay than were their paternal uncles (7.3 percent versus. 1.7 per-
cent). A follow-up study found a 67 percent correlation among the five
markers for thirty-three pairs of gay brothers (Hu et al., 1995). Hamer con-
cluded that at least one form of male-male attraction is transmitted maternally.
Lesbian sisters, however, did not share these markers with their gay brothers,
suggesting a different mechanism for female homosexuality. Other investiga-
tors have reported less convincing data (Rice, Anderson, Risch, & Ebers,
1999), but failed to replicate Hamer’s methods.
Traits highly correlated among twins who share identical genetic material
also support a biological basis for sexual orientation. Bailey and Pillard (1991)
found a 52 percent correlation for homosexuality among fifty-six gay men
with an identical twin. Only 22 percent of fifty-four gay men with a non-
identical twin were gay, and 11 percent of adopted brothers were gay. A second
study found a 48 percent correlation for homosexuality among seventy-one
lesbians with an identical twin compared to 16 percent of thirty-seven lesbians
with a nonidentical lesbian twin (Bailey, Pillard, Neale, & Agyei, 1993). Only
14 percent of adopted sisters identified as lesbian. Other researchers have
reported a 65 percent correlation for homosexuality among identical male
twins and a 30 percent correlation among nonidentical male twins (Whitam,
Diamond, & Martin, 1993). This study also included three sets of triplets. In
one set, the identical twin brothers were gay, but their nonidentical sister was
heterosexual. In the second set, the identical twin sisters were lesbian and their
nonidentical sister was not. And, in the third set, all three identical brothers
were gay. However, a large sample of Australian twins found only a 20 percent
correlation for homosexuality among identical twin males and 24 percent
Sexual Orientation and Identity 165
among identical twin females (Bailey et al., 2000). The investigators con-
cluded that subject recruitment procedures inadvertently inflated earlier cor-
relations.
These studies suggest two important points: (a) male homosexuality is more
strongly biological (heritable) than female homosexuality, and (b) heritable
biological factors alone fail to account for same-sex attraction. Genes only
influence neurochemical processes such as hormone production. These pro-
cesses in turn are influenced by prenatal and postnatal environments. Even
identical twins do not share identical prenatal environments.
gay men had a lower 2D:4D ratio than age-matched heterosexual men, and
bisexual men had a lower ratio than gay men. However, Williams et al. (2000)
have found a lower ratio only for gay men with older brothers, although
Robinson and Manning had reported no birth order effect. Williams et al.
(2000) also found a lower 2D:4D ratio among lesbians compared with het-
erosexual women. Contrary to the conventional androgen theory, these
studies suggest that same-sex attraction is associated with high prenatal an-
drogen levels, particularly among males with older brothers. Attraction to both
sexes may be associated with very high fetal androgen exposure.
Leftward asymmetry in fingerprint pattern is a female-typical effect that is
present as early as seven weeks postconception, and gay men are more likely
than heterosexual men to evidence this leftward asymmetry (Hall, 2000).
Finger-ridge counts on the left and right hands also differ by sex and sexual
orientation. However, among a set of identical male triplets, the predicted
pattern held for one heterosexual (HT1) and one gay brother (HM), but the
most masculine heterosexual triplet (HT2) had a ridge count similar to his gay
brother (Hershberger & Segal, 2004).
Handedness may be determined before birth and shows sex differences.
Most adults (89 percent) are right-handed, and men are more likely to be left-
handed than women (Gilbert & Wysocki, 1992). Left-handedness is associated
with a number of developmental problems that are more typical in males and,
thus, linked to androgen exposure. Left-handedness has sometimes been as-
sociated with male homosexuality (Lindesay, 1987). A meta-analysis of twenty
studies found a higher frequency of non–right-handedness among gay men and
lesbians compared to heterosexuals (Lalumière, Blanchard, & Zucker, 2000).
However, these studies obscure the point that most gay men and lesbians are
right-handed and most left-handers are heterosexual. Among one set of iden-
tical male triplets, the heterosexual brothers differed on handedness, and the
gay brother was right-handed (Hershberger & Segal, 2004).
A small but consistent birth-order effect has been found among gay
men (Blanchard, Zucker, Siegelman, Dickey, & Klassen, 1998). That is, gay men
have more older brothers than heterosexual men. Lesbians, however, do not
differ from heterosexual women in number of any category of siblings.
Blanchard and Klassen (1997) have hypothesized that this birth-order effect
among gay men might be explained by a maternal immune response to male-
specific fetal hormones, inhibiting heterosexual orientation. The idea that
maternal stress blocks male fetal hormones comes from studies of laboratory
rats (Dörner, 1979). Male rats exposed to maternal stress hormones more
frequently present themselves to males and show little interest in females.
Dörner et al. (1980) speculated that pregnant human mothers living during
periods of extreme social stress (e.g., the war years in Germany) would have
more gay male children and, indeed, they found such an effect although
greater social acceptance of homosexuality over time may explain this finding.
When mothers of adult gay and heterosexual men were questioned about
Sexual Orientation and Identity 167
stressors during pregnancy, they reported similar levels of stress across stages of
pregnancy (Bailey, Willerman, & Parks, 1991). Mothers of lesbians reported
greater stress during the first and second trimesters, although the theory did not
predict female homosexuality. However, other researchers have found that
mothers of gay men reported higher levels of stress during the first and second
months of pregnancy than mothers of heterosexual men, consistent with the
maternal stress theory (Ellis & Cole-Harding, 2001). No differences were
found between mothers of lesbian and heterosexual women.
The auditory system differs by sex. Males have an advantage in auditory
discrimination tasks, and females hear high frequencies better than males
(McFadden & Pasanen, 1998). The inner ear also typically emits sounds—
otoacoustic emissions (OAEs)—that differ by sex and are, thus, related to
prenatal androgen exposure. Yet, contrary to the conventional androgen
theory, McFadden and Pasanen (1998, 1999) found no differences in OAEs
among gay, bisexual, and heterosexual men. They hypothesized that variation
in timing and concentration of androgen at different brain sites accounts for
variation in sexual orientation among men. McFadden and Pasanen (1998,
1999) also found that lesbian and bisexual women demonstrated OAEs that
were more male-typical. This is the strongest evidence to date that links
female-female attraction to prenatal androgen exposure.
Sex differences in neuroanatomic size and functioning are well docu-
mented. Some structural differences in a number of sites have been linked to
male homosexuality, but the relevance of these findings is unclear. In most cases,
these sites are not known to influence sexual attraction. However, the anterior
hypothalamus in humans is thought to be functionally similar to the preoptic
area in male rats, which regulates sexual behavior, specifically, mounting be-
havior. LeVay (1991) found that for presumed gay men and heterosexual
women a section of the anterior hypothalamus (third interstitial nuclei) was
similar in size but was two to three times larger for heterosexual men. Of course,
human sexual behavior is more complex than mounting behavior in rats, and
what role the anterior hypothalamus plays in sexual orientation is uncertain.
As for cognitive differences, generally gay men have performed less well
on mental rotations (spatial ability) but better on verbal fluency tasks than
heterosexual men (Gladue, Beatty, Larson, & Staton, 1990; McCormick &
Witelson, 1991). Most functional differences have been attributed to non–
right-handed gay men, although right-handed and non–right-handed het-
erosexual men are more variable than gay men on fluency tasks (McCormick
& Witelson, 1991). Gay men and heterosexual women have performed sim-
ilarly on tests of spatial abilities, which differed significantly from heterosexual
men (Rahman & Wilson, 2003; Rahman, Wilson, & Abrahams, 2003). By
contrast, lesbians have performed similarly to heterosexual women on many
cognitive tasks (Gladue et al., 1990) or better than heterosexual women on
visuomotor targeting, and similarly to heterosexual men (Hall & Kimura,
1995). Among a set of identical male triplets discordant for sexual orientation,
168 Sexuality Today
Heterosexual Identity
Freud (1905/1953) proposed one of the few theories of heterosexual
development. He supposed that all children are born capable of erotic at-
traction to anyone or anything. According to Freud, from an early age, male
children know that they have a penis and believe that everyone else does too.
Upon learning that females do not have a penis, boys fear that theirs will be
taken from them. Gradually, young boys identify with their penis-bearing
father and view him as a rival for their mother’s affection, a psychic conflict
that Freud called the Oedipal complex. The fear that father (or other males) will
cut off a boy’s penis prohibits him from forming erotic attachments to males.
Young girls allegedly go through a parallel process. Girls, upon realizing that
they do not have a penis but boys do, become jealous. They identify with their
mother but blame her for their lack of a penis. The young girl believes that she
and her mother are competing for her father’s affection, a psychic conflict
called the Electra complex. In directing their erotic attachments to the other-sex
and engaging in heterosexual activity, boys and girls reach Freud’s final stage of
psychosexual development.
A study of fourteen heterosexual men and twelve women who wrote
about their own identity development provides some support for Freud’s ideas
about male psychosexual development (Eliason, 1995). Most men described
arriving at their heterosexual identity by first rejecting a gay identity. How-
ever, women often considered a lesbian or bisexual identity before choosing a
heterosexual one.
Bisexual Identity
Female bisexuals have reported experiencing other-sex attraction and be-
havior before same-sex feelings, while male bisexuals have reported experiencing
170 Sexuality Today
same-sex eroticism before or at the same time as other-sex behavior (Fox, 1995).
Despite these differences in timing of attractions, Weinberg et al. (1994) have
proposed four stages of collective bisexual identity formation: (a) initial confusion—
erotic feelings for both sexes are recognized, producing confusion and discomfort;
(b) applying a label—acknowledgment that sex with both men and women is
pleasurable; (c) settling into an identity—greater self-acceptance is often associated
with meaningful relationships; and (d) continued uncertainty—confusion regarding
identity stems from exclusive relationships and difficulty managing multiple
partners. Although most participants in this study thought that bisexuality was not
a phase for them, 40 percent acknowledged that their identity might change in the
future (Weinberg et al., 1994). Rust (2000) has noted that bisexuals experience
multiple shifts in identity depending on their current partner and argued that a
stable identity is not the endpoint for bisexuals. Ault (1996) has noted that some
bisexual women adopt ‘‘fractured’’ identities—e.g., lesbian-identified bisexual—
in an attempt to maintain identification with lesbian communities and politics.
For bisexuals in this culture, sexual identity appears to be a fluid characteristic.
Gay/Lesbian Identity
Cass (1979) proposed an early model of gay/lesbian development, in-
volving six stages: (1) identity confusion—uncertainty generated by awareness of
same-sex feelings; (2) identity comparison—awareness of being different pro-
duces a great deal of internal conflict; (3) identity tolerance—self-acceptance of
same-sex desires and sense of belonging to a stigmatized minority; (4) identity
acceptance—openly disclosing one’s same-sex feelings and identity; (5) identity
pride—newly out gay men and lesbians relish their supportive gay family of
friends and reject the heterosexual community as intolerant and hostile; and (6)
identity synthesis—public and private identities meld into a single self-concept.
Collective stage models like this one have been criticized for their simplistic
view of development; for ignoring gender, ethnic, class, and geographical
differences; and for stressing identification with the gay community (Reynolds
& Hanjorgiris, 2000).
Recent models have emphasized that sexual identity development is a
diverse and continuous life process. McCarn and Fassinger (1996) have pro-
posed separate but parallel processes for individual and group identity for
lesbians, with each process having four phases: awareness, exploration, deepening/
commitment, and internalization/synthesis. Fassinger and Miller (1996) have
proposed similar parallel processes for gay men. Both evidence-based models
de-emphasize public disclosure and attempt to account for the many influences
on identity development.
Gays, lesbians, and bisexuals manage multiple identities, depending on
their openness about their sexuality. However, the problem is especially
complex for people of color who must balance a sexual minority identity with
Sexual Orientation and Identity 171
a racial or ethnic identity that may be more central to their sense of self
(Fukuyama & Ferguson, 2000).
Sodomy Laws
In 2003, the U.S. Supreme Court (Lawrence and Garner v. Texas, 2003)
struck down a Texas state law banning private consensual sex between adults
of the same sex. The case stemmed from the 1998 arrest of two Houston men
after police entered their home on a false report of a man with gun, filed by a
disgruntled former lover. Delivering the majority opinion, Justice Anthony
Kennedy stated:
The Court also reversed its 1986 Bowers v. Hardwick decision that upheld
state criminalization of private homosexual conduct. In his dissenting opinion,
Justice Antonin Scalia accused the Court of granting a ‘‘fundamental right’’ to
homosexual sodomy and buying into the ‘‘homosexual agenda’’ (Lawrence and
Garner v. Texas, 2003).
In 2003, fourteen states had sodomy laws. Four states—Texas, Kansas,
Oklahoma, and Missouri—prohibited oral and anal sex between members of
the same sex only; the other ten states—Alabama, Florida, Idaho, Louisiana,
Michigan, Mississippi, North Carolina, South Carolina, Utah, and Virginia—
and the territory of Puerto Rico banned sodomy for everyone (Summersgill,
2005). Although rarely enforced, sodomy laws created a permanent criminal
class of citizens and were routinely cited by courts and legislatures to deny
parental rights to gay people. One infamous example is the 1995 Virginia case,
Bottoms v. Bottoms, in which Sharon Bottoms lost custody of her son to her
mother who invoked state sodomy laws to demonstrate Sharon’s criminal
status and unfitness to raise her child. The child’s father had no objections to
172 Sexuality Today
Sharon having custody. Sodomy laws have also been used to prohibit gays and
lesbians from becoming foster parents or adopting, to deny individuals em-
ployment, and to harass student organizations (Summersgill). Justice Kennedy
acknowledged the overreaching impact of sodomy laws: ‘‘When homosexual
conduct is made criminal by the law of the State, that declaration in and of
itself is an invitation to subject homosexual persons to discrimination both in
the public and private spheres’’ (Lawrence and Garner v. Texas, 2003, p. 18).
Justice Scalia, however, lamented the loss of criminal status for gay men and
lesbians: ‘‘Many Americans do not want persons who openly engage in ho-
mosexual conduct as partners in their businesses, as scoutmasters for their
children, as teachers in their children’s schools, or as boarders in their home’’
(p. 18).
About eighty-five countries currently criminalize same-sex sodomy (In-
ternational Gay and Lesbian Human Rights Commission [IGLHRC], 2003).
Penalties vary widely, from two to twenty-five years in prison (Mali and Saint
Lucia), a life sentence (e.g., India, Singapore, Uganda), or death (e.g., Iran,
Pakistan, Saudi Arabia). Same-sex relations still occur in these countries, but
are hidden. Sodomy is legal in another 125 countries, including Albania,
Argentina, Austria, Cambodia, Canada, Central African Republic, Colombia,
Denmark, Eritrea, France, Germany, Israel, Italy, Japan, Jordan, Mexico, New
Zealand, Netherlands, Poland, Rwanda, Spain, Sweden, and the United
Kingdom (Summersgill, 2005). In addition, a number of countries (e.g.,
Ecuador, Finland, Israel, South Africa) and ten U.S. states also have policies
prohibiting discrimination in the workplace based on sexual orientation
(IGLHRC, 1999).
Gay Marriage
The current controversy over gay marriage in the United States began in
1993 when the Hawaii Supreme Court ruled that the state’s disallowance of
same-sex marriage amounted to gender discrimination (‘‘Trial challenging,’’
1996). Three gay couples promptly sued for the right to marry. As the court
case opened, members of Congress passed the Defense of Marriage Act, al-
lowing states the right to deny recognition of gay marriages licensed in other
states, which President Clinton signed into law in 1996 (‘‘Anti gay marriage
act,’’ 1996). Critics argued that the law violated the constitutional requirement
that states recognize legal contracts in other states. Meanwhile, the Hawaii
Legislature amended the state constitution to define marriage as only between
a man and a woman.
Three years later, the Vermont Supreme Court ruled that the state con-
stitution allowed gays and lesbians the benefits of marriage (‘‘Vermont’s top
court,’’ 1999). After contentious debate, the state enacted same-sex civil un-
ions that carried all the benefits of marriage. Then, in 2003, the Massachusetts
Sexual Orientation and Identity 173
Supreme Court ruled that the state had no constitutionally valid reason to deny
gays and lesbians the right to marry (Arce, 2004). The Court later clarified that
only full marriage rights, not civil unions, would conform to the state con-
stitution.
The Massachusetts Court decision came on the heels of the U.S. Supreme
Court ruling on sodomy laws. In his dissent, Justice Scalia had noted, ‘‘This
reasoning leaves on pretty shaky grounds state laws limiting marriage to
opposite-sex couples’’ (Lawrence and Garner v. Texas, p. 17). Events in Canada
also fueled the issue. In 2001, a favorable court ruling in Ontario allowed the
first gay male couple to marry in a church (Struck, 2004). Soon, six Canadian
provinces had begun issuing marriage licenses to gay couples. The Ontario
premier Dalton McGuinty proclaimed: ‘‘The fact that a gay couple might
happen to marry does not threaten me or my marriage or my children’s future
in any way, shape or form’’ (Cotter, 2004). In 2003, Canadian prime minister
Paul Martin, with approval from the Supreme Court, asked the government to
draft a federal law to standardize gay marriage rights.
In the United States, however, President Bush called for a constitutional
amendment defining marriage as a union between a man and woman, making
gay marriage a presidential campaign issue (‘‘Our government,’’ 2004). Bush
accused ‘‘activist judges’’ of making ‘‘arbitrary’’ court decisions and redefining
marriage. He openly worried that the Defense of Marriage Act would not
‘‘protect’’ traditional marriage, meaning that states and cities might have to
recognize gay marriages.
Despite attempts by the state governor and legislature to halt the process,
in May 2004, Massachusetts became the first state to allow same-sex couples to
marry (‘‘Same-sex couples,’’ 2004). By November, Congress had failed to pass
a federal constitutional amendment, but eleven states approved constitutional
amendments to ban same-sex marriage, with eight states also prohibiting same-
sex civil unions (‘‘Voters in 11 states,’’ 2004). These eleven states included
Arkansas, Georgia, Kentucky, Michigan, Mississippi, Montana, North Dakota,
Oklahoma, Ohio, Oregon, and Utah. Missouri and Louisiana had already
passed gay-marriage ban amendments. Another fifteen states are prepared to
introduce same-sex marriage bans over the next two years. In early 2005, a
district court upheld the Defense of Marriage Act, dismissing a lawsuit by two
women seeking to have their Massachusetts marriage recognized in Florida
(Chachere, 2005). No doubt the U.S. Supreme Court will ultimately decide
the legality of gay marriage.
In countries like the Netherlands, where same-sex marriage has been legal
for years, the issue is rarely a topic of discussion (‘‘Global view,’’ 2004).
Currently, Denmark, the Netherlands, and Belgium grant same-sex couples
full civil marriage, while Brazil, Croatia, Finland, France, Germany, Hungary,
Iceland, Israel, New Zealand, Norway, Portugal, and Sweden recognize same-
sex ‘‘domestic partnerships’’ or civil unions with limited rights (‘‘Global view,’’
174 Sexuality Today
2004; IGLHRC, 2003b). A number of provinces and cities around the world
also recognize civil unions.
Relationships
The 2000 Census has reported 54.5 million married couples and another
4.9 million unmarried heterosexual couples living in the United States (Sim-
mons & O’Connell, 2003). Almost 600,000 unmarried couples were of the
same sex. Most same-sex couples (51 percent) were male. One study has
estimated that 40–60 percent of gay men and 45–80 percent of lesbians are
currently in relationships (Kurdek, 1995). On average, married men were 2.4
years older (49) than their wives, and unmarried men (36.8) were 2.1 years
older than their partners (Simmons & O’Connell, 2003). Same-sex couples
were in their early forties on average. Male partners differed in age by an
average of two years, while female partners differed by only one year.
Nearly nine in ten people marry sometime in their lives, but about half of
first marriages end in divorce (U.S. Census Bureau, 2002). The median
length of first marriages was eight years. In 2000, 120.2 million Americans were
married, and 41 million were widowed, separated, or divorced (Kreider &
Simmons, 2003). Just over one-quarter of the population had never married.
Nationally, almost half (46 percent) of married couples and 43 percent of
unmarried heterosexual couples had at least one child under age 18 living in
the household. One-third of female couples and almost one-quarter of male
couples also had children living with them. Perhaps 2–8 million gay men and
lesbians are parents of between 4 million and 14 million children (Patterson,
1995). Most same-sex couples bring children from heterosexual relationships.
Some couples adopt or foster children where laws permit, and a number of
lesbians conceive via artificial insemination.
SUMMARY
Sexual orientation is largely unexplored territory, and its origins have yet
to be discovered. The study of male homosexuality has dominated research on
sexual orientation in an attempt to explain less common, socially stigmatized
sexual behaviors. However, many cultures have noted considerable diversity
in sexual attractions. Understanding attraction to both sexes may be the key to
explaining human sexuality.
Conventional theories of sexual orientation have found little support.
Interactionist theories are relatively new and have yet to be tested. Some
theorists suggest that same-sex attraction developed to facilitate intra-sex re-
lationships and reduce conflict. If so, many people would be capable of at-
traction to both sexes.
While many other countries have enacted antidiscrimination laws and
granted same-sex couples the benefits of marriage, it seems likely that in the
current political climate, homosexuality will continue to be a pressing issue in
the United States.
176 Sexuality Today
REFERENCES
Alford, G. S., Plaud, J. J., & McNair, T. L. (1995). Sexual behavior and orien-
tation: Learning and conditioning principles. In L. Diamant and R. D.
McAnulty (Eds.), The psychology of sexual orientation, behavior, and identity: A
handbook (pp. 121–135). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Almaguer, T. (1993). Chicano men: A cartography of homosexual identity and
behavior. In H. Abelove, M. Barale, and D. M. Halperin (Eds.), The lesbian
and gay studies reader (pp. 255–273). New York: Routledge.
Anti gay marriage act clears Congress. (1996, September 10). CNN.com. Re-
trieved January 17, 2005, from www.cnn.com
Arce, R. (2004, February 6). Massachusetts court upholds same-sex marriage.
CNN.com. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.cnn.com
Ault, A. (1996). Ambiguous identity in an unambiguous sex/gender structure:
The case of bisexual women. Sociological Quarterly, 20(3), 449–463.
Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Genetic and environmental
influences on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin
sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 524–536.
Bailey, J. M., & Pillard, R. D. (1991). A genetic study of male sexual orientation.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 1089–1096.
Bailey, J. M., Pillard, R. D., Neale, M. C., & Agyei, Y. (1993). Heritable factors
influence sexual orientation in women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50,
217–223.
Bailey, J. M., Willerman, L., & Parks, C. (1991). A test of the maternal stress
theory of human male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20(3),
277–293.
Barlow, D. H., & Agras, W. S. (1973). Fading to increase heterosexual respon-
siveness in homosexuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 355–366.
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive
as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 347–374.
Bayer, R. (1987). Homosexuality and American psychiatry: The politics of diagnosis.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.
Beckstead, A. L. (2001). Cures versus choices: Agendas in sexual reorientation
therapy. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Psychotherapy, 5(3/4), 87–115.
Bell, A. P., & Weinberg, M. S. (1978). Homosexuality: A study of diversity among
men and women. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Bell, A. P., Weinberg, M. S., & Hammersmith, S. K. (1981). Sexual preference: Its
development in men and women. Bloomington: Indiana University.
Bem, D. (1996). Exotic becomes erotic: A developmental theory of sexual ori-
entation. Psychological Review, 103(2), 320–335.
Bergler, E. (1947). Differential diagnosis between spurious homosexuality and
perversion homosexuality. Psychiatric Quarterly, 31, 399–409.
Besen, W. R. (2003). Anything but straight: Unmasking the scandals and lies behind the
ex-gay myth. New York: Harrington Park.
Sexual Orientation and Identity 177
Collaer, M. L., & Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioral sex differences: A role for
gonadal hormones during early development? Psychological Bulletin, 118(1),
55–107.
Cotter, J. (2004, December 9). Gay marriage decision sparks diverse reaction.
CNews. Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.cnews.canoe.ca
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A biobehavioral
model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. Psychological Review,
110(1), 173–192.
Dittman, R. W., Kappes, M. E., & Kappes, M. H. (1992). Sexual behavior in
adolescent and adult females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psycho-
neuroendocrinology, 17, 153–170.
Dörner, G. (1979). Psychoneuroendocrine aspects of brain development and re-
production. In L. Zichella & E. Pancheir (Eds.), Psychoneuroendocrinology
and reproduction. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Dörner, G., Geier, T., Ahrens, L., et al. (1980). Prenatal stress as a possible
aetiogenetic factor of homosexuality in human males. Endokrinologie, 75,
365–368.
Eliason, M. J. (1995). Accounts of sexual identity formation in heterosexual stu-
dents. Sex Roles, 32(11/12), 821–833.
Ellis, L., & Cole-Harding, S. (2001). The effects of prenatal stress and of prenatal
alcohol and nicotine exposure on human sexual orientation. Physiology &
Behavior, 74, 1–14.
Espin, O. M. (1993). Issues of identity in the psychology of Latina lesbians. In
L. D. Garnets & D. C. Kimmel (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on lesbi-
ans and gay male experiences (pp. 348–363). New York: Columbia Uni-
versity.
Faderman, L. (1981/1998). Surpassing the love of men: Romantic friendship and love
between women from the Renaissance to the present. New York: Quill/William
Morrow.
Fassinger, R. E., & Miller, B. A. (1996). Validation of an inclusive model of
homosexual identity formation on a sample of gay men. Journal of Ho-
mosexuality, 32(2), 53–78.
Fay, R. E., Turner, C. F., Klassen, A. D., & Gagnon, J. H. (1989). Prevalence and
patterns of same-gender sexual contact among men. Science, 243, 338–348.
Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of sexual behavior. New York: Harper
and Brothers.
Foucault, M. (1978/1990). The history of sexuality: Vol. 1. An introduction (R. Hurley,
Trans.). New York: Vintage Books.
Fox, R. C. (1995). Bisexual identities. In A. R. D’Augelli & C. J. Patterson (Eds.),
Lesbian, gay and bisexual identities over the lifespan: Psychological perspectives
(pp. 48–86). New York: Oxford University.
Freud, S. (1905/1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In J. Strachey
(Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud
(Vol. 7, pp. 123–246). London: Hogarth.
Sexual Orientation and Identity 179
Herdt, G. H., & McClintock, M. (2000). The magical age of 10. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 29, 587–606.
Hershberger, S. L. (2001). Biological factors in the development of sexual ori-
entation. In C. J. Patterson & A. R. D’Augelli (Eds.), Lesbian, gay, and
bisexual identities and youth: Psychological perspectives (pp. 27–51). New York:
Oxford University.
Hershberger, S. L., & Segal, N. L. (2004). The cognitive, behavioral, and per-
sonality profiles of a male monozygotic triplet set discordant for sexual
orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(5), 497–514.
Hu, S., Pattatucci, A. M. L., Patterson, C., Li, L., Fulker, D. W., Cherny, S. S.,
et al. (1995). Linkage between sexual orientation and chromosome Xq28
in males but not in females. Nature Genetics, 11, 248–256.
International Gay and Lesbian Human Rights Commission. (1999, April). Anti-
discrimination legislation. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.iglhrc.org
International Gay and Lesbian Human Rights Commission. (2003a, July). Where
having sex is a crime: Criminalization and decriminalization of homosexual acts.
Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.iglhrc.org
International Gay and Lesbian Human Rights Commission. (2003b, November).
Where can you marry: Global summary of registered partnership, domestic part-
nership, and marriage laws. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.iglhrc.org
International Lesbian and Gay Association. (2000, July). World legal survey: The
armed forces. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.ilga.info/Information/
Legal_survey/ Summary%20information/armed_forces.html
Johnson, L., George, F. W., Neaves, W. B., Rosenthal, I. M., Christensen, R. A.,
DeCristoforo, A., et al. (1986). Characterization of the testicular abnor-
mality in 5-alpha-reductase deficiency. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and
Metabolism, 63, 1091–1099.
Katz, J. N. (1995). The invention of heterosexuality. New York: Dutton.
Kauth, M. R. (2000). True nature: A theory of sexual attraction. New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum.
Kauth, M. R. (2002). Much ado about homosexuality: Assumptions underlying
current research on sexual orientation. Journal of Psychology and Human
Sexuality, 14(1), 1–22.
Kauth, M. R. (2005). Revealing assumptions: A guide for describing sexual ori-
entation. Journal of Bisexuality, 5(4), xx.
Kauth, M. R., & Landis, D. (1996). Applying lessons learned from minority
integration in the military. In G. M. Herek, J. B. Jobe, & R. M. Carney
(Eds.), Out in force: Sexual orientation and the military (pp. 86–105). Chicago:
University of Chicago.
King, J. L. (2004). On the down low: A journey into the lives of ‘‘straight’’ black men
who sleep with men. New York: Broadway.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual
behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Sexual Orientation and Identity 181
Roscoe, W. (1998). Changing ones: Third and fourth genders in Native North America.
New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Ross, M. W. (1987). A theory of normal homosexuality. In L. Diamant (Ed.),
Male and female homosexuality: Psychological approaches (pp. 237–259). New
York: Hemisphere.
Rust, P. C. R. (2000a). The biology, psychology, sociology, and sexuality of
bisexuality. In P. C. R. Rust (Ed.), Bisexuality in the United States: A social
science reader (pp. 403–470). New York: Columbia University.
Rust, P. C. R. (2000b). Bisexuality in the United States: A social science reader. New
York: Columbia University.
Saghir, M. T., & Robins, E. (1973). Male and female homosexuality. Baltimore:
Williams & Wilkins.
Same-sex couples exchange vows in Massachusetts. (2004, May 17). CNN.com.
Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.boston.com
Sandberg, D. E., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Yager, T. J., Hensle, T. W., Levitt,
S. B., Kogan, S. J., et al. (1995). Gender development in boys born with
hypospadias. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20(7), 693–709.
Servicemembers Legal Defense Network. (2004, March 24). Conduct unbecoming:
The tenth annual report on ‘‘Don’t Ask, Don’t Tell, Don’t Pursue, Don’t
Harass.’’ Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.sldn.org
Shepherd, G. (1987). Rank, gender, and homosexuality: Mombasa as a key to
understanding sexual options. In P. Caplan (Ed.), The cultural construction of
sexuality (pp. 240–270). London: Tavistock.
Shively, M. G., & DeCecco, J. P. (1977). Components of sexual identity. Journal
of Homosexuality, 3, 41–48.
Shostak, M. (1981). Nisa, the life and words of a !Kung woman. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University.
Simmons, T., & O’Connell, M. (2003, February). Married-couple and unmarried-
partner households: 2000. Census 2000 Special Reports. U.S. Census Bureau.
Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.census.gov/prod/2003pubs/
censr-5.pdf
Socarides, C. (1968). The overt homosexual. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Spitzer, R. L. (2003). Can some gay men and lesbians change their sexual ori-
entation? 200 participants reporting a change from homosexual to het-
erosexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32(5), 403–417.
Stein, E. (1999). The mismeasure of desire: The science, theory, and ethics of sexual
orientation. New York: Oxford University.
Storms, M. D. (1981). A theory of erotic orientation development. Psychological
Review, 88, 340–353.
Struck, D. (2004, December 10). High court in Canada backs gay mar-
riage. Washingtonpost.com. Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.wash
ingtonpost.com
Summersgill, B. (2005). Sodomy laws. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from
www.sodomylaws.org
184 Sexuality Today
Trial challenging same-sex marriage ban opens in Hawaii. (1996, September 11).
CNN.com. Retrieved January 17, 2005, from www.cnn.com
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2002, February 8). Nearly 9-in-10 people may marry, but half
of first marriages may end in divorce, Census Bureau says. Retrieved January
17, 2005, from www.census.gov/Press-Release/www/releases/archives/
marital_status_living_arrangements/000500.html
Van Wyck, P. H., & Geist, C. S. (1984). Psychosocial development in hetero-
sexual, bisexual, and homosexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13,
505–544.
Vermont’s top court backs rights for same-sex couples. (1999, December 21).
CNN.com. Retrieved January 15, 2005, from www.archives.cnn.com
Voters in 11 states overwhelmingly vote against same-sex marriage. (2004, No-
vember 3). The Advocate. Retrieved December 31, 2004, from www.ad
vocate.com
Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Pryor, D. W. (1994). Dual attractions: Un-
derstanding bisexuality. New York: Oxford University.
Wellings, K., Field, J., Johnson, A. M., & Wadsworth, J. (1994). Sexual behaviour
in Britain: The national survey of sexual attitudes and lifestyles. Harmonds-
worth, UK: Penguin Books.
Whitam, F. L., Diamond, M., & Martin, J. (1993). Homosexual orientation in
twins: A report on 61 pairs and 3 triplet sets. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22,
187–206.
Williams, T. J., Pepitone, M. E., Christensen, S. E., Cooke, B. M., Huberman,
A. D., Breedlove, T. J., et al. (2000, March). Finger-length ratios and
sexual orientation. Nature, 404, 455–456.
Williams, W. (1986). The spirit and the flesh: Sexual diversity in American Indian
culture. Boston: Beacon Press.
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Wolff, C. (1971). Love between women. New York: Harper & Row.
Woodson, J. C., & Gorski, R. A. (2000). Structural sex differences in the mam-
malian brain: Reconsidering the male/female dichotomy. In A. Matsumoto
(Ed.), Sexual differentiation of the brain (pp. 229–255). Boca Raton, FL: CRC
Press.
8
M. Michele Burnette 1
Do men have stronger sex drives than women? Are women more emotionally
expressive than men? These are common assumptions in today’s world that
often guide our thoughts and behaviors toward others. The assumption that
women and men are characteristically very different is a highly popularized
notion and a major topic of discussion in the media (e.g., talk shows, maga-
zines) and popular self-help books. However, scientific research on men and
women suggests that they are far more similar than different. Thus, while
categorizing and generalizing about what males and females are like may
simplify our lives (and provide interesting and lucrative fodder for book writers
and the media), these assumptions do not often hold true.
Before moving forward in this discussion, it is important to understand a
few general concepts that will be discussed throughout this chapter. Sex refers
to the biological differences in the sex chromosomes and sex organs of males
and females. Gender refers to the psychosocial condition of being feminine or
masculine, or those traits, interests, and behaviors assumed to be appropriate
for a given sex. For example, the sex of a person born with an XY chro-
mosomal makeup, with a penis and testicles, is assumed to be male, but his
behavior, personality, and general lifestyle will determine whether or not his
gender is masculine or feminine. Gender roles are those social behaviors, life-
styles, and personality characteristics that women and men are expected to
exhibit. People who adhere closely to these roles are gender-typed. A gender-
typed female, for example, might dress in feminine clothing, become
186 Sexuality Today
emotional easily, not show interest in or participate in sports, and dedicate her
life to rearing children rather than having a career outside the home. A gender-
typed male might work in road construction, watch or participate in sports,
and show very little emotion in the face of an upsetting event. But when we
assume that all members of a sex possess characteristics and behave in ways that
are consistent with gender role expectations, we are gender stereotyping—we
expect females to be feminine and males to be masculine. Though many men
and women do exhibit several characteristics and behaviors in common with
other members of their identified sex, this is not universally true. Consider the
stay-at-home dad or the woman fighter pilot. When we stereotype, we might
fall prey to gender bias, that is, we treat men and women differently based on
assumptions about members of their sex. Gender bias often leads to unfair
treatment. For example, we often rob men of the opportunity to play a
primary role in rearing their children because we assume women are by nature
more nurturing than men. Or we restrict women from jobs assumed to be
suitable only for men. The truth is, many people are androgynous, meaning that
they exhibit a balance of masculine and feminine characteristics. One final
concept is gender identity or one’s personal view of oneself as male or female,
which also may be inconsistent with the individual’s biological sex. In general,
the dichotomy of maleness and femaleness is an arbitrary notion that can be
challenged on both social and biological levels.
These individuals are infertile and many are mentally handicapped. Turner’s
syndrome results from a female having only one X chromosome (XO). Indi-
viduals with this condition appear female but have no ovaries (or underdevel-
oped ovaries) and, therefore, do not produce egg cells or sex hormones. In the
absence of sex hormones, these women do not develop at puberty without
hormone replacement therapy. They also tend to be short and have various
physical defects. Unlike these two conditions, the cause of intersexuality is
unknown. Hermaphrodites usually have female chromosomes (XX) but genital
development is abnormal, resulting in ambiguous genitalia and internal repro-
ductive structures (e.g., one ovary and one testicle, or uterus with male external
genitals).
Over- or underexposure to hormones during prenatal development can
also cause abnormal sexual development. The embryo may produce abnormal
amounts of hormones, or the mother may produce or ingest hormones that
affect prenatal development. The most common hormonal abnormalities are
congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) and androgen insensitivity syndrome
(AIS). In CAH, the adrenal glands of the fetus produce too much testosterone.
The result is premature puberty in boys and malelike external genitals in girls.
For example, the clitoris may be enlarged and have the appearance of a penis.
In AIS, the male fetus has a genetic disorder, which causes the person to be
insensitive to testosterone. As a result, the newborn male will not have an
internal reproductive system and will have a clitoris and a shallow vagina. At
puberty, he is likely to develop breasts. Because they appear female at birth,
these chromosomal males are ordinarily reared as females—in fact, by surgi-
cally lengthening the vagina and taking estrogen supplements, these individuals
usually have satisfactory lives as women (Hines, Ahmed, & Hughes, 2003;
Wisniewski et al., 2000). AIS is a condition that highlights the difference
between biological sex and gender as well as the difficulty of dichotomizing
male and female. According to their chromosomal makeup, these individuals
are male; however, their visible physical traits would suggest they are female.
In addition, they are generally socialized and identify themselves as female.
GENDER IDENTITY
It is common to assume that one’s biological sex will always match one’s
perception of self as male or female. However, as we just discussed in the case
of AIS, this assumption is simply false. Even in the absence of any genetic or
hormonal abnormalities, some individuals’ gender identity does not corre-
spond to their biological sex. In our society, we find a highly diverse group of
individuals who do not conform to traditional notions of a one-to-one cor-
respondence between biological sex and gender in the way they look, behave,
or self-identify. We collectively refer to them as transgendered. This term is
often used to refer to a broad range of individuals, including the intersexed
individuals we just discussed, as well as cross-dressers, gay men in drag,
188 Sexuality Today
not accept a person’s individuality. Because so many stereotypes are not only
inaccurate but also negative, strict adherence to such stereotypes can harm or
oppress stereotyped groups. Such gender bias, for example, has often pre-
vented women from pursuing and succeeding in their chosen careers. A
contemporary example is the debate over whether or not women should be
placed in or near combat (Stone, 2005). Many of the arguments against
women in the military, in general, are based on stereotypes and are not sup-
ported by research (RAND Research Brief, 1997), and the cost to women is
not having equal access to a tremendous number of jobs and advanced posi-
tions in the armed services.
ANDROGYNY
So far, this chapter has illuminated the fact that neither sex nor gender is a
dichotomous characteristic, and that we cannot easily assume that a person will
think, behave, or possess a particular set of characteristics based on our per-
ception of the individual’s sex. In fact, people fall on a continuum of masculine
and feminine—most men and women are best described as androgynous,
having both masculine and feminine characteristics. Only a small number of
individuals possess very few either feminine or masculine characteristics—they
are referred to as undifferentiated. In neither case are these people gender-typed.
Where once individuals who were not gender-typed were viewed as deviant,
much evidence today suggests that androgyny is the picture of well-being. No
longer under pressure to prove one’s masculinity or femininity, the androgy-
nous person has a broader repertoire of possible responses to draw from and can
choose the most appropriate response for a particular situation. With such a
broad repertoire, an individual can function effectively in a variety of situations.
To take the case of women in war again, one advantage to having women in the
Iraq war is their ability to deal sensitively and effectively with other women and
children whom they approach during door-to-door searches (Stone, 2005).
Thus, these women are true warriors who also make use of more traditionally
‘‘feminine’’ qualities in a unique and challenging environment.
Researchers have addressed the question of whether or not androgynous
individuals enjoy greater psychological well-being than their more gender-
typed counterparts owing to their broader repertoires and, therefore, greater
ability to act and respond to a variety of demands. Because adolescence is the
time when young people begin to take on more adult roles, this question has
been studied most extensively in adolescents. Research does suggest that an-
drogynous children and adolescents enjoy greater psychological well-being but
so do more masculine children and adolescents (Allgood-Merten & Stockard,
1991; Markstrom-Adams, 1989). This latter finding may be because masculine
traits have traditionally been more highly valued by society than feminine
traits, giving the more masculine adolescent greater status and acceptance in
her or his environment.
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 191
Behavioral Genetics
The behavioral genetics viewpoint posits that gender-typed behaviors are
determined through genetic inheritance. In other words, our adoption of these
behaviors as well as others is determined by our genetic makeup passed down
to us through our biological parents. This position is tested by two primary
means. One is by demonstrating that adopted children demonstrate more
similar behavior patterns to their biological parents (inheritance) than to their
adoptive parents (environment). It is also tested by looking at similarities be-
tween monozygotic twins, who are genetically identical, and comparing the
extent of those similarities to similarities between dizygotic twins, who are no
more genetically similar than any other pair of siblings—they share only about
50 percent of their genes. If a behavioral trait is totally determined by genetics,
you would expect identical twins to behave exactly the same 100 percent of
the time. This is called a concordance rate. You would also expect dizygotic
twins to have about a 50 percent concordance on that trait. Few genetic
studies have been conducted to assess the relative influence of genetics on
gender role development. One study did show some evidence that genetics
accounted for the variance in masculine and feminine characteristics in chil-
dren (20 to 48 percent of the variance). However, they concluded that ex-
periences outside the home, such as peers, have a greater impact than genetics
or even parental influence (Mitchell, Baker, & Jacklin, 1989). A recent twin
study addressed the contribution of genetics to atypical gender development
(e.g., boys playing with jewelry and girls playing with swords). This study
found that environment contributed more to gender role development than
genetic factors, except perhaps in the case of girls who were high in mascu-
linity and low in feminine characteristics. In this case, genetic factors appeared
to be the primary determinant (Knafo, Iervolino, & Plomin, 2005).
192 Sexuality Today
Evolutionary Theory
The evolutionary position is that gender-typed behaviors that present
themselves across all cultures have been selected throughout the centuries in
order to insure survival of the species and that these behaviors cannot be
explained by environmental factors alone. One would expect to see more
variability in these behaviors if they were environmentally determined. Ex-
amples of cross-cultural invariability are that in all cultures men are more likely
than women to be polygamous (have multiple partners), and women are less
likely to commit murder than men. Evolutionary psychologists claim that they
can predict similarities between the sexes in those areas where both have been
challenged by similar adaptive problems, and differences between the sexes in
those areas where they have faced different adaptive problems, throughout
time.
For example, David Buss (1995), a prominent evolutionary psychologist,
suggested that women are less likely to engage in casual sex than men because
the costs of sex are much greater for women. Women are likely to get
pregnant and then bear the burden of caring and providing for the child. Men
tend to engage in more casual sexual interactions because their evolutionary
‘‘goal’’ is to propagate the species. In addition, they suffer few, if any, lifelong
negative consequences. Because of the additional burden women bear, they
are more likely to be discriminating and seek out only those sexual partners
who appear oriented toward commitment and assuming responsibility for their
offspring. While these tendencies may be true in a general sense, in reality
there is tremendous variability in the behaviors of men and women. Certainly,
we all know men who are very focused on finding the right lifelong mate and
women who engage in casual sex without fear of the potential consequences.
Perhaps it is best to think of the evolution of behaviors as predispositions that
are either exaggerated or diminished by environmental factors (Buss &
Schmitt, 1993; Kenrick & Trost, 1993). For example, the invention of ef-
fective contraception has given women much greater sexual freedom (Buss,
1994); therefore, women do not have to worry as much about pregnancy and
can be less discriminating in their sexual choices.
Overall, it appears that these biological theories of gender role develop-
ment provide incomplete explanations of how common behaviors are estab-
lished. It is most likely that environmental factors play as much of, if not a
greater, role in gender role development.
Freudian Theory
Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) lived primarily in Vienna, Austria, during
what was known as the Victorian era. The Victorian era was marked by ex-
treme sexual oppression. Women, in particular, were denied sexual expression,
and, in fact, were considered potential prostitutes if they expressed any sexual
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 193
feelings at all. This is the context in which Freud’s view of gender identifi-
cation evolved. By today’s standards, aspects of this view are sexist. Freud’s
overall idea of development was that children proceed through a relatively
predictable set of stages, including the phallic stage, from about age 3 to 6. He
believed that boys and girls learn about what it is to be ‘‘male’’ or ‘‘female’’ by
observation and imitation of the same-sex parent’s behavior. During the phallic
stage, children develop their gender roles through this process. According to
him, a boy experiences what Freud called the Oedipal complex, during which
the boy desires his mother and comes to envy his father, the primary rival for
his mother’s affection. He then becomes fearful that his father, upon discov-
ering the boy’s feelings, will castrate him. Thus, he develops castration anxiety.
To relieve his anxiety, he suppresses his carnal desires for his mother and
identifies with his father by imitating his behaviors, attitudes, and appearance.
The girl is also attached to the mother, her primary caregiver. However,
during the phallic phase, when genitalia become the object of attention, she
discovers that she lacks a penis, and blames her mother and becomes hostile
toward her. She then attaches to her father and imagines that she will become
pregnant by him and that this will cause her to develop a penis and gain equal
status with her father. This is due mostly to the idea that the girl is ‘‘envious’’
of her father’s penis and wants to possess it so strongly that she dreams of
bearing his children, thus the term penis envy. Eventually, recognizing that she
cannot possess her father, she identifies and imitates her mother, the woman
who does possess him. Freud argued, however, that because the girl is am-
bivalent about being female (i.e., has penis envy), she adopts an inferiority
complex as the ‘‘inferior sex.’’ She does not fully adopt her mother’s charac-
teristics and her identification is incomplete, causing her to have a poorly de-
veloped superego, or conscience. As a result, her values and morals are seen as
weaker than men’s. Clearly, these are antiquated and sexist notions about the
sexes. Freudian theory has little relevance to our current understanding of
gender role development except that it opened the door for theorizing about
how the social world impacts on the developing child.
plays with a doll, but his little male friends make fun of him, so he no lon-
ger plays with dolls. In this case, the teasing from his friends punishes playing
with dolls. If he picks up a truck and his friends come over to play with him,
then playing with trucks is likely to be rewarded by their approval. Rein-
forcement of gender-typed behavior and punishment of deviations from them
establish gender-typed patterns early in life, and these may even take place
before birth. Take, for example, parents’ preparation for a new baby. If they
know the sex, they generally decorate a nursery differently for a boy or a girl—
a boy’s room might be decorated with sports-related themes, while a girl’s
might be decorated with cute bunnies. As children grow up, they are influ-
enced by more than their parents’ behavior. We are inundated in our society
with messages that tell us that boys and girls should behave differently. Take,
for example, the highly gender-typed toy commercials that children see while
watching cartoons, or the ads on radio and television and in the written media
that stress beauty for women and achievement for men.
Beyond the more direct forces of reinforcement and punishment is role
modeling. Role modeling is the imitation of behaviors of someone admired or
liked. Children are most likely to imitate the behavior of someone of the same
sex when several members of that sex exhibit that behavior (Bussey & Ban-
dura, 1984; Bussey & Perry, 1982). When, for example, boys see only one or
two men (if any) staying home to care for their children while most men are
working, they are not likely to aspire to be stay-at-home dads. They are much
more likely to model ‘‘climbing the corporate ladder’’ as they see numerous
examples of men doing just that.
Social influences on gender role development in our culture cannot be
ignored. There is a plethora of research on the influence of parents, peers,
teachers, and the media on the development of gender roles, and there is little
doubt that all these influence children to develop gender-typed behavior. One
of the criticisms of social learning theory is that it focuses almost exclusively on
the external environment and ignores the role of other factors, such as thought
processes, in gender socialization.
Cognitive-Developmental Theory
There are several cognitive theories of gender development. All are con-
sistent with the social learning perspective in that they assert that children learn
gender roles through interactions with their environment. But while social
learning theory paints a picture of children as somewhat passive with respect
to the influence of their learning environments, most, if not all, cognitive
theories emphasize the active role that children play in their own gender so-
cialization. One prominent cognitive theory is cognitive-developmental the-
ory. A major tenet of this theory is that a child’s ability to develop gender roles
relies on her or his ability to develop gender constancy, which occurs when the
child accomplishes three things: (1) gender identity—identifies self as male or
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 195
female: (2) gender stability—recognizes that one’s gender does not change over
time: and (3) gender consistency—recognizes that one’s identity is not altered
by changes in gender-typed activities, traits, or appearance. Once the child
identifies as male or female, the child will seek out same-sex role models to
imitate ‘‘appropriate’’ behaviors. A final tenet of cognitive developmental
theory is that a child is motivated internally to bring her or his behavior and
thinking in line with one’s gender category (Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002).
All of the theories of development that we have reviewed have value.
Today’s gender role theorists, for the most part, maintain that in order to
completely understand gender role development, we need a comprehensive
model of development that takes into account biological, social, and cognitive
influences (Martin et al., 2002; Martin, Ruble, Szkrybalo, 2004). In fact, some
theories, such as social cognitive theory, attempt to integrate cognition into a
social learning theory of development and also make note of the contribution
of biology and other sociocultural factors (Bussey & Bandura, 1999). Ulti-
mately, all of these factors probably contribute to gender role development,
but a truly comprehensive model of these contributions has not yet emerged.
Numerous studies have shown that while women and men are both able
to accurately identify flirting behaviors, men have a much greater tendency
than women to identify flirting as imparting sexual interest (Henningsen,
2004). Even in casual and brief interactions, men are more likely than women
to see members of the opposite sex as being seductive, sexy, and even
promiscuous (Harnish, Abbey, & DeBono, 1990; Haworth-Hoeppner, 1998;
Johnson, Stockdale, & Saal, 1991). There are several possible reasons why men
might overinterpret flirtatious or even neutral interactions as sexual in nature.
One may be that men are unduly influenced by the media’s tendency to place
greater emphasis on women’s physical attractiveness and sexual availability as
opposed to other attributes such as character and personality. Second, men
may be on the lookout for signs of interest from women because men have
traditionally been expected to play the role of initiating dates and sexual
interactions (Muehlenhard & Rodgers, 1998). Finally, men have traditionally
been taught that women play ‘‘hard to get’’ and avoid giving off signs of
interest even when they are interested.
The discrepancy in male and female interpretations of sexual interest may,
in part, be at the core of the most common form of rape—acquaintance
(‘‘date’’) rape. When males believe that females are interested in a sexual
encounter when they are not, and/or that females are more likely to act as if
they are not interested, males may continue to pursue sexual interactions even
when the female is not interested. In fact, research has shown that males are
less likely than females to identify scenarios depicting nonconsensual sex as
unacceptable (Freetly & Kane, 1995) perhaps because they do not perceive it as
nonconsensual at all, but rather they see it as part of a ‘‘mating game.’’
The traditional view of sexual interactions between men and women has
been that men are the initiators, and, as we discussed above, women are the
resistors. However, a fairly sizable number of studies now suggest that many
women do initiate sexual activity (Clements-Schreiber, Rempel, & Desmarais,
1998). Up to 93 percent of females report that they have initiated sex at some
time (Anderson & Aymami, 1993). More surprisingly, research has also docu-
mented that women use pressure tactics to get men to engage in sexual inter-
actions with them. Results from a study of coercive sexual strategies used by
women suggest that there is at least a modest relationship between a woman’s
willingness to use coercive strategies and adherence to the gender stereotype
that men are always ready and willing to engage in sexual activity (Clements-
Schreiber et al., 1998). Thus, stereotypes about men may contribute to inap-
propriate and potentially harmful behavior in women directed at men. While
much attention has been given to how stereotypes about women put them at
risk for sexual victimization, little attention has been given to how men might
likewise be victimized. The role of harmful gender stereotypes of men in co-
ercive sexuality needs to be explored further.
With the spread of HIV over the last few decades, more attention has been
given to how gender stereotypes might have an impact on safer sex practices.
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 197
The only way to practice safer sex, besides complete abstinence, is to use a
condom each and every time one has oral-genital or genital-genital contact
with another person. Until the female condom gains greater popularity, the
primary means of protecting against sexually transmitted infections, including
HIV/AIDS, is to use a male condom. Obviously, use of a male condom requires
significant male cooperation. Women who adhere to traditional notions of male
as the aggressor and woman as the passive, less sexual one in heterosexual
interactions are not likely to take the initiative to purchase, and, much less,
require that their male partners use condoms. In fact, in a study of African
American women, those women who never used condoms and were hence
characterized as sexually nonassertive expressed that they did not use condoms
out of concern for how their male partners would react to their requests to wear
a condom (Wingood & DiClemente, 1998). Furthermore, a more traditional
woman is more likely to prefer sex in the context of an ongoing relationship as
opposed to casual sex. When a woman waits until she gets to know a man well
before having intercourse, she is more likely to assume that he is ‘‘safe’’ (i.e., does
not carry any sexually transmittable infections). In fact, one study supported this
idea that women preferring ‘‘relational’’ sex were less likely to use a condom
than women who did not place an emphasis on relational sex (Hynie, Lydon,
Cote, & Weiner, 1998).
in men and women, but there is also not always a clear distinction between male
and female sex. Some individuals have genetic or hormonal conditions that
make their biological sex ambiguous as well. If people do not fit into clear
categories of male and female, masculine and feminine, what about similarities
and differences between men and women? Overall, men and women have more
similarities than differences in how they behave, especially with respect to
interpersonal and sexual relationships.
REFERENCES
Allgood-Merten, B., & Stockard, J. (1991). Sex role identity and self-esteem: A
comparison of children and adolescents. Sex Roles, 25, 129–139.
Anderson, P. B., & Aymami, R. (1993). Reports of female initiation of sexual
contact: Male and female differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22(4),
335–343.
Brehm, S. S. (1992). Intimate relationships. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bullough, V. L. (2000). Transgenderism and the concept of gender. International
Journal of Transgenderism, 4(3). Retrieved July 22, 2002, from www
.symposion.com/ijt/gilbert/bullough.htm
Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York:
Basic Books.
Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selec-
tion. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
perspective on human nature. Psychological Review, 2, 204–232.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1984). Influence of gender constancy and social power
on sex-linked modeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42,
1292–1302.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development
and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.
Bussey, K., & Perry, D. G. (1982). Same-sex imitation? Sex Roles, 8, 773–784.
Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. S. (1993). Is there any reason to research sex differ-
ences in communication? Communication Quarterly, 41, 129–144.
Clements-Schreiber, M. E., Rempel, J. K., & Desmarais, S. (1998). Women’s
sexual pressure tactics and adherence to related attitudes: A step toward
prediction. Journal of Sex Research, 35(2), 197–205.
Doorn, C. D., Poortinga, J., & Verschoor, A. M. (1994). Cross-gender identity in
transvestites and male transsexuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 185–201.
Freetly, A. J. H., & Kane, E. W. (1995). Men’s and women’s perceptions of non-
consensual sexual intercourse. Sex Roles, 33, 785–803.
Green, B. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (1994). The attractiveness of gender-typed traits of
different relationship levels: Androgynous characteristics may be desirable
after all. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 244–253.
Green, R. (2000). Birth order and ratio of brothers to sisters in transsexuals.
Psychological Medicine, 30(4), 789–795.
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 199
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early
gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903–933.
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2004). Recognizing the centrality of
gender identity and stereotype knowledge in gender development and
moving toward theoretical integration: Reply to Bandura and Bussey.
Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 702–710.
Mitchell, J. E., Baker, L. A., & Jacklin, C. N. (1989). Masculinity and femininity
in twin children: Genetic and environmental factors. Child Development,
60, 1475–1485.
Money, J. (1980). Love and sickness: The science of sex, gender difference, and pair-
bonding. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Money, J., & Lehne, G. K. (1999). Gender identity disorders. In R. T. Am-
merman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of prescriptive treatments for children
and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 214–228). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Muehlenhard, C. L., & Rodgers, C. S. (1998). Token resistance to sex: New
perspectives on an old stereotype. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 447–
461.
RAND Research Brief. (1997). Military readiness: Women are not the problem. Re-
trieved June 20, 2005, from www.rand.org/publications/RB/RB7515/
Rehman, J., Lazer, S., Benet, A. E., Schaefer, L. C., & Melman, A. (1999). The
reported sex and surgery satisfactions of 28 postoperative male-to-female
transsexual patients. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28(1), 71–89.
Schmidt, G., & Sigusch, V. (1970). Sex differences in responses to psychosexual
stimulation by films and slides. Journal of Sex Research, 6, 268–283.
Schmidt, G., Sigusch, V., & Schafer, S. (1973). Responses to reading erotic stories:
Male-female differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 2, 181–199.
Sigusch, V., Schmidt, G., Reinfeld, A., & Wiedemann-Sutor, I. (1970). Psy-
chosexual stimulation: Sex differences. Journal of Sex Research, 6, 10–24.
Smith, Y. L., Van Goozen, S. H., Kuiper, A. J., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2005).
Sex reassignment: Outcomes and predictors of treatment for adolescent
and adult transsexuals. Psychological Medicine, 35(1), 89–99.
Steinman, D. L., Wincze, J. P., Sakheim, D. K., Barlow, D. H., & Mavissakalian,
M. (1981). A comparison of male and female patterns of sexual arousal.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 10(6), 529–547.
Stone, A. (2005, May 20). Panel’s decision reheats women-in-combat debate:
Pentagon to review issue that some say renews cultural battle. U.S.A.
Today. Retrieved June 7, 2005, from www.usatoday.com/printedition/
news/20050520/a_womencombat20.art.htm
Vogel, D. L., Wester, S. R., Heesacker, M., & Madon, S. (2003). Confirming
gender stereotypes: A social role perspective. Sex Roles: A Journal of Re-
search, 48(11/12), 519–528.
Wingood, G. M., & DiClemente, R. J. (1998). Pattern influences and gender-
related factors associated with noncondom use among young adult Afri-
can American women. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26,
29–51.
Gender, Gender Identity, and Sexuality 201
Laina Y. Bay-Cheng 1
It is important to remember that helping your child stay healthy is an ongoing
job. Parents need to know that sexually active adolescents face greater physical
and emotional health risks than any other segment of the population. . . . With
so much at stake, it is more important than ever for parents to encourage their
adolescents to delay sexual involvement, preferably until marriage. Abstinence
is, without question, the healthiest choice for adolescents, both physically and
emotionally.
In most dating relationships there is a natural progression of physical intimacy. If
no barriers are put into place, this progression generally leads to sexual inter-
course. Most experts agree that once couples move beyond hugging and light
kissing, hormones encourage further physical contact.
Progression of physical intimacy:
Holding hands
Holding each other around the waist or shoulder (hugs)
Kissing on the lips
‘‘French’’ (open mouth) kissing
Touching breasts
Touching sexual organs
Sexual intercourse.1
204 Sexuality Today
have been trained to see? An essentialist answers this final question with the
former: we are observing actual reality, which exists regardless of whether we
are there to observe it or not. A social constructionist, on the other hand,
endorses the latter: what we see is a reality that we are simultaneously con-
structing to be ‘‘true’’; motives for pleasure and aggression exist precisely
because we look for—and decide that we find—them.
vaginal lubrication, penile erection) and orgasm. Everything beyond that most
basic physiological potential (e.g., what cues provoke arousal, how an indi-
vidual responds after arousal, etc.), however, is socially constructed (Tiefer,
2004; White, Bondurant, & Travis, 2000). What is more, social construc-
tionists also argue that it is precisely these other, socially dependent aspects of
sexuality that are meaningful, not the base potential for arousal. For instance,
Paul Abramson and Steven Pinkerton (2002) explain that until it is processed
by the brain according to cultural scripts, a kiss or a caress has no sexual
significance: ‘‘The sensory signals arriving at the brain following stimulation of
an erogenous zone are not inherently pleasurable, or even inherently sexual.
Instead, interpretation of these signals by the brain is required for the im-
pinging sensations to be recognized as sexually pleasurable. It is this inter-
pretive stage that admits the profound influences of culture and context in the
experience of sexual pleasure’’ (pp. 8–9).
In this sense, sexuality is thoroughly social and context dependent. The act
of coitus, for instance, might be construed as an expression of intimacy
(‘‘making love’’), a casual act of physical gratification (‘‘hooking up’’), a vio-
lation of bodily and personal integrity (rape), a form of labor performed in
exchange for resources (prostitution), or even a military strategy (as in the case
of systematic wartime rape). These varied interpretations of coitus depend not
just on the time, place, and situation but also on which participant you ask.
These examples show how an identical physical act, penile-vaginal intercourse,
may have radically divergent meanings and consequences. On a similar note,
Hope Landrine (1998) argues for the need for contextualized understandings
of behavior by comparing the distinct meanings of anal intercourse among
young Latina women and gay men:
1. Our instinctual sex drive is constantly surging and must be actively restrained
by laws, morals, and individual willpower (i.e., the drive reduction model).
2. ‘‘Real’’ sex is coitus (i.e., penile-vaginal intercourse)—everything else is fore-
play (e.g., hand-genital contact, oral sex) or perverted (e.g., anal sex; bondage).
3. Heterosexuality is normal (statistically, clinically, and ideally).
4. In sexual relationships and encounters, men and women occupy distinct gender
roles.
5. Sexuality is for adults only.
Religion
Prior to the Enlightenment and the premium placed on science in the
modern world, the Christian church (which I use to refer to Catholic and
Protestant denominations) served as the ultimate authority governing sexuality.
Relying on and generating doctrines that associated certain (most) sexual rela-
The Social Construction of Sexuality 211
tionships and behaviors with varying levels of sin, the church established and
disseminated norms, including strict prohibitions regarding masturbation and
premarital or extramarital sexual relations (especially for women). The concept
of ‘‘sin,’’ which characterized all violations of the religious boundaries placed on
sexuality, played a central role in pre-Enlightenment constructions of sexuality.
With the rise of modernism, however, the scientific concepts of disease and
disorder replaced sin as the marker of deviations from sexual norms. Kim Phillips
and Barry Reay (2002) articulate this by quoting Donzelot: ‘‘The priest pre-
ceded the doctor as the manager of sexuality’’ (p. 10).
Although religion is no longer the sole authority regarding sexuality, it
would be misguided to minimize its continued role in the construction of
sexuality. This is especially true in the United States, which is widely regarded
as the most religious among industrialized nations (Whitehead, Wilcox, Ros-
tosky, Randall, & Wright, 2001). For instance, despite the racial and ethnic
diversity of the country, the majority of Americans claim theistic beliefs and
specifically identify with Christian denominations. According to the results of a
recent Gallup poll reported in the New York Times, 46 percent of all Americans
identify as evangelical or born-again Christians (Kristof, 2003). Indeed, through
its significant influence on American politics and therefore federal funding, the
Christian church exerts substantial control over sexuality-related research,
policies, and programs such as sexuality education (Irvine, 2002).
In addition to this indirect influence, religion also plays a direct role in the
construction of sexuality at local and personal levels. Research regarding the
association between religion and sexuality has been primarily focused on
whether religion serves as a protective factor against the perceived risks of
sexuality by reducing nonmarital sexual activity itself. If we take a moment to
deconstruct this basic research question, it becomes clear that it operates on the
drive reductionist assumption that something is needed to help people withstand
the urge to have sex; that without religious, legal, or social deterrents of some
sort, people will engage in sex willy-nilly and put themselves, and others, at all
kinds of risk. Nevertheless, the preponderance of evidence does indicate that
religiosity, both in terms of personal beliefs and public practices such as at-
tending services, is associated with less frequent sexual activity among adults
(Poulson, Eppler, Satterwhite, Wuensch, & Bass, 1998; Wyatt, 1997) and ad-
olescents (Holder et al., 2000).
Although this appears to suggest that religiosity is ‘‘good’’ for one’s sexual
health (presuming one strives for lower rates of sexual activity), another set of
research findings complicates this picture: religion is also associated with lower
levels of safe sexual activity (i.e., condom and contraceptive use) (Holder et al.,
2000; Wilcox, Rostosky, Randall, & Wright, 2001). Another study by Marlena
Studer and Arland Thornton (1987) indicated that religious teen women were
less likely to use ‘‘medical’’ contraceptives (e.g., methods such as the pill, which
require a medical examination and prescription) than other methods, including
condoms, spermicides, withdrawal, and the rhythm method. They explain this
212 Sexuality Today
Medicine
One of the hallmark features of modernism is its conviction in the su-
premacy of objectivity and logic. Medicine, as the scientific study and treatment
of the human body, is revered as the ultimate authority on the conditions and
potential of human life. Through its predominant focus on physiological, as
opposed to social, grounds and remedies for sexual ‘‘dysfunctions’’ and abnor-
malities, the interdisciplinary study of sexuality reveals its reliance on medicalized
notions of human experiences and relationships. Tiefer (2004) defines medi-
calization as ‘‘a major social and intellectual trend whereby the concepts and
practices of medicine come to exercise authority over particular areas of life’’
(p. 181). She continues to describe American culture as in a state of ‘‘biomania,’’
in which our attention is exclusively trained on the body (hormones, brain
chemistry, brain structure, DNA, and so on), and we are fixated on medical
explanations and techniques to reveal the reasons and cures for all conditions and
complaints. Although advances in medical knowledge and technology have
certainly enriched many aspects of life, Tiefer warns that the total dominance of
the medical model in sexuality, both as a field of research and as an aspect of
The Social Construction of Sexuality 213
of ‘‘sick.’’ However, as Tiefer (2004) points out, the health model does not
quite work with sexuality:
The Media
Magazines, television, movies, and the Internet all serve as the mouthpiece
of medicine and play a critical role in the medicalized construction of sexu-
The Social Construction of Sexuality 215
ality. A glance at the headlines of current women’s and men’s magazines will
offer clear evidence of the media’s role in the construction of sexuality as well
as the dissemination of sexual ‘‘expert’’ advice: ‘‘Secrets of your sex drive: Why
you want it when you want it . . . and how to want it more’’ (Cosmopolitan,
2005, June); and ‘‘Sex by sundown: Cheap tricks that pay off big’’ (Maxim,
2005, May). In addition, the news media play an important role in relaying
scientific and medical discoveries and knowledge to the general public. Most
recently, a Swedish study regarding differences between the reactions of ho-
mosexual and heterosexual men to particular scents made the headlines of
major print, television, and online media. The study’s design does not allow
for conclusions about which came first: the difference in scent receptivity or
the identity as gay. That is, it is possible that men who frequently or exclu-
sively have sex with men develop a different smell response as a result of their
sexual behaviors, rather than having homosexual relationships as a result of an
inborn biological difference. However, as Anne Fausto-Sterling (2000) warns,
popular media reports of scientific studies frequently distort research findings
by overlooking or misrepresenting more nuanced aspects of the work or by
only choosing to report studies that confirm particular positions or viewpoints
(thereby increasing their audience and subsequent profit). Indeed, with regard
to the study about scent reception, some media outlets, such as the New York
Times (Wade, 2005), mentioned (albeit at the end of the article) that causality
cannot be deduced from this study, while others blatantly distorted the find-
ings with headlines such as ‘‘Hormone sniff test indicates biological base for
sexual orientation, researchers report’’ (Schmid, 2005). Rather than present a
more complex, ambiguous, and accurate story of the research findings, the
media packaged a simpler, more popular version, one that bolsters an essen-
tialist construction of sexual orientation.
In addition to its role in perpetuating a medicalized construction of sex-
uality, the popular media also socialize viewers and readers into other aspects of
‘‘normal’’ sexuality. Specifically, through their explicit and implicit messages,
media such as magazines and television contribute to the coitus-centered,
heteronormative, and traditionally gendered norms of sexuality. Studies by
Laura Carpenter (1998) and Meenakshi Durham (1998) deconstructed the
gendered messages contained in girls’ magazines such as Seventeen, noting the
ways in which they teach girls to adhere to gendered sexual norms of feminine
appearance and the role of girls and women to please others (especially boys
and men). At the same time, such magazines send messages of the importance
of sexual virtue, warning girls not to give in to sexual temptation. In this way,
they direct girls to be sexual looking but not sexual acting. Men’s magazines
such as Maxim and Stuff contain similarly gendered content, though it is aimed
at encouraging a male target audience to objectify women and view them-
selves as entitled to sexual fulfillment (Krassas, Blauwkamp, & Wesselink,
2003; Ward, 2003). In her extensive review of the media’s role in sexual
socialization, Ward (1995) found that although television programming tends
216 Sexuality Today
their impressions that there were not enough images of black girls and women
and that those that were included made them seem less attractive and feminine
than their white counterparts. Indeed, viewers and readers are not passive or
blank slates that mindlessly consume whatever media is in front of them; in-
stead, not only do we choose what we watch or what we read, we also interpret
them differently based on our particular histories and backgrounds.
When comparing the sexual content of the news media (e.g., newspapers)
with that of the entertainment media (e.g., television sitcoms), a somewhat
divided picture emerges. While both these media construct sexuality according
to the drive reduction, coitus-centered, heteronormative, gendered, and adults-
only principles, they also diverge from one another. On the one hand, enter-
tainment media such as movies, music videos, and fictional television programs
tend to glamorize and simplify sex and sexuality: the people are beautiful and
successful and the sex tends to be exciting and consequence-free (Cope-Farrar &
Kunkel, 2002). The news media, on the other hand, tend to report on the darker
sides of sexuality: rates of STIs in the United States and around the world, sexual
predators seeking victims over the Internet, and the incidence of date rape,
especially among youth. As we will see in the next section, school-based sex-
uality education delivers a far less mixed message, focusing almost exclusively
on the dangers of sex.
Schools
Without a doubt, sexuality education is an ongoing process that involves
familial, peer, romantic, and sexual relationships; it certainly is not confined to
classroom lessons in middle and high school. Indeed, much of the point of this
chapter is that we are constantly learning about sexuality and soaking up norms
of sexuality from all different social institutions and relationships. However,
formal school-based sexuality education (SBSE) warrants distinct attention as
it serves as a public, official face of our culture’s sexual norms and ideals.
Recently, SBSE has been at the center of a cultural debate between a con-
servative, largely Christian sexual agenda—in favor of abstinence until mar-
riage and in opposition to abortion and equal rights for lesbian, gay, bisexual,
and transgendered-identified individuals—and a liberal agenda that advocates
for safer sex (including, but not limited to, abstinence) and respect for a range
of reproductive and sexual choices. Beginning with federal legislation in 1996
as part of welfare reform (Personal Responsibility and Work Opportunity
Reconciliation [PRWOR] Act), states have received significant federal
funding for abstinence-only SBSE. This funding has been regularly increased
since 1996, including an 18.5 percent funding increase for abstinence-only
programs in President Bush’s proposed budget for 2006 (SIECUS, 2005). In
order for a program to qualify for this support, it should satisfy a list of
criteria—the program ‘‘teaches that sexual activity outside of the context of
marriage is likely to have harmful psychological and physical effects’’ and
218 Sexuality Today
Families
The social construction of sexuality in the context of familial relationships
has received surprisingly little and fairly superficial attention. I refer to this as
‘‘superficial’’ given that research in the field has been limited to a focus on
adult-adolescent communication, even though sexual socialization is a process
that occurs over the entire course of a parent-child relationship, and because it
is typically only concerned with explicit, verbal communication rather than
the multitude of nonverbal forms of teaching and learning that parents and
their children engage in. What is more, this section of the chapter should
probably be called ‘‘Parents’’ as opposed to ‘‘Families’’ since there has been
220 Sexuality Today
very little research on the role of siblings in sexual socialization. The research
that has been conducted has yielded mixed findings: some studies suggest that
younger siblings become sexually active at a younger age than their older sib-
lings (Rodgers, Rowe, & Harris, 1992) or are more likely to become pregnant if
an older sister is a teenaged mother (East & Shi, 1997), whereas others find that
younger siblings are more conservative in their sexual attitudes and behaviors,
perhaps out of disapproval of their older siblings’ sexual activity (Kornreich,
Hearn, Rodriguez, & O’Sullivan, 2003).
Much of the research on parent-child communication regarding sexual-
ity has been focused on whether parents are having ‘‘sex talks’’ with their
children and what specific topics they include (DiLorio, Pluhar, & Belcher,
2003). Surveys have shown that both youth and their parents want parents to
be a main source of sexuality education (Wyatt & Riederle, 1994). However,
despite this mutual interest, research about what is being communicated, along
with other studies of exactly who is communicating with whom and how,
reveals a pretty substantial disconnect between parents and their children.
First of all, the confluence of three factors—the drive reduction model of
sexuality, the position that only adults should be sexual, and the belief that teens
are hypersexual—leaves parents fearful that talking about sex will somehow
encourage youth to have sex (Fine, 1988). This worry lingers despite the fact
that there is no evidence that talking to youth about sex compels them to have
more of it (Kirby & Coyle, 1997). In her interviews with American and Dutch
parents regarding the rules and limits they set for their teenaged children’s
sexuality, Amy Schalet (2000) found that the American parents frequently
described their teens as not only too young to have sex, but also too young to
know that they were too young to have sex (again, reflecting the essentialist
presumptions regarding the developmental capabilities and limitations of ado-
lescents described earlier in this chapter). Therefore, the American parents felt
that setting limits on potentially sexual interactions (e.g, coed parties) was part
of being a good, responsible caregiver. The Dutch parents, tapping into a
different construction of both sexuality and adolescence, felt that their role was
to adjust to and accommodate their adolescents’ emerging sexual interests, re-
lationships, and behaviors. Schalet describes Dutch parents’ efforts to negotiate
with their adolescent children and employ an ethic of mutual consideration that
involves compromise and communication among all family members, children
and, adults alike. She uses the following quote from a Dutch father to illustrate
such negotiation and reconciliation:
You [live] here with each other, [so] you have to take each other into
account. That means that it can be necessary to consult with one another
about what television program to watch, or what time to eat dinner.
From time to time, someone will have to compromise. We [the parents]
too. [This applies also] to whether boyfriends can sleep here, or whether
they cannot because we have other guests. (p. 92)
The Social Construction of Sexuality 221
Aside from this fear that talk about sex will incite the act itself, parents
frequently report feeling like they do not know how or what to say to their
adolescents. In fact, this is regarded to be such a common obstacle that there
are public service announcements on television encouraging parents to talk
with their teenagers, and community programs designed to educate parents
about sexuality so that they will be able to educate their children at home.
Indeed, in their review of related research, DiLorio et al. (2003) cite studies
indicating that the more knowledgeable and confident in their knowledge
parents are, the more likely they are to discuss sexuality with their children.
However, while increasing a parent’s knowledge about sexuality may
increase the likelihood that they will talk with their children about sexuality,
this does not remedy another major problem: many parents report that they
and their children have conversations about sex, but unfortunately, their
children do not agree. Across studies that asked parents and their children if
they had talked with one another about sex, 72–98 percent of the parents
reported that they had; however, only 13–83 percent of the children recalled
such conversations taking place (DiLioro et al., 2003). In another study, Jac-
card, Dittus, and Gordon (1998) found that 73 percent of the mothers in their
sample claimed to have discussed sex with their teenagers, but only 46 percent
of the teenagers corroborated this.
Researchers who are curious about the reasons for this apparent disconnect
between what parents say and what youth hear have suggested that parents may
be talking as they claim, but that they are communicating in ineffective ways.
Studies have shown that when discussing sexual matters, mothers become more
authoritative and didactic in their style and are more inclined to lecture than
to engage in a conversation of mutual turn-taking (Kahlbaugh, Lefkowitz,
Valdez, & Sigman, 1997; Lefkowitz, Kahlbaugh, & Sigman, 1996). This may be
due to a few factors: a lack of modeling about how to talk about these issues
(because parents’ own parents did not have such conversations with them), the
concern mentioned earlier that talking about sex will somehow encourage sex,
and the discomfort and embarrassment that parents frequently report when
talking to their children about sexuality (which, of course, may be due to the
first two factors) (Wyatt & Riederle, 1994).
Research in this area also demonstrates that parents are communicating in
selective ways. That is, who says what to whom varies significantly and largely
according to gender. In their review, DiLorio et al. (2003) cite evidence that
when it comes to sex talks, mothers are doing more of the talking than fathers.
What is more, mothers are more likely to talk with daughters than with sons;
and when fathers do talk about sexuality, they are more likely to do so with sons
than with daughters. Of greatest importance to the subject of this chapter,
however, is the difference in what parents (typically mothers) tell their
daughters compared to their sons. In keeping with the gender norms that
dominate mainstream sexual scripts, daughters are more frequently instructed in
ways to be successful sexual gatekeepers (Downie & Coates, 1999; O’Sullivan,
222 Sexuality Today
CONCLUSION
The focus of this chapter has been on the social institutions that authorize
and disseminate norms of sexuality in the contemporary United States, norms
that essentialize sexuality as driven by a deep-seated, instinctual urge to have
penile-vaginal intercourse, within traditionally gendered adult relationships. In
the past, religion served as the primary authority regarding sexuality. Although
it retains significant influence, this role has largely been usurped by science,
which presents a seemingly objective, medicalized view of sexuality. Het-
eronormative, coitus-centered, and gendered norms are further transmitted
through the social institutions of the media, schools, and families, reproducing
a narrow, essentialist construction of sexuality.
However, as discussed at the beginning of this chapter, social constructionism
allows us to revisit and reevaluate what seem to be foregone conclusions about
what is natural—what must be, or has always been. In doing so, social con-
structionism shows us how to read between lines and recognize the implicit
meanings and norms that shape our behavior, which in turn further shapes our
reality. Thus, social constructionism creates new ways of viewing social issues.
What is more, it also allows us to see ourselves and the potential for
change in new and exciting ways. In contrast to the inevitability and stability
proposed by essentialist perspectives, social constructs are dynamic and must be
constantly produced and reproduced in order to exist. If, for example, we all
stopped using race in our identification and categorization of ourselves and
others, if no one called anyone ‘‘white,’’ ‘‘black,’’ or ‘‘Asian’’ anymore, then
these labels would cease to have meaning or even exist. Race is not an in-
dependent entity; it is fueled by our thoughts and interactions. Of course, one
point here is absolutely critical: this example is not meant to suggest that the
repercussions of race, specifically, the damage done by oppression and the
benefits afforded by privilege, are somehow imagined; or that these do not
affect the lived experiences of individuals, families, communities, and nations;
or that the legacy and injustices of racism can be easily undone. The point and
perspective offered by social constructionism is not that race and racism do not
exist in our world; it is that they do not have to. Race exists as it does only
because we think and say so (see Chapter 10 by Lewis in this volume).
The Social Construction of Sexuality 223
Indeed, social constructs are not stable, and social construction is not a
one-way, top-down process. Individuals are not passive recipients of institu-
tions’ teachings; they also exert influence and play an active role in the exis-
tence not only of social constructs but also of institutions themselves. Without
sufficient membership, for instance, churches must close their doors. In a
recent study, Ellingson, Tebbe, van Haitsma, and Laumann (2001) described
the challenge faced by churches that must negotiate between denominational
doctrine (the source of their institutional authority and legitimacy) and the
needs and norms of their local communities (the source of their institutional
viability). They cite several examples of congregations diverging from larger
denominational conventions (e.g., bans on performing same-sex unions) in
response to the more accepting and open norms of the local culture. In doing
so, these individual churches influence the ‘‘normative frameworks’’ for sex-
uality in the local community, but also may have an impact on the larger
denomination’s policies and stances toward particular sexual issues. This is an
example of how social construction is an ongoing, dynamic process that occurs
through the relations and transactions between social systems of all sizes: in-
dividuals, local churches, and the upper levels of denominational leadership
and doctrine.
Similarly, dominant sexual norms and constructs are also altered and
influenced through alternative channels and communities. The Internet has
emerged as an alternative site of sexuality expression, interaction, and edu-
cation. To use sexuality education as an example, in sharp contrast to school
settings (where the curricular content is closely regulated by federal, state, and
local funders and administrators), anyone can say anything on the Web—a
double-edged sword, to be sure. However, despite the numerous risks (e.g.,
spreading misinformation), the opportunities for sexuality education offered by
the Internet are exciting. While conservative positions regarding sexuality are
well represented online (see Irvine, 2005), they are counterbalanced by al-
ternative, sex-positive Web-based sources of information and interaction
(Bay-Cheng, 2005). Furthermore, the Internet offers unique opportunities for
building communities among sexually stigmatized and marginalized individ-
uals (Stern & Handel, 2001) and for challenging the narrow construction of
sexuality that is being produced and reproduced through religion, medicine,
the media, schools, and families.
Much of the content here has criticized biomania and the medicalization
of sexuality for disguising variable, alterable cultural biases as universal, fixed
biological truths and for stigmatizing our deviations from those constructed
norms as signs of inherent inadequacy or dysfunction. If we did not have
science to draw boundaries of normal and abnormal; healthy and sick; big,
good, or frequent enough, what would sexuality look like? Would we see it as
a relationship that we engage in rather than an individual capacity? Tiefer
(2004) draws a humorous analogy to friendship:
224 Sexuality Today
People use the phrase ‘‘my sexuality’’ as though they are only sexual in
one way. They say that really comfortably, but I’m not so comfortable
with that, because the sexuality that I have with one person is very
different than I have with another person. My experience—I think
everybody’s experience as they get older—is one of enormous fluctu-
ation in my sexual life. Sexuality is more situational, like friendship. You
have the potential for friendship, but it’s not like you walk around saying,
‘‘Gee, my friendship is really going strong today.’’ (p. 93)
NOTES
1. Abstinence (n.d.). Retrieved May 15, 2005, from www.4parents.gov/
topics/abstinence.htm.
2. This conception of gender as a polarized categorical variable is itself a
social construct that has been rigorously critiqued and deconstructed. For exam-
ple, see Fausto-Sterling (2000).
REFERENCES
Abramson, P. R., & Pinkerton, S. D. (2002). With pleasure: Thoughts on the nature
of human sexuality (Rev. ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bay-Cheng, L. Y. (2005). Left to their own devices: Disciplining youth discourse
on sexuality education electronic bulletin boards. Sexuality Research and
Social Policy, 2, 37–50.
Bearman, P. S., & Brückner, H. (2000). Promising the future: Virginity pledges as they
affect transition to first intercourse. New York: Columbia University Press.
Berger, P., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality: A treatise in the
sociology of knowledge. Garden City, NY: Basic Books.
Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selec-
tion. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168.
Carpenter, L. M. (1998). From girls into women: Scripts for sexuality and romance
in Seventeen magazine, 1974–1994. Journal of Sex Research, 35, 158–168.
Cope-Farrar, K. M., & Kunkel, D. (2002). Sexual messages in teens’ favorite prime-
time television programs. In J. D. Brown, J. R. Steele, & K. Walsh-Childers
The Social Construction of Sexuality 225
Stern, S. E., & Handel, A. D. (2001). Sexuality and mass media: The historical
context of psychology’s reaction to sexuality on the Internet. Journal of Sex
Research, 38, 283–291.
Studer, M., & Thornton, A. (1987). Adolescent religiosity and contraceptive
usage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 117–128.
Tiefer, L. (2004). Sex is not a natural act and other essays (2nd ed.). Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Tyler, M. (2004). Managing between the sheets: Lifestyle magazines and the
management of sexuality in everyday life. Sexualities, 7, 81–106.
Wade, A. (2005, May 10). For gay men, different scent of attraction. New York
Times, p. A1.
Ward, L. M. (1995). Talking about sex: Common themes about sexuality in the
prime-time television programs children and adolescents view most.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 595–615.
Ward, L. M. (2003). Understanding the role of entertainment media in the sexual
socialization of American youth: A review of empirical research. Devel-
opmental Review, 23, 347–388.
Ward, L. M., Hansbrough, E., & Walker, E. (2005). Contributions of music video
exposure to black adolescents’ gender and sexual schemas. Journal of Ad-
olescent Research, 20, 143–166.
Welsh, D. P., Rostosky, S. S., & Kawaguchi, M. C. (2000). A normative per-
spective of adolescent girls’ developing sexuality. In C. B. Travis & J. W.
White (Eds.), Sexuality, society, and feminism (pp. 111–140). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
White, J. W., Bondurant, B., & Travis, C. B. (2000). Social constructions of
sexuality: Unpacking hidden meanings. In C. B. Travis & J. W. White
(Eds.), Sexuality, society, and feminism (pp. 11–33). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Whitehead, B. D., Wilcox, B. L., Rostosky, S. S., Randall, B. A., & Wright,
M. L. C. (2001). Keeping the faith: The role of religion and faith communities in
preventing teen pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent
Teen Pregnancy.
Wilcox, B. L., Rostosky, S. S., Randall, B. A., & Wright, M. L. C. (2001).
Reasons for hope: A review of research on adolescent religiosity and sexual
behavior. In B. D. Whitehead, B. L. Wilcox, S. S. Rostosky, B. A.
Randall, & M. L. C. Wright (Eds.), Keeping the faith: The role of religion and
faith communities in preventing teen pregnancy. Washington, DC: National
Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Wyatt, G. E. (1997). Stolen women: Reclaiming our sexuality, taking back our lives.
New York: Wiley.
Wyatt, G. E., & Riederle, M. H. (1994). Reconceptualizing issues that affect
women’s sexual decision-making and sexual functioning. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 18, 611–625.
10
Linwood J. Lewis 1
How can we understand the relationship between race/ethnicity and sexual-
ity? Do persons of different ethnic groups have different ways of being sexual?
There are many correlates of sexual behavior, orientation, and identities—
social class, education, neighborhood organization, and gender, as well as race
and ethnicity. We cannot reduce the diversity of human sexual experiences to
differences between persons based on ethnic or racial characteristics. The
sexual experiences of persons in the world clearly occur at the intersection of
multiple social identities. Yet, race and sexuality (as well as gender) seem to be
important, basic aspects of the Western sense of ‘‘self.’’ Michel Foucault (1978)
suggests that sexuality captures a sense of ‘‘truth’’ about ourselves; Jeffrey
Weeks (1986) suggests that there is an assumption in Western culture that our
sexuality is ‘‘the most spontaneously natural thing about us’’ and that it gives us
our identities and a sense of self as man or woman, ‘‘normal’’ or ‘‘abnormal’’ or
‘‘natural’’ or ‘‘unnatural’’ (p. 13). I would suggest that race also gives us a basic
sense of identity and our place within society, which is more apparent for
ethnic and racial minorities, but in fact exists for all of us.
For many audiences, both academic and nonacademic, ethnicity and race
may seem to be natural categories, which emerge from a biological or phys-
iological base and do not change over historical time (Morning, 2004). Sex-
uality also shares this sense of the natural, meaning that it is presocial, or falling
outside of human societies to alter or control; highlights essential or intrinsic
and fixed defining aspects of a person, particularly in reference to their gender;
230 Sexuality Today
WHAT IS RACE?
Race is most often defined as the placement of individuals and groups into
categories based on physical characteristics such as skin color, hair texture, and
facial structure (Braun, 2002; Goldberg, 1993). It is commonly assumed that
members of a race also share a host of nonphysical characteristics including
behaviors, customs, and belief systems. Race differs from ethnicity, which we
will define here as a category of persons who share language, culture, history,
religion, and/or geographic origin (Haney-Lopez, 1997). All members of an
ethnic group need not share all of the characteristics of the group, but there is a
sense of shared origins and familial roots (e.g., blood). Race may be seen today
as primarily physical (with a subtext of culture added), while ethnicity is more
cultural (with a subtext of biology added) (Haney-Lopez; Omi & Winant,
1994). There is enormous confusion about the differences between race and
ethnicity: each is often defined in terms of the other, and this confusion has
existed since the beginning of the use of the term ‘‘race’’ in its modern sense in
the seventeenth century. The differentiation of these concepts is important in
understanding the relationship among race, ethnicity, and sexuality.
One example of this is the confusion between the racial term ‘‘black’’ and
the ethnic term ‘‘African American.’’ In this chapter, I use the former to refer
to the broadest grouping of persons whose ancestors were of African descent,
and the latter to refer to those specific Americans whose ancestors were
brought to the United States as African slaves. Thus, the racial term black
contains various ethnicities, including African American, a number of Afro-
Caribbean ethnic groups (e.g., Jamaican, Haitian, Trinidadian), as well as re-
cent immigrants from various countries in Africa (e.g., Nigerian Americans,
South African Americans). The distinction between race and ethnicity is
important because very different ethnic groups are often collapsed into the
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 231
same racial demographic category; yet, they may have very different histories,
cultures, and conflicting relations with other members of their racial category.
This is especially important when looking at issues of sexuality. For example,
some Afro-Caribbeans hold themselves in contradistinction to African Amer-
icans, whom they may perceive as morally suspect, lazy, and sexually undisci-
plined (Kasinitz, Battle, & Miyares, 2001; Waters, 1999). The use of broad
racial/ethnic categories in the sexuality literature masks ethnic, generational,
and cultural diversities.
Given the indeterminacy of race as a category, it may not seem that it is a
useful construct in understanding sexuality. In fact, there are calls from various
disciplines to remove race as a scientific construct, because it does not appear
to have biological meaning. There is no biological evidence that humans can
be reliably organized into racial categories that mirror common categories of
race (Lewontin, 1972; Montagu, 1942/1997). It has been demonstrated that
85.4 percent of genetic variation occurs within racial groups and 8.3 percent
between population groups within a race; only 6.3 percent of genetic variance
occurs between racial groups (Braun, 2002; Lewontin). However, to do away
with the concept of race would be a mistake because although race may not
appear to have biological currency, it does have enormous social significance.
At the microsocial level of individual interactions, we notice the physical
characteristics of a person that are associated with different races, and this
informs our expectations of that person. Omi and Winant (1994), in their
classic work Racial Formation in the United States, note the discomfort that arises
when we meet someone who is racially ambiguous, and our comments when
someone violates our perception of what that person’s race should be—
‘‘Funny, you don’t look [b]lack’’ (p. 59). Racial expectations and stereotypes
(e.g., whites’ inability to jump higher than blacks, the exaggerated sexuality
and criminality of blacks and Latinos) are testimony to a racially organized
social structure. These beliefs are used to justify broader, macrolevel social
‘‘policies’’ such as housing and job discrimination as well as to explain why
differences in the life circumstances of different racial groups, such as poverty
and high rates of incarceration, exist in the first place. Beliefs and expectations
about sexuality are organized racially, and so, ignoring race would deprive us
of a tool in understanding human sexual experience, particularly in twenty-
first-century America.
peoples of the Americas were incapable of being converted because they were
so inferior in terms of wisdom, virtue, and basic humanity. Bartolemé de Las
Casas idealized the lives of the native peoples and argued that they were in fact
capable of understanding Christianity and should be converted. In the end, Las
Casas may have been indirectly responsible for the continued miseries of the
native peoples as forced conversions to Christianity destroyed native cultures
(Goldberg, 1993).
From the sixteenth to the eighteenth century, there were changes in the
way slavery was practiced, which required changes in justification for Euro-
peans and Americans. Initially, slavery was an extended form of indentured
servitude, with the prospect of freedom for African slaves. Many of the
American colonies had substantial populations of freed Africans who were
Christianized and partly integrated into colonial society as artisans and other
laborers (Adams & Sanders, 2003; D’Emilio & Freedman, 1997). As economic
reliance on the forced labor of slaves to clear and work the land increased in
the American South, it added impetus for change to a permanent enslavement
for both slaves and their descendents. The practice of slavery made fortunes in
the New World and in Europe, as the infamous Triangle Trade moved slaves
from Africa to the New World in return for rum, molasses, and other goods,
which bought manufactured goods for sale in Africa. As slavery became in-
creasingly integrated into the economic and social fabric of the colonies,
Americans attempted to account for this practice to each other and to the
world by asserting the inferiority of Africans.
As Enlightenment ideas came to the fore in the eighteenth century, dis-
cussion shifted to concepts of basic human rights. Humans have rights to life
and liberty, and we have a moral obligation to guarantee those rights; we also
have the right to rebel in the face of abrogation of those rights by an unjust
government. Enlightenment values suggest that what separates humans from
other animals is the ability to reason. Linnaeus used the binomial Homo sapiens
(‘‘thinking man’’) to differentiate humans, while John Locke (1689/1931)
suggested that only rational beings should be afforded natural equality and,
thus, full moral treatment. There was a logical disconnect between the bru-
tality and permanence of slavery and the increased calls for basic human rights
to life and liberty by American colonists. Abolitionists on both sides of the
Atlantic remarked on this hypocrisy at the time (Adams & Sanders, 2003).
There needed to be a resolution of the dichotomy between the ideal of the
human right to life and liberty and the loss of these rights for African slaves
and, to a lesser extent, Native Americans. The solution was found in redefining
the important characteristics that determined whether one was human, and
pointing to the lack of these characteristics in Africans.
John Locke’s writings are widely cited as strong influences on American
ideas on liberty and political Liberalism. But it has also been suggested that some
of his work served as direct justification for African slavery and confiscation of
Native American lands (Goldberg, 1993; London, Pieterse, & Parekh, 1995).
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 235
Locke’s concept of nominally essential qualities, qualities that are created from a
society’s shared ideas about an object, was used to justify racial hierarchy
(Goldberg). The logic is as follows: societies identify qualities that define objects.
So, we note that balls are round and roundness is an essential quality of an object
called ball. If Englishmen decide that the color black is an indication of irra-
tionality, then irrationality becomes part of the essence of blackness. All objects
(including persons) that share that essence also partake of the quality of irra-
tionality. Africans are black of skin; therefore, they are irrational and irrationality
is a nominally essential quality of African-ness. Tying the nature of Africans to
irrationality is an important step in the moral justification of slavery because
rationality is seen as the hallmark of humanity (Goldberg). By defining Africans
as irrational beings, they were denied moral treatment as well as the right to
engage in rebellion and escape. For those who trafficked in African slaves, it
provided a fig leaf to defend their social and moral right to engage in the
practice. At the end of the eighteenth century, it allowed a compromise be-
tween Southern slave owners and Northerners who were uncomfortable with
the practice so as to ratify the Articles of Confederation and, later, the U.S.
Constitution (Adams & Sanders, 2003).
This act of intellectual legerdemain was not universally agreed upon at the
time of the Enlightenment. Johann Gottfried von Herder (1800) criticized
Kant’s anthropological views on race and on the supposed inferiority of blacks.
In Outlines of a Philosophy of the History of Man, Herder pointed out that dark
skin color is not uniform in Africans; that the cuticles, bodily fluids, and other
tissues are not black in color; and that the climate darkens pale European
skin to resemble Africans; thus implying that these differences are not per-
manent. He also pointed out that the lack of empirical knowledge of African
civilizations precluded comparisons between African and European cultural
achievements. Finally, he pointed out that the known diversity of physical forms
in Africa makes it difficult to form judgments of Africans as a whole.
James Beattie (1770/1997) argued against Hume’s ideas of the innate inferiority
of Africans by pointing out that if a slave can neither read, write, nor speak
a European language and is not permitted to do anything without the permis-
sion of his master, then Europeans should not expect the slave to distinguish
himself.
In summary, these ideas of the inferiority of Africans, which were floated as
reasons for justifying slavery, became reified as ‘‘truths’’ of what it means to be
African. Reification occurs when a society creates an interpretation of an event,
and then, without additional evidence, believes that the interpretation reflects
concrete reality. It was forgotten that the viability of organizing people into
races was argued and contested from race’s inception. It was also forgotten that
the stigmatization of the black race was originally posited as a justification for
African enslavement. Ideas of irrationality and inferiority came to be seen as a
natural entailment, or the essence, of blackness. These ideas about race were
then generalized, so that race was thought to represent natural categories for all
236 Sexuality Today
humans, categories that exist independent of social forces. This forgotten set of
controversies explains why there has been an obsession about racial differences
in intelligence in the United States until the present day (e.g., Herrnstein &
Murray, 1994). Ideas about the irrationality of Africans, which allowed their
poor treatment, became reified ideas about native intelligence of persons of
African descent in general. These reified ideas also become justification for
present social inequalities and inequalities in distribution of resources.
Section 4189 of the same Code declares that ‘‘if any white person and any
negro, or the descendant of any negro to the third generation, inclusive,
though one ancestor of each generation was a white person, intermarry
or live in adultery or fornication with each other, each of them must, on
conviction, be imprisoned in the penitentiary or sentenced to hard labor
for the county for not less than two nor more than seven years.’’ (Code of
Alabama, cited in Pace v. State [of Alabama], 1883)
Although these laws were largely found in the South and were enforced
largely among black-white unions, they were more often enforced when black
men attempted to marry white women. White men still maintained sexual
access to black women, whether that access was voluntary or not. Often, white
men were not prosecuted for the rape of black women; when blacks acted in
extralegal ways by attacking white male rapists to protect themselves from
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 239
sexual assault, they were themselves the target of lynching (D’Emilio &
Freedman, 1997, p. 217).
In the mid-to-late twentieth century, there was a gradual increase in the
number of interracial unions in the United States. Interethnic marriages were
fairly common among European ethnic groups, and antimiscegenation laws
were not uniform across the country (Pagnini & Morgan, 1990). As the lib-
eration movements of the 1960s and 1970s (Civil Rights and Black Power
movements, Gay Liberation and Women’s Liberation movements) liberalized
sexual interactions in general, pressure was applied to overturn these laws at
the national level. The U.S. Supreme Court declared antimiscegenation laws
unconstitutional in Loving v. State of Virginia in 1967. The number of inter-
racial unions increased from 310,000 in 1970 to 1,160,000 in 1992. This was
an increase from 0.7 percent of all marriages in 1970 to 2.2 percent in 1992
(Qian, 1999). At present, the ways in which ethnic and racial boundaries
relating to sex are enforced are informal and implicit (such as through social
pressure and stigma) rather than through the formal actions of the government.
If we examine the research literature on sexual networks, we see that sexual
interactions among racial groups are still largely endogamous (within racial
groups) (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994); when exogamy
(between racial groups) occurs, it tends to recapitulate class and gender
boundaries (Qian).
In summary, the history of the concept of race and its implementation in
American society has led to a sociocultural context in which sexuality, race,
and ethnicity form a mutually reinforcing framework. As individuals negotiate
this framework, they may or may not be aware of the ways in which the
sociocultural context affects the sexual choices that one makes as an individual.
However, individuals are also integrated into this framework—each one of us
has a sexual, ethnic, and racial identity that influences our sexual interactions as
members of the groups we see ourselves a part of.
composition of these groups. These data will therefore focus on broader racial
categories in examining sexual behavior and beliefs. Each of the following
sections will (1) describe the group under study, (2) focus on elements of the
history and present social conditions relevant to sexual behavior and beliefs
within the racial group, and (3) describe data on sexual behaviors and beliefs
collected within the past twenty years.
White Americans
White Americans form the largest racial group in the United States,
topping 194 million or 69.1 percent in the 2000 U.S. Census. This does not
include the 3.4 million who self-identified as white and another race and are
not of Latino origin, or those Latinos who also self-identified as white (16.9
million). In terms of ancestry, the 2000 Census asked respondents to write in
their ancestry or ethnic origin. The three highest ethnic ancestries reported in
the census data by those who self-identified as white-only were German (29.8
million), Irish (18.9 million), and English (16.4 million).
History
The importance of white racial identity and culture for sexual behavior
may not be apparent to most Americans. White identity often goes unre-
marked and unnoticed by the individual and the society because of the way in
which ideas about whiteness and American identity are created and experi-
enced in the United States.
Because there was no long-standing American ethnic identity in terms of a
shared history, ties of kinship and blood, or geographic origin held by all
Americans, such an identity had to be created and recreated throughout the
nation’s history. Through most of American history, one of the basic elements
of American citizenship was a white identity. This was policed through custom
and law; nonwhites were denied citizenship by law (e.g., Naturalization Act of
1790). Racial ideas were applied not only to those groups whose skin color,
hair texture, and facial structure marked them as different, but also to Euro-
pean ethnic groups as well. For example, in the United States during the
eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries, the Irish were not considered to be
members of the white race. Differences in language, religion, and cultural
practices, particularly around sexual and courtship practices, led to widespread
discrimination for the Irish.2 Similar experiences awaited other European
immigrants (e.g., Italians, Russians, Greeks) into the United States during the
nineteenth and twentieth centuries. Discriminatory practices led to extreme
pressure for those groups who might become white to assimilate or subsume
their ethnic identities and cultural practices into an undifferentiated American
identity. The rhetoric around the concept of the ‘‘melting pot’’ suggests that
immigrants shed their differences and become American. Immigrant groups
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 241
who do not choose to ‘‘fit in’’ by retaining their language of origin or cultural
practices face pressure from society to conform.
The loss of a specific ethnic identity and cultural experience may lead
white Americans to perceive themselves as lacking a race or ethnicity because
these two factors may not appear to be salient in their daily life. In many parts
of the United States, whites may not have any meaningful interaction with
ethnic minority persons, either because of de facto segregation or because of
low numbers of ethnic minority persons in general. Researchers who are not
immune to social and cultural influences on theorizing may also perceive race
and ethnicity as factors that affect nonwhites primarily, and thus ignore the
possible effects of white racial cultures on sexuality. While there is substantial
empirical evidence for white sexual behaviors and attitudes, as well as differ-
ences between racial groups, there is not as much empirical or theoretical work
on the effect of white racial identity on sexual behavior or attitudes.
18 for whites was three, which was higher than for all other groups except
blacks (four partners) (Laumann et al., 1994, p. 181).
Homosexuality. In the sexuality literature, there are distinctions made
among sexual identity, sexual behavior, and sexual orientation or desire. The
percentage of homosexual- and bisexual-identified persons varies from less
than 1 percent for black women to 3.7 percent for Hispanic men in the
NHSLS. For white men, this is 3 percent and for white women, 1.7 percent.
For women, this was the highest reported percentage, and for men it was the
second highest (Laumann et al., 1994, p. 305).
Stereotypes in American society suggest that homosexuality or rather gay
and lesbian identities seem to be associated with whites, particularly among
ethnic minority groups (Boykin, 2005). Media depictions of gay men and
lesbians are more likely to be white, which is consistent with the numbers of
whites in the population as a whole. The Gay Liberation movement in the
1970s was a largely white male movement, and much of the activism around
HIV/AIDS in the 1980s and 1990s was conducted by middle- and upper-
middle-class white gay men (Epstein, 1996). Whites also tend to be less likely
to hold negative attitudes toward homosexuality in general, although religious
conservatives (Burdette, Ellison, & Hill, 2005) and white men tend to have
higher levels of sexual prejudice (Bonilla & Porter, 1990; Herek & Capitanio,
1996).
Sexual dysfunction. Sexual dysfunction has not been discussed in racial terms
in the research literature until recently (Lewis, 2004; Working Group on a New
View of Women’s Sexual Problems, 2001). But examination of the literature
suggests that quite a bit is known about sexual dysfunction in white populations.
Very often, the data on sexual dysfunction does not contain the ethnic back-
ground of the research participants; when it does, the vast majority of the studies
examine largely white samples and cannot make informed analyses of the causes
or prevalence of sexual dysfunction in ethnic minority populations (e.g., Benet
& Melman, 1995; Feldman, Goldstein, Hatzichristou, Krane, & McKinlay,
1994). Eleven to 29 percent of white women reported sexual problems in the
NHSLS; these ranged from a lack of interest in sex (29 percent), to inability to
experience orgasm (24 percent), to anxiety about sexual performance (11
percent). Seven to 29 percent of white men reported sexual problems such as
climaxing too early (29 percent) and an inability to experience orgasm (7 per-
cent) (Laumann, Paik, & Rosen, 2001). While these rates seem high, they are
lower in comparison to other racial/ethnic groups.
Latinos
Latinos form the second largest racial group in the United States.3 In the
2000 U.S. Census, 35.3 million people (12.5 percent of the U.S. population)
self-identified as Hispanic or Latino. The largest ethnic ancestries reported
were Mexican (20.6 million), Puerto Rican (3.4 million), and Cuban
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 243
(1.2 million). There is enormous diversity found within Latino ethnic groups.
Major challenges lie in defining this population, deciding exactly who fits into
this category, accounting for differences in self-definition, and understanding
the relationship between culture of origin and host culture and its effects on
sexuality.
For example, do we define being a Latino as being a citizen of a Spanish or
Portuguese language country? How then do we account for Chinese Cubans,
who came to Cuba in the nineteenth century as laborers? Are they Asian or
Latino? In the U.S. Census, Hispanic/Latino status is described as an ethnicity
and Latinos can also self-identify as members of other races. Thus, a Latino
respondent can identify as black and Hispanic, or white and Hispanic, or
Native American and Hispanic. On the face of it, this makes sense; because of
the diversity of skin colors and ethnic group intermarriage and immigration,
Latinos can be of African, Chinese, Amerindian, or Asian Indian descent
among other groups, sometimes within the same person. The individual his-
tories of Latin countries may also determine the racial characteristics of their
inhabitants. Argentina was founded by the Spanish and for many years has
carried out an explicit policy of promotion of white European immigration.
Because of this, the ethnic breakdown in Argentina is 97 percent white (mostly
Spanish and Italian) and 3 percent nonwhite, mestizo (mixed white and
Amerindian ancestry), Amerindian, or other groups (Central Intelligence
Agency, 2005a). The Dominican Republic, on the other hand, because of a
larger African slave population and high rates of intermarriage, has an ethnic
breakdown of 16 percent white, 11 percent black, and 73 percent mixed (CIA,
2005b).
Another challenge lies in the expectation that a person from Argentina and
a person from the Dominican Republic have had similar experiences before
immigration to the United States. A white Argentinean may be of high social
class in Argentina, and if they appear white to American eyes (including
English fluency), they may also enjoy that class and racial status in the United
States without ever experiencing racial discrimination. Yet, they are members
of the same racial/ethnic category as a dark-skinned Dominican, who may
experience quite a bit of racial discrimination. Social class is an important
aspect of identity and varies across national origin and immigration status in the
United States. Given this, it may be difficult to characterize Latino sexual
behavior as a whole, although there is some research suggesting that there are
enough similarities between Latino ethnic groups to make such a discussion
meaningful (Carballo-Dieguez, Dolezal, Nieves-Rosa, & Diaz, 2000). There
are multiple levels of complexity in the analysis of Latino sexualities. The best
course available in assessing the work in this area is to be as painstaking as
possible in the ethnic identification of Latino respondents—what is the
country of origin of the sample; what is the level of acculturation, age of
arrival, generation, and length of residence in the United States for immigrant
samples.
244 Sexuality Today
History
From reading this chapter, as well as much of the scholarship on race, it
may seem that racial interactions were primarily between whites and blacks in
the United States. However, there are long and continuing histories of Latino-
white interactions that vary across particular Latino ethnic groups. In general,
the treatment of Latinos often paralleled the treatment of Native Americans in
the United States. For example, the appropriation of land and other material
resources belonging to Mexicans and Mexican Americans occurred in the
Southwest and California before and after the Treaty of Guadalupe Hidalgo in
1848 (Carrasco, 1998). The interactions between whites and Mexicans in the
Southwest were complex. Mexicans were seen as inferior, particularly as
tensions rose between Mexico and the United States. But it is important to
remember that in the early nineteenth century, white Americans were initially
immigrants into much of the Southwest, and so there was integration of whites
into Mexican culture. After the Treaty of Guadalupe Hidalgo and the dis-
covery of gold in California, the relationship between the numbers of whites
and Mexican Americans changed, and we see the implementation of pub-
lic policy to limit the economic power of Mexican Americans (Takaki, 1993).
Sexual relations between Mexicans and whites were different than that
between blacks and whites. They typically occurred in one of three fashions
(D’Emilio & Freedman, 1997). At first, white men intermarried Mexican
women and assimilated into Mexican society. This occurred earlier in the
nineteenth century, when small numbers of white male trappers, miners, and
traders interacted with Mexican society. Later, as more whites entered the
Southwest, Mexican women assimilated into white society through inter-
marriage. They often had to endure increased stigmatization as dirty and
immoral, particularly as relations between Mexico and the United States
soured in the wake of the Mexican-American War (1846–1848). Their hus-
bands attempted to reinvent stigmatized Mexican women as Spanish ‘‘ladies’’
in order to ease their integration into white society. The third type of sexual
relation was more exploitative, as white men used their social privilege in
similar ways as with blacks to subjugate and control Mexican mestizo sexuality
(D’Emilio & Freedman, 1997). This exploitative relationship was mirrored in
the law as well, as Mexican labor was alternatively called for and cracked down
upon by the United States throughout the nineteenth and twentieth centuries
(Carrasco, 1998). Tensions in the southwestern part of the country and Cali-
fornia among whites, blacks, and Latinos exist to the present day, and continue
to affect the perception of Mexicans and Mexican Americans in the United
States.
Acculturation is a significant issue for Latino immigrants (Zea, Reisen, &
Dı́az, 2003). The terms of immigration—whether there was a significant
population of the migrating group in the United States, the social class and
resources of the group, and the relationship between the home country and
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 245
et al., 1994, p. 305). But this percentage may be compromised by the meaning of
this self-identify for Latinos. For many heterosexually identified Latino men,
being the insertive partner in same-sex anal intercourse (el bugarron) does not
make a man a homosexual; it is the act of being penetrated that makes one
homosexual. Homosexuality is thought of as a problem of gender such that men
who have same-sex desires and act on them by being penetrated are not real men
(no hombre hombre) (Carrier, 1976; Dı́az, 1998).
For many homosexually oriented men, their identities as a man and as a
Latino are at odds with their sexual desires, and thus they may organize their
behavior to conform to ideas of masculinity. Some men may internalize en-
gendered sexual prejudice, be more concerned with the loss of an erection and
avoid becoming comfortable using condoms, and ignore HIV prevention
messages aimed at gay men because those men are loca (crazy females) and thus
different from themselves (Dı́az, 1998). Others may choose to drink alcohol
or use drugs in order to help dissociation from sexual feelings or feelings of
shame (Hughes & Eliason, 2002). Familism, a cultural ideal that emphasizes the
importance of family and family emotional bonds, also makes it difficult for
men to negotiate homosexuality. Since homosexuality is so stigmatized, it
increases the difficulty of coming out to one’s family and increases the shame
and emotional pain if a man is rejected by his family. There may also be
conflict between cultural values of respeto (respect for others) and familism such
that Latinos may not come out to their families because to do so would breach
the respect shown to elders (Rosario, Schrimshaw, & Hunter, 2004). Some
Latino men choose to avoid this possibility by living a double life, which can
lead to more stress (Meyer, 2003).
Latina lesbians experience significant stigma as well, as they must negotiate
compulsory heterosexuality and the expectation that as women they must have
a family and children of their own (Espin, 1987). There are cultural possi-
bilities for strong and enduring emotional ties between women (amigas intimas)
(Espin, 1993), but a lesbian identity is strongly stigmatized in Latino com-
munities. There is the perception here, as in black communities, that ho-
mosexuality is a behavior that was forced on Latinos by (white) Americans.
The stigma attached to the word may be so strong that some Latinas use the
words amigas or companeras in their self-identification. Again, there is an ex-
pectation that homosexuality is a gender problem, so that homosexually ori-
ented women may feel pressure to conform to a butch or masculinized role
(Espin, 1993). These cultural meanings may also compromise the percentage
(1.1 percent) of Latina women who self-identified as either homosexual or
bisexual (Laumann et al., 1994, p. 305).
Attitudes toward homosexuality are largely negative among Latinos. In the
NHSLS data, Mexican American men and women constitute the highest
percentage of respondents who stated that homosexuality was wrong (84
percent and 85 percent, respectively) (Mahay et al., 2001, p. 215). Controlling
for other variables, Mexican American men are twice as likely and women
248 Sexuality Today
three times as likely as whites to endorse this negative attitude. Many Latinos
face racial and ethnic discrimination and prejudice within white gay and les-
bian communities as well (Dı́az, Ayala, Bein, Henne, & Marin, 2001; Siegal &
Epstein, 1996).
Sexual dysfunction. Interestingly, when examining the NHSLS data on
sexual dysfunction, Latinos tend to endorse the lowest levels of these sexual
problems. However, most of the differences between Latinos and whites in
the statistical analyses conducted by Laumann et al. (2001) are not significant.
Twelve to 30 percent of Latina women reported sexual problems in the
NHSLS. These ranged from a lack of interest in sex (30 percent), to inability to
experience orgasm (22 percent), to anxiety about sexual performance (12
percent). Five to 27 percent of Latino men reported sexual problems such as
climaxing too early (27 percent) and anxiety about their performance (5 per-
cent) (Laumann et al., 2001).
When we examine women’s experience of sexuality in marriage, there is
evidence of generational and perhaps migrational differences in their percep-
tions of their sexual lives. Hirsch (2003) reported that in more traditional
marriages based on mutual respect and social decorum (respeto), Mexican
women expected no sexual pleasure in their sexual interactions with their
husbands. If it did occur, then it was seen as a bonus; if it did not, then it was a
private matter for the woman to deal with (Hirsch, 2003, p. 213). In the more
companionate marriages experienced by the urban, more educated, and/or
immigrant women, trust and mutual disclosure (confianza) led to greater ex-
pectation of intimacy and sexual pleasure for women and for men. Although
this division between immigration, education, location, and type of marriage
expectations is not a hard and fast rule (respeto and confianza are found
through Mexican society in Mexico and abroad), these factors are important in
understanding how sexual problems are defined and discussed (Fontes, 2001).
Without knowing the salience of these ethnic identities for the individual, it is
impossible to determine their effects on the sexual experiences of individuals,
but there are broad statements that can be made about cultural scenarios about
African Americans in general.
Twenty-first-Century Perceptions/Stereotypes of
African American Sexuality
In many ways, the present perceptions of African American male and
female sexualities are an extension of the ideas we have explored earlier. For
the past thirty years, the sexuality of native African Americans has continued
to be presented largely as a problem. In the psychological and public health
literatures, much focus has been placed on increases in adolescent pregnancy
and illegitimacy as problems found in black communities, although the
birthrate in all communities had increased during the mid-twentieth century,
and actually peaked in the 1950s (Luker, 1996). The number of babies born to
women out of wedlock across all age-groups has remained stable or declined,
depending on which group of women one chooses to focus on, and the rates
of black adolescent pregnancies out of wedlock dropped 40 percent in the
1990s (Martin et al., 2002). Yet, in popular culture and the media, myths of
black women as ‘‘baby factories,’’ and high rates of illegitimacy in black
communities as indicative of black sexual immorality continued to be pro-
duced throughout the twentieth century (Walker, 1998).
Multiple sexual stereotypes about blacks are continually in flux, trans-
formed by new representations in popular culture both within and outside of
black communities. Present scenarios about black women include the ‘‘black
bitch,’’ a loud, rude, and sexually aggressive woman, and the ‘‘ho’’ or ‘‘hoochie
mama,’’ a materialistic, sexually available woman who uses her sexuality for
material gain (Collins, 2004). These stereotypes, which are stigmatized because
they violate patriarchal expectations about women’s sexuality and its control,
coexist in hip hop culture alongside black female performers who speak out
against misogyny and objectification.
Much has been made of hip hop as the major creator of sexual stereo-
types about black men and women, as well as its production by African
Americans. Because it was created by poor and working-class African Americans
and Latinos (hip hop was invented in dance clubs and parties in the Bronx, in the
late 1970s), hip hop has heightened credibility as an authentic portrayal of
African American (sexual and gender) values. But the largest consumers of hip
hop are white: according to music industry sales statistics, 70–75 percent of
hard-core rap albums are purchased by white consumers (Speigler, 1996). Al-
though the musicians and producers are largely persons of color, consumers
ultimately control what is produced by their purchasing patterns; many of
the major labels that were originally owned by African Americans (e.g., Def Jam
Records, Russell Simmons) have been bought by mainstream recording
250 Sexuality Today
companies. This suggests that although hip hop may have reflected, and for
many still reflects, working-class and poor African American cultural values of
masculinity and femininity, there may also be a commodification and packaging
of hip hop to reflect expectations about African American male and female
sexuality by non–African American consumers. This suggestion would be
anathema for hip hop artists, for whom ‘‘keeping it real’’ or maintaining black
working-class ties and values, and not selling out, is a core value.
African American male sexuality has continued to be represented as dan-
gerous and violent, and also close to the primitive, and thus more potent than
other groups who have been exposed to the emasculating effects of civilization.
Past cultural scenarios of the primitive, rapacious black ‘‘Buck’’ and the Re-
construction-era rapist, who is a danger to white womanhood, have been up-
dated to presentations of the hypermasculine black (working-class) man. Images
of black hypermasculinity present an urban aesthetic of physical prowess and
strength, respect through the imminent threat or use of violence, misogyny,
leeriness of emotional ties to lovers, and a particular kind of sexual aggressiveness
(Henry, 2002). This aggressiveness is demonstrated through the persona of the
‘‘playa,’’ who collects sexual conquests, often through implicit or explicit de-
ception of their female partners (Anderson, 1997; Collins, 2000; hooks, 2004).
‘‘Pimp culture’’ is also an element found within hip hop; the pimp is glorified as
a man who makes money and controls women despite social sanctions against
him. The pimp is transgressive—he goes against mainstream social values and so
holds street credibility for black and white consumers alike.
Yet, hip hop culture is multifaceted. It is not only ‘‘booty flyin’ across the
screen,’’ but is also a major venue for poetry, political commentary, and ac-
tivism by young African Americans and Latinos (Kitwana, 2003; Rivera,
2002). Some of the same artists who extend hypermasculinity and misogyny in
their work are also advocates for political change, often within the same album
(Kitwana, 2003; Powell, 1998). Older African Americans, as well as feminist-
oriented critics, contest the values displayed in hip hop culture as misogynistic
and counter to the values of civil society (Powell).
Hip hop, however, has a visibility and perceived authenticity in the
United States that makes it a major player in the production and distribution of
ideas about African Americans (Kitwana, 2003). Its worldwide distribution
through music, videos, and motion pictures inspired by hip hop sensibilities
makes it central among the first experiences of non-American audiences to
African Americans.
In the context of the protean nature of hip hop and black cultures in
general, it is interesting to examine the sexual behavior of blacks in the United
States. High rates of HIV infection in black communities may also have had an
effect on individual sexual behaviors, even though it may not have had an effect
on the discussion of HIV in American popular culture at large. A Kaiser Family
Foundation report focusing on media coverage of HIV/AIDS from 1981 to
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 251
2002 noted that less than 2 percent of all recent HIV/AIDS reporting centered
on ethnic minorities, although HIV/AIDS is the leading cause of death for
African Americans between 25 and 44 years of age (Kaiser Family Foundation,
2004).
monogamy, which in turn led to family dissolution, lack of moral values, and,
ultimately, criminal behavior, alcoholism, and other forms of damage to Af-
rican American communities. The balance of work on ethnic/racial minority
sexuality in the twentieth century can be seen as a tension between biological
models of essentialism and sociocultural explanations for differences between
white and ethnic minority, largely, African American samples.
As sociocultural explanations of racial/ethnic differences became more
social constructionist in their approach, they moved from suggesting that all
members of a race receive the same socialization to questioning the basis
of shared ideas about sexuality. There also came to be a focus on the meaning
of sexual behavior to the individual and the society of which they were a part.
At the present time, essentialist ideas about race and sexuality have fallen out
of vogue in academic circles, particularly if they focus on biological ideas
about race. The biological meaningfulness of race has been challenged by
biologists, anthropologists, and now geneticists, who suggest that genetic data
does not support the typology of race as conceived in the nineteenth and early
twentieth centuries (e.g., Graves, 2001; Lewontin, 1972; Montagu, 1942/
1997).
called intrapsychic scripts. Each of these levels is analytically distinct yet inex-
tricably linked to the other.
Cultural scenarios are the scripts built by cultural or social groups for
explaining the (sexual) experiences of people. Cultural scenarios provide the
raw material for the construction of more personal scripts by members of that
culture by limiting choices of behavior and beliefs and by setting rules of
appropriate behavior and responses to others’ behavior. The majority of ideas
about black sexuality discussed earlier in this chapter are cultural scenarios.
These scenarios include beliefs concerning black (and white) masculinity held
by African Americans, European Americans, and other racial/ethnic groups;
expectations of male-female relations; and moral prescriptions for appropriate
and inappropriate sexual practices, among others. Scenarios can be about
persons outside of the group as well as within the group. These scenarios
impact on relations between groups; sexual scenarios can have a deadly effect
when combined with social power because of the emotional and societal
valence contained within them. Historical analyses, such as the analysis of race
in this chapter, can highlight the construction of cultural scenarios over time,
making clear why some scenarios have more currency at present than others.
Cultural scenarios, which refer to the beliefs about the behavior of
groups, are abstract in SST—the theory does not directly address the actual
behaviors and beliefs in individuals acting in a concrete world. An excellent
example of a cultural scenario is the concept of marianismo. The actual be-
havior engaged in by Latinas, and the extent to which marianismo is adhered
to or resisted by individuals can be very different from the scenario, and is
affected by the social environment in which the actual behavior occurs. The
actual ‘‘rules’’ that an individual woman holds for how she interacts with her
partners are interpersonal scripts. Some women may enact the marianismo
scenario with their husbands in their own life without thinking about or
changing their behavior at all. Other women may interact with their partner
and find that neither is satisfied with enacting marianismo between themselves.
Individuals take these external cultural scenarios, internalize them as a part of
the creation and continuation of the self, and in conjunction with other
persons, and use transformed cultural scenarios as interpersonal scripts to or-
ganize actual behavior in real-world contexts.
Interpersonal scripts govern the actual interactions between the actor and
other persons, but also allow room for improvisation. Cultural scenarios are
not simply downloaded verbatim into individuals. Individuals select the cul-
tural scenarios that are most consistent with their own ideas and experiences of
sexuality and incorporate them into their own menu of sexual acts. Inter-
personal scripts help in the production of behavior—overt, observable events
that occur in the interaction of social actors in the real world. Because in-
terpersonal scripts are behavioral scripts, this is the level that is usually ex-
amined in much of the sexuality research. There tends to be little reference to
cultural scenarios or intrapsychic scripts.
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 257
CONCLUSION
Examining race or ethnicity is just one way of organizing and under-
standing sexuality. Sexuality is defined and experienced within a web of bi-
ological, psychological, and sociocultural factors. This web is dynamic,
meaning that it changes in historical time, in ontogenetic (individual devel-
opmental) time, and as individuals become more experienced sexually within
the context of their society and the sexual communities in which they live.
Other important loci of identity (e.g., gender, social class, religion) also clearly
impact on the construction of sexuality and in turn are also constructed by
sexuality. This brief overview does not do justice to the rich literatures nec-
essary to develop a complete snapshot of this moving target. But it is clear that
reductionistic approaches that narrow and homogenize do not lead to a clear
and veridical understanding of race, ethnicity, and sexuality. Interdisciplinary
approaches with multiple methods of analysis should allow the field to move
from a mere description of sexuality to a deeper appreciation of sexuality in
context.
258 Sexuality Today
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by center grant P50 MH43520 from the
National Institute of Mental Health to the HIV Center for Clinical and Be-
havioral Studies (Anke A. Ehrhardt, Ph.D., Principal Investigator).
NOTES
1. Of course, the issue of agency calls into question whether any sexual
relationship between slaves and their owners can be considered consensual in the
face of this clearest of power differentials. How does one describe the choices
faced by slaves such as Harriet Jacobs (1813–1897), who was forced to choose
between her owner, a man who wished to make her his concubine, and another
white man, whose sexual interest and fathering of her children would protect her
from her owner’s advances? This tactic did not work in the end. After the birth of
her two children, Jacobs was forced to live in a crawlspace in her grandmother’s
home for seven years to hide from the advances of her owner. Jacobs’s experiences
were narrated in her autobiography, Incidents in the Life of a Slave Girl (1861/2000).
2. During the mid–nineteenth century, political deals made between Irish
American ward leaders and white, Anglo Saxon elites for Irish votes led to in-
creasing civil rights for Irish Americans and assimilation into white racial culture
(Ignatiev, 1996).
3. I use the term ‘‘race’’ here because although the U.S. Census designated
Hispanic/Latino as an ethnic category, the social treatment of Latinos in the
United States suggests that Latinos are treated as if they are a different race (Haney-
Lopez, 1997). Since race has little biological validity, the only currency that race
holds is in its social effects (Graves, 2001). Concerns about intermarriage, passing,
structural inequalities such as access to health care and housing, as well as the
grouping of multiple national groups into a single category suggest that ‘‘Latino/a’’
acts as a race in all but name.
REFERENCES
Adams, F. S., & Sanders, B. (2003). Alienable rights: The exclusion of African
Americans in a white man’s land, 1619–2000. New York: HarperCollins.
Anderson, E. (1997). Code of the street. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Azibo, D. (1988). Understanding the proper and improper usage of the com-
parative research framework. Journal of Black Psychology, 15, 81–91.
Battle, J., & Lemelle, A. J. (2002). Gender differences in African American atti-
tudes toward gay males. Western Journal of Black Studies, 26, 134–139.
Beattie, J. (1770/1997). A response to Hume. In E. C. Eze (Ed.), Race and the
Enlightenment: A reader (pp. 34–37). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Benet, A. E., & Melman, A. (1995). The epidemiology of erectile dysfunction.
Urologic Clinics of North America, 22, 699–709.
Sexuality, Race, and Ethnicity 259
Bergreen, L. (2004). Over the edge of the world: Magellan’s terrifying circumnavigation of
the globe. New York: HarperCollins.
Bernier, F. (1684/2000). A new division of the earth. In R. Bernasconi & T. L.
Lott (Eds.), The idea of race (pp. 1–4). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.
Bohan, J. S. (1993). Regarding gender: Essentialism, constructionism, and feminist
psychology. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 5–21.
Bonilla, L., & Porter, J. (1990). A comparison of Latino, black and non-Hispanic
white attitudes toward homosexuality. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sci-
ences, 12, 437–452.
Bowser, B. P. (1994). African-American male sexuality. In A. S. Rossi (Ed.),
Sexuality across the life course (pp. 127–150). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Boykin, K. (2005). Beyond the down low: Sex, lies and denial in black America. New
York: Carroll & Graf.
Braun, L. (2002). Race, ethnicity, and health: Can genetics explain disparities?
Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 45, 159–174.
Brooks-Gunn, J. (1987). The impact of puberty and sexual activity upon the
health and education of adolescent girls and boys. Peabody Journal of Ed-
ucation, 64, 88–112.
Burdette, A. M., Ellison, C. G., & Hill, T. D. (2005). Conservative Protestantism
and tolerance toward homosexuals: An examination of potential mecha-
nisms. Sociological Inquiry, 75, 177–196.
Carballo-Dieguez, A., Dolezal, C., Nieves-Rosa, L., & Diaz, F. (2000). Simi-
larities in the sexual behavior and HIV risk factors of Colombian, Do-
minican, Mexican, and Puerto Rican MSM residing in New York City.
Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 12, 49–67.
Carrasco, G. P. (1998). Latinos in the United States: Invitation and exile. In
R. Delgado & J. Stepancic (Eds.), The Latino/a condition: A critical reader
(pp. 77–85). New York: New York University Press.
Carrier, J. M. (1976). Cultural factors affecting urban Mexican male homosexual
behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 5, 103–124.
Central Intelligence Agency. (2005a). World factbook—Argentina. Retrieved March
20, 2005, from www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook/geos/ar.html#
People
Central Intelligence Agency. (2005b). World factbook—Dominican Republic. Re-
trieved March 20, 2005, from www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook/
geos/dr.html
Cheng, C. (1999). Marginalized masculinities and hegemonic masculinity: An
introduction. Journal of Men’s Studies, 7, 295–315.
Collins, P. H. (2000). Black feminist thought: Knowledge, consciousness and the politics
of empowerment (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
Collins, P. H. (2004). Black sexual politics: African Americans, gender and the new
racism. New York: Routledge.
DeLamater, J., & Hyde, J. S. (1998). Essentialism vs. social constructionism in the
study of human sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, 35, 10–18.
260 Sexuality Today
Gutman, H. G. (1976). The black family in slavery and freedom, 1750–1925. New
York: Pantheon Books.
Haney-Lopez, I. (1997). Retaining race: LatCrit theory and Mexican American identity
in Hernandez v. Texas. Retrieved April 2, 2005, from personal.law.mi
ami.edu/~fvaldes/latcrit/archives/harvard/haney.htm#N_1_
Henry, M. (2002). He is a ‘‘bad mother $%§!#’’: Shaft and contemporary black
masculinity. Journal of Popular Film and Television, 30, 114–119.
Herder, J. G. von (1800). Outlines of a philosophy of the history of man. London:
Printed for J. Johnson by Luke Hansard.
Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). ‘‘Some of my best friends’’: Intergroup
contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men
and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve. New York: Free Press.
Hirsch, J. S. (2003). A courtship after marriage: Sexuality and love in Mexican trans-
national families. Berkeley: University of California Press.
hooks, b. (2004). We real cool: Black men and masculinity. New York: Routledge.
Hughes, T. L., & Eliason, M. (2002). Substance use and abuse in lesbian, gay,
bisexual and transgender populations. Journal of Primary Prevention, 22,
263–298.
Ignatiev, N. (1996). How the Irish became white. New York: Routledge.
Jacobs, H. A. (1861/2000). Incidents in the life of a slave girl. New York: Signet Books.
Kaiser Family Foundation. (2004). AIDS at 21: Media coverage of the HIV ep-
idemic 1981–2002 (Publication # 7023). Menlo Park, CA: Author. Re-
trieved January 6, 2006 from www.kff.org/kaiserpolls/7023.cfm
Kant, I. (1764/1997). On national characteristics. In E. C. Eze (Ed.), Race and the
Enlightenment: A reader (pp. 48–57). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Kant, I. (1775/1997). On the different races of man. In E. C. Eze (Ed.), Race and
the Enlightenment: A reader (pp. 38–48). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Kasinitz, P., Battle, J., & Miyares, I. (2001). Fade to black? The children of West
Indian immigrants in southern Florida. In R. G. Rumbaut & A. Portes
(Eds.), Ethnicities: Children of immigrants in America (pp. 267–300). Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kitwana, B. (2003). The hip hop generation: Young blacks and the crisis in African
American culture. New York: Perseus Books Group.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (2001). Sexual dysfunction in the
United States: Prevalence and predictors. In E. O. Laumann & R. T.
Michael (Eds.), Sex, love and health in America: Private choices and public
policies (pp. 352–376). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lewis, L. J. (2004). Examining sexual health discourses in a racial/ethnic context.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 223–234.
262 Sexuality Today
Painter, N. I. (1991, November 11). Who was lynched? Nation, 253(16), 577.
Parrinder, G. (1987). A theological approach. In J. H. Geer & W. T. O’Donohue
(Eds.), Theories of human sexuality (pp. 21–48). New York: Plenum Press.
Peterson, J. L. (1992). Black men and their same-sex desires and behaviors. In
G. H. Herdt (Ed.), Gay culture in America: Essays from the field (pp. 147–
164). Boston: Beacon Press.
Powell, K. (1998). Keepin’ it real: Post-MTV reflections on race, sex, and politics. New
York: Random House.
Qian, Z. (1999). Who intermarries? Education, nativity, region, and interracial
marriage, 1980 and 1990. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 30, 579–
597.
Rivera, R. (2002). Hip hop and New York Puerto Ricans. In M. Habell-Palan &
M. Romero (Eds.), Latino/a popular culture (pp. 127–143). New York:
New York University Press.
Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2004). Ethnic/racial differences in
the coming-out process of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: A comparison
of sexual identity development over time. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Mi-
nority Psychology, 10, 215–228.
Rose, T. (2003). Longing to tell: Black women talk about sexuality and intimacy.
New York: Picador.
Seal, D. W., Wagner-Raphael, L. I., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (2000). Sex, intimacy, and
HIV: An ethnographic study of a Puerto Rican social group in New York
City. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 11, 51–92.
Siegel, K., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Ethnic-racial differences in psychological stress
related to gay lifestyle among HIV-positive men. Psychological Reports, 79,
303–312.
Simon, W. (1996). Postmodern sexualities. New York: Routledge.
Simon, W., & Gagnon, J. H. (1987). A sexual scripts approach. In J. H. Geer &
W. T. O’Donohue (Eds.), Theories of human sexuality (pp. 363–383). New
York: Plenum Press.
Smith, E. A., & Udry, J. R. (1985). Coital and non-coital sexual behaviors of
white and black adolescents. American Journal of Public Health, 75, 1200–
1203.
Speigler, M. (1996). Marketing street culture: Bringing hip-hop style to the
mainstream. American Demographics, 18(11), 29–34.
Stevenson, M. R. (2002). Conceptualizing diversity in sexuality research. In
M. W. Wiederman & B. E. Whitley, Jr. (Eds.), Handbook for conducting
research on human sexuality (pp. 455–478). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Takaki, R. (1993). A different mirror: A history of multicultural America. Boston: Little,
Brown.
Tiefer, L. (2004). Sex is not a natural act and other essays (2nd ed.). Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Upchurch, D. M., Levy-Storms, L., Sucoff, C. A., & Aneshensel, C. S. (1998).
Gender and ethnic differences in the timing of first sexual intercourse.
Family Planning Perspectives, 30, 121–127.
264 Sexuality Today
Dan Brown 1
Human sexuality has perhaps stimulated more attention than any other aspect
of human behavior (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948). From ancient times,
artistic representations of human sexuality have portrayed such images as ex-
aggerated sexual organs (Webb, 1982) and explicit participation in sexual in-
tercourse (Brewer, 1982). Perhaps the height of commercialized use of sexual
images in the ancient world occurred by 800 B.C.E. with the works of the
Greeks and the Romans (Lane, 2001). Pornography, derived from pornographos,
the Greek term for writing about prostitutes (Hyde, 1964), is at least as old as
prostitution itself (Zillmann & Bryant, 1989). Walter Kendrick (1987) traces
the term pornography through successive generations of dictionaries, finding
that it did not appear in Samuel Johnson’s work of 1755 and concluding that
the term was born sometime between 1755 and 1857, when the word appeared
in a medical dictionary. However, scientific and legal consensus about a defi-
nition of pornography remains elusive. A 1986 federal government commission
charged with studying pornography defined the term as ‘‘material predomi-
nantly sexually specific and intended for the purpose of sexual arousal’’ (U.S.
Department of Justice, 1986, pp. 228–229). A more complete discussion than
that contained here of definitional factors related to pornography is available
elsewhere (Brown, 2003).
James V. P. Check and Ted H. Guloien (1989) describe the distinctions
between erotica, degrading pornography, and violent pornography. Dolf
Zillmann (1984b), distinguishing pornography from erotica by the absence of
266 Sexuality Today
somewhat ‘‘chaotic’’ (p. 7), rather than well organized and centralized. Richard
Morais (1999) recalls that Forbes magazine reported in the 1970s about por-
nography as a business of organized crime that operated as a largely underground
cash enterprise. He observes that the Supreme Court’s Miller decision, applying
the First Amendment protection of free speech for pornography, allowed
pornography to be sold openly as a legal product.
Schlosser found that about 100 hard-core pornographic films were pro-
duced in 1978. Subsequently, Morais reports, legal pornography revenues
reached $56 billion worldwide by 1998, and he lists several large multinational
companies offering pornographic products and enjoying listing on major stock
exchanges. Businesses, such as Castle Superstores, openly sell pornographic and
sex-related products that once were hidden from public view.
its first year and 400,000 in its second, and selling 5.5 million copies per month
in 1969 (Lane, 2001).
purchasing all sorts of books and maps during that year. Pornography spending
in 1996 exceeded the $6.3 billion spent by Americans in 1996 at motion-
picture box offices and the $6.4 billion that they spent attending all spectator
sporting events (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1998). Seidman (2003) cited
reports by Egan (2000), Laslow (1999), and Rich (2001) that yearly sales of
pornographic materials surpassed $10 billion, more than half of which went for
videos and films.
The focus of pornography on home consumption also brought new
business entities into the industry that previously had nothing to do with
selling sex (Schlosser, 1997). Major corporations in industries such as tele-
phone, cable television, hotel, and motel chains provide pornography for fees.
Because pornography consumers no longer need adult theaters, many of them
folded. Los Angeles adult theaters dwindled to about six by 1997, after once
exceeding thirty. Similarly, adult bookstores declined and began showing
videos on-site.
Nonchain video stores often turn to pornography to compete with chains.
Schlosser (1997) cites Paul Fishbein of Adult Video News as reporting that
25,000 American video stores, almost twenty times the number of adult
bookstores, offered hard-core pornography in the mid-1990s.
representing about 15 percent of the total number of Web users in that year
(Cooper, Scherer, Boies, & Gordon, 1999).
Small hobbyists were not the only people exchanging pornography on the
Internet. Print pornography peddlers like Playboy, Penthouse, and Hustler host
some of the most popular online sites. The Playboy Web site opened in 1994,
offering free previews of monthly magazines. The site received 4.3 million
visits in October 1997 and generated revenues of $2.5 million in one quarter
during 1999 (Lane, 2001).
Morais (1999) cited Forrester Research reports of Internet pornography
revenues reaching about $1 billion annually, including such enterprises as the
Internet Video Network, a channel reaping $7 million each year from such
content as strip shows. Lane (2001) cites estimates by industry analysts of Internet
pornography revenues of nearly $2 billion annually by the fall of 1999. Ac-
cording to Paul (2004), by July 2003, the Internet contained 260 million pages
of pornography, up by 1800 percent from 1998. She reports that pornography
accounted for 7 percent of Web pages indexed by Google, an Internet search
engine. That proportion represents 231 million pages of Web pornography.
The federal government’s General Accounting Office (GAO) reported that
400,000 commercial pornographic Web sites operated in 2003, and the number
grew to 1.6 million by the issuance of a report by a congressional subcommittee
in May 2004 (Subcommittee on Commerce, Trade, and Consumer Protection,
2004). The report notes that 34 million people visit pornographic Web sites
each month, about 25 percent of the monthly Web traffic.
Rosen (2004) cites data from the Internet Filter Review showing that
pornography on the Internet makes up $2.5 billion of the worldwide $57
billion business of pornography. This Internet contribution includes, accord-
ing to the review, 4.2 million pornographic Web sites that are visited annually
by 72 million people, including 40 million Americans. About 25 percent of all
searches conducted daily on the Internet seek pornographic materials (Rosen),
and sex is the most frequently sought topic on the Internet (Cooper et al.,
1999).
Internet pornography has become so widespread that people confront it
without meaning to. A national survey (Finkelhor, Mitchell, & Wolak, 2000)
of 1,501 children of ages 10–17 who regularly use the Internet found that
25 percent of the respondents had found on the Internet, within the previous year,
unwanted images of naked people or of people having sex. Among these in-
cidents, 94 percent involved naked people, 38 percent referred to people engaged
in sex, and 8 percent portrayed violence in addition to the nudity or sex.
A congressional report (Subcommittee on Commerce, Trade, and Con-
sumer Protection, 2004) found that Internet pornography peddlers frequently
use false domain names and fake advertising to entice people to their Web sites
when they are not seeking sexual content. Searches conducted by the GAO
found pornography in 56 percent of searches using terms that are popular in
finding children’s materials online. Pornographers intentionally lure children
Commercial Sex: Pornography 271
to their Web sites by using nonsexual terms, such as Cinderella, enticing visitors
to retrieve files on peer-to-peer exchanges that allow Internet users to trade
files of various types, including music.
In 1996, Tomb Raider was released for game consoles, becoming popular and
spawning spin-offs in various media, including motion pictures. The game sold
2.3 million copies in 1998. Although Tomb Raider was not sexually explicit,
amateur Web sites began offering chances to see the buxom lead character, Lara
Croft, in the nude and play games with Croft appearing naked. None of the
explicit imitators sold as well as Tomb Raider because of their inability to market
through mainstream retail outlets (Lane, 2001). Clearly, video game pornography
justifies increased research attention because of popularity and the child audience.
A Gallup poll of more than 1,000 adults conducted in 1985 for Newsweek
magazine reported that 37 percent of the respondents reported having pur-
chased a magazine like Playboy (Press et al., 1985). Hustler magazine purchases
were reported by 13 percent of the respondents, 7 percent reported attending
an X-rated movie within the previous year, and 9 percent reported having
purchased or rented such a videotape or film within the previous year.
Bryant and Brown cited a summary by T. W. Smith (1987) of polling data
showing reported the proportion of respondents who had seen sexually ex-
plicit films within the previous year, covering 1973–1986. Fluctuating as low
as 15 percent in 1978, the data reflected a steady upward trend from 1978
through 1986 to 24.8 percent.
Testifying before the Attorney General’s Commission on Pornography,
Jennings Bryant (1985b) reported data from telephone surveys showing a
much higher rate of using pornography. From three groups of respondents,
each including 100 males and 100 females, he found that 94.3 percent of the
total 600 respondents had seen sexually explicit R-rated films, with the mean
at 14.4 films per person. The groups included students aged 13–15 and 16–18
and adults aged 19–39. Nearly everyone in the latter two groups reported
having looked at or read Playboy or Playgirl or similar magazines, with adult
males reporting experience with an average of 26.5 issues. The average age of
first experience with such materials among all three groups was 11 years for
males and 12 years for females.
When the survey addressed consumption of hard-core pornography de-
picting people engaged in sexual activity, the average age among all respon-
dents was 13.5 years, and 69 percent of both males and females reported having
seen X-rated films. Among the members of the group aged 13–15, 92 percent
reported having seen such a film, whereas the adults reported first exposure
at an average age of about 18 years. Among the16- to 18-year-olds, 84 percent
reported having seen an X-rated film. Among all respondents, 70 percent of
females and 55 percent of males reported having been introduced by someone
else to hard-core films.
Bryant compared these figures with those obtained from students enrolled
in college classes, which provided nearly 100 percent participation in the sur-
veys, finding about half of the usage reported in telephone surveys. He ac-
knowledged that people willing to participate in a telephone survey about their
experience with pornography might be more liberal in their sexual attitudes
than those who would not consent, calling for more research to clarify the
differences in findings from the different surveys.
Alan Sears (1989) cited a July 1986 Time magazine poll showing that 72
percent of the surveyed members of the public wanted a government crack-
down on pornography, and 92 percent favored such action against child
pornography. He also cited a 1986 Gallup poll reporting that 73 percent of the
respondents would ban certain types of extremely explicit pornography.
274 Sexuality Today
that differences in use of pornography exist between males and females, some
authors (e.g., Burt, 1976; Thomas, 1986) contend that such differences stem
from the type of pornographic content offered in the marketplace.
CONTENT OF PORNOGRAPHY
Print Media
Through the 1950s, open display of pornography was rare in the United
States, with such printed materials being relegated to back rooms of places that
appeared from the street to be ordinary newsstands (Brown & Bryant, 1989).
With increasing public display of printed pornography by the 1960s, the
materials used for pornographic magazines improved through the use of four-
color printing on glossy paper. The predominant sexually explicit magazine
content portrayed simulated sex acts with no exposed genitals because such
display would have likely opened the publishers to prosecution for distributing
obscenity (U.S. Department of Justice, 1986).
By the mid-1960s, nudist magazines dared to display genitalia, and soon
after, other magazines began to feature attractive models engaged, not only in
nudist camp activities, but also in sexual behavior. By the late 1960s, magazines
featured photographs of both male and female genitalia, usually portraying a
single individual but sometimes more than one person. Although sadomas-
ochism emerged in publications during the period, portraying less explicit
sexual photographs, such content was not a major factor in the commercial
marketing of pornography.
Textual descriptions of sexual matters became generally considered to be
immune from prosecution for obscenity law violations after U.S. Supreme
Court decisions liberalized views about published obscenity by the late 1960s.
The U.S. Commission on Obscenity and Pornography reported that many
textual works published by the early 1970s presented extremely graphic de-
scriptions of sexual matters. Most of these books were ‘‘designed to appeal
to heterosexual males, but about 10% portrayed material attractive to male
homosexuals, about 5% focused on fare catering to fetish enthusiasts, and almost
none were intended to appeal to females’’ (Brown & Bryant, 1989, p. 6).
Romance magazines also seemed to promise in photographs more sexual
emphasis than the text delivered in the 1960s. An analysis of eight different
romance magazines of the era revealed frequent kissing and coitus, but rarely
found homosexual activity or oral-genital contact. The practice of promiscuity
in the stories resulted in severe consequences (Sonenschein, 1970).
The 1970 Commission on Pornography and Obscenity identified adult
paperback novels as containing a considerable amount of pornography
(Massey, 1970). One of the largest examinations of these books reported
increasing frequency of paperback sexual content from 1967 through 1970,
followed by a leveling off from 1970 through 1974. This degree of emphasis
276 Sexuality Today
With the introduction of the video cassette recorder in the latter part of
the decade, the most popular-selling prerecorded video cassettes contained
pornography, and the industry grew bolder in a variety of ways. More graphic
content, including homosexuality and sadomasochism, became common, and
adult theaters openly promoted their wares (U.S. Department of Justice,
1986). During this period, public protests tended to focus on hard-core ma-
terials that were often promoted as triple-X to highlight their explicit brand
of pornography, as opposed to adult or X-rated films and videos. The latter
classification appeared in full view of the buying public in legitimate multi-
interest outlets (Palys, 1986).
Palys studied 150 sexually explicit videos, finding that they contained an
average of eleven sex scenes each. Among his sample videos, 77 percent of the
analyzed scenes contained sex acts, and he separated the videos into two ca-
tegories: adult and triple-X videos. The fifty-eight adult videos included more
aggression, and the triple-X videos contained more oral sex.
Bradley S. Greenberg (1994) cites findings from an examination (Cowan,
Lee, Levy, & Snyder, 1988) of forty-five pornographic videos as showing that
more than half of the scenes portrayed ‘‘domination or exploitation’’ (p. 168),
usually men abusing women. These videos resembled Palys’s (1986) triple-X
category. More than half of the videos (60 percent) included sex acts, with the
average video including ten such acts. Heterosexual activity occurred 78
percent of the time, 11 percent included lesbian sexual behavior, 2 percent
portrayed bisexual activity, and 9 percent displayed masturbation. No male
homosexuality occurred in the sample.
EFFECTS OF PORNOGRAPHY
Much of the literature about the effects of pornography deals with neg-
ative impact, but Kinsey suggested using sexually explicit films for sex
education in the late 1940s (Yaffe, 1982). For example, preadolescent and
adolescent school children, normal adults, medical students, health profes-
sionals, people with mental disabilities, and people with sexual dysfunction
have been involved with such therapeutic use of pornography (Bryant &
Brown, 1989).
Observing that many Americans believe pornography is vulgar but
without effects on its viewers, Victor Cline (1994) suggests that such a view
denies the idea of education in suggesting that people are not affected ‘‘by
what they see’’ (p. 230). He notes that pornography has useful results, spe-
cifically for couples who want to modify their sexual behavior or attitudes. He
observes that such patients would have specific prescriptions of materials to
view delivered by a licensed therapist, similar to the way a patient with a
different problem would receive a prescription for chemical medication. The
patients would not merely turn to whatever pornographic images happened to
become available because so much of pornography that is commercially
Commercial Sex: Pornography 279
with the sex life with a current partner. Bryant and Zillmann observe that
research evidence documents the occurrence of these harms.
RESEARCH ON PORNOGRAPHY
The 1970 Commission on Pornography financed a great deal of new
research about pornography, and the Attorney General’s Commission at-
tempted to call forth the results of scientific research in policy formation, but
the results were more or less surprising, depending on whether the researchers
maintained objectivity. Zillmann and Bryant (1989) pointed out that not
everyone in the scientific community agreed that social science research was
the best available method to inform such policy making. They observe that
some social scientists used the opportunity to advocate positions consistent
with personal consumption preferences and attitudes, leading to questioning of
the validity of research on pornography. To address that problem, they pro-
duced a book designed to offer diverse views from which readers could draw
more informed conclusions.
In that book, Kathryn Kelley, Lori Dawson, and Donna M. Musialowski
(1989) describe the three faces of sexual explicitness as including empirical
investigations, the interface of internal fantasy and external stimuli, and the
potential of sexually explicit materials in dealing with sexual deviance and
dysfunction. These authors defined sexual explicitness as clearly obvious
representation of sexual activity and found that many of the variations ob-
served in responses are associated with a theme of personality traits. They list
sexual attitudes and experience as other important mediators of responses to
sexually explicit content. They acknowledge that the same content may pro-
duce opposite reactions in different people and that sexually explicit content
may produce responses deemed both negative and positive.
Physiological changes such as blood pressure readings and other mea-
surements of the sympathetic nervous system are readily observable in people
viewing pornography. Enjoyment of pornography, in fact, depends on in-
creased levels of sympathetic activity (Zillmann, 1984b). Long-term con-
sumption of pornography tends to reduce initial resistance to such material
(Zillmann, 1989), and enjoyment of pornography has been demonstrated to
remain even or decrease after prolonged exposure. Zillmann and Bryant (1982,
1984) showed that these effects persisted after two weeks following the end of
such viewing. In plain language, consuming pornography quickly tends to
reduce the enjoyment of consuming it. The activity no longer produces the
level of excitement that occurred at first look, and more extreme imagery is
needed to produce the same degree of early excitement.
A systematic study of the research literature published in peer-reviewed
scholarly journals between 1971 and 1991 produced 152 empirical studies of
the effects of pornography (Lyons, Anderson, & Larson, 1994). Analysis of this
body of research resulted in the conclusion that using pornography produces
Commercial Sex: Pornography 281
Viewing Behavior
The primary research technique for measuring prolonged consumption of
pornography also emerged from the work of the 1970 Commission (Howard,
Reifler, & Liptzin, 1971). The protocol involved exposure to pornography
followed by an interval of no exposure before taking measurements of possible
effects. The pioneering study showed pornographic films to college students over
a three-week period. Eight weeks after the end of the screening, measurements
were taken during the showing of a pornographic film and also afterward. The
physiological measures confirmed that the primary response after steady exposure
to pornography was boredom from seeing similar material. Although pornog-
raphy’s initial impact on physiological arousal is powerful, content that once was
exciting soon loses its capacity to elicit a thrill (Zillmann, 1984b).
Zillmann and Bryant (1986) found that participants who engaged in
prolonged pornography consumption and were subsequently allowed to
choose their own viewing materials preferred more unusual forms of sexually
explicit content, such as bestiality and sadomasochism. This effect occurred
with both male and female participants and reflected the results of interviews
about repeat customers at adult book and video stores. Zillmann (1994a)
predicts that experienced consumers of pornography will turn to sexually
explicit materials containing violence because of the loss of excitement gen-
erated by more common forms. He suggests that if explicit sexuality appears
openly in nonsexual environments, such as on billboards, on public trans-
portation, on materials at checkout counters, on television, and other generally
available public displays, people seeking sexually gratifying stimulation will
soon turn to more radical forms of sexual imagery to meet their requirements
(Zillmann, 1984a). This same habituation to pornography, he explains, cor-
responds with frequent references in clinical literature that habituation occurs
282 Sexuality Today
1988), scientific proof of a causal connection is not available, and research has
not demonstrated that youths’ consumption of pornography leads to their
becoming sex offenders (Davis & McCormick, 1997).
Ethical and legal restrictions on research protocols make it difficult, if not
impossible, to prove that using pornography causes violent behavior. Conse-
quently, the frequency with which pornography viewers imitate what they see
lacks scientific analysis (Harris, 1994).
College men who saw pornography were more likely than viewers who
did not, to say that they might rape someone if they could be sure of avoiding
prosecution (Check & Guloien, 1989). Describing such admissions by research
participants as evidence of sexual callousness, Bryant and Zillmann (2001)
found that sexual callousness strengthens from viewing of both violent and
nonviolent pornography. Research findings have not yet demonstrated that
violent pornography is more powerful than nonviolent pornography in pro-
ducing impact on imitative behavior (Zillmann, 2000). Even when the por-
nographic content includes scenes of suffering by rape victims, participants
viewing the content show no greater tendency to admit a willingness to com-
mit rape than those who saw noncoercive pornography.
It is also difficult to prove a link between actual illegal behavior and
viewing pornography. Researchers acting ethically and legally cannot conduct
studies that would show such causation. Doing so would subject the research
participants to the possibility of great harm. Therefore, whatever researchers
know about such connections comes from interviewing criminals and studying
their crimes. Bryant and Zillmann (1996) found such evidence conflicting and
inconclusive.
When pornography was more easily available, rates of sex crimes in Japan
and Denmark declined, but the same was not true in Australia and the United
States. Mere availability does not explain these results, and researchers need
more knowledge of the effects of cultural differences when evaluating the
effects of pornography on criminal sexual behavior.
James Weaver (1994), summarizing research connecting pornography
with sexual callousness, concedes that research fails to prove the connection.
Young people today are more likely to have received initial sexual orientation
from pornography than are people of earlier generations. Weaver further notes
that consuming pornography produces a loss of respect for ‘‘female sexual
autonomy’’ (p. 224) and lessening of male restraint of aggression against
women. These two factors commonly appear in the beliefs and attitudes of
male sexual offenders. M. Douglas Reed, analyzing the clinical evidence about
pornography, states: ‘‘Pornography to addicted sex offenders is as dangerous as
matches and gasoline to an arsonist’’ (1994, p. 265).
Child pornography involves criminal behavior. Federal law (U.S. Code,
2004) prohibits producing, advertising, or distributing sexually explicit images
of models under the age of 18. Kenneth Lanning and Ann Burgess (1989)
describe child pornography as a record of child abuse because it cannot be
284 Sexuality Today
seemed less attractive and less satisfying as sexual performers. Questions about
the partners’ willingness to remain faithful emerged, as did greater willingness
for pornography viewers and their partners to sanction sexual relationships
outside of marriage. Recreational sex attained higher-rated importance for
prolonged pornography viewers, and they expressed greater willingness to use
sex as a tool for gaining favor.
Prolonged consumption of pornography itself significantly changed per-
ceptions about family values and fostered greater acceptance of sexual pro-
miscuity (Zillmann, 1994a). These findings were attributable specifically to the
pornography use, as distinguished from being the result of generally changing
attitudes in society. Such prolonged pornography use was accompanied by a
decline in expectations that intimate sexual partners will remain faithful to each
other, a finding that consistently occurred among males, females, students, and
nonstudents. Prolonged use of pornography produced greater acceptance of
sexual behavior with people other than marriage partners as well as with
people other than the regular partner outside of marriage. Additionally, the
prolonged pornography users showed greater tolerance for their partners
straying from faithfulness. In other words, this consumption weakened notions
that sexual intimacy should be reserved for a person’s exclusive sexual partner,
whether in or out of marriage.
This fostering of acceptance for multiple sex partners also demonstrates
consequences for ideas about sexual activity and health. Prolonged use of
pornography fostered beliefs that sex without restraints is ‘‘wholesome and
healthy; and moreover, that any sexual restraint poses health risks’’ (Zillmann,
1994a, p. 206).
Prolonged pornography use led to lowered acceptance of the importance
of marriage to society and greater likelihood of believing that marriage will
become obsolete. Again, these findings emerged in males, females, students,
and nonstudents. Pornography consumption also reduced expressed desire to
have children, apparently making family commitments seem unnecessarily bur-
densome.
In addition to affecting ideas about marriage, prolonged consumption of
pornography affected perceptions of grounds for divorce. Sexual infidelity
became less accepted as suitable grounds for divorce among pornography users,
and unacceptable sexual interest and initiative became more acceptable grounds.
For reasons unrelated to sex, pornography consumption produced no differ-
ences in perceptions about grounds for divorce.
Penrod, 1997). Research support for this phenomenon is available from studies
showing aggressive pornography to males (Donnerstein, Linz, & Penrod,
1987). Aggressive pornography was defined as sexually explicit images of
the use of force or coercion, usually involving rape or assault. A lesser degree
of support emerged from display of such content to females (Krafka et al.,
1997).
The rape myth suggests that women invite rape, and research shows that
the influence of mild pornography leads men to hold women responsible for
being raped. People who saw such images also gave less credibility to claims by
women that they were raped (Wyer, Bodenhausen, & Gorman, 1985). The
stimulus materials contained images of women in sexually alluring poses.
Women who saw the materials were less inclined to react the same way,
however, giving the alleged rape victim more credibility and less responsibility
for the alleged rape than women in a control group.
Another study assessed recommended rape sentences after conviction
based upon whether the respondents had experienced prolonged exposure to
pornography (Zillmann & Bryant, 1982). Although women issued longer
prison sentences to the convicted rapists than did men, both women and men
gave more lenient sentences after three weeks of exposure to pornography
during a six-week study.
Prolonged exposure to both nonviolent and violent pornography has been
demonstrated to increase the likelihood that males report willingness to
commit rape (Check, 1985). Even eliminating from experiments men with
predispositions to violence and men who might have been angry before seeing
violent pornography produced this same attitude toward women (Donner-
stein, 1984). After viewing pornography in which women were victimized by
violence in scenes rated as sexually arousing, men rated the female victims as
having suffered less injury than did men who did not see that content (Linz,
1985).
Edward Donnerstein and Daniel Linz (1986) found that young adults
displayed increased tendencies of sexual callousness toward women after seeing
violent pornography over a prolonged period. Unlike Zillmann and Bryant,
however, these authors concluded that the violence, instead of the sexually
explicit content, produced the effect. The same authors were involved in other
published research that found desensitization of male viewers of violent por-
nography in which females were victimized (Linz et al., 1989). Studies do,
however, reveal that realistic portrayals of the consequences of sexual violence
produced less male sexual arousal than violent pornography portraying female
victims who become sexually aroused during the assault against them (Linz &
Donnerstein, 1989).
Seeing films portraying sexually aggressive females led males to project
similar perceptions onto women judged by others as not being sexually per-
missive (Zillmann & Weaver, 1989). In other words, women perceived in
pretests as nice girls were seen by male viewers of sexually explicit content as
Commercial Sex: Pornography 287
being promiscuous. Women who saw such films were less willing to see
innocence in female victims in cases involving clearly guilty male perpetrators
of violence against women, including rape. The same effect appeared in male
viewers when they saw sexually explicit films portraying rape and erotic vi-
olence, but the effect failed to appear when males saw content portraying
consensual sex and female-instigated sex. The authors concluded that viewing
pornography trivialized rape among both men and women, contradicting
earlier studies by Linz and Donnerstein (1990) and Donnerstein et al. (1987)
that the violence, not the sex, produced sexual callousness.
After analyzing the available research on television violence, Haejung Paik
and George Comstock (1994) supported Zillmann and his colleagues, con-
cluding that the sexual content, not the violence, was primarily responsible for
generating sexual callousness. Other authors ( Jansma, Linz, Mulac, & Imrich,
1997) suggest that measuring interactions between men and women after they
view sexually violent pornography would improve the research about sexual
callousness, citing Elizabeth Perse’s (1994) finding that acceptance of rape
myths influences decisions to use pornography. Bryant and Zillmann (2001)
observed that meta-analyses (e.g., Allen, D’Alessio, & Brezgel, 1995; Allen,
Emmers, & Giery, 1995), or comprehensive studies of wide collections of
research investigations, have concluded that nonviolent pornography is almost
as powerful as violent pornography in promoting sexual callousness against
women.
availability of content to children. The Court also criticized the law for using
vague definitions of such terms as indecent and patently offensive in describing
content that could be restricted, noting that valuable educational and artistic
materials could be included using such terms as the basis for regulation (Rosen,
2004).
Congress passed the Child Pornography Prevention Act of 1996, in-
cluding computer-generated sexually explicit images of children under content
banned as child pornography. Again, the U.S. Supreme Court found the law
unconstitutionally vague (Ashcroft v. Free Speech Coalition, 2002).
Congress again focused on Internet pornography and children with the
Child Online Protection Act (COPA) in 1998, particularly on content de-
signed for commercial purposes. Congress used language from Miller v. Cali-
fornia (1973) to specify what would be restricted. The U.S. Supreme Court
ruled in the Miller case that community standards would govern what is ob-
scene, but the Court ruled (Ashcroft v. Free Speech Coalition, 2002) that local
standards could not effectively be applied to the Internet and called instead for a
national standard in such a venue (Rosen, 2004). These cases illustrate the
conflict between protecting the rights of those considered to be particularly
vulnerable to the influence of Internet and computer-generated pornography
and the First Amendment.
CONCLUSIONS
Reaching the bottom line on the impact of pornography depends on
perspective. The economic impact is unquestionable. Researchers differ about
the effects on attitudes and behavior. For example, some analyses of the re-
search on pornography effects (e.g., Fisher & Grenier, 1994) question the
findings and methods of research on aggressive pornography. These objections
have been criticized (Malamuth et al., 2000) as being inaccurate, unrepre-
sentative, and lacking validity.
Existing research tends to focus mostly on sexually violent pornography
and its impact on rape to the exclusion of emphasis on more subtle and
common forms of pornography (Zillmann, 1989). Strategies used in this re-
search often follow disparate methodologies that produce findings that fail to
fit into a consistent body of knowledge, resulting in a lack of clear under-
standing of research results and fodder for those who would attack research
about pornography. Such critics use different studies with inconsistent findings
as ammunition for their attacks. Despite clear social science research findings
about the harms of using pornography and despite clear testimony about
suffering from victims of pornography, some researchers and members of the
legal community do not accept that pornography causes harm.
However, other systematic meta-analyses (e.g., Allen, D’Alessio, et al.,
1995; Allen et al., 2000; Allen, Emmers, et al., 1995) have been praised
Commercial Sex: Pornography 291
(Malamuth et al., 2000) for their rigorous methodology. These studies report
consistent and strong effects from the use of pornography.
Despite these differences, Zillmann (1994b) argues that research provides a
strong foundation for public policy dealing with pornography. Inconsistencies
in the body of research are explainable to the satisfaction of informed re-
searchers, and the bulk of the findings are consistent in demonstrating harm
from pornography. Conceding that censorship is neither desirable nor con-
stitutional, he recommends that policy makers focus on education designed to
enlighten the public about such ills as sexual callousness, support for rape and
rapists, and other improper sexual attitudes. While falling short of demon-
strating that pornography ruins families and personal relationships, the scien-
tific evidence supports the contention that prolonged consumption of por-
nography influences the attitudes and dispositions of the users toward intimate
sexual partners, sexual health, marriage, and family values.
REFERENCES
Abelson, H., Cohen, R., Heaton, E., & Suder, C. (1971). National survey of
public attitudes toward and experience with erotic materials. Technical
report of the Commission on Obscenity and Pornography (Vol. 6). Washington,
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Allen, M., D’Alessio, D., & Brezgel, K. (1995). A meta-analysis summarizing the
effects of pornography. Part II: Aggression after exposure. Human Com-
munication Research, 22, 258–283.
Allen, M., D’Alessio, D., & Emmers-Sommer, T. M. (2000). Reactions of criminal
sexual offenders to pornography: A meta-analytic summary. In M. Rolloff
(Ed.), Communication Yearbook 22 (pp. 139–169). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Allen, M., Emmers, T., & Giery, M. A. (1995). Exposure to pornography and
acceptance of the rape myths. Journal of Communication, 45, 5–26.
Ashcroft v. Free Speech Coalition. (2002). 122 S. Ct. 1389, 1406.
Baldwin, M. (1984). The sexuality of inequality: The Minneapolis pornography
ordinance. Law and inequality, 2, 629–653.
Baron, R. A. (1979). Heightened sexual arousal and physical aggression: An ex-
tension to females. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 91–102.
Brewer, J. S. (1982). A history of erotic art as illustrated in the collections of the
Institute for Sex Research (The ‘‘Kinsey Institute’’). In A. Hoch & H. I.
Lief (Eds.), Sexology: Sexual biology, behavior and therapy (pp. 318–321).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Exerpta Medica.
Brosius, H. (1992). Sex und pornographie in den Massenmedien: Eine Analyse
ihrer Inhalte, ihrer Nutzung und ihrer Wirkung [Sex and pornography in
the mass media: An analysis of content, use, and effects]. In R. Frohlich
(Ed.), Der andere Blick: Aktuelles zur Massenkommunikation aus weiblicher Sicht
[The different view: Current research in mass communication from a
female viewpoint] (pp. 139–158). Bochum: Brockmeyer.
292 Sexuality Today
Brosius, H., Staab, J., & Weaver, J. B. (1991, May). Exploring the social ‘‘reality’’ of
contemporary pornography. Paper presented at the International Communi-
cation Association Meeting, Chicago.
Brown, D. (2003). Pornography and erotica. In J. Bryant, D. Roskos-Ewoldsen, &
J. Cantor (Eds.), Communication and emotion: Essays in honor of Dolf Zillmann
(pp. 221–253). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, D., & Bryant, J. (1989). The manifest content of pornography. In
D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Pornography: Research advances and policy
considerations (pp. 3–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: Men, women, and rape. New York: Simon
and Schuster.
Bryant, J. (1985a). Effects of pornography: Research findings. Testimony to the
U.S. Attorney General’s Commission on Pornography. Houston, TX.
Bryant, J. (1985b, March). Frequency of exposure, age of initial exposure, and reactions to
initial exposure to pornography. Report presented to the Attorney General’s
Commission on Pornography, Houston, TX.
Bryant, J., & Brown, D. (1989). Uses of pornography. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant
(Eds.), Pornography: Research advances and policy considerations (pp. 25–55).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1996). Violence and sex in the media. In D. W. Stacks
& M. B. Salwen (Eds.), An integrated approach to communication theory and
research (pp. 195–209). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (2001). Pornography: Models of effects on sexual
deviancy. In C. D. Bryant (Ed.), Encyclopedia of criminology and deviant
behavior (pp. 241–244). Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge.
Burt, M. E. H. (1976). Use of pornography by women: A critical review of the
literature. Case Western Reserve Journal of Sociology, 8, 1–6.
Check, J. V. P. (1985). The effects of violent and nonviolent pornography. Ottawa:
Department of Justice.
Check, J. V. P., & Guloien, T. H. (1989). Reported proclivity for coercive sex
following repeated exposure to sexually violent pornography, non-violent
dehumanizing pornography, and erotica. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant
(Eds.), Pornography: Research advances and policy considerations (pp. 159–184).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cline, V. B. (1994). Pornography effects: Empirical and clinical evidence. In
D. Zillmann, J. Bryant, & A. C. Huston (Eds.), Media, children, and the
family: Social scientific, psychodynamic, and clinical perspectives (pp. 229–247).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cooper, A., Scherer, C. R., Boies, S. C., & Gordon, B. L. (1999). Sexuality on
the Internet: From sexual exploration to pathological expression. Profes-
sional Psychology: Research and Practice, 30(2), 154–164.
Cooper, A., & Sportolari, L. (1997). Romance in cyberspace: Understanding
online attraction. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 22, 7–14.
Cowan, G., Lee, C., Levy, D., & Snyder, D. (1988). Dominance and inequality in
X-rated videocassettes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 299–311.
Commercial Sex: Pornography 293
Lyons, J. S., Anderson, R. L., & Larson, D. B. (1994). A systematic review of the effects
of aggressive and nonaggressive pornography. In D. Zillmann, J. Bryant, &
A. C. Huston (Eds.), Media, children, and the family: Social scientific, psychody-
namic, and clinical perspectives (pp. 271–310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
MacKinnon, C. A. (1997). The roar on the other side of silence. In C. A.
MacKinnon & A. Dworkin (Eds.), In harm’s way (pp. 3–36). Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
MacKinnon, C. A., & Dworkin, A. (Eds.) (1997). In harm’s way. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Malamuth, N. M., Addison, T., & Koss, M. (2000). Pornography and sexual
aggression: Are there reliable effects and can we understand them? In J. R.
Heiman (Ed.), Annual Review of Sex Research (Vol. 11, pp. 26–91). Mason
City, IA: Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality.
Malamuth, N. M., & Ceniti, J. (1986). Repeated exposure to violent and non-
violent pornography: Likelihood of raping ratings and laboratory aggres-
sion against women. Aggressive Behavior, 12, 129–137.
Malamuth, N. M., & Check, J. V. P. (1981). The effects of mass media exposure
on acceptance of violence against women: A field experiment. Journal of
Research in Personality, 15, 436–446.
Malamuth, N. M., & Check, J. V. P. (1983). Sexual arousal to rape depictions:
Individual differences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92(1), 55–67.
Malamuth, N. M., Check, J. V. P., & Briere, J. (1986). Sexual arousal in response
to aggression. Ideological, aggressive and sexual correlates. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 50, 330–340.
Malamuth, N. M., & Spinner, B. (1980). Longitudinal content analysis of sexual
violence in the best selling erotica magazines. Journal of Sex Research, 16, 226–
237.
Mann, J., Sidman, J., & Starr, S. (1971). Effects of erotic films on sexual behavior
of married couples. Technical report of the Commission on Obscenity and
Pornography (Vol. 8, pp. 170–254). Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Marshall, W. L. (1988). The use of explicit sexual stimuli by rapists, child mo-
lesters, and nonoffender males. Journal of Sex Research, 25, 267–288.
Massey, M. (1970). A marketing analysis of sex-oriented materials. Technical report
of the Commission on Obscenity and Pornography (Vol. 4, pp. 3–98). Wash-
ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Miller v. California. (1973). 413 U.S. 15.
Morais, R. C. (1999, June 14). Porn goes public. Forbes, 163(12), 214–221.
Paik, H., & Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of television violence on antisocial
behavior: A meta-analysis. Communication Research, 21(4), 516–546.
Palys, T. S. (1984, June). A content analysis of sexually explicit videos in British
Columbia. Working papers on pornography and prostitution (Research
Report No. 15). Ottawa: Department of Justice.
Palys, T. S. (1986). Testing the common wisdom: The social content of video
pornography. Canadian Psychology, 27(1), 22–35.
296 Sexuality Today
Paul, P. (2004, January 19). The porn factor: In the Internet age, pornography is
almost everywhere you look. Time, 163(3), 99.
Perse, E. M. (1994). Uses of erotica and acceptance of rape myths. Communication
Research, 21(4), 488–518.
Press, A., Namuth, T., Agrest, S., Gander, M., Lubenow, G. C., Reese, M., et al.
(1985, March 18). The war against pornography. Newsweek, 58–66.
Prince, S. (1990). Power and pain: Content analysis and the ideology of por-
nography. Journal of Film and Video, 42(2), 31–41.
Reed, M. D. (1994). Pornography addiction and compulsive sexual behavior. In
D. Zillmann, J. Bryant, & A. C. Huston (Eds.), Media, children, and the
family: Social scientific, psychodynamic, and clinical perspectives (pp. 249–269).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Reno v. American Civil Liberties Union. (1997). 521 U.S. 844.
Rich, F. (2001, May 20). Naked capitalists. New York Times Magazine, 50.
Rideout, V. (2004). Parents, media, and public policy: A Kaiser Family Foundation
Survey. Retrieved February 11, 2005, from www.kff.org/entmedia/entme
dia092304pkg.cfm
Rimm, M. (1995). Marketing pornography on the information superhighway: A
survey of 917,410 images, descriptions, short stories, and animations down-
loaded 8.5 million times by consumers in over 2000 cities in forty countries,
provinces, and territories. Georgetown Law Journal, 83(5), 1849–1934.
Rosen, J. (2004). The end of obscenity. New Atlantis, 75–82.
Russell, D. E. H. (1988). Pornography and rape: A causal model. Political Psy-
chology, 9(1), 41–73.
Sapolsky, B. S., & Zillmann, D. (1981). The effect of soft-core and hard-core
erotica on provoked and unprovoked hostile behavior. Journal of Sex
Research, 17, 319–343.
Schlosser, E. (1997, February 10). The business of pornography. U.S. News &
World Report, 122(5), 42–51.
Scott, J. E., & Cuvelier, S. J. (1987). Sexual violence in Playboy magazine: A
longitudinal content analysis. Journal of Sex Research, 23(4), 534–539.
Sears, A. (1989). The legal case for restricting pornography. In D. Zillmann &
J. Bryant (Eds.), Pornography: Research advances and policy considerations (pp.
323–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Seidman, E. L. (2003). The pornographic retreat: Contemporary patterns of
pornography use and the psychodynamic meaning of frequent pornogra-
phy use for heterosexual men. Dissertation Abstracts International, 64(8),
4063. (UMI No. 3102959).
Slade, J. W. (1984). Violence in the hard-core pornographic film. Journal of
Communication, 26(1), 148–163.
Smith, D. D. (1976). The social content of pornography. Journal of Communication,
26(1), 16–24.
Smith, T. W. (1987). The polls—a review: The use of public opinion data by the
Attorney General’s Commission on Pornography. Public Opinion Quarterly,
51, 249–257.
Commercial Sex: Pornography 297
Sonenschein, D. (1970). Love and sex in the romance magazines. Journal of Popular
Culture, 4(2), 398–409.
Subcommittee on Commerce, Trade, and Consumer Protection. (2004). Online
pornography: Closing the door on pervasive smut. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Thomas, S. (1986). Gender and social–class coding in popular photographic
erotica. Communication Quarterly, 34(2), 103–114.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1998). Statistical abstract of the United States: 1999
(119th ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
U.S. Code. (2004). 18 § 2251.
U.S. Commission on Obscenity and Pornography. (1970). Report of the Commis-
sion on Obscenity and Pornography. New York: Random House.
U.S. Department of Justice. (1986, July). Attorney General’s Commission on Pornog-
raphy: Final Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Weaver, J. B. (1994). Pornography and sexual callousness: The perceptual and
behavioral consequences of exposure to pornography. In D. Zillmann,
J. Bryant, & A. C. Huston (Eds.), Media, children, and the family: Social
scientific, psychodynamic, and clinical perspectives (pp. 215–227). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Weaver, J. B., Masland, J. L., & Zillmann, D. (1984). Effect of erotica on young
men’s aesthetic perception of their female sexual partners. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 58, 929–930.
Webb, P. (1982). Erotic art and pornography. In M. Yaffe & E. C. Nelson (Eds.),
The influence of pornography on behavior (pp. 80–90). London: Academic
Press.
Wilson, W. C., & Abelson, H. I. (1973). Experience with and attitudes toward
explicit sexual materials. Journal of Social Issues, 29(3), 19–39.
Winick, C. (1985). A content analysis of sexually explicit magazines sold in an
adult bookstore. Journal of Sex Research, 21(2), 206–210.
Wishnoff, R. (1978). Modeling effects of explicit and nonexplicit sexual stimuli
on the sexual anxiety and behavior of women. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
7, 455–461.
Wyer, R., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Gorman, T. F. (1985). Cognitive mediators of
reactions to rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 324–338.
Yaffe, M. (1982). Therapeutic uses of sexually explicit material. In M. Yaffe &
E. C. Nelson (Eds.), The influence of pornography on behavior (pp. 119–150).
London: Academic Press.
Zillmann, D. (1984a). Connections between sex and aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum.
Zillmann, D. (1984b). Effects of nonviolent pornography. In D. Scott (Ed.),
Symposium on media violence and pornography: Proceedings and resource book
(pp. 95–115). Toronto, Ontario: Media Action Group.
Zillmann, D. (1989). Effects of prolonged consumption of pornography. In
D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Pornography: Research advances and policy
considerations (pp. 127–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
298 Sexuality Today
Every night, between the peak hours or 9 p.m. and 1 a.m., perhaps a quarter
of a million Americans pick up the phone and dial a number for commercial
phone sex. The average call lasts 6 to 8 minutes, and the charges range from
89 cents to $4 a minute. . . . Three quarters of the callers are lonely hearts
seeking conversation with a woman. The sexual content of the call is often of
secondary importance. . . . most calls are answered by ‘‘actresses’’—bank tellers,
accountants, secretaries, and housewives earning a little extra money at the end
of the day.
—Schlosser, 1997, pp. 48–49
We are not bad people. We are regular people. You know, I live in a regular
neighborhood. I’m a regular person. . . . Just I’m a dancer. I think it’s
important for people to know that we are just regular people and that we do
have lives. We do have families, we do have kids, you know, and we’re
300 Sexuality Today
not . . . out to wreck homes. . . . We’re just out there to make a dollar, just like
anybody else.
—H. Bell, Sloan, & Stickland, 1998, p. 358
In its various forms, the sex trade is one of the most lucrative industries in the
world. The global pornography industry alone generates over $55 billion in
revenues each year, making it the third most profitable industry after the trade
of weapons and of illicit drugs (Morais, 1999). The global sex industry thrives
in some parts of the world, southeast Asia in particular. In Thailand, prosti-
tution fuels the sex industry, which employs tour operators and travel agencies
that offer special package deals. Sex tourism contributes upward of $4 billion
per year to the Thai economy (Bishop & Robinson, 1998). The sex trade
caters to men. The vast majority of the sex tourists to the capital of Thailand,
nearly 90 percent, are men. In fact, the customers of prostitutes, whether male
or female, are men. Although less visible in other parts of the world, including
the United States, the sex trade thrives in most cultures.
Of all the forms of sex trade, one in particular has a long and controversial
history—prostitution. The lengthy existence of the various forms of com-
merce reveals that there are sufficient numbers of consumers to sustain these
trades. Topless or exotic dancing also has a long history; as a form of enter-
tainment for hire, however, it is probably a more recent phenomenon.
This chapter offers an overview of the above two common forms of sex
trade. Research on prevalence and the different forms of these practices is
reviewed. Additionally, research findings on sex trade workers and their
customers are summarized. Finally, we also consider the sociocultural and
individual factors that support the sex trade, along with divergent perspectives
on the benefits and problems associated with the trade.
EXOTIC DANCING
We use the term ‘‘exotic dancing’’ to refer to any form of sexually sug-
gestive dancing for hire. The dancers are designated by a variety of terms,
including topless dancers, strippers, exotic dancers, and adult entertainers. The
latter term is apparently preferred by dancers because it carries less of a negative
connotation than the other labels. Entertainment featuring nude or topless
dancing has gained popularity in recent years. In one national sex survey,
16 percent of men and 4 percent of women reported having been to a club
featuring nude or seminude dancers (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels,
1994). According to the Exotic Dancer Directory, an industry publication, there
are over 2,000 clubs offering adult entertainment in the United States. It is
estimated that most major metropolitan areas have several dozen clubs that
offer adult entertainment, and these numbers seem to be steadily growing.
These clubs provide acts consisting of performances by dancers in various
stages of undress. Depending on local ordinances, the entertainers may be
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 301
profession like exotic dancing will tend to be more disinhibited and more
comfortable with their bodies than most. Some of these same issues will be
revisited as we cover another profession, prostitution. However, unlike topless
and nude dancing, prostitution is almost always considered a deviant occu-
pation.
PROSTITUTION
Prostitution refers to the profession involving the indiscriminate exchange
of sexual favors for economic gain, or the commercialized sale of sexual ser-
vices in which sex is a commodity (de Zalduondo, 1991). For the prostitute,
the practice represents a means of deriving or supplementing an income.
Therefore, prostitutes are referred to as sex trade workers. In some cases, the
prostitute may exchange sexual acts for illicit drugs; for example, the so-called
crack whores trade sex for crack cocaine (Fullilove, Lown, & Fullilove, 1992).
A person who trades sexual favors for a job promotion would not be labeled
a prostitute, although this includes some of the same elements as prostitution.
What separates prostitution from this example is the repeated and indiscrim-
inate nature of selling sexual services.
Historical Perspective
Prostitution has been called the ‘‘oldest profession.’’ In reality, it is
probably not any older than such social roles as medicine man and priest.
However, prostitution has always been and continues to be one of the most
controversial occupations. Much ambivalence toward the practice of selling
sexual favors as an occupation is reflected throughout history. On one hand,
prostitution is often viewed as a deplorable practice, but its lengthy existence
reveals that there has always been a demand for sex at a price. This ambiva-
lence is illustrated by the writings of early religious figures. Biblical texts refer
to Mary Magdalene as a ‘‘woman of the city, a sinner,’’ and many references to
harlots are found throughout the Bible. In the fourth century, the Christian
bishop Augustine viewed prostitutes as shameful while also noting that they
served as useful outlets for lustful desires. Similarly, Thomas Aquinas, Italian
priest and philosopher of the Middle Ages, believed that prostitutes helped
prevent the spread of lustful sins (Bullough & Bullough, 1977). Napoleon
Bonaparte is quoted as saying that ‘‘prostitutes are a necessity. Without them,
men would attack respectable women on the street.’’ In Victorian-era Eng-
land, prostitutes were viewed as unfortunate but essential sexual outlets for
men’s needs; the trade prevented ‘‘worse offenses’’ such as having sexual en-
counters with other men’s wives or with virgins (Taylor, 1970).
The ambivalence toward prostitution is evident in the various govern-
mental policies and interventions. President Juan Perón of Argentina ordered
the legalization of prostitution in 1954. The Argentine government and public
304 Sexuality Today
health department reasoned that legalizing the commerce of sex would help
control the spread of sexually transmitted diseases and prevent men from
engaging in sexually deviant behavior (Guy, 1991). In the same country, Dr.
Nicolás V. Greco wrote that banning prostitution led men to seek ‘‘artificial
methods’’ (such as masturbation) or ‘‘sexual perversions’’ (homosexuality in
this case) for sexual release. Greco and others believed that prostitution en-
couraged heterosexuality and, therefore, reinforced the institutions of marriage
and family. Lacking any scientific evidence to support these views, Greco
quoted St. Thomas Aquinas and St. Augustine. Prostitution has remained a legal
institution in Argentina since 1955. Historical records suggest that prostitution
has generally been viewed as a necessary ‘‘evil,’’ one that might be tolerated to
prevent worse evils.
male partners, and some men will be willing to pay for sexual encounters.
Declining numbers of eligible women could be due to higher mortality rates
(which was common in the past due to severe anemia and complications during
childbirth), a cultural requirement of female virginity, or a double standard that
tolerates male sexual experimentation but demands female sexual restrictive-
ness. In general, prostitution is most common in restrictive societies and least
common in sexually open societies. Presumably, in an open and tolerant so-
ciety, where the genders have equal rights and opportunity, there would be no
need for a clandestine sex trade (Goode, 1990). However, it should be noted
that even liberal countries such as Denmark report a thriving sex trade. And in
a study of Norwegian men, 13 percent admitted having paid for sex with a
prostitute (Høigård & Finstad, 1986/1992).
twenty years later (Hunt, 1974). This pattern was also documented by Lau-
mann et al. (1994) who found that 7 percent of 55- to 59-year-old men had
their first sexual encounter with a prostitute, compared to 1.5 percent of 18- to
24-year-olds. The preferred explanation for this trend is the decreased double
standard during the second half of the twentieth century, which made more
women open to premarital sexual experimentation. Consequently, this in-
crease in available sexual partners reduced men’s inclination to pay a stranger
for sexual activity (Edgley, 1989).
The Prostitute
The term ‘‘prostitute’’ is derived from the Latin word prostitutus, meaning
‘‘to set forth’’ or to be exposed for sale. This refers to the advertising of sexual
services by the prostitute, whether in manner of dress, verbal propositions, or
location. The corresponding legal term, ‘‘solicitation,’’ is a reference to the
offer of sexual activity for a fee.
The consistent pattern in contemporary society and throughout history is for
the customers of prostitutes to be men. The most common form of prostitution
involves women who sell sexual favors to heterosexual men. The second most
common group consists of homosexual male prostitutes who cater to gay men.
Male prostitutes who make themselves available to women are called ‘‘gigolos,’’
and they are reportedly very uncommon. Lesbian prostitutes are considered ex-
tremely rare. Despite what customers may believe, the prostitute does not engage
in the practice for personal sexual satisfaction, but rather as a financial enterprise.
Prostitutes do not generally derive pleasure from their repeated encounters with
customers and, in fact, generally resent them. For the practitioner, prostitution
entails the provision of a service for a fee. Female prostitutes earn over thir-
teen times the salary of nonprostitutes (Earls & David, 1989). By definition, the
transactions are void of emotional involvement. The briefer the encounter is,
the sooner the prostitute can return to work and generate more income. One
prostitute commented, ‘‘[W]hen I have intercourse I move around just a little.
Then the customers get more turned on, so it goes faster. Otherwise, it’s so gross;
besides, I get sore if it takes too long’’ (Høigård & Finstad, 1986/1992, p. 68).
Street prostitutes may have a dozen or more anonymous sexual encounters during
the course of an evening (Cordelier, 1976/1978; Heyl, 1979). In one study of
prostitutes, the average number of customers per day was four, with some
prostitutes reporting up to ten encounters in a day (Freund et al., 1989).
Female Prostitutes
There are different classes of female prostitutes. From society’s perspec-
tive, the most deviant form involves streetwalkers, the most common and visible
prostitutes. Streetwalkers are virtually indiscriminate in accepting customers,
have a relatively low fee for services, and generally have numerous patrons in
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 307
one night. Compared to the other types of prostitutes, streetwalkers are more
vulnerable to arrest and abuse by customers. Most streetwalkers work for a
pimp, usually a man who provides protection in return for a large percentage
of monies earned by the prostitute.
Over 60 percent of the streetwalkers studied by Freund et al. (1989)
engaged in fellatio and 23 percent had vaginal intercourse with customers.
Consistent with reports from female prostitutes, kissing is uncommon (Freund,
Lee, & Leonard, 1991). As one prostitute stated, ‘‘Like most girls, I personally
refuse to let a client kiss me on the mouth. . . . I make a distinction between my
vagina and my mouth. I think it’s only normal, we’ve got our dignity too’’
( Jaget, 1980, p. 167).
Streetwalkers advertise their services in several ways. They tend to wear
provocative and revealing clothing and will generally frequent areas known for
prostitution. Finally, upon gaining the attention of potential customers, they
often make subtle (‘‘Want to party?’’) or direct (‘‘I can show you a good time’’)
propositions. The cost of services is negotiated early in an encounter, with fee
varying depending on the type of sexual act requested; fellatio is often cheaper
than intercourse (Winick & Kinsie, 1971).
Prostitutes who work in brothels (whorehouses), and special massage par-
lors or clubs have higher status. Being in an establishment that employs
prostitutes has some advantages: it is safer, and business is often more regular.
However, there is also a risk of arrest because police squads periodically raid
such facilities. Outside of a few counties in Nevada, there are no legally
recognized brothels in the United States although some clandestine facilities
definitely exist. In countries where prostitution is legal and regulated, brothel
prostitution is prevalent. Typically, brothels are managed by a ‘‘madam’’ who
collects a percentage of all earnings in the establishments (Heyl, 1979). Massage
parlors are sometimes fronts for brothels. The masseuse will generally provide a
massage, with other services (often fellatio or masturbation) available to clients
for an additional charge. Such extra services are of course illegal in the United
States. This association between prostitution and massage parlors has led many
legitimate masseurs and masseuses to emphasize that they do not provide
sexual services. One way to stress the legitimacy of massage services is by
specifying that they consist of ‘‘therapeutic massages.’’
At the highest level of prostitution are the call girls, who often operate
through an escort service. Escort services advertise that they provide male or
female escorts for social occasions. Their advertisements stress that their ser-
vices are confidential and discreet. One large city in the southeastern United
States advertised no less than 106 such escort agencies. The nature of their
advertisements suggests that these agencies are thinly veiled covers for pros-
titution (e.g., ‘‘Fantasy Girls,’’ ‘‘Wild College Girls,’’ ‘‘Affairs of the Heart’’).
Call girls demand a higher price, are more selective, and typically have a small
regular clientele (Greenwald, 1970). They often operate independently and live a
luxurious life in comparison to other types of prostitutes (Winick & Kinsie, 1971).
308 Sexuality Today
Contrary to streetwalkers and brothel prostitutes, call girls do not usually have
multiple encounters in one evening. The case of Heidi Fleiss, ‘‘the Hollywood
Madam,’’ made the headlines in 1993 for operating an exclusive call-girl service
that catered to wealthy men in California (Fleiss was quoted as saying, ‘‘I took the
oldest profession on Earth, and I did it better than anyone on Earth’’). This case
was sensational to the media and public not because it involved prostitution but
due to the allegations that Fleiss’s customers included politicians and popular
actors (Birnbaum, 1993).
Equivalent roles to those of contemporary prostitutes could be found in
ancient Greece, where pornoi (a term meaning ‘‘the writing about [or by]
prostitutes’’) referred to the lowest class of prostitutes and hetairae (meaning
‘‘companion’’) represented the higher-class courtesans (Bullough & Bullough,
1978, 1995). The latter held high unofficial status, were educated and socially
sophisticated, and commanded a higher price for their services. Ancient Greek
culture epitomized gender inequality. Therefore, wives were responsible for
childrearing and domestic duties, while the hetairae served as social and sexual
companions. In both cases, woman was considered man’s property, either for
his sexual enjoyment or for domestic comfort.
Male Prostitutes
Although both are old practices and probably involve similar prevalence
rates, male prostitution has received less attention than female prostitution.
Prior to 1963, even less was known of male prostitution than is known today.
That year, John Rechy published City of Night, a novel about the travels of a
boy from Texas who becomes a prostitute and plies his trade throughout the
United States. The novel served as a window into the seamier side of male
prostitution and increased public awareness about the ‘‘profession.’’
Male prostitutes tend to practice their trade intermittently in compari-
son to female prostitutes (Winick & Kinsie, 1971). The vast majority of male
prostitutes offer their services to gay men. Interestingly, the majority of male
prostitutes do not describe themselves as being gay. In their study of 224
male street prostitutes, Boles and Elifson (1994) found that only 18 percent
described themselves as homosexual, while nearly 36 percent considered
themselves to be bisexual, and 46 percent were heterosexual in their self-
reported sexual orientation. The average age was 28 years and most had been
in the trade for close to ten years. The services offered by male prostitutes vary
as a function of their reported sexual orientation: heterosexual prostitutes were
unlikely to participate in anal intercourse whereas nearly 65 percent of ho-
mosexual male prostitutes engaged in receptive anal intercourse. Twenty-three
percent of bisexual male prostitutes participated in receptive anal intercourse
(Boles & Elifson). In a study of male prostitutes in London, West and de
Villiers (1993) reported that the ages ranged from 16 to 21 years. Fellatio and
masturbation of customers are the most commonly reported sexual activities
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 309
Child Prostitutes
One of the most disturbing aspects of the sex trade is the exploitation of
children. Worldwide, it is estimated that several million children and adoles-
cents are involved in prostitution (Willis & Levy, 2002). In the United States
alone, over 244,000 children are at risk of sexual exploitation, which includes
prostitution and child pornography (Estes & Weiner, 2001). Child prostitution
has been reported throughout the world, from Boise, Idaho, to Bangkok,
Thailand, and London, England. Both boys and girls are involved and their
ages range from 10 to 17. Many adult prostitutes actually started their careers in
adolescence when they ran away to escape physical, mental, or sexual abuse at
home. According to Williard (1991), low self-esteem and a lack of marketable
skills may lead some runaways into prostitution as a means of survival on the
streets. Most commonly, an adult, either a pimp or even a parent, is involved
in their initiation in the trade. Campagna and Poffenberger (1988) described a
pimp who recruited 12- to 14-year-old girls from a shelter for runaways while
310 Sexuality Today
another met desperate youths at bus stations. The initiation into prostitution
often included instilling a false sense of security and the lure of easy money.
The use of drugs is another common means of facilitating the exploitation of
children and adolescents.
Child prostitution has long-term adverse effects on the victims. Of par-
ticular concern is the global trafficking of women and children as sex trade
workers involving as many as 700,000 victims per year (U.S. Department of
State, 2002), 30 percent of whom are children (Kelly, 2002). With increased
recognition of the problem of sexual exploitation of children, efforts are being
made to prosecute the exploiters and prevent the tragic effects on the victims.
In August 1996, nearly 2,000 representatives from 122 countries assembled in
Stockholm, Sweden, for the first World Congress against Sexual Exploitation
of Children. This meeting called for international attention on the plight of
sexually exploited children, concluding that it is the responsibility of each
nation to protect children and to prosecute perpetrators of such crimes, which
are apparently increasingly prevalent.
Customers of Prostitutes
Far more interest has been shown in prostitutes than in their customers by
both researchers and the legal system. We know that prostitutes are more likely
to be arrested than their customers, and the criminal charges are more serious
(Boyle & Noonan, 1987). According to Margo St. James (1987), a former
female prostitute and an advocate for the rights of sex trade workers, few men
are arrested for prostitution. Those few men are usually male prostitutes rather
than customers. This trend in prosecuting prostitutes more severely than their
customers is not new. In the eighteenth century, convicted male customers
were fined but the female prostitute was publicly flogged. Her crime has
consistently been viewed as worse than his. Prostitution remains the only
sexual offense for which more women than men are convicted.
In the trade, customers are referred to as ‘‘johns’’ or ‘‘tricks.’’ Their de-
mographic characteristics cross all socioeconomic and racial strata. In one study
of the customers of an escort service, based on an address listing obtained
during a raid, the majority of clients were Caucasian, married, and affluent
(Adams, 1987). Monto (2005) reported that a significant proportion of cus-
tomers are married, although they are less likely to be married than noncus-
tomers, and they are more likely to be dissatisfied with their marriages.
Customers tend to visit prostitutes from their own ethnic and racial back-
ground (Monto, 2004). From their interviews with 101 customers of New
Jersey streetwalkers, Freund et al. (1991) reported that 42 percent were
married and most resided in the surrounding areas. Average age in the sample
was 40. Most men were regular customers (93 percent made monthly visits and
63 percent reported weekly encounters) and had been visiting the prostitutes
for more than one year. Furthermore, 55 percent of the customers were
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 311
‘‘regulars’’ who had sex with the same prostitute during their outings. Sex
usually occurred outdoors, such as in back alleys, or in the client’s car. The
preferred sexual activity was fellatio, and vaginal intercourse was a close second
(see also Monto, 2001).
Several motives for using prostitutes have been delineated, including va-
riety, loneliness, sexual deviance, curiosity, and deprivation (Edgley, 1989;
Monto, 2004, 2005; Pitts, Smith, Grierson, O’Brien, & Misson, 2004). Some
customers seek the anonymous and indiscriminate nature of sex with a pros-
titute. The encounters do not require emotional commitment or preliminary
courting, just sex. The prostitute holds no expectation of the client other than
financial remuneration. The customer may believe that he is unable to obtain
sexual favors without paying for them. Several situations would apply, in-
cluding men whose wives object to certain sexual practices (such as fellatio),
men who have serious physical deformities or social anxiety, and those who
have deviant or kinky sexual proclivities. For example, some customers solicit
from prostitutes unusual sexual activities that their regular partners find objec-
tionable, such as bondage, spanking, or the use of unusual costumes or sex toys.
The illicit and risky nature of an encounter with a prostitute is attractive to some
men. Finally, some customers employ the services of prostitutes when their
regular partners are unavailable, due to travel or illness.
Although there is an implicit agreement between the customer and the
prostitute that the exchange will be superficial and temporary, customers
usually want the illusion that the prostitute is interested in them personally and
sexually. In fact, customers may become frustrated or angry if the prostitute
seems detached, unresponsive, or hurried (Monto, 2004). As with exotic
dancing (Enck & Preston, 1988), the customers of prostitutes are paying for a
semblance of intimacy; some even describe the transactions as love relation-
ships and insist that they have a special place in the prostitute’s life. The
challenge for the prostitute is to negotiate a fee for a brief encounter while
maintaining the illusion of sexual desire and interest in the customer (Berstein,
2001).
There are a host of reasons for visiting a prostitute, but sexual initiation is
no longer a common motive. Whereas prostitutes functioned essentially as sex
educators by initiating many young men in the 1930s and 1940s, in con-
temporary society, few men seek their sexual initiation from sex trade workers.
States. Simon et al. (1992) reported that nearly 18 percent of the 211 male
prostitutes they studied tested positive for HIV. Boles and Elifson (1994) found
that 35 percent of the 224 male prostitutes in their study carried HIV and 28
percent tested positive for syphilis. Male prostitutes who identified themselves
as homosexual had higher rates of HIV infection (50 percent) than those who
described themselves as heterosexual (18.5 percent).
Prostitutes are a potentially high-risk group for contracting HIV because
they often engage in two high-risk behaviors for exposure to HIV: having sex
involving fluid exchange with multiple partners and injectable drug use. The
nature of prostitutes’ work, the sex trade, puts them in frequent contact with
bodily fluids. Prostitutes who practice unprotected receptive anal sex are es-
pecially vulnerable to HIV infection (Karim & Ramjee, 1998). Their lifestyle
commonly involves injectable drug use. Although there has been a trend for
increased condom use among prostitutes, it is by no means consistent and
universal. Many prostitutes who are not injectable drug users have a regular sex
partner who is. Therefore, the partner’s behavior puts the prostitute at risk for
HIV disease since male and female prostitutes rarely practice safer sex with
their regular partners (Albert, Warner, & Hatcher, 1998).
Recognizing these problems, several programs have been implemented to
reduce health risks among prostitutes and their clients. One such program,
EMPOWER (Education Means Protection of Women in Recreation), was
developed in Thailand to provide HIV testing, education, and health coun-
seling. Preliminary results reveal that this and other programs are effective in
reducing rates of STDs, including HIV, among prostitutes (Hanenberg &
Rojanapithayakorn, 1998).
Prostitution in Perspective
Sociologists and feminists have emphasized the gender inequality that is
evident in prostitution. Prostitution primarily benefits men, both customers
and pimps. Customers obtain sexual enjoyment from prostitutes who, in turn,
financially support their pimps. Those women who are most likely to enter the
trade are the economically disadvantaged, with limited education and skills,
and commonly having a background of abuse (L. Bell, 1987; Shaver, 2005).
They apparently enter prostitution because they believe they have few viable
resources in society other than their sexuality.
Prostitution is never really accepted in any modern society. Prostitutes
generally are held at the lowest ranks of the social ladder. Even in societies
where prostitution is legalized, it is ‘‘not an expression of society’s acceptance
of prostitution but instead epitomizes a policy of isolation and stigma toward
the prostitute’’ (Hobson, 1987, p. 233). In other words, prostitution policies
and regulations in tolerant societies represent subtle attempts to control or
segregate prostitutes from this perspective. In a fair, egalitarian society, many
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 315
[a] society that institutionalizes prostitution as a work option for the poor
makes a statement about its position on inequality. One can see this in
the policy toward prostitution in countries like Korea, and until recently,
the Philippines. The governments have sought to legitimize prostitution
as work, even elevated it to a patriotic endeavor, since sex commerce has
brought in foreign tourism and reduced the national debt. (p. 235)
From any perspective, the sex trade is flourishing and will continue to
do so despite efforts to regulate or eliminate it (Bullough & Bullough, 1995).
Clearly, commercial sex, in all its forms, represents one of the most contro-
versial aspects of human sexuality.
NOTE
1. Most drag queens are not prostitutes.
REFERENCES
Adams, R. (1987). The role of prostitution in AIDS and other STDs. Medical
Aspects of Human Sexuality, 21, 27–33.
Albert, A. E., Warner, D. L., & Hatcher, R. A. (1998). Facilitating condom use
with clients during commercial sex in Nevada’s legal brothels. American
Journal of Public Health, 88, 643–647.
Bell, H., Sloan, L., & Stickland, C. (1998). Exploiter or exploited: Topless dancers
reflect on their experiences. Affilia: Journal of Women and Social Work, 13,
352–369.
Bell, L. (Ed.). (1987). Good girls/bad girls: Sex trade workers and feminists face to face.
Toronto: Women’s Press.
Berstein, E. (2001). The meaning of purchase: Desire, demand and the commerce
of sex. Ethnography, 2, 389–420.
Birnbaum, J. (1993, August). Heidi does Hollywood. Time, pp. 56–57.
Bishop, R., & Robinson, L. S. (1998). Night market: Sexual cultures and the Thai
economic miracle. London: Routledge.
Boles, J., & Elifson, K. W. (1994). Sexual identity and HIV: The male prostitute.
Journal of Sex Research, 31, 39–46.
Boyle, C., & Noonan, S. (1987). Gender neutrality, prostitution, and pornogra-
phy. In L. Bell (Ed.), Good girls/bad girls: Sex trade workers and feminists face to
face (pp. 34–37). Toronto: Women’s Press.
Buckingham, R. W., Meister, E., & Webb, N. (2004). Condom use among the
female sex worker population in Thailand. International Journal of STD and
AIDS, 15, 210–211.
Bullough, B., & Bullough, V. L. (1995). Female prostitution: Current research
and changing interpretations. Annual Review of Sex Research, 7, 158–180.
Bullough, V. L., & Bullough, B. (1977). Sin, sickness, and sanity: A history of sexual
attitudes. New York: Garland.
Bullough, V. L., & Bullough, B. (1978). Prostitution: An illustrated social history.
New York: Crown.
Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Campagna, D. S., & Poffenberger, D. L. (1988). The sexual trafficking of children: An
investigation of the child sex trade. Dover, MA: Auburn House.
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 317
Janus, S. S., & Janus, C. L. (1993). The Janus Report on sexual behavior. New York:
Wiley.
Karim, S. S. A., & Ramjee, G. (1998). Anal sex and HIV transmission in women.
American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1265–1267.
Kelly, L. (2002). Journeys of jeopardy: A review of research on trafficking in women and
children in Europe. Geneva, Switzerland: IOM Migration Research Series.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kramer, L. A., & Berg, E. C. (2003). A survival analysis of timing of entry into
prostitution: The differential impact of race, educational level, and child-
hood/adolescence risk factors. Sociological Inquiry, 73, 511–528.
Lambert, B. (1988, September 20). AIDS among prostitutes not as prevalent as
believed, studies show. New York Times, p. D9.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Lewis, J. (1998). Lap dancing: Personal and legal implications for exotic dancers.
In J. Elias, V. L. Bullough, V. Elias, & G. Brewer (Eds.), Prostitution: On
whores, hustlers, and johns (pp. 377–391). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
Manderson, L. (1992). Public sex performances in Patpong and explorations of the
edges of imagination. Journal of Sex Research, 29, 451–475.
McAnulty, R. D., Satterwhite, R., & Gullick, E. (1995, March). Topless dancers:
Personality and background. Paper presented at the meeting of the South-
eastern Psychological Association, Savannah, GA.
McCaghy, C. H., & Hou, C. (1994). Family affiliation and prostitution in a
cultural context: Career onsets of Taiwanese Prostitutes. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 23, 251–266.
Monto, M. A. (2001). Prostitution and fellatio. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 140–145.
Monto, M. A. (2004). Female prostitution, customers, and violence. Violence
against Women, 10, 160–188.
Monto, M. A. (2005). A comparison of male customers of female street prostitutes
with national samples of men. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 49, 505–529.
Morais, R. C. (1999, June 14). Porn goes public. Forbes, 163, 214–221.
Nadon, S. M., Koverola, C., & Schludermann, E. H. (1998). Antecedents to
prostitution: Childhood victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13,
201–221.
Pitts, M. K., Smith, A. M. A., Grierson, J., O’Brien, M., & Misson, S. (2004).
Who pays for sex and why? An analysis of social and motivational factors
associated with male clients of sex workers. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33,
353–358.
Pleak, R. R., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1990). Sexual behavior and AIDS
knowledge of young male prostitutes in Manhattan. Journal of Sex Research,
27, 557–587.
The Sex Trade: Exotic Dancing and Prostitution 319
Sexual Risk-Taking:
Correlates and Prevention
HEALTH CONSEQUENCES OF
SEXUAL RISK-TAKING
Sexually transmitted diseases are one of the major health concerns in the
United States with an estimated annual medical cost of $15.5 billion (CDC,
2004c). Recent estimates by the CDC indicate that 19 million Americans
become infected every year, and nearly half of those infected (46.7 percent) are
adolescents and young adults (Weinstock, Berman, & Cates, 2004). These
estimates also revealed that women, ethnic minority groups, and men who have
sex with men (MSM) are particularly vulnerable to STD infections (CDC,
2004c).
Approximately twenty-five diseases are primarily transmitted through
sexual contact (CDC, 2001c). Not including infection with HIV, the most
common STDs in the United States are chlamydia, gonorrhea, syphilis, genital
herpes, and human papillomavirus (HPV). Of these diseases, chlamydia, gon-
orrhea, syphilis, and HIV/AIDS are closely monitored due to their significant
impact on the health of the American population. Chlamydia and gonorrhea are
particularly common among women, younger individuals (15–24 years old),
and African Americans (CDC, 2001c). These bacterial infections are frequently
asymptomatic and can only be detected through testing. If untreated, these
infections can result in pelvic inflammatory disease (PID), lead to ectopic
pregnancy (pregnancies occurring outside the uterus), undesirable pregnancy
outcomes (e.g., eye disease and pneumonia in infants), and permanent infertility
for both men and women (CDC, 2001b, 2004a). Moreover, chlamydia in-
fections place women at a greater risk for contracting HIV if exposed to the virus
(CDC, 2001a).
Syphilis is a curable sexually transmitted bacterial infection that can also be
transmitted to the fetus during pregnancy or childbirth. Infection rates are
higher among African Americans, Hispanics, women aged 20–24 years, and
Sexual Risk-Taking 323
men aged 35–39 years (CDC, 2004a). In the late stage of the disease, infected
individuals develop cardiovascular and neurological diseases (e.g., loss of motor
coordination, paralysis), blindness, and may eventually die (American Social
Health Association, 2005; CDC, 2001c). Furthermore, individuals with
syphilis sores are two to five times more likely to contract HIV compared to
those without this condition.
Approximately 40,000 individuals contract HIV every year in the United
States (CDC, 2004a), and about 1,039,000 to 1,185,000 were living with
HIV/AIDS by the end of 2003 (Glynn & Rhodes, 2005). The highest rates of
infection in 2003 occurred among MSM, followed by infections among ad-
olescents and adults through heterosexual contact. Racial minority groups
have been disproportionately impacted by HIV/AIDS, accounting for about
68 percent of all HIV/AIDS diagnoses in 2003 (CDC, 2004c). Worldwide, an
estimated 39.4 million people were living with HIV/AIDS, and 4.9 million
became infected by the end of 2004 (United Nations Program on HIV/AIDS/
World Health Organization, 2004).
HIV is transmitted primarily through sexual contact (exchange of semen,
blood, or vaginal fluids) and blood-to-blood contact (e.g., needle sharing).
Although the virus may not produce symptoms years after infection, it can be
transmitted to others during this time. Over time, HIV destroys immune cells
(CD4 and T cells) and, eventually, symptoms of infection appear. If untreated
or undiagnosed, the immune system will be gradually damaged and the in-
dividual’s ability to fight infections seriously compromised, leading to the
diagnosis of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) and eventual death.
Although there is no cure for AIDS, improvements in HIV/AIDS treatment
have resulted in a growing number of persons living longer, healthier, and
more productive lives (CDC, 2003). Unfortunately, approximately 25–40
percent of those infected continue to engage in sexual risk-taking, increasing
the likelihood of HIV transmission or reinfection (Kalichman et al., 2001).
Despite these potential health consequences associated with sexual risk-
taking, and the public health efforts to increase awareness of these risks, rates of
STD and HIV infection remain extremely high among the U.S. population. In
the next section, we provide a brief review of individual and contextual factors
that independently and jointly contribute to sexual risk-taking.
(McKirnan, Ostrow, & Hope, 1996; Zweig, Lindberg, & McGinley, 2001). At a
global level, heavy and frequent alcohol use is associated with a greater likelihood
of having multiple sex partners and unprotected intercourse (Cooper, 2002; Stall
& Purcell, 2000). For example, in a national study of adolescents and young
adults, Santelli, Brener, Lowry, Bhatt, and Zabin (1998) found that heavier
alcohol use was associated with having a greater number of sexual partners.
However, situation studies (studies that examine the co-occurrence of alcohol
use and sexual risk-taking on particular occasions) have not found a consistent
association between alcohol use and sexual risk-taking. In particular, alcohol use
is not predictive of the frequency of unprotected intercourse, suggesting that
although these behaviors co-occur, alcohol does not play a causal role.
Several explanations for the association between alcohol use and sexual risk-
taking have been offered. Some researchers have suggested that alcohol has the
ability to reduce sexual inhibitions through its impairing effects on individuals’
ability to think about potential negative consequences. Thus, under the influence
of alcohol, individuals mainly focus on their sexual arousal and have a limited
ability to focus on the more distant consequences associated with these behaviors
(Steele & Josephs, 1990). Others have suggested that this association is not the
result of the pharmacological effects of alcohol, but rather the result of individ-
uals’ beliefs about alcohol’s ability to increase their sexual arousal and reduce
inhibitions. Therefore, individuals are likely to drink in anticipation of sexual
encounters, or in response to specific situations, and then behave according to
those expectations (George, Stoner, Norris, Lopez, & Lehman, 2000; Lang,
1985). To date, the empirical evidence has provided some support for both of
these explanations. Specifically, the effect of alcohol on sexual risk-taking appears
to vary depending on the strength of coexisting inhibiting forces (e.g., perceived
costs and benefits) and disinhibiting forces (arousal). Simultaneously, individuals’
alcohol expectancies play a role in promoting alcohol use in sexual situations or in
anticipation of these situations, leading to sexual risk-taking (Cooper, 2002;
George et al., 2000). For example, a recent study by Vanable et al. (2004)
concluded that the social context, specifically, sex with a casual partner, predicted
both alcohol consumption and sexual risk-taking. The authors concluded that
their findings might be explained by both the impairing effects of alcohol in-
toxication and by individuals’ expectations of increased arousal and disinhibition
in situations that are viewed as desirable but would be avoided while sober.
As in the case of alcohol, the relationship between drug use and sexual
risk-taking is complex. At a global level, drug use before or during sex is
associated with having multiple sex partners, trading sex for drugs or money,
and weak peer norms for condom use. However, it is unclear whether this
association is causal or holds with all types of sex partners (Stall & Purcell,
2000) or situations. In fact, several factors might explain this association.
Among some groups, exchanging sex for drugs is common, and in these
situations, individuals are less likely to practice safe sex (Windle, 1997).
Some drugs can have a significant impact on sexual behavior. For example,
326 Sexuality Today
amphetamine and crack cocaine users frequently report that these drugs in-
crease their levels of sexual desire, sexual stamina, and reduce sexual inhibitions
(Ross & Williams, 2001). In some circumstances, these drugs are used ‘‘stra-
tegically’’ to achieve these effects, particularly in situations or settings that
promote sexual risk-taking behaviors (e.g., bars, dance clubs, parties) (Green,
2003). In sum, these findings suggest that the use of alcohol and drugs may not
play a causal role in sexual risk-taking; rather, it appears that other individual
(e.g., personality, beliefs), contextual (e.g., partner type, social setting), and
social factors (e.g., relationship type and quality) may explain their co-
occurrence (Ross & Williams, 2001; Weinhardt & Carey, 2000).
Personality
Sensation seeking (the tendency to seek novelty and excitement and the
willingness to take risks in order to have these experiences), and impulsivity
(the tendency to act without planning or deliberation) (Zuckerman, 1994)
have been found to be associated with several risky behaviors, including
smoking, alcohol and drug use, and sexual risk-taking (Zuckerman & Kuhl-
man, 2000). For example, a recent quantitative review by Hoyle, Fejfar, and
Miller (2000), of fifty-three studies of college and high-risk populations (e.g.,
MSM), concluded that these two personality characteristics predicted a variety
of sexual risk behaviors, including frequent unprotected sexual intercourse, sex
with strangers, multiple sexual partners, and having sex while intoxicated.
Sensation seeking appears to contribute to sexual risk-taking in the following
ways: (1) by interfering with individuals’ ability to engage in safe sex in the
‘‘heat of the moment’’ (Bancroft et al., 2003; Pinkerton & Abramson, 1995),
and (2) causing individuals who are high in sensation-seeking to report low
levels of perceived risk after engaging in these behaviors (Zuckerman, 1979).
The empirical evidence also suggests that personality characteristics in-
teract with other individual and situational factors to predict sexual risk-taking
(Hoyle et al., 2000). More recently, researchers have focused on identifying
factors that might clarify the nature of these associations. For example, Ka-
lichman, Cain, Zwebebm, and Geoff (2003) found that sensation seeking was
directly associated with higher expectations of increased sexual arousal and
disinhibition while under the influence of alcohol. In turn, these expectations
were associated with alcohol use in sexual situations and with unprotected
intercourse with nonprimary sex partners. Thus, it is possible that personality
plays a role in sexual risk-taking through its impact on individuals’ expecta-
tions, beliefs, attitudes, and norms.
Cognitive Factors
Several theoretical perspectives, such as the theories of reasoned action
(Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977) and planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), have proposed
Sexual Risk-Taking 327
Gender Roles
Gender roles also play an important role in predicting sexual risk be-
haviors, behavioral choices, and the ability to initiate and maintain behavioral
changes. Among women, the tendency to place a greater emphasis on main-
taining harmony and connectedness and providing support in their relation-
ships (Simon, 1995) contributes to their tendency to put their partners’ needs
above their desire to protect themselves from STD/HIV infection (Misovich,
Fisher, & Fisher, 1997). In fact, women frequently avoid making requests to
use condoms if they expect that such requests will lead to conflict or violence
(Wingood & DiClemente, 1998). Cultural beliefs about women’s sexual roles
and behavior also act as barriers for women’s ability to negotiate safe sex
practices with their partners (Gomez & Marin, 1996; St. Lawrence et al.,
1998). For example, traditional gender roles assign women less decision-
making power, interfering with their ability to make decisions that go against
their partners’ wishes (Amaro, 1995). These power inequalities also contribute
to women’s vulnerability to violence within their intimate relationships
(Amaro). Among college women, nearly one-third report having been pres-
sured or forced to engage in sexual activities (Muehlenhard, Goggins, Jones, &
Satterfield, 1991). Women with a history of sexual victimization are more
likely to experience further victimization, to report a history of STDs (El-
Bassel, Gilbert, Rajah, Foleno, & Fyre, 2000), and to be at a greater risk for
HIV infection (Garcia-Moreno & Watts, 2000) compared to women without
such histories. Thus, the amount of power women hold in relationships is an
important predictor of both the frequency of condom use and exposure to
sexual coercion (Pulerwitz, Gortmaker, & DeJong, 2000). These findings
suggest that women’s intentions to avoid risky behaviors, and their knowledge
and skills about how to prevent HIV/STD infection, might not be the strongest
predictors of sexual risk-taking. Rather, women’s behaviors are greatly deter-
mined by their partners’ attitudes and behaviors about safe sex practices (Logan,
Cole, & Leukefeld, 2002).
Power inequalities and sexual coercion do not occur only among women;
in fact, a study conducted by Kalichman and Rompa (1995) found that 29
percent of gay and bisexual males had experienced sexual coercion involving
attempted or completed unprotected anal intercourse. Men with a history of
victimization are also more likely to avoid talking with their partners about the
use of condoms for fear of the potential consequences (Kalichman et al., 2001).
These findings suggest that aspects of close interpersonal relationships are
important contributors to sexual risk-taking and STD/HIV infection.
330 Sexuality Today
Social Influences
Outside of the dyadic relationship, members of one’s social network play
an important role in predicting sexual behaviors. Among adolescents, aspects
of the parent-adolescent relationship are associated with the age of sexual
initiation and sexual risk-taking. Specifically, greater parental warmth and
acceptance (Markham et al., 2003), more frequent parent-adolescent discus-
sions about sex and sexual risk-taking (Hutchinson, Jemmott, Jemmott,
Braverman, & Fong, 2003; Miller, Forehand, & Kotchick, 2000), and parental
knowledge of teen’s activities (Huebner & Howell, 2003; Luster & Small,
1994) predict adolescents’ decision to delay the initiation of sexual activity,
greater use of condoms and contraceptives, and fewer sexual partners. Peers
also play an important role in predicting sexual behaviors and sexual risk-
taking (K. S. Miller et al., 2000). For example, adolescents’ perceptions of their
peers’ attitudes toward risky behaviors in general (e.g., alcohol and drug use)
and norms regarding sexual intercourse and condom use (Kinsman, Romer,
Furstenberg, & Schwarz, 1998) are associated with the initiation of sexual
activity and the frequency of condom (K. S. Miller et al., 2000) and birth
control use (Vesely et al., 2004), even after accounting for individual char-
acteristics (e.g., gender) and parental influences. Similarly, among adults, in-
dividuals’ perceptions of group norms for condom use are important predictors
of intentions to use condoms (Boyd & Wandersman, 1991) and the frequency
of unprotected sexual intercourse (Hart, Peterson, Community Intervention
Trial for Youth Study Team, 2004). For example, among women (Sikkema
et al., 2000) and gay men (Kegeles, Hays, & Coates, 1996), individuals’ beliefs
about the attitudes toward safer sex held by members of their community and
social groups are important predictors of the frequency of condom use.
Environmental Influences
Poverty has been shown to be associated with a variety of health indicators
such as health status, physical functioning, and mortality (Kawachi & Berkman,
2000). Several factors associated with poverty, such as violent behavior, sub-
stance use, and the exchange of sex for money, drugs, or goods may also
contribute to sexual risk-taking and higher rates of STD/HIV infections
(Miles-Doan, 1998). Individuals living in poverty frequently experience high
levels of stress, greater exposure to community violence (Catalano, 2004),
limited access to health services, and might be overwhelmed by the tasks
associated with meeting their basic needs for food, shelter, and safety. Thus,
under these conditions, individuals may place less emphasis on avoiding sexual
risk practices (Logan et al., 2002).
As suggested by the aforementioned review, various individual, social,
and contextual factors contribute to sexual risk-taking. Several prevention
intervention strategies have been developed with the goal of modifying these
Sexual Risk-Taking 331
PREVENTION INTERVENTIONS
Primary prevention interventions are aimed at modifying behavioral,
cognitive, social, and environmental factors that have been shown to be asso-
ciated with sexual risk-taking and STD/HIV infection. These interventions
have been largely guided by health behavior theories (e.g., health belief model,
social cognitive theory) that propose that individuals’ intentions, beliefs, and
expectations are important predictors of sexual risk-taking and behavior change
(Logan et al., 2002). During the past decade, researchers have tailored inter-
ventions based on these models to address specific developmental, cultural,
social, and situational factors that influence individuals’ ability to initiate and
maintain behavioral changes (Kelly & Kalichman, 2002).
Prevention intervention approaches can be directed to the individual and
to the community (Coates, 1990). Successful prevention interventions aimed
at the individual involve face-to-face counseling and group sessions. In gen-
eral, these programs are based on the social cognitive and reasoned action
theories and include the following components: risk-reduction education,
activities aimed at encouraging behavioral change and positive attitudes toward
safe sex practices, and exercises to increase safe sex communication and ne-
gotiation and assertiveness skills. Typically, these interventions involve several
group or individual sessions that are provided in community- or clinic-based
programs (Kelly & Kalichman, 2002). Small group meetings are thought to
provide an opportunity for individuals to interact with peers who support safe
sex strategies and who can help them increase their sense of self-efficacy
(DiClemente et al., 1999). Overall, interventions aimed at the individual have
been shown to reduce the frequency of unprotected intercourse and increase
condom use. However, individuals may not always be motivated to participate
in multiple session programs that require a considerable time commitment
on their part. Several brief interventions modeled after these multiple-session
programs have been developed; unfortunately, these interventions have been
shown to have minimal effects on sexual risk-taking (Kelly & Kalichman,
2002).
Community-level prevention interventions seek to reduce sexual risk-
taking by changing norms and practices within an entire target population.
These interventions frequently focus on leaders or popular individuals within a
particular community with the goal of promoting changes in beliefs and at-
titudes toward safe sex practices across existing social networks. These efforts
are often implemented in settings frequented by members of the target pop-
ulation, such as bars, barbershops, grocery stores, and restaurants (Kelly &
Kalichman, 2002; Ross & Williams, 2002).
332 Sexuality Today
Adolescents
Prevention intervention programs for adolescents are typically delivered in
schools, clinics, and community settings (Pedlow & Carey, 2003). School-
based interventions are implemented as part of the school curricula and are
provided to a broad range of students. The materials covered in these inter-
ventions range from abstinence-only messages, STD/HIV education, to the
discussion of safe sex practices (Kirby, 2001). The Safer Choices program is an
example of a school-based prevention intervention that has shown some
promising results. The intervention was implemented in ten schools and was
delivered by a teacher and peer leaders. High school students participated in
ten sessions that involved role-playing activities, groups, and exercises aimed at
building skills. These activities targeted both individual (i.e., attitudes) and
social (i.e., parent-adolescent communication) factors that have been shown to
influence sexual risk-taking. This program also included student homework
that required parental involvement. Seven months after the intervention,
students who participated in this program were less likely to engage in un-
protected intercourse, more likely to report condom use during their last
intercourse, and reported fewer barriers to condom use compared to those
who received only AIDS education (Coyle et al., 1999). Although some
school-based programs have shown some success, these programs cannot
reach out-of-school adolescents who are typically at high risk for STD/HIV
infection.
Community-based prevention programs for adolescents are better able to
reach high-risk populations. For example, Rotheram-Borus, Feldman, Ro-
sario, and Dunne (1994) conducted a prevention intervention program with
runaway homeless adolescents that involved multiple (more than fifteen) face-
to-face individual sessions led by skilled trainers. The intervention included
HIV/AIDS education, coping skills training, activities aimed at reducing
barriers for safe sex, and the provision of medical and health care services.
Adolescents who participated in the intervention reported increases in condom
use and fewer sex partners at the six months and twelve months follow-ups,
compared to those who received only counseling.
Several common key characteristics of successful prevention intervention
programs for youths have been identified. The critical components are (1) a
focus on reducing one or more sexual risk-taking behaviors, (2) use of health
behavior theories to develop the interventions, (3) consistent focus on one
clear message about abstaining from sexual activity and/or using condoms, (4)
provision of education about how to avoid risky situations and the use of
effective prevention methods, (5) provision of modeling and practice of
Sexual Risk-Taking 333
Women
Successful prevention interventions for women are frequently guided by
the principles of social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1994), and include skills
training in condom use, safe sex communication and negotiation, and gender-
related factors (i.e., power imbalances) that influence sexual risk-taking (Di-
Clemente et al., 1999). These prevention intervention programs have been
implemented in community and clinic settings and are frequently peer led.
Kelly et al. (1994) conducted a clinic-based intervention with inner-city
African American women. The five-session intervention focused on HIV/
AIDS education, addressed individual attitudes toward safe sex, and provided
activities aimed at identifying and handling barriers for condom use. Women
who participated in this intervention reported greater use of condoms and
fewer episodes of unprotected intercourse three months postintervention,
compared to those women in a control condition. In addition, women also
reported improvements in their ability to negotiate condom use and safe sex
practices with their partners.
A community-level intervention for women was implemented by Sikkema
et al. (2000). This intervention was delivered in nine low-income housing
communities in five different cities. First, the investigators offered workshops
on HIV risk reduction. These workshops were followed by ongoing con-
versations, social events, and community activities led by women who had
been identified as popular among their neighbors. Nine other low-income
communities served as a comparison group; these communities received AIDS
education materials and condoms as part of the intervention. One year after
the intervention, women in the communities that received the intervention
reported a greater decline in the percentage of unprotected intercourse epi-
sodes and increases in condom use, compared to women in the control
condition.
Overall, these interventions have had moderate success. In particular, it is
unclear to what extent they have resulted in changes in sexual risk-taking in
the context of primary committed relationships, the main route of STD/HIV
infection among women (Logan et al., 2002; O’Leary, 2000).
intervention programs have been developed to address the factors associated with
sexual risk-taking in this population. An example of a successful individual-level
intervention for MSM is the National Institute of Mental Health Multisite HIV
Prevention Trial Group (1998), a program that was implemented in STD and
health clinics in seven U.S. cities. This intervention involved small-group sessions
that used teaching, group discussion, role-play and practice exercises, and activ-
ities aimed at creating attitude changes and helping individuals set behavioral
goals. Those men who participated in these sessions were more likely to use
condoms and less likely to report an STD infection twelve months after the
intervention, compared to those who received only one AIDS education session.
Kelly et al. (1991, 1992, 1997) developed a series of community-level
interventions to reduce sexual risk behaviors among gay men at high risk for
HIV infection. These interventions were based on the diffusion of innovation
theory (Rogers, 1983), which proposes that new behavior patterns in a pop-
ulation can be initiated by targeting key opinion leaders. Key opinion leaders
were trained on how to influence others’ views about AIDS-related risks, how
to recommend sex-related risk reduction strategies, and how to endorse the
benefits and norms associated with safe sex. These interventions resulted in
significant declines in sexual risk-taking among residents of the targeted
communities.
In general, successful prevention intervention programs for MSM are
guided by theoretical perspectives that emphasize the role of cognitive and
attitudinal factors, intentions for behavior change, self-efficacy beliefs, and
social norms in predicting sexual risk-taking behaviors. In addition, researchers
have become increasingly aware of the importance of modifying these inter-
ventions to respond to changes in attitudes about safe sex. For example, in
recent years, increases in rates of sexual risk-taking among MSM have been
reported (CDC, 2000). These increases appear to be explained at least in part
by improvements in the efficacy of HIV/AIDS treatment (Halkitis et al., 2004)
and ‘‘safer sex burnout’’ among young MSM (Rofes, 1998).
REFERENCES
Adimora, A. A., Schoenbach, V. J., Bonas, D., Martinson, F. E. A., Donaldson,
K. H., & Stancil, T. R. (2002). Concurrent partnerships among women in
the United States. Epidemiology, 14, 155–160.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical
analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–
918.
Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & Muellerleile, P. A. (2001).
Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom
use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 142–161.
Albarracin, D., Kumkale, G. T., & Johnson, B. T. (2004). Influences of social
power and normative support on condom use decisions: A research syn-
thesis. AIDS Care, 16(6), 700–723.
Amaro, H. (1995). Considering women’s realities in HIV prevention. American
Psychologist, 50(6), 437–447.
American Social Health Association. (2005). Facts and answers about STDs. Re-
search Triangle Park, NC: Author. Retrieved July 31, 2005, from
www.ashastd.org/stdfaqs/syphilis.html
Annie E. Casey Foundation. (1999). When teens have sex: Issues and trends—A Kids
Count special report. Baltimore: Author. Retrieved July 9, 2005, from
www.aecf.org/kidscount/teen/foreword.htm
336 Sexuality Today
Gowen, L. K., Feldman, S. S., Dı́az, R., & Yisrael, D. S. (2004). A comparison of
the sexual behaviors and attitudes of adolescent girls with older vs. similar-
aged boyfriends. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33(2), 167–175.
Green, A. I. (2003). ‘‘Chem friendly’’: The institutional basis of ‘‘club drug’’ use in
a sample of urban gay men. Deviant Behavior, 24, 427–447.
Grunbaum, J. A., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Roos, J., Hawkins, J., Lowry, R., et al.
(2004). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2003. Morbidity
and Mortality Weekly Report, 53, SS-2.
Halkitis, P. N., Wilton, L., Parsons, J. T., & Hoff, C. (2004). Correlates of sexual
risk—taking behavior among HIV seropositive gay men in concordant
primary partner relationships. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 9(1), 99–113.
Halperin, D. T., Steiner, M. J., Cassell, M. M., Green, E. C., Hearst, N., Kirby,
D., et al. (2004). The time has come for common ground on preventing
sexual transmission of HIV. Lancet, 364, 1913–1915.
Hart, T., Peterson, J. L., & the Community Intervention Trial for Youth Study
Team. (2004). Predictors of risky sexual behavior among young African-
American men who have sex with men. American Journal of Public Health,
94(7), 1122–1123.
Hays, R. B., Kegeles, S. M., & Coates, T. J. (1997). Unprotected sex and HIV
risk-taking among young gay men within boyfriend relationships. AIDS
Education and Prevention, 9, 314–329.
Hoyle, R. H., Fejfar, M. C., & Miller, J. D. (2000). Personality and sexual risk
taking: A quantitative review. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 1203–1231.
Huebner, A. J., & Howell, L. W. (2003). Examining the relationship between
adolescent sexual risk-taking and perceptions of monitoring, communi-
cation, and parenting styles. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(2), 71–78.
Hutchinson, M. K., Jemmott, J. B., Jemmott, L. S., Braverman, P., & Fong, G. T.
(2003). The role of mother–daughter sexual risk communication in re-
ducing sexual risk behaviors among urban adolescent females: A pro-
spective study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 98–107.
Jessor, R. (1991). Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for
understanding and action. Journal of Adolescent Health, 12(8), 597–605.
Kalichman, S. C., Benotsch, E., Rompa, D., Gore-Felton, C., Austin, J., Web-
ster, L., et al. (2001). Unwanted sexual experiences and sexual risk in gay
and bisexual men: Associations among revictimization, substance use, and
psychiatric symptoms. Journal of Sex Research, 38(1), 1–9.
Kalichman, S. C., Cain, D., Zwebebm, A., & Geoff, S. (2003). Sensation seeking,
alcohol use and sexual risk behaviors among men receiving services at a
clinic for sexually transmitted infections. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 64(4), 564–569.
Kalichman, S. C., & Rompa, D. (1995). Sexually coerced and noncoerced gay and
bisexual men: Factors relevant to risk for human immunodeficiency virus
(HIV) infection. Journal of Sex Research, 32, 45–50.
Kaplowitz, P. B., Oberfield, S. E., & Drug and Therapeutics and Executive
Committees of the Lawson Wilkins Pediatric Endocrine Society. (1999).
Sexual Risk-Taking 339
LoConte, J., O’Leary, A., & Labouvie, E. (1997). Psychosocial correlates of HIV-
related sexual behavior in an inner-city STD clinic. Psychology and Health,
12, 589–601.
Logan, T. K., Cole, J., & Leukefeld, C. (2002). Women, sex, and HIV: Social and
contextual factors, meta-analysis of published interventions, and implica-
tions for practice and research. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 851–885.
Longshore, D., Stein, J. A., Kowalewski, M., & Anglin, M. D. (1998). Psycho-
social antecedents of unprotected sex by drug-using men and women.
AIDS and Behavior, 2(4), 293–306.
Luster, T., & Small, S. A. (1994). Factors associated with sexual risk-taking be-
haviors among adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 622–632.
Lynch, D., Krantz, S., Russell, J., Hornberger, L., & Van Ness, C. (2000). HIV
infection: A retrospective analysis of adolescent high-risk behaviors.
Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 14, 20–25.
Markham, C. M., Tortolero, S. R., Escobar-Chavez, S. L., Parcel, G. S., Harrist,
R., & Addy, R. C. (2003). Family connectedness and sexual risk-taking
among urban youth attending alternative high schools. Perspectives on
Sexual and Reproductive Health, 35(4), 174–179.
McKirnan, D. J., Ostrow, D. G., & Hope, B. (1996). Sex, drugs and escape: A
psychological model of HIV-risk sexual behaviors. AIDS Care, 8, 655–669.
Miles-Doan, R. (1998). Violence between spouses and intimates: Does neigh-
borhood matter? Social Forces, 77(2), 623–645.
Miller, B. C., Christopherson, C. R., & King, P. K. (1993). Sexual behavior in
adolescence. In T. P. Gullotta, G. R. Adams, & R. Montemayor (Eds.),
Adolescent sexuality (pp. 57–76). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Miller, K. S., Forehand, R., & Kotchick, B. A. (2000). Adolescent sexual behavior
in two ethnic minority groups: A multisystem perspective. Adolescence,
35(138), 313–333.
Misovich, S., Fisher, J., & Fisher, W. (1997). Close relationships and elevated HIV
risk behavior: Evidence and possible underlying psychological processes.
Review of General Psychology, 1, 72–107.
Molitor, F., Facer, M., & Ruiz, J. D. (1999). Safer sex communication and unsafe
sexual behavior among young men who have sex with men in California.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28(4), 335–343.
Muehlenhard, C. L., Goggins, M., Jones, J., & Satterfield, A. (1991). Date rape and
sexual coercion in close relationships. In K. McKinney & S. Sprecher (Eds.),
Sexuality in close relationships (pp. 155–175). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
National Institute of Mental Health Multisite HIV Prevention Trial Group.
(1998). The NIMH Multisite HIV prevention trial: Reducing HIV sexual
risk behavior. Science, 280, 1889–1894.
Odets, W. (1994). AIDS education and harm reduction for gay men: Psycho-
logical approaches for the 21st century. AIDS and Public Policy Journal, 9,
1–18.
O’Leary, A. (2000). Women at risk for HIV from a primary partner: Balancing
risk and intimacy. Annual Review of Sex Research, 11, 191–234.
Sexual Risk-Taking 341
Ostrow, D. G. (2000). The role of drugs in the sexual lives of men who have sex
with men: Continuing barriers to researching this question. AIDS and
Behavior, 4(2), 205–219.
Parson, J. T., Halkitis, P. N., Bimbi, D., & Borkowski, T. (2000). Perceptions of
the benefits and costs associated with condom use and unprotected sex
among late adolescent college students. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 377–391.
Pedlow, C. T., & Carey, M. P. (2003). HIV sexual risk-reduction interventions
for youth: A review and methodological critique of randomized controlled
trials. Behavior Modification, 27(2), 135–190.
Pinkerton, S. D., & Abramson, P. R. (1995). Decision making and personality
factors in sexual risk-taking for HIV/AIDS: A theoretical integration.
Personality and Individual Differences, 19(5), 713–723.
Polacsek, M., Calentano, D. D., O’Campo, P., & Santelli, J. (1999). Correlates of
condom use stage of change: Implications for intervention. AIDS Educa-
tion and Prevention, 11, 38–52.
Pulerwitz, J., Gortmaker, S., & DeJong, W. (2000). Measuring sexual relationship
power in HIV/STD research. Sex Roles, 42, 637–660.
Rofes, E. (1998). Dry bones breathe: Gay men creating post-AIDS identities and cul-
tures. Binghamton, NY: Harrington Press.
Rogers, E. M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations. New York: Free Press.
Ross, M. W., & Williams, M. L. (2001). Sexual behavior and illicit drug use.
Annual Review of Sex Research, 12, 290–310.
Ross, M. W., & Williams, M. L. (2002). Effective targeted and community HIV/
STD prevention programs. Journal of Sex Research, 39(1), 58–62.
Rotheram-Borus, M. J., Feldman, J., Rosario, M., & Dunne, E. (1994). Pre-
venting HIV among runaways: Victims and victimization. In R. J. Di-
Clemente & J. Peterson (Eds.), Preventing AIDS: Theories and methods of
behavioral interventions (pp. 175–188). New York: Plenum Press.
Santelli, J. S., Brener, N. D., Lowry, R., Bhatt, A., & Zabin, L. S. (1998).
Multiple sexual partners among U.S. adolescents and young adults. Family
Planning Perspectives, 30(6), 271–275.
Sikkema, K. J., Kelly, J. A., Winett, R. A., Solomon, L. J., Cargill, V. A.,
Roffman, R. A., et al. (2000). Outcomes of a randomized community-
level HIV prevention intervention for women living in 18 low-income
housing developments. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 57–63.
Simon, R. (1995). Gender, multiple roles, role meaning, and mental health.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 36, 182–194.
Sobo, E. (1995). Women and AIDS in the United States. In E. Sobo (Ed.),
Choosing unsafe sex: AIDS-risk denial among disadvantaged inner city women
(pp. 9–24). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Stall, R. D., & Purcell, D. W. (2000). Intertwining epidemics: A review of
research on substance use among men who have sex with men and its
connection to the AIDS epidemic. AIDS and Behavior, 4, 181–192.
Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous
effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921–933.
342 Sexuality Today
Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 51–58.
St. Lawrence, J., Eldridge, G., Reitman, D., Little, C., Shelby, M., & Brasfield, T.
(1998). Factors influencing condom use among African American women:
Implications for risk reduction interventions. American Journal of Commu-
nity Psychology, 26, 7–28.
Tubman, J. G., Windle, M., & Windle, R. C. (1996). Cumulative sexual intercourse
patterns among middle adolescents: Problem behavior precursors and con-
current health risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, 182–191.
United Nations Program on HIV/AIDS/World Health Organization. (2004).
AIDS epidemic update: 2004. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Retrieved July
8, 2005, from www.unaids.org/wad2004/report_pdf.html
Vanable, P. A., McKirnan, D. J., Buchbinder, S. P., Bartholow, B. N., Douglas,
J. M., et al. (2004). Alcohol use and high-risk sexual behavior among men
who have sex with men: The effects of consumption level and partner
type. Health Psychology, 23(5), 525–532.
Vesely, S. K., Wyatt, V. H., Oman, R. F., Aspy, C. B., Kegler, M. C., Rodine,
S., et al. (2004). The potential protective effects of youth assets from
adolescent sexual risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 34, 356–365.
Weinhardt, L. S., & Carey, M. P. (2000). Does alcohol lead to sexual risk be-
havior? Findings from event-level research. Annual Review of Sex Research,
11, 125–157.
Weinstock, H., Berman, S., & Cates, W. (2004). Sexually transmitted diseases
among American youth: Incidence and prevalence estimates, 2000. Per-
spectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 36(1), 6–10.
Windle, M. (1997). The trading of sex for money or drugs, sexually transmitted
diseases (STDs), and HIV-related risk behaviors among multisubstance
using alcohol inpatients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 49, 33–38.
Wingood, G., & DiClemente, R. (1998). Relationship characteristics associated
with noncondom use among young adult African-American women:
Implications for HIV prevention. Journal of Black Psychology, 19, 190–203.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking and risk taking. In C. E. Izard (Ed.),
Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 163–197). New York:
Plenum Press.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Impulsive unsocialized sensation seeking: The biological
foundations of basic dimension of personality. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs
(Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior
(pp. 219–258). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Com-
mon biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 999–1029.
Zweig, J. M., Lindberg, L. D., & McGinley, K. A. (2001). Adolescent health risk
profiles: The co-occurrence of health risks among females and males.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30(6), 707–728.
14
best a peripheral factor. The question of plasticity thus lies at the heart of one
of the most far-reaching and bitter debates in the field of sexuality theory. To
be sure, it is possible to state the issue in a less antagonistic manner: high
plasticity can be considered an adaptation by which nature makes creatures
better able to adjust and change in response to meaningful experience.
The most discussed application of this concept concerns the possibility of
gender differences. An article by Baumeister (2000) contended that a basic,
fundamental difference is that female sexuality has higher erotic plasticity than
male sexuality. In relative terms, this means that women’s sexual responses and
feelings are more affected by social, cultural, and situational factors, whereas
male sexuality is relatively more shaped by genetic, hormonal, and other bi-
ological factors. The bulk of this article will focus on the question of gender
differences in erotic plasticity.
Assuming that plasticity is not a strictly constant quantity, there is no single
answer to the great and hotly debated question of nature versus nurture in
sexuality. For some people, the sex drive would be a relatively fixed biological
fact, whereas for others it would be subject to considerable influence from the
social environment.
Value judgments also introduce a dimension of sensitive problems into
debates about sexual behavior. We concur with the view that erotic plasticity
is not an inherently evaluative dimension, in the sense that it is not clearly or a
priori better to have high versus low plasticity. There might be some ways in
which high or low plasticity is better, but these largely cancel each other out,
and for the most part it is not clearly better to be one or the other. However,
the difference can be hugely influential on behavior, and failure to appreciate
its importance can introduce deeply divisive or even offensive misunder-
standings.
Ultimately, it may emerge that some individuals have higher erotic plas-
ticity than others. At present, there is no published scale available to assess these
differences, but some researchers have begun discussions about creating one,
and it is possible that after this work is published, a trait scale may become
available.
Whisman, 1996), and they are also more likely to have heterosexual
relationships even after having been exclusively gay for years (Rust, 1992).
Circumstances that promote sexual experimentation, such as swinging (i.e.,
mate-swapping) parties, seem to induce a fair number of heterosexual women
but hardly any heterosexual men to experiment with same-gender sex (Fang,
1976; O’Neill & O’Neill, 1970; Smith & Smith, 1970). Likewise, some evi-
dence suggests that there is more consensual same-gender activity in women’s
than in men’s prisons, and women seem to make the transition much more
smoothly and easily from an exclusively heterosexual orientation prior to their
imprisonment, to homosexual while in prison, and then back to heterosexual
upon release from prison than do men (Gagnon & Simon, 1968; Giallom-
bardo, 1966; Ward & Kassebaum, 1965). All of this supports the view of
greater plasticity in sexual orientation among women.
appears to have a stronger effect on the daughter’s subsequent sex life (e.g.,
toward starting earlier and having more partners) than the son’s, and it is fair to
assume that almost no divorces are motivated by the goal of making the
daughter more promiscuous.
The question whether sexual orientation is chosen or not has been de-
bated at some length and with some political and religious bias (e.g., is it fair to
reproach people as sinful for feeling ways they cannot help). One creative and
novel approach has been to ask people whether they feel they had some choice
as to whether be homosexual or heterosexual. Having choice is one sign of
plasticity, insofar as one must be capable of more than one possible orientation
in order to be able to choose from them. Only a minority of people claim to
feel that their sexual orientation was a matter of choice, but this minority is
almost entirely female (Rosenbluth, 1997; Whisman, 1996). Indeed, gay males
are more likely than gay females to express the wish that they could change to
a heterosexual orientation, but apparently most men feel that this is impossible.
Another approach to assessing social and cultural influences is to consider
the environmental factors and compare them against genetic influences.
Stronger effects of genes indicate low plasticity. Although the information base
for this sort of comparison is limited, there are several findings suggesting that
genetic influences on sexuality are stronger among males than among females.
One finding is that male identical twins are more likely than other pairs to
have begun having sex at the same age (though the finding is limited to more
modern times, after the sexual revolution, probably because of limited op-
portunities available to males before this) (Dunne et al., 1997). Female iden-
tical twins are less likely to start having sex at the same age, which implies that
the onset of sexual behavior has a stronger genetic component among males
than among females (and therefore, conversely, the onset of sexual behavior is
more shaped by social and situational factors among females than among
males).
The issue of genetic influences on homosexuality has attracted consider-
able research attention. Most studies find stronger evidence of some genetic
input among males than among females. In particular, the preliminary finding
of a possible ‘‘gay gene’’ was based solely on a male sample, and no such claim
has been made regarding females. A review by Bailey and Pillard (1995)
concluded that either male homosexuality is more genetically determined than
female homosexuality, or the state of evidence remains inadequate to draw a
conclusion. At the time, they favored the latter (more cautious) position, but
we suspect that in time the former conclusion will be confirmed. Recent work
by Lippa (2003) further supports a greater biological contribution to homo-
sexuality in men than in women. In a study of more than 2,000 participants, he
found that the ratio of index to ring-finger lengths differed in males and
females. Men typically have a lower index to ring-finger ratio than do females.
Homosexual and heterosexual men also showed different ratios. Homosexual
men had higher, more typically feminine ratios. However, finger-length ratios
350 Sexuality Today
Attitude-Behavior Consistency
A third way that erotic plasticity manifests itself is in low correlations
between general attitudes and specific behaviors. If plasticity is low, then the
person’s general attitudes are likely to predict what he will feel and want (and
presumably do) in most situations. In contrast, if plasticity is high, then the
Erotic Plasticity 351
person will find that her behavior depends on specific aspects of the situation,
and her general attitudes will not apply all the time. High plasticity means that
social and situational factors are influential, in which case, behavior is less
consistent.
A variety of evidence confirms that attitude-behavior consistency is lower,
at least in the sexual realm, among women than among men. Many researchers
have confirmed that girls and women are more likely than boys and men to
engage in sexual behaviors of which they do not approve, and, indeed, they
may continue doing them despite their own ongoing disapproval (Anto-
novsky, Shoham, Kavenocki, Modan, & Lancet, 1978; Christensen & Car-
penter, 1962). These inconsistencies ranged from adolescent girls who were
having intercourse despite advocating abstinence, to adult women who dis-
approved of casual sex but engaged in it anyway (Croake & James, 1973;
Herold & Mewhinney, 1993). A variation on this inconsistency is having sex
when one does not desire to have it. Although both men and women in
committed relationships periodically report engaging in sex when they did not
feel desire (usually because they wanted to please a partner), women report this
more frequently than men (Beck, Bozman, & Qualtrough, 1991).
Most people advocate using condoms, especially when having sex for the
first time or with a partner one does not know well. But many people act
contrary to this, by having sex without condoms or other protection under
those circumstances. Still, some work suggests that the gap is larger for women
than for men (Herold & Mewhinney, 1993). This is ironic because most
people believe that condoms detract from male sexual pleasure more than from
that of female, so one might have predicted the opposite result. Plasticity can,
however, explain the greater gap for women.
Many people disapprove of extramarital sexual activity or extradyadic
sexual activity (e.g., sex outside of a committed relationship) but engage in it
anyway. Such inconsistency appears to be higher among women. Hansen
(1987) showed that attitudes toward extramarital sex predicted actual behavior
fairly closely for men but not for women. Thus, many women may regard
extradyadic sex as desirable and exciting yet never engage in it, while others
may disapprove of it but do it anyway.
Similar findings emerged regarding same-sex activity. The NHSLS (Lau-
mann et al., 1994) asked respondents whether they liked the idea of having sex
with a member of their own gender and whether they had done so during the
past year. For males, these questions were very highly correlated, but for women
there was much less connection. Thus, again, many women liked the idea but
never did it, whereas others disliked the idea but had done it anyway. Specific
and situational factors presumably overrode the general attitudes, consistent
with high plasticity.
Attitudes about sexuality are conducive to making specific predictions
about behavior. One would expect behavior to correspond to the atti-
tude. However, there are other dispositional variables that do not lend
352 Sexuality Today
Any Exceptions?
A determined search for counterexamples yielded only a handful of sug-
gestive findings. For instance, not all evidence points in the direction of greater
354 Sexuality Today
biological contributions in sexual orientation for men than for women. A study
investigating the relationship between fingerprint patterns and handedness
found a significant relationship between handedness and sexual orientation in
women, but not in men (Mustanski, Bailey, & Kaspar, 2002). Fingerprint
patterns were unrelated to orientation in both genders. The aforementioned
difference in finger-length ratios is a natural point of comparison to this finding.
Finger-length ratios are more compelling evidence of biology than is handed-
ness because handedness is subject to social factors, and finger lengths are not. A
study investigating shifting handedness trends in Japan found some evidence that
men’s hand preferences are more malleable than women’s (Iwasaki, Kaiho, &
Iseki, 1995). This might account for the lack of relationship between handed-
ness and sexual orientation in men.
The most instructive counterexamples of erotic plasticity point toward
childhood. For example, males are more likely than females to acquire sexual
paraphilias (sexual arousal to atypical stimuli, e.g., nonhuman objects or un-
consenting humans), and although the origins of these are poorly understood,
most evidence points to some kind of childhood experience that creates the
unusual sexual desire. In adulthood, paraphilias have low plasticity and are
quite difficult to change or erase.
There is also some evidence that childhood sexual abuse has more severe
and long-lasting effects on boys than on girls. A follow-up to the NHSLS
found that people who had suffered sexual abuse as children were more likely
to have sexual health problems as adults if they were men rather than women
(Laumann, Paik, & Rosen, 1999).
Such findings suggest that there may be a phase of plasticity in male sexual
development, but it is apparently in childhood. Once the boy reaches puberty,
the pattern of sexual tastes and preferences is largely set (though the person
may not discover all these until some time later, especially if he regards his
desires as socially unacceptable). In contrast, female sexuality may continue to
develop and change throughout adulthood. This may help women recover
from events of childhood, and, as such, would be one clear benefit of plasticity
for some people.
Some research with animals confirms the conclusion of a brief phase of
plasticity during male childhood, although cross-species generalizations about
sex must be made very cautiously. An experimental study by Kendrick, Haupt,
Hinton, Broad, and Skinner (2001) swapped baby sheep and goats at birth, so
that each was raised by the other species. The adults were allowed access to
both species, and their mating preferences were observed. Consistent with
high plasticity, the females copulated with the other species. The males ex-
hibited low plasticity but in a most curious manner: they would only mate
with their adoptive species, and not their own true species. This indicates that
the male sexual preferences were shaped during childhood and remained fixed
during adulthood, even though those preferences were such that they would
prevent offspring.
Erotic Plasticity 355
CONCLUSION
Erotic plasticity makes the sex drive malleable and enables cultural and
situational factors to shape and alter it, not least by use of meanings. Plainly,
many animals in nature have satisfactory, efficacious sex without any influence
of culturally constructed or individually interpreted meanings. Yet, just as
356 Sexuality Today
plainly, many human sexual responses depend heavily and sometimes crucially
on meaning. The great variety of human sexual response is partly attributable
to the plasticity that is prepared by nature and activated by cultural meanings.
A substantial body of evidence indicates that female sexuality has higher
plasticity, and is therefore more open to social and cultural influences, than
male sexuality. The reason for the gender difference in plasticity is not es-
tablished with anywhere near the certainty that the fact of the difference is, but
at present the best guess is that it is linked to the mildness versus intensity of the
desire. High erotic plasticity is not necessarily better or worse than low, but it
has wide-ranging implications, including ease of self-knowledge, ease of ad-
aptation to new demands and circumstances, capacity for change across the
lifespan, and optimal type of therapeutic intervention. Future work is needed
to extend and verify the implications of gender differences in plasticity as well
as to establish its basic causes. Future work is also desirable to map out di-
mensions other than gender that can promote differential plasticity.
REFERENCES
Adams, C. G., & Turner, B. F. (1985). Reported change in sexuality from young
adulthood to old age. Journal of Sex Research, 21, 126–141.
Antonovsky, H. F., Shoham, I., Kavenocki, S., Modan, B., & Lancet, M. (1978).
Sexual attitude-behavior discrepancy among Israeli adolescent girls. Journal
of Sex Research, 14, 260–272.
Ard, B. N. (1977). Sex in lasting marriages: A longitudinal study. Journal of Sex
Research, 13, 274–285.
Bailey, J. M., & Pillard, R. C. (1995). Genetics of human sexual orientation.
Annual Review of Sex Research, 6, 126–150.
Barry, H., & Schlegel, A. (1984). Measurements of adolescent sexual behavior in
the standard sample of societies. Ethnology, 23, 315–329.
Bart, P. B. (1993). Protean women: The liquidity of female sexuality and the
tenaciousness of lesbian identity. In S. Wilkinson & C. Kitzinger (Eds.),
Heterosexuality: Feminism and psychology reader (pp. 246–252). London:
Sage.
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex
drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 347–374.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a gender
difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual dis-
tinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 5, 242–273.
Beck, J. G., Bozman, A. W., & Qualtrough, T. (1991). The experience of sexual
desire: Psychological correlates in a college sample. Journal of Sex Research,
28, 443–456.
Bell, A. P., & Weinberg, M. S. (1978). Homosexualities: A study of diversity among
men and women. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Erotic Plasticity 357
Iwasaki, S., Kaiho, T., & Iseki, K. (1995). Handedness trends across age groups in
a Japanese sample of 2316. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 80, 979–994.
Kendrick, K. M., Haupt, M. A., Hinton, M. R., Broad, K. D., & Skinner J. D.
(2001). Sex differences in the influence of mothers on the socio-sexual
preferences of their offspring. Hormones and Behaviour, 40, 322–338.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the
human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual
behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kitzinger, C., & Wilkinson, S. (1995). Transitions from heterosexuality to les-
bianism: The discursive production of lesbian identities. Developmental
Psychology, 31, 95–104.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the
United States: Prevalence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281, 537–544.
Lippa, R. A. (2003). Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related per-
sonality traits in men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 103–114.
McCauley, E. A., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1980). Sexual behavior in female transsexuals
and lesbians. Journal of Sex Research, 16, 202–211.
Miller, B. C., & Moore, K. A. (1990). Adolescent sexual behavior, pregnancy, and
parenting: Research through the 1980s. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
52, 1025–1044.
Mirande, A. M. (1968). Reference group theory and adolescent sexual behavior.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 30, 572–577.
Mustanski, B. S., Bailey, J. M., & Kaspar, S. (2002). Dermato-glyphics, hand-
edness, sex and sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 113–122.
Newcomer, S., & Udry, J. R. (1987). Parental marital status effects on adolescent
sexual behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 235–240.
O’Neill, G. C., & O’Neill, N. (1970). Patterns in group sexual activity. Journal of
Sex Research, 6, 101–112.
Reiss, I. L. (1967). The social context of premarital sexual permissiveness. New York:
Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Rosario, M., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Hunter, J., Exner, T. M., Gwadz, M., &
Keller, A. M. (1996). The psychosexual development of urban lesbian,
gay, and bisexual youths. Journal of Sex Research, 33, 113–126.
Rosenbluth, S. (1997). Is sexual orientation a matter of choice? Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 21, 595–610.
Rust, P. C. (1992). The politics of sexual identity: Sexual attraction and behavior
among lesbian and bisexual women. Social Problems, 39, 366–386.
Sack, A. R., Keller, J. F., & Hinkle, D. E. (1984). Premarital sexual intercourse: A
test of the effects of peer group, religiosity, and sexual guilt. Journal of Sex
Research, 20, 168–185.
Erotic Plasticity 359
role, 24, 193; homosexuality theories impact of, 191; in homosexuality, 164,
of, 160–161; ‘‘latency’’ period theory 349; of sexual orientation, 164–165
of, 48; Oedipal/Electra complex of, genital stimulation: by infants and
24, 169, 193; projection theory of, toddlers, 45
24; psychosexual theory of, 37–38; GID. See gender identity disorder
on sexual perversions, 24–25 gigolos, 308
GSS. See General Social Survey
Gagnon, John, 255
Gay Liberation movement, 242 Herder, Johann Gottfried von, 235
gay marriage: in Canada, 173; hip hop culture: African American
Massachusetts Supreme Court on, sexual stereotypes and, 249–250
171–173; in Netherlands, 173; state Hispanics/Latinos: acculturation for,
constitutional amendments banning, 244–245; familism among, 247;
173; in U.S., 172–173 gender expression among, 246;
gays. See also lesbians: in military, homosexuality among, 246–248;
174–175; among older adults, 145; hypermasculinity in, 156; lesbian
in urban areas, 110 stigmatization among, 247; NHSLS
gender bias, 186; women’s impact of, data on, 245–248; preadolescent
190 intercourse rates among, 52; racial
gender constancy, 194–195 group profile of, 242–248; sexual
gender differences: in short-term sex, behavior/beliefs of, 245; sexual
28 dysfunction among, 248; sexual
gender identity disorder (GID), 188; practices v. whites of, 246
environmental factors in, 188 HIV/AIDS, 197. See also HIV-related
gender role, 185–186; atypical, 191; behavior change; among African
behavioral genetics and, 191; in Americans, 250–251; deaths from,
children, 35; cognitive-development 103; in developing countries, 81–82;
theory and, 194–195; evolutionary gay community impact of, 115;
theory of, 192; Freudian theory of, intervention programs for, 334;
24, 193; genetic basis of, 191; among latency period of, 114; prostitution
Latinos, 246; among older adults, and, 313–314; rates of, 323; sex
146; personal relationships impact of, education and, 42; sexual behavior
189; in sexual behavior, 21; sexual impact of, 113–118
expression impact of, 155–156; HIV-related behavior change, 115
sexual interactions impact of, 195; homosexuality. See also gays; lesbians;
sexual risk-taking and, 329; social same-sex behavior: American society
influences on, 194, 204–205; social stereotypes of, 242; atypical prenatal
learning theory and, 193–194; androgen and, 165–166, 168; birth-
stereotypes in, 186, 189–190; order effect on, 166; childhood
theories of, 191–195; women’s femininity and, 163; church
flexibility in, 189 attendance and, 111; cognitive
gender stereotyping, 186, 189–190 differences and, 167–168;
General Social Survey (GSS), 106 dysfunctional families and, 161;
genetics. See also behavioral genetics: evolutionary theory and, 29–30;
erotic plasticity and, 349; gender role Freud’s theories of, 160–161;
366 Index
impact on, 154; as survival strategy, sample representation in, 4, 11, 135;
159 scales in, 8; self-report measures in,
school-based sexuality education 6; settings for, 1; on sex ratio, 26;
(SBSE), 217–219 sexual values/morals impact of, 13
scientific racism, 238 sex researchers, 2–3, 12; assumptions
self-reporting: accuracy of, 6; of sexual about, 3; sexist bias of, 17
activity, 7 sex trade: business of, 300, 316; social
Seventeen, 216–217 desirability response bias in, 7–8;
sex education: abstinence-only statistical significance in, 11–12;
programs in, 85–87, 217, 219; stigmatization within, 3; in Thailand,
American v. Dutch parents and, 220; 300; variable correlation in, 10–11;
biological approach to, 85; in vested interests in, 12–13
childhood sexuality development, sexual abuse in childhood, 54–55
41–42; community-wide sexual activity: adult frequency of, 112;
interventions in, 87–88; cohabitation and, 105–106;
comprehensive programs of, 86; premarital rates of, 108; premarital/
content evolution in, 85; in adolescent, 108; self-report measures
developing countries, 89–90; in of, 7
European countries, 90; sexual attraction: Kinsey scale of, 157
noncurriculum based interventions sexual behavior. See also sexual risk-
in, 87; in North America, 84; taking: of adolescents, 73; of adults,
parental role in, 219–222; 108–113; age impact on, 119; age-
pornography and, 278–279; on inappropriate, 55; in childhood,
pregnancy and STD transmission, 35–36; by community type, 119;
85; school-based, 217–219; service criminal problems and, 103;
learning programs of, 88; U.K. v. ecological impact on, 52; health
Netherlands, 91; U.S. v. Europe, 42 problems and, 103; HIV/AIDS
sex research: anonymous surveys in, 1, impact on, 113–118; infants and
7–8; assumptions in, 10; attitudes/ toddlers v. preschoolers, 47;
behavior in, 10; cause and effect in, language use and, 69; male v. female
9; college students v. public in, 4; standards of, 43–44; marriage and,
consumers of, 5; on couples, 14; 103; of masturbation, 70–71;
diary method in, 7; direct midlevel theories about, 31; parental
observation in, 5; experimental/ reports of, 49–50; of pre-adolescents,
control groups in, 9; face-to-face 50–51; precoital, 71–72;
interviews in, 8; by faculty members, reproductive problems and, 103;
2; on general population, 4; grants social importance of, 103;
for, 3; measures used in, 8–9; media sociodemographic differences and,
reports of, 12, 14; midlevel 104; trends in, 104; value judgments
theorizing in, 27, 31; participants in, about, 344; of white Americans,
3–5; penile arousal measurement in, 72; of whites v. African Americans,
4; physiological measures in, 4; 119
politics and, 13–14; positive effects Sexual Behavior in the Human Male
of, 14; process of, 1–2; procreation (Kinsey), 253
focus of, 213; researchers in, 2–3, 12; Sexual Conduct (Gagnon, Simon), 255
370 Index
include self and identity, self-control, the need to belong and interpersonal
rejection, sexuality, aggression, and violence. His books include The Cultural
Animal: Human Nature, Meaning, and Social Life; The Social Dimension of Sex; Evil:
Inside Human Violence and Cruelty; and Meanings of Life.
Volume 2
SEXUAL FUNCTION
AND DYSFUNCTION
1
PRAEGER PERSPECTIVES
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Contents
Preface vii
Introduction ix
1. Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 1
M. Michele Burnette
2. Sex and the Brain 19
George J. Demakis
3. The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response:
Physical and Psychological Factors That Control Our
Sexual Response 37
David L. Rowland
4. An Evolutionary Perspective on Sexual and Intimate
Relationships 67
David C. Geary
5. Love 87
Pamela C. Regan
6. Sexual Desire Issues and Problems 115
Anthony F. Bogaert and Catherine Fawcett
vi Contents
We have had many opportunities to teach and interact with both college
students and professional audiences about some very important topics and
issues in human sexuality in our roles as authors and college professors. When
we were approached to write this three-volume set on sex and sexuality, we
were intrigued with the idea of having a forum in which to reach a broader
audience. That is our goal for this work. With that in mind, we encouraged
our contributors to ‘‘talk to’’ a general audience when writing about the topics
that were most important to them. The authors we selected to write these
chapters represent both established authorities and budding scholars on the
various topics in human sexuality. We are confident that they have all helped
us accomplish our goal.
To us, few, if any, other topics in the realm of human behavior are more
interesting, exciting, or controversial than sex. And we hope that you will
agree after reading the chapters from this set. Each chapter stands alone, and
you can choose to read as many or as few as you would like—pick the ones
that interest you. We hope that you will find this work to be of significant
value to you, whether you are in pursuit of a better general understanding of
sexuality or are looking for answers to specific questions.
One theme you will find throughout these texts is that human sexual
function is affected by a whole host of factors. These factors are biological,
sociocultural, and psychological in nature. The scientific study of sexuality is
for all practical purposes a ‘‘young’’ field, and we have only touched the
viii Preface
surface in an attempt to fully understand how these factors interact and impact
sexuality.
Another theme or concern you will find throughout this work is the
question whether ‘‘scientific’’ views of sex are biased by social judgments about
normal versus abnormal and/or functional versus dysfunctional sexual be-
havior. U.S. culture, in particular, holds many strong values and prohibitions
about sex. In this context, studying and interpreting research on sexuality in an
unbiased manner can be a challenge. Many of our authors caution the reader
about this concern.
We wish to thank all the researchers and clinicians, past and present, who
have contributed to the science of sex. Many of them have contributed
chapters to this set, and for that we are grateful. We also thank our colleagues,
families, and friends who supported us during the writing and editing process.
Finally, we thank ‘‘the team’’ at Praeger Publishers.
Introduction
Sexual arousal and response is a natural and essential condition of life. Without
it, animals would not reproduce and would cease to exist. It seems that a
function so essential to our survival would be straightforward so as to ensure
that this process would not fail. But observe human sexual behavior, and you
will see that it is far from uncomplicated. Human sexual arousal and response is
multiply influenced by the integration of emotional, cognitive, interpersonal,
physiological, biological, sociocultural, environmental, and perhaps even
evolutionary factors. So complex and dynamic are these interrelationships that
we will likely always lack a full and complete understanding of them.
Volume 2 of Sex and Sexuality opens with an overview of our remarkable
reproductive anatomy. Chapter 1, by Burnette, covers the structure and
function of all the major reproductive organs of the male and female. This
chapter, combined with Chapter 2 by Demakis on the role of the brain and the
endocrine system, enhances our understanding of how multiple body systems
interact to produce sexual arousal and response. This complex picture becomes
even clearer with the addition of Rowland’s chapter, Chapter 3, on the
psychobiology of sex, in which he discusses models of sexual arousal and
response as well as physiological mechanisms (e.g., the senses) and psycholo-
gical processes (e.g., thoughts, feelings) that play a part in human sexual be-
havior.
Next, this volume addresses perspectives on sex and intimate relationships.
In Chapter 4, Geary offers an interesting discussion of how male and female
x Introduction
differences in sexual preferences and behavior may have evolved through time
to improve the chances of species survival. The research on evolution and
sexual behavior has largely involved nonhuman species and has been extra-
polated to humans. A discussion of sex and interpersonal relationships would
be incomplete without a discourse on love. Regan points out in Chapter 5 that
a majority of adolescents and adults in the United States believe that sexual
interactions should generally occur within the context of a love relationship.
Regan discusses types and theories of love with an emphasis on the two types
most linked to sexual expression, passionate and companionate love.
Most research aimed at understanding sexual response and function has
focused on understanding what has caused inhibition or disruption in the
process of sexual arousal and response. The next several chapters address this
topic. As we learn from the models of sexual arousal and response, this process
is arbitrarily divided into phases, including sexual desire, excitement, orgasm,
and resolution. Dysfunction can occur in any of the first three phases. In
Chapter 6, Bogaert and Fawcett talk about factors that increase, maintain, and/
or decrease a person’s desire to engage in sex. In Chapter 7, Febbraro addresses
sexual problems that occur during the excitement phase of the sexual response
related to difficulties either with feelings of sexual pleasure or with the phy-
siological changes associated with sexual excitement (e.g., failure of a female to
adequately lubricate, or premature ejaculation in a male). In Chapter 8,
Millner discusses orgasmic problems and disorders, in which she raises and
addresses the issue of what constitutes a true orgasmic disorder—for example,
if a woman can successfully achieve an orgasm while masturbating but not
while having intercourse, does she have an orgasmic disorder? This situational
inorgasmia might be considered a problem if, for example, it caused discord
between the couple, but it is not necessarily a dysfunction or disorder.
In light of the various sexual dysfunctions that individuals sometimes
experience, research and clinical work over the years have focused on finding
effective ways to resolve these problems for individuals and couples. Chapter 9
by Kleinplatz provides an overview of interventions for sexual problems and
again touches on some of the controversies inherent in determining what
constitutes an actual disorder, given the subjective nature of the human sexual
experience.
Past research has focused mostly on sexual inhibition when addressing
disorders of the desire and arousal phases of sexual response. More recently,
hypersexuality, also called sexual compulsivity, has become a focus of research.
Sexual compulsivity has become especially popularized by reports of in-
dividuals who access pornographic Internet sites uncontrollably. Reece,
Dodge, and McBride provide a stimulating discussion of this popular topic and
urge caution in making value judgments about what is and is not an appro-
priate level of sexual interest or activity in Chapter 10. Finally, this volume
would be incomplete if we did not include a chapter addressing a long-ignored
issue—sexuality in people affected in various degrees by chronic disease,
Introduction xi
M. Michele Burnette 1
The reproductive or sex organs are a remarkable set of anatomical structures.
These organs are referred to as both reproductive and sexual because they
perform two interrelated functions. They produce and support the developing
fetus, and they can provide intense sexual pleasure and intimacy between
people. In this chapter, you will learn about all the major reproductive
structures in the male and female, as well as the breasts, since they are so closely
linked to sexual pleasure in the U.S. culture.
Female and male anatomy are presented in separate sections in this
chapter; however, keep in mind that for each structure in the female sexual
anatomy there is a corresponding structure in the male anatomy. These are
called homologous structures because they develop from the same cells in the
developing fetus. As you may know, the chromosomes of males and females
are different, with males having an XY sex chromosome configuration, and
females having an XX configuration. In addition, although we tend to refer to
testosterone as a ‘‘male’’ hormone and estrogens as ‘‘female’’ hormones, males
and females have varying amounts of both of these hormones in their bodies.
However, if a fetus has a Y chromosome, male hormones are produced in
greater amounts. And in the presence of adequate amounts of testosterone, the
cells that are intended to become reproductive organs differentiate into male
organs (e.g., penis, testes). In the absence of a Y chromosome, all fetuses
develop female organs (e.g., uterus, ovaries). Thus, this chapter will begin the
study of sexual anatomy with that of the female.
2 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
two spongy bodies, called corpora cavernosa, which are located inside the shaft,
will become engorged with blood and cause the clitoris to expand in size and
become more accessible to stimulation. The clitoral hood, which results from
the labia minora joining together, generally covers the clitoris but will retract
somewhat during arousal. Because the glans is so sensitive, it can become over
stimulated in some women, causing an unpleasant sensation. At later points of
sexual arousal, the glans actually retracts under the hood somewhat, protecting
it from excessive stimulation. The amount of stimulation of the glans that is
perceived as pleasurable varies considerably among women. Verbal and non-
verbal communication from a woman about what does and does not feel good
should always be a partner’s guide during sexual interactions.
The Perineum
The perineum is more of an area than an actual structure, but it is im-
portant in that touching or stroking this area can be highly sexually stimu-
lating. This area is found between the vaginal and anal openings. It is also the
Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 5
The Vagina
The vagina, or vaginal canal, is a tubular structure, but the walls rest against
each other when in a nonaroused state. The canal is about four inches long
when not in an aroused state and runs between the vaginal opening and the
cervix. The walls of the vagina contain folded layers of muscle, which can
stretch considerably during childbirth. The walls also constantly secrete a fluid,
which provides an optimal environment for ‘‘good bacteria’’ that maintain
vaginal pH at a healthy level. These secretions increase considerably during
sexual arousal to lubricate the walls for vaginal intercourse. All women expe-
rience a small amount of vaginal discharge because of these fluids. This is natural
and expected, but if the discharge seems excessive, causes irritation, or has a foul
or strong odor, it may indicate infection, such as a yeast infection or sexually
transmitted infection. If any of these signs are present, the woman should
consult a medical professional.
Only the outer one-third of the vaginal canal is sensitive to touch; thus, it
is the only part that plays much of a role in sexual stimulation. Some experts on
female sexual anatomy and lay women alike contend that there is an area in the
outer third of the vagina which is especially rich with nerve endings and, when
stimulated, is most likely to cause an orgasm. This is called the Grafenberg or
G-Spot. It is reportedly located about two inches up from the entrance of the
vagina on the front of the body. Women report that they can feel a raised spot
or series of ridges in this area.
The Cervix
The vaginal canal ends at the cervix. The cervix is located between the
vaginal canal and the uterus. Viewing the cervix up through the vagina, it
appears somewhat like a donut with a very small opening in the center. The
opening is to the uterus and is called the cervical os. Two important substances
Figure 1.2. Female reproductive system internal structures.
Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 7
pass through the cervical os—menstrual flow passes out of the uterus through
this canal and into the vaginal canal, and during intercourse, ejaculate con-
taining millions of sperm (the male contribution to ultimately producing a
baby) must pass through the cervix to the uterus on the way to the fallopian
tubes, where an egg, if present, might be fertilized. Mucus produced by glands
in the cervix forms a plug in the cervical os, which protects the uterus from
harmful bacteria. This plug dissolves during menstruation and ovulation (when
an egg cell is available for fertilization by the sperm) to allow passage through
the os.
The Uterus
The uterus is the structure that bears the resemblance of an upside-down
pear. It has muscular walls and is hollow within. Measuring on average about
three by two inches, it is obviously capable of expanding tremendously as this is
the reproductive organ that is home to the developing fetus. The uterine walls
are formed by three layers. The thin, outer layer is called the perimetrium. The
middle layer, or myometrium, contracts during labor to help move the prenate
into the vaginal canal. And the inner layer, or endometrium, thickens in re-
sponse to hormonal changes in anticipation of pregnancy. Once an egg from
the female is fertilized by a sperm from a male, the mass of cells that will
become a human form attaches to the uterine wall and is nourished by the
endometrium. If pregnancy does not occur, the inner lining sheds in the form
of menstrual flow.
The Ovaries
The ovaries, located at the end of the fallopian tubes, are held in place by a
ligament attached to the uterine wall. They are two egg-shaped structures
measuring approximately 3⁄4–11⁄2 inches long. These critical organs produce egg
cells as well as hormones that are essential to female reproduction.
8 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
THE BREASTS
The breasts, also called mammary glands, are unique to mammals, which are
capable of producing milk and nourishing their young. In the center of the
surface of the breast is the areola, the circular, darkened area. The nipple, a
rounded, protruding, and also darkly pigmented structure, is located in the
center of the areola. Beneath the skin of the breasts lies a layer of fatty tissue
(adipose). Found within this tissue are the alveolar glands and the lactiferous ducts.
The alveolar glands produce breast milk after delivery of an infant. The al-
veolar glands empty into the lactiferous ducts. These ducts store milk produced
by the alveolar glands, and they also open into the nipples, where they release
milk when stimulated by a suckling infant.
The amount and distribution of adipose tissue determines the shape and
size of the breasts; thus, the size of the breasts bears no relationship to how well
the breasts function (e.g., milk production) or how sensitive they are to
stimulation. Why, then, is our U.S. culture, and so many others, so concerned
about the size of a woman’s breasts? In fact, this obsession seems to be a
Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 9
The Penis
Please refer to the figure of the external male genitals (Figure 1.3). Perhaps
the most prominent genital structure in the male is the penis. The penis is a
cylindrical structure consisting of a glans and a shaft. The penis actually runs
beyond the body wall into the pelvic region—this unexposed area is referred
to as the root. The glans and shaft of the penis are homologous to the glans and
shaft of the clitoris. The shaft runs the full length of the penis up to the glans,
which is the acorn-shaped structure at the end of the penis. The opening to
the urinary tract, called the meatus, is located at the tip of the glans. The raised
edge where the shaft connects to the glans is called the coronal ridge. The glans
and the area of the coronal ridge in particular are most heavily innervated and
are, therefore, very sensitive to touch. As in women, direct stimulation to this
area might become too intense at times for some men.
The skin covering the penis is hairless and very elastic, moving freely across
the underlying structures and stretching when the penis becomes erect. The
area of tissue on the underside of the penis is homologous to the labia minora in
Figure 1.3. Male reproductive system external structures.
Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 11
the female. A visible line that runs along the underside of the penis is where the
tissues fused during prenatal development. Covering the glans is a fold of tissue
called the foreskin. The foreskin usually retracts beyond the glans during an
erection and only slightly when urinating. The foreskin is sometimes removed
for cultural or religious reasons in a procedure called circumcision. This procedure
is usually performed just after birth, although some adults choose to have it
removed later in life. Contrary to popular opinion, circumcision is not a med-
ical necessity. Circumcision has been shown to reduce the risk of infections
such as urinary tract infections, and more seriously the human immunodefi-
ciency virus; however, equal risk reduction occurs simply by practicing good
hygiene (i.e., cleaning under the foreskin while bathing). Given the fact that
circumcision is painful and has its own risks (e.g., infection, deformation),
parents are urged to weigh all costs and benefits carefully (Kinkade & Meadows,
2005).
Penile Augmentation
Another current social concern regarding the penis is that of penile
augmentation. Conduct a quick search of the Internet (or check your junk
email box), and you will see that there are countless products being marketed
to increase a man’s penis size . . . because ‘‘size does matter.’’ Manual stretching
exercises, stretching by using penile weights, vacuum pumps, pills, and lotions
are all scams designed to entice men who have fallen prey to the ‘‘male enhance-
ment’’ industry’s campaign to make men feel insecure about their penises and
erections. In fact, some of these enhancement strategies can be harmful. For
example, both manual stretching and using weights can damage penile tissue.
Vacuum pumps can also damage the elastic penile tissue and eventually cause
less-firm erections (Mayo Clinic Staff, 2005).
In recent years, penis augmentation surgery has become available, but
medical societies do not endorse augmentation surgery for cosmetic reasons
only. Surgery typically involves making an incision near the base of the penis
and cutting the suspensory ligament that attaches the penis to the pubic bone
allowing the root of the penis to hang outside the body. Skin is also grafted
from the abdomen to the penile shaft. A potentially significant problem cre-
ated by this procedure is that because the suspensory ligament stabilizes and
supports the upward tilt of the erect penis, the penis may now wobble, or
erections may occur at unusual angles. The girth of a penis can also be in-
creased through several methods. A couple of common ones include injecting
fat cells from another part of the body into the penile shaft or grafting skin and
fat to the outside of the penis. These procedures are of questionable safety,
and additional surgery is sometimes required to correct negative effects of
the surgery. Some of these complications include low-hanging penises, loss of
sensitivity, scarring, shorter penises, hair at the penis base, and fat concentrated
in one or more areas causing lumps (Mayo Clinic Staff, 2005).
12 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Interestingly, the typical male requesting this surgery has a penis length
within the normal range (Mayo Clinic Staff, 2005; Mondaini et al., 2002).
What is ‘‘normal?’’ A recent Italian study showed that the typical flaccid penis is
9 centimeters (3.54 inches) long while the stretched penis is 12.5 centimeters
(4.92 inches). The typical circumference at the middle of the shaft is 10 cen-
timeters (3.94 inches; Ponchietti et al., 2001). Other research has shown that 70
percent of men’s erect penises range from 5 inches to 7 inches, and a penis is
considered ‘‘abnormally’’ small only when it measures smaller than 3 inches
when erect (Mayo Clinic Staff, 2005). Men seeking penile augmentation are
typically in their late twenties, and they tend to think that the typical penis is
larger than the above findings (i.e., they estimate that the normal flaccid penis is
12 centimeters or 4.72 inches). A large number of these males said their con-
cerns started in childhood when they observed that a friend had a larger penis,
and a smaller but significant number of them began to worry about their penis
size in their teen years after viewing erotic images (Mondaini et al., 2002).
So, does size matter? In a recent Dutch study, 77 percent of the sexually
active women surveyed responded that penis size was ‘‘unimportant’’ or ‘‘to-
tally unimportant.’’ Women who were concerned about length were also
concerned about girth, with girth being more important to them than length
(Francken, van de Wiel, van Driel, & Weijmar Schultz, 2002). Other re-
search has shown that women tend to prefer average-sized penises. And some
men with large penises express concerns about being too large because women
respond in fear to the sight of a large penis or the man fears hurting his
partner. In fact, some women do feel discomfort if an especially long penis is
thrust against the cervix, and the man must be careful not to insert the penis
too far. In addition, it is important to remember that the female has very little
sensation in the upper two-thirds of her vagina, meaning that stimulation in
this area is unlikely to enhance sexual arousal. In short, bigger is not necessarily
better.
The Scrotum
The scrotum, a saclike structure behind the penis, is the homologous
structure to the labia majora in the female. The scrotum houses organs called the
testes. This sac is thin, hairless or slightly hair-covered skin, which hangs and
moves loosely around the testes. The function of the scrotum is critical to
reproduction—its job is to maintain the testes at a temperature that is neither too
cold nor too hot and therefore damaging or lethal to sperm, which are formed,
matured, and stored in the testes. Sperm must be maintained at a temperature of
about 938 Fahrenheit, more than five degrees lower than normal body tem-
perature. To maintain this safe temperature, the dartos muscle, in the middle layer
of the scrotal sac, contracts and draws the testes up closer to the body when they
become too cold and loosens so that the testes will fall farther away from the
body when they become too hot. The scrotum sometimes contracts when a
Figure 1.4. Male reproductive system internal structures.
14 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
man senses fear, an involuntary response intended to protect the testes from
harm (Baldwin, 1993).
The Perineum
The perineum on the male body is the area located between the scrotum
and the anal opening. Similar to the female, the perineum of the male is richly
innervated and, therefore, sensitive to touch.
one coiled tube, which, if stretched and measured from end-to-end, is about
20 feet long. You can actually feel the epididymis by gently rolling the testis
between your fingers. Each testis is held in place by a spermatic cord, which
contains nerves and blood vessels that support the testes. The cremaster muscle is
also located in the spermatic cord. During sexual arousal, the cremaster muscle
contracts, pulling the scrotal sac closer to the body. As with the dartos muscle,
contraction and expansion of the cremaster muscle also functions to regulate
scrotal temperature. Finally, the spermatic cords also contain the vas deferens,
which are discussed next.
condom) should be applied before Cowper’s gland emissions and any penis to
vulva contact if one would like to be absolutely certain to avoid pregnancy.
CONCLUSION
At first, the idea of studying sexual anatomy may sound boring, but
hopefully you learned that the sexual anatomy is quite interesting. The ben-
efits of understanding how these organs function are considerable from both
a psychological and physical standpoint. Such knowledge can make you
more comfortable with your sexuality in general and help you to know how
you and your partner can derive the most enjoyment from your sexual in-
teractions. In addition, understanding how these organs work might help you
to recognize when you might have a physical concern that needs to be ad-
dressed.
Reproductive and Sexual Anatomy 17
REFERENCES
American Society of Plastic Surgeons. (n.d.). Top 5 cosmetic procedures. Retrieved
September 8, 2005, from www.plasticsurgery.org/public_education/
loader.cfm?url¼/commonspot/security/getfile.cfm&PageID¼16732
American Society of Plastic Surgeons. (2005). 2004 cosmetic surgery trends.
Retrieved September 8, 2005, from www.plasticsurgery.org/public_
education/loader.cfm?url¼/commonspot/security/getfile.cfm&PageID¼
16150
Baldwin, D. (1993). Understanding male sexual health. New York: Hippocrene
Books.
Discovery Health Channel. (2002). All about breasts [TV series episode]. Berman
and Berman: For Women Only. Retrieved September 8, 2005, from
www.newshe.com/allaboutbreasts1.shtml
Francken, A. B., van de Wiel, H. B., van Driel, M. F., & Weijmar Schultz, W. C.
(2002). What importance do women attribute to the size of the penis?
European Urology, 42(5), 426–431.
Kinkade, S., & Meadows, S. (2005). Does neonatal circumcision decrease mor-
bidity? Journal of Family Practice, 54(1), 81–82.
Mayo Clinic Staff. (2005, May 24). Beware of penis-enlargement scams. Retrieved
September 11, 2005, from www.mayoclinic.com/invoke.cfm?id¼
MC00026
Mondaini, N., Ponchietti, R., Gontero, P., Muir, G. H., Natali, A., Caldarera, E.,
Biscioni, S., & Rizzo, M. (2003). Penile length is normal in most men
seeking penile lengthening procedures. International Journal of Impotence
Research, 14(4), 283–286.
Ponchietti, R., Mondaini, N., Bonafe, M., Di Loro, F., Biscioni, S., & Masieri, L.
(2001). Penile length and circumference: A study on 3,300 young Italian
males. European Urology, 39(2), 183–186.
2
George J. Demakis 1
The brain is the most complex organ in the human body and is involved in
nearly all that we do, from dancing to reading to having sex. Though its role in
sexual behavior is still not fully understood, the increasing research on this
topic in recent years has documented the brain’s importance—one writer
(Rodgers, 2002) even titled her chapter on the topic ‘‘Where It Really
Happens’’—in various areas such as sexual interest, arousal, and orientation, as
well as sex differences. Many researchers, in fact, would agree that one cannot
understand sex without understanding the brain and, more broadly, how it
interacts with other organs and systems in the body to produce, control, and
regulate sexual behavior. This chapter explores these issues. I first review
common methods of studying sexual behavior, then outline basic neuroanat-
omy with an emphasis on brain regions important for sex, and then summarize
the findings about brain-sex relationships framed by common research meth-
ods. What is not covered here and beyond the scope of this chapter are (a)
physiological changes that accompany sexual arousal in the genitalia, such as
erection in males, (b) brain differences between men and women and how
such differences are manifested in behavior and cognition, and (c) brain dif-
ferences that may be associated with sexual orientation. Each of the literatures
on the above topics is voluminous, at times controversial, especially the final
point, and deviates from our goal of understanding the brain’s role in sexual
behavior.
20 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
The third and final common type of research is the study of changes in
sexual functioning or behavior in individuals with brain damage or disease or,
less commonly, psychiatric conditions, such as depression or schizophrenia.
Ideally, the researcher examines sexual behavior in individuals with damage to
specific brain regions and compares them to those with damage to other
specific brain regions. Much like the animal studies mentioned above, when
damage to an area is related to a decrease in sexual interest or motivation, that
area is assumed to be involved in this aspect of sexuality. A variety of patient
groups have been studied, including those with traumatic brain injury, stroke,
and even Alzheimer’s disease. The true challenge of this approach is that
naturally occurring brain damage, as opposed to the experimentally placed
lesions in animal studies, typically does not occur in only one specific area of
the brain, making determination of the role of specific brain areas difficult.
Moreover, damage to one area with a tumor or a stroke may also affect other
brain areas far from the area of damage as blood flow is altered throughout the
brain. A second related approach has evaluated patients treated for psychiatric
disorders with psychosurgery, which entails surgical destruction of certain
disordered brain regions thought to be causing the psychiatric difficulties. In
the past, this approach was used for severe psychiatric difficulties, notably
schizophrenia, and is used rarely today. At various times, the destruction or
inhibition of brain regions can also be done with medications, an approach
sometimes used in the treatment of individuals with severe sexual disorders,
such as pedophilic sex offenders. Interpretation of such findings is complicated
because there are likely to be complex differences in psychiatric patients in-
volving not only the brain, but also cognition and behavior, that potentially
limit our understanding of brain-sex relationships.
In total, what we know about the brain and sexual behavior comes from a
variety of research approaches using a wide range of animal and human par-
ticipants. Rather than competing, it is probably best to see these approaches as
complementary—each provides a unique perspective and set of findings for
our understanding of the brain’s role in sexual behavior, but together, they
provide the fullest understanding of brain-sex relationships. Before addressing
what we know about such relationships, I briefly outline the main subdivisions
and structures of the brain, to guide that discussion, with focus on those areas
involved in sexual behavior.
Figure 2.1. The four lobes of the brain, as viewed in the left hemi-
sphere.
Sex and the Brain 25
(often termed subcortical), each cerebral hemisphere includes the basal ganglia,
a set of structures important for the control of movement, and the limbic system,
several connected brain structures important for, among other things, emo-
tion, memory, and sex. Limbic means ‘‘border’’ in Latin; this system is a
horseshoe-shaped rim at the junction of the diencephalon and each cerebral
hemisphere. Key structures here include the hippocampus, curled into the base
of the temporal lobe, responsible for the formation of new memories; and the
amygdala, which sits just in front of the hippocampus and is involved in certain
aspects of sexual and emotional behavior (see Figure 2.3). Other structures of
the limbic system include the cingulate gyrus, the septal area, and, as described
above, the hypothalamus. We will return to the limbic system, particularly the
role of the hypothalamus, below.
26 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
hypothalamus
hypothalamic area
Figure 2.4. A medial view of the male rat brain illustrating the pre-
optic area (POA), which appears to be particularly important for
successful copulation.
broadly, testosterone has been widely demonstrated to be necessary for male sex-
ual behavior, including erections and ejaculations—and even sexual thoughts
in humans—in a variety of species.
While the findings described above confirm the importance of the MPOA
for male sexual behavior, it is important to be more precise about what this
region actually accomplishes. At least as determined from studies on the rat, on
which most of the research has been done, it seems important for the inte-
gration and organization of multiple sources of information, including that
from environmental, physiological, and psychological sources, to copulate (see
Nelson, 2000, pp. 199–271). Such information includes, for instance, data
about the state of the male’s own endocrine system and stimuli associated with
the female rat, such as auditory, olfactory, and tactile information about her
sexual availability. Without it, the male is unable to generate appropriate
sexual behavior and responses in the presence of a receptive mate. Because
it serves to integrate such diverse sources of bodily and environmental infor-
mation, the MPOA is not only critical for sex, but also plays an important role
in other motivated behaviors, such as thirst, temperature regulation, and
maternal behavior in the female.
In females, the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMN), quite similar in lo-
cation to the MPOA in males, appears to be critical for mating. In female
rats, this region controls lordosis, the characteristic arching of the back and
28 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
elevation of the rump while the animal remains still, necessary for copulation.
In many animal species, this posture displays females’ receptivity for sex and is
stereotyped, showing little variability across individuals. Experimental damage
to the VMN decreases or abolishes lordosis and other sex-related female be-
havior (e.g., ear wiggling), whereas electrical stimulation does the opposite.
Similar to the males, when certain hormones, in this case estradiol or pro-
gesterone, are injected into this area, sexual behavior is activated, even in rats
whose ovaries have been removed. The VMN also appears similar to the
MPOA in the sense that it serves to integrate multiple sources of information
such as that from the motor system, necessary for the characteristic posture of
lordosis, and the endocrine system, necessary for the secretion of female sex
hormones of estrogen and progesterone that accompany lordosis (see Nelson,
2000, pp. 273–335).
In addition to the findings on the hypothalamus, animal research has also
demonstrated the importance of the amygdala—a limbic structure closely
connected with the hypothalamus—and the temporal lobes for sexual activity.
Another part of the Everitt (1990) study described above lesioned the amyg-
dala rather than the hypothalamus in male rats. These rats did not seek access to
females, suggesting decreased sexual motivation, but were capable of mating
with females if they were provided. Later when amphetamines that induce the
release of the neurotransmitter dopamine were injected into this area, sexual
motivation increased. The amygdala, therefore, seems important for sexual
motivation and initiation. In a quite different type of study, Kluver and Bucy
(1939) removed bilateral (from both cerebral hemispheres) amygdalae and
anterior temporal lobes of monkeys and observed a constellation of unique
behaviors: tameness and loss of fear, hyperorality (i.e., a tendency to put any-
thing into the mouth whether food or not), visual agnosia (i.e., inability to
visually recognize objects), attentional difficulties (i.e., inability to focus on
both relevant and irrelevant stimuli), and hypersexuality. In fact, such monkeys
demonstrated indiscriminate sexual activity—whether heterosexual, homo-
sexual, autoerotic (masturbation), and sexual activity with inanimate objects,
such as chairs. While this research is usually used to highlight the role of the
amygdala and related structures in emotion, it is clear that this region is in-
volved in other activities, including sexual behavior. This issue and the po-
tential causes of the increased sexuality will be described below in discussion of
individuals who have suffered neurological damage.
right parietal regions became more activated than left parietal regions in
both heterosexual men and women who self-stimulated to orgasm. This asym-
metry (i.e., difference between sides of the brain) increased as sexual arousal
increased. Another study by Waismann, Fenwick, Wilson, Hewett, and
Lumsden (2003) using heterosexual men also found activation in right parietal
regions when these men viewed sexually explicit slides. The authors suggested
that these findings were obtained because regions of the parietal lobe are
responsible for complex aspects of visual processing, such as visual association
and pattern recognition. Given the visual nature of the stimuli, these findings
would not be surprising. Using a somewhat different approach, Tucker (1983)
had a small sample of experienced actors generate either intense feelings of
sexual arousal or depression in a laboratory condition. Self-reported sexual
arousal was associated with higher right-hemisphere activity. One exception to
the above research was a study by Heath (1972), who found increased activity
in the septum, a limbic system structure, but none in the right hemisphere,
during orgasm in a man and woman undergoing psychiatric treatment. It is
unclear how well these findings generalize to other populations because of the
severe psychiatric difficulties of the sample. These various EEG studies have
now been supplanted by more sophisticated neuroimaging technologies that
provide a more detailed analysis of regional brain activation. Simply noting
that right hemisphere activity increases during sexual arousal, as has been done
in the past, does not provide much specificity nor does it advance our un-
derstanding of brain-sex relationships.
The newer generation of neuroimaging studies has provided better neu-
roanatomical detail of sexual arousal in humans. One well-done and techno-
logically advanced study by Redoute et al. (2000) used PET scans to evaluate
cerebral blood flow in heterosexual men presented with sexually explicit (but
silent) video clips. This study used a whole-brain scanner that could evaluate
specific cortical as well as subcortical brain areas. Specific areas of increased
activation, as compared to when neutral video clips were observed, included
the following brain regions: limbic system and related structures (anterior
cingulate, right frontal orbitofrontal region), parietal lobes, basal ganglia, and
the posterior hypothalamus. Areas of deactivation were primarily in the
temporal lobes, bilaterally. Karama et al. (2002) used somewhat different
technology, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analysis of brain
activation, during silent video clips of sexual interactions between a man and a
woman. These authors found activation in the following brain regions for both
sexes: bilateral medial prefrontal, orbitofrontal, insular, and occipitotemporal
regions, as well as the amygdala, ventral striatum, and anterior cingulate cortex.
Interestingly, hypothalamic activation was observed only in men and was
positively correlated with self-reported sexual arousal during the video clips.
Males also demonstrated higher levels of self-reported sexual arousal than fe-
males. Together, these studies have demonstrated that multiple cortical and
subcortical brain regions are involved in sexual arousal; Redoute et al. (2000)
30 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
have argued that these areas highlight the cognitive, emotional, motivational,
and physiological aspects of sexual arousal. As a caveat, it should be noted that
these studies only evaluated arousal and not actual sexual behavior, which
might be expected to involve other brain regions as well, particularly those
responsible for motor behavior.
Moving from only sexual arousal, Tiihonen et al. (1994) conducted a
unique and technically difficult study. They had heterosexual men masturbate
to orgasm while being monitored by a PET scan and found increased acti-
vation in right prefrontal regions. Blood flow to other brain areas decreased.
These authors concluded that this region was important for human male
sexuality, particularly its emotional aspects, but not actual genital somatosen-
sory stimulation. In fact, there was no activation in this brain area devoted to
processing sensory information from the genitalia, suggesting that orgasm is
more than simply the mechanical stimulation of these organs, but rather is a
‘‘higher’’ process mediated by the cortex, including sexually related thoughts,
perceptions, memories, and fantasies. While this finding is confined to men
and should be considered tentative until replicated, it does accord well with
some clinical findings (see below).
Together with the older EEG research, the more recent PET and f MRI
findings suggest the importance of the right hemisphere, particularly frontal
regions, as well as a variety of limbic regions and structures connected to them
for sexual arousal (and, in one study, orgasm). Unfortunately, because much of
this research has been done with males, it is unclear how well these findings hold
up in women, and one wonders if such patterns of brain activity associated with
sexual arousal in a laboratory setting are the same as those experienced in real
life. While these are reasonable caveats, research on individuals who have had
brain surgery or have suffered neurological damage (described below) confirm
these findings and provide an additional method of studying brain-sex relations.
man with ‘‘religious obsessions’’ who had not had intercourse for twenty years
prior to the surgery, but postsurgery commented on the ‘‘increased pleasure’’
he experienced with sex as he apparently sought out prostitutes. Freeman also
described one woman who continued to live with her husband, but had many
(fifty!) extramarital sexual relationships. In a classic case study, Ackerly (1964)
described J. P., a patient born without a right frontal lobe and with only
approximately 50 percent of his left frontal lobe. When he was in school his
behavior was odd—at times he was seemingly polite and well-mannered,
but at other times he would behave in a socially unacceptable fashion. For
instance, he would excessively boast about accomplishments, steal things,
wander about, expose himself, and masturbate in public. He was of normal
intelligence and did not show remorse for such behavior. By the time he was
an adult, he had been arrested for automobile theft, at which time the frontal
lobe damage was identified. Similar hypersexuality can also occur in humans
who have suffered Kluver-Bucy syndrome, which is relatively rare in complete
form, as initially observed in monkeys after removal of the temporal lobes. In
one case (Shraberg & Weisberg, 1978), a 23-year-old woman displayed the
classic elements of this syndrome following a stroke after childbirth. For our
purposes, most relevant was her hypersexuality—she would throw off her
hospital gown and then writhe and gyrate her hips to simulate intercourse.
This behavior often occurred when hospital staff was present; after approxi-
mately two months of hospitalization, it ceased. EEG analysis of this woman’s
brain revealed abnormalities in the right hemisphere, particularly in the parietal
regions and the junction between the parietal and temporal lobes. In addition
to the above disorders, poor control over sexual impulses and desires has been
observed in many neurological conditions, including dementia and traumatic
brain injury, which can obviously be a management and treatment challenge.
Briefly, in one of the few group studies on the topic, Sandel, Williams,
Dellapietra, and Derogatis (1996) examined sexual functioning in a group of
individuals several years after traumatic brain injury. Though as a group no
major differences in sexual functioning were found compared to normative
data, individuals with either a damaged right hemisphere or frontal lobe ex-
perienced increased sexual functioning on certain variables. Frontal lobe dam-
aged individuals reported more sexual cognitions or thoughts and fantasies,
whereas right hemisphere damaged individuals reported higher sexual arousal
and more sexual experiences.
What do the above case studies and the research study on sexual impulsivity
teach us about the brain-sex relationship? These abnormal increases in sexual
interest and activity are likely due to the diminishment of the customary in-
hibiting effect of cortical structures, primarily frontal and anterior temporal lobes
on subcortical brain structures. More precisely, the frontal lobes serve to inhibit
or constrain these structures and, when they can no longer do so, the relative
influence of these regions is magnified. Because these regions are involved
in motivated behavior, such as sex and emotion, when they are damaged these
32 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
have also been reported in which the sexual behavior ceased or was reduced or
eliminated in male sex-offenders (see Grossman, Martis, & Fichtner, 1999).
While chemical castration is legal in some states in the United States, there have
been numerous legal challenges to this practice, including that it violates
constitutional guarantees, including the right to privacy, equal protection, and
the prohibition against cruel and unusual punishment (Miller, 1998). Partly as a
result of this, and of the reluctance of some in the medical field to fully par-
ticipate in such experiments, these approaches remain controversial and are not
as widely used or advocated today as they have been in the past. Nevertheless,
such findings highlight the important role of androgens in male sexual behavior.
The second and a quite different area of research, the sexual behavior and
responsivity of spinal cord damaged patients, provides a somewhat different
perspective on sex and behavior. While there is significant variability in lo-
cation of injury on the spinal cord as well as the resulting sexual behavior, an
interesting finding to emerge from this area is that individuals with complete
severing of the spinal cord who are quadriplegic can still respond sexually.
For instance, with genital stimulation of the penis, males are able to achieve
erections and ejaculate, though they typically do not experience orgasm, as the
sensory information necessary for this to occur from the penis and genitalia
does not reach the brain because of the spinal injury. These men are also
typically not able to achieve erection by simply thinking about or fantasizing
about sex. Similar findings about the capacity of spinal cord damaged animals
to achieve erections and to ejaculate have been observed. In a related case, a
young man who was brain-damaged and in a coma was mechanically stimu-
lated to ejaculate so that his wife could be artificially inseminated (Townsend,
Richard, & Witt, 1996). Findings in women are a bit more complex, but it
also appears that sexual arousal and stimulation are more common than orgasm
in women with a variety of spinal cord injuries (Sipski, 2002). Together, these
findings indicate that certain reflexive aspects of sexual behavior can be
achieved without direct brain involvement and, traditionally, have been con-
ceived of as spinal reflexes. Such reflexes are due to the connection of the
external genitalia with the spinal cord and do not require higher-up cortical
processing. Before moving on from this issue, it is important to note that while
these reflexes may be retained after injury, sexual interest, arousal, and mo-
tivation typically decline post–spinal cord injury, which would be expected
given the many physical, psychological, and medical challenges these indi-
viduals face. For instance, even assuming all aspects of sexuality were func-
tioning normally, the motoric difficulties of an individual with quadriplegia
would certainly make sexual activity challenging.
REFERENCES
Ackerly, S. S. (1964). A case of paranatal bilateral frontal lobe defect observed for
thirty years. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex
and behavior (pp. 192–218). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Sex and the Brain 35
Tiihonen, J., Kuikka, J., Kupila, J., Partanen, K., Vainio, P., & Airaksinen, J.
(1994). Increase in cerebral blood flow of right prefrontal cortex in man
during orgasm. Neuroscience Letters, 170, 241–243.
Townsend, M. F., Richard, J. R., & Witt, M. A. (1996). Artificially stimulated
ejaculation in the brain-dead patient: A case report. Urology, 47, 760–762.
Tucker, D. M. (1983). Asymmetries of coherence topography: Structural and
dynamic aspects of brain lateralization. In P. Flor-Henry & J. Gruzelier
(Eds.), Laterality and psychopathology (pp. 349–362). Amsterdam: Elsevier
Science.
Waismann, R., Fenwick, P. B., Wilson, G. D., Hewett, T. D., & Lumsden, J.
(2003). EEG responses to visual erotic stimuli in men with normal and
paraphilic interests. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 135–144.
3
David L. Rowland 1
INTRODUCTION AND OVERVIEW
The ease with which most people experience sexual arousal and response
belies the complex integration of physiological, psychological, relationship,
and cultural factors underlying it. Research over the past decade has revealed
much of this complexity, yet, even now, our understanding of sexual arousal
and response remains very incomplete. In fact, many of our insights about
normal sexual arousal and performance have resulted from the study of dis-
turbances in the system. Such situations provide strong motivation for us to
analyze the system in order to understand and remediate the problem. In doing
so, we discover that, despite its critical role in so basic a function as procre-
ation, sexual arousal and response requires a great deal of integration at all
levels of the organism.
In this chapter, we discuss the process of sexual arousal and response,
including:
brain); and (3) peripheral response systems that prepare the person for sexual
activity (e.g., vaginal lubrication, erection of the penis).
Psychological processes, such as attention, thoughts, and feelings, which provide the
link between erotic stimulation and sexual arousal.
psychological aspects for each component, but it also pointed out the inter-
dependence among the response components. For example, problems with
orgasm could result from insufficient arousal; or problems with arousal might
actually be seated in the desire phase.
Since these initial conceptualizations, a number of alterations or alter-
native models have been proposed. For example, a distinction between spon-
taneous desire (libido) and stimulus-driven desire (arousability) has been
suggested. Whereas the former seems more typical of men, the latter is more
descriptive of women, a difference that may have evolutionary, physiological,
and clinical significance (Basson, 2002; Levin, 2002, in press; Tiefer, 1991).
Regarding the evolutionary perspective, males’ success at producing offspring
is largely tied to their willingness and ability to compete for females—hence a
high level of biologically mediated sexual drive increases the likelihood of
participating and succeeding in the competition. Because mammalian females
expend greater effort than males toward the offspring (gestation, lactation,
etc.), their success depends more heavily on choosing males with the ‘‘right’’
set of credentials or qualities. As a result, females will be more discriminating in
their choice of males than vice versa, and this discrimination (and subsequent
interest) will be driven largely by external (physical and behavioral) cues
provided by the potential mate (Daly & Wilson, 1978). Such differences in
reproductive strategies would be supported by physiological infrastructure
(e.g., gonadal, endocrinal, sensory-perceptual, and emotional differences) be-
tween the sexes.
Most would agree that this sharpening of the conceptualization of desire is
both warranted and overdue. Not only do the above distinctions fit well with
many men’s and women’s reports of their own experiences of arousability, but
they also recognize the importance of both internally and externally mediated
stimuli essential for sexual interest and arousal. They also appear consistent
with the kinds of problems that often surface in sexual dysfunction clinics.
In addition, this reconceptualization provides greater linkage of desire with
arousal, as for most women and many men, the stimuli that generate interest in
becoming aroused (sexual interest) are the same as those that elicit arousal itself
(e.g., behaviors and sight of the partner). Finally, this reconceptualization is
important from a clinical perspective, as it formally incorporates an idea that
has long been known to clinicians in the treatment of sexual problems, namely
the importance of taking a systemic approach that includes physiological,
psychological, and relationship factors toward understanding sexual response
and its disorders.
Given the previous comment, it is not surprising that increasing emphasis
has been placed on the role of the dyadic (i.e., a couple’s) relationship in un-
derstanding (and treating) sexual response (Schnarch, 1988, 1991). According
to the systemic or ‘‘biopsychosocial’’ model, sexual response is the culmina-
tion of three interacting domains. (1) The biological—the physiological mech-
anisms that prepare and enable genital response. (2) The psychological—the
40 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Synthesis of Models
Because various models differ in their focus or utility, some are frequently
cited by clinicians, others by researchers. No single model can adequately serve
all the needs within the field of sexology. Clearly, a good understanding of
sexual arousal incorporates important aspects of a number of the models.
Second, certain preconditions are necessary for sexual response to occur, in-
cluding the appropriate external stimuli (partner, sexual situation, etc.) and
internal conditions (endocrine, neurophysiological pathways, etc.). These in-
ternal conditions are mediated through both psychological and physiological
pathways and contribute to the ability to respond to psychosexual stimuli and
thus experience sexual arousal.
Third, sexual response itself consists of a progression of responses, beginning
with sexual arousal, which has both a central (brain) component and a peripheral
(autonomic/genital) component. The subsequent behavioral response (a sexual
act) is maintained through ongoing psychological and peripheral physiologi-
cal processes, which, through feedback mechanisms, may culminate in orgasm
and resolution.
Finally, the positive experiences associated with sexual arousal and response
often reinforce and increase feelings of passion and intimacy between the
couple, strengthening the partners’ sexual bond. These, together with a sense of
commitment, typically contribute to the satisfaction, longevity, and success of
long-term relationships (Sternberg & Barnes, 1988).
The extent to which the preceding findings apply to humans is only now
being clarified through MRI and PET research, procedures that can detect
changes in brain activity during states of sexual arousal. Interestingly, pre-
liminary studies on humans suggest that some of the neural activation during
sexual arousal may be shifted from lower (MPOA and VMN) to higher brain
centers in men and women. This finding is not surprising in view of the fact
that sexual response in humans depends more heavily upon contextual factors
such as those arising from the relationship with the sexual partner, those related
to social behavior, and those related to attitudes, beliefs, and moral codes. In
men, changes have been noted in the ventral tegmental area, a midbrain-
forebrain region involved in mediating pleasure and reward. Concomitant
changes in the frontal, occipital, and temporal lobes have also been noted
(Holstege et al., 2003; Stoléru et al., 1999). Generally, these brain regions are
responsible for the processing of external (sensory) stimuli by giving meaning
and interpretation to them and for evaluating, deciding, and executing specific
appropriate motor/behavioral responses. Activity changes in hypothalamic and
amygdaloid areas have also been noted in men, but not necessarily in the areas
known for the involvement in sexual response in rodents. In women, many of
these same structures appear to undergo change during arousal and orgasm
(Karama et al., 2002). However, there appears to be less activation of hypo-
thalamic and thalamic regions in women, perhaps offering an explanation for
differences in sexual arousal typically seen across men and women when
viewing erotica. The limitation of these MRI and PET studies is that although
we have learned which specific brain areas are activated (or deactivated) during
sexual arousal and orgasm in men and women, exactly how their activation
relates to the subjective experience of arousal and orgasm is unclear (Levin, in
press). Indeed, many of these same brain structures underlie other (i.e., non-
sexual) cognitive and mental functions.
Nevertheless, several conclusions relevant to understanding human sexual
behavior may be drawn from both animal and human studies.
1. Grouping human sexual response into discrete phases or components
(e.g., sensory, motivational, affective, cognitive, etc.) is largely based upon
introspective analyses. While such conceptualizations may prove useful for
research and communication purposes, discrete neural analogs (in terms of
structures or activities within the brain) for these constructs are unlikely to
exist.
2. Although many similar brain regions appear to underlie sexual behavior
across species, differences do emerge, at least when comparing rodents and cats
with humans. Not surprisingly, in humans there appears to be a greater in-
volvement of higher brain structures responsible for information processing
and decision making, and less involvement of lower centers responsible for
biologically motivated behaviors. Thus, substantial differences probably exist
in the precise role that various neural structures play, as well as the way in
which they interact with other structures.
The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response 49
3. Differences also occur across the sexes, both at the human and non-
human level. With respect to these differences in humans, the findings imply
that sexual arousal and response, although sharing common elements in many
ways, may also be experienced quite differently by men and women.
the sacral area of the spinal cord can generate an erection. Recent studies,
however, also suggest a possible role for the sympathetic nervous system in
erection (Benson, 1988), since blockage of this system produces penile en-
gorgement and erection as well (Rowland & Burnett, 2000; Siroky & Krane,
1983).
The ANS influences erectile tissue through changes in the dynamics of
blood flow of the pudendal arteries (Rowland & Burnett, 2000). These arteries
supply blood to the corpora cavernosa, the two lengthwise chambers on the
back side of the penis, and the corpus spongiosum, the chamber that runs
down the front side of the penis (i.e., if you are facing a man with a flaccid
penis) and expands to form the glans penis. Erection is the result of increased
arterial flow through vasodilation and shunting of the arterial blood away from
immediate venal flow into the cavernous spaces of the penis. At first, this
increase in arterial flow occurs without an increase in blood pressure, and
therefore is probably the result of smooth muscle relaxation of the arterial
walls. When full erection occurs, intracavernosal pressure is increased. Al-
though restricted venal drainage from the increasingly erect penis presumably
contributes to inducting or maintaining erection, its role has only recently
received possible clarification (Batra & Lue, 1990). During erection, when
intracavernosal pressure is high, small blood vessels are compressed against the
relatively unyielding walls of the chambers, known as the tunica albuginea, and
the resulting blockage may decrease venal outflow, increasing the effect of the
inflow of blood in keeping the penis erect and firm.
Ejaculation is generally viewed as the efferent (motor) component of a
reflex process resulting from sensory stimulation of the coronal region of the
penis, although in rare instances it appears that the sensory component is not
critical to this process (e.g., spontaneous ejaculation). At the genital level,
ejaculation involves two steps, including (1) seminal emission and bladder neck
closure, and (2) forced expulsion of fluid (Motofei & Rowland, in press), and
requires involvement of the sympathetic, parasympathetic, and somatic motor
systems. During the first stage—emission—semen is deposited into the urethral
tract, an event associated with ‘‘ejaculatory inevitability’’ in men. At this time,
the bladder neck also closes to prevent urine from mixing in the urethral tract
and semen from flowing back toward the bladder. The deposition of semen in
the urethral tract then triggers the spasmodic (clonic) contractions responsible
for ejaculation—a complex process that involves involuntary contraction of
muscles that are normally under voluntary control. Local sensory receptors
transmit this information to the brain, which is then associated with the
subjective experience of orgasm. Obviously, the trigger for the ejaculatory
sequence is under the control of brain systems and is related to the man’s level
of sexual excitement and arousal (Motofei & Rowland, in press). However,
since ejaculation involves a series of muscle contractions, local mechanisms at
the level of the pelvic musculature have also been suggested (e.g., stretching of
muscles to a point of vigorous contraction). Furthermore, there is evidence to
The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response 51
suggest that the posterior pituitary hormone, oxytocin, may facilitate these
contractions (Carmichael et al., 1987). Exactly what makes these rapid con-
tractions so rewarding is simply unknown. However, other autonomic func-
tions in the pelvic region such as urination and defecation appear to share
similar, though less intense, properties. For example, smooth muscle stretching
and tension buildup from withholding urine and/or feces is associated with
pleasant sensations when release finally does occur.
urethra) are homologous to the penis and are for most women the epicenter of
orgasm. While there is no universally accepted homologue to ejaculation in
the female, some women may produce an ejaculate-like fluid from the anterior
wall of the vagina, an area sometimes referred to as the G-spot (Alzate &
Hoch, 1986; Levin, in press).
In contrast to the male, there is ongoing debate regarding the function of
orgasm in the woman, as it plays no critical physiological role in successful
reproduction (i.e., in women, pregnancy occurs without orgasm, whereas in
men ejaculation/orgasm is necessary for reproduction). Hypotheses regarding
the presumed role of female orgasm abound, and include such functions as
preparation of the uterus for impregnation, facilitated transport of sperm to-
ward the uterus, or even dissipation of vasocongestion in the vaginal region.
Regarding this last point, Levin (in press) argues that relaxation of vaginal tone
from orgasm allows continued blood flow through capillaries as muscle tone
reaches a maximum and ensures maintained vaginal lubrication during sexual
intercourse. A second function of female orgasm may be that of facilitating
vaginal tenting and allowing for elevation of the cervix, a crucial movement
for facilitated sperm transport toward the uterus.
Given these sex differences in structure and function related to orgasm, as
well as the brain structures involved, the mechanisms of orgasm may be suf-
ficiently dissimilar in women and men so that the experience of orgasm is
different as well. Specifically, female orgasm tends to be more variable in its
description, longer in duration, more dependent on learning factors, less re-
liable in its occurrence, and less sensitive to refraction than male orgasm. In
reference to this last point, various studies have estimated that anywhere from
15 percent to 42 percent of women experience multiple orgasms—one orgasm
right after another (Darling, Davidson, & Jennings, 1991). In contrast, multiple
orgasm in men is still viewed as ‘‘case study’’ material, although the traditional
view of a prolonged male refractory period (a time just after ejaculation during
which no amount of stimulation will result in excitation) has recently been
challenged by research suggesting that a subpopulation of men may be capable
of achieving multiple orgasms, although each may not be accompanied by
ejaculation (Dunn & Trost, 1989).
Hanewald, 1994). Second, while autonomic responses such as heart rate and
pulse can be compared across sexes, and do indeed show similar patterns
during arousal (e.g., Heiman, 1977), there is no means of directly comparing
magnitude of penile versus vaginal responses. Third, even though both men
and women may exhibit physiological arousal, they may report different
emotions and feelings associated with the sexual stimuli. Finally, in studies of
this type, participants tend to engage in a self-selection process, particularly
when the study is conducted in a laboratory setting where sexual response is
actually monitored (as opposed to surveys or questionnaires). Women vol-
unteers for such studies tend to be less sex-role stereotyped than non-
volunteers, whereas men volunteers tend to be more sex-role stereotyped
(Wolchik, Brever, & Jensen, 1985). Because sex-role stereotyping could ex-
plain these findings as readily as sex differences per se, one has to be cautious
about drawing strong conclusions regarding the nature of sexually arousing
stimuli for the two sexes.
Recent research has also investigated the role of other sensory systems on
sexual arousal. Olfactory stimulation from a potential mate is essential to
normal copulatory behavior in most mammalian species, including nonhuman
primates, but its role in human sexual attraction and arousal appears to be more
subtle and variable (see Vandenbergh, 1988). Among humans, the scent of
one’s partner may play an important role in sexual arousal and response. The
ability of olfactory stimuli to augment arousal may be more than just a con-
ditioned response; some argue that mammals (including humans) are bio-
logically predisposed to recognize and respond to certain smells as sexual.
For example, women may find musky smells, a typical ‘‘male’’ smell that is a
byproduct of androgen, to be sexy. In many species, musk presumably plays
an important role in reproduction by serving as a male identifier and attractant
to females seeking a fertile mate. The argument has been made that in humans
the effect of such smells may be subliminal, that is, below people’s level of
conscious awareness (Cutler et al., 1987).
In contrast with the subtle and variable effects olfactory stimuli have on
arousal, tactile stimulation of the genitals is strongly associated with sexual
arousal in both men and women. Yet, even this most basic type of stimulation
is context dependent. In laboratory studies on men, penile tactile stimulation
presented without visual erotic stimulation is only mildly arousing compared
with the same stimulation given in conjunction with visual sexual stimulation
(providing an appropriate sexual context) (Rowland & Slob, 1992). Further-
more, loss of sensitivity in the genital region—from aging or disease—is
associated with impaired sexual response in men, although this probably ex-
acerbates existing problems rather than actually causing them (Rowland &
Perelman, in press). In women, the role of genital sensory stimulation in sexual
arousal has received only passing attention in laboratory studies, probably
because its role is so obvious and because of difficulties in applying controlled
tactile stimulation to the vaginal and clitoral regions. One recent study (Slob,
The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response 55
Bax, Hop, Rowland, & van der Werff ten Bosch, 1996) investigating the effect
of vibratory stimulation of the labial region found that when women viewed
an erotic videotape, the vibratory stimulation enhanced self-reported sexual
arousal, but did not augment genital response.
studies showing a common underlying basis for emotion and sexual arousal,
one recent report (Everaerd & Kirst, 1989) used ‘‘prototypes’’ (clusters of at-
tributes or qualities that people use to describe that emotion) of various emo-
tional states and compared them against ones for sexual arousal. The prototype
for sexual arousal overlapped considerably with the emotional prototypes for
‘‘joy,’’ ‘‘warm feeling,’’ and ‘‘merry.’’ Given the above, it is not surprising that
in sexually functional individuals, sexual arousal and positive affect appear to be
strongly interwoven (Rowland et al., 2003).
Perhaps in summary, one can safely say that positive feelings both facilitate
and result from sexual arousal and successful performance. At the same time,
negative emotions (fear, embarrassment, worry, or anxiety), particularly when
they emanate from issues of performance evaluation, are often associated with
sexual impairment and may contribute to, maintain, or result from the dys-
function. Yet, negative emotions, because of their ability to increase general
levels of arousal, may in some instances also facilitate sexual arousal.
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS
Individuals vary considerably in the intensity and frequency of sexual
arousal and behavior. These differences can be attributed to myriad physio-
logical, psychological (cognitive-affective), and sociocultural factors. Specifi-
cally, physiological systems involved in sexual response can be altered by such
conditions as disease, aging, pathophysiological agents, and pharmacological
substances. For example, prolonged and heavy use of a pathophysiological
agent such as nicotine, which diminishes vasomotor response, may have
deleterious effects on erectile response in men; antidepressant drugs are
known to inhibit ejaculation in men and produce anorgasmia in women.
Some conditions may affect sexual response in one sex, while having minimal
or no effect in the other. Diabetes, a condition known to produce peripheral
neuropathy, often interferes with erectile ability in men but appears to have
negligible effects on sexual arousal in women (Slob, Koster, Radder, & van
der Werff ten Bosch, 1990). At the other end of the spectrum, some phys-
iological agents may facilitate sexual arousal and response. Throughout the
ages, reports abound on the use of putative aphrodisiacs (e.g., the bark of the
yohimbine tree supposedly enhances arousal) (see Rowland & Tai, 2003, for a
review).
The importance of psychological factors to functional sexual response
cannot be overstated. Numerous factors, ranging from the erotic value of the
stimuli, expectations of the situation, and self-efficacy on the one hand, to
affective response, self-perceptions, and methods of cognitive processing on
the other, have been shown to impact sexual arousal significantly, and may
account for differential patterns of responding between sexually functional
and dysfunctional individuals. Even when a sexual dysfunction has a strong
somatic basis, psychological factors are implicated. Men and women who fail
sexually, whether from somatic or psychogenic causes, are likely to react
with worry and feelings of loss of control which affects future sexual re-
sponses.
Beyond physiological and psychological influences, relationship and so-
ciocultural factors play important roles in sexual arousal. Among other things,
the quality of the relationship between the sexual partners, the individual’s
personal priorities and values, and customs and expectations of one’s culture
play critical roles in defining any sexual situation, and therefore will impact
sexual arousal and behavior.
The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response 61
REFERENCES
Abrahamson, D. J., Barlow, D. H., & Abrahamson, L. S. (1989). Differential
effects of performance demand and distraction on sexually functional and
dysfunctional males. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 241–247.
Alzate, H., & Hoch, Z. (1986). The G-spot and female ejaculation: A current
appraisal. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 11, 211–220.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th Ed., Text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Bancroft, J. (1989). Human sexuality and its problems. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingston.
Barbach, L. G. (1974). For yourself: A guide to female orgasmic response. New York:
Doubleday.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction: the role of anxiety and
cognitive interference. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
140–148.
Basson, R. (2002). A model of women’s sexual arousal. Journal of Sex and Marital
Therapy, 28, 1–10.
Batra, A. K., & Lue, T. F. (1990). Physiology and pathology of penile erection.
Annual Review of Sex Research, 1, 251–263.
Baum, M. J. (1992). Neuroendocrinology of sexual behavior in the male. In
J. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology
(pp. 97–130). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Beck, J. G., Barlow, D. H., & Sakheim, D. K. (1983). The effects of attentional
focus and partner arousal on sexual responding in functional and dys-
functional men. Behavior Research and Therapy, 21, 1–8.
Beck, J. G., Barlow, D. H., Sakheim, D. K., & Abrahamson, D. J. (1987). Shock
threat and sexual arousal: The role of selective attention, thought content,
and affective states. Psychophysiology, 24, 165–172.
Benson, G. S. (1988). Male sexual function: Erection, emission, and ejaculation.
In E. Knobil & J. Neill (Eds.), The physiology of reproduction (pp. 1121–
1139). New York: Raven Press.
Byrne, D., & Kelley, K. (Eds.) (1986). Alternative approaches to the study of sexual
behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carmichael, M. S., Humbert, R., Dixen, J., Palmisano, G., Greenleaf, W., &
Davidson, J. M. (1987). Oxytocin increase in human sexual response.
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 64, 27–31.
Carter, C. S. (1992a). Hormonal influences on human sexual behavior. In
J. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology
(pp. 131–142). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Carter, C. S. (1992b). Neuroendocrinology of sexual behavior in the female. In
J. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology
(pp. 71–95). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clark, A. S., Pfeifle, J. K., & Edwards, D. A. (1981). Ventromedial hypothalamic
damage and sexual proceptivity in female rats. Physiology & Behavior, 27,
597–602.
62 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Nobre, P., & Pinto Gouveia, J. (2000). Erectile dysfunction: An empirical ap-
proach based on Beck’s cognitive theory. Sexual and Relationship Therapy,
15(4), 351–366.
Pfaff, D. W., & Schwartz-Giblin, S. (1988). Cellular mechanisms of female re-
productive behaviors. In E. Knobil & J. Neill (Eds.), The physiology of
reproduction (pp. 1487–1568). New York: Raven Press.
Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., & Coria-Avila, G. (2003). What can animal models tell
us about human sexual response? Annual Review of Sex Research, 14, 1–63.
Robinson, P. (1976). The modernization of sex. New York: Harper & Row.
Rosen, R. C., & Beck, J. G. (1988). Patterns of sexual arousal: Psychophysiological
processes and clinical applications. New York: Guilford.
Rowland, D. L., & Burnett, A. (2000). Pharmacotherapy in the treatment of male
sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 226–243.
Rowland, D. L., & Cooper, S. E. (2005). Behavioral and psychologic models in
ejaculatory function research. Current Sexual Health Reports, 2, 29–34.
Rowland, D. L., & Heiman, J. (1991). Self-reported and genital arousal changes in
sexually dysfunctional men following a sex therapy program. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 35, 609–619.
Rowland, D. L., & Perelman, M. (in press). Inhibited or retarded ejaculation. In
M. O’Leary (Ed.), Handbook of premature ejaculation. Abingdon, Oxon, UK:
Taylor & Francis.
Rowland, D. L., & Slob, A. K. (1992). Vibrotactile stimulation enhances sexual
arousal in sexually functional men: A study using concomitant measures of
erection. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 387–400.
Rowland, D. L., & Tai, W. (2003). A review of plant-derived and herbal remedies
in the treatment of sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy,
29(3), 185–205.
Rowland, D. L., Tai, W., & Slob, A. K. (2003). Preliminary exploration of
emotional response in men with premature ejaculation: Effects of clomi-
pramine treatment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 145–154.
Sachs, B. D. (2003). The false organic-psychogenic distinction and related
problems in the classification of erectile dysfunction. International Journal of
Impotence Research, 15, 72–78.
Sachs, B. D., & Meisel, R. L. (1988). The physiology of male sexual behavior. In
E. Knobil & J. Neill (Eds.), The physiology of reproduction (pp. 1393–1486).
New York: Raven Press.
Sarrel, P. M. (1999). Psychosexual effects of menopause: Role of androgens.
American Journal of Obstetric Gynecology, 180, 319–324.
Sbrocco, T., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Conceptualizing the cognitive component
of sexual arousal: Implications for sexuality research and treatment. In
P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 419–449). New
York: Guilford.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determi-
nants of emotional state. Psychological Reports, 69, 379–399.
The Psychobiology of Sexual Arousal and Response 65
Weisberg, R. B., Brown, T. A., Wincze, J. P., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). Causal
attributions and male sexual arousal: The impact of attributions for a bogus
erectile difficulty on sexual arousal, cognitions, and affect. Journal of Ab-
normal Psychology, 110, 324–334.
Whipple, B., Ogden, G., & Komisaruk, B. (1992). Physiological correlates of
imagery-induced orgasm in women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 121–
134.
Wilcox, A. J., Day Baird, D., Dunson, D. B., McConnaughey, D. R., Kesner,
J. S., & Weinberg, C. R. (2004). On the frequency of intercourse around
ovulation: Evidence for biological influences. Human Reproduction, 19(7),
1539–1543.
Wolchik, S. A., Brever, S. L., & Jensen, K. (1985). Volunteer bias in erotica
research: Effect of intrusiveness of measure and sexual background. Ar-
chives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 93–107.
Wylie, K., Levin, R. J., & Goddard, A. (2004).Vasomotion and the vaginal re-
sponse to sexual arousal assessed by photoplethsymography—a proposed
new quantitative analysis. (11th World Congress of the International So-
ciety for Sexual and Impotence Research, October 17–21, Buenos Aires,
Argentina). Journal of Sexual Medicine, 1(Suppl. 1), 92 (MP102).
4
David C. Geary 1
After a hiatus of more than 100 years following Darwin’s (1871) and other
naturalists’ early theories on human evolution, social and biological scientists
are once again using our understanding of evolution to shed light on human
behavior (Alexander, 1979; Betzig, 1986; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Geary, 1998).
The topics that have captured much of this recent attention are sexuality and
sex differences in sexual preferences and behaviors. Fortunately, biologists have
studied sexual and reproductive behavior and its evolution in hundreds of
nonhuman species and now have a firm grasp of the how and why of these
behaviors, including an understanding of when and why there is variation in
their expression across social and ecological contexts (Amundsen, 2000; An-
dersson, 1994; Dunbar, 1995; Zahavi, 1975). In the first section, I provide a
brief introduction to theory and research on sexual and other reproductive
behaviors in nonhuman species. This introduction provides a new perspective
for thinking about and coming to fully understand human sexuality, and as-
sociated sex differences. These topics are in fact quite broad and complex and
so I focus on two features of human reproductive behaviors in the second
section; specifically, the mate-choice preferences of women and men, and
differences in these preferences. I conclude the section with a discussion of
historical and cultural variation in mate-choice preferences and sexual be-
haviors. My goal here is to illustrate the power and utility of the evolutionary
approach for conceptualizing human sexuality and sex differences in sexuality.
68 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Compete or Choose?
For most species, sexuality is about reproducing, and in order to reproduce
one must compete for mates or chose the right mate. As I stated, males tend to
compete and females, choose. But why this pattern of sex differences? About 100
years after Darwin’s insights regarding sexual selection, scientists determined that
sex differences in the tendency to compete or choose depend largely, but not
exclusively, on the degree of each sex’s investment in parenting (Trivers, 1972).
The sex that provides more than his or her share of parental investment becomes,
in effect, an important reproductive resource for members of the opposite sex.
One important result is competition among members of the lower investing sex,
typically males, over the parental investment of members of the higher investing
sex, typically females. Members of the higher investing sex are thus in demand,
and can be choosy when it comes to mates. In turn, any sex difference in the
tendency to parent is related to a more fundamental sex difference in the potential
rate with which males and females can produce offspring (Clutton-Brock &
Vincent, 1991). This potential rate of reproduction interacts with social condi-
tions, the operational sex ratio (OSR, which is discussed later) in particular, to
create the mating dynamics that are observed in many species, including humans.
Choosing a Mate
One of the advantages of investing more in parenting than in competing is
that this investment becomes a valuable resource, one that members of the
70 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
opposite sex will fight over (Andersson, 1994). The resulting demand for this
investment creates the ability to choose mates. Because females invest more in
parenting than males, female choice is much more common than male choice
across bird, insect, fish, reptile, and mammal species. One result of female
choice is the evolution of exaggerated male traits, such as the colorful plumage
of the males of many species of birds. These exaggerated traits are often an
indicator of the physical or genetic health of the male, or serve as an indicator
of his ability (e.g., vigor in searching for food) to provide parental investment
(Andersson, 1994; Zahavi, 1975).
The physical and genetic health of males is related, in part, to their im-
mune system; specifically, the ability to resist infection by parasites, such as
worms, viruses, and so forth in the local ecology (Folstad & Karter, 1992;
Hamilton & Zuk, 1982). It appears that a healthy immune system is partly
heritable, and thus the offspring of males with exaggerated traits survive in
greater numbers than do the offspring of other males (Saino, Møller, & Bol-
zern, 1995). Thus, male ornaments are barometers that are strongly affected by
the condition of the male, and female mate choice reflects the evolution of
females’ ability to read these barometers. Although the research is less ex-
tensive, there is evidence that similar mechanisms may operate in species in
which males parent or females vary greatly in their reproductive success. In
these species, males tend to be choosy when it comes to mates and females
often have exaggerated traits (Amundsen, 2000; Andersson, 1994).
Long-Term Partners
Cultural success. One finding that has consistently emerged across Western
and traditional societies is that women prefer long-term partners who are
culturally successful, or are likely to become successful, all other things being
equal (Buss, 1989; Irons, 1979; Sprecher, Sullivan, & Hatfield, 1994). The
specifics of this success vary from one culture to the next, and can range from
ownership of cows to ownership of stock portfolios. Across these contexts,
culturally successful men are those who wield social influence and have control
of the forms of resource that women can use for their own well-being and that
of their children; money buys safe housing, health care, food, and social
influence. The reason for this is clear: In all cultures that have been studied, the
children of culturally successful men have lower mortality rates than the
children of other men (Geary, 2000). Even in cultures where mortality rates
are low, children of culturally successful men benefit in terms of psycholog-
ical and physical health, longevity in adulthood, and opportunities (e.g., ed-
ucational access) to become culturally successful themselves (Adler et al.,
1994). These are exactly the conditions that would result in the evolution of
women’s preference for socially dominant and culturally successful marriage
partners.
72 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
appears to be more of a luxury than a necessity. In fact, the preference for this
type of relationship is more common in middle-class and upper-middle-class
Western culture than in many other cultures or, in fact, in the working-class of
Western societies (Argyle, 1994; Hewlett, 1992). I am not saying that the
development of an intimate relationship is not important or not preferred by
women in non-Western cultures. Rather, in many non-Western contexts
women are more focused on keeping their children alive than on developing
intimacy with their husband.
Good looks and good genes. As I noted above, women will often make trade-
offs between a partner’s cultural success and his physical attractiveness. This
does not mean that a partner’s attractiveness is not important—it is—but,
rather, it is more of a luxury than a necessity. Indeed, in romance novels and
other literatures that appeal to many women, the central male character is
almost always socially dominant, culturally successful, and handsome as well,
and this makes biological sense (Whissell, 1996). Handsome husbands are more
likely to sire children who are attractive and thus sought out as mating and
marriage partners in adulthood, and these men and their children may be
physically healthier than other men and their children, but these relations
are complex and remain to be resolved (Geary, 2005b; Hume & Mont-
gomerie, 2001; Weeden & Sabini, 2005). Whether or not handsome husbands
are healthier, women prefer men who are somewhat taller than average, and
have an athletic (but not too muscular) and symmetric body shape, and
shoulders that are somewhat wider than their hips (Cunningham, 1986; Singh,
1995; Waynforth, 2001). Women rate symmetric facial features as attractive, as
well as somewhat larger than average eyes, a large smile area, and prominent
cheekbones and chin. When they can, women put these preferences into
practice; for instance, physically smaller and less-robust men are less likely to
be chosen as marriage partners than are taller and more-robust men (Nettle,
2002).
There is also evidence that women’s mate and marriage choices are
influenced by men’s immune-system genes (Ober, Elias, Kostyu, & Hauck,
1992; Wedekind, Seebeck, Bettens, & Paepke, 1995). Women, of course, are
not directly aware of these genetic differences: Immune-system genes are
signaled through pheromones and women are sensitive to, and respond to,
these scents, especially during the second week of their menstrual cycle, that is,
when they are most fertile (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998). And women show
a preference for the scents of physically attractive men, even though they have
never seen these men. This suggests that attractive and presumably healthy
men have a variety of related physical and pheromonal traits that distinguish
them from other men and that can influence women’s choices of sexual
partners. It is not simply the quality (i.e., presumed resistance to disease) of the
man’s immune-system genes; what matters is how these genes match up with
those of the woman. In terms of disease resistance, the best outcome for
offspring occurs when there is high variability in immune-system genes, and
74 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
one way to achieve this is through having children with a partner with dif-
ferent immune-system genes. Women, in fact, find scents of men with dis-
similar immune-system genes as more pleasant and sexy than the scents of men
with similar immune-system genes, and conceive more easily with these men
(Ober et al., 1992; Wedekind et al., 1995).
Some of the more intriguing research in this area has revealed that women’s
preference for physically attractive men varies across the menstrual cycle and with
her physical attractiveness (Gangestad, Thornhill, & Garver, 2002; Little, Burt,
Penton-Voak, & Perrett, 2001). Penton-Voak et al. (1999) demonstrated that
women preferred men with masculine facial features (e.g., prominent chin)
around the time of ovulation, and men with more feminine facial features at other
times in their cycle; implications are discussed below, under ‘‘Short-term part-
ners.’’ Little et al. (2001) found that physically attractive women rated masculine
looking men as more attractive long-term partners than did other women, pre-
sumably because attractive women are better able to keep these men focused on
the primary relationship.
Short-Term Partners
Because women pay the cost of pregnancy, they are on average more
sexually cautious than men, but do at times engage in short-term sexual re-
lationships (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Bellis & Baker, 1990; Essock-Vitale &
McGuire, 1988; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Oliver & Hyde, 1993; Symons,
1979). Sometimes women engage in these relationships when they perceive
the potential for development of a longer-term relationship, suggesting that
they sometimes use sexuality as a means to initiate a relationship with a po-
tential marriage partner. At other times, women initiate a short-term sexual
relationship outside of the context of a marriage or other long-term rela-
tionship, and in still other contexts women and their children may be better off
when the women have multiple sexual partners (Bellis & Baker, 1990; Lan-
caster, 1989). I first provide a brief description of the dynamics of women’s
extra-pair relationships, and then describe the contexts in which most women
benefit from multiple sexual relationships.
Extra-pair sex. It has been estimated that between 12 percent and 25
percent of women will engage in some type of affair during their lifetime
(Banfield & McCabe, 2001; Bellis & Baker, 1990; Glass & Wright, 1992). The
reasons for these affairs are many, but the most potentially volatile situation is
one in which the woman’s affair results in pregnancy by her extra-pair partner
and cuckoldry of her husband; cuckoldry means that the husband has been
deceived into raising the child of another man. The definitive study on how
often this happens has not yet been conducted, but it is clear that it happens
more frequently than many people wish to admit. The best estimate at this
time is that as many as 10 percent of children may be the result of these
relationships, although the rate varies widely across contexts and ranges from
An Evolutionary Perspective on Sexual Relationships 75
women who excluded males from the domestic unit and maintained
multiple liaisons were more fecund, had healthier children with fewer
pre- and post-natal mishaps, were able to raise more children over the
age of five, had better nourished children (as measured by protein per
76 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Among the Ache and Barı́, South American Indian societies, women will
often engage in sexual relations with men who are not their social partners,
especially after becoming pregnant (Beckerman et al., 1998; Hill & Hurtado,
1996). By tradition, these men are called secondary fathers and are socially
obligated to provide food and other resources, as well as social protection, to
the woman’s child, although not all of them do so. The result seems to be a
confused paternity such that both primary and secondary fathers invest in the
child. The advantages of having a secondary father are substantial. The mor-
tality rate of Ache children with one secondary father is about half that of
children with no secondary father or two or more secondary fathers; with
more than one secondary father paternity is too uncertain, and thus these men
do not invest in the child. The benefit of a secondary father cannot be at-
tributed to qualities of the mother, as 80 percent of Barı́ children with a
secondary father survived to adulthood, as compared to 61 percent of their
siblings without a secondary father (Beckerman et al., 1998).
Long-Term Partners
When men are looking for long-term partners, typically for marriage, they
are in effect committing to invest a significant amount of their time and
resources in the relationship with their partner and any resulting children.
This is not to say that they are willing to invest as much as their partners would
prefer for them to, as it is typically not the case. Still, from an evolutionary
perspective, men are predicted to be, and are, more similar than different from
women in terms of the traits they seek in a long-term partner (Kenrick, Groth,
Trost, & Sadalla, 1993). In the following sections, I highlight a few of the key
areas in which men and women differ in their mate preferences.
Cultural success. Outside of the strictures of Western culture, men are
typically allowed to marry as many women as they can support, although only
about 10–15 percent of men actually marry polygynously (Murdock, 1981);
even in Western culture, the same end can be achieved with serial marriages
(Forsberg & Tullberg, 1995). In these non-Western societies, men are more
An Evolutionary Perspective on Sexual Relationships 77
concerned with the traits described in the ‘‘Good looks and fertility’’ section
than with their partners’ cultural success (Gil-Burmann, Peláez, & Sánchez,
2002; Li et al., 2002; Sprecher et al., 1994). They do expect their wives to
contribute to the family, as with foraging, but they are not typically concerned
about their cultural success per se. In Western societies, monogamous mar-
riages are socially imposed and thus marriage for culturally successful men—
those who would have several wives in other societies—has a sexual and
reproductive cost: Their sexual behavior is restricted, at least in terms of
marriage vows, and sometimes legally, to a single relationship, and they typ-
ically have fewer children (Flinn & Low, 1986; Forsberg & Tullberg, 1995). As
a result, culturally successful men in such cultures and those who strive for
cultural success tend to be more choosy when it comes to marriage partners
than other men, or successful men in polygynous cultures. For many of these
men, the cultural success of their prospective wife is important, but is more of
a luxury than a necessity, in contrast with women’s expectations for the cul-
tural success of their husbands.
Personality and behavior. When it comes to a marriage partner, men
throughout the world prefer women who are intelligent and kind, although
these traits are often a luxury and not a necessity (Buss, 1989; Li et al., 2002).
One behavior that is a necessity for men, however, is their partner’s sexual
fidelity. Men’s concern for their partner’s sexual fidelity is an evolutionarily
coupled feature of the earlier described cuckoldry risks, and the costs associated
with investing in the child of another man. The social and psychological
manifestation of this concern is sexual jealousy, which has a near universal
influence on the dynamics of men’s and women’s relationships (Buss, 1994;
Symons, 1979). It is not that women do not become sexually jealous, they do:
it is a matter of degree and a matter of how men and women react to an actual
or perceived infidelity. In one study, women reported their partner engaged in
more monitoring of their behavior during the week the women were most
likely to ovulate, the time frame when these same women reported an increase
in sexual fantasy and interest in an extra-pair man (Gangestad et al., 2002).
This and related studies are consistent with the view that men’s sexual jealousy
evolved at least in part as a response to women’s ability to cuckold.
Good looks and fertility. Both women and men prefer attractive partners,
but this preference is consistently found to be more important—a necessity and
not a luxury—for men than for women (Buss, 1989; Li et al., 2002). Men’s
ratings of women’s physical attractiveness are driven by several specific physical
traits, including a waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) of 0.7; facial features that signal a
combination of sexual maturity but relative youth; body and facial symmetry;
and age (Cunningham, 1986; Kenrick et al., 1993; Kenrick & Keefe, 1992;
Sprecher et al., 1994). A measure of leanness to obesity independent of height,
that is, the body mass index (BMI), is also associated with rated attractiveness
(Weeden & Sabini, 2005). Leaner women tend to be rated more attractive
than heavier women, although, as noted later, the attractiveness of relatively
78 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
thinner to relatively heavier women varies with availability of food and other
resources (Anderson, Crawford, Nadeau, & Lindberg, 1992; Pettijohn &
Jungeberg, 2004).
In any case, this combination of cues has been hypothesized to be indi-
cators of women’s health and fertility. To illustrate, women’s fertility is low in
the teen years, peaks at about age 25, and then gradually declines to near zero
by age 45 (Menken, Trussell, & Larsen, 1986). Teenage mothers experience
more complications during pregnancy than do women in their twenties, and
these risks begin to increase in the thirties, and increase sharply after age 35.
Given this, it is not surprising that men’s preferences are sensitive to indica-
tions of a women’s age (Kenrick & Keefe, 1992). Other aspects of men’s
preferences may or may not be indicators of health and fertility. In a review of
this literature, Weeden and Sabini (2005) found that women with attractive
faces, as rated by men, and a waist-to-hip ratio in the middle range tended to
be in better health than their peers, but the strength of these relations was not
large. Women with ratios greater than 0.85 are at risk for a number of
physiological disorders and appear to have greater difficulty conceiving than do
women with lower ratios. Other studies suggest that BMI might be a better
predictor of health than WHR. Facial and body symmetry, in contrast, was not
found to be consistently correlated with women’s health. One possible ex-
ception is breast symmetry. Women with symmetric breasts are rated as at-
tractive by men, and these women appear to be more fertile than other women
(Møller, Soler, & Thornhill, 1995).
Short-Term Partners
Unlike women, men, like the males of most other species, often pursue
short-term sexual relationships as an end initself. At least it seems to be so; but
in fact, it can result in the currency of evolution—children. One important
difference between these sexual relationships and those described in the
‘‘Long-term partners’’ section is men’s investment in children; for short-term
partners, men have no intention of investing in any resulting children, whereas
for long-term partners they typically do. That men are interested in short-term
sexual partners and differ in important respects from women on this dimension
of sexuality is illustrated in the respective sections below on sexual attitudes
and fantasy, and use of prostitutes.
Sexual attitudes and fantasy. Some of the largest sex differences in this area
involve attitudes toward casual sex and the frequency of masturbation (Buss &
Schmitt, 1993; Clark & Hatfield, 1989; Oliver & Hyde, 1993). About four out
of five men were more enthusiastic about the prospect of casual sex than the
average woman, and about six out of seven men report masturbating more
frequently than the average woman; the latter suggests that men are more likely
on average to be sexually frustrated than women. Men’s attitudes toward casual
sex are put into practice if the opportunity arises. In a set of studies in which
An Evolutionary Perspective on Sexual Relationships 79
demand than are their same-sex peers and therefore are able to exert more
influence in their inter-sexual relationships. However, wider social and eco-
logical factors also influence the sexual behavior and choices of marriage part-
ners of these and other people, as I briefly overview in the following sections.
unpredictable, but thinness was not necessarily the preferred standard in so-
cieties with excess food. In a related study, Pettijohn and Jungeberg (2004)
found that economic and social well-being in the United States were related to
the facial and body features of the Playboy playmate of the year. When times
were difficult (e.g., increase in unemployment rate), the playmates tended to
be taller, heavier, and had more mature (e.g., smaller eyes) facial features.
Thinner playmates tended to be found with economic prosperity and higher
levels of social well-being. These studies suggest that cultural and historical
variation in the ideal for women’s beauty varies, at least in part, with stability
and availability of food and other resources.
CONCLUSION
Many people are uncomfortable with the proposal that human behavior in
general and human sexuality in particular are the result of a long evolutionary
history and are essentially about surviving and reproducing. But this discom-
fort does not make these proposals incorrect, and in fact an evolutionary
perspective on human sexual behavior is the only theoretical lens that provides
the full range of explanation and understanding of these phenomena. Darwin’s
(1871) insights on the processes of sexual selection and later discoveries re-
garding the importance of parenting and potential rates of reproduction for
shaping the evolution and here-and-now expression of sexual behavior and sex
differences in this behavior have provided compelling explanations of these
behaviors in hundreds of species (Andersson, 1994; Clutton-Brock & Vincent,
1991; Trivers, 1972). It readily follows that these same mechanisms will yield
many insights on human sexuality and human sex differences (Geary, 1998;
Symons, 1979), and this is indeed the case, as I illustrated with women’s and
men’s mate preferences and variation in these preferences across historical
periods and different cultures. With this chapter, I have in fact only scratched
the surface regarding the power of this approach and hope that it has piqued
the readers’ interest in reading more and thinking more about this topic.
REFERENCES
Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., &
Syme, S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the
gradient. American Psychologist, 49, 15–24.
Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and human affairs. Seattle, WA: University of
Washington Press.
Amundsen, T. (2000). Why are female birds ornamented? Trends in Ecology and
Evolution, 15, 149–155.
Anderson, J. L., Crawford, C. B., Nadeau, J., & Lindberg, T. (1992). Was the
Duchess of Windsor right? A cross-cultural review of the socioecology of
ideals of female body shape. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 197–227.
An Evolutionary Perspective on Sexual Relationships 83
Love
Pamela C. Regan 1
Well, I can’t speak for anyone else, but for me, the only way I’d have
sex with someone was if we were deeply in love. If two people are
in love, then sex seems like a natural way to express those feelings.
(19-year-old woman interviewed by the author)
As the quotations above illustrate, many people view love and sex as intimately
connected. In fact, attitude surveys conducted around the United States reveal
that the majority of adults and teenagers feel that sexual activity is most ap-
propriate when it occurs between two people who are involved in a loving,
committed relationship (Reiss, 1964; Sprecher, McKinney, Walsh, & Ander-
son, 1988). And being in love—and wanting to express those feelings of love
to the partner—is one of the primary reasons couples engage in intercourse
with one another ( Jessor, Costa, Jessor, & Donovan, 1983; Leigh, 1989). Love
also is related to many other significant interpersonal events in human life,
88 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Love Component
Like the primary colors, the primary love styles can be combined to form
secondary colors or styles of love. The three secondary styles identified by Lee
contain features of the primary styles, but they also possess their own unique
characteristics. Pragma is a variety of love that combines elements of storge and
ludus. Lee (1973) referred to this love style as ‘‘the love that goes shopping for
a suitable mate’’ (p. 124). The pragmatic lover has a practical outlook to love
and seeks a compatible lover. Essentially, he or she creates a shopping list of
desirable attributes and selects a mate based on how well that individual meets
these requirements (and, not surprisingly, the pragmatic lover will drop a
partner who fails to ‘‘measure up’’ to expectation).
Mania is a secondary love style that reflects the combination of eros and
ludus. However, manic lovers lack the self-confidence associated with eros
and the emotional self-control associated with ludus. This obsessive, jealous love
style is characterized by self-defeating emotions, desperate attempts to force
affection from the loved one, and the inability to believe in or trust any affection
the partner or loved one actually does display. The manic lover is desperate to
fall in love and to be loved, begins immediately to imagine a future with the
partner, wants to see the partner daily, tries to force the partner to show love and
commitment, distrusts the partner’s sincerity, and is extremely possessive. Lee
(1973) felt that this was the most potentially destructive love style, calling it
‘‘irrational, extremely jealous, obsessive, and often unhappy’’ (p. 15).
The last secondary color of love is agape, which combines eros and storge.
Agape is similar to Lewis’s concept of ‘‘charity,’’ and represents an altruistic,
selfless love style that implies an obligation to love and care for others without
any expectation of reciprocity or reward. This love style is universal in the sense
that the typical agapic lover feels that everyone is worthy of love and that loving
and caring for others is a duty of the mature person. With respect to personal love
relationships, agapic lovers will put aside their own needs and interests and
devote themselves to the partner, even stepping aside in favor of a rival who
seems more likely to meet the partner’s needs. Although Lee felt that many
people respected and strived to attain the agapic ideal, he believed that the give-
and-take that characterizes most romantic relationships precluded the occur-
rence of purely altruistic love.
colleagues (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986, 1990; Hendrick, Hendrick, & Dicke,
1998; Hendrick, Hendrick, Foote, & Slapion-Foote, 1984). Called the Love
Attitudes Scale, this instrument contains items that reflect the important com-
ponents of the six love styles as originally conceptualized by Lee. Sample items
include:
My partner and I have the right physical ‘‘chemistry’’ between us. Eros
I feel that my partner and I were meant for each other. Eros
I believe that what my partner does not know about me would not hurt him/
her. Ludus
When my partner gets too dependent on me, I want to back off a little. Ludus
Our friendship merged gradually into love over time. Storge
Our love is really a deep friendship, not a mysterious, mystical emotion. Storge
In choosing my partner, I believed it was best to love someone with a similar
background. Pragma
A main consideration in choosing my partner was how he/she would reflect on
my family. Pragma
When my partner does not pay attention to me, I feel sick all over. Mania
I cannot relax if I suspect that my partner is with someone else. Mania
I cannot be happy unless I place my partner’s happiness before my own. Agape
I would endure all things for the sake of my partner. Agape
The Love Attitudes Scale has been used in many empirical investigations.
In general, the results of these studies reveal that love experiences vary as a
function of individual difference and group variables. For example, many
researchers find that women score higher on the love styles of storge and
pragma than do men, whereas men tend to score higher on ludus (Hendrick &
Hendrick, 1988, 1987, 1995; Rotenberg & Korol, 1995). There also are
multicultural and cross-cultural differences in love style. Within the United
States, Asian American adults often score lower on eros and higher on pragma
and storge than Caucasian, Latino, and African American adults (Dion &
Dion, 1993; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986). Latino groups, on the other hand,
often score higher on ludus than Caucasian groups (Contreras, Hendrick, &
Hendrick, 1996). Cross-cultural comparisons reveal that Americans tend to
endorse a more storgic and manic approach to love than do the French, who,
in turn, tend to demonstrate higher levels of agape (Murstein, Merighi, &
Vyse, 1991).
Interestingly, only a few researchers have examined the role of love styles in
ongoing romantic relationships. In general, there is a tendency for individuals to
pair with people who have a similar love style—erotic lovers fall passionately in
love with other erotic lovers, agapic people pair with other equally selfless
96 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
individuals, and so on (Davis & Latty-Mann, 1987; Morrow, Clark, & Brock,
1995). In addition, there is some evidence that love styles are associated with
relationship outcomes. For example, research on dating couples conducted by
Hendrick and her colleagues revealed that men and women who adopted an
erotic style of loving tended to feel particularly satisfied with their romantic
relationships (Hendrick, Hendrick, & Adler, 1988). In addition, the partners of
women who scored high on eros (erotic or passionate love) or agape (selfless
love) were highly satisfied, whereas the partners of women who scored high on
ludus (game-playing love) were not very satisfied. More recently, Brenda
Meeks, Susan Hendrick, and Clyde Hendrick (1998) examined the correlation
between relationship satisfaction and various love styles in a sample of dating
couples. Their results revealed that men and women who endorsed an erotic or
storgic approach to love also tended to be highly satisfied with their relation-
ships. Those who possessed a ludic love style, however, were less satisfied.
Taken as a whole, these results suggest that game playing, lack of friendship, and
lack of passion are not conducive to interpersonal happiness.
When considering this particular area of research, it is important to keep in
mind that not everyone possesses one style of loving. Some people may have
several love styles that characterize their approach to relationships. It is also
possible for a person’s love style to change over his or her lifetime or during the
course of a given relationship. For example, the emotional intensity and pas-
sionate attraction associated with an erotic love style, or the jealous pre-
occupation associated with a manic love style, may occur more often during the
beginning stages of a romance when the partners are uncertain about their
feelings and the future of their relationship. Over time, however, these feelings
may be replaced by more storgic or agapic feelings as the partners grow closer
and their attachment stabilizes.
Both Sternberg and Lee developed their love classification systems by
examining the self-reported experiences of adult men and women involved in
romantic (dating, cohabiting, or marital) relationships. Similarly, the mea-
surement instrument developed to reflect Lee’s theory of love, the Love
Attitudes Scale, also focuses on these kinds of relationships (for example, re-
spondents are instructed to answer the items with respect to their current or
previous romantic partner). Thus, the classification systems proposed by
Sternberg and Lee probably are more appropriately considered taxonomies of
adult romantic love rather than of general love varieties.
think he’s very attractive,’’ ‘‘I wonder what our children would look like’’), and
behaviors (‘‘I arrange my schedule so that we can spend time together,’’ ‘‘We go
everywhere together’’) that she has experienced during their relationship with
her prototype—her mental model—of ‘‘being in love’’ (‘‘People who are in love
miss each other when they’re apart, think about each other a lot, imagine a future
life together, and spend a lot of time with each other’’). If what she is experi-
encing matches her prototype, she will probably conclude that she is, in fact, in
love with her partner.
The prototype approach has been used to explore the horizontal structure
of a variety of relational concepts, including love. Beverley Fehr and James
Russell (1991), for example, asked men and women to generate as many types
of love as they could in a specified time and then asked another sample
of individuals to rate these love varieties in terms of ‘‘prototypicality’’ or
‘‘goodness of example.’’ Of the ninety-three subtypes of love that participants
generated, maternal love was rated as the best or most prototypical example of
love, followed by parental love, friendship, sisterly love, romantic love, brotherly love,
and familial love. Infatuation and puppy love were considered two of the least
prototypical examples of love.
Researchers also have identified the prototypic features (as opposed to
types) of love. For example, Fehr (1988) asked one group of participants to list
the characteristics of the concept love and a second group of participants to rate
how central each feature was to the concept of love. Features that her parti-
cipants believed were central or prototypical to love included the following:
Trust
Caring
Honesty
Respect
Concern for the other’s well-being
Loyalty
Commitment
Acceptance
Fear
Uncertainty
Dependency
Seeing only the other’s good qualities
Euphoria
Love 99
PASSIONATE LOVE
Out of all the many varieties of love that theorists and researchers have
identified, passionate love has received the most sustained attention. This focus
is justified by the fact that passionate love appears to be a universal human
experience. Social scientists have found evidence for the existence of passion-
ate love in virtually all known human societies ( Jankowiak & Fischer, 1992;
Sprecher et al., 1994). Additionally, many people place a high degree of value
on this particular kind of love. For example, increasing numbers of men and
100 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
women around the world are basing their selection of marital and other long-
term romantic partners on passionate love (Goodwin, 1999). Cross-cultural
surveys reveal that most people say that they will not marry unless they are in
love with their partner (Levine, Sato, Hashimoto, & Verma, 1995; Simpson,
Campbell, & Berscheid, 1986). And finally, passionate love appears to be a
unique experience. As we will discuss below, this particular variety of love pos-
sesses several features that clearly differentiate it from other kinds of love.
For example, Krafft-Ebing (1945) posited that sensual love (his label for
passionate love) consisted largely of the romantic idealization of the loved
one’s qualities coupled with intense sexual desire, and he stated that this
particular variety of love was ‘‘never true or lasting’’ and died quickly. In the
1940s, noted love theorist and psychotherapist Theodor Reik (1944, 1945)
expressed a similar view of passionate love, arguing that it was a mixture of
three unique characteristics—sexual desire or the ‘‘sex urge,’’ a short life span,
and idealization of the loved one—combined with affection (which he be-
lieved was present in many types of love). A decade later, Ellis (1954) also
concluded that the distinguishing features of passionate love were the un-
realistically positive evaluation and ‘‘fictionalization’’ of the loved one, intense
and changeable emotions and feelings, fragility, exclusivity, and sexual desire.
He believed that sexual desire, in particular, was the most powerful force
behind the development of passionate love, and that this type of love would
inevitably perish once desire was sated—‘‘sexual and marital consummation
indubitably, in the vast majority of instances, maims, bloodies, and finally kills
romanticism’’ (p. 116).
Like their predecessors, contemporary love theorists have continued to
emphasize the intense, idealistic, emotional, sexual, and short-lived nature of
passionate love. As we have discussed earlier, Lee (1973, 1977) viewed erotic
Love 101
‘‘mere’’ sexual partners are evaluated, and sex is seldom the main focus of
limerence. Either the potential for sexual mating is felt to be there,
however, or the state described is not limerence. (1979, p. 25)
Q. How much passionate love do you currently feel for your partner?
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
None at all A great deal
Q. Rate the intensity of your feelings of passionate love for your current partner.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Not at all intense Extremely intense
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Not at all in love Very much in love
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Extremely weak Extremely strong
The items clearly reflect what theorists believe are the essential ingredients of
passionate love: Intense physiological arousal, emotional turbulence and in-
tensity, cognitive preoccupation, idealization of the loved one, and physical or
sexual attraction.
their feelings of passion to each other. The most common method of ex-
pressing passionate attraction was through sexual activities, including ‘‘making
love.’’
As the majority of love theorists have speculated, one particular aspect of
sexuality—sexual desire or sexual attraction (i.e., lust)—appears to have the
strongest association with passionate love. Many men and women cer-
tainly seem to think so. For example, Robert Ridge and Ellen Berscheid
(1989) asked a sample of college-aged men and women whether they believed
that there was a difference between the experience of ‘‘being in love’’ with
someone and the experience of ‘‘loving’’ someone. Fully 87 percent emphat-
ically claimed that there was indeed a difference between the two experiences.
In addition, when asked to specify the nature of that difference, participants
uniformly cited sexual attraction as descriptive of the passionate, ‘‘being in
love’’ experience and not of the ‘‘loving’’ experience. Similar results have been
reported by Regan et al. (1998). These researchers asked a sample of men and
women to list in a free response format all of the features that they considered
to be characteristic or prototypical of the state of ‘‘being in love.’’ Out of 119
spontaneously generated features, sexual desire received the second highest
frequency rating (66 percent; trust was first, cited by 80 percent). In other
words, when thinking about passionate love, two-thirds of the participants
automatically thought of sexual desire.
Person perception experiments provide additional support for these pro-
totype results. Person perception experiments are commonly used in social
psychological research and essentially involve manipulating people’s percep-
tions of a relationship and then measuring the impact of that manipulation on
their subsequent evaluations and beliefs. In one such experiment, Regan
(1998) provided a sample of forty-eight undergraduate men and women with
two self-report questionnaires ostensibly completed by ‘‘Rob’’ and ‘‘Nancy,’’ a
student couple enrolled at their university. The members of this couple re-
ported that they were passionately in love with each other, that they loved
each other, or that they liked each other. Participants then estimated the
likelihood that the members of the couple experience sexual desire for each
other and the amount of desire that they feel for each other. Analyses revealed
that participants perceived partners who are passionately in love as more likely
to experience sexual desire than partners who love each other or who like each
other. Similarly, partners who are passionately in love were believed to ex-
perience a greater amount of sexual desire for each other than partners who
love each other or who like each other. Sexual desire is viewed, at least by
young men and women, as an important feature or component of passionate
love relationships—and not of relationships characterized by feelings of loving
(i.e., companionate love) or liking (i.e., friendship).
Not only do people believe that passionate love is characterized by sexual
desire, but most men and women report experiencing sexual desire for the people
with whom they are passionately in love. For example, Ellen Berscheid and Sarah
106 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Meyers (1996) asked a large sample of undergraduate men and women to list the
initials of all the people they currently loved, the initials of all those with whom
they were currently in love, and the initials of all those toward whom they
currently felt sexual attraction/desire. Their results indicated that 85 percent of
the persons listed in the ‘‘in love’’ category also were listed in the ‘‘sexually desire’’
category, whereas only 2 percent of those listed in the ‘‘love’’ category (and not
cross-listed in the ‘‘in love’’ category) were listed in the ‘‘sexually desire’’ cate-
gory. Thus, the objects of respondents’ feelings of passionate love (but not their
feelings of love) also tended to be the objects of their desire.
Research with actual dating couples yields similar results. Regan (2000)
found that the self-reported amount of sexual desire experienced by men and
women for their dating partners was significantly positively correlated with the
level of passionate love they felt for those individuals. Their feelings of desire
were unrelated, however, to the amount of companionate love and liking they
experienced for their partners. In other words, the more sexual desire a person
reported feeling for his or her partner, the more strongly he or she reported
being in love with (but not liking or loving) that individual. In sum, research
reveals that passionate love is a sexualized experience that is strongly associ-
ated with feelings of sexual desire for the partner, tends to result in the oc-
currence of sexual activity, and appears to be linked with sexual arousal and
excitement.
COMPANIONATE LOVE
In addition to passionate love, most early and contemporary love theorists
include a type of love known today as companionate love in their classification
schemes. Variously described as affectionate love, friendship love, true love,
attachment, storge, and conjugal love, companionate love reflects ‘‘the affec-
tion and tenderness we feel for those with whom our lives are deeply en-
twined’’ (Hatfield & Rapson, 1993).
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
None at all A great deal
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Not at all Very much
Companionate love can also be assessed with multi-item scales that are
designed to reflect the features that theorists believe to be important elements of
this particular variety of love. For example, the storge subscale on the Love
Attitudes Scale (discussed earlier in this chapter) has been used as a measure of
companionate love. Perhaps the most commonly utilized measure of compan-
ionate love, however, is the thirteen-item Love Scale created by psychologist
Zick Rubin (1970). Sprecher and Regan (1998) subsequently modified this
scale by adding an item that assesses interpersonal trust and removing several
items that reflected a more passionate love experience. Sample items on the
resulting Companionate Love Scale include:
Love 109
SUMMARY
In an effort to understand the nature of love, scholars from a variety of
disciplines have proposed various typologies or classification schemes that
specify types of love. Although there is disagreement about the number and
the nature of the different love types, there are several points of rapproche-
ment. Virtually all early and contemporary love theorists agree that love is
intricately associated with the quality of human life, that different varieties of
love exist, and that at a minimum, there are two commonly experienced types
of adult romantic love—a passionate variety that is intense, emotional, fragile,
and sexually charged, and a companionate variety that is durable, stable, and
infused with warmth, intimacy, affection, and trust. These theoretical sup-
positions are largely supported by empirical research on people’s implicit
conceptions of love and self-reports of ongoing experiences in love relation-
ships. Of course, it is important to recognize that other types of love also exist
and are experienced by men and women over the course of their lifetimes,
ranging from the vague liking felt for casual acquaintances to the intense
devotion often experienced for family members, children, and beloved pets.
An important task for future researchers is to determine the unique features
and consequences of these other important varieties of love.
REFERENCES
Aron, A., & Henkemeyer, L. (1995). Marital satisfaction and passionate love.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12, 139–146.
Baumeister, R. F., Wotman, S. R., & Stillwell, A. M. (1993). Unrequited love:
On heartbreak, anger, guilt, scriptlessness, and humiliation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 377–394.
Berscheid, E. (1988). Some comments on love’s anatomy: Or, whatever happened
to old-fashioned lust? In R. J. Sternberg & M. L. Barnes (Eds.), The
psychology of love (pp. 359–374). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Berscheid, E., & Meyers, S. A. (1996). A social categorical approach to a question
about love. Personal Relationships, 3, 19–43.
Berscheid, E., & Regan, P. (2005). The psychology of interpersonal relationships.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1974). A little bit about love. In T. L. Huston (Ed.),
Foundations of interpersonal attraction (pp. 355–381). New York: Academic
Press.
110 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Murstein, B. I., Merighi, J. R., & Vyse, S. A. (1991). Love styles in the United
States and France: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 10, 37–46.
Regan, P. C. (1998). Of lust and love: Beliefs about the role of sexual desire in
romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 5, 139–157.
Regan, P. C. (2000). The role of sexual desire and sexual activity in dating
relationships. Social Behavior and Personality, 28, 51–60.
Regan, P. C., & Berscheid, E. (1999). Lust: What we know about human sexual
desire. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Regan, P. C., Kocan, E. R., & Whitlock, T. (1998). Ain’t love grand! A pro-
totype analysis of romantic love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
15, 411–420.
Reik, T. (1944). A psychologist looks at love. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.
Reik, T. (1945). Psychology of sex relations. New York: Grove Press.
Reiss, I. L. (1964). The scaling of premarital sexual permissiveness. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 26, 188–198.
Ridge, R. D., & Berscheid, E. (1989, May). On loving and being in love: A necessary
distinction. Paper presented at the annual convention of the Midwestern
Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories.
In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language
(pp. 111–144). New York: Academic Press.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 104, 192–233.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.),
Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rotenberg, K. J., & Korol, S. (1995). The role of loneliness and gender in in-
dividuals’ love styles. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 537–546.
Rubin, Z. (1970). Measurement of romantic love. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 16, 265–273.
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowl-
edge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.
Simpson, J. A., Campbell, B., & Berscheid, E. (1986). The association between
romantic love and marriage: Kephart (1967) twice revisited. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 363–372.
Sprecher, S., Aron, A., Hatfield, E., Cortese, A., Potapova, E., & Levitskaya, A.
(1994). Love: American style, Russian style, and Japanese style. Personal
Relationships, 1, 349–369.
Sprecher, S., McKinney, K., Walsh, R., & Anderson, C. (1988). A revision of the
Reiss premarital sexual permissiveness scale. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 50, 821–828.
Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. C. (1998). Passionate and companionate love in
courting and young married couples. Sociological Inquiry, 68, 163–185.
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93, 119–135.
Love 113
coworkers; and although you do not follow him around at work (hopefully!),
you have never noticed him to have a ‘‘wandering eye’’ for any of the new and
attractive women who happen to come to your place of employment. What
are you to conclude? A classic case of a person with lifelong low sexual desire;
perhaps you might even consider him to be an ‘‘asexual?’’ But wait. A rea-
sonable alternative to this conclusion (at least if you are not blind to human
diversity) is that he is gay and effectively hides his interest in, and sexual
activity with, men from his coworkers. Another alternative is that he does have
sexual desires for women, but he is very shy and withdrawn; thus, he may have
an active fantasy and masturbation life but does not express this desire within
the context of his public life; still another alternative is that he has some level of
desire for women, but that this sexual desire is secondary to a strong preference
for some illegal activity (e.g., voyeurism) or a nonhuman object (e.g., a fetish),
and he again expresses these desires in a very private manner.
How can we incorporate all of these alternatives, along with, of course,
the expression of the sexual desires that often occur in standard heterosexual
relationships? Kaplan (1995) argues that sexual desire is ‘‘an interest in sexual
activity, leading the individual to seek out sexual activity or to be pleasurably
receptive to it.’’ Note that in this definition there is no assumption about the
gender of the partner(s); in fact, there is no assumption that a partner is even
necessary (it could be fantasy or even masturbation). Our own definition of
sexual desire is similar to Kaplan’s, and includes one or more of the following
aspects: (1) the interest in (or being pleasurably receptive to) stimulation of
one’s own genitals and sexual release (e.g., orgasm); (2) the interest in (or being
pleasurably receptive to) genital/sexual contact with another; and (3) interest
in (or being pleasurably receptive to) thinking about, seeing, approaching, and
touching one’s preferred sexual partners and/or objects (e.g., a partner’s
genitals). Note that in our definition, we specify what is ‘‘sexual’’ in sexual
desire (i.e., genital stimulation/contact); in Kaplan’s definition, it is implied.
Keep these definitions in mind when you read the remainder of the chapter.
Aside from these definitions, it is also important to draw a distinction at
this point between desire and another aspect of sexual functioning, arousal.
Sexual arousal has to do with the physical changes that occur during sexual
activity, such as an erection and vaginal lubrication. Often, desire for sex (e.g.,
feelings of ‘‘horniness’’) comes before and leads to physical stimulation and
arousal. For example, let’s say Sally has been feeling sexual desire (‘‘horny’’) all
day, and when she goes home, she masturbates, arousing herself (e.g., vaginal
lubrication) until she has an orgasm. However, one’s subjective desire for sex
can also occur (or at least intensify) once physical stimulation and arousal has
occurred. For example, Marcia has been feeling tired and not very sexy all day.
When she goes home after work, her boyfriend, Ted, starts to massage her
body, including her breasts and labia, she becomes physically aroused and her
desire for sex intensifies; soon they begin to engage in passionate lovemaking.
Recent research indicates that it is common for people, particularly women, to
Sexual Desire Issues and Problems 117
indicate that desire and arousal aspects of their sexuality overlap (Graham,
Sanders, Mihausen, & McBride, 2004) and that—as the example with Marcia
indicates—sexual desire sometimes occurs during or even after arousal. On the
other hand, although very related and often reinforcing, it is important to note
that one can feel desire without physical arousal, and one can feel arousal
without desire. Furthermore, sexual desire disorders may not necessarily ac-
company sexual arousal problems (e.g., erectile problems) and vice versa.
Sexual desire issues (and not arousal per se) will be the topic of this chapter.
midbrain and hippocampus, which contain hormone receptor sites, but most
of the relevant areas seem to be concentrated at the base of the brain (e.g.,
hypothalamus; see Chapter 2 in this volume).
A minimal level of testosterone is important to sexual interest, but it is
important to keep in mind that sexual desire in humans is also a function of
numerous psychosocial factors, including learning (memories), fantasies, and
the quality of one’s relationships. Thus, higher brain areas and the social
context, not just testosterone and basic areas of the brain, are important.
One psychosocial factor that may be important in affecting desire is one’s
learning or conditioning history with regard to sex (see Ågmo, Turi, Ellingsen,
& Kaspersen, 2004). Some people have had a history of sexually positive
experiences, and this would likely maintain, or even increase, one’s sexual
interest. On the other hand, some people may have had primarily neutral or
even negative experiences, which may serve to decrease sexual interest. What
are these ‘‘positive’’ or ‘‘negative’’ (or ‘‘neutral’’) experiences? The most rel-
evant positive experience is sexual pleasure, including orgasm. The neutral
experiences would be a lack of pleasure (e.g., no orgasm), and the negative
experiences would include boredom, fear, anxiety, and perhaps even pain.
From a learning perspective, whether we have positive, neutral, or negative
experiences during sex has consequences. Specifically, the more we have
positive experiences (e.g., orgasm), the stronger the association or condi-
tioning between these positive, rewarding experiences and the stimuli/context
that brings it about. It should also increase the incentive or motivation to seek
out those stimuli or contexts in which the reward takes place. So, let us say that
Wendy has been masturbating (and having orgasms) since age 15. During
college, she was sexually active with a number of boyfriends, and then later,
with her husband, Mark. She typically has had orgasms with all of her partners
including Mark. Now, at 45, she is still strongly interested in sex, and desires it
regularly. From a learning perspective, this may be because she has learned to
have positive associations/memories to sex throughout her adolescence and
adulthood, and these positive associations/memories maintain her incentive to
engage in it regularly.
Is there any research support for this perspective? There is, although most
of it is indirect. First, orgasm frequency has been found to be an important
factor in sexual behavior and motivation (Bentler & Peeler, 1979; Arafat &
Cotton, 1974). For example, those activities in women that most consistently
induce an orgasm (e.g., cunnilingus) are rated as the most satisfying (Hurlbert,
Apt, & Rabehl, 1993). Second, because women are less likely than men to
have an orgasm consistently, one might expect that more women than men
should be diagnosed with low sexual desire. This is in fact the case (also see
‘‘Gender Differences in Sexual Desire,’’ below). Third, animal models of
sexual desire have demonstrated that in rats injected with naloxone, which
prevents a positive affective state (reward), sexual behavior takes on aversive
properties (Ågmo et al., 2004). Fourth, there is some evidence that people
Sexual Desire Issues and Problems 119
who have had sex early and more frequently in their lives (e.g., in adolescence
and young adulthood) are the ones most likely to continue to engage in it later
on in life (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994). Although alter-
native explanations may account for this (e.g., a person may have had a strong
sex drive in the first place), one plausible explanation is that these people have
built up a reward history with regard to sex and now the incentives and
motivations for sex continue throughout their lives.
Sexual fantasies are another factor affecting sexual desire. Although they
are difficult to define, sexual fantasies or daydreams are considered acts of the
imagination; thoughts that are not simply orienting responses to external
stimuli or immediately directed at solving a problem or working on a task
(Leitenberg & Henning, 1995). They can be realistic or bizarre, elaborate or
fleeting, and can result from memories or be entirely made up. Sexual fantasies
can occur spontaneously or intentionally, and can be provoked by other
thoughts, feelings, and sensory cues. They can take place during sexual ac-
tivity, or outside of it, often during masturbation. In short, the term ‘‘sexual
fantasy’’ refers to almost any mental imagery that is sexually arousing or erotic
to the individual (Leitenberg & Henning).
Factor analysis has revealed that the majority of sexual fantasies fall into
one of four basic categories:
The first category is by far the most common. The content of sexual fantasies
in gay men and lesbian women tends to be the same as in their heterosexual
counterparts, except that homosexuals imagine same-sex partners rather than
opposite-sex partners (Leitenberg & Henning, 1995).
Although some traditional approaches (e.g., classical psychoanalysis) have
advocated negative views about sexual fantasies, it is now often considered a
sign of pathology not to have sexual fantasies rather than to have them (Lei-
tenberg & Henning, 1995). For instance, infrequent sexual fantasy is one of the
defining criteria for hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD), described in
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric
Association, DSM-IV, 1995). In addition, a positive association has been found
between sexual fantasy frequency and orgasmic frequency during intercourse,
for both men and women (Arndt, Foehl, & Good, 1985; Epstein & Smith,
120 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
1957; Lentz & Zeiss, 1983). The experience of sexual fantasy is also positively
associated with sexual arousability (Stock & Greer, 1982). Considering these
findings, it is not surprising that individuals with sexual desire disorders are
often encouraged by sex therapists to use sexual fantasies during masturbation,
intercourse, and even nonsexual activities (Leitenberg & Henning, 1995).
Research has also shown that frequency of sexual fantasy is either positively
correlated with ratings of general satisfaction (particularly in women), or un-
related to sexual satisfaction. In addition, in contrast to the popular belief that
sexual deprivation leads to more sexual thoughts, those with the most active
sex lives seem to have the most sexual fantasies (Crepault, Abraham, Porton, &
Couture, 1976). Thus, sexual fantasy is generally considered a normal and
healthy part of one’s sexuality.
Relationship factors are also likely important in sexual desire. For ex-
ample, have you ever started a sexual relationship with someone and you just
cannot get enough of them (at least, sexually speaking)? For some reason, those
‘‘horny’’ feelings just seem to stay with you, and all you want to do is stay in
bed with them (the only interruption being room service). Of course, this
intensity of sexual desire usually fades, despite the affection and love that may
persist and begin to grow for a partner over time. Thus, on the other side of
novelty is familiarity and boredom, and these may dampen one’s sexual desires.
Research supports this view: sexual activity typically decreases for couples over
time. For example, data from a well-conducted American study indicates that
married couples in their forties have intercourse, on average, about 1.3 times
per week, whereas married couples in their twenties have intercourse, on
average, 2.2 times per week (Laumann et al., 1994). Part of this decline may be
an age factor (e.g., declining health), but a large part of it probably has to do
with ‘‘habituation,’’ the tendency to lose interest in one’s partner with in-
creasing familiarity. For example, this habituation explanation makes sense of
recent findings suggesting that there is usually a sharp decline after the first year
of a relationship and then a slow and fairly steady decline thereafter (Call,
Sprecher, & Schwartz, 1995). This trend is probably best explained by
habituation/boredom rather than age and declining health.
Aside from novelty factors in a relationship, there are other relationship
issues that can increase or decrease desire. Physical attractiveness may be one of
them. Those who perceive themselves and/or their partners to be physically
attractive may have more interest in, and desire for, sex. The role of one’s
partner’s attractiveness in increasing desire may be obvious because the more
attractive the partner, the more desirable sex with that partner may be.
However, the role of one’s own (perceived) sexual attractiveness is also likely
to be important, particularly for women (see Ackard, Kearney-Cooke, &
Peterson, 2000). This is partly because women, relative to men, place a higher
value on their ability to attract and turn on a partner with their own beauty/
sexiness. Indeed, part of the ‘‘turn on’’ for a woman in sexual situations may be
the knowledge that she is beautiful and sexy in her partner’s eyes. Thus, if a
Sexual Desire Issues and Problems 121
woman perceives (rightly or wrongly) that she does not possess this ability, her
desire to put herself into sexual situations may be low. If, however, she be-
lieves that her body is very sexy and beautiful to her partner(s), then her desire
for sexual situations may be high. For example, Holly likes it when men look
at her body appreciatively, and when she has sexual fantasies, she often begins
them by imagining herself wearing underwear (e.g., a teddy and a lace bra) that
she believes will turn on her partners. When interacting with her boyfriend,
Luke, her desire is similar: she likes it when he tells her how beautiful and sexy
she is, and her interest for sex is usually increased by wearing underwear that
she knows will turn him on. In contrast, Maria has a very negative image of
her own body and does not believe that men like looking at her. She also
believes that her body is unattractive to her husband, Jack, and that there is
little that she can do to increase her attractiveness in his eyes. Consequently,
she does not desire sex and tends to avoid sexual thoughts and situations.
Another relationship issue is satisfaction and conflict. The more a rela-
tionship is mutually satisfying with a high degree of intimacy, the more the
couple may have desire for sex. Conversely, frequent arguments, anger, and
resentment may lead to a lack of sexual interest in one’s partner. After all, it
is hard to have desire for someone who you currently disdain! Also, such
conflict and distress may work to diminish sexual desire indirectly by causing
anxiety and/or depression in a partner, which has been shown to be negatively
related to sexual desire (van Minnen & Kampman, 2000; Trudel, 1991).
and ICD-II (Vroege, Gijs, & Hengeveld, 1998). One of the reasons that it is
not a diagnosable disorder is because hyperactive sexual desire disorder often
accompanies paraphilias (e.g., fetishes, exhibitionism). However, this is not
always the case. Thus, one argument in favor of including hyperactive sexual
desire disorder as a diagnosable problem is that it may be associated with
nonparaphilic activities (e.g., masturbation or sexual activity with a partner)
and may be a source of significant distress for the individual. Given that
hyperactive sexual desire is not yet a diagnosable phenomenon, this chapter
will primarily address issues of low sexual desire.
TREATMENT
Can low sexual desire be treated? Some degree of success has been re-
ported in the literature. Examples of treatment strategies, along with the ef-
ficacy of these treatments, are presented below.
Hawton, Catalan, and Fagg (1991) used an intervention to treat low desire
problems in women based on Masters and Johnson’s (1970) classic approach
to sex therapy. Masters and Johnson’s approach to sex therapy was couple-
oriented (i.e., must have a partner or a surrogate) and used an intensive two-
week program with different techniques and homework assignments. One
such technique was sensate focus, a technique still widely used today by many
different therapists for a variety of sexual dysfunctions. While in the nude,
partners take turns giving and receiving pleasurable stimulation to nongenital
(e.g., face, back, belly) areas of the body. Because touching the genitals is off
limits (at least initially), the sensate focus approach is meant to decrease the
anxiety that may accompany sexual performance issues. Hawton et al. (1991)
reported a high level of success in treating low desire in women using Masters
and Johnson’s approach, but their report should be viewed cautiously because
they did not include an adequate control group.
128 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
intrinsic desire) problem (Stoléru et al., 2003), then one might expect that
testosterone, which may be more associated with intrinsic desire issues, may
only be modestly effective. Indeed, although administering higher-than-
typical levels of this hormone did increase desire in the HSDD participants,
they did not exhibit lower than typical levels of testosterone prior to treatment.
There is recent research on a drug known as buproprion in the treatment
of low desire in women with HSDD. This drug is not a hormone but a
chemical that affects neurotransmitters in the brain (dopamine, noradrenaline)
thought to be important in sexual functioning. Evidence exists that this drug
does indeed increase sexual desire in a substantial minority of women with
HSDD (Segraves et al., 2001); although it is important to keep in mind that
the majority did not respond. Interestingly, there is also evidence that bu-
proprion also increases sexual desire in people with depression and other
conditions that may relate to low sexual desire (Modell, Katholi, Modell, &
Depalma, 1997). Some other chemicals may have an indirect effect on sexual
desire through increasing physical arousal, including Viagra-like drugs and
polyphenolics (Kang, Park, Hwang, Kim, Lee, & Shin, 2003). Polyphenolics
are chemicals derived from plants and have a high concentration in certain
foods (e.g., red wine). They seem to have positive (anti-oxidant) effects on the
cardiovascular system and increase blood flow in certain areas of the body
including the pelvic region. The use of polyphenolics has just begun within the
context of sexual problems, and much more research, including with control
groups, is necessary.
Another treatment is the use of certain behaviors that increase the like-
lihood of orgasm (i.e., orgasm consistency treatment). Note that this treatment
is the only one recognized by the American Psychological Association as being
efficacious (Chambless et al., 1998). This therapy is meant to build up a history
of rewarding experiences (i.e., orgasm) with sex and thus to increase the
incentives and interest in it. Usually, this begins by directed (or guided)
masturbation and then later, by a coital alignment technique. This latter
technique makes it more likely that an orgasm will occur because it entails
adjustment of the position of the partners so that thrusting leads to more direct
clitoral stimulation. There have been some reports of success with this
treatment, at least in women (Hurlbert, 1993; Hurlbert, Apt, Rabehl, 1993;
Hurlbert, White, Powell, & Apt, 1993; LoPiccolo & Stock, 1986; Pierce,
2000). However, although these results seem promising, it is unclear whether
proper control groups were included in these studies as well.
In summary, a number of different approaches have been used to treat low
sexual desire, with some degree of success, although more research is needed
(e.g., more controlled studies). It is also important to note that low sexual desire
issues have been considered difficult sexual dysfunctions to treat, and they will
probably remain a challenge for therapists. Furthermore, not all people with
low desire necessarily want treatment (e.g., are not distressed about it). Some
of these individuals with low or absent desire, particularly if it is a lifelong
130 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
phenomenon, may in fact feel that they have a separate sexual identity, unique
from the three traditional sexual orientations (heterosexuality, homosexuality,
bisexuality) routinely discussed in the sexuality literature. This unique fourth
identity is called ‘‘asexuality.’’ Research on asexuality is just beginning (Bogaert,
2004). Thus, we clearly need more research on low desire issues, along with
potentially related phenomena such as asexuality, in the future.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Re-
search Council of Canada Grant 410–2003–0943 to Anthony F. Bogaert
(tbogaert@brocku.ca; 905-688-5550, ext. 4085). The authors wish to thank
Carolyn Hafer and Luanne Jamieson for their help at various stages of this
research.
REFERENCES
Ackard, D. M., Kearney-Cooke, A., & Peterson, C. (2000). Effect of body image
and self-image on women’s sexual behaviors. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 28, 422–429.
Ågmo, A., Turi, A. L., Ellingsen, E., & Kaspersen, H. (2004). Preclinical models
of sexual desire: Conceptual and behavioral analyses. Pharmacology, Bio-
chemistry, & Behavior, 78, 379–404.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1995). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Arafat, I., & Cotton, W. (1974). Masturbation practices of males and females.
Journal of Sex Research, 10, 293–307.
Arnal, A., Llario, R., & Gil, M. D. (1995). Estudio epidemiológico sobre la
pravalencia de disfunciones sexuales en la comunidad valenciana [Epide-
miological studies on the prevalence of sexual dysfunctions in a commu-
nity of Valencia]. Psicothema, 7, 95–104.
Arndt, W., Foehl, J., & Good, F. (1985). Specific sexual fantasy themes: A
multidimensional study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
472–480.
Baumeister, R., Catanese, K., & Vohs, K. (2001). Is there a gender difference in
strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a
review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,
242–273.
Bentler, P. M., & Peeler, W. H. (1979). Models of female orgasm. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 8, 405–423.
Sexual Desire Issues and Problems 131
van Anders, S. M., Chernick, A. B., Hampson, E., & Fisher, W. A. (2005).
Preliminary clinical experience with androgen administration for pre- and
postmenopausal women with hypoactive sexual desire. Journal of Sex and
Marital Therapy, 31, 173–187.
van Minnen, A., & Kampman, M. (2000). The interaction between anxiety and
sexual functioning: A controlled study of sexual functioning in women
with anxiety disorders. Sex and Relationship Therapy, 15, 47–57.
Ventegodt, S. (1998). Sex and quality of life in Denmark. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 28, 295–307.
Vroege, J. A., Gijs, L., & Hengeveld, M. W. (1998). Classification of sexual
dysfunctions: Towards DSM-V and ICD-11. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39,
333–337.
Warnock, J. K. (2002). Female hypoactive sexual desire disorder. CNS Drugs, 16,
745–753.
World Health Organization. (1992). International statistical classifications of diseases
and related health problems (1989 revision). Geneva: Author.
7
Greg A. R. Febbraro 1
This chapter will provide an overview of the sexual arousal disorders: female
sexual arousal disorder (FSAD) and male erectile disorder (MED). The diag-
nostic features of FSAD and MED as well as their potential causes will be
discussed. In addition, current available treatments for FSAD and MED will
be briefly reviewed. Issues for future consideration will also be raised.
Both FSAD and MED are classified under the ‘‘sexual dysfunctions’’
category in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, fourth edition, text revision
(DSM-IV-TR). The DSM-IV-TR published by the American Psychiatric
Association is the primary classification system used by mental health profes-
sionals in North America. Sexual dysfunctions are persistent and recurrent
problems in the appetitive, excitement, and orgasm phases of the human sexual
cycle. Dysfunctions are either psychological or psychophysiological in nature.
Sexual arousal disorders are sexual problems that occur during the excitement
phase and that relate to difficulties with feelings of sexual pleasure or with the
physiological changes associated with sexual excitement (APA, 2000; Sue, Sue,
& Sue, 2003). The diagnostic and associated features of FSAD and MED will
now be discussed.
ways. For example, someone with FSAD may engage in infrequent sexual
activity or only certain types of sexual activity. The impact of sexual avoidance
on relationships is discussed later in this chapter, when reviewing interpersonal
causal factors for sexual arousal disorders.
Prevalence rates for FSAD have been highly variable. More recent re-
search indicates that between 10 percent and 20 percent of women experience
the disorder over the course of a lifetime. Difficulties with lubrication itself
were reported among 19 percent of women in the NHSLS study (Laumann,
Paik, & Rosen, 1999). There are a couple of potential reasons for the vari-
ability in the estimates of FSAD. First, FSAD often co-occurs with sexual
desire disorders (e.g., hypoactive sexual desire disorder) and orgasmic disorders
(e.g., female orgasmic disorder), making it difficult to differentiate from those
disorders (APA, 2000; Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Lo-
Piccolo, 1997). Second, some women with FSAD may have little or no
subjective sense of sexual arousal, making its diagnosis difficult.
injuries) (APA, 2000; Hyde & DeLamater, 2006). MED often co-occurs with
other sexual disorders, particularly hypoactive sexual desire and premature
ejaculation disorders. Furthermore, individuals with mood disorders, such
as depression, and substance-related disorders, like alcoholism, often report
problems with sexual arousal (APA, 2000).
In the past, MED has been attributed primarily to psychological factors.
For example, in their pioneering work on human sexuality, William Masters
and Virginia Johnson estimated that only 5 percent of erectile dysfunctions
were due to physical conditions (Masters & Johnson, 1970). However, more
recent studies indicate that from 30 percent to 70 percent of erectile dys-
functions are caused by some form of vascular insufficiency, diabetes, ath-
erosclerosis, traumatic groin injury, or other physiological factors (Hooper,
1998; Segraves, Schoenberg, & Ivanoff, 1983).
According to the NHSLS study, approximately 10 percent of men have
experienced an erection problem within the past twelve months (Laumann,
Paik, & Rosen, 1999). This statistic varies by age with only 7 percent of 18- to
29-year-olds experiencing erection problems, but 18 percent of 50 to 59-year-
olds and 39 percent of men who were 60 years and older experiencing such
problems. Similar rates for erection problems have been found in Germany and
France (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006). Problems with erection are one of the most
embarrassing ones many men can imagine or experience. In addition, de-
pression may result from repeated experiences of erection problems. Further-
more, erection problems can also be a cause of concern for the male’s partner.
Biological Causes
As previously mentioned, Masters and Johnson (1970) had speculated that
the large majority of sexual disorders were psychological in nature. However,
the potential role of biological factors is increasingly being recognized (Rosen
& Leiblum, 1995). Biological factors that explain the development of sexual
disorders such as FSAD and MED include organic factors (e.g., diseases, in-
juries) and drugs. With regard to MED, approximately 50 percent or more of
cases may result from organic factors or a combination of organic and other
factors such as psychological factors (Buvat et al., 1990; Richardson, 1993).
Heart and circulatory diseases are often associated with MED as erections
themselves depend on the circulatory system ( Jackson, 1999). Troubles in
the vascular system can create erection problems as the ‘‘production’’ of an
Sexual Arousal Disorders 139
erection depends upon having a large amount of blood flowing into the penis
by way of the arteries, with simultaneous constricting of the veins so that the
blood cannot flow out as rapidly as it is coming in. Damage to these arteries or
veins can result in MED (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006). In women, vascular
disease associated with diabetes can lead to FSAD (Phillips, 2000).
MED is also associated with diseases like diabetes mellitus and kidney
problems. Diabetes, for example, can cause circulation problems and peripheral
nerve damage, both impacting the ability to produce erections. Some studies
have found that 28 percent of men with diabetes have erectile disorders, making
it one of the most common medical causes (de Tejada et al., 2005). Hypogo-
nadism, a condition characterized by the underfunctioning of the testes resulting
in low testosterone levels, is also associated with MED (Morales & Heaton,
2001). MED is also associated with hyperprolactinemia, a condition characterized
by the excessive production of prolactin ( Johri, Heaton, & Morales, 2001).
Any injury causing damage to the lower part of the spinal cord may cause
MED, since that is where the erection reflex center is located. MED may also
result from some types of prostate surgery, although this is not a common
phenomenon (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006).
Prior pelvic trauma, such as injury sustained during childbirth, can result
in FSAD. In addition, urogenital atrophy (shrinkage of genital and urinary
tissues) in menopausal and postmenopausal women can lead to FSAD (Phillips,
2000; Goldstein, 2000).
Drugs can also cause arousal and erectile problems. Examples of such drugs
decreasing sexual arousal and causing erection problems include certain anti-
hypertensive medications, certain antidepressants, overuse of alcohol, and the
use of illicit substances like heroin and marijuana. Furthermore, long-term use
of nicotine can also cause erection problems (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006).
FSAD can also result from the intake of antihistamines and hypertensive
medications. Furthermore, antidepressants, such as selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors (SSRIs) and tricyclics (TCAs), can result in FSAD (Millner, 2005).
Psychological Causes
Psychological causes of sexual disorders are often categorized into im-
mediate causes and prior learning. Immediate causes refer to problems oc-
curring during intimacy that inhibit the sexual response. Prior learning refers
to any beliefs or responses that people have learned or experienced earlier in
life, that now affect their sexual response. Therefore, anxiety/fear can result in
a vicious cycle, impacting sexual responding.
Prior Learning
Prior learning as a psychological cause of sexual disorders may be due to
various things learned or experienced during childhood, adolescence, or even
adulthood. In some cases of sexual disorders, the person’s first sexual act was
traumatic. Child sexual abuse is one of the most serious of the traumatic early
experiences that lead to later sexual disorders such as FSAD. A history of sexual
abuse is frequently reported by women seeking therapy for problems with
sexual desire, arousal, or aversion (Leonard & Follette, 2002). Similar findings
emerge for men with desire or arousal problems (Wyatt, Chin, & Asuan-
O’Brien, 2002; McCarthy, 1990).
Cultural or societal factors may also contribute to the development of sexual
problems. For example, growing up in a very strict religious family where sex
is viewed as being dirty and sinful can play a factor in the development of
142 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Interpersonal Causes
Problems in a couple’s relationship are another leading cause of sexual
disorders. Anger or resentment toward one’s partner does not create an op-
timal environment for sexual satisfaction. Furthermore, in relationships in
which there is anger or resentment, sex can be used as a weapon by one or
both partners to psychologically hurt the other (e.g., by refusing to engage
in any sexual behavior or in a particular sexual behavior the other desires). In
addition, struggle for power in a relationship may add to sexual problems
(Hyde & DeLamater, 2006).
Problems with intimacy are often an issue in relationships, which results in
sexual disorders. Intimacy involves becoming emotionally close to one an-
other. This may or may not involve physical contact. Intimacy problems
typically represent a combination of individual psychological factors and re-
lationship problems. Some individuals in relationships may enjoy the sex in the
relationship but fear becoming intimate with their partner. Intimacy often
involves allowing oneself to be emotionally vulnerable, and this is often very
frightening to some individuals. Individuals with intimacy problems may be
very good in the early stages of relationships but then lose interest or look for
reasons to end relationships once it appears that things are becoming more
Sexual Arousal Disorders 143
Behavior Therapy
The basic assumption of behavior therapy is that sexual problems are the
result of prior learning and that they are maintained by ongoing reinforce-
ments and punishment (immediate causes). Therefore, problematic behaviors
can be unlearned and replaced by more adaptive ones (Hyde & DeLamater,
2006). This is consistent with the basic principles of behaviorism and learning
theory, major influences in psychology.
A variety of different behavioral techniques have been used to treat sexual
problems. Historically, one of the key behavior therapy techniques has been
systematic desensitization. This behavioral technique involves first, teaching
individuals an incompatible behavior to anxiety/fear, usually muscle relaxation.
Individuals also construct a fear hierarchy consisting of a series of increasing
fearful situations. Once the fear hierarchy has been constructed, and individuals
have learned how to use muscle relaxation, they are then asked to imagine each
step of the fear hierarchy. When feeling anxious, they are asked to use their
relaxation skills. Individuals cannot progress to the next step of the fear hierarchy
until they have significantly reduced their fear to the preceding step (Hyde &
DeLamater, 2006).
Most behavior therapy programs are multidimensional in nature. They
include education about sexual anatomy and functioning, use of anxiety re-
duction techniques (e.g., systematic desensitization), structured behavioral
exercises, and communication training. Behavior therapy programs for erectile
dysfunction have had fairly good success rates. However, most of them have
144 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
had fairly high relapse rates as well. Therefore, most behavior therapy pro-
grams should include what are called ‘‘relapse prevention procedures,’’ should
problems reoccur (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006). Relapse prevention procedures
assist the individual in coping with setbacks.
Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy
Many sex therapists currently use a combination of behavioral strategies
and exercises and cognitive therapy. This is referred to as cognitive-behavioral
therapy (CBT). A key component of the cognitive approach to sex therapy is
cognitive restructuring (Hyde & DeLamater, 2006). In cognitive restructuring,
the therapist basically assists the client in restructuring his or her thought
patterns, helping them to become more positive and realistic about sexual
expectations. Often, negative attitudes are challenged, and individuals examine
realistic alternatives to negative attitudes. Cognitive restructuring was pre-
viously described when reviewing psychological causes of sexual arousal
disorders.
As for women with FSAD, they may have beliefs/attitudes about sexual
activity that are associated with shame or guilt. Several myths have been
identified that can create difficulties with sexual arousal in women. These
include: (1) women must not be sexual, (2) women’s responses to sex should
be similar to men’s responses, and (3) there are correct and incorrect ways to
become aroused. Furthermore, a woman’s negative beliefs or feelings about
the partner can create negative sexual experiences (Charlton & Brigel, 1997).
In CBT, the above myths would be challenged and modified.
Bibliotherapy
Bibliotherapy involves the use of written and other (e.g., use of videos)
materials in the treatment of psychological and physical problems. Biblio-
therapy materials typically describe how particular treatment methods are to be
implemented by the individual. It can function as a stand-alone treatment or as
a complement to ongoing therapy or medical care (van Lankveld, 1998).
To date, the vast majority of bibliotherapy approaches for sexual dys-
function have targeted orgasmic disorders. In addition, bibliotherapy ap-
proaches have predominantly used behavior therapy techniques pioneered by
Masters and Johnson, or variations of them. A recent statistical review of
bibliotherapy interventions targeting sexual dysfunctions conducted by Jacques
van Lankveld found bibliotherapy to be moderately effective at the end of
treatment. However, there was a relatively small effect at follow-up (van
Lankveld, 1998). van Lankveld noted that 87 percent of the studies reviewed
dealt with orgasmic disorders, thus limiting the generalizability of the findings.
More recently, bibliotherapy approaches have begun to use cognitive-
behavioral techniques in the treatment of sexual dysfunction. In 2001, Jacques
Sexual Arousal Disorders 145
van Lankveld, Walter Everaerd, and Yvonne Grotjohann published the only
cognitive-behaviorally-based BT study to date. In a randomized clinical trial,
couples were assigned to either the BT group or a waiting-list group. After
the ten-week BT treatment, participants (N ¼ 199 couples) reported fewer
complaints of low frequency of sexual interaction and general improvement of
sexual problems, and lower male posttreatment ratings of problem-associated
distress. Unlike previous studies, the above study targeted a wider variety of
sexual problems, including sexual arousal problems.
Couples Therapy
Due to the role that interpersonal factors may play in sexual disorders,
couples therapy is often used as part of an overall treatment strategy when
relevant. Couples therapy assumes that the relationship difficulties between
two people can cause sexual problems. Therefore, couple/relationship issues
need to be addressed in order for sexual problems to improve. Couples therapy
has often been combined with cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment
of MED. A multicomponent treatment for MED developed by Raymond
Rosen et al. (1994) involves sexual and performance anxiety reduction; ed-
ucation and cognitive intervention; script assessment and modification; con-
flict resolution and relationship enhancement; and relapse prevention.
For women with FSAD, many experts recommend that couples therapy
should explore the type and quality of intimacy within a relationship. This
would include exploring the willingness of each partner to trust the other, the
ability of each partner to share self with the other, and fears of negative
evaluation by one or both partners (Gehring, 2003). This approach to couples
therapy is consistent with research findings that marital discord is often asso-
ciated with the four factors of criticism, stonewalling, nonverbal or verbal
expression of contempt, and defensiveness (Gottman, 1994).
Biomedical Therapies
There are an increasing number of biomedical treatments available for
individuals experiencing sexual arousal disorders. These include medication/
drug and surgical treatments, which will now be briefly reviewed.
Medication/Drug Treatments
The best-known medication currently is Viagra (sildenafil) released in 1998
for the treatment of erectile disorder. Viagra is taken orally before engaging in
sexual activity. Viagra does not directly produce an erection; however, when
males are stimulated sexually after taking Viagra, the drug facilitates the physi-
ological processes that produce erection. Viagra serves to relax the smooth
muscles in the corpora cavernosa, allowing blood flow in and creating an
146 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
(pre- and postmenopausal women between the ages of twenty-one and sixty-
five) diagnosed with FSAD. Participants were randomly assigned to either a
Femprox group (groups differed by strength of Femprox) or a placebo group.
Overall, participants in the Femprox group showed improvement in sexual
arousal over the course of treatment, compared to the placebo group. The
results of this preliminary study are promising for women with FSAD.
smaller when erect after the surgery and the experience of different sensations
during both arousal and ejaculation (Steege, Stout, & Carson, 1986).
Another surgical therapy technique involves the implantation of a semi-
rigid, silicone-like rod into the penis (Melman & Tiefer, 1992; Shandera &
Thompson, 1994). This technique has fewer complications and is less costly
than a penile prosthesis (Rosen & Leiblum, 1995).
With regard to FSAD, and female sexual dysfunction in general, a medical
device available is the EROS-Clitoral Therapy Device. The device is designed
to increase blood flow to the clitoris, and provides a vacuum suction to the
clitoris in one of three levels of intensity. Several studies have provided pre-
liminary evidence for the physiological effectiveness of the EROS device in
women with FSAD (Billups et al., 2001; Munarriz, Maitland, Garcia, &
Goldstein, 2003; Wilson, Delk, & Billups, 2001).
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Sexual Arousal Disorders 149
Araujo, A., et al. (1998). The relationship between depressive symptoms and male
erectile dysfunction: Prospective results from the Massachusetts Male
Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 152, 533–541.
Back, A. K., Wincze, J. P., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). Sexual dysfunction. In D. H.
Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders (3rd ed.). New
York: Guilford.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction: The role of anxiety and cog-
nitive interference. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 140–148.
Billups, K. L., Berman, L., Berman, J., Metz, M. E., Glennon, M. E., & Gold-
stein, L. (2001). A new nonpharmacological vacuum therapy for female
sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 27, 435–441.
Brock, G. B., McMahon, C. G., Chen, K. K., Costigan, T., Shen, W., Watkins, V.,
et al. (2002). Efficacy and safety of Tadalafil in men with erectile dys-
function: An integrated analysis of registration trials. Journal of Urology, 168
(178 Suppl. S.), 1332–1336.
Buvat, J., Buvat-Herbaut, M., Lemaire, A., Marcolin, G., & Quittelier, E. (1990).
Recent developments in the clinical assessment and diagnosis of erectile
dysfunction. Annual Review of Sex Research, 1, 265–308.
Charlton, R. S., & Brigel, F. W. (1997). Treatment of arousal and orgasmic
disorders. In R. S. Charlton (Ed.), Treatment of sexual disorders (pp. 237–
280). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Female sexual arousal disorder, positive Femprox study results. Medical News
Today. Retrieved on May 25, 2005, from www.medicalnewstoday.com/
medicalnews.php?newsid¼24977
Gehring, D. (2003). Couple therapy for low sexual desire: A systematic approach.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 29, 25–38.
Goldstein, I. (2000). Female sexual arousal disorder: New insights. International
Journal of Impotence Research, 12(Suppl. 2), S152–S157.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Heaton, J. P. W. (2001). Key issues from the clinical trials of apomorphine SL.
World Journal of Urology, 19, 25–31.
Hellstrom, W. J., Gittelman, M., Karlin, G., Segerson, T., Thibonnier, M.,
Taylor, T., et al. (2003a). Sustained efficacy and tolerability of vardenafil, a
highly potent selective phosphodiestase type 5 inhibitor, in men with
erectile dysfunction: Results of a randomized double-bind, 26-week
placebo-controlled pivotal trial. Urology, 61(4 Suppl. 1), 8–14.
Hellstrom, W. J., Overstreet, J. W., Yu, A., Saikali, K., Shen, W., Beasley, Jr., M.,
et al. (2003b). Tadalafil has no detrimental effect on human spermato-
genesis or reproductive hormones. Journal of Urology, 170, 887–891.
Hooper, J. (1998, August). Science in the sack: Beyond Viagra. Health and Fitness,
pp. 108–113.
Hyde, J. S., & DeLamater, J. D. (2006). Understanding human sexuality (9th ed.).
New York: McGraw Hill.
Jackson, G. (1999). Erectile dysfunction and cardiovascular disease. International
Journal of Clinical Practice, 53, 363–368.
150 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Johri, A., Heaton, J., & Morales, A. (2001). Severe erectile dysfunction is a marker for
hyperprolactinemia. International Journal of Impotence Research, 13, 176–182.
Kabalin, J. N., & Kuo, J. C. (1997). Long-term follow-up of and patient satis-
faction with the Dynaflex self-contained inflatable penile prosthesis. Journal
of Urology, 158, 456–469.
Laan, E., Everaerd, W., van Aanhold, M., & Rebel, M. (1993). Performance
demand and sexual arousal in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31,
25–35.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the
United States: Prevalence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281, 537–544.
Leonard, L. M., & Follette, V. M. (2002). Sex functioning in women reporting a
history of child sexual abuse: Clinical and empirical considerations. Annual
Review of Sex Research, 13, 346–388.
Levitt, E. E., & Mulcahy, J. J. (1995). The effect of intracavernosal injection of
papaverine hydrochloride on orgasm latency. Journal of Sex and Marital
Therapy, 21, 39–41.
LoPiccolo, J. (1997). Sex therapy: A postmodern model. In S. J. Lynn & J. P.
Garske (Eds.), Contemporary psychotherapies: Models and methods. New York:
Guilford.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. London:
Churchill.
McCarthy, B. W. (1990). Treating sexual dysfunction associated with prior sexual
trauma. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 16, 142–146.
Melman, A., & Tiefer, L. (1992). Surgery for erectile disorders: Operative pro-
cedures and psychological issues. In R. C. Rosen & S. R. Leiblum (Eds.),
Erectile disorders (pp. 255–282). New York: Guilford.
Millner, V. S. (2005). Female sexual arousal disorder and counseling deliberations.
Family Journal, 13(1), 95–100.
Montorsi, F., & Althof, S. (2004). Partner responses to sildenafil citrate (Viagra)
treatment of erectile dysfunction. Urology, 63, 762–767.
Morales, A., & Heaton, J. (2001). Hormonal erectile dysfunction: Evaluation and
management. Urologic Clinics of North America, 28, 279.
Munarriz, R., Maitland, S., Garcia, S. P., Talakoub, L., & Goldstein, I. (2003). A
prospective duplex Doppler ultrasonographic study in women with sexual
arousal disorder to objectively assess genital engorgement induced by
EROS therapy. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 29(1), 85–94.
Padma-Nathan, H., et al. (2001). On-demand IC351 (Cialis) enhances erectile
function in patients with erectile dysfunction. International Journal of Im-
potence Research, 13, 2–9.
Palace, E. M., & Gorzalka, B. B. (1990). The enhancing effects of anxiety on
arousal in sexually dysfunctional and functional women. Journal of Ab-
normal Psychology, 99, 403–411.
Sexual Arousal Disorders 151
Vaughn S. Millner 1
What could possibly go wrong with orgasm? Orgasm, considered by many to
be one of the most basic physiological functions and one of life’s greatest
pleasures, is an activity that can result in ecstasy, a child, a warm glow, or, at
the very least, a mild purr.
As a sex researcher, therapist, counselor, educator, and human sexuality
course instructor, I have had the opportunity to observe and hear reports from
clients, students, and colleagues about many misconceptions regarding the
natural phenomenon of orgasm. Consider the following examples. During a
gynecological exam, a physician asked his 22-year-old, recently married pa-
tient if she had orgasms. ‘‘I don’t know,’’ she replied. He quickly added,
‘‘Then you haven’t.’’ On another day, the physician asked his 19-year-old,
college student patient if he had engaged in sex yet. His patient honestly
responded that he had not yet ‘‘gotten involved.’’ Instead, had the physician
asked his male patient if he had experienced intercourse yet, he would have
received a different answer. Some college students believe they have not had
sex unless they have an orgasm; and some believe that if they had engaged in
anal intercourse, but not vaginal intercourse, then they have not had sex
(Randall & Byers, 2003). The bottom line is that there is much confusion in
North America about accurate information about sex, and imbedded in this
misinformation is uncertainty about orgasmic function.
In this chapter, I will describe orgasm as well as orgasmic problems and the
more serious orgasmic disorders. I also will ask readers to consider the context
154 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
of the sexual experience and recognize how aging does and does not play a
part in orgasmic satisfaction. In addition, readers will be provided a sampling of
treatment options for orgasmic problems.
WHAT IS ORGASM?
Orgasm is considered to be the culminating event in what is termed the
‘‘sexual response cycle.’’ Several versions of the sexual response cycle exist. Sex
research pioneers Masters and Johnson (1966) described the male’s and female’s
sexual response cycle in four parts: (1) the excitement phase, (2) the plateau
phase, (3) the orgasmic phase, and (4) the resolution phase (also includes the
refractory phase for males). This chapter focuses on the orgasmic phase; but it
should be remembered that other phases are both directly and indirectly linked
to the orgasmic phase. The first stage, excitement, refers to the body’s phys-
iological response to psychological and/or physical sexual stimulation. If the
individual maintains the excitement phase, the second, or plateau, phase is
entered. Sexual tension increases if the individual continues to find the sexual
stimuli exciting. The ultimate release of continuing tension results in orgasm,
contraction of the sex organs. During the orgasmic phase, involuntary climax
is reached, with sensations occurring for females primarily in the clitoris, va-
gina, and uterus. For men, orgasm includes sensations mainly in the penis,
prostate, and seminal vesicles. The final stage, resolution, is the inevitable loss
of tension. Added to this cycle for men is the refractory period, the time
during which the penis once again achieves tumescence. The refractory period
varies considerably among men; and its description has been contradicted by
some researchers, who rejoin that some men have multiple orgasms (Dunn &
Trost, 1989).
The ejaculation process (i.e., the emission and expulsion of the whitish
seminal fluid in men during the orgasm phase) is described by Masters and
Johnson (1966) to occur within two stages. The first stage includes the ex-
pulsion of seminal fluid into the prostatic urethra via contractions. In this stage,
the sphincter of the urinary bladder closes, or remains closed to prevent leakage
into the bladder, as well as to contain the urine in the bladder, thereby effec-
tively eliminating the possibility of the mixture of the urine and seminal fluid.
In the second stage, the seminal fluid progresses from the prostatic urethra to
the urethral opening. Subsequently, ejaculatory contractions project seminal
fluid. Once males have begun ejaculatory contractions, the seminal fluid pro-
jections cannot be stopped, at least not in younger males. Not all older males
have a clear differentiation of the two stages, that is, there may be a projection of
the seminal fluid without the first stage’s clear caution of irreversibility. Perry
and Whipple (1981) studied female ejaculators and asserted that the two-phase
ejaculatory process is as appropriate for women as it is for men.
In contrast to the Masters and Johnson (1966) model developed for both
men and women, Walsh and Wilson (1987) developed a normal sexual response
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 155
cycle for men and suggested five interrelated occurrences during the sexual
cycle: libido (desire), erection, ejaculation, orgasm, and detumescence. All of
these are related, of course, to orgasmic function. Kandeel, Koussa, and
Swerdloff (2001) describe the stages as follows: The first stage, desire, varies in
intensity and is influenced by many factors, including pharmacological agents
and erotica. Desire may also be influenced by elevated levels of testosterone in
older men, but not in younger men. Erection, the second stage, is the result of
multiple stimuli that impact neurological and vascular pathways that eventually
produce tumescence in the penis rigid enough for penetration. Tumescence
occurs as a result of increased blood flow to the penis. The third stage, ejac-
ulation, is a reflex, and results from action taking place in the sympathetic
nervous system. The next phase, orgasm, is influenced by both physical and
psychological factors. Orgasm involves contraction of the sex organs as well as
pressure release in the urethra, contractions of the pelvic floor muscles, ejac-
ulation, and, ultimately, release of tension. Orgasms, however, do not always
include ejaculation. Orgasm and ejaculation, although two interrelated events,
are separate physiological functions. Orgasm may occur with or without
emission or ejaculation, and conversely, ejaculation can occur with or without
orgasm (Kandeel et al., 2001; Wylie & Ralph, 2005). Finally, during the de-
tumescence stage, the penis relaxes to a flaccid condition. The blood is drained
away from the penis until, over time, it returns to the pretumescent level.
Overall, it is to be noted that orgasms for both men and women can occur
alone or with a partner of either sex. Therefore, they can occur through coitus,
oral sex, masturbation, or other means. In addition, orgasmic feelings can
occur within the genital and pelvic area or extend to other areas of the body
(Mah & Binik, 2002).
women view their changing bodies and to the extent that they feel attractive to
their partner. The greatest fear is that of harming the baby during sexual
activity (von Sydow, 1999).
Another age-related factor that affects sexual functioning via impact to the
uterus, hormones, and nerve endings is a hysterectomy. All women experience
hormone fluctuation or hormone decline after a hysterectomy. In addition,
as a result of the trauma on the body, both physically and psychologically,
women can experience difficulties achieving orgasm postsurgery, although
this is not an inevitable outcome (Sholty et al., 1984). Indeed, many women
express increased sexual satisfaction after a hysterectomy (Goetsch, 2005). What
is often missing from study analysis is the evaluation of women’s subjective
sexual experience, which may be the component that makes the difference
between an orgasm or lack thereof.
ever-increasing population, are living longer, and those who have been cured
of cancer often have residual long-lasting psychosocial and sexual needs that
formed during cancer treatment (Reuben, 2004). For example, consider sur-
gery for prostate cancer. Prostate cancer sometimes results in a radical prosta-
tectomy, removal of the prostate. This can result in erectile dysfunction. In
recent years, however, surgery has advanced to the extent that men often regain
their ability to engage in sexual intercourse within two years (Walsh, Marschke,
Ricker, & Burnett, 2000). Many men, nevertheless, maintain a sense of em-
barrassment or shame, which prevents them from seeking help. For those men
who express concern and are open to assistance, relief is available in the form of
medication, injections, or penile implants (Burnett, 2005). Recognition of
sexual needs during and after cancer treatment is an essential quality of life issue.
Another medical factor sometimes leading to orgasmic problems is injury
to the body. One of the most debilitating injuries as it relates to sexual activity
is a spinal cord injury (Charlifue, Gerhart, Menter, Whiteneck, & Manley,
1992; Sipski, Rosen, Alexander, & Gomez-Marin, 2004). The sympathetic
arousal mechanism impacted by spinal cord injuries has been shown to in-
fluence genital sensitivity (Sipski et al., 2004), and anxiety and negative body
image are often added to the physiological impairment. Such injuries do not
inevitably cause sexual dysfunction, however. For instance, reports indicate
that about one-half of women with spinal cord injuries report the ability to
achieve orgasm (Charlifue, Gerhart, Menter, Whiteneck, & Manley, 1992;
Sipski & Alexander, 1993). In some cases, research shows that for women who
have injuries at T6 and above, the ability to experience orgasm is not related to
their injuries (Sipski, Alexander, & Rosen, 1995); whereas those with total
disruption have a much more difficult experience. One expert in this area,
Sipski (2002), states that women with various types of spinal cord injuries can
experience the sensations associated with orgasm. Sipski urges women with
injuries to become more sexually self-aware, masturbate, and use mechanical
devices such as vibrators to help improve their sexual response.
Sexual functioning can also be hampered by substances such as alcohol. A
study by Johnson, Phelps, and Cottler (2004) found an association between
inhibited orgasm and marijuana and heavy alcohol use for both men and
women. Heavy drinking in this study was defined as seven or more drinks
every day for a period of two weeks or longer, or drinking heavily at least once
a week for a period of two months or longer. Drinking also interferes with
one’s ability to make wise sexual choices. Some mistakenly make decisions to
drink based on the intention that alcohol will reduce their inhibition and
enhance their sexual satisfaction. Instead, excessive drinking minimizes se-
lectivity of a partner and creates vulnerability to sexual aggression (Klassen &
Wilsnack, 1986).
Another substance-related contribution to orgasmic problems is medication
side effects. Medication serves multiple roles in its association with sexual func-
tioning. Although in some cases, pharmaceutical options such as sildenafil can
164 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
2. Attitudes. Women who expected little from their relationships were also those
women who reported an inability to reach orgasm. For men, erectile problems
resulted from being in an uncommitted relationship.
3. Health. Poor health for women was associated with sexual pain whereas poor
health for men was related to an increased risk of all sexual dysfunctions.
4. Lifestyle factors. For both women and men, low feelings of emotional satisfaction
were associated with more sexual dysfunction, but especially so for women. In
addition, infrequent sexual activity resulted in both lubrication and erectile
problems.
5. Sexual trauma. Both men and women who experienced sexual victimization
reported long-term problems in their sexual functioning. This is consistent with
others’ findings (Dennerstein, Guthrie, & Alford, 2004), wherein women who
had been abused were found to have fewer sexual activities than women
who had not experienced abuse.
6. Stress. For both men and women, all phases of the sexual response cycle are
negatively impacted by emotional and stress-related problems.
A male has no such body part as his penis also serves other purposes. The range
of physical differences is actually quite complex, and there is considerable
ongoing study about the multiple interrelated physiological systems that dis-
tinguish men and women’s sexual behavior.
Mentally and socially, there also appear to be differences in the ways men
and women experience orgasm. Perhaps this is because right from birth women
are socialized differently from men. Society impinges nonpermissive sexual
messages upon women more so than with men. This could be one reason why
men masturbate more often than women—a fact that leads to more frequent
orgasms than women (Oliver & Hyde, 1993).
In examining sexual differences between men and women, Peplau (2003)
found four other primary distinctions that I believe have implications for the
orgasmic experience. The first difference was sexual desire. Men demonstrated
greater sexual desire more consistently than women. Second, men were not as
likely to stress committed relationship as a necessary ingredient for sexual be-
havior as women. Third, men were found to be more sexually aggressive than
women. Aggression may be related to sexual self-concept, coercion in sexual
relationships, and the decision as to who initiates sex. Fourth, men’s sexual
behavior did not change as much as women’s sexual behavior over time. This
fourth condition leaves one to ponder how willingness to change or adapt over a
lifetime might very well influence orgasmic outcomes.
When considering differences, there is variability of orgasmic experiences
within one’s own gender, as well as between males and females. In the 1980s,
a group of researchers (Sholty et al., 1984) studied a small group of women
to determine how they experienced orgasm. Although the women differed
considerably in their preferred methods, several factors positively influenced
their orgasmic satisfaction, such as an improved attitude toward experimenting
with sex with a long-term partner, improved sexual self-awareness, less fear of
pregnancy due to better birth control measures, increased interest in sex and
orgasm, and decreased level of shame and inhibition. In this study, women
over the age of 40 were more likely to experience orgasm in several anatomic
sites.
TREATMENT OPTIONS
Most sexual problems can be resolved with medical or psychological
treatment. Generally, a combination of the two is the best approach, and both
require education. In addressing treatment, there are several considerations.
First, treatment for orgasm problems must be designed to address the origin
of the difficulty. Orgasmic problems and dysfunctions can result from inter-
ruption to normal sexual development, aging, medical conditions, restrictive
social conditioning, relationship problems, and psychological challenges.
Successful treatment must begin with assessment of the problem. Gener-
ally, a physician should be consulted to rule out a physiological problem or
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 167
medical diagnosis. Physicians can also check for medication side effects that
impact sexual functioning. Physicians should be chosen for their expertise as
well as their ability to accept that a sexual problem could have psychological,
social, or physical origins. Of course, if it is obvious that the problem is purely
psychological in origin, then treatment would begin with a mental health
professional with expertise in sex therapy. For example, for a man or woman
who has been sexually abused as a child and has sexual issues related to the
abuse, therapy may be the best place to begin.
Second, it is important to remember that change will be required, and
people are sometimes resistant to change. To overcome a sexual problem, one
must generally implement changes in one’s behavior, thoughts, or relationship.
This will require self-awareness, intention to change, actually doing something
different, and assessing the effectiveness of that behavior change.
Third, sex therapy often proves to be a useful and effective solution for
orgasm problems (D’Amicis, Goldberg, LoPiccolo, Friedman, & Davies, 1985;
Pierce, 2000; see also chapter 9 in this volume), but it is important to realize
that the interventions vary according to the clinician as well as to the problem.
Some clinicians concentrate on behavioral techniques whereas others may
focus on relational dynamics. Others, such as Bianchi-Demicheli and Zutter
(2005) base their interventions on a holistic approach addressing the origin of
the problem including biological, intrapsychic, relational, social factors, or a
combination of all. Their holistic model offers interventions that attempt to
bridge psychological and physical components of sexual problems.
In the following sections, I briefly list a few medical options followed by a
short sampling of psychological and behavioral treatments. Medical treatment
options include both mechanical devices (for the physical component of or-
gasm) and pharmaceutical choices (for physical and, sometimes, mental
components of orgasm). Psychological interventions include a wide range of
therapies, usually involving some type of communication training for partners.
Behavioral interventions begin with sex education and debunking of inaccu-
rate sexual myths. Clients are subsequently taught new techniques to assist
them in becoming more self-aware and to help them learn how to better
please themselves or their partners. Often, sex therapy is fashioned from a
combination of medical, psychological, and behavioral options. Collaborative
relationships among a client’s health care providers would probably best serve
the client’s interests (Millner & Ullery, 2002).
Medical Interventions
light vacuum over the clitoris with the expectation that the clitoral erectile
chambers and labia would fill with blood (Bhugra, 2003). Approved by the
U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for treatment of sexual arousal
disorder and orgasm disorders in otherwise healthy women, the device also has
shown to have promising results for women with sexual disorders resulting
from cervical cancer radiation treatment (Schroder et al., 2005).
Pharmaceutical Options
The most popular treatment for men with orgasmic problems is sildenafil
(McCarthy & Fucito, 2005). Not an ‘‘orgasm’’ pill, sildenafil (brand name is
Viagra) helps men sustain erections by relaxing smooth muscles, expanding the
arteries, and swelling the penis when they receive psychological or physical
sexual stimulation (Lue, 2000; Rosenberg, 1999). If sildenafil fails as a treat-
ment option, it may bring other issues into consideration, such as lack of
desire. Sildenafil appears safe for most men, but there is a health risk for some,
especially if they smoke or have underlying cardiovascular disease (Lue, 2000),
diabetes, high blood pressure, high cholesterol, or certain eye problems (U.S.
Food and Drug Administration, 2005). Sildenafil has not been approved by the
FDA for women, although clinical trials are under way.
For women, pharmaceutical options such as estrogen creams and testos-
terone in combination with estrogen can offer a treatment option for vaginal
irritation or dryness that occur with age-related changes (Kingsberg, 2002;
‘‘Overview: A Woman’s Guide to Hormone Therapy,’’ 2003; Sarrel, 1990).
Estrogen, often combined with progestogen, helps to decrease cancer risk for
women who have not had a hysterectomy. The risks and benefits are still being
studied, however. Taking the hormones is not a simple decision-making pro-
cess, and women should weigh the risks and the benefits of hormone replace-
ment therapy with their physician.
There is a caveat when looking at a medication solution for sexual
problems. Both men and women may choose a drug or medical device as a
quick remedy to orgasmic problems that perhaps evolved from other factors
such as social conditioning that created anxiety about sexuality (Tiefer, 2002).
Although these rapid fixes work up to a point, medical interventions are most
successful when partners are psychologically strong and solid in their rela-
tionships (McCarthy & Fucito, 2005). Partners may experience a great deal of
relief and more satisfactory sexual performance if they focused additionally on
the interpersonal aspects of the sexual experience. For example, as men age,
arousal becomes increasingly important. Medication alone will not return the
men to their adolescent sexual prowess. Additionally, older couples can benefit
from learning how to redefine normal sexual activity (Kingsberg, 2002).
Medication facilitates the sexual response, but ignoring the rest of the human
sexual experience can result in an orgasmic experience that may not be psy-
chologically stimulating (Mah & Binik, 2005).
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 169
Intimacy
‘‘What’s love got to do with it?’’ asks a popular song title. This time-honored
question as it relates to sexual satisfaction is important. Many people, especially
women, require their own emotional investment before relaxing themselves
enough to experience orgasm. Love, shown to be an emotion as well as a
neurochemical reflection within the human brain, can be an integral component
to intimacy, passion, and subsequently orgasm. For many, there is a relationship
between intimacy and sexual function (McCabe, 1997), particularly orgasmic
satisfaction (Mah & Binik, 2005). It has been shown that those who experience
orgasm with a partner are reported to experience greater physiological satisfac-
tion, more intimacy, and deeper pelvic feelings (Newcomb & Bentler, 1983).
One of the fundamental principles in intimacy-based sex therapy is that
having an orgasm is not a requirement, but rather a part of an overall satis-
factory, intimate sexual experience (Ellison, 2001). An orgasm may or may not
happen and the outcome is irrelevant. What is relevant is having a mutually
enjoyable sensual and sexual experience with a partner. With intimacy-based
sex therapy, partners learn to express their feelings and thoughts truthfully.
Cognitive Restructuring
Cognitive restructuring techniques are interventions that facilitate
changing internal, automatic thoughts that interfere with one’s sexual func-
tioning. Cognitive-behavioral techniques have been effective in treating sev-
eral sexual problems such as female orgasm disorders and premature ejaculation
(McCabe, 2001). For example, difficulty in attaining an orgasm may result
from what thoughts are going through one’s mind while engaged in sexual
activity. For example, what might be the orgasmic outcome of a woman who
says to herself, ‘‘I wonder if he’s noticed my stomach is as big as a house’’ or
‘‘All men are creeps. This one’s probably no different’’ or ‘‘He’s just using
me.’’ In a second example, what would be the satisfactory sexual outcome of a
man who bases his sexuality on performance, but who is not able to consis-
tently achieve an erection? Thoughts may enter his mind such as ‘‘I’m a
failure.’’ Feelings of guilt, shame, self-rebuke, and embarrassment are negative
feelings that often correspond with negative thoughts about self or partner. If
these thoughts are going through one’s mind, then it seems reasonable that
thoughts of exultation and feelings of ecstasy are far away. It is important,
therefore, for those experiencing orgasmic problems to examine cognitions,
both before and during the sexual experience, for any pessimistic messages that
could interfere with achieving sexual satisfaction. If thoughts are interfering
with sexual functioning, then visits to a therapist or counselor would be
advisable, particularly one who is trained in cognitive-behavioral therapy.
170 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Communication Training
Partners who communicate about what is most pleasing for each are most
likely to receive the most pleasurable experience. Usually, sex therapy involves
some form of communication training ranging from assertiveness training to
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 171
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Anderson, R. N., & Smith, B. L. (2005). Deaths: Leading causes for 2002. Nat-
ional Vital Statistics Report, 53(17). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for
Health Statistics.
Anderson, K. E., & Wagner, G. (1995). Physiology of penile erection. Physiological
Reviews, 75(1), 191–236.
Araujo, A. B., Mohr, B. A., & McKinlay, J. B. (2004). Changes in sexual function
in middle-aged and older men: Longitudinal data from the Massachusetts
Male Aging Study. American Geriatrics Society, 52, 1502–1509.
Ashton, A. K., & Rosen, R. C. (1998). Accommodation to serotonin reuptake
inhibitor-induced sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy,
24(3), 191–192.
Bachman, G., Bancroft, J., Braunstein, G., Burger, H., Davis, S., Dennerstein, L.,
et al. (2002). Female androgen insufficiency: The Princeton consensus
statement on definition, classification, and assessment. Fertility and Sterility,
77, 660–665.
Bancroft, J. (2002). The medicalization of female sexual dysfunction: The need for
caution. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 451–456.
Basson, R. (2002). Are our definitions of women’s desire, arousal and sexual pain
disorders too broad and our definition of orgasmic disorder too narrow?
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 28, 289–300.
Berman, J. R., Berman, L. A., & Goldstein, I. (1999). Female sexual dysfunction:
Incidence, pathophysiology, evaluation and treatment options. Urology,
54, 385–391.
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 173
Dunn, M. E., & Trost, J. E. (1989). Male multiple orgasms: A descriptive study.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 377–387.
Edwards, W. M., & Coleman, E. (2004). Defining sexual health: A descriptive
overview. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(3), 189–195.
Ellison, C. R. (2001). Intimacy-based sex therapy: Sexual choreography. In P. J.
Kleinplatz (Ed.), New directions in sex therapy (pp. 163–184). Ann Arbor,
MI: Taylor & Frances.
Feldmeier, H., Leutscher, P., Poggensee, G., & Harms, G. (1999). Male genital
schistosomiasis and haemospermia [Editorial]. Tropical Medicine and Inter-
national Health, 4(12), 791–793.
Geboes, K., Steeno, O., & DeMoor, P. (1975). Primary anejaculation: Diagnosis
and therapy. Fertility and Sterility, 26, 1018–1020.
Goetsch, M. F. (2005, June). The effect of total hysterectomy on specific sexual
sensations. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 192(6), 1922–1927.
Gökyildiz, S., & Beji, N. K. (2005). The effects of pregnancy on sexual life.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 31, 201–215.
Holstege, G., Georgiadis, J. R., Paans, A. M. J., Meiners, L. C., Ferdinand, H. C.
E., van der Graff, et al. (2003). Brain activation during human male ejac-
ulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9185–9193.
Hurlbert, D. F. (1993). A comparative study using orgasm consistency training in
the treatment of women reporting hypoactive sexual desire. Journal of Sex
and Marital Therapy, 19(1), 41–55.
Hurlbert, D., & Apt, C. (1994). Female sexual desire, response, and behavior.
Behavior Modification, 18, 488–504.
Jannini, E. A., & Lenzi, A. (2005). Ejaculatory disorders: Epidemiology and cur-
rent approaches to definition, classification and subtyping. World Journal of
Urology, 23(2), 68–75.
Johnson, S. D., Phelps, D. L., & Cottler, L. B. (2004). The association of sexual
dysfunction and substance use among a community epidemiological sam-
ple. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(1), 55–63.
Kandeel, F. R., Koussa, K. T., & Swerdloff, R. S. (2001). Male sexual function
and its disorders: Physiology, pathophysiology, clinical investigation, and
treatment. Endocrine Reviews, 22(3), 342–388.
Kaschak, E., & Tiefer, L. (Eds.). (2001). A new view of women’s sexual problems.
New York: Haworth Press.
Kelly, M. P., Strassberg, D. S., & Turner, C. M. (2004). Communication and
associated relationship issues in female anorgasmia. Journal of Sex and Mar-
ital Therapy, 30, 263–276.
Kennedy, S. H., Eisfeld, B. S., Dickens, M. A., Bacchiochi, B. A., & Bagby, R.
M. (2000). Antidepressant-induced sexual dysfunction during treatment
with moclobemide, paroxetine, sertraline, and venlafaxine. Journal of Clin-
ical Psychiatry, 61, 276–281.
Kingsberg, S. A. (2002). The impact of aging on sexual function in women and
their partners. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31(5), 431–437.
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 175
Klassen, A. D., & Wilsnack, S. C. (1986). Sexual experience and drinking among
women in a U.S. national survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15(5), 363–392.
Koeman, M., van Driel, M. F., Schultz, W. C., & Mensick, H. J. (1996). Orgasm
after radical prostatectomy. British Journal of Urology, 77, 861–864.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the
United States: Prevalence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281, 537–544.
Laumann, E. O., Nicolosi, A., Glasser, D. B., Paik, A., Gingell, C., Moreira, E.,
et al. (2004). Sexual problems among women and men aged 40–80 years:
Prevalence and correlates identified in the Global Study of Sexual Atti-
tudes and Behaviors. International Journal of Impotence, 17, 39–57.
Lue, T. F. (2000). Erectile dysfunction. New Journal of Medicine, 342, 1802–1813.
Mah, K., & Binik, Y. M. (2002). Do all orgasms feel alike? Evaluating a two-
dimensional model of the orgasm experience across gender and sexual
context. Journal of Sex Research, 39(2), 104–113.
Mah, K., & Binik, Y. M. (2005). Are orgasms in the mind or the body? Psych-
osocial versus physiological correlates of orgasmic pleasure and satisfaction.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 31, 187–200.
Master, V. A., & Turek, R. J. (2001). Ejaculatory physiology and dysfunctions.
Urologic Clinics of North America, 28, 363–375.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little,
Brown.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. Boston: Little,
Brown.
McCabe, M. P. (1997). Intimacy and quality of life among sexually dysfunctional
men and women. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 23(4), 276–290.
McCabe, M. P. (1999). The interrelationship between intimacy, sexual func-
tioning, and sexuality among men and women in committed relationships.
Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 8, 31–38.
McCabe, M. P. (2001). Evaluation of a cognitive behavior therapy program for
people with sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 27,
259–271.
McCarthy, B. W., & Fucito, L. M. (2005). Integrating medication, realistic ex-
pectations, and therapeutic interventions in the treatment of male sexual
dysfunction. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 31, 319–328.
Melman, A., & Gingell, J. C. (1999). The epidemiology and pathophysiology of
erectile dysfunction. Journal of Urology, 161, 5–11.
The Merck Manual of Diagnosis and Therapy. (n.d.). Female orgasm disorder. In
Sexual dysfunction in women (chap. 243). Retrieved July 30, 2005, from
www.merck.com/mrkshared/mmanual/section18/chapter243/243c.jsp
Metz, M. C., & Pryor, J. L. (2000). Premature ejaculation: A psychophysiolog-
ical approach for assessment and management. Journal of Sex and Marital
Therapy, 26, 293–320.
Michael, A. (2000). Venlafaxine-induced painful ejaculation. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 177, 282–283.
176 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Michelson, M. D., Bancroft, J., Targum, S., Kim, Y., & Tepner, R. (2000).
Female sexual dysfunction associated with antidepressant administration: A
randomized, placebo-controlled study of pharmacologic intervention. Amer-
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 239–243.
Millner, V. (2005). Female sexual arousal disorder and counseling deliberations.
Family Journal, 11(10), 1–6.
Millner, V. S., & Ullery, E. K. (2002). A holistic treatment approach to male
erectile disorder. Family Journal, 10(4), 443–447.
Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1983). Dimensions of subjective female
orgasmic responsiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44,
862–873.
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.
Overview: A woman’s guide to hormone therapy. (2003, April). National Women’s
Health Report, 26(2), 5.
Papadimas, J., Ioannidis, S., Kiouras, S., Tarlatzis, B., Papanicolaou, A., Bondis, I.,
et al. (1999). Spontaneous pregnancy following therapeutic approach of an
infertile man with aspermia/obstructive azoospermia. Archives of Andrology,
42, 105–108.
Papp, G. K., Kopa, Z., Szabó, F., & Erdei, E. (2003). Aetiology of haemospermia.
Andrologia, 35, 317–320.
Penson, D. F., Latini, D. M., Lubeck, D. P., Wallace, K. L., Henning, J. M., &
Lue, T. F. (2003). Do impotent men with diabetes have more severe
erectile dysfunction and worse quality of life than the general population of
impotent patients? Diabetes Care, 26, 1093–1099.
Peplau, L. A. (2003). Human sexuality: How do men and women differ? Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 12(2), 37–40.
Perelman, M. A. (2001). Integrating sildenafil and sex therapy: Unconsummated
marriage secondary to erectile dysfunction and retarded ejaculation. Journal
of Sex Education and Therapy, 26, 13–21.
Perry, D. P., & Whipple, B. (1981). Pelvic muscle strength of female ejaculators:
Evidence in support of a new theory of orgasm. Journal of Sex Research, 17,
22–39.
Pierce, A. P. (2000). The Coital Alignment Technique (CAT): An overview of
studies. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 257–268.
Raja, M. (1999). Risperidone-induced absence of ejaculation. International Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 14(5), 317–319.
Ralph, D. J., & Wylie, K. R. (2005). Ejaculatory disorders and sexual function.
British Journal of Urology International, 95, 1181–1186.
Randall, H. E., & Byers, E. S. (2003). What is sex? Students’ definitions of having
sex, sexual partner, and unfaithful sexual partner. Canadian Journal of Hu-
man Sexuality, 12(2), 87–96.
Reuben, S. H. (2004). Living beyond cancer: Finding a new balance. President’s
Cancer Panel 2003–2004 Annual Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Depart-
ment of Health and Human Services.
Orgasmic Problems and Disorders 177
Rosenberg, K. P. (1999). Sildenafil. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 25, 271–
279.
Rowland, D. L., Keeney, C., & Slob, A. K. (2004). Sexual response in men with
inhibited or retarded ejaculation. International Journal of Impotence Research,
16, 270–274.
Rowland, D. L., Myers, L., Culver, A., & Davidson, J. M. (1997). Bupropion
and sexual function: A placebo-controlled prospective study on diabetic
men with erectile dysfunction. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17(5),
350–357.
Sarrel, P. M. (1990). Sexuality and menopause. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 75,
(Suppl. 4), 26S–30S.
Schroder, M. A., Mell, L. K., Hurteau, J. A., Collins, Y. C., Rotmensch, J.,
Waggoner, S. E., et al. (2005). Clitoral therapy device for treatment of sex-
ual dysfunction in irradiated cervical cancer patients. International Journal
of Radiation Oncology, 61(4), 1078–1086.
Sholty, M. J., Ephross, P. H., Plaut, S. M., Fischman, S. H., Charnas, J. F., &
Cody, C. A. (1984). Female orgasmic experience: A subjective study.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13(2), 155–392.
Sipski, M. L. (2002). Central nervous system based neurogenic female sexual
dysfunction: Current status and future trends. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
31(5), 421–424.
Sipski, M. L., & Alexander, C. J. (1993). Sexual activities, response and satisfaction
in women pre- and post-spinal cord injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 74, 1025–1029.
Sipski, M. L., Alexander, C. J., & Rosen, R. C. (1995). Orgasm in women with
spinal cord injuries: A laboratory-based assessment. Archives of Physical
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 76, 1097–1102.
Sipski, M. L., Rosen, R. C., Alexander, C. J., & Gomez-Marin, O. (2004). Sexual
responsiveness in women with spinal cord injuries: Differential effects of
anxiety-eliciting stimulation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(3), 295–302.
Tiefer, L. (2002). Beyond the medical model of women’s sexual problems: A
campaign to resist the promotion of ‘‘female sexual dysfunction.’’ Sexual
and Relationship Therapy, 17(2), 127–135.
U.S. Food and Drug Administration (2005). Patient information sheet: Sildenafil cit-
rate. Retrieved July 30, 2005, from www.fda.gov/cder/drug/InfoSheets/
patient/sildenafilPIS.htm
von Sydow, K. (1999). Sexuality during pregnancy and after childbirth: A meta-
content analysis of 59 studies. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 27–49.
Waldinger, M. D. (2003). Use of psychoactive agents in the treatment of sexual
dysfunction. CNS Drugs, 6(3), 204–216.
Waldinger, M., & Schweitzer, D. (2005). Retarded ejaculation in men: An
overview of psychological and neurobiological insights. World Journal of
Urology, 23(2), 76–81.
Walsh, P. C., & Wilson, J. D. (1987). Impotence and infertility in men. In
E. Braunwald, K. J. Isselbacher, R. S. Petersdorf, J. D. Wilson, J. B.
178 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Peggy J. Kleinplatz 1
This chapter will give the reader an overview of the field of sex therapy. There
will be a brief description of sex therapy and its beginnings. This will be
followed by a discussion of the major factors that lead to sexual difficulties.
How to tell whether a sexual difficulty counts as a ‘‘problem’’ and what kind of
expert to consult will be considered next. Of course, individuals and couples
deal with all sorts of sexual concerns that are not necessarily classified as
‘‘official’’ sexual dysfunctions. Sex therapists deal with these issues daily, and
some of the more common ones will be enumerated here. Then, the major
male and female sexual dysfunctions will be discussed, followed by the most
common problem for couples, that is, differences in sexual desire. Recom-
mended resources follow the conclusion.
for eleven years, Masters and Johnson released their findings in 1966. They
described the stages of sexual arousal and response they had observed as the
Human Sexual Response Cycle: excitement, plateau, orgasm, and resolution
(Masters & Johnson, 1966). This model became the basis for their designation—
and later, the entire field’s ideas—of what constitutes ‘‘normal’’ sexual func-
tioning. Deviations from this model during the stages they had described were
designated as sexual dysfunctions. For example, difficulties among men during
the sexual excitement phase were referred to as ‘‘impotence’’ (and are more
commonly known today as ‘‘erectile dysfunction’’). In 1970, Masters and
Johnson released their second book, Human Sexual Inadequacy, in which they
described the sexual dysfunctions in men and women and the approach they
had developed for treatment of them. This was the genesis of the field of sex
therapy and this new book became its Bible.
The dysfunctions described by Masters and Johnson were later enshrined in
the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM )
published by the American Psychiatric Association (1980). The desire disorders
were described by pioneers Helen Singer Kaplan (1977, 1979) and Harold Lief
(1977). These dysfunctions continue to comprise the bulk of the categories of
sexual difficulties listed in the current edition of the DSM (APA, 2000). The
remaining categories of the DSM are not listed as dysfunctions but as disorders.
These are the paraphilias, that is, various forms of unconventional sexuality,
including cross-dressing and sexual sadomasochism (known as SM or BDSM
colloquially) and the gender identity problems. There is considerable controversy
over whether these categories belong in the DSM or their World Health Or-
ganization counterpart, the International Classification of Diseases (ICD; see below).
Early treatment methods in sex therapy were notably successful, at least in
the reversal of the symptoms of sexual dysfunctions (Kaplan, 1974; Masters &
Johnson, 1970). In fact, they were so impressive that the field was able to
ignore the difference between treatment of symptoms versus attention to the
underlying problems, not to mention overlooking the individuals or couples
who were suffering. Solving mechanical problems expediently was sex ther-
apy’s initial claim to fame, but this approach made it all too convenient to
focus on the ‘‘easy’’ cases and be caught unawares when confronted with an
avalanche of ‘‘harder’’ cases, some of which resulted from earlier ‘‘successes.’’
That is, the fact that sex therapy was so effective at eliminating the target
symptoms meant that the field did not have to spend a lot of time considering
and debating whether the minimal goals aspired to were all that could or
should be achieved. Practitioners were operating—and often still do—without
a theory of human sexual experience to ground and orient clinical work
(Kleinplatz, 2003; Wiederman, 1998). As such, sex therapy may be very ef-
fective at helping couples to ameliorate their dysfunctional sex lives while
being conspicuously ill-equipped to attain intense erotic intimacy (Kleinplatz,
1996, 2001; Schnarch, 1991; Shaw, 2001). For example, helping a couple
182 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
learn to overcome a problem with early ejaculation does not guarantee that
their sexual relationship will improve.
Intrapsychic Factors
Intrapsychic factors are those psychological elements within the individual
that bear on his or her sexual expression. Many of these are related to early
childhood experiences and the messages received in childhood about sexuality.
For example, what was the nature of the parents’ sexual relationship and how was
it perceived within the family? Were the parents sexually open and expressive,
affectionate and demonstrative, or more reserved? Did they seem loving to one
another and to the children, or cool and distant? Were they happily married,
miserable together, or divorced? What did the parents teach about sex? What did
the children learn from what the parents said or, more commonly, from what
was never spoken, about sexuality? How did the family deal with nudity, self-
stimulation in childhood, privacy, questions about where babies come from, and
about what to expect during puberty? The values learned about one’s body, and in
particular the genitalia, as well as about pleasure and sexuality, overall, have a
tremendous impact on future sexual attitudes, comfort, or discomfort. Too many
young people are still being told, albeit nonverbally, ‘‘Sex is dirty—save it for
someone you love.’’ No wonder sex therapists often have extensive waiting lists!
Clearly, childhood sexual abuse or incest can lead to feelings of shame,
guilt, and fear around sexuality and generally diminished self-esteem (Herman,
1992; Maltz, 1998). Many survivors of sexual abuse do not feel entitled to
consensual, mutually respectful, and loving sexual relations. Furthermore, they
are often unable to imagine sex that is chosen freely and how that would look
and feel, rather than sex occurring for someone else’s sake—to fulfill another’s
needs. These are often contributing factors to the future development of sexual
problems including low sexual desire, sexual aversion, and various sexual
dysfunctions. Unfortunately, given that many people raised in ‘‘normal’’ homes
contend with sex-negative environments, they too are subject to anxiety and
discomfort regarding sexuality with similar consequences, even if their con-
cerns are not as intense as among those who were sexually abused. In other
words, the notion that all sexual abuse survivors will develop sexual problems
is an overstatement; correspondingly, in a sex-negative culture, ‘‘normal’’ sex-
ual development may breed sexual problems.
Another common contributor to sexual problems is the focus on sex as
performance rather than as a source of mutual pleasure. To the extent that one
is observing one’s body while in bed, concerned about getting or keeping an
erection, reaching orgasm, or, more likely, reaching orgasm ‘‘soon enough’’ or
delaying orgasm ‘‘long enough,’’ one is typically unable to connect fully and
joyfully with one’s partner(s). Masters and Johnson (1970) referred to this as
‘‘spectatoring’’ and indeed, it often feels as if one is off in the bleachers,
watching and worrying from a distance, instead of being embodied within and
present with one’s partner(s). Thus, performance anxiety interferes with sexual
satisfaction even if it does not always technically impede ‘‘functioning.’’
184 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Interpersonal Factors
Interpersonal factors include all those elements that affect one’s ability to
be engaged in sexual relationships or for a couple to be mutually engaged.
These factors include difficulties with trust, fears of rejection, power struggles,
disappointment, the aftermath of affairs, and ‘‘goodness of fit’’ (i.e., the extent
to which partners share compatible visions of sex, eroticism, pleasure, etc.).
The most prominently reported interpersonal factor is communication dif-
ficulties. Typically, couples arrive in sex therapists’ offices complaining of
problems with ‘‘communicating about intimacy.’’ Sometimes this phrase is a
euphemism for fears of talking openly about sexual likes and dislikes, wishes,
preferences, and fantasies. However, the difficulty is often with conflict res-
olution per se and its impact on the couple’s willingness to risk emotional and
sexual intimacy together. Couples who cannot imagine disagreements as
leading to anything other than attacks, recriminations, hurt feelings, and im-
plicit or explicit threats eventually learn to avoid conflict. In fact, such couples
may announce early in therapy that they are one another’s best friends and that
they get along beautifully . . . except for this one glitch around ‘‘intimacy.’’ In
such cases, it is not ‘‘sex’’ per se that is the problem; rather, these couples
discover in therapy that couples who cannot really afford to get angry with one
another also cannot afford to feel much of anything, let alone to share passion.
Above and beyond these ‘‘communication difficulties’’ is the difficulty in
literally talking about and defining sexual terms. Many people associate their
Latinate sex vocabularies with physicians’ offices, and perhaps biology classes,
and thus claim such terms ‘‘sound clinical.’’ In contrast, the slang terms for
sexual parts and functions (in English) have derogatory connotations and make
people uncomfortable, too. As such, people are often at a loss as to how to ask
for what they want. The usual default, nonverbal communication, is an inad-
equate substitute for simple, clear requests. Furthermore, the seriousness of this
seemingly trivial problem is exacerbated by the lack of consensus as to what
constitutes ‘‘sex.’’ The Clinton-Lewinsky scandal highlighted how different
individuals disagree on the meanings of the same terms and even manipulate
language to obscure understanding. As such, it is fairly common for sex ther-
apists to ask couples about their sexual behaviors, only to be met with eu-
phemisms such as ‘‘making love’’ or ‘‘down there.’’ These terms often turn out
to signify entirely different things to each partner. Partners who argue endlessly
about wanting to ‘‘take more time making love’’ may unknowingly have
mistaken his self-doubts surrounding rapid ejaculation with her desire to re-
ceive more oral stimulation or their concerns about how frequently sex occurs.
Sociocultural/Economic/Political Factors
Personal and sociocultural values have an enormous impact on sexuality.
In North America, sexual values tend to be sex-negative and thus contribute to
Sex Therapy 185
Biomedical Factors
Any factor that affects the human body can affect sexuality. Although
physicians may not specifically inquire as to the impact of medical conditions
on one’s sex life, the impact is there and should not be underestimated or
ignored (Maurice, 1999). It is very useful for people to see their physicians and
ask to be examined for any medical problems that might affect sexuality
(Moser, 1999), particularly when embarking on sex therapy. A wide variety of
diseases or their treatments can affect sexual desire, arousal, and response di-
rectly or indirectly. Particular categories of relevance here include any illness
or injury that affects the neurological, endocrine (i.e., hormonal), or cardio-
vascular systems. For example, cardiovascular disease can create difficulties
with sexual response, including difficulties with blood flow to the genitals in
men and women. Actually having a heart attack often terrifies individuals and
their partners into refraining from sexual relations, fearing that strenuous ac-
tivity might trigger a subsequent heart attack. Unfortunately, providing the
information that might help couples resume sexual relations safely in the
presence of heart disease is often overlooked during a crisis and forgotten
during the rehabilitation phase. Many health professionals are uncomfortable
bringing up the subject of sexuality with their patients. Furthermore, medi-
cations for high blood pressure often create or exacerbate sexual dysfunctions.
The fact that many patients are not warned about these potential side effects
makes people feel all the more isolated and defective.
Chronic illness of any kind, disability and, ongoing pain, whatever the
source, each has an impact on sexuality. These effects can be very subtle and
barely noticeable at first, as might be the case with lower back pain, or sudden
and dramatic, as in a spinal cord injury. Consideration of these effects should
be included in the course of medical treatment or rehabilitation. Two medical
problems that have a direct bearing on sexuality are sexually transmitted in-
fections (STIs) and infertility. Diagnosis of these conditions often makes people
feel defective immediately. People with STIs may describe feeling contami-
nated and untouchable. Infertility typically forces people to question their
adequacy as sexual beings and their conceptions of ‘‘normalcy,’’ while its
treatment interferes with their previous sexual patterns.
A wide variety of commonly used drugs affect sexual functioning. For
example, most psychotropic drugs, that is, medications used for psychiatric
purposes, tend to affect sexual functioning adversely. A popular category of
antidepressant medications, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs),
which includes such drugs as Prozac and Paxil, can diminish or prevent sexual
arousal, orgasm, or even sexual desire itself. On the other hand, this very side
effect has been used to assist men who have been diagnosed with rapid ejac-
ulation to slow their responses. In general, sexual side effects tend to be
underreported, both because of the methodology used by pharmaceutical
companies in their recording of adverse effects in the process of clinical trials,
Sex Therapy 187
and because many people are too embarrassed to volunteer information about
sexual difficulties. They may not even make the connection between medi-
cation usage and sexual problems. Some commonly used drugs do not even
‘‘count’’ in patients’ minds when giving a sexual/medical history. Over-the-
counter drugs such as antihistamines and decongestants dry out mucous
membranes in the nose and mouth; as such, they may well limit a woman’s
lubrication, though it typically does not even occur to her that her allergy pills
are making sex uncomfortable. Similarly, women using hormonal contra-
ception (that is, oral contraceptive pills, ‘‘the patch,’’ or injections, as in Depo-
Provera) may not be aware of their possibly adverse effects on sexual desire in
some women and, unfortunately, are not typically warned of such possibilities
in advance. Recreational drugs need to be considered as well. People often use
alcohol to ‘‘loosen up’’ before sex. However, the same cautions that apply to
drinking and driving are relevant for sex as well. Because alcohol depresses the
central nervous system, it affects reflexes and judgment. Alcohol can impair the
ability to make clear and consensual choices about sex, thereby contributing to
unwanted sex or unsafe sex. Furthermore, excess alcohol will also impair the
mechanisms involved in sexual excitement and orgasm. Long-term alcoholism
inevitably creates sexual dysfunction.
H O W D O Y O U K N O W I F I T ’ S ‘‘ N O R M A L ’’
OR IF IT’S A PROBLEM?
If it is bothersome, it could be a problem for the person who feels
bothered. As obvious as that may seem, there is a lack of consensus among sex
therapists as to whether or not distress is necessary or sufficient to indicate a
problem. Some sex therapists believe that even when one feels no distress,
certain symptoms should automatically qualify for diagnosis of pathology
(Althof, 2001). Althof argues that until symptoms of sexual problems (e.g.,
women’s difficulties with orgasm) are regarded as ‘‘objective’’ indicators of
pathology regardless of subjective distress or lack thereof, sexual disorders will
not be given their due and taken seriously as obstacles to patients’ quality of
life. This argument is analogous to the reasoning that although high blood
pressure generally causes no symptoms, it signifies pathology and requires
treatment nonetheless. Althof would say that there is a parallel with sexual
problems. Others argue that personal distress is the most important criterion in
determining whether or not we are to conceptualize some difficulty as worthy
of treatment (Nathan, 2003). If it does not bother the individual in question,
how can anyone else determine that the ‘‘patient’’ has a dysfunction, disorder,
or even a problem?
Furthermore, these issues become even murkier when considering
‘‘problems’’ that create no distress for the ‘‘identified patient’’ but do bother
others. For example, low sexual desire is not necessarily deemed problematic
yet qualifies for the diagnosis of hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD)
188 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
may feel distress, most sex therapists will provide reassurance and help to
normalize the concern, but will not ‘‘treat’’ the alleged ‘‘frigidity.’’
Other couples come into therapy not because they have any problem but
because they want more joy, meaning, eroticism, and intimacy out of sex.
They are dismayed, disillusioned, and disgruntled by the quality of their sex
lives. Although it would be hard to classify these couples as having dysfunc-
tions or disorders, something seems to be missing. Such individuals often speak
in terms of lack of connection or the absence of passion, or they state that the
‘‘mystery is gone.’’ The excitement of the honeymoon phase of any rela-
tionship cannot be recaptured and must evolve into something deeper and
ultimately more fulfilling (McCarthy & McCarthy, 2003). Nonetheless, erotic
intimacy can be heightened in sex therapy by helping the people involved to
grow as individuals and partners (Kleinplatz, 1996, 2001; Schnarch, 1991).
The work of sex therapy in such cases may entail helping make the relationship
safe enough to enable the individuals involved to be emotionally naked and to
take the risks involved in being vulnerable.
Furthermore, some therapists consider using whatever concerns clients bring
into therapy as openings toward personal growth and integration, regardless of
whether or not their problems qualify for diagnosis (Mahrer & Boulet, 2001).
The most common presenting problems are related to sexual desire. These
problems encompass low sexual desire, very high sexuality (often referred to
as ‘‘sexual addiction’’), sexual/erotic desire discrepancies, and unusual sexual
desires (typically classified as the ‘‘paraphilias’’).
Erectile Dysfunction
Of all male sexual dysfunctions, the one that has received the greatest
attention in recent years has been erectile dysfunction, largely because of the
marketing of pharmacological treatments for this condition since the late
1990s. Many people will have greater familiarity with erectile dysfunction than
with other sexual problems. It will serve as the prototype here for sexual
dysfunctions and their treatment, and will be discussed at greater length to
introduce the basic principles of sexual problems and sex therapy.
Sex Therapy 191
nature of sexual problems and depending on how a given couple might re-
spond to the initial sensate focus exercises in the course of therapy. For ex-
ample, in the case of erectile dysfunction, the man was encouraged to allow
himself to become sexually aroused and erect through penile stimulation
by his partner, but to allow his erection to subside repeatedly. Eventually, his
comfort level with, and enjoyment from, sexual contact would supersede
his performance anxiety and thus enable him to return to engaging in inter-
course without concern.
Although Masters and Johnson’s approach was quite successful at allevi-
ating the symptom of erectile dysfunction, at least in the short term, their
method has been eclipsed by more recent medical interventions. Interestingly,
as medically based treatments have become increasingly available, so has the
popularity of the belief that sexual problems are caused by underlying medical
disorders. Some have argued that this shift from the notion that sexual
problems are primarily psychosocial to the current emphasis on the biomedical
has been brought about by the pharmaceutical industry, which profits, quite
literally, from the latter way of thinking (Tiefer, 2000, 2001). Others would
say that men are only too eager to believe that their problems are organic
because it relieves them of having to confront themselves in therapy. In any
case, the notion that problems are either medical or psychosocial does a dis-
service to men. As discussed above, sexual problems are generally caused by a
combination of factors but inevitably, have an impact on the whole person,
mind, and body alike. Furthermore, regardless of who is identified as the
patient, sexual difficulties typically affect all parties in a relationship. Therefore,
attempts to deal with sexual problems should ideally include the ‘‘whole’’
person and the couple.
During the 1980s, urologist Giles Brindley discovered that injecting the
penis with papaverine, an enzyme from a papaya extract, would cause auto-
matic, hard, long-lasting erections. This breakthrough allowed men to ensure
strong erections without having to engage in conventional sex therapy. In fact,
they could do so without informing their partners and, for that matter, in the
absence of any stimulation. These injections into the side of the penis, more
specifically, into the corpora cavernosa, would create erections within about
twenty minutes, regardless of what the man was feeling or doing. Whether that
is a good or bad outcome is a value judgment. Nonetheless, it at least allowed
men dealing with erectile dysfunction a new treatment option. (These in-
tracavernosal injections would later be filled most commonly with a combi-
nation of papaverine, phentolamine and prostaglandin E1.) However, many
men were understandably queasy at the thought of having to stick a syringe
into their penises every time they wanted to produce an erection.
This obstacle was overcome with the introduction of sildenafil citrate,
better known as Viagra, in 1998 (Goldstein et al., 1998). Viagra is ingested
orally and, like the intracavernosal injections, works by dilating the arteries of
the penis to permit an erection. Unlike the injection method, Viagra requires
Sex Therapy 193
sexual stimulation to work effectively. If the man is not in the mood, it will
not work. There is also a relatively high placebo rate. The same holds true for
the other drugs in this class, vardenafil (i.e., Levitra) and tadalafil (i.e., Cialis),
all known as PDE5 inhibitors. These relatively safe drugs have grown enor-
mously popular, enough so to encourage the pharmaceutical industry to invest
heavily in the development of products for the treatment of other sexual
problems. Unfortunately, while these drugs may be effective at treating the
symptom of sexual dysfunction, they often leave the problems within the man
untouched, not to mention circumventing the couple entirely. For example,
these drugs may allow the couple to temporarily ignore his feelings of worth-
lessness as a man unless he performs to an arbitrary and unrealistic standard;
thus, his unspoken problems of self-doubt and resentment remain. Perhaps
for this reason, or perhaps for other reasons (including embarrassment), arti-
cles have appeared on the problem of ‘‘patient compliance’’ (Althof, 1998;
Perelman, 2000; Wise, 1999).
It is noteworthy that Masters and Johnson treated only couples—never
individuals alone—but that practical considerations have led most sex thera-
pists today to see people individually. Certainly, the increasing use of medical
interventions and the lack of reimbursement in many American insur-
ance plans mean that couples therapy is unlikely to occur as often as desired
(Stock & Moser, 2001). Many experts still believe that ideally the relationship
should be the target of sex therapy and continue to provide therapy to het-
erosexual or same-sex couples. Furthermore, in some instances, what appear to
be mechanical difficulties in functioning can only be identified correctly as
appropriate bodily responses to unsatisfactory sex or relationship issues when
both partners are present (Kleinplatz, 2004; Schnarch, 1991). For example, it is
only when both are in the same room that one detects the simmering conflict
between them—or even their outright dislike for one another—such that his
‘‘dysfunction’’ is more likely a solution than a problem. Perhaps rather than
being problematic, the man’s soft penis provides evidence of good judgment
demonstrated via his body.
Rapid Ejaculation
Notwithstanding the greater public attention to erectile dysfunction, rapid
ejaculation is probably the most common male sexual dysfunction (Polonsky,
2000). As stated above, there has been considerable controversy in defining
rapid ejaculation. How do you know how soon is too soon? All sorts of
definitions have been proffered over the years with attempts at scientific ob-
jectivity. Some have focused on the number of minutes prior to ejaculation
and others on the number of thrusts. There are several problems with such
definitions: First, people who are enjoying sex rarely employ a stopwatch
(fortunately!), so it is hard to ascertain what is ‘‘normal’’ and what is not.
Second, much as scientist-clinicians seek objective criteria, how long sex lasts
194 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
is only a problem when it creates distress. Surely, we can imagine the movie
scene of lovers working feverishly to meet surreptitiously, ripping each other’s
clothes off in an empty corridor and enjoying a ‘‘quickie’’ before they can even
catch their breath. In such instances, the sex seems torrid—not problematic.
Thus, more recent approaches have focused on the man or the couple’s
subjective experience. For example, Metz and McCarthy (2003) feel that the
best professional description of this problem is that ‘‘the man does not have vol-
untary, conscious control, or the ability to choose in most encounters when to ejaculate’’
(p. 1; emphasis in the original).
Typically, by the time men seek sex therapy for rapid ejaculation, they
have already tried numerous delaying tactics regardless of the cost they may
be paying. Common examples of ‘‘home remedies’’ include numbing creams,
doubling up on condoms, and trying to think about disgusting things to turn
themselves off. To the extent that these strategies ‘‘work,’’ they deprive the
man of pleasure, deprive the partners from feeling present with one another,
and reinforce the notion that sex equals performing during intercourse.
Fortunately, rapid ejaculation generally responds quite readily to sex ther-
apy. Most therapists deal with rapid ejaculation using a combination of sensate
focus exercises, other cognitive-behavioral exercises and bibliotherapy. The
tone set in therapy and in the exercises/readings is particularly important in
counteracting the mindset common among men concerned about rapid
ejaculation. The client’s beliefs are explored and myths challenged both during
therapy sessions and in assigned readings (e.g., Metz & McCarthy, 2003;
Zilbergeld, 1999). For example, many men feel pressured to ‘‘last’’ as long as
possible in the assumption that extensive thrusting is what it takes to satisfy
female partners. Such men often feel quite relieved when they learn that most
women prefer external, clitoral stimulation when seeking orgasm. As such,
‘‘lasting longer’’ becomes a choice for prolonging lovemaking, when he or
they desire it, rather than an obligation. Therapists may have couples use
sensate focus exercises in order to demonstrate that pleasure may abound
regardless of the duration of intercourse. Other exercises are typically assigned
to help the man gain more of a sense of voluntary control over his ejaculation.
Ideally, the emphasis is on increasing his tolerance for high levels of pleasure
rather than reducing his sensitivity to partner stimulation (Zilbergeld, 1999).
The man or couple is then usually instructed in the use of the ‘‘squeeze’’ or
‘‘stop-start’’ techniques. Both techniques are used when the man is feeling very
aroused and close to orgasm. The squeeze technique involves having the man
or his partner apply pressure with the thumb and forefinger to the front and
back of his penis—never the sides—either just under the penile glans or at the
base of the penis to delay ejaculation. The stop-start technique is exactly what
the name conveys: when the man feels that any further stimulation will lead to
orgasm, he or they stop their activities until his level of excitement subsides.
Then, stimulation is resumed. These exercises are intended to help him learn
how it feels to be highly aroused without ‘‘going over the edge.’’
Sex Therapy 195
Delayed Ejaculation
On the other end of the spectrum are men who are unable to reach
orgasm with a partner. (Men who have never had an orgasm in their lives
under any circumstances, including via masturbation, probably have under-
lying medical problems requiring evaluation.) This problem had been known
as ‘‘ejaculatory incompetence’’ and then later as ‘‘retarded ejaculation,’’ but
both these pejorative terms have been replaced by the term ‘‘delayed ejacu-
lation.’’ Most people do not think of men capable of thrusting away endlessly
during intercourse as having a problem. On the contrary, many would be
envious of such a capacity. As such, it has been estimated that delayed ejac-
ulation is the most underreported of male sexual problems just as corre-
spondingly, rapid orgasm in women is probably the most underreported of
women’s sexual difficulties. Many people would just scratch their heads at
hearing of such conditions and ask, ‘‘So what’s the problem?’’ The assumption
in our sex scripts is that no man can stay hard for too long and that no woman
can reach orgasm too soon. However, Apfelbaum (2000) would respond that
men who cannot ejaculate with a partner are the misunderstood ‘‘workhorses’’
of the sex world. Apfelbaum has argued that although such men have often
been treated as if they were withholding during sex, on the contrary, they are
giving too much. These are individuals who are trying so hard to please their
partners that they continue to perform while subjectively feeling minimal
arousal, numb, or even turned off. It seems that their desires are out of sync
with their penises, which continue to remain erect despite lack of excitement
(Apfelbaum). Why this should be the case is not clear. During the 1970s and
1980s, sex therapy techniques involved attempting to stimulate such indi-
viduals ever more aggressively to the point of orgasm. Apfelbaum suggests
instead that sex therapists help them to acknowledge their feelings. In other
words, whereas previous treatment models had suggested that these men just
keep on moving, even if this meant ignoring their own reluctance (a strategy
associated with the work of Kaplan [1974] and referred to as ‘‘bypassing’’),
Apfelbaum recommends ‘‘counterbypassing.’’ Here, the men are encouraged
to pay more attention to their reluctance and lack of desire for intercourse—
not to override them—and to be true to themselves by acknowledging these
196 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
feelings. It is only via authenticity that enduring change can come about
(Apfelbaum).
The latter remains the goal for many couples, notwithstanding our ex-
panding knowledge of female genitalia and sexuality. The number and in-
tensity of nerve endings in the clitoris far exceed those in the vagina, and even
those found in the vagina are located primarily near the entrance. (Any woman
who has ever used tampons can remember her surprise upon first reading the
package instructions; these indicated that once the tampon is inserted cor-
rectly, the woman will be unable to feel it in the course of normal activities.) It
can be difficult for women to get enough direct clitoral stimulation during
intercourse to bring about orgasm. Indeed, for some women the trick is to
arrange enough external pressure and friction to trigger orgasm (almost) despite
intercourse! Although this information is increasingly widespread, given a so-
ciety that defines ‘‘sex’’ as intercourse—nothing else quite counts as ‘‘going all
the way’’—the objective of ‘‘climaxing during sex’’ endures. Even the obvious
solution, for the woman or her partner to stimulate her clitoris manually during
intercourse, strikes many people as ‘‘cheating,’’ at least initially. It is as though
the ‘‘hands-free orgasm’’ remains the cultural gold standard.
The pressure on women to perform corresponds to the pressure on men
discussed earlier. If this theme is becoming repetitive, that is because its impact
is ubiquitous. To the extent that sex must be heterosexual and that the ultimate
end of sex is penis in vagina, we are creating obstacles to, and ‘‘dysfunctions’’
in the way of, sexual pleasure. Furthermore, we thereby limit what we can
hope to attain during sex. For example, in 1982, sexologists Ladas, Whipple,
and Perry wrote a best-selling book about the ‘‘G-spot’’ (i.e., the Grafenberg
spot), which described a sensitive area that could be accessed via stimulation of
the anterior wall, (the roof or front wall) of the vagina. (This area is now
referred to as the ‘‘female prostate’’ [Zaviacic, 1999].) Rather than being
welcomed as a new possibility for further sexual exploration, this important
contribution was misinterpreted in some quarters as setting a new imperative,
or perhaps reasserting the notion that there is one right way to reach orgasm
after all (Tavris, 1992).
For the sex therapist, that leaves the problem of figuring out what con-
stitutes a problem and precisely what requires ‘‘treatment.’’ Increasingly, sex
therapists have come to believe that couples who insist upon the woman
achieving orgasm via sexual intercourse are in need of psychoeducational
counseling rather than ‘‘treatment’’ as such. That is, they may need to learn
enough about women’s bodies to readjust their expectations and to expand
their criteria for valid orgasm pathways (e.g., manual or oral sex). Alternately,
they may be encouraged to use their hands or sex toys to provide direct clitoral
stimulation during intercourse (Dodson, 2002).
But what of the woman who has never had an orgasm by any means and
wants to do so? This problem is sometimes called inhibited female orgasm or
anorgasmia. Some would say that the term really should be ‘‘preorgasmic’’ rather
than ‘‘anorgasmic’’ because all women are capable of orgasm, whether it has
198 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
happened yet or not (Barbach, 2000). In Masters and Johnson’s (1970) approach,
the couple began, as always, with sensate focus exercises. For many women, the
taboo against exploring their bodies was strong enough to have prevented them
from ever really discovering what gave them pleasure. Sensate focus exer-
cises gave them permission to enjoy touching and being touched for their own
sake. This theme has been emphasized in other programs for women who have
difficulty with orgasm, such as those of Barbach (2000) and Heiman and Lo-
Piccolo (1988). These popular cognitive-behavioral treatment plans involve
teaching women about the anatomy of their genitalia and the physiology of
sexual response, generally through readings and exercises, and via discussion
during therapy sessions. Women also review their sexual histories, current be-
liefs, and especially myths to uncover the obstacles to pleasure. Individual or
relationship problems, which make it hard to feel desirable, to feel worthy of
sexual attention, to trust a partner, or to let go, are discussed and dealt with as
necessary.
Bodily awareness and self-stimulation are emphasized, as most women
who have never had an orgasm with a partner will probably find it easiest to
experience it alone at first. There are often fewer inhibitions when one is
alone, and the focused concentration on oneself makes it easier to discover
one’s own sensitivities and triggers to orgasm. It also seems safer to take all the
time one needs alone; this is a serious consideration for many women (and
their partners) who worry, ‘‘It’s taking too much time. . . . I’m afraid [he/she]
will get bored and give up on me.’’ The implication that the couple should be
able to rush through sex betrays their beliefs about sex and often the under-
lying fear of wanting more pleasure than entitled. It also suggests that the
couple’s existing sexual activities may not be particularly erotic, at least for her.
As such, she will be encouraged to explore her own wishes and fantasies, too,
on her way to orgasm and beyond.
Once she has found out how to have orgasms alone, she will need to show
and tell her partner what pleases her. Here, it is often literally a matter of
teaching her partner about her desires and her body, and demonstrating what
kinds of stimulation she finds exciting. To the extent that they are able to
communicate effectively and get over the initial awkwardness, this approach
usually allows her to begin having orgasms with a partner in relatively short-
term therapy.
lack of arousal has been subsumed under treatment for difficulties with orgasm
or desire.
Another reason for the lack of clinical or research attention to women’s
arousal difficulties is the conventional North American sexual script that
emphasizes functioning over subjective experience. To the extent that
men’s difficulties with arousal are evident in erectile dysfunction, intercourse
is impeded and, therefore, male arousal difficulties command the spotlight.
Women’s sexual arousal difficulties are manifest in terms of lack of vaginal
lubrication and absent, minimal, or diminished subjective feelings of excite-
ment. Neither of these difficulties necessarily obstructs intercourse per se and,
therefore, they have been ignored by researchers. There has been a great deal
of research on factors that might reduce or increase the blood flow to male
genitalia. We need more data on factors affecting the psychology and physi-
ology of female sexual response.
Women who see their gynecologists for lack of lubrication are sometimes
assessed for underlying physical, psychological, or interpersonal causes and of-
fered appropriate treatment; however, it is not unusual for them simply to
receive instructions on the use of lubricants or hormonal creams. This treatment
enables them to engage in intercourse whether or not they feel aroused. Lu-
bricants are an important adjunct in helping women to engage in pain-free
intercourse when their own, natural lubrication has been diminished by disease,
by various prescription (e.g., antihypertensives, diuretics) or over-the-counter
medications (e.g., antihistamines, decongestants), or by aging. However, such
measures, when applied whenever women are slow to lubricate, are treating a
symptom rather than what may be the underlying cause of the problem. It may
simply be that she is not lubricating because the sex or her partner is not arousing
to her. In such cases, the ‘‘treatment’’ may actually mask the problem—or the
fact that there is no problem: it is healthy and normal not to be aroused if the sex
or the relationship are not to one’s liking or are actually a turn-off. The reasons
that she is not lubricating or subjectively aroused warrant attention.
Given the paucity of literature on female sexual arousal problems, little is
discussed in the way of treatment outside the use of lubricants and hormone
creams. There are some valuable exceptions, including the integrated mind-
body-relationship program of Foley, Kope, and Sugrue (2002). Their self-help
program involves assessment and bodily awareness exercises for the woman,
with emphasis on the pelvic floor muscles, sensate focus exercises for the
couple, the use of fantasy, and designing the ideal sexual encounter.
It is worth remembering that some of the so-called inevitable changes
associated with aging and decreasing hormone levels may be subject to pre-
vention. Masters and Johnson, among others, would say ‘‘use it or lose it.’’
Women who remain sexually active, whether alone or with partners, are less
likely to have problems with vaginal dryness or lack of lubrication than women
who undergo prolonged periods of abstinence.
200 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Vaginismus
Perhaps the ultimate obstacle to intercourse is vaginismus. Vaginismus has
been described as an involuntary, reflexive spasm of the muscles of the outer
third of the vagina and perineum, preventing vaginal penetration. It varies in
severity from women who cannot tolerate vaginal penetration in any form to
women who can insert tampons and perhaps a finger during sexual stimula-
tion. Some can even weather a gynecological exam (albeit, with hesitation and
discomfort) but still tighten up at the prospect of intercourse. Men who at-
tempt penile penetration with women diagnosed with vaginismus report that
as they try to get past the vaginal entrance, ‘‘It feels like I’m hitting a brick
wall.’’ If they endeavor to push further, it will hurt the woman as well as the
man’s penile glans.
It is common for women with vaginismus to have orgasms via self-
stimulation as well as manual and oral stimulation with a partner. What, then,
brings them to therapy? Sometimes they seek treatment because they or their
partners would like to be able to engage in intercourse. Sometimes they are
referred for treatment by physicians unable to perform a pelvic exam. In many
cases, the partners have developed a broad enough sexual repertoire such that
they are able to satisfy one another (Hawton & Catalan, 1990; Kaplan, 1987;
Pridal & LoPiccolo, 1993; Valins, 1992) with no particular desire for inter-
course. In these instances, there is no need for therapy unless and until the
couple wants to conceive a baby. Indeed, therapists note that the timing
of couples seeking sex therapy for treatment of vaginismus is often linked to
increased pressure from in-laws for grandchildren. It is striking that there is not
a single case of vaginismus in lesbian couples reported in the literature.
There is extensive speculation about the origins of vaginismus. Certainly,
any time a person feels tension or fear, the body responds accordingly: the
muscles contract. Women diagnosed with vaginismus tend to have consider-
able anxiety about intercourse. More specifically, many are afraid of pain during
intercourse, which results in the muscles at the vaginal opening tightening,
making intercourse very difficult, if not impossible. Some, still virgins, are afraid
based on horror stories they have heard about pain and bleeding on the wed-
ding night. Others have already attempted intercourse but the pain they have
dreaded leads to a self-fulfilling prophecy; sure enough, attempting intercourse
when terrified leads the body to shut down, the vaginal muscles to shut tightly,
and then inevitably to pain. Sometimes, vaginismus results from a history of
pain during intercourse, in which case it is important to evaluate the cause of
this pain. Others are afraid because of a prior history of sexual abuse and
assault. (There is debate about the prevalence of trauma history among women
with this condition.) Almost inevitably, women diagnosed with vaginismus
come from sex-negative backgrounds with little to no sex education. One cue
mentioned in gynecological textbooks for diagnosing vaginismus is that the
vaginal muscles are positioned just as the lips might be in saying ‘‘no.’’ Perhaps
Sex Therapy 201
her body is sending a message about her reservations, which should be taken
seriously rather than just trying, literally, to push past it. It may be advisable to
conceive of the vaginal spasm as a symptom, related to her underlying fear(s)
and feelings about intercourse rather than a disorder per se. However, all the
speculation about causes, meaning, or even the purpose of vaginismus is
generally considered irrelevant (at least in the literature) for treatment.
To the extent that the goal in treating vaginismus is for women to gain
control over their vaginal muscles, thereby enabling intercourse, most of the
time this objective is attained regardless. In fact, of all the sexual dysfunctions
in women, vaginismus was long considered the easiest to treat, with the
highest success rate (i.e., virtually 100 percent in the short term [Kaplan, 1974;
Masters & Johnson, 1970]). The major treatment method involves the use of
graduated, plastic, vaginal dilators. Women are trained in the insertion and
‘‘containment’’ of the dilators, progressing in size from the narrowest to the
widest over the course of the treatment program. Women are encouraged also
to learn and practice relaxation techniques and Kegel (i.e., vaginal muscle)
exercises. Biofeedback is increasingly employed as an adjunct to therapy.
Eventually, women are to make the transition from plastic dilators to penises,
preferably with the man encouraged to lie motionless on his back until the
woman feels comfortably in control.
Notwithstanding the effectiveness of these ‘‘desensitization’’ techniques in
eliminating the symptom of vaginismus, this treatment approach has been crit-
icized as dehumanizing to the woman and her partner (Kleinplatz, 1998;
Nicolson, 1993; Shaw, 1994; Ushher, 1993). It may ‘‘work,’’ but the therapy
process may ‘‘succeed’’ at overcoming a vaginal spasm by ignoring the rest of
the person. Overriding her anxieties and fears in order to achieve mastery over
her body may leave her feeling disconnected and alienated from her partner. It
also ignores the possibility that she may simply not be willing to engage in
sexual intercourse. Suggested alternatives help the woman to become centered
and integrated enough to deal with her feelings openly and directly rather than
through the symptom of the vaginal spasm (Kleinplatz, 1998; Shaw, 1994).
genital pain, it is very unlikely that it will be detected. The ‘‘clean bill of
health’’ does not mean much if the physician did not ask the right questions. It
is important and helpful for the woman to tell her physician in as much detail
as possible what is bothering her. Correspondingly, the physician will not
be able to learn much without asking specific questions about where exactly
it hurts, during which sexual (or other) acts, what triggers it and makes it
stop, how intense the pain is and what is the nature of the pain. Given that
many women and their physicians are uncomfortable discussing these subjects
with one another, the sex therapist often plays the role of detective and
facilitator.
The questions suggested above help to track the source of the pain. For
example, burning pain on the external genitalia during oral sex or manual
stimulation would likely lead to rather different investigations and diagnoses
than sharp pain at the vaginal opening at the beginning of intercourse versus an
aching thud on deep penetration. Pain can be caused by anything from STIs to
yeast infections, endometriosis, allergies, hormonal changes, and vaginismus to
episiotomy scars that have healed poorly. (An episiotomy is the incision often
made into a woman’s perineum during childbirth to prevent tearing. It is more
routine in the United States than anywhere else. Research and experience in
other countries have demonstrated clearly that most women usually do not
need an episiotomy and that the risks outweigh the benefits.) Much of the
recent focus has been on the pain of vulvar vestibulitis (i.e., generalized,
chronic pain of the external genitalia [see Bergeron et al., 2002]) and vulva-
dynia (i.e., chronic pain at the vaginal opening).
There is quite a bit of controversy as to whether or not dyspareunia should
be classified as a sexual dysfunction or as a pain disorder (Binik, Meana, Berkley,
& Khalifé, 1999; Binik, 2005). As Binik et al. asked in the title of one of their
articles, ‘‘Is the pain sexual or is the sex painful?’’ They argue that the current
classification of dyspareunia as a psychologically based disorder manifested as a
physical ‘‘sexual’’ symptom is erroneous and conspicuous. All other pain dis-
orders (e.g., back pain, headaches) are classified and treated together using the
same rationale; it is only dyspareunia that seems to have been singled out as
different, with the focus on the sex rather than the pain. Binik (2005) states that
our understanding of women’s genital pain and its treatment must be revised. It
should be reconceptualized as a pain disorder that interferes with a woman’s
quality of life, including but not limited to her sexual expression.
The treatment of dyspareunia must, of course, be related to the underlying
cause of the pain. The ideal goal is to cure the source of the pain. When that is
not possible, and even when it is, considering that she is now suffering from
chronic pain, treatment should be multifaceted. Experts trained in sex therapy,
biofeedback, pain management techniques, and physiotherapy will be required
to work together with gynecologists and other physicians to diagnose and treat
women or couples whose lives have been limited by pain.
Sex Therapy 203
Key phrases for therapists to note include ‘‘The passion is gone’’ and ‘‘I
don’t feel any connection.’’ Such statements may indicate that although
nothing is wrong with either of them, there may not be anything right going
on, either. The couple may be technically proficient and there may be no
mechanical failures. However, the sex may be lackluster enough that it is not
really worth wanting (Kleinplatz, 1996; Schnarch, 1991). Many people are
justifiably disillusioned by their sex lives. One or both may be having their
required quota of orgasms yet disappointed at the absence of eroticism during
sex. In such cases, the question is: ‘‘What would make it worth your while to
get excited?’’ It is all too common for people to be perceived as having low
sexual desire when, actually, they refuse to settle for mediocre sex and are
geared toward ecstasy (Ogden, 1999). Sometimes, sex therapists can be most
helpful by going beyond the purely ‘‘clinical’’ to deal with all the dimen-
sions of desire including the emotional, intellectual, erotic, and even spiritual
(Ogden, 1999, 2001).
Given the complexity of sexual desire complaints, therapies must be in-
dividualized to deal with the particular problems of each unique individual or
couple. There are few standardized therapy methods. This is greatly disap-
pointing to many of those who come to therapy expecting quick-fix solutions.
The popular discourse in the media suggests that desire is just a matter of
hormones. Although hormone levels are relevant, simply increasing them is
rarely sufficient to solve desire problems. Sometimes, the cause of the desire
problems is primarily biochemical, as when a person’s desire drops dramatically
after starting to use certain drugs (e.g., SSRIs, sedatives, alcohol, cocaine).
Even in these relatively straightforward instances, depending on how long
it has been since the individual last experienced desire, it may be awkward
getting back into having sex and may often require some coaching during sex
therapy. Bibliotherapy may offer useful suggestions and exercises for couples
who are struggling with uncomplicated desire problems or may be used as an
adjunct to sex therapy (e.g., McCarthy & McCarthy, 2003). Couples must do
their own work between sessions, arranging time to be both intimate and
sexual (Ellison, 2001).
Most of the time, sexual desire problems require high levels of honesty in
therapy in order to tease out the interplay of contributing factors and to deal
with each of them. These may be as diverse as history of incest, treatment for
cancer, arguments about child-rearing practices, reluctance to engage in oral
sex, and distrust following an affair. Thus, a one-size-fits-all solution to these
problems, all appearing as ‘‘low sexual desire,’’ is unlikely to help.
CONCLUSION
Sex therapy is still in its infancy. For a field not yet fifty years old, a great
deal has been accomplished. The complexity of sexual difficulties and the
many reasons, meanings, and purposes underlying them remain a challenge.
Sex Therapy 205
REFERENCES
Advocates for Youth (1999). European approaches to adolescent sexual behavior and
responsibility. Washington, DC: Author.
Althof, S. E. (1998). New roles for mental health clinicians in the treatment of
erectile dysfunction. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 23(3), 229–231.
Althof, S. E. (2001). My personal distress over the inclusion of personal distress.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 27(2), 123–125.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., Text Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Apfelbaum, B. (2000). Retarded ejaculation: A much misunderstood syndrome.
In S. R. Leiblum & R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Principles and Practice of Sex
Therapy (3rd ed., pp. 205–241). New York: Guilford.
Assalian, P. (1994). Premature ejaculation: Is it really psychogenic? Journal of Sex
Education and Therapy, 20(1), 1–4.
Barbach, L. (2000). For yourself: The fulfillment of female sexuality. New York: Signet
Books.
Barth, R. J., & Kinder, B. N. (1987). The mislabeling of sexual impulsivity.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 13, 15–23.
Bergeron, S., Brown, C., Lord, M.-J., Oala, M., Binik, Y. M., & Khalifé, S.
(2002). Physical therapy for vulvar vestibulitis syndrome: A retrospective
study. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 28(3), 183–192.
Binik, Y. M. (2005). Should dyspareunia be retained as a sexual dysfunction in
DSM-V? A painful classification decision. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
34(1), 11–22.
Binik, Y. M., Meana, M., Berkley, K., & Khalifé, S. (1999). The sexual pain
disorders: Is the pain sexual or is the sex painful? Annual Review of Sex
Research, 10, 210–235.
Carnes, P. (1991). Don’t call it love: Recovery from sexual addiction. New York:
Bantam.
206 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
RECOMMENDED READINGS
Barbach, L. (2000). For yourself: The fulfillment of female sexuality. New York: Signet
Books.
Courtois, C. (1988). Healing the incest wound. New York: W. W. Norton.
Dodson, B. (2002). Orgasms for two. New York: Harmony.
Daniluk, J. C. (1998). Women’s sexuality across the life span: Challenging myths,
creating meanings. New York: Guilford.
Ducharme, S. H., & Gill, K. M. (1997). Sexuality after spinal cord injury: Answers to
your questions. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Ellison, C. R. (2000). Women’s sexualities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Feuerstein, G. (1992). Sacred sexuality: Living the vision of the erotic spirit. New York:
Putnam.
Foley, S., Kope, S. A., & Sugrue, D. (2002). Sex matters for women. New York:
Guilford.
Francouer, R., Cornog, M., & Perper, T. (Eds.). (2000). Sex, love and marriage in the
21st century: The next sexual revolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Gottman, J. M. (1999). The marriage clinic: A scientifically-based marital therapy. New
York: W. W. Norton.
Helminiak, D. A. (1996). The human core of spirituality: Mind as psyche and spirit.
New York: State University of New York Press.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
Kaplan, H. S. (1974). The new sex therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Kaschak, E., & Tiefer, L. (Eds.). (2001). A new view of women’s sexual problems.
New York: Haworth.
Kleinplatz, P. J. (Ed.). (2001). New directions in sex therapy: Innovations and alter-
natives. Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge.
210 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Krotoski, D. M., Nosek, M. A., Turk, M. A. (Eds.). (1996). Women with physical
disabilities: achieving and maintaining health and well-being. Baltimore: Paul H.
Brookes.
Leiblum, S. R., & Rosen, R. C. (2000). Principles and practice of sex therapy. (3rd
Ed.) New York: Guilford.
Levant, R. F., & Brooks, G. R. (Eds.). (1997). Single Men and sex: New psychological
perspectives. New York: Wiley.
Levine, S., Althof, S., & Reisen, C. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of clinical sexuality for
mental health professionals. New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Lusterman, D. (1998). Infidelity: A survival guide. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Malone, T. P., & Malone, P. T. (1987). The art of intimacy. New York: Prentice
Hall.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. Boston: Little,
Brown.
Moser, C. (1999). Health care without shame: A handbook for the sexually diverse and
their caregivers. San Francisco: Greenery Press.
Nelson, J. B., & Longfellow, S. P. (1994). Sexuality and the sacred: Sources for
theological reflection. Louisville, KY: Westminster/John Knox Press.
Ogden, G. (1999). Women who love sex. Boston: Womanspirit Press.
Reiss, I. L. (1990). An end to shame: Shaping our next sexual revolution. Buffalo, NY:
Prometheus Books.
Schiavi, R. C. (1999). Aging and male sexuality. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Schnarch, D. (1991). Constructing the sexual crucible: An integration of sexual and
marital therapy. New York: W. W. Norton.
Schnarch, D. (1997). Passionate marriage: Love, sex, and intimacy in emotionally
committed relationships. New York: Wiley.
Schover, L. R., & Jensen, S. B. (1998). Sexuality and chronic illness: a comprehensive
approach. New York: Guilford.
Schover, L. R. (1997). Sexuality and fertility after cancer. New York: Wiley.
Schwartz, P. (1994). Peer marriage: How love between equals really works. New York:
Free Press.
Sipski, M. L., & Alexander, C. J. (Eds.). (1997). Sexual function in people with disability
and chronic illness: A health professional’s guide. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
Tiefer, L. (2004). Sex is not a natural act and other essays. Boulder: Westview Press.
Treadway, D. (2004). Intimacy, change and other therapeutic mysteries. New York:
Guilford.
Wincze, J. P., & Carey, M. P. (2001). Sexual dysfunction: A guide for assessment and
treatment. New York: Guilford.
Zilbergeld, B. (1999). The new male sexuality. New York: Bantam Books.
T o L o c a t e a C e r t i fi e d S e x T h e r a p i s t
The Board of Examiners in Sex Therapy and Counseling of Ontario,
www.BESTCO.info
Sex Therapy 211
Sexual Compulsivity:
Issues and Challenges
Michael Reece, Brian M. Dodge,
and Kimberly McBride 1
For the most part, the world seems to operate under the assumption that sexual
decision making occurs as a result of some logical and rational process.
However, these logical and rational foundations become questioned when
individuals participate in sexual activities that appear to be inconsistent with
others’ perceptions of their professional or societal roles or their personal
perceptions of themselves. Perhaps one of the most famous public cases to be
considered in recent times was the notion, widely circulated in the popular
press, that President Bill Clinton was a ‘‘sex addict’’ due to the inconsistency
between his role in society and his apparent inability to control his sexual
urges. Mostly this was related to his affair with a White House intern and the
ensuing public debate and attempts to remove him from office. This con-
troversy launched the notion of ‘‘sexual addiction’’ into the national spotlight,
with numerous television shows, magazines, newspapers, and Internet sites
discussing the concept of sex addiction and using the behavior of the president
of the United States as an example for debate and discussion.
While public discussion over this particular episode has subsided, one
remaining artifact is that individuals throughout society continue to attempt
to understand the sexual behaviors of others and themselves that they perceive
to be ‘‘out of control’’ or ‘‘compulsive.’’ These sexual behaviors and their
characteristics have been called by many different names, with the most
common being nymphomania, hypersexuality, sexual compulsivity, sexual
addiction, and sexual impulsivity. These terms are used interchangeably by
214 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
some professionals who work in the sexuality field, and often one’s choice of a
term reflects his or her personal beliefs and professional training. These terms
are also used in different ways, depending on whether one is describing a
psychological characteristic of an individual (i.e., someone is a ‘‘sex addict’’ or
a ‘‘sexually compulsive person’’) or describing a particular behavior (i.e., some
believe that excessive use of the Internet for sexual purposes is an example of a
‘‘sexually compulsive behavior’’). That different terms are used to describe the
phenomenon of a person being sexually ‘‘out of control’’ or that a behavior is
‘‘sexually compulsive’’ is also the result of the fact that there remains a great
deal of debate as to whether the phenomenon of sexual compulsivity truly
exists, and if it does, how it is measured. For consistency throughout this
chapter, we will use the label ‘‘sexual compulsivity’’ to refer to behaviors and
their characteristics that some people perceive to be problematic.
A range of sexual behaviors have been considered to be indicative of sexual
compulsivity, such as excessive masturbation, having high numbers of sex-
ual partners, excessive use of the Internet for sexual purposes (sexual chatting
online, viewing sexually explicit videos, etc.), and looking for sex or having
sexual interactions in public spaces (often called ‘‘cruising’’ or ‘‘dogging’’),
among others. However, the problem with any of these behaviors, either alone
or in combination, being indicative of sexual compulsivity is that there are no
established criteria for distinguishing among behaviors that are a normal part of
one’s sexual repertoire and when these behaviors have become excessive. How
much masturbation is too much masturbation? How many sexual partners are
too many? How many hours can one spend chatting with others about sex on
the Internet before it is considered problematic? The lack of a solid answer to
any of these questions makes reaching consensus on the phenomenon of sexual
compulsivity very challenging for both professionals and members of the general
population.
Scale Items
various areas of their lives. For example, a person spending a great deal of time
viewing sexually explicit materials on the Internet may not necessarily be in-
dicative of sexual compulsivity, but if that behavior results in the individual’s
inability to relate to a romantic or relational partner or if it creates financial
challenges, then it might indicate that their Internet-based sexual activities have
become problematic. Table 10.2 provides a list of the impacts that could be
indicative of sexually compulsive behavior.
Regardless of the lack of consensus on the meaning of sexual compul-
sivity, and on how to assess it and treat it, researchers have devoted a significant
amount of energy into studying it and trying to understand how it is related to
sexual health issues. In the following section of this chapter, we will present
three examples of this work from our own research.1 Three specific lines of
research are presented, including work that has focused on sexual compulsivity
and its associations with continued high-risk sexual behaviors among those
Impact Description
living with HIV, work exploring the extent to which sexual compulsivity is an
issue among young adults, and research that looked into sexual compulsivity
among individuals who seek sexual interactions in public places, known as
cruising (which is perhaps one of the issues that is often cited as being a clear
example of out-of-control sexual behavior).
In a separate work, we also found some other trends that suggest we need to
understand more about sexual compulsivity as we continue to respond to the
challenges of the HIV/AIDS epidemic. Gay and bisexual men who scored
highly on a measure of sexual compulsivity and who reported that they had been
participating in unprotected intercourse indicated that they were most likely to
participate in these behaviors with individuals they met in anonymous settings
(sex clubs, bathhouses, etc.) and with men whose HIV status was unknown
(Reese & Dodge, 2003, 2004).
These studies are highly consistent with the work of other researchers
in the field of HIV/AIDS and, while certainly not conclusive, indicate a
continuing need for us to understand whether knowledge of a phenomenon
like sexual compulsivity may help public health professionals better target
their efforts to reduce the incidence of HIV infections. It may be that those
who are struggling to control their sexual behaviors are among those most
appropriate for specialized interventions if we were able to more appropriately
assess them and deliver programs to them in a way that does not further
stigmatize them or label their sexual behaviors as pathological. (See HIV Case
Scenario.)
meet men other than on the Internet? Does he lack the skills to in-
troduce condoms into his sexual interactions?
Linkages to Research: Alfred is obviously struggling to control his
constant need to seek sexual partners on the Internet and this is some-
thing that he is wanting to better control. His Internet-based behavior
seems compulsive on the surface, and his lack of HIV disclosure to his
partners and his high rates of unprotected insertive intercourse is con-
sistent with our research among men who score highly on measures of
sexual compulsivity. It is certain that the potential for him to have
transmitted HIV to other men is a problem that needs to be addressed
immediately. However, rather than assume his claim that he is a ‘‘sex
addict’’ is correct, his therapist may want to work with him to increase
his knowledge of the non–bar-oriented venues for meeting men in his
city, teach him skills for using condoms and ask him to make a com-
mitment to use condoms in all sexual interactions, regardless of the
extent to which his partners ask about HIV. While some aspects of his
behaviors may be compulsive, Alfred is an example of those who are
labeled (or who self-labeled) sexually compulsive, but perhaps his
concerns over the inability to control his behavior can be resolved with
strategic methods introduced by his therapist that increase his comfort
with his bisexuality and his ability to use condoms consistently in all
interactions.
this an example of normal sexual behavior given Jason’s age and the
social norms of the college environment?
Linkages to Research: Jason is clearly uncomfortable with his sexual
behaviors. However, research has yet to establish what sexual compul-
sivity truly looks like among young adults. Late adolescence and early
adulthood are often a time of sexual exploration. Research conducted
with college students has shown that having multiple sexual partners is
not uncommon. Further, among young men, masturbation frequencies
have been found to be higher than among older men or women. On the
surface, it is difficult to assess whether or not Jason’s behaviors truly are
‘‘out of control’’ or if they are relatively ‘‘normal.’’ Certainly, his lack of
condom use is an issue in terms of both pregnancy and HIV/STI
transmission. If his masturbation is causing him to miss classes or perform
poorly on homework assignments, then it may be indicative of sexual
compulsivity. In this case, the therapist would need to assess to what
extent Jason’s behaviors are impacting his life. Often times young adults
need education about what is normal sexual behavior and what is ex-
cessive. The therapist would definitely need to provide education about
HIV and STI transmission and encourage condom use. The therapist
should also determine how many women Jason has ‘‘hooked up’’ with
and explore his feelings of guilt related to these encounters. Until a
proper assessment is performed, it would be premature to make any
assumptions about Jason’s behavior.
are totally committed to our kids, and our nights are pretty full taking care of
them. My other sex life . . . my cruising sex life is very intense. And those
guys, I meet them all in public sex environments. I feel so ‘‘complicated’’
sometimes.
Considerations: Is Michael experiencing sexual compulsivity in his life
or is he merely enjoying the ‘‘sexual freedom’’ that many individuals
report as a reason for engaging in sex with multiple partners? At what
point can the line be drawn? How are Michael’s body image issues
related to his cruising behaviors? Do his self-esteem issues act to re-
inforce, or hinder, the cruising? What are the implications of Michael’s
behaviors in terms of his job and home life, in terms of health, legal
repercussions, etc.?
Linkages to Research: Although he does not explicitly identify ‘‘sexual
compulsivity’’ as a problem in his life, Michael has presented concerns in
terms of his sexual behaviors being out of control and causing problems
in his life, particularly in terms of mental and social health. He has
described problems in several key areas of the SASH (see Table 10.2)
framework for understanding sexual compulsivity. Bill may benefit from
consulting with a health care provider who is trained to diagnose and
treat compulsive sexual behavior, if he is deemed to be dealing with
such issues.
While these three areas of research indicate support for the notion that
sexual compulsivity exists in some form among a diverse range of individuals
and that it is associated with certain sexual behaviors, there is a need for much
more research in these areas before any solid conclusions can be drawn. Ad-
ditionally, there is a need to continue identifying ways of responding to sexual
compulsivity among those who appear to be struggling with it by making
available effective forms of treatment.
TREATMENT ISSUES
One of the most obvious challenges to providing treatment for clients
presenting with sexual compulsivity is the lack of a clear set of diagnostic and
treatment criteria. Without a clear set of criteria to serve as a guide, diagnosis
and treatment can become an arbitrary endeavor. The Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association,
1994) contains three general categories under which a diagnosis can be made:
Paraphilia (either specific or not otherwise specified [NOS]); Impulse Control
Disorder NOS; or Sexual Disorder NOS.
For mental health practitioners who may have little or no specialized
training in the treatment of sexuality related issues, this ambiguity is problem-
atic. Selecting the most effective treatment for a given disorder presumes a clear
226 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Twelve-Step Groups
The earliest approach to treatment, and one that is still common today,
was adapted from the twelve-step model of Alcoholics Anonymous. The
underlying premise of this approach is that sexual compulsivity represents a
form of addiction much like alcoholism, drug addiction, or compulsive
gambling. In this treatment paradigm, individuals are believed to be powerless
over the amount or kind of sexual activity in which they engage (Carnes,
1989; Myers, 1995). One of the key components in this approach is the belief
that the sexually addicted individual is overwhelmed by shame. In order for
sex addicts to recover, they must progress through the twelve steps by ad-
mitting they have a problem, relinquishing control to God, admitting the
nature of their wrongs, making amends when possible, and sharing the message
with other addicts (Sex and Love Addicts Anonymous, 1985).
The twelve-step approach relies on groups of self-identified addicts
coming together to share their stories and support one another through the
recovery process. Typically, the groups do not include a trained facilitator or
therapist. Instead, the groups are facilitated solely by members who encourage
one another to abstain from problematic sexual behaviors and carry the
message to other sex addicts. The only requirement for membership in such
groups is the desire to stop acting out a pattern of sex addiction.
Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy
Cognitive-behavioral therapies refer to a set of techniques that are based
on the assumption that behavior can be altered by changing cognitive pro-
cesses (thoughts, beliefs, attitudes, assumptions). In this approach, maladaptive
cognitive processes are assumed to be the underlying cause of maladaptive
behaviors, and these negative thoughts are believed to be modifiable, both
directly and indirectly, through therapeutic techniques (Montesinos, 2003).
Within the cognitive-behavioral approach, there are two different groups
of strategies that are assumed to influence behavioral change. The first group of
strategies is referred to as cognitive restructuring. Cognitive restructuring fo-
cuses specifically on changing maladaptive thought processes (internal dialogue
or self-talk) in a clear and direct manner. The second group of strategies is
Sexual Compulsivity 227
Pharmacotherapy
The appropriateness of using psychotropic medications in the treatment of
sexual compulsivity is still controversial in the scientific community. Certain
scientists and therapists believe that medications called selective serotonin re-
uptake inhibitors (SSRIs, such as Prozac and Paxil), which are commonly used
to treat depression, are highly effective for treating sexual compulsivity (Kafka,
1991; Ragan & Martin, 2000). Their argument is that these medications both
decrease sexual urges and alleviate the depression that results from feeling out
of control. Opponents to this approach argue that a common side effect of
antidepressant medication is a diminished libido, therefore these medications
may be temporarily masking the issues rather than treating it. So, unless an
individual wants to commit to a lifetime of daily use, antidepressants should
only be used in combination with other treatment approaches.
Antianxiety medications are another type of psychotropic drugs that have
been used to treat sexual compulsivity. These medications are thought to
reduce anxieties that either drive compulsive behaviors or result from them.
Again, critics argue that these treatments may be useful in reducing the neg-
ative thoughts and feelings associated with sexual compulsivity but should not
be used as a primary means of treatment.
Psychodynamic Psychotherapy
Psychodynamic therapy evolved from Sigmund Freud’s psychoanalytic
theory, which assumes that sexual and aggressive impulses are the primary
228 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Twelve-Step Program
Sex Addicts Anonymous—SAA www.sexaa.org
Sexual Compulsives Anonymous—SCA www.sca-recovery.org
Sex and Love Addicts Anonymous—SLAA www.slaafws.org
Sexaholics Anonymous—SA www.sa.org
Sexual Recovery Anonymous—SRA sexualrecovery.org
For Family and Friends
Recovering Couples Anonymous (for couples www.recovering-couples.org
when one member of the couple goes to an-
other twelve-step group)
Codependents of Sex Addicts (related to SAA) www.cosa-recovery.org
S-Anon (related to SA) www.sanon.org
Professional Association
Society for the Advancement of Sexual Health www.ncsac.org
SUMMARY
Clearly, there is a great deal of professional and public interest in the
notion of sexual compulsivity. While much attention has been given to
probable consequences of such behavior, particularly in terms of HIV/STI
risk, little is actually known about the underlying causes or the actual outcomes
of such behavior. As often is the case with any type of science, answering
questions related to sexual compulsivity is an arduous process. Unfortunately,
the pace at which research is being conducted is not keeping up with the
demand of those who want or need answers. HIV and STDs continue to be
transmitted, and a growing number of individuals are self-identifying, or are
being identified by others, as ‘‘sex addicts.’’ Further complicating the matter, a
growing number of mental health practitioners are seeing clients with issues
related to out-of-control sexual behavior without knowing if these treatments
actually work. There are currently a number of efforts underway to answer
these pressing questions; however, until the results are in, we cannot make
assumptions about the behaviors or the individuals who present with them. It
is highly likely that for some individuals certain sexual behaviors are, indeed,
problematic. For others, applying the sexually compulsive label may be in-
appropriate and damaging. Until we know what sexual compulsivity really is
and how it is manifested behaviorally, we need to acknowledge what we do
not know and avoid unwarranted speculation and moral judgments.
230 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
NOTE
1. The studies were conducted by Reese, Dodge, and colleagues.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Benotsch, E. G., Kalichman, S. C., & Kelly, J. A. (1999). Sexual compulsivity and
substance use in HIV-seropositive men who have sex with men: Prev-
alence and predictors of high-risk behaviors. Addictive Behaviors, 24(6),
857–868.
Carnes, P. (1983). Out of the shadows: Understanding sexual addiction. Minneapolis,
MN: Compcare.
Carnes, P. (1989). Contrary to love: Helping the sex addict. Center City, MN: Hazeldon.
Coleman, E. (1990). The obsessive-compulsive model for describing compulsive
sexual behavior. American Journal of Preventive Psychiatry and Neurology,
2(3), 9–14.
Dodge, B., Reece, M., Cole, S., & Sandfort, T. G. M. (2004). Sexual compul-
sivity among heterosexual college students. Journal of Sex Research, 41(4),
343–350.
Gold, S. N., & Heffner, C. L. (1998). Sexual addiction: Many conceptions,
minimal data. Clinical Psychology Review, 18, 367–381.
Heckman, T. G., Kelly, J. A., & Somlai, A. M. (1998). Predictors of continued
high-risk sexual behavior in a community sample of persons living with
HIV/AIDS. AIDS and Behavior, 2(2), 127–135.
Kafka, M. P. (1991). Successful antidepressant treatment of nonparaphilic sexual
addictions and paraphilias in men. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 52(2), 60–65.
Kalichman, S. C., & Fisher, J. D. (1998). Introduction to the special issue on risk
behavior practices of men and women living with HIV-AIDS. AIDS and
Behavior, 2(2), 87–88.
Kalichman, S. C., Greenberg, J., & Abel, G. G. (1997). HIV-seropositive men
who engage in high-risk sexual behavior: Psychological characteristics and
implications for prevention. AIDS Care, 9(9), 441–450.
Kalichman, S. C., Johnson, J. R., Adair, V., Rompa, D., Multhauf, K., & Kelly,
J. A. (1994). Sexual sensation seeking: Scale development and predicting
AIDS-risk behavior among homosexually active men. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 62, 385–397.
Kalichman, S. C., Kelly, J. A., & Rompa, D. (1997). Continued high-risk sex
among HIV seropositive gay and bisexual men seeking HIV prevention
services. Health Psychology, 16(4), 369–373.
Kalichman, S. C., Roffman, R. A., Picciano, J. F., & Bolan, M. (1997). Sexual
relationships, sexual behavior, and HIV infection: HIV-seropositive gay
and bisexual men seeking prevention services. Professional Psychology: Re-
search and Practice, 28(3), 355–360.
Sexual Compulsivity 231
Kalichman, S. C., & Rompa, D. (2001). The sexual compulsivity scale: Further
development and use with HIV-positive persons. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 76(3), 379–395.
Kalichman, S. C., & Rompa, D. (1995). Sexual sensation seeking and compul-
sivity scales: Reliability, validity, and predicting HIV risk behavior. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 65, 586–601.
Marks, G., Burris, S., & Peterman, T. A. (1999). Reducing sexual transmission of
HIV from those who know they are infected: The need for personal and
collective responsibility. AIDS, 13(3), 297–306.
Montesinos, L. (2003). Behavioral analysis, therapy, and counseling. In J. Donnelly
(Ed.), Health counseling: Application and theory (pp. 55–71). Belmont, CA:
Thomson-Wadsworth.
Myers, W. A. (1995). Addictive sexual behavior. American Journal of Psychotherapy,
49, 473–483.
Quadland, M. C., & Shattls, W. D. (1987). AIDS, sexuality, and sexual control.
Journal of Homosexuality, 14(1/2), 277–298.
Ragan, P. W., & Martin, P. R. (2000). The psychobiology of sex addiction. Sex
Addiction and Compulsivity, 7, 161–175.
Reece, M. (2003). Sexual compulsivity and HIV serostatus disclosure among men
who have sex with men. Sexual Addiction and Compulsivity, 10, 1–11.
Reece, M., Plate, P., & Daughtry, M. (2001). HIV prevention and sexual com-
pulsivity: The need for an integrated strategy of public health and mental
health. Sexual Addiction and Compulsivity, 8, 157–167.
Reece, M., & Dodge, B. (2003). Exploring the physical, mental and social well-
being of gay and bisexual men who cruise for sex on a college campus.
Journal of Homosexuality, 46(1/2), 111–126.
Reece, M., & Dodge, B. (2004). Exploring indicators of sexual compulsivity
among men who cruise for sex on campus. Sexual Addiction and Compul-
sivity, 11, 87–113.
Sex and Love Addicts Anonymous. (1985). Twelve steps and twelve traditions.
Retrieved October 1, 2005, from www.slaafws.org/12and12.html.
Society for the Advancement of Sexual Health. (2005). Sexual addiction. Retrieved
October 3, 2005, from www.ncsac.org/addicts/papers_sexual_addiction
.aspx
11
The brain, it has been said, is the most important sex organ—the implication
being that thoughts and feelings about sex may provide sufficient motivation to
overcome some physical limitations and allow for a sexually satisfying experi-
ence despite physical disability or other obstacles. And the truth of this statement
becomes quite clear when considering the sexual functioning of individuals
living with spinal cord injuries, individuals recovering from heart attacks or
strokes, and individuals living with other chronic health conditions that impede
physical activity or functioning and interfere with the performance of sexual
activities—especially in the manner presented by filmmakers and advertisers in
mainstream media. In fact, many individuals living with disabilities (and espe-
cially disabilities associated with physical limitations) are seen as asexual. But
sexuality is central to our being, regardless of physical appearance or health status
(Kroll & Klein, 1995).
Sexuality, while fundamental, is a delicate area of life, and often the first area
to suffer a major disruption in the face of stress or illness. For individuals in
relationships with difficulties prior to the onset of illness or disability, these
difficulties are likely to be magnified when health issues arise (Kievman, 1989;
McGonigle, 1999; McInnes, 2003; Wallerstein & Blakeslee, 1996). This is
especially true when such conditions result from accidents or when the disability
comes about in an otherwise unexpected manner.
There can be considerable variation in the manner and degree to which an
illness or disability affects sexuality. Conditions may be congenital or present
234 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
from birth (e.g., cerebral palsy), acquired (e.g., spinal cord injuries or acute
illness), or developed slowly over time (e.g., osteoarthritis or heart disease).
Some conditions may have signs that are easily seen by even casual observers
(e.g., paraplegia or paralysis) while others may not carry any outwardly visible
signs (e.g., chronic pain or diabetes—the ‘‘invisible disabilities’’). Many health
problems significantly limit physical mobility due to functional impairments
(e.g., muscular or nerve disorders) while others limit the functioning of specific
body systems (e.g., cardiovascular or pulmonary disease). And finally, some
conditions may directly affect sexual functioning (e.g., vascular or neurological
disease resulting in erectile dysfunction) while others may have only indirect
effects (e.g., chronic low back pain). Regardless of the distinctions that can be
made and the various methods for categorizing chronic illness and disability, the
impact on sexuality and sexual functioning can be significant, and, in nearly all
cases, involves multiple factors.
Sexuality, likewise, involves multiple factors, and vital to any discussion
of sexuality and disability is the reminder that sexuality is not just about activities
culminating in sexual intercourse—it is about intimacy. Communication, trust,
confidence, and pleasurable touching are all critical components of sexuality.
Psychologist Jackson Rainer, who provides therapy to individuals with arthritis
and other chronic health conditions, suggests redefining sexuality as ‘‘an energy
that is healing, warming, and operates more outside of the genitals than in one
specific place on the body’’ (Arthritis Foundation, 2004). When viewed in
those terms, sex becomes less daunting—and becomes more about possibilities.
In the face of illness or disability, individuals and relationships may be
redefined. And while the adverse effects on a relationship are the most obvious,
some suggest that dramatic changes brought about by illness or disability disrupt
the routine and create an opportunity for a new beginning—a chance to re-
discover one’s body and what feels good, or a chance to rebuild a relationship
and improve sexual relations with a partner (Carlson, 1996; Kroll & Klein, 1995;
Maurer & Strausberg, 1989; Wallerstein & Blakeslee, 1996).
In order to emerge from a health crisis with a ‘‘new, improved’’ sexual
relationship, couples must overcome numerous obstacles. In addition to any
direct disease/disability effects on sexuality, there are often undesirable side
effects from interventions or medications used to treat the condition. The
individual’s psychological response to having a given disease or disability and
the partner’s response to changing roles are also key factors in determining the
level of disruption to a couple’s sexual life. While the focus of this chapter is a
discussion of how various chronic diseases and disabilities impact sexuality, an
underlying goal is to inform readers of the benefits that result when individuals
with chronic health problems or disabilities can find ways to express sexuality
and experience sexually satisfying activities.
The disabilities and chronic conditions included in this chapter are far from
exhaustive but are believed to be representative of a wide range of conditions that
can adversely affect sexual functioning. While there is necessarily some overlap in
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 235
CONCERNS COMMON TO
VARIOUS DISABILITIES
For each of the disabilities and limitations discussed in this chapter, the
challenge is twofold: not only is a person confronted with exploring and
identifying changes in physical capacity and sexual response, but this may also
occur in the context of psychological adjustment to a newly acquired or diag-
nosed disability. The role of both physical and psychological factors must be
considered because both of these domains are essential to healthy sexual ex-
pression (Merritt, 2004). And because sexuality is affected by multiple factors,
any difficulties that are present may require various forms of intervention.
Therefore, assistance from a physician, psychologist, occupational or physical
therapist, or personal attendant (or other health care provider) may be necessary
to address issues adequately. If the assistance of a health care specialist is nec-
essary, there is one caveat: the healthcare professional must have expertise in
treating the disability and must be open and comfortable with frank discussions
about sexuality.
Being comfortable with oneself and a partner is required when exploring or
rediscovering sexual functioning and sexuality. Whether this exploration is
through masturbation by oneself, or through mutual pleasuring, feeling com-
fortable with the process is key—if it does not feel safe, do not do it. For partners
and individuals with disabilities alike, it is important to remember that com-
munication about the experience is paramount. In order to break down ster-
eotypes that the only ‘‘real’’ sex is genital intercourse, open and honest
communication must play a central role. Individuals with disabilities must
feel free to investigate what is arousing and pleasurable, and determine what
physical capacity is available to meet these needs sexually, or how sex toys, a
partner, or an attendant may be able to help meet these needs and desires.
Limitations may force individuals to become creative in thinking about ways to
give and receive pleasure. Being flexible in thinking about sex and having fun
with it is essential. After all, ‘‘sex’’ is a broadly defined act, and encompasses
anything and everything an individual and her/his partner(s) find satisfying.
While sexuality encompasses a broad range of activities that do not always
involve genital contact, such activity may be part of intimate encounters. Dis-
abilities, whether acquired or developmental, do not prevent sexually trans-
mitted infections, nor do they always impact fertility (Hammond & Burns,
2000). Therefore, if sex involves intercourse or other contact with genitals, all
236 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
individuals are encouraged to follow ‘‘safe sex’’ practices (i.e., use condoms to
prevent transmission of sexually transmitted diseases and birth control methods
to prevent unwanted pregnancies).
Sensation Changes
Changes in sensation occur with many different disabilities. To be sure,
sensation is an important part of sexual arousal and response. After an injury or
diagnosis, it is important to discover how different areas of the body are func-
tioning with regard to sensation (Merritt, 2004). Sensation can be reduced or
heightened, or hypersensitivity may even result. Changes in sensation can be
explored alone or with a partner. While changes in genital sensation are the
focus of most discussions, the whole body should be explored for erogenous
zones and areas of sexual response. Individuals can be pleasantly surprised at the
sexual responsiveness when an earlobe or inner arm is properly stimulated.
Doing a ‘‘body mapping’’ exercise as described in Kaufman et al. (2003) can
provide a wealth of information concerning sensate pleasure. For some partners,
this may seem too ‘‘clinical,’’ and can rob the spontaneity of exploring each
other sexually. Clearly, each couple must decide on the best process for this or
similar exercises. However, some partners begin such an exercise with ‘‘good
intentions’’ and never complete the task because they become distracted by the
pleasure they are giving and receiving along the way (Kaufman et al., 2003).
In addition to seeking pleasure through sensation, safety and skin integrity
must be a focus as well. With loss in sensation comes the need to protect these areas
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 237
Impaired Mobility
For individuals facing mobility challenges, exploring these with a partner
or an attendant can be very helpful in promoting a quality sex life. As
with reduced sensation, those with mobility limitations are at risk for skin
breakdown and other injuries if care is not taken with positioning. Exploration
of different sexual positions will enable partners to determine how to max-
imize pleasure and comfort. Additionally, employing pillows, furniture (and
cushions), and sex toys can enhance the sexual experience for both partners.
Other couples may choose to use specially designed foam positioning pieces,
or slings to enable different positions (Hammond & Burns, 2000; Kaufman
et al., 2003).
Spasticity
One challenge facing individuals with a variety of disabilities is spasticity
(exaggerated, deep tendon reflexes and muscle cramps that are involuntary, and
often, painful contractions of the muscles). Because sexual arousal can bring on
spasms, it is important for people to understand how and when spasms occur,
and take steps to prevent or minimize the effects of spasticity. Bathing in warm
water is one technique for reducing spasticity, and the bath can be incorporated
into the sex play (Kaufman et al., 2003). Taking medication for spasticity prior
to engaging in sexual activity can also help reduce the disruptive effects of
spasticity on the sexual experience. As an individual becomes more comfortable
and accepting of spasticity experiences, it can be utilized to the their advantage
to enhance the sexual experience. Some people report that tongue spasticity is
wonderful for nipple stimulation, and others suggest that a hand tremor can be a
convenient tool for genital or body stimulation. Learning to channel physical
reactions into sex play can provide unusual but highly enjoyable enticement,
which further enhances the sexual experience (Kaufman et al., 2003).
Psychological Effects
Persons who suffer a traumatic injury or who are diagnosed with a dis-
abling condition can experience a variety of reactions, which can vary widely
in nature and severity. Not all individuals become clinically depressed or
anxious following such a life-altering event, and the person, treatment team,
and support persons should not ‘‘expect’’ psychopathology to arise in the wake
of an injury or diagnosis. However, adjustment to the disability and the re-
sulting life changes can take time. For some, sexual activity can take a back seat
to other life tasks during this process. Therefore, information regarding sex
238 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
should be provided and processed at various points during the treatment and
rehabilitation process, so that this important issue is not overlooked (Merritt,
2004).
NEUROLOGICAL/NEUROVASCULAR
CONDITIONS
In the following section, sexuality issues associated with neurological and
neurovascular disabilities will be addressed. Sections covering traumatic brain
injury (TBI), spinal cord injury (SCI), and cerebrovascular accident (‘‘stroke’’
or CVA) will provide more specialized information. Slightly less detailed
sections will address issues specific to multiple sclerosis (MS), epilepsy, and
cerebral palsy (CP). While this is clearly not an exhaustive review of sexuality
and neurological conditions, it is hoped that the following sections will pro-
vide a jumping-off point for persons with disabilities and their partners to
begin exploring sexuality in a healthy and fulfilling manner. At the end of the
chapter, resources to promote further exploration and support are provided.
Physiological Impact
Physical changes following TBI are variable, depending on the nature of
the injury. Very often, spinal cord injuries (SCI) occur concomitantly with the
TBI (Merritt, 2004). Hemiplegia or hemiparesis (weakness or paralysis of one
side of body) may also occur in varying degrees of severity and sensation may
be impaired as well. Additionally, spasticity can be an issue depending on the
nature of the injury. Cognitive deficits can often accompany a TBI, and like
physical changes, these deficits can vary according to the location and severity
of the injury to the brain. Issues that impact sexual functioning directly may be
problems with social or interpersonal relationships, problems with accurately
perceiving and expressing emotions, and limited insight into these concerns
(Rosenthal & Ricker, 2000).
Psychological Impact
Given the complex nature of traumatic brain injury (TBI), those who
suffer such injury and those who care for them can face a variety of issues
postinjury. In particular, an individual with TBI may experience cognitive
dysfunction, physical limitations, and personality changes, which can be dis-
tressing for both the individual and those in the individual’s social support
network. At times, personality changes can be more disturbing than any
physical or cognitive difficulties, simply because friends and family may ex-
perience the person quite differently than before the injury. Anger, irritability,
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 239
Physiological Impact
Spinal cord injury (SCI) affects men and women in different ways. How-
ever, both genders will likely be concerned with mobility, sensation, and per-
formance. As with other injuries, in SCI the level (cervical through sacral) and
completeness of the injury (as measured by the ASIA scale) to the cord will have
varying effects on physiological response. Men may experience changes in their
ability to achieve or maintain an erection, whereas women may have reduced
lubrication and ability to tighten vaginal muscles (Merritt, 2004). Both will
experience varying levels of muscle control and sensation below the level of the
injury, depending on the nature of the SCI.
For individuals with SCI, one negative side effect that can crop up during
sexual activity is autonomic dysreflexia. Signs and symptoms include pounding
headache, flushed skin, high blood pressure, slow pulse, and blurry or spotty
vision. Prompt medical treatment is a must, as the person is at high risk for
stroke, convulsions, or other medical complications if this is left untreated
(Hammond & Burns, 2000).
For women, there are often no physiological changes after injury that
prevent them from engaging in sexual activity ( Jackson & Lindsey, 1998;
Kaufman et al., 2003). Some women experience a diminished ability to produce
vaginal lubrication owing to an interruption in the nerve signals from the brain
to the genital area (Merritt, 2004). This is remedied by using a water-based
lubricant; readily available at pharmacies or drugstores. Additional varieties of
lubricants may be found online or at specialty sex shops. With the loss of muscle
control that follows, some women may not be able to tighten vaginal muscles,
resulting in reduced friction during intercourse. In order to improve friction,
some women contract their urinary muscles to increase vaginal tightness. Dif-
ferent sexual positions may improve this as well (Kaufman et al., 2003).
The ability to achieve orgasm after SCI is another prominent concern for
women. One study indicated that 54 percent of women were able to achieve
orgasm after sexual activity, and another 30 percent reported extragenital
240 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
pleasure. Some women report achieving orgasm after breast and upper body
stimulation.
For men, the ability to achieve and maintain an erection is a primary
concern. Men have two types of erections, psychogenic and reflex. In a
psychogenic erection, sexual thoughts or feelings prompt signals from the
brain to the penis that exit at the T-10 to L2 levels, resulting in an erection.
Reflex erections, on the other hand, result from direct physical contact to the
penis or stimulation of other erogenous zones such as the neck or nipples. A
reflex erection is involuntary and can occur without sexual thoughts. The
nerves that control impulses which stimulate a reflex erection are located in the
sacral area (S2–S4) of the spinal cord. If the S2–S4 pathway is not damaged,
men can generally have a reflex erection with proper stimulation (Merritt,
2004).
There are a variety of treatment options available to men who are unable
to achieve an erection sufficient for sexual activity. Medications such as Viagra
and Cialis are available with a prescription from a physician. Another option is
penile injection therapy, in which medications are injected into the shaft of the
penis, producing an erection for one to two hours following administration
(Lindsey & Klebine, 2000; Merritt, 2004). Medicated Urethral System Erec-
tion or MUSE is another treatment option that involves placing a small,
medicated pellet into the urethra. Once absorbed into the surrounding tissue,
an erection can result (Lindsey & Klebine, 2000). Vacuum pumps enable the
production of erections through mechanical means. Erections are maintained
by placing a constriction ring (also termed ‘‘cock ring’’) around the base of the
penis to prevent the blood from draining out prior to completion of sexual
activity (Merritt, 2004). Use of the ring also prevents urinary leakage that can
often occur during sex. Permanent penile prostheses are also available, but
often are a last resort given the risk of infection or injury to the penis due to
low levels of sensation (Lindsey & Klebine, 2000).
For both men and women, SCI can impact fertility levels. Following SCI,
women may experience a disruption in their menstrual cycle, but as the body
adjusts to the injury, normal cycles often resume with no effects on fertility
( Jackson & Lindsey, 1998). Therefore, birth control should be employed
accordingly, under the supervision of a physician. For women who wish to
conceive, pregnancy can present some challenges physically and psychologi-
cally, making close prenatal monitoring essential. For men, ejaculation can be
disrupted in upwards of 90 percent of men with SCI (Lindsey & Klebine,
2000). Retrograde ejaculation (where the semen travels up the urethra and is
deposited in the bladder) can also occur. Sperm count does not change;
however, the motility of sperm cells may decrease significantly. Treatment
options such as penile vibratory stimulation and rectal probe electroejaculation
are available to assist in the conception process (Kaufman et al., 2003; Lindsey
& Klebine, 2000). Both of these treatment options are performed under the
supervision of a physician.
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 241
Psychological Impact
Psychological adjustment to disability following a spinal cord injury is
complex and can present significant challenges for individuals and their fam-
ilies. It can, of course, greatly affect sexual activities as well. After surviving
the trauma of a serious injury, individuals often ask themselves and others if
they are different, aside from the obvious changes in mobility. Basic person-
ality features, styles of relating to others, or level of intelligence are unlikely to
change. However, persons with SCI and their support persons may feel a
variety of emotions including shock, sadness, and anger in response to the
injury. This is very common following a traumatic event and part of the
grieving process and part of psychological adjustment to the effects of
the injury. While this is a natural part of recovering from an injury, profes-
sional assistance may be necessary if the emotional response becomes un-
manageable and impedes progress in rehabilitation or life in general.
There are many ways to navigate the emotional recovery process, which
can be as individualized as the rehabilitation process itself. Individuals with SCI
and their partners/family members may begin examining their feelings and
attitudes toward one another in addition to their feelings toward others with
disabilities. Working on communication skills can promote healthy discussion
between those with SCI and family members and can prepare a person with
SCI to negotiate social situations following the injury. Issues such as main-
taining friendships, dating, and engaging in sexual relationships are frequent
concerns in promoting quality of life. Improving coping strategies, assertive-
ness training, and social skills during the adjustment phase can assist individuals
with SCI and families in ‘‘getting back to life.’’ When individuals with SCI and
their significant others are given adequate support in regaining psychological
intimacy, discussions concerning physical intimacy and experimentation with
sexual expression are likely to follow. As alluded to in the previous section,
psychological factors may play a more important role than the physiological
factors in all aspects of sexual functioning following SCI (Kaufman et al., 2003;
Merritt, 2004).
Multiple Sclerosis
Physiological Impact
Sexual dysfunction is commonly reported in MS. The areas of neurolog-
ical involvement and the type of MS typically coincide with the type of
sexual problems experienced by people living with MS. At times, sexual
dysfunction itself may be the initial symptom prior to diagnosis. Men fre-
quently experience erectile dysfunction (either an erection insufficient for
intercourse, or an adequate erection for an insufficient duration), difficulties
with sensation (either diminished or hypersensitivity), slowed ejaculation,
242 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Psychological Impact
As with other disabilities, those who live with MS often experience
persistent fatigue or low energy that can curtail sexual activity and diminish
sexual drive (Devins & Schnek, 2000). Although incontinence does not typ-
ically interfere with sexual activity, the potential for it or the presence
of catheters can be a source of anxiety or shame, which can detract from
the sexual experience (Leonard, 2005). Depression, a common emotional
correlate of MS, can also reduce libido and make it more difficult to become
aroused.
Open communication, understanding and being flexible about sexual
experiences can promote enjoyment for those involved. Varying the timing of
sex play, positioning, and having a good understanding of sensation and
mobility issues can enhance sexual encounters considerably (Merritt, 2004).
Additionally, emptying bowel and bladder prior to engaging in sex and using
adequate lubrication can engender additional comfort and pleasure (Leonard,
2005).
Epilepsy
Physiological Impact
As with MS, sexual problems associated with epilepsy are multifaceted,
and can involve different areas of the brain, hormone levels, physical and
psychological difficulties, along with sexual dysfunction associated with medi-
cations taken to control seizures (Morrell, 1997). Both genders may encounter
low levels of sexual desire, and difficulties with arousal. Women report ex-
periencing pain during intercourse (dyspareunia) and painful vaginal spasms
during intercourse (vaginismus) (Epilepsy Foundation, 2005). Both of these
issues are generally unrelated to diminished sexual desire or arousal, but can
often lead to avoidance of sexual intercourse due to intense discomfort. Men
with epilepsy can experience erectile problems, most often with achieving or
sustaining erections. Ejaculation can be slowed as well. Individuals living with
epilepsy can experience sexual arousal and sensations, physical exertion, and
faster breathing similar to a preseizure aura (Morrell, 1997). Because the
physiological symptoms of sexual arousal and climax are similar to preseizure
auras, sexual activity can be an especially distressing phenomenon and can have
profound effects on psychological and sexual functioning (Epilepsy and
Sexuality, 2005).
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 243
Treatment Impact
Individuals living with epilepsy often take a variety of medications to
control their seizure activity. Common side effects of these medications may
interfere with both sexual desire and the ability to become aroused. Sedation is
the most common side effect of antiepileptic medications and can be so severe
as to interfere with sex play (Morrell, 1997). Because medications often affect
individuals somewhat differently, a frank discussion with a health care provider
can assist in finding a medication regimen that is effective in controlling seizure
activity with fewer sexual side effects. For women experiencing pain during
intercourse, there are gynecological treatments available to assist with this
(Epilepsy and Sexuality, 2005).
Cerebral Palsy
Impact
For those coping with the effects of cerebral palsy (CP), addressing issues
of sexual functioning can take many paths. To be sure, CP itself does not
typically bring about any changes in arousal or erectile function (Disability and
Illness, 2004). However, issues such as social stigma, communication, spastic-
ity, pain, and positioning can affect sexual activity significantly. Research
indicates that when compared to a sample of unmarried, able-bodied men,
single men with CP demonstrated less facility with sexual information, ex-
perience, libido, and satisfaction, and experienced more psychological symp-
toms. Unmarried women with CP, conversely, demonstrated less satisfaction
with body image as compared to single, able-bodied women (Cho, Park, Park,
& Na, 2004). This highlights the idea that social stigma and psychological
factors can significantly hamper efforts aimed at achieving satisfying sexual
experiences (Disability and Illness, 2004). Partners may express concerns about
spasticity and limited range of motion as new positions are explored. And as
with other disabilities that limit mobility, this requires some patience, crea-
tivity, and openness to trying new positions, but this may lead to greater
satisfaction and excitement with partners (Disability and Illness, 2004). Ex-
perimentation should be undertaken with consideration of the safety and
comfort needs of those involved.
Cerebrovascular Accident
Physiological Impact
Much like TBI and SCI, stroke can affect a person’s sexual functioning in
a variety of ways. Physical difficulties caused by stroke include weakness,
paralysis, diminished sensation, and pain (Westcott, 2002). Reduced sensation
244 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
and pain are symptoms associated with other conditions, and the reader is
referred to the later sections of this chapter for more detailed discussion re-
garding those symptoms.
Weakness and paralysis following a stroke may make coping with the
physical changes a special challenge: activities of daily living such as bathing,
dressing, grooming, and household chores take longer and are more energy
consuming than before the stroke. Therefore, fatigue can interfere with in-
timacy (Westcott, 2002). Since sex does not always have to occur just prior to
retiring for the evening, a couple should explore alternate times to engage in
sexual activity, most notably, times when energy is higher (Merritt, 2004).
Making time for sex when energy levels are higher can result in more satisfying
sex (and potentially a cheerier attitude when tackling those other, less exciting
activities of daily living). Trying different positions that compensate for limited
mobility or low energy can also be of help. A ‘‘spooning’’ position with both
partners lying on their sides (whatever side is more comfortable) and facing the
same direction can allow for penetration, stroking of erogenous zones, and
cuddling. Both partners lying on their sides facing one another can provide a
similar result (Kaufman et al., 2003).
Treatment Impact
Some medications for high blood pressure are known to produce side
effects that may have a negative effect on sexual desire and functioning
(Caplan & Moelter, 2000). Sexual arousal and ability to achieve orgasm can
also be affected by these medications. Discussing these side effects with a
physician may allow him/her to prescribe different medications that may
produce fewer side effects and promote effective blood pressure control. In-
dividuals should not, under any circumstance, alter a dosage or stop taking
these medications without discussing it with a health care provider first, as this
may pose serious health risks.
Psychological Impact
One of the most common fears following a stroke is having another one,
and many individuals fear engaging in sexual activities because of concern about
increasing blood pressure to a level that might cause another stroke (Westcott,
2002). Because many underlying illnesses (such as diabetes) can increase a
person’s risk for stroke, it is important for individuals to have frank discussions
with their physicians prior to resuming sexual activity. If fears of sexual activity
causing a stroke cannot be put to rest by such a discussion, a referral for
psychotherapy can assist with exploring underlying fears and issues that may be
complicating the resumption of sexual activity.
Many individuals report other emotional changes following stroke, in-
cluding depression, diminished self-esteem, and body image issues (Merritt,
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 245
2004; Caplan & Moelter, 2000). Sexual problems and emotional difficulties
often coincide, especially following the traumatic experience of stroke. Emo-
tional problems such as depression can result from the enormous adjustment
process of recovery and coping with changes in physical and cognitive ability.
However, some emotional issues can also be linked to damage in specific
areas of the brain following the stroke. People may notice mood swings,
inappropriate sadness or tearfulness, anger, depression, or anxiety, among other
things. These changes should be discussed with a physician and a psychologist
or neuropsychologist in order to receive the proper treatment to avoid pro-
longing or furthering the issues.
Lastly, communication difficulties can also hamper efforts to engage in
fulfilling sexual activity. Frustration and anger are common reactions to prob-
lems with verbal expression or comprehension of what others are saying.
Receptive and expressive aphasia are frequently associated with stroke and can
present problems for those with a stroke and support persons alike. Patience is
required for both the individual and partners or support persons. Individuals
who experience communication difficulties often report that frustration and
anger can further impede successful communication efforts and diminish af-
fectionate or sexual feelings along the way. If communication issues are
identified as a problem, a physician, speech therapist (preferably one special-
izing in assistive technology devices that can produce words or phrases), or
psychologist may be able to assist in evaluating and treating this problem.
CARDIOVASCULAR DISEASE
Cardiovascular disease (CVD, or coronary heart disease; CHD) is the
number one cause of death in men and women in the United States (National
Heart, Lung and Blood Institute, n.d.). While the previous section on CVA,
which included some of the factors affecting sexual activity following a stroke,
is applicable here as well, CVD and sexuality requires further discussion.
Approximately 25 percent of people with CVD report a discontinuation of
sexual activity after a heart attack, and another 50 percent report a decrease
in sexual activity. The percentages are smaller, but similar, for patients with
cardiac chest pain, also called angina pectoris (Taylor, 1999). While this de-
cline in sexual activity occurs primarily due to psychological factors, other
factors include the effects of the disease on blood vessels and other body
systems important to sexual functioning, the effects of medication on physical
functioning, and the psychological effects on the individual’s sexual partner.
Physiological Impact
Sexual dysfunction in men is frequently associated with cardiovascular
disease because any condition that inhibits blood flow to the genital region can
lead to erectile dysfunction (ED) in men and, some believe, to similar arousal
246 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
problems in women (Buvat & Lemaire, 2001). While other common causes of
the disruption of blood flow, including atherosclerosis (hardening of the ar-
teries) and hypertension (high blood pressure), are frequently present before an
individual suffers from a heart attack they may not be detected until afterward.
Despite the well-known connection between cardiovascular disease and sexual
dysfunction, many individuals are nervous about bringing up these concerns
with physicians. Additionally, because some changes in sexual functioning are
expected to occur as one ages, it is common for an individual to fail to
mention such changes to her/his physician because these changes are mis-
takenly attributed solely to the aging process. And while some decline in sexual
activity is normal as we age, current medical technology allows people to
remain safely sexually active well into their eighties and nineties (Thorson,
2003). The first step to resuming sexual activity after a cardiac event is for the
patient to discuss any worries with her/his physician or with another qualified
health care provider. Physicians, too, may be hesitant to raise the topic of
sexual functioning for fear of offending a patient or because of the physician’s
perceived lack of expertise, lack of time, or discomfort in discussing the
topic (Haboubi & Lincoln, 2003; Stead, Brown, Fallowfield, & Selby, 2003;
Sundquist & Yee, 2003).
After a cardiac event, many patients report being much more aware of
heart activity (such as how fast it is beating) and other physical symptoms
(such as rate of or difficulty breathing). Such a common response to a life-
threatening event, such as a heart attack, can also change the focus of, and
interfere with, engaging in sexual activity. Because of fears of recurrence of
CHD symptoms, changes in the body during sex are to be expected and
should not cause worry. However, the energy and effort required for sexual
intercourse is the equivalent of mild to moderate physical activity and re-
quires about the same amount of effort required to climb up two flights of
stairs, meaning that for most people recovering from a heart attack, sexual
activity is safe (Douglas & Wilkes, 1975; Hellerstein & Friedman, 1970; Stein,
1977).
More specifically, as one becomes sexually aroused, the body goes through
many changes. The skin may become flushed, blood pressure rises, and heart
beat increases to 90–130 beats per minute during orgasm. Increases in blood
pressure and heart rate are considered safe even for those who have recently
experienced a myocardial infarction.
Additional reassurance regarding the safety of sex is provided by guidelines
developed to help determine the safety of sexual activity for individual cardiac
patients. The Princeton Consensus Panel, a group of experts on sex and
cardiac patients, determined that patients can be put into one of three risk
categories: low, intermediate, or high. The majority of patients are in the low
risk category, which includes patients with controlled hypertension (high
blood pressure); mild, stable angina (chest pain); successful cardiac bypass
surgery or stent placement; a history of an uncomplicated heart attack; mild
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 247
heart valve disease; and no symptoms and less than three cardiac risk factors
(Debusk et al., 2000). For individuals in this category, resuming sexual activity
is generally safe within three to six weeks after a cardiac event.
Individuals in the other risk groups or those with lingering concerns
should discuss the safety of resuming sexual activity with his/her physician. For
such individuals a graded exercise tolerance test, or cardiac stress test, might be
recommended. During a stress test, the patient is hooked up to monitors that
measure heart and body functions. The patient is asked to walk on a treadmill
or ride a stationary cycle at different speeds and levels of difficulty while the
body’s response is monitored. The physician can then determine how much
physical activity the heart can safely handle and will be able to inform the
patient whether sexual activity is safe.
As long as the physician has determined that it is safe, engaging in regular
physical activity can be extremely helpful in returning to normal sexual rou-
tines. All individuals recovering from heart attack are encouraged to participate
in exercise to strengthen the heart muscle, but physical activity has additional
benefits as well. It can increase coordination and muscle tone as well as im-
prove self-assurance, self-worth, and ‘‘staying power.’’ Exercise not only leaves
one in better health but also allows for better sexual performance.
Despite the popular belief that many people have heart attacks during
sexual activity, it is simply not true. While having sex in an unfamiliar place or
with a new partner can cause additional stress on the heart, even these kinds of
activities do not lead to heart attacks very frequently. For healthy people with
no previous history of cardiac disease, the chance of having a heart attack
during sex is one to two in a million. For individuals with a previous heart
attack, the risk increases to ten or twenty in a million (Kloner, 2000).
To further lower the risks of suffering a heart attack and increase the
enjoyment of sexual activity, people are advised to be well rested before sex.
This might mean having sex in the morning or soon after a nap. Food and
drink (especially alcohol) should be avoided for one to three hours before
having sex, as digestion diverts blood flow from the heart to the stomach. Very
hot or cold showers or sitting in a sauna or whirlpool should also be avoided, as
this can cause an additional increase in blood pressure.
Treatment Impact
Cardiovascular disease is frequently managed with a variety of medica-
tions. Most of the common classes of drugs used in the treatment of cardio-
vascular disease will not affect sexual functioning. These include ACE
inhibitors (used to treat high blood pressure), calcium channel blockers (used to
treat angina and high blood pressure), and statins (used to treat high choles-
terol).
However, beta-blockers such as Toprol XL, Lopressor, and Tenormin
(atenolol and metoprolol), which are used to treat high blood pressure, have
248 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
been frequently associated with decreased sexual ability and erectile dysfunc-
tion. Beta-blockers can also be used to relieve angina (chest pain) and can help
prevent additional heart attacks.
Despite the commonly made assumptions, a recent review of published
articles and medical texts revealed that there are no scientific studies supporting
the belief that the use of beta-blockers is highly associated with sexual dys-
function (Lama, 2002). This review failed to find any connection between
erectile dysfunction or decreased libido and the use of beta-blockers. How-
ever, there has been difficulty separating out the effects of hypertension
on sexual functioning from the contribution of age combined with the use of
multiple medications. While there have been a number of well-reported,
isolated cases linking beta-blockers and sexual dysfunction, reexamining the
existing data suggests that the link is not strong.
While medications used to treat heart disease are of primary concern,
medications used to treat other conditions may also raise special concerns for
cardiac patients. Viagra (sildenafil) and other drugs of the same class including
Levitra (vardenafil) and Cialis (tadalafil) have revolutionized the treatment of
erectile dysfunction or ED. Along with that, it has also increased the awareness
of risks of sexual activity in patients being treated for cardiovascular disease.
According to current research, the only contraindication to Viagra use is the
use of organic nitrates (such as nitroglycerin or isosorbide dinitrate, sold under
the trade names of Isordil, Nitrogard, Nitrostat, Sorbitrate). Viagra causes a
mild decrease in blood pressure, and when combined with nitrates, it can lead
to a major decrease in blood pressure. How safe it is for people with recent
(within six months) heart attack, unstable angina, stroke, or life threatening
arrhythmias to use ED medication has not been adequately studied. It has been
found that caution should be used by patients with unstable cardiac conditions
when taking Viagra (Kloner, 2000). However, in patients with heart disease,
placebo-controlled drug trials did not show an increase in heart attacks or
serious cardiac events with the use of Viagra.
Psychological Impact
Any life-threatening event (such as a heart attack or stroke) can have an
adverse psychological impact on an individual and on the individual’s loved
ones, making it difficult for life to return to normal. Patients often report a
fear of death and anxiety about any activity that puts stress on the heart.
Feelings of sadness following a cardiac event are also common and may be
associated with changes in eating and sleeping habits. Some individuals may
experience irritability and withdraw from those around them. Changes like
those may be a sign of depression and should not be ignored. While about 85
percent of cases of depression following a heart attack resolve in about three
months (American Heart Association, 1990), even moderate levels of de-
pression can interfere with recovery from a heart attack (Carney, Freedland,
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 249
Rich, & Jaffe, 1995; Frasure-Smith, Lesperance, & Talajic, 1996). Depression
can affect sexual desire, resulting in greatly reduced interest in sexual activity.
Because there are so many effective treatments available for depression, it is
important that health care providers be consulted when symptoms of de-
pression persist.
Partners of individuals who have had a cardiac event also may experience
depression or other psychological symptoms. Partners may become afraid or
anxious (especially regarding sexual activities) and the negative impact of these
fears on individuals recovering from heart attacks has been well documented
(Ben-Sira & Eliezer, 1990; Levin, 1987; Thompson & Meddis, 1990). Often,
partners have difficulties expressing their concerns directly, instead doing so by
being patronizing and over-protective. Therefore, it is important for couples to
make a special effort to communicate openly with each other about any fears or
concerns in order to manage the new challenges following such an event.
When it comes to resuming sexual activity, couples can start out by just
enjoying the sensations of being together. Cuddling and caressing are good
ways to enjoy each other’s bodies and affection without the performance-
driven demands of sexual intercourse. Lowering one’s expectations of a sexual
encounter can help ease the stress as well. While orgasms are often seen as the
goal of sex, feelings of tenderness and sensuality should also be appreciated.
Physical affection can serve not only as a path back to sexual intercourse but
also as a reward in its own right.
Once sexual intercourse has been resumed, one should be aware that
research has suggested that no one sexual position is better than any other for
cardiac patients, but there are a few guidelines to keep in mind (Taylor, 1999).
In sexual positions where one partner is on top, this partner typically reaches a
higher heart rate and engages in more strenuous physical exertion. Therefore,
it may be better, particularly when first returning to sexual intercourse, for the
person with cardiac disease to be on the bottom during sex.
Moreover, care should be taken when engaging in any positions that
require the individual who has had a heart attack to put pressure on her/his
arms for an extended amount of time, particularly for individuals who have
had open-heart surgery, as this puts more stress on the incision. Some sug-
gestions for less strenuous positions include both partners lying on their sides
or sitting face to face in a chair. Oral sex is also an excellent way for couples to
enjoy each other. Couples are encouraged to consult a physician prior to
engaging in anal sex because it can lead to irregular heart rhythms (Cambre,
1990). When returning to sexual activity, the most important thing for couples
to remember is to go slowly and not to do anything that makes either partner
anxious. Still, some early research suggested that resuming sexual activity
sooner, rather than later, after a heart attack could result in a faster recovery
(Scalzi & Dracup, 1978), and a subsequent study showed similar benefits, with
recovery being quicker when spousal fear is alleviated and sexual activities
resumed (Beach et al., 1992).
250 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
DIABETES
Many individuals with cardiovascular disease also have other chronic health
problems such as Type II diabetes mellitus, an endocrine disorder that results in
poorly regulated blood glucose (sugar) levels due to reduced production of insulin
(a hormone that helps turn sugar into stored energy) and insulin resistance (body’s
decreased sensitivity to insulin). Type II diabetes mellitus is frequently associated
with ED in men (Wandell & Brorsson, 2000). Obesity is also commonly asso-
ciated with Type II diabetes and makes it more likely that those who develop
diabetes will also have heart disease and hypertension (high blood pressure). The
previous sections have described the impact of cardiovascular and neurovascular
conditions on sexuality and these apply to individuals who have Type II diabetes
in addition to heart disease. And while cardiovascular disease may account for
much of the sexual dysfunction observed in diabetes, there are several other ways
in which Type II diabetes can impact sexual functioning.
Physiological Impact
In addition to the cardiovascular effects on sexual functioning, some re-
search also suggests that obesity may negatively affect sexual functioning
through hormonal changes that decrease sexual desire (Stahl, 2001; Trischitta,
2003). Individuals with poorly regulated Type II diabetes often report symp-
toms of fatigue, blurred vision, headache, and irritability—all symptoms that
may interfere with an individual’s desire for sexual activity. While those
symptoms tend to be transient and associated with extremes in glucose levels,
the complications that develop over years of poorly controlled blood glucose
levels can be enduring and debilitating.
The complications of Type II diabetes affect multiple systems that directly
and indirectly affect sexual functioning. These complications may include
neuropathies (nerve damage) that reduce sensitivity to touch, especially in the
feet and hands; retinopathy (damage to the blood vessels in the eye) that can
lead to blindness; and nephropathy (kidney disease) that may lead to kidney
failure and the need for hemodialysis (pumping blood through a machine that
cleans the blood). Neuropathies and nephropathy have been associated, both
directly and indirectly, with sexual dysfunction.
Nephropathy or kidney disease may progress to the point that the kidneys
can no longer effectively clean the waste products out of the blood. When this
happens, the individual may undergo hemodialysis. This generally requires visits
to a dialysis center three times a week for three to five hours each visit. Studies of
both men and women undergoing dialysis show that sexual dysfunction is
common and related to changes in hormonal, nerve, and blood vessel conditions
as well as psychological response to kidney disease and treatments (Gipson, Katz,
& Stehman-Breen, 1999; Peng et al., 2005; Rosas et al., 2003).
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 251
Treatment Impact
As discussed in the section on the treatment impact of cardiovascular dis-
ease, the beta-adrenergic blocking agents used to treat hypertension may be
associated with ED. And because many individuals with Type II diabetes have
high blood pressure and hyperlipidemia (too much fat in the blood), the in-
formation on medications from the section on treatment impact of cardiovas-
cular disease also applies to those who have diabetes and cardiovascular disease.
In addition to medications taken to treat hypertension and elevated cho-
lesterol, individuals with Type II diabetes may take oral medications (pills) to
lower blood glucose or they may inject insulin. Medications to treat diabetes
help lower blood glucose in different ways. Medications such as Glucotrol,
Glucotrol XL, Mycronase, and Glynase help to reduce blood sugar levels by
causing the pancreas to produce more insulin. Because there is more insulin
present with these medications, it is possible for individuals to experience a low
blood sugar reaction. A low blood sugar reaction can make one behave as if
intoxicated (drunk) and can lead to death if not treated immediately.
Some diabetes medications lower blood glucose by making the body more
sensitive to insulin and by reducing the amount of glucose produced in the
body, while others slow the breakdown of starches into sugar. Some diabetes
medications may cause serious liver damage. Other less serious, but sometimes
intolerable side effects of these medications include: nausea, diarrhea, and
other gastrointestinal distress. While these symptoms are generally not life
threatening, they can be quite distressing and therefore interfere with sexual
activity, particularly if they occur frequently.
Psychological Impact
As with other life-threatening conditions, a diagnosis of diabetes can lead
to heightened anxiety and fear in both the individual with diabetes and his/her
252 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
partner. In addition to any anxieties about the serious side effects of medicines,
many individuals with diabetes report a depressed mood and extreme fatigue
resulting from the rather burdensome regimen involved in diabetes care. In
fact, some suggest that individuals with diabetes are twice as likely to develop
depressive symptoms as those without the disease (Ciechanowski, Katon,
Russo, & Hirsch, 2003; Lustman et al., 2000).
Depression, as stated previously, is associated with a significant decline in
sexual desire. Other symptoms of depression include: decreased energy, appetite
extremes, and difficulty with concentration and attention, all of which impact
diabetes management. So in the context of depression, diabetes care declines and
may lead to a cycle of worsening depression and increasing physical symptoms.
Psychological interventions and/or antidepressants should be sought if symp-
toms persist beyond several weeks, bearing in mind that the SSRI (serotonin
selective reuptake inhibitors) antidepressants are linked with disruption of sexual
functioning at all stages of sexual response: desire, arousal, and climax (Stahl,
2001). Discussing any sexual side effects of the medication with a qualified
health care provider is critical as there are medications available that may not
interfere with sexual functioning.
When a partner’s anxieties about an individual’s health status persist de-
spite reassurances by health care providers, marital or individual therapy may
be indicated. Various kinds of professionals are available to address these
concerns including psychologists, social workers, nurses, and counselors. A
referral from a trusted health care provider is the first step to resolving any
psychological factors disrupting sexual activity.
AUTOIMMUNE DISORDERS
While diabetes is most frequently thought of as an endocrine disorder,
Type I diabetes is also classified as an autoimmune disorder because the in-
dividual’s immune system has destroyed the cells in the pancreas responsible
for producing insulin. Individuals with Type I diabetes share similar symptoms
(e.g., fatigue, frequent urination, irritability) and complications (e.g., neuro-
pathies, cardiovascular disease) as those with Type II diabetes. Many of the
other more common autoimmune diseases also may be classified under the
other categories, and they also may occur with, be accompanied by, or increase
the risk for conditions from the other categories discussed in this chapter.
Because the primary presenting symptoms of these conditions overlap con-
siderably with those discussed in the other sections of this chapter, the reader is
referred to the neurological, cardiovascular, endocrine, and chronic pain
sections for information regarding the disease and treatment impact on sexual
functioning. Some of the more common disorders include: (1) systemic lupus
erythematosus (SLE), which can affect a wide range of body tissues (e.g.,
joints, skin, kidneys, heart, lungs, and blood vessels); (2) scleroderma (‘‘hard
skin’’), which results in thickening and tightness of the skin of the fingers or
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 253
toes but can affect other organs; and (3) rheumatoid arthritis (RA) in which
the synovium or lining of joints becomes inflamed.
Physiological Impact
Individuals living with pain often report decreased sexual activity with
some estimates as high as 46 percent (Maigne & Chatellier, 2001). While the
main reasons for decreased sexual activity in chronic pain are psychological in
nature, sexuality can be affected by changes in sensation and mobility with
254 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Treatment Impact
Many of the medications used to treat chronic pain conditions can in-
terfere with sexual functioning at all stages. Antiepileptics or anticonvulsant
medications (e.g., gabapentin, carbamazepine, or lamotrigine) are often used to
treat neuropathic or nerve pains. As described earlier, a common side effect of
these medications is extreme sedation.
Sedation is also associated with the use of opioid analgesics, muscle re-
laxants, and benzodiazepines—especially at the initial stages of treatment.
Tricyclic antidepressants (e.g., amitriptyline and nortriptyline) are often used
in the treatment of neuropathies as well. These medications are well known
for their sedating side effect; however, dry mouth, dizziness, and feeling as if
one has a ‘‘hangover’’ (especially upon first awakening) are frequently reported
and distressing side effects of amitriptyline and the other tricyclics. These side
effects can interfere with one’s ability to feel attractive and may reduce desire
to engage in sexual activity. Having a frank discussion of these concerns with a
physician or other members of the pain treatment team can lead to the ap-
propriate medication changes.
Chronic Disease, Disability, and Sexuality 255
Psychological Impact
Chronic pain conditions often lead to dramatic changes in lifestyle, and
adapting to life with pain can be a lengthy process. Many chronic pain
treatment centers have multidisciplinary teams comprised of physicians, nurses,
behavioral health psychologists, occupational and physical therapists, and social
workers because of the many factors that contribute to the chronic pain ex-
perience and the impact chronic pain has on various areas of life.
Individuals living with chronic pain frequently develop psychological
symptoms as a result, and these can interfere with sexual activities in varying
degrees. Some individuals may experience mild anxieties and fears about in-
creased pain associated with sex while others develop major depressive dis-
orders resulting in decreased libido. Furthermore, depression has been linked
to increased pain severity—a vicious cycle that produces greater emotional
distress followed by increasing pain severity (Swanson, 1999). If depression is
implicated in the decreased desire for sex in a person with chronic pain, the
first step to resolving this problem is discussing these concerns with a qualified
health care professional. There are many effective treatments for depression,
and treating depression may, as stated previously, result in decreased pain as
well as improved mood.
SUMMARY
Many of the disabilities and chronic health conditions covered in this
chapter directly impact sexual functioning because of damage to nerves or blood
vessels, decreased hormone production, and chronic pain. The treatments for
each of these conditions may also interfere with the satisfying expression of
sexuality. Furthermore, there are often psychological factors such as depression
or anxiety that individuals with chronic health problems experience. These
psychological factors may also affect an individual’s partner.
Despite the multiple factors that can adversely affect sexuality and intimacy,
there remain a wide variety of means for achieving satisfying physical intimacy
and enjoying the pleasures of sexual activity whether by oneself or with a
partner. Sexuality is, as repeated throughout this chapter, an important aspect of
life and critical to the quality of life and overall health and well-being of in-
dividuals living with disabilities and recovering from health crises.
REFERENCES
American Heart Association. (1990). Sex and heart disease. Dallas: Author.
American Pain Society. (2002). Guidelines for the management of pain in os-
teoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, and juvenile chronic arthritis. Glenview,
IL: Author.
256 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
Video
Alexander, C. J., & Sipski, M. L. (Coproducers). (1993). Sexuality reborn: Sexuality
following spinal cord injury [Videotape]. West Orange, NJ: Kessler Institute for
Rehabilitation.
Books
Griffith, E., & Lemberg, S. (1993). Sexuality and the person with traumatic brain
injury: A guide for families. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
Kroll, K., & Klein, E. (1992). Enabling romance: A guide to love, sex and relationships
for people with disabilities and the people who care about them. Horsham, PA: No
Limits Communications.
Web Sites
www.americanheart.org/presenter.jhtml?identifier¼1200000
American Heart Association Web site that answers questions about heart disease
and provides information about community supports, activities, and treat-
ments.
www.arthritis.org/default.asp
A Web site about arthritis, community supports, healthcare providers, medica-
tions, and treatments.
www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/healthtopics.html
A Web site designed to provide answers about health problems, medications, and
organizations that offer support and services to individuals with specific health
concerns.
www.sexualhealth.com
A Web site with a wealth of information about sexuality, education, counseling,
therapy, medical attention, and other resources for persons with disabilities
and their partners.
260 Sexual Function and Dysfunction
www.spinalcord.uab.edu/show.asp?durki¼24434
A Web site by the University of Alabama at Birmingham. In addition to copious
information about sexuality following spinal cord injury, there is abundant
information about a variety of issues related to spinal cord injury.
www.newmobility.com
A Web site and magazine for persons with disabilities that addresses a variety of
issues concerning living with disabilities.
Index
of, 165–166; shaft of, 2; stimulation Cowper’s gland, 13, 15–16. See also
of, 188–189 bulbourethral gland
cognition: attention and, 58; human CP. See cerebral palsy
behavior and, 20; in sexual arousal, cremaster muscle, 15
58–60 cross-dressing, 188
cognitive arousal theory, 57–58 cruising, 222, 224–225
cognitive interference, 139–140 CSB. See compulsive sexual behavior
cognitive restructuring, 128, cultural mores, 80–82
140–141, 169; sexual compulsivity cultural success: competition and, 81;
and, 227 female preference for, 71–72; male
cognitive-behavioral therapy, 144; preference for, 76–77; physical
bibliotherapy and, 144–145; for attractiveness and, 72; women and,
sexual compulsivity, 226–227 71–72
coital alignment technique, 129 CVA. See cerebrovascular accident
Coleman, Eli, 215 CVD. See cardiovascular disease
common love, 88 cyproterone acetate, 32
communication, 108; failure of,
140–141; intimacy and, 236; sexual dartos muscle, 12
dysfunction and, 184; sexuality and, decision/commitment component, 90
234; techniques of, 172; training in, Depo Provera, 32
170–171 desire, 38, 117. See also sexual desire:
companionate love, 92, 106–109 arousal and, 46; conceptualization of,
competition: choice vs., 68; cultural 39; relationship outcomes and,
success and, 81; parental investment 120–121; testosterone increase with,
vs., 69 155
compulsive sexual behavior (CSB), diabetes mellitus: CVA and, 244; MED
215. See also sexual compulsivity and, 139; sexual activity and,
condoms, 15–16 250–252
conflict, 121 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual,
conjugal love, 89 fourth edition, text revision
consummate love, 92 (DSM-IV-TR), 136; female orgasmic
contraception, 187 disorder in, 159; FSAD criteria
copulation, 20 of, 136; male orgasmic disorder in,
coronal ridge, 9, 10 160; premature ejaculation in, 161;
coronary heart disease (CHD): sexual disorders in, 158–159
recurrence of, 246; sexual activity diencephalon, 23
after, 245 Dion, Karen, 101
corpora cavernosa: of female, 4, 8; of Dion, Kenneth, 101
male, 14, 16 disabilities, 235–238
corpus callosum, 27 divorce rates, 80
corpus spongiosum, 14, 16 dorsolateral cerebral cortex, 25
cortex. See also cerebral cortex: dorsomedial hypothalamus, 27
information of, 23; inhibition by, 34; DSM-IV-TR. See Diagnostic and
regions of, 24 Statistical Manual, fourth edition,
couples therapy, 145 text revision
264 Index
male erectile disorder (MED), 135, minor lips, 2. See also labia majora/
137–138, 190–193, 245–246; minora
complexity of, 148; diseases mobility impairment, 237
influencing, 138–139; drug monogamous marriage, 81
treatments for, 145–146; DSM-IV- mons pubis, 2, 8
TR criteria for, 137; ICI treatment mons veneris, 2–3
for, 147; medical treatments for, 248; MPOA. See medial preoptic region
suction device treatment for, 147; MRI. See magnetic resonance imaging
surgical treatment of, 147–148 MS. See multiple sclerosis
male hormones, 1 multiple sclerosis (MS), 238, 241–242
male orgasmic disorder, 160–161 mutual respect, 108
male-on-male aggression, 81 myometrium, 6, 7
mammary glands, 6, 8
mania, 94 National Health and Social Life Survey
Masters, William, 138; bibliotherapy (NHSLS), 135; FSAD occurrences
pioneered by, 144; couples in, 136–137; MED occurrences in,
treatment by, 193; sensate focus 138
exercises of, 191–192; sex therapy natural language concepts, 97
and, 180–182 neuroanatony, 22–26
masturbation, 28, 116; brain activity neurological conditions, 238–245
during, 29; brain blood flow during, neuron activity, 21
30; comfort with, 235; frequency of, neurovascular conditions, 238–245
121; guided, 129; as malady, 123; NHSLS. See National Health and
PET scan of, 30; as self-discovery, 198 Social Life Survey
mate-choice preferences, 69–70; age nipple, 8
and, 78; cross-cultural variation in, nonlove, 91–92
79–80; cultural success and, 71–72;
evolution and, 67; of females, 71–76; OA. See osteoarthritis
historical variation in, 79–80; obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD),
immune system and, 73–74; of 215
males, 76–79; personality/behavior occipital lobe, 24
and, 72–73 OCD. See obsessive-compulsive
maternal love, 89, 98 disorder
meatus, 9–10 odynorgasmia, 162
MED. See male erectile disorder olfactory bulb, 27
medial cerebral cortex, 25 olfactory stimulation, 54
medial orbitofrontal cortex, 126 operational sex ratio (OSR), 69, 80
medial preoptic region (MPOA): male optic chiasm, 27
sexual behavior and, 26; sexual oral sex, 202, 249
response mediation by, 46; orbitofrontal cerebral cortex, 25
testosterone and, 26–27; VMN orgasm(s): age-related problems of,
similarity to, 27–28 158; arousal and, 196; attitudes
medroxyprogesterone, 32 toward, 165; clitoris and, 51–52;
menstrual flow, 7, 20–21 consistency training for, 170;
mental love models, 96–97 definition of, 154–155;
268 Index
184–185; substance abuse as cause for, 255; love and, 88; medication
163–164; therapy for, 180; treatment solution and, 168, 189; orgasm and,
of, 166–171 171; passionate love and, 101, 105;
sexual excitement, 38 PET scan and, 21; study of, 180–181
sexual experience: context of, 159, sexually transmitted infections (STIs),
182; passionate love as, 104–105; 185–186, 202
sexual health and, 155–156; sexual shaft: of clitoris, 2; of penis, 9–10, 16
response and, 168 sharing, 108
sexual fantasy, 119; experience of, 120; Shaver, Phillip, 97
factor analysis of, 119; orgasm short-term partners: female preferences
frequency and, 119–120; role of, 55; in, 74–76; male preferences in,
sexual desire and, 121–122 78–79
sexual fulfillment obstacles, 182 sildenafil. See Viagra
sexual health, 155–156 sisterly love, 98
sexual interest, 26 skin, of penis, 9–10
sexual knowledge, 172 SLE. See systemic lupus erythematosus
sexual love, 89 Society for Advancement of Sexual
sexual minorities, 188 Health (SASH), 216–217
sexual motivation, 45–49 somatic motor system, 49–52
sexual pleasure, 1 somatic nervous system, 22
sexual positions, 249 somatosensory stimulation, 49
sexual preferences, 67 SOR. See stimulus-organism-response
sexual preparation, 43–45 paradigm
sexual relationships, 80–81 spasticity, 237
sexual response. See also sexual arousal; spectatoring, 59, 183
sexual desire: conceptualization of, sperm: pH of, 15; production of, 14;
38–42; culmination of, 154; domains seminal fluid and, 15
of, 39–40; emotions and, 55; general spermatic cord, 15
models for, 38–40; in humans, 48; sphincter muscle, 15
model synthesis for, 41–42; MPOA spinal cord injury (SCI), 23, 33, 238,
and, 46; normal, 154–155; 239–241; erections after, 240;
physiology of, 42–43, 60; fertility levels following, 240; MED
psychobiological components of, 47; and, 139
sexual experience and, 168 spinal mediation systems, 42
sexual selection: in humans, 70; spinal reflexes, 33
parental investment and, 68 spongy erectile tissue, 13
sexual sensitivity, 170 spontaneous desire, 39
sexual stimulation, 5; failure of, Sprecher, Susan, 103
140–141; of women, 51 squeeze technique, 194
sexual thoughts, 27 start-stop technique, 194
sexual trauma, 165 Stillwell, Arlene, 104
sexuality: activities included in, stimulation, 20, 49. See also sexual
235–236; chronic disease and, 233; stimulation: body parts prone to, 43;
cognitive aspects of, 34; EEG and, conditioning and, 53; erotic, 53–55;
21; evaluation of, 21; factors of, 234, olfactory, 54; tactile, 54–55
272 Index
Regan has served on the editorial board of Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, Personal Relationships, the Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, and
the Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality.
Volume 3
SEXUAL DEVIATION AND
SEXUAL OFFENSES
1
PRAEGER PERSPECTIVES
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Contents
Preface vii
Introduction ix
1. Exhibitionism 1
William D. Murphy and I. Jacqueline Page
2. Sadomasochism 21
Pekka Santtila, N. Kenneth Sandnabba, and Niklas Nordling
3. Female Sex Offenders 47
Donna M. Vandiver
4. Pedophilia 81
Richard D. McAnulty
5. Sexual Assault 97
Karen S. Calhoun, Jenna McCauley, and Megan E. Crawford
6. Incest Victims and Offenders 131
Rita Kenyon-Jump
7. Treatment of Sex Offenders 159
Lester W. Wright, Jr., and Angela P. Hatcher
vi Contents
We have had many opportunities to teach and interact with both college
students and professional audiences about some very important topics and
issues in human sexuality in our roles as authors and college professors. When
we were approached to write this three-volume set on sex and sexuality, we
were intrigued with the idea of having a forum in which to reach a broader
audience. That is our goal for this work. With that in mind, we encouraged
our contributors to ‘‘talk to’’ a general audience when writing about the topics
that were most important to them. The authors we selected to write these
chapters represent both established authorities and budding scholars on the
various topics in human sexuality. We are confident that they have all helped
us accomplish our goal.
To us, few, if any, other topics in the realm of human behavior are more
interesting, exciting, or controversial than sex. And we hope that you will
agree after reading the chapters from this set. Each chapter stands alone, and
you can choose to read as many or as few as you would like—pick the ones
that interest you. We hope that you will find this work to be of significant
value to you, whether you are in pursuit of a better general understanding of
sexuality or are looking for answers to specific questions.
One theme you will find throughout these texts is that human sexual
function is affected by a whole host of factors. These factors are biological,
sociocultural, and psychological in nature. The scientific study of sexuality is
for all practical purposes a ‘‘young’’ field, and we have only touched the
viii Preface
surface in an attempt to fully understand how these factors interact and impact
sexuality.
Another theme or concern you will find throughout this work is the
question whether ‘‘scientific’’ views of sex are biased by social judgments about
normal versus abnormal and/or functional versus dysfunctional sexual be-
havior. U.S. culture, in particular, holds many strong values and prohibitions
about sex. In this context, studying and interpreting research on sexuality in an
unbiased manner can be a challenge. Many of our authors caution the reader
about this concern.
We wish to thank all the researchers and clinicians, past and present, who
have contributed to the science of sex. Many of them have contributed
chapters to this set, and for that we are grateful. We also thank our colleagues,
families, and friends who supported us during the writing and editing process.
Finally, we thank ‘‘the team’’ at Praeger Publishers.
Introduction
Few topics inspire more curiosity than sexual practices deemed unusual,
deviant, or deplorable. It is, however, very challenging to define deviance with
respect to sexual preferences. Norms regarding sexual behavior vary over time
and across cultures. Consider, for example, the changing perspective on ho-
mosexuality. Although we currently view homosexuality as a normal variation
or alternative lifestyle, it was officially classified as a sexual deviation until fairly
recently. It was not until 1973 that the American Psychiatric Association
elected to drop homosexuality from its official list of mental disorders.
Modern culture has brought many previously taboo and forbidden topics
out of the bedroom into the living room. Many sexual practices that were
previously considered obscure and uncommon are discussed openly on the
Internet. Bondage, domination, and fetishism, for example, are terms that are
familiar to many people. The extent to which these represent deviant sexual
practices is the subject of debate. There is little disagreement, however, that
they qualify as atypical; these sexual practices are not considered mainstream in
any culture.
Some sexual practices are unquestionably maladaptive and deviant, often
even criminal. Sexual activity that involves force and coercion is deviant in
every sense of the word. Rape is a legal term that can be applied to any form of
sexual assault. Sexual activity with persons below the age of consent, such as
children, is illicit and criminal. Child molestation therefore is another form of
sexual coercion since children are incapable of providing consent. This volume
x Introduction
allowing them to draw conclusions from the crime scene alone. A more
sensible approach to profiling involves spelling out which claims are purely
speculation and intuition and which are based on sound research. This ap-
proach discourages investigators from relying too heavily on information that
may not be very accurate; it also recognizes that not all information generated
by profilers is equally useful.
In Chapter 10, Marshall and Hucker address the various definitions of
severe sexual sadism. Their review concludes that some features that are con-
sidered classic signs, such as torture, cruelty, and humiliation of victims, are
not seen in every case. In Chapter 11, on sexual homicide, Wright, Hatcher,
and Willerick explore this disturbing phenomenon. Sensational depictions in
the media have fueled the public’s fascination with murders that occur in the
context of lust, power, and brutality. Interestingly, the authors conclude that
there may be as many as 200 serial killers at large at any point in time.
1
Exhibitionism
INTRODUCTION
The term ‘‘exhibitionism’’ is attributed to the French physician Lasègue (cited
in MacDonald, 1973), who in 1877 described a number of cases he had seen.
Also during the late 1800s, Krafft-Ebing published his classic book, Psycho-
pathia Sexualis (1965), describing a variety of deviant sexual behaviors includ-
ing exhibitionism. These early scientists and clinicians described exhibitionism
as exposure of one’s genitals by males, generally to females, for sexual plea-
sure without any attempt at further sexual contact (MacDonald, 1973). They
proposed that exhibitionism is related to some type of pathology, either some
type of brain disease that interferes with behavioral control, or a mental
disorder.
Over 125 years later, the clinical description of exhibitionism has changed
little. Exhibitionism is considered a psychiatric disorder by the mental health
field and is one of the paraphilias described in the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders of the American Psychiatric Association (1994). In
this psychiatric nomenclature, exhibitionism is described as meeting the fol-
lowing two criteria: (1) recurrent, intense sexually arousing fantasies, sexual
urges, or behaviors involving exposing one’s genitals to an unsuspecting
stranger, over a period of at least six months; (2) the fantasies, sexual urges, or
behaviors caused clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occu-
pational, or other important areas of functioning.
2 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
1. Who are the victims and how often does this behavior occur?
2. Does exhibitionistic behavior cause harm to the victim?
3. Are exhibitionists dangerous?
4. Do exhibitionists share certain mental disorders or psychological problems that
cause exhibitionism?
5. Can exhibitionists be ‘‘cured’’ or treated?
This chapter will provide some information on these questions, but as the
reader will see, for many, we do not have a definitive answer.
random samples of the population, but they show surprising consistency across
time and countries. Cox and MacMahon (1978) found that 32 percent of 405
female college students from four universities across the United States reported
being victims of exhibitionism, while Cox, Tsang, and Lee (1982) found
almost the same percentage in a sample of undergraduate students in Hong
Kong. Gittelson, Eacott, and Mehta (1978) found that 44 percent of a sample
of 100 British nurses with a mean age of 37 had been exposed to outside the
work situation. Over twenty years later, a study by Riordan (1999) that in-
volved both college students and women in the community in the United
Kingdom found that 48.6 percent reported being exposed to, and a 2004 study
in Germany of 309 female college students indicated that 39.5 percent re-
ported being exposed to (Kury, Chouaf, Obergfell-Fuchs, & Woessner, 2004).
A study conducted in the United States (Finkelhor, Ormrod, Turner, &
Hamby, 2005) surveyed a national sample of children and youth between the
ages of 2 and 17. Either the child or the parent was interviewed, depending on
the child’s age, regarding the child’s exposure to a variety of victimization
experiences. Of this group of children and youth, 2.8 percent indicated that
they had been exposed to in the last year. This percentage would indicate that
approximately 1.6 million children between the ages of 2 and 17 are exposed
to each year.
Although none of the above studies are perfect, there is a very clear
pattern across time, and a good deal of consistency across countries. Surveys
are very consistent that 30 percent to over 40 percent of women have an
experience of being exposed to, and so have a significant number of children
and adolescents. This is a rather alarming number of individuals being sub-
jected to an unwanted and intrusive sexual behavior.
reported fear, 30 percent disgust, and 9 percent anger. Cox and MacMahon
(1978) found that 14 percent of their sample indicated that the experience
seriously or very seriously affected their attitude toward men. Riordan (1999)
is the only study that could be located since the 1970s, and its findings were
very similar, with about 49 percent reporting shock and about 26 percent fear,
and 68 percent considering the exhibitionist dangerous.
It should be noted that in most cases, the negative reactions were not long
lasting and tended to resolve within a month. However, another factor that
was found across studies was that many women avoided places where the act
occurred.
Although for most women exhibitionism does not lead to long-term neg-
ative sequelae, a small percentage experience more long-term negative effects,
and a high percentage report initial negative reactions. In addition, being a
victim does seem to impact a woman’s (as most reported cases are female)
feeling of safety in her own community. The fact that being exposed to does
not lead to long-term trauma for most victims should not lead to seeing the
behavior as only a nuisance. It is not appropriate to create short-term fear in
women and to impact their feelings of safety in their own community. Nor is
exhibitionism an appropriate way to introduce children to sexual behavior.
Recidivism
Determining the true reoffense rate or recidivism rate for sex offenders is
difficult. What is considered recidivism is usually based on individuals who
have at least been charged for a new offense or who have been reconvicted for
a new offense. However, as we have seen, many victims of exhibitionists never
report the offense, and when the offense is reported, the police may not be
able to arrest the suspect, or may not be able to charge the offender, or pros-
ecutors may elect not to prosecute. Therefore, official recidivism rates gen-
erally underestimate true rates of reoffending.
Given these limitations, the research suggests that exhibitionists tend to
have high recidivism rates as compared to other sex offenders. For example, in an
early study, Frisbee and Dondis (1965), found that 40.7 percent of treated ex-
hibitionists reoffended after five years as compared to 18.2 percent of pedophiles
who targeted female children and 34.5 percent of pedophiles who targeted male
6 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
children. It should be noted that although they were labeled treated offenders,
the treatment program at that time would not meet current standards for ef-
fective treatment. It should also be noted that this was a population of indi-
viduals sentenced to a secure residential treatment program under what were at
that time termed sexual psychopath laws. Today, exhibitionists would not be
sent to such secure facilities.
More recent data also suggest fairly high rates of recidivism. Marshall,
Barbaree, and Eccles (1991), in a small study of forty-four exhibitionists, found
a 39 percent recidivism rate for treated offenders and a 57 percent rate for
untreated offenders. Because of these high rates, Marshall et al. (1991) de-
scribed a second study where they modified their treatment program to address
a broader range of treatment issues. Their previous program had focused more
on strategies directed toward the urges in exhibitionists to expose themselves,
but the new program focused on factors such as the exhibitionists’ perceived
need to be perfect and assisted them in developing more intimate relationships.
In this study, although there was no control group, the recidivism rate was
down to 24 percent. These results, although still somewhat high for reof-
fending, are promising. Firestone, Kingston, Wexler, and Bradford (2005) fol-
lowed over 200 exhibitionists for an average of thirteen years and found that
23.6 percent reoffended sexually.
It should also be noted that those who expose to children have higher
recidivism rates than those who expose to adults (Frisbee & Dondis, 1965). In
addition, those who have antisocial personalities, have higher scores on an
alcohol measure, and are less educated (Firestone et al., 2005; Greenberg,
Firestone, Bradford, Greenberg, 2002) have higher rates of reoffending.
Criminality
Before answering the question, ‘‘What is the cause of exhibitionism?’’ we
need to ask, ‘‘Do exhibitionists differ from other criminals?’’ There has been a
tendency when looking at the causes of sex offending to assume that somehow
sex offenders are different in some ways from other criminal populations. As
one will see in later parts of this chapter, there have been a number of attempts
to explain the behavior of sex offenders in general and exhibitionists in par-
ticular as being related to specific psychiatric disorders or psychological
problems.
Exhibitionism 9
Neurological Impairment
Krafft-Ebing (1965), in explaining exhibitionism, stated that there was a
group that suffered from ‘‘acquired states of mental weakness,’’ which were
caused by ‘‘cerebral (or spinal) disease.’’ He went on to describe a number of
cases where there appeared to be some brain impairment. There has been a
continued interest in this area, although there are limited studies with exhi-
bitionists. Although case studies have appeared that describe exhibitionists
having certain types of brain pathology (see Murphy, 1997, for review), there
have been very few controlled studies. The most extensive data comes from
Flor-Henry and Lang (1988) and Flor-Henry, Koles, Reddon, and Baker
(1986). In studies that use both neuropsychological test data and EEGs
(measures of brain electrical activity), they found deficits in the left hemisphere
of the brain. The neuropsychological data suggested left frontal temporal lobe
dysfunction. The temporal lobe is important because early animal studies have
shown that temporal lobe injury in primates can lead to hypersexuality (Kluver
& Bucy, 1939). However, as pointed out by O’Carroll (1989), subjects used in
Flor-Henry’s studies were recidivist, incarcerated offenders who averaged five
to six convictions.
At this time, data is too limited to indicate whether there is any specific
neurological impairment in exhibitionists. It is likely, based on the case reports
that have appeared in the literature, that there are some individuals who
expose themselves as a result of some type of brain pathology, although it is
unlikely that this applies to the vast majority of people who expose themselves.
have been unable to identify any one specific factor. Beginning in the late
1960s and continuing from then, researchers in the field of sex offense began
questioning whether such behavior occurred not because of psychopathology
but because the individual had a specific interest in different forms of deviant
sexual behavior. A technology was developed called penile plethysmography
(also called phallometry) that used a gauge that directly measured changes in
penis size while subjects were exposed to specific sexual stimuli. The stimuli
could include slides depicting people of different ages, videotaped depictions
of sexual behavior, or audiotaped descriptions of sexual behavior. Subjects’
responses to deviant stimuli were compared to responses to nondeviant stimuli,
and studies have compared a variety of sex offenders to non-sex offenders. The
history of this research and the data related to it have been reviewed by
Murphy and Barbaree (1994) and Marshall and Fernandez (2003).
There have been numerous studies investigating this assessment meth-
odology with child molesters and rapists. Results have suggested that certain
subtypes of child molesters, those who molest nonrelatives, have a tendency to
respond more to child-type stimuli than normal.
Marshall and Fernandez (2003) reviewed approximately ten studies that
have attempted to determine whether exhibitionists have specific sexual at-
traction to exhibitionistic behavior. None of the studies indicated that exhi-
bitionists responded more to exhibitionistic stimuli than nonexhibitionistic
stimuli; nor did any study indicate that exhibitionists differed in any mean-
ingful way from nonexhibitionistic controls. These findings are fairly clear that
exhibitionists’ arousal patterns do not differ from what would be expected of
men in the general population.
However, Firestone et al. (2005) and Greenberg et al. (2002) found that
exhibitionists who reoffended showed more arousal to child stimuli than those
who did not reoffend. This suggests that, although exhibitionists may not show
arousal to exhibitionist stimuli, their arousal to other deviant stimuli may have
value in predicting those more likely to reoffend.
recidivism rate of 9.9 percent for treated offenders and a 17.4 percent rate for
untreated offenders, which is highly significant. The most recent Lösel study
included many topics of the Hanson study, as well as those from studies
published since that time, including a number of European studies. They were
able to locate eighty independent comparisons between treated and untreated
offenders that included over 22,000 offenders. Their findings were very similar
to those of Hanson et al., with a recidivism rate of 12 percent for the treated
groups and 24 percent for the comparison groups. Again, most of the studies
reviewed in the Lösel and Schmucker meta-analysis were not on exhibition-
ists. However, these authors did separate out four studies that focused spe-
cifically on exhibitionists. Although this is only a small number of treatment
studies, results did indicate that treated exhibitionists did significantly better
than untreated exhibitionists, although they did not provide specific recidivism
rates.
Although the area of sex offender treatment lacks studies that have used
the strongest research designs, there have been multiple studies using relatively
appropriate control groups. When such studies are combined, there seems to
be an indication that sex offender treatment can be effective. Although we do
not have the definitive answer to the effectiveness of treatment, the general
public’s view that sex offenders cannot be treated does not appear to be
warranted, and the evidence does suggest that recidivism can be reduced by
appropriate treatment. This also appears to apply to exhibitionists. However, it
is also clear that although treatment may reduce recidivism, exhibitionists still
have higher rates of reoffense as compared to other offenders.
What to Treat?
The above review suggests that sex offender treatment can be effective.
However, the second question is what does one actually treat to reduce re-
cidivism? This is another area where there have been significant advances in
our research understanding of what kind of factors need to be targeted in
treatment to actually reduce recidivism. Some of the public’s skepticism of sex
offender treatment is probably due to many early studies that provided treat-
ment that was not highly effective. These studies provided treatment from a
psychoanalytic model focusing on early childhood experiences and relation-
ship with mother. There is little evidence that focusing on these types of
factors will reduce recidivism.
What research has shown is that to reduce the recidivism of sex offenders
in general, and we feel this applies to exhibitionists too, we must focus on what
are termed dynamic risk factors. Research has identified approximately four
broad areas related to recidivism (Beech, Fisher, & Thornton, 2003; Hanson &
Harris, 2000; Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2004). Although different re-
searchers use different terminology for each of the broad areas, we will try to
summarize each below.
Exhibitionism 15
Social-Emotional Functioning
Another broad area that relates to reoffending can be deficits in estab-
lishing relationships and in handling negative emotional states. These include
16 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Treatment Style
Another advance in the treatment of sex offenders, which also applies to
exhibitionists, is the way in which treatment is delivered. Early treatment of sex
offenders tended to be very confrontive in nature. It was felt that offenders were
always lying, and that they had to be constantly challenged. Current treatment
styles recognize that such an approach is probably not effective, and are very
different compared to some of the early treatments. People specializing in sex
offender treatment still believe offenders need to accept responsibility for their
behavior but have begun to recognize that if one expects change, then one must
collaboratively work with offenders. In addition to focusing on negative aspects
of the offenders’ behavior and functioning, therapists must help establish more
positive goals to replace the inappropriate behavior and assist the offender to lead
a more prosocial life (Marshall et al., 2005).
Exhibitionism 17
CONCLUSIONS
Compared to other paraphilias, we have much less knowledge of this
population. We do know that the behavior occurs frequently and impacts
upward of 40 percent of women and up to 1.6 million children each year. We
have learned that there is no one type of exhibitionist and that they vary across
a number of characteristics. It is also clear that not all exhibitionists are
‘‘harmless’’ and that a significant minority goes on to commit more serious sex
offenses and other violent offenses. We also have some limited evidence that
treatment can be effective, although exhibitionists still seem to reoffend at
higher rates than other paraphiliacs.
A prime area for the future is to focus on adolescents who expose. We
know that up to 50 percent of exhibitionists begin in adolescence. It is also this
population that authorities may not take seriously. We need to be able to
identify those who are more at risk to continue the behavior or to escalate into
more serious offenses, and to provide interventions earlier.
REFERENCES
Abel, G. G., Jordan, A., Rouleau, J.-L., Emerick, R., Barboza-Whitehead, S., &
Osborn, C. (2004). Use of visual reaction time to assess male adolescents
who molest children. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16,
255–265.
Abel, G. G., & Osborn, C. (1992). Stopping sexual violence. Psychiatric Annals,
22, 301–330.
Abel, G. G., & Rouleau, J.-L. (1990). The nature and extent of sexual assault. In
W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual
assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender (pp. 9–21). New York:
Plenum Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2005). Evaluating the effects of birth complica-
tions on low self-control in a sample of twins. International Journal of Of-
fender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 49, 450–471.
Beech, A. R., Fisher, D. D., & Thornton, D. (2003). Risk assessment of sex
offenders. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 339–352.
18 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Berah, E. F., & Myers, R. G. (1983). The offense records of a sample of convicted
exhibitionists. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 11,
365–369.
Blair, C. D., & Lanyon, R. I. (1981). Exhibitionism: Etiology and treatment.
Psychological Bulletin, 89, 439–463.
Cox, D. J., & MacMahon, B. (1978). Incidence of male exhibitionism in the
United States as reported by victimized female college students. National
Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 1, 453–457.
Cox, D. J., & Maletsky, B. M. (1980). Victims of exhibitionism. In D. J. Cox &
R. J. Daitzman (Eds.), Exhibitionism: Description, assessment, and treatment
(pp. 289–293). New York: Garland.
Cox, D. J., Tsang, K., & Lee, A. (1982). A cross cultural comparison of the
incidence and nature of male exhibitionism in college students. Victimology,
7, 231–234.
Dahlstrom, W. G., Welsh, G. S., & Dahlstrom, L. E. (1972). An MMPI hand-
book: Vol. 1. Clinical interpretation. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press.
English, K., Jones, L., Pasini-Hill, D., Patrick, D., & Cooley-Towell, S. (2000).
The value of polygraph testing in sex offender management. Denver, CO:
Colorado Department of Public Safety, Office of Research and Statistics.
Fehrenbach, P. A., Smith, W., Monastersky, C., & Deisher, R. W. (1986). Ad-
olescent sexual offenders: Offender and offense characteristics. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 225–233.
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., Turner, H., & Hamby, S. L. (2005). The victimi-
zation of children and youth: A comprehensive, national survey. Child
Maltreatment, 10, 5–25.
Firestone, P., Kingston, D. A., Wexler, A., & Bradford, J. M. (2005). Long term
follow-up of exhibitionists: Psychological, phallometric, and offence. Unpublished
manuscript.
Flor-Henry, P., Koles, Z. L., Reddon, J. R., & Baker, L. (1986). Neu-
ropsychological studies (EEG) of exhibitionism. In M. C. Shagrasi, R. C.
Josiassen, & R. A. Roemer (Eds.), Brain electrical potentials and psychopa-
thology (pp. 279–306). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Flor-Henry, P., & Lang, R. (1988). Quantitative EEG analysis in genital exhi-
bitionists. Annals of Sex Research, 1, 49–62.
Forgac, G. E., Cassel, C. A., & Michaels, E. J. (1984). Chronicity of criminal
behavior and psychopathology in male exhibitionists. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 40, 827–832.
Forgac, G. E., & Michaels, E. J. (1982). Personality characteristics of two types of
male exhibitionists. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 287–293.
Frenken, J., Gijs, L., & Van Beek, D. (1999). Sexual offender research and
treatment in the Netherlands. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 347–371.
Freund, K. (1990). Courtship disorder. In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E.
Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the
offender (pp. 331–342). New York: Plenum.
Exhibitionism 19
Frisbee, L. V., & Dondis, E. H. (1965). Recidivism among treated sex offenders
(California Mental Health Research Monograph No. 5). Sacramento, CA:
Department of Mental Hygiene.
Gittelson, N. L., Eacott, S. E., & Mehta, B. M. (1978). Victims of indecent
exposure. British Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 61–66.
Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Greenberg, S. R., Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., & Greenberg, D. M. (2002). Pre-
diction of recidivism in exhibitionists: Psychological, phallometric, and of-
fense factors. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14, 329–347.
Hanson, R. K., Gordon, A., Harris, A. J. R., Marques, J. K., Murphy, W.,
Quinsey, V. L., et al. (2002). First report of the collaborative outcome data
project on the effectiveness of psychological treatment for sex offenders.
Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14, 169–194.
Hanson, R. K., & Harris, A. J. R. (2000). Where should we intervene? Dynamic
predictors of sexual offense recidivism. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27,
6–35.
Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. (2004). Predictors of sexual recidivism: An
updated meta-analysis. Ottawa, Canada: Public Works and Government
Services.
Hill, A., Briken, P., Kraus, C., Strohm, K., & Berner, W. (2003). Differential
pharmacological treatment of paraphilias and sex offenders. International
Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 47, 407–421.
Kluver, H., & Bucy, P. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal
lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 42, 979–1000.
Krafft-Ebing, R. V. (1965). Psychopathia sexualis. New York: Bell.
Kury, H., Chouaf, S., Obergfell-Fuchs, J., & Woessner, G. (2004). The scope
of sexual victimization in Germany. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19,
589–602.
Langevin, R., Paitich, D., Ramsey, G., Anderson, C., Kamrad, J., Pope, S., et al.
(1979). Experimental studies of the etiology of genital exhibitionism.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 8, 307–331.
Långström, N., & Seto, M. C. (in press). Exhibitionistic and voyeuristic behavior
in a Swedish national population survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior.
Lee, J. K. P., Jackson, H. J., Pattison, P., & Ward, T. (2002). Developmental risk
factors for sexual offending. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 73–92.
Longo, R. F., & Groth, A. N. (1983). Juvenile sexual offenses in the histories of
adult rapists and child molesters. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 27, 150–155.
Lösel, F., & Schmucker, M. (2005). The effectiveness of treatment for sexual
offenders: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Crimi-
nology, 1, 117–146.
MacDonald, J. M. (1973). Indecent exposure. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Marshall, W. L., Barbaree, H. E., & Eccles, A. (1991). Early onset and deviant
sexuality in child molesters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 323–336.
20 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Marshall, W. L., & Fernandez, Y. M. (2003). Phallometric testing with sexual of-
fenders. Brandon, VT: Safer Society.
Marshall, W. L., Ward, T., Mann, R. E., Moulden, H., Fernandez, Y. M., Serran, G.,
et al. (2005). Working positively with sexual offenders: Maximizing the
effectiveness of treatment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 1096–1114.
McCreary, C. P. (1975). Personality profiles of persons convicted of indecent
exposure. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 260–262.
Mohr, J. W., Turner, R. E., & Jerry, M. B. (1964). Pedophilia and exhibitionism.
Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Press.
Murphy, W. D. (1990). Assessment and medications of cognitive distortions in sex
offenders. In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.),
Handbook of sexual assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender
(pp. 331–342). New York: Plenum.
Murphy, W. D. (1997). Exhibitionism: Psychopathology and theory. In D. R.
Laws & W. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment, and
treatment (pp. 22–39). New York: Guilford.
Murphy, W. D., & Barbaree, H. E. (1994). Assessments of sexual offenders by
measures of erectile response: Psychometric properties and decision making. Bran-
don, VT: Safer Society Press.
O’Carroll, R. (1989). A neuropsychological study of sexual deviation. Sexual and
Marital Therapy, 4, 59–63.
Pfäfflin, F. (1999). Issues, incidence, and treatment of sexual offenders in Ger-
many. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 372–395.
Riordan, S. (1999). Indecent exposure: The impact upon the victim’s fear of
sexual crime. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 10, 309–316.
Rooth, G. (1973). Exhibitionism, sexual violence and paedophilia. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 122, 705–710.
Saunders, E. B., & Awad, G. A. (1991). Male adolescent sexual offenders: Ex-
hibitionism and obscene phone calls. Child Psychiatry and Human Devel-
opment, 21, 169–178.
Sjöstedt, G., Långström, N., Sturidsson, K., & Grann, M. (2004). Stability of modus
operandi in sexual offending. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 609–623.
Smallbone, S. W., & Wortley, R. K. (2004). Criminal diversity and paraphilic
interests among adult males convicted of sexual offenses against children.
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 48,
175–188.
Templeman, T. L., & Stinnett, R. D. (1991). Patterns of sexual arousal and
history in a ‘‘normal’’ sample of young men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20,
137–150.
2
Sadomasochism
Pekka Santtila, N. Kenneth Sandnabba,
and Niklas Nordling 1
The perspectives on sadomasochistic sexual behavior range from seeing it as a
crime or a psychiatric disorder to seeing it as ‘‘a sophisticated erotic activity
with several layers of meaning and significance’’ (Henkin, 1992). Modern
popular culture and fashion contain stylized representations of fetishistic and
sadomasochistic activity in, for example, mainstream music videos (chains and
leather clothes have been popular features). You can also find an advertisement
on the Internet for ‘‘a practical 4-day course to explore SM sex’’ that promises
a practical and hands-on workshop with lots of demonstrations and useful tips
about SM equipment, etiquette, and negotiation of safer sex in SM scenes
(MetroM8, n.d.), and a description of the diagnostic criteria for sexual mas-
ochism (diagnosis number 302.83) and sexual sadism (diagnosis number
302.84), which include clinically significant distress or impairment in social,
occupational, or other important areas of functioning in the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).
The question becomes, is sadomasochistic sexuality a more or less voluntary
variant of human sexuality that does not necessarily imply psychiatric illness
and need for treatment, or is it a deviant behavior with serious consequences
(psychological distress and physical injury) both for the individual and the
society? Also, it is open to question whether sadomasochistic sexuality can be
viewed as a single, homogenous phenomenon or whether the different per-
spectives might be describing relatively independent phenomena that have
been loosely labeled as sadomasochistic.
22 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
not, in the large majority of cases, reveal pathological patterns of family inter-
action, although a number of clinical case reports have suggested this to be the
case (Blos, 1991; Blum, 1991; Coen, 1988; Rothstein, 1991). These reports have
two flaws. On one hand, they are lacking in systematic empirical support, and on
the other, they are exclusively based on people who have sought psychological
help. So far, no studies have focused on exploring associations between child-
hood experiences and the way in which a nonclinical sample of SM practitioners
express their sexuality. For example, attachment theorists (e.g., Shaver, Hazan, &
Bradshaw, 1988) have shown that attachment style affects the expression of
sexuality in a number of areas including trust, desire for reciprocation, and fear of
closeness. They also suggest that for every feature of adult love relationships,
there is either a documented or a plausible infant parallel.
Likewise, the question of whether childhood abuse experiences have
etiological significance for sadomasochistic behavior has largely been ignored.
Due to the complexity of sadomasochistic sexual behavior (SM sex) and the
significance of social influences on it, it is unlikely that any simple associations
between childhood abuse experiences and later SM sex could be found.
Nevertheless, it is important to ascertain empirically what the role of sexual
abuse—if any—is for the development of sadomasochistic sexual interests and
for the choice of either sadistic or masochistic position.
The empirical results reported in this chapter to clarify some of these
questions mainly derive from a series of seven empirical articles describing
different aspects of sadomasochistic sexual behavior based on a large number
of SM practitioners (Alison, Santtila, Sandnabba, & Nordling, 2001; Nord-
ling, Sandnabba, & Santtila, 2000; Nordling, Sandnabba, Santtila, & Alison,
2005; Sandnabba et al., 1999; Sandnabba, Santtila, Beetz, Nordling, & Alison,
2002; Santtila et al., 2000; Santtila, Sandnabba, Alison, & Nordling, 2002).
our study was aimed at getting about equal numbers of straight and gay SM
practitioners by targeting different kinds of clubs. Of all the SM practitioners
27 percent identified themselves as mainly sadistic, 23 percent as both sadistic
and masochistic, and 50 percent as mainly masochistic in their behavior.
On average, first awareness of sadomasochistic interest had taken place
when the male SM practitioners had been between 18 to 20 years of age. Also,
first experience with SM sex and onset of regular sadomasochistic behavior
took place mostly between the ages of 21 and 25 years. The relatively late
awareness of sadomasochistic interests and late start of behavior is noteworthy.
Most of the SM practitioners (88 percent) had practiced ordinary sex, that
is, consensual heterosexual or gay male sexual activity without sadomasochis-
tic elements, before engaging in sadomasochism. Some 5 percent of the SM
practitioners no longer practiced ordinary sex. On average, they stopped having
ordinary sex at 25 years of age. On the other hand, approximately a fourth (27
percent) of the male SM practitioners endorsed a statement suggesting that only
sadomasochistic sex could satisfy them. These results suggest that the devel-
opment of sadomasochistic sexual behavior starts after experience with more
ordinary sexual behavior and the establishment of a sexual orientation.
These results relate to the relationship between sadomasochistic sexual
practices and other sexual activities. The cue-response model of sexual arousal
(Suppe, 1985) is a suitable model for analyzing this question, since it stresses
inflexibility as a primary criterion for paraphilia. The model presents a classi-
fication of how specific cues stimulate or inhibit an individual’s arousal. Cues
interfering with sexual arousal are classified as inhibitory, while cues that nei-
ther inhibit nor intensify sexual arousal are seen as nonfacilitative. Facilitative
cues, on their part, enhance but are not necessary for sexual arousal. Finally,
cues that are necessary for sexual arousal are named paraphiliac. This differs
from the definition of paraphilia in DSM-IV, where a clinically important
distress or impairment of work, social, or personal functioning is required for
diagnosis. According to Suppe’s model, sadomasochism is paraphiliac if it is the
only way for an individual to get sexually aroused and satisfied. This also means
that if a person also engages in sex without sadomasochistic elements, sado-
masochism should not be viewed as a paraphiliac cue for him or her. These
results are also relevant for viewing sadomasochism as a sexual minority. This
would not seem to be the case; instead, sadomasochistic sexuality is, for the
most part, an additional feature of the sexuality of the SM practitioners who also
engage in more ordinary sexual behavior.
It has been suggested that it would be a development from taking mostly
the masochistic role in an SM scene toward taking mostly the sadistic position
(Baumeister, 1988). This hypothesis of development has not been unequivo-
cally supported, however. When exploring the changes in sadomasochistic
preference, it was found that almost half of the male SM practitioners had not
changed their preference at all. About a fifth had changed their behavior to-
ward sadistic and another fifth toward masochistic preferences. Changes toward
Sadomasochism 25
Percent of SM
Practitioners Who
Completely or
Statements Partly Agreed
I wanted to do it again 97
I felt happy 86
I was glad 86
I felt safe 79
I was proud 59
I felt guilty 24
I was troubled 22
I was afraid about the future 20
I thought it was immoral 12
I was disgusted with myself 9
WHAT DO SADOMASOCHISTS DO
(IN BED AND ELSEWHERE)?
Most people have some kind of an image of what sadomasochistic sex
involves. Still, there are no generally accepted definitions of what constitutes
sadomasochistic sex, and such popular images may be more reflective of cultural
and media influences than of the reality of sadomasochistic sex. Therefore, a
good way of looking at the question is to ask individuals who consider
themselves to be sadomasochists to report what they actually do. Table 2.2
presents the proportions of male participants who have engaged in different
Table 2.2. Frequencies with which Male SM Practitioners Participated
in Different Sexual Behaviors and Role-Plays
Proportion of
Sexual Practice or All-Male SM
Behavior/Role-Play Practitioners Prevalence
1. Oral sex 95.2 E
2. Bondage 88.7 E
3. Flagellation 82.8 S
4. Anal intercourse 80.6 G
5. Handcuffs 74.7 E
6. Rimming 73.1 G
7. Dildos 72.6 G
8. Leather outfits 72.6 G
9. Chains 71.0 E
10. Verbal humiliation 69.9 S
11. Clothespins, clamps 66.6 E
12. Mask, blindfold 66.2 S
13. Spanking 65.5 E
14. Cockbinding 64.5 G
15. Gag 53.8 S
16. Biting 53.3 E
17. Rubber outfits 52.1 S
18. Cane whipping 50.6 S
19. Vaginal intercourse 47.3 S
20. Water sports 47.3 E
21. Wrestling 45.7 G
22. Body odors 42.5 E
23. Face slapping 40.3 E
24. Use of weights 39.3 E
25. Enema 39.3 E
26. Special equipment, e.g. slings, crosses, cages 38.1 G
27. Hot wax 35.0 E
28. Ice 33.9 E
29. Fist fucking 33.3 E
30. Cross-dressing 28.5 S
31. Piercing 21.0 E
32. Skin branding 17.3 E
33. Scat (coprophilia) 17.3 E
34. Hypoxyphilia 16.7 E
35. Straitjacket 15.6 S
36. Electric shocks 15.0 S
37. Knives, razor blades 13.4 E
38. Mummifying 12.9 E
39. Catheter 9.2 E
40. Zoophilia 6.4 E
Role-Plays E
1. Master/Madame-slave 55.9 S
2. Uniform scenes 38.8 G
3. Teacher-student 29.1 S
4. Execution scenes 23.6 E
5. Hospital scenes 15.7 S
6. Rape scenes 13.5 E
E¼equally prevalent among gay and straight males; G¼more prevalent among gay males; S¼more prevalent
among straight males.
Sadomasochism 27
kinds of sexual behaviors and role-plays during the twelve months preceding
our survey.
This information indicates that, in accordance with the results of the
studies of Moser and Levitt (1987), flagellation and bondage were among the
most popular activities. Additionally, some activities not specific to sadomas-
ochism, for example, oral sex and anal intercourse, that these authors did not
investigate, were also quite popular. The similarities between the percentages
of some behaviors (bondage, verbal humiliation, gag, biting, cane-whipping,
water sports, enema, face-slapping, hot wax, cross-dressing, piercing, skin
branding, and zoophilia) in our study and that of Moser and Levitt were
noteworthy.
Most SM practitioners had engaged in sadomasochistic sex during the
preceding twelve months from two to five times overall. The analysis of
the effect of sadomasochistic preference on the frequency of sadomasochistic
sessions gave some indication of the sadistic males being more often engaged in
sadomasochistic sex than the masochistic males. The average number of sa-
domasochistic sessions during a month that the SM practitioners would have
liked to have was six. Sadomasochistic preference had no effect on this vari-
able. It seems that sadomasochists are not able to have SM sex as often as they
would have wanted. However, as pointed out above, most of them also en-
gaged in ordinary sexual activity.
The discrepancy between how often the SM practitioners engaged in
sessions, and how often they would have liked to, may depend on difficulties
in finding a partner who would share the same sexual interests, because this in
most cases requires involvement in the sadomasochistic subculture. Also, the
high number of masochistic heterosexual men and the relative lack of women
create difficulties, a result established earlier by researchers in the field (e.g.,
Moser & Levitt, 1987) and confirmed again. The difficulties experienced by
the masochistic SM practitioners may also have been reflected in their ex-
pressed desire for having steady relationships.
more easily. Previous studies (Kamel, 1983; Spengler, 1977) have shown gay
men to be more integrated in the sadomasochistic subculture. However,
during the last two decades, sadomasochism has become more visible and it
may be that such differences no longer exist. To the extent that gay male
sadomasochistic subculture still offers more role models and possibilities of
engaging in sexual behavior than the straight sadomasochistic subculture
(Kamel, 1983), it may be that gay male sadomasochists are more satisfied with
their sex lives than straight male sadomasochists.
There was some indication that the gay male SM practitioners are better
educated (43 percent had college education) compared to straight men (29
percent had college education). The gay male SM practitioners also had a
higher income than the population in general. To the extent that educational
attainment is viewed as a measure of social and psychological well-being, it
seems that the gay male SM practitioners had succeeded well in this respect
(Sandnabba et al., 1999).
Exclusively straight males became aware of their sadomasochistic prefer-
ences at a younger age than other groups of SM practitioners. Further, they
also had their first experience at a younger age. In a similar manner, there was a
tendency for them to differ from the other groups in terms of the onset of
regular sadomasochistic activity (Sandnabba et al., 1999). This means that the
gay male SM practitioners became aware and started practicing their sado-
masochistic interests later.
One reason might be that the gay male SM practitioners only become
interested in sadomasochistic activities after they have resolved issues related to
sexual interest. This would agree with findings showing that gay male indi-
viduals establish their sexual orientation and start sexual activity later than
heterosexual males (Coleman, 1982; Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 1993). This
is also consistent with Kamel’s idea of sadomasochism as a reaction to dissat-
isfaction with the ordinary gay male scene (1983). These results also suggest
that the development of sadomasochistic sexual behavior starts after experience
with sexual behavior without sadomasochistic elements and the establishment
of a sexual orientation.
Correlations between sexual orientation (rated on a five-point scale with
anchors exclusively homosexual and exclusively heterosexual with the middle
point being bisexual) and sadomasochistic preference (also rated on a five-
point scale with anchors exclusively sadistic and exclusively masochistic with
the middle point being equally sadistic and masochistic) were computed sep-
arately for male and female SM practitioners. Both male and female SM
practitioners with a more heterosexual orientation were more likely to have a
more masochistic preference while the more gay SM practitioners were more
likely to be sadistically oriented. This association was stronger in females when
compared to male SM practitioners.
As already reported above, 27 percent of all male SM practitioners en-
dorsed a statement suggesting that only sadomasochistic sex could satisfy them.
Sadomasochism 29
The straight males were somewhat more likely to endorse this statement than
the gay males (Sandnabba et al., 1999).
Hypermasculinity
Among the 184 SM practitioners studied (see Table 2.3), only 57 of the
256 possible combinations of the 7 hypermasculinity behaviors (i.e., profiles)
occurred, which suggests the existence of an underlying structure in how the
behaviors are combined, that is, sexual scripts (Santtila et al., 2002). This means
that the behaviors in this group combine in certain predictable ways and not
randomly. Therefore, from knowing whether a subject has engaged in a
particular behavior, for example fisting, it is possible to tell something about
other behaviors he or she is likely to have engaged in as well.
SM practitioners who engaged in water sports were also engaged in rim-
ming. Cockbinding, however, was a qualitatively different aspect of hyper-
masculinity. SM practitioners who engaged in fisting most certainly also had
experienced scat, and those with experience of scat in turn had also experi-
enced enema. Thus, the presence of these behaviors combined with either the
rimming/water sports behaviors or the cockbinding behavior identify SM
practitioners with the most experience. However, all of these behaviors seem
more related to the rimming/water sports dimension, pointing to a script
within the hypermasculinity theme starting with rimming and ending at fisting.
Use of dildo together with catheter had no clear relationship with either se-
quence. This may have something to do with them being pieces of technical
equipment. In conclusion, the use of dildos does not give information on the
level of an SM practitioner’s experience with respect to other hypermasculinity
behaviors. The use of catheter was quite rare, which may explain why it was not
a structured part of the hypermasculinity scripts.
Pain
Forty out of the 128 possible combinations of the presence of the be-
haviors in this group were observed, again suggesting the existence of a clear
Sadomasochism 33
Humiliation
Of the thirty-two possible profiles, eighteen were identified, again sug-
gesting the existence of a clear underlying structure. The major distinction
could be drawn between flagellation and knives on one hand and face slapping
on the other. SM practitioners who had used knives in their SM sessions had
most certainly also been involved in sessions where flagellation had been
enjoyed. The remaining two behaviors, verbal humiliation and gag did not
clearly belong to any of these two major distinctions. However, when ex-
amining the results of the analysis further, it was noticed that gag was more
associated with the flagellation/knives script. In contrast, there was some in-
dication that verbal humiliation was more associated with a script involving
face slapping. Verbal humiliation, which was a relatively common behavior,
may, therefore, be seen to express similar intentions as face slapping, albeit in a
milder form.
Restraint
Results concerning the nine restraint behaviors were somewhat less clear
when compared to the above results. However, an interesting sequence was
revealed. Six of the behaviors were ordered in a sequence of restraint behaviors
34 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
starting from the less extreme bondage, and going through chains, handcuffs,
slings, and straitjacket, and finally ending with the most extreme variation of
hypoxyphilia. This is understandable since all of them (with the possible ex-
ception of hypoxyphilia) involve the use of some kind of equipment. Also, the
roles of the sadist and the masochist are clearly defined in scripts involving
these behaviors.
One of the behaviors, wrestling, represented a qualitatively different kind
of restraint behavior. It differs from the above behaviors in that it does not
require clearly defined dominant and submissive positions and also in that no
equipment is necessary in order to engage in this behavior. The use of ice
could not be associated to the other behaviors in a structured way. This can be
explained because ice is not a restraint behavior per se, rather it is used for
additional fun in an SM script involving restraint. Finally, mummifying rep-
resented an extreme form of both the group of six behaviors described above
as well as an extreme form following wrestling. The association between
mummifying and wrestling could be understood as two different ways to limit
the freedom of movement.
Clearly, the sadomasochistic behaviors are not haphazardly combined with
each other. Rather, evidence for structured patterns of co-occurrences was
found. Further, the combining of the behaviors was also theoretically mean-
ingful, indicating the existence of progressions of sadomasochistic behaviors
which can be likened to sexual scripts for ordinary heterosexual sexual be-
havior (Gagnon, 1990; Gagnon & Simon, 1987).
These results have important implications for the understanding and
conceptualization of sadomasochism as a sexual phenomenon. Some of the
behaviors are observed in almost all participants, such as bondage, flagellation,
rimming, and clothespin torture (Alison et al., 2001). However, in addition to
these ‘‘core’’ behaviors, there are a number of less-common activities expressed
in sadomasochistic sex that form specific and distinct scripts. Different persons,
creating subgroups of individuals, differentially engage in these scripts. The
existence of such emphases suggests that individual careers within the sado-
masochistic subcultures are determined in an interplay between the individ-
ual’s own developmental history, psychological characteristics (see Santtila,
Sandnabba, & Nordling, 2000), and the subcultural context within which the
individual faces information concerning possible pathways of expression and
conformist group processes that make the development of certain scripts more
likely than others (cf. social constructionist approaches to sexuality, e.g., Hart,
1985). This process may be more transparent in sadomasochism due to its
highly ritualized nature, but the process itself is probably shared in most ex-
pressions of human sexuality.
Further, the SM practitioners’ involvement in the subculture through
sexual contacts and porn was positively associated with greater variability in
their sexual behavior. Although the design of the study does not warrant any
causal conclusions, the results nevertheless imply that sadomasochistic behavior
Sadomasochism 35
SM Practitioners with
Behavior/Role-Play Experience in Bestiality Comparison Group
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: Assessed in the strange situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Alison, L., Santtila, P., Sandnabba, N. K., & Nordling, N. (2001). Sadoma-
sochistically-oriented behavior: Diversity in practice and meaning. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 30, 1–12.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Baumeister, R. F. (1988). Masochism as escape from self. Journal of Sex Research,
25, 28–59.
Blos, P. (1991). Sadomasochism and the defense against recall of painful affect.
Journal of American Psychoanalytical Association, 39, 417–430.
Blum, H. P. (1991). Sadomasochism in the psychoanalytic process, within and
beyond the pleasure principle: Discussion. Journal of American Psychoana-
lytical Association, 39, 431–450.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. London: Hogarth
Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
London: Hogarth Press.
Brewin, C. A., Andrews, B., & Gottlib, I. H. (1993). Psychopathology and early
experience: Reappraisal of retrospective reports. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
82–98.
Coen, S. J. (1988). Sadomasochistic excitement: Character disorder and perver-
sion. In R. A. Glick & D. I. Meyers (Eds.), Masochism, current psychoanalytic
perspectives (pp. 43–59). London: Analytic Press.
Coleman, E. (1982). Developmental stages of the coming out process. Journal of
Homosexuality, 7, 31–43.
Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S. S. (1988). Mothers’ internal models of relationships
and children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of mother-
child interaction. Child Development, 59, 1273–1285.
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrela-
tionships among components and gender label. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 46, 991–1004.
DeLamater, J. D., & MacCorquodale, P. (1979). Premarital sexuality: Attitudes,
relationships, behavior. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Falk, G., & Weinberg, T. S. (1983). Sadomasochism and popular Western culture.
In T. S. Weinberg & G. W. Kamel (Eds.), S and M studies in sadomasochism
(pp. 137–146). New York: Prometheus Books.
Ferrenbach, P. A., Smith, W., Monastersky, C., & Deisher, R. W. (1986). Ad-
olescent sexual offenders: Offender and offense characteristics. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 225–233.
Foucault, M. (1999). Sexualitet, makt och identitetens politik [Sexuality, power,
and the politics of identity]. Lambda Nordica, 2, 122–137.
Sadomasochism 43
Scharff, D. E. (1988). The sexual relationship: An object relations view of sex and the
family. London: Routledge Press.
Shaver, P., Hazan, C., & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The inte-
gration of three behavioral systems. In R. J. Steinberg & M. L. Barnes
(Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68–99). New York: Vail-Ballou Press.
Spengler, A. (1977). Manifest sadomasochism of males: Results of an empirical
study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 6, 441–456.
Suppe, F. (1985). Classifying sexual disorders: The diagnostic and statistical manual
of the American Psychiatric Association. In J. P. DeCecco (Ed.), Gay
personality and sexual labeling (pp. 9–28). New York: Harrington Park Press.
Taywaditep, K. J. (2001). Marginalization among the marginalized: Gay men’s negative
attitudes toward effeminacy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of
Chicago.
Townsend, L. (1983). The leatherman’s handbook. New York: Modernismo.
Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive affect, and
competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct validation. Child
Development, 50, 821–829.
Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Moser, C. (1984). The social constituents of
sadomasochism. Social Problems, 31, 379–389.
Weinberg, T. S. (1987). Sadomasochism in the United States: A review of recent
sociological literature. Journal of Sex Research, 23, 50–69.
Widom, C. S., & Shepard, R. L. (1996). Accuracy of adult recollections of
childhood victimization: Part I. Childhood physical abuse. Psychological
Assessment, 8, 412–421.
3
Donna M. Vandiver 1
Recently, media attention has been drawn to females who have sexually assaulted
young boys. One of the most highly publicized cases was a teacher from Seattle,
Mary Kay LeTourneau, who had an affair with her sixth grade student, Vili
Fualaau (‘‘Le Tourneau says,’’ 2004). She spent six months in jail for the offense.
Despite being arrested on two occasions, she continued to see her former stu-
dent. She had two children with him and later married him in March 2005 after
he had turned 21 years of age. Despite extensive media coverage of Mary Kay
LeTourneau’s love affair with her student, few equated this type of behavior
with a bona fide ‘‘sex offender.’’ The majority of people had not even heard of
females sexually offending; sex offenders are typically thought to be male.
Researchers have identified relationships such as the one Mary Kay Le-
Tourneau established with her student as a specific category of female sex
offense, teacher/lover. In fact, this is only one of the many types of female
sex offenses. A high percentage of females who sexually offend do not offend
alone; they often sexually abuse a child with a male, possibly their husband.
High rates of incest have also been found among this population of offenders,
with mothers often abusing a son or daughter. For some females who sexually
offend, the stereotypical rapist is turned on its head—an adult woman rapes an
adult man. Other females start out on the wrong side of the law; the women
are criminals, committing nonsexual offenses and the sexual offense is just
another offense. Usually this is for the purpose of economic gain; it includes
48 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Percentage of
Offenders Who
Researchers Are Female Sample Size and Source
PROBLEMS IN RECOGNIZING
FEMALE SEX OFFENDERS
Many researchers have speculated that sex offenses in general are largely
underreported (Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith, 1990), and data from the
NCVS support this notion. Measurement problems are inherent in reporting
sexual abuse. While the majority of people are willing to call the police if
someone steals their wallet or purse or their car, not everyone is willing to
report a sexual offense. Many times the offender is a friend, relative, or even a
caretaker ( Johnson, R., & Shrier, 1987), and in these cases the victim is even
less likely to report the offense. When the victim is a young child, he or she
may not recognize the behavior as something that is wrong (Groth & Birn-
baum, 1979). Victims may feel the abuse is their fault ( Johnson, R., & Shrier,
1987) or may fear additional abuse if they tell someone. Detection and
prosecution of sexual abuse is also problematic. Sexual abuse is more difficult
to detect than physical abuse (Farrell, 1988). Physical abuse results in bruises
and broken bones whereas sexual abuse does not result in such obvious
physical markings. The victim is not always encouraged to tell his/her story
because of the difficulty associated with criminal prosecution. Some believe
sexual abuse is a mental health problem rather than a criminal justice problem;
thus, it is better handled by social service agencies rather than criminal courts
(Berliner & Barbieri, 1984).
The problems associated with reporting sexual abuse are compounded
when the offender is female. The thought of a woman sexually offending is a
perplexing concept to most; in our society we are not geared toward thinking
that a female is physically capable of ‘‘rape’’ or any other type of sexual assault
(Denov, 2004). This obstacle in our thinking is conveyed well by the title of a
book chapter, ‘‘What harm can be done without a penis?’’ (Hislop, 2001).1
Males are typically associated with violent crimes, and there is often an in-
ability to associate a ‘‘submissive and passive’’ woman with a violent offense
(Scavo, 1989). The problem is perpetuated by organizational structures within
agencies such as law enforcement departments and treatment centers that rely
on traditional constructs of who can and cannot sexually offend (Denov,
2004).
Researchers have identified many reasons why females are underrepre-
sented in official data. Sexual abuse by a woman is often considered harmless
despite research findings indicating the effects are prominent for victims of
female sexual abuse (Hetherton, 1999). Women sex offenders often go un-
noticed because women are able to disguise sexual offenses when engaging in
routine child-rearing activities such as bathing and dressing (Groth & Birn-
baum, 1979). Females who act with a male co-offender may be seen as less
culpable than their male partner (Mayer, 1992).
Female Sex Offenders 51
1. clinical sources (see Faller, 1987, 1995; Johnson, R., & Shrier, 1987; Peluso &
Putnam, 1996; Rosencrans, 1997; Rudin, Zalewski, & Bodmer-Turner, 1995;
Travin, Cullen, & Protter, 1990);
2. incarcerated samples (see Kaplan & Green, 1995; O’Connor, 1987; Syed &
Williams, 1996);
3. medical samples (i.e., hospital) (see Duncan & Williams, 1998; Marvasti, 1986);
and
4. sex offender registries (see Vandiver & Kercher, 2004; Vandiver & Walker,
2002).
Each of the above sources includes information from the female sex offender
herself (or her records) and from victims of female sex abuse (Denov, 2004;
Johnson, R., & Shrier, 1987; Krug, 1989; Peluso & Putnam, 1996; Rudin et al.,
1995; Sarrel & Masters, 1982). Most of these sources are limited in that they
include offenses known only to social service agencies, medical personnel, or law
enforcement; thus, they provide only the narrowest view into the world of female
sex offenders, given that many do not come to the attention of such agencies.
While the number of studies specifically focused on female sex offenders is
growing and includes approximately thirty empirical studies, many are limited
by small sample sizes. Only about ten studies include more than thirty female
sex offenders (see Duncan & Williams, 1998; Faller, 1987, 1995; O’Connor,
1987; Pothast & Allen, 1994; Rosencrans, 1997; Rudin et al., 1995; Vandiver
& Kercher, 2004; Vandiver & Walker, 2002). With the exception of Vandiver
and Kercher’s (2004; n ¼ 471), no study included more than 100 subjects.
Many studies included fewer than fifteen female sex offenders (see Chasnoff
et al., 1986; Chow & Choy, 2002; Denov, 2003; Johnson, R., & Shrier, 1987;
Kaplan & Green, 1995; Krug, 1989; Marvasti, 1986; Nathan & Ward, 2002;
Peluso & Putnam, 1996; Rowan, Rowan, & Langelier, 1990; Sarrel & Mas-
ters, 1982; Travin et al., 1990; Wolfe, 1985).
(Faller, 1987, 1995; Lewis & Stanley, 2000; Vandiver & Kercher, 2004;
Vandiver & Walker, 2002).
High rates of mental illness are reported by various studies. For example,
Lewis and Stanley (2000) found in a study of fifteen women, 66 percent had
a psychotic disorder (n ¼ 2), schizophrenia (n ¼ 1), or depressive symptoms
(n ¼ 7). Nathan and Ward (2002) also found 66 percent of the twelve female
sex offenders had either depression (n ¼ 4), an eating disorder (n ¼ 3), or ex-
perienced self-mutilation with suicidal ideations. In an assessment of eleven
female sex offenders, Kaplan and Green (1995) found 72 percent experienced
posttraumatic stress syndrome, 63 percent had experienced major depression,
63 percent had avoidant personality disorder, and 45 percent had dependent
personality disorder. In O’Connor’s (1987) study, 48 percent of eighty-one
incarcerated female sex offenders had a history of some type of psychiatric
disorder. Additionally, 40 percent of the eighty-one women had psychotic
features. In a study of seventy-two female sex offenders, 32 percent had some
type of mental illness (Faller, 1995). A study of sixteen female sex offenders
included 31 percent who had either borderline personality disorder or psy-
chotic features. In a study of forty female sex offenders, 18 percent had
psychotic features (Faller, 1987).
Even though the rate of mental illness has been found to be high, caution is
suggested in interpreting these findings. Many of the sources relied upon are
clinical sources (Faller, 1995; Lewis & Stanley, 2000; Matthews, Hunter, &
Vuz, 1997); thus, many of the women were likely being treated primarily for a
mental illness and the sexual offending was then discovered. Relying on clinical
sources is likely to yield high percentages of persons with a mental illness.
A moderate number of cases with mental retardation and borderline
intellectual functioning have also been found among this population of of-
fenders. Thirty-three percent of forty cases in one study were mentally retarded
or had brain damage (Faller, 1987). Twenty-seven percent of the fifteen cases in
Lewis and Stanley’s (2000) research had mild mental retardation. Twenty-two
percent of seventy-two cases in another study were mentally retarded (Faller,
1995). Rowan et al. (1990) reported one of nine cases had mental retardation.
A few studies reported many female sex offenders had a history of drug
and/or alcohol abuse. Slightly more than half of the forty cases in Faller’s 1987
study and seventy-two cases in Faller’s 1995 study had a substance-abuse
history. In Rosencrans’s (1997) study of ninety-three female sex offenders, 32
percent had abused alcohol and 19 percent had a substance-abuse history. The
drug or alcohol abuse for many women may be evidence of poor coping
strategies in general.
Experiencing sexual abuse as a child is also a common characteristic of
female sex offenders. Eighty percent of the fifteen cases in Lewis and Stanley’s
(2000) study were sexually abused by either someone they knew or a family
member. Approximately three-quarters (76 percent) of the thirty-eight female
sex offenders in one study (Pothast & Allen, 1994) were sexually abused as a
Female Sex Offenders 53
child. Fifty-eight percent of the twelve female sex offenders in Wolfe’s (1985)
study had a history of sexual abuse. In Miccio-Fonseca’s (2000) study, 56
percent of the eighteen female sex offenders were sexually abused as a child.
Almost half of the forty female sex offenders in Faller’s (1987) study reported
experiencing sexual abuse. In another study, the victims of female sex of-
fenders believed 20 percent of their abusers had been abused by their father
and 20 percent were abused by their mother (Rosencrans, 1997). Many of the
reports of sexual abuse, however, were self-reported.
Relationship to Offender
Number
of Related Acquaintances Stranger Other
Researchers Victims (%) (%) (%) (%)
Faller, 1987 63 90 10
Faller, 1995 72 75 Nonfamilial: 25
Lewis & Stanley, 22* 76 24
2000
Peluso & Putnam, 2 50 50
1996
Rudin et al., 1995 87 56 22 3 Caretaker: 19
Vandiver & 471 37 46 7 Missing or not
Kercher, 2004 applicable: 10**
Vandiver & Walker, 16* 94 Nonfamilial: 6
2002
Syed & Williams, 18* 76 Nonfamilial: 24
1996
* The original data did not include information on all victims; the number reported here includes
the number of victims with available information.
** Several offenders did not have a specific victim (i.e., possession of pornography).
The victims are typically young, with an average age of less than 12 years
(Faller, 1987, 1995; Lewis & Stanley, 2000; Nathan & Ward, 2002; Vandiver
& Kercher, 2004). With regard to the sex of the victim, the studies vary. Many
researchers report a slightly higher number of female victims as compared to
male victims (Faller, 1987, 1995; Rowan et al., 1990; Rudin et al., 1995). One
study, however, included reports that all of the victims, or a majority, were
male (Lewis & Stanley, 2000). Vandiver and Kercher (2004), relying on 471
subjects, found half of the victims were male and half were female. Subsequent
research by Vandiver (in press) indicated that the sex of the victim varies
depending on whether the woman was acting by herself or with a co-offender.
Those acting alone are more likely to have male victims while co-offenders are
more likely to have a combination of male and female victims.
Sarrel & Masters, 1982 Forced assault Male victims of female sexual abuse; n ¼ 11.
Babysitter abuse
Incestuous abuse
Dominant woman abuse
McCarty, 1981; 1986 Independent offenders of males (1986) Female sex offenders identified by child protective
Independent offenders of females (1986) services who engaged in mother-child incest;
Co-offenders and accomplices (1986) n ¼ 26.
Severely psychologically disturbed abuser (1981)
Mathews, 1987; Mathews et al., Teacher/lover Female sex offenders sentenced to community
1989 Predisposed correctional center; n ¼ 16.
Male-coerced molester
Exploration/exploitation
Psychologically disturbed (McCarty, 1986)
Mayer, 1992 Female rapist Prior empirical reports of female sex offenders.
Female sexual harassment
Mother molester
Triads
Homosexual molestation
Syed & Williams, 1996 Teacher/lover (Mathews et al., 1989) Female sex offenders incarcerated in Canada;
(building on Mathews et al.’s Male-coerced (Mathews et al., 1989) n ¼ 19.
[1989] categories) Angry-impulsive
Male-accompanied, familial
Male-accompanied, nonfamilial
(continued)
Table 3.3. continued
Nathan & Ward, 2002 Male-accompanied, the rejected/revengeful Female sex offenders incarcerated in Australia;
(building on Mathews et al.’s n ¼ 12.
[1989] categories)
Vandiver & Kercher, 2004 Heterosexual nurturers Registered adult female sex offenders in Texas;
Noncriminal homosexual offenders n ¼ 471.
Female sexual predators
Young adult child exploiters
Homosexual criminals
Aggressive homosexual offenders
Female Sex Offenders 57
Nurturer
Nurturing abuse typically involves an inappropriate relationship between a
woman and someone she knows. Several researchers have described different
types of female sex offenders who fit into this category; they are summarized as
heterosexual nurturer, teacher/lover, and babysitter abuse. Each involves a woman in
a position of authority who engages in a sexual relationship with (usually) a
young boy, often a teenager, whom she is responsible for in some way. This
type of sex offender is not ‘‘predatory’’ in terms of the woman specifically
going to certain locations (i.e., school, parks, etc.), yet there does appear to be
a grooming process where the woman becomes ‘‘friends’’ with the youth.
Thus, there may be a grooming process where boundaries are slowly redefined
over the period the relationship exists.
Heterosexual Nurturer
Vandiver and Kercher (2004) reported a broad category of inappropriate
relationships, including any woman in a caretaking or nurturing role. For
example, Vandiver (2003) described a woman who worked at a youth facility
and ‘‘fell in love’’ with a young teenage boy; thus a mentor-mentee rela-
tionship existed. The woman had no history of sexual abuse. She was divorced
with two children. She indicated the victim was a 14-year-old male whom she
met through her work. She described the sexual act between her and the
teenager as consensual, but followed up by stating that she knew it was wrong
and did not want to make an excuse for what she had done. She indicated that
the teenager came from a ‘‘bad family.’’ He did not know his father and had
been sexually abused by his grandfather.
The relationship began at the youth facility and the boy began to come
over to her residence to talk and get something to eat. The relationship
progressed into a sexual one after he kissed her once. She had sex with him
approximately seven times over a six-month period. She stated, ‘‘When it
happened it seemed natural—but I shouldn’t say natural because it’s not
natural to have sex with a teenage boy. He kissed me and I didn’t stop it.’’
The woman was with the boy when she had a car accident, which led to
her arrest when law enforcement suspected the abuse. After she was arrested,
she still tried to contact the young boy and was ‘‘taken in [by law enforcement]
several times.’’ At the time of the interview she had not seen the boy in several
years.
Table 3.4. Summary of Female Sex Offender Typologies
Nurturer Heterosexual nurturer (Vandiver & Kercher, 2004) Adult female molests young male (approximately 12 years
old). Female has a mentorship role (i.e., teacher,
caretaker, etc.) to the young male.
Teacher/lover (Mathews et al., 1989) A teacher who has a sexual relationship with a young boy,
usually her student.
Babysitter abuse (Sarrel & Masters, 1982) Older woman or girl seducing a young boy whom she is not
related to; abuse occurring while she is babysitting.
Exploration/exploitation (Mathews et al., 1987) Often abuse in a babysitting situation; young (14 to 16);
typically no victimization history.
58
Angry-impulsive (Syed & Williams, 1996) Female violently assaults an adult male; motivated by anger.
Aggressive homosexual criminal (Vandiver & Adult female molests adult female.
Kercher, 2004)
Criminal Female sexual predator (Vandiver & Kercher, 2004) Adult who molests young boys; high rate of rearrest.
Offenders Homosexual criminals (Vandiver & Kercher, 2004). Adult who molests young girls; high rate of rearrest.
Psychologically Severely psychologically disturbed abuser (McCarty, 1986) Has a history of adolescent psychological problems.
Impaired
Homosexual Homosexual molestation (Mayer, 1992) Woman with homosexual tendencies; abuses child of same
Molester or opposite sex; may or may not have a co-offender;
victim may or may not be her own child.
* The original research did not include whether the female acted alone or with someone. The characteristics of the women and victim were similar to latter research of
co-offending women (see Vandiver, in press).
60 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
One of the interesting points to note about this situation is that the
woman described the young boy as having nowhere else to go and no one else
for support. In other words, he was ‘‘social junk.’’2 She could not do anything
to harm him—he was already damaged goods, so to speak. Similarly, Mary
Kay LeTourneau also took a young boy under her wing who was in a similar
situation.
The heterosexual nurturer category identified by Vandiver and Kercher
(2004) was the most common category of female sex offenders. The largest
percentage (31 percent, n ¼ 146) of 471 adult women were classified in this
category. This category of offenders would also include teachers who fall in
love with their male students.
Teacher/Lover
As portrayed in the media, women in a position of authority (i.e., teacher)
have engaged in a sexual relationship with a younger male, often a teenager
(i.e., student). Mathews, Matthews, and Speltz (1989) found a case that fits the
teacher/lover category. A teacher who ‘‘fell in love’’ with her adolescent student
reported that she saw nothing wrong with the relationship. The teacher was
not the victim of sexual abuse as a child; however, she was forced into
prostitution as an adolescent. Turning to an adolescent male was described by
Mathews et al. (1989) as the result of feeling fearful toward men.
Babysitter Abuse
Mathews et al. (1989) defined exploration/exploitation abusers who typically
abuse in a babysitting situation. Sarrel and Masters (1982) also defined babysitter
abuse as a category of female sex offense. Two cases of babysitter abuse were
described by Sarrel and Masters (1982). In one case, a 25-year-old man re-
ported he was sexually abused by his babysitter when he was only 10 years old.
The young man described the event as pleasurable and reported it had oc-
curred for approximately one year. The young man reported ‘‘she frequently
manipulated his penis and that sometimes there was an erection, but he had no
ejaculatory experience’’ (p. 122). The boy later told his family about the
experience. His father whipped him severely. He then took his son to a priest
and a psychiatrist. The father often referred to his son’s ‘‘shameful conduct’’
and told his son that he should have reported the sexual activity sooner. He did
not know what happened to the babysitter. The young boy reported that
afterward he never masturbated and had overwhelming feelings of guilt. He
did not date regularly and was not receptive to sexual advances made by
women. The man, after establishing a platonic relationship with a young
woman at the age of 24, discussed his fears regarding sex and the incidents that
occurred with the babysitter. He then began psychiatric treatment.
Female Sex Offenders 61
Co-offender
Several of the typologies include a distinction between women who act
alone (i.e., solo offenders) and those who act with another person (i.e., co-
offenders) (Mathews et al., 1989; Mayer, 1992; McCarty, 1986; Nathan &
Ward, 2001; Syed & Williams, 1996). The number of co-offenders in a given
population is high, meaning it is not uncommon for women who have sex-
ually offended to have a partner, usually male. In a recent study including
a cross-national sample of 227 women arrested for a sexual offense, approxi-
mately half acted with another person (Vandiver, in press). This is indicative
of high rates of coercion among this population of females who sexually
offend.
Researchers reported co-offenders were significantly different than those
who act alone. Co-offenders had more victims per incident. They were more
likely to abuse a relative and to have both male and female victims. The type of
behavior the women exhibit, however, includes a broad continuum from
passive to active, with more cases of passive participation cited in the research
(Vandiver, in press).
Characteristics that vary among co-offenders are the woman’s relationship
to the victim and the co-offender, her motivation (i.e., revengeful), whether
she was coerced, and level of contact with the victim during abuse (i.e., hands-
off or hands-on). Researchers have relied more on the relationship between
the woman and the victim (related or not related) and her motivation for
engaging in the sexual abuse (i.e., feeling of rejection and revenge) in de-
veloping classifications of co-offending women. For the purpose of this dis-
cussion, the following subcategories under male-accompanied are discussed:
(1) male-accompanied, familial, (2) male-accompanied, nonfamilial, and (3)
male-accompanied, rejected/revengeful.
62 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Male-Accompanied
Mayer (1992) proposed a typology of five categories of female sex of-
fenders, which includes a category of triads in which female offenders were not
acting alone in the abuse. The typical combination includes a mother, father,
and victim. The victim may include a child of the mother and father or a
nonrelated child. The mother or the father may be the coercer. It has been
speculated that when the mother is the initiator she may feel dependent or is
seeking to nurture the child. She may be reenacting her own abuse. When the
father is the initiator, the mother may be coerced.
Mathews et al. (1989) identified a category of women who acted with
another male (male-coerced ). This category depicts female offenders who were
coerced into sexually abusing a victim, usually their daughters. After Mathews
et al. proposed a male-coerced category, other researchers further developed
this category by breaking it into those who were related to the victim (male-
accompanied, familial), not related to the victim (male-accompanied, nonfa-
milial), and acting out of revenge (male-accompanied, the rejected/revengeful).
Male-Accompanied, Familial. Syed and Williams (1996), relying on the
typology developed by Mathews et al. (1989), found in an examination of
nineteen female sex offenders that not all of the cases could be classified within
the proposed categories. Instead of relying on a male-coerced category, they
found it more appropriate to create a new category: male-accompanied, familial.
They found four of the offenders in their study fit into a male-accompanied,
familial category. Likewise, McCarty (1986) identified a category of co-
offending mothers who abused their own child/children.
An example of a mother abusing her children was described in Syed and
Williams’s (1996) research. A woman allowed her common-law husband to
have sex with her daughter. The daughter was not biologically related to the
common-law husband. The stepfather took the daughter’s privileges away
(i.e., telephone use) if she refused to participate in the sexual abuse. The
mother assisted in the abuse by striking her daughter when she refused. The
mother had a history of sexual victimization by her family members and
nonfamily members. Her father was one of her abusers. Psychological tests
were administered to the mother, which ‘‘indicated she had severe assertive
and relationship deficits and, as a consequence, was a woman who did not
possess the necessary skills to defend her rights’’ (n.p.).
Male-Accompanied, Nonfamilial. Syed and Williams (1996) also proposed a
second category: male-accompanied, nonfamilial. Mathews et al. (1989) had also
identified women who fit into this category. They described a husband and
wife who molested a pair of 13-year-old twins who lived in the same apart-
ment complex. The details are given below:
The woman was arrested after the female victim’s boyfriend reported the
behavior. The woman was described as cooperative with law enforcement.
She spent time in jail and participated in a sex offender treatment program.
This example of a male-accompanied, nonfamilial situation highlights the use
of coercion by the dominant male. While not all male-accompanied cases
include a male who coerces the female, it does show that women, even though
they may actively participate in the sexual abuse, are highly vulnerable to
coercion into sexual abuse. Additionally, the woman in this situation was the
primary source of income for this family; thus, economic reliance on a
dominant male was not a factor in her situation.
Male-Accompanied, the Rejected/Revengeful. Nathan and Ward (2002) sug-
gested adding a category of female offenders who had a male partner, but were
not coerced, male-accompanied, the rejected/revengeful. The authors noted that
prior case studies included descriptions of female sex offenders who were
motivated by feelings of rejection in their primary relationship; the sexual
abuse appears to be out of revenge. One example included a woman who was
a victim of chronic domestic violence and she reported that she was motivated
by extreme jealousy.
Incestuous
Incestuous relationships with female offenders have been identified in
many typologies of female sex offenders. Prior research has found that women
64 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Sister-Brother Incest
A case of sister-brother incest was discussed in Sarrel and Masters’s (1982)
research. A 14-year-old girl began molesting her 10-year-old brother, and the
abuse occurred for two years. The researchers describe the abuse thus: ‘‘She
stimulated him manually and orally and then inserted his penis into her vagina.
At first he only felt frightened and did not understand what was happening.
She usually threatened to beat him or attack him with a knife if he told
anyone. He does not recall if he ejaculated. He was too frightened to tell his
parents’’ (p. 125). Later, his sister went to psychiatric treatment; the victim
subsequently became suicidal and he too was placed in psychiatric treatment.
He entered treatment again when he married and was unable to consummate
his marriage.
Mother-Son Incest
Several researchers have reported instances of mother-son incest (Lawson,
1993; Mayer, 1992). Two cases of incest were reported in Sarrel and Masters’s
(1982) research. A 30-year-old man reported to his therapist that his mother
who had been divorced since he was 2 years old began playing with his genitals
when he was 13 years old. The sexual activity later included her performing
oral sex on him and having sex with him. They had sex two to three times a
week until he left for college. When he went home, he continued having sex
with his mother. His mother died during his senior year of college. He re-
ported that he never approached his mother, but rather she always approached
him. The researchers noted, ‘‘He felt strongly devoted to her, stating that he
enjoyed her obvious pleasure during their sexual encounters far more than his
own’’ (p. 124). After he left for college, he reported he was not able to achieve
an erection when he attempted to have sex with a girl his own age. He felt
guilty and felt he was not being faithful to his mother. Once he became so
nauseated after having foreplay with a girl that he threw up. He resumed
dating but was not able to have sex. He later married and entered therapy.
Female Sex Offenders 65
Mother-Daughter Incest
Accounts of mothers abusing their own daughters have been reported and
identified by researchers as a salient category of female sex offenders. Mathews
et al. (1989) identified a mother whose husband had passed away and she
began first physically abusing her 4-year-old daughter and then sexually
abusing her.
When feeling alone and wanting to be close, ‘‘I would go into the
bedroom and touch [her daughter].’’ The abuse consisted of kissing and
fondling the child, usually over her pajamas or underwear. Initially the
abuse occurred when her daughter was awake. As the child grew older,
however, [the mother] would wait for [her daughter] to fall asleep before
touching her. (p. 15)
The mother was abused by her own father when she was a child. After the
mother entered substance abuse treatment, she reported the sexual abuse she
had with her daughter. She was referred to sexual abuse treatment.
Adult on Adult
Several typologies include adult women who sexually assault another
adult. Most of the categories describe (adult) women who sexually assault
(adult) men. One category, however, includes an adult woman who sexually
assaults another adult woman. This category of female sex offenders, therefore,
is divided into two groups: female-on-male and female-on-female.
Female-on-Male
Several typologies have been proposed that include an adult woman
specifically sexually assaulting an adult man: female sexual harassment and female
rapist (Mayer, 1992), dominant woman abuse, forced assault (Sarrel & Masters,
1982), and angry-impulsive (Syed & Williams, 1996). Mayer (1992) described a
continuum of this type of behavior that includes sexual harassment (a woman
sexually harassing a man, possibly at the workplace) and female rape, in which
she has sex with the man against his will.
Sarrel and Masters (1982) identified dominant woman abuse and used the
term to describe cases where there was at least one episode of a woman
engaging in overt sexual aggression—a complete role reversal where women
behaved as men have in sexually aggressive incidents. Three cases were used to
describe dominant woman abuse. In one of the cases, a man was sexually abused
by his wife after they were legally separated. She aggressively approached him
sexually and he reported feeling scared and not ejaculating as she had sex with
66 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
him. She had an orgasm. It was confirmed by the wife that she did attack her
husband and was ‘‘in a state of fury.’’ She expressed wanting to hurt him and
use sex as a way to express her rage.
In another case a 33-year-old woman who had only homosexual expe-
riences forced a 35-year-old male to have sex with her. He reported being
fearful and later suffered from sexual dysfunction. Another case involved a 17-
year-old male, who was forced to have intercourse by a 23-year-old woman
who was a friend of his family. He was a Mormon and expressed extreme guilt
over the sexual incident because it was in conflict with his religious beliefs. No
weapon was used, but he felt intimidated by her use of force (Sarrel & Masters,
1982).
Likewise, forced assault describes a woman who sexually assaults an adult
male (Sarrel & Masters, 1982). The male is described as being fearful, not
enjoying the experience. Four cases of forced assault were identified from
eleven male victims of female sexual assault. One of the victims was a truck
diver who was 27 years old. After meeting a woman whom he had known
previously, he went to a motel with her and the following occurred:
[H]e was given another drink and shortly thereafter fell asleep. He awoke
to find himself naked, tied hand and foot to a bedstead, gagged, and
blindfolded. As he listened to voices in the room, it was evident that
several women were present . . . he was told that he had to ‘‘have sex
with all of them.’’ He thinks that during his period of captivity four
different women used him sexually, some of them a number [of] times.
Initially he was manipulated to erection and mounted. . . . He be-
lieves that the period of forcible, restrained and repeated sexual as-
saults continued for [more] than 24 hours. (Sarrel & Masters, 1982,
pp. 120–121)
After the incident the man sought therapy. He never reported the incident
to law enforcement. He suffered from psychological distress and was not able
to complete sexual intercourse. He married later, but still was unable to engage
in sexual intercourse. His wife was unaware of the rape that he endured (Sarrel
& Masters, 1982).
Three other cases identified by these researchers included a 37-year-old
married man who was forced at gunpoint to have sex and receive oral sex from
several women. Another case involved a 23-year-old male medical student
who was forced to have sex with his female aggressor. He was threatened with
a scalpel. Another teenager who was 17 years old was forced by a group of five
people (three women and two men) to have oral sex performed on him and
was masturbated (Sarrel & Masters, 1982).
In all of the four cases, force or threatened use of force occurred. The men
were constrained physically in some way and were fearful of the attackers. All
Female Sex Offenders 67
were able to function sexually during the incident, yet none were able to
adequately perform sexually after the incident (Sarrel & Masters, 1982).
A similar situation of a woman violently sexually assaulting a male (angry-
impulsive) was described by Syed and Williams (1996). This type of female sex
offender was motivated by anger. In one case, a woman violently assaulted her
victim, an adult male. The researchers noted that none of the prior categories
had addressed anger as the central feature of the sexual abuse.
Female-on-Female
One study yielded a unique category of sex offenders not identified in
previous research. Vandiver and Kercher (2004) described aggressive homosexual
offenders, which included women who were typically in their thirties with a
victim also in her thirties. The offense of arrest was sexual assault. It was
speculated that this group included domestic violence between homosexual
couples. Furthermore, this type of female sex offender does not fit the typical
female sex offender in that her motivations are different. Her motivations are
likely to be similar to male sex offenders who sexually assault their spouse.
Criminal Offenders
Women who have a history of nonsexual arrests in conjunction with at
least one sex offense have also been identified. Vandiver and Kercher (2004)
identified two categories of such offenders, female sexual predator and homosexual
criminal. The female sexual predator has male victims whereas the homosexual
criminal has female victims. The women in the homosexual criminal category
were arrested for ‘‘forcing behavior,’’ including sexual performance on a child
and compelling prostitution. An article appearing in the Houston Chronicle
provides an example of such incidents.
Reports such as these are not uncommon. The Associated Press (‘‘Police
rescue,’’ 2001), for example, also reported of a 17-year-old girl from Mil-
waukee who was forced into a prostitution ring by a man and woman.
This category of offender can also include having children pose nude for
photographs to be sold privately or made available on websites for the purpose
68 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Psychologically Impaired
A category of female sex offenders has been identified that includes psy-
chologically impaired women. These women have been described as aggres-
sive, impulsive, poorly socialized, depressed, and guilty (McCarty, 1981).
Additionally, in many of the other identified categories of sex offenders, some
form of mental illness has been found. For example, Mayer (1983) noted that
mothers who abuse their daughters often exhibit psychotic behavior. As noted
in the section titled ‘‘Description of Female Sex Offenders,’’ many samples of
female sex offenders included high rates of mental illness. Thus, mental illness
may be a characteristic that occurs with many other identified characteristics of
female sex offenders.
Homosexual Molester
Mayer (1992) identified a category of female sex offenders, homosexual
molesters, that had many overlapping characteristics with other identified ca-
tegories of female sex offenders. This type of offender has homosexual ten-
dencies, possibly latent. She may molest a child who is the same sex or even
the opposite sex. Additionally, she may act with a male offender or by herself.
When she acts with a co-offender, she may assume a passive role to explore
homosexual feelings. This category is unique from other categories of sex
offenders in that she is motivated by her homosexual feelings; the abuse allows
her the opportunity to explore such feelings.
MOTIVATIONS/EXPLANATIONS OF BEHAVIOR
Past research, specifically, has identified the following motivations for
female sexual offending: reenactment of sexual abuse (Mayer, 1992; Saradjian
& Hanks, 1996), emotional women acting out their feelings, narcissistic
women abusing their own daughters (Mayer, 1992), extension of battered-
woman syndrome, socialization to follow their male accomplices (Davin,
Hislop, & Dunbar, 1999), desire for intimacy, economic gain, and domestic
violence among homosexual couples (Vandiver & Kercher, 2004).
Reenactment of early trauma has been proposed as a primary explanation
of females who sexually abuse (Mayer, 1992; Saradjian & Hanks, 1996). It is
proposed that the victim experiences displaced anger and, thus, identifies with
the aggressor. The victim later becomes an offender and acts out her experi-
ences on another person. Typically, researchers will cite the high rates of abuse
many sex offenders experienced themselves to support this notion; however,
the extent to which one affects the other has not been fully supported. While
there is a high rate of correlation between experiences of abuse and later abusing
cited in studies (see Knopp, 1984), this does not necessarily translate into
causation. In fact, as noted by Salter (2003), studies including more objective
measures (i.e., polygraph) result in the number of victims-turned-victimizer
70 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
less likely than male sex offenders to have legal problems (68 percent compared
to 63 percent) (Miccio-Fonseca, 2000). The sexual offense arrest was likely to
be the first arrest for women, but not the first arrest for the men (Vandiver &
Walker, 2002). High rates of substance abuse exist among both populations of
sex offenders (Faller, 1987). Female sex offenders reported fewer sexual
partners when compared to male sex offenders. While both groups reported
having experienced abuse as children (Mayer, 1992; Miccio-Fonseca, 2000),
women were more likely to report being a victim of incest when compared to
men (approximately 33 percent compared to 13 percent) and being a victim of
rape (39 percent compared to 4 percent) (Miccio-Fonseca, 2000). Addition-
ally, one study reported that 76 percent of the women, compared to 36
percent of the men, reported they had been sexually abused (Pothast & Allen,
1994). In another study, 54 percent of the women compared to 33 percent of
the men were sexually abused by 6 years of age (Miccio-Fonseca, 2000).
Prevalence
In 2003, 59 of the 247 (23.8 percent) females arrested for forcible rape
were juveniles according to the UCR (U.S. Department of Justice, 2005b).
Additionally, juvenile females accounted for 21.9 percent of the females ar-
rested for a sex offense other than forcible rape and prostitution. Although the
number of juvenile females arrested for a sex offense is low, they make up a
substantial portion (approximately 20 percent) of females arrested for a sex
offense. Again, caution should be made in drawing conclusions from the
numbers because law enforcement data do not fully capture the scope of this
group of offenders.
Data from the NCVS regarding the number of juvenile females who
sexually offended is not available.3 NIBRS data indicates that juveniles were
arrested for 172 of the 404 (42.6 percent) sexual offenses committed by females
in 2001 (U.S. Department of Justice, 2004). Thus, information from UCR
and NIBRS indicates that the number of female sex offenders is low, yet
juvenile sex offenders make up a substantial portion of females who sexually
offend.
Fromuth & Conn, 1997; Johnson, T. C., 1989; LeTourneau, Schoenwald, &
Sheidow, 2004). The number of juvenile females in the empirical studies ranges
from eight to only sixty-one; thus, no study has been conducted on samples
larger than sixty-one. The majority of the studies also rely on clinical sources.
The source and the number of subjects limit the ability to fully describe this
population of offenders.
victims per offender whereas Bumby and Bumby (1997) reported an average
of two. Fromuth and Conn (1997) found the average number of victims to be
slightly more than one. The victims were very young, sometimes even in their
infancy (Hunter et al., 1993). Few victims were older than 12 (Fehrenbach &
Monastersky, 1988; Fromuth & Conn, 1997; Hunter et al., 1993; Vandiver &
Teske, in press).
Most often, the victims knew or were related to their abuser. Only one
study (Hunter et al., 1993) reported that strangers were molested. In this study,
39 percent of the thirty-three victims were strangers to their abuser. The sex of
the victim appears to have no distinct pattern (see Table 3.6). Some studies
reported a high proportion of juvenile female sex offenders choosing both
males and females (see Hunter et al., 1993), while other studies report more
male victims as compared to female victims (see Vandiver & Teske, in press).
Still other studies report just the opposite: more female victims as compared to
male victims (see Bumby & Bumby, 1997; Fehrenbach et al., 1986).
Motivations/Explanations of Behavior
Explanations of juvenile female sexual offending have been similar to the
ones given for adult female sexual offending. For example, reenactment of
abuse has been proposed ( Johnson, T. C., 1989). One researcher noted that
some young female sex offenders appear to act out their own sexual abuse
experiences;4 another suggests it to be the result of being sexually victimized
(Araji, 1997). It has also been found in some cases that the child identifies with
the aggressor (Turner & Turner, 1994).
It has also been reported that in many of the families of the juvenile female
sex offender, sexual abuse is pervasive and the child may engage in the be-
havior on a younger sibling because it is inevitable he or she will be abused. If
the juvenile female sex offender is the abuser, the abuse may be less severe as
compared to being victimized by an older member of the family (Turner &
Turner, 1994).
likely to choose young victims, females were more likely than males to choose
those in the infancy to 5-years-of-age range (52 percent ccompared to 38
percent).
Kubik, Hecker, and Righthand (2002) found in a comparison of eleven
juvenile female to eleven juvenile male sex offenders that females experienced
more severe and pervasive abuse. They also found that the juvenile female and
male sex offenders exhibited similar sex offender behaviors, criminal histories,
and psychosocial characteristics.
to the adult population, the juveniles exhibit high rates of mental illness,
particularly depression. This group of offenders also reports a high rate of being
sexually victimized. Their victims were very young, usually less than 12 years.
The victims typically knew or were related to their abuser.
Female sex offenders, whether adult or juvenile, engage in a broad range
of sexual behavior. The effects are long lasting and, many times, severe. The
motivations appear to be complex and research has only recently emerged on
this topic. As society becomes increasingly less tolerant of sex offenses in
general, there may be a greater willingness to report, arrest, charge, and
convict females who commit such offenses. It is likely in the future, as more
law enforcement and social service agencies become knowledgeable about this
type of abuse, that the numbers will increase and subsequent research will
include increased sample sizes.
NOTES
1. The same statement was also made by J. L. Mathis (1972, p. 54).
2. ‘‘Social junk’’ is a term coined by Steven Spitzer. He applied Marxist
theory in developing the term. It refers to those who make up a segment of society
who are not in a position to acquire adequate resources for themselves, often
falling between the cracks of social service agencies.
3. The NCVS publications do not include this information; however, the
original data may contain such information.
4. T. C. Johnson’s (1989) research focused on young juvenile sex offenders;
many of the subjects were younger than 13 years. The term ‘‘young’’ sex offender
rather than ‘‘juvenile’’ sex offender is used when discussing this research.
REFERENCES
Araji, S. (1997). Sexually aggressive children: Coming to understand them. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Berliner, L., & Barbieri, M. K. (1984). The testimony of the child victim of sexual
assault. Journal of Social Issues, 40(2), 125–137.
Bumby, K. M., & Bumby, N. H. (1997). Adolescent sexual offenders. In B. K.
Schwartz & H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sex offender: New insights, treatment,
innovations, and legal developments (Vol. 2, pp. 10.11–10.16). Kingston, NJ:
Civic Research Institute.
Chasnoff, I. J., Burns, W. J., Schnoll, S. H., Burns, K., Chisum, G., & Kyle-
Spore, L. (1986). Maternal-neonatal incest. American Journal of Orthopsy-
chiatry, 56(4), 577–580.
Chow, E. W. C., & Choy, A. L. (2002). Clinical characteristics and treatment
response to SSRI in a female pedophile. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31(2),
211–215.
Davin, P. A., Hislop, J., & Dunbar, T. (1999). Female sexual abusers: Three views.
Brandon, VT: Safer Society Press.
Female Sex Offenders 77
Nathan, P., & Ward, T. (2001). Females who sexually abused children: Assess-
ment and treatment issues. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 8(1), 44–55.
Nathan, P., & Ward, T. (2002). Female sex offenders: Clinical and demographic
features. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 8(1), 5–21.
O’Connor, A. A. (1987). Female sex offenders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150,
615–620.
Peluso, E., & Putnam, N. (1996). Case study: Sexual abuse of boys by females.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(1),
51–54.
Petrovich, M., & Templer, D. I. (1984). Heterosexual molestation of children
who later became rapists. Psychological Reports, 54, 810.
Police rescue kidnapping victim, suspects arrested in prostitution ring. (2001, June
29). Associated Press.
Pothast, H. L., & Allen, C. M. (1994). Masculinity and femininity in male and
female perpetrators of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18(9),
763–767.
Righthand, S., & Welch, C. (2001). Juveniles who have sexually offended: A review of
the professional literature (report). Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Jus-
tice and Delinquency Prevention.
Rosencrans, B. (1997). The last secret: Daughters sexually abused by mothers. Orwell,
VT: Safer Society Press.
Rowan, E. L., Rowan, J. B., & Langelier, P. (1990). Women who molest children.
Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 18(1), 79–83.
Rudin, M. M., Zalewski, C., & Bodmer-Turner, J. (1995). Characteristics of
child sexual abuse victims according to perpetrator gender. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 19(8), 963–973.
Salter, A. C. (2003). Predators: Pedophiles, rapists, and other sex offenders. New York:
Basic Books.
Saradjian, J., & Hanks, H. (1996). Women who sexually abuse children: From research
to practice. New York: John Wiley.
Sarrel, P. M., & Masters, W. H. (1982). Sexual molestation of men by women.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11(2), 117–131.
Scavo, R. R. (1989). Female adolescent sex offenders: A neglected treatment
group. Journal of Contemporary Social Work, 70, 114–139.
Syed, F., & Williams, S. (1996). Case studies of female sex offenders. Ottawa: Cor-
rectional Service of Canada.
Teachey, L. (2000, November 10). Mom, boyfriend gets 40 years for prostituting
girls. The Houston Chronicle, p. A40.
Travin, S., Cullen, K., & Protter, B. (1990). Female sex offenders: Severe victims
and victimizers. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 35(1), 140–150.
Turner, M., & Turner, T. (1994). Female adolescent sexual abusers: An exploratory
study of mother-daughter dynamics with implications for treatment. Brandon, VT:
Safer Society Press.
U.S. Department of Justice. (2004). Compiled by the U.S. Dept. of Justice, Fed-
eral Bureau of Investigation. ICPSR ed. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university
80 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Pedophilia
Richard D. McAnulty 1
INTRODUCTION
Sexuality has always been viewed as a force that must be tightly controlled and
regulated. Sexual behavior has only been considered a legitimate topic of
scientific inquiry for the past fifty years, beginning with Alfred Kinsey’s
landmark surveys. Before that time, sexuality and its problems were to be
regulated by the church, the government, or medicine. Accordingly, sexual
behavior that deviated from the established norm, however it was defined at
the time, was declared sinful, criminal, or sick. Deviant sexual behavior, then,
called for penitence, punishment, or a cure. This historical heritage still in-
fluences our thinking about sexual behavior. For example, do people who
molest children suffer from an evil nature, a criminal mindset, or a mental
illness?
I cannot answer such a question since it, like most questions relating to
sexual behavior, hinges on personal values and beliefs. There are currently no
universal and objective criteria for judging sexual attitudes and practices.
Outside of sexual homicide, no sexual behavior is universally viewed as
harmful or abnormal. Even sexual practices that would be condemned as child
molestation in the United States took place openly and regularly in some
cultures in the past (see Green, 2002). Although most definitions of sexual
disorders emphasize that the associated patterns of sexual arousal deviate from
normative patterns (hence the term sexual deviations), there are no clear criteria
82 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
DEFINITIONS
The official definition of sexual deviation, including pedophilia, has
changed considerably over time. According to the original definition by the
American Psychiatric Association (1952), sexual deviations (or perversions as
they were once called) were related to the ‘‘sociopathic personality distur-
bance.’’ The sociopathic personality applied to individuals whose behavior
failed to comply with social or cultural guidelines; in a sense, they were
extreme nonconformists with respect to sexual practices. Pedophilia was for-
mally introduced as a sexual deviation in 1968. The term ‘‘sexual deviation’’
Pedophilia 83
was renamed paraphilia in 1980. Paraphilia was adopted because it was pre-
sumably more descriptive: para-, referring to an abnormality to which the
person is attracted, -philia. According to the official description (American
Psychiatric Association, 2000), the paraphilias involve recurrent and intense
sexually arousing fantasies, sexual urges, or behaviors that typically involve (1)
nonhuman objects (such as a shoe fetish), (2) the suffering or humiliation of
oneself or of one’s partner (as in sexual sadism), or (3) children or other
nonconsenting persons (which would include exhibitionism and pedophilia).
For some individuals with paraphilias, their unusual urges or practices are
necessary, even required, for sexual arousal. The person may not be able to per-
form sexually without the preferred item, situation, or partner. In other cases,
they are not essential but desirable. In any case, the definition of a paraphilia
requires that the urges or sexual practices are either distressing to the individual
or cause impairments in one or more areas of life. The unusual sexual practices
may cause problems in intimate relationships and lead to criminal arrest when
they involve nonconsenting partners. Paraphilias entail sexual arousal that of-
ten interferes with ‘‘the capacity for reciprocal, affectionate sexual activity’’
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 524). In cases where the sexual
urges and fantasies are essential for the person’s sexual arousal, they may become
a focus of the person’s life. A man with a foot fetish, for example, may take a job
as a salesperson in footwear. A man whose sexual urges involve immature
children may coach youth sports teams to have access to potential victims.
Pedophilia (literally, love of young children) is officially described as involving
recurrent, intense sexually arousing urges, fantasies, or behaviors involving
sexual activity with a prepubescent child (usually 13 years old or younger).
The person must be at least 16 years old and at least five years older than the
victim. A relationship between an older adolescent and a 12- to 13-year-old
would not qualify as pedophilia. Although the terms ‘‘pedophile’’ and ‘‘child
molester’’ are often used interchangeably, there are important differences
between them (Barbaree & Seto, 1997; McAnulty, Adams, & Dillon, 2001).
Pedophilia is usually reserved for those individuals who show some degree of
sexual preference for children: their urges and fantasies often focus on children,
sometimes exclusively. Child molestation, however, is a broad term that can
be applied to any person who engages in inappropriate sexual behavior with a
child. Child molestation may be motivated by the unavailability of an adult
partner (Freund, McKnight, Langevin, & Cibiri, 1972). It could be due to
cognitive deficits such as mental retardation or dementia, or conditions related
to lowered inhibitions, such as alcoholism or a psychopathic personality in-
volving traits such as impulsiveness and thrill-seeking (Dorr, 1998). In these
cases, the person’s sexual actions with a child probably do not result from
persistent and intense urges. In other words, pedophilia is but one of several
possible motives for molesting a child. This distinction in terms is not simply a
matter of semantics, because it has important implications for understanding
offenders and making decisions about their treatment.
84 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
CHARACTERISTICS OF PEDOPHILES
One consistent conclusion is that pedophiles represent a very diverse
group of individuals. There is a high degree of variability in their personal
characteristics, life experiences (including their family backgrounds), criminal
histories, and reasons for molesting children. As Prentky, Knight, and Lee
(1997) concluded, ‘‘there is no single ‘profile’ that accurately describes or
accounts for all child molesters’’ (p. v). With this caution in mind, several
consistent findings have emerged in studies of incarcerated pedophiles.
words, men who have sexually abused multiple young boys who are not
relatives are more likely to be true pedophiles. As discussed in the next section,
men who show a marked sexual preference for children with little to no
arousal to adult partners are often labeled ‘‘preferential pedophiles.’’ A con-
sistent finding is that preferential pedophiles generally have deficits in their
social and sexual relationships with adults. The majority of child molesters do
not appear to be preferential pedophiles because they do not display an intense
and exclusive focus on children.
they are self-centered and insincere, and they seem to experience little remorse
or guilt.
Recent studies suggest that a majority of incarcerated child molesters have
committed nonsexual crimes. Criminal diversity, which refers to the range of
criminal offenses in a person’s past, is quite common among sex offenders in
prison. Smallbone and Wortley (2004), who examined the criminal records of
362 convicted child molesters, reported that nearly two-thirds (64.4 percent)
had prior criminal arrests, the majority of which were for nonsexual offenses
such as theft, traffic violations, and drug offenses. In other words, this group of
child molesters was criminally diverse. Nonsexual offenses accounted for 86
percent of all previous criminal offenses. These findings have led some re-
searchers to question whether sex offenders are truly unique and different from
incarcerated nonsex offenders (see Simon, 2000).
Studies suggest that 8–30 percent of child molesters have an antisocial
or psychopathic personality disorder (Quinsey, Harris, & Rice, 1995; Serin,
Malcolm, Khanna, & Barbaree, 1994; for a review see Dorr, 1998). Alcohol
abuse is one of the most commonly reported problems among child molesters
(Marshall, 1997). Antisocial traits, though, are not found in all cases. Para-
doxically, some pedophiles are moralistic, conservative individuals in other
aspects of their lives (Marshall & McKnight, 1975).
Troubled Childhoods
One final consistent finding is that pedophiles as a group are more likely to
have had troubled childhoods. Their developmental histories often include
being the victim of physical, emotional, or sexual abuse. Half or more of child
molesters report having been the victims of sexual abuse during childhood
(Bard et al., 1987; Marshall, 1997). Childhood emotional abuse is a devel-
opmental risk factor for sexual deviation, including pedophilia (Lee, Jackson,
Pattison, & Ward, 2002). Childhood sexual abuse is also related to the risk of
becoming a perpetrator (Lee et al., 2002). Research shows that families that
provide ineffective socialization, that are characterized by problematic parent-
child relationships, and that involve high levels of parental conflict and vio-
lence may place children at a higher risk of sexual offending later in life.
Parental absence or inconsistency, as when a parent is emotionally unavailable
to the child, increases the likelihood of later emotional and interpersonal
problems, including anxiety, distrust, insecurity, excessive anger, and poor
social skills (Prentky et al., 1997; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998). Parental in-
sensitivity to a child’s needs, in particular, is believed to compromise the
child’s ability to feel secure in adult relationships. Detrimental childhood ex-
periences, such as having a severely dysfunctional family, can lead to social
skills deficits and feelings of inadequacy, which can ultimately interfere with
healthy adult relationships and intimacy.
Pedophilia 87
Victim Preferences
Pedophiles often have very specific preferences for victims. They may
differ in terms of preferred victim gender (male, female, or both), relationship
to the victim (incestuous versus nonincestuous), and whether their sexual pref-
erence is exclusive (i.e., the pedophile is attracted only to children) or non-
exclusive. The distinction between homosexual, bisexual, and heterosexual
pedophilia is well established (Langevin, 1983; Lanyon, 1986). Heterosexual
pedophiles (men who prefer immature girls) are apparently more common than
homosexual pedophiles (men who prefer boys), whereas the bisexual subtype is
uncommon. Homosexual pedophiles, however, tend to have a larger number
of victims than the heterosexual pedophiles. For example, Abel et al. (1987)
found that their sample of heterosexual child molesters (nonincestuous) re-
ported an average of twenty victims, compared to 150 for the homosexual
pedophiles (nonincestuous). Incestuous offenders in the same study admitted to
an average of 1.8 female victims and 1.7 male victims. As with nonincestuous
child molestation, most incestuous pedophiles chose female victims. In contrast
to homosexual pedophilia, heterosexual child molesters are more likely to be
married (Langevin, Hucker, Handy, et al., 1985).
Contrary to popular belief, pedophiles are not ‘‘dirty old men,’’ as most
incarcerated pedophiles are in their midtwenties to midthirties (Groth &
Birnbaum, 1978; Langevin, Hucker, Handy, et al., 1985). By definition, the
victims in both groups are prepubescent; average victim age is approximately
10 years (Groth & Birnbaum, 1978). Approximately 25 percent of victims are
less than 6 years of age, another 25 percent are between 6 and 10 years of age,
and roughly 50 percent are between 11 and 13 years of age (Erickson, Walbek,
& Seely, 1988). Type of sexual activity with victims ranges from fondling to
oral sex and actual penetration (Erickson et al., 1988). Among heterosexual
pedophiles, fondling of the victim is by far the most common (54 percent),
although vaginal contact (41.5 percent) and cunnilingus (19 percent) are not
rare. For homosexual offenders, fondling of the victim is also most common
(43 percent), followed by the performance of fellatio on the victim (41 per-
cent). Anal contact in the latter group occurs in one-third of cases. In cases
involving younger children, actual anal or vaginal penetration is uncommon;
contact usually entails rubbing the penis against the orifice or between the
thighs (Erickson et al., 1988; Langevin, Hucker, Handy, et al., 1985). In cases
of intrafamilial incest, there tends to be a progression from masturbation and
fondling to actual attempts at intercourse over time. Methods of obtaining
victim compliance include enticement via bribery, seduction, appeal to curi-
osity, and intimidation and threats in some cases. The majority of pedophiles
are at least acquainted with their victims. Incestuous pedophiles commonly
molest biological, adoptive, or stepchildren, whereas the victims of non-
incestuous offenders may include neighbors, relatives, and acquaintances.
88 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Pedophiles typically have beliefs about sexual contact with children that
facilitate acting out their deviant sexual urges (Hanson, Gizzarelli, & Scott,
1994; Ward, Hudson, Johnston, & Marshall, 1997). In general, the beliefs of
pedophiles involve some degree of denial and minimization: they deny or
minimize the actual harm suffered by their victims and they also minimize
their own responsibility for the offenses. Specifically, they often claim that
adult sexual contacts are beneficial to children (‘‘it teaches them about sex’’).
Offenders not only deny or minimize their own responsibility for the offense,
but also tend to view the victim as an instigator or willing participant (Stermac
& Segal, 1989). Pedophiles often claim that a child’s sexually provocative
appearance or behavior actually invited the offense. These rationalizations are
often used by offenders to justify their actions while reducing any sense of
shame or remorse.
TYPES OF PEDOPHILES
One classification of pedophiles involves the distinction between prefer-
ential and situational pedophilia (Lanyon, 1986). This is similar to the ex-
clusive-versus-nonexclusive (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and
fixated-versus-regressed classifications proposed by Cohen, Seghorn, and
Calmas (1969) and Groth and Birnbaum (1978). Preferential or fixated pedo-
philes are primarily, and often exclusively, interested in children as sexual
partners and tend to be unmarried. Homosexual pedophiles are usually pref-
erential molesters. Their sexual experiences with adults tend to be very lim-
ited; they commonly have experienced lifelong difficulties in relating to adults,
and their sexual development is described as fixated or blocked. For these
offenders, encounters with children are usually premeditated rather than im-
pulsive. These individuals tend to be more comfortable emotionally, socially,
and sexually with children. The situational or regressed pedophiles tend to be
primarily heterosexual child molesters. Incestuous offenders would generally
be classified as situational offenders. These individuals are more likely to be
married and to have more extensive sexual experience with adult partners than
do preferential pedophiles. A common pattern is to have an apparently normal
development with adequate social and heterosexual skills. As the person enters
adulthood, however, his social, occupational, and marital adjustment become
tenuous and marginal. The pedophilic acts are typically precipitated by direct
confrontation with a female or a threat to the person’s masculinity. These
individuals’ sexual encounters with children are more impulsive, usually with
older but prepubescent females, and tend to occur intermittently rather than
continuously (Lanyon, 1986). A major question is whether these individuals
have always had some sexual arousal to children as well as adults. It is im-
portant to note that there is overlap between these categories. For example,
Prentky et al. (1997) found that social skills problems could be found in cases
of both preferential and situational pedophiles.
Pedophilia 89
THEORIES OF PEDOPHILIA
Psychoanalytic theories emphasize that pedophiles choose children as
partners because they elicit less castration anxiety than do adults (Fenichel,
1945). Others have hypothesized an aversion to adult females and an associ-
ation with homosexuality. The research evidence, however, does not support
these theoretical views (Langevin, 1983; Langevin, Hucker, Ben-Aron, et al.,
1985). The behavioral or social learning theories stress the importance of early
conditioning, direct reinforcement, or modeling experiences, such as the
presence of sexual abuse in the offender’s past or an early sexual experience
with a younger child. The single most popular theory of pedophilia is the
‘‘abused-abuser hypothesis,’’ which proposes that individuals who were sex-
ually abused in childhood are predisposed to developing pedophilia. As Gar-
land and Dougher (1990) commented, despite the popularity of this view there
is surprisingly little empirical support. There are at least three problems with
this hypothesis: (1) although most victims of child sexual abuse are females, the
vast majority of pedophiles are males; (2) only half of all child molesters have a
personal history of sexual victimization by an adult in childhood (Weeks &
Widom, 1998); and (3) some pedophiles allege having been sexually abused in
childhood as a ploy to reduce their perceived responsibility for their sexual
offenses (Freund, Watson, & Dickey, 1990). Thus, most individuals who were
sexually molested as children do not develop pedophilia (Hanson & Slater,
1988; Salter et al., 2003).
Araji and Finkelhor (1985) have advanced a four-factor model of pedo-
philia. According to them, pedophilia may be understood in terms of (a)
emotional congruence, or the emotional need to relate to children (e.g., faulty
emotional development, feelings of inadequacy); (b) blockage, or the inability to
attain alternative sources of gratification (e.g., social skills deficits, fear of adult
partners); (c) disinhibition, referring to any influence that lowers the person’s
90 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
factors that could shape deviant urges and fantasies. It is also supported by a
wealth of research that demonstrates that inadequate parent-child attachment
bonds are linked to a host of behavioral and emotional problems later in life
(see Goldberg, 1997). Sex offenders consistently report insecure attachment as
predicted by the theory (Lyn & Burton, 2005).
CONCLUSIONS
Pedophilia is a serious problem in society. Many men and women report
being victims of sexual abuse during childhood. According to one national
survey (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994), nearly 12 percent of
men and 17 percent of women reported that they had been sexually touched as
children by an older adolescent or an adult. In most cases, it occurred between
the ages of 7 and 10, and it progressed to oral sex and intercourse in 10–30
percent of the cases. For many victims, the abuse causes short-term and long-
term problems in life (see Chapters 5 and 6 in this volume).
Pedophiles are usually men who have some degree of sexual preoccupa-
tion with immature children. For some, it is an exclusive sexual preference,
but others have also had adult partners. Men with an exclusive sexual pref-
erence for children tend to have a larger number of victims and it is often a
focus of their emotional, social, and sexual lives. As a group, pedophiles often
lack social skills and feel inadequate in their relationships. This is not, however,
universal. In some cases, child molestation is only part of a lengthy pattern of
problematic behaviors at home and at school; these offenders tend to be
antisocial, impulsive, and emotionally immature. Some pedophiles have a
marked preference for a type of victim and specific sexual activities; others
seem more indiscriminate. Many if not most pedophiles report troubled
childhoods involving emotional, physical, or sexual abuse. Inadequate parent-
child attachment and related problems are commonly reported by pedophiles
and other sex offenders. These experiences are likely important factors in the
developmental pathway to pedophilia.
REFERENCES
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., Mittelman, M., Cunningham-Rathner, J., Rouleau,
J. L., & Murphy, W. D. (1987). Self-reported sex crimes of nonincar-
cerated paraphiliacs. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 3–25.
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). The diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed., Text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
92 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Hanson, R. K., Gizzarelli, R., & Scott, H. (1994). The attitudes of incest of-
fenders: Sexual entitlement and the acceptance of sex with children.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 21, 187–202.
Hanson, R. K., & Slater, S. (1988). Sexual victimization in the history of sexual
abusers: A review. Annals of Sex Research, 1, 485–499.
Langevin, R. (1983). Sexual strands: Understanding and treating sexual anomalies in
men. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Langevin, R., Hucker, S. J., Ben-Aron, M. H., Purins, J. E., & Hook, H. J.
(1985). Why are pedophiles attracted to children? Further studies of erotic
preference in heterosexual pedophilia. In R. Langevin (Ed.), Erotic prefer-
ence, gender identity, and aggression in men: New research studies (pp. 181–210).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Langevin, R., Hucker, S. J., Handy, L., Purins, J., Russon, A. E., & Hook, H. J.
(1985). Erotic preference and aggression in pedophilia: A comparison of
heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual types. In R. Langevin (Ed.), Erotic
preference, gender identity, and aggression in men: New research studies (pp. 137–
160). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lanyon, R. I. (1986). Theory and treatment in child molestation. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 176–182.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social
organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Laws, D. R., & O’Donohue, W. (1997). Introduction: Fundamental issues in
sexual deviance. In D. R. Laws & W. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance:
Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 1–21). New York: Guilford.
Lee, J. K. P., Jackson, H. J., Pattison, P., & Ward, T. (2002). Developmental risk
factors for sexual offending. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 73–92.
Levin, S. M., & Stava, L. (1987). Personality characteristics of sex offenders: A
review. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 57–79.
Lyn, T. S., & Burton, D. L. (2005). Attachment, anger, and anxiety of male sexual
offenders. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 11, 127–137.
Marshall, W. L. (1989). Intimacy, loneliness, and sexual offenders. Behaviour Re-
search and Therapy, 27, 491–503.
Marshall, W. L. (1997). Pedophilia: Psychopathology and theory. In D. R. Laws &
W. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment, and treatment
(pp. 152–174). New York: Guilford.
Marshall, W. L., Barbaree, H. E., & Christophe, D. (1986). Sexual offenders
against female children: Sexual preferences for age of victims and type of
behaviour. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 18, 424–439.
Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Hodkinson, S. (1993). The importance of
attachment bonds in the development of juvenile sex offending. In H. E.
Barbaree, W. L. Marshall, & S. M. Hudson (Eds.), The juvenile sex offender
(pp. 164–181). New York: Guilford.
Marshall, W. L., & McKnight, R. D. (1975). An integrated treatment program for
sexual offenders. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 20, 133–138.
94 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
McAnulty, R. D., Adams, H. E., & Dillon, J. (2001). Sexual deviation: The
paraphilias. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook
of psychopathology (3rd ed.). New York: Plenum.
Okami, P., & Goldberg, A. (1992). Personality correlates of pedophilia: Are they
reliable indicators? Journal of Sex Research, 29, 297–328.
Overholser, J. C., & Beck, S. (1986). Multimethod assessment of rapists, child
molesters, and three control groups on behavioral and psychological
measures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 682–687.
Prentky, R. A., Knight, R. A., & Lee, A. F. S. (1997). Child sexual molestation:
Research issues. National Institute of Justice Research Report. Washington,
DC: U. S. Department of Justice.
Quinsey, V. L., Chaplin, T. C., & Carrigan, W. F. (1979). Sexual preferences
among incestuous and nonincestuous child molesters. Behavior Therapy, 10,
562–565.
Quinsey, V. L., Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (1995). Actuarial prediction of sexual
recidivism. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 85–105.
Salter, D., McMillan, D., Richards, M., Talbot, T., Hodges, J., Bentovim, A.,
et al. (2003). Development of sexually abusive behaviour in sexually vic-
timized males: A longitudinal study. Lancet, 361, 471–476.
Segal, Z. V., & Marshall, W. L. (1985). Heterosexual social skills in a population
of rapists and child molesters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
53, 55–63.
Seidman, B. T., Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. J. (1994). An
examination of intimacy and loneliness in sex offenders. Journal of Inter-
personal Violence, 9, 518–534.
Serin, R. C., Malcolm, P. B., Khanna, A., & Barbaree, H. E. (1994). Psychopathy
and deviant sexual arousal in incarcerated sexual offenders. Journal of In-
terpersonal Violence, 9, 3–11.
Seto, M. C. (2004). Pedophilia and sexual offenses against children. Annual Review
of Sex Research, 15, 321–361.
Simon, L. (2000). An examination of the assumptions of specialization, mental
disorder, and dangerousness in sex offenders. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 18, 275–308.
Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (1998). Childhood attachment and adult
attachment in incarcerated adult male sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 13, 555–573.
Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (2001). Further evidence for a relationship
between attachment insecurity and coercive sexual behavior in non-
offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 22–35.
Smallbone, S. W., & Wortley, R. K. (2004). Criminal diversity and paraphilic
interests among adult males convicted of sexual offenses against children.
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 48,
175–188.
Stermac, L. E., & Segal, Z. V. (1989). Adult sexual contact with children: An
examination of cognitive factors. Behavior Therapy, 20, 573–584.
Pedophilia 95
Sexual Assault
Karen S. Calhoun, Jenna McCauley,
and Megan E. Crawford 1
Sexual assault is an enormous problem in the United States as well as inter-
nationally. Not only does it impact the lives of millions of individuals, but it
also has huge costs to society in the form of economic loss, health burdens, and
social problems. The emotional aftermath, short- and long-term disruption in
functioning, psychological and physical health problems, increase in suicide
risk, and increased vulnerability to additional forms of sexual and physical
violence are but a few of the consequences. Survivors are affected most di-
rectly, but others in their lives (family, partners, friends, etc.) suffer serious
consequences as well.
In this chapter, we will review definitions, impact, risk factors, prevention
and intervention, and support services available to victims. The focus will be
on adult victims of sexual assault, since child sexual abuse has somewhat dif-
ferent rates, definitions, and related issues.
issues will be briefly discussed below. While these discrepancies affect any
review of prevalence data, the important point is this: rape is a far too common
occurrence and has a broad range of potential consequences and risk factors.
In defining various degrees of sexual violence, two main approaches are
followed. The first set of definitions is found within the legal realm and is used
for the periodic reporting of incidence rates. Forcible rape is a crime punishable
by law, and thus it is legally defined by the FBI Uniform Crime Report as ‘‘the
carnal knowledge of a female forcibly and against her will’’ (Rantala, 2000).
This definition includes both attempted and completed rapes, but only in-
cludes female victims. The National Incidence Based Reporting System
broadens the definition to include both female and male victims, defining rape
as ‘‘the carnal knowledge of a person forcibly, and/or against that person’s will;
or not forcibly or against that person’s will where that person is incapable of
giving consent because of his/her temporary or permanent mental or physical
incapacity.’’ Additional legal terms which may apply to sexual assault experi-
ences include aggravated assault, an unlawful attack by one person upon another
for the purpose of inflicting severe or aggravated bodily injury; this type of
assault is usually accompanied by the use of a weapon or another force likely to
produce death or great bodily harm, and simple assault, aggravated assault
without the display of a weapon. It is worth noting that legal definitions vary
by state.
While important for purposes of filing legal charges and calculating annual
incidence rates, the definitions are not as commonly used in research on sexual
assault as are those germane to the public health sector. Within this perspec-
tive, sexual violence is defined as ‘‘any sexual act, attempt to obtain a sexual act,
unwanted sexual comments or advances, or acts to traffic, or otherwise di-
rected, against a person’s sexuality using coercion, by any person regardless of
their relationship to the victim, in any setting, including but not limited to
home and work’’ ( Jewkes, Sen, & Garcia-Moreno, 2002). As you can see, this
definition is quite broad, leaving room for a variety of victim experiences.
More specifically, rape may be defined as ‘‘physically forced or otherwise
coerced penetration of the vulva or anus using a penis, other body parts, or an
object.’’ Attempted rape is a noncompleted rape. Within the context of these
definitions, coercion applies to physical force, psychological intimidation,
blackmail or other threats, or taking advantage of an individual who is inca-
pable of giving consent. (For a review of the discrepancies of legal and public
health sector definitions, see Kilpatrick, 2004.)
In an effort to resolve the disparate and often overlapping terminology, the
Centers for Disease Control (Basile & Saltzman, 2002) published a compen-
dium of uniform definitions and recommended data elements. Sexual violence
was divided into five categories: a completed sex act without victim consent or
involving a victim unable to consent or refuse, an attempted sex act without
victim consent or involving a victim unable to consent or refuse, abusive
sexual contact, noncontact sexual abuse (such as exhibitionism), and sexual
Sexual Assault 99
PREVALENCE ESTIMATES
Even considering underreporting and the lack of uniform definitions, the
prevalence rates for rape are notably high. Prevalence of rape within a com-
munity sample has been estimated at 14 percent (Kilpatrick, Edmunds, &
Seymour, 1992). According to the National Violence Against Women Survey
(NVAWS), one in six women have experienced an attempted or completed
100 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
37%
36% Rape
Attempted Rape
Sexual Assault
27%
12%
8%
30%
Intimate
Relative
Acquaintance
Stranger
50%
rape (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000). Among college women, estimates are even
higher than those within the general population (Sorenson, Stein, Siegel,
Golding, & Burnham, 1987). In a college sample of women, Gidycz et al.
(1997) found that approximately 17 percent had experienced a rape, while an
additional 33 percent had experienced some other form of sexual assault.
Greene and Navarro (1998) found 27 percent of first-year college women were
sexually assaulted over the course of a twelve-week semester. The combined
effects of these estimates indicate that sexual assault is a frequent occurrence,
impacting thousands of women each year. Although adolescent and young
Sexual Assault 101
adult women are the most common targets of sexual assault, women of all ages,
ethnicities, and backgrounds are victims.
Figure 5.1 gives an estimate of the relative incidence of rape, attempted
rape, and sexual assault, according to the most recent National Crime Victim
Survey results. It also indicates the incidence broken down by perpetrator
type. Half of all perpetrators are acquaintances, including dates, neighbors,
friends of friends, classmates, etc. Just under one third are strangers, 12 percent
are spouses or intimate partners, and the rest are relatives.
CONSEQUENCES
Rape is a trauma that is often a devastating experience for its survivors. It
results in both acute and chronic symptoms that may range from temporary
emotional and physical reactions like fear and bruising, to more long-term
psychological and health conditions such as major depression or fibromyalgia.
Early research on the recovery of rape victims reported that almost 40 percent
of women stated that recovery took ‘‘several years,’’ while over 25 percent
reported that they did not feel they had yet fully recovered at four to six years
postrape. It is important to keep in mind that each person is different and may
present with a wide array of immediate symptoms as a result of sexual assault,
which may or may not progress to the development of chronic conditions.
The subsequent material summarizes some of the potential immediate and
long-term consequences with which rape survivors may be faced. It is by no
means exhaustive, but represents a guide to the more common and prevalent
reactions of women following the experience of rape.
Acute reactions that appear within the first few weeks following the assault
included a range of somatic and emotional conditions. For women experi-
encing physically forced sex, physical trauma may be common, including
bruising and irritation. Other somatic reactions include tension headaches,
fatigue, sleep disturbances, appetite reduction, nausea, hypersensitivity, and
gynecological symptoms such as vaginal discharge, bleeding, itching, burning
sensation, and generalized pain.
In terms of emotional reactions, fear is a primary feeling described by rape
survivors. Women may report being afraid to be alone or fear of places that
resemble the site of their attack. Women may also experience heightened
anxiety throughout the course of their normal day and during their partici-
pation in routine activities. Other reactions common to victims of rape include
humiliation, embarrassment, anger, revenge, and self-blame.
Psychological Sequelae
Rape often leaves its mark via psychological conditions such as fear,
anxiety, low self-esteem, social adjustment issues, depression, dysthymia, and
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). One study, which surveyed over 3,000
households and compared women with a history of sexual assault to those
without such history, found that sexual assault predicted the later onset of
major depressive episodes, substance use disorders, and anxiety disorders such
as phobia, panic disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and
PTSD are the two most commonly experienced long-term psychological
consequences of rape.
Depression is characterized by depressed mood most of the day every day,
diminished interest and pleasure in once pleasurable activities, significant
weight loss or gain, sleep disturbances, diminished ability to concentrate, and
recurrent thoughts of death. In addition to acute symptoms of depression
following a rape, women may also experience long-term depression (for re-
views, see Koss, Bailey, & Yuan, 2003; Crowell & Burgess, 1996). Approxi-
mately 50 percent of rape survivors report depressive symptoms one month
following the attack. Of these, over 40 percent meet the diagnostic criteria for
depression. For comparison, the normal rate of depression for women in the
United States is approximately 20 percent (Atkeson, Calhoun, Resick, & Ellis,
1982). Results from epidemiological research have indicated that those with a
history of childhood or adult sexual assault are 2.4 times more likely to be
diagnosed with major depression than controls. Other research has shown that
104 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
prevalence rates of substance abuse or dependence. One study found that the
odds for developing alcohol or drug use disorders more than doubled for
women with a history of sexual assault (Burnam, Stein, & Golding, 1988).
Clinicians and researchers report that women seeking treatment for substance
use disorders (SUDs) have much higher rates of physical and sexual assault
when compared with women in the general population. This relationship
appears to be evident among women with a history of childhood sexual
abuse.
Although the link between sexual assault and SUDs has been well docu-
mented, it is difficult to disentangle the direction and time sequencing of this
relationship. Citing research designs reliant upon retrospective report of data,
insufficient methodology, psychological symptomology, substance use, and
often complex interactions of abuse, researchers have yet to fully deter-
mine which comes first—sexual assault or increased use of substances. However,
attempts are being made to more clearly delineate the order of onset of these
conditions. For example, White and Humphrey (1997) collected longitudinal
data over the course of three years of over 700 college women. They found that
women who experienced a sexual assault in one given year of the study had
nearly doubled their odds of reporting heavy drinking in the following year.
Currently, support is strongest for the existence of a bidirectional, at times even
cyclical, relationship between sexual assault and substance use.
Supporting Theory
While the temporal directionality of the relationship between sexual as-
sault and alcohol abuse appears to be bidirectional, researchers have formed
and tested a hypothetical explanation for the occurrence of alcohol abuse
subsequent to sexual assault. This theory has been coined the ‘‘self-medication
hypothesis.’’ Although this theory has been applied to a broad spectrum of
substances, a majority of the research involving women who have experienced
sexual assault focuses upon alcohol specifically. According to this theory,
women who have experienced a traumatic event (such as sexual assault) are left
to deal with the aftermath, which may include many of the symptoms pre-
viously discussed, like depressed mood, general anxiety, and PTSD. Alcohol
use serves as a coping mechanism in that it may temporarily numb symptoms
of anxiety, depression, or PTSD. Some researchers have termed the use of
alcohol for the reduction of unpleasant emotions as ‘‘chemical avoidance.’’
Because chemical avoidance is often effective on a short-term basis (e.g., it
reduces anxiety), the drinking behavior is reinforced by the reduction in
distress. This increases the likelihood of that same drinking behavior upon
subsequent experience of unpleasant emotions. Over time, when used re-
peatedly and often exclusively, drinking becomes a maladaptive mechanism for
coping. Laboratory research has supported this model. Results of studies, such
as one by Levenson, Oyama, and Meek (1987) demonstrating that alcohol was
106 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
who are then revictimized as adults) show the greatest odds of exhibiting
lifetime suicide attempts. The relationship between victimization and suicidal
behavior is often mediated by the occurrence of other stressful life events,
depression, PTSD, and alcohol dependence symptoms. Other suggested me-
diators of the sexual assault–suicide relationship that have not yet received
as much empirical support include attributions of blame, hopelessness, and
searching for meaning in one’s victimization.
REVICTIMIZATION
In addition to the many physical and psychological consequences com-
mon in the aftermath of a sexual assault, women with a history of victimization
also appear to be at increased risk for future sexual assaults. Although many
variables have been recognized as potential risk factors for victimization, re-
search consistently identifies a previous history of sexual abuse as one of the
strongest predictors of future sexual victimization (Himelein, 1995; Koss &
Dinero, 1989). Although prevalence rates vary, they suggest that up to 72
percent of women with a child or adolescent sexual abuse history will expe-
rience additional sexual assaults, with similar rates being documented across a
variety of samples including college students, clinical populations, and com-
munity samples (Messman & Long, 1996). While most of this research has
relied on retrospective reporting of victimization experiences, studies that
follow women over time have identified a trend for revictimization as well.
Gidycz, Hanson, and Layman (1995) followed female college students over a
nine-month period, and found that sexual assault survivors were approximately
twice as likely to experience victimization during their first three months of
participation, and at subsequent six- and nine-month follow-up periods, this
risk increased significantly. Specifically, women who were victimized during
the initial three months of participation were three times more likely than
nonvictims to experience victimization during the subsequent three-month
follow-up period, and at nine months, participants were twenty times more
likely than nonvictims to experience additional sexual victimization if they had
been assaulted during the earlier time period.
These high prevalence rates are particularly alarming when one considers
the impact of multiple assault experiences on a woman’s functioning and well-
being. All of the negative consequences experienced by rape survivors become
exponentially worse for revictimized women, including higher rates of de-
pression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress symptoms, hostility, somatic complaints,
and suicide attempts (Ellis, Atkeson, & Calhoun, 1982; Messman-Moore, Long,
& Siegfried, 2000). In addition, revictimized women experience more inter-
personal dysfunction than single-assault victims, with fewer and less fulfilling
social relationships, and problems with assertiveness, sociability, submissive-
ness, intimacy, responsibility, and control (Classen, Field, Koopman, Nevill-
Manning, & Spiegel, 2001; Cloitre, Scarvalone, & Difede, 1997; Ellis et al.,
108 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
turn significantly increased their vulnerability for adult sexual assault expe-
riences (Orcutt, Cooper, & Garcia, in press). Therefore, the distress caused
by exposure to early traumatic experiences may lead to maladaptive coping
strategies that increase a woman’s risk for sexual assault, such as contact with
multiple sexual partners and impaired sexual decision making (Orcutt et al., in
press).
Social-psychological characteristics have been commonly suggested as
potential sources of risk for sexual victimization. Although there is some ev-
idence supporting the role of assertiveness and social poise in protecting a
woman from experiencing victimization, research has failed thus far in iden-
tifying any consistent personality profile that distinguishes rape survivors from
nonvictims. Some characteristics that are frequently suggested as possible risk
variables are a woman’s attitudes and beliefs about violence, traditional views
of femininity, and acceptance of rape myths. The social control theory posits
that some women are socialized to be more accepting of violence, to submit to
traditional passive female roles, and to believe in common rape myths, all of
which increase their likelihood of being targeted by potential perpetrators
(Koss & Dinero, 1989). Research thus far has failed to support this theory,
however, with the majority of findings suggesting that there are no differences
between nonvictims and sexual assault survivors on measures of sex-role ste-
reotyping, acceptance of interpersonal violence, and adversarial sexual beliefs
(Amick & Calhoun, 1987; Koss, 1985).
The third model, suggested by Koss and Dinero (1989), consists of various
situational characteristics surrounding the assault itself and the possible role that
these variables play in increasing a woman’s risk for sexual assault. Studies aimed
at understanding these risk factors have emphasized the possible influence of
alcohol or substance use by both the survivor and perpetrator, location of the
assault, dating behaviors, and frequency of sexual activity. Being in an isolated
location with a potential acquaintance rapist has been considered a risk factor.
As many as 75 percent of sexual assaults occur within private residences, with
these experiences being approximately twice as likely to occur in the man’s
apartment (Miller & Marshall, 1987; Muehlenhard & Linton, 1987). The se-
clusion of these environments may reduce a woman’s opportunity to escape the
situation and successfully resist any unwanted sexual advances. The location
may also influence the perceived justifiability of sexual aggression, as research
suggests that both men and women believe that rape is more justified if a couple
goes to the man’s apartment (Muehlenhard, 1988). Other context-specific
variables that influence people’s perceptions of rape justifiability include the
woman initiating the date and allowing the man to pay for all the dating
expenses (Muehlenhard, 1988). These research findings imply that risk for
victimization may increase in these dating contexts. However, it is important to
note that while for some participants the perceived justifiability of rape in-
creased in these dating situations, the vast majority of students (77.5 percent)
surveyed indicated that rape was never justifiable under any circumstance.
Sexual Assault 111
Therefore, while women may benefit from increased awareness of how these
dating behaviors may be misinterpreted by potential assailants, they do not
represent negligent actions or behaviors that justify the sexual assault.
In an attempt to integrate the risk factors suggested by these three theo-
retical models, Koss and Dinero (1989) examined the combined influence of
these variables on risk for sexual victimization in order to identify the best set
of predictors or risk factors. Looking at fourteen variables hypothesized to
increase a woman’s vulnerability, findings suggested that taken together, only
four of these variables were responsible for predicting a woman’s odds of being
raped at a rate greater than chance: a previous history of sexual abuse, sexual
attitudes, alcohol use, and sexual activity. Thus, the strongest predictors seem
to be related to sexual history and alcohol use.
sexual assault and alcohol consumption does not mean that if women abstained
from drinking, their risk for assault would disappear. There are potentially
confounding factors that may better account for the occurrence of assault than
consumption of alcohol. Future research hopes to more closely elucidate ad-
ditional mechanisms for this relationship, both direct and indirect, as well as
search for intervening factors that may moderate this relationship, reducing the
risk of sexual assault for those women who drink.
Identification of risk factors has been an important step in research on
sexual assault as it increases our understanding of how to take preventative
action and educate women on how they can reduce their risk. However, it is
important to emphasize that even the most conscientious efforts made by a
woman to avoid engaging in any of these risky behaviors, such as alcohol use
or increased sexual activity, cannot by itself guarantee that she will not ex-
perience a sexual assault in her lifetime. The ultimate responsibility for any
victimization experience lies with the perpetrator, and although knowledge of
risk factors can reduce prevalence rates, every sexual assault occurs under
different circumstances and is precipitated by different events or behaviors,
making all risk factors equally worthy of attention and consideration.
Primary Prevention
Although research on sexual victimization and our understanding of
associated risk factors have steadily increased in recent years, rates of victim-
ization continue to be distressingly high. Given the well-established and ex-
tensive range of negative consequences suffered by women in the aftermath of
assault, the importance of directing attention toward prevention and risk-
reduction efforts cannot be overemphasized. In order to eliminate or at least
reduce the occurrence of sexual victimization, the most logical place to make
an impact is with prevention programs aimed at potential perpetrators, as their
actions ultimately determine whether an assault takes place. Programs aimed at
male audiences typically involve components related to reducing rape-myth
acceptance and increasing empathy for victims, and some recent programs
have shown success at changing participants’ attitudes toward rape (Foubert,
2000; O’Donohue, Yeater, & Fanetti, 2003). Although these attitudes are
related to sexual aggression, there is still a lack of evidence for the success of
these programs in changing actual behavior and reducing rates of sexual assault
(Yeater & O’Donohue, 1999).
While the effectiveness of prevention programs targeting males remains in
question, however, there is a more pressing need to educate women on rape
statistics and potential risk factors so that they may begin to take steps on their
own to reduce their vulnerability. Although sexual assault prevention pro-
grams have been widely implemented on college campuses, the effectiveness of
116 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
these programs at producing lasting change and reducing prevalence has not
been studied adequately. Most of these programs have emphasized attitudinal
change through education and awareness of prevalence, rape myths, societal
factors involved in promoting the occurrence of rape, and common risk fac-
tors and consequences of sexual victimization, and very few investigated
whether program participation actually influenced subsequent victimization
rates (Yeater & O’Donohue, 1999). In one of the first studies to evaluate the
success of a prevention program in producing both an increase in protective
dating behaviors and a decrease in rates of victimization, Hanson and Gidycz
(1993) implemented a program for female college students that focused on
awareness of rape myths, risk factors, and available strategies and precautionary
behaviors associated with rape resistance. The program was successful not only
in increasing knowledge about sexual assault and reducing risky dating be-
haviors, but also in reducing rates of sexual victimization for program par-
ticipants in comparison to the control group. However, while these results
appear promising, the success of this program was not universal, as the re-
duction in victimization experiences was only true for women without a
previous history of victimization. For those women who had already expe-
rienced a sexual assault, this particular prevention failed to decrease their risk
for future victimization. Subsequent modifications of this program have pro-
duced inconsistent findings regarding its effectiveness at reducing the inci-
dence of sexual assault (Breitenbecher & Gidycz, 1998; Breitenbecher &
Scarce, 1999). Evaluations of programs targeting mixed-gender audiences have
also documented positive changes in attitudes toward rape, but the magnitude
of this change was small and was unrelated to a reduction in victimization
experiences (Gidycz et al., 2001; Pinzone-Glover, Gidycz, & Jacobs, 1998).
More research is needed not only to identify the critical elements of these
programs that contributed to their relative success, but also to identify ways to
generalize their findings to women with a previous history of victimization
and women in community and clinical populations. However, the original
findings produced in the Hanson and Gidycz (1993) study are encouraging
because they highlight the potential that women have to successfully reduce
their risk for sexual assault. Because sexual victimization has such devastating
costs to both women and society in general, prevention on this level is ideal,
and although initial findings from these studies are limited, they suggest that
with further research, this may be an attainable goal.
Support Services
Although some of these recently developed early interventions for rape
victims show potential for reducing the immediate and long-term distress
experienced by women in the aftermath of an assault, the value of both formal
and informal support in helping women cope with victimization cannot be
overemphasized. For many years now, increased awareness and activism on
behalf of rape survivors has led to the creation of more widely available support
services in a variety of contexts that women may utilize as part of their healing
process. Some of these formal community resources include the legal or
criminal justice system, medical services, mental health professionals, and rape
crisis centers, all of which offer varying types of support, information, and
advocacy for victimized women. While every survivor must decide for herself
which of these services to make use of during her recovery process, each of
these resources can provide valuable support in helping women cope with a
sexual assault experience.
The emergence of rape crisis centers nationwide has created a particu-
larly important resource for sexually victimized women. In 2001, there were
118 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
mental health professionals, as well as publications and books that are available
to educate loved ones on rape and how to best support the survivor (see
McEvoy & Brookings, 1991).
conducted in a gradual manner. That is, the client progresses through her
particular set of exposure situations in a way that allows her to progressively
face more difficult (anxiety-producing) situations.
Anxiety management training aims to teach survivors an assortment of
skills that will help them cope with and manage (as the name suggests) their
anxiety and its symptoms. This form of treatment may include things such as
anger management skills, relaxation training, trauma education, interpersonal
skill training, job skills training, etc. While, in general, research has shown that
this form of treatment alone is not as effective in the long term as exposure
treatment, it has shown effectiveness and may be an especially beneficial ad-
dition to a treatment package that includes exposure.
Resick and Schnicke (1993) have developed a treatment for PTSD spe-
cifically geared toward survivors of rape. The treatment, called cognitive pro-
cessing therapy, combines the third element, cognitive restructuring, with a
form of exposure that involves writing about the traumatic event in graded
levels of detail. The cognitive restructuring component provides a means of
addressing potential distortions in thoughts about or resulting from the assault.
An additional avenue of assistance for survivors of assault suffering from
PTSD is psychopharmacological intervention. While pharmacological treat-
ments for PTSD are in somewhat earlier stages of development, treatment
outcome studies have lent support to the use of selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors such as sertraline (Brady, Pearlstein, Asnis, Baker, Rothbaum, Sikes,
et al., 2000). In most cases, these should be combined with psychotherapy.
Whatever the treatment option a survivor may choose to pursue, the most
important point is that there are options. In order to become more informed of
the options that may exist in any particular area, there are several worthwhile
resources that are a good start for seeking out treatment. Some of these re-
sources include state psychological associations, which will typically be able to
provide a list of therapists and some brief description of the populations they
serve. An additional potential resource for those seeking a treatment similar to
the ones described above is the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive
Therapies (www.aabt.org). This site has listings of member psychologists and
other professionals around the country, many of whom specialize in the
treatment of PTSD and other related problems, and may be a helpful starting
point for those seeking treatment. Finally, for those living near a large research
university, doctoral programs often operate training clinics, which may serve as
a resource for further information and treatment.
Regardless of the services and resources a sexual assault survivor uses to
best cope with her experience, it is clear that the reactions and quality of
services provided by formal and informal support systems can impact the
overall adjustment and well-being of survivors in the aftermath of an assault. As
suggested by Koss and Harvey (1991), ‘‘together these reactions will define the
victim’s position relative to the larger society and will contribute to or detract
from her sense of personal and social power. As these intersecting communities
124 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
act or fail to act on her behalf, the woman raped literally will rebuild her sense
of self ’’ (p. 96).
REFERENCES
Abbey, A., Ross, L. T., McDuffie, D., & McAuslan, P. (1996). Alcohol and dating
risk factors for sexual assault among college women. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 20(1), 147–169.
Abbey, A., Zawacki, T., Buck, P. O., Testa, M., Parks, K., Norris, J., et al.
(2002). How does alcohol contribute to sexual assault? Explanations from
laboratory and survey data. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
26(4), 575–581.
Ahrens, C. E., & Campbell, R. (2000). Assisting rape victims as they recover
from rape: The impact on friends. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15(9),
959–986.
Amick, A. E., & Calhoun, K. S. (1987). Resistance to sexual aggression: Perso-
nality, attitudinal, and situational factors. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16,
153–163.
Arata, C. M. (2002). Child sexual abuse and sexual revictimization. Clinical Psy-
chology: Science & Practice, 9(2), 135–164.
Atkeson, B. M., Calhoun, K. S., Resick, P. A., & Ellis, E. M. (1982). Victims of
rape: Repeated assessment of depressive symptoms. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 50(1), 96–102.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Basile, K. C., & Saltzman, L. E. (2002). Sexual violence surveillance: Uniform defi-
nitions and recommended data elements. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Con-
trol and Prevention.
Brady, K., Pearlstein, T., Asnis, G. M., Baker, D., Rothbaum, B., Sikes, C. R.,
et al. (2000). Efficacy and safety of sertraline treatment of posttraumatic
stress disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Me-
dical Association, 283(14), 1837–1844.
Breitenbecher, K. H. (2001). Sexual revictimization among women: A review of
the literature focusing on empirical investigations. Aggression and Violent
Behavior, 6, 415–432.
Breitenbecher, K. H., & Gidycz, C. A. (1998). An empirical evaluation of a
program designed to reduce the risk of multiple sexual victimization.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 472–488.
Breitenbecher, K. H., & Scarce, M. (1999). A longitudinal evaluation of the
effectiveness of a sexual assault education program. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 14, 459–478.
Burgess, A.W., & Holmstrom, L. L. (1974). Rape trauma syndrome. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 131(9), 981–986.
Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. (1979). Rape: Sexual disruption and reco-
very. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49(4), 648–657.
Sexual Assault 125
Burnam, M. A., Stein, J. A., & Golding, J. M. (1988). Sexual assault and mental
disorders in a community population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 56(6), 843–850.
Calhoun, K. S., Atkeson, B. M., & Resick, P. A. (1982). A longitudinal exami-
nation of fear reactions in victims of rape. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
29(6), 655–661.
Calhoun, K. S., Gidycz, C. A., Wilson, A. E., Van Wynsberghe, A., Outman, R.,
Marioni, N., et al. (2002, November). Long-term evaluation of a group risk
reduction program for sexual revictimization. Poster presented at the 36th A-
nnual Meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
Reno, NV.
Campbell, R. (1998). The community response to rape: Victims’ experiences with
the legal, medical, and mental health systems. American Journal of Comm-
unity Psychology, 26, 355–379.
Campbell, R., & Martin, P. Y. (2001). Services for sexual assault survivors: The
role of rape crisis centers. In C. M. Renzetti, J. L. Edleson, & R. K.
Bergen (Eds.), Sourcebook on violence against women (pp. 227–241). Thou-
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Campbell, R., Wasco, S. M., Ahrens, C. E., Sefl, T., & Barnes, H. E. (2001).
Preventing the ‘‘second rape’’: Rape survivors’ experiences with commu-
nity service providers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 1239–1259.
Classen, C., Field, N. P., Koopman, C., Nevill-Manning, K., & Spiegel, D.
(2001). Interpersonal problems and their relationship to sexual revictimi-
zation among women sexually abused in childhood. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 16, 495–509.
Cloitre, M., Scarvalone, P., & Difede, J. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder, self
and interpersonal dysfunction among sexually retraumatized women. Jour-
nal of Traumatic Stress Disorder, 10, 437–452.
Corbin, W. R., Bernat, J. A., Calhoun, K. S., McNair, L. D., & Seals, K. L.
(2001). The role of alcohol expectancies and alcohol consumption among
sexually victimized and nonvictimized college women. Journal of Interper-
sonal Violence, 16(4), 297–311.
Crowell, N. A., & Burgess, A. W. (Ed.). (1996). Understanding violence against
women. National Research Council, Commission on Behavioral & Social
Sciences & Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Cue, K. L., George, W. H., & Norris, J. (1996). Women’s appraisals of sexual-
assault risk in dating situations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 487–504.
Dunlap, H., Brazeau, P., Stermac, L., & Addison, M. (2004). Acute forensic
medical procedures used following a sexual assault among treatment-see-
king women. Women and Health, 40, 53–65.
Ellis, E. M., Atkeson, B. M., & Calhoun, K. S. (1982). An examination of
differences between multiple- and single-incident victims of sexual assault.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 221–224.
Filipas, H. H., & Ullman, S. E. (2001). Social reactions to sexual assault victims
from various support sources. Violence and Victims, 16, 673–692.
126 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse:
A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 530–541.
Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D., & Perry, K. J. (1995). Evaluation of a brief cognitive-
behavioral program for the prevention of chronic PTSD in recent assault
victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 948–955.
Foubert, J. D. (2000). The longitudinal effects of a rape-prevention program on
fraternity men’s attitudes, behavioral intent, and behavior. Journal of Am-
erican College Health, 48, 158–163.
Frazier, P. A., & Haney, B. (1996). Sexual assault cases in the legal system: Police,
prosecutor, and victim perspectives. Law and Human Behavior, 20(6), 607–
628.
Gidycz, C. A., Coble, C. N., Latham, L., & Layman, M. J. (1993). Sexual assault
experience in adulthood and prior victimization experiences: A prospec-
tive analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 151–168.
Gidycz, C. A., Hanson, K., & Layman, M. J. (1995). A prospective analysis of the
relationships among sexual assault experiences. Psychology of Women Qua-
rterly, 19, 5–29.
Gidycz, C. A., Layman, M. J., Crothers, M., Gylys, J., Matorin, A., & Dowdall,
C. (1997, May). An evaluation of an acquaintance rape prevention program:
Impact on attitudes and behavior. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Gidycz, C. A., Layman, M. J., Rich, C. L., Crothers, M., Gylys, J., Matorin, A.,
et al. (2001). An evaluation of an acquaintance rape prevention program:
Impact on attitudes, sexual aggression, and sexual victimization. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 16, 1120–1138.
Golding, J. M., Cooper, M. L., & George, L. K. (1997). Sexual assault and health
perceptions: Seven general population studies. Health Psychology, 16(5),
417–425.
Golding, J. M., Siegel, J. M., Sorenson, S. B., Burnam, M. A., & Stein, J. A.
(1989). Social support sources following sexual assault. Journal of Commu-
nity Psychology, 17, 92–107.
Goodman, L. A., Koss, M. P., & Russo, N. F. (1993). Violence against women:
Mental health effects: Part II. Conceptualizations of posttraumatic stress.
Applied and Preventive Psychology, 2(3), 123–130.
Greene, D. M., & Navarro, R. L. (1998). Situation-specific assertiveness in the
epidemiology of sexual victimization among university women: A pros-
pective path analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 589–604.
Hanson, K. A., & Gidycz, C. A. (1993). Evaluation of a sexual assault prevention
program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 1046–1052.
Himelein, M. J. (1995). Risk factors for sexual victimization in dating: A longit-
udinal study of college women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 31–48.
Jewkes, R., Sen, P., & Garcia-Moreno, C. (2002). Sexual violence. In E. G. Krug,
L. L. Dahlberg, J. A. Mercy, A. B. Zwi, & R. Lozano (Eds.), World report
on violence and health (pp. 149–181). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health
Organization.
Sexual Assault 127
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Brown, A. L. (in press). Risk perception, rape and
sexual revictimization: A prospective study of college women. Psychology of
Women Quarterly.
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Long, P. J. (2000). Child sexual abuse and revictim-
ization in the form of adult sexual abuse, adult physical abuse, and adult
psychological maltreatment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15(5), 489–
502.
Messman-Moore, T. L., Long, P. J., & Siegfried, N. J. (2000). The revictimization
of child sexual abuse survivors: An examination of the adjustment of co-
llege women with child sexual abuse, adult sexual abuse, and adult physical
abuse. Child Maltreatment, 5, 18–27.
Miller, B., & Marshall, J. C. (1987). Coercive sex on the university campus.
Journal of College Student Personnel, 28, 38–47.
Miranda, R., Mironda, R., Meyerson, L. A., Long, P. J., Marx, B. P., & Simpson,
S. M. (2002). Sexual assault and alcohol use: Exploring the self-medication
hypothesis. Violence and Victims, 17(2), 205–217.
Mohler-Kuo, M., Dowdall, G. W., Koss, M. P., & Wechsler, H. (2004). Corr-
elates of rape while intoxicated in a national sample of college women.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65(1), 37–45.
Mosher, D. L., & Anderson, R. D. (1986). Macho personality, sexual aggression,
and reactions to guided imagery of realistic rape. Journal of Research in
Personality, 20(1), 77–94.
Muehlenhard, C. L. (1988). Misinterpreted dating behaviors and the risk of date
rape. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 20–37.
Muehlenhard, C. L., & Linton, M. A. (1987). Date rape and sexual aggression in
dating situations: Incidence and risk factors. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
ogy, 34(2), 186–196.
Norris, J., Nurius, P., & Dimeff, L. (1996). Through her eyes: Factors affecting
women’s perception of and resistance to acquaintance sexual aggression
threat. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 123–145.
O’Donohue, W., Yeater, E. A., & Fanetti, M. (2003). Rape prevention with
college males: The roles of rape myth acceptance, victim empathy, and
outcome expectancies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 513–531.
Orcutt, H. K., Cooper, M. L., & Garcia, M. (in press). Use of sexual intercourse
to reduce negative affect as a prospective mediator of sexual revictimiza-
tion. Journal of Traumatic Stress.
O’Sullivan, E., & Carlton, A. (2001). Victim services, community outreach, and
contemporary rape crisis centers: A comparison of independent and mu-
ltiservice centers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 343–360.
Parks, K. A., & Zetes-Zanatta, L. M. (1999). Women’s bar-related victimization:
Refining and testing a conceptual model. Aggressive Behavior, 25(5),
349–364.
Pinzone-Glover, H. A., Gidycz, C. A., & Jacobs, C. D. (1998). An acquaintance
rape prevention program: Effects on attitudes toward women, rape-related
Sexual Assault 129
Wilson, A. E., Calhoun, K. S., & Bernat, J. A. (1999). Risk recognition and
trauma-related symptoms among sexually revictimized women. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 705–710.
Yeater, E. A., & O’Donohue, W. (1999). Sexual assault prevention programs:
Current issues, future directions, and the potential efficacy of interventions
with women. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 739–771.
6
Rita Kenyon-Jump 1
Incest, the sexual exploitation of a child by a family member, is socially ab-
horred, yet occurs far too frequently and is clearly linked to psychopathology
and social dysfunction in children, adolescents, and adults. What follows is a
review of the research on incest, including the most widely accepted defini-
tion, prevalence data, (the number of cases found in a population at a given
time), descriptions of the characteristics of both survivor and offender, the
psychological effects (problems or disorders) that result from experiencing
childhood sexual abuse by a family member, and effective treatments to assist
survivors of childhood sexual trauma in reclaiming their lives. I have attempted
to cover the effects of incest on male and female survivors at various devel-
opmental stages, such as during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood as well
as in different victim-perpetrator relationships, such as mother-son incest,
father-daughter incest, and with extended family members. I have also pro-
vided current thinking into male and female juvenile and adult incest of-
fenders.
Much of the research on childhood sexual abuse combines intrafamilial
(immediate and extended family members) and extrafamilial (acquaintances
and strangers) child sexual abuse victims into one sexually abused group, and
typically they are compared with a group of persons who have not experienced
sexual abuse. Studies will report the percentage of incest and nonfamilial
sexually abused participants in the sexual abuse group but use the combined
group for the statistical comparisons. This makes it more difficult to determine
132 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
the differences and similarities between people who have experienced incest
and those who have experienced sexual abuse by nonfamily members. When
possible, I have delineated the effects of childhood sexual abuse specifically
attributed to intrafamilial abuse.
FAMILY ENVIRONMENT
There have been conflicting reports of the independent effects of family
environment and childhood sexual abuse in predicting long-term negative
outcomes for males and females who were sexually abused in childhood. Some
report that the childhood family environment rather than the abuse is re-
sponsible for the psychological difficulty that survivors of childhood trauma
exhibit (Beitchman et al., 1992; Flemming, Mullen, Sibthorpe, & Bammer,
1999; Merrill, Thomsen, Sinclair, Gold, & Milner, 2001; Peters, 1988, cited in
Fassler, Amodeo, Griffin, Clay, & Ellis, 2005), while others assert that child-
hood sexual abuse has been linked to multiple short- and long-term psycho-
logical problems both in child and adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse
and has been associated with psychiatric disorders whether or not the abuse is
part of a larger collection of family adversities (Dinwiddie et al., 2000; Fassler
et al., 2005; Kendler et al., 2000; Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001; Mullen,
Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1996; Nelson et al., 2002; Peleikis,
Mykletun, & Dahl, 2004; Stevenson, 1999; Weiss, Longhurst, & Mazure,
1999). Rind, Tromovitch, and Bauserman (1998) indicated in their meta-
analysis of child sexual abuse studies using college samples that the family
background risk factors predicted more risk for adult psychological distress
than did child sexual abuse in the college student population. They also
suggested that in clinical samples with a higher proportion of persons with
intrafamilial childhood sexual abuse, the experience of childhood sexual abuse
may be more of a predictor than the family background. Thus, it may be in the
case of incest that sexual abuse is the significant risk factor.
134 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
PSYCHOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF
INCESTUOUS ABUSE
Sexual abuse by a family member is associated with more psychological
distress and social adjustment symptoms than extrafamilial childhood abuse
(Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Finkelhor, 1979; Herman, Russell, & Trocki,
1986; Jackson, Calhoun, Amick, Maddever, & Habif, 1990; Kelly, Wood,
Gonzalez, MacDonald, & Waterman, 2002; Ketring & Feinauer, 1999; Molnar,
Buka, & Kessler, 2001; Russell, 1986; Tsai, Feldman-Summers, & Edgar, 1979;
Wind & Silvern, 1992). When comparing female victims of childhood sexual
abuse, father-daughter incest is the most psychologically damaging sexually
abusive relationship (Finkelhor,1979; Russell, 1986) and when both male and
female intrafamilial sexual abuse victims are studied, those abused by their
fathers also scored significantly higher on symptoms related to dissociation,
anxiety, depression, postsexual abuse trauma, and sleep disturbance than males
and females abused by acquaintances, strangers, and other family members
(Ketring & Feinauer, 1999). Perpetration by a father/father figure produced
significantly higher mean trauma scores even when controlling for the severity
of the sexual trauma. In addition, abuse by other family members resulted in
significantly more negative symptoms than that experienced by persons who
were sexually abused by strangers (Ketring & Feinauer, 1999).
Younger children are more likely to be abused by family members (Fi-
scher & McDonald, 1998) and sexual abuse at a young age, before 7–8 years of
age, has been associated with more psychological disturbance (Nash, Zivney,
& Hulsey, 1993) and more physical injury (Fischer & McDonald, 1998). In-
trafamilial sexual trauma is typically repetitive and of a longer duration than
abuse by nonfamily members (Cole & Putnam, 1992; Fischer & McDonald,
1998; Ruggiero, McLeer, & Dixon, 2000). Duration of the abuse has been
found to be associated with poorer adjustment in adulthood (Bennett, Hughes,
& Luke, 2000; Rodriguez, Vande Kemp, & Foy, 1998). Internalization of the
abuse (i.e., attributing self-blame) is correlated with duration of abuse and has
been found to explain more of the adulthood maladjustment of childhood
sexual abuse survivors than duration of the abuse, relationship to the offender,
or age of onset of the abuse (Steel, Sanna, Hammond, Whipple, & Cross,
2004).
There is a longer delay in disclosure of abuse in incest, with more coercion
to keep the abuse a secret and greater fear of what will happen to the family
if the sexual abuse is revealed (Fischer & McDonald, 1998; Kogan, 2004;
Lawson, 1993; Russell, 1986; Sheinberg & Fraenkel, 2001). The more closely
related the sexually victimized child is to the perpetrator, the less likely that the
child will disclose the abuse (Wyatt & Newcomb, 1990). The younger the
child is when disclosing incest, the more likely she will receive a negative
reaction whether she discloses to a parent or friend (Roesler & Wind, 1994)
Incest Victims and Offenders 135
and the less likely that the sexual assault will result in an arrest (U.S. De-
partment of Justice, 2000). In fact, over half of a sample of female incest
survivors reported that their parent either ignored the disclosure, responded
with anger, or blamed the victim (Roesler & Wind, 1994). Mothers with a
history of childhood sexual trauma display more distress than mothers who
have not been sexually abused when their children disclose sexual abuse
(Deblinger, Stauffer, & Landsberg, 1994). Mothers have been found to be
most supportive of their children upon disclosure of incest when the perpe-
trator is an ex-spouse (Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelsohn, & Coulter, 1989),
biological father, uncle, cousin, or grandfather (Sirles & Franke, 1989) and least
supportive when a current, unmarried partner (Everson et al., 1989; Sirles &
Franke, 1989) or stepfather (Sirles & Franke, 1989) is the offender. Telling of
the abuse will not ensure that the abuse will stop. In a retrospective survey of
228 female survivors of incest, the abuse continued for at least a year for over
half of the respondents who had disclosed their abuse prior to age 18, and in
over a fourth of the cases, the abuse did not stop for over a year following the
disclosure (Roesler & Wind, 1994).
Intrusiveness (i.e., anal, vaginal, oral penetration) of the sexual abuse does
not differ between groups of children abused by family members or nonfamily
members; both groups of children experience force and penetration (Hall,
Mathews, & Pearce, 2002).
abused by stepfathers and fathers had higher global functioning and fewer
symptoms of avoidant behavior. Their finding was opposite of those who
report that abuse by fathers and stepfathers is the most damaging (Finkelhor,
1979; Ketring & Feinauer, 1999; Russell, 1986). A possible explanation of this
could be that they included adult relatives other than fathers and stepfathers in
their extrafamilial group and did not define if the perpetrator children in the
extrafamilial group were related to the offended children. Including family
members may have lowered the functioning of the extrafamilial group.
Adolescents are at particular risk for developing psychological problems as
a result of sexual abuse (Feiring, Taska, & Lewis, 1999). Peer interaction is
crucial for normal adolescent development, and the secrecy of sexual abuse,
especially incestuous abuse, can isolate an adolescent from his/her peer group
(Marvasti & Dripchak, 2004a; O’Brien, 1987; Schultz, 1990). Adolescence is
also a time for developing one’s identity, including a sexual identity. It is
confusing for young girls to view themselves as their own age when they
have been forced to engage in activities intended for adults (Sheinberg &
Fraenkel, 2001). Girls, more so than boys, experience increased personal vul-
nerability and perceive the world as a dangerous place (Feiring et al., 1999).
Male and female adolescents with histories of sexual abuse report more dis-
tress than adolescents who have not experienced sexual abuse, with sexually
abused female adolescents more likely to experience depression than sexually
abused male adolescents (Meyerson, Long, Miranda, & Marx, 2002). Both
male and female sexually abused adolescents report more symptoms of de-
pression and hopelessness than similar adolescents without histories of sexual
trauma (Martin, Bergen, Richardson, Roeger, & Allison, 2004). Sexually
abused girls with severe family conflict experience higher levels of depression
(Meyerson et al., 2002). Incest prior to the age of 14 significantly increases
the risk of a lifetime incidence of major depression (Pribor & Dinwiddie,
1992).
Adolescent male and female sexual abuse victims are more likely to think
about suicide, make suicidal threats or attempts, or deliberately self-injure than
their nonabused counterparts. However, boys who experienced sexual abuse
respond more negatively than girls (Martin et al., 2004; Molnar, Berkman, &
Buka, 2001; Ystgaard, Hestetun, Loeb, & Mehlum, 2004). They have a
tenfold increased risk of making suicidal plans and threats and a fifteenfold
increased risk of attempting suicide compared to nonabused males, even after
controlling for symptoms of depression, hopelessness, and family functioning.
In abused girls, depression, hopelessness, and poorer family functioning make
it more likely that they will become suicidal (Martin et al., 2004). High levels
of distress related to the sexual trauma are strongly associated with suicidal
ideas and plans in both male and female adolescents (Martin et al., 2004).
Males with low levels of distress are also likely to make suicidal threats while
those with high levels of distress will deliberately harm themselves in addition
to making threats (Martin et al., 2004).
Incest Victims and Offenders 137
Sexually and physically abused adolescent boys and girls are more likely to
use alcohol and other drugs at a young age, including a greater variety of
substances, and engage in more frequent attempts to ‘‘self-medicate painful
emotions’’ than are nonabused adolescents (Harrison, Fulkerson, & Beebe,
1997, p. 536). In a sample of 122,824 public school sixth, ninth, and twelfth
grade students in Minnesota, 25.6 percent of the female students and 6.8 percent
of the male students endorsed having experienced sexual abuse either by an
older family member or an older nonfamily member; of this group of sexually
abused adolescents, 64 percent of the males and 46 percent of the females who
reported intrafamilial sexual abuse also reported extrafamilial sexual abuse
(Harrison et al., 1997). Substance use and abuse is especially problematic for
adolescents, as it interferes with the development of appropriate coping skills
and negatively impacts social and academic functioning (Harrison et al., 1997).
Childhood sexual abuse during adolescence increases the risk for later
sexual victimization (Ryan, Kilmer, Cauce, Wantanabe, & Hoyt, 2000) and is
strongly associated with attempted suicide in homeless adolescents (Feitel,
Margetson, Chamas, & Lipman, 1992; Ryan et al., 2000) and with high levels
of depression and anxiety in adolescents (Meyerson et al., 2002; Ryan et al.,
2000). Adolescent male victims of sexual abuse have a higher risk of suicidal
ideation, sexual risk-taking, substance abuse, delinquency, and eating disorders
than adolescent males without a history of sexual abuse (Chandy, Blum, &
Resnick, 1999). Male and female high school students with a history of
childhood sexual victimization engage in high-risk sexual behaviors, such as
having consensual intercourse before age 15, having two or more sexual
partners in the previous three months, and sex resulting in pregnancy. The risk
of multiple partners, substance use at last intercourse, and sex resulting in
pregnancy is four to five times greater for high school boys who experienced
sexual abuse compared with boys with no such history (Raj, Silverman, &
Amaro, 2000). High school-aged girls who experienced childhood sexual
abuse are twice as likely to have early intercourse, three or more sex partners
ever, and to become pregnant than are girls who have not experienced sexual
abuse during their childhood (Raj et al., 2000).
Adolescents who have a history of childhood sexual abuse are at an in-
creased risk of eating disorders as well (Chandy et al., 1997; Neumark-Sztainer,
Story, Hannan, Beauhring, & Resnick, 2000). Those who experienced incest
are significantly more likely than adolescents who have not been sexually
abused to engage in bingeing and purging with their eating, to express dissat-
isfaction with their bodies, and to report a loss of control with regard to their
eating habits (Wonderlich et al., 2001).
have been linked to the experience of childhood sexual abuse in adult women
(Cole & Putnam, 1992; Gladstone, Parker, Mitchell, Malhi, Wilhelm, &
Austin, 2004; Hofmann, Levitt, Hofman, Greene, Litz, & Barlow, 2001;
Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001; Owens & Chard, 2003; van Gerko, Hughes,
Hamill, & Waller, 2005; Weiss et al., 1999).
Female inpatients who have been diagnosed with major depressive dis-
order and who have experienced childhood sexual trauma are four times less
likely to recover from their depression within twelve months than those de-
pressed female inpatients with no history of child sexual abuse and, at a five-
year follow-up, chronic depression is significantly more prevalent in those
with histories of childhood sexual abuse (Zlotnick, Ryan, Miller, & Keitner,
1995). Childhood sexual abuse has also been linked to longer episodes of
depression in female outpatients (Zlotnick, Mattia, & Zimmerman, 2001) and
in females with comorbid anxiety and major depressive disorders (Zlotnick,
Warshaw, Shea, & Keller, 1997). Outpatients with histories of childhood
sexual abuse are significantly more likely to be diagnosed with PTSD, multiple
psychological disorders (e.g., major depression combined with an anxiety
disorder), and borderline personality disorder (Zlotnick et al., 2001) than
patients with no histories of childhood sexual abuse. Although this study of
235 depressed outpatients combined those who experienced incest with those
who experienced extrafamilial abuse in their sample of sexually abused sub-
jects, 25 percent of the sample endorsed having experienced childhood sexual
abuse and 52 percent of those experienced incest (Zlotnick et al., 2001). The
odds of PTSD are higher for sexually abused females who have experienced
rape by steprelatives and acquaintances than by strangers (Molnar, Buka, &
Kessler, 2001). Others have concluded that childhood sexual abuse is a sub-
stantial risk factor for chronic, recurrent major depressive episodes (Browne &
Finkelhor, 1986; Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992; Saunders, Villeponteaux,
Lipovsky, Kilpatrick, & Veronen, 1992). In fact, a history of childhood sexual
abuse in females was associated with fourteen of seventeen lifetime mood,
anxiety, and substance abuse disorders (Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001). In a
study of 301 women in New Zealand who met lifetime criteria for an eating
disorder, affective disorder, and/or substance abuse, women with a lifetime
diagnosis of depression were twice as likely to report a history of incest than
women who had no history of depression (Bushnell, Wells, & Oakley-Brown,
1992, cited in Weiss et al., 1999).
Substance abuse is strongly linked with childhood sexual abuse, more so
than any other psychiatric disorder (Kendler et al., 2000; Teusch, 2001) and, in
particular, with victims of incest (Wonderlich et al., 2001). Female victims of
incest who do not receive treatment for their trauma have more difficulty in
the early stages of alcohol sobriety than women without a history of incest or
childhood sexual trauma and are also more likely to relapse (i.e., use alcohol
again) (Kovach, 1986). Incest is also associated with lifetime difficulty with
crack cocaine (Freeman, Collier, & Parillo, 2002).
Incest Victims and Offenders 139
and explained that they had ‘‘not recorded in a sufficiently systematic way’’ to
allow this information to be used in the analysis (p. 377). Other studies have
found similar links to eating disorders and body image disturbance (Kendler
et al., 2000; Waller, 1992; Waller, Hamilton, Rose, Sumra, & Baldwin, 1993).
COPING
Negative attributional style has been defined as the tendency for people to
ascribe the ‘‘cause of a negative event to themselves (internal), across situations
(global), and over time (stable)’’ and has been associated with psychological
distress following childhood sexual abuse (Steel et al., 2004, p. 787). Attri-
butions of shame and self-blame are associated with depression and lower self-
esteem in children as early as two months after disclosure of the abuse (Feiring
et al., 1999). Accepting responsibility for the abuse, internalization of the
abuse, resistance, and confrontive coping all contribute to serious psycho-
logical problems in adulthood (Steel et al., 2004). Failing to use social support
as a coping strategy also leads to more psychological distress in adulthood (Steel
et al., 2004).
Duration of the abuse is correlated with internalization of abuse, which in
turn is related to a poorer adjustment in adulthood. For example, the longer
the abuse takes place, the more likely the victim will blame him-/herself.
Others (Bennett et al., 2000; Rodriguez et al., 1998) also found a positive
relationship between duration of abuse and significant psychological distress
but did not look at the coping styles. The older the child at the time of abuse,
142 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
the poorer the adjustment in adulthood, as the older the victim, the more
likely he/she will accept responsibility for the abuse (Steel et al., 2004). Older
victims may experience increased negative symptomatology because they have
awareness that the sexual experiences are unacceptable and harmful (Ruggiero
et al., 2000).
In general, victims appear to have poorer outcomes if they deny the abuse,
distance themselves from the abuse, or otherwise minimize the abuse. Fur-
thermore, the long-term impact of childhood sexual abuse may be lessened if
the victim tells another person about the abuse and problem-solves possible
strategies to end the abuse (Guelzow, Cornett, & Dougherty, 2002). Endler
and Parker (as cited in Guelzow et al., 2002) proposed three coping strategies
used by persons when placed in stressful situations. They identified emotion-
focused coping (i.e., self-blame for being too emotional, preoccupation with
worry) and avoidance-focused coping (i.e., engaging in activities to ignore the
abuse) as maladaptive, while task-focused coping (i.e., outlining priorities and
developing and following through on a course of action) is viewed as adaptive.
Family support is likely to reduce extreme long-term consequences from
childhood sexual abuse and decrease the impact of childhood sexual abuse
(Guelzow et al., 2002). A mother’s support is an important mediating variable
when the perpetrator of the abuse is familial, while lack of a father’s support
increases the likelihood of emotion-focused coping (Guelzow et al., 2002).
In their study of over 100 female survivors of incest, Brand and Alexander
(2003) found that avoidance-focused coping and emotion-focused coping
were the most used styles of coping and were associated with poorer adult
functioning. Use of avoidance seems to be dysfunctional in the long run as it
prevents the development of effective coping strategies. They found little use
of task-focused coping and explained how this highlights the extreme pow-
erlessness of children who are being sexually abused. They suggested that
sexually victimized children’s main method of protecting themselves is to
attempt to manage their emotional reactions. Contrary to the finding of
others, seeking social support was associated with more distress. Victims of
incest typically report significantly less family support than victims of extra-
familial sexual abuse (Stroud, 1999) and for this reason telling of the abuse is
not always effective in ending the abuse (Roesler & Wind, 1994). Brand and
Alexander (2003) also suggest that when ‘‘abuse is frequent, chronic, and/or
perpetrated by a family member, distancing from the current abuse may be
beneficial’’ (p. 291). They also hypothesized that children who are able to
distance themselves may function better as adults because the distancing
strategy could make them more resilient. Thus, it is not clear-cut when
seeking support will be helpful or harmful. Perhaps if the mother believes her
child and takes action to protect the child, then the social support may prevent
adulthood distress.
Developmental factors affect a child’s capacity to handle stress and sexually
abused children react to incest differently depending upon age and develop-
Incest Victims and Offenders 143
mental level (Cole & Putnam, 1992). Preschool victims use the coping style of
denial and dissociation and are unable to use instrumental coping strategies of
refusal and avoidance because they cannot tell others of the abuse and cannot
get away from their family member perpetrator. Sexual abuse during the
elementary school years interferes with development of social self-competence
because children who experience severe guilt, shame, and confusion regarding
their sexual abuse are unlikely to feel secure enough to make friends or create
any type of social support away from home. Adolescence is a significant period
of social and sexual identity development. Incest during adolescence may
interrupt learning to use reasoning, reflection, and planning, which leaves
incest survivors relying on denial, dissociation, and other immature coping
strategies and places them at greater risk for severe psychological problems
(Cole & Putnam, 1992).
NONOFFENDING MOTHERS
Early work in the area of incest tended to blame mothers and promoted a
belief that mothers were aware of the incest prior to the disclosure and col-
luded with the perpetrator on a conscious or unconscious level. However, the
vast majority of mothers believe their children when they disclose the sexual
abuse and make an effort to protect them ( Joyce, 1997).
Crawford (1999) concluded in her review of literature on the role of
nonoffending mothers in intrafamilial sexual abuse of daughters that these
mothers are a mixed group with each needing individual assessment by pro-
fessionals to determine if that mother is capable of supporting, protecting, and
assisting in her daughter’s healing. Lastly, Bolen (2003) also focused on the
literature pertaining to nonoffending mothers of sexually abused children with
an emphasis on intrafamilial abuse and suggested that there has been a socio-
historical context in which nonoffending mothers are held accountable for the
abuse of their children in the eyes of child protection professionals, whereas
there appears to be no similar level of responsibility for the nonoffending
father.
There is conflicting information regarding a mother’s history of childhood
sexual abuse being a risk factor for sexual abuse of her own children. Some
studies have shown that a mother’s history of childhood sexual abuse, espe-
cially incest, is a risk factor for her own children to be sexually abused, es-
pecially by a father or stepfather (Faller, 1989; Joyce, 1997; McCloskey &
Bailey, 2000; Russell, 1986), while others found no difference in the mother’s
history of sexual abuse with regard to sexual abuse of her children, including
incest (Estes & Tidwell, 2002). Mothers who abuse alcohol and drugs place
their children at greater risk for sexual abuse than mothers who do not
(McCloskey & Bailey, 2000).
Daughters whose mothers experienced incest have reported a negative
impact upon them as children that transcended into their adulthood. Research
144 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
suggests that mothers who have been victims of incest themselves suffer long-
term negative effects, including parenting difficulties as a result of problems
related to their own sense of organization, control, and confidence (Cole &
Putnam, 1992). A negative attitude toward one’s own body and sexuality,
viewing all women as victims, difficulty integrating sex and intimacy, impaired
judgment of the trustworthiness of others, hypervigilance, lack of assertiveness,
lack of parenting skills, difficulty recognizing and expressing anger, eating
disorders, and external locus of control were found in both the mothers who
experienced incest and their daughters. In addition, the daughters attributed
many of their own psychological problems to their mother’s parenting and
history of incest, including sexually acting out, sexualized relationships with
males, feeling defective, fear of all men, impaired functioning in their occu-
pations, and substance abuse (Voth & Tutty, 1999).
OFFENDERS
record and had been placed outside of their homes prior to age 16 (Greenberg
et al., 2005).
Childhood sexual victimization of incest offenders presents a complex
picture. Male sex offenders who were abused by family members as children
are more likely to have female victims and less likely to bribe their victims than
are male offenders who were abused by strangers or acquaintances (Craissati,
McClurg, & Browne, 2002). Those males abused by strangers are more likely
to abuse males or a combination of both males and females (Craissati et al.,
2002).
Familial offenders more so than nonfamilial offenders minimize their
behavior (Webster & Beech, 2001) and view their victims as adults (Wilson,
1999). Some studies have shown that nonfamilial offenders are more likely to
blame their victims and are less likely to admit their responsibility (Miner &
Dwyer, 1997; Webster & Beech, 2001), while others found the contrary
(Parton & Day, 2002).
Recidivism
While researchers have consistently demonstrated lower sexual and violent
recidivism (i.e., offending again) rates for those molesting their biological
daughters and stepdaughters as compared to child molesters who molest ac-
quaintances, strangers, and extended family members (Firestone et al., 1999;
Greenberg et al., 2000; Hanson & Bussière, 1998), it is difficult to know true
146 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
TREATMENT
Group treatments are particularly effective for victims of incest because
being in a group decreases isolation and provides an awareness that others have
also experienced incest. Groups typically last ten weeks with follow-up six
months later. Group treatments for women victims of childhood incest ef-
fectively reduce symptoms of anxiety, avoidance, dissociation, and depression,
increase self-esteem and the ability to protect oneself, and decrease feelings
of guilt, shame, and self-blame (Alexander, Neimeyer, Follette, Moore, &
Harter, 1989; Carver, Stalker, Stewart, & Abraham, 1989; Hazzard, Rogers, &
Angert, 1993; Herman & Schaatzow, 1984; Morgan & Cummings, 1999;
Roberts & Lie, 1989; Zlotnick, Shea, et al., 1997).
Several studies showed conflicting results regarding the impact of indi-
vidual therapy in addition to the group experience. Some research shows that
prior individual therapy contributes to more successful outcomes from the
group treatment (Hazzard et al., 1993; Westbury & Tutty, 1999), while other
research found no additional benefit from concurrent individual therapy
(Morgan & Cummings, 1999).
A treatment model that focuses on shame-based behaviors may signifi-
cantly decrease a woman’s risk for sexual revictimization, increase her ability
to express emotions, such as rage and humiliation, and decrease self-blame
(Kessler & Bieschke, 1999). Shame is also a significant emotion that results in
negative consequences for male victims of incest and is important to target in
treatments for men and boys. Psychiatric disorders most prevalent with sur-
vivors of childhood incest reflect impairments in self- and social functioning
and suggest use of a developmental model for treatment (Cole & Putnam,
148 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
CONCLUSIONS
Incest is a serious ongoing problem in our society that continues to be
unreported and underreported and has significant negative, long-term con-
sequences for its victims, both male and female. Untreated childhood sexual
abuse creates problems that last a lifetime. We must do a better job of detecting
incest by specifically inquiring of this experience in our clinical populations.
The unexplored nature of incest with males needs to be studied along all
developmental levels in order to design effective treatments. In the outcome-
based world of psychological treatment, we need to provide treatment that has
proven to be effective and efficacious. Discovering and treating sexual abuse
during childhood and adolescence could prevent significant impairment, re-
victimization, and disruption in the lives of adult survivors of childhood incest
as well as reduce the transgenerational risk of incest.
REFERENCES
Alexander, P. C., Neimeyer, R. A., Follette, V. M., Moore, M. K., & Harter, S.
(1989). A comparison of group treatment of women sexually abused as
children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 479–483.
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., DaCosta, G. A., Akman, D., &
Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of the long-term effects of child sexual
abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16, 101–118.
Bennett, S. E., Hughes, H. M., & Luke, D. A. (2000). Heterogeneity in patterns
of child sexual abuse family functioning and long-term adjustment. Journal
of Interpersonal Violence, 15(2), 134–137.
Bolen, R. M. (2003). Nonoffending mothers of sexually abused children: A case
of institutionalized sexism? Violence Against Women, 9(11), 1336–1366.
Brand, B. L., & Alexander, P. C. (2003). Coping with incest: The relationship
between recollections of childhood coping and adult functioning in female
survivors of incest. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(3), 285–293.
Briere, J., & Elliott, D. M. (2003). Prevalence and psychological sequelae of self-
reported childhood physical and sexual abuse in a general population
sample of men and women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 1205–1222.
Browne, M. W., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abuse: A review
of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66–77.
Carver, C. M., Stalker, C., Stewart, E., & Abraham, B. (1989). The impact of
group therapy for adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 753–758.
Chandy, J. M., Blum, R. W., & Resnick, M. D. (1996). Gender-specific out-
comes for sexually abused adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20(12),
1219–1231.
Classen, C., Koopman, C., Nevill-Manning, K., & Spiegel, D. (2001). A pre-
liminary report comparing trauma-focused and present-focused group
150 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Everson, M., Hunter, W., Runyon, D., Edelsohn, G., & Coulter, M. (1989).
Maternal support following disclosure of incest. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 59, 197–207.
Faller, K. C. (1989). Why sexual abuse? An exploration of the intergenerational
hypothesis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 13, 543–548.
Fassler, I. R., Amodeo, M., Griffin, M. L., Clay, C. M., & Ellis, M. E. (2005).
Predicting long-term outcomes for women sexually abused in childhood:
Contribution of abuse severity versus family environment. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 29, 269–284.
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Lewis, M. (1999). Social support and children’s and
adolescents’ adaptation to sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
13(2), 240–260.
Feitel, B., Margetson, N., Chamas, J., & Lipman, C. (1992). Psychosocial back-
ground and behavioral and emotional disorders of homeless and runaway
youth. Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 43, 155–159.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children. New York: Free Press.
Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., McCoy, M., Greenberg, D. M., Curry, S., &
Larose, M. R. (1999). Predictions of recidivism in incest offenders. Journal
of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 511–531.
Fischer, D. G., & McDonald, W. L. (1998). Characteristics of intrafamilial and
extrafamilial child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(9), 915–929.
Flemming, J., Mullen, P. E., Sibthorpe, B., & Bammer, G. (1999). The long-term
impact of childhood sexual abuse in Australian women. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 23(2), 145–159.
Foa, E. B., & Meadows, E. A. (1997). Psychosocial treatments for posttraumatic
stress disorder: A critical review. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 449–480.
Fontella, C., Harrington, D., & Zuravin, S. J. (2000). Gender differences in the
characteristics and outcomes of sexually abused preschoolers. Journal of
Child Sexual Abuse, 9(2), 21–40.
Freeman, R. C., Collier, K., & Parillo, K. M. (2002). Early life sexual abuse as a risk
for crack cocaine use. American Journal of Alcohol Abuse, 28(1), 109–131.
Freund, K., Watson, R., & Dickey, R. (1991). Sex offenses against female chil-
dren perpetrated by men who are not pedophiles. Journal of Sex Research,
28, 409–423.
Gill, M., & Tutty, L. M. (1999). Male survivors of childhood sexual abuse: A
qualitative study and issues for clinical consideration. Journal of Child
Sexual Abuse, 7(3), 19–33.
Gladstone, G. L., Parker, G. B., Mitchell, P. B., Malhi, G. S., Wilhelm, K., &
Austin, M. (2004). Implications of childhood trauma for depressed
women: An analysis of pathways from childhood sexual abuse to deliberate
self-harm and revictimization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(8), 1417–
1425.
Gordy, P. L. (1993). Group work that supports adult victims of childhood incest.
Social Casework, 64, 300–307.
152 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Greenberg, D., Bradford, J., Firestone, P., & Curry, S. (2000). Recidivism of child
molesters: A study of victim relationship with the perpetrator. Child Abuse
& Neglect, 24(11), 1485–1494.
Greenberg, D. M., Firestone, P., Nunes, K. L., Bradford, J. M., & Curry, S.
(2005). Biological fathers and stepfathers who molest their daughters:
Psychological, phallometric, and criminal features. Sexual Abuse: A Journal
of Research and Treatment, 17(1), 39–46.
Guelzow, J. W., Cornett, P. F., & Dougherty, T. M. (2002). Child sexual abuse
victims’ perception of paternal support as a significant predictor of coping
style and global self-worth. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 11(4), 53–72.
Hall, D. K., Mathews, F., & Pearce, J. (2002). Sexual behavior problems in
sexually abused children: A preliminary typology. Child Abuse & Neglect,
26, 289–312.
Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1998). Predicting relapse: A meta-analysis of
sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
chology, 66, 348–362.
Harrison, P. A., Fulkerson, J. A., & Beebe, T. J. (1997). Multiple substance abuse
among adolescent physical and sexual abuse victims. Child Abuse & Neglect,
21(6), 529–539.
Hazzard, A., Rogers, J. H., & Angert, L. (1993). Factors affecting group therapy
outcomes for adult sexual abuse survivors. International Journal of Group
Psychotherapy, 43, 453–468.
Herman, J., Russell, D., & Trocki, K. (1986). Long-term effects of incestuous
abuse in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 1293–1296.
Herman, J., & Schaatzow, E. (1984). Time-limited group therapy for women
with a history of incest. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 34,
605–616.
Hetherton, J., & Beardsall, L. (1998). Decisions and attitudes concerning child
sexual abuse: Does the gender of the perpetrator make a difference to child
protection professionals? Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(12), 1265–1283.
Hofmann, S. G., Levitt, J. T., Hofman, E. C., Greene, K., Litz, B. T., & Barlow,
D. H. (2001). Potentially traumatizing events in panic disorder and other
anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 13, 101–102.
Jackson, J. L., Calhoun, K. S., Amick, A. E., Maddever, H. M., & Habif, V. L.
(1990). Young adult women who report intrafamilial sexual abuse: Sub-
sequent adjustment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19(3), 211–221.
Joyce, P. A. (1997). Mothers of sexually abused children and the concept of
collusion: A literature review. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 6(2), 75–92.
Kelly, R. J., Wood, J. J., Gonzalez, L. S., MacDonald, V., & Waterman, J. (2002).
Effects of mother-son incest and positive perceptions of sexual abuse ex-
periences on the psychosocial adjustment of clinic-referred men. Child
Abuse & Neglect, 26, 425–441.
Kendall-Tackett, K. A., Williams, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). The effects of
sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical
findings. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164–181.
Incest Victims and Offenders 153
Kendler, K., Bulik, C. M., Silberg, J., Hettema, J. M., Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A.
(2000). Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use
disorders in women: An epidemiological and cotwin control analysis.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(10), 953–959.
Kessler, B. L., & Bieschke, K. J. (1999). A retrospective analysis of shame, dis-
sociation, and adult victimization in survivors of childhood sexual abuse.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46(3), 335–341.
Ketring, S. A., & Feinauer, L. L. (1999). Perpetrator-victim relationship: Long-
term effects of sexual abuse for men and women. American Journal of Family
Therapy, 27, 109–120.
King, N. J., Tongue, B. J., Mullen, P., Myerson, N., Heyne, D., Rollings, et al.
(2000). Treating sexually abused children with posttraumatic stress symp-
toms: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1347–1355.
Kogan, S. M. (2004). Disclosing unwanted sexual experiences: Results from
a national sample of adolescent women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28,
147–165.
Kovach, J. (1986). Incest as a treatment issue for alcoholic women. Alcoholism
Treatment Quarterly, 3, 1–15.
Lawson, C. (1993). Mother-son sexual abuse: Rare or underreported? A critique
of the research. Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 261–269.
Martin, G., Bergen, H. A., Richardson, A. S., Roeger, L., & Allison, S. (2004).
Sexual abuse and suicidality: Gender differences in a large community
sample of adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 491–503.
Marvasti, J. A., & Dripchak, V. L. (2004a). Psychiatric co-morbidities in female
survivors of sexual trauma: Trauma related syndrome and revictimization.
In J. A. Marvasti (Ed.), Psychiatric treatment of victims and survivors of sexual
trauma: A neurobiological approach (pp. 19–35). Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
Marvasti, J. A., & Dripchak, V. L. (2004b). Psychopathology in survivors of
incestuous abuse: Self-mutilation, trichotillomania, restless leg syndrome,
& anorexia nervosa. In J. A. Marvasti (Ed.), Psychiatric treatment of victims
and survivors of sexual trauma: A neurobiological approach (pp. 37–50).
Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
McCloskey, L. A., & Bailey, J. A. (2000). The intergenerational transmission of
risk for child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15(10), 1019–
1035.
Merrill, L. L., Thomsen, C. J., Sinclair, B. B., Gold, S. R., & Milner, J. S. (2001).
Predicting the impact of child sexual abuse on women: The role of abuse
severity, parental support, and coping strategies. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 69(6), 992–1006.
Meyerson, L. A., Long, P. J., Miranda, R., Jr., & Marx, B. P. (2002). The
influence of childhood sexual abuse, physical abuse, family environment,
and gender on the psychological adjustment of adolescents. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 26, 387–405.
154 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Raj, A., Silverman, J. G., & Amaro, H. (2000). The relationship between sexual
abuse and sexual risk among high school students: Findings from the 1997
Massachusetts Youth Risk Behavior Survey. Maternal and Child Health
Journal, 4(2), 125–134.
Ray, S. L. (1996). Adult male survivors of incest: An exploratory study. Journal of
Child Sexual Abuse, 5, 103–114.
Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (2002). Men who molest their sexually immature
daughters: Is a special explanation required? Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
111(2), 329–339.
Rind, B., Tromovitch, P., & Bauserman, R. (1998). A meta-analytic examination
of assumed properties of child sexual abuse using college samples. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 124, 22–53.
Roberts, L., & Lie, G. (1989). A group therapy approach to treatment of incest.
Social Work with Groups, 12, 77–90.
Rodriguez, N., Vande Kemp, H., & Foy, D. W. (1998). Posttraumatic stress
disorder in survivors of childhood sexual and physical abuse: A critical
review of the empirical research. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 7(2), 17–45.
Roesler, T. A., & Wind, T. W. (1994). Telling the secret: Adult women de-
scribe their disclosures of incest. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9(3),
327–338.
Ruggiero, K. J., McLeer, S. V., & Dixon, J. F. (2000). Sexual abuse characteristics
associated with survivor psychopathology. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(7),
951–964.
Russell, D. E. H. (1986). The secret trauma: Incest in the lives of girls and women. New
York: Basic Books.
Ryan, K. D., Kilmer, R. P., Cauce, A. M., Wantanabe, H., & Hoyt, D. R.
(2000). Psychological consequences of child maltreatment in homeless
adolescents: Untangling the unique effects of maltreatment and family
environment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(3), 333–352.
Saunders, B. E., Villeponteaux, L. A., Lipovsky, J. A., Kilpatrick, D. G., &
Veronen, L. J. (1992). Child sexual assault as a risk factor for mental
disorders among women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 189–204.
Schultz, R. (1990). Secrets of adolescence: Incest and developmental fixations. In
R. P. Kluft (Ed.), Incest-related syndromes of adult psychopathology (pp. 133–
159). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Seto, M. C., Lalumiere, M. L., & Kuban, M. (1999). The sexual preferences of
incest offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(2), 267–272.
Sharpe, J., Selley, C., Low, L., & Hall, Z. (2001). Group analytic therapy for male
survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Group Analysis, 34, 195–209.
Shaw, J. A., Lewis, J. E., Loeb, A., Rosado, J., & Rodriguez, R. A. (2000). Child
on child sexual abuse: Psychological perspectives. Child Abuse & Neglect,
24(12), 1591–1600.
Sheinberg, M., & Fraenkel, P. (2001). The Relational Trauma of Incest: A family-
based approach to treatment. New York: Guilford.
156 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Sirles, E., & Franke, R. (1989). Factors influencing mothers’ reactions to in-
trafamily sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 13, 131–139.
Spiegel, D., Classen, C., Thurston, E., & Butler, L. (2004). Trauma-focused
versus present-focused models of group treatment for women sexually
abused in childhood. In L. Koenig, L. S. Doll, A. O’Leary, and W. Pe-
quegnat (Eds.) From child sexual abuse to adult sexual risk: Trauma, re-
victimization, and intervention (pp. 251–268). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Spiegel, J. (2003). Sexual abuse of males: The SAM model of theory and practice. New
York: Brunner-Routledge.
Steel, J., Sanna, L., Hammond, B., Whipple, J., & Cross, H. (2004). Psychological
sequelae of childhood sexual abuse: Abuse-related characteristics, coping
strategies, and attributional style. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 785–801.
Stevenson, J. (1999). The treatment of long-term sequelae of child abuse. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 89–111.
Stroud, D. D. (1999). Familial support as perceived by adult victims of childhood
sexual abuse. Sexual Abuse, 11, 159–175.
Tardif, M., Auclair, N., Jacob, M., & Carpentier, J. (2005). Sexual abuse per-
petrated by adult and juvenile females: An ultimate attempt to resolve
a conflict associated with maternal identity. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29,
153–167.
Teusch, R. (2001). Substance abuse as a symptom of childhood sexual abuse.
Psychiatric Service, 52, 1530–1532.
Tsai, M., Feldman-Summers, S., & Edgar, M. (1979). Childhood molestation:
Variables related to differential impact on psychosexual functioning in
adult women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 407–417.
U.S. Department of Justice. (2000). Sexual assault of young children as reported to law
enforcement: Victim, incident, and offender characteristics. (NCJ 182990). Wash-
ington, DC: Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics.
Vandiver, D. M., & Kercher, G. (2004). Offender and victim characteristics of
registered female sex offenders in Texas: A proposed typology of female
sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16(2),
121–137.
van Gerko, K., Hughes, M. L., Hamill, M., & Waller, G. (2005). Reported
childhood sexual abuse and eating-disordered cognitions and behaviors.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 29, 375–382.
Voth, P. F., & Tutty, L. M. (1999). Daughter’s perceptions of being mothered by
an incest survivor: A phenomenological study. Journal of Child Sexual
Abuse, 8(3), 25–43.
Waller, G. (1992). Sexual abuse and the severity of bulimic symptomology. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 90–93.
Waller, G., Hamilton, K., Rose, N., Sumra, J., & Baldwin, G. (1993). Sexual
abuse and body-image distortion in the eating disorders. British Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 32, 350–352.
Incest Victims and Offenders 157
Webster, S. D., & Beech, A. R. (2001). The nature of sexual offenders’ affective
empathy: A grounded theory analysis. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research
and Treatment, 12, 249–261.
Weiss, E. L., Longhurst, J. G., & Mazure, C. M. (1999). Childhood sexual abuse
as a risk factor for depression in women: Psychosocial and neurobiological
correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(6), 816–828.
Westbury, E., & Tutty, L. M. (1999). The efficacy of group treatment for sur-
vivors of childhood abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 31–44.
Wilson, R. J. (1999). Emotional congruence in sexual offenders against children.
Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 11, 33–48.
Wind, T. W., & Silvern, L. (1992). Type and extent of child abuse as predictors of
adult functioning. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 261–281.
Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Mitchell, J. E., Thompson, K. M., Redlin, J.,
Demuth, G., et al. (2001). Eating disturbance and sexual trauma in childhood
and adulthood. New York: Wiley.
Wyatt, G. E., & Newcomb, M. (1990). Internal and external mediators of
women’s sexual abuse in childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 58, 758–767.
Ystgaard, M., Hestetun, I., Loeb, M., & Mehlum, L. (2004). Is there a specific
relationship between childhood sexual and physical abuse and repeated
suicidal behavior? Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 863–875.
Zlotnick, C., Mattia, J., & Zimmerman, M. (2001). Clinical features of survivors
of sexual abuse with major depression. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 357–367.
Zlotnick, C., Ryan, C. E., Miller, I. W., & Keitner, G. I. (1995). Childhood
abuse and recovery from major depression. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19,
1513–1516.
Zlotnick, C., Shea, T. M., Rosen, K., Simpson, E., Mulrenin, K., Begin, A., et al.
(1997). An affect-management group for women with posttraumatic stress
disorder and histories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
10, 425–436.
Zlotnick, C., Warshaw, M., Shea, M. T., & Keller, M. B. (1997). Trauma and
chronic depression among patients with anxiety disorders. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 333–336.
7
INTRODUCTION
The term ‘‘sex offender’’ can be defined as any individual who, because of the
nature of his or her sexual behavior, has come into contact with the legal
system. The sexual behavior for which the individual has gotten into trouble
might have been coercive in nature, as in the case of rape, or it might have
been for what is known in psychology as paraphilic behavior. Paraphilias,
formerly known as sexual deviations, represent a group of heterogeneous
disorders. The common theme in this group is that they involve sexual urges,
fantasies, and behaviors that are viewed as atypical and often socially unac-
ceptable (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The essential features of a
paraphilia in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-
TR, a diagnostic guide used by mental health professionals) are recurrent,
intense sexually arousing fantasies, sexual urges, or behaviors generally in-
volving (1) nonhuman objects, (2) the suffering or humiliation of oneself or
one’s partner, or (3) children or other nonconsenting persons. For some cli-
ents, the atypical fantasies are essential for sexual arousal and, therefore, they
are always incorporated into sexual behavior, even if just in fantasy. For others,
the atypical urges and fantasies are occasional, and they may still engage in
normative sexual practices.
The DSM-IV-TR states that paraphilias, regardless of their prevalence, are
typically persistent; however, they do sometimes diminish with age. Because
160 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
perpetrators of sexual offenses are most commonly men, we will limit our
discussion to the treatment of male sex offenders.
It is important to remember that not all atypical sexual behaviors fall into
the sex offense category. Likewise, what is considered deviant behavior can
change and vary across cultures. As a result, treatment is generally limited to
those individuals whose sexual behavior involves nonconsenting partners,
including pedophilia, exhibitionism, voyeurism, and frotteurism, or to those
individuals whose atypical sexual arousal is personally problematic. It has been
found that men whose sexual fantasies center on fetishes, including cross-
dressing or masochism, rarely request treatment; they seek a consenting partner
who either shares or tolerates their atypical sexual interests (McAnulty &
Burnette, 2004).
THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS
In order to effectively treat sex offenders, it is important to have a thorough
understanding of how atypical sexual arousal is developed and maintained.
Behavioral modification is the main component of most contemporary treat-
ment programs for paraphilias and other types of sex offenses (Abel et al., 1984;
Barnard, Fuller, Robbins, & Shaw, 1989). Behavioral treatments are based on
the assumption that atypical sexual interest is primarily learned behavior.
Conditioning, the process whereby learning occurs, can either be operant or
classical in nature. In the case of atypical sexual arousal, it is thought that a
combination of both types of conditioning is responsible for the learning pro-
cess. Operant conditioning generally occurs when a reinforcing consequence
immediately follows a response and increases the future frequency of that re-
sponse, or when an aversive consequence immediately follows a response and
decreases the future frequency of that response. Classical conditioning occurs
when a neutral stimulus acquires the eliciting properties of an unconditioned
stimulus through pairing the unconditioned stimulus with a neutral stimulus.
As for the role of conditioning of atypical sexual interest, with regard
to classical conditioning, a neutral, or in this case previously nonsexually
arousing, stimulus becomes sexually arousing by repeatedly being paired with
a stimulus that is sexually arousing. The sexually arousing stimulus that the
neutral stimulus is paired with may be an unconditioned, or unlearned stim-
ulus, or it might be a conditioned, or learned stimulus. Operant conditioning,
in this situation, would be important in the reinforcement of behavior as
sexually arousing. Specifically, masturbation to atypical fantasies will increase
their strength to sexually arouse and also function as mental rehearsals in which
future sexual offenses are planned (Marshall & Barbaree, 1990). Evidence for
the relationship between atypical stimuli and orgasm is provided by a few early
analogue laboratory studies. These studies (Rachman, 1966; Rachman &
Hodgson, 1968) paired pictures of boots to nude pictures of adult women and
Treatment of Sex Offenders 161
led to increased responding (i.e., sexual arousal) to the fetishistic stimuli (i.e.,
the boots). Abel and Blanchard (1974), Evans (1968), Laws and Marshall
(1991), and McGuire, Carlisle, & Young (1965) provide support for the use of
masturbation to alter sexual interests; however, Herman, Barlow, and Agras
(1974) and Marshall (1974) found it difficult to produce such arousal. It is
important to remember that while atypical sexual arousal can be conditioned
in a laboratory, this does not provide proof that atypical sexual interests de-
velop along similar lines (Laws & O’Donohue, 1997). Regardless of how it
developed, inappropriate or atypical sexual arousal is thought to be a signifi-
cant factor in the cause and maintenance of sexual offending (Ward, Hudson,
& Keenan, 2000), and, as such, learning to have a sexual preference for, and
arousal to, consenting adult partners is viewed as necessary for changing one’s
sexual practices (Ward & Stewart, 2003).
There is good evidence that atypical sexual behavior, at least in the short
term, is amenable to behavioral modification techniques (Feierman & Feier-
man, 2000), which are based on the above theoretical assumptions. The goal is
to decrease the strength of the atypical arousal and to increase normative sexual
arousal. Support for these principles comes from several lines of research.
Stava, Levin, and Schwanz (1993), for example, demonstrated that it was the
aversive component of covert sensitization trials, rather than merely distraction
or habituation, that was responsible for reductions in sexual arousal to pedo-
philic stimuli in a 30-year-old pedophile.
Broadly speaking, treatment for sex offenders includes strategies to de-
crease atypical arousal and increase appropriate arousal. However, because men
who commit sex offenses often have multiple deficits, including social skills
deficits, poor impulse control, and low victim empathy, most treatment
programs employ multiple modalities to address these problems. Likewise,
treatment for sex offenders may also include skills training, sex education, and
cognitive restructuring. Finally, relapse prevention is included in most sex of-
fender treatment programs as a means of preventing the offender from com-
mitting another sex offense.
Aversion Therapy
The goal of aversion therapy is to decrease the sexually arousing prop-
erties of atypical fantasies and images; this is accomplished by pairing the
atypical fantasies and images with an aversive stimulus. McAnulty and Adams
(1992) noted that while there does appear to be evidence in the literature
for the effectiveness of aversive conditioning in treating paraphilic disorders,
the use of aversion therapy is sometimes challenged on ethical and moral
grounds.
There are two types of aversion therapy, differentiated primarily by the
type of aversive stimulus that is used. Electrical aversion involves the use of a
mild but painful shock (McAnulty & Adams, 1992). Olfactory aversion in-
volves the use of a noxious odor, such as ammonia. For each type of aversion
therapy, the aversive stimuli are immediately presented when the individual
engages in atypical imagery or fantasy.
Covert Sensitization
Another form of aversive conditioning, covert sensitization, was intro-
duced by Cautela (1967). Just as with other forms of aversive conditioning, the
purpose of this procedure is to decrease the level of an undesired behavior.
While covert sensitization is a form of aversive conditioning, it does not involve
the actual presentation of an aversive stimulus. Cautela claimed that this pro-
cedure is covert in that the aversive stimuli are presented in the imagination
only.
In this procedure the individual is asked to fantasize using his atypical
fantasy; however, before the fantasy reaches the point of actually engaging
in the atypical behavior, the individual is instructed to imagine an aversive
image (e.g., getting caught by the police, spending time in jail, etc.) as a way
to reduce the sexually arousing properties of the atypical fantasy (Cautela,
1967). In order for this procedure to be most effective, it is typically re-
commended that the aversive image that is used be one that the client would
find most aversive. Likewise, images that are realistic are likely to be most
effective.
With regard to effectiveness, in a review of the literature, Little and
Curran (1978) noted that there have been several controlled, within-subject
studies that provide empirical support for the use of covert sensitization in the
treatment of sexual deviance. Likewise, Brownell, Hayes, and Barlow (1977)
effectively used covert sensitization in the treatment of two exhibitionists, a
sadist, a transvestite, and a child molester. Using a combination of orgasmic
reconditioning, described below, and covert sensitization, Lande (1980) suc-
cessfully treated an individual with a history of fire setting accompanied by
masturbation.
Treatment of Sex Offenders 163
Masturbatory Satiation
The technique of masturbatory satiation is intended to decrease an indi-
vidual’s arousal to atypical fantasies by pairing the atypical fantasies with
boredom. When using the procedure, the individual is instructed to mastur-
bate to orgasm while fantasizing about something that is normal or appro-
priate. Once the individual has reached orgasm, he is instructed to continue
masturbating for forty-five minutes to one hour during which time the pre-
ferred atypical fantasy is used (Witt & Sager, 1988). Continuing to masturbate
to an atypical fantasy after orgasm is punishing to the person, rather than
rewarding and makes the atypical fantasy less exciting and, therefore, less likely
to be used in the future. Using this technique the person learns that the normal
fantasies help him have an orgasm and that the atypical fantasies produce
boredom and may cause him pain or embarrassment.
Medication Management
The use of medications to treat sex offenders is controversial. Grubin
(2000) pointed out that some cognitive-behavioral therapists perceive the use
of medications as ‘‘cheating’’ and that the use of medications might suggest to
the offender that his ability to control his offending is limited because his
sexual drives are not completely under his power. The most commonly used
classes of medications are antiandrogens and selective serotonin reuptake in-
hibitors (SSRIs).
Hyde and DeLamater (2006) stated that sexual arousability is dependent
on maintaining the level of androgen in the bloodstream above a certain level.
Therefore, antiandrogen drugs are sometimes used either to reduce the pro-
duction of androgen or to block the effects of androgen. The two most
commonly used antiandrogen medications are cyproterone acetate and me-
droxyprogesterone acetate (Depo-Provera). By either blocking the production
of androgens or blocking their effects, these medications reduce sexual drive as
well as the individual’s ability to respond physically (i.e., get an erection) to
sexual stimuli (Grubin, 2000).
The SSRIs, frequently prescribed for depression and anxiety, are also
sometimes used in the treatment of sex offenders. In their review of the
literature, Greenberg and Bradford (1997) reported that SSRIs have been
useful in reducing fantasies, sexual urges, masturbation, and paraphilic behavior
in exhibitionists, fetishists, voyeurs, and child molesters. Grubin (2000) sug-
gested that SSRIs may be most beneficial for those individuals whose sexual
offending has an obsessive-compulsive quality to it.
Hyde and DeLamater (2006) suggested that antiandrogen and SSRIs in the
treatment of sex offenders should only be used as one element of a more
comprehensive treatment program. Likewise, Grubin (2000) stated that most
164 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
men want to quit taking the medication at some point as a test, because of side
effects, or because they are unhappy with being asexual. Regardless of the
reason, it is important that individuals who have committed sex offenses have
other skills at their disposal to prevent committing another offense. It should
also be mentioned that the use of these medications, just as with any other
medication, does not guarantee the desired effect.
Orgasmic Reconditioning
Orgasmic reconditioning is one of the techniques designed to enhance
arousal to appropriate stimuli (e.g., adult heterosexual and/or homosexual
partners) by pairing appropriate stimuli with orgasm. It is also sometimes re-
ferred to as directed masturbation, masturbation training, or masturbatory re-
conditioning; however, some of these terms have specific meanings. According
to McAnulty and Adams (1992), ‘‘The rationale for orgasmic reconditioning is
based on the assumption that stimuli acquire sexually arousing properties
through their pairing with pleasurable sensations, namely sexual arousal and
orgasm’’ (p. 188). There are currently four distinct forms of masturbatory re-
conditioning: (a) thematic shift (Marquis, 1970; Thorpe, Schmidt, & Castell,
1964); (b) fantasy alternation (Abel, Blanchard, Barlow & Flanagan, 1975; Van-
Deventer & Laws, 1978); (c) directed masturbation (Kremsdorf, Holmen, &
Laws, 1980; Maletzky, 1985); and (d) satiation (Marshall & Lippens, 1977). The
most frequently used type of orgasmic reconditioning, and the one that we will
discuss, is thematic shift.
Thematic shift orgasmic reconditioning, as originally used by Marquis
(1970), required the client to masturbate using atypical stimuli until the point
of ejaculatory inevitability, at which time the client was to switch to an
appropriate fantasy. The aim is to increase the attractiveness of the conven-
tional fantasy by such association (Hawton, 1983). As therapy progresses, the
client is instructed to begin using an appropriate fantasy earlier and earlier in
his masturbatory sessions until he can ultimately use appropriate fantasies from
the beginning of masturbation to orgasm (McAnulty & Adams, 1992). Initially,
Treatment of Sex Offenders 165
the client may lose his arousal and erection when he switches to the appro-
priate fantasy. If this happens, the client is instructed to switch to his preferred
fantasy to achieve a high level of arousal and then return to the appropriate
fantasy. Bancroft (1974) found that gradual reshaping of atypical fantasies is
more effective than trying to masturbate without using the atypical/nonnor-
mative fantasy at all.
Quinsey and Earls (1990) concluded that evidence for the effectiveness of
orgasmic reconditioning is limited in depth and significance. Laws and Mar-
shall (1991), while optimistic, stated that there are insufficient data to conclude
that orgasmic reconditioning is a clearly effective treatment for sexual devia-
tions of any kind over the long term. Several studies (Davison, 1968; Marquis,
1970) reported success in using orgasmic reconditioning with clients, but
treatment did not rely on orgasmic reconditioning exclusively. Marshall and
Eccles (1991) consider orgasmic reconditioning to be one component of a
comprehensive program to modify one’s sexual behavior.
SKILLS TRAINING
treatment to enable the use of these skills. Usually social skills are inhibited by
anxiety/irrational fears. The individual may require some anxiety reduction
technique, such as desensitization, that is, helping the person to relax in the
presence of something that makes him anxious or scared; or perhaps some
cognitive restructuring, which is explained below, to challenge rationaliza-
tions, or the thoughts people have that make it okay for them to engage in
atypical behavior, and change distorted thinking (McMullin, 1986; Murphy,
1990).
Assertiveness Training
Stermac and Quinsey (1986) found that a significant number of rapists lack
social competence skills, particularly assertiveness skills. Likewise, it is often the
case that sex offenders need to be taught the difference between assertive,
aggressive, passive, and passive-aggressive behaviors. An individual who pos-
sesses assertive skills is able to respond to problematic situations by making
requests when something is wanted and by refusing inappropriate requests
(Schroeder & Black, 1985). When acting in an assertive manner, one protects
his rights as well as the rights of others (Lange & Jakubowski, 1976). When
acting aggressively, one is protecting one’s rights but trampling on the rights of
others. An aggressive person may overact in some situations, become angry,
and be abusive toward others; these types of people usually want things done
their way and do not take turns or negotiate fairly (Dow, 1994). A person who
is passive is allowing his rights to be neglected while protecting the rights of
others. A passive person may not express opinions, does not refuse unreason-
able requests, and allows others to have their way most of the time. A person
who is passive-aggressive acts as if he is subjugating his rights and protecting the
rights of others but will trample on the rights of others behind their backs.
Individuals who are constantly passive may lash out in an aggressive manner
when they get tired of having their rights violated. The goal of assertiveness
training is to teach individuals that everybody has rights and how to protect
their rights without violating the rights of others. Cognitive restructuring may
be necessary to help the offender realize that he has rights and that it is alright to
say no to certain requests or that it is alright to make appropriate requests of
others. Individuals with low self-esteem and negative core beliefs, such as ‘‘I am
stupid’’ or ‘‘I am worthless,’’ often place the rights of others ahead of their own
rights. When this happens the person often feels angry and taken advantage of
and may act out sexually in an atypical way to get even or to feel better. Several
models of assertiveness training can be used, depending upon the situation.
Basic assertion involves a simple expression of standing up for one’s rights,
beliefs, feelings, or opinions and can also be used to express affection (Lange &
Jakubowski, 1976). Empathic assertion allows you to convey sensitivity to
another and is useful in situations in which you have a relationship with an
individual (Lange & Jakubowski, 1976). Escalating assertion (Rimm & Masters,
Treatment of Sex Offenders 167
1974) involves starting with a response that can accomplish the speaker’s goal
with a minimum of effort and negative emotion and has a small possibility of
negative consequences (Lange & Jakubowski, 1976). If, however, the other
person fails to respond to a request and continues to violate one’s rights, the
speaker escalates the assertion and becomes increasingly firm. The broken
record model of assertiveness (Smith, 1975) is probably best used in situations in
which one does not have a relationship with the individual and does not plan
to start such a relationship. When using the broken record model, the indi-
vidual basically sounds like a record that is stuck repeating the same phrase over
and over.
Anger Management
For some offenders, sex and aggression are inextricably linked. Individuals
develop scripts for interpersonal relationships through their observations and
interactions with others. Negative events from their past, such as poor parenting,
parental rejection, inconsistent and harsh discipline, violence between parents,
physical and sexual abuse, being exposed to inappropriate models, as well as
many others can lead to distorted internal dialogue and a faulty belief system
about one’s environment (Fagen & Wexler, 1988; Marshall & Barbaree, 1990).
These aggressive cognitive scripts that develop throughout one’s childhood and
adolescence could become blueprints for aggression depending on whether the
behaviors exhibited are punished or rewarded (Huesmann, 1988). The aim of
anger management is to reduce the intensity of the anger that is experienced and
to control the way the individual behaves when anger is elicited.
Turkat (1990) proposed treating aggression problems using graduated
exposure to stimuli that elicit anger. He suggested constructing a hierarchy of
anger-eliciting stimuli and training the individual to engage in a competing
response, such as distracting oneself with another thought or activity or using a
relaxation technique to get rid of the anger. The idea is that one cannot be
angry and relaxed at the same time. Cognitive restructuring, or changing how
one thinks about something, can be used with offenders who hold adversarial
attitudes toward their partners and for those who use interpersonal violence to
attain desired goals. Skills training—for example, assertiveness and social skills
training—may be necessary to supplement the offender’s armament of tools to
use in interpersonal relationships. Teaching the client to take a time-out is also
a good technique to allow him to compose his thoughts and calm down before
he responds when he is angry. Stress management and communication skills
training may be implemented if necessary.
empathy plays a major role in the etiology and maintenance of sex offend-
ing. The data from studies assessing empathy in sex offenders, however, have
provided mixed results (Geer, Estupinan, & Manguno-Mire, 2000). Re-
searchers examining empathy in sex offenders have recently begun to in-
vestigate the nature of empathic responding to determine if this is a general
deficit or if it is circumscribed to a class of victims. Fernandez, Marshall,
Lightbody, and O’Sullivan (1999) assessed the level of empathy in child
molesters and a control group of nonoffenders for three types of victims: an
accident victim, a general sexual abuse victim, and their own victim. They
found that child molesters displayed the same amount of empathy as non-
offenders toward the victim of an accident. However, relative to the accident
victim, the child molesters demonstrated a deficit in empathy toward a general
sexual abuse victim, that is, not their own victim. Similarly, the child mo-
lesters displayed significantly less empathy toward their own victim than to-
ward the general sexual abuse victim. This finding is important as it calls into
question a long-held assumption that sex offenders lack empathy/lack the
ability to experience empathy. Despite the lack of clear-cut evidence for the
role of empathy, most treatment programs for sex offenders include a com-
ponent designed to increase an offender’s capacity for victim empathy to
reduce recidivism.
SEX EDUCATION
Barbaree and Seto (1997) suggested that sex education be included in a
comprehensive treatment program for sex offenders. Given that myths and
misinformation about sexuality abound, it is likely that an offender lacks in-
formation and/or has incorrect information. The aim of sex education is to
make the individual more comfortable with sexual information and to im-
prove one’s sexual skills by providing comprehensive knowledge of the sexual
anatomy, sexual response, sexual technique, and communication skills. Kolvin
(1967) suggested that sexual education, counseling, and reassurance alone
could generate behavior change. Sex education can be provided in a group
format, and/or self-help books can be given to the client to read on his own
time.
SEX THERAPY
Clients who have a sexual dysfunction may require sex therapy to correct
the problem. Conditions such as erectile disorder, premature ejaculation, or
delayed ejaculation may cause the client to be embarrassed or to get angry and
act out with his partner. The end result is that the person could seek out
nonconsenting or underage partners and become abusive if provoked. The
goal of sex therapy is to restore normal functioning so that he will be com-
fortable with his sexual performance and will hopefully seek out appropriate
Treatment of Sex Offenders 169
partners, or be comfortable with his dysfunction and learn other ways to please
his partner. There are empirically validated techniques for treating sexual
dysfunctions and these can be administered in individual therapy. Leiblum and
Rosen (2000) and Wincze and Carey (2001) offer treatment recommendations
for sexual dysfunctions.
COGNITIVE RESTRUCTURING
Our thoughts, appraisals, and expectancies can elicit or modulate our mood
and physiological processes, influence the environment, and serve as stimuli for
behavior (Turk, Rudy, & Sorkin, 1992). Conversely, mood, physiology, en-
vironmental factors, and behavior can influence thought processes (Turk et al.,
1992). The goals of cognitive therapy are to help the client identify and correct
maladaptive thoughts, to retrain the client to think more logically and realis-
tically, and to modify any irrational core beliefs (Abel et al., 1984; Turk et al.,
1992). Murphy (1990) found that sexual aggressors reported such cognitive
distortions as claims that the victim enjoyed the assault, blaming the victim, and
a general belief in rape myths. This self-deceptive and distorted thinking, which
is based on false assumptions, misperceptions, and self-serving interpretations,
helps the sex offender justify his behavior (Feierman & Feierman, 2000). The
application of cognitive-behavioral therapy to sexual disorders evolved from
the research on anxiety disorders and depression.
Cognitive restructuring involves teaching the client to challenge irrational
attitudes and beliefs, not only about sexuality, but also about how he views the
world and life in general. The client is taught to self-monitor his thoughts, to
recognize maladaptive thought patterns, and to log his irrational thoughts on
tracking sheets that are used in therapy to monitor progress (Beck, Rush,
Shaw, & Emery, 1979). The client is instructed to dispute the irrational
thoughts and state evidence as to why the thoughts are irrational. The client is
then encouraged to state a rational response and provide evidence for the
validity of this response. Clients usually have difficulty recognizing their dis-
torted thoughts and may argue that their distorted thoughts are accurate since
they have evidence as to the veracity of their thoughts and beliefs. However,
the evidence provided by the client can usually be identified as another type of
cognitive distortion. As the client becomes better at identifying his irrational
thoughts and beliefs, he reduces the amount of time spent thinking irrationally
and is able to make better decisions that lead to more desirable outcomes.
Once the client starts to change his attitude and thought patterns and begins to
see events and situations more realistically, the therapist can begin to work on
the client’s core beliefs to help the client see himself in a more positive
manner. Negative core beliefs such as ‘‘I’m stupid’’ or ‘‘I’m powerless’’ can be
tackled and replaced by more accurate beliefs once the client no longer en-
gages in distorted thinking. By experiencing these negative core beliefs about
oneself—for example, ‘‘I’m stupid’’—the person feels inadequate around
170 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
people his own age. He then spends time with individuals younger than
himself, perhaps children, so that he feels smarter than them, which can lead to
sexual abuse. If a man has a negative core belief that he is powerless, he might
rape a woman or a child to prove that he has power over that person. By
helping the offender to realize that the negative core belief is incorrect, he can
then develop an accurate core belief that does not lead to distorted thinking
and/or inappropriate behavior.
An alternative to using cognitive restructuring to help clients change their
thinking in order to change their negative feelings or problem behavior is to use
acceptance-based therapy to help people accept their negative thoughts and
feelings rather than change them (Hayes, Stossahl, & Wilson, 1999; Hayes &
Wilson, 1994). Acceptance, in this paradigm, refers to the willingness to expe-
rience a full range of thoughts, emotions, memories, bodily states, and behav-
ioral predispositions, including those that are problematic, without necessarily
having to change them, escape from them, act on them, or avoid them (Paul,
Marx, & Orsillo, 1999). According to LoPiccolo (1994), using acceptance-
based therapies allows the client to relinquish the struggle to gain control over
his thoughts, which then allows him to develop and engage in more adaptive,
alternative behaviors.
RELAPSE PREVENTION
Relapse prevention is a self-control program designed to teach individuals
who are trying to change their behavior how to anticipate and cope with the
problem of relapse (Laws, 1989, p. 2). The aim of relapse prevention is to
prevent the recurrence of a problematic behavior (Hanson, 2000; Ward &
Hudson, 1998), which is frequently accomplished by helping the individual
identify and control or avoid triggers of the behavior (Hyde & DeLamater,
2006). Relapse prevention is frequently used as an adjunct to cognitive-
behavioral therapy but has also been used as a stand-alone program. The pro-
gram has a psychoeducational thrust that combines behavioral skills training,
cognitive therapy, and lifestyle change (Larimer & Marlatt, 1994; Laws, 1989).
This method of treatment teaches coping strategies to avoid lapses, which are
viewed as opportunities to learn which stimuli control behavior, and relapses,
which are viewed as failures (Maletzky, 1997). The model, as described by
Pithers (1990), is based on the work of Marlatt and Gordon (1985), who
developed this procedure for treating addictive behaviors.
Behavior chains and cycles are central concepts to relapse prevention
(Maletzky, 1997). A sex offense is viewed as a sequence or chain of behaviors
that ultimately leads to the offending behavior. A number of antecedents and
assumptions precede the final act. With relapse prevention, the client is taught
to analyze the chain of behaviors and assumptions that lead to the offending
behavior. Clients are taught the value of breaking the chain of behaviors as
early in the chain as possible to avert another offense. In the early stages of
Treatment of Sex Offenders 171
treatment the client is instructed to keep records of his lapses and triggers to
create self-awareness and self-scrutiny (Maletzky, 1997). By helping the client
anticipate events that predispose a lapse—that is, making a mistake, such as
a child molester wanting to engage in sexual fantasies about children (as op-
posed to actually masturbating to fantasies about children, which would be a
relapse)—and by having escape strategies to exit high-risk situations, lapses can
often be circumvented. Additionally, stimulus control procedures are put in
place to make the client accountable and to decrease offending opportunities.
If a client has a relapse, the event is used as an opportunity to learn from his
mistakes, and he is encouraged to begin using his treatment plan immediately.
In summary, the essential components of relapse prevention include:
does seem to help some offenders. Hall (1995) concluded that treatment
produces a 30 percent reduction in recidivism. Treatment outcome rates, on a
short-term basis, are significantly better than no treatment at all and often rival
the outcome rates for many other DSM-IV-TR psychiatric disorders (Feier-
man & Feierman, 2000).
Treatment plans should be tailored to the individual offender based on his
need and risk level in order to increase his chances of overcoming his prob-
lems. Anyone working with sexual offenders should be prepared to be patient
and flexible because many offenders do not wish to change but are forced to
do so as a result of external pressure, usually a family member or the legal
system. Laws (2003) stated that resistance should be viewed as a phase of
treatment rather than an obstacle. Therapists must help clients work through
the resistance, or precontemplation, phase before contemplating actual change
(Laws, 2003).
In most cases, any treatment technique in isolation will be ineffective and a
combination of procedures is likely to be needed (Hawton, 1983). The client’s
unique behavioral excesses and deficits should guide the therapist in choosing
which techniques to use. Assessment should include the presence of disin-
hibiting factors such as alcohol or drugs, stress or emotional states, the use of
pornography, and the role of atypical fantasies in the commission of a sexual
offense (Finkelhor, 1984). Treatment for sex offenders should generally cover a
variety of issues such as distorted cognitions, sexual issues, victim empathy,
social skills training, problem-solving, life skills, stress management, and relapse
prevention training (Hudson, Marshall, Ward, Johnston, & Jones, 1995). We
concur with Grossman, Martis, and Fichtner (1999), who concluded, ‘‘What
emerges from the literature is a strong suggestion that a comprehensive
cognitive-behavioral program should involve components that reduce atyp-
ical arousal while increasing appropriate arousal and should include cognitive
restructuring, social skills training, victim empathy awareness, and relapse pre-
vention’’ (p. 360). The research assessing the treatment of sex offenders dem-
onstrates that treatment does seem to reduce recidivism among sexual offenders.
REFERENCES
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. B., Cunningham-Rathner, J., Rouleau, J. L., Kaplan, M., &
Reich, J. (1984). Treatment manual: The treatment of child molesters. (Available
from G. G. Abel, Behavioral Medicine Institute of Atlanta, 1401 Peachtree
street, Northeast Atlanta, GA 30309).
Abel, G. G., & Blanchard, E. B. (1974). The role of fantasy in the treatment of
sexual deviation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 467–475.
Abel, G. G., Blanchard, E. B., Barlow, D. H., & Flanagan, B. (1975, December).
A case report of the behavioral treatment of a sadistic rapist. Paper presented at
the 9th Annual Convention of the Association for the Advancement of
Behavior Therapy, San Francisco, CA.
Treatment of Sex Offenders 173
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York: Free
Press.
Geer, J. H., Estupinan, L. A., & Manguno-Mire, G. M. (2000). Empathy, social
skills, and other relevant cognitive processes in child molesters. Aggression
and Violent Behavior, 5, 99–126.
Greenberg, D., & Bradford, J. M. W. (1997). Treatment of the paraphilic dis-
orders: A review of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Sexual
Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 9, 349–360.
Grossman, L. S., Martis, B., & Fichtner, C. G. (1999). Are sex offenders treatable?
A research overview. Psychiatric Services, 50, 349–361.
Grubin, D. (2000). Complementing relapse prevention with medical intervention.
In D. R. Laws, S. M. Hudson, & T. Ward (Eds.), Remaking relapse prevention
with sex offenders: A sourcebook (pp. 201–212). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hall, G. C. N. (1995). Sexual offender recidivism revisited: A meta-analysis of
recent treatment studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63,
802–809.
Hanson, R. K. (2000). What is so special about relapse prevention? In D. R. Laws,
S. M. Hudson, & T. Ward (Eds.), Remaking relapse prevention with sex
offenders: A sourcebook (pp. 3–26). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1998). Predicting relapse: A meta-analysis of
sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
chology, 66, 348–362.
Hawton, K. (1983). Behavioural approaches to the management of sexual devi-
ations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 248–255.
Hayes, S. C., Stossahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment
therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford.
Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1994). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
Altering the verbal support for experiential avoidance. Behavior Analyst,
17, 289–304.
Herman, S. H., Barlow, D. H., & Agras, W. S. (1974). An experimental analysis
of exposure to ‘‘elicit’’ heterosexual stimuli as an effective variable in
changing arousal patterns in homosexuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
12, 315–345.
Hudson, S. M., Marshall, W. L., Ward, T., Johnston, P. W., & Jones, R. L. (1995).
Kia Marama: A cognitive-behavioural program for incarcerated child
molesters. Behaviour Change, 12, 69–80.
Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information-processing model for the development
of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 13–24.
Hyde, J. S., & DeLamater, J. D. (2006). Understanding human sexuality (9th ed.).
Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Kolvin, I. (1967). Aversion imagery treatment in an adolescent. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 5, 245–248.
Kremsdorf, R. B., Holmen, M. L., & Laws, D. R. (1980). Orgasmic re-
conditioning without deviant imagery: A case report with a pedophile.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 203–207.
Treatment of Sex Offenders 175
SUGGESTED READINGS
Laws, D. R., & O’Donohue, W. (Eds.). (1997). Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment,
and treatment. New York: Guilford.
Ward, T., Laws, D. R., & Hudson, S. M. (Eds.). (2003). Sexual deviance: Issues and
controversies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wincze, J. P., & Carey, M. P. (2001). Sexual dysfunction: A guide for assessment and
treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
8
problematic for effective sex offender management using this approach. Sec-
ond, we describe the relatively new approach to sex offender management
called the Good Lives Model (GLM) and describe how the GLM incorporates
the risk management principles while also adopting a much more holistic
approach to sex offender management. Finally, we outline how treatment
could be implemented using the GLM and draw main conclusions about this
approach and its strengths. We note from the outset that we use male pro-
nouns when referring to offenders. Although there is increasing recognition of
women as perpetrators, by far the vast majority of sexual offenses are com-
mitted by men, and almost all research has been conducted with male of-
fenders.
(historical) risk factors for sexual recidivism than the dynamic (psychological)
risk factors. Figure 8.1 summarizes the static and dynamic risk factors identified
in the two most recent and complementary meta-analyses on sexual recidivism
(see Hanson & Bussière, 1998; Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2004). Of note,
the risk factors presented are those that were consistently associated with sexual
recidivism across several studies (each risk factor is aggregated across three to
thirty-one studies). Also, the risk predictors vary in their predictive strength;
some are more strongly associated with future sexual recidivism than others.
For example, sexual deviancy and antisocial/psychopathic personality were
found to be stronger predictors of sexual recidivism than intimacy deficits or
adverse childhood environment.
Research has only more recently begun to address the situational or acute
risk factors for sexual recidivism. Results from the most comprehensive study
of acute risk factors are presented in Figure 8.2 (see Hanson & Harris, 2000).
Situational or acute risk factors are usually not predictive of sexual recidivism
over the longer term, but instead they indicate when a particular offender
is more likely to reoffend. For example, personal distress variables, such as
negative mood, show no or only a very weak association with future sexual
recidivism in studies of large groups of sexual offenders (Hanson & Bussière,
1998; Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2004), but in those offenders who do
recidivate, sexually negative mood is often reported in the relapse process. One
way of understanding this fact is that negative mood per se does not predict
future sexual recidivism across offenders but the way that negative mood is
managed by offenders (e.g., regulating mood using deviant sexual fantasy and/
or masturbation), particularly by those at higher risk for reoffending, is linked
to sexual recidivism (Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2004).
Risk management has become synonymous with the Risk-Need ap-
proach. According to the Risk-Need Model (RNM), effective treatment
depends upon classifying offenders according to three main principles: Risk,
Need, and Responsivity (Andrews & Bonta, 2003). First, the risk principle states
that offenders’ risk of reoffending should be assessed and that the intensity of
treatment delivered to the offender should match this assessed level of risk.
Thus, according to the risk principle, offenders with highest risk should re-
ceive the most intense treatment (i.e., the largest ‘‘dose’’), whereas offenders
with lowest risk should receive minimal or no treatment. In practice, risk
assessment instruments combine a number of risk factors to produce an esti-
mate of risk. Thus, the more risk factors present for an offender, the higher the
level of assessed risk and in turn the greater the intensity of recommended
treatment.
Second, the need principle specifies that treatment should primarily target
dynamic risk factors (i.e., those factors potentially amenable to change) asso-
ciated with risk of future offending. In the RNM, psychological and situa-
tional dynamic risk factors are relabeled as criminogenic needs.1 Examples of
sexual offending criminogenic needs are deviant sexual arousal, intimacy
184 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Self-management
Victim access
Sees self as no risk to recidivate
Attitudes
Low remorse/victim blaming
Psychological Symptoms
Anger
Negative mood
Psychiatric symptoms
General hygiene problems
Drug Use
Substance abuse
Started anti-androgens (sex drive reduction medication) the month before
recidivating
Social Adjustment
General social problems
Cooperation with Supervision
Overall cooperation with supervision (low)
Disengaged from supervision
Manipulative
No-show/late for appointments
Figure 8.2. Acute (situational) risk factors for sexual offense recidi-
vism.
Source: Hanson & Harris (2000).
deficits and loneliness, and problems with emotional regulation (Hanson &
Harris, 2000). In contrast, other clinical problems with weak or nonexistent
statistical relationships to reoffending are labeled noncriminogenic needs. Exam-
ples of noncriminogenic needs are low self-esteem, anxiety, and personal
distress (Andrews & Bonta, 2003). Noncriminogenic needs are deemed largely
irrelevant as primary treatment targets because changing them is not linked to
reductions in reoffending (Ward & Stewart, 2003).
In practice, the RNM is often accompanied by a relapse prevention treat-
ment framework or component. Relapse prevention teaches offenders to
recognize the situational and psychological dynamic risk factors associated with
past offending, such as being alone with children or feeling very down. Of-
fenders are then taught how best to avoid or respond to these risk situations
and psychological states so as to minimize their chances of reoffending (Ward
& Hudson, 2000).
The Management of Sex Offenders 185
offenders (i.e., those with the most risk factors). Conversely, giving low-risk
offenders high levels of treatment is wasteful and may actually increase their
chances of reoffending (Andrews & Bonta, 2003). Third, treatment that di-
rectly targets criminogenic needs or dynamic risk factors rather than other
clinical problems (i.e., noncriminogenic needs) will result in better outcomes.
Fourth, offenders’ other clinical problems or characteristics that affect their
responsiveness to treatment should be addressed to the extent necessary for the
offender to engage and learn in the program.
Norm Building
The main aims are to establish the social rules for the group, encourage
motivation to engage in the program, and encourage accepting personal re-
sponsibility for offending and offense-related risk factors. The treatment phi-
losophy is explained; the men are told that the program does not aim to cure
them but rather to teach them to control their behavior through understanding
their offending and learning ways to break the offense pattern. Each group
generates group rules that will assist them to function effectively to achieve the
program aim. Rules typically cover confidentiality (prohibiting the discussion
of issues raised in the group with people outside the group), communication
procedures (e.g., using ‘‘I’’ statements, turn-taking), the importance of ac-
cepting responsibility for one’s own issues, and challenging other group
members constructively and assertively (rather than aggressively or colluding).
Understanding Offending
The main aims are for each man to fully understand his offense cycle and
the role of his various risk factors, and to understand how distorted thinking
has facilitated his cycle. Men are encouraged to develop an understanding of
how background factors (e.g., low mood, lifestyle imbalances,2 sexual diffi-
culties, intimacy problems) set the scene for their own offending. The men are
then encouraged to be honest about the steps taken to set up an opportunity
for offending, whether involving explicit planning or unintentional choices,
188 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
and to be honest about the nature of their offending. Men are assisted to see
how their own particular postoffense reactions added to background diffi-
culties and perpetuated reoffending risk.
Arousal Reconditioning
This module focuses on the role of deviant arousal in offending and
teaches techniques to reduce deviant sexual interest. Inappropriate or deviant
sexual arousal to children is hypothesized to be an important factor causing and
maintaining sexual offending (Marshall & Barbaree, 1990). In essence, the
pairing of orgasm to imagined or real sexual contact with children is thought
to condition offenders’ sexual responsiveness to children. Thus, the arousal
reconditioning module aims to teach each man techniques to unpair or re-
condition deviant sexual arousal patterns. Men are taught imaginal (or covert)
sensitization, a technique that involves pairing deviant sexual arousal (and
other early aspects of the offense cycle) with the negative consequences of
apprehension in the offenders’ imagination and with an alternate escape script.
Directed masturbation is another technique used that attempts to strengthen
sexual arousal to appropriate images and thoughts, while satiation procedures
attempt to reduce arousal to deviant sexual fantasies.
Mood Management
The mood management module aims to teach knowledge and skills to
enhance emotion regulation. Men are taught to identify and distinguish a
range of feelings that are commonly linked with offending, such as sadness,
fear, and anger, and to focus on those feelings associated with their risk of
reoffending. A range of cognitive-behavior techniques used in mainstream
mood or emotion management are taught in the module, the main aim being
to help men avoid making emotion-focused snap decisions.
The Management of Sex Offenders 189
Relationships
In this module, men consider the importance of intimate relationships and
the ways that they can enhance appropriate intimate relationships through a
variety of psycho-educational tasks. Communication and problem-solving
techniques are taught. Education about healthy sexuality and sexual dysfunc-
tion is also included.
Relapse Prevention
The final module is an extension of the relapse prevention focus that has
run throughout the program. By this stage, men should have learned to self-
monitor their risk factors and to use a range of cognitive and behavioral
techniques to respond more effectively when risk factors emerge or are oper-
ating. Particular emphasis is placed on ‘‘breaking the cycle’’ as early as possible
to ensure that the risk of reoffending is always minimized. The men present a
revised understanding of their offense cycle in the form of a personal statement.
Men are encouraged to understand that risk management incorporates both an
internal risk management component (i.e., internal self-monitoring and coping
skills) and an external risk management component that involves external
monitoring and support from prosocial family and friends who are prepared to
help him achieve his goal of avoiding reoffending. Thus, in his personal
statement, each man is required to link each of his risk factors to the internal and
external risk management strategies that are designed to reduce risk.
Reintegration Component
A reintegration component runs alongside the group program that focuses
on each man’s release planning and strengthens his proposed support network
in the community. Prosocial support people (e.g., professionals, family, and
friends) are identified by program staff together with the man, and these
support people are informed about the man’s participation and progress in
treatment. In the latter stages of the program, reintegration meetings are
typically held that involve the man and his support network. In these meetings
the man presents and discusses his offense pattern, relapse prevention, and
release plan with his support network, who in turn evaluate and strengthen his
understanding, relapse, and release plan. A key purpose of these reintegration
meetings is to equip the man’s support network to be able to externally
monitor the man’s progress in the community and to act to reduce or disclose
high-risk situations when they emerge.
Although the various sexual offender programs differ in their organizing
structure, a number of common features characterize the RNM of sexual of-
fender treatment. First, the treatment emphasizes a formulation of the offense-
related risk factors. Second, treatment is problem-focused. Third, treatment
190 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
and shows knowledge of the situations he should avoid (i.e., relapse preven-
tion). What may not be addressed or considered is whether the offender has
other socially acceptable and personally satisfying ways of meeting the psy-
chological and social needs once met by offending.
Like all humans, sex offenders have inherent human needs that require
fulfillment (Deci & Ryan, 2000). In contrast to the pincushion model, we
argue that the primary purpose of treatment should be to help offenders learn
new ways of living that are both socially acceptable and personally satisfying. In
essence, we believe that ‘‘good lives’’ and risk management are like two sides
of the same coin. Focusing on a good life and offender well-being helps of-
fenders learn what to do to have a satisfying life where offending is unnec-
essary. In turn, this approach results in the automatic reduction of risk factors
that once flagged a good life problem.
A number of related concerns about the basic RNM stem from or have
contributed to the development of the GLM that we present in the following
section. First, many of the core treatment techniques using the RNM and
attendant relapse prevention approach are framed in negative terms. For ex-
ample, treatment focuses on extinguishing deviant sexual arousal, eliminating
problematic attitudes, reducing cognitive distortions (i.e., biased thinking), and
avoiding high-risk situations (e.g., avoiding use of substances or babysitting
young children for friends). We think an important focus of treatment is on
what kind of life to lead, not simply what problems or situations to avoid or
reduce. Stopping offending involves replacing the old patterns associated with
offending with new ways of living life. Broadly discussing or speculating about
alternative prosocial options is insufficient. The best way to learn something new
is to develop specific goals and focus attention on achieving those goals. We
suggest that treatment should focus on building strengths or capabilities to
enable offenders to meet their needs in acceptable ways, rather than promoting
narrow skills purely for managing risk factors.
Second, human beings have a range of basic or inherent human needs that
motivate us to pursue certain experiences and outcomes (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
The categorization of needs into criminogenic and noncriminogenic does not
reflect this kind of understanding of human need (Ward & Stewart, 2003). In
the RNM ‘‘needs’’ are defined entirely by their statistical relationship to
subsequent offending. No attempt is made to link an understanding of crim-
inogenic needs to broader psychological models of human need and func-
tioning. Although knowing the correlates or predictors of offending is relevant
information, what is more important for treatment is why an offender sexually
offended (i.e., knowing the cause, not just the symptoms).
In the GLM, criminogenic needs/dynamic risk factors are reframed as the
internal or external obstacles that interfere with offenders’ meeting their basic
human needs in personally rewarding and socially acceptable ways. For ex-
ample, offenders’ antisocial attitudes are viewed as an internal obstacle to
meeting the basic human need of intimacy in relationships. A common theme
192 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
to antisocial thinking is that other people are hostile and malevolent in-
dividuals who will hurt or take advantage if given the opportunity. Thus
antisocial thinking creates suspicion, hostility, and mistrust that distorts the
perception of interpersonal encounters and interferes with establishing the
trusting and secure relationships that provide intimacy.
Fourth, the RNM gives no attention to the role of personal identity and
personal agency in the change process. Although both are intuitively relevant,
little research exists on the role of personal identity or personal agency in the
process of desisting from offending. A notable exception is Maruna’s (2001)
study on the self-narratives of offenders who either desisted from crime or
persisted with crime. The results revealed that desisters and persisters differed
little in their personality traits, but substantially in their personal identities.
Persistent offenders tended to live according to a condemnation script that empha-
sized little possibility for change and an impoverished sense of personal agency or
self-efficacy. In contrast, desisters tended to live according to a redemption script
where they viewed themselves as inherently good people whose pattern of crime
resulted from negative external events and misdirected attempts to assert some
form of power or control. For desisters, change involved giving new meaning to
past events and gaining a sense of power and control over their destiny.
Fifth, the principle and issue of treatment responsivity is not sufficiently
developed, a fact that is acknowledged also by advocates of the RNM (An-
drews & Bonta, 2003; Ogloff & Davis, 2004). Treatment responsivity barriers
can be both criminogenic needs (e.g., impulsivity and antisocial thinking) and
noncriminogenic needs (e.g., low self-esteem, anxiety, and psychological
distress). Although the responsivity principle affords a valid reason for ad-
dressing noncriminogenic needs, the primary focus of treatment always em-
phasizes criminogenic needs. Instead, we believe treatment engagement and
effectiveness can be maximized if issues of emotional safety, self-esteem, and
emotional well-being are explicitly considered and attended to throughout the
treatment process.
Sixth, the RNM is silent on the crucial role of context or ecological
variables in the process of rehabilitation. Offenders do not commit offenses in a
vacuum and equally cannot be expected to make changes in a vacuum. In-
stead, each offender is embedded in a local social, cultural, personal, and
environmental context. Offenders’ contexts should be considered so that
treatment focuses on the specific skills and resources necessary to function
adaptively in those specific contexts. For example, an offender who returns to
live in a rural area will face somewhat different barriers to social integration
than an offender who returns to live in an urban area. Equally, the skills and
resources relevant to individuals from various ethnic or socioeconomic groups
are likely to be different in important ways. Tailoring the development of
internal (e.g., skills, attitudes) and external resources (e.g., social supports,
work opportunities) to each offender’s distinct social contexts is likely to
improve treatment relevance and effectiveness.
The Management of Sex Offenders 193
human goods (i.e., meet their inherent human needs), their well-being flour-
ishes. For such individuals their good lives plan is working well. However, when
individuals are unable to secure a number of primary human goods, constructing
meaningful and purposeful lives is frustrated and well-being is compromised; the
good lives plan is dysfunctional. According to the GLM, the presence of dynamic
risk factors simply alerts clinicians to problems in the way offenders are seeking to
achieve primary human goods and construct meaningful and purposeful lives.
Different categories of risk factors point to problems in the pursuit of different
types of primary human goods. For example, social isolation indicates difficulties
in the ways the goods of intimacy and community are sought and may indicate
social skills deficits and/or lack of social opportunities and resources.
Research findings from a number of disciplines (i.e., anthropology, evo-
lutionary theory, philosophy, practical ethics, psychology, social policy, and
social science) appear to converge on nine types of primary goods (see Arnhart,
1998; Aspinwall & Staudinger, 2003; Cummins, 1996; Emmons, 1999; Linley
& Joseph, 2004; Murphy, 2001; Nussbaum, 2000; Rescher, 1990). No one of
these goods is ‘‘better’’ to attain than others; rather, all in some form or another
are necessary for a fulfilling life. The main categories of primary human goods
sought are life (i.e., healthy living, optimal physical functioning, sexual satis-
faction), knowledge (i.e., wisdom and information), excellence in work and play (i.e.,
mastery experiences), excellence in agency (i.e., autonomy, self-directedness), inner
peace (i.e., freedom from emotional turmoil and stress), relatedness (i.e., intimate,
family, romantic, and community relationships), spirituality (i.e., finding mean-
ing and purpose in life), happiness, and creativity. As a comprehensive list, these
nine primary human goods are, of course, multifaceted and may be broken
down into related subclusters of goods. For example, the primary good of
relatedness may be further subdivided into the goods of intimacy, friendship,
support, caring, reliability, honesty, and so on. Table 8.1 summarizes these
primary human goods and outlines potential secondary (instrumental) human
goods that individuals may use to secure their primary human goods.
Individuals are unique in the priorities or weight they give to different
types of goods due to cultural context, personal preferences, strengths, and
opportunities. For example, an individual from a culture that places greater
social value on relatedness than excellence in agency per se may internalize that
value and prioritize pursuit of group mastery over individual mastery (i.e.,
greater well-being is achieved when the group does well rather than when the
individual does well). Thus, all individuals have their own unique good lives
plan that reflects the priority given to the various primary human goods and
the secondary goods or strategies chosen to achieve the primary goods. In
essence, an individual’s good lives plan reflects an individual’s personal identity;
it is like an internalized metascript that guides the kind of life a person seeks
and the type of person he tries to be.
A good life is attainable when an individual possesses both the internal skills
and capabilities and external opportunities and supports to achieve primary
The Management of Sex Offenders 195
Life
Healthy living Leisure & sporting involvement
Optimal physical functioning Attention to diet
Sexual satisfaction Maintain intimate relationship
Knowledge
Wisdom Work, career
Information Education
Reading
Excellence in work & play
Mastery experiences Involvement in work, career, sport,
hobbies, interests
Engage in training, mentoring program
Excellence in agency
Autonomy Achieve financial independence
Self-directedness Seek employment that matches desire for
autonomy/direction
Inner peace
Freedom from emotional Achieve lifestyle balance
turmoil and stress Maintain positive relationships
Learn emotional regulation skills
Physical exercise
Relatedness
Intimate Work on building intimacy within
Family relationships
Romantic Invest in establishing & maintaining a
Community romantic relationship
Have children, be an active parent
Involvement in community groups & activities
Spirituality
Meaning & purpose in life Practice religious beliefs
Live life according to values
Engage in cherished life projects
Happiness Engage in relationships & activities that
bring joy & pleasure
Creativity Work, parenting, music, art, gardening
human goods in a socially acceptable manner. For a fulfilling and balanced life, it
is important that the full range of primary goods is attained within an individual’s
lifestyle. In the case of individuals who offend, problems reside in four major
types of difficulties: (1) problems in the means used to secure goods (e.g., seeking
intimacy through child sexual abuse); (2) a lack of scope or variety in the goods
196 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
GLM Assessment
The GLM approach to assessment has distinctive content and style di-
mensions. As described earlier, the traditional RNM focuses assessment on
eliciting offenders’ personal history relevant to offending, offenders’ under-
standing of their offending, and measuring a range of potential risk factors with
psychological tests. In addition to these traditional foci, the GLM assessment
model places equal importance on discovering the offenders’ own goals, life
priorities, strengths, achievements, and aims for their intervention. The purpose
is to understand how clients conceptualize their own lives, and how they
prioritize and operationalize their range of primary human goods. The result is a
balance between assessment of risk and vulnerability, and assessment of client
strengths and personal identity.
198 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
GLM Treatment
A GLM approach to sex offender treatment is informed by an explicit and
particular understanding of sex offenders and the therapeutic task. First, the
GLM acknowledges that a large proportion of sex offenders have develop-
mental histories marked by a diversity of adversarial experiences. These ad-
versarial experiences may involve negative developmental experiences (e.g.,
physical or sexual abuse, instability in the family or caregiver arrangements,
The Management of Sex Offenders 199
and so on) and/or may involve experiences that were missing in development
(e.g., there was emotional neglect, insecure relationships, lack of positive
personal and interpersonal modeling, and so on). Hence, sex offenders are seen
as individuals who have lacked the opportunity and resources necessary to
develop an adequate good lives plan. Second, sexual offending represents an
attempt to achieve human goods that are desired and normative, but where the
skills or capabilities necessary to achieve them are lacking. Third, the absence
or problems in achieving some primary human goods appear to be more
strongly related to sexual offending than others. These goods are agency (i.e.,
autonomy and self-directedness), inner peace (i.e., freedom from emotional
turmoil and stress), and relatedness (i.e., including intimate, romantic, family,
and community) (Ward & Mann, 2004). Fourth, reducing the risk of sexual
reoffending is achieved by assisting sexual offenders to develop the skills and
capabilities necessary to achieve the full range of primary human goods, with
particular emphasis on agency, inner peace, and relatedness. Fifth, treatment
is seen as an activity that adds to a sexual offender’s repertoire of personal
functioning, rather than being an activity that removes or manages a problem.
Restricting activities that are highly related to sexual offending or offense-
related problems may be necessary but should not be the primary focus of
treatment. Instead, the goal should be to assist clients to live as normal a life as
possible, where restrictions are only used when necessary.
The aims of GLM treatment are always specified as approach goals (Em-
mons, 1999; Mann, 2000; Mann, Webster, Schofield, & Marshall, 2004).
Approach goals involve defining what individuals will achieve and gain, in
contrast to avoidance goals that specify what will be avoided or ceased. Spec-
ifying the aims of treatment as approach goals has several advantages. For ex-
ample, goals that are life-enhancing rather than problem-avoiding are more
likely to create intrinsic motivation for change rather than the motivation for
change being extrinsically driven (i.e., to avoid trouble with the law). Goals that
focus on what the offender wants to obtain in life are more in line with what
offenders want to achieve. The reality is that most offenders are much more
focused on their own problems and quality of life than the harm they have
caused their victims. Hence, incorporating offenders’ goals as well as society’s
goals into treatment is more likely to tap into offenders’ intrinsic motivation for
change.
Research shows some advantages to using approach goal programs. Cox,
Klinger, and Blount (1991) found that alcohol abusers who participated in an
approach-goal focused program were less likely to lapse than individuals
working toward avoidance goals. Mann et al. (2004) found teaching tradi-
tional relapse prevention ideas and skills to sex offenders with an approach-goal
focus rather than the traditional avoidance and risk reduction focus resulted
in greater engagement in treatment (i.e., greater homework compliance and
disclosure of problems). Instead of teaching offenders what risk factors to
notice and avoid, offenders were taught personal and interpersonal qualities to
200 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
notice and work toward for a more adaptive personal identity. At pro-
gram completion, offenders in the approach-goal group were equally able to
articulate their personal risk factors but were rated as more genuinely moti-
vated for living a nonoffending lifestyle than offenders in the avoidance-goal
group.
Treatment using the GLM involves two broad steps: First, the offender
must learn to think of himself as someone who can secure all the important
primary human goods in socially acceptable and personally satisfying ways. In
other words, the offender has to learn to believe that change is possible and
that change is worthwhile. Second, the treatment program should aim to help
offenders develop the scope, strategies, coherence, and capacities necessary for
living a healthy personal good lives plan. To achieve this, individuals’ of-
fending should be understood in the context of the problematic or unhealthy
good lives plan operating when the offending occurred, or until now. Also,
the treatment goals should be understood as the steps necessary to help the
individual construct and achieve the healthy personal good lives plan.
Many of the specific activities of traditional RNM programs can be uti-
lized in a GLM program. However, the goal of each intervention will be
explicitly linked to the GLM theory and offered in a style consistent with the
GLM principles. Ward and colleagues (Ward & Mann, 2004; Ward, Mann, &
Gannon, 2005) recently reviewed the traditional targets of sex offender treat-
ment and reinterpreted these in light of the GLM. For example, a common
target of sex offender treatment is offenders’ sexual preferences for children.
According to the GLM, sexual preferences for children point to the following
potential problems and treatment approaches: (i), the offender uses inappro-
priate means to achieve sexual satisfaction and sexual intimacy (through which
the primary human goods of life and relatedness that we outlined earlier are
achieved, respectively). Treatment should focus on helping the offender de-
velop a wider range of strategies for achieving sexual satisfaction and sexual
intimacy (i.e., provide appropriate means to achieve these goods); (ii), the
offender lacks scope in his good lives plan and places too much emphasis on
achieving sexual satisfaction or sexual intimacy at any cost. The offender
should be helped to learn to value and invest in a broader range of primary
human goods (i.e., improve the scope of the good lives plan); and (iii), the
offender uses inappropriate means to achieve agency or mastery and attempts
to achieve these through sexual domination of a minor. Treatment should help
the offender develop a wider range of strategies for achieving agency and
mastery in both appropriate sexual relationships and in nonsexual situations
(i.e., provide appropriate means for achieving these goods). The extent that
any one of these formulations is accurate for an individual offender would be
ascertained through the assessment process. It is also entirely feasible that a
different link to a primary human good may exist. The GLM is not intended to
be a rigidly prescriptive approach; rather, what is important is that the problem
area is understood in terms of the individual’s good lives plan and treatment
The Management of Sex Offenders 201
aims to achieve a healthy good lives plan (in which offending is not necessary
or compatible).
Adopting a combined GLM and RNM treatment approach requires re-
thinking some of the ways that sex offender treatment programs are packaged
and operationalized. As discussed earlier, RNM sex offender treatment programs
tend to be highly structured psycho-educational programs where a series of skills
are taught in sequential modules. Although a one-size-fits-all program structure
has advantages in terms of the simplicity of streaming individuals for treatment,
the rigidity of such an approach is inconsistent with the emphasis on making
treatment explicitly relevant and tailored to the individual offender. An alter-
native approach is to develop individualized formulation-based GLM treatment
programs that tie intervention modules or areas specifically to the offenders’
good lives formulations and plans. Offering formulation-based interventions is
not the same as offering unstructured treatment. Unstructured treatments have
been shown to have no impact on recidivism rates (Gendreau, 1996; Andrews &
Bonta, 2003), so they obviously are not sufficient. Formulation-based treatment
derives clear structure from the formulation, treatment methods, and treatment
processes used, and is capable of providing a transparent program model that
is auditable.
Few formulation-based treatment programs for sex offenders currently
exist on which to base a GLM treatment approach. One exception is a pro-
gram run by William Marshall et al. in Canada (Marshall, Anderson, & Fer-
nandez, 1999), where group members work through a series of assignments at
their own pace. Assignments include both offense-related topics and topics
related to achieving human goods, such as intimacy, attachment, and emo-
tional well-being. While one way to deliver an assignment or topic-based
program is for each participant to complete each assignment at his or her own
pace, another option is for participants to only complete treatment compo-
nents or modules derived from their formulation. Although a departure from
the current practice, it would be possible to manualize the major clinical areas
addressed in a modularized program. Each program participant would have a
selection of the modules based on his or her individualized formulation. In
practice, some modules could be designed as core modules that are relevant to
all program participants so participants can continue to meet as a group and
obtain the benefits of a group process. Others could be selected based on
individual need. For example, a core module focused on building and main-
taining progress toward a good lives plan could include psycho-education about
good lives plans, basic self-management skills, problem-solving and motivation
enhancement skills. Other modules could focus on sexual health, interpersonal
competence, self-esteem, anxiety management, and so on, and be completed
on an as needed basis either in other groups or individually, depending on the
resources and operational constraints of the agency. An example of a highly
individualized program that uses manualized treatment components, much like
choosing the best tools from a tool kit, is Multi-Systemic Therapy (MST;
202 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
CONCLUSIONS
In this chapter we have presented a new theory of sex offender rehabil-
itation. The GLM is a strength-based approach to working with offenders that
has the major aim of equipping offenders with the necessary internal and
external resources to live better lives. In the GLM, criminogenic needs or
dynamic risk factors are conceptualized as distortions in the internal and ex-
ternal resources necessary to live healthy lives. Although criminogenic needs
are important for understanding the occurrence of sexual offending, they
should not be the sole focus of treatment. Instead, we advocate embedding the
RNM within the GLM to create a twin focus on establishing good lives and
avoiding inflicting harm. Such an approach grounds individuals’ offending
within a broad understanding of their functioning, personal identity, lifestyle,
and social context and provides a rich and comprehensive guide for clinicians
who undertake the difficult task of treating sex offenders.
By making treatment more meaningful for offenders and optimizing of-
fenders’ intrinsic motivational and change processes, we believe we can in-
crease the effectiveness of treatment and, in turn, increase public safety. In
particular, the GLM provides us an opportunity to explore a better means of
reaching unmotivated or treatment-resistant offenders and enhancing the
maintenance of positive changes following treatment completion in the
community. The combined approach also provides the potential to be more
efficient with those offenders who are already motivated and well on the road
to change.
We believe that the GLM and principles will continue to grow and exert
influence in clinical practice with sex offenders. Of course, full integration of
such principles is dependent upon the outcome data from programs that have
begun to pilot the model. It is our hope that researchers’ and practitioners’
interest in the GLM will flourish and produce a sizable evidence base upon
which the GLM can be more fully evaluated. However, adopting the GLM
The Management of Sex Offenders 203
NOTES
1. Strictly speaking, some dynamic risk factors may not be criminogenic needs.
For example, some risk assessment instruments look at the recency of criminal be-
havior, such as the number of assaults committed over the last twelve months.
Although such dynamic risk factors may change over time (e.g., there are more or
fewer assaults committed over the last twelve months), they still do not represent the
psychological or situational clinical problems that are targeted in treatment.
2. Lifestyle imbalance is created when life is dominated by activities per-
ceived as hassles or demands (shoulds) compared with activities perceived as
pleasures or self-fulfilling (wants). Lifestyle imbalance is often associated with a
perception of self-deprivation that can trigger a desire for indulgence in an avoided
or abstained behavior (Marlatt, 1985).
REFERENCES
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2003). The psychology of criminal conduct (3rd ed.).
Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.
Arnhart, L. (1998). Darwinian natural right: The biological ethics of human nature.
Albany: State University of New York Press.
Aspinwall, L. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (Eds.). (2003). A psychology of human
strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Beech, A. R., & Ward, T. (2004). The integration of etiology and risk in sex-
ual offenders: A theoretical framework. Aggression & Violent Behavior, 10,
31–63.
Cox, M., Klinger, E., & Blount, J. P. (1991). Alcohol use and goal hierarchies:
Systematic motivational counseling for alcoholics. In W. R. Miller &
S. Rollnick (Eds.), Motivational interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive
behavior (pp. 260–271). New York: Guilford.
Cummins, R. A. (1996). The domains of life satisfaction: An attempt to order
chaos. Social Indicators Research, 38, 303–328.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry,
11, 227–268.
Den Uyl, D. (1991). The virtue of prudence. New York: Peter Lang.
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns. New York: Guilford.
Gendreau, P. (1996). Offender rehabilitation: What we know and what needs to
be done. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23, 144–161.
Gendreau, P., Little, T., & Goggin, C. (1996). A meta-analysis of the predictors of
adult offender recidivism: What works! Criminology, 34, 575–607.
204 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Ward, T., & Mann, R. (2004). Good lives and the rehabilitation of offenders: A
positive approach to sex offender treatment. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph
(Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 598–616). New York: Wiley.
Ward, T., Mann, R., & Gannon, T. (2005). The comprehensive model of good lives
treatment for sex offenders: Clinical implications. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Ward, T., Polaschek, D. L., & Beech, A. R. (2005). Theories of sexual offending.
Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Ward, T., & Stewart, C. A. (2003). Criminogenic needs or human needs: A
theoretical critique. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 9, 125–143.
9
Offender Profiling
‘‘ A M O R O N I N A H U R R Y C O U L D S E E I T
W O U L D N O T S T A N D U P I N C O U R T ’’ 1
In the United Kingdom, a severe blow was delivered to offender profiling
in the Rachel Nickell murder case.2 A clinical psychologist developed a profile
based on the supposed fantasies on the apparently sexually related murder of a
young woman in one of London’s public parks. The resultant advice supplied
by the profiler was used to direct a police undercover operation, in which the
enquiry team hoped the suspect (Colin Stagg) would reveal some guilty
knowledge of the offense (i.e., that only the actual offender would know
about). This guilty knowledge or a confession was never forthcoming and
there was no compelling evidence against Stagg. Indeed, there is some recent
plausible speculation that a variety of other suspects who were not focused on
with the same verve are far more compelling. However, at the earliest stages of
the trial, even before the evidence was heard before the jury, the case was
thrown out. The judge, Justice Ognall, severely criticized the use of ‘‘profil-
ing’’ and behavioral advice in this particular investigation—in part because of
its lack of scientific foundations and the apparent unquestioned intuition on
which it was based (Britton, 1997). He argued that the enquiry was run with
an ‘‘excess of zeal’’ and classified the case as an example of ‘‘misconduct of the
grossest kind.’’
This case doubtless had a negative impact on profiling in the United
Kingdom. However, the hiatus provided academics and practitioners with the
time to review the subject and how to move profiling forward to standardize
advisory output. Moreover, there was a clear remit to ensure that advice
should offer practical results and be based on a firmly scientific foundation.
Prior to this painful introspection, profiling in the United Kingdom had been a
phenomenon that was trying to run before it could even crawl, let alone walk.
The approach taken by Britton was largely based on his experience and the
format generally involved the provision of inferences about the ‘‘type’’ of
offender responsible and the ‘‘types’’ of characteristics associated with this
‘‘type’’ of offense behavior. Alison and West (2005) referred to this approach,
which is still used by many advisors in the United States and a number of
European countries, as ‘‘traditional’’ profiling.
By traditional offender profiling, we broadly mean the range of advice
given in major criminal investigations in which an ‘‘expert’’ draws up a list of
demographic details of an offender based upon inferences about the type of
person whom she or he thinks has committed the crime (see Alison & West,
2005 for a detailed review). We need to distinguish this from more con-
temporary approaches, which are less well considered, but will be covered in
the latter half of this chapter. Traditional profiling is now less well used than it
was ten years ago and is best exemplified in the accounts of ex-FBI officers in
the United States (for example, Ressler & Shactman’s Whoever Fights Monsters,
1992) and by one or two clinical psychologists in the United Kingdom (e.g.,
Offender Profiling 209
see Britton’s 1997 The Jigsaw Man). It is the most frequently represented
portrayal of what offender profilers do in crime fiction (TV, films, and books)
although its weaknesses have gradually led to a decline in how often it is used
by police investigators. In traditional methods, ‘‘types’’ of offenders are in-
ferred from characteristics of the crime scene (i.e., whether the offender used a
garrotte or a gun, whether he bound the victim or spent time speaking with
him or her). The idea of an expert with special insight into the minds of killers
and who can, through an examination of the crime scene, draw conclusions
about the type of person who committed it is an enticing prospect and perhaps
is the reason why this approach is the one most frequently reflected in the
media. The archetype of visionary crime fighter, succeeding where the rest of
the enquiry team has failed, seems to have a very firm grip on the public’s
imagination. However, we will show that such portrayals, as well as some of
the now actual but outdated approaches that these were based on, represent a
very incomplete and naı̈ve view of theories of personality as well as what is
possible in profiling. Doubtless, such archetypes will continue to entertain us
in TV shows, films, and books, but it is important that we understand that this
glorification of the expert as mystical crime fighter should be appreciated as
based on anecdote and fiction and not on fact.
In contrast, in this chapter, we will show that what is far more important
for us to gain a psychological ‘‘purchase on’’ is the central figure of any
investigation, namely the senior investigating officer (SIO), not the ‘‘expert’’
pulled in to assist the enquiry. Indeed, psychologists wishing to contribute to
major investigations would do well to consider their expertise in relation to
understanding how the SIO will make decisions, lead his or her team, and
network with the local community, the family of the victim(s), and, poten-
tially, even state or federal government. We illustrate that the skills required to
be a successful detective are far from just being a good sleuth and, although the
detective’s role can be enhanced by working with psychologists, a good in-
vestigating officer cautiously approaches external advice and is fully aware of
how to critically evaluate it and incorporate it wisely into the investigation.
However, before we consider the range of contributions to investigation
beyond the simpleminded view of profiling types of offenders, we outline the
justifications for our very skeptical view of traditional profiling.
BASIC ASSUMPTIONS
According to Sexual Homicide: Patterns and Motives, an often-cited ‘‘hand-
book’’ on offender profiling, it is possible to establish what sort of person has
committed a crime based on the offender’s behavior at a crime scene (Ressler,
Burgess, & Douglas, 1988). So, with this assumption in mind, a profiler could,
for example, in examining the injuries of the victim, the level of ransacking,
and the method of entry to the property, predict the type of person ‘‘who-
dunnit.’’ This process would involve reading the fine details of the crime scene
210 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
and coming up with the kind of person responsible. This might include the
offender’s age, personality, social competencies, and even the type of car the
offender drives. Perhaps the most popular example of this style of profiling is
the supposed distinction between organized and disorganized killers. This
model of offender behavior assumes that each type will have a distinct and
consistent method of committing crimes. The details of this model are sum-
marized in the following tables. Burgess, Douglas, and Ressler (1985) derived
the system in interviews with thirty-six offenders who volunteered to speak to
FBI officers.
Burgess et al. argue that these two ‘‘styles of offending’’ match up with
two equally distinct offender types, organized and disorganized (see Table 9.1).
For example, an individual engaging in the organized behaviors is the sort of
person who would be married, have a good work record, and generally be
more social than his dysfunctional, socially inept, disorganized counterpart.
The style of offending is also thought to reflect the poor personal hygiene
habits associated with disorganized offenders (see Tables 9.2 and 9.3).
Although this system holds great appeal (once one had learned what was
present in an organized killing and a disorganized killing, one would simply
learn by rote memory the list of characteristics associated with one or the
other), criminal behavior is, sadly, much more complex than this simple
twofold system. Let us consider an analogy: If we think about the behavior of
people we know, we might be able to say whether they keep a reasonably tidy
house or not (‘‘organized’’ or ‘‘disorganized’’ house owners). We might even
find that there are some very basic differences in the way these people think.
Thus, we might want to measure the extent to which individuals who keep
their house in pristine condition are more particular in the organization of
their office space. We might also measure the extent to which their level
of organization relates to other behaviors such as punctuality. The former
measure would tell us how consistently tidy they are in different environments
and the latter measure would be one indication of how the organization of
their house measures up (or not) with other behaviors that we might hy-
pothesize are organized behaviors. Both of these are plausible hypotheses
(although similar efforts have not been tested or examined in relation to
1. The offender did not leave any fingerprints at the rape crime scene. It is
therefore my assertion that this offender is likely to be a prolific burglar.
Research by Professor X (1987) indicates that 76 percent of offenders who
do not leave fingerprints have more than seven previous convictions for
burglary.
2. This offense demonstrates that the offender is a ‘‘planner’’ rapist—there are no
fingerprints, the crime scene is tidy, there is no ransacking, and he has only
stolen electrical goods and children’s clothes (both of which can be easily sold
for gain). ‘‘Planner’’ rapists are between the ages of 25 and 30, feel no remorse,
are likely to be in a semiskilled job, and are likely to be married.
214 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
The first is called a ‘‘one to one relationship’’ and typifies the sorts of
claims made by profilers who may refer to themselves as crime analysts or
behavioral advisors (these individuals might be considered the ‘‘new genera-
tion’’ of contemporary profilers). The second example reflects the more tra-
ditional method of profiling and is in line with the previous work of some FBI
agents (most of whom are now retired) who advised in the early days of
profiling in the 1970s, as well as an increasingly dwindling selection of indi-
viduals from a variety of backgrounds who appear to be happy to put them-
selves forward as expert profilers.
Traditional profiling methods (as in point 2) make far more ambitious
claims than those offered by the behavioral advisor approach. Indeed, what is so
enticing is the seeming promise of a rich and detailed character assessment or
‘‘pen portrait’’ of the offender. However, this approach assumes that offenders’
behaviors are a product of stable personality traits (consistency) and that all
offenders who share a particular personality (a ‘‘planner type’’) will behave in
the same way (homology). Thus, the traditional view makes a number of
inferential leaps (see Figure 9.1) in which one derives a type from a cluster of
behaviors and a cluster of background characteristics from those different
types.
However, research has indicated that this model fails to hold water.
Several studies have now tested this process and consistently failed to find these
sorts of relationships (see Davies, Wittebrood, & Jackson, 1998; House, 1997;
Mokros & Alison, 2002).
Many developments have emerged since, and the FBI and their associated
academic colleagues have begun to produce many more academically rigorous
studies (particularly in relation to the study of child abduction—see, for ex-
ample, Boudreaux, Lord, & Dutra, 1999; Boudreaux, Lord, & Jarvis, 2001;
Prentky, Knight, & Lee, 1997), but prior to these more recent studies, and even
though there was little to no empirical support for the theories upon which
traditional methods relied. Witkin (1996) demonstrated that the demand for
1. Crime scene: Perpetrator takes a weapon to the crime scene, gags victim,
leaves no fingerprints
profiles was high, with the FBI having a number of full-time profilers who,
collectively, were involved in around 1,000 cases per year. Unfortunately, it has
taken some time for science to catch up with and question the methods that had
previously been relied upon. Furthermore, science is only just beginning to
develop more reliable bases upon which to advise crime investigations (issues
that we shall consider shortly). However, despite its more labored journey, the
scientific method is gradually weeding out the bogus approaches and providing
more fruitful, reliable, tested, and transparent evidence-based methods for as-
sisting the police. Part of the contribution lies in a change of tact, from the
exclusive focus on the killer and his likely ‘‘psychological profile,’’ to contri-
butions that consider the way the police collect information, make decisions,
and direct and lead a team that they must motivate during times of stress, often
with difficult challenges that require them to deal effectively and sensitively
with the community they serve and often rely upon. Thus, behavioral advisors
and profiles are now realizing that their contribution may lie more productively
in a greater appreciation of the myriad issues that are involved in investigating
crime.
of the police’s role in the investigation and have frequently concluded that
the police force has had little to do with solving crime (Greenwood, 1970;
Greenwood, Chaiken, & Petersilia, 1977). Even certain FBI officers (Ressler,
Burgess, D’Agostino, & Douglas, 1984) have admitted that many of the United
States’s most notorious serial murderers have been caught either through
happenstance or during some unrelated routine police procedure. Keppel
(1989), though, is dismissive of the role of chance. Instead, he views this as an
opportunity eagerly grasped by a smart cop: ‘‘what usually occurs is that some
patrol officer on routine duty comes across the killer, it then takes alert and
intelligent investigators to turn the opportunity into a final resolution of the
case’’ (p. 68). We have argued that bringing to bear a psychological and sys-
tematic approach to several aspects of policing (leadership, information col-
lection, decision making, and so on) can make these ‘‘chances’’ more probable.
Alison and Whyte (2005) identified a number of factors involved in ap-
prehension. Their descriptive study considered 101 single-offender American
serial murder cases. The average age for these offenders was approximately 30
years old, with the youngest at 17 years old and the eldest 52 years old. Many
previous studies have indicated that serial killers are in their late twenties or
early thirties (Hickey, 1991). In our sample these offenders killed a total of 617
victims ranging from three to twenty-three people killed as a series.
Figure 9.2 outlines the frequency distribution for the methods by which
the present sample of serial murderers were apprehended (the apprehension
variables). In many cases there were several apprehension variables that con-
tributed to an individual’s capture. However, the most frequent contributions
were from eyewitness testimony, the fact that the offender had previously been
institutionalized, that he had committed another crime in a similar way (and so
the crimes were linked) and, most frequently of all, because the offender
committed another (often less serious) crime that led to the offender’s capture.
The overlapping nature of these apprehension variables is captured in
Figure 9.3. We have classified these as belonging to five central issues: the way
in which the offender’s own behavior assists in his own apprehension, the role
of an informant, the role of the direct work of a detective, the role of the
victim, or, at the core of the overwhelming majority of cases, the role that the
offender’s previous crimes have in apprehension.
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
Frequency (%)
OFFENDER INFORMANT
Lives/works near victim, knows victim, Police sting, tip-off, complaints
confesses from neighbor
CORE VARIABLES
Prior institutionalization, similar MO,
prior crimes
DETECTIVE VICTIM
Forensic evidence, eyewitness, Survivor testimony, victim’s
car trace, routine policing belongings found at
suspect’s home
them is one small component of detective work. The job of the detective will
seldom be restricted to ‘‘sleuthing’’ and, instead, involves a multilayered set of
aims and objectives. This is perhaps best summed up by the management
pyramid of Figure 9.4.
Indeed, far from being a macho ‘‘supercop,’’ successful SIOs will possess a
myriad of interpersonal and diplomatic, managerial, media-handling, and ad-
ministrative and logistical skills—a far cry from the stereotypical TV portrayal
of the ‘‘rogue cop’’ who bends the rules but gets results! The structure of
the pyramid implies that if the broader foundations are rocked or managed
ineffectively, the integrity of the management of the incident will be com-
promised. For example, insensitivity to the local community can damage
community relations that can last for many years and suppress intelligence and
information. This has been the sad inheritance of the Stephen Lawrence en-
quiry in England.4 The case involved the murder of a young black male in
London. The subsequent public inquiry flagged institutional racism as endemic
within the Metropolitan Police, an allegation given more credence by the
perceived lack of police success with the case. This subsequently led to a
fractious relationship between the police and the local black community, se-
verely damaging relationships for many years. The police have had to work
very hard to regain the trust of the community, and a variety of initiatives in
recent years have been largely successful in this process of repair. Efforts in-
cluded changes in how the police liaise, recruit from, and work alongside
ethnic groups. It is therefore important to have some understanding of
the wider context within which policing and investigation emerge and to rec-
ognize that behavioral advice is one component in a very large and sophis-
ticated system. Profiling is simply one element within the wider remit of
behavioral advice and, as such, really is at the very tip of the narrowest point of
the management pyramid.
Incident
(the enquiry itself)
Enquiry team
(those who are working on the incident, team skills, team atmosphere)
Family and local community
(race and diversity, family liaison)
Organizational climate of the police environment
(current attitudes that impact on the police generally)
Political and local/national government issues
(federal and state government-driven agendas and current practice)
F R O M ‘‘ P R O F I L I N G ’’ T O ‘‘ P R I O R I T I Z I N G ’’
Bearing in mind that a profile should yield operationally useful informa-
tion to the police, there needs to be some way of describing how each piece of
advice can be practically applied to an investigation. It is hard to argue why it
might be useful to know whether an offender collects pornography. However,
indicating a likely area in which he may reside may prove more operationally
useful. Suspect prioritization is aimed at reducing the pool of suspects. In
achieving this, each item in an investigative report would have to demonstrate
on what basis a range of suspects could be narrowed down. Alison and Wilson
(2005) have argued that this is most productively considered through a range
of filters, rather than a list of likely characteristics of the offender. Thus, one
would provide the following sort of filter:
‘‘First consider all individuals within a 1 mile radius of the crime scene,
who have any prior convictions for any kind of offense within the last year’’
(Geographic Filter).
‘‘Then prioritize all those with any prior violent or sexual convictions’’
(Preconvictions Filter).
‘‘Of this group, first consider all those matching the physical description
provided by your victim’’ (Eyewitness Filter).
This system carries the assumption that the most reliable information is the
geographic, followed by the preconvictions, followed by the eyewitness in-
formation, because if we are wrong about the first filter (and the offender lives
farther than one mile away) all the other filters become redundant. However,
if the eyewitness information is incorrect (research does indicate that it can be
quite unreliable) and the geographic and preconviction filters are correct, our
offender is still within the right search parameters. This system has proved
effective in a variety of investigations, from rapes to kidnaps and child ab-
ductions (Alison, 2005). However, it is important to establish the right filters
and their levels of reliability. This is important because it allows us (and the
SIO) to know how much trust to put in any one filter. Thus, as well as moving
toward a different approach in terms of the content of the advice, Alison,
Goodwill, and Alison (2005) have argued that the structure of advisory reports
is critical in ensuring that they are clearly interpreted and used judiciously.
AN INVESTIGATIVE CREDO
Although the temptation for psychologists to assist police with their en-
quiries can be great, and there is a laudable desire to ‘‘do good,’’ inappropriate
and unclear advice in high-profile cases can actually prove quite dangerous and
drag an enquiry team in entirely the wrong direction. Sadly, there is little to
assist psychologists in making decisions about whether to engage in an enquiry,
or indeed, how to engage. The current British Psychological Society Codes of
Conduct, as well as the American Psychological Association Ethical Guide-
lines, do provide a framework, but they are not designed specifically for con-
tributing to crime investigations. However, Alison and West (2005) have put
forward a number of questions that the profiler should contemplate during the
initial contact, reviewing evidence, and writing-up stage. This Investigative
Credo is as follows:
researchers test hypotheses and return to the tried and tested methods to
scrutinize a variety of methods for assisting the police. With this has come the
recognition that profiling is a small cog in a far larger machine, and for that
machine to work effectively psychologists need to be more creative and ex-
pansive in their thinking. Gradually, work is emerging that has assisted us in our
understanding of information collection, decision making, leadership, com-
munity relations, media, and prioritization. It is through this joint, multivariate
endeavor that science will gradually catch up with and overtake the anecdotes
and fictional portrayals and make a real contribution to crime investigation.
NOTES
1. A reference in The Independent (a UK newspaper) to the psychological
evidence against Colin Stagg in the Nickell enquiry.
2. The first author was one of the several psychologists who provided a
defense report in this case.
3. There are now several articles that have failed to find such relationships in
offending behavior (see Alison, 2005 for a review).
4. ‘‘The Stephen Lawrence Inquiry,’’ http://www.archive.official-documents
.co.uk/document/cm42/4262/sli-00.htm
5. Jim Sturman, the legal representative for the defense, provides an inter-
esting overview of this case in Sturman and Ormerod (2005).
REFERENCES
Alison, L. J. (2005). (Ed.). The forensic psychologist’s casebook: Psychological profiling
and criminal investigation. Cullumpton, Devon: Willan.
Alison, L. J., & Canter, D. V. (1999). Professional, legal, and ethical issues in
offender profiling. In D. V. Canter & L. J. Alison (Eds.), Profiling in policy
and practice (pp. 21–54). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
Alison, L. J., Goodwill, A., & Alison, E. (2005). Guidelines for profilers. In L. J.
Alison (Ed.), The forensic psychologist’s casebook: Psychological profiling and
criminal investigation (pp. 277–297). Cullumpton, Devon: Willan.
Alison, L. J., Smith, M. D., Eastman, O., & Rainbow, L. (2003). Toulmin’s
philosophy of argument and its relevance to offender profiling. Psychology,
Crime and Law, 9(2), 173–183.
Alison, L. J., & West, A. (2005). Conclusions: Personal reflections on the last
decade. In L. J. Alison (Ed.), The forensic psychologist’s casebook: Psychological
profiling and criminal investigation (pp. 380–392). Cullumpton, Devon:
Willan.
Alison, L. J., & Whyte, C. (2005). Informant, offender, detective and victim roles in
apprehension methods in serial murder (Master’s thesis, University of Liver-
pool, UK). Unpublished document.
Alison, L. J., & Wilson, G. (2005). Suspect prioritization in the investigation of sex
offenders: From clinical classification and profiling to pragmatism. In L. J.
224 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Ressler, R. K., Burgess, A. W., D’Agostino, R. B., & Douglas, J. E. (1984). Serial
murder: A new phenomenon of homicide. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the International Association of Forensic Science, Oxford,
England.
Ressler, R. K., Burgess, A. W., & Douglas, J. E. (1988). Sexual homicide: Patterns
and motives. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Ressler, R. K., & Shachtman, T. (1992). Whoever fights monsters: My twenty years
tracking serial killers for the FBI. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Salfati, C. G., & Taylor, P. J. (forthcoming). Differentiating sexual violence: A
comparison of sexual homicide and rape.
Toulmin, S. (1958). The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Witkin, G. (1996). How the FBI paints portraits of the nation’s most wanted. U.S.
News & World Report, 32.
10
INTRODUCTION
The idea that some people are sexually stimulated by inflicting suffering
(physical and psychological) on others has a long history in mental health
literature. Krafft-Ebing (1886) was the first to clearly describe this clinical
entity and his description of the features of sexual sadists has influenced di-
agnostic criteria ever since. However, instances of sexually sadistic acts ap-
peared in the more popular literature far earlier than Krafft-Ebing’s description.
Baron Gilles de Rais was hanged in the fifteenth century for the rape, torture,
and murder of several hundred children, and most people are aware of the
behavior of the notorious Donation Alphonse François under his rather
grandiose, self-adopted name of the Marquis de Sade. In the early part of the
twentieth century, Stekel (1929) expanded on Krafft-Ebing’s work distin-
guishing masochism (the sexualized experience of being subject to suffering)
from sadism (the sexualized experience of inflicting suffering). It was Stekel’s
work, in particular, that led to the adoption of these terms in clinical work. In
this chapter we will restrict our concerns to those people identified as sexual
sadists and, more particularly, to those sexual offenders who meet criteria for
sexual sadism.
Some sexual sadists, like Gilles de Rais, also murder their victims.
However, some sexual offenders who are not sadists likewise kill their victims,
sometimes to eliminate the only witness (other than themselves), sometimes as
228 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
a result of rage, and sometimes for other reasons (see Grubin [1994] for a
discussion of these reasons). Unfortunately, the literature on sexual sadism does
not always clearly distinguish sadistic sexual murderers from other types of
sexual murderers, and similarly, articles on serial sexual killers may fail to
identify the subgroup of sexual sadists among their samples.
These problems make it difficult to review or summarize the existing
literature. In addition, we (Marshall & Kennedy, 2003) found that although
most authors indicated they had followed the criteria outlined in a relevant
edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), the actual criteria used to identify their
samples of sexual sadists was not a match for DSM criteria. Quite a number of
researchers (Apsche, 1993; Brittain, 1970; Dietz, Hazelwood, & Warren,
1990; Egger, 1998; Fromm, 1973; Giannangelo, 1996; Gratzer & Bradford,
1995; Levin & Fox, 1985; Langevin, Ben-Aron, Wright, Marchese, & Handy,
1988; Myers, Scott, Burgess, & Burgess, 1995) claim that the crucial feature of
sexual sadists is the exercise of power and control over the victim, while the
other features (e.g., torture, humiliation, aggression) are seen as the means by
which power and control is exercised. At other times, some of these same
authors (e.g., Myers, Burgess, Burgess, & Douglass, 1999; Ressler, Burgess, &
Douglas, 1988), as well as others (Seto & Kuban, 1996), describe the ex-
pression of violence or aggression as the key feature of sexual sadists. Whatever
features are seen as diagnostic, all authors view sexual sadists as being sexually
aroused by these features. DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association,
2000) describes sexual sadism as a paraphilia that involves ‘‘recurrent, intense
sexually arousing fantasies, sexual urges, or behaviors involving acts (real, not
simulated) in which the psychological or physical suffering (including humi-
liation) of the victim is sexually exciting to the person’’ (p. 574). Like all the
features described in the literature (e.g., violence, torture, control, power,
aggression, humiliation), the DSM requires that the psychological and physical
suffering of the victim must be sexualized for the offender to meet criteria as a
sexual sadist. The problem with this requirement is that the only person who
can know with any degree of certainty that these acts generate (or are necessary
for) sexual arousal, is the offender. Not surprisingly, few sexual offenders who
commit violent or degrading acts willingly admit that such acts are sexually
arousing to them. Indeed, very few sexual offenders of any type are readily
forthcoming about what sexually excites them. Thus, the diagnostician must
make an inference based on other information (e.g., details of the crime scene,
reports by victims, the offender’s life history and offense history) about the
client’s sexual motivation. The authors of DSM-III (American Psychiatric
Association, 1980) recognized that the rather poor interdiagnostician reli-
ability of earlier versions of DSM was the result of requiring diagnosticians to
make inferences about their client’s motivations or about other unobservable
processes. As a result, from DSM-III onward, the subsequent editions of DSM
have, for almost all diagnoses, attempted to specify observable criteria; the
Severe Sexual Sadism 229
reliably conclude that the features described in these studies accurately convey
what it is that sexual sadists do when they sexually offend, and we are on far
more shaky ground when it comes to inferring sexual motivation in relation to
any of these behaviors.
Proulx et al. also described personality features of their putative sexual
sadists. The sadists, in comparison with a matched group of nonsadistic sexual
offenders, displayed more schizoid, schizotypal, histrionic, and avoidant per-
sonality features. More interesting perhaps was Proulx et al.’s observations of
the behaviors and experiences of the sadists in the forty-eight hours preceding
their offense. They found that sadists were more likely than nonsadistic sexual
offenders to have had a specific conflict with a woman during this preoffense
period and to have had conflicts with women in general. They were also far
more angry and sexually excited, and they reported fantasizing about deviant
sexual acts prior to their offenses. In addition, the sadists in Proulx et al.’s study
were far more likely to have planned their offense and to have deliberately
selected the victim than were the nonsadistic offenders.
Although the differences in the percentage of subjects who displayed each
feature across these three studies no doubt reflect both differing diagnostic
practices and different samples of sexual sadists, there is some consistency in the
features seen as crucial to this diagnosis. However, diagnostic inconsistency
presents a real problem that, to date, has to some extent obfuscated the iden-
tification of the primary characteristics of sexual sadists. We now turn to a
consideration of diagnostic issues.
DIAGNOSTIC ISSUES
In order to address the problems presented by sadistic sexual offenders, it is
necessary to have a clear and agreed-upon definition of what constitutes sexual
sadism. Research cannot proceed in a useful way unless all researchers are in
agreement on the criteria necessary to identify sexual sadism. If researchers
employ different definitions of the problem, then the data derived from re-
search cannot be integrated in a way that would lead to an understanding of
sexual sadism that could guide assessment and treatment. Since sadistic sexual
offenders constitute a real threat to the community, it is essential that meth-
odologically sound research be conducted that can appropriately inform treat-
ment providers and decision-makers. Sexual sadists who commit offenses to
satisfy their deviant desires may not always present as a high risk to reoffend
based on actuarial measures, but their risk to harm if they do reoffend is always
high. Thus, an agreed-upon set of criteria necessary to reliably diagnose sexual
sadism is critical.
The DSM in its various incarnations has ostensibly been the agreed-upon
guide for researchers attempting to study sexual sadism. We say ‘‘ostensibly’’
because in our review of the existing literature (Marshall & Kennedy, 2003)
we found that although almost all authors claimed to adhere to DSM criteria,
Severe Sexual Sadism 231
the actual criteria they specified were rarely a match for those specified in the
DSM. Given these differences in the diagnostic criteria employed in various
studies, it is no surprise that estimates of the incidence of sexual sadism range
from 5 percent to 80 percent (Marshall & Kennedy, 2003). Of course, these
different estimates may also result from the different samples examined in the
various studies of sexual offenders.
Fortunately, there was a good deal of overlap in the studies we reviewed
concerning the features that were considered indicative of sexual sadism. While
our review revealed thirty-plus supposedly critical features, there was agree-
ment among most researchers that the following are essential to the diagnosis of
sexual sadism: torture or cruelty, victim suffering, humiliation of victim, use of
force or violence, control over victim, sexual mutilation. Some authors (e.g.,
Brittain, 1970; Gratzer & Bradford, 1995; Myers et al., 1999) suggest that the
motivation behind sexual sadism is the exercise of power and control over the
victim, which has become sexualized in these men; that is, the exercise of
power and control is sexually arousing to sexual sadists. These authors claim that
all the other features (e.g., torture, cruelty, force and violence, humiliation) are
enacted for the sole purpose of achieving and demonstrating this power and
control.
Faced with the evident disagreement across studies revealed on the
specifics of diagnostic criteria, Marshall and collegues (2002) conducted two
studies meant to evaluate the reliability of the diagnosis of sexual sadism. First,
they extracted from the files held in three Canadian federal prisons psychiatric
reports where the psychiatrists were asked to evaluate the dangerousness of
various sexual offenders. All these assessments occurred over a ten-year period
(1989–1999). All the evaluated offenders had previously been assessed as high
risk to reoffend sexually using one or another actuarial risk assessment in-
strument. Combined with the actuarial instruments, the psychiatrists’ evalua-
tions were intended to reveal each offender’s risk to reoffend and the associated
likelihood of harm. Fifty-nine evaluations were located of which forty-one
involved cases where the offender was diagnosed as a sexual sadist, while the
remaining eighteen cases were given various other diagnoses. The fourteen
evaluators were all experienced forensic psychiatrists who reported using
DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria.
Marshall, Kennedy, and Yates (2002) compared the offenders who were
given a diagnosis of sexual sadism with those who were not given such a
diagnosis. The groups were compared on twenty offense features (extracted
from extensive police and victim reports and from court records), ten sets of
self-reported information, and seven data sets derived from phallometric as-
sessments of sexual interests. All this information was available to the psy-
chiatrists doing the evaluations. Contrary to expectations, those offenders who
were not diagnosed as sexual sadists were significantly more likely to have
beaten or tortured their victims than were those deemed to be sexual sadists. In
addition, the nonsadists showed greater sexual arousal to nonsexual violence
232 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
while the sadists displayed greater arousal to consenting sexual scenes. Marshall
et al. calculated a composite sadistic score based on offense details, but again it
was the nonsadists who scored the highest. It appears that the diagnosticians in
the study did not systematically employ the information Marshall et al. used to
compare the sadists and nonsadists. Perhaps they relied more on how the
offender presented at interview. However, the only thing that predicted
their diagnoses was what an earlier psychiatric report identified as the diag-
nosis. Apparently, once a diagnosis is made it tends to be perpetuated by
subsequent diagnosticians even when the available information contradicts the
diagnosis.
Since Marshall and Kennedy’s (2002) literature review revealed that the
authors of each study used idiosyncratic criteria to apply the diagnosis of sexual
sadism, it may be that Marshall et al.’s (2002) first study simply revealed the
idiosyncratic tendencies of each of the psychiatrists the prisons hired to eval-
uate the offenders. Marshall, Kennedy, Yates, and Serran (2002), therefore,
decided to conduct a further study to examine the interdiagnostician reli-
ability of sexual sadism. They carefully extracted information from the files of
twelve of the offenders in their first study, six of whom had been identified as
being sexual sadists while the other six were identified as having some other
diagnosis (e.g., pedophilia, antisocial personality disorder). The information
contained details of the life history of each offender, crime scene data and
other details of his offense(s), the results of various psychological tests as well as
the results of phallometric evaluations (which measure a person’s sexual arousal
to selected cues), and self-reported sexual interests and activities provided by
the offenders. All this information on each of the twelve offenders was pro-
vided to fifteen internationally renowned forensic psychiatrists, each of whom
had experience working specifically with sexually sadistic offenders. These
experts were asked to complete several tasks, but the one of prime interest was
the requirement that they decide whether each offender was or was not a
sexual sadist. A resultant calculation revealed a percentage of agreement among
the experts that was marginally above chance (75 percent agreement where
chance agreement would be 53.3 percent). Generally, the statistic considered
appropriate to determine inter-rater agreement is the kappa coefficient. For
very important decisions it is generally agreed that a kappa coefficient of .9 is
necessary, whereas for a decision having rather trivial consequences a kappa of
.6 is acceptable (American Educational Research Foundation, 1999). Given
that psychiatric diagnoses of sexual sadism (or not) markedly influence a variety
of decisions (e.g., determining if an offender meets criteria for Dangerous
Offender status, or is a Sexually Violent Predator, or a decision to release the
offender to the community) that have very important implications for the
safety of the community and for the offenders’ freedom, it would seem ne-
cessary for interdiagnostician agreement to be high. Unfortunately, not only
was the percent agreement among the experts quite low, but the kappa statistic
revealed completely unsatisfactory interdiagnostician agreement (kappa ¼ .14).
Severe Sexual Sadism 233
Clearly Marshall et al.’s two studies do not encourage confidence in the ap-
plication of the diagnosis of sexual sadism.
Perhaps the best study yet of psychiatric diagnoses applied to sexual of-
fenders was conducted by Levenson (2004). She examined diagnoses given to
sexual offenders being considered by Florida courts for the application of a
civil commitment as a Sexually Violent Predator (SVP). The successful ap-
plication of this status means the offender is to be incarcerated indefinitely until
it can be shown that he has so profited from treatment as to no longer be an
unacceptable risk to the community. In these cases the courts require two
acknowledged experts to independently evaluate the offender. For the SVP
status to be applied, the offender must meet criteria for a paraphilia and be
determined to be at high risk to reoffend. Levenson compared the diagnoses
identified by each of the two independent assessors. The resultant kappa
coefficient for sexual sadism was 0.3, which is far below acceptable standards.
Evidently so-called experts in Florida do no better at diagnosing sexual sadism
than did Marshall et al.’s samples of Canadian psychiatrists or international
forensic psychiatrists.
Despite these disappointing data, we are not inclined to dismiss the re-
levance of sexually sadistic behaviors, but rather, we believe the present di-
agnostic practices are inadequate. We could urge forensic psychologists and
psychiatrists to exercise greater care, or we could insist that they employ the
same criteria. Perhaps, however, the problem resides in the insistence of the
diagnostic manual that the sadist must be sexually aroused by the suffering and
humiliation of the victim. Since no one but the offenders can know the answer
to this question, and they are unlikely to reveal such interests, the diagnosis
requires the clinician to infer sexual motivation in the infliction of cruelty,
torture, or degradation. Such a reliance on inference is almost certain to limit
the reliability of the diagnosis.
One way around this problem of inferring sexual motivation that has been
adopted by some authors is to employ phallometric assessment (see Marshall &
Fernandez [2003] for a review). Phallometric assessments involve the mea-
surement of the client’s erectile changes in response to the presentation of
various sets of sexual stimuli. No one has yet developed a satisfactory stimulus
set specifically for sadists, but several researchers have adapted current assess-
ment stimuli designed for men who sexually assault adult females. Seto and
Kuban (1996), for example, used arousal to a description of a brutal rape as an
index of sexual sadism. Unfortunately, they found no differences between ra-
pists they defined as sadists and rapists whom they determined were not sadists.
Seto and Kuban’s data match those found in similar studies (Barbaree, Seto, Serin,
Amos, & Preston, 1994; Langevin et al., 1985; Rice, Chaplin, Harris, & Coutts,
1994). Proulx et al. (2005) modified their standard phallometric stimuli to
include sets describing rapes that involved either extreme physical violence or
had additional elements involving the humiliation of the victim. Proulx et al.’s
stimulus sets are closer to the DSM criteria for sexual sadism than are any other
234 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
available stimulus sets. In comparing rapists who were deemed to be sadists with
rapists who did not meet the criteria, Proulx et al. found that the sadists showed
significantly greater arousal to both the physically violent and humiliating
scenes. These data suggest that specifically designed sadistic stimuli may reliably
distinguish sadistic from nonsadistic sexual offenders. We have designed such
stimuli but because of the extreme nature of the content we have not yet been
able to get ethics approval for a study to examine the value of this phallometric
procedure. In addition, such studies suffer from a seemingly inescapable con-
undrum; namely, that each group (i.e., sadists and nonsadists) must be dis-
tinguished prior to the phallometric evaluation and yet the phallometric test is
being evaluated as a diagnostic tool in identifying sadism.
As an alternative to current diagnostic practices, we suggest that the actual
behaviors of sexual sadists may provide a basis for more accurately and more
reliably identifying these problematic offenders. In the study by Marshall, Ken-
nedy, Yates, et al. (2002) where international experts were asked to identify sexual
offenders as sadists or not, these experts were also required to rate the importance
for diagnostic purposes of a variety of features of the offender’s behavior. While the
Experts’ Ratings
Feature of Importance
experts were not, as we have seen, able to agree on the diagnosis, they were in
general agreement on what features are important in making a diagnosis. Table
10.2 describes the features identified by these experts as relevant and records the
average ratings of the importance of each of these features. The rating scale ranged
from 0 to 4 where 4 indicated that the feature was crucial to the diagnosis and 1
indicated it was somewhat relevant; 0 meant the feature was not relevant. In
addition to the features described in Table 10.2, both cross-dressing and fire-setting
have been suggested by some authors (Dietz et al., 1990; Gratzer & Bradford, 1995)
as distinguishing features of sexual sadists, but experts in Marshall, Kennedy, Yates,
et al.’s (2002) study rated both of these features as zero (i.e., not relevant).
We intend to develop a rating scale based on the features listed in Table
10.2 with ratings for each feature being weighted according to the values
assigned by the experts in the Marshall, Kennedy, Yates, et al. (2002) study. The
scale has been developed to the stage where we are now conducting inter-rater
reliability studies in several locations worldwide. We will also examine the
relationship between scores on the rating scale and various other features of the
offense, offender, and actuarial risk measures. Whether such a dimensional
approach, rather than a categorical diagnosis, will prove helpful remains to be
seen, but there have been calls for the DSM to move to a more dimensional
approach across all diagnoses (Widiger & Coker, 2003). In any event, it is clear
that current diagnostic practices, as they apply to sexual sadism, are in need of
serious repair.
TREATMENT
Given the present state of knowledge, we firmly believe that if a sexual
offender clearly meets criteria for sexual sadism (or scores high on our Sadism
Scale), then psychological treatment alone is insufficient. A combination of
antiandrogens and psychological treatment (specifically, cognitive behavioral
therapy) is, in our view, necessary to effectively minimize the risk of re-
offending and thereby maximally protect the public. For sexual sadists the risk
not only concerns the likelihood of a reoffense, but also includes the very high
risk of considerable harm to the victim. For some sexual sadists (particularly
those who have only one identified victim), scoring actuarial risk assessment
instruments (see Doren [2002] for details) may indicate a low risk to reoffend,
but this will not reveal anything about risk to harm. Antiandrogens may serve
to reduce both the risk to reoffend and the risk to harm, but psychological
interventions may also equip the offender with the skills, attitudes, and beliefs
necessary to meet his needs in a prosocial manner.
Psychological Intervention
We have elsewhere described in detail the application of a cognitive
behavioral treatment program designed specifically for sexual offenders and we
236 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
particular relevance that guide their perceptions, expressed attitudes, and be-
haviors, concern sex, violence, women and children, and their own sense of
entitlement. Every surface expression of these schemas (e.g., their expressed
attitudes, beliefs, and perceptions) needs to be challenged and alternative views
need to be encouraged and reinforced.
Self-esteem is enhanced because doing so appears to enhance the offen-
der’s involvement and commitment to treatment, and enhancements of self-
esteem are related to improvements in various other targets of treatment (see
Marshall et al. [1999] for details). Having clients complete an autobiography
helps them recognize the origins of their problems, assists the therapist in
developing a broader understanding of the client, and facilitates, along with the
offense disclosure, the beginning of the development of the offender’s path-
ways to offending. The offense pathway identifies the background factors that
led to the creation of a frame of mind that allowed the offender to develop the
specific steps required to offend. The background factors (e.g., problems with
adult relations, anger at the world or women), the preparations to offend (e.g.,
planning, getting intoxicated), and the specific steps taken to be able to offend
all need to be clearly elucidated. This is necessary so that eventually steps to
circumvent these problems can be identified (i.e., self-management plans) and
skills training (e.g., mood management, and the enhancement of coping skills
and relationship skills) implemented to facilitate putting the client’s self-
management plans into action.
In discussions, numerous therapists have expressed concerns about having
sexual sadists understand the harmful effects they have inflicted on their vic-
tims, as is typically done to increase the empathy sexual offenders have toward
their victims. Since sexual sadists are, by definition, excited by the prospect of
harming their victims, it is suggested by these therapists that helping such
offenders recognize the harm they have done will enhance their motivation to
offend rather than reduce it. It is thought that sadists would enjoy, rather than
be deterred by, the idea that their victim has suffered harm in the aftermath of
the offense. This may, however, be a misplaced concern. Sadists clearly derive
some pleasure (whether sexual or as a result of control, etc.) from their victim’s
suffering during the offense, but this does not mean that they are necessarily
excited by postoffense suffering. Indeed, it seems to us unlikely that they are.
To be aroused by suffering, the sadist has to be in the process of inflicting it on
the victim. In his sexual fantasies the sadist imagines hurting and humiliating
the victim, but he does not imagine this suffering to last after the imagined
offense is over. There is no evidence suggesting that sexual sadists dwell on or
are excited by the prospect of their victim continuing to suffer long-term. In
our treatment of sadists, we find that most of them have either not thought
about the postoffense suffering of their victim or they have expressed some
degree of regret about the suffering. It appears they enjoy victim suffering
during the offense but they are either indifferent to long-term consequences or
they may actually prefer their offense not to have long-term consequences for
238 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
the victim. In any event, sadists depersonalize their victims during the offense
in order to inflict pain and degrade the victim. The process of identifying
postoffense victim harm and its spillover effects on the victim’s family serves to
make the victim a real person with feelings, hopes, and all the other features
that make someone human. Reducing the sadist’s capacity to depersonalize
other people should make it harder for him to offend in the future. Therefore,
in alerting a sexual sadist to the long-term harm that his victim has experienced
(or is likely to experience), the therapist must portray the victim as a real
person with hopes and aspirations that have been disrupted by the offense.
This, we believe, will cause the sadist to think of his victims (and all potential
future victims) as fully formed people, thus reducing his capacity to de-
personalize them in a way that allows him to treat them as objects for his
peculiar pleasure.
Sadists are typically isolated individuals, or at least have serious problems in
forming deep attachments to others. Teaching them the skills, attitudes, and
self-confidence needed to effectively relate to others should allow them to feel
not only more connected to others (and consequently less likely to deperso-
nalize others) but also to feel less need to control others. The desperate need to
have control over another person reflects the sadist’s inability to feel any sense
of control over various aspects of his own life, particularly in terms of his
relationships with other people. Giving him the skills needed to meet his needs
(including the need for control) should serve to reduce his attempts to control
others by inappropriate means.
It appears that sadists exercise strong control over the expression of their
emotions except when offending. They often present as cold and detached
individuals, devoid of any real emotions. Attempts at suppressing emotions fail
to give the person any experience at enjoying and modulating their emotions
in an appropriate and satisfying way. Everybody experiences emotions, but
some people attempt to suppress the expression of feelings, and this leads to all
manner of problems (Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999). Also, poor emo-
tional regulation (which in the case of sadists typically manifests as over-
controlled emotional regulation) has numerous damaging consequences both
for the individual and others (Baumeister & Vohs, 2004). Encouraging sadists
to become more emotionally expressive may not only be beneficial; it should
also provide the therapist with a window into the world of the client, which
would not be available were the sadist to remain emotionally unexpressive.
Problematic schemas and problematic motivations are frequently obscure in
emotionally unresponsive clients. Also, emotional expression helps to reveal
the things that distress clients so that coping skills can be developed to reduce
distress that may otherwise initiate the chain of events leading to offending.
There can be no doubt that sexual sadists have deviant sexual fantasies.
Whether they enjoy these fantasies as persistent and preferred sexual interests,
or whether the fantasies occur only under stressful or other problematic
circumstances, does not matter. These sexual fantasies involve the control of,
Severe Sexual Sadism 239
as well as the physical and psychological suffering of, their victims. Clearly,
even occasional fantasies of this kind, in someone who has committed a serious
sexual offense, need to be eliminated. We can expect antiandrogens to reduce
the frequency and intensity of deviant sexual fantasies (Bradford, 2000), but
behavioral procedures should also be employed to reduce the possibility of
these fantasies recurring in the future. Marshall et al. (in press) provide detailed
descriptions of appropriate behavioral strategies to achieve this goal. Our
preference is to employ the combination of deliberately masturbating to ap-
propriate fantasies (which we help the client construct) until orgasm and then
shifting (during the refractory period—see Masters & Johnson [1966] for a
description) to articulating all possible variants of the deviant themes for a
further ten minutes. This latter aspect of the procedure is called ‘‘satiation.’’
This combination of masturbating to orgasm while fantasizing appropriate sex
and then engaging the satiation procedure has been shown to be effective
across a range of deviant sexual fantasies and behavior ( Johnston, Hudson &
Marshall, 1992; Marshall, 1979; Marshall, in press).
Finally, the sadist, like all other sexual offenders in treatment, must for-
mulate plans for the future that he will implement after discharge from the
program. These plans include some limited avoidance strategies (i.e., what
have been called ‘‘relapse prevention plans’’) meant to reduce contact with
potential victims or to prevent the reemergence of risk factors, but they should
emphasize the development of what Ward (2002; Ward & Marshall, 2004)
calls a ‘‘good life plan’’ designed specifically for, and with, the client. This good
life plan is meant to encourage the offender to build a new life that will prove
to be more fulfilling across various domains of functioning such as health,
knowledge, work and leisure, creativity, and relationships. Associated with
these plans, the client’s plans for accommodation, work, and friendships need
to developed, and he needs to identify support groups who will help him with
both the transition back to the community and with his attempts to remain
offense-free.
Pharmacological Treatment
There has been a wide range of medical intervention used to treat sexual
offenders, from psychosurgery at one end of the body to castration at the
other. Clearly, the motivations for their use can be considered as either pu-
nitive or therapeutic depending on one’s point of view. The scientific grounds
for operating on an otherwise healthy brain are highly suspect and the evi-
dence for so doing in terms of control of sexually deviant behaviors is ques-
tionable (e.g., Rieber & Sigush, 1979) such that the procedure has not been
used for many years (Pfäfflin, 1995). Physical castration is no less controversial
(Berlin, 2005; Weinberger, Sreenivasan, Garrick, & Osran, 2005) and in most
Western countries it is not a practical consideration. As a result, since World
War II, pharmacological approaches have been explored.
240 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Bradford, 2003). These effects are more likely to occur with prolonged usage
(Gijs & Gooren, 1996).
Both MPA and CPA are artificial steroid hormones chemically related to
the sex steroid hormones that occur naturally in the body. More recently,
interest has focused on a different class of drug, a nonsteroid, which lowers the
blood testosterone levels even more dramatically than MPA and CPA. This
type of drug, known as a luteinizing hormone releasing hormone (LHRH)
agonist, is a peptide or protein substance similar to a naturally occurring hor-
mone that is released from the hypothalamus. This LHRH agonist mimics this
naturally occurring hormone and stimulates the anterior pituitary at the base of
the brain to produce luteinizing hormone (LH), which, in turn, acts on the
testes to stimulate release of testosterone. After injecting the LHRH agonist,
by a feedback loop the initially increased testosterone levels circulating in the
blood quickly cause the hypothalamus to cease producing LHRH and tes-
tosterone levels then fall.
The LHRH agonists have to be administered by injection, as they would,
like any other protein, be digested in the stomach if taken by mouth. Two
special precautions have to be taken with these drugs. First, a small test dose
has to be given with the first injection to ensure that the patient is not allergic
to the foreign protein that the drug constitutes. Second, to combat the po-
tential increase in libido that the initial surge of testosterone might cause, it
is important to concurrently administer an anti-androgen, such as CPA, for
about the first two weeks. Longer-term side effects with LHRH agonists
include hot and cold flashes, loss of facial and body hair, asthenia, diffuse
muscle pain, and loss of bone density (Briken, Nika, & Berner, 2001; Grass-
wick & Bradford, 2003). Unlike CPA and MPA, LHRH agonists have not yet
been subjected to the same degree of scrutiny. Briken, Hill, and Berner (2003)
reported that there had been only four case reports, one single-case controlled
study, seven open uncontrolled studies, and one study comparing an LHRH
agonist with CPA. Nonetheless, it appears that this type of drug is a safer
alternative to CPA and MPA and likely to be more effective as testosterone
suppression is more complete (Briken et al., 2003, 2001; Dickey, 1992). How-
ever, more research is needed (Briken et al., 2003) and the issue of bone loss as
a potentially serious side effect needs further exploration, especially with re-
spect to preventative measures (Grasswick & Bradford, 2003).
In the face of such potentially serious side effects as have been described
above in connection with hormonal treatments, it is not surprising that interest
has been shown in more common psychotropic drugs, such as antidepressants,
as well as the antipsychotics already described. The possible benefits of the
traditional antidepressants and lithium carbonate were explored a number of
years ago (e.g., Snaith & Collins, 1981; Ward, 1975), but it was not until the
introduction of the specific serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) that their
utility as sex-drive suppressants became fully exploited. These drugs, of which
Prozac is one major type, have a high incidence of sexual side effects. In fact,
242 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
improve, or if there are medical complications such as liver disease that pre-
clude treatment with CPA or MPA, Briken et al. switch to an LHRH agonist,
which is the treatment they recommend for all severe cases. Briken et al. also
recommend that all sexual offenders should receive psychotherapy together
with pharmacotherapy for comorbid disorders.
Properly identified sexual sadists would fit into Bradford’s classification as
at least severe, and more likely catastrophic, cases, and would be included in
Brinker et al.’s severe cases. Thus, sexual sadists would appear to warrant both
extensive psychological treatment, either cognitive behavioral/relapse pre-
vention or some other form of psychotherapy, as well as either CPA, MPA, or
an LHRH agonist. Whether it is necessary with sexual sadists to apply a dosage
that would completely suppress testosterone production, as Bradford rec-
ommends, remains to be seen. However, when sexual sadists are released to
the community, it would seem prudent, given the threat for harm that they
pose, to aim for complete suppression as the first step in a process of careful
monitoring of their functioning and behavior.
So far, no research has demonstrated the effectiveness of treatment with
sexual sadists, although, as we have seen, there is evidence of its effectiveness
with other sexual offenders. It will be difficult to evaluate treatment for these
individuals because (fortunately) they constitute a small proportion of sexual
offenders and thus there are rarely enough available to justify an outcome
study. In addition, quite a number of sexual sadists are incarcerated indefi-
nitely, further reducing the number available for an outcome study. However,
the following case description illustrates the potential benefits of combining
medications and psychological treatment.
A CASE STUDY
Donald, now in his 30s, is currently free of any legal constraints and living and
working in the community with periodic visits to his psychiatrist and a relapse
prevention group. He rarely experiences sadistic fantasies but has them well under
control, thanks to many years of combined psychological therapy and pharma-
cological treatment. However, Donald’s early offense history was truly alarming.
Donald had his first and only girlfriend, who was a year younger than
himself, at age 14 years. They had enjoyed sexual contacts short of intercourse
over a fifteen-month period before her parents found them in bed together
and terminated their relationship. This experience left Donald feeling angry
and frustrated.
Donald’s first sexual assault occurred at the age of fifteen when he attacked
a 10-year-old girl. He maintained that this was a spontaneous act and that he
experienced ‘‘raw’’ feelings of ‘‘anger, fear, and rejection’’ at the time, ac-
companied by the urge to hurt her. Using a knife he used to carry to fix his
bike, Donald coerced the girl into removing her clothing and then tied her feet
and hands with her shoelaces. She became uncooperative, which increased
244 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Donald’s anger. He slapped her face and buttocks, forced her to fellate him,
and attempted anal and vaginal intercourse but was only able to manage digital
penetration. He held her captive for a short period and then allowed her to
dress and leave.
Within the next three days, Donald committed two other similar sexual
attacks on girls of a similar age. Shortly thereafter, Donald was apprehended.
He received a twelve-month sentence followed by two years’ probation. Dur-
ing his incarceration Donald ruminated on his offenses, and his fantasies of
kidnapping, raping, and bondage became more intense. Before the end of his
sentence, Donald was transferred to a psychiatric facility, but he was afraid to
disclose the extent of his fantasies to clinicians and was not motivated to seek
treatment.
Within four months of his discharge from the hospital, Donald was
working in the community. Although he was on probation, Donald’s sexual
fantasies of kidnapping and raping young girls, and his feelings of revenge
against females who had spurned or ridiculed him, were beginning to pre-
occupy him. He also engaged in voyeurism and cross-dressing at that time and
started breaking into houses.
Donald was laid off from work and as a result had more time to fantasize.
Shortly after becoming unemployed, Donald broke into a home he had been
observing voyeuristically. After entering the house, Donald discovered the
young woman he had been watching. Using scissors he had taken with him,
Donald forced her to perform fellatio. He was unable to complete sexual
intercourse, but he cut up her clothes and hair in order to frighten her; then
he left.
A few days later Donald broke into another home of a victim he knew
who had once refused to date him. Donald was very resentful and angry
toward her. This victim was humiliated by having kitchen refuse smeared on
her. Unfortunately for Donald, she recognized him as he ran off. Donald
turned himself in shortly afterward.
Donald was found ‘‘not guilty by reason of insanity’’ (NGRI), though he
has never shown any signs of major mental illness. This verdict reflects the
somewhat idiosyncratic way the insanity defense was applied at the time in
Canada. His clinical diagnoses have included sexual sadism together with
various combinations of personality disorder, though he has never been con-
sidered psychopathic or antisocial.
As was typical at that time, Donald began his treatment in a maximum-
security forensic facility. There he eventually elaborated his sexual fantasies to
his therapists. He admitted to having intense and frequent sadistic sexual
fantasies involving tormenting females, though not involving cutting or stab-
bing them, but sometimes breaking their fingers while raping them. Donald
expressed a strong interest in pornography, especially bondage, as well as
fetishism for female clothing and transvestic fetishism, which he had practiced
Severe Sexual Sadism 245
on a number of occasions. His stated interest at that time was in females aged
12–16 years. Phallometric testing demonstrated a clear sexual preference for
pubescent females and he responded to both rape and nonsexual violence. It
was noted that Donald was introverted, egocentric, and emotionally con-
stricted, had great difficulty forming relationships with others, and spent much
of his time sexually fantasizing. His primary treatment was in a social therapy
program and also a variety of behavioral treatments, but he made little headway
in controlling his fantasies.
Eventually, Donald was transferred to a medium-security facility where
he participated voluntarily in a research study of cyproterone acetate (CPA).
However, this produced only a limited reduction in his arousal to violent
themes. Although Donald remained a loner in most of his interpersonal rela-
tions on the unit, he did become attracted to a female copatient. Donald’s
fantasies about her quickly became sadistic in nature and he admitted to en-
tering her dormitory at night to watch her sleep.
Another attempt was made a few years later at suppressing Donald’s de-
viant fantasies with CPA. At this time he responded better to an increased oral
dose of 200 mg daily. Donald’s interpersonal behavior also began to improve
and there was noticeable increase in his self-confidence and socialization.
About seven years after his NGRI finding, he was enjoying an open ward in
the hospital and was able to have access to the hospital grounds in the company
of staff.
Donald began a cognitive behavioral relapse prevention (CBT/RP)
program at this time, during which he admitted that he was spending several
evenings a week writing out his sexual fantasies and pornographic letters. He
showed his therapist a large pile of these writings that he had collected. Donald
willingly allowed nursing staff to confiscate these and any others that were later
found in his possession. He also indicated that he felt depressed and over-
whelmed at times, so he was given antidepressant medication, which he said
elevated his mood and controlled his deviant thoughts.
Donald’s progress was such that he was transferred to another psychiatric
facility with a minimum-security forensic unit. He began attending an up-
grading course in the community and did very well. Meanwhile, Donald
continued with individual psychological counseling aimed at improving his
relapse prevention strategies.
Phallometric testing was repeated ten years after his index offense. Un-
fortunately, this demonstrated that Donald’s maximum response was still to
pubescent females and he continued to be aroused by sadistic/bondage stimuli
despite his claim that CPA had effectively suppressed his arousal. The results of
the assessment and pressures of his schoolwork increased Donald’s depression
and he was given an increase in antidepressant medication (Fluvoxamine 300
mg per day). On this regimen, combined with psychotherapy, Donald stabi-
lized over the following year. He began attending a regular CBT/RP group
246 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
and he functioned extremely well in that program. His compliance and at-
tendance were excellent. However, a random check of Donald’s computer
revealed a number of sadistic images. As a result, his medication was switched
to medroxyprogesterone acetate, but this was discontinued because of lack of
efficacy and it was decided to change to leuprolide acetate (an LHRH agonist),
which can only be given by injection. Donald felt that this was much more
successful in curbing his paraphilic desires than his previous medications had
been.
Since beginning treatment with leuprolide acetate nearly ten years ago,
Donald has been regularly monitored by an endocrinologist with annual bone
scans. He takes supplementary calcium and vitamin D but has not so far
suffered any significant bone loss or other serious side effects from the med-
ication. He discontinued the antidepressant medication two years ago with no
relapse into depression.
After nearly twenty-five years of progressive treatment in the forensic
system, Donald was given an absolute discharge, which means that he is no
longer subject to legal restrictions of any kind. He is free in the community
and working at a job that gives him satisfaction, and he is no longer tormented
by the sadistic and pedophilic fantasies that caused suffering in himself and
others. Donald appears to have been successfully rehabilitated through a
combination of psychological and pharmacological approaches.
REFERENCES
American Educational Research Foundation. (1999). Standards for educational and
psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed., Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., Text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (1998). The psychology of criminal conduct (2nd ed.).
Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.
Apsche, J. A. (1993). Probing the mind of a serial killer. Morrisville, PA: International
Information Associates.
Bancroft, J., Tennant, G., Loucas, K., & Cass, J. (1974). The control of deviant
sexual behaviour by drugs: Behavioural changes following oestrogens and
anti-androgens. British Journal of Psychiatry, 125, 310–315.
Barbaree, H. E., Seto, M. C., Serin, R. C., Amos, N. L., & Preston, D. L. (1994).
Comparisons between sexual and nonsexual subtypes: Sexual arousal to
rape, offense precursors, and offense characteristics. Criminal Justice and
Behavior, 21, 94–114.
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.). (2004). Handbook of self-regulation: Re-
search, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford.
Severe Sexual Sadism 247
Marshall, W. L., Kennedy, P., Yates, P., & Serran, G. A. (2002). Diagnosing
sexual sadism in sexual offenders: Rehability across diagnosticians. Inter-
national Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 46, 668–
676.
Marshall, W. L., Marshall, L. E., Serran, G. A., & Fernandez, Y. M. (in press).
Treating sexual offenders: An integrated approach. New York: Routledge.
Masters, W., & Johnson, V. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little, Brown.
McConaghy, N., Blaszcznski, A., & Kidson, W. (1988). Treatment of sex of-
fenders with imaginal desensitization and/or medroxyprogesterone. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 77, 199–206.
Money, J. H. (1968). Discussion of the hormonal inhibition of libido in male sex
offenders. In R. Michael (Ed.), Endocrinology and human behavior (p. 169).
London: Oxford University Press.
Myers, W. C., Burgess, A. W., Burgess, A. G., & Douglas, J. E. (1999). Serial
murder and homicide. In V. Van Hasselt & M. E. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook
of psychological approaches with violent offenders (pp. 153–172). New York:
Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Myers, W. C., Scott, K. I., Burgess, A. W., & Burgess, A. G. (1995). Psycho-
pathology, biopsychosocial factors, crime characteristics and classification
of 25 homicidal youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1483–1489.
Pearson, H. J. (1990). Paraphilias, impulse control and serotonin (letter). Journal of
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 10, 233.
Pfäfflin, F. (1995, September). Issues, incidence and treatment of sex offenders in
Germany. Paper presented at the International Expert Conference on Sex
Offenders, Utrecht, Netherlands.
Proulx, J., Blais, E., & Beauregard, E. (2005). Sadistic sexual aggressors. In W. L.
Marshall, Y. M. Fernandez, L. E. Marshall, & G. A. Serran (Eds.), Sexual
offender treatment: Controversial issues (pp. 61–77). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Reilly, D., Delva, N., & Hudson, R. W. (2000). Protocols for the use of cy-
proterone, medroxyprogesterone and leuprolide in the treatment of
paraphilia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, 559–563.
Ressler, R. K., Burgess, A. W., & Douglas, J. E. (1988). Sexual homicide: Patterns
and motives. New York: Free Press.
Rice, M. E., Chaplin, T. C., Harris, G. T., & Coutts, J. (1994). Empathy for the
victim and sexual arousal among rapists and nonrapists. Journal of Inter-
personal Violence, 9, 435–449.
Rieber, I., & Sigush, V. (1979). Psychosurgery on sex offenders and sex deviants
in West Germany. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 8, 523–527.
Seto, M. C., & Kuban, M. (1996). Criterion-related validity of a phallometric test
for paraphilic rape and sadism. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 175–
183.
Snaith, R. P., & Collins, S. A. (1981). Five exhibitionists and a method of
treatment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 126–130.
250 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Stekel, W. (1929). Sadism and masochism: The psychology of hatred and cruelty
(L. Brink, Trans.). New York: Horace Liveright.
Tennant, G., Bancroft, J., & Cass, J. (1974). The control of deviant sexual be-
haviour by drugs: A double-blind controlled study of benperidol, chlor-
promazine and placebo. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 3, 261–271.
Ward, N. (1975). Successful lithium treatment of transvestism associated with
manic depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 161, 204–206.
Ward, T. (2002). Good lives and the rehabilitation of offenders: Promises and
problems. Aggression and Violent Behavior: A Review Journal, 7, 513–528.
Ward, T., & Marshall, W. L. (2004). Good lives, aetiology and the rehabilitation
of sex offenders: A bridging theory. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 10, 153–
169.
Weinberger, L. E., Sreenivasan, S., Garrick, T., & Osran, H. (2005). The impact
of surgical castration on sexual recidivism risk among civilly committed
sexual offenders. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 33,
16–36.
Widiger, T. A., & Coker, L. A. (2003). Mental disorders as discrete clinical
conditions: Dimensional versus categorical classification. In M. Hersen &
S. M. Turner (Eds.), Adult psychopathology and diagnosis (4th ed., pp. 3–35).
New York: Wiley.
Wincze, J., Bansal, S., & Malamud, S. (1986). Effects of medroxyprogesterone
acetate on subjective arousal, arousal to erotic stimulation, and nocturnal
penile tumescence in male sex offenders. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15,
293–305.
11
INTRODUCTION
Most of us fear being the victims of violent crimes, particularly ones that are
sexual in nature. It is horrific enough to lose one’s life to criminal behavior,
but to be captured and tortured sexually prior to being killed is beyond
comprehension. Violent sex crimes often get sensationalized in the media and
then take on lives of their own. The hype associated with these cases leads to
inaccurate information and heightened fears, many of which are unfounded.
Additionally, there have been many movies, novels, and works of fiction about
violent sexual crimes that are often confused with actual cases. What follows is
a review of the literature regarding violent sex crimes. The focus of this
chapter will be on the act of sexual homicide, on the individuals who commit
these crimes, and on how sexual homicide is investigated.
Sexual homicide has been defined as the killing of another person in the
context of power, sexuality, and brutality (Ressler, Burgess, & Douglas, 1996).
A murder may be classified as a sexual homicide when the evidence or ob-
servations at the crime scene indicate that the murder was sexual in nature.
The evidence or observations could include the attire or lack of attire of the
victim; exposure of the sexual organs of the victim; sexual positioning of the
victim’s body; the insertion of foreign objects into the victim’s body cavities;
evidence of oral, anal, or vaginal sexual intercourse; and/or evidence of
substitute sexual activity, interest, or sadistic fantasy (Ressler et al., 1996).
252 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Individuals who commit this type of crime derive some sort of sexual satis-
faction from committing the crime, either as a result of a connection between
sexual satisfaction and violence or as the result of dominating the victim.
It is difficult to assess the number of sexual homicides for several reasons.
For instance, many crimes get reported as ordinary or motiveless homicides
even when it is obvious that the crimes were sexual in nature (Ressler et al.,
1996). Additionally, the evidence may have been inadequate to state con-
clusively that the crime was sexual in nature (Groth & Burgess, 1977), the
investigators at the scene may not have been trained to detect the underlying
sexual dynamics of the crime scene (Cormier & Simon, 1969; Revitch, 1965),
or the investigators may not have shared their findings (Ressler, Douglas,
Groth, & Burgess, 1980). It is largely agreed upon that the majority of serial
murders are sexual in nature (Lunde, 1976; Ressler, 1985; Ressler et al., 1996;
Revitch, 1965).
Hickey (2002) suggested that while serial murder is rare, the number of
serial killers surged between the years 1950 and 1995, and since 1975 the rise
has been even more dramatic. Similarly, Jenkins (1992) suggested that the
number of serial murders in American society is increasing. Schlesinger (2001)
argued that this increase in serial homicide may be an artifact of the increase in
contract killings; thus, while the rate of serial murder may have increased, the
rate of sexual homicide may not have increased. According to Hickey, the FBI
estimates that there are between 35 and 100 serial killers active in the United
States at any one time; however, Holmes and Holmes (1998) estimated that
there are as many as 200 serial killers at large. Regardless of the actual number
of serial killers at large, the odds of becoming a victim of a serial killer are
minuscule when one takes into account the size of the population as a whole
(Hickey, 2002). Serial killing does appear to be correlated with population
density, that is, states with the largest populations and large metropolitan areas
are most likely to report cases of serial murder; however, Hickey pointed out
that researchers have not been able to find any regional subcultural variables,
that is, poverty or race, that correlate with serial violence.
Mass Murderer
Mass murder is a situation in which several victims are killed within a few
moments or hours of each other (Hickey, 2002) and in one place (Holmes &
Violent Sex Crimes 253
Holmes, 1998). Mass murderers, except those who kill their own families, will
usually commit their crimes in public places (Hickey, 2002). Douglas and
Olshaker (1999) reported that a mass murderer often kills in a place that is
familiar to him, a place where he feels comfortable. The victims of mass
murderers are often intentionally selected, such as a former boss, ex-wife, or
friend, but other people who happen to be in the vicinity may also be killed.
Sometimes, however, an offender gets so frustrated that he lashes out at groups
of people who have no relationship to him. When an offender is angry at
society in general, the best way to get even is to kill innocent children, perhaps
in a schoolyard (Hickey, 2002).
Spree Killer
The spree killer kills a number of victims, usually at least three, at different
locations in a short period of hours or days (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999;
Hickey, 2002), and the killings are usually accompanied by the commission of
another felony (Holmes & Holmes, 1998). Hickey noted that spree killers
‘‘often act in a frenzy, make little effort to avoid detection, and kill in several
sequences’’ (p. 16). There appears to be no cooling-off period even though the
murders occur at different places over what may be several hours or days
(Greswell & Hollin, 1994; Hickey, 2002). Fox and Levin (2005) explained that
the short amount of time between murders for a spree killer is spent planning
and executing his crimes or evading the police.
Serial Killer
A serial killer is an individual who kills three or more people over a pe-
riod of more than thirty days with a ‘‘cooling-off’’ period between killings
(Holmes & Holmes, 1998). This is a person who hunts human beings for the
sexual thrill it gives him, and he will do it over and over again. The serial killer
individualizes his murders and often continues to kill over a longer period of
time than the types of killers described above (Hickey, 2002). During the
cooling-off periods the serial killer may continue about a daily routine that
could include going to work and spending time with friends and family (Fox &
Levin, 2005). He believes he can outwit and outmaneuver the police, sometimes
posing as a police officer, and never expects to get caught (Douglas & Olshaker,
1999; Fox & Levin, 2005). If a serial killer is apprehended, it is typically only
after he has eluded detection for weeks, months, or even years (Hickey, 2002).
In some instances a serial killer will appear to stop killing; however, it is not
known if the killer was jailed for another offense, that is, a single murder or
another crime not linked to the other murders, died due to an illness or an
accident, changed his modus operandi (method of operation), moved to a new
location, or simply decided to stop killing. A serial killer is capable of producing
quite a bit of fear since he is often thought to be killing for sport and is able to
254 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
blend in with others, making him difficult to detect. Hickey noted that he may
be one of the nicest people by day and a killer by night. Serial killers are often
described as charming, ordinary, are often loners or asocial, and may have a
good relationship with a wife or a girlfriend; likewise, it has been noted that
when a serial killer does get apprehended, neighbors, acquaintances, or co-
workers will often express shock and report that he was the last person they
would have suspected of being a vicious murderer (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999).
Fox and Levin (2005) noted that the typical serial killer does not ‘‘look or act
like the strangers that our mothers always warned us about’’ (p. 36). They added
that many serial killers are clever, inventive, and project a ‘‘nice guy’’ image that
makes them so difficult to apprehend.
Within serial murder, it is frequently noted that there are different cate-
gories. Hickey (2002) distinguished between the visionary, mission-oriented,
hedonistic, and power/control-oriented serial killers. According to Hickey’s
classification system, a visionary serial killer is an individual who murders at the
command of voices that he hears or visions that he sees; a missionary-oriented
serial killer is an individual who murders because it is his mission to get rid of
certain groups of people; a hedonistic serial killer is an individual who obtains
some sort of pleasure from the murders that he commits; and a power/control-
oriented serial killer is an individual who obtains pleasure by exerting control
over others. A sexual homicide might fall into either the hedonistic type or
power/control-oriented type of serial killing.
From a psychological perspective, the evidence at crime scenes seems to
indicate that there are two types of sexual murderers: the rape or displaced anger
murderer (Cohen, Garofalo, Boucher, & Seghorn, 1971; Groth, Burgess, &
Holmstrom, 1977; Prentky, Burgess, & Carter, 1986; Rada, 1978), and the
sadistic or lust murderer (Becker & Abel, 1978; Bromberg & Coyle, 1974;
Cohen et al., 1971; Groth, et al., 1977; Guttmacher & Weihofen, 1952; Po-
dolsky, 1966; Prentky et al., 1986; Rada, 1978; Ressler, 1985; Scully & Marolla,
1985). The rape or displaced anger murderer kills his victim after committing the
rape to avoid getting caught (Podolsky, 1966). It has been noted (Rada, 1978)
that these murderers rarely report sexual satisfaction from their murders and do
not perform postmortem sexual acts with their victims. The sadistic murderer,
however, kills as part of a ritualized, sadistic fantasy (Groth et al., 1977). As
Ressler et al. (1996) explained, for the sadistic murderer, ‘‘aggression and sex-
uality become fused into a single psychological experience—sadism—in which
aggression is eroticized’’ (p. 6). Additionally, Brittain (1970) pointed out that the
subjugation of the victim is important to the sadistic murderer, and cruelty and
infliction of pain are merely the means by which this subjugation is achieved.
(Hickey, 2002). Team killers, that is, dyads, triads, or even larger groupings,
are thought to be less common than solo killers (Hickey, 2002). With regard
to relationship, Hickey noted that sometimes the members of a team are
related, either legally (for example, spouses or stepsiblings) or by blood (for
example, siblings or parent-child combinations). However, members of a team
may also be intimately involved with each other but not related, acquain-
tances, or even strangers. As with solo serial killers, team killers are likely to
have had a sexual motivation for committing their murders (Hickey, 2002).
An example of team sexual serial killers is Kenneth Bianchi and Angelo
Buono, better known as the Hillside Stranglers, who were adoptive cousins.
It has been noted (Hickey, 2002) that within teams there is always one
person who maintains psychological control over the other member(s) of the
team. Likewise, Kelleher and Kelleher (1998) noted that while the dynamics of
team killers are frequently volatile, these teams will be dominated by a single
individual who attempts to organize the criminal activities of the team, taking a
leadership role in most of the homicides. Sometimes the control seems mystical,
as in the case of Charles Manson; however, in other cases the control may take
the form of coercion, intimidation, and persuasion (Hickey, 2002). Some
leaders have reported experiencing a sense of power and gratification, not only
through the deaths of their victims, but from getting others to do the killing for
them. If caught, the leader of the team will usually turn on the other member(s)
of the team and blame them for the murders (Hickey, 2002). Hickey also
pointed out that not all members of the team share equally in the thrill of the
kill; although, for some, killing not only becomes acceptable, but desirable.
Kelleher and Kelleher (1998) noted that the primary criminal activity of
serial-killing teams that include one male and one female is sexual homicide.
Kelleher and Kelleher stated, ‘‘male/female teams that specialize in sexual
homicide are maintained by the synergy of the sexual relationship between the
partners and their combined pathological obsession with sexual domination
and control’’ (p. 121). While a majority of the female serial killers are part of a
male-dominated team, there have been a number of cases in which the female
member is very active in murders and whose magnitude of sexual psychopathic
killing rivals that of her male counterpart (Kelleher & Kelleher, 1998).
Hickey (2002) determined that among nonrelative team killers, a man al-
most exclusively assumed leadership; in fact, very few cases have been docu-
mented in which a woman masterminded multiple homicides, was the main
decision maker, or was the main enforcer. Kelleher and Kelleher (1998) noted
that while the male partner of a serial-killing team was usually the dominant
partner, a man was often a solo sexual serial killer as well. Hickey noted that some
women who were followers went on to become ‘‘ ‘equal partners in the killing’
and participated directly in some of the bloodiest murder cases ever chronicled’’
(p. 187).
Team killers, according to Hickey (2002), are not responsible for as many
victims as solo killers. Specifically, he noted that on average team killers were
256 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
responsible for four to five killings per offender whereas solo killers had a
slightly higher average number of victims. He also noted that team killers were
most likely to remain in local proximity to their killing sites and were not as
mobile as other types of offenders (Hickey, 2002).
Team killers were similar to their solo counterparts on many background
variables. However, some of the dissimilarities are that team offenders had a
slightly higher rate of psychiatric problems, were less likely to have criminal
records for sex-related crimes, and seemed more interested in financial gain
than solo killers (Hickey, 2002). Solo killers, however, were more likely to
report feelings of rejection in childhood, remember more beatings as a child,
report having been adopted, and report parents dying or being an orphan.
Team killers, for the most part, did not receive college educations and few
received postsecondary training, such as vocational training. Most were em-
ployed in blue-collar work (Hickey, 2002).
FEMALE KILLERS
It is commonly thought that most women who kill do so in domestic
situations and, thus, are not multiple killers (Hickey, 2002). However, some of
these women go on to remarry and kill again. The notion of women as mass
murders or serial killers goes against some long-held, and perhaps sexist, views
of women, and is still quite controversial. These beliefs, held by mainstream
society, as well as those of the courts, about female killers make it less likely
that a woman will come under suspicion for multiple killings. According to
Hickey, in 1991 the FBI labeled Aileen Wuornos the nation’s first female serial
killer basically because she was the first woman to kill like a man. She was not,
however, the first female serial killer. It has been noted that there have been
approximately fifty-six female serial killers since 1900; however, statistics have
shown that the number of women who kill is relatively low in comparison to
the number of men (Hickey, 2002).
One facet that sets female serial killers apart from male killers is their
preferred choice of weapon—more specifically, poison; however, other female
killers, particularly those with an accomplice, may also resort to more violent
methods, such as shooting, bludgeoning, or stabbing (Hickey, 2002). Ac-
cording to the data in the Hickey study, female offenders differed from their
male counterparts in several ways. Female offenders, in general, selected less-
violent methods of killing, did not sexually attack the victim, did not mutilate
the corpse, and were generally not sexually involved with their victims.
Kelleher and Kelleher (1998) pointed out that the ‘‘female serial murderer was
most successful when motivated by reasons other than sex and when operating
alone’’ (p. 15). They also found that compared to the male serial killer, who
frequently attacks strangers, the female serial killer’s victim of choice is usually
someone who depends on her for care or a person with whom she has some
type of relationship. Hickey pointed out that the motives for female serial
Violent Sex Crimes 257
crimes are largely unknown, but financial security, revenge, enjoyment, and
sexual stimulation have been identified as reasons for killing. However, as
previously mentioned, women who commit serial murder are less likely to
commit sexual homicide (Keeney & Heide, 1994; Kelleher & Kelleher, 1998).
MOTIVATIONAL FACTORS
Serial killers have frequently reported experiencing trauma during their
formative years (Hickey, 2002). This trauma often took the form of instability in
the home and included such things as alcoholic parents, prostitution by mother,
incarceration of parent(s), periodic separation from parents due to trouble at
home, and psychiatric problems involving the parents (Hickey, 2002). Many
childhood factors, such as experiencing a trauma, and family of origin variables
for sexual murderers have been examined in order to determine what motivated
their acts. Several characteristics have been identified and include childhood
abuse, neglect, poverty, violence in the home, violence in the media, exposure
to pornography, genetics, cognitive disabilities, insanity, PMS, blood sugar
imbalance, and/or substance abuse. However, these variables either singularly
or in combination have been identified in the backgrounds of many individuals
who do not grow up to be serial killers. While these variables have been
identified in, and seem to be correlated with deviant offenders, these variables
are not causal, that is, possessing these characteristics does not cause an individual
to commit serial murder.
Hazelwood and Michaud (2001) have noted two disturbing trends with
regard to sexual fantasies: (1) ‘‘Offenders today are conceptualizing their crimes
at a much earlier age than their predecessors did,’’ and (2) ‘‘Their fantasies are
growing more complex and, in some cases, deadlier over time’’ (p. 19). As
previously mentioned, Hazelwood and Michaud noted that an individual
might fantasize about engaging in sexual murder but will not actually engage in
the behavior. The distinction between individuals who only fantasize about
committing a sexual murder and those who actually commit a sexual murder is
that the latter actually made the choice to cross over from fantasy to reality.
Salter (2003) stated that individuals who actually commit rape differ from those
who only fantasize about rape in that the rape fantasies are more prevalent, are
more obsessive, and are more important to the rapist.
It is not enough to know that deviant fantasies are part of the motivation
for committing a sexual homicide; it is also important to examine what actually
motivates an individual to act out his aberrant sexual fantasies. It is sometimes
mistakenly believed that rape is a sexually motivated act and that the offender
committed the crime because he was ‘‘horny’’ (Hazelwood & Michaud, 2001);
however, sexual assault is an act of aggression, an assertion of power, an
expression of anger, or some combination thereof. The rapist achieves grati-
fication, not from the sexual release, but from the result of having power,
exerting aggression, and expressing anger, while gaining the thrill of domi-
nation and control. Rapists are basically using sex as a tool of aggression as it
serves nonsexual needs (Hazelwood & Michaud, 2001). Hickey (2002) stated
that sexual assault appears to be the method of gaining control over the vic-
tims, which is very similar to an individual who commits sexual homicide for
which he might receive sexual gratification from the power, control, or
domination that he has over his victim.
and the partner may or may not consent to the activity. What is important is the
suffering of the partner; this is the stimulus that elicits sexual arousal in these
individuals. Sadistic fantasies or behaviors may include activities that indicate the
dominance of the sadist over the partner, such as being forced to engage in
submissive behavior and restraints, bondage, beating, burning, rape, cutting,
stabbing, strangulation, torture, and/or mutilation. Killing of the partner may
also occur.
Necrophilia, a sexual attraction to corpses, may take a variety of forms.
Rosman and Resnick (1989) distinguished between three forms of genuine
necrophilia—necrophilic homicide, the commission of murder in order to
obtain a corpse for sexual purposes; regular necrophilia, the use of already dead
bodies for sexual purposes; and necrophilic fantasy, the fantasizing about sexual
activity with a corpse without actually engaging in necrophilic behavior. In
examining the motivations behind necrophilia, it was noted that 68 percent of
the individuals in their sample (n ¼ 34) engaged in necrophilia in order to have
an unresisting and unrejecting partner, and 12 percent of the sample engaged
in necrophilia in an ‘‘attempt to gain self-esteem by the expression of power
over homicide victims’’ (p. 159). Other reasons for engaging in necrophilia
included reunion with a romantic partner (21 percent), conscious sexual at-
traction to corpses (15 percent), and attempting to gain comfort or overcome
feelings of isolation (15 percent).
PROFILING
The examination of criminal behavior, particularly sexual homicide, is mul-
tidisciplinary with fields such as psychology, sociology, geography, biology,
260 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
History of Profiling
Attempts at profiling prior to the 1970s were basically composite con-
struction of murderers, which described the ‘‘typical murderer’’ (Palmer, 1960;
Rizzo, 1982). Criminal profiling, as we now think of it, began informally in the
early 1970s by using crime scene information to infer various offender charac-
teristics to help in the apprehension of the criminal (Ressler et al., 1996). During
the early days, criminal profiling was referred to as ‘‘psychological profiling’’ or
‘‘criminal personality profiling’’ and was a little-known and spare-time service
that was provided to local law enforcement officers if requested (Hazelwood &
Michaud, 2001). These newer criminal profile analyses conducted by the FBI
proved useful in identifying offenders and, due to the requests of local author-
ities, were then made available to all law enforcement agencies (Ressler et al.,
1996). Within a span of a decade, criminal profiling became very popular. The
results of a 1981 evaluation questionnaire revealed that criminal profiling had
helped to focus 77 percent of the cases in which the subjects were later identified
(Ressler et al., 1996). Today, criminal profiling is just part of a more compre-
hensive behavioral assessment program called criminal investigative analysis
(McCrary & Ramsland, 2003). For more information on criminal investigative
analysis, see McCrary and Ramsland, The Unknown Darkness (2003).
Violent Sex Crimes 261
What Profilers Do
Criminal profilers examine crime scenes for clues that reveal behaviors
that are characteristic of, and perhaps unique to, the offender. Hazelwood and
Michaud (2001) stated that although investigators can find patterns and
common elements among offenders, no two offenders ever commit the exact
same sexual crime. ‘‘The crime scene may be the point of abduction, a location
where the victim was held, the murder scene, and/or the final body location’’
(Ressler et al., 1996, p. xiii). Crime scene characteristics, as described by
Ressler et al., include ‘‘those elements of physical evidence found at the crime
scene that may reveal behavioral traits of the murderer’’ (p. xiii). This could
include a variety of physical remnants of the crime, including a weapon, tools
used in the crime, positioning of the victim, and evidence of various acts
committed against the victim, just to name a few. Ressler et al. defined profile
characteristics as ‘‘those variables that identify the offender as an individual and
together form a composite picture of the suspect’’ (p. xxii). Profile charac-
teristics may consist of any defining feature about an individual, including the
perpetrator’s sex, age, occupation, level of intelligence, acquaintance with the
victim, residence, and mode of transportation. According to Hazelwood and
Michaud, the most difficult crimes to profile are those in which there is no
known cause of death, an unidentified victim, and/or a lack of behavior to
study and analyze.
Criminal profilers assist with investigations by describing the type of indi-
vidual who committed a crime (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999). A major fallacy that
exists is that profilers can identify a specific person as the individual who com-
mitted a crime. Ressler et al. (1996) pointed out that rather than providing the
identity of the offender, a criminal profile indicates the kind of person who is most
likely to have committed a crime based on observation of the characteristics at
the crime scene. Thus, criminal profilers can help law enforcement narrow its
field of investigation and concentrate its efforts in a particular area, but cannot tell
the law enforcement personnel who is responsible for the crime, (Ressler et al.,
1996; Hazelwood & Michaud, 2001; see also Chapter 9 in this volume).
Criminal investigative analysis has been shown to be especially useful in
solving cases of sexual homicide (Ressler et al., 1996). To an untrained in-
vestigator, many of these crimes appear to be motiveless and the crime scenes
seem to offer few obvious clues about the killer’s identity, which may often be
the case in many sexual homicide crime scenes. Since obvious as well as
implied clues are pieced together to form leads that contribute to the killer’s
profile, attention to detail is extremely important when profiling crime scenes
(Ressler et al., 1996). Hazelwood and Michaud (2001) stated that when cre-
ating a profile for any type of crime, it is very important for the profiler to
maintain an open mind and not lock in on only one possibility. Victims of a
killer often share common characteristics that may or may not, at first, be
262 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Criminal/Offender Profiling
Criminal profiling is the way in which law enforcement has sought to
combine the information from research in other disciplines with more tradi-
tional investigative techniques in an effort to combat violent crime (Ressler
Violent Sex Crimes 263
Victim Profiling
In order for a profiler to assist in identifying an offender, he or she must
understand the motive for the crime, and, according to Douglas and Olshaker
(1999), the key to understanding motive is in the victimology. Specifically, the
profiler wants to know who the offender has chosen as his victim and why
(that is, if it was a victim of opportunity or if a careful and deliberate choice
was made). This is based on the assumption that behavior reflects personality
and, even though every crime is unique, behavior fits into certain patterns
(Douglas & Olshaker, 1999). Identifying significant pieces of the crime pattern
enables the profiler to determine why the offender committed the crime,
which will aid in answering the ultimate question of who committed the
crime. For example, when examining a break-in, it is important to know what
items were taken because this can provide valuable information as to what type
of perpetrator you should be looking for and what his motive was for com-
mitting the crime (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999). Specifically, if the property
taken was valuable and could be sold for cash, you will have one type of
offender; however, if the property taken was some personal item of little value,
such as women’s underwear, then you have a very different type of offender.
The offender who stole the women’s underwear had a very different motive
than the offender looking for valuable property to sell. Knowing the motive
helps the officials know the dangerousness of the offender. A panty thief does
not take women’s underwear because he cannot afford to buy them; the theft
is motivated by the sexual images related to the items, the fantasy, and the
associated sexual arousal (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999). Based on experience
264 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
from other crimes, profilers know that fetish burglars are not likely to stop on
their own; however, criminals who commit crimes for different motives, that
is, for money or for drugs, may stop when they gain employment or enter
rehab (Douglas & Olshaker, 1999).
An examination of the victim in relation to the offender helps us to un-
derstand the social dynamics of serial murder (Hickey, 2002). It allows investi-
gators to clarify the victim side of the killer-victim relationship and to measure, in
part, the degree of vulnerability and culpability of some victims. Research on
victims of serial killers has shown that they were more likely to be killed away
from their homes, which means that they may have been in areas of the com-
munity where their assailants had easy access. Three categories of potential
victims—family, acquaintances, and strangers—have been identified. Research has
demonstrated that serial killers most often kill strangers, whereas with homicide
in general, relatives and close friends are most often the victims (Hickey, 2002).
Various reasons have been offered as to why the majority of serial killers
focus on strangers. It may be easier to dehumanize a stranger, which enables
the killer to view the victims as objects of hatred and lust; the offender likely
perceives that killing strangers provides some level of safety from detection
(Hickey, 2002); and the offender might get a thrill from seeking out unsus-
pecting strangers (Leyton, 1986). Most homicides are committed by an indi-
vidual with whom the victim had a relationship, and, as a result, the focus of
the investigation will be on the victim’s friends and family members until the
death is linked to a serial killer.
With regard to victim selection, some victims are chosen because they
match the killer’s paraphilic fantasy; some murderers engage in proxy killings in
which they focus on individuals who remind them of someone, perhaps their
mother (Hickey, 2002); and there have been victims who have just been at the
wrong place at the wrong time. Some offenders are drawn to victims who
represent what they want for themselves, such as beauty, wealth, or assertive-
ness; other offenders destroy those who symbolize what they fear or loathe,
such as gay individuals, the homeless, the elderly, and the infirm (Hickey,
2002). The latter groups of individuals represent what Egger (2003) calls the
‘‘less dead,’’ or the ‘‘devalued stratum of humanity’’ (p. 48). He refers to these
groups—gays, homeless, prostitutes, migrant workers, runaways, elderly, in-
firm—as ‘‘less dead’’ because they were ‘‘less alive’’ before their violent deaths.
In other words, these groups were marginalized and devalued members of the
community who were seen as vulnerable and powerless by the perpetrator.
There is, unfortunately, support for Egger’s proposition that marginalized
groups are viewed as being ‘‘less alive’’ and that their deaths will not cause a
public outcry. For example, the task force for the Green River serial killer was
disbanded prior to the conclusion of their work due to lack of public support
and dwindling leads even though the investigation produced over fifty verifi-
able victims, most of whom were prostitutes (Egger, 2003).
Violent Sex Crimes 265
The degree of power and control the killer is able to exert over the
victim is another factor influencing victim selection (Hickey, 2002). Serial
killers seem to carefully target and prey upon individuals whom they perceive
as less physically and intellectually capable than themselves. These categories of
strangers, while not mutually exclusive, were reportedly the most frequently
sought after by serial killers: young women alone, including female college
students and prostitutes; children, both boys and girls; and travelers, including
hitchhikers. On the one hand, when acquaintances were killed, the top three
categories that represent the majority of victims were friends and neighbors,
children, and women alone; on the other hand, when the victims were family
members, children, husbands, and wives were the top three categories. When
the three categories of victims (strangers, acquaintances, and family) were
combined, women and children made up the majority of the victims, which
makes sense if these offenders prey on those they perceive as weaker, helpless,
or as having less power and control. This statistic is in sharp contrast to
homicide in general, in which 78 percent of the victims were men. Another
difference between the types of homicide is that the majority of victims of
serial killers are Caucasian, whereas overall, the majority of general homi-
cide victims are African American. Young and middle-aged adults and teens
were the most likely targets of serial killers, but the very young and the elderly
were also represented. Hickey has noted that since 1975, there has been an
increase in those offenders who target only the elderly. Hickey suggested that
with the aging of the population in the United States, nursing homes and
hospitals may need to pay close attention to employees to prevent individuals
from living out their ‘‘angel of death’’ fantasies.
Victim facilitation, or the degree to which victims make themselves ac-
cessible or vulnerable to attack, is another factor that needs to be considered
(Hickey, 2002). Most serial killers murder strangers and their victimization
may be determined by the degree to which the victim placed him- or herself
in a vulnerable situation (Hickey, 2002). Reiss (1980) determined that victims
who had been multiply victimized were more likely to experience the same
form of victimization than be subject to two different criminal acts. McDo-
nald (as cited in Hickey, 2002) determined that victim-prone individuals
developed certain attitudes and lifestyle choices that increased their vulnera-
bility. People who hitchhike or prostitute themselves, as well as individuals
who pick up hitchhikers and prostitutes, are considered high facilitators. Low-
facilitation victims can be thought of as sharing little or no responsibility for
victimization, such as when a stranger kidnaps a child playing in a yard or when
a nurse poisons a patient in a nursing home. The risk of being a victim of a
serial killer is small and most victims are considered low facilitation, but there
are those who are at greater risk as a result of their age, gender, place of res-
idence, or lifestyle, and the number of these individuals is increasing (Hickey,
2002).
266 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
Children are more likely to die in domestic homicide than at the hands of
a serial killer, but they can be at risk both in and out of the home (Hickey,
2002). Female serial killers of children are more likely to murder either their
own children or those of relatives, whereas male offenders were seven times
more likely to be strangers to their victims. The primary motives for female
offenders of children from highest to lowest were financial, to collect insur-
ance money; to exert control; enjoyment; and sexual gratification (the last two
were tied at 8 percent). The primary motives of male offenders of children in
order from highest to lowest were sexual gratification, to exert control, en-
joyment, and financial reasons; however, the majority of men reported hav-
ing a combination of reasons for why they murdered children. Serial killers
who kill children engage in a variety of methods to lure children. They will
sometimes ask for the child’s assistance, perhaps in looking for a lost puppy;
they will sometimes tell the children that there has been an emergency and that
they are there to escort them home; they will sometimes use a badge to look as
if they are an authority figure; or they may sometimes appeal to the child’s ego
by telling the child that he or she should be in a beauty contest or in a
television commercial (Wooden, 1984; Hickey, 2002). As is true with those
who kill adults, the offender is usually a psychopath and will use a combination
of techniques that begin with charisma or manipulation, move to intimidation,
and, ultimately, become brutal (Hickey, 2002).
Linkage Analysis
Criminal investigative analysts use a different profiling procedure called
linkage analysis to determine if a murder is linked to other murders and to help
the investigators determine if the same killer is responsible for multiple ho-
micides (Hazelwood & Michaud, 2001). Investigators using linkage analysis
look for a particular modus operandi and ritual behaviors, that is, type of
weapon used, age and gender of victim, performance of sexual acts, amount of
violence used, etc. They also look to see if the murders were grouped to-
gether, both geographically and chronologically. According to Hazelwood and
Michaud, in addition to examining the similarities in the crime scenes, it is also
important to look at the differences, as dissimilarities in crime scenes do not
necessarily mean that different people committed the crimes. Dissimilarities in
various aspects of two crime scenes could merely mean that as the killer
committed more murders, he altered his preferred method of killing (that is,
changed type of weapon, acted alone rather than as part of a team, chose a
different type of victim, used a different method to dispose of the body, etc.)
(Hazelwood & Michaud, 2001). Differences or inconsistencies in crime scenes
could also be due to variables such as the specific crime scene circumstances,
victim behavior, the amount of time the offender has, and even the killer’s
mood. Linkage analysis can be a valuable tool to link cases together in situa-
Violent Sex Crimes 267
tions where there are no reliable witnesses or physical evidence (Hazelwood &
Michaud, 2001).
Problems in Profiling
Profiling should be considered one tool that can assist in the investigation
of criminal behavior. It is often, as stated earlier, a technique that is employed
as a last resort, when all other leads have been exhausted. When using profiling
as a tool in investigations, one has to always consider that the profile could lead
investigators in the wrong direction, which is a waste of resources and could
268 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
facilitate the loss of additional lives (Hickey, 2002). Even if the profile is based
on the best crime scene evidence and employs the most precise attention to
detail, it is rarely 100 percent accurate, which, again, may cause investigators to
ignore other leads that do not match the information in the profile (Goodroe,
1987; Hickey, 2002). Additionally, some investigators may not understand
how to properly use the information in the profile. They may base their
conclusions about the identity of a suspect or whether two cases can be linked
to a common suspect on one piece of physical evidence from the crime
scene(s) that was mentioned in the report (Hickey, 2002). In conclusion, it
takes experienced investigators to thoroughly investigate a crime and create a
profile, and it takes investigators trained in their use to properly use profiles
once they are obtained (see Chapter 9 in this volume).
REFERENCES
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., Cunningham-Rathner, J., Mittelman, M., & Rouleau,
J. L. (1988). Multiple paraphilic diagnoses among sex offenders. Bulletin of
the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 16, 153–168.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., Text Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Becker, J. V., & Abel, G. G. (1978). Men and the victimization of women. In
J. R. Chapman & M. R. Gates (Eds.), Victimization of women. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage.
Brittain, R. P. (1970). The sadistic murderer. Medical Science and the Law, 10, 198–207.
Bromberg, W., & Coyle, E. (1974). Rape! A compulsion to destroy. Medical
Insight, April, 21–22, 24–25.
Casey-Owens, M. (1984). The anonymous letter-writer—a psychological profile?
Journal of Forensic Sciences, 29, 816–819.
Cohen, M. L., Garofalo, R. F., Boucher, R., & Seghorn, T. (1971). The psy-
chology of rapists. Seminars in Psychiatry, 3, 307–327.
Cormier, B. S., & Simon, S. P. (1969). The problem of the dangerous sexual
offender. Canadian Psychiatric Association, 14, 329–334.
Douglas, J., & Olshaker, M. (1999). The anatomy of motive. New York: Pocket
Books.
Egger, S. (2003). The need to kill: Inside the world of the serial killer. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Fox, J. A., & Levin, J. (2005). Extreme killing: Understanding serial and mass murder.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Geberth, V. J. (1981). Psychological profiling. Law and Order, 29, 46–49.
Goodroe, C. (1987). Tracking the serial offender. Law and Order, 35, 29–33.
Gresswell, D. M., & Hollin, C. R. (1994). Multiple murder: A review. British
Journal of Criminology, 34, 1–13.
Groth, A. N., & Burgess, A. W. (1977). Sexual dysfunction during rape. New
England Journal of Medicine, 297, 764–766.
Violent Sex Crimes 269
Groth, A. N., Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. (1977). Rape: power, anger
and sexuality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1239–1243.
Guttmacher, M. S., & Weihofen, H. (1952). Psychiatry and the law. New York:
Norton.
Hazelwood, R. R. (1983). The behavior oriented interview of rape victims: The
key to profiling. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 52, 8–15.
Hazelwood, R., & Michaud, S. G. (2001). Dark dreams: Sexual violence, homicide,
and the criminal mind. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Hickey, E. W. (2002). Serial murderers and their victims (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Holmes, R. M., & Holmes, S. T. (1998). Serial murder (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Holmes, R. M., & Holmes, S. T. (2002). Profiling violent crimes: An investigative tool
(3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jenkins, P. (1992). A murder ‘‘wave?’’ Trends in American serial homicide 1940–
1990. Criminal Justice Review, 17, 1–19.
Keeney, B. T., & Heide, K. M. (1994). Gender differences in serial murderers: A
preliminary analysis. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 383–398.
Kelleher, M. D., & Kelleher, C. L. (1998). Murder most rare: The female serial killer.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Leyton, E. (1986). Hunting humans: Inside the minds of mass murderers. New York:
Pocket Books.
Lunde, D. T. (1976). Murder and madness. San Francisco: San Francisco Books.
McCrary, G. O., & Ramsland, K. (2003). The unknown darkness: Profiling the
predators among us. New York: HarperCollins.
Miron, M. S., & Douglas, J. E. (1979). Threat analysis: The psycholinguistic
approach. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 48, 5–9.
Palmer, S. (1960). A study of murder. New York: Thomas Crowell.
Podolsky, E. (1966). Sexual violence. Medical Digest, 34, 60–63.
Prentky, R. A., Burgess, A. W., & Carter, D. L. (1986). Victim responses by rapist
type: An empirical and clinical analysis. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1,
73–98.
Rada, R. T. (1978). Clinical aspects of the rapist. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Reiss, A., Jr. (1980). Victim proneness in repeat victimization by type of crime. In
S. Fineberg & A. Reiss, Jr. (Eds.), Indicators of crime and criminal justice:
Quantitative studies (pp. 41–54). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice.
Ressler, R. K. (1985). Violent crimes. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 54, 1–31.
Ressler, R. K., Burgess, A. W., & Douglas, J. E. (1996). Sexual homicide: Patterns
and motives. New York: The Free Press.
Ressler, R. K., Douglas, J. E., Groth, A. N., & Burgess, A. W. (1980). Offender
profiling: A multidisciplinary approach. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, 49,
16–20.
Revitch, E. (1965). Sex murderer and the potential sex murderer. Diseases of the
Nervous System, 26, 640–648.
270 Sexual Deviation and Sexual Offenses
abuse. See also alcohol abuse; child sexuality and, 89, 167; treatment of,
sexual abuse; childhood abuse; 167
childhood sexual abuse; drug/ AIDS. See HIV/AIDS
alcohol abuse; sexual abuse; alcohol abuse: approach goal programs
substance abuse: exploration/ for, 199; among female sexual
exploitation and, 60; by female offenders, 52; myopia theory and,
babysitters, 60 113; sexual assault and, 105–106,
abuse reenactment, by juvenile female 111–112, 114
sex offenders, 73 American Psychiatric Association
abused-abuser hypothesis, of (APA), 1; sexual deviation according
pedophilia, 89 to, 82
acceptance-based therapy, 170 American Psychological Association
adolescence. See also juvenile female (APA) Ethical Guidelines, 221
sex offenders: bestiality in, 35; incest anger management training, 167
impact during, 143 antiandrogen treatment: cyproterone
adolescent sexual abuse victims, 136; acetate, 163, 240, 245;
eating disorders among, 137; sub- medroxyprogesterone acetate, 163,
stance abuse by, 137; suicide and, 136 240, 246; for sex offenders, 163–164;
adult male incest offenders, 144–145 for sexual sadists, 239
adult on adult assault, by female sex anxiety management training, for
offenders, 65 sexual assault victims, 122–123
aggression: among homosexual APA. See American Psychiatric
offenders, 67; among pedophiles, 89; Association
272 Index
codirector of the Centre for the Study of Critical Incident Decision Making
(www.incscid.org). His work involves training and evaluating responses to
high-profile incidents, including security planning, siege, anticorruption in-
vestigations, and most recently, debriefing international negotiating teams. His
work has attracted the attention of the United Kingdom police, the fire ser-
vice, specialist operations, the intelligence services and, more recently, the
United Nations in New York. Professor Alison has published widely on of-
fender profiling and psychological contributions to law enforcement practice
in major incidents. His current research focuses on decision-making and
leadership qualities in critical incident management. He has provided psy-
chological advice in armed robbery, murder, rape, and child abduction cases.
He has presented his work in several international journals and recently
published The Forensic Psychologists Casebook: Psychological Profiling and Criminal
Investigation.
and revictimization. Her more specific research interests address the role of
alcohol and other risk factors in impacting women’s risk for assault with the
ultimate aim of integrating this information into intervention programs aimed
at victims of sexual assault.