CDPS Reappraisal 1
CDPS Reappraisal 1
CDPS Reappraisal 1
Current Directions in Psychological Science XX(X) 16 The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721412461500 http://cdps.sagepub.com
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester; 2Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco; and 3Department of Psychology, Harvard University
Abstract Contrary to lay beliefs, physiological changes that co-occur with stress are not necessarily bad. Much can be done during stressful experiences to promote adaptive responses. In this article, we review recent research on one method for improving acute stress responses: reappraising arousal. A growing body of research suggests that cognitive appraisals are powerful tools that help shift negative stress states to more positive ones. Arousal reappraisal instructs individuals to think of stress arousal as a tool that helps maximize performance. By reframing the meaning of the physiological signals that accompany stress, arousal reappraisal breaks the link between negative affective experiences and malignant physiological responses. We demonstrate how this approach can benefit physiological reactivity, attention, and performance and explore its potential applications. Keywords stress, reappraisal, emotion regulation, psychophysiology Stress is ubiquitous in daily life and typically is a negative experience. Indeed, people devote considerable time and resources to reducing stress levels via regulatory behaviors, such as vacationing, exercising, or having some cocktails at the local pub. These efforts, however, do not change stressful experiences but rather provide an escape from day-to-day stressors. What are the options for coping with acute stress when escape is not possible? What can people do in the moment to modify the stressful experience? Research indicates that cognitive processes, particularly reappraisal, can shift negative stress to positive stress. other way off the mountain than plunging down this trail. Regardless of your affinity for skiing, this situation would likely elicit an increase in physiological arousal. Avid skiers might experience excitement, believing that they could handle the difficult trail, whereas novices would be more likely to experience fear if the difficulty of the trail were perceived to exceed their skill level. Thus, arousal is semantically and psychologically fuzzy (Blascovich, 1992). Our responses depend in large part on how a situation and our bodys responses are construed. The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat offers an explanation of how appraisals and situations interact to shape stress responses (see Blascovich & Mendes, 2010, for a review). Both challenge and threat states are experienced during acute stress but differ in antecedent appraisal processes and downstream physiological responses. Individuals experience challenge when appraisals of personal resources exceed situational demandslike the expert skiers in the example above. Alternatively, threat manifests when perceived demands exceed resources. Although both states are accompanied by sympathetic activation, challenge is characterized by improved cardiac efficiency and dilation of the peripheral vasculature,
Corresponding Author: Jeremy P. Jamieson, University of Rochester, 437 Meliora Hall, Rochester, NY 14627 E-mail: jeremy.jamieson@rochester.edu
Background
Scientists have long believed that the mind and body are tightly linked, with changes in one directly affecting the other. Seminal work by Schachter and Singer (1962), for example, specified that cognitive processes, physiological signals, and situational cues interact to determine emotions. The idea that the mind and body operate in concert to produce psychological states is evident in current models of emotion. For instance, Conceptual Act Theory argues that appraisal transforms internal states into emotions by integrating bodily changes with external sensory information and knowledge of the situation (Barrett, 2006). To understand how the body and mind work together, imagine you are a skier staring down a steep, icy slope with no
2 whereas threat decreases cardiac efficiency and constricts the vasculature in anticipation of damage or defeat. Commonly held beliefs suggest that arousal experienced during stress is bad, but sympathetic activation may actually be greater during approach-motivated challenge states than during threat states. This notion is consistent with the idea of physiological toughness, which suggests that activation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) facilitates effective coping and improves performance in situations of acute stress (Dienstbier, 1989). Using the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat as a framework, recent studies have sought to improve acute stress responses by altering appraisals of arousal (e.g., Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, 2010; Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, 2012a, 2012b). In this line of research, research participants are told that the physiological arousal experienced during stressful situations can be thought of as a resource that aids performance. Participants who reframe the meaning of the physiological signals that accompany stress as beneficial experience more positive outcomes than those who do not (Fig. 1). Research on reappraising arousal has extended work on emotion regulation (Gross, 1998, 2002) and cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT; Hofmann & Smits, 2008). The underlying
Jamieson et al. theme of these approaches is that changing cognitions produces downstream benefits. Reappraisal, as specified by emotion-regulation models, typically involves the reinterpretation of the affective meaning of contextual cues. In other words, emotionally charged stimuli are presented, and participants are instructed to reinterpret the stimuli (e.g., The images are fake) or distance themselves from the stimuli (e.g., by adopting a third-person perspective; Kross & Ayduk, 2011; Ochsner & Gross, 2008). Clinical researchers developed CBT to help improve patient outcomes by modifying faulty affective responses and cognitions (Barlow, 2004). For instance, depressive patients are taught to identify errors in thinking (e.g., Everyone hates me and always will) and replace them with more rational thoughts. In the classic emotion-regulation literature, reappraisal has often (but not always) centered on decreasing sympathetic activation during passive tasks (e.g., Gross, 2002). Likewise, reappraisal in clinical research typically either decreases arousal (e.g., mindfulness meditation; Cincotta, Gehrman, Gooneratne, & Baime, 2011) or teaches individuals to accept arousal (e.g., interoceptive exposure; Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004). Decreased SNS arousal might be adaptive when no instrumental cognitive or physical responses are
a
Stressful Situation Increased Physiological Arousal Negative Arousal Appraisals
Negative Affect Maladaptive Physiological Responses Vigilance for Threat Cues Performance Impairments
b
Increased Physiological Arousal
Reduced Negative Affect Adaptive Physiological Responses Reduced Bias for Threat Cues Facilitated Performance
Stressful Situation
Fig. 1. The impact of stress on downstream outcomes and the effects of arousal reappraisal. As shown in panel (a), stressful situations are accompanied by increased physiological arousal, which is typically construed in a negative manner. These negative appraisals of arousal feed forward to produce myriad negative outcomes, including negative affect, maladaptive patterns of physiological reactivity, increased vigilance for threat cues, and impaired performance. As shown in panel (b), arousal-reappraisal manipulations break the association between stress-based arousal and negative appraisals. By severing this link, arousal-reappraisal techniques help shift negative stress states to more positive ones, leading to a reduction in negative affect, more adaptive patterns of physiological reactivity, reduced attentional bias for threat cues, and improved performance.
Reappraisal and Acute Stress Reponses required, but during active tasks, increased SNS arousal can facilitate mobilization of oxygenated blood to the brain and periphery, thereby improving performance. Arousal reappraisal narrows in on acutely stressful events that require active responding and identifies bodily responses, specifically sympathetic arousal, as a coping tool. That is, arousal reappraisal seeks to alter cardiovascular responses so as to promote adaptive responding during acute stress (cf. Dienstbier, 1989; Mendes & Jamieson, 2011). It is not aimed at eliminating or dampening stress arousal but instead focuses on changing the type of stress response.
3 activation (Nater & Rohleder, 2009) that tends to covary with catecholamines (e.g., dopamine, epinephrine [adrenaline], and norepinephrine; Rohleder, Nater, Wolf, Ehlert, & Kirschbaum, 2004). Participants assigned to reappraise arousal exhibited an increase in alpha amylase and improvements in their performance on the quantitative section of the practice GRE, relative to no-instruction controls. One to three months after the laboratory session, participants returned to the lab with their score reports from the actual GRE. Compared with controls, reappraisal participants scored higher on the quantitative section of the actual GRE and reported that arousal on the day of the test had aided their performance. These findings demonstrate that a brief laboratory-based reappraisal manipulation may have sustained effects on stress appraisals and performance. Taken together, the aforementioned research demonstrates that reappraising arousal as a coping tool during acutely stressful episodes that require instrumental responses can promote adaptive physiological responses, reduce attentional bias, and improve performance. However, the literature has yet to pin down the exact mechanisms of change. For example, in the GRE study, reappraisal participants exhibited long-term benefits, but we are uncertain of why the effects persisted. Reappraisal participants may have engaged in the same reappraisal they learned in the lab during the actual test, or their success in the lab may have reinforced studying and improved performance through learning. Future research should attempt to uncover when and why reappraisal might stick.
4 situations so as to decrease their emotional impact exhibit more adaptive responses to anger provocation compared with individuals low in reappraisal tendencies (Mauss, Cook, Cheng, & Gross, 2007; Schmader, Forbes, Zhang, & Mendes, 2009). Thus, some people might simply be better than others at applying reappraisal instructions. Another variable to consider is interoceptive accuracy the ability to perceive ones bodily changes (Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004). It is unclear whether individuals with high interoceptive accuracy would benefit more or less from arousal reappraisal. If sympathetic activation produces the improvements seen in our work (cf. Dienstbier, 1989), then accurate interoceptors may exhibit greater improvements than poor interoceptors because of their ability to perceive increases in sympathetic arousal (Werner, Duschek, Mattern, & Schandry, 2009). Conversely, stress and arousal tend to be negatively perceived, so good interoceptors may exhibit more rigid negative-arousal appraisals and be less inclined to believe reappraisal instructions. Alternatively, interoception may be unrelated to reappraisal, given that research has yet to show whether good interoceptors can differentiate types of stress. Studying moderators will also help specify the conditions necessary for reappraisal effects to manifest while highlighting limitations. One likely necessary condition is motivation. If individuals are disengaged, then altering arousal appraisals will not influence outcomes. To illustrate this point, in the GRE study (Jamieson et al., 2010), if we had recruited college freshmen for whom this particular test was not currently selfrelevant, reappraisal may not have affected performance. In other words, the stakes must be high. Another limitation evident in the GRE study is that arousal reappraisal instructions benefited only quantitative performance; verbal performance was unaffected. Compared with verbal problems, math problems generally require more active processing. Thus, reappraisal may benefit performance only when active-processing demands are high.
Jamieson et al. Notably, people with SAD display a strong attentional bias for emotionally negative information and interpret ambiguous social situations as threatening (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007). As we have shown, arousal reappraisal attenuates threat vigilance, which may lessen the likelihood that anxious individuals will experience future situations as threatening. In fact, recently completed research (Jamieson et al., 2012b) has provided initial evidence that reappraisal can improve socially anxious individuals responses to evaluative stress. Of course, additional work is needed to further explore potential clinical applications, but these initial results are promising. More broadly, research on psychological treatments has primarily focused on improving patient outcomes rather than identifying mechanisms of change (Kazdin, 2011; Kazdin & Nock, 2003). That is, clinical trials typically test packages of techniques, not individual components, with careful measurement of immediate outcomes. So, although reappraisal is a primary component of CBT, we do not necessarily know how effective reappraisal is itself or, if reappraisal is effective, how it works. The paradigms and experimental procedures reviewed here can help researchers test putative mechanisms. Finally, the translational implications of reappraisal have yet to be explored. The medical literature suggests that prevention is more effective than curative treatments (Leaf, 1993). Forestalling disease development is preferred to treating symptoms. In the work described here, reappraisal instructions were sufficient to alter affective, physiological, and cognitive processes. Given that adaptive responses to acute stress improve peoples ability to cope with future stressors (Dienstbier, 1989), health education might seek to incorporate information about the functionality of stress. The potential for such an approach can be seen in research showing that a brief self-affirmation intervention at the outset of a semester improved students classroom performance and reduced racial achievement gaps months and even years later (Yeager & Walton, 2011). In sum, recent research on arousal reappraisal has taken seriously the idea that the body and mind interact reciprocally and that embodiment effects are situated in a broader context. Taking this work into a translational arena may have myriad benefits, including assisting clinical psychologists in identifying mechanisms of change in CBT, providing guidance to organizational and sports psychologists on improving performance, helping educational psychologists facilitate learning, and more.
Recommended Reading
Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2010). (See References). A review of social psychophysiology. Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2008). (See References). A metaanalysis of CBT. Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012a). (See References). An empirical study of the effects of arousal reappraisal on cardiovascular functioning. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). (See References). A review of selfdistancing emotion-regulation techniques.
5
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012b). Changing the conceptualization of stress improves affective and physiological outcomes in social anxiety disorder. Manuscript submitted for publication. Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Evidence-based treatment research: Advances, limitations, and next steps. American Psychologist, 66, 685698. Kazdin, A. E., & Nock, M. K. (2003). Delineating the mechanisms of change in child and adolescent therapy: Methodological issues and research recommendations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 11161129. Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The Trier Social Stress TestA tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 7681. Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 187191. Leaf, A. (1993). Preventive medicine for our ailing health care system. Journal of the American Medical Association, 269, 616618. Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 35, 747766. Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., Cheng, J. Y., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal: Experiential and physiological responses to an anger provocation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 116124. Mendes, W. B., & Jamieson, J. P. (2011). Embodied stereotype threat: Exploring brain and body mechanisms underlying performance impairments. In M. Inzlicht & T. Schmader (Eds.), Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application (pp. 5168). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The link between social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 13061315. Nater, U. M., & Rohleder, N. (2009). Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: Current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 486 496. Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 12151229. Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 153158. Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., Wolf, J. M., Ehlert, U., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Psychosocial stress-induced activation of salivary alpha amylase: An indicator of sympathetic activity? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032, 258263. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379399. Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & Mendes, W. B. (2009). A metacognitive perspective on the cognitive deficits experienced in intellectually threatening situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 584596.
References
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 124. Barlow, D. H. (2004). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of panic. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Barlow, D. H., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Master your anxiety and panic: Workbook. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 2046. Blascovich, J. (1992). A biopsychosocial approach to arousal regulation. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 11, 213237. Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2010). Social psychophysiology and embodiment. In S. T. Fiske & D. T. Gilbert (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 194-227). New York, NY: Wiley. Cincotta, A. L., Gehrman, P., Gooneratne, N. S., & Baime, M. J. (2011). The effects of a mindfulness-based stress reduction programme on pre-sleep cognitive arousal and insomnia symptoms: A pilot study. Stress and Health, 27(3), e299-e305. Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189195. Dedovic, K., Duchesne, A., Andrews, J., Engert, V., & Pruessner, J. C. (2009). The brain and the stress axis: The neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress. NeuroImage, 47, 864871. Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84100. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224237. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281291. Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 621632. Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 208212. Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012a). Mind over matter: Reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 417422.
6
Stein, M. B., & Kean, Y. M. (2000). Disability and quality of life in social phobia: Epidemiologic findings. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 16061613. Werner, N. S., Duschek, S., Mattern, M., & Schandry, R. (2009). Interoceptive sensitivity modulates anxiety during public speaking. Journal of Psychophysiology, 23, 8594.
Jamieson et al.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stoop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 324. Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: Theyre not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267301.