Illustrations of The IPA - Modern Greek
Illustrations of The IPA - Modern Greek
Illustrations of The IPA - Modern Greek
AMALIA ARVANITI
Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures, University of Cyprus, PO Box 20537, Nicosia, Cyprus
e-mail: amalia@ucy.ac.cy
Modern Greek is a descendant of Classical Greek and is spoken today by approximately
11,000,000 people living in Greece. In addition, it is spoken (with various modifications) in large
Greek immigrant communities in North America, Australia and elsewhere. Although the Modern
Greek dialects had largely been shaped by the 10th c. A.D. (Browning 1983), the linguistic
situation in Greece has been one of diglossia from the middle 19th c. (the early beginnings of the
independent Greek state) and until 1976. The High and Low varieties of Greek diglossia are
known as Katharevousa and Dhimotiki respectively. Katharevousa was a purist, partly invented,
variety that was heavily influenced by Classical Greek; the term Dhimotiki, on the other hand,
loosely describes the mother tongue of the Greeks, which was confined to oral communication. In
1976 the use of Katharevousa was officially abolished and gradually a new standard based on
Dhimotiki as spoken in Athens has emerged. This variety is adopted by an increasingly large
number of educated speakers all over Greece, who choose it over regional varieties (Mackridge
1985). In spelling, Modern Greek has kept many of the conventions of Ancient Greek, although
several simplifications have taken place since 1976. Perhaps the most dramatic of these has been
the decision to stop using accent and breath marks (which have not had phonetic correspondents
in the language for nearly 2,000 years); these marks were replaced by one accent on the stressed
vowel of each word with two or more syllables. The variety described here is Standard Modern
Greek as spoken by Athenians. The sample text in particular is based on recordings of two
Athenian speakers, a male in his mid-twenties and a female in her mid-thirties. Both speakers
read the passage twice in relatively informal style.
Consonants
The consonant system of Greek comprises voiced and voiceless plosives and fricatives,
nasals, and liquids.
Bilabial
Plosive
Fricative
Nasal
Tap
Lateral approx
Labiodental
Dental
b
f
m
t
T
d
D
n
Alveolar
Velar
k
x
g
V
|
l
p
b
f
v
m
t
d
T
D
s
z
n
|
l
k
g
x
V
"pi|
"bi|
"fsi
"vsi
"monos
"tino
"dino
"TEm
"DEm
"so
"zo
"nomos
"|im
"lim
"kom
"gm
"xom
"Vom
I took
beer
phase
base
alone
to tend
to dress
topic
parcel
safe fem.
animals
law
verb
nail-file
comma
range
soil
rubber
Plosives. The voiceless plosives are unaspirated with very short VOTs. The voiced
plosives are fully prevoiced and oftenespecially in more formal speechaccompanied by
prenasalisation, which can vary in duration from being very brief to taking up almost the entire
closure of the plosive; in fast speech prenasalisation is rarer and voiced plosives are sometimes
lenited to fricatives (Arvaniti & Joseph in press). Voiced plosives never appear prenasalised
word-initially or in clusters in which they are preceded by /r/ or /l/ (e.g. [b|"buni] red mullet is
never pronounced *[mb|"mbuni]). The prenasalisation of voiced plosives is also subject to
sociolinguistic variation. Specifically, prenasalisation seems to be in decline, with younger
Athenian speakers producing few or no prenasalised tokens (Arvaniti & Joseph, in press).
However, because of the possibility of prenasalisation, there is considerable disagreement as to
whether the voiced plosives should be treated as separate phonemes (e.g. Householder 1964) or as
sequences of homorganic nasal+voiceless plosive in which the plosive assimilates for voice to the
nasal (e.g. Newton 1972).
Nasals. Greek has only two nasal phonemes, /m/ and /n/. However, in clusters nasals
share the place of articulation of the plosives and fricatives they precede; e.g. [fi"vlo] I
doubt, ["iVm] touch, ["Nxos] stress.
Rhotics. Greek has one rhotic sound. This is pronounced as an alveolar tap [|] wordinitially and intervocalically, or when it is followed by another consonant; in fast speech it may
o
u
pis
pEs
ps
pos
pu
u
o
The Greek vowels do not exhibit much variation in terms of quality. In casual speech
unstressed /i/ and, to a lesser extent, /u/ become devoiced and even elided. The factors that trigger
devoicing are not fully understood. However, it can safely be stated that devoicing is mostly
triggered by the presence of voiceless consonants flanking the high vowels and is more common
when the vowel is in a syllable immediately following a stressed syllable (Dauer 1980).
The high vowel /i/ is realised differently depending on the position it occupies in the
syllable. Specifically, when /i/ is followed by another vowel in the same syllable (and is not
preceded by one of the consonants with palatal allophones), then it is pronounced as a palatal
fricative, which is voiced after voiced consonants and voiceless after voiceless ones; e.g. ["l.DJ]
oils, [V."t] kittens (c.f. ["l.Di] oil, [V."ti] kitten); but [".Di.]
leave (cf. [".DJ] empty neu.pl.). In /m/+/i/+vowel sequences forming one syllable, /i/ is
pronounced //, e.g. [m."lo] brain (cf. [mi.E."los] bone marrow).
Conventions
Greek shows several types of juncture phenomena, mostly, but not exclusively, between
proclitics (such as articles, weak forms of pronouns, and the negative particles [DEn] and [min])
and their hosts. When word-initial voiceless plosives are preceded by a proclitic that ends in /n/
they become voiced; the nasal may assimilate for place of articulation to the following plosive or
be elided; e.g. [toN gTiJi"ti] or [to gTiJi"ti] the professor ACC. (/ton kTiJi"ti/).
The final /n/ of proclitics is elided (with some exceptions) when the host begins with a fricative or
a liquid; e.g. [to "filo mu] my friend (/ton "filo mu/), [ti "|otis] I
asked her (/tin "|otis/). When word-final /s/ precedes a word-initial voiced consonant (and the
two words belong to the same intonational phrase) /s/ becomes voiced; e.g. [o k"fEz lE"czi]
coffee stains (/o k"fEs lE"czi/). Generally, word-final consonants resyllabify, if
the following word (within the same phrase) begins with a vowel. Sequences of identical
consonants across word boundaries are simplified, except in very formal and careful speech; e.g.
[o "mno si"koTicE] Manos got up (/o "mnos si"koTicE/). Sequences of
identical vowels across word boundaries also degeminate, unless (a) vowel elision will result in a
stress clash or (b) both vowels are stressed; e.g. [i "po|t "niJi] the door opens
(/i "po|t "niJi /). Vowel elision also takes place between certain sequences of different vowels;
which vowel is elided depends on a complex combination of stress patterns, relative vowel
sonority and the morphological role of the vowels (e.g. the verb-initial past tense marker /E/ is
more likely to be elided than a word-initial /E/ that belongs to the words stem). When neither of
the vowels is elided, the sequence is often pronounced as a short diphthong, e.g. ["inE8"po8ifzm]
[it] is of cloth (/"inE "po "ifzm]).
. ,
,
.
.
.
References
ARVANITI, A. (1992). Secondary stress: evidence from Modern Greek. In Docherty, G. J. &
Ladd, D. R. (editors), Papers in Laboratory Phonology II: Gesture, Segment, Prosody, 398423. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
ARVANITI, A. (1994). Acoustic features of Greek rhythmic structure. Journal of Phonetics 22,
239-268.
ARVANITI, A. (2000). The phonetics of stress in Greek. Journal of Greek Linguistics 1.
ARVANITI, A. & JOSEPH, B. D. (in press). Variation in voiced stop prenasalisation in Greek.
Glossologia 11-12, 121-156.
BROWNING, R. (1983). Medieval and Modern Greek. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
DAUER, R. M. (1980). The reduction of unstressed high vowels in modern Greek. JIPA 10, 17-27.
HOUSEHOLDER, F. W. (1964). Three dreams of Modern Greek phonology. In Austerlitz, P.
(editor), Papers in memory of George C. Pappageotes. Supplement to Word 20, 17-27.
JONGMAN, A., FOURAKIS, M. & SERENO, J. A. (1989). The acoustic vowel space of Modern
Greek and German. Language and Speech 32, 221-248.
MACKRIDGE, P. (1985). The Modern Greek Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
NESPOR, M. & VOGEL, I. (1989). On clashes and lapses. Phonology 6, 69-116.
NEWTON, B. (1972). The generative interpretation of dialect: A study of Modern Greek
phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.