Assessing Reading Fluency
Assessing Reading Fluency
Reading
Fluency
Timothy V. Rasinski, Ph.D.
Assessing Reading Fluency is intended to assist practitioners in monitoring students fluency development. Assessments are discussed in terms
of three components of fluency:
Accuracy, or accurate decoding of words in text;
Automaticity, or decoding words with minimal use of
attentional resources; and
Prosody, or the appropriate use of phrasing and expression to
convey meaning.
Assessing Reading Fluency is written by Dr. Timothy V. Rasinski (Ph.D.,
Ohio State University), a professor of education in the Department of
Teaching, Leadership, and Curriculum Studies at Kent State University.
He has published over 100 articles and 10 books on various aspects of
reading education, including The Fluent Reader: Oral Reading Strategies
for Building Word Recognition, Fluency, and Comprehension. Dr.
Rasinski recently served on the Board of Directors of the International
Reading Association and is an editor for the Journal of Literacy
Research.
The Regional Educational Laboratory at Pacific Resources for Education
and Learning would like to express sincere thanks to the following
reviewers:
Dr. David J. Chard, University of Oregon
Dr. Melanie R. Kuhn, Rutgers University
Dr. Wayne M. Linek, Texas A&M University Commerce
to decode all the words they encounter and seem to have a pretty good understanding of them as well. Moreover, they appear to
be of average to above average intelligence and are knowledgeable about the world around them. But, Mr. Lee also knows that
both Kimberly and Thomas do not comprehend what they read.
When he asks them questions about what they read, they usually respond I dont know, I dont remember, or give an incorrect or incomplete answer. Interestingly, when Mr. Lee reads to
the class, both children seem to have a good understanding of
what is read.
Mr. Lee refers Kimberly and Thomas to the school reading specialist, Mrs. Pearce, for further testing. Mrs. Pearce works with
Kimberly and Thomas separately. She asks each of them to read
aloud for her, after which she asks them to retell what they read.
Mrs. Pearce confirms Mr. Lees observations about accuracy in
decoding and poor comprehension. She also notes something
else that may be the cause of their reading comprehension problems: both read without appropriate phrasing or interest.
Thomas reads in a slow and labored word-by-word manner. His
reading rate is 56 words correct per minute. Kimberly buzzes
through the passage; she reads the words, but pays little attention to sentence juncture or other punctuation. Her reading rate
is 178 words correct per minute. Mrs. Pearce thinks she has
found the source of Kimberly and Thomass difficulty in reading
reading fluency.
Prosody in Reading
While it is good for readers to have the additional cognitive capacity
that comes from automaticity in word decoding, they also need to
actively use that capacity to make sense of the text. Readers can
employ their attention for comprehension or for other tasks. All readers have had the experience of accurately and automatically decoding
words while thinking about something else and, as a result, not comprehended the passage.
This is the point where fluency connects directly to comprehension.
The prosody component of reading fluency stresses the appropriate
use of phrasing and expression (Dowhower, 1987, 1991; Schreiber,
1980, 1987, 1991; Schreiber & Read, 1980). When readers embed
appropriate volume, tone, emphasis, phrasing, and other elements in
oral expression, they are giving evidence of actively interpreting or
constructing meaning from the passage. Just as fluent musicians
interpret or construct meaning from a musical score through phrasing, emphasis, and variations in tone and volume, fluent readers use
cognitive resources to construct meaning through expressive interpretation of the text.
In a sense, then, reading fluency is multidimensional one dimension stresses the importance of accuracy in word decoding, a second
dimension focuses on quick and automatic recognition of words in
connected text, and a third dimension stresses expressive and meaningful interpretation of text. These dimensions are related to one
another accurate and automatic reading creates the conditions for
expressive reading. All three are important for effective comprehension and overall good reading. All must be taught, and all must be
monitored.
Osborn and Lehr (2003) provide an excellent summary of ways in
which reading fluency can be taught and nurtured in classrooms.
Methods for assessing a students level of achievement at any given
moment and for determining growth over time are part of any good
instructional program. This paper explores how reading fluency can
be assessed in valid and efficient ways.
Fluency Assessments
The ability to measure students level of achievement in fluency and
monitor their progress is key to successful fluency teaching.
Teachers need to be able to gauge the effectiveness of their instruction in fluency; to do this, they need ways to assess student fluency
validly and efficiently. The next section of this paper explores methods for assessing reading fluency. The inclusion of assessment
approaches in this booklet was guided by two important criteria.
First, fluency assessments must have some degree of reliability and
validity. Users of the assessments must be assured that the results
they obtain are reliable that the results will provide consistent
measures of fluency and will not vary because of imperfections in the
assessment itself. Users must also be assured that the assessments
are valid that they actually measure reading fluency. The assessments themselves should resemble the ways in which reading fluency is defined. In this booklet, fluency is defined in terms of three key
components: accuracy in reading, automaticity in reading, and
prosody (or expression) in reading. Moreover, since fluency is a contributor to overall reading proficiency, the fluency assessments presented here should correlate with other, more general measures of
reading proficiency.
Second, the assessments must be efficient in administration, scoring,
and interpretation. Assessments should be as quick and easy to use
as possible. If they are not, teachers may not find time to use them
or may use them in ways that are inconsistent with their intent.
Moreover, time given to assessment is usually time taken away from
instruction. Thus, quick and easy assessments will allow teachers to
gauge students progress and maximize teaching time so that academic progress can be made.
Since current views suggest that reading fluency consists of three
distinct components, this booklet aligns its approach to assessment
with these components:
Decoding accuracy the ability of readers to decode words
accurately in text.
Automaticity the ability of readers to decode words in text
with minimal use of attentional resources.
Prosody the ability of readers to appropriately use phrasing
and expression.
97-100%
Instructional Level:
90-96%
Frustration Level:
< 90%
Figure 1
Procedures for Measuring Accuracy and Rate in CBM/ORF
1. Find a passage(s) of approximately 250 words written at
the students grade placement. Submit the passage to a
text readability formula to estimate its grade appropriateness.
2. Ask the student to read the passage for one minute and
tape-record the reading. Emphasize that the text should be
read aloud in a normal way, and not faster than normal.
3. Mark any uncorrected errors made by the student. Errors
include mispronunciations, substitutions, reversals, omissions, or words pronounced by the examiner after a wait of
2-3 seconds without an attempt or response from the student. Mark the point in the text the student has come to
after one minute of reading.
4. Repeat steps 1 and 2 with two different passages (optional). If you choose to repeat the process, use the median or
middle score for analysis.
5. Determine accuracy by dividing the number of words read
correctly per minute (WCPM) by the total number of words
read (WCPM + any uncorrected errors). This number will
be a percentage. Compare the students performance
against the target norms in Table 1.
6. Determine the rate by calculating the total number of
WCPM and comparing the students performance against
the target norms in Table 2.
Fall
(WCPM)
Winter
(WCPM)
Spring
(WCPM)
10-30
30-60
30-60
50-80
70-100
50-90
70-100
80-110
70-110
80-120
100-140
80-120
100-140
110-150
100-140
110-150
120-160
110-150
120-160
130-170
120-160
130-170
140-180
Readers who perform at or near these target norms should be considered as progressing adequately in automaticity. Readers who are
significantly and consistently below (or above) the norm span for
their grade level and time of year may be at risk in their reading fluency development. We generally think of disfluent readers as reading
in a very slow and disjointed manner; disfluency, however, can come
from readers who read too fast and fail to pay attention to intra- and
inter-sentential boundaries or the meaning of the text.
Figure 2
Classroom Fluency Chart
Teacher:
Student
Name
Year:
Fall
Accuracy
Winter
Spring
Accuracy Accuracy
Fall
Rate
Winter
Rate
Spring
Rate
CBM/ORF reading assessments that include accuracy and rate provide teachers with a workable and valid approach to documenting
student performance and progress in reading. Although only a
snapshot of a students reading, the assessments nonetheless align
well with other, more comprehensive measures. Moreover, they can
guide teachers instruction to meet students specific needs. Students
who perform poorly on the assessments can be identified for more
thorough and comprehensive reading assessment.
A Note of Caution
There are limitations to these assessments, and caution has been
raised by researchers such as Deno, Mirkin, and Chiang (1982).
Although reading rate appears to be a good measure of the decoding
automaticity component of reading fluency and of reading achievement in general, it does not mean that students should receive overt
and intensive instruction and practice in becoming fast readers.
Reading rate appears to reflect students ongoing development of
automaticity in their decoding, which can be developed through practiced and assisted readings (see Kuhn & Stahl, 2000; Osborn & Lehr,
2003). If teachers provide the kind of instruction in fluency that
works, then fluency, comprehension, and rate will improve. If teachers choose instead to focus primarily on developing students reading
rate at the expense of reading with expression, meaning, and comprehension, students may read fast but with insufficient comprehension. Their goal may be to get from one point in the text to another
as fast as possible, without understanding the nuances of meaning in
the text. This would be a grave misinterpretation of the research
related to reading fluency development and a disservice to the students.
Similarly, teachers need to be cautious in using reading rate to
assess English language learners (ELLs). Many ELLs can be deceptively fast and accurate in their reading, yet demonstrate little understanding of the text. Teachers cannot assume that such students are
progressing well in reading based solely on their reading rate. Other
issues such as vocabulary and language proficiency may impede the
students growth in reading and require instructional intervention.
are embedded in texts and intended for readers to recognize, understand, and express through intonation, pause, voice, and emphasis.
This coaching role is analogous to a teacher-student conference during a writing workshop, in which a students writing efforts are
shared and examined. During the conference the teacher notes positive aspects of the students composition as well as areas that may
need revision for clarity or style. The teacher will share or model
ways in which the student may express meaning in writing. Similarly,
a teacher who acts as a coach during oral reading encourages and
applauds reading that expresses meaning at a variety of levels, notes
areas for further work, and models ways in which the student may
try reading the passage. Regular opportunities for coaching will lead
the student to higher levels of fluent and expressive reading as well
as comprehension. Moreover, students oral reading will have an
impact on their silent reading (Pinnell et al., 1995). Most readers
hear an internal voice while reading silently; the internal voice is
developed through opportunities for reading orally and silently.
Assessing students oral interpretive reading is a key to developing
their prosodic or expressive reading competencies. Interpretation of
text is more complex because it is more subjective than accuracy levels and reading rates. Nevertheless, methods have been developed to
help teachers measure the extent to which students provide a fluent
interpretation while reading.
Since expression or interpretation of text is difficult to quantify,
researchers have turned to qualitative rubrics or rating scales to
guide the assessment process and assign a grade or level. The
rubrics range from well-phrased, expressive reading at one end to
word-by-word, monotonic reading at the other.
The rubrics are quite simple to use. A student reads a grade-level
passage and a teacher or other rater listens to the student reading or
to a recording of the reading. The listening period can be short;
teachers are able to make reliable and valid measurements in 60 seconds or less. At the end of the listening period, the teacher consults
the rubric and assigns a score that most closely aligns with the students reading. In using a rubric, teachers and other raters need to
share a well-established sense of what constitutes appropriate phrasing and expressiveness in reading for their assigned grade level.
Several fluency rubrics have been developed and found to work well
in assessing fluency and overall reading proficiency. In one study,
Rasinski (1985) adapted a six-point fluency rubric devised by
Allington (1983; Allington & Brown, 1979). Using the rubric, raters
listened to and rated recordings of third and fifth grade students
reading. Raters did not have a copy of the passage that students
read, and to make the task as efficient as possible, raters were asked
to listen to a reading for no more than 30 seconds. This instrument
was highly reliable (test-retest reliability = .90) and was strongly
associated with the students performance on a standardized test of
reading proficiency.
In a more recent large-scale study of fourth graders oral reading fluency, a group of researchers headed by Pinnell (1995) rated fourth
graders oral reading using a four-point rubric (see Figure 3). In this
study, students whose oral reading was assigned a score of one or
two were not considered fluent; they had yet to achieve even a minimally acceptable level of fluency. The researchers found that ratings
of students oral reading performance were strongly associated with
their performance on the silent reading comprehension test that was
part of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. These studies suggest that rating students for the level of expressive or prosodic reading is a reliable and valid way for assessing the prosodic reading
component of fluency and for assessing overall reading performance.
The use of such rubrics can assist teachers in coaching students to
higher levels of interpretive reading. Rubrics can also help students
develop a greater internalized (metacognitive) awareness of their ability to interpret text orally and to guide their development in oral interpretive reading.
Figure 3
Oral Reading Fluency Scale
4. Reads primarily in larger, meaningful phrase groups.
Although some regressions, repetitions, and deviations
from the text may be present, these do not appear to
detract from the overall structure of the story. Preservation
of the authors syntax is consistent. Some or most of the
story is read with expressive interpretation. Reads at an
appropriate rate.
3. Reads primarily in three- and four-word phrase groups.
Some smaller groupings may be present. However, the
majority of phrasing seems appropriate and preserves
the syntax of the author. Little or no expressive interpretation is present. Reader attempts to read expressively and some of the story is read with expression.
Generally reads at an appropriate rate.
2. Reads primarily in two-word phrase groups with
some three- and four-word groupings. Some
word-by-word reading may be present. Word
groupings may seem awkward and unrelated to
the larger context of the sentence or passage. A
small portion of the text is read with expressive
interpretation. Reads significant sections of the
text excessively slowly or fast.
1. Reads primarily word-by-word. Occasional
two- or three-word phrases may occur but
these are infrequent and/or they do not preserve meaningful syntax. Lacks expressive
interpretation. Reads text excessively slowly.
A score of 1 should also be given to a student
who reads with excessive speed, ignoring
punctuation and other phrase boundaries, and
reads with little or no expression.
Source: Adapted from Listening to Children Read Aloud: Oral Fluency, by G.
S. Pinnell, J. J. Pikulski, K. K. Wixson, J. R. Campbell, P. B. Gough, & A. S.
Beatty, 1995, Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National
Center for Education Statistics. Available at http://nces.ed.gov/pubs95/web/
95762.asp
Assessing Reading Fluency 17
Frequent extended pauses, hesitations, false starts, sound-outs, repetitions, and/or multiple attempts.
Moderately slow.
Consistently conversational.
Source: Adapted from Training Teachers to Attend to Their Students Oral Reading Fluency, by J. Zutell and T. V. Rasinski, 1991, Theory Into Practice, 30, pp. 211-217.
D.Pace (during
sections of minimal disruption)
C.Smoothness
B.Phrasing
A.Expression and
Volume
Dimension
Use the following scales to rate reader fluency on the dimensions of expression and volume, phrasing, smoothness, and pace. Scores range from 4 to 16. Generally, scores
below 8 indicate that fluency may be a concern. Scores of 8 or above indicate that the student is making good progress in fluency.
Figure 4
Multidimensional Fluency Scale
References
Allington, R. L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal. The Reading
Teacher, 36, 556-561.
Allington, R. L., & Brown, S. (1979). Fact: A multi-media reading program.
Milwaukee, WI: Raintree Publishers.
Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. (2002). A synthesis of research on
effective interventions for building fluency with elementary students
with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 386-406.
Deno, S. L. (1985). Curriculum-based measurement: The emerging alternative. Exceptional Children, 52, 219-232.
Deno, S. L. (1997). Whether thou goest: Perspectives on progress monitoring. In J. L. Lloyd, E. J. Kameenui, & D. Chard (Eds.), Issues in educating students with disabilities (pp. 77-99). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Deno, S. L., Mirkin, P., & Chiang, B. (1982). Identifying valid measures of
reading. Exceptional Children, 49, 36-45.
Dowhower, S. L. (1987). Effects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional readers fluency and comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly,
22, 389-407.
Dowhower, S. L. (1991). Speaking of prosody: Fluencys unattended bedfellow. Theory Into Practice, 30, 165-175.
Edformation. (2003). AIMSweb: Charting the path to literacy. Retrieved September 17, 2003, from www.aimsweb.com/norms/reading_fluency.htm
Fuchs, L. S., Deno, S. L., & Mirkin, P. (1984). The effects of frequent curriculum-based measurement and evaluation on pedagogy, student
achievement, and students awareness of learning. American
Educational Research Journal, 21, 449-460.
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (1986). Effects of systematic formative evaluation:
A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 53, 199-208.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Deno, S. L. (1982). Reliability and validity of curriculum-based informal reading inventories. Reading Research
Quarterly, 18, 6-26.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal measures of reading comprehension. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2),
20-28.
Hasbrouck, J. E., & Tindal, G. (1992). Curriculum-based oral reading fluency
norms for students in grades 2 through 5. Teaching Exceptional
Children, 24, 41-44.
Johnson, M. S., Kress, R. A., & Pikulski, J. J. (1987). Informal reading
inventories. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2000). Fluency: A review of developmental and
remedial practices (CIERA Rep. No. 2-008). Ann Arbor, MI: Center for
the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. A. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293-323.
Marston, D. (1989). A curriculum-based measurement approach to assessing academic performance: What it is and why do it. In M. R. Shinn
(Ed.), Curriculum-based measurement: Assessing special children (pp.
18-78). New York: Guilford.
Marston, D., & Magnusson, D. (1985). Implementing curriculum-based
measurement in special and regular education settings. Exceptional
Children, 52, 266-276.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report
of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidencebased assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its
implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769).
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Osborn, J., & Lehr, F. (with Hiebert, E. H.). (2003). A focus on fluency.
Honolulu, HI: Pacific Resources for Education and Learning. Available at
www.prel.org/products/re_/fluency-1.pdf
Pikulski, J. J. (1990). Informal reading inventories. The Reading Teacher, 11,
514-516.
Pinnell, G. S., Pikulski, J. J., Wixson, K. K., Campbell, J. R., Gough, P. B., &
Beatty, A. S. (1995). Listening to children read aloud: Oral fluency.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for
Education Statistics. Retrieved February 20, 2004, from
http://nces.ed.gov/pubs95/web/95762.asp
Rasinski, T. V. (1985). A study of factors involved in reader-text interactions
that contribute to fluency in reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
The Ohio State University, Columbus.
Rasinski, T. V. (2003). The fluent reader: Oral reading strategies for building
word recognition, fluency, and comprehension. New York: Scholastic.
Samuels, S. J. (2002). Reading fluency: Its development and assessment. In
A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about
reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 166-183). Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Schreiber, P. A. (1980). On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of
Reading Behavior, 12, 177-186.
Schreiber, P. A. (1987). Prosody and structure in childrens syntactic processing. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral
and written language (pp. 243-270). New York: Academic Press.
Schreiber, P. A. (1991). Understanding prosodys role in reading acquisition.
Theory Into Practice, 30, 158-164.
Schreiber, P. A., & Read, C. (1980). Childrens use of phonetic cues in
spelling, parsing, andmaybereading. Bulletin of the Orton Society,
30, 209-224.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of
individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research
Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Word recognition: Changing perspectives. In R.
Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of
reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 418-452). New York: Longman.
Zutell, J., & Rasinski, T. V. (1991). Training teachers to attend to their students oral reading fluency. Theory Into Practice, 30, 211-217.