The CPH in Sla
The CPH in Sla
The CPH in Sla
Robert M. DeKeyser
University of Pittsburgh
This study was designed to test the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis (Bley-Vroman, 1988), which states that, whereas children are
known to learn language almost completely through (implicit) domain-specific mechanisms, adults have largely lost the ability to
learn a language without reflecting on its structure and have to use
alternative mechanisms, drawing especially on their problem-solving
capacities, to learn a second language. The hypothesis implies that
only adults with a high level of verbal analytical ability will reach
near-native competence in their second language, but that this ability will not be a significant predictor of success for childhood second
language acquisition. A study with 57 adult Hungarian-speaking immigrants confirmed the hypothesis in the sense that very few adult
immigrants scored within the range of child arrivals on a grammaticality judgment test, and that the few who did had high levels of
verbal analytical ability; this ability was not a significant predictor for
childhood arrivals. This study replicates the findings of Johnson and
Newport (1989) and provides an explanation for the apparent exceptions in their study. These findings lead to a reconceptualization of
the Critical Period Hypothesis: If the scope of this hypothesis is lim-
First and foremost, I thank Anna Fenyvesi and Donald Peckham, who helped with the test design and
were in charge of the data collection, and Mary Connerty who helped with the data collection. I am
grateful to Jacqueline Johnson and Elissa Newport for sharing their original grammaticality judgment
test items with me, to Zoltan Dornyei for providing me with the Hungarian Language Aptitude Test
designed by his student Istvan Otto, and to Istvan Otto himself for providing me with a copy of his
thesis. David Birdsong, Michael Long, Barry McLaughlin, Brian MacWhinney, Christina Bratt Paulston, Liliana Sanchez, G. Richard Tucker, and three anonymous SSLA reviewers provided constructive criticism on drafts of this paper. I gratefully acknowledge the financial support of the University
of Pittsburghs Central Research Development Fund. A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the Second Language Research Forum, University of Arizona, Tucson, October 26, 1996.
Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to: Robert M. DeKeyser, Department of
Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260; e-mail: RDK1@pitt.edu.
2000 Cambridge University Press 0272-2631/00 $9.50
499
500
Robert M. DeKeyser
ited to implicit learning mechanisms, then it appears that there may
be no exceptions to the age effects that the hypothesis seeks to
explain.
The popular belief that adults are much worse at learning a second language
than children has been supported in part by the professional literature, especially since Lenneberg (1967). Evidence for the Critical Period Hypothesis,
which states that individuals past a certain age are worse at learning a language than younger individuals, has been accumulating, for both pronunciation (e.g., Asher & Garcia, 1969; Munro, Flege, & MacKay, 1996; Oyama, 1976;
Tahta, Wood, & Loewenthal, 1981) and grammar learning (e.g., Coppieters,
1987; Harley, 1986; Harley & Hart, 1997; Johnson, 1992; Johnson & Newport,
1989, 1991; Patkowski, 1980; Schachter, 1990; Sorace, 1993). Empirical research
has refined the popular concept in a number of ways, however: Children have
an advantage in ultimate attainment, not in rate of learning (e.g., Krashen,
Long, & Scarcella, 1979; Slavoff & Johnson, 1995); the decline of language
learning ability does not suddenly occur around puberty but seems to take
place gradually from ages 6 or 7 to 16 or 17 and beyond (e.g., Bialystok &
Hakuta, 1994; Johnson & Newport, 1989, 1991; Oyama, 1978).
In spite of this body of research, the concept of a critical period for second
language acquisition continues to be a controversial topic. Not only is there
no agreed-upon explanation (see, e.g., Goldowsky & Newport, 1993; Harley &
Wang, 1997; Long, 1990; for a recent overview of explanations, see Birdsong,
1999), but the very existence of the phenomenon is denied or played down by
some researchers (e.g., Bialystok, 1997; Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994, 1999; Birdsong, 1992, 1999; Flynn & Manuel, 1991; Ioup, Boustagui, El Tigi, & Moselle,
1994; White & Genesee, 1996, in the domain of morphosyntax; Bongaerts, 1999;
Bongaerts, Planken, & Schils, 1995; Bongaerts, van Summeren, Planken, &
Schils, 1997; Flege, 1999; Flege, Takagi, & Mann, 1995; Moyer, 1999; Neufeld,
1978, in the domain of pronunciation).
A new impetus for research in this area came from Bley-Vromans (1988)
formulation of the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, adults can no longer rely on the innate mechanisms for implicit language acquisition and must, therefore, rely on alternative, problem-solving
mechanisms.1 Even though the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis is well
known in the field of second language acquisition, no empirical study on age
effects has been conducted specifically to test the strong prediction it impliesthat is, that only adults with a high level of verbal ability are expected
to succeed fully at second language acquisition. Children, of course, all learn
their native dialect completely, regardless of their level of verbal ability (except in cases of a clear handicap), because they rely on language-specific
mechanisms of implicit learning instead of on general mechanisms for explicit
learning. If the implicit learning mechanisms used by the child are no longer
available, then the adult must bring alternative, verbal-analytic problem-solv-
Critical Period
501
ing skills to the process of language acquisition in order to succeed, and these
analytical verbal skills are characterized by strong individual differences.
Therefore, the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis predicts that those adults
who appear to be successful at learning a second language will necessarily
have a high level of verbal ability. In other words, if the Critical Period Hypothesis is interpreted as applying only to implicit language acquisition, no
exceptions should be foundthat is, no adults should be found who are successful in acquiring a second language without having a high level of verbal
ability, which allows for explicit learning.
Testing this hypothesis was the main goal in carrying out the experimental
research described in this article. A secondary aim was to replicate Johnson
and Newports (1989) study, which found that ultimate attainment in the second language (L2) was strongly correlated with age of acquisition for people
who started acquiring the language before age 17; no correlation with age was
found for those who arrived past age 17. Adult acquirers showed a wide
spread, mostly scoring far below the level of early childhood acquirers, but
with a sizable number of apparent exceptions who scored within the range of
these early acquirers. That study is probably the best known critical period
investigation in the area of L2 morphosyntax, probably largely because of its
very clear-cut results. A number of methodological criticisms of Johnson and
Newports work have been voiced in the last few years, however. The present
study, then, is an attempt at replicating their findings with a very different
population, while avoiding certain methodological problems and testing the
hypothesis that the exceptions can be explained by some adults use of alternative, explicit mechanisms of learningmechanisms not accessible to the
majority of learners.
REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE
Age Effects on the Acquisition of Morphosyntax
in a Second Language
Several researchers (e.g., Eubank & Gregg, 1999; Long, 1990; Schachter, 1996;
Scovel, 1988; Seliger, 1978; Walsh & Diller, 1981) have argued that multiple
critical periods may exist for various aspects of L2 skills; more specifically,
the decline in learners ability to acquire a native-sounding pronunciation may
have different causes and show different age effects compared to the ability
to acquire a nativelike mastery of grammar. The literature review presented
here focuses on ultimate attainment in morphosyntax. Recent reviews for age
effects on ultimate attainment in pronunciation can be found in Bongaerts et
al. (1997), Flege (1999), Harley and Wang (1997), Long (1990, 1993), and Patkowski (1994); a literature review for age effects on rate of acquisition can be
found in Slavoff and Johnson (1995).
Relatively few studies have investigated the effect of age of first exposure
on ultimate attainment in morphosyntax. Patkowski (1980, see also 1990) ob-
502
Robert M. DeKeyser
tained global syntactic proficiency ratings for 67 nonnative speakers of English. He had native-speaking judges rate the subjects on the basis of written
transcripts corresponding to 5-minute recorded segments, thus avoiding a
confounding influence of accent. Subjects who had been exposed to English
before age 15 received much higher ratings on average than those who arrived in the United States after age 15. The pre-puberty group showed a
strong ceiling effect, whereas the ratings for the post-puberty group were
normally distributed. The results for a grammaticality judgment test administered to the subjects were essentially the same as for the syntax ratings.
Hyltenstam (1992) analyzed written retelling and transcriptions of oral retelling of stories by 24 speakers of L2 Swedish whose native language was either Spanish or Finnish, as well as the corresponding data from 12 native
speakers. He found no overlap in the distribution of number of errors between
the native speakers and those who acquired the L2 after age 7; the distribution for those who acquired the L2 before age 6 overlapped with the distributions for both the native speakers and the late acquirers.
Johnson and Newport (1989) administered a grammaticality judgment test
with orally presented sentences, covering a wide variety of basic morphosyntactic structures of English, to a group of 46 native speakers of Chinese and
Korean who had immigrated to the United States at various ages (339). They
found a gradual decline of the level of proficiency from ages 67 to 1617;
proficiency leveled off past that age. Adults showed a wide variety in proficiency but no clear age effect within their group. The correlation between age
of arrival and test score was .77 for all the subjects together and .87 for
those who arrived before age 17. Individual elements of grammar varied
widely in their correlation with age, even though this correlation was significant for all 12 rule types examined.
This study has drawn widespread attention. It is one of the most frequently
cited references in the second language acquisition literature of the last 10
years, but it has also been criticized on a number of points. The length of residence (minimum 5 years) may not have been enough in all cases for the learners to have reached ultimate attainment levels (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994;
Juffs & Harrington, 1995). The test may have been too long (276 items) for
the participants to concentrate on every item; the test required mental vigor
(Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994, p. 70). Age of arrival was to some extent confounded with age at test taking, which may explain to some extent the lower
scores of the older learners, who may have lost some of their attentional vigilance (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994, p. 70; see also Bialystok, 1997). The biggest
change may be around age 20 rather than around age 16, and a statistically
significant decline continues into adulthood; therefore there is no strong qualitative change around age 16 (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994; see also Bialystok,
1997; Bialystok & Hakuta, 1999). Research on learners of an L2 more closely
related to their L1 shows less dramatic age effects (Kellerman, 1995). The latter point is no threat to the concept of a critical period; clearly, the more
closely related the L1 and L2 are, the fewer structures have to be acquired
Critical Period
503
from scratch, and the fewer structures, therefore, are eligible to show an age
effect. The exact shape of the age function is a difficult point; it is hard to find
enough subjects in the critical age range of 1520,2 and the exact point in the
curve where the decline has bottomed out may depend on the structures investigated (see the hypothesis of multiple critical periods mentioned previously) and on individual differences among learners. The other three
criticisms, however, should certainly be taken into account by anybody trying
to replicate Johnson and Newports (1989) findings: Participants should preferably have been using the L2 for 10 years or more,3 any confound of age of
arrival with age of test taking should be avoided, and a shorter test may be
advisable in order to avoid excessive fatigue.
Johnson (1992) replicated the previous study with the same subjects a year
later, this time in a written format. The results were essentially the same as
those obtained with orally presented stimuli, even though the correlation between age and test score was somewhat lower than in the previous study (r =
.54 for all subjects together; .73 for those who arrived before adulthood),
and the number of morphosyntactic structures that showed a correlation between age and test score was smaller.
Johnson and Newport (1991) found very much the same results for oral
grammaticality judgments concerning subjacency in L2 English among 21 native speakers of Chinese who had their first exposure to English between ages
4 and 38. Performance was negatively correlated with age of arrival for those
who immigrated before adulthood (r = .63) and then leveled off to barely
above chance.
Schachter (1990; see also 1989) also investigated subjacency as a function
of age, this time with 79 Korean, Chinese, Indonesian, and Dutch speakers of
L2 English, who had their first exposure to the language after age 12. She
found that many learners in the first three groups had not acquired the constraints on wh-movement, referred to as subjacency, even where the various
forms of wh-movement themselves had been acquired. Korean speakers, in
particular, whose native language shows no evidence of subjacency, performed at chance level. The Dutch speakers, however, whose native language
shows the same range of subjacency phenomena as English, performed at the
same level as the native speaker control group.
Lee (1992, quoted in Schachter, 1996) found not only that Korean learners
ability to judge the grammaticality of reflexives in L2 English, after three years
of exposure, was negatively correlated with age of first exposure after age 14,
but also that it was positively correlated with age of first exposure before age
11. In other words, for this particular element of grammar, there seemed to be
not just a critical period with an end point, but one bounded on both sides,
which Schachter called a window of opportunity (p. 185). Given the relatively short period of exposure, however, this is probably a matter of speed
of acquisition rather than of ultimate attainment.
Even when the L1 and L2 are more closely related, strong age effects have
been documented. Coppieters (1987) administered a grammaticality judgment
504
Robert M. DeKeyser
test on subtle points of French grammar to 21 highly educated and very highly
proficient speakers of French as an L2, six of whom did not even have a detectable accent. The statistical analysis of the results, corroborated by qualitative data from follow-up interviews, showed that there was not even any
overlap in the distribution of the scores from 21 nonnatives with those from
native-speaking controls.
Sorace (1993) obtained similar results in a study with 24 L1 English and 20
L1 French near-native speakers of L2 Italian, who had not been exposed to the
L2 before age 18. Her study was more narrowly focused than Coppieters
(1987) experiment (it dealt exclusively with auxiliary choice) but similar in its
methodology (elicitation of grammaticality judgments). Just as in Coppieterss
study, the results indicated that, even for these near-native L2 speakers whose
length of residence in Italy ranged from 5 to 15 years, intuitions about grammaticality were substantially different from those of a control group of 36 native speakers.
Four studies published so far appear to strike a dissenting note. They all
assess the linguistic competence of highly proficient L2 speakers to determine
whether their knowledge of L2 grammar is indistinguishable from that of native speakers, and all of the studies have been quoted as evidence against the
Critical Period Hypothesis.
Ioup et al. (1994) documented the successful acquisition of Arabic as an L2
by two adult native speakers of English, one tutored, the other untutored. The
differences between the two were very small and both were relatively close to
native norms on a variety of tests. The tutored learner had a doctorate in Arabic. The other individual was a teacher of ESL who had studied Latin and who
paid close attention to inflectional morphology from the beginning of her
learning. At the time of testing, she had been in Egypt for 26 years. Although
both learners did very well on their tests, they were still far from a perfect
approximation of the native norm and, therefore, do not really provide evidence against the Critical Period Hypothesis. The focus of the study, in fact,
was on the role of instruction.
Bialystok (1997) reported briefly on two studies of L2 acquisition as a function of age, one with university students who were native speakers of English
or German and who had started studying L2 French at different ages, and one
with native speakers of Chinese who had immigrated to Canada and learned
L2 English at different ages. In both cases, those who learned L2 at a later
age (after age 15) did better than the younger learners. Bialystok takes this as
evidence against the Critical Period Hypothesis, but the fact that the older
learners did better suggests that what was measured was rate of learning
rather than ultimate attainment. As no minimal length of residence is mentioned in Bialystoks report, it is possible that many learners had not reached
their ultimate level of attainment yet. In that case, the level of performance
is to some extent a function of the rate of learning, and several studies have
documented rate advantages for older learners (Ekstrand, 1976; Krashen,
Long, & Scarcella, 1979; Snow & Hoefnagel-Hohle, 1978). Moreover, for the L2
Critical Period
505
506
Robert M. DeKeyser
Critical Period
507
508
Robert M. DeKeyser
2. Those adult acquirers, however, who score within the range of child acquirers will
all have high verbal aptitude, which may have allowed them, at least in part, to
learn the L2 grammar through explicit reflection on rules.
3. Different elements of grammar will show different correlations with age of acquisition; not all structures are equally sensitive to the critical period effect.
METHOD
Participants
Fifty-seven native speakers of Hungarian participated in this study. Hungarian
was chosen because it was desirable to have speakers of one language in order to eliminate variability due to the L1 within one study, and because even
the basic structures of English morphosyntax present many elements that
have to be acquired by speakers of a non-Indo-European language such as
Hungarian. Other desiderata were a long period of residence, to eliminate the
risk of confounding ultimate attainment with rate of acquisition, and a wide
spread in the two main independent variables, age of arrival and socioeconomic status (the latter being a first approximation of verbal ability). The Hungarian community around Pittsburgh met all these requirements.
Of the 57 participants, 32 were male and 25 female. They all lived in a 200mile radius around Pittsburgh (western Pennsylvania, eastern Ohio, northern
West Virginia, western Maryland) and were recruited by advertising in Hungarian-American newsletters, by distributing fliers at community events, and
by word of mouth in the community. All participants were volunteers and
were paid $20 for their efforts. Sixty Hungarian Americans volunteered, but
the data from three (elderly) people had to be discarded owing to problems
with hearing the test stimuli or concentrating on the test. Of the 57 remaining
participants, 42 (25 males and 17 females) were older than 16 when they immigrated; 15 (8 females and 7 males) were younger than 16 at the time of their
arrival in the United States (or, in a few cases, a different English-speaking
country before moving on to the United States); the range of age of arrival was
140. All participants had resided in the United States for at least 10 years;
average length of residence was 34 years (35.6 for the younger and 33.7 for
the older group). Average age at testing was 55 (43.2 for the younger and 60
for the older group), ranging from 16 to 81. Level of education and occupational status also varied widely, from blue-collar workers with an eighth-grade
education to professionals with doctoral degrees; the average number of
years of schooling was 14.6 for the younger and 13.4 for the older group.
Although all the participants were native speakers of Hungarian, some had
also had varying degrees of exposure to other languages such as Russian,
Czech, Slovak, Romanian, German, or Hebrew before moving to an Englishspeaking country. None reported any substantial exposure to English before
emigrating. When asked about their proficiency in Hungarian compared to English now, 22 said they still felt more comfortable in Hungarian than in English
Critical Period
509
(1 in the younger and 21 in the older group), 20 said they were more comfortable with English (10 in each group), and 15 said it made no difference (4 in
the younger and 11 in the older group).
The measures for age of arrival, age at time of testing, aptitude, years of
schooling, and grammar test scores for all individuals are given in Appendix A.
Instruments
4
by Johnson and Newport (1989) was made available to me by the authors and
was adapted in a variety of ways. The test was shortened to include 200 instead of 276 items; most subcategories now include six rather than eight items
(three- rather than four-item pairs). Four practice items were added at the beginning to ensure the testing procedure was understood perfectly by all participants. A few items were deleted or changed on J. Johnsons advice (personal
communication, May, 1996). Several subcategories were deleted or reorganized; a few were added to include structures that tend to be problematic for
Hungarian speakers. The reliability coefficient (KR-20) obtained was .91 for
grammatical items and .97 for ungrammatical items.
The complete set of items is given in Appendix B; the items in the Appendix
are grouped according to the structure being tested. Incorrect items are
marked with an asterisk. Items from Johnson and Newport (1989) are marked
JN; new items are marked DK. A more detailed list of changes compared to
the test in Johnson and Newport is given in Appendix C.
Language Learning Aptitude Test. Carroll and Sapons (1959) Modern Language Aptitude Test is usually considered the best verbal aptitude test in
terms of its predictive validity for L2 learning. Some of the minor technical
problems discovered over the years do not affect the Words in Sentences
part (Carroll, 1990), which is specifically aimed at measuring grammatical sensitivity and therefore should be the best predictor of grammar learning. (This
subtest has been shown to correlate highly with verbal intelligence or even
general intelligence, however. Wesche, Edwards, and Wells, 1982, for instance,
found a correlation of .56 with the overall score on Thurstone and Thurstones
[1962] Primary Mental Abilities.) Because the participants in this study were
native speakers of Hungarian, a corresponding test in Hungarian was needed.
The Hungarian Language Aptitude Test, Words in Sentences (Otto, 1996b), is
an adaptation of the Words in Sentences part of the Modern Language Aptitude Test (Carroll & Sapon). The test consists of 20 five-way, multiple-choice
items; these were selected as the best items from a pool of 50 items piloted
with 177 high school students in Budapest. The difficulty level for the 20 items
ranged from .23 to .68 with a mean of .52. Item-total correlations were all significant at the p < .001 level, with a mean of .52 (Z. Dornyei, personal communication, October 21, 1996; see also Otto, 1996a).
510
Robert M. DeKeyser
Background Questionnaire. All participants filled out a two-page questionnaire about their language background, educational background, age of arrival
in North America, and age at the time of the test.
Procedure
The tests and the questionnaire were administered individually to the participants at their home (or, in some cases, a room on campus). They filled out
the background questionnaire first, with the help of the experimenter.
The grammaticality judgment test came immediately after the questionnaire. The 200 items were tape-recorded by a male native speaker of English,
with one of the two items of each correct-incorrect pair in the first 100 items,
and the other in the second 100. Within each set of 100, the items came in a
fixed random order, with the exception that no two items of the same category ever occurred consecutively. Each item was read twice in a row, with a
3-second interval between repetitions and a 6-second interval between items.
Participants indicated whether they considered an item to be correct or incorrect by blackening the corresponding circle on an standardized answer sheet
for computer processing. The tape recording guided them through the first
four items, giving feedback on which were the correct answers. Participants
could ask the experimenter to stop the tape at any time if they were tired;
there was always a break of a couple of minutes after the first 100 items, while
the tape was turned around and rewound. The test took about 55 minutes.
Finally, the aptitude test was administered; 20 minutes were allotted for
this test. The participants read the stimulus sentences on a test sheet, and
marked their answers on a specially designed multiple-choice answer sheet
(Otto, 1996b).
RESULTS
Age of Acquisition and Ultimate Attainment
The correlation between the grammaticality judgment test score and age of
acquisition was .63 (p < .001) for the group as a whole (N = 57). For adult arrivals (n = 42), the correlation was .04 (ns); for participants who arrived before the age of 16 (n = 15), the correlation was .26 (ns). Figure 1 presents a
scatterplot for age of acquisition and grammaticality judgment test score.
Separate analyses were performed for high- and low-aptitude learners. The
average score on the aptitude test was 4.7 out of 20 (4.3 for the younger and
4.9 for the older group), with a standard deviation of 2.79. A somewhat arbitrary cut-off point was established between high- and low-aptitude learners, at
a place where the high-aptitude group would be sufficiently different from the
average, but without making it too small for statistical analysis. High aptitude
was operationalized as having a score of 6 or more (at least .46 standard deviations above average); the resulting high-aptitude group was composed of 15
Critical Period
511
512
Robert M. DeKeyser
ysis of specific types of errors. Although it is never 100% certain that a given
test taker has rejected an item because of the error built in by the test designer (see, e.g., Kellerman, 1995), this built-in error is certainly by far the
most likely reason for the rejection and can safely be interpreted as such
when summing results over individuals, with only a small margin of error.
Therefore, following Johnson and Newport (1989), I only included the 100 ungrammatical items in the analysis of errors for individual structures.
Contrary to Johnson and Newport (1989), however, in order to avoid the
language-as-fixed-effect fallacy (Clark, 1973), that is, the error of assuming
that all instances of a given structure are equally good tests of that structure,
scores were not summed over items testing the same structure. Instead, the
scores for individual items were correlated with age of arrival, and the items
were ranked according to this correlation. In this way, one can see whether a
given structure correlates moderately with age of arrival because all items
correlate moderately, or because some correlate and others do not. Table 1
shows the ranking of items from highest to lowest correlation with age of arrival.
Dividing the items into three groups, depending on whether the correlation
of the corresponding scores with age of arrival was high (r significant at p <
.01), marginal (.01 < p < .05), or low (r not significant at p .05), consistently
or almost consistently high correlations with age of arrival were found for the
following structures: present progressives with auxiliary omitted (PPAO: 3/3
items in top group), determiners omitted (DEOM: 3/3 items in top group), determiners used with abstract nouns (DEAB: 4/4 items in top group), plurals
marked on mass nouns (PLMN: 3/3 items in top group), wh-questions without
do-support (WHNA: 3/3 items in top group), wh-questions without subject-verb
inversion (WHNI: 3/3 items in top or in-between group), irregular plurals regularized (3/3 items in top or in-between group), wrong subcategorization of
verb for gerund, infinitive, to + infinitive (SUBC510: 5/6 items in top group),
and adverbs between the verb and the object (WOAD: 2/3 items in top group).
Consistently low correlations were found for word-order problems in declarative sentences not involving adverbs (WODO, WOIO, WOPP, WOVS: 12/12
items in bottom group), yes-no questions that lack do-support (YNVS: 3/3
items in bottom group), and gender errors in pronouns (PRGE: 4/4 items in
bottom or in-between group).
DISCUSSION
Hypothesis 1
The hypothesis of a strong negative correlation between age of acquisition
and score on the grammaticality judgment test was confirmed. A correlation
of .63 was found, which is similar to the .77 reported by Johnson and Newport (1989). More importantly, the extent of overlap between the range of
scores for adult acquirers and for acquirers below age 16 is very small.
Critical Period
513
.4071 PPAO03
.4026 PTII01
.4026 YNDT02
.3964 WHNA02
.3961 PTIR03
.3941 SUBC08
.3940 PMTF02
.3896 PTMO02
.3871 DEOM01
.3833 PROM03
.3821 WHNA03
.3803 PPAO01
.3767 TPSM02
.3743 WOAD01
.3729 DEOM03
.3632 TPSM01
.3592 DEAB01
.3521 SUBC05
.3410 PLIR01
p .05
.2565 PPMO02
.2530 TPSM04
.2516 YNDT01
.2441 PTII03
.2423 PROM01
.2408 TPSO02
.2407 PROM04
.2360 PMTF01
.2326 PLMO03
.2289 PMTF04
.2281 YNVS01
.2279 WOIO03
.2259 SUBC09
.2180 YNVS02
.2156 WOVS03
.2078 WODO03
.2071 PRAC03
.1918 TPSM03
.1903 PTMF03
.1832 TPSM05
.1777 WOIO02
.1766 PMSE04
.1757 PRGE01
.1735 PTMO01
.1719 PPMO03
.1685 PROM02
.1609 YNAA03
.1504 WOPP03
.1491 WOAD03
.1458 WOPP01
.1452 WOIO01
.1294 SUBC01
.1265 PLMO01
.1220 PTIR01
.1093 WOPP02
.1052 PRAC01
.0984 WODO01
.0915 YNVS03
.0814 YNAV02
.0649 PRGE03
.0572 SUBC04
.0267 PMSE02
.0218 WOVS02
.0189 WODO02
.0116 PRGE02
WOVS01
Note. PTMO = past tense marking omitted; PTIR = irregular verbs regularized; PTII = regular ending on irregular stem;
PLMO = plural marking omitted; PLIR = irregular plurals regularized; PLMN = mass nouns used with plural marker;
TPSO = third-person -s omitted; TPSM = third-person -s marked on main verb after modals; PPMO = progressive -ing
omitted; PPAO = progressive auxiliary omitted; DEOM = determiner omitted; DEAB = determiner used with abstract
nouns; PROM = Pronoun omitted (transfer from Hungarian object pro-drop); PRGE = gender errors; PMSE = phrasal
verb separation not allowed; PMTF = phrasal verb separation allowed, but particle moved too far; SUBC = subcategorization; YNAA = Aux Aux order; YNAV = Aux Verb order; YNVS = Verb Subject order; YNDT = double tense marking;
WHNI = no aux inversion; WHNA = no aux; WODO = S V DO order violated; WOIO = S V IO DO order violated; WOVS =
S V order violated; WOPP = S V PP order violated; WOAD = incorrect adverb placement.
Whereas roughly half of the adult arrivals in the Johnson and Newport study
scored within the range of the pre-16 arrivals, only a few adult arrivals performed as well in the present study. As Figure 1 shows, the dividing line between those who started acquiring English before age 16 and those who
started later falls around a score of 180 (out of 200). Among the younger arrivals, only 1 out of 15 scored below that point (a person with an eighth-grade
education and a blue-collar job); among the older arrivals only 3 out of 42
scored above that point.
Johnson and Newport (1989) reported an even stronger correlation between age of arrival and test performance for the early arrivals (r = .87) than
for the group as a whole, but that result was not replicated here; among those
who arrived before age 16 the correlation was .26 (ns). This discrepancy is
hard to interpret because, in both the present study and Johnson and New-
514
Robert M. DeKeyser
ports, the number of data points in the age range of 1216 was very small
(2 and 5, respectively).2 It should be pointed out, however, that Johnson and
Newports data show a dip in proficiency scores for the age range 1216 compared to both earlier acquirers and adults (see their Figure 2, p. 80). As there
is no theoretical explanation for such a decline in adolescent learners only,
the dip in their Figure 2 is probably an artifact, and the correlation coefficient
of .87 probably strongly overestimates the true r.
To assess Bialystoks (1997; see also Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994) interpretation of Johnson and Newports (1989) correlation coefficients (i.e., test scores
correlated strongly with age of arrival, because the latter was confounded
with age at the time of the test), partial correlation coefficients were calculated. The correlation between test score and age of arrival remained a robust
.54 (p < .001) after age at time of test was partialed out; the correlation between test score and age at time of test with age of arrival partialed out was
.13 (ns). These results confirm Johnson and Newports interpretation of their
correlation coefficients and refute Bialystoks criticism.
Length of residence, which was another potential confound with age of arrival, turned out not to be correlated with test scores at all in my data; r = 0.00
(sic). This clearly shows that length of residence no longer plays a role past
the first 10 years (see Oyama, 1978).
Hypothesis 2
It was predicted that no adult acquirers would score within the range of child
acquirers unless they had high verbal aptitude. Using the same operational
definition of high aptitude as above (a score of 6 or higher on the aptitude
test), it was found that, among the participants who started acquiring English
after age 16 but obtained a high score on the grammaticality test (over 180,
i.e., in the upper right quadrant of Figure 1; or even including those who came
close, with a score over 175), all but one had an aptitude score of 6 or above.
Their aptitude scores and grammaticality judgment test scores were 11/177,
10/190, 8/194, 7/184, 6/176, and 3/186. This last case, the only exception, is
that of a participant in the study who was doing postdoctoral studies in the
natural sciences; this suggests that he must be of above-average analytical
ability and that his aptitude test score is not indicative of his analytical abilities. It should also be pointed out that from age 15 to age 26 he lived in Israel
and that he felt most comfortable in Hebrew, followed by English, followed by
Hungarian. (Of the other five highly successful high-aptitude learners, two said
they were more comfortable in English, one in Hungarian, and two equally proficient in both.) Even though the data are not 100% clear-cut, one cannot escape the conclusion that there were few adult acquirers who scored within
the child acquirers range or even came close (6/42) and that none of those
who did were clearly of average or below-average aptitude. This finding is in
line with what the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis predicts: The only way
that an adult learner can achieve grammatical competence similar to that of a
Critical Period
515
516
Robert M. DeKeyser
into either the easy (low correlation with age) or difficult (high correlation
with age) category in this study fell into the same category in Johnson and
Newports.
The fact that certain structures seem impervious to otherwise strong age
effects begs for an explanation. What characteristics do pronoun gender, subject-verb inversion and do-support in yes-no questions, and basic word order
have in common that could explain why they are learned easily, that is, by
virtually everybody, even by adults whose verbal ability is below average?
Transfer from Hungarian cannot be an explanation because Hungarian makes
no gender distinction in personal pronouns and has no do-support, no subjectverb inversion for questions, and a rather variable word order (see, e.g., Kenesei, Vago, & Fenyvesi, 1998). What errors against the three structures in question do have in common is that they are all perceptually salient. Pronoun
gender errors are so irritating to native speakers that they will almost always
correct them when their nonnative interlocutors make such mistakes, even
though overt correction of grammar errors is otherwise rare in adult nativenonnative interaction; therefore the gender distinction becomes very salient
to the learner. In the case of basic word order, errors lead to differences from
the norm that are very salient themselves: sentences that begin with a (lexical) verb (WOVS, e.g., *Bites the dog), end with a verb (WODO, e.g., *The girl
the movie likes; WOPP, e.g., *The students to the movies went), or begin with
two consecutive noun phrases (WOIO, e.g., *The woman the policeman asked
a question). In all three cases a salient position (sentence initial or sentence
final) is occupied by a syntactic constituent that can never occupy that position in English (WOVS, WODO, WOPP) or it is occupied twice (WOIO). Finally,
in the case of yes-no questions without do-support or subject-verb inversion,
the error has the same highly salient effect: a lexical verb in initial position.
This last case is particularly interesting because it contrasts clearly with
errors in wh-questions, which are similar and yet go undetected by most
adults: lack of inversion (WHNI, e.g., *What Martha is bringing to the party?)
and lack of do-support (WHNA, e.g., *What they sell at the corner store?). In
these cases the errors are less salient because they occur after the question
word that occupies the initial, salient position; as a result they go undetected.
It is known that salience of specific structures plays a role in the ease or
difficulty with which they are acquired in an L2. Bardovi-Harlig (1987), for instance, showed in a study of 95 college-age learners acquiring L2 English that
wh-questions with preposition stranding (e.g., Whom did John give the book
to?) were acquired before pied piping (e.g., To whom did John give the book?),
even though the latter is unmarked and would therefore be expected to be
acquired first. As Robinson (1996) pointed out, preposition stranding is characterized by the fact that the question word and the preposition occupy the
two most salient positions in the sentencefirst and last. Salience clearly prevailed over markedness in Bardovi-Harligs study. In the same way, in the
present study, the salient inversion pattern in yes-no questions was learned
before inversion in wh-questions, even though the latter is considered the
Critical Period
517
least marked (Eckman, Moravcsik, & Wirth, 1989; Greenberg, 1963). Doughty
(1991) also provided evidence for the importance of the salience factor in L2
acquisition by showing that learners who had received treatment consisting
of bringing to prominence (p. 462) the structural components of relative
clauses did as well on a subsequent production task as the learners who were
taught the rules.
It is also known that perceptual salience interacts with the implicit-explicit
learning distinction. Reber, Kassin, Lewis, and Cantor (1980, Experiment 1, pp.
494497), in particular, showed that the structure of letter strings representing an artificial grammar was better learned explicitly than implicitly when the
stimuli were organized in a way that made their similarities salient, and it was
better learned implicitly than explicitly when the similarities were not made
salient. The explicit-salient combination was by far the best of the four. Therefore, it is not surprising that more salient patterns are learned before less salient, yet otherwise similar, structures by adult learners, who, as argued
above, have to rely on explicit learning.
The nature of the few structures that were learned well by the adults in the
present study is further evidence, then, for the fact that L2 grammar learning
in adults is largely limited to explicit processes: Not only are the learners with
high ability for explicit learning the only adults who are successful overall, but
those structures whose salience makes them particularly good candidates for
explicit learning are also the only ones that are learned by all immigrants, regardless of age.
In conclusion, perceptually very salient aspects of morphosyntax such as
basic word order and pronoun gender appear to have been acquired by most
learners at any age of arrival, whereas many other basic structures such as
the use of articles, the use and position of auxiliaries, the position of adverbs,
certain elements of verb subcategorization, and even some uses of the plural
morpheme had not been learned well by many of the learners who arrived
after the age of 16, not even after decades of residence in an English-speaking
environment.
CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS
This study has addressed a number of questions and challenges in the SLA
literature. It has accepted Longs (1990) challenge to look at what grammaticality judgments for specific structures reveal about the linguistic competence
of very advanced nonnative speakers who learned the L2 as adults.
It replicated Johnson and Newports (1989) landmark study and found remarkably similar results, having addressed the various methodological criticisms leveled at Johnson and Newport (1989) by Bialystok and Hakuta (1994),
Bialystok (1997), Kellerman (1995), and others, and having investigated a population that differed significantly from Johnson and Newports in terms of native language, education, and socioeconomic status.
This study provided an explanation for why certain learners and certain
518
Robert M. DeKeyser
Critical Period
519
This restriction of the concept of critical period does not automatically answer the question of its ultimate cause, but it is an important step in that direction. Most literature reviews mention four different hypotheses: (a) Adults
are more self-conscious and less able to identify with speakers of the target
language than children, (b) adults receive less adequate input than children
do, (c) adults have different cognitive mechanisms, and (d) adults show the
effects of reduced neurological plasticity (see, e.g., Birdsong, 1999; Harley,
1986; Harley & Wang, 1997; Long, 1990; Singleton, 1989). If the critical period
is defined as an absolute phenomenon, in the sense of unavoidable loss at a
certain age of the capacity to induce abstract patterns implicitly, then affective variables are clearly an inadequate explanation. They may explain part of
the large variance typically seen for adults (see Figure 1; see, e.g., also Johnson & Newport, 1989; Krashen, 1981; Patkowski, 1990); in other words, they
may correlate with ultimate achievement, but they show too much interindividual variation to account for as absolute a phenomenon as the critical period. (For other arguments against affect as an explanation for age effects, see
Long, 1990.)
Input to adults must also vary from one individual situation to another.
More importantly, input differences are not a good explanation for age effects,
because it is precisely in the linguistic domain where input varies the least
phonologythat the age effects are most readily apparent, and it is at the
stage where the comprehensibility of input should be the least problematicin the later stages of acquisitionthat adults clearly perform worse
than children. (For other arguments against input as an explanatory factor,
see Long, 1990.)
As neither input nor affective variables can explain the range of phenomena observed in the literature, differences in cognitive functioning must certainly be involved; to what extent these follow from independent neurological
changes is still an open question. It may be that the severe decline of the ability to induce abstract patterns implicitly is an inevitable consequence of fairly
general aspects of neurological maturation and that it simply shows up most
clearly in language acquisition, because any human language is an exceedingly
complex web of highly abstract patterns. It may also be that (not neurologically determined) developmental differences in memory are involved, either
quantitative (Deacon, 1997, ch. 4; Elman et al., 1996; Goldowsky & Newport,
1993; Newport, 1990; Kareev, Lieberman, & Lev, 1997) or qualitative in the
sense of cognitive restructuring (Birdsong, 1994; Flege, 1992; Karmiloff-Smith,
1992; Wode, 1994). To what extent the change that researchers call the critical period phenomenon follows from deeper cognitive principles, and to what
extent these principles may follow from aspects of neurological maturation is
a question that will probably be with us for some time to come.
Further research should be carried out with other structures in other languages to ascertain that the most salient structures are learned by virtually
all adult learners, and the less salient abstract patterns only by those with
high verbal ability. If that research confirms the patterns found in this study,
520
Robert M. DeKeyser
the search for a neurological basis for the maturational decline in learning will
come into clearer focus: Is there anything in developmental cognitive neuropsychology that can explain why the ability to induce abstract patterns implicitly declines with age?
In the meantime, it is important that the practical implications of age effects in L2 learning not be overstated. The maturational effects described in
this study do not mean, for instance, that children should simply learn a foreign language in elementary school rather than in high school (Patkowski,
1994). Children are better than adults at acquiring a language implicitly, not
at figuring out its structure explicitly (on the contrary, adolescents and adults
are far better at that because of their higher level of general cognitive maturity). Implicit acquisition processes, however, require massive amounts of input, which only a total immersion program can provide, not a program
consisting of a few hours of foreign language teaching per week. The findings
of this study do imply, however, that explicit learning processes are a necessary condition for achieving a high level of competence in a nonnative language after childhood. Therefore foreign language teaching policies that deny
explicit focus on form to academically oriented adults, who can handle such
analytical approach of linguistic structure, should be considered as fundamentally flawed. They deny learners with high analytic ability the use of the only
mechanism at their disposal to master certain basic structures in the L2.
(Received 6 August 1999)
NOTES
1. Felix (1985) also saw age effects as the inevitable result of a shift from language-specific implicit learning mechanisms to explicit learning through general problem-solving skills, but in his view
explicit learning is a competitor that, as soon as it is fully developed, starts interfering with the
implicit mechanisms. This view seems to imply, though, that the stronger a persons problem-solving
skills are, the worse language learning will be; this implication is clearly not borne out by the facts
as discussed later in this paper.
2. Learners in the 1218 age range are too young to immigrate by themselves, whereas their parents are likely to be around 40 years of age already and therefore too old to be likely to immigrate
(note that neither the Johnson and Newport [1989] study nor the present one included any participants whose age of arrival was above 40). Bialystok and Hakuta (1999) reported a continuous decline
throughout the lifespan in a study with thousands of subjects, presumably including a fair number
in the 1218 range, but the outcome measure was a global self-rating, which is likely to have been
influenced by the popular conception that one learns language skills less well as one gets older.
3. Oyama (1978) found no difference in aural comprehension at various levels of noise interference between Italian learners of ESL who had been in the United States for 511 years and those
whose length of residence was 1218 years.
4. It is well known that grammaticality judgments do not constitute a perfectly valid measure of
linguistic competence (for thorough discussions see, e.g., Cowart, 1997, and Schutze, 1996). One of
the most frequently made objections is the role that metalinguistic competence plays in grammaticality judgments. The importance of this (generally valid) concern is mitigated in this study because
of the simple nature of both the stimuli (in terms of vocabulary and syntax) and the format (yes-no).
Furthermore, the lack of significant correlation between performance on the grammaticality judgment test and the language-learning aptitude test (see the following section) for the prepuberty arrivals seems to confirm that metalinguistic competence at the time of taking the test was not a major
causal factor in the grammaticality judgment scores of the participants in this study.
5. The low scores on the aptitude test may have been due in part to the fact that the participants
Critical Period
521
did not use Hungarian as extensively as monolinguals. Otto (1996b) did not give average scores for
his monolingual sample, but given that the mean level of item difficulty was .52, the average scores
must have been around 10. Given the low average aptitude scores in this study, they are to be
treated as relative rather than absolute values.
6. Figure 3 in Johnson and Newport (1989, p. 87) is a better basis for comparison with the correlations in the present study than the correlation coefficients they list on p. 88, because these coefficients obscure whether a lower correlation is due to older learners doing well or to younger arrivals
not doing as well as one might expect. As the proportion of young learners in my study is much
smaller than in Johnson and Newports, the difference between the two oldest (>16) and four youngest groups (<16) in their Figure 3 is a better point of comparison for the correlation coefficients in
the present study, which largely reflect the difference between those who arrived after age 16 and
those who arrived before.
7. Johnson and Newport (1989, p. 87, fn. 8) interpreted the items with missing do-support as a
subcategory of yes-no question problems that reflects word-order errors. In fact, in these sentences,
the word order always shows the correct inversion pattern in the sense that the tensed verb comes
before the subject; the error is one of lack of do-support (which entails the erroneous tensing of the
lexical verb). It is true, however, that the order of the subject and the lexical verb is incorrect, and
learners may, therefore, perceive the items as evidencing an incorrect word order. The fact that the
items in question fall into the easy group along with other word-order items, and not into the difficult
group with other items in which the auxiliary is lacking (WHNA) or not inverted (WHNI), lends support to Johnson and Newports point of view that the learners experience these yes-no questions
without do-support as word order errors rather than as auxiliary errors.
REFERENCES
Asher, J. J., & Garca, R. (1969). The optimal age to learn a foreign language. The Modern Language
Journal, 53, 334341.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (1987). Markedness and salience in second-language acquisition. Language Learning, 37, 385407.
Bialystok, E. (1997). The structure of age: In search of barriers to second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 13, 116137.
Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1994). In other words: The science and psychology of second-language acquisition. New York: BasicBooks.
Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1999). Confounded age: Linguistic and cognitive factors in age differences
for second language acquisition. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the Critical
Period Hypothesis (pp. 161181). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Birdsong, D. (1992). Ultimate attainment in second language acquisition. Language, 68, 706755.
Birdsong, D. (1994). Decision making in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 169182.
Birdsong, D. (1999). Introduction: Whys and why nots of the Critical Period Hypothesis for second
language acquisition. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the Critical Period
Hypothesis (pp. 122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bley-Vroman, R. (1988). The fundamental character of foreign language learning. In W. Rutherford &
M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and second language teaching: A book of readings (pp. 19
30). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Bongaerts, T. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 pronunciation: The case of very advanced L2 learners. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 133
159). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bongaerts, T., Planken, B., & Schils, E. (1995). Can late starters attain a native accent in a foreign
language? In D. Singleton & Z. Lengyel (Eds.), The age factor in second language acquisition (pp.
3050). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Bongaerts, T., van Summeren, C., Planken, B., & Schils, E. (1997). Age and ultimate attainment in the
pronunciation of a foreign language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 447465.
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In K. C. Diller (Ed.),
Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Carroll, J. B. (1990). Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: Then and now. In T. S. Parry &
C. W. Stansfield (Eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered (pp. 1129). Washington, DC: Center for
Applied Linguistics.
522
Robert M. DeKeyser
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test: Form A. New York: The Psychological
Corporation.
Clark, H. (1973). The language as fixed effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335359.
Coppieters, R. (1987). Competence differences between native and near-native speakers. Language,
63, 544573.
Corno, L., & Snow, R. E. (1986). Adapting teaching to individual differences among learners. In M. C.
Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 605629). New York: Macmillan.
Cowart, W. (1997). Experimental syntax: Applying objective methods to sentence judgments. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1981). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on
interactions. New York: Irvington.
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York:
Norton.
Doughty, C. (1991). Second language instruction does make a difference. Evidence from an empirical
study of SL relativization. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 431469.
Eckman, F. R., Moravcsik, E. A., & Wirth, J. R. (1989). Implicational universals and interrogative structures in the interlanguage of ESL learners. Language Learning, 39, 173205.
Ekstrand, L. H. (1976). Age and length of residence as variables related to the adjustments of migrant
children, with special reference to second language learning. In G. Nickel (Ed.), Proceedings of
the Fourth International Congress of Applied Linguistics (pp. 179197). Stuttgart, Germany: Hochschul.
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Eubank, L., & Gregg, K. (1999). Critical periods and (second) language acquisition: Divide et impera.
In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 6599).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Felix, S. (1985). More evidence on competing cognitive systems. Second Language Research, 1, 4772.
Flege, J. E. (1992). Speech learning in a second language. In C. A. Ferguson & C. Stoel-Gammon (Eds.),
Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 565604). Timonium, MD: York
Press.
Flege, J. E. (1999). Age of learning and second language speech. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language
acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 101131). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Flege, J., Takagi, N., & Mann, V. (1995). Japanese adults can learn to produce English /r/ and /l/ accurately. Language and Speech, 38, 2555.
Flynn, S., & Manuel, S. (1991). Age-dependent effects in language acquisition: An evaluation of critical period hypotheses. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the second
language (pp. 117145). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Goldowsky, B. N., & Newport, E. L. (1993). Modeling the effects of processing limitations on the acquisition of morphology: The less is more hypothesis. In E. Clark (Ed.), The proceedings of the 24th
annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 124138). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language (pp. 5890). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Harley, B. (1986). Age in second language acquisition. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Harley, B., & Hart, D. (1997). Language aptitude and second language proficiency in classroom learners of different starting ages. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 379400.
Harley, B., & Wang, W. (1997). The Critical Period Hypothesis: Where are we now? In A. M. B. de
Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 1951). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hyltenstam, K. (1992). Non-native features of near-native speakers: On the ultimate attainment of
childhood L2 learners. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 351368). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., El Tigi, M., & Moselle, M. (1994). Reexamining the Critical Period Hypothesis:
A case study of successful adult SLA in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 16, 7398.
Johnson, J. S. (1992). Critical period effects in second language acquisition: The effect of written
versus auditory materials on the assessment of grammatical competence. Language Learning,
42, 217248.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influ-
Critical Period
523
ence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1991). Critical period effects on universal properties of language:
The status of subjacency in the acquisition of a second language. Cognition, 39, 215258.
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject
and object asymmetries in wh-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 483516.
Kareev, Y., Lieberman, I., & Lev, M. (1997). Through a narrow window: Sample size and the perception of correlation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 278287.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: Developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kellerman, E. (1995). Age before beauty. In L. Eubank, L. Selinker, & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), The
current state of interlanguage: Studies in honor of William E. Rutherford (pp. 219231). Amsterdam:
Benjamins.
Kenesei, I., Vago, R. M., & Fenyvesi, A. (1998). Hungarian. New York: Routledge.
Krashen, S. D. (1981). Aptitude and attitude in relation to second language acquisition and learning.
In K. C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 155
175). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Krashen, S. D., Long, M., & Scarcella, R. (1979). Age, rate, and eventual attainment in second language
acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 13, 573582.
Lee, D. (1992). Universal Grammar, learnability, and the acquisition of English reflexive binding by L1
Korean speakers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los
Angeles.
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251285.
Long, M. (1993). Second language acquisition as a function of age: Research findings and methodological issues. In K. Hyltenstam & A. Viberg (Eds.), Progression and regression in language (pp. 196
221). New York: Cambridge University Press.
McLaughlin, B. (1990). The relationship between first and second languages: Language proficiency
and language aptitude. In B. Harley, P. Allen, J. Cummins, & M. Swain (Eds.), The development of
second language proficiency (pp. 158174). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology: The critical factors of age, motivation, and
instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 81108.
Munro, M. J., Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A. (1996). The effects of age of second language learning on
the production of English vowels. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 313334.
Neufeld, G. (1978). On the acquisition of prosodic and articulatory features in adult language learning. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 163174.
Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 1128.
Otto, I. (1996a). The development of a language aptitude test for Hungarian learners of foreign languages. Unpublished masters thesis, Eotvos Lorand University, Budapest.
Otto, I. (1996b). Hungarian language aptitude test: Words in sentences. Unpublished manuscript, Department of English Applied Linguistics, Eotvos Lorand University, Budapest.
Oyama, S. (1976). A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system. Journal
of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 261283.
Oyama, S. (1978). The sensitive period and comprehension of speech. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 16, 117.
Patkowski, M. (1980). The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language. Language Learning, 30, 449472.
Patkowski, M. S. (1990). Age and accent in a second language: A reply to James Emil Flege. Applied
Linguistics, 11, 7389.
Patkowski, M. S. (1994). The Critical Age Hypothesis and interlanguage phonology. In M. Yavas (Ed.),
First and second language phonology (pp. 205221). San Diego, CA: Singular.
Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1991). Implicit acquisition of abstract knowledge about artificial grammar: Some methodological and conceptual issues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
120, 112116.
Pulvermuller, F., & Schumann, J. H. (1994). Neurobiological mechanisms of language acquisition. Language Learning, 44, 681734.
Reber, A., Kassin, S., Lewis, S., & Cantor, G. (1980). On the relationship between implicit and explicit
modes in the learning of a complex rule structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Learning and Memory, 6, 492502.
524
Robert M. DeKeyser
Redington, M., & Chater, N. (1996). Transfer in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 125, 123138.
Robinson, P. (1996). Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental,
rule-search, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 2767.
Sasaki, M. (1993). Relationships among second language proficiency, foreign language aptitude, and
intelligence: A structural equation modeling approach. Language Learning, 43, 313344.
Schachter, J. (1989). Testing a proposed universal. In S. M. Gass & J. Schachter (Eds.), Linguistic
perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 7388). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schachter, J. (1990). On the issue of completeness in second language acquisition. Second Language
Research, 6, 93124.
Schachter, J. (1996). Maturation and universal grammar. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 159193). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schutze, C. (1996). The empirical base of linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak: A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech.
Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Seliger, H. W. (1978). Implications of a multiple critical periods hypothesis for second language learning. In W. C. Ritchie (Ed.), Second language acquisition research: Issues and implications (pp. 11
19). New York: Academic Press.
Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367395.
Singleton, D. (1989). Language acquisition: The age factor. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Skehan, P. (1986). The role of foreign language aptitude in a model of school learning. Language Testing, 3, 188221.
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second language learning. London: Edward Arnold.
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Slavoff, G. R., & Johnson, J. S. (1995). The effects of age on the rate of learning a second language.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 116.
Snow, C., & Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from
second language learning. Child Development, 49, 11141128.
Snow, R. E. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in learning. In P. L. Ackerman, R. J. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Learning and individual
differences: Advances in theory and research (pp. 1359). New York: Freeman.
Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. (1984). Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 347376.
Sorace, A. (1993). Incomplete vs. divergent representations of unaccusativity in non-native grammars
of Italian. Second Language Research, 9, 2247.
Tahta, S., Wood, M., & Loewenthal, K. (1981). Foreign accents: Factors relating to transfer from the
first language to a second language. Language and Speech, 24, 265272.
Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, C. T. (1962). Primary Mental Abilities Test. Chicago: Science Research
Associates.
Walsh, T., & Diller, K. C. (1981). Neurolinguistic considerations on the optimum age for second language learning. In K. C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 321). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Wesche, M., Edwards, H., & Wells, W. (1982). Foreign language aptitude and intelligence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 127140.
White, L., & Genesee, F. (1996). How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult
second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12, 233365.
Wode, H. (1994). Nature, nurture, and age in language acquisition: The case of speech perception.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 325345.
Critical Period
525
APPENDIX A
Table A1. Main variables for all participants
Age of
arrival
Age
at test
Grammaticality
judgment test score
Aptitude
test score
Years of
schooling
28
8
11
20
23
22
30
27
27
9
11
13
27
27
23
26
21
23
17
4
1
25
20
34
25
22
33
12
35
29
28
26
3
40
33
37
21
26
26
10
28
25
26
22
20
68
46
51
59
60
56
75
50
64
54
57
58
67
66
63
66
60
62
56
53
40
65
60
70
39
58
73
28
75
69
64
65
49
54
59
60
61
40
51
55
72
35
76
68
30
136
170
181
132
139
153
141
147
175
198
194
196
170
126
110
119
134
118
167
193
199
122
126
143
129
190
143
195
175
153
184
129
197
125
146
111
127
123
151
194
143
176
176
164
152
3
4
5
5
10
8
5
9
9
4
8
4
4
4
3
5
5
6
4
5
4
2
2
4
8
10
7
2
3
0
7
4
4
2
3
3
4
4
3
2
2
8
6
5
3
6
6
12
14
12
14
17
18
17
16
16
20
18
16
8
8
6
8
12
18
18
12
8
16
8
16
14
16
18
17
17
12
16
16
12
10
14
16
16
20
16
12
12
7
13
526
Robert M. DeKeyser
6
21
38
24
26
32
11
5
23
9
27
3
16
70
52
35
37
81
51
21
55
20
73
49
183
162
177
174
186
132
183
196
155
197
155
199
3
4
11
3
3
5
4
4
4
7
6
4
8
12
20
18
20
12
12
12
16
13
9
16
Critical Period
527
APPENDIX B
GRAMMATICALITY JUDGMENT TEST ITEMS GROUPED BY STRUCTURE
Practice Items
1. A snake bit she on the leg.*
2. Susan is making some cookies for us.
3. The baby bird has fall from the oak tree.*
4. The little boy was counting all his pennies last night.
Past Tense
A. Past tense marking omitted in obligatory context (PTMO).
1. JN
JN
2. DK
DK
3. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
4. JN
JN
5. JN
JN
6. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
7. JN
JN
8. DK
DK
9. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
Plural
A. Plural marking omitted in obligatory context (PLMO).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
4. JN
JN
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
528
Robert M. DeKeyser
7. DK
DK
8. DK
DK
9. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
Third-Person Singular
A. Third-person -s omitted in obligatory context (TPSO).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
4. DK
DK
5. DK
DK
6. DK
DK
7. JN
JN
8. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
2
1
Present Progressive
A. Progressive -ing omitted in obligatory context (PPMO).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
4. DK
DK
5. DK
DK
6. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
Determiners
A. Determiner omitted in obligatory context (DEOM).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
Critical Period
3. JN The boy is helping the man build house.*
JN The boy is helping the man build a house.
529
1
2
4. DK
DK
5. DK
DK
6. DK
DK
7. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
Pronominalization
A. Pronoun omitted in obligatory context
(transfer from Hungarian object pro-drop) (PROM).
1. DK
DK
2. DK
DK
3. DK
DK
4. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
5. JN
JN
6. DK
DK
7. DK
DK
8. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
Particle Movement
A. Phrasal verb separation not allowed (PMSE).
1. JN
JN
2. DK
DK
3. DK
DK
4. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
B. Phrasal verb separation allowed, but particle moved too far (PMTF).
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
7. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
530
Robert M. DeKeyser
1
2
Subcategorization (SUBC)
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. DK
DK
4. DK
DK
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
7. JN
JN
8. JN
JN
9. JN
JN
10. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
Yes-No Questions
A. *aux Aux s[ . . . (YNAA).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
4. JN
JN
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
C. *V s[ . . . (YNVS).
7. JN
JN
8. JN
JN
9. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
1
2
Critical Period
11. JN
JN
12. JN
JN
531
2
1
1
2
Wh-Questions
A. No aux inversion (WHNI).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
B. No aux (WHNA).
4. JN
JN
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
Word Order
A. S V DO order violated (WODO).
1. JN
JN
2. JN
JN
3. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
4. JN
JN
5. JN
JN
6. JN
JN
1
2
2
1
1
2
7. JN
JN
8. JN
JN
9. JN
JN
2
1
1
2
2
1
10. DK
DK
11. DK
DK
1
2
2
1
532
Robert M. DeKeyser
1
2
13. DK
DK
14. DK
DK
15. DK
DK
2
1
1
2
2
1
Critical Period
533
APPENDIX C
ADAPTATIONS MADE TO THE TEST ITEMS IN JOHNSON
AND NEWPORT (1989)
1. Four practice items were added at the beginning of the test.
2. In cases (pertaining to the categories Word Order and Subcategorization) where there were
three items or options rather than two, the number of items was reduced to two (one correct,
one incorrect), in line with all other cases.
3. Three sentences were modified on the advice of Johnson and Newport (J & N).
4. Two items were deleted on J & Ns advice (these items were not counted in their analysis).
5. Two J & N items had a violation of agreement on top of another grammar problem; this agreement error was eliminated.
6. Some items that were correct, but slightly awkward, were modified.
7. Three items labeled as Inappropriate Context in J & N were deleted because of the semantic
rather than morphosyntactic nature of the error (past tense, third-person singular, present
progressive).
8. A few items labeled as Questions with Tense, a subdivision of both the Past Tense and
Third-Person Singular categories, were moved to the category Yes-No Questions.
9. Some nouns used to test the mass-count distinction were changed to items known to lead to
transfer problems in Hungarian speakers.
10. A few items with erroneous -s marking on the main verb following a model were added to the
category Third-Person Singular, including two items that J & N listed in the subdivision Tense
Placement of the Aux Rule category.
11. The Present Progressive category now includes two items from the Be Takes -ing subdivision
of J & Ns Aux Rule category; two simpler items were dropped; items in which the progressive
auxiliary is omitted were added.
12. Part B of the category Determiners was made more homogeneous; all items are now abstract
nouns (transfer of the determiner from Hungarian is expected).
13. In the category Pronominalization, the nominative- versus objective-case items were dropped,
as well as the possessive adjective items; examples of pronoun omission were included
(transfer from Hungarian object pro-drop is expected); reflexives were dropped as a category;
gender errors were added (including one item from J&Ns Reflexives category).
14. In the category Particle Movement, items that concerned prepositional phrases rather than
particles were deleted and replaced by others (some for which separating is okay, some for
which it is not).
15. The category Aux Rule was eliminated (but two items from its subcategory Tense Placement
are now classified under Third-Person Singular).
16. In the category Subcategorization, items 5 and 6 were replaced by other items specifically
chosen to catch transfer from Hungarian.
17. In the category Yes-No Questions, the subcategory Invert + Stay was replaced with items
showing double marking (present and past).
18. In the category Word Order, items were added in which the SVPP order is violated, and (erroneous) SVAO items were added.
19. In many categories, the number of items was reduced in order to limit the test to 200 items
(from mostly four- to mostly three-item pairs per subcategory).