Theories of Attitude Formation and Change
Theories of Attitude Formation and Change
Theories of Attitude Formation and Change
Learning theories of attitude change, no longer as popular as they once were, focus
on reinforced behavior as the primary factor responsible for attitude development.
Early research on attitude change drew on Festinger's cognitive dissonance theory,
which posits that, when a person is persuaded to act in a way that is not congruent
with a pre-existing attitude, he or she may change the attitude to reduce
dissonance (Smith & Ragan, 1999). To use dissonance to produce attitude change,
the persuader must first establish the dissonance, and then provide a method to
reduce it. Ideally, this will involve making the chosen alternative attractive, showing
a social group with the desired attitude, demonstrating the issue's importance,
providing free choice, and establishing a wide latitude of acceptance through
successive approximation (Martin & Briggs, 1986).
Although the fact that attitudes are stored separately from their related cognitions
means that a person may experience a feeling without remembering the
information or event that triggered it, attitudes will generally be stronger when the
link between their cognitive and affective components is consciously recalled
(Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991). For this to work, of course, the recipient must attend to
the message providing that information. A tendency toward passive viewing of
mediated messages may be reduced by instructing students to attend and alerting
them to the fact that the content will be tested (Wetzel et al., 1994). According to
Zimbardo and Leippe (1991), "a persuasive message is most likely to cause attitude
and behavior change if it can shape both beliefs about its topic and beliefs about
what important individuals and social groups think about the topic and how they
behave toward it" (p. 188). The most effective persuasive messages are those "that
get the audience to think about an issue or object in concrete, vivid images that
have definite implications for behavior" (Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991, p. 194).
Social judgment theories emphasize the role of prior attitudes in shaping attitude
formation and change. They describe attitude as a kind of spectrum with a "latitude
of acceptance" surrounding a current attitude; a new position is more likely to be
accepted if it falls within this latitude and less likely to be accepted if it does not
(Smith & Ragan, 1999). This theory suggests that change in attitude position might
be greater in response to the presentation of a moderate persuasive position than in
response to a more extreme message. As with dissonance theory, social judgment
theory presents attitude change as a response to the receipt of a message that is
not entirely congruent with the currently held attitude. Acceptance of the new
position is contingent upon its falling within the latitude of acceptance of the
receiver. "The use of successive approximations can expand the latitude of
acceptance and thereby permit greater attitude change than might otherwise be
possible" (Bednar & Levie, 1993, p. 295). The latitude of acceptance is analogous to
the zone of proximal development in social development theory as discussed in the
chapter on Vygotsky's Constructionism.
their effectiveness (Bednar & Levie, 1993). Social learning theories of attitude
change are closely related to theories that emphasize the role of social learning in
cognitive development. See the chapters on Social Constructivism and Cognitive
Apprenticeship, for example, for discussions of the importance of the social context
for cognitive development. Social learning theory also shares cognitive
apprenticeship's emphasis on modeling as a way of sharing knowledge.
There have been several attempts to classify types and levels of learning in the
affective domain. Perhaps the best-known classification was developed by
Krathwohl, Bloom, and Masia in 1964 (Smith & Ragan, 1999). The Krathwohl
taxonomy, as it is known, has five major categories, each with several subcategories. These levels are:
The following table provides an overview of the basic premises and key instructional
implications of these theories.