How To Negotiate With A Liar PDF
How To Negotiate With A Liar PDF
How To Negotiate With A Liar PDF
BROSMIND
obust social psychology research indicates that people lieand lie often. One
prominent study found that people tell, on average, one or two lies every day.
Negotiators are no exception. Judging from studies done in 1999 and 2005, roughly
half of those making deals will lie when they have a motive and the opportunity to do so.
Typically they see it as a way to gain the upper hand (although it can actually cause backlash and
prevent the kind of creative problem solving that leads to win-win deals). Deception is thus one
of the intangibles that negotiators have to prepare for and take steps to prevent.
Many people assume that the solution is to get better at detecting deception. Theres a
widespread notion that one can reliably spot a liar through subtle behavioral cuesor tells, in
the parlance of poker and other games that involve blung. But the evidence doesnt support
that belief. One meta-analysis (a study of studies) found that people can correctly identify
whether someone is telling a lie only 54% of the timenot much better odds than a coin ip. Even
the polygrapha technology specically engineered to detect lies in a controlled settingis
riddled with problems and comes to the wrong conclusion about a third of the time. Humans are
particularly inept at recognizing lies that are cloaked in attery: your bosss promise that a
promotion is coming any day now; the suppliers assurance that your order is his top priority.
Were wired to readily accept information that conforms to our preexisting assumptions or hopes.
Is there anything you can do to ensure youre not duped in a negotiation? Yes, if you focus on
these methods arent fail-safe, they will leave you better positioned in your deal making and help
you to create maximum value.
1. Encourage Reciprocity
Humans have a strong inclination to reciprocate disclosure: When someone shares sensitive
information with us, our instinct is to match their transparency. In fact, simply telling people that
otherseven strangershave divulged secrets encourages reciprocation. In a series of studies
that I conducted with Alessandro Acquisti and George Loewenstein, we presented readers of the
New York Times with a list of unethical behaviors, such as making a false insurance claim and
cheating on ones tax return. People who were told that most other participants had admitted
doing those things were 27% more likely to reveal that they had done likewise than were people
who were told that only a few others had made such admissions.
disclosure exercise eventually married!) Inducing a close relationship is not the primary goal of
most negotiations, of course. But other research, by Maurice Schweitzer and Rachel Croson,
shows that people lie less to those they know and trust than they do to strangers.
breakthroughs.
benet of letting you frame the negotiation, which can enhance your chances of nding
The risk of not getting the whole story is why its so important to test your negotiating partners
with direct questions. Schweitzer and Croson found that 61% of negotiators came clean when
asked about information that weakened their bargaining power, compared to 0% of those not
asked. Unfortunately, this tactic can backre. In the same experiment, 39% of negotiators who
were questioned about the information ultimately lied. But you can go a long way toward
avoiding that outcome by posing your queries carefully. Research by Julia Minson, Nicole Ruedy,
and Schweitzer indicates that people are less likely to lie if questioners make pessimistic
assumptions (This business will need some new equipment soon, right?) rather than optimistic
ones (The equipment is in good order, right?). It seems to be easier for people to lie by arming
an untrue statement than by negating a true statement.
Savvy counterparts often get around direct questions by answering not what they were asked but
what they wish theyd been asked. And, unfortunately, we are not naturally gifted at detecting
this sort of evasiveness. As Todd Rogers and Michael Norton have found, listeners usually dont
notice dodges, often because theyve forgotten what they originally asked. In fact, the
researchers discovered that people are more impressed by eloquent sidestepping than by answers
that are relevant but inarticulate.
Dodge detection is improved, however, when listeners are prompted to remember the question
for example, when it is visible as the speaker replies. In a negotiation, therefore, its a good idea to
come to the table with a list of questions, leaving space to jot down your counterparts answers.
Take time after each response to consider whether it actually provided the information you
sought. Only when the answer to that question is yes should you move on to the next issue.
My colleagues and I have discovered that strong privacy protections can also increase lying. In
addition, weve found that when questions are posed in a casual tone rather than a formal one,
people are more likely to divulge sensitive information. Imagine you are negotiating a job oer
with a prospective employee and would like to assess the strength of her other options: Does she
have competitive oers? Shes likely to be more forthcoming if you avoid or at least minimize
condentiality assurances and instead nonchalantly broach the topic: We all know there are tons
of great rms out there. Any chance you might be considering other places? Of course, you
should still properly protect any condential information you receive, but theres no reason to
announce that unless asked.
5. Cultivate Leaks
People inadvertently leak information in all kinds of ways, including in their own questions. For
example, suppose you are in charge of procurement for a rm and youre about to sign a contract
with a supplier who has promised to deliver goods within six months. Before signing, he asks you
what happens in the event of late delivery. The question could be innocent, but it might also
signal his worries about meeting the schedule. So you need to pay attention.
When people leak mindlessly, the information tends to be accurate. Astute negotiators realize
that valuable knowledge can be gleaned simply by listening to everything their counterparts say,
even seemingly extraneous or throwaway commentsin the same way that interrogators look for
statements from criminal suspects that include facts not known to the public.
WHAT NOT TO DO
WHAT TO DO
Redirect. In the short term, the
strategies deployed by politicians, who
routinely face tough, direct questions,
can be instructiveparticularly for oneshot negotiations (when you are unlikely
to meet your counterpart again). A
familiar tactic is to dodge the question
by changing the subject to something
seemingly related. As noted earlier,
people are generally not very good at
detecting dodges, so you have an
opportunity to selectively disclose
information of your choosing. A second
strategy is to turn the tables and
question the questioner. Responding in
this way can deect attention and enable
you to take control of the topic.
Share carefully. If youre playing a longer
game, disclosure can work in your favor;
it can foster trust and facilitate better
INFLUENCE
Comments
Leave a Comment
POST
11 COMMENTS
Aditya Sedani
a month ago
A very good read for negotiation and managerial conversation as well. At times, while negotiating it is helpful if
you anchor the deal on extreme your level and then apply the "pessimistic assumptions" mentioned in the article.
It creates additional pressure and also give other cues to know how much condent the person is in making his
statement.
But good to read and mane of such incidences we do observe or recall.
00
REPLY
POSTING GUIDELINES
We hope the conversations that take place on HBR.org will be energetic, constructive, and thought-provoking. To comment, readers must sign in or
register. And to ensure the quality of the discussion, our moderating team will review all comments and may edit them for clarity, length, and
relevance. Comments that are overly promotional, mean-spirited, or off-topic may be deleted per the moderators' judgment. All postings become
the property of Harvard Business Publishing.